Anda di halaman 1dari 9

Aesth Plast Surg (2008) 32:200208 DOI 10.

1007/s00266-007-9080-z

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Marquardts Phi Mask: Pitfalls of Relying on Fashion Models and the Golden Ratio to Describe a Beautiful Face
E. Holland

Published online: 4 January 2008 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Abstract Background Stephen Marquardt has derived a mask from the golden ratio that he claims represents the ideal facial archetype. Many have found his mask convincing, including cosmetic surgeons. However, Marquardts mask is associated with numerous problems. The method used to examine goodness of t with the proportions in the mask is faulty. The mask is ill-suited for non-European populations, especially sub-Saharan Africans and East Asians. The mask also appears to approximate the face shape of masculinized European women. Given that the general public strongly and overwhelmingly prefers above average facial femininity in women, white women seeking aesthetic facial surgery would be ill-advised to aim toward a better t with Marquardts mask. This article aims to show the proper way of assessing goodness of t with Marquardts mask, to address the shape of the mask as it pertains to masculinity-femininity, and to discuss the broader issue of an objective assessment of facial attractiveness. Methods Generalized Procrustes analysis is used to show how goodness of t with Marquardts mask can be assessed. Thin-plate spline analysis is used to illustrate visually how sample faces, including northwestern European averages, differ from Marquardts mask. Results Marquardts mask best describes the facial proportions of masculinized white women as seen in fashion models. Conclusions Marquardts mask does not appear to describe ideal face shape even for white women because

its proportions are inconsistent with the optimal preferences of most people, especially with regard to femininity. Keywords Aesthetics Golden ratio Phi mask Procrustes analysis Stephen Marquardt Thin-plate splines

E. Holland (&) P.O. Box 2994, Tempe, AZ 85280, USA e-mail: fbeauty_webmaster@yahoo.com URL: http://www.femininebeauty.info/

The nature of beauty is an intriguing topic. It is tempting to describe beauty in terms of a simple, recurring theme such as the golden ratio. Dividing a line segment into two parts such that the ratio of these parts equals the ratio of the larger part and the line segment gives us the golden ratio (Phi). A number of attempts have been made to describe ideal facial proportions in terms of the golden ratio [14,17,30,34]. However, several studies have not found any relationship between facial attractiveness and compliance with golden ratio proportions [20,22,27,33,38] or evidence of any special aesthetic preference for the golden ratio among humans [6]. Stephen Marquardt [19] has tortuously derived supposedly ideal facial proportions from the golden ratio. Marquardts Phi mask has been favorably reviewed by some authors [1,14,16]. Kim [16] reported that Marquardts mask is convenient for facial analytics, and Bashour [1] has claimed that Marquardts mask is a suitable tool for developing an objective system to assess facial beauty. However, Marquardts mask is associated with a number of problems. A preliminary examination of Marquardts mask does not suggest that it describes ideal face shape. Figure 1 shows the front and side views of the mask with most internal lines in the original mask erased so as not to obscure overall face shape. The mask appears to trace best the outline of an individual with European ancestry.

123

Aesth Plast Surg (2008) 32:200208 Fig. 1 The outline of Marquardts mask. See the complete version at http://www.beautyanalysis.com/

201

Marquardt claims that his mask applies equally well to attractive individuals across geographic populations. However, in W. W. Howells data set involving 57 linear craniofacial interlandmark distances, the proportion of global variation between populations was 24% for the rst principal component of shape variation and 33% for the second principle component [32]. Large as these values are, because a correlation structure underlying shape variation across populations also exists, the face shape differences across continental populations are striking. Therefore, how can an ideal face shape be conceived across populations as distinct as Norwegians, Tanzanians, and the Chinese? Using the ratio of the basion-prosthion length to the basion-nasion length as an index of prognathism, Hanihara [11] reported that the mean of this index was 94.9 3.45 for Norwegians, 104.1 4.45 for Tanzanians, and 96.6 4.01 for northern Chinese. Similarly, when the ratio of the simotic subtense to the simotic chord was used as an index of the prominence of the nasals, the result was 52 10.20 for Norwegians, 30.3 6.49 for Tanzanians, and 34.8 12.13 for northern Chinese [11]. Well-separated population means make it difcult to conceive the ideal Homo sapiens face. Even among individuals of European ancestry, the mask outline does not appear to describe a typical man or woman. With reference to face shape variation resulting from masculinization and feminization [12,31], the outline of Marquardts mask appears to be that of a masculinized woman, as evident from the eminent supraorbital ridges, low-set eyebrows, strong nasoglabellar curvature, cheekbone placement high on the face, retracted mandibular symphysis, and a prominent and squared chin.

By Marquardts own admission, his mask describes a womans shape. Therefore, the masculine element is curious given that a strong preference for facial femininity has been described in the general population. For instance, in a metaanalysis of the effect of facial femininity on attractiveness ratings, the effect size correlation was r = 0.64, with a standard deviation of 0.39 and a 95% condence interval of 0.51 to 0.74 [29]. Marquardts mask curiously leans toward a class 2 prole. A class 1 prole is reported as the most attractive [4,7,15,18,26], and minor deviations from a class 1 prole are assigned higher ratings among individuals of European ancestry if they lean toward the class 3 type rather than the class 2 type [4,5,15,21,26]. Additionally, Marquardts mask is inconsistent with the optimal preferences of white North Americans regarding soft tissue nasion placement [23] and earlobe proportions [24]. Therefore, for a number of reasons, as a rst approximation, it is difcult to see Marquardts mask as a descriptor of ideal facial proportions. There also is a problem with the method Marquardt recommends for comparing a given face with the mask. He instructs that before the mask is superimposed on a persons photo to assess goodness of t, the distance between the lips and the midpupil level should be equalized for the front view, and the inferior irion-lip distance should be equalized for the side view. Bashour [1] has carried out the superimposition by equalizing interpupillary distance. However, when control is used for face size, neither of these distances nor other conceivable measures such as face length or face breadth are constant for all people, even when applied to only one specic ethnic group. Therefore, how can goodness of t be assessed?

123

202

Aesth Plast Surg (2008) 32:200208

To compare shapes, control must be used for three factors: location, size, and rotational effects. A standard procedure is to use Procrustes analysis [2,25], which proceeds as follows. Landmarks on the shapes to be compared are identied, and their Cartesian coordinates are measured. Then the centroid of each shape, which is the center of mass of a physical system with unit mass at its landmarks, is computed. The centroid size of each shape, which is the sum of squared distances of a shapes landmarks around its centroid, also is computed. After this, each shape is scaled to unit centroid size. Subsequently, the shapes to be compared are aligned with respect to centroid position and rotated so that the sum of squared distances between corresponding landmarks is minimized. Now the shapes can be compared, as in computation of the Riemannian distance (0 B q B p/2) between the shapes. The shape differences can be visualized by using thin-plate spline analysis [10]. This article aims to use the geometric morphometric methods of generalized Procrustes analysis and thin-plate spline analysis to illustrate how goodness of t can be assessed with Marquardts mask, where it stands with respect to masculinity-femininity, and how valid Marquardts claims are. It also aims to address the issue of an objective evaluation of facial beauty.

surface tangents corresponding to the angular outline of Marquardts mask. A better method exists [3], but because few such semi-landmarks exist, this article is well served by the aforementioned approximation. The choice of the landmarks is based on a need to focus on masculinityfemininity and the limitations of a paper by Hennessy et al. [12]. Hennessy et al. [12] provided front and prole views of a very masculine northwestern European male average, a northwestern European malefemale (population) average, and a very feminine northwestern European female average. The masculine and feminine extremes provided by the authors exaggerated the average maleaverage female shape difference threefold. These averages were produced via geometric morphometric methods, as opposed to the typical composite made by blending faces with interpupillary distance equalized, and hence were suitable for this article. To facilitate a better understanding of Marquardts mask, a three-dimensional (3D) face roughly corresponding to it was generated using Poser (http://www.e-frontier.com/) and Victoria 4 from DAZ3D (http://www.daz3d.com/) and contrasted with a more feminine face (Figs. 3 and 4). Generalized Procrustes analysis and thin-plate spline analysis were carried out using the Shapes package [9] for the R software environment for statistical computing and graphics (http://www.r-project.org/).

Methods The landmarks used are shown in Fig. 2. If a point approximately homologous to a landmark on Marquardts mask lay along a curve, making it difcult to pinpoint, then the y coordinate was dened at the point of intersection of
Fig. 2 Landmarks used to compare face shapes

Results Tables 1 and 2 list the Riemannian distances between Marquardts mask and other faces. Table 1 shows that

123

Aesth Plast Surg (2008) 32:200208 Fig. 3 Rough threedimensional approximation of Marquardts mask in front view (left) and a more feminine face

203

Fig. 4 Rough threedimensional approximation of Marquardts mask in side view (left) and a more feminine face

Marquardts mask is closest to its rough 3D approximation, which should not be surprising. Marquardts mask also appears to be closer to the masculine side of the population average for northwestern Europeans, but caution is required here because of the limited number of landmarks used and the large distances between all the northwestern European averages and Marquardts mask. In Table 2, we again observe that Marquardts mask is closest to its rough 3D approximation. Marquardts mask also appears to be closer to the population average for northwestern Europeans than to the very masculine and very feminine shapes. Again, however, the large distances between all the northwestern European averages and Marquardts mask should be noted.

How Marquardts mask differs from the other faces can be visualized using thin-plate splines, whereby the deformation of a 2D grid in the background shows how Marquardts mask can be deformed to make it approach the other shapes. Figure 5 shows the thin-plate splines for the front view of the face, and Fig. 6 shows those for the side view of the face. Figure 5 shows that some features of Marquardts mask are on the masculine side of the population average for northwestern Europeans, particularly the high placement of the cheekbones, the facial narrowing, and, to a lesser extent, the interexocanthion length and the width of the chin. The data from Hennessy et al. [12] show that the superior medial orbital margins are displaced inferiorly

123

204 Table 1 Riemannian distances between Marquardts mask (front view) and other faces

Aesth Plast Surg (2008) 32:200208

1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Very masculine northwestern European average man Northwestern European malefemale average Very feminine northwestern European average woman Rough three-dimensional approximation of Marquardts mask (Fig. 3, left) The face on the right in Fig. 3

0.085 0.109 0.122 0.050 0.128

Table 2 Riemannian distances between Marquardts mask (side view) and other faces

1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Very masculine northwestern European average man Northwestern European malefemale average Very feminine northwestern European average woman Rough three-dimensional approximation of Marquardts mask (Fig. 4, left) The face on the right in Fig. 4

0.132 0.129 0.148 0.050 0.074

with increasing masculinization. The closest analogs in Marquardts mask are the low-set eyebrows (Fig. 1), clearly masculine in their placement. Figure 6 shows that the nasoglabellar curvature, the retraction of the mandibular symphysis, and the prominence of the chin in Marquardts mask are on the masculine side of the male female northwestern European average. Figure 6 also shows that Marquardts mask leans toward a class 2 prole compared with the average northwestern European proles. The thin-plate splines show, within the limits of the landmarks used, how the 3D approximation of the Phi mask can be ne-tuned to make it better match the masks proportions. The chin must be made more prominent in both the side and front views and slightly wider. Additionally, the prominence of the zygomatic arches must be increased. Although the eyebrows are not addressed in this article, the 3D approximation would need to have its eyebrows lowered as well. These changes are consistent with a slightly more masculine look and a more robust appearance of the cheekbones.

Discussion Marquardts claims regarding his mask are not consistent with the extant literature on correlates of facial beauty, particularly regarding placement along the masculine-tofeminine discriminant, mandibular prole and soft tissue nasion placement. The masculine element in a mask supposedly describing the ideal female face is curious given how strongly above average femininity in women is preferred by most people [29]. But then, in coming up with his mask, Marquardt relied heavily on female fashion models as a reference standard of beauty. Therefore, a brief review examining the looks of female fashion models, henceforth fashion models, is pertinent. Marquardt has shown that fashion models, on the average, have masculinized faces. Because sex hormones have a global effect, the masculinized faces of fashion

models would be expected to correspond to masculinized physiques as well. Indeed, this is readily observed by a perusal of mainstream fashion magazines such as Elle and Vogue. However, Tovee et al. [37] have described fashion models as hourglass shaped by virtue of a reported average circumferential waist-to-hip ratio of 0.7 and a circumferential bust-to-hip ratio of 0.99. An hourglass approximation applies to a front view, not a 3D view (i.e., the circumferential measurements should be viewed with caution). Given the typically small breasts of high-fashion models, the data of Tovee et al. [37] show that they have a larger rib cage for a given hip size than normal women and glamour models. A larger rib cage will stretch out the waist in front view, but the thinness of high-fashion models will translate to a small waist circumference, not much higher than that of shorter glamour models. A large rib cage is not consistent with an hourglass approximation because an hourglass has a narrow midsection. Therefore, Tovee et al.s [37] conclusion is mistaken. The typical thinness of fashion models also is noteworthy. Most individuals prefer women eshier than fashion models [8,28]. In a sample of 300 fashion models, the average body mass index (BMI: weight divided by the square of the height) was 17.57 kg/m2 [37], whereas the optimum BMI of women rated by Western adults is reported to be 2 to 3 kg/m2 higher [35,36]. In short, female fashion models obviously do not reect the aesthetic preferences of the general public and should not be used to develop an aesthetic archetype. A perusal of fashion models shows a preponderance of Nordic features. Therefore, it may have been better to compare Marquardts mask to Nordic facial norms instead of a population with a dominant Celtic element (northwestern Europeans). However, I am not aware of data on any Nordic population similar to the data provided by Hennessy et al. [12], and there is a signicant Anglo-Saxon element in northwestern Europe, too. Additionally, because Marquardt claims that his mask applies to Homo sapiens,

123

Aesth Plast Surg (2008) 32:200208 Fig. 5 Thin-plate splines showing how Marquardts mask can be deformed to transform it to various faces addressed in Table 1

205

123

206 Fig. 6 Thin-plate splines showing how Marquardts mask can be deformed to transform it to various faces addressed in Table 2

Aesth Plast Surg (2008) 32:200208

123

Aesth Plast Surg (2008) 32:200208

207

the differences among various northern European populations are not relevant to his argument. It could be argued that Marquardt is mistaken in believing his mask describes the ideal proportions of a woman, and that instead, using objective methodology, he has come across a template for Homo sapiens modied into male and female forms as a result of sex differentiation. However, why are parts of Marquardts mask on the masculine side of the population average for northwestern Europeans? Additionally, why does this template most closely approximate, with some distortions, an outlier variety of Homo sapiens? For instance, a tendency for greater attening of the nasal bones/midface to correlate with more protruding jaws has been clearly documented in humans, and northern Europeans, with the most prominent nasal bones and the most regressed jaws, are at an extreme of face shape [11]. How can a mask with prominent nasals and a regressed jaw describe the ideal face of Homo sapiens? More importantly, how objective is Marquardts methodology? Using the golden ratio to describe facial beauty does not make the attempt objective. The human face presents a tremendous number of proportions, and with a hard enough look, some of them are bound to approximate the golden ratio. Marquardts methodology, detailed in his U.S. patents (nos. 5,659,625 and 5,867,588), is similar to that of a researcher carrying out a huge number of statistical tests on the data he possesses in the hope of coming across a statistically signicant nd, which, if found, cannot make the researcher condent that the nd is of any real world signicance. For instance, even after years of working on a 3D version of his mask, Marquardt has yet to come up with it, obviously because he has not come across an intuitive and simple way of putting together geometric structures derived from the golden ratio to form an aesthetically pleasing human face in 3D that resembles his 2D masks in front and side views. If it is possible to derive the outline of a beautiful face from the golden ratio, then Marquardt certainly has not achieved this. The preceding discussion leads to the issue of an objective assessment of attractiveness. Can this be achieved? If a reference template or face outline representing the ideal existed, then geometric morphometric methods could be used to compare individual faces with it and to describe the discrepancies numerically and visualize them graphically. A detailed comparison would require the use of a sufciently large number of landmarks. This could be cumbersome, especially in 3D comparisons (e.g., photogrammetry or laser surface scanning), and could pose a problem if homologous locations need to be identied for points lying along a curve. These points could not be considered true landmarks but rather semi-landmarks or sliding landmarks. However, geometric morphometric methods that deal with semi-

landmarks [3] and interpolate pseudo-landmarks [13] between landmarks have been developed. In addition, assessment of Cartesian coordinates is not more cumbersome than Bashours [1] method. Therefore, an objective comparison can be made, but how is a reference standard objectively obtained in the rst place? Development of an aesthetic reference standard should start with well-documented correlates of beauty, namely, averageness, above average femininity in women, and low uctuating asymmetry [29]. For example, development of an aesthetic female mask for a given ethnicity could involve assessing population norms for women, developing a mask with zero uctuating asymmetry to reect this average, feminizing it to varying degrees, and assessing a large and random sample of individuals from this ethnic group to determine what degree of above average feminization is regarded as the most appealing by most individuals. This modied average could be altered further on a computer by judges to obtain a more pleasing mask and the central tendency of the alteration noted. The nal product would be a mask describing the face shape of a woman that most individuals of her ethnicity would nd very attractive, which in reference to Kims [16] contention, would serve much better than Marquardts mask with respect to facial analytics. This method will not produce a reference standard based on a simple, recurring theme, but it remains to be shown that the latter is possible.

Conclusions Stephen Marquardt has shown how misleading overenthusiasm for the golden ratio and a reliance on fashion models can be. His mask is unable even to describe aesthetically pleasing proportions for white women, especially because of its masculinization. References
1. Bashour M (2006) An objective system for measuring facial attractiveness. Plast Reconstr Surg 118:757774, discussion 775 756 2. Bookstein FL (1996) Biometrics, biomathematics and the morphometric synthesis. Bull Math Biol 58:313365 3. Bookstein FL (1996/1997) Landmark methods for forms without landmarks: Morphometrics of group differences in outline shape. Med Image Anal 1:225243 4. Cochrane SM, Cunningham SJ, Hunt NP (1997) Perceptions of facial appearance by orthodontists and the general public. J Clin Orthod 31:164168 5. Czarnecki ST, Nanda RS, Currier GF (1993) Perceptions of a balanced facial prole. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 104:180187 6. Davis ST (2007) Aesthetic preferences for the unity ratio resist the inuence of color illusions. Am J Psychol 120:4771

123

208 7. De Smit A, Dermaut L (1984) Soft-tissue prole preference. Am J Orthod 86:6773 8. Dodd P: How thin is too thin? New survey reveals overwhelming global support for too thin fashion models debate. Neilsen Breaking News. Retrieved on 14 March 2007 at http://www2. acnielsen.com/news/20070202.shtml 9. Dryden IL, Mardia KV (1998) Statistical shape analysis. John Wiley, Chichester 10. Duchon J (1977) Splines minimizing rotation-invariant seminorms in Sobolev spaces. In: Schempp W, Zeller K (eds) Constructive theory of functions of several variables: Lecture notes in mathematics. Vol 571. Springer, Berlin, pp. 85100 11. Hanihara T (2000) Frontal and facial atness of major human populations. Am J Phys Anthropol 111:105134 12. Hennessy RJ, McLearie S, Kinsella A, Waddington JL (2005) Facial surface analysis by 3D laser scanning and geometric morphometrics in relation to sexual dimorphism in cerebral craniofacial morphogenesis and cognitive function. J Anat 207:283295 13. Hutton TJ, Buxton BF, Hammond P, Potts HW (2003) Estimating average growth trajectories in shapespace using kernel smoothing. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 22:747753 14. Jefferson Y (2004) Facial beauty: Establishing a universal standard. Int J Orthod Milwaukee 15:922 15. Johnston C, Hunt O, Burden D, Stevenson M, Hepper P (2005) The inuence of mandibular prominence on facial attractiveness. Eur J Orthod 27:129133 16. Kim YH (2007) Easy facial analysis using the facial golden mask. J Craniofac Surg 18:643649 17. Levin EI (1978) Dental esthetics and the golden proportion. J Prosthet Dent 40:244252 18. Maple JR, Vig KW, Beck FM, Larsen PE, Shanker S (2005) A comparison of providers and consumers perceptions of facialprole attractiveness. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 128:690 696, quiz 801 19. Marquardt SR (2002) Dr. Stephen R. Marquardt on the Golden Decagon and human facial beauty. Interview by Dr. Gottlieb. J Clin Orthod 36:339347 20. Matoula S, Pancherz H (2006) Skeletofacial morphology of attractive and nonattractive faces. Angle Orthod 76:204210 21. Michiels G, Sather AH (1994) Determinants of facial attractiveness in a sample of white women. Int J Adult Orthodon Orthognath Surg 9:95103 22. Moss JP, Linney AD, Lowey MN (1995) The use of three-dimensional techniques in facial esthetics. Semin Orthod 1:94104

Aesth Plast Surg (2008) 32:200208 23. Mowlavi A, Meldrum DG, Wilhelmi BJ (2003) Implications for nasal recontouring: Nasion position preferences as determined by a survey of white North Americans. Aesthetic Plast Surg 27:438445 24. Mowlavi A, Meldrum DG, Wilhelmi BJ, Ghavami A, Zook EG (2003) The aesthetic earlobe: Classication of lobule ptosis on the basis of a survey of North American Caucasians. Plast Reconstr Surg 112:266272, discussion 273264 25. OHiggins P (2000) The study of morphological variation in the hominid fossil record: biology, landmarks and geometry. J Anat 197:103120 26. Orsini MG, Huang GJ, Kiyak HA, Ramsay DS, Bollen AM, Anderson NK, Giddong DB (2006) Methods to evaluate prole preferences for the anteroposterior position of the mandible. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 130:283291 27. Qualtrough AJ, Burke FJ (1994) A look at dental esthetics. Quintessence Int 25:714 28. Rand CS, Wright BA (2000) Continuity and change in the evaluation of ideal and acceptable body sizes across a wide age span. Int J Eat Disord 28:90100 29. Rhodes G (2006) The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annu Rev Psychol 57:199226 30. Ricketts RM (1982) Divine proportion in facial esthetics. Clin Plast Surg 9:401422 31. Rosas A, Bastir M (2002) Thin-plate spline analysis of allometry and sexual dimorphism in the human craniofacial complex. Am J Phys Anthropol 117:236245 32. Roseman CC, Weaver TD (2004) Multivariate apportionment of global human craniometric diversity. Am J Phys Anthropol 125:257263 33. Rosenstiel SF, Ward DH, Rashid RG (2000) Dentists preferences of anterior tooth proportion: A Web-based study. J Prosthodont 9:123136 34. Seghers MJ, Longacre JJ, Destefano GA (1964) The golden proportion of beauty. Plast Reconstr Surg 34:382386 35. Swami V, Tovee MJ (2007) Perceptions of female body weight and shape among indigenous and urban Europeans. Scand J Psychol 48:4350 36. Tovee MJ, Cornelissen PL (2001) Female and male perceptions of female physical attractiveness in front view and prole. Br J Psychol 92(Part 2):391402 37. Tovee MJ, Mason SM, Emery JL, McCluskey SE, Cohen-Tovee EM (1997) Supermodels: Stick insects or hourglasses? Lancet 350:14741475 38. Ward DH (2001) Proportional smile design using the recurring aesthetic dental (red) proportion. Dent Clin North Am 45:143154

123

Anda mungkin juga menyukai