Anda di halaman 1dari 8

Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 477484

Misleading funnel plot for detection of bias in meta-analysis


Jin-Ling Tanga,*, Joseph LY Liub
b a Department of Community and Family Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong Centre for Clinical Trials and Epidemiological Research, Faculty of Medicine, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong Received 23 April 1999; received in revised form 24 August 1999; accepted 6 October 1999

Abstract
Publication and other forms of selection biases pose a threat to the validity of meta-analysis. Funnel plots are usually used to detect such biases; asymmetrical plots are interpreted to suggest that biases are present. Using 198 published meta-analyses, we demonstrate that the shape of a funnel plot is largely determined by the arbitrary choice of the method to construct the plot. When a different definition of precision and/or effect measure were used, the conclusion about the shape of the plot was altered in 37 (86%) of the 43 meta-analyses with an asymmetrical plot suggesting selection bias. In the absence of a consensus on how the plot should be constructed, asymmetrical funnel plots should be interpreted cautiously. These findings also suggest that the discrepancies between large trials and corresponding meta-analyses and heterogeneity in meta-analyses may also be determined by how they are evaluated. 2000 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Funnel plot; Meta-analysis; Randomized controlled trials; Selection bias; Publication bias; Statistical method; Systematic reviews

1. Introduction Meta-analysis is increasingly used to summarize healthrelated evidence as an aid to medical decision making. One of the main criticisms about the technique is that it is prone to publication and other forms of selection biases [15]. Meta-analyses should therefore be routinely scrutinized for the possible presence of such biases in practice [6]. The funnel plot (with the associated significance test for asymmetry of the plot) has been advocated by many for this purpose [1,68]. However, the procedure lacks empirical evidence to support what is observed is, indeed, selection bias. It was suggested that the method may result in false-positive results [9]. The validity of funnel plots needs to be scrutinized. A funnel plot is constructed by plotting the trial-specific effect against a measure of its precision. Precision may be defined differently according to the number of subjects in the trial or a function of the standard error for the effect, such as the inverse of the standard error and the statistical power [1,6,10,11]. A commonly held interpretation is that the points will be symmetrically distributed around the overall effect in the shape of an inverted funnel; when selection bias is present, the plot will become asymmetrical and the overall effect of the meta-analysis will be biased [1,6]. However, this reasoning is overly simplistic because other
* Corresponding author. Tel: 852-2692 8779; fax: 852-2606 3500. E-mail address: jltang@cuhk.edu.hk (J.-L. Tang)

factors may also affect the shape of the funnel plot. As is well recognized, the asymmetry of the funnel plot may be due to true heterogeneity. In other words, less precise trials may show a different effect from that of more precise trials because they differ in their design, participants characteristics, regimen of treatment, etc., and thus are not measuring the same underlying effect [7,1215]. Furthermore, the funnel plot can be constructed in different ways. The treatment effect may be expressed as the relative risk or risk difference [1618]. Precision may also be defined differently, as mentioned above [1,6,10,11]. Little is known about whether the choice of the definition of precision and effect measure in constructing the funnel plot may affect the shape of the plot. Using published meta-analyses, this article examines whether differently constructed funnel plots may result in discrepancies in conclusions about the symmetry of the plot and how often this discrepancy occurs. 2. Methods 2.1. Materials and general methods Meta-analyses were identified by search of every systematic review electronically published in the second issue of the 1998 Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews [19]. Meta-analyses having trial(s) using continuous variables as the outcome or trials having a proportion of outcome events of 0% or 100% in either of the two compared groups were

0895-4356/00/$ see front matter 2000 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights reserved. PII: S0895-4356(99)00 2 0 4 - 8

478

J.-L. Tang, J.L.Y. Liu / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 477484

Fig. 1. Funnel plots of the relative risk of three meta-analyses according to the definition of precision.

J.-L. Tang, J.L.Y. Liu / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 477484

479

Fig. 1. Continued.

excluded. We also excluded meta-analyses with five or fewer trials. The arbitrary cutoff used here is based on the minimum number of six trials adopted in Egger et als paper in which the funnel plot method and the associated significance test are described in more detail [6]. A total of 198 meta-analyses were included in this analysis. The precision is defined in two ways: one according to the inverse of the standard error and the other the trial size. We will call the funnel plot using the former definition of precision the standard error- or SE-based funnel plot and the latter the trial size- or N-based funnel plot. The measure of effect is expressed as both the relative risk and risk difference. Heterogeneity was tested by the method suggested by DerSimonian and Laird [20]. All the analyses were conducted using the Statistical Analysis System (version 6.11). 2.2. Standard error-based funnel plot and related asymmetry test The SE-based funnel plot is constructed by plotting the effect measure (e.g., the natural logarithm of the relative risk) against the inverse of its standard error. Its asymmetry is tested by a significance test using the linear regression method suggested by Egger and colleagues [6]. Briefly, in the regression the standardized effect (STE), defined as the effect divided by its standard error, is regressed against the precision of the effect, defined as the inverse of the standard error (regression equation: STE precision). The

intercept provides a quantitative measure of the asymmetry and is of major interest. The more the intercept deviates from zero, the more pronounced the asymmetry. Negative values will indicate that less precise studies have a more pronounced effect than more precise studies and may thus suggest selection bias. If the P-value of the intercept is 0.10 or smaller, the asymmetry is considered to be statistically significant and accepted. 2.3. Study size-based funnel plot and related asymmetry test In the N-based funnel plot the effect is plotted against the number of subjects or study size. Using the same principles as in the SE-based regression [6,21,22], we suggest here an N-based linear regression in which the product (E) of the effect and the square root of the average number of subjects in the two compared groups of the trial (n) is regressed against the latter (regression equation: E n ). The interpretation of the intercept and its P-value is the same as that of the SE-based regression outlined above. 2.4. Comparison of differently constructed funnel plots Five meta-analyses from the Cochrane Database [2327] are used to demonstrate how the funnel plot of the same set of studies may differ from each other when different definitions of precision and/or effect measures are used. In order to show how often the funnel plots may differ from each other, we divided the 198 meta-analyses into the following three

480

J.-L. Tang, J.L.Y. Liu / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 477484

Fig. 2. SE-based or N-based funnel plots of three meta-analyses according to the measure of effect.

J.-L. Tang, J.L.Y. Liu / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 477484

481

Fig. 2. Continued.

groups: 1) consistently symmetrical: the funnel plot is symmetrical regardless of the definition of precision and effect measure used, 2) consistently asymmetrical: the funnel plot is asymmetrical regardless of the definition of precision and effect measure used, and 3) inconsistently asymmetrical: the funnel plot is asymmetrical when only some, but not all, combinations of the two definitions of precision and two effect measures are used. Group 3 meta-analyses may have a symmetrical or asymmetrical funnel plot depending on how the plot is constructed and are considered to have discrepant funnel plots. Meta-analyses of groups 2 and 3 are further divided into two groups according to the sign of intercept: 1) the intercept is negative in at least one of the asymmetrical funnel plots differently constructed and 2) the intercept is positive in all the asymmetrical funnel plots. The former could suggest selection bias while the latter would not.

3. Results 3.1. Comparison of funnel plots constructed with the same effect measure Fig. 1 shows that the funnel plot for the relative risk may differ from each other when different definitions of preci-

sion are used in constructing the plot. In meta-analysis A [23] the SE-based funnel plot (Fig. 1, A1) is largely symmetrical and the P-value for the significance test of the asymmetry (i.e., the regression intercept) is 0.32, suggesting that the effect is not associated with precision and small trials have a relative risk similar to that of the large trials. Conversely, the N-based plot (Fig. 1, A2) is asymmetrical (P 0.09 for the regression intercept, and the same for the P-values below) and shows that small trials have a much smaller effect than that of the large trials. Meta-analysis B [24] shows the opposite. The SE-based plot (Fig. 1, B1) and test for the intercept (P 0.10) suggest that small trials produce more pronounced effects than the large trials, while the N-based plot (Fig. 1, B2) and test (P 0.24) suggest a lack of strong evidence for such an association and that the overall effect of small trials is close to that of the large ones. In meta-analysis C [25] both the SE-based and N-based plots look asymmetrical but in opposite directions. The SEbased plot (Fig. 1, C1) indicates that small trials produce more pronounced effect than large ones (P 0.09), while the N-based plot (Fig. 1, C2) seems to indicate the opposite, although the test for asymmetry was not statistically significant (P 0.17).

