Anda di halaman 1dari 11

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Sleep Medicine Reviews (2008) 12, 307 317

www.elsevier.com/locate/smrv

CLINICAL REVIEW

The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures


Leon C. Lacka,, Michael Gradisara, Eus J.W. Van Somerenb,c, Helen R. Wrighta, Kurt Lushingtond
School of Psychology, Flinders University, South Australia, Australia Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an Institute of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam, The Netherlands c Departments of Clinical Neurophysiology, Neurology and Medical Psychology, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands d School of Psychology, University of South Australia, SA, Australia
b a

KEYWORDS
Sleep; Insomnia; Core body temperature; Circadian rhythms; Thermoregulation

Summary Sleepiness and sleep propensity are strongly inuenced by our circadian clock as indicated by many circadian rhythms, most commonly by that of core body temperature. Sleep is most conducive in the temperature minimum phase, but is inhibited in a wake maintenance zone before the minimum phase, and is disrupted in a zone following that phase. Different types of insomnia symptoms have been associated with abnormalities of the body temperature rhythm. Sleep onset insomnia is associated with a delayed temperature rhythm presumably, at least partly, because sleep is attempted during a delayed evening wake maintenance zone. Morning bright light has been used to phase advance circadian rhythms and successfully treat sleep onset insomnia. Conversely, early morning awakening insomnia has been associated with a phase advanced temperature rhythm and has been successfully treated with the phase delaying effects of evening bright light. Sleep maintenance insomnia has been associated not with a circadian rhythm timing abnormality, but with nocturnally elevated core body temperature. Combination of sleep onset and maintenance insomnia has been associated with a 24-h elevation of core body temperature supporting the chronic hyper-arousal model of insomnia. The possibility that these last two types of insomnia may be related to impaired thermoregulation, particularly a reduced ability to dissipate body heat from distal skin areas, has not been consistently supported in laboratory studies. Further studies of thermoregulation are needed in the typical home environment in which the insomnia is most evident. & 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Corresponding author. Tel.: +61 8 8201 2391;

fax: +61 8 8201 3877. E-mail address: leon.lack@inders.edu.au (L.C. Lack). 1087-0792/$ - see front matter & 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2008.02.003

ARTICLE IN PRESS
308 L.C. Lack et al. Habitual wake up time usually occurs about 13 h after the CBTmin, when core body temperature begins to rise.7,8 These relationships can be seen in Figure 1. However, of particular relevance to insomnia are two zones of the CBT rhythm during which sleep is inhibited.9,10 One zone occurs in the early evening, about 69 h before the CBTmin and 14 h before habitual bedtime (18:0022:00). It has been termed a wake-maintenance zone,10 because sleep onsets are delayed during this period.8,11 Another zone occurs in the morning about 47 h after the CBTmin (typically between 08:00 and 11:00) and is associated with increasing wakefulness in sleep or with wake up.10,11 Therefore, if the timing of the individuals endogenous circadian temperature rhythm is timed relatively late or relatively early with respect to the attempted sleep period, difculty initiating or maintaining sleep can occur. The endogenous melatonin circadian rhythm is highly correlated with the CBT rhythm and is sometimes used to estimate the timing of the circadian system.12,13 Plasma melatonin level begins to increase in the evening typically about 23 h before habitual bedtime (about 21:00, see Figure 1). Levels peak between 02:00 and 04:00, the midpoint of the sleep period and about one to two hours before the CBTmin, decreasing to barely detectable levels before 09:00, about 1 h after habitual wake time.12,14

Insomnia
Insomnia is a very generic term with the criteria including difculty initiating sleep, maintaining sleep, and early morning awakening, accompanied by some form of daytime impairment (e.g., fatigue).1 This review will focus on these three areas of sleep difculties and the relationship with body temperatures. The International Classication of Sleep Disorders (ICSD-2)1 elaborates several categories of insomnia primarily on the basis of its etiology and contributing causes such as psychophysiological insomnia, inadequate sleep hygiene, etc. There are also insomnias arising from circadian rhythm sleep disorders described in a separate section of the ICSD. We will elaborate on the contribution of circadian rhythm timing disorders as indicated, in particular, by the timing of the core body temperature rhythm. These timing disorders will relate to both difculties initiating sleep (sleep onset insomnia) and early morning awakening insomnia. Difculty maintaining sleep and excessive nocturnal wakefulness will be related to nocturnal hyper-arousal as indicated primarily by elevated nocturnal core temperature. In addition, the review will look at the relationship of insomnia to the regulation of body temperature.

Body temperature and circadian rhythm timing


Periodic circadian (24-h) variations or rhythms are ubiquitous in the human body and include physiological and behavioral rhythms of core body temperature, melatonin secretion, sleep propensity and subjective alertness. The sleepwake cycle and body temperature rhythms have a stable internal phase relationship in normally entrained conditions with the timing of sleep highly correlated with the timing or phase of the circadian body temperature rhythm.2 The temperature and melatonin circadian rhythms remain synchronized primarily by the entraining effects of the light/dark cycle. Endogenous core body temperature variations over time follow a near sinusoidal rhythm with the temperature minimum normally occurring between 04:00 and 06:00.3,4 Sleep propensity is closely linked to core body temperature. Sleep is typically initiated on the falling limb of the temperature rhythm, and about 56 h before the core body temperature minimum (CBTmin), with maximum circadian sleepiness occurring at the CBTmin.5,6 For an individual whose habitual sleep onset is 23:00, the CBTmin would occur between 04:00 and 05:00.

Sleep onset insomnia and delayed sleep phase disorder


Sleep onset difculties may occur when an individuals temperature rhythm is timed relatively late with respect to their attempted sleep period. In these cases the delayed temperature rhythm may result in a delay of the evening wake maintenance zone such that it coincides with the attempt to sleep. The evening wake maintenance zones of normal and sleep onset insomniacs are illustrated in Figure 2. Some studies have found that individuals experiencing sleep onset insomnia have a relatively delayed circadian rhythm. In one study,15 the sleep and temperature rhythm of 13 subjects (average age 33.7 years) who experienced sleep onset insomnia were compared to a similarly aged group of good sleepers. The polysomnographically measured (PSG) sleep onset latency of the insomnia group was 42 min compared to 11 min for the good sleepers. The insomniacs endogenous mean CBTmin, measured in a constant routine methodology, was at 07:18 and their typical

ARTICLE IN PRESS
The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures 309

Core Temperature

Sleep Propensity

Melatonin

SLEEP PERIOD

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21 22

23

24

10

11

12

TIME (24 Hour Clock)

Figure 1 Diagrammatic representation of normally entrained endogenous rhythms of core body temperature (solid curve), plasma melatonin (dotted curve), and objective sleep propensity (dashed curve) placed in the context of the 24-h clock time and normal sleep period (shaded area).8,13

38 Wake Maintenance Zones

CORE BODY TEMP. C

37

36

EMA Tmin

Normal Tmin

SOI Tmin

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21 22 23 24

2 3

7 8

10

11

12

TIME (24 Hour Clock)

Figure 2 Diagrammatic representation of the circadian core body temperature rhythms of normally entrained good sleepers (solid line), sleep onset insomniacs (SOI, dotted line), and early morning awakening insomniacs (EMA, dashed line) showing their temperature minima, wake maintenance zones, and morning wake zones (thick dotted line).

bedtimes were at 24:00. Therefore, the insomnia group was attempting sleep within their wake maintenance zone estimated to occur between 22:18 and 01:18. On the other hand, the good

sleepers were attempting sleep at about 23:00, closer to their mean CBTmin of 03:15, and therefore, about 2 h after the evening wake maintenance zone.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
310 In another pilot study we assessed the circadian timing of the melatonin rhythm and sleep of 16 participants (average age 29 years) with sleep onset insomnia.16 The timing of salivary dim light melatonin onset (DLMO)17 which is estimated to occur about 7 h before the CBTmin18,19 was used as an indicator of the timing of their circadian rhythm.17,20 The time of DLMO for this group was later than 24:00, indicating a delayed circadian rhythm. Their average bedtime was 00:30 and average sleep onset time was 01:45. As in the previous study these subjects were also attempting to fall asleep during their predicted wake maintenance zone. Cases with more severely delayed circadian rhythms can result in delayed sleep phase disorder (DSPD), previously known as delayed sleep phase syndrome.1 Weitzman and colleagues21 rst described the syndrome of delayed sleep phase insomnia in patients who were unable to fall asleep until about 02:00 or even as late as 06:00, and therefore had chronic difculty falling asleep at the desired time necessary to meet their daily commitments. When ambulatory core body temperature was monitored in individuals with DSPD, their CBTmin was approximately 2 h later when compared to a control group of good sleepers.22 Watanabe and colleagues23 assessed the sleep and circadian rhythm of individuals with DSPD. Their ambulatory core body temperature minimum occurred at 07:43, over 2.5 h later than the control group. On the two nights of PSG, their sleep onset latency was over 100 min and they were not able to fall asleep until 02:15. In contrast, the control group had a latency of only 5 min and a sleep onset time at 23:30. Those individuals with DSPD were attempting sleep in their wake maintenance zone that would have extended from about 23:0002:00, and hence had difculty falling asleep at their chosen bedtime. Therefore, considerable evidence relates sleep onset insomnia and a delayed sleep onset time with a delayed core body temperature rhythm and late timed circadian clock in general. L.C. Lack et al. and wake times at 03:00. If the person tries to obtain more sleep in the morning by staying in bed, frequent unsuccessful attempts to reinitiate sleep may lead to conditioned insomnia.1,24 An advanced sleep phase is associated with an advanced circadian core body temperature rhythm25,26 and melatonin rhythm.27 In individuals with ASPD, Jones and colleagues25 found their mean temperature and melatonin rhythms were timed about 4 h earlier than normal. This would place their typical wake up time of 04:18 at about the start of their morning wake zone. Those who present symptoms of early morning awakening insomnia (i.e., chronic difculty waking before intended, getting inadequate sleep, and reporting daytime tiredness) may also show an advanced temperature rhythm. Using a laboratory controlled constant routine procedure to eliminate external masking effects28 we evaluated the endogenous core body temperature timing of individuals who experienced early morning awakening insomnia.29 Individuals with early morning awakening insomnia were compared with an agematched group of good sleepers. The mean CBTmin of the insomnia group occurred at 00:20, almost 4 h earlier than the control group. The wake up time of the insomnia group at 04:49 occurred in their estimated morning wake zone preventing them from falling back to sleep and limiting their total sleep amount to less than 6 h. In a further constant routine study evaluating the effectiveness of light therapy for early morning awakening insomnia,24 participants average baseline CBTmin occurred at 01:40 with the wake zone estimated to start before 06:00. This coincided closely with their subjective (sleep diary) wake up time at about 05:45 and an objective wake up time (actigraphy) at 06:15. Again, participants were unable to sleep-in later and could obtain no more than 6 h total sleep. Therefore, ASPD and early morning awakening insomnia are associated with an early timed temperature rhythm that would predictably limit the ability to extend sleep in the morning. The relatively early timed temperature rhythm and associated morning wake zone of early morning awakening insomniacs is illustrated in Figure 2 as the dashed curve advanced about 3 h earlier than normal.

Early morning awakening insomnia and advanced sleep phase disorder


The ICSD-2 nosology describes advanced sleep phase disorder (ASPD), or earlier terminology syndrome, as a disorder in which the major sleep period is early (advanced) with respect to the desired sleep/wake period.1 Individuals with ASPD have overwhelming early evening sleepiness, an early sleep onset and early morning awakenings. For example, bedtimes could be as early as 19:00

The treatment of insomnia by phase shifting (re-timing) circadian rhythms


If treatments that appropriately shift the rhythms of sleep onset and early morning awakening insomnia are efcacious, it would support the

ARTICLE IN PRESS
The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures prediction that temperature rhythm de-synchrony contributes to the etiology of these two insomnias. Initial studies have demonstrated that bright light can phase shift circadian rhythms,3032 with bright light presented before the CBTmin delaying the circadian rhythm and bright light administered after the CBTmin advancing the rhythm.3337 Morning bright light has advanced the sleep and temperature rhythm of individuals with DSPD3840 and DSPD with associated sleep onset insomnia.4143 Conversely, evening bright light has been shown to be effective in phase delaying the sleep and body temperature rhythms of insomniacs with an early CBTmin (before 02:30) and lengthening the sleep of early morning awakening insomnia.24,44 A nal example is an experimental study in hemodialysis patients demonstrating that early morning awakening is associated with an early morning advanced drop in skin temperature.45 If the early morning drop in skin temperature is prevented by conditioning of the dialysate temperature, the patients sleep better in the early morning. 311

Practice points
1. Sleep onset insomnia can be caused by a delayed circadian rhythm and can be treated with morning bright light therapy. 2. Early morning awakening insomnia can be caused by an early timed circadian rhythm that can be treated with evening bright light therapy.

Insomnia from hyper-arousal


Independent of circadian factors, it is clear that insomnia can arise from a wide range of stimuli that promote arousal. These include environmental (e.g., noise, elevated ambient temperature), social (e.g., poor sleep habits, shiftwork), psychological (e.g., conditioned responses, anxiety) and physiological (respiratory impairment, restless legs, pain) stimuli. At a broad conceptual level it is generally held that such stimuli lead to a hyper-aroused state that is the immediate cause of poor sleep and secondary daytime symptoms of insomnia (e.g., fatigue, impaired mood).4650 However, whether the secondary daytime symptoms associated with poor sleep arise from the poor sleep or chronic hyper-arousal separately or in combination remains contentious.

From a series of studies, Bonnet and Arand47,51,52 suggested that the secondary symptoms of insomnia (e.g., fatigue, cognitive impairment, and general dysphoria) are not due to loss of sleep per se, but may be better explained instead by persistent hyper-arousal. For example, disrupting the sleep of normal sleepers to simulate that of a typical insomniac resulted in increased sleepiness that is not usually associated with chronic insomnia despite their reduced total sleep.47 In another study where sleep maintenance insomniacs had their poor sleep further disrupted by articially increasing their wakeful periods they showed increased sleepiness rather than the more typical secondary symptoms of insomnia.51 By contrast, when hyper-arousal was induced in a group of 12 good sleepers over a week by administering a total of 1200 mg of caffeine across each day, they found the changes in physiological, sleep, and daytime functioning measures were comparable to those seen in chronic primary insomnia.52 Further support for the hyper-arousal model of insomnia has come from evidence showing that physiological arousal (e.g., adrenocortical activity, catecholamine levels, electrodermal conductance, frontalis muscle tension, heart rate, metabolic rate, and peripheral vasoconstriction) is raised in insomniacs compared to controls.46,49,5364 A particular focus in the arousal and sleep literature has been an exploration of the relationship between nocturnal core body temperature and sleep onset. Compared to controls, insomniacs are reported to have higher oral pre-sleep onset temperatures58 and, under ad lib sleep conditions, both higher rectal61 and oral49 temperatures over the sleep period. Finally, consistent with the inverse relationship reported between core body temperature and distal peripheral temperature,4 insomniacs are also reported to have lower nger temperatures in the minutes prior to sleep onset.56 By contrast, two studies reported no difference between insomniacs and controls in core body temperature and other correlates of arousal. Adam et al.58 observed no signicant difference under ad lib sleep conditions in either post-sleep onset oral temperature or daytime adrenocortical activity in middle-aged to elderly insomniacs compared to controls. Similarly, Freedman and Sattler56 observed no difference in a variety of autonomic activity measures (e.g., EMG, heart rate, nger temperature) between young sleep onset insomniacs and controls during ad lib sleep. However, it is to be noted in both cases that the trends were in the expected direction. Despite evidence of an association between physiological arousal and insomnia it is noteworthy

ARTICLE IN PRESS
312 that the majority of studies have measured arousal under ad lib sleep conditions. This methodological limitation may account for some of the group differences reported between insomniacs and controls. For example, group differences in physical activity and posture rather than sleep quality may account for temperature differences.6568 Similarly, core temperature may be elevated in insomniacs compared to controls during ad lib sleep conditions simply because temperature is elevated during wakefulness and by denition insomniacs spend a greater period of the night awake.69 In brief, it could be argued that elevated core body temperature is simply a result of sleepwake state rather than an endogenous contributor to insomnia as the hyper-arousal model would suggest. More direct evidence that core body temperature is elevated in insomniacs has come from work by our group70 where we examined temperature under wakeful constant routine conditions. In our study we found that aged insomniacs (primarily sleep maintenance insomniacs) compared to controls had higher core body temperatures prior to their habitual sleep onset time at home and that this persisted across the wakeful constant routine night until early morning when both groups converged and showed similar core temperature values across the remainder of the day. These ndings suggest that aged sleep maintenance insomniacs are not chronically hyper-aroused across the 24-h period but are endogenously hyper-aroused at night. It should be noted that one other study of core temperature evaluated in a constant routine protocol failed to nd differences between good sleepers and insomniacs during the day or at night.71 Whether this lack of difference arose from the use of a younger age group or smaller differences in objective sleep parameters between groups needs further investigation. Our data support Sewitchs50 proposition that insomnia is characterized by a failure to down regulate temperature at night. It is also consistent with a large body of evidence that a rise in distal peripheral temperature and a concomitant fall in core body temperature presages successful sleep onset.7276 This last nding anticipates the next section that reviews the possible relationship of insomnia to abnormal thermoregulation. In conclusion, there is some evidence to suggest that hyper-arousal can account for the sleep loss and perhaps the daytime symptoms of insomnia. What remains to be more fully determined is whether hyper-arousal is simply restricted to the L.C. Lack et al. night time period or whether it persists over the day in insomnia.

Practice points
1. Considerable evidence supports the chronic hyper-arousal model of insomnia. 2. The evidence from constant routine studies of core body temperature only supports nocturnal elevated CBT in aged sleep maintenance insomniacs.

Thermoregulation and Insomnia


Skin temperatures role in thermoregulation
We have already described the relationships between core body temperature and poor sleep. However, a review of insomnia and temperature would be incomplete without reference to the role of skin temperature in thermoregulation. The regulation of core body temperature occurs as a combination of heat production and heat loss.3 When heat production is greater than heat loss, core body temperature increases and, conversely, when heat loss exceeds heat production, as in the late evening, it decreases. As the circadian rhythm of heat loss is phase advanced with respect to heat production, this has led many researchers to conclude that the process of heat loss rather than changes in heat production primarily drives the circadian rhythm of core body temperature.3,4 With regards to insomnia and in particular, sleep onset insomnia, the mid to late evening could be considered the critical part of the circadian body temperature rhythm. Prior to retiring to bed, the core temperature rhythm should be on the decline. However, this requires heat loss from the core through the transference of heated blood to various blood vessels located in the skin. The vessels most effective at rapidly losing heat are known as arterio-venous anastomoses (AVAs), and are concentrated in more distal regions of the skin (i.e., hands, feet, nose, lips, and ears).75,77 Increased transference of blood to distal skin regions can be indirectly measured via increases of distal skin temperatures. Since the proximal skin region (i.e., forehead, abdomen, thigh, and infraclavicular) blood vessels are less involved with thermoregulation,78,79 this section will focus on distal skin temperature in relation to insomnia.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures 313

Skin temperature and the sympathetic nervous system


Heat loss from distal skin areas occurs most rapidly when AVAs are maximally dilated.4,77 Conversely, when vasoconstriction occurs, the amount of blood ow is restricted causing distal skin temperature values to decrease towards ambient air temperature (e.g., 2022 1C in a laboratory). Vasomotor activity of AVAs is directed primarily from sympathetic nervous system constrictor neurons. When these neurons are activated, smooth muscles around AVAs constrict causing vasoconstriction, sometimes to the point of ceasing blood ow.77 Alternatively, when there is reduced activation of these sympathetic constrictor neurons, the smooth muscles relax, vasodilation takes place, and blood ow increases.77 As a result, skin temperature rises towards values close to core body temperature and heat loss occurs through convection to cooler ambient air. Many studies of good sleepers have found a close association between distal skin temperature increases and the onset of sleep.76,8085 Some pathological conditions also support this relationship. Pache et al.86 found that those with vasospastic syndrome from chronic peripheral vasoconstriction causing cold hands and feet also suffer from difculty initiating sleep. At the other extreme narcoleptics who have abnormally short sleep onset latencies also have chronically elevated distal/proximal skin temperature gradients (DPG) and showed increases of DPG during sleep arising mainly from robust increases in distal skin temperature.87 A suggested causal mechanism involves warm sensitive neurons in the pre-optic area/anterior hypothalamus that receive input from skin and that are also involved in sleep initiation.79,8890 This suggestion is consistent with recent evidence that inducing an elevation in skin temperature promotes sleep onset,90,91 and enhances sleep depth both in normal sleepers and elderly insomniacs.92 However, these processes have received little attention in insomniacs. It has been postulated that distal skin temperatures around sleep onset for insomniacs may undergo a different process.76,93

within a reasonable amount of time (e.g., within 1015 min). In contrast, insomniacs take longer to fall asleep initially and following awakenings from sleep.15,50,56,58 Unlike good sleepers, insomniacs are not relaxed, even to the point of being anxious when attempting sleep.58,94 This view has been supported very recently with the nding that the normal reduction of cognitive arousal and alertness at bedtime is attenuated in those with sleep onset insomnia.95 Their anxiety and alertness may be associated with an activation of their sympathetic nervous system, resulting in vasoconstriction of AVAs, and indicated by decreased distal skin temperatures. Some studies have found relationships between anxiety-provoking stimuli and lower nger skin temperatures in good sleepers.96 Hence, if insomniacs nd that the attempt to sleep produces even mild anxiety, it would be expected to increase sympathetic nervous system activation and lower skin temperatures (at least compared to good sleepers).

Supporting research
The rst study to investigate the skin temperatures of insomniacs attempting sleep was conducted in 1979 by Brown97 who found that insomniacs did show increases in toe skin temperature when attempting sleep, but that sometimes there was no observable change. Yet, when toe temperature increases were observed, they were more variable, and took twice as long to reach the same amount of temperature change compared to good sleepers. Freedman and Sattler56 found that compared to good sleepers insomniacs have signicantly lower nger skin temperatures from lights out through to Stage 2 sleep onset. However, it appears that once sleep is achieved, differences in distal skin temperatures between insomniacs and good sleepers disappear, suggesting a critical period between lights out and sleep onset. It is worth noting that Freedman and Sattler56 found their insomniacs scored signicantly higher on measures of general anxiety and worry compared to good sleepers, providing support for a link between anxiety, insomnia and lower distal skin temperature. More recent attempts to investigate the notion that insomniacs have attenuated distal skin temperature increases when attempting sleep have found conicting results. Research by our group conrmed that middle-aged insomniacs with sleep onset and maintenance difculties reported both higher levels of general anxiety and sleep anticipatory anxiety in their home environments compared to good sleepers.98 Surprisingly though, we

Does insomnia limit the down regulation of core temperature as a result of impaired distal skin vasodilation?
Good sleepers are typically known to be relaxed when attempting sleep, and manage to fall asleep

ARTICLE IN PRESS
314 found the rapid increases in nger temperature of the insomniacs to be greater than those of good sleepers when they were falling asleep. However, it should be noted that in the laboratory the insomniacs fell asleep as quickly as the good sleepers and, in that sense, were not displaying their characteristic insomnia. The insomniacs did have signicantly higher core body temperatures (by approximately 0.2 1C) throughout the circadian rhythm thus supporting the chronic hyper-arousal theory for those suffering combined sleep onset and sleep maintenance insomnia. It would have been of interest for this study98 to have measured proximal skin temperature, as, along with core temperature, it would be likely to be higher in the insomniacs prior to the sleep attempt. If so, this would produce a lower distal/ proximal skin temperature gradient associated with longer sleep latencies in normal sleepers.82 However, such a result would not negate the nding of a robust increase of distal skin temperature when insomniacs fell asleep. The chronically higher core temperature also suggests that there was a greater need for the insomniacs to lose core heat via the ngers and they appeared able to do so in the laboratory free of their previous insomnia symptoms. On the other hand, a recent study by van den Heuvel et al.99 suggested that insomniacs have less ability to vasodilate in distal skin. They found that younger sleep onset insomniacs had no greater nger temperature increase when challenged by a warm (45 1C) contralateral hand bath than a neutral bath (3035 1C), whereas good sleepers showed a greater increase to the warm bath. However, because the baseline nger temperatures were not reported, it is not possible to rule out the possibility that the attenuated response in the insomnia group was due to a ceiling effect in their nger temperature. Regardless of whether insomniacs show attenuated vasodilation responses, it is still relevant, at least from a clinical perspective, to investigate whether skin warming would facilitate sleep onset. Raymann et al.100 have recently found that foot (distal) skin warming signicantly facilitated shorter sleep onsets in young and elderly healthy sleepers, but not signicantly in sleep disturbed elderly. Interestingly though, this elderly insomnia group showed the greatest slowing of reaction speed during proximal skin warming.100 Reaction speed and the ability to maintain wakefulness have also been shown to be sensitive to mild skin warming in narcolepsy.101 Therefore, in good sleepers, narcoleptics and possibly in insomniacs skin warming seems conducive to sleepiness which L.C. Lack et al. is consistent with the earlier suggested link between warm sensitive and sleep inducing neurons in the hypothalamus. However, as in the two previous studies this one was also conducted in the laboratory during the day without insomnia evident according to their relatively short sleep latencies. Further research is needed to determine whether vasodilation attenuation or non-reactivity to skin warming can be demonstrated in the bedroom environment where, presumably, their sleep onset is typically impaired and delayed. There are now wireless skin temperature sensors that would facilitate such home based studies.102

Practice points
1. While there is considerable understanding of the normal role of distal skin vasodilation in body heat loss and down regulation of core body temperature prior to bedtime and with the attempt to sleep, the few relevant studies have not consistently shown this process to be abnormal with insomnia. 2. One of these studies, however, has supported the chronic hyper-arousal model of combined sleep onset and maintenance insomnia.

Conclusion
Insomnia subtypes may be differentiated in terms of the aspect of sleep that is the predominant chronic difculty (difculty falling asleep, waking frequently for long periods during the night, early nal awakening before sufcient sleep is obtained). There appear to be specic abnormalities of body temperature associated with some of these subtypes. Difculty initiating sleep at a normal time (sleep onset insomnia) is associated with a circadian core body temperature rhythm that is delayed from normal timing indicating a delayed circadian clock likely to be a contributor to the insomnia. Morning bright light therapy has been used to phase advance the circadian rhythm and treat this type of insomnia. Early awakening insomnia has been associated with a circadian temperature rhythm timed much earlier than normal. Evening bright light therapy has been used to delay the circadian rhythms and treat this type of insomnia. Difculty with excessive nocturnal wakefulness has been

ARTICLE IN PRESS
The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures associated with elevated nocturnal core temperature, even in controlled laboratory constant routines, suggesting an endogenous nocturnal hyperarousal state at least in sleep maintenance insomnia. There is some evidence that the combination of sleep onset and maintenance insomnia is associated with elevated core temperature throughout the 24-h period and supports the chronic hyper-arousal model of insomnia. The possibility that elevated core temperature reects an impairment of the distal skin regions to vasodilate and dissipate body heat has received inconsistent support. Further research is needed to explore the etiological role of body temperature and thermoregulation under the conditions in which chronic insomnia is manifest in the home setting. 315

Research agenda
Future research should: 1. Examine further the 24-h core, proximal, and distal skin temperature rhythms and other correlates of arousal under controlled conditions for the differing insomnia subtypes compared to controls. 2. Extend the chronobiologic treatment research of those insomnia types showing an apparent mis-timing etiology (sleep onset, early morning awakening) in order to optimize treatment methods (e.g., timing and type of light therapy and other chronobiotic effectors). 3. Measure subjective arousal/anxiety in conjunction with sleep latency and distal skin temperature for sleep attempts of insomniacs in the typical home environment in which the insomnia is evident.

References
1. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The international classication of sleep disorders, (ICSD-2): diagnostic and coding manual, 2nd ed. Westchester, Illinois: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2005. 2. Czeisler CA, Weitzman ED, Moore-Ede MC, Zimmerman JC, Knauer RS. Human sleep: its duration and organization depend on its circadian phase. Science 1980;210:12647. 3. Aschoff JJ. Circadian control of body temperature. In: Hardy JD, Gagge AP, Stowijk AJ, editors. Physiological and behavioral temperature regulation. Springeld: Charles C Thomas; 1970. p. 90519.

4. Kra uchi K, Wirz-Justice A. Circadian rhythm of heat production, heart rate, and skin and core temperature under unmasking conditions in men. Am J Physiol 1994; 267:R81929. 5. Czeisler CA, Dumont M, Duffy JF, Steinberg JD, Richardson GS, Brown EN, et al. Association of sleepwake habits in older people with changes in output of circadian pacemaker. Lancet 1992;340:9336. 6. Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Sleepiness and sleep state on a 90-min schedule. Psychophysiology 1977;14:12733. 7. Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. Paradoxical timing of the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity serves to consolidate sleep and wakefulness in humans. Neurosci Lett 1994;166:638. 8. Lack LC, Lushington K. The rhythms of human sleep propensity and core body temperature. J Sleep Res 1996; 5:111. 9. Strogatz SH. The mathematical structure of the human sleepwake cycle. Lecture notes in biomathematics no. 69. Heidelberg: Springer; 1986. *10. Strogatz SH, Kronauer RE, Czeisler CA. Circadian pacemaker interferes with sleep onset at specic times each day: role in insomnia. Am J Physiol 1987;253:R1728. 11. Dijk D-J, Czeisler CA. Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans. J Neurosci 1995;15:352638. 12. Shanahan TL, Czeisler CA. Light exposure induces equivalent phase shifts of the endogenous circadian rhythms of circulating plasma melatonin and core body temperature in men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1991;73:22735. 13. kerstedt T, Fro berg JE, Friberg Y, Wetterberg L. Melatonin excretion, body temperature and subjective arousal during 64 hours of sleep deprivation. Psychoendocrinology 1979; 4:21925. *14. Monk TM, Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Berga SL, Jarrett DB, Begley AE, et al. Circadian rhythms in human performance and mood under constant conditions. J Sleep Res 1997; 6:918. 15. Morris M, Lack L, Dawson D. Sleep-onset insomniacs have delayed temperature rhythms. Sleep 1990;13(1):114. 16. Lack L, Wright H, Paynter D. The treatment of sleep onset insomnia with morning bright light. Sleep Res 1995;24:338. 17. Lewy AJ, Sack RA, Singer CL. Assessment and treatment of chronobiologic disorders using plasma melatonin levels and bright light exposure: the clock-gate model and the phase response curve. Psychopharmacol Bull 1985;21:36872. 18. Brown EN, Choe Y, Shanahan TL, Czeisler CA. A mathematical model of diurnal variations in human plasma melatonin levels. Am J Physiol 1997;272:E50616. 19. Eastman CI, Martin SK, Hebert M. Failure of extraocular light to facilitate circadian rhythm re-entrainment in humans. Chronobiol Int 2000;17:80726. 20. Lewy AJ, Sack RA. The dim light melatonin onset as a marker for circadian phase position. Chronobiol Int 1989; 6(1):93102. 21. Weitzman ED, Czeisler CA, Coleman RM, Spielman AJ, Zimmerman JC, Dement W, et al. Delayed sleep phase syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981;38:73746. 22. Ozaki S, Uchiyama M, Shirakawa S, Okawa M. Prolonged interval from body temperature nadir to sleep offset in patients with delayed sleep phase syndrome. Sleep 1996;19(1):3640. *23. Watanabe T, Kajimura N, Kato M, Sekimoto M, Nakajima T, Hori T, et al. Sleep and circadian rhythm disturbances in
The most important references are denoted by an asterisk.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
316
patients with delayed sleep phase syndrome. Sleep 2003;26(6):65761. Lack L, Wright H, Kemp K, Gibbon S. The treatment of early-morning awakening insomnia with 2 evenings of bright light. Sleep 2005;28:61623. Jones CR, Campbell SS, Zone SE, Cooper F, DeSano A, Murphy P, et al. Familial advanced sleep-phase syndrome: a short-period circadian rhythm variant in humans. Nat Med 1999;5(9):10625. Ondze B, Espa F, Ming LC, Chakkar B, Besset B, Billiard M. Advanced sleep phase syndrome. Rev Neurol 2001;157(11):S1305. Reid R, Chang AM, Dubocovich ML, Turek F, Takahashi JS, Zee PC. Familial advanced sleep phase syndrome. Arch Neurol 2001;58:108994. Czeisler CA, Brown EN, Ronda JM, Kronauer RE, Richardson GS, Freitay WO. A clinical method to assess the endogenous circadian phase (ECP) of the deep circadian oscillator in man. Sleep Res 1985;14:295. Lack LC, Mercer JD, Wright H. Circadian rhythms of early morning awakening insomniacs. J Sleep Res 1996;5: 2119. Czeisler CA, Allan JS, Strogatz SH, Ronda JM, Sanchez R, Rios CD, et al. Bright light resets the human circadian pacemaker independent of the timing of the sleepwake cycle. Science 1986;233:66771. Lewy AJ, Sack RL, Miller LS, Hoban TM. Antidepressant and circadian phase-shifting effects of light. Science 1987; 235:3524. Lewy AJ, Wehr TA, Goodwin FK, Newsome DA, Markey SP. Light suppresses melatonin secretion in humans. Science 1980;210:12678. Czeisler CA, Kronauer RE, Allan JS, Duffy JF, Jewett MR, Brown EN, et al. Bright light induction of strong (type 0) resetting of the human circadian pacemaker. Science 1989;244:132833. Jewett ME, Rimmer DW, Duffy JF, Klerman EB, Kronauer RE, Czeisler CA. Human circadian pacemaker is sensitive to light throughout subjective day without evidence of transients. Am J Physiol 1997;273:R18009. Khalsa SBS, Jewett ME, Cajochen C, Czeisler CA. A phase response curve to single bright light pulses in human subjects. J Physiol 2003;549.3:94552. Lewy AJ, Sack RL, Fredrickson RH, Reaves M, Denney D, Zielske DR. The use of bright light in the treatment of chronobiologic sleep and mood disorders: the phase response curve. Psychopharmacol Bull 1983;19(3):5235. Minors DS, Waterhouse JM, Wirz-Justice A. A human phaseresponse curve to light. Neurosci Lett 1991;133: 3640. Rosenthal NE, Joseph-Vanderpool JR, Levendosky AA, Johnston SH, Allen R, Kelly KA, et al. Phase-shifting effects of bright morning light as treatment for delayed sleep phase syndrome. Sleep 1990;13(4):35461. Watanabe T, Kajimura N, Kato M, Sekimoto M, Takahashi K. Effects of phototherapy in patients with delayed sleep phase syndrome. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999;53: 2313. Lack L, Bramwell T, Wright H, Kemp K. Morning blue light can advance the melatonin rhythm in mild delayed sleep phase syndrome. Sleep Biol Rhythms 2007;5:7880. Czeisler CA, Kronauer RE, Johnson MP, Allan JS, Johnson TS, Dumont M. Action of light on the human circadian pacemaker: treatment of patients with circadian rhythm sleep disorders. In: Horne J, editor. Sleep 88. Stuttgart: Verlag; 1989. p. 427.

L.C. Lack et al.


42. Dawson D, Morris M, Lack L. The phase shifting effects if a single 4 h exposure to bright morning light in normals and DSPS subjects. Sleep Res 1989;17:415. 43. Lack L, Wright H, Paynter D. The treatment of sleep onset insomnia with bright morning light. Sleep Biol Rhythms 2007;5:1739. 44. Lack L, Wright H. The effect of evening bright light in delaying the circadian rhythms and lengthening the sleep or early morning awakening insomniacs. Sleep 1993;16: 43643. 45. Parker KP, Bailey JL, Rye DB, Bliwise DL, Van Someren EJW. Lowering dialysate temperature improves sleep and alters nocturnal skin temperature in patients on chronic hemodialysis. J Sleep Res 2007;16:4250. 46. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 24-h metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Sleep 1995;18(7):5818. 47. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. The consequences of a week of insomnia. Sleep 1996;19(6):45361. 48. Mendelson WB, Garnett D, Gillin JC, Weingartner H. The experience of insomnia and daytime and nighttime functioning. Psychiatry Res 1984;12:23550. 49. Monroe LJ. Psychological and physiological differences between good and poor sleepers. J Abnormal Psychol 1967;72(3):25564. 50. Sewitch DE. Slow wave sleep deciency insomnia: a problem in thermoregulation at sleep onset. Psychophysiology 1987;24(2):20015. 51. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. The consequences of a week of insomnia II: patients with insomnia. Sleep 1998;21(4): 35968. 52. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Caffeine use as a model of acute and chronic insomnia. Sleep 1992;15(6):52636. 53. Johns M, Gay T, Masterton J, Bruce D. Relationship between sleep habits, adrenocortical activity and personality. Psychosom Med 1971;33:499507. 54. Haynes S, Sides H, Lockwood G. Relaxation instructions and frontalis electromyographic feedback intervention with sleep-onset insomnia. Behav Ther 1977;8:64452. 55. Haynes SN, Adams A, Franzen M. The effects of pre-sleep stress on sleep-onset insomnia. J Abnormal Psych 1981;90: 6016. *56. Freedman RR, Sattler HL. Physiological and psychological factors in sleep-onset insomnia. J Abnormal Psychol 1982; 91(5):3809. 57. Haynes SN, Fitzgerald SG, Shute GE, Hall M. The utility and validity of daytime naps in the assessment of sleep-onset insomnia. J Behav Med 1985;8:23747. 58. Adam K, Tomeny M, Oswald I. Physiological and psychological differences between good and poor sleepers. J Psychiatr Res 1986;20(4):30116. 59. Stepanski E, Zorick F, Roehrs T, Young D, Roth T. Daytime alertness in patients with chronic insomnia compared to asymptomatic control subjects. Sleep 1988;11(1):5460. 60. Broman J-E, Hetta J. Electrodermal activity in patients with persistent insomnia. J Sleep Res 1994;3:16570. 61. Vgontzas AN, Tsigos C, Bixler EO, Stratakis CA, Zachman K, Kales A, et al. Chronic insomnia and activity of the stress system: a preliminary study. J Psychosom Res 1998;45(1): 2131. 62. Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Lin H-M, Prolo P, Mastorakos G, Vela-Bueno A, et al. Chronic insomnia is associated with nyctohemeral activation of the hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal axis: clinical implications. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2001;86(8):378794. 63. Rodenbeck A, Hajak G. Neuroendocrine dysregulation in primary insomnia. Rev Neurol 2001;157(11):S5761.

*24.

25.

26.

27.

28.

29.

*30.

31.

32.

33.

34.

35.

36.

37.

38.

39.

40.

41.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures
64. Rodenbeck A, Huether G, Ru ther E, Hajak G. Interactions between evening and nocturnal cortisol secretion and sleep parameters in patients with severe chronic primary insomnia. Neurosci Lett 2002;324:15963. 65. Kleitman N, Doktorsky A. The effects of the position of the body and of sleep on rectal temperature in man. Am J Physiol 1933;104:3403. 66. Saltin B, Hermansen L. Esophageal, rectal, and muscle temperature during exercise. J Appl Physiol 1966;21: 175762. 67. Gander PH, Graeber RC, Connell LJ. Masking of the circadian rhythms of heart rate and core temperature by the restactivity rhythm in man. Sleep Res 1985;14:298. 68. Wever RA. Internal interactions within the human circadian system: the masking effect. Experientia 1985;41:33242. 69. Barrett J, Lack L, Morris M. The sleep-evoked decrease of body temperature. Sleep 1993;16(2):939. *70. Lushington K, Dawson D, Lack L. Core body temperature is elevated during constant wakefulness in elderly sleepers. Sleep 2000;23(4):17. 71. Varkevisser M, Van Dongen HPA, Kerkhof GA. Physiologic indexes in chronic insomnia during a constant routine: evidence for general hyperarousal? Sleep 2005;28: 158896. 72. Campbell SS, Broughton RJ. Rapid decline in body temperature before sleep: ufng the physiological pillow? Chronobiol Int 1994;11:12631. 73. Murphy PJ, Campbell SS. Nighttime drop in body temperature: a physiological trigger for sleep onset? Sleep 1997; 20:50511. 74. Kra uchi K, Wirz-Justice A. Circadian clues to sleep onset mechanisms. Neuropharmacol 2001;25(5 suppl.):S926. 75. Van Someren EJW. More than a marker: interaction between the circadian regulation of temperature and sleep, age-related changes, and treatment possibilities. Chronobiol Int 2000;17:31354. 76. Lack L, Gradisar M. Acute nger temperature changes preceding sleep onsets over a 45-h period. J Sleep Res 2002;11:27582. 77. Morris JL. Autonomic vasoconstriction in the skin. In: Morris JL, Gibbins IL, editors. Autonomic innervation of the skin. Australia: Harwood Academic; 1997. p. 5785. 78. Charkoudian N. Skin blood ow in adult human thermoregulation: how it works, when it does not, and why. Mayo Clin Proc 2003;78:60312. 79. Kra uchi K. The human sleepwake cycle reconsidered from a thermoregulatory point of view. Physiol Behav 2007;90: 23645. 80. Kleitman R, Ramsaroop A, Engelmann T. Variations in skin temperatures of the feet and hands and the onset of sleep. Fed Proc 1948;7:66. 81. Kra uchi K, Cajochen C, Werth E, Wirz-Justice A. Warm feet promote the rapid onset of sleep. Nature 1999;401:367. 82. Kra uchi K, Cajochen C, Werth E, Wirz-Justice A. Functional link between distal vasodilation and sleep-onset latency? Am J Physiol 2000;278:R7418. 83. Kra uchi K, Cajochen C, Werth E, Renz C, von Arb M, WirzJustice A. Thermoregulatory changes begin after lights off and not after onset of sleep stage 2. Sleep 2001;24:A165. 84. Kubo H, Yanase T, Akagi H. Sleep stage and skin temperature regulation during night-sleep in winter. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999;53:1213.

317

85. van den Heuvel CJ, Noone JT, Lushington K, Dawson D. Changes in sleepiness and body temperature precede nocturnal sleep onset: evidence from a polysomnographic study in young men. J Sleep Res 1998;7:15966. 86. Pache M, Kra uchi K, Cajochen C, Wirz-Justice A, Bubler B, Flammer J, et al. Cold feet and prolonged sleeponset latency in vasospastic syndrome. Lancet 2001;358: 1256. 87. Fronczek R, Overeem S, Lammers GJ, van Dijk JG, Van Someren EJW. Altered skin-temperature regulation in narcolepsy relates to sleep propensity. Sleep 2006;29: 14449. 88. Van Someren EJW. Sleep propensity is modulated by circadian and behavior-induced changes in cutaneous temperature. J Thermal Biol 2004;29:43744. 89. Van Someren EJW. Mechanisms and functions of coupling between sleep and temperature rhythms. Prog Brain Res 2006;153:30924. 90. Raymann RJEM, Swaab DF, Van Someren EJW. Cutaneous warming promotes sleep onset. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2005;288:R158997. 91. Raymann RJEM, Swaab DF, Van Someren EJW. Skin temperature and sleep-onset latency: changes with age and insomnia. Physiol Behav 2007;90:25766. 92. Raymann RJEM, Swaab DF, Van Someren EJW. Skin deep: cutaneous temperature determines sleep depth. Brain 2008;131:50013. 93. Gilbert SS, Van den Heuvel CJ, Dawson D. Daytime melatonin and temazepam in young adult humans: equivalent effects on sleep latency and body temperatures. J Physiol 1999;514:90514. 94. Bootzin R, Shoham V, Kuo T. Sleep anticipatory anxiety questionnaire: a measure of anxiety about sleep. Sleep Res 1994;23:188. 95. Robertson JA, Broomeld NM, Espie CA. Prospective comparison of subjective arousal during the pre-sleep period in primary sleep onset insomnia and normal sleepers. J Sleep Res 2007;16:2308. 96. Kistler A, Mariauzols C, von Berlepsch K. Fingertip temperature as an indicator for sympathetic responses. Int J Psychophysiol 1998;29:3541. 97. Brown CC. Toe temperature change: a measure of sleep onset? Waking Sleeping 1979;3:3539. *98. Gradisar M, Lack L, Wright H, Harris J, Brooks A. Do chronic primary insomniacs have impaired heat loss when attempting sleep? Am J Physiol 2006;290:R111521. *99. Van den Heuvel C, Ferguson S, Dawson D. Attenuated thermoregulatory response to mild thermal challenge in subjects with sleep-onset insomnia. Sleep 2006;29: 116773. 100. Raymann RJEM, Van Someren EJW. Time-on-task impairment of psychomotor vigilance is affected by mild skin warming and changes with aging and insomnia. Sleep 2007;30:96103. 101. Fronczek R, Raymann RJEM, Romeijn N, Overeem S, Fischer M, Van Dijk JG, et al. Manipulation of core body and skin temperature improves vigilance and maintenance of wakefulness in narcolepsy. Sleep 2008;31:23340. 102. van Marken Lichtenbelt WD, Daanen HA, Wouters L, Fronczek R, Raymann RJ, Severens NM, et al. Evaluation of wireless determination of skin temperature using iButtons. Physiol Behav 2006;88:48997.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai