Anda di halaman 1dari 13

Variance uctuations in nonstationary time series: a comparative study of music genres

Heather D. Jennings a,b, Plamen Ch. Ivanov c, A. M. Martins d,e, P. C. da Silva d and G. M. Viswanathan c,d,f
de Artes, Universidade Federal de Alagoas, MaceiAL, 57072970, Brazil o
b Department a Departamento

arXiv:cond-mat/0312380 v3 21 Dec 2003

of Music, Wesleyan University, Middletown, CT 06459, USA for Polymer Studies and Department of Physics, Boston University, Boston, MA 02215, USA

c Center

de Fsica Terica e Experimental, o Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, NatalRN, 59072-970, Brazil de Engenharia Eltrica, e Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, NatalRN, 59072-970, Brazil de Fsica, Universidade Federal de Alagoas, MaceiAL, 57072970, Brazil o
f Departamento e Departamento

d Departamento

Abstract An important problem in physics concerns the analysis of audio time series generated by transduced acoustic phenomena. Here, we develop a new method to quantify the scaling properties of the local variance of nonstationary time series. We apply this technique to analyze audio signals obtained from selected genres of music. We nd quantitative dierences in the correlation properties of high art music, popular music, and dance music. We discuss the relevance of these objective ndings in relation to the subjective experience of music. Key words: Time series analysis, variance uctuations, music PACS: 05.45.T, 05.40.F, 43.75

Introduction

An important problem in physics concerns the study of sound. Music consists of a complex Fourier superposition of sinusoidal waveforms. A person with very good hearing can hear continuous single frequency (monochromatic)
Preprint submitted to Elsevier Science 21 December 2003

musical tones in the range 20 Hz to 20 kHz [1]. Audio CD players can reproduce high delity music using a 44 kHz sampling rate for two channels of 16 bit audio signals, corresponding to a maximum audible frequency of 22 kHz [1,2], according to the Nyquist sampling theorem. In practice, band pass or other lters limit the range of frequencies to the audible spectrum referred to above. Systematic studies of the amazing complexity of music have focused primarily on using FFT- or DFT-based spectral techniques that detect power densities in frequency intervals [1,3,4,5]. For example, 1/f -type noise in music has received considerable attention [3]. Another approach to musical complexity involves studies of the entropy and of the fractal dimension of pitch variations in music [6]. Such systematic analyses have shown that music has interesting scaling properties and long-range correlations. However, quantifying the differences between qualitatively dierent categories of music [7,8] still remains a challenge. Here, we adapt recently developed methods of statistical physics that have found successful application in studying nancial time series [9], DNA sequences [10] and heart rate dynamics [11]. Specically, we develop a new adaptation of Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (DFA) [12,13,14] to study nonstationary uctuations in the local variance [9] of time seriesrather than in the original time seriesby calculating a function (t) that quanties correlations on time scale t. This method can detect deviations from uniform power law scaling [10,11,13,14] embedded in scale invariant local variance uctuation patterns. We apply this new method to study correlations in highly nonstationary local variance (i.e., loudness) uctuations occurring in audio time series [4,9]. We then study the relationship of such objectively measurable correlations to known subjective, qualitative musical aspects that characterize selected genres of music. We show that the correlation properties of popular music, high art music, and dance music dier quantitatively from each other.

Methods

The loudness of music perceived by the human auditory system grows as a monotonically increasing function of the average intensity. One typically measures the intensity of sound signals in dB (deci-Bells or decibels) [1,2,15]. Hence, one conventionally also measures loudness in dB, even though the subjectively perceived loudness scales as a non-linear function of the intensity [15]. The subjective perception of loudness varies according to frequency and depends also on ear sensitivity, which in turn can depend on age, sex, medication, etc (see, e.g., Refs. [1,2,15]). For all practical purposes, however, the objective measurement of sound intensity provides a good means to quantify loudness. In the remainder of this article, we use the term loudness to refer to the instantaneous value of the running or moving average of the intensity. 2