482

J.-L. Tang, J.L.Y. Liu / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 477484

3.2. Comparison of funnel plots constructed with the same definition of precision Fig. 2 shows that funnel plots based on the same definition of large trials may differ from each other when different effect measures are used. The SE-based funnel plot of meta-analysis D [24] is asymmetrical for both the risk difference (Fig. 2, D1, P 0.03) and relative risk (Fig. 2, D2, P 0.10) but in opposite directions. The N-based funnel plot of meta-analysis E [26] is symmetrical for the risk difference (Fig. 2, E1, P 0.91) but asymmetrical for the relative risk (Fig. 2, E2, P 0.02). In all five examples above, there is obvious heterogeneity in either the risk difference or the relative risk or both (P 0.10). However, funnel plots may still differ in meta-analyses in which there is insufficient evidence for the presence of heterogeneity. In meta-analysis F [27], for example, heterogeneity is neither observed for the risk difference (P 0.78) nor for the relative risk (P 0.88). The SE-based funnel plot is symmetrical for the risk difference (Fig. 2, F1, P 0.90) but asymmetrical for the relative risk (Fig. 2, F2, P 0.06). 3.3. Frequency of meta-analyses having discrepant funnel plots Table 1 shows how often the funnel plot differs when different definitions of precision and/or effect measures are used. Of the 198 meta-analyses, 122 (61.6%) consistently showed a symmetrical funnel plot (group 1). 8 (4.0%) an asymmetrical plot regardless of how the plot was constructed (groups 2 and 3), and 68 (34.3%) had discrepant funnel plots when a different definition of precision and/or effect measure were used (groups 4 and 5). The asymmetry of funnel plots is dependent on how the plot is constructed in 37 (86.0%, 95% confidence interval: 76%96%) (group 4) of the 43 meta-analyses with an asymmetrical funnel plot, which suggests selection bias (groups 2 and 4). The number is 31 (93.5%, 95% confidence interval: 86%100%) (group 5) of the 33 meta-analyses with an asymmetrical plot, which is unlikely to be a result of selection bias (groups 3 and 5).

The proportion of discrepant meta-analyses suggestive of selection bias is 18.5% (12/65) in meta-analyses with 6 trials, 21.4% (18/84) in those with 79 trials, and 26.5% (13/49) in those with 10 trials or more. In these discrepant meta-analyses, the proportion of those that are method-dependent is 100%, 88.9%, and 69.2% respectively, for the three groups of metaanalyses with different number of trials (data are not tabulated). In addition, in 95 meta-analyses in which there is no sufficient evidence for heterogeneity (P 0.10) in either the risk difference or the relative risk, 23 (24.2%) show an asymmetrical funnel plot (i.e., equivalent to groups 2 and 4 meta-analyses in Table 1), suggesting selection bias, and in 19 (82.6%) of them the asymmetry is method dependent (data are not tabulated).

4. Discussion We found that the shape of the funnel plot depends to a large degree on how the precision is defined and what effect measure is used in constructing the plot. The funnel plot is asymmetrical and suggests selection bias in 43 (21.7%) of the 198 meta-analyses examined in this study. However, the plot became symmetrical in 86.0% of the 43 meta-analyses when a different definition of precision and/or effect measure were used. This proportion was slightly smaller in meta-analyses with no sufficient evidence for heterogeneity and in those with a relatively large number of trials. In the absence of a consensus on which definitions of precision and effect measures are the preferred methods, this large number of inconsistently asymmetrical funnel plots should be interpreted cautiously. Selection bias is possibly a reasonable explanation in the 6 of the 43 meta-analyses that consistently showed an asymmetrical funnel plot suggestive of selection bias. However, explanations other than selection bias may also need to be sought in the rest of the 43 meta-analyses. First, whether selection bias is present or not should not depend on how the funnel plot is constructed. Second, there were also 33 asymmetrical metaanalyses that have a positive intercept and dont suggest selection bias. This may imply that a similar number of meta-analyses having a negative intercept suggestive of selection bias may also arise due to reasons other than selection bias. One explanation suggested is true heterogeneity or more precisely an association of the effect with the precision of the trials [7,1215]. True heterogeneity, again, does not explain why most of the asymmetrical funnel plots are method dependent. The reasons for such dependence have yet to be found. We believe that the dependence can partly be explained by the variation in the frequency of the outcome events (or simply the risk) in the compared groups of the trials. An examination of how the standard error is determined may explain this claim. The standard error is a function of the sample size and risk. Precision as defined by the inverse of standard error is thus a function of the sample size and risk, while precision as defined by the trial size depends by definition on sample size only. When the risk varies in compared groups among the trials, the precision of the same trials will

Table 1 Disagreements in the direction of and significance test for the asymmetry of differently constructed funnel plots of 198 meta-analyses Categories 1) Consistently symmetrical 2) Consistently asymmetrical with at least one negative intercept 3) Consistently asymmetrical with intercepts all positive 4) Inconsistently asymmetrical with at least one negative intercept 5) Inconsistently asymmetrical with intercepts all positive Total
a

Number 122 6 2 37a 31 198

% 61.6 3.0 1.0 18.7 15.7 100.0

The further breakdown of this group of meta-analyses according to the definition of precision and effect measure are tabulated in Table 2.

J.-L. Tang, J.L.Y. Liu / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 477484

483

be rated differently by the two definitions of precision. As a result, the two funnel plots may differ. Meta-analyses in Fig. 1 provide cases in point when the precision is defined differently. In addition, the standard error is a different function of sample size and risk for the risk difference and the logarithm of the relative risk [16,18,28]. Once again, when the risk varies in the trials, the precision of the same trials will be rated differently when different effect measures are used, resulting in discrepant SE-based funnel plots (Fig. 2, D and F). Furthermore, an asymmetrical funnel plot indicates that the treatment effect varies according to a third factor: the precision. This phenomenon is known as effect modification (or interaction) in epidemiology and the precision is the effect modifier [29]. The presence of effect modification largely depends on the choice of the effect measure when the risk in the control group differs [13,30,31]. Typically, no effect modification when the risk difference is used as the effect measure may indicate an effect modification when the relative risk is used, and vice versa [31]. Thus, the shape of funnel plots using different effect measures may differ in a manner similar to the dependence of effect modification on the effect measure used, even when trial size is used as precision (Fig. 2, E and Table 2). To illustrate, assume that trials in a meta-analysis have the same number of patients equally divided in the two compared groups, and the risk in the treated group is 4% lower than that in the control group, which varies between 5% and 20% in the trials. The risk difference will be constant in the trials, but the relative risk varies; those with a larger relative risk will be rated as less precise than those with a smaller relative risk. Consequently, the SE-based funnel plot for the relative risk will be asymmetrical, indiTable 2 Thirty-seven meta-analyses with discrepant funnel plots some of which suggest selection bias, according to the definition of precision and effect measure Categoriesa 1) 2) 3) 4) 5) 6) 7) 8) 9) 10) 11) 12) 13) 14) Total
a

Number 5 0 1 0 1 15 1 1 1 2 1 2 3 4 37

% 13.5 0.0 2.7 0.0 2.7 40.5 2.7 2.7 2.7 5.4 2.7 5.4 8.1 10.8 100.0

cating that less precise trials have a greater relative riska feature of publication bias but not a result of selective publication, whereas the SE-based funnel plot for the risk difference and the N-based plots will not suggest publication bias. These discussions also indicate that the N-based funnel plots in which the precision is not affected by the risk may be the preferable method for detection of bias. However, in practice the trial size may not be independent of, but often determined by, the level of the risk studied [13,14]. Hence, like the SE-based funnel plots, the N-based funnel plots may cause false-positive results. Furthermore, if publication bias exists, the selection procedure is likely to be determined by a function of the magnitude of the effect and its standard error (as reflected by the associated confidence interval or P-value), rather than directly by study size [3]. In this case the N-based funnel plot may fail to detect the presence of the bias. How often would the shape of funnel plots depend on the way the plot is constructed? The answer is whenever an asymmetrical funnel plot occurs. The variation in effect due to either true heterogeneity or chance is the prerequisite for the occurrence of an asymmetrical plot. Whenever the effect varies, the risk varies either in the control or treated group or in both. Thus, the risk variation, which is an inevitable consequence of effect variation, will always play a role, if not the only role, in the occurrence of an asymmetrical plot. In other words, all asymmetrical funnel plots are dependent more or less on the way they are constructed. These findings raise a question about the validity of the funnel plot as a tool for the detection of selection-related biases in meta-analyses of binary outcomes because of its inability of distinguishing selection bias from true heterogeneity. Recognition of the dependence of the shape of the funnel plot on the definition of precision and/or effect measure used has several implications. First, it is overly simplistic to conclude that a meta-analysis is biased on an observation of an asymmetrical funnel plot. Second, most observed discrepancies between large trials and corresponding meta-analyses [3234] may be dependent on how large trials are defined and what effect measures are used in the comparison. Third, the presence of heterogeneity in a meta-analysis may also depend on the choice of effect measure used [28]. Fourth, the effect measure that has a symmetrical funnel plot should probably be used to summarize the trials if it is also clinically desirable. Alternatively, an asymmetrical funnel plot should be interpreted as suggestive of a special form of heterogeneity in which the effect is associated with the precision (i.e., the risk, trial size or both) of the trials. This association can be simply called the precision-related heterogeneity. The determinants of the association, one of which is selection bias, should be further investigated and such information would be of great value for clinical decision making [3539]. Acknowledgments We thank Shelly LY Tse and Lan Kwok for assistance in hand searching the Cochrane Database and Aprille Sham in

: the 1st place, risk difference (RD) as effect measure and study size as precision; The 2nd place, relative risk (RR) as effect measure and study size as precision; The 3rd place, RD as effect measure and the inverse of standard error as precision; The 4th place, RR as effect measure and the inverse of standard error as precision; and , asymmetrical funnel plot; , symmetrical funnel plot.

484

J.-L. Tang, J.L.Y. Liu / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 477484 [23] Thornley B, Adams CE, Awad G. Chlorpromazine versus placebo for those with schizophrenia. (Comparison: Chlorpromazine vs placebo; Outcome: behaviourdeteriorated / disturbed / unco-operative.) Cochrane Library: Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Oxford: Update Software, 1998. Disk issue 2. [24] Jewell D, Young G. Treatments for nausea and vomiting in early pregnancy. (Comparison: all antiemetic medication compared with placebo; Outcome: effect on nausea.) Cochrane Library: Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Oxford: Update Software, 1998. Disk issue 2. [25] Handoll HHG, Farrar MJ, McBirnie J, Tytherleigh-Strong G, Awal KA, Milne AA, Gillespie WJ. Prophylaxis using heparin, low molecular weight heparin and physical methods against deep vein thrombosis and pulmonary embolism in hip fracture surgery. (Comparison: any heparin vs control / placebo; Outcome: DVTany.) Cochrane Library: Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Oxford: Update Software, 1998. Disk issue 2. [26] Silagy C, Ketteridge S. Physician advice for smoking cessation. (Comparison: effects aids adjuncts to advice; Outcome: smoking cessation (maximal followup).) Cochrane Library: Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Oxford: Update Software, 1998. Disk issue 2. [27] Boulvain M, Irion O. Stripping/sweeping of the membranes to induce labour or to prevent post-term pregnancy. (Comparison: stripping/ sweeping of the membranes vs controls; Outcome: forceps/vacuum extraction.) Cochrane Library: Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Oxford: Update Software, 1998. Disk issue 2. [28] Tang JL. Weighting bias in meta-analysis of binary outcomes. J Clin Epidemiol (in press). [29] Greenland S, Rothman KJ. Concepts of Interaction. In: Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Eds. Modern Epidemiology, 2nd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven; 1998:32942. [30] Rothman K, Poole C. A strengthening programme for weak associations. Int J Epidemiol 1968;17(4)(suppl.):9559. [31] Rothman KJ. Modern epidemiology, 1st ed. Boston: Little, Brown & Company, 1986. pp. 177236, 31126. [32] Villar J, Carroli G, Belizn JM. Predictive ability of meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. Lancet 1995;345:7726. [33] Cappelleri JC, Ioannidis JPA, Schmid CH, de Ferranti SD, Aubert M, Chalmers TC, Lau J. Large trials and meta-analysis of smaller trials: how do their results compare? JAMA 1996;276:13328. [34] LeLorier J, Grgoire G, Benhaddad A, Lapierre J, Derderian F. Discrepancies between meta-analyses and subsequent large randomized, controlled trials. N Engl J Med 1997;337:53642. [35] Davey-Smith G, Song F, Sheldon TA. Cholesterol lowering and mortality: the importance of considering initial level of risk. BMJ 1993; 306:136773. [36] Davey-Smith G, Egger M. Who benefits from medical interventions? BMJ 1994;308:724. [37] Berlin JA. Invited commentary: benefits of heterogeneity in metaanalysis of data from epidemiologic studies. Am J Epidemiol 1995; 142:3837. [38] Sharp SJ, Thompson SG, Altman G. The relation between treatment benefit and underlying risk in meta-analysis. BMJ 1996;313:7358. [39] Thompson SG, Smith TC, Sharp SJ. Investigating underlying risk as a source of heterogeneity in meta-analysis. Stat Med 1997;16:274158.

statistical graphics. This project was supported in part by a grant from the Chinese University of Hong Kong (6900511). References
[1] Light R, Pillemer DB. Summing up: the science of reviewing research. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1984. [2] Koren G, Graham K, Shear H, Einarson T. Bias against the null hypothesis: the reproductive hazards of cocaine. Lancet 1989;II:14403. [3] Easterbrook PJ, Berlin JA, Gopalan R, Matthews DR. Publication bias in clinical research. Lancet 1991;337:86772. [4] Dickersin K, Min YI, Meinert CL. Factors influencing publication of research results: follow-up of applications submitted to two institutional review boards. JAMA 1992;267:3748. [5] Egger M, Davey Smith G. Bias in location and selection of studies. BMJ 1998;316:616. [6] Egger M, Davey-Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in metaanalysis detected by a single, graphical test. BMJ 1997;315:62934. [7] Berlin JA, Begg CB, Louis TA. An assessment of publication bias using a sample of published clinical trials. J Am Stat Assoc 1989;84: 38191. [8] Elvik R. Evaluating the statistical conclusion validity of weighted mean results in meta-analysis by analysing funnel graph diagrams. Accid Anal Prev 1998;30:25566. [9] Irwig L, Macaskill P, Berry G, Glasziou P. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test: graphical test is itself biased. BMJ 1998;316:740. [10] Vandenbroucke JP. Passive smoking and lung cancer: a publication bias. BMJ 1988;296:3912. [11] Ioannidis JPA, Cappelleri JC, Lau J. Issues in comparison between meta-analyses and large trials. JAMA 1998;279:108993. [12] Stuck AE, Rubenstein LZ, Wieland D. Asymmetry detected in funnel plot was probably due to true heterogeneity. BMJ 1998;316:469. [13] Schmid CH, Lau J, McIntosh M, Cappelleri JC. An empirical study of the effect of the control rate as a predictor of treatment efficacy in meta-analysis of clinical trials. Stat Med 1998;17:192342. [14] Gonser M, Vetter K, Noack F. Meta-analyses of interventional trials done in populations with different risks. Lancet 1995;345:1304. [15] Ioannidis JPA, Cappelleri JC, Lau J. Meta-analyses and large randomized, controlled trials. N Engl J Med 1998;338:59. [16] Pocock SJ. Clinical Trials: A Practical Approach. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, 1983. pp. 187210. [17] Fletcher RH, Fletcher SW, Wagner EH. Clinical Epidemiology: The Essentials, 3rd ed. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, 1996:13664. [18] Greenland S, Rothman KJ. Measures of effect and measures of association. In: Rothman KJ, Greenland S, editors. Modern Epidemiology, 2nd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven, 1998. pp. 4764. [19] Cochrane Library Database of Systematic Reviews. Oxford: Update Software, 1998. Disk issue 2. [20] DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials 1986;7:17788. [21] Galbraith RF. A note on graphical presentation of estimated odds ratios from several clinical trials. Stat Med 1988;7:88994. [22] Galbraith RF. Graphical display of estimates having differing standard errors. Technometrics 1988;30:27181.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai