Anda di halaman 1dari 19

Training and Supervision in Cognitive Therapy

THE ACQUISITION OF COMPETENCE IN COGNITIVE THERAPY


When, some years ago, the author took part in a national project to define competencies for the different models of psychological therapy, it was apparent that cognitive therapy practitioners had a very particular take on the acquisition of competence.1 A large group of therapists of different orientations gathered in London, first together and then separately in different orientation groups, to come back together at the end of the day. By the end of the day, the cognitive therapy group had pretty well completed a first draft describing the different skills and competencies associated with the different stages of their model. When all the groups had reassembled, however, it quickly became clear that hardly any of the other groups had got so far one group was still discussing who was the client and who was the therapist! This story signals both the strengths and dangers of cognitive therapy. It is obviously good to have a set of skills and competencies that can be clearly described, discussed and learnt. It may, however, be problematic if the parsimonious simplicity of the cognitive model is learnt in a way that lacks therapeutic artistry and subtlety. This piece will suggest that, just as the cognitive model has developed a more rounded and subtle model of therapeutic intervention, so must its methods of training and supervision. The challenge for trainers and supervisors is to help trainees and practitioners to develop sophistication in both scientific and artistic competence in cognitive therapy, without losing the benefit of the simple clarity of the model. The common core content of training and key points about the training process will be described, including new training models incorporating an opportunity for a personal experience of therapy, in the form of self-practice and self-reflection. Descriptions of the different elements of and approaches to supervision will follow. The subjects of training and supervision in cognitive therapy would each warrant coverage as a separate volume. This piece will therefore focus on two particularly common issues of contemporary training and supervision. The first issue is reflected in a common experience of cognitive therapy training, such as what occurs when trainees may have to let go of at least some previous ideas and skills let go of one trapeze before they have got a reliable set of new, cognitive skills and concepts to catch the other trapeze. Clutching at fresh air is an experience that invariably leads to trepidation. As cognitive therapists become more aware of the interpersonal processes in therapy, so cognitive therapy trainers can use the same skills to reflect the interpersonal processes of training. The second issue is the debate that concerns the degree of systemisation that should be used in implementing cognitive therapy. One side of the coin is the

clarity and help that a therapist can gain from following a relatively prescribed format of therapy, such as that described in a manual or protocol. The other side of this coin could be that over-adherence could easily lead to a mechanistic format of therapy that is imposed upon the client.

TRAINING IN COGNITIVE THERAPY


From its inception, cognitive therapy has always been well received in Britain (Beck, 2004). Various well-known British trainers, such as Melanie Fennell and Adrian Wells, trained with Beck in Philadelphia and incorporated much of his training structure and many of his methods into the training course at Oxford. The Oxford course has also subsequently influenced other courses in Britain and abroad. As the demand has grown for accredited therapists and accredited training, the Oxford and other training traditions, including that of behaviour therapy courses, have informed the course content which has come to be recommended for cognitive therapy by the British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies (BABCP; see www.babcp.org.uk)

CONTENT OF TRAINING
Aaron Beck notes that, by the mid-1970s, cognitive therapy had warranted admission to the arena of controversy (1976: 7) alongside other approaches to psychological therapy.Beck has frequently argued that this admission was warranted by the fact that cognitive therapy could claim to be:
a system of psychotherapy which provides (1) a comprehensive theory of psychopathology that drives the structure of psychotherapy, (2) a body of knowledge and empirical findings which supports the theory, and (3) research findings that demonstrate its effectiveness. (Weishaar, 1993: 47)

These three factors may be regarded as the bedrock features of cognitive therapy and the bedrock for training. The strength of the model derives further from the fact that there is a well-established set of principles that operationalise the above points in a down-to-earth and often parsimonious way. The principles are set out in Table 1. These principles help to ensure that the therapeutic model is focused on both short-term interventions and pragmatic problem-solving and that, at the same time, through informed and collaborative therapeutic relationships, it is able to switch into other modes and to take on other types of issues. Principle 10, suggesting as it does that a map of the clients problems be drawn up, also ensures that research-based conceptual models can be shared with the client and used to guide therapy, and thereby underpin the whole enterprise. The teaching content of cognitive therapy is generally divided into theory and practice elements (see Table 2) and application elements (Table 3) Most training courses are likely to include these elements but, as we have noted many times throughout Cognitive therapy, second edition (Sanders and Wills, 2005), the applications of cognitive therapy have now expanded to the point where particular courses may seek to include some and exclude others, perhaps depending on the needs of the trainees and/or training establishment.

Table 1 Principles of Cognitive Therapy (based on Beck & Emery, 1985; Beck, 1995)
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. Cognitive Cognitive Cognitive Cognitive Cognitive Cognitive Cognitive Cognitive Cognitive Cognitive therapy therapy therapy therapy therapy therapy therapy therapy therapy therapy is short term, at least initially. uses problem-solving. is structured. is collaborative. is active and directional. is based on a sound therapeutic relationship. uses regular homework assignments. uses guided discovery and Socratic dialogue. is educational. is based on cognitive conceptualisation.

Table 2

Theory and practice content of cognitive therapy training

Principles of cognitive therapy. Cognitive model of psychological problems. Conceptualisation behavioural therapy. Behavioural interventions. Problem-solving. Cognitive interventions. Schema-focused therapy. Interpersonal and therapeutic processes.

