Anda di halaman 1dari 15

Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Biological Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho

The error processing system in major depressive disorder: Cortical


phenotypal marker hypothesis
Poppy L.A. Schoenberg a,b,c,∗
a
Faculty of Science, Intelligent Systems, Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands
b
Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, University Medical Centre Nijmegen, The Netherlands
c
Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study, Wassenaar, The Netherlands

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Major depressive disorder (MDD) ensues reduced goal-directed cognition and behaviour. Cognitive and
Received 17 April 2013 emotional flexibility to disengage and adapt future responses was examined in the error processing sys-
Accepted 19 March 2014 tem (error-related negativity/ERN, error-positivity/Pe event-related potentials) of 58 depressed patients
Available online 26 March 2014
(21 current, 37 remitted) vs. 27 controls undergoing cognitive and affective Go/NoGo paradigms. ERN
was equivalent between patient and controls for the cognitive task, albeit amplitude attenuated in
Keywords:
patients during the affective task. Blunted ERN amplitudes were evident between patients and controls
Major depressive disorder
in males compared to females, plausibly underpinned by disparities in dopaminergic pathways. Patients
ERN
Pe
displayed enhanced Pe amplitudes for both cognitive and affective tasks. Abberations in cortical error
ERP processing in MDD appear specific to affective systems for the pre-attentive ERN, opposed to cognitive
Error processing system and affective processing for the consciously-integrated Pe. Heightened Pe, observed in both current and
Phenotypal marker remitted patients, advocates the possibility of the Pe waveform as a candidate intermediate phenotype
of depression.
© 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction 2013). How these affective and cognitive dynamics operate mech-
anistically within MDD, and to what extent they are interrelated,
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is an intensely debilitating are yet to be precisely formulated within a pathophysiological
psychiatric condition, with an estimated 75–85% risk of lifetime system.
recurrence, marked by severe detriments in functioning and quality Two principle neuroanatomical structures correlate to the aeti-
of life (Greden, 2001; Lai, 2011). Higher prevalence and greater risk ology and maintenance of MDD. Namely, the prefrontal cortex (PFC:
have been identified in females compared to males (Kessler, Chiu, BA 10) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC: BA 24, 32, 33), asso-
Demler, Merikangas, & Walters, 2005), although predominantly ciated with, particularly emotion, self-regulatory circuitries of the
psycho-social factors have explored this apparent pattern (Nolen- brain (Maletic et al., 2007), paradox to their mediatory roles in
Hoeksema, 2001; Piccinelli & Wilkinson, 2000). Furthermore, the cognitively engaging tasks. Sub-systems of the PFC, i.e. the ventro-
disorder appears to be moderately heritable with particular sur- medial and lateral orbital PFC, are associated with basal regulative
mised candidate genetic factors predisposing higher liability to states related to mood, aggression, pain processing, and perser-
MDD (for comprehensive reviews, see Elder & Mosack, 2011; vatory behaviour. These regions also mediate more cognitively
Levinson, 2006). demanding processes in collaboration with the dorsolateral PFC,
Converging evidence suggests MDD reflects a complex cul- specifically executive functioning, working memory, and sustained
mination of affective and cognitive deficits, characterised by attention (Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000; Maletic et al., 2007). Col-
valence-specific maladaptions, such as hyperfocus and sensitivity lectively, ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical regions transduce
to negative stimuli, impaired cognitive control in processing nega- information from key cortical pathways related to an array of cog-
tive stimuli (Fossati, 2008), and an inability to disengage from asso- nitive and affective domains towards the generation of affective
ciated negative emotionality and experience (Foland-Ross et al., meaning and subsequent goal-directed emotional response and
behaviour (Roy, Shohamy, & Wager, 2012).
The ACC is analogous to an epicentral monitoring hub of the
∗ Correspondence to: Radboud University Nijmegen, Postbus 9010, 6500GL, complex transmittal network within the brain, whose primary role
Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Tel.: +31 24 365 2632; fax: +31 24 365 2728. is to gauge conflicts between brain regions elicited to opposing
E-mail addresses: schoenberg@scientist.com, P.Schoenberg@cs.ru.nl response options, and activate further processing via the PFC for

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.03.005
0301-0511/© 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114 101

subsequent performance monitoring towards goal-directed out- unexamined and undefined. Non-clinical studies allude healthy
comes. Sub-components of the ACC indicate diverging mechanisms, males present enhanced ERN and N2, also regulated by the ACC dur-
such as the mediation of cognitive and executive functioning asso- ing conflict monitoring, amplitudes compared to females (Clayson,
ciated with the dorsal ACC, contrary to the ventral ACC involved Clawson, & Larson, 2011; Larson, South, & Clayson, 2011), suggest-
in the processing of affective information and adaptive response ing overall males require greater ACC activation to engage similar
(Bush et al., 2000). Moreover, the ACC is purportedly an anatomi- levels of performance monitoring as females (Li, Huang, Constable,
cal generator associated with the brain’s error processing system, & Sinha, 2006). This pattern is mediated by anxiety, where ERN
alongside reciprocal interplays with regions of the PFC and basal amplitudes significantly increase in females scoring high in the
ganglia (Falkenstein et al., 2001; Ullsperger & von Cramon, 2004). worry dimension (Moran, Taylor, & Moser, 2012). Thus, a sup-
Electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings have shown this sys- plementary line of interest was to examine whether sex-related
tem can be temporally mapped from tasks designed to elicit error cortical differences in performance monitoring, specific to error
responses, reflected by an early negative voltage event-related processing, present in healthy populations diverge in depression,
potential (ERP), the error-related negativity (ERN), and recipro- potentially contributing towards a neural explanation for report-
cal correct-related negativity (CRN), peaking around 50–150 ms. edly dissimilar female/male depressive symptom prevalence and
A later evoked (200–400 ms) positive voltage ERP, the error- expression.
positivity (Pe) and reciprocal correct-positivity (Pc), are evoked in Coupled with MDD’s genetic heritability, a final encompass-
response to consciously detected error-making. ing aim was to explore the concept that aberrations in error
Remaining a point of contention, the ERN component rep- processing may represent an intermediate cortical phenotypal
resents a polyfactorial cortical index of error processing, such marker, expressing underlying dysfunction and dysregulation in
as overall activation of the error detection system to mismatch neurophysiological and neurotransmissional systems associated
(Gehring, Goss, Coles, Meyer, & Donchin, 1993; Vidal, Hasbroucq, with the examined ERPs, potentially useful for wider research clas-
Grapperon, & Bonnet, 2000), conflict monitoring generated by error sifying endophenotypes.
vs. correct response choice (Yeung, Botvinick, & Cohen, 2004), an
emotional index of error processing (Luu, Collins, & Tucker, 2000), 2. Experiment I: cognitive error processing
or the interplay between these cognitive and affective dynam-
ics (Yeung, 2004). The later neural correlate of error processing 2.1. Methods
(the Pe), is thought to reflect error awareness and affective
2.1.1. Sample
evaluation to error significance (Falkenstein, 2004; Nieuwenhuis, Table 1 illustrates demographic and clinical measures. Overall, 85 participants;
Ridderinkhof, Blom, Band, & Kok, 2001; Overbeek, Nieuwenhuis, & 27 HCs recruited via public advertisements, and 58 depressed patients (21 current,
Ridderinkhof, 2005). Minimal preceding research pertains to error 37 remitted) were recruited via the Radboud University Medical Centre Nijmegen
processing ERPs in MDD, despite presenting a logical avenue of (UMCN) psychiatric clinic and associated UMCN Mindfulness Centre. Patients were
either currently being treated at the UMCN or undergoing an intake assessment
inquiry based on excessive sensitivity to negative cues charac-
to receive treatment, and were screened for eligibility to take part in the research
terised by the illness. Extant studies indicate patients suffering a by a specialist team at the clinic. Patient inclusion criteria comprised current pri-
current depressive episode yield significantly higher ERN ampli- mary DSM-IV-TR diagnosis of major depressive disorder, diagnosed by a qualified
tude compared to non-depressed controls (Chiu & Deldin, 2007), consultant psychiatrist, in people aged 21–65 years. Current or remitted depres-
also found in a small sample of patients with moderate depres- sion was classified by the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I:
Sheehan et al., 1998). Included patients (current/remitted) had suffered between 1
sion (Holmes & Pizzagalli, 2010). The variant feedback-dependent and 3 previous episodes. Exclusion criteria (also for HCs) were alcohol/substance
ERN presents blunted amplitude for post-error trials (negative abuse/dependence within the last 6 months, current or previous co-morbid bipolar
feedback-dependent) in remitted depressed patients compared to disorder-, psychosis-, obsessive compulsive disorder-, eating disorders-, personality
healthy controls (Ruchsow et al., 2004, 2006). Attenuated ERN disorders-, neurological disorders (e.g. ADHD, ASDs, epilepsy)-, and learning dif-
ficulties. Informed written consent to participate in an ethically approved (CMO,
amplitudes are also present in children and adolescents with MDD
Arnhem-Nijmegen) research study was obtained.
(Ladouceur et al., 2012), suggesting neural expression is mediated
by degrees of illness severity and brain maturation. Severity of 2.1.2. Cognitive Go/NoGo task
illness, clinical co-morbidity, and maturation potentially modu- Five letters (A, F, H, Y, X): h = 2 cm, w = 1.5 cm, white on black background, were
late contradictory directions in the Pe waveform within MDD. For sequentially presented in random order. Overall, 495 stimuli (393 Go, 99 NoGo = 20%
inhibition rate) were presented in 3 × 165 stimuli blocks, with rest intervals between
example, geriatric depressed patients (Alexopoulos et al., 2007), each block. Stimulus duration was 500 ms, with a randomised interstimulus inter-
and patients with psychomotor retardation (Schrijvers et al., 2008) val (ISI) between 750 and 2200 ms. Participants were instructed to press a button
show reduced Pe amplitude, whereas these findings have not been as quickly and accurately as possible whenever the letters ‘A’, ‘F’, ‘H’, or ‘Y’ were
replicated elsewhere (Chiu & Deldin, 2007; Holmes & Pizzagalli, presented, and to not press whenever an ‘X’ was presented. Before recording, a 30
stimuli (24 Go) practise block ensured task comprehension.
2008).
One aim of this investigation was to examine further whether 2.1.3. Electrophysiological recording
cortical aberrations in error processing in MDD have implications EEG data were acquired using Brain Vision Recorder 1.03 and QuikAmps 72 hard-
for the global waveform, or are specific to early (ERN) or late (Pe) ware (www.BrainProducts.com), recorded from 30 Ag/AgCl active electrode sensors
ERP components. Based on heightened sensitivity to negative cues, with integrated noise subtraction circuits (actiCAP: Brain Products) located in accor-
dance with the 10–10 electrode system (sites: Fp1, Fp2, AFz, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FC5,
it was predicted enhancements in both components of the wave- FC1, FCz, FC2, FC6, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8, CP5, CP1, CP2, CP6, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, O1, Oz,
form would be observed in patients. A related question inquired O2). Average online reference was used, and referenced to the right mastoid offline.
whether these aberrations in error processing operate relative to Ground electrode was placed on the forehead. Vertical and horizontal ocular activity
cognitive or affective functioning, and to what extent dysregulation were calculated by bipolar derivations of electro-oculogram signals recorded using
Ag/AgCl cup electrodes above and below the left eye, and 1 cm to the outer can-
in cognitive and affective dynamics are interrelated in MDD? Thus,
thi of each eye, respectively. Impedance was maintained ≤10 K. Electrical signal
two experiments utilising similar paradigms were conducted; a was continuously sampled at a digitization rate of 500 Hz, with a band-pass filter of
cognitive Go/No-Go task presenting letter stimuli, and an affective 0.1–100 Hz.
Go/No-Go task comprising valenced word stimuli.
Moreover, despite the apparent female preponderance in the 2.1.4. Signal processing
ERP analysis was conducted using Brain Vision Analyzer 2.0.2. Data were fil-
prevalence, morbidity risk, and incidence related to MDD, possible tered between 0.1 and 30 Hz (24-dB/octave slope), using zero-phase shift band-pass
sex-related differences in the neural substrates of cognitive and (Infinite Impulse Response Butterworth) filters, and a 50 Hz notch. Occular arte-
affective processing which may account for such remain relatively facts were corrected using the regression method (Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1983).
102 P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114

Table 1
Demographic and clinical data.

Demographic Patients Controls Between-group comparison

All (CD + RD) CD RD Patients vs. HC CD vs. RD

Age/Range 49.2 (10.3)/24–64 47.5 (9.5)/24–61 50.2 (10.7)/25–64 48.9 (7.7)/37–60 F = 0.023, p = 0.88 F = 0.902, p = 0.35
Sex: F/M (%) 37/21 [64/36] 16/5 [76/24] 21/16 [57/43] 16/11 [59/41] 2 = 2.781, p = 0.60 2 = 2.190, p = 0.14
Medicated: Y/N (%) 40/18 [69/31] 17/4 [81/19] 23/14 [62/38] – – 2 = 2.210, p = 0.14
Educationa 4.94 (1.9) 4.22 (2.0) 5.33 (1.8) 5.48 (1.6) 2 = 5.472, p = 0.60 2 = 9.183, p = 0.24
Clinical Variable
IDSb 25.9 (11.0) 33.3 (11.0) 22.0 (8.9) 7.3 (6.2) F = 64.130, p < 0.0001*** F = 13.336, p = 0.001**
STAI-Sb 39.5 (10.3) 43.0 (12.0) 37.6 (8.9) 29.4 (8.1) F = 17.710, p < 0.0001*** F = 10.912, p < 0.0001***
a
Education categories (according to education system in the Netherlands): 1 = LWOO (pre-vocational education with learning support), 2 = VMBO (12–16 yrs), 3 = HAVO
(12–17 yrs), 4 = VWO (12–18 yrs), 5 = MBO (16–20 yrs), 6 = HBO (vocational/professional training), 7 = WO (university level higher education/training).
b
IDS: Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (Rush, Gullion, Basco, Jarrett, & Trivedi, 1996), STAI (* clinically relevant scores = >45): State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
(Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983).

Data were segmented into the following epochs (length = −200 to 600 ms relative 2.2.2. Event-related potentials (ERPs)
to response or stimulus onset): (1) response-locked false alarms to NoGo stimuli Four ERP datasets (1CD, 2RD, 1 HC) were dropped from the
(FA), (2) response-locked correct hits to Go stimuli (CH). Data were baseline cor-
analyses due to having too few error trials (<5) available for the
rected from −200 to −50 ms epochs. Visual inspection of individual participant
averages defined error processing ERPs within the following temporal epochs: ERN ERN/Pe ERP averaging procedure. Of the remaining statistically
(30–150 ms), Pe (200–450 ms) on false alarm (FA) trials to NoGo stimuli; and CRN viable N = 81 sample, there were no significant differences between
(30–150 ms), Pc (200–450 ms) on correctly rejected NoGo trials. Individual averages groups pertaining to the mean (SD), and range, of the number of tri-
for each valence condition were then calculated, prior to grand average calculation. als used to calculate individual averages for the ERN/Pe. As follows:
The adaptive mean value around the identified peak amplitude, specifically defined
as 3−/+ data points (12 ms) within each ERP temporal epoch, were extracted for sub-
3-level Group (CD, RD, HC) (p = 0.31: CD = X̄21.0( 13.8), range
sequent statistical analyses. Adaptive mean ERP amplitude has been suggested to 5–49; RD = 17.5 (10.3), range 5–40; HC = 15.9 (8.7), range 5–34); 2-
offset low signal-to-noise ratio more reliably whilst maintaining individual ERP vari- level Group (patients, HCs) (p = 0.28: patients = 18.6 (11.6), range
ability (Clayson, Baldwin, & Larson, 2012). It should be noted, the peak amplitude 5–49; HC = above). Topographical voltage maps for the ERN and Pe
measure was also analysed, whereby overall results concurred with the adaptive
components are presented in Fig. 1.
mean amplitude extraction, albeit due to reportage length confines are not explicitly
reported hereafter.
2.2.2.1. ERN/CRN. A significant main effect of Condition (F(1,
2.1.5. Statistical analyses 78) = 122.977, p < 0.0001), but no main effect of 3-level Group
Data from the FCz electrode were used in statistical analyses, as ERN and Pe (p = 0.38), were apparent. Examining both 2-level Group (patients
adaptive mean amplitudes were maximal at this site across groups for both compo- vs. HC) and Sex IVs, main effect of Condition (p < 0.0001) was con-
nents.
firmed. There was no main effect of Group (p = 0.10), although
Condition (Go, NoGo) × Group (CD, RD, HC) repeated-measures ANOVA (r-
ANOVA), with Greenhouse Geisser correction where appropriate, examined significant Group × Sex (F(1, 77) = 6.156, p = 0.02) and trend Condi-
ERN/CRN and Pe/Pc amplitude and latency measures separately. Posthoc contrasts tion × Group × Sex (F(1, 77) = 3.361, p = 0.07) interactions revealed
were examined using the conservative Scheffe test to avoid Type I errors. male patients had significantly lower ERN amplitudes compared
Sex was also examined as an IV via a Condition × Group × Sex r-ANOVA.
to male HCs (F(1, 30) = 7.584, p = 0.01 – (♂patients: −6.8 (5.7) ␮V
However, due to lower, and unequal, patient numbers per cell for CD and RD
when also stratified by Sex; Group was defined by 2-levels for these analyses,
vs. ♂HCs: −13.3 (7.2) ␮V), see Fig. 2). The opposite, of higher ERN
i.e. patient (CD + RD) vs. HC, so to maintain viable statistical power. Multivari- amplitude, was evident in female patients compared to female HCs,
ate GLM analysed behavioural measures, i.e. number of correct hits (CH), false albeit to non-significant levels (p = 0.53–(♀patients: −9.4 (7.8) ␮V
alarms (FA), correct NoGo (C-NoGo), error of omission (EoO), and response vs. ♀HCs: −7.9 (5.5) ␮V).
time (RT) data. Significant findings were followed up with posthoc one-way
Examining latency, main effect of Condition (F(1, 78) = 5.823,
ANOVAs.
p = 0.02), but not 3-level Group (p = 0.13), were evident. How-
ever, main effect of 2-level Group (F(1, 77) = 3.921, p = 0.05),
2.2. Results and Group × Sex (F(1, 77) = 4.957, p = 0.03) interaction, illustrated
male patients yielded overall slower latencies compared to male
2.2.1. Task measures HCs, and significantly so for CRN (F(1, 30) = 4.788, p = 0.04 –
Two behavioural log-files (1 patient) were corrupted by a tech- (♂patients: 71.9 (50.9) ms vs. ♂HCs: 37.8 (8.8) ms)). A less clear
nical failure, thus absent from the statistical analyses. Performance pattern was evident with regards to latency data between female
data are outlined in Table 2. Patients yielded slower RTs for FAs groups.
compared to HCs (Table 2).
2.2.2.2. Pe/Pc. Pe amplitudes were greater than Pc for all groups
(F(1, 78) = 126.897, p < 0.0001). Main effect of 3-level Group (F(2,
Table 2
Accuracy number (N) and reaction times (RT) for the cognitive Go/NoGo task in 78) = 4.029, p = 0.022), illustrated both CD and RD patients had
patients vs. healthy controls. higher amplitudes compared to HCs, significantly so between RD
patients and HCs (Scheffe p = 0.04 – (RD patients: 12.7 (6.1) ␮V
Patient () X̄ Control () X̄ Comparison
vs. HCs: 9.4 (3.2) ␮V)), and approaching significance between CD
FA (N) 19.4 (12.5) 15.5 (9.4) p = 0.16 patients and HCs (Scheffe p = 0.07 – (CD patients: 12.9 (6.0) ␮V)).
FA (RT) 325.3 (52.1) 307.9 (32.2) p = 0.12
CH (N) 392.8 (6.8) 395.0 (1.5) p = 0.12
Examining 2-level Group × Sex IVs: main effect of Condition
CH (RT) 398.9 (53.9) 413.8 (58.7) p = 0.26 (F(1, 77) = 97.134, p < 0.0001) was confirmed. A main effect of
C-NoGo 79.6 (12.5) 83.5 (9.4) p = 0.16 Group (F(1, 77) = 7.345, p = 0.008), and Condition × Group (F(1,
EoO (N) 3.2 (6.8) 1.0 (1.5) p = 0.12 77) = 4.420, p = 0.04) interaction was also confirmed, i.e. patients
FA, false alarms to NoGo stimuli; CH, correct hits to Go stimuli; C-NoGo, correctly yielded enhanced Pe amplitudes compared to HCs (Fig. 3).
rejected NoGo stimuli; EoO, error of omission to Go stimuli. Lack of significant main effect of Sex (p = 0.64), or Group × Sex
P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114 103

Fig. 1. Topographical maps for ERN (left) and Pe (right) epochs in patients (above) compared to HCs (below) during the cognitive Go/NoGo task. (For interpretation of the
references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)

interaction (p = 0.70), reflected this was true for both female 3. Experiment II: affective error processing
and male data. Interestingly, follow-up ANOVAs revealed female
patients yielded significantly higher Pe amplitudes compared 3.1. Method
to female HCs (F(1, 49) = 4.808, p = 0.033 – (♀patients: 12.6
(6.0) ␮V vs. ♀HCs: 9.0 (3.33) ␮V)), not found in the male data The sample and electrophysiological (online) recording protocol
(p = 0.15–(♂patients: 12.9 (6.1) ␮V vs. ♂HCs: 10.0 (3.1) ␮V)). was the same as Experiment I (see Sections 2.1.1 and 2.1.3). Task
There were no significant findings pertaining to latency presentation order was counter-balanced.
measures.

3.1.1. Affective Go/NoGo task


The task comprised 12 × 100 stimuli blocks with rest inter-
2.2.3. Medication effects vals between each block. Stimuli consisted of positive, negative,
Although number of medicated (n = 40) vs. non-medicated and neutral Dutch words extracted from two standardised word
(n = 18) patients was not statistically significant, multi-variate databases rating/categorising words by valence (refer to, 1.
GLM analysis was conducted with medication status cate- Arnold et al., 2011; Fitzgerald et al., 2011; and 2. Hermans
gory as fixed factor, to rule out the possibility that the & De Houwer, 1994). Each block consisted of two possible
found neurophysiological differences between patients and “word valence types” defined as Go or NoGo stimuli within
HCs were not due to anti-depressant pharmacology. No sig- each block (80 × Go − 20 × NoGo (20% inhibition rate)), consti-
nificant differences for any of the ERP (amplitude/latency) tuting six possible “block types” (i.e.: 1. Positive (Go)-Negative
measures between medicated and non-medicated patients were (NoGo); 2. Positive-Neutral; 3. Negative-Positive; 4. Negative-
found. Neutral; 5. Neutral-Positive; 6. Neutral-Negative). Each block type
104 P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114

Fig. 2. Cognitive Go/NoGo task: False Alarms to NoGo stimuli at FCz for females (green) vs. male data (black) in patients (thick) and HCs (thin) (0 ms = response onset). (For
interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)

was repeated in the experimental constituting 12 overall randomly (FCz/ + Pz) × 3-level Group, r-ANOVAs analysed ERN and Pe
presented blocks, where word stimuli were randomly presented amplitude and latency data, respectively. The r-ANOVAs were
within each block. Within the overall experiment, 600 different re-run with 2-level Group (patient vs. HCs) + Sex IVs. Posthoc
words were used to reduce stimuli habituation/familiarity. Stim- contrasts were examined using the conservative Scheffe test, and
ulus duration was randomly presented between 500 and 1500 ms, Greenhouse Geisser correction was applied when assumptions of
with a randomised ISI between 800 and 1750 ms. Prior to each block sphericity had been violated. Follow up between subjects com-
standardised instructions were given onscreen, verified verbally by parisons utilised one-way ANOVA with applied Scheffe posthoc
the experimenter, indicating which word valence to press vs. not contrasts.
press for, of the two possible valence types included in the forth-
coming presented block. Instruction presentation doubled as a rest 3.2. Results
period, the duration of which was determined by the participant.
Three patients (1 CD) and one HC did not participate in this task
3.1.2. Signal analysis because Dutch was not their first language.
Evoked potentials were extracted using the same signal
processing parameters as Experiment I (Sections 2.1.3 and 2.1.4). 3.2.1. Behavioural data
However, data were segmented into response-locked CH and FA Behavioural log-files for five participants (1 CD, 3RD, 1HC) were
trials also categorised by valence block type, i.e. POS, NEG, and corrupted due to technical equipment failure, so could not be
NEU stimuli. Adaptive mean amplitudes were maximal at FCz for included in the behavioural statistical analyses.
the ERN component across groups, and at Pz for the Pe compo- Main effect of Group for CH response time (RT) to NEG stimuli
nent in HCs, compared to FCz for POS and NEG stimuli and Pz (F(2, 73) = 3.092, p = 0.05), revealed CD patients yielded faster RTs
for NEU stimuli in patients. As such, subsequent statistical anal- (see Table 2) compared to RD (Scheffe, p = 0.99) and HCs (Scheffe,
yses included FCz electrode data for the ERN, further to FCz and Pz p = 0.09), although posthoc contrasts indicated differences were
electrode sites for analyses on the Pe component. not significant. Main effect of Group for CH rate to NEU stimuli
(F(2, 73) = 4.266, p = 0.02), showed CD yielded fewer CHs com-
3.1.3. Statistical analysis pared to RD (Scheffe, p = 0.19), and significantly so compared to
Condition (ERN, CRN) × Valence (POS, NEG, NEU) × 3- HCs (Scheffe, p = 0.02). Finally, main effect of Group for EoO (erro-
level Group (CD, RD, HC), and Condition × Valence × Site neously omitted Go trials) rate for NEU stimuli (F(2, 73) = 4.291,
P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114 105

Fig. 3. Cognitive Go/NoGo task: NoGo (black) and Go (blue) at FCz in patients (thick) vs. healthy controls (thin) for ERN (30–150 ms) and Pe (200–450 ms) waveforms at FCz
(0 ms = response onset). (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)

p = 0.02), indicated CD patients yielded the most EoOs compared Two-level Group × Sex IV analyses indicated main effects of
to RD (Scheffe, p = 0.16), and a significant margin compared to HCs Condition (p < 0.0001), Valence (F(2, 136) = 3.178, p = 0.05), and
(Scheffe, p = 0.02) (Table 2). Group (F(1, 68) = 7.132, p = 0.009), highlighting a general pat-
Two-level Group × Sex analyses, showed main effect of Group tern of lower ERN amplitudes in patients compared to HCs
for CH rates to NEU stimuli (F(1, 72) = 5.127, p = 0.03), where (Fig. 5). Furthermore, a significant Group × Sex (F(1, 68) = 10.584,
patients yielded fewer CHs (Table 2). Main effect of Group for EoOs p = 0.002) interaction revealed male patients yielded consistently
for NEU stimuli (F(1, 72) = 4.837, p = 0.03) revealed patients com- lower ERN amplitudes compared to male HCs, for POS (p = 0.07
mitted more EoO. Finally, a main effect of Sex showed females had (♂patients = −1.4(2.7) ␮V vs. ♂HCs = −3.2(1.9) ␮V)), NEG (p = 0.005
consistently faster RTs compared to males across conditions and (♂patients = −1.5(2.4) ␮V vs. ♂HCs = −4.2(2.0) ␮V)), and NEU
significantly so for FAs to NEG stimuli (F(1, 72) = 6.649, p = 0.01 – (p = 0.10 (♂patients = −2.4(5.5) ␮V vs. ♂HCs = −5.6(2.4) ␮V)) condi-
(♀: 478.9 (55.2) ms vs. ♂: 519.5 (70.8) ms). However, no Group × Sex tions, not evident between female patients and female HCs. Within
interactions were evident. the patient group, female patients yielded consistently higher ERN
amplitudes compared to the male patients, albeit to non-significant
3.2.2. Event-related potentials (ERPs) margins. Within the HC group, HC males yielded consistently
Two ERP datasets (1RD, 1HC) for POS, four (2RD, 2HC) for higher ERN amplitudes compared to HC females (Fig. 6), signifi-
NEG, and seven (2CD, 3RD, 2HC) for NEU valence conditions were cantly so for NEG (F(1, 25) = 12.241, p = 0.002 (♀HC = −1.6(1.8) ␮V
dropped from statistical analyses due to having too few error trials vs. ♂HC = −4.2(2.0) ␮V)), and NEU (F(1, 23) = 17.924, p < 0.0001
(<5) available for the ERN/Pe ERP averages. There were no signif- (♀HC = −2.1(1.7) ␮V vs. ♀HC = −5.6(2.4) ␮V)) conditions.
icant differences in mean (SD), and range of the number of trials Examining latency, main effect of Condition was evident for 3-
used to calculate ERN/Pe individual averages (Table 3). Topograph- level (F(1, 69) = 40.579, p < 0.0001), and 2-level (F(1, 68) = 33.365,
ical voltage maps for the ERN and Pe components are presented in p < 0.0001) Group analyses. No main effects of 3-level (p = 0.76), or
Fig. 4a and b. 2-level (p = 0.56) Group, Sex (p = 0.56), or interactions were found.

3.2.2.1. ERN/CRN. Main effect of Condition (F(1, 69) = 107.913, 3.2.2.2. Pe/Pc. Main effect of Condition (F(1, 74) = 67.962,
p < 0.0001) indicated amplitudes were greater for ERN compared p < 0.0001) revealed amplitudes were greater for Pe than Pc
to CRN across the sample. No main effect of Group (p = 0.16) or across the sample. Significant interactions Condition × Group (F(2,
interactions were found. 74) = 3.059, p = 0.05), Valence × Site × Group (F(4, 148) = 2.453,
106
Table 3
Accuracy number (N) and reaction times (RT) for affective Go-NoGo task in: (a) patients vs. healthy controls, and (b) current vs. remitted patients.

(a) Patient (CD + RD) (␴)X̄ Control (never-depressed) (␴)X̄ Effects/Interactions

Positive Negative Neutral Positive Negative Neutral 2-level group (patient, HC)

FA (N) 28.5 (11.7) 22.5 (12.1) 22.0 (12.9) 27.9 (7.7) 24.5 (9.2) 23.0 (9.5)
FA (RT) 500.4 (75.9) 496.9 (69.9) 507.4 (83.5) 490.9 (70.8) 487.7 (50.8) 505.0 (60.4) Sex, p = 0.01 (NEG)
CH (N) 283.6 (29.6) 284.0 (36.9) 277.1 (27.8) 288.8 (22.7) 282.7 (32.2) 290.4 (15.0) Group, p = 03 (NEU)

P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114


CH (RT) 574.33 (69.3) 584.5 (73.4) 568.8 (71.0) 586.9 (54.6) 604.3 (60.5) 574.2 (64.3)
C-NoGo 50.7 (11.9) 56.9 (73.4) 51.8 (11.4) 52.1 (7.7) 59.1 (8.1) 50.4 (11.1)
EoO 36.3 (29.6) 32.9 (31.4) 42.4 (27.9) 31.2 (22.7) 37.3 (32.2) 29.6 (15.0) Group, p = 0.03 (NEU)
No. Trials available 16.33 (10.6), range 5–54 15.2 (9.5), range 5–42 13.6 (10.33), range 5–62 15.8 (6.6), range 5–25 15.1 (7.7), range 5–38 13.1 (4.4), 5–25 POS: p = 0.09
for ERN/Pe ERP
individual
averages
NEG: p = 0.06
NEU: p = 0.17

(b) Current depressed (␴)X̄ Remitted depressed (␴)X̄ 3-level Group (CD, RD, HC)

Positive Negative Neutral Positive Negative Neutral

FA (N) 32.1 (13.9) 25.1 (14.9) 25.2 (16.0) 26.3 (9.8) 21.0 (10.0) 20.0 (10.4)
FA (RT) 481.2 (67.4) 492.3 (69.5) 504.9 (75.2) 511.7 (79.3) 499.5 (71.2) 508.9 (89.3)
CH (N) 286.2 (33.0) 282.2 (36.5) 269.1 (37.0) 282.1 (27.9) 285.1 (37.8) 281.8 (19.6) Group, p = 0.05 (NEG)
Group, p = 0.02 (NEU)
CH (RT) 555.6 (72.6) 557.7 (70.6) 556.2 (80.8) 585.5 (66.0) 600.5 (71.4) 576.2 (64.8)
C-NoGo 47.9 (13.9) 53.2 (16.1) 49.9 (14.0) 52.4 (10.5) 59.1 (9.8) 52.8 (9.6)
EoO 33.8 (33.0) 37.8 (36.5) 50.9 (37.0) 37.9 (27.8) 29.9 (28.2) 37.4 (29.7) Group, p = 0.02 (NEU)
No. Trials available 20.0 (14.0), range 6–54 19.2 (11.4), range 5–42 16.7 (14.4), range 5–62 14.2 (7.4), range 5–32 13.0 (7.6), range 5–32 11.8 (6.8), range 5–27 POS: p = 82
for ERN/Pe ERP
individual
averages
NEG: p = 0.97
NEU: p = 0.84

FA, false alarms to NoGo stimuli; CH, correct hits to Go stimuli; C-NoGo, correctly rejected NoGo stimuli; EoO, error of omission to Go stimuli.
P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114 107

p = 0.05), and an overall main effect of 3-level Group (F(2, F(1, 78) = 7.845, p = 0.006 (patients = 8.0(7.2) ␮V; HC = 4.0(1.9) ␮V);
74) = 4.534, p = 0.01), indicated HCs yielded consistently lower Pz (F(1, 78) = 7.534, p = 0.008 (patients = 8.1(6.0) ␮V;
amplitudes across valence conditions compared to CD (Scheffe, HCs = 4.8(2.2) ␮V))) conditions. There was no main effect of
p = 0.02), and RD (Scheffe, p = 0.08). A marked pattern highlighted Sex (p = 0.96), or Group × Sex (p = 0.79) interaction.
CD patients had significantly higher Pe amplitudes for NEG Examining latency, main effect of Condition was found for 3-
stimuli (Fig. 5) at FCz (CD > HC: Scheffe, p = 0.02 (CD = 8.8(7.3) ␮V; level (F(1, 74) = 6.946, p = 0.01) and 2-level (F(1, 73) = 4.091, p = 0.05)
RD = 7.0(4.0) ␮V; HC = 4.9(2.2) ␮V)), and Pz (CD > HC: Scheffe, Group, indicating generally longer latencies for Pe compared to Pc.
p = 0.01 (CD = 8.9(5.2) ␮V; RD = 6.2(4.6) ␮V; HC = 5.0(2.3) ␮V)). A No main effects of 3-level Group (p = 0.15), 2-level Group (p = 0.58),
further pattern showed RD patients yielded significantly higher or Sex (p = 0.28), were evident.
Pe amplitudes for NEU stimuli (Fig. 7), compared to HC at FCz
(RD > HC: Scheffe, p = 0.01(CD = 6.7 (4.7) ␮V; RD = 8.7 (8.2) ␮V; 3.2.3. Medication effects
HC = 4.0(1.9) ␮V)), and Pz (RD > HC: Scheffe, p = 0.05 (CD = 8.1 Significant differences exclusively pertained to ERN latency data
(3.8) ␮V; RD = 8.1 (7.0) ␮V; HC = 4.8(2.2) ␮V)). for the POS (F(1, 53) = 4.642, p = 0.04 (medicated = 57.6(27.1) ms vs.
Analyses with 2-level Group × Sex IVs confirmed main effects of non-medicated = 42.5(14.6) ms)), and NEU (F(1, 49) = 6.863, p = 0.01
Group (p = 0.01), Condition (p < 0.0001), and Valence × Site × Group (medicated = 57.4(27.5) ms vs. non-medicated = 82.9(40.4) ms)),
(p = 0.02), in addition to a Condition × Group (F(1, 73) = 5.868, conditions. No medication effects pertained to amplitude variables.
p = 0.02) interaction. Overall, the patient group showed consis-
tently higher Pe and Pc amplitudes compared to HCs (Fig. 5), 4. Discussion
significantly so at FCz and Pz for NEG (FCz: F(1, 80) = 6.005, p = 0.02
(patients = 7.7(5.5) ␮V; HC = 4.9(2.2) ␮V); Pz: F(1, 80) = 4.875, The encompassing and intertwining aims of the present
p = 0.03 (patients = 7.3(4.9) ␮V; HC = 5.0(2.3) ␮V)), and NEU (FCz: research pertained to examine the cortical error processing system

Fig. 4. (a) Topographical maps for the ERN (left) and Pe (right) epochs in patients (above) compared to HCs (below) for positive NoGo conditions. (b) Topographical maps
for the ERN (left) and Pe (right) epochs in patients (above) compared to HCs (below) for negative NoGo conditions. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure
legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)
108 P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114

Fig. 4. (Continued ).

in MDD, specifically; (1) whether aberrations are linked to cogni- Flanker task findings examining the effects of negative feed-
tive and/or affective processing, during early or later mechanistic back upon error processing in exclusively remitted patients also
neural stages; (2) to explore any sex-related differences in error- report no comparable differences in error rates, reaction times,
related functionality; (3) to question and contribute towards the or neurophysiology of the ERN (Ruchsow et al., 2004). However,
idea that cortical disparities in error processing reflect a possible within the patient group blunted ERN amplitudes for error trials
intermediate phenotype of depression. preceded by an error were present, but no difference in ERN for
error trials preceded by a correct response. Results were replicated
4.1. Early error detection/conflict monitoring using an adapted Go/NoGo task (Ruchsow et al., 2006), suggesting
a detrimental perception towards failure and negative feedback
It was hypothesised that non-feedback dependent ERN would attenuates neurophysiological indexes of error processing within
be enhanced in depressed patients due to heightened sensitivity the disorder, plausibly attributed to hypoactivity of underlying cen-
to error-related information and negative stimuli, ergo, generating tral reward pathways associated with the left prefrontal cortex
greater activation of a pre-conscious conflict monitoring mecha- (Ruchsow et al., 2004). Furthermore, depressed patients presenting
nism in response to errors. This conjecture was not supported by severe symptomatology including anhedonia, apathy, and psy-
data from the cognitive task as no significant findings pertained chomotor retardation, also show reduced ERN (Schrijvers et al.,
to ERN/CRN waveforms between patients and healthy controls, 2008).
further evidenced by group equivalence in behavioural task perfor- Increased ERN has been recorded in remitted patients (Holmes
mance. Moreover, the hypothesis was refuted based on the findings & Pizzagalli, 2008), further to ERN magnitude and symptom sever-
from the affective Go/NoGo task, whereby an opposite pattern ity positively correlating (Chiu & Deldin, 2007). However, findings
emerged. That is, globally, patients showed significantly reduced appeared to be mitigated by co-morbid anxiety, suggesting the
ERN amplitudes compared to HCs. neurobiological mechanisms underlying anxiety plausibly have
P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114 109

Fig. 5. Affective Go/NoGo task: False Alarms to NoGo at FCz in for Positive (green), Negative (black), and Neutral (blue) conditions in patients (thick) vs. HCs (thin)
(0 ms = response onset). (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)

activating effects upon cortical error processing, supported by the and mood. During the remissive phase of MDD enhanced activation
presence of enhanced ERN in anxiety disorders (Endrass, Klawohn, in this early juncture of error processing increases vulnerability to
Schuster, & Kathmann, 2008; Hajcak, McDonald, & Simons, 2003). relapse, maintaining the cyclical nature of the disorder. Whereas,
In accord with the present study findings, blunted ERN amplitudes blunted ERN characterises severe symptomatology and covert
were present in a depressed patient sample with no co-morbidity cognitive dysfunction specific to emotion processing, adjunct to
anxiety disorders or clinically relevant levels of anxiety at the emotional and behavioural ‘shut down’, i.e. anhedonia, apathy,
time of testing as measured by the STAI. Overall, early phase fatigue; clinical hallmarks of full manifestation of the illness.
error processing in MDD appears dependent upon affective mate-
rial linked to subsequent dysregulation in emotion processing, 4.2. Error awareness and evaluative significance
rather than exclusive cognitive-based impairment, and viably has
a complex intrinsic relationship with degree of negative affect. In line with MDD reflecting propensity for greater motiva-
For example, when delaying the feedback signal in a Flanker task, tional salience and evaluation of performance errors (Ridderinkhof,
greater medial frontal negativity waveforms for all feedback types Ramautar, & Wijnen, 2009), in turn, sensitivity to error aware-
characterise depressed patients compared to non-depressed con- ness (Hughes & Yeung, 2011); globally, higher Pe amplitudes were
trols. However, enhanced neurophysiological response is evident apparent in patients compared to controls. Moreover, disentangling
in moderately depressed, compared to blunted response in the current from remitted depression showed Pe amplitudes were gen-
more severely depressed (Tucker, Luu, Frishkoff, Quiring, & Poulsen, erally higher in the currently depressed patients, where discernable
2003). heightened neurophysiological response pertained to errors for
Collectively, it could be surmised that attenuated ERN is associ- negative stimuli. Curiously, higher Pe amplitudes for errors to neu-
ated with greater negative affect and symptom severity, whereas tral stimuli were present in the remitted depressed group. The
enhanced ERN activation characterises mild to moderate depres- cognitive task also showed patients to have heightened Pe ampli-
sion, plausibly representing a ‘maintenance mechanism’. That is, a tudes compared to non-depressed controls, although no distinction
core composite of error processing involves an automatic template between current and remitted depression. These findings suggest
upgrading mechanism to mismatch and related conflict detec- Pe aberrations in MDD are emotion-specific, and also present in
tion. Thus if heightened, potentially renders vulnerability towards the absence of a current depressive episode, whereby degree of
misinterpreting and perceptually inflating negative environmen- illness and negative affect appear to have mediating effects upon
tal cues, a risk factor for maintaining negatively-biased cognition the conscious stage of error evaluation.
110 P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114

Extant enquiry into the Pe in MDD remains equivocal. Reduced


Pe has been found in depressed patients with psychomotor slow-
ing (Schrijvers et al., 2008), geriatric MDD patients (Alexopoulos
et al., 2007), and higher anxiety severity in MDD patients (Olvet,
Klein, & Hajcak, 2010). Studies incorporating reward trials in error
eliciting task designs report null results in moderately depressed
patients for non-reward (neutral) and incentive conditions (Chiu &
Deldin, 2007). Further observations cite equivalent Pe waveforms
in moderately depressed patients vs. controls on no-incentive tri-
als, contrary to attenuated Pe amplitude in patients for reward
conditions (Holmes & Pizzagalli, 2010). Blunted Pe observations in
depression may lie with task paradigm rather than symptom sever-
ity, as a study investigating the impact of symptom reduction on
action monitoring ERPs in severely depressed patients found no
correlation between changes in symptom severity after a 7-week
intervention and blunted Pe amplitude. However, one patient was
also diagnosed with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and a
further two with chronic fatigue syndrome, possibly contaminating
the overall patient sample (n = 15) (Schrijvers et al., 2009).
Enhanced Pe amplitude found here in a larger sample of exclu-
sively MDD patients, supports a concept of ‘error fixation’ in
depression and increased evaluative significance towards errors
made for negatively valenced material. These results corrobo-
rate with neuroimaging studies mapping attentional biases to
negative material in MDD, reporting increased activation in the
rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) and precuneus (Elliott,
Rubenstein, Sahakian, & Dolan, 2000; Mitterschiffthaler et al.,
2008; Vuilleumier, Armony, Driver, & Dolan, 2001), indicating
aberrations in top-down cognitive control of attention to emo-
tional, particularly negatively valenced, stimuli. Moreover, the
fact remitted patients exhibited inflated Pe amplitudes to neu-
tral stimuli is plausibly connected to the inherent vulnerability in
MDD, particularly when acute symptoms have subsided, to inter-
pret ambiguous material as negative (Gur, Erwin, Gur, & Zwil, 1992;
Keeley, Davidson, Crane, Matthews, & Pace, 2007), in turn, sustain-
ing a disposition to experience negative mood states, and potential
vulnerability to relapse.

4.3. Sex differences in error processing in MDD

Neurophysiological differences between female and male


patients were exclusive to the ERN. Overall, male patients showed
consistently lower ERN amplitudes compared to female patients,
for both the cognitive and affective tasks. Reduced monitoring
at this early stage of error processing suggests impaired ACC
regulation. For example, it is proposed that disinhibition of the
ACC is facilitated by dopaminergic neurotransmission (Holroyd
& Coles, 2002); a neuromodulator predominantly involved in
the central cortical reward pathway (Arias-Carrión, Stamelou,
Murillo-Rodríguez, Menéndez-González, & Pöppel, 2010; Munro
et al., 2006), impacting self-regulatory processes and related goal-
directed outputs. Moreover, dopamine is closely linked to the
ERN as the key neurotransmissional modulator for the waveform
(Holroyd & Coles, 2002; Meyer et al., 2012). As such, attenuated
ERN amplitudes in male patients relative to female patients may
have been linked to reduced underlying dopaminergic activity.
In this vein, sex differences in dopaminergic neurotransmis-
sion have found to be primarily dependent upon gonadal steroids,
reflected by enhanced straital dopamine activity, specifically medi-
ated by connection between oestrogen receptors and the basal
ganglia (Di Paolo, 1994), accounting for the general trend for higher
Fig. 6. Affective Go/NoGo task: False Alarms to NoGo stimuli at FCz in Female (green)
dopamine levels in healthy females compared to healthy males.
and Male (black) data for Positive (above), Negative (middle) and Neutral (below)
conditions in patients (thick) vs. HCs (thin) (0 ms = response onset). (For interpreta-
Supporting evidence includes Positron Emission Tomography (PET)
tion of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web studies investigating sex differences in d-amphetamine-induced
version of the article.) dopamine catabolism (Riccardi et al., 2006, 2011), where females
reveal considerably heightened dopamine release in subcortical
P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114 111

Fig. 7. Affective Go/NoGo task: False Alarms to NoGo at FCz for Negative condition (sbove) and Neutral condition (below), in CD (black), RD (green), and HCs (blue)
(0 ms = response onset). (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)
112 P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114

regions of the globus pallidus and inferior frontal gyrus, specific increase pertinent excitatory neurotransmitter pathways may be
to the right hemisphere, in addition to temporal and parietal more effective with male patients. Rigorous empirical investiga-
cortical areas, thus, greater induced dopamine release glob- tions are required to explore these theorisations further.
ally within the brain relative to males (Riccardi et al., 2006).
Furthermore, d-amphetamine-induced dopaminergic antagonism 4.4. Limitations
reveals sex-related changes in cognition and affect indexed by
the Stroop task, where increased dopamine release in the subs- There are several limitations of this research. Adhoc anal-
tantia nigra, a key cortical structure connected to the central yses with the (un)/medicated patient samples showed no ERP
reward pathway and regulation of striatal and limbic function- amplitude differences attributed to medication status, although
ing, correlated with positive affect in males only (Riccardi et al., an ideal sample would include non-medicated patients or medica-
2011). Non-clinical studies reveal healthy males demonstrate tion flush-out preceding EEG recording. Larger participant samples
higher ERN amplitudes compared to healthy females (Larson systematically examining sex-related differences in ERP param-
et al., 2011), further to neuroimaging data suggesting males eters associated with MDD will provide considerable empirical
require increased ACC cortical activity to achieve the similar strength than the findings presented here. Previous research hints
behavioural performance of females during performance monitor- that paradigm design may affect ERP result outcomes in MDD;
ing (Li et al., 2006). These neuroanatomical findings coupled with replication studies utilising Flanker or Stroop tasks, for example,
the effects on positive mood in the males, point to consideration may help to consolidate the wider picture regarding the error
of sex-related differences in dopamine catabolism and neuro- processing system in MDD. Furthermore, the affective Go/NoGo
transmissional release upon associated symptomatic clusters in task utilised valenced words for affective stimuli, which incor-
MDD. porates a high cognitive element in the semantic processing of
A recent review found no conclusive data-driven evidence the word to extract emotional content. As such, affective pictorial
for discrete symptomatic clinical subtypes within the disorder, stimuli (e.g. IAPS) may have been more appropriate to examine
although possible sex-related subtypes were not explored (van Loo, emotion system involvement in error processing within the disor-
de Jonge, Romeijn, Kessler, & Schoevers, 2012). Female patients der. Moreover, incorporating a feedback-dependent element to the
show a general propensity to experience more somatic-related design towards error elicitation would aid more substantive theori-
problems (Lai, 2011), commonly associated with stress-based sations regarding the concept of subclinical profiles in depression,
factors (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2001). Increased dopamine release particularly to investigate the plausibility of greater hypoactivity
in the accumbens nucleus within the ventral striatum, has of the central reward system in male patients, opposed to primary
shown to correlate to perceived stressful environmental cues stress-based neuro-biological systems underlying female patient
(Mora, Segovia, del Arco, de Blas, & Garrido, 2012), indicating aetiology and symptom expression. No explicit measures of verbal
greater dopaminergic system activation, which would translate IQ or general IQ assessments were conducted, so we cannot be sure
to higher ERN amplitudes. Furthermore, stress-induced dopamine whether these factors were matched between patients and con-
release is largely mediated by functional interaction between trols, accounting for ERP anomalies. However, similar behavioural
the accumbens nucleus and PFC, the latter interplaying with the task performances across groups and matched level of education
ACC towards ERN generation (Ullsperger & von Cramon, 2004). suggest such parameters were relatively equal.
Enhanced ERN in the female patient group suggests heightened
dopaminergic activation, contrary to the male patients yield- 4.5. Error processing ERPs as intermediate phenotypes of MDD
ing blunted ERN associated with reduced dopaminergic release
involved in the reward system and processing of motivationally- Overall, MDD can be characterised by neural abnormalities
salient stimuli. In this vein, males show greater vulnerability to of the error processing system. Mesocorticalimbic dysregulation
externalising disorders (Kessler et al., 2005) attributed to dysregu- detrimentally affects dopaminergic reward pathway dynamics, in
lation of the dopaminergic reward system (e.g. ADHD, addiction, turn mediating ERN waveform characteristics. Given the intercon-
psychopathy), where such disorders also display reduced ERN nection between the mesolimbic system and PFC, in conjunction
amplitudes (Franken, van Strien, Franzek, & van de Wetering, with associated dopamine receptor activity, the ERN presents a
2007; Herrmann et al., 2010; Littel et al., 2012; Munro et al., plausible intermediate phenotypal cortical marker for depression.
2007). Albeit, an extant hypothesis (Olvet & Hajcak, 2008) suggests the
The possibility of blunted reward pathway activation in the ERN waveform represents a non-specific biomarker for internal-
male patient group is supported by the reverse pattern observed ising disorders (Hajcak, Franklin, Foa, & Simons, 2008), whereby
in the HC group; HC females had lower ERN amplitude compared the empirical findings indicate heightened ERN amplitudes define
to HC males, in line with the previous findings (Larson et al., anxiety (Hajcak, 2012; Weinberg, Olvet, & Hajcak, 2010 – also find-
2011). In non-psychiatric populations, relevant cortical pathways ing no differences in the Pe component between GAD patients
in males are more strongly affected by anticipated reward than and controls), particularly aspects related to worry and apprehen-
females (Munro et al., 2006; Spreckelmeyer et al., 2009), where sion (meta-analysis: Moran et al., 2012; Moser, Moran, Schroder,
error-making presents a threat to the neural reward system. Ergo, Donnellan, & Yeung, 2013). This report found attenuated ERN
beneficial future enquiry may define possible underlying cortical amplitudes in depressed patients, in the absence of co-morbid
and biochemical signatures pertaining to sex-related depressive anxiety disorder or state anxiety levels. Moreover, the lack of signif-
profiles. Symptoms in females may be underlined and associated icant ERN finding here from the cognitive task, alongside equivocal
by greater stress response bio-mechanisms, leading to heightened observations from existing research, weakens any argument of
help-seeking behaviours, perhaps explaining the apparent female consistency relating the ERN as a specific phenotype for MDD. Alter-
over-representation in this clinical group (Piccinelli & Wilkinson, natively, this study indicated enhanced Pe amplitude in a relatively
2000). Whereas, depressed males may be more vulnerable to ‘pure’ depression sample, devoid of anxiety or other co-morbidities
blunted motivational salience and mesolimbic hypoactivity related present in the prevailing evidence, also during remission in the
to the anticipatory reward pathway. Such exploration could have absence of a current depressive episode.
implications for treatment programmes, i.e. interventions aiming Collectively, these findings suggest aberrations of the pre-
to inhibit maladaptive neuromodulatory systems may have opti- attentive ERN appear specific to emotion systems in depression,
mal success in female patients, whereas those that target and expressed as blunted amplitude. Modulatory effects in the
P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114 113

opposite direction of waveform enhancement may be observed in Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural
less severe depression or co-morbid anxiety. Whereas, aberrations system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385–390.
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal
of the consciously-integrated Pe component reflects heightened of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55(4),
error awareness and evaluation traversing both cognitive and affec- 468–484.
tive systems regardless of stage of illness, presenting specificity as Greden, J. F. (2001). The burden of recurrent depression: Causes, consequences, and
future prospects. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 62(S22), 5–9.
a potential intermediate phenotype candidate for depression. Gur, R. C., Erwin, R. J., Gur, R. E., & Zwil, A. S. (1992). Facial emotion discrimination:
II Behavioral findings in depression. Psychiatry Research, 42, 241–251.
Financial support Hajcak, G. (2012). What we have learned from mistakes: Insights from error-related
brain activity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 101–106.
Hajcak, G., Franklin, M. E., Foa, E. B., & Simons, R. F. (2008). Increased error-related
This work was funded by the Netherlands Organisation for Sci- brain activity in pediatric obsessive–compulsive disorder before and after treat-
entific Research (NWO) (SSM06011) in support of the BrainGain ment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 116–123.
Hajcak, G., McDonald, N., & Simons, R. F. (2003). Anxiety and error-related brain
SmartMix Programme for the Netherlands Ministry of Economic activity. Biological Psychology, 64, 77–90.
Affairs and Netherlands Ministry of Education, Culture and Science; Hermans, D., & De Houwer, J. (1994). Affective and subjective familiarity ratings of
and the Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study in the Humanities 740 Dutch words. Psychologica Belgica, 34(2–3), 115–139.
Herrmann, M. J., Mader, K., Schreppel, T., Jacob, C., Heine, M., Boreatti-Hümmer, A.,
and Social Sciences. et al. (2010). Neural correlates of performance monitoring in adult patients with
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The World Journal of Biological
Conflict of interest Psychiatry, 11(2), 457–464.
Holmes, A. J., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2008). Spatiotemporal dynamics of error processing
dysfunctions in major depressive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(2),
None. 179–188.
Holmes, A. J., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2010). Effects of task-relevant incentives on the
electrophysiological correlates of error processing in major depressive disorder.
Acknowledgements Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 119–128.
Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing:
Reinforcement learning, dopamine and the error-related negativity. Psycholog-
Much appreciation and gratitude to all those who participated
ical Review, 109, 679–709.
in the experiments. Many thanks to Addy de Graaf for generous Hughes, G., & Yeung, N. (2011). Dissociable correlates of response conflict and error
assistance with research co-ordination; and to Magdalena Kowal- awareness in error-related brain activity. Neuropsychologia, 49, 405–415.
czuk and Katrin Scheibe for their help with the data collection. Keeley, R. D., Davidson, A. J., Crane, L. A., Matthews, B., & Pace, W. (2007). An associ-
ation between negatively biased response to neutral stimuli and antidepressant
Thank you to the team at the Radboud University Medical Centre nonadherence. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 62(5), 535–544.
for Mindfulness for helpful assistance with patient recruitment. Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Preva-
lence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the national
comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 617–
References 627.
Ladouceur, C. D., Slifka, J. S., Dahl, R. E., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D. A., & Ryan, N.
Alexopoulos, G. S., Murphy, C. F., Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Kalayam, B., Katz, R., Kanel- D. (2012). Altered error-related brain activity in youth with major depression.
lopoulos, D., et al. (2007). Event-related potentials in an emotional go/no-go task Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 351–362.
and remission of geriatric depression. Neuroreport, 18(3), 217–221. Lai, C.-H. (2011). Major depressive disorder: Gender differences in symptoms, life
Arias-Carrión, O., Stamelou, M., Murillo-Rodríguez, E., Menéndez-González, M., & quality, and sexual function. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 31(1), 39–44.
Pöppel, E. (2010). Dopaminergic reward system: A short integrative review. Larson, M. J., South, M., & Clayson, P. E. (2011). Sex differences in error-related
International Archives of Medicine, 3, 24. performance monitoring. Neuroreport, 22, 44–48.
Arnold, J. F., Fitzgerald, D. A., Fernández, G., Rijpkema, M., Rinck, M., Eling, P. A., Levinson, D. F. (2006). The genetics of depression: A review. Biological Psychiatry,
et al. (2011). Rose or black-coloured glasses? Altered neural processing of posi- 60(2), 84–92.
tive events during memory formation is a trait marker of depression. Journal of Li, C. R., Huang, C., Constable, R. T., & Sinha, R. (2006). Gender differences in the
Affective Disorders, 131(1–3), 214–223. neural correlates of response inhibition during a stop signal task. Neuroimage,
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior 32, 1918–1929.
cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 215–222. Littel, M., van den Berg, I., Luijten, M., van Rooij, A. J., Keemink, L., & Franken, I. H.
Chiu, P. H., & Deldin, P. J. (2007). Neural evidence for enhanced error detection in A. (2012). Error processing and response inhibition in excessive computer game
major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 608–616. players: An event-related potential study. Addiction Biology, 17(5), 934–947.
Clayson, P. E., Baldwin, S. A., & Larson, M. J. (2012). How does noise affect amplitude Luu, P., Collins, P., & Tucker, D. M. (2000). Mood, personality, and self-monitoring:
and latency measurement of event-related potentials (ERPs)? A methodological negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mechanisms of error
critique and simulation study. Psychophysiology, 50, 174–186. monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), 43–60.
Clayson, P. E., Clawson, A., & Larson, M. J. (2011). Sex differences in electrophysio- Maletic, V., Robinson, M., Oakes, T., Iyengar, S., Ball, S. G., & Russell, J. (2007). Neu-
logical indices of conflict monitoring. Biological Psychology, 87, 282–289. robiology of depression: An integrated view of the key findings. International
Di Paolo, T. (1994). Modulation of brain dopamine transmission by sex steroids. Journal of Clinical Practice, 61(12), 2030–2040.
Reviews in the Neurosciences, 5, 27–42. Meyer, A., Klein, D. N., Torpey, D. C., Kujawa, A. J., Hayden, E. P., Sheikh, H. I., et al.
Elder, B. L., & Mosack, V. (2011). Genetics of depression: An overview of the current (2012). Additive effects of the dopamine D2 receptor and dopamine transporter
science. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 32, 192–202. genes on the error-related negativity in young children. Genes, Brain and Behav-
Elliott, R., Rubinsztein, J. S., Sahakian, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2000). Selective attention ior, 11(6), 695–703.
to emotional stimuli in a verbal go/no-go task: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 11, Mitterschiffthaler, M. T., Williams, S. C. R., Walsh, N. D., Cleare, A. J., Donaldson, C.,
1739–1744. Scott, J., et al. (2008). Neural basis of the emotional Stroop interference effect in
Endrass, T., Klawohn, J., Schuster, F., & Kathmann, N. (2008). Overactive performance major depression. Psychological Medicine, 38(2), 247–256.
monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ERP evidence from correct and Mora, F., Segovia, G., del Arco, A., de Blas, M., & Garrido, P. (2012). Stress, neuro-
erroneous reactions. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1877–1887. transmitters corticosterone and body–brain integration. Brain Research, 1476,
Falkenstein, M. (2004). Errors, conflicts and the brain. Journal of Psychophysiology, 71–85.
18, 153–163. Moran, T. P., Taylor, D., & Moser, J. S. (2012). Sex modulates the relationship between
Falkenstein, M., Hielscher, H., Dziobek, I., Schwarzenau, P., Hoormann, J., Sunder- worry and performance monitoring brain activity in undergraduates. Interna-
mann, B., et al. (2001). Action monitoring, error detection, and the basal ganglia: tional Journal of Psychophysiology, 85(2), 188–194.
An ERP study. Neuroreport, 12(1), 157–161. Moser, J. S., Moran, T. P., Schroder, H. S., Donnellan, M. B., & Yeung, N. (2013). On
Fitzgerald, D. A., Arnold, J. F., Becker, E. S., Speckens, A. E. M., Rinck, M., Rijpkema, the relationship between anxiety and error monitoring: A meta-analysis and
M., et al. (2011). How mood challenges emotional memory formation: An fMRI conceptual framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 466.
investigation. Neuroimage, 56(3), 1783–1790. Munro, C. A., McCaul, M. E., Wong, D. F., Oswald, L. M., Zhou, Y., Brasic, J., et al.
Foland-Ross, L. C., Hamilton, P. J., Joormann, J., Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., & Gotlib, I. (2006). Sex differences in striatal dopamine release in healthy adults. Biological
H. (2013). The neural basis of difficulties disengaging from negative irrelevant Psychiatry, 59, 966–974.
material in major depression. Psychological Science, 24(3), 334–344. Munro, G. E. S., Dywan, J., Harris, G. T., McKee, S., Unsal, A., & Segalowitz, S. J.
Fossati, P. (2008). Neural signatures of cognitive and emotional biases in depression. (2007). ERN varies with degree of psychopathy in an emotion discrimination
Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 10(3), 358–361. task. Biological Psychology, 76(1–2), 31–42.
Franken, I. H. A., van Strien, J. W., Franzek, E. J., & van de Wetering, B. J. (2007). Error- Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band, G. P. H., & Kok, A. (2001).
processing deficits in patients with cocaine dependence. Biological Psychology, Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response
75(1), 45–51. errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology, 38, 752–760.
114 P.L.A. Schoenberg / Biological Psychology 99 (2014) 100–114

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Gender differences in depression. Current Directions in major depressive disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 218–
Psychological Science, 10(5), 173–176. 224.
Olvet, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2008). The error-related negativity (ERN) and psy- Schrijvers, D., de Bruijn, E. R. A., Maas, Y., De Grave, C., Sabbe, B. G. C., & Hulstijn,
chopathology: Towards an endophenotype. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, W. (2008). Action monitoring in major depressive disorder with psychomotor
1343–1354. retardation. Cortex, 44, 569–579.
Olvet, D. M., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Depression symptom severity and Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E.,
error-related brain activity. Psychiatry Research, 179, 30–37. et al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The
Overbeek, T. J. M., Nieuwenhuis, S., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dissociable compo- development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for
nents of error processing: On the functional significance of the Pe vis-à-vis the DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 22–33.
ERN/Ne. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 319–329. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual
Piccinelli, M., & Wilkinson, G. (2000). Gender differences in depression: Critical for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
review. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 486–492. Spreckelmeyer, K. N., Krach, S., Kohls, G., Rademacher, L., Irmak, A., Konrad,
Riccardi, P., Park, S., Anderson, S., Doop, M., Sib Ansari, M., Schmidt, D., et al. (2011). K., et al. (2009). Anticipation of monetary and social reward differently
Sex differences in the relationship of regional dopamine release to affect and activates mesolimbic brain structures in men and women. Scan, 4, 158–
cognitive function in striatal and extrastriatal regions using poitron emission 165.
tomography and [18 F] Fallypride. Synapse, 65, 99–102. Tucker, D. M., Luu, P., Frishkoff, G., Quiring, J., & Poulsen, C. (2003). Frontolim-
Riccardi, P., Zald, D., Li, R., Park, S., Sib Ansari, M., Dawant, B., et al. (2006). Sex bic response to negative feedback in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal
differences in amphetamine-induced displacement of [18 F] Fallypride in striatal Psychology, 112(4), 667–678.
and extrastriatal regions: A PET study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(9), Ullsperger, M., & von Cramon, Y. D. (2004). Neuroimaging of performance monitor-
1639–1641. ing: Error detection and beyond. Cortex, 40, 593–604.
Ridderinkhof, R. K., Ramautar, J. R., & Wijnen, J. G. (2009). To PE or not to PE : A van Loo, H. M., de Jonge, P., Romeijn, J-W., Kessler, R. C., & Schoevers, R. A. (2012).
P3-like ERP component reflecting the processing of errors. Psychophysiology, 46, Data-driven subtypes of major depressive disorder: A systematic review. BMC
531–538. Medicine, 10, 156.
Roy, M., Shohamy, D., & Wager, T. D. (2012). Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical Vidal, F., Hasbroucq, T., Grapperon, J., & Bonnet, M. (2000). Is the ‘error negativity’
systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, specific to errors? Biological Psychology, 51, 109–128.
16(3), 147–156. Vuilleumier, P., Armory, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and
Ruchsow, M., Herrnberger, B., Beschoner, P., Grön, G., Spitzer, M., & Kiefer, M. (2006). emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study.
Error processing in major depressive disorder: Evidence from event-related Neuron, 30, 829–841.
potentials. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40, 37–46. Weinberg, A., Olvet, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Increased error-related brain activity
Ruchsow, M., Herrnberger, B., Wiesend, C., Grön, G., Spitzer, M., & Kiefer, M. (2004). in generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychology, 85, 472–480.
The effect of erroneous responses on response monitoring in patients with major Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of errordetection:
depressive disorder: A study with event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 41, Conflict monitoring and error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111(4),
833–840. 931–959.
Rush, A. J., Gullion, C. M., Basco, M. R., Jarrett, R. B., & Trivedi, M. H. (1996). The Yeung, N. (2004). Relating cognitive and affective theories of the error-related neg-
Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (IDS): Psychometric properties. Psy- ativity. In M. Ullsperger, & M. Falkenstein (Eds.), Errors, conflicts, and the brain;
chological Medicine, 26, 477–486. Current opinions on performance monitoring (pp. 63–70). Liepzig: Max Planck
Schrijvers, D., de Bruijn, E. R. A., Maas, Y. J., Vancoillie, P., Hulstijn, W., Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience.
& Sabbe, B. G. C. (2009). Action monitoring and symptom reduction in

Anda mungkin juga menyukai