An important fact that deserves a detailed explanation concerns how the human ear cannot perceive any variation in loudness (i.e., amplitude modulation) that occurs at frequencies f > 20 Hz. Humans hear frequencies in the audible range 20 Hz < f < 20 kHz and therefore do not perceive amplitude modulation or instantaneous intensity uctuations in this frequency range as variations in loudness, but rather as having constant loudness. We briey explain this point as follows. We can consider the the human auditory system, in a limiting approximation, as a time-to-frequency transducer that operates in the audible range of 20 Hz < f < 20 kHz. Any monochromatic signal in this frequency range will lead to the perception of an audible tone of that same frequency or pitch. A linear combination of such signals can give a number of impressions to the human ear, depending on the exact Fourier decomposition of the signal. Specically, a combination of monochromatic signals may sound as having a nontrivial timbre, [1,15] and if the signal frequencies have special arithmetic relationships, then they may sound as a harmony [1,15]. Beats and heterodyning, for two or more closely spaced frequencies, can also arise. Most importantly, a linear superposition of monochromatic signals can sound either as having constant loudness, or else as having varying loudness. We discuss this last point in some detail: If a monochromatic carrier signal U of frequency f becomes amplitude modulated by a modulating signal v of frequency fM f , then the Fourier decomposition of the modulated signal Uv will include monochromatic sidebands of sum and dierence frequencies f fM , but no power at frequency fM [16]. Moreover, amplitude modulation with fM < 20 Hz results in sidebands close to the carrier frequency, whereas fM > 20 Hz leads to signicant changes in the perceived sound timbre, due to the distant sidebands f fM . Indeed, if fM > 20 Hz, the sidebands fall far enough away from the carrier to enable the ear to pick up the sidebands as having distinct frequencies, thereby leading to the perception of a changed timbre. Only if fM < 20 Hz do the sidebands fall suciently close to the carrier to fool the auditory system into perceiving a monochromatic signal of varying loudness. Specically, humans hear fM < 8 Hz as a tremolo (i.e., a periodic oscillation in the intensity of the carrier tone), whereas for 8 Hz < fM < 20 Hz we perceive a transition from the tremolo eect to the timbre eect (see Refs. [1,15] for more information). The reader should not confuse tremolos with vibratos, which arise from frequency modulation rather than amplitude modulation. We now devise methods suitable for studying the scaling properties of the intensity of music signals over a range of times scales [1,2,4]. We begin with selected pieces of music taken from CDs and digitize them using 8 bit sampling at fs =11 kHz. Since each piece lasts several minutes, therefore, this low 11 kHz bit sampling rate suces for obtaining excellent statistics. Similarly, since we aim not to listen to music, but to study correlations in intensity, 8 bit sound adequately satises basic signal-to-noise requirements (better than 3

100 : 1). We choose 4 min stretches of music, and to each piece of music assign a time series U(i), where 0 U(i) (28 1) and i represents the sample index (Fig. 1(a)). We generate another series v(j) dened as the standard deviation of every non-overlapping 110 samples of U(i). The variance [v(j)]2 thus represents the average intensity of the sound (loudness) over intervals of 0.01 s (Fig. 1(b)). Concerning the choice of the windowing time interval, we have found the exact value of the time interval to have little or no importance; we have veried that our central results do not depend on the exact value chosen, since we aim to study uctuations in the intensity of the signal. We have found, e.g., that using a time interval ve times larger, 0.05 s, equivalent to the minimum audible tone frequency of 20 Hz, leads to no signicant changes to our main results. In this context, we note that the measurement of the loudness of music has some similarities to the measurement of volatility in nancial markets, since in both cases the variance measurement eectively involves a moving window of xed but arbitrary size [9]. We dene the power spectrum S(k) of the signal as the modulus squared of the discrete Fourier transform U (k) of U(i): S(f ) |U (k)|2 , (1)

where f = fs k = 11 000 k represents the frequency measured in Hz. At the lowest frequencies, the spectrum appears distorted by artifacts of the fast Fourier transform (FFT) method. Specically, at small frequencies approaching 1/N, where N represents the FFT window size, a spurious contribution arises from the treatment of the data as periodic with period N [17]. The last few decades have seen extensive studies of the audio power spectra, considered nowadays well understood (Fig. 1(c)). The spectral power in the range 20 Hz < f < 20 kHz arises due to audible sounds, while lower frequency contributions emerge due to the structure of the music on sub-audible scales larger than 201 s (see Fig. 1(c)). Since we primarily aim to study loudness uctuations at these larger time scales t > 201 s, we nd it more convenient to study the power spectrum S (f ) of the series v(j) rather than of the series U(i). This spectrum allows us to study correlations related to loudness at these higher time scales. However, v(j) behaves as a highly nonstationary variable and the power spectrum of nonstationary signals may not converge in a well behaved manner. Therefore, conclusions drawn from such spectra may lead to questions about their validity. In order to circumvent these limitations, we use DFA. Like the power spectrum, DFA can measure two-point correlations in time series, however unlike power spectra, DFA also works with nonstationary signals [10,11,13,14,18]. The DFA method has been systematically compared with other algorithms for measuring fractal correlations in Ref. [19], and Refs. [13,14] contain compre4

hensive studies of DFA. We use the variant of the DFA method described in Ref. [20]. We dene the net displacement y(n) of the sequence v by y(n) n v(j), which can be thought of graphically as a one-dimensional j random walk. We divide the sequence y(n) into a number of overlapping subsequences of length , each shifted with respect to the previous subsequence by a single sample. For each subsequence, we apply linear regression to calculate an interpolated detrended walk y (n) a + b(n n0 ). Then we dene (y)2 , where y y(n) y (n), and the DFA uctuation by FD ( ) the angular brackets denote averaging over all points y(n). We use a moving window to obtain better statistics. We dene the DFA exponent (t) by (t) d log FD ( ) , d log ( + 3) (2)

where t = 100 gives the real time scale measured in seconds. Uncorrelated data give rise to = 1/2, as expected from the central limit theorem, while correlated data give rise to = 1/2. Specically, a value = 1/2 corresponds to uncorrelated white noise, = 1 corresponds to 1/f -type noise with complex nontrivial correlations, and = 1.5 corresponds to trivially correlated Brown noise (integrated white noise). Refs. [10,21] discuss in further detail the relationship between DFA and the power spectrum. A constant value of (t) indicates stable scaling [10,11], while departures indicate loss of uniform power law scaling. We obtain the best statistics by studying time scales that range from 100.5 s to 10 s, hence we focus on these scales.

Results

We have recorded 10 tracks from each of 9 genres: music from the Western European Classical Tradition (WECT), North Indian Hindustani music, Javanese Gamelan music, Brazilian popular music, Rock and Roll, Techno-dance music, New Age music, Jazz, and modern electronic Forr dance music (with roots o in traditional Forr, from Northeast Brazil). We have chosen these genres of o music somewhat arbitrarily, noting that our main interest lies not in the music itself but rather in developing quantitative methods of analyzing music that canin principlebe applied in future studies systematically to compare and contrast diverse audio signals originating in music. Fig. 2(a) shows the the power spectrum S (f ) of the series v(j). As noted previously, v(j) does not have stationarity and therefore the meaning of such spectra may appear ambiguous. Nevertheless, we can observe clear dierences in the spectra of each genre of music. 5

Figs. 2(b,c) show the DFA functions FD (t) and (t), respectively. Each genre of music has a dierent (t) signature. In Jazz, Javanese music, New Age music, Hindustani music and Brazilian Pop, (t) decreases with t. WECT music appears characterized by extremely high (t) in the region of interest from 100.5 s to 101.0 s, with lower values for rock and roll. Techno-dance and Forr music have characteristic (t) patterns marked by dips near 0.8 s. o These characteristics also appear in Fig. 3, which shows (t) for each data set separately. We also compute the average DFA exponent in the region of interest 100.5 s t 10 s for each genre of music (Fig. 4). We emphasize that these values of measure the scaling exponents in the variancehence, loudness uctuations of the music signals. Any conclusions derived from the results presented here must carefully consider this point.

Discussion

Javanese Gamelan and New Age, and to a lesser extent Hindustani and WECT, have the values closest to = 1, corresponding to the most complex, nontrivial correlations (1/f -type behavior). We note that WECT music has the highest value of , indicating that loudness uctuations have the strongest correlations in this genre. Hence, from the point of view of loudness level changes, WECT music appears the most correlated, and modern electronic Forr music the least correlated. None of the results reported here have a dio rect bearing on harmony, melody or other aspects of music. Our results apply only to loudness uctuations, which can reect aspects of the rhythm of the music [1]. Another observation concerns how the extremely predictable periodic rhythmic structure of Techno-dance music and Forr shows up as minima in (t) o near 0.8 s (Figs. 2(c), 3). This nding suggests that the periodic beat of the music, considered abstractly as a superposition of periodic trends and the acoustic signal, leads to signicant deviations from uniform power law scaling at that time scale [10,13,14]. The above results seem to suggest that the qualitative dierences between genreswell known to music loversmay in fact be quantiable. For example, WECT music, Hindustani music and Gamelan music, which have the highest average 1 (suggesting almost perfect 1/f scaling behavior), usually belong to the general category of high art music. On the other end, electronic Forr and Techno-dance music, where periodic tends dominate, have the lowest o average , and arguably belong to the category of dance or danceable music. The lower observed in these genres is due to a a bump and horizontal 6

shoulder in the DFA uctuation uctation FD (t) that emerges at time scales corresponding to the pronounced periodic beats [13] (see Figs. 2(c), 3). Such genres might have evolved primarily for dancing, rather than for listening. We can speculate from this point of view that Jazz, Rock and Roll, and Brazilian popular music may occupy an intermediary position between high art music and dance music: complex enough to listen to, but periodic and rhythmic enough to dance to. Finally, we discuss the relevance of these ndings to the possible eects of music on the nervous system [24]. Studies of heart rate dynamics using the DFA method have shown that healthy individuals have values relatively close to = 1, corresponding to 1/f correlations, while subjects with heart disease have higher values (typically > 1.2) that indicate a signicant shift towards less complex behavior in heart rate uctuations, since = 1.5 corresponds to trivially correlated Brown noise (e.g., see [11,22,23]). Hence, listening to certain kinds of music may conceivably bestow benets to the health of the listener [24,25,26]. The hypothesis that music with 1 confers health benets still requires systematic testing. For example, the so-called Mozart eect refers to the conjecture that listening to certain types of music may correlate with higher test scores and more generally to intelligence [24]. If ever such ndings become substantiated, then a new approach to the study of music (and perhaps other forms of art) might become a necessity. We note, however, that the Mozart eect has not been legitimately established as a real phenomenon. Nevertheless, the results reported hereand more importantly, the approach used in obtaining the results point towards the possibility of objectively analyzing subjectively experienced forms of art. Such an approach may nd relevance in the academic study of music, and of art in general. In summary, we have developed a method to study loudness uctuations in audio signals taken from music. Results obtained using this method show consistent dierences between dierent genres of music. Specically, dance music and high art music appear at the lower and upper endpoints respectively in the range of observed values of , with Rock and Roll, Jazz, and other genres appearing in the middle of the range.

Acknowledgements

We thank Ary L. Goldberger, Yongki Lee, M. G. E. da Luz, C.-K Peng, E. P. Raposo, Luciano R. da Silva and Itamar Vidal for helpful discussions. We thank CNPq and FAPEAL for nancial support. 7

References
[1] John R. Pierce, in The Psychology of Music, Ed. Diana Deutsch, Academic Press (2nd edition), 1998. [2] William M. Siebert, Circuits, Signals, and Systems, The MIT Press, Cambridge, 1986. [3] Y. L. Klimontovich and J. P. Boon, Europhys. Lett. 3 (1987) 395. [4] R. F. Voss and J. Clarke, Nature 258 (1975) 317. [5] M. Dorer, J. New Mus. Res. 30 (2001) 3. [6] J. P Boon and O. Decroly, Chaos 5 (1995) 501. [7] K. P. Han et al., Eletronics 44 (1998) 33. [8] N. P. M. Todd, G. J. Brown, Articial Intelligence Review 10 (1996) 253. [9] G. M. Viswanathan, U. L. Fulco, M. Lyra and M. Serva, Physica A 329 (2003) 273. [10] G. M. Viswanathan, S. V. Buldyrev, S. Havlin and H. E. Stanley, Biophys. Journal 72 (1997) 866. [11] G. M. Viswanathan, C.-K. Peng, H. E. Stanley and A. L. Goldberger, Physical Review E 55 (1997) 845. [12] C. K. Peng, S. V. Buldyrev, M. Simons, H. E. Stanley and A. L. Goldberger, Phys. Rev. E 49 (1994) 1695. [13] K. Hu, Plamen Ch. Ivanov, Zhi Chen, Pedro Carpena and H. E. Stanley, Phys. Rev. E 64 (2001) 011114. [14] Zhi Chen, Plamen Ch. Ivanov, Kun Hu and H. E. Stanley, Phys. Rev. E. 65 (2002) 041107. [15] B. Truax, Ed., Handbook for Acoustic Ecology [CD-ROM], Vancouver, Cambridge Street Publishing, 2001. [16] D. Panter, Modulation, Noise and Spectral Analysis, McGraw-Hill, New York, 1965. [17] W. H. Press, B. P. Flannery, S. A. Teukolsky and W. T. Vetterling, Numerical Recipes in C : The Art of Scientic Computing, Cambridge University Press, 1993. [18] Jan W. Kantelhardt, Eva Koscielny-Binde, Henio H. A. Rego, Shlomo Havlin and Armin Bunde, Physica A 295 (2001) 441. [19] M. S. Taqqu, V. Teverovsky, W. Willinger, Fractals 3 (1995) 785.

[20] S. V. Buldyrev, A. L. Goldberger, S. Havlin, R. N. Mantegna, M. E. Matsa, C.-K. Peng, M. Simons and H. E. Stanley, Phys. Rev. E 51 (1995) 5084. [21] K. Wilson, D. P. Francis, R. Wensel, et al., Physiol. Meas. 23 (2002) 385. [22] C.-K. Peng, Shlomo Havlin, H. E. Stanley and A. L. Goldberger, Chaos 5 (1995) 82. [23] P. Ch. Ivanov, L. A. N. Amaral, A. L. Goldberger, S. Havlin, M. G. Rosenblum, H. E. Stanley and Z. Struzik, Chaos 11 (2001) 641. [24] F. H. Rauscher, G. L. Shaw and K. N. Ky, Nature 365 (1993) 611. [25] A. Tornek, T. Field, M. Hernandez-Reif, et al., Psychiatry 66 (2003) 234. [26] G. Martin, M. Clarke, C. Pearce, J. Am. Acad. Child. Psy. 32 (1993) 530.

100 50
U

(a)

0 -50 -100 50 40 30 20 10 0

(b)

2 time [s]

2
(c)
=1 =0

0
log10 S(f)

-2 -4 -6
inaudible audible

-8

1 log10 f

Fig. 1. (a) The original signal U (i) and (b) local standard deviation v(j) for a 4 s stretch of music as a function of real time measured in seconds. We can relate the value of v(j) to the instantaneous loudness of the music, as described in the text. (c) double log plot of the power spectrum S(f ) as a function of frequency f measured in Hz of U (i). The human ear can only detect monochromatic tones of frequencies in the range 20 Hz < f < 20 kHz. We instead perceive frequencies f < 20 Hz as giving rise to melodic, rhythmic, speech and other such structures that have time scales t > 201 s. Such spectra have previously been studied comprehensively. Note that we nd 1/f -type behavior for audible frequencies. The spectrum scales approximately as S(f ) f , with 1. In contrast, for lower frequencies we nd behavior more reminiscent of white noise, with 0. Such spectra, while useful for studying power densities in audible frequencies, do not easily adapt to the study of loudness uctuations. This forms the fundamental basis motivating the development here of a new method that can detect deviations from uniform power law scaling at a given time scale t in the instantaneous loudness of the music.

10

5 4

(a)

log10 S(f)

3 2 1 0 -2 -1 0 log10 f

Techno-dance Forr Hindustani Javanese Jazz Brazilian Pop New Age Rock W.E.C.T.

(b)

log10 FD(t)

3 2 1 0 -1 0 log10 t 1 2

(c)
2

(t)

1 0 0

-1

log10 t

Fig. 2. (a) Double log plot of the power spectrum S (f ) of the variable v(j) for various genres of music. For every genre we averaged the spectrum for each individual piece of music, found using a windows of size 213 samples (corresponding to 81.29 s of music), with shifts of 210 samples (10.24s). We applied logarithmic binning to smooth the spectrum by averaging over windows which grow in size as 21/4 . These spectra suggest quantitative dierences in the scaling properties of the loudness uctuations that depend on the genre of music. (b) Double log plot of the average DFA functions FD (t) as a function of the time scale t (in seconds) for each genre of music. (c) Log-linear plot of the DFA correlation exponents (t) obtained from local slopes in (b), according to Eq. 2. Note the striking dierences between genres, which also appear in Fig. 3.

11

2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0 1.5 Javanese Jazz Brazilian pop Techno Forr Hindustani

(t)

1.0 0.5 0.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0 -1 0 1 2 -1 0 1 2 -1 0 1 2 New Age Rock WECT

log10 t
Fig. 3. DFA exponents (t) for 9 genres of music, with 10 representative signals each. We have calculated (t) according to Eq. 2.

12

1.1 1.0 0.9 0.8

<>

0.7 0.6

Rock and Roll

Techno-dance

Brazilian Pop

Hindustani

Javanese

New Age

Forr

Jazz

Fig. 4. Average values for each genre, ranked in increasing order. The standard deviation of the values of varies from genre to genre, but averages = 0.09. We note the remarkable relationship between and the music genre. As discussed in the text, the presence of dominant periodic trends arizing from the regular rhythmic beats can lead to lower values of . The results raise the possibility that the qualitative dierences between high art, popular, and dance music genres may be quantiable.

13

WECT

Anda mungkin juga menyukai