Many courses in cognitive therapy use measures of therapist competence, such as The Cognitive Therapy Scale (CTS) to assess whether therapists are doing what they are supposed to do. The CTS (Young and Beck, 1988) was revised in 2000 (James et al., 2000) with 12 items measuring: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. Agenda setting and adherence to the agenda; Providing and eliciting feedback; Collaboration; Pacing and efficient use of time; Interpersonal effectiveness: genuineness, warmth, empathy, trust; Eliciting appropriate emotional expression; Eliciting key cognitions; Eliciting and planning behaviours; Guided discovery; Conceptual integration; Application of methods of change: use of cognitive and behavioural methods; Homework.

Each category is given a six-point rating, from 0 (absence of feature or highly inappropriate performance) to 6 (excellent performance, even in the face of patient difficulties). The rating scale specifies exactly what the therapist should be doing within each category, thereby giving highly specific instruction and

Table 3

Application elements of cognitive therapy

Depression. Anxiety. Types of anxiety panic, social and simple phobias, agoraphobia, OCD, etc. Post-traumatic stress disorder. Stress and low self-esteem. Family and couple problems.

feedback. It is very valuable in this way, giving detailed guidance about the form of therapy. We also know that therapist competence, along with therapeutic alliance, is related to outcome and may well be one of the key common factors across the psychotherapies (Trepka et al., 2004).

LEARNING PROCESSES IN TRAINING


Although the content of training may look straightforward, learning cognitive therapy, either as a beginning trainee, or in ongoing professional development, is a more complex process. Because cognitive therapy is often understood as incorporating psycho-education teaching clients about the nature of psychological problems and about the way out of them as part of its working methods, there are inevitable parallels between the educational process in therapy and the education process in training. Although there is a distinct content that needs to be imparted, attention also needs to be paid to the processes that trainees follow when learning cognitive therapy. Such processes are described in adult education theory, such as the Lewin-Kolb cycle and Schons concept of reflective practice (see Bennett-Levy, 2002; Bennett-Levy et al., 2004). In brief, the LewinKolb cycle suggests that there are different stages in the cycle of learning and that there is a characteristic sequence of stages, as shown in Figure 1. Learning theorists such as Honey and Mumford (1992) have suggested that, although this is a common sequence, people vary in how they move through it: some people reflectors need lots of reflection time, whereas others pragmatists need more action-based experiences. In educational terms, this means that trainers and tutors probably need to spend time thinking about the balance between different types of learning activities and the links between them, such as following a slab of theory with some kind of practical exercise. For example, in a session looking at some of the principles in Table 1, it can be useful to operationalise one of the principles in a practice application. Trainees could be asked to take one of the principles, such as agenda setting or homework, and practise a possible line of rationale-giving that one might use to introduce it to a client. An additional dimension might be added to this by suggesting that trainees select principles with which they have some difficulty.

Using Uncertainty and the Socratic Method


Given the cognitive model of understanding belief and attitude change, we should not expect that all trainees will immediately and fully accept all of the

Experience

Planning

Observation

Reflection

Figure 1 The Experiential learning cycle (Bennett-Levy et al., 2004; adapted from Lewin, 1952; Kolb, 1984).

training content, particularly the principles (Table 1) Indeed we should be surprised if they did. One way of conceptualising the principles is to view them not as blind facts, but as beliefs about therapy (Wills, 2005), with belief in this context following the definition of Alford and Beck as ideas that are worthy of being tested (1997: 85). Thus, by introducing principles as testable hypotheses, such as therapy works better when it is structured, we are introducing trainees to the ideas of questioning, finding out, and hypothesis testing, which resonate throughout cognitive therapy. What Padesky (1996) calls therapist beliefs the beliefs of individual therapists about what they are doing in therapy may also come into play. For example, the therapist may believe that clients will not like it when I ask them to do homework. Therapist beliefs may be in the form of learning heuristics or rules governing the way in which the trainee will implement the therapy principles. In a longitudinal study of cognitive therapy trainees, Wills (2005). found that many trainees had an initial learning heuristic that told them that I must follow the steps of cognitive therapy very exactly, otherwise it will not work. This heuristic often led to rigid and ineffective practice. A more effective and personal style only developed when trainees were able to change this heuristic to something more like, I can try to do this my way and see if it works, thus introducing an experimental and discovery mode into the learning process. Counsellors learning cognitive therapy experienced the most difficulty when they were trying to implement the structure of therapy, especially in setting an agenda (Wills, 2005). This seemed to relate to a conjunction between a previous immersion in person-centred counselling and sets of therapist beliefs that defined structuring sessions as being directive and disrespectful of the client. Sometimes these beliefs would be accompanied by catastrophic predictions about the clients likely reaction to such disrespect. Again, such beliefs are open to be tested, by trying out the difference between bringing little structure or more structure to the sessions. Other cognitive therapy skills seem, in their own right, to be particularly hard to learn to do really well. Trainees often report difficulties in learning with Socratic dialogue, finding it difficult not to lead clients towards certain answers that seem obvious.

Sometimes the client is saying something like, Im such a failure, so useless, or whatever, and you just feel like saying to them, oh, for Gods sake, what about the two wonderful kids you have brought up? Not very Socratic, eh? (Wills, research interviews, 2005)

This may be because at this moment, the therapist has moved from a guided discovery perspective to a changing minds perspective (Padesky, 1993) Padesky makes the point that one of the characteristics of good guided discovery may be that the therapist does not always know where the dialogue is heading and is genuinely open to wherever it may lead. If we translate that thought into the training situation, it may be that there are times when the trainer has to find that same interest in the wanderings of the trainee as he or she lurches towards the aha moment of learning. In order to learn cognitive therapy, particularly for those who have several years or decades of experience in other therapeutic models, trainees might have to go through a considerable period of confusion and discomfort while their therapy beliefs and learning heuristics adapt to their new situation. The period of confusion has a striking similarity to the phenomenon of aporia, described in Chapter 5 of Cognitive therapy, second edition (Sanders and Wills, 2005) as a necessary part of cognitive change, facilitated through Socratic dialogue and guided discovery. Such confusion is a defining experience of training, as described in the seminal paper by Worthless et al. (2002). There may be trainees who have moved effortlessly towards competence but we would struggle to recall them. However, the cognitive dissonance generated by such confusion may be a necessary motivator for movement towards insight. If they wish to help trainees through this period of confusion, trainers in cognitive therapy may wish to ensure that their teaching methods are as Socratic as they would want their trainees to be with their clients, as strongly suggested by Christine Padesky and others (Padesky, 1996). Training in cognitive therapy cannot therefore just be about imparting knowledge in a context-free way but should, as far as possible, mirror the principles of cognitive therapy itself. The concept of training heuristic used in Cognitive therapy, second edition can be applied to trainees search for meaning and competence, clearly heard in this reflective account of a trainee after finishing training:
I am normally a confident person and I back myself but for a short while there, in the middle of training, I lost that trust. I thought that I had to apply this model as a 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 And it really wasnt working but it became like a safety blanket 1, 2, 3 And then I thought, well, this is no good and at that same time, I talked to the tutor and he said, A., you could just try doing it and I thought, yes, come on A., just do it And I realized that I could do it 1, 2, 3, 4 and it would still be wrong So I just did it, I did it in my own way, I suppose, and it worked, and, to my amazement, the tutors thought it worked too! (Wills, 2005).

This silent dialogue going on inside the trainees head is very reminiscent of the way Beck (1976) describes finding out about the two streams of thinking going on within some of his patients. This discovery led on to the concept of negative automatic thoughts and to the basis of the theory of cognitive therapy. Just as these silent ways of negative thinking could be related to feelings of dysphoria

and anxiety in patients, it is easy to see how negative training heuristics can be associated with such feelings in trainees. A finding that surprised me in my research (Wills, 2005) was the pervasive anxiety that seems to surround all aspects of training, captured in Worthless et al.s paper (2002) Sometimes it is helpful to link what one might call negative thoughts about training and anxiety with trainees, although, in our experience, they may be guarded in discussing them until after being awarded their certificates of competence! The training arena in which these thoughts are most likely to come up is within case discussions or supervision groups. Being aware of negative training thoughts and feelings can help a supervisor to respond more sensitively and helpfully to the needs of trainees. Such thoughts and feelings do not of course entirely disappear after the completion of training and so can also be helpfully addressed in ongoing supervision.

Personal Therapy and Cognitive Therapy Training


In many counselling and psychotherapy traditions, personal therapy is seen as an essential requirement for training and practising as a therapist (Wilkins, 2006) and therapists rate this as an important aspect of training (Macran and Shapiro, 1998; Orlinsky et al., 2001). Although it is accepted wisdom that personal therapy is useful, there has not been a great deal of research on its effect on subsequent therapeutic practice, nor on what learning mechanisms can explain its effectiveness (Bennett-Levy, 2002). In a review, Macran and Shapiro (1998) found that personal therapy improved empathy, warmth and genuineness and, given the importance of these factors in therapy, such improvements in themselves are likely to have a good impact on outcome. By contrast, in the world of cognitive and behavioural therapies, personal therapy is not currently seen as an essential, or widely accepted, aspect of training (EABCT, 2001). The British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies (BABCP, 2000), while not insisting on personal therapy for accreditation, does stipulate: therapists must ensure that they can identify and manage appropriately their personal involvement in the process of cognitive and/or behaviour therapy and therapists must have developed an ability to recognise when they should seek other professional advice. Counsellors and therapists from other traditions have reported being somewhat mystified by such lack of interest in individual therapy. What they have gained from their own therapy an awareness and understanding of their own issues and how these might impact on the therapeutic relationship, the experience of being a client, a place to take difficulties that might arise during training or as a result of client work, material with which to conceptualize difficulties in therapy are all seen as equally valid within cognitive therapy but have not, until recently, been explicitly focused on. In addition, we know that reflection is an important aspect of learning, and personal therapy provides the opportunity to reflect on both professional and personal issues. Part of the reason for the lack of focus on personal therapy may be historical: many of the old school practitioners of cognitive therapy came from behavioural traditions and clinical

psychology, where personal therapy has not always been seen as essential or even relevant. Those seeing cognitive therapy as an educational model, involving the application of appropriate techniques, again might see therapy for therapists as unimportant. Given the strong belief in the benefits of personal therapy for therapists, it may be rather baffling that there is little evidence of its overall effectiveness (Roth and Fonagy, 1996). This may be because of the way it has sometimes been implemented as a compulsory element in some training courses. We think that this may not be a good enough reason to undertake therapy, partly because it is important for the client to be able to decide when the time is exactly right for this commitment. We are also aware that undertaking therapy in this way has led to some quite adverse and even abusively exploitative outcomes for some of the trainees and therapists involved. We are glad to report the need for personal experience of therapy is increasingly recognised within cognitive therapy and that our colleague, James BennettLevy, has devised a method for facilitating such an experience in a way that overcomes some of the difficulties described earlier by formatting it as a safer and more educational experience. Working in Australia and more recently in Oxford, he has developed a training method called Self Practice/Self Reflection, where trainees undergo cognitive therapy with a training partner, and reflect in writing on the process of each session, thinking through the implications of the therapy experience for themselves, for their clients and for cognitive therapy. Bennett-Levy found that SP/SR impacted on therapy in a number of ways. The trainees reported a deeper sense of knowing of cognitive practices. They gained a deeper understanding of therapy, understood themselves better and demonstrated improvement in cognitive therapy skills (Bennett-Levy, 2001; BennettLevy et al., 2003). They also noted a re-emphasis on therapeutic relationship skills: the experience of being in the clients shoes demonstrated starkly some of the anxiety and difficulties in making changes, even as high functioning individuals; and served to emphasize how valuable empathy, understanding, respect, tolerance and guidance of the therapist is (Bennett-Levy et al., 2003: 150) The study found that SP/SR helped trainees to develop self-reflection, enabling them to reflect both during and after sessions. We know that client perception of empathy is correlated with positive outcome; and if SP/SR leads therapists to be more empathic, as judged by our clients, then it is likely to lead to better outcomes. A review of SP/SR in CBT training (Laireiter and Willutzki, 2003) confirms Bennett-Levys findings, showing that trainees report substantial personal and professional gains from using SP/SR. They report improved self-insight and selfawareness, and a better understanding of the role of the therapist and the process of therapeutic change, as well as a better understanding of and skills in CBT methods. As summarised by Laireiter and Willutzki: Although empirical evidence is not extensive at present, it supports the notion that most of these goals may be attained by a combination of person- and practice-related self-reflection together with self-practice of CBT methods (2003: 28) Personal therapy or SP/SR may take a while to filter through to become incorporated within cognitive therapy training, but is certainly becoming recognised as a means of improving understanding of ourselves and of personal aspects of our clinical practice.

LEARNING THE STRUCTURE: THERAPEUTIC ARTISTRY VERSUS STANDARD THERAPY FOR STANDARD PROBLEMS
nor should standard, protocol-based practice stifle innovation, but should serve as a springboard to it, since there are many situations in the psychotherapies where there is no research evidence or even clinical consensus to indicate the best approach. (Parry, 1996) Once the basics are learned, a competent cognitive therapist develops an artful ability to conceptualize interlocking problems, make intervention choices, and solve problems in an efficient and effective manner to facilitate client learning and change. These ideals are challenging to achieve. (Padesky, 1996: 269)

Many therapy procedures are now well written up into manual and protocol formats, and one approach to training can include introducing trainees to the use of these materials. On the one hand, cognitive therapy does promote the importance of protocols, since research has suggested that therapists who do not follow the structure are not as effective as those that do (Schulte et al., 1992; Wilson, 1997) On the other hand, however, developing an individual conceptualisation for each client we see, and following therapy accordingly, is recommended (Butler, 1998; Jacobson et al., 1999) Jacqueline Persons (1989) argues that a failure to use individual conceptualisation means that our treatment would be overly dictated by symptom identification. She uses the analogy of treating fever. If we were to only treat fever according to the symptoms, we would not be able to distinguish between fever caused by malaria and that caused by pneumonia, and this would mean that we would be likely to make treatment mistakes. Learning to balance a structured, standard approach in therapy with working with individual clients can be an extremely challenging part of training, particularly for those therapists used to a looser, client-centred and client-directed way of working, who fear that structure will lose the essence of working with the person rather than with symptoms. However, to reject standard therapy on this basis may risk losing the therapys effectiveness or at least diluting its value. The therapeutic procedures of cognitive therapy have gained a lot from being clearly delineated and structured, and there are strong arguments for therapists to follow them closely. For many trainees, using a standard protocol may come as a relief, offering clear guidelines as to what to do in therapy. But even this is not without its problems. Henry et al. (1993) report, admittedly in the context of training in psychodynamic therapy, that training using protocols and/or manuals is likely to result in therapist hostility towards clients presumably when the clients do not conform to the client behaviour required by the protocol and thus to adverse results. The problem raised by Henry and colleagues is one that is frequently reported in training. Trainees, coping with the aporia crisis of leaving one form of practice behind without having fully developed a new form of practice, see the process of mastering the protocol as a way out of this anxiety. They therefore frequently report going through a stage where they overapply the protocol or the model:

I seemed to spend more time thinking about applying the model than thinking about the person in front of me. This felt very uncomfortable to me. In retrospect, I think that I got into a way of thinking about how to do cognitive therapy that was almost neurotically structured (cognitive therapy trainee, quoted in Wills, 2005)

More optimistically, however, trainees in the Wills (2005) research reported that as time went on they were able to reconceptualise the situation with a learning heuristic that was more helpful to them. The solution for these trainees seemed to lie in developing the paradoxical ability to follow the protocol in their own way and in a way that was responsive to the needs of their individual clients. In some ways the protocol versus individual therapy argument has become more polarised than it needs to be, and successful therapy involves a balance between science, or following the structure, or art, working with the individual. Artistry and science are clearly evident in Aaron Becks therapeutic abilities and his ideas. It is perhaps significant that, for cognitive therapy, he has been an advocate for both modes of operation. This fact in itself may give us hope that the two approaches are not necessarily mutually exclusive. Faced with a bewildering mass of issues and symptoms, a therapist can find help in both the individual conceptualisation and in the treatment methods and rationales of the manuals. There are, also, different understandings of what structure means in practice. One notion would hold that inside the therapists head there is a very clear and structured scheme of rules on how to proceed and this is matched by another consisting of clear and structured therapeutic behaviours. Another notion is that there is a clear and precise structure of rules inside the therapists head but that the art of therapy depends on using the rules to steer between different potential actions. Alford and Beck use the term in both these different ways: The therapist needs a clear structure a clear rationale on which to base a way of proceeding without that it will be difficult to maintain a scientific stance in practice (1997: 72); Structure is a defining characteristic of cognitive therapy and a reliable way to discriminate between it and other therapeutic modes [it includes] the presence of an agenda, setting homework and asking the client for feedback at the end of the session (1997: 76) The first usage may be deemed as a kind of structured way of thinking about and understanding a situation, a way of conceptualising or formulating a clients history, for example. The latter we might better call steps that structure action. While a scheme of such steps can act as a facilitator of clear work with clients, it may also be open to the danger of resulting in mechanical work. This is why trainees also need to be wired to the cognitive way (i.e. having a thorough way of using cognitive conceptualisations), as Young (in Weishaar, 1993: 38.) puts it. He goes on to say: Technical cognitive therapists [are] not really attuned to the interpersonal component of the therapy, just with delivering a technique (1993: 39) What we are talking about here may be the art of learning to sail a yacht by catching the wind rather than by resorting to the outboard motor. In some ways, cognitive therapy may itself have to accept at least part of the blame for the state of things regarding its implementation. As a discipline, it has been over-reliant on highly quantitative outcome research and has not encouraged much qualitative process research, with the notable exception of the in-depth, qualitative work of Bennett-Levy (2002) A strongly quantitative bias

may have been necessary in the opening phase of the history of cognitive therapy but development of qualitative research of cognitive therapy is now long overdue (Grant et al., 2004) As we will argue again later, we may need to develop different research modes probably those of the more qualitative, process-oriented style to get at more subtle questions such as, when is it good to depart from the structure/protocol and when is it necessary to stay with it? These are questions that crop up time after time when an open learning approach is used in cognitive therapy training. The question can also become a major focus of supervision.

SUPERVISION IN COGNITIVE THERAPY


Supervision in cognitive therapy probably occupies less of a hinterland than it does in some of the other major models of psychotherapy and counselling. Although it has always been a part of the actual practice of the model, it has not been much written of or theorised about, perhaps once again showing the practical and pragmatic nature of the model. That it has always been there, however, can be seen in the fact that Beck strongly argued for the presence of cognitive therapy supervisors in the major research trial conducted by the US National Institute for Mental Health in the 1980s (Weishaar, 1993) There are signs now that cognitive and cognitive-behavioural therapists are beginning to think much more about the nature of supervision (Bennett-Levy, 2002; OCarroll, 1999; Padesky, 1996; Townend et al., 2002) There are two main forums in which cognitive therapy supervision takes place: supervision as part of a training course (more likely to be group supervision) and supervision as part of ongoing development after training (more likely to be individual supervision) Both authors have practised and experienced individual and group supervision in both of these contexts. Supervision after training may be particularly important in helping to prevent practice reverting to the mean after training. It is easy to understand how, when the frequently intensive atmosphere of the training situation is left behind, therapeutic practice can, first, stop developing and, then, revert back to previously established models of practice (MacKay et al., 2000) Post-training environments, despite what quality assurance and mission statements may say, may not nurture innovative practice, even when it is empirically supported (Wills, 2005) There is also a significant problem whereby highly trained therapists may be quite quickly promoted into posts where they do less, or sometimes no, direct therapy work (Ashworth et al., 1999) In addition, supervision can be informal peer supervision, as in a discussion between colleagues, or self-supervision, where we allow time to reflect on our work (Padesky, 1996) Christine Padeskys (1996) excellent chapter on supervision in cognitive therapy gives clear guidelines about how to bring cognitive therapy principles into supervision. For example, she recommends that supervision reflect the structure of cognitive therapy by using pre-planned questions as a focus for each supervision session, with themes such as: Is there a cognitive model for understanding and treating this client problem and is this being followed?

Does the therapist have the knowledge and skills to implement therapy properly? Is the therapy following expected patterns? What might be interfering?

CONTENT AND PROCESS IN COGNITIVE THERAPY SUPERVISION


Cognitive therapy is a vast and growing area of application, and in comparison to the early models, where depression was the only area to learn about, now there is much extra that one could learn about. Thankfully, this does not necessarily mean that we have to know in detail the content of every intervention for every problem. Many of the different applications operate to the same sort of principles and offer similar types of interventions using similar skills. The main variation, as indicated earlier, would be on the development of a conceptualisation for particular problems, mindful that our clients are individual people, whom we understand and aim to help in individual ways. From a training and supervision point of view, the variety in cognitive therapy is really more about being able to find out about rather than knowing about. Curiosity is a quality that is good for trainees and supervisees to foster, backed up by the ability to think in a clear and scientific way. Therefore, knowledge about the content of therapy for particular client difficulties will probably always play quite a significant role in cognitive therapy supervision. Being able to use diagnostic system criteria to get in the right ballpark with clients is often something that more senior practitioners can help trainees with. However, we would stress the use of such criteria as ballpark estimates rather than rock-solid facts. Help with understanding symptoms may be less relevant to more seasoned supervisees a good example of how supervisory needs vary over the development of a career. Townend et al. (2002) found in their survey of cognitive behavioural practitioners that conceptualisation was the issue most raised in CBT supervision. Conceptualisation can contribute to the review of either the content or process of the therapy. It is also inevitable that issues concerning therapeutic process will often be raised in supervision. Interestingly, I kept a supervision case book of issues raised in supervision over a period of two years and this record shows that over twothirds of all supervision discussions were more focused on therapeutic process than on therapeutic content. This was made all the more remarkable by the fact that the greatest proportion of the supervision discussions had taken place within the context of a training course. Perhaps the reason for this is that many cognitive therapy techniques are relatively simple to execute and are often backed by very clear rationales. This is not by any means saying that cognitive therapy is easy to do but that its clarity means that most of the difficulties occur in relation to its application, rather than to its execution. Process issues in cognitive therapy supervision often seem to carry the flavour of Im doing great cognitive therapy but the client isnt! Some examples of typical discussions are presented in Figure 2. We described how such cognitive-interpersonal events can be understood and worked with in cognitive therapy in Cognitive therapy, second edition. The seven-eyed model developed by Hawkins and Shohet (2000) can be very useful

I get the impression that this client doesnt suffer fools gladly and I wonder if she sees me as a fool! M. is an extremely bright but depressed student client. She is very committed to doing CT therapy and indeed sees it as the answer to all her problems. The supervisee had only seen this client three times but on each occasion she had made a criticism of the way the therapy was being conducted and each time had asked for specific changes in the way it was being done. The supervisee made the above comment. I described one aspect of the clients behaviour as over the top and he stormed out of the room. F. was a depressed middle-aged man who was finding it difficult to sustain relationships with women. The female supervisee was exploring with the client his thoughts and behaviours as he showered his girlfriend with gifts, and tentatively wondered how the girlfriend might experience it. The client reacted badly though he did call back and some discussion followed. The supervisee was able to reflect on how the client was affecting her and how she might use such cognitive interpersonal information better in the future.

Figure 2

Process Issues in Cognitive Therapy Supervision

for identifying and working with such interpersonal markers as those noted earlier (see Figure 3) The first three process modes of the Hawkins and Shohet model relate mainly to fairly conscious material content of the therapy. The other process modes relate mainly to less conscious processes in both the therapy and the supervision. Processes 1 and 2 of the Hawkins and Shohet model focus on the content of the therapy sessions and the strategies being used in them respectively. In the case of client M. (see Figure 2), the client had been diagnosed as being dysthymic and was indeed very emotionally flat and hard to engage. One of her criticisms of the therapy, however, concerned filling out an inventory to measure her depression. The therapist uses many of these inventories and to save resources and the rainforest had started getting clients to fill them out in pencil and then erasing their marks for reuse later. M. had objected to this and had asked for a fresh sheet each time. This was the first time that any client had made this point and the therapist had begun to wonder if the client might have an entitlement schema (Young and Klosko, 1994) The supervisor helped him think this through and how this might link to the choice of which strategies to use, for example, whether he should agree to this request or not. What would be the implications of acceding or not acceding to the request for the therapeutic relationship, the third process mode of the Hawkins and Shohet model? The therapist spontaneously began to consider, I wonder if I like this client? What is she doing to me? Am I a bit attracted to her? This is the therapist process focus process mode 4 of the model. The supervisor was now aware of a reaction in himself: I have to make this therapist stand his ground here. I cant let him be pushed around by this client. This has been a weak side revealed in his work before. This is process mode 5 of the model: focusing on the supervisory relationship itself. Finally, the supervisor decided to step back from this latter reaction and ask himself, Am I getting drawn into this? Is this perhaps driven by my stuff about my strong need to patrol my own boundaries? I could just go to a more

Organizational context Professional codes and ethics

Organizational constraints and expectins

6 Supervisor 5 6a Fantasy relationship 4 Therapist 2 1 Client 3

Economic realities/pressures Family

Social context Social norms

Figure 3

The seven-eyed model of supervision

neutral position and help the therapist review the pros and cons of acceding or not acceding to the request. Hed probably learn more from that. This is supervision process mode 6: focusing on the supervisors process. Since devising the six process modes of the original model published in 1989, Hawkins and Shohet (2000) have added a seventh process mode in their second edition. The seventh process mode concerns how the therapy and the supervision relate to the wider context of the therapy and supervision. The case of client F. (see Figure 2) could profit by analysis from the perspective of the same six modes that have been applied to client M., above. In this case, however, the seventh process mode may also become relevant. Clients who storm out of sessions may choose to go into complaint mode. Most organisations now have well worked-out, formalised ways of dealing with complaints and these may relate to wider quality assurance systems. Such systems and procedures may strike us as bureaucratic and/or covering ones backside but do at least offer some protection to potentially vulnerable human service workers. Single-handed practitioners may not expect to get caught up in such situations and may have very little in the way of organisational coping systems in place. Good supervision would not only help the therapist reflect on the practice issues, as per Figure 2, but also on the systems and potential personal liability issues in such incidents. This might include severely practical aspects concerning whether the incident had been recorded and whether any witnesses were available. In general, supervision in therapy and counselling has been good and proactive in dealing with psychological process issues, and passive and poor in dealing with system

process issues. Townend et al. (2002), for instance, report that, in their survey of supervision, therapist safety was never discussed. A further systems issue that has arisen in the nascent literature on supervision for cognitive therapists is that of dual relationships (Townend et al., 2002) In the NHS, for example, where there are strong management systems, conflict may arise when one person is trying to hold the roles of both managerial supervision and developmental supervision. Townend and colleagues report that supervisees did not generally report this as a problem but suggest caution in this regard. They further suggest that a written contract covering such matters as expectations, methods and procedures can act as a useful protection against contamination effects between the two roles. Townend et al.s (2002) survey reports that many cognitive practitioners are influenced by the supervision models offered by Padesky (1996) and Beck and Liese (1997) These models are based on the desirability of congruence between the way therapy is conducted and the way supervision is conducted. This point covers issues such as congruent structure, setting agendas, goals and using feedback to guide the process, and congruent methods, such as conceptualisations, role-plays and use of tapes. In this way, the supervision session mirrors cognitive therapy itself, and is a powerful means of experiencing the process while working on the supervision issues themselves.

CONCLUSION: HELPING THERAPISTS TO ESTABLISH THEIR OWN STYLES


Cognitive therapy walks a narrow line between scientific precision and therapeutic artistry. As the cognitive model has developed, it has become abundantly clear that the therapy itself and the training and supervision modes informed by this model are usually accompanied by subtle interpersonal processes. Such processes were not entirely expected in the early days of cognitive therapy. Interpersonal processes receive much of their subtlety from their idiosyncratic nature, a factor relatively easily integrated into a cognitive approach, based as it is on uncovering individual appraisal and meaning. One of the challenges facing trainee cognitive therapists, and indeed those longer in the tooth, is in balancing structured, standard therapy with working with our individual clients. There is no doubt that the therapeutic procedures of cognitive therapy have gained much from being clearly delineated and structured. There are therefore strong arguments for therapists to follow them quite closely. These arguments also stand in favour of supervisors helping trainees and supervisees to follow them quite closely. The devil, however, is quite often in the detail. How closely should cognitive therapists stick to the protocol and what licence can they give to their therapeutic artistry? We wonder if the debate between these two approaches has not become rather polarised into an either/or mode. It seems extremely unlikely that one therapist would implement even highly specified treatments in exactly the same way as another therapist or that either of them would work in exactly the same way with different clients. Our own experience tells that even when a client seems to be very much with the programme, there are hundreds of little decisions specific to that client and that therapy that still have to be made. Equally, even

when client and therapist are not following a manual, it is also extremely unlikely that the therapy is not following some kind of sequence known to the therapist from his or her reading of the literature and from previous experience with clients. It is likely then, that there is far more of a continuum between high and low structure than is sometimes implied in the current debate. Even when following an established sequence of therapy, clinicians themselves consider that their clinical judgement is a vital part of the equation for success (Butler, 1998; Nelmes, 2001) Huey argues that therapy would be likely to be enhanced by effective clinical reasoning, i.e., teaching future practitioners how to, rather than what to, think about complex psychopathology (1999: 2) It would be helpful now to have more research on the kind of small decisions that sometimes can keep things on track but at other times allow for successful diversion. Such research would give guidance on what one might called the natural history of the therapy process. The forum of supervision, both during and after training, seems to be the most likely location where such data would naturally accumulate. Within psychotherapy generally, supervision has been more prevalent and prominent than it has been in the cognitive and behavioural field and yet has also been somewhat under-researched. A recent review of research on supervision in counselling and psychotherapy (Wheeler, 2003) has, however, revealed a new focus on outcome and it is now time for cognitive therapists to add its experience to this developing and important field.

Further Reading
Bennett-Levy, J. (2006) Therapist skills: their acquisition and refinement. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 34 (1): 5778. Goldfried, M.R. (ed.) (2000) How therapists change: personal and professional reflections. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Padesky, C.A. (1996) Developing cognitive therapist competency: teaching and supervision models. In P.M. Salkovskis (ed.), The frontiers of cognitive therapy (pp. 26692) New York: Guilford Press.

NOTE
1 The cognitive therapists also tended to dress more conventionally. Goldfried (2000) claims that, as a behaviourist travelling to the humanistic Mecca at Esalen in the 1970s, he used a two-sided case so that the two appropriate dress styles could be kept separate. The Esalen side was smaller, clothing being, at that time in Esalen, optional!

REFERENCES
Alford, B.A. & Beck, A.T. (1997) The integrative power of cognitive therapy. New York: Guilford Press.

Ashworth, P., Williams, C. & Blackburn, I.-M. (1999) What becomes of cognitive therapy trainees? A survey of trainees opinions and current clinical practice after postgraduate cognitive therapy training. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 27 (3): 26777. BABCP (2000) Minimum training standards for the practice of CBT. London: BABCP. Beck, A.T. (1976) Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press. Beck, A.T. (2004) Origin, evolution and current state of cognitive therapy: The inside story. Keynote address, Congress of the European Association of Behavioural and Cognitive Therapies, Manchester, September. Beck, A.T. & Emery, G. with Greenberg, R.L. (1985) Anxiety disorders and phobias: a cognitive perspective. New York: Basic Books. Beck, A.T. & Liese, B.S. (1997) Back to basics: fundamental cognitive therapy skills for keeping drug-dependent individuals in treatment. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph Series, 165: 41130. Beck, J. (1995) Cognitive therapy: basics and beyond. New York: Guilford Press. Bennett-Levy, J. (2001) The value of self-practice of cognitive therapy techniques and selfreflection in the training of cognitive therapists. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29: 20320. Bennett-Levy, J. (2002) Navel gazing or valuable training strategy? Self-practice of therapy techniques, self-reflection, and the development of therapist expertise. In J. Henry (ed.), First European positive psychology conference proceedings. Leicester: British Psychological Society. Bennett-Levy, J., Lee, N., Travers, K., Pohlman, S. & Hamernik, E. (2003) Cognitive therapy from the inside: enhancing therapist skills through practising what we preach. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 31: 14358. Bennett-Levy, J., Butler, G., Fennell, M., Hackmann, A., Mueller, M. & Westbrook, D. (2004) The Oxford guide to behavioural experiments in cognitive therapy. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Butler, G. (1998) Clinical formulation. In A.S. Bellack & M. Hersen (eds), Comprehensive clinical psychology. Oxford: Pergamon, (pp. 124). European Association of Behavioural and Cognitive Therapies (EABCT) (2001) Minimum training standards. London: EABCT. Goldfried, M.R. (ed.) (2000) How therapists change: personal and professional reflections. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Grant, A., Mills, J., Mulhern, R. & Short, N. (2004) Cognitive behavioural therapy in mental health care. London: SAGE. Hawkins, P. & Shohet, R. (2000) Supervision in the helping professions: an individual, group, and organisational approach. Buckingham: Open University Press. Henry W.P., Strupp, H.H., Butler S.F., et al. (1993) Effects of training in time-limited dynamic psychotherapy: Changes in therapist behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61: 43440. Honey, P. & Mumford, A. (1992) The manual of learning styles. Maidenhead: Peter Honey. Huey, D. (1999) The potential utility of problem-based learning in the education of clinical psychologists. British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies Annual Conference, London. Jacobson, N.S., Schmaling, K.B., Holz-Munroe, A., Katt, J.L., Wood, I.F. & Follette, V.M. (1999) Research structured versus clinically flexible versions of social learning based marital therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27: 17380. James, I.A., Blackburn, I.M. & Reichelt, F.K. (2000) Manual of the revised cognitive therapy scale. Newcastle upon Tyne: Newcastle Cognitive & Behavioural Therapies Centre.

Kolb, D.A. (1984) Experiential learning: experience as the source of learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Laireiter, A.R. & Willutzki, U. (2003) Self-reflection and self-practice in training of cognitive behaviour therapy: an overview. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 10: 1930. Lewin, K. (1952) Field theory in social science: selected theoretical papers. Edited by Dorwin Cartwright. London: Tavistock. MacKay, H., West, W., Moorey, J., Guthrie, E. & Margison, F. (2000) Training in psychodynamic-interpersonal therapy: primary care counsellors experiences of changing their practice. British Association for Counselling and Psychotherapy Research Conference, Manchester, May. Macran, S. & Shapiro, D. (1998) The role of personal therapy for therapists: a review. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 71: 1325. Nelmes, M. (2001) Therapy protocols versus individual conceptualisation: the views of CBT therapists. Unpublished MA thesis, University of Wales, Newport. OCarroll, P. (1999) Cognitive-behaviour therapy supervision: evidence-based? Paper given at BABCP Annual Conference, Bristol, July. Orlinsky, D.E., Botermans, J.F. & Rnnestad, M.H. (2001) Towards an empirically grounded model of psychotherapy training: four thousand therapists rate influences on their development. Australian Psychologist, 36: 13948. Padesky, C. (1995) Cognitive therapy of anxiety: key treatment principles and methods. Audiotape. Newport Beach, CA: Center for Cognitive Therapy. www.padesky.com Padesky, C. A. (1993) Socratic questioning: Changing minds or guided discovery? Congress of the European Association of Behavioural and Cognitive Therapies. London, September. Padesky, C.A. (1996) Developing cognitive therapist competency: teaching and supervision models. In P.M. Salkovskis (ed.), Frontiers of cognitive therapy. New York: Guilford Press, (pp. 26692). Parry, G. (1996) NHS psychotherapy services in England: review of strategic policy. London: NHS Executive. Persons, J.B. (1989) Cognitive therapy in practice: a case formulation approach. New York: W.W. Norton. Roth, A. & Fonagy, P. (1996) What works for whom? A critical review of psychotherapy research. New York: Guilford Press. Sanders, D. & Wills, F. (2005) Cognitive therapy (2nd edn). London: SAGE. Schulte, D., Knzel, R., Pepping, G. & Schulte-Bahrenberg, T. (1992) Tailor made versus standardized therapy of phobic patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14: 6792. Townend, M., Iannetta, L.E. & Freeston, M.H. (2002) Clinical supervision in practice: a survey of UK cognitive behavioural psychotherapists accredited by the BABCP. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30: 485500. Trepka, C., Rees, A., Shapiro, D.A. & Hardy, G.E. (2004) Therapist competence and outcome of cognitive therapy of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28: 14357. Weishaar, M.E. (1993) Aaron T. Beck. London: SAGE. Wheeler, S. (2003) Research on supervision of counsellors and psychotherapists: a systematic scooping study. Rugby: British Association for Counselling and Psychotherapy. Wilkins, P. (2006) Professional and personal development. In C. Feltham & I. Horton (eds), Handbook of counselling and psychotherapy (2nd edn). London: SAGE, (pp. 15865). Wills, F. (2005) The influence of trainees beliefs about therapy on the acquisition of competence in CBT training. Paper presented at the British Association for Counselling and Psychotherapy Research Conference.

Wilson, G.T. (1997) Treatment manuals in clinical practice. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34: 295314. Worthless, I.M., Competent, U.R. & Lemonde-Terrible, O. (2002) Cognitive therapy training stress disorder: a cognitive perspective. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30 (3): 36574. Young, J. & Beck, A.T. (1988) The cognitive therapy rating scale manual (rev. edn). Philadelphia, PA: Center for Cognitive Therapy, University of Pennsylvania. Young, J. & Klosko, J. (1994) Reinventing your life. New York: Plume.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai