Anda di halaman 1dari 29

Verb-particle constructions in Romance:

A lexical-syntactic account
1
JAUME MATEU AND GEMMA RIGAU
Probus 22 (2010), 241269 09214771/10/022-0241
DOI 10.1515/prbs.2010.009 Walter de Gruyter
Abstract
In this paper, we deal with some important differences between Romance verb
particle constructions and Germanic ones. Despite claims to the contrary (Ia-
cobini & Masini 2007), we argue that Romance languages (Italian included)
consistently obey the Talmian generalization that non-directional manner verbs
do not coappear with non-adjunct paths in Romance. We relate this generaliza-
tion to the important restriction that those verbs that enter into verb-particle
constructions in Romance encode or involve Path (e.g., It. Gianni entrato den-
tro lit.Gianni entered inside; Gianni corso via Gianni ran away; Gianni ha
lavato via la macchia Gianni washed the stain away); crucially, unlike Ger-
manic, Romance does not allow verb-particle constructions where the verb en-
codes or involves pure manner (John danced away; John worked his debts off).
We use Haugens (2009) syntactic distinction between Incorporation and Con-
ation to draw some relevant syntactic differences between Romance and Ger-
manic verb-particle constructions: those ones formed via Incorporation are
possible in Romance, while those ones formed via Conation/Compounding
are only possible in Germanic.
1. Introduction
It has been pointed out that verb-particle constructions fascinate syntacticians
because they do not fall neatly within the distinctions between syntax and mor-
phology and between idioms and compositional constructions. As a result, one
1. The authors are grateful to M. Luisa Zubizarreta and Violeta Demonte and to two anony-
mous reviewers for their observations and suggestions. This research has been sponsored
by grants HUM2006-13295-C02-01/02 (Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnologa-FEDER) and
2009SGR1079 (Generalitat de Catalunya).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
242 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
nds different proposals in a vast literature, which is mostly about Germanic
languages (e.g., see Deh et al. (2002) and McIntyre (2007), for a review).
2
More recently, verb-particle constructions have also attracted the attention of
Romance linguists since Italian verbi sintagmatici (phrasal verbs) like those
in (1) turn to be quite productive in this Romance language as well (e.g., see
Grcia 1988; Simone 1996; Masini 2005, 2006; Iacobini & Masini 2007; Ia-
cobini 2009; Cini 2008, among others).
(1) buttare gi throwdown, uscire fuori exit out, correre via run away,
tirare su bring up, lavare via wash away, etc.
In this paper, we deal with some important differences between Germanic verb
particle constructions and Romance ones. For example, we want to call into
question one recent important claim found in Iacobini & Masini (2007): it is
evident that Italian does not conformto Talmys generalization, since it behaves
more like English than Spanish.
3
Masini (2005, 2006), Iacobini & Masini
(2007), and Iacobini (2009) provide an interesting descriptive account of how
verb-particle constructions like those in (1) emerged in Italian. There is, how-
ever, a glaring omission in their descriptive works: i.e., they do not address
what we argue is the most important difference between Italian and Germanic
phrasal verbs. By addressing this crosslinguistic point in the present paper, we
reach a conclusion that is in fact opposite to theirs: Italian verbi sintagmatici
resemble Germanic phrasal verbs but only supercially. That is to say, when
a more abstract level of analysis is adopted (e.g., the one provided by Hale
and Keysers (2002) l(exical)-syntactic theory of argument structure), Italian
can be shown to behave more like Spanish than English in that relevant sense,
namely both Romance languages consistently obey the Talmian generalization
that non-directional manner verbs do not coappear with (non-adjunct) paths in
Romance (see Section 4 below).
In part, the present paper can be taken as a response to Masinis (2005)
and Iacobini and Masinis (2007) Construction Grammar approaches to Italian
verb-particle constructions. Not only will we show that the lexical variation
which inspired their surface-based works can be structurally accounted for, we
will also argue that it actually constitutes support for the structural position
2. Two renowned proposals are the Complex Predicate approach (Johnson 1991; Neeleman
1994; Stiebels & Wunderlich 1994; Zeller 2001, among others) and the Small Clause ap-
proach (Kayne 1985; Hoekstra 1988; den Dikken 1995; Ramchand & Svenonius 2002, Mateu
2008, among others).
3. As shown below in Section 4, they refer to Talmys (1991, 2000) highly inuential typology
of motion expressions, with its dichotomy of verb-framed languages like Spanish (where
path is expressed as a semantic component of a motion verb) and satellite-framed languages
like English (where path is expressed as a satellite around the verb, e.g., as a particle).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 243
against the continuum position found in some cognitive and functionalist ap-
proaches. We will show that this lexical variation is not arbitrary nor can be
accounted for in the everything is listed kind of Construction Grammar uni-
verse, but instead is tightly constrained by the same structural factors that can
be observed to be in effect in canonical motion-construction instances in the
languages under consideration.
Accordingly, we will argue, for example, that there is a principled explana-
tion for contrasts like the one exemplied in (2): the ungrammaticality of (2b)
shows that there is no Italian counterpart of dance away. Of course, one could
say that idiosyncrasy is expected if phrasal verbs are formed in the lexicon (i.e.,
some are possible, some are not). However, we will show that there is a general
structural explanation of this difference.
(2) a. Gianni
Gianni

is
corso
run
via.
away
Gianni ran away.
b.
*
Gianni
Gianni

is
danzato
danced
via.
away
Gianni danced away.
We claim that phrasal verbs like those in (1) are possible in Italian precisely
because their verbs already encode or involve directionality, which is further
specied by the particle. In contrast, this restriction does not hold in Germanic:
accordingly, examples like those in (3) are impossible in Italian because the
verb does not involve directionality, this meaning component being only en-
coded in a true Path satellite. Masini (2005, 2006), Iacobini and Masini (2007),
and Iacobini (2009) do not offer any general explanation of why verb-particle
constructions like those in (2b) and (3) are impossible in Italian.
(3) a. John worked his debts off.
b. John danced the night away.
Interestingly, Masini (2005: 167) claims that the existence of Italian phrasal
verbs like lavare via (wash away) or raschiare via (scrape away) in (4)
depends on the removal sense of the verbs involved (cf. 5).
(4) a. Gianni
Gianni
ha
has
lavato
washed
via
away
la
the
macchia.
stain
Gianni washed the stain away.
b. Gianni
Gianni
ha
has
raschiato
scraped
via
away
la
the
vernice.
paint
Gianni scraped the paint away.
(5) a. [V [
P
via]]
b. V= agentive verb of removing
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
244 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
c. to remove by V
Masini (2005: 167)
However, what Masini misses is the crucial observation that what seems to
be an idiosyncratic restriction on removal verbs in (4) is actually related to
a more general fact of Italian: i.e., its verb-framed nature with respect to Path
(see Section 4 below); that is, when dealing with the particular via case, Masini
(2005) and Iacobini and Masini (2007) are not successful in explaining what
(4a,b) and (2a) have in common that makes them possible in Italian, while
(2b) and (3) are not. As noted, there is a Talmian generalization involved here:
Italian verbs like correre, lavare or raschiare can be expected to enter into
verb-particle constructions like those in (2a) and (4) because these verbs can
be claimed to acquire directionality. In contrast, examples like those in (2b)
and (3) are expected to be impossible in Italian, since their verbs do not encode
nor involve any directionality component.
Our point is then clear: one cannot simply state that Italian behaves more
like English than Spanish on the basis of surface facts, without comparing the
entire array of paradigms in the two languages (see Section 4 below).
Moreover, in the next section, we show that verb-particle constructions are
not a quirk of Italian, but this phenomenon is also found within other Romance
languages with different degrees of productivity (see also Iacobini 2009; Mateu
and Rigau 2009). Once again the Talmian prediction is that Germanic phrasal
verbs like those in (2b) and (3) should not be found in those other Romance
languages that have verb-particle constructions. We will show that such a pre-
diction is borne out.
To advance our conclusion, we claim that, unlike English, Italian (and, more
generally, Romance) prevents pure (i.e., non-directional) manner verbs from
taking Paths as complements, whereby the relevant structural meaning com-
ponent in the Romance examples of verb-particle constructions (the apparently
problematic examples in (2a) and (4) included) will be shown to be Path, rather
than Manner. Accordingly, our conclusion to be exposed in Section 4 will be
that Italian does conform to Talmys (1991, 2000) generalization concerning
Romance languages: i.e., they lack the so-called Co-event pattern.
In Section 4, we will also show that our syntactic proposal can be related to
Follis (2002) and Folli & Ramchands (2005) interesting proposals concerning
the differences between English and Italian goal of motion constructions: we
will argue that their [R]esult feature, which can also be claimed to be lexically
encoded in verbs like those in (2a) and (4), corresponds to our Talmian Path.
Accordingly, we will show that their criticisms of Talmys typology are not
well-grounded since, despite appearances, these constructions in Romance do
not counterexemplify our structural version of the Talmian typology but rather
support it.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 245
The organization of the paper is as follows: in Section 2 we show that
verb-particle constructions are not a quirk of Italian but a Pan-Romance phe-
nomenon. In Section 3 we present our syntactic analysis of Italian phrasal verbs
like lavare via wash away, which is inspired by Hale & Keysers (2000) in-
sightful l-syntactic account of English complex verbs like cool down or heat
up. In Section 4 we deal with the important differences between Romance vs.
Germanic phrasal verbs within the context of Talmys (1991, 2000) typology of
motion events. In Section 5 we present our conclusion and some nal remarks.
2. Romance phrasal verbs: A Pan-Romance phenomenon
In this section, we show that the presence of phrasal verbs in Romance lan-
guages is productive and non-sporadic (see also Iacobini 2009). To begin with,
it is interesting to point out that the presence of verb-particle constructions is
widely attested in Old Romance languages: for example, see the data from Old
Spanish in (6) (fromthe CORDE data base), Old Catalan in (7), and Old French
in (8) (see Rouget & Tremblay 2003; Burnett & Tremblay 2007, 2009).
4
(6) a. echar fuera throw out
(Nebrija, A. de (1492) Gramtica castellana, 24V)
b. echar arriba throw up
(Herrera, G. A. de (1513) Obra agricultura, 2)
c. echar delante throw forward
(Pineda, J. de (1589) Dilogos familiares de la agricultura cris-
tiana, IV, 174)
d. venir delante come forward
((c 1400) Biblia Escorial I-j-4: Pentateuco, 136R)
e. volver atrs turn back
(Annimo (15751580) Relaciones topogrcas de los pueblos de
Espaa, 469)
f. subir arriba rise/raise up
(Annimo (1293) Gran Conquista de Ultramar. ms. 1187 BNM)
(7) a. anar defora go out
(Sant Gregori (1340) Dilegs, 24v
b. gitar fora throw out
(Sant Gregori (1340) Dilegs, 5v)
c. metre sus put up
(Annim (XVc.) Curial e Gelfa, 155)
4. Actually, verb-particle constructions were already common in pre-classical Latin: see Iacobini
(2009: 37) for examples from Plautus (254184 BC).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
246 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
d. pujar sus rise up
(Pere IV (XIVc) Crnica, 201)
e. tirar defora throw out
(Sant Gregori (1340) Dilegs, 1.19)
f. tornar amunt turn up
(135760, Llibre de Cort Reial mallorqu, 242)
g. treure sus draw up
(135760, Llibre de Cort Reial mallorqu, 277)
h. venir dessus come over
(R. Muntaner (13251328), Crnica, CCXLIX)
(8) a. aller ariere to go back
b. aller avant to go forward
c. courir su to pursue, to attack
d. mettre sus to put on
e. issir fors to go out
According to Rouget & Tremblay (2003), while during Middle Ages, French
had an extensive system of separated particles used to modify the aspectual/
locative value of verbs (e.g.: issir hors), during the Renaissance, such construc-
tions had almost completely disappeared.
5
Interestingly, this was not the fate
of Italian, Spanish, Catalan, and other Romance languages, which still show
a rich range of verb-particle constructions at present. In order to show that
phrasal verbs are quite widespread in Romance languages other than Italian, it
is interesting to see that some examples from Dantes dialect (see Masini 2006:
8799) can also be found in Catalan and Spanish: see (9). See also Calvo Rigual
(2008) and Mateu & Rigau (2009), for more data and additional discussion.
6
5. Nevertheless, nowadays phrasal verbs are used in less formal registers and non-standard
French: sauter dessus to jump up, courir aprs lit. to run after: to pursue, aller en ar-
rire go back, revenir en arrire come back, etc. (see Iacobini 2009: 22, and references
therein).
6. Some verbi sintagmatici can often acquire a metaphorical use based on their basic locative
meaning. For instance, It. tirare su throw up can mean build (a building) and comfort
someone, while Old Fr. mettre sus (lit. to put on) meant to accuse, and Cat. tirar endavant
throw ahead and Sp. sacar adelante bring ahead mean to get something off the ground,
to keep something going or to give ones family a good start in life. The fact that the
meaning of verb-particle constructs like It. andare avanti is literal (go ahead) or gurative
(to make progress) will be claimed to be irrelevant with respect to their associated l-syntactic
analyses (see Section 3).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 247
(9) Dantes dialect Catalan Spanish
andare avanti go ahead anar endavant ir/salir adelante
andare fore go out anar fora ir fuera
andare suso/su go up anar amunt ir arriba
buttare fuori throw out tirar fora echar fuera
discendere gi/giuso
descend down
baixar avall bajar abajo
gittare gi throw down tirar avall echar abajo
mettere avanti put ahead tirar endavant sacar adelante
tirare su throw up tirar amunt echar arriba
uscire fuori exit out sortir fora salir fuera
Phrasal verbs are also present in other Romance languages, as exemplied in
the sample in (10):
(10) a. Friulan (Vicario 1995):
l fur to go out, l su to go up, l vie to go away, mont su
to go up, etc.
b. Piedmont (Iacobini 2009):
all via to go away, bt gi to throw down, tira avanti to
throw ahead, i.e., to manage, etc.
c. Sardinian (Iacobini 2009):
besari a foras to exit out, andarei abbasciu to go down/to fall,
fuliare foras to throw away, etc.
d. Sicilian (Iacobini 2009, from Amenta 2008):
iri avanti to go ahead, ittari fuori to throw away, mttiri iusu
to put down, etc.
e. Venetan (Beninc and Poletto 2006):
ndar fora go out, buttar fora throw out, tirar su bring up,
saltar fora jump up, i.e., crop up, vegner fora come out, etc.
Although examples in (9) and (10) show that phrasal verbs can be found in
many Romance languages, it is true that Italian and other languages such as
Venetan and Friulan can indeed be considered exceptional among other Ro-
mance Languages since they have developed a pattern where the verb is not a
motion verb:
7
e.g., lavare to wash, pulire to clean, fregare to rub, grattare
to scrape, eliminare to eliminate, etc., and also mangiare to eat, bere to
drink, fumare to smoke, etc.; see the examples in (11) through (13). Two
particles can typically go with these verbs in Italian: via away and fuori out.
7. Interestingly, these examples are not attested in Dantes works (see Masini 2006), whereby
they can be considered as a more recent innovation.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
248 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
However, what will be shown to be crucial in the next sections is that this in-
novative pattern is allowed in Italian (and other languages such as Venetan and
Friulan) as long as the verbal basis involves an abstract directionality compo-
nent. By contrast, Germanic languages are not subject to this lexical restriction,
whereby their verb-particle constructions are much more diverse.
(11) a. Riuscirai
manage-fut.2sg
a
to
lavare
wash
via
away
il
the
sudore.
sweat
You will manage to wash the sweat away.
b. Voglio
want.1sg
lavare
wash
via
away
i
the
miei
my
peccati.
sins
I want to wash my sins away.
c. Lavi
wash
fuori
out
le
the
scatole
box
del
of.the
pranzo
lunch
ogni
every
giorno.
day
Wash up your lunch box every day.
d. Devono
must-3pl
raschiare
scrape
via
away
la
the
sporcizia.
dirt
They have to scrape out the dirt.
e. Lintonaco
the plastering

is
stato
been
mangiato
eaten
via
away
dalla
by the
umidit.
humidity
The plastering was completely damaged by the humidity.
f. Lui
he
ha
has
bevuto
drunk
via
away
il
the
vino
wine
ad
in
un
one
tiro.
go
He drank the wine up all at once.
(12) a. (Venetan) S-ciopa
(it) burst
fora
out
tuto.
everything
Everything is going to burst. (Beninc and Poletto 2006: 11)
b. El
he
se
refl
ga
has
magn
eaten
fora
out
i
the
schei.
money
He spent his money. (Beninc and Poletto 2006: 13)
(13) (Friulan) Al
he

has
bevt
drunk
fr
out
dute
all
la
the
butilie
bottle
di bessl.
alone
He drank the bottle up on his own. (Vicario 1995: 191)
Having established that phrasal verbs are possible and even productive within
the Romance languages, a structural analysis remains to be provided, a non-
trivial matter as we will see immediately in the next section.
3. On the l-syntax of verbi sintagmatici
Before dealing with the l(exical)-syntax of Romance phrasal verbs, it will be
useful to sketch out Hale and Keysers (1998, 2002) basic elements of argument
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 249
structure, whose syntactic approach is assumed here:
8
in particular, our present
analysis of Romance phrasal verbs will be shown to be inspired by Hale &
Keysers (2000) analysis of English complex verbs like heat up, cool down or
widen out.
As is well-known, argument structure is conceived of by Hale and Keyser
as the syntactic conguration projected by a lexical item, that is, argument
structure is the system of structural relations holding between heads (nuclei)
and the arguments linked to them. Their main assumptions can be expressed as
follows: argument structure is dened in reference to two possible relations be-
tween a head and its arguments, namely, the head-complement relation and the
head-specier relation. A given head (i.e., x in 14) may enter into the following
structural combinations in (14): these are its argument structure properties, and
its syntactic behavior is determined by these properties.
9
(14) a. x
x y
b. x
z x
x y
c.
z
y
d. x
The main empirical domain on which Hale and Keysers hypotheses have been
tested includes unergative denominal verbs like dance in (15a), transitive loca-
tion verbs like shelve in (15b) or transitive locatum verbs like saddle in (15c),
and (anti)causative deadjectival verbs like clear in (15d). Unergative verbs are
argued to be hidden transitives in the sense that they involve merging a non-
relational element (typically, a noun) with a verbal head (see 16a); both lo-
cation verbs (e.g., shelve) and locatum verbs (e.g., saddle) involve merging
the structural combination in (14b) with the one in (14a): see (16b). Finally,
unaccusative verbs involve the structural combination in (14c), the causative
one involving two structural combinations,
10
i.e., the one depicted in (14c) is
merged with the one in (14a): see (16c).
11
8. See Zubizarreta & Oh (2007), for an excellent review of Hale & Keysers (1993, 2002) theory
of l(exical)-syntax.
9. According to Hale & Keyser (2002), the prototypical or unmarked morphosyntactic realiza-
tions of the x head in English are the following ones: V in (14a), P in (14b), Adj in (14c), and
N in (14d).
10. See Hale and Keyser (2002: Chap. 6) on why unaccusatives of the arrive-type do not
causativize. See also Zubizarreta and Oh (2007: 192; Fn. 14).
11. Hale and Keyser (1993, 2002) argue that the external argument is truly external to argument
structure congurations: for example, they typically appeal to this proposal when accounting
for why unacusative structures can be causativized (cf. 15d), while unergatives ones can-
not (e.g., *Mary danced John; cf. Mary made John dance). Accordingly, the external argu-
ment can be claimed to occupy the specier position of a functional projection in so-called
s(entential)-syntax.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
250 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
(15) a. John danced.
b. John shelved the book.
c. John saddled the horse.
d. The strong winds cleared the sky / The sky cleared.
(16) a. V
V
[ / 0]
N
dance
b. V
V
[ / 0]
P
DP P
the {book/horse} P N
[ / 0]{shelf/saddle}
c. V
V
[ / 0]
V
DP V
the screen V
[ / 0]
A
clear
Applying the incorporation operation to (16a) involves copying the full phono-
logical matrix of the noun dance into the empty one corresponding to the verb.
Applying it to (16b) involves two steps: the full phonological matrix of the
noun {shelf /saddle} is rst copied into the empty one corresponding to the
preposition. Since the phonological matrix corresponding to the verb is also
empty, the incorporation operation applies again from the saturated phonolog-
ical matrix of the preposition to the unsaturated matrix of the verb. Finally, ap-
plying incorporation to (16c) involves two steps as well: the full phonological
matrix of the adjective clear is rst copied into the empty one corresponding
to the internal verb. Since the phonological matrix corresponding to the ex-
ternal verb is also empty, the incorporation applies again from the saturated
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 251
phonological matrix of the inner verb to the unsaturated matrix of the external
verb.
However, as pointed out by Hale &Keyser (1997, 2002), hyponymous object
constructions like the ones exemplied in (17) pose a non-trivial problemto the
previous system:
(17) a. John danced a polka. (cf. John danced)
b. John shelved the books on the windowsill. (cf. John shelved the
books)
In (17a) an overt DP (a polka) is allowed to be inserted in the position that
would in principle correspond to the trace left by incorporation of N into V.
Similarly, in (17b) an overt PP (on the windowsill) can be inserted in the posi-
tion that would correspond to the traces left behind in N and P en route to the
null V. The solution put forward by Hale & Keyser (1997) involved delinking
the incorporated noun from its base-generated position via index-deletion
(see their work for details). However, here we will not assume their trace/index-
deletion analysis but rather Haugens (2009) alternative proposal. In order to
avoid the cyclicity problems involved in Hale &Keysers (1997) analysis, Hau-
gen (2009) has recently argued for an insertion of non-cognate roots into the
upper and lower copies after a movement (i.e., Copy) operation has applied. In
particular, Haugen (2009: 248251) argues that, once a Late Insertion account
is accepted,
12
it is possible to spell-out two different roots (e.g.,

dance and

polka in [18]) for the purpose of expressing identical abstract syntactic fea-
tures. According to this author, the Projection Principle is not violated because
the lower copy remains coindexed with the upper copy, and no features are ever
deleted. Notice then that in his account there is a trace of movement, i.e., a
bundle of abstract syntactic features in the lower copy. For example, dance a
polka can be analyzed as follows:
13
12. See also Hale & Keyser (2002: 7778), who point out that arguments for late insertion of
vocabulary items, in the sense of Halle & Marantzs (1993) theory of Distributed Morphology,
are extraordinarily compelling. In particular, see Hale and Keyser (2002: 98), for their last
DM-like account of denominal verbs like dance.
13. A sentence like #John danced a table would not be ungrammatical but rather pragmatically
ill-formed: in Haugens (2009: 253) account the hyponymy interpretation occurs due to prag-
matic constraints.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
252 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
(18) V
V
[, , . . . n]
i

dance
DP
D N
[, , . . . n]
i

polka
According to Haugen (2009), there are two ways of forming denominal verbs:
i.e., via Incorporation or via Conation. To put it in Haugens (2009: 260)
words, Incorporation is conceived of as head-movement (as in Baker 1988;
Hale & Keyser 1993), and is instantiated through the syntactic operation of
Copy, whereas Conation is instantiated directly through Merge (compound-
ing). Accordingly, in Incorporation cases, the denominal verb (e.g., see 15a:
John danced) is formed via Copying the relevant set of features of the nominal
complement into the null verb. In contrast, in Conation cases, the denominal
verb (e.g., see 19a) is formed via Compounding a nominal root with the null
verb (see 19b).
14
(19) a. The factory horns sirened midday and everyone broke for lunch
(ex. from Clark & Clark 1977, apud Borer [2005])
b. [
FP
[
DP
the factory horns] . . . [
V
[
V

siren V] [
DP
midday]]]
With the previous background in mind, let us deal with our analysis of Ro-
mance phrasal verbs, which is inspired by Hale & Keysers (2000: 4547)
analysis of English complex verbs like heat up, cool down or widen out, whose
directional particles are claimed by them to be analyzed as cognate (sic)
complements of an abstract P incorporated in the verb. According to these au-
thors, it is not the case that the root heat in (20) incorporates into the particle
up; rather their claim is that this prepositional-like element is inserted into the
P head after the simple verb has been formed.
15
Furthermore, Hale & Keyser
(2000: 45-46) point out that in (20) P does not head a separate, autonomous
predicate. Instead, it is as if A and P jointly head one and the same predicate.
And this, like any adjectival predicate, nds its subject external to its own pro-
jection.
14. For further discussion on the so-called Manner Conation, see also Mateu & Rigau (2002),
Mateu (2002, 2008, 2010), McIntyre (2004), Harley (2005), Zubizarreta & Oh (2007), Acedo-
Matelln & Mateu (2008), i.a. As we will see in Section 4, this Compounding operation
accounts for the way the Manner component is introduced in Germanic phrasal verbs like the
ones exemplied in (3a) John worked his debts off and (3b) John danced the night away.
15. The upper verbal head in (20) is only posited in the causative use in (21a).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 253
(20) V
V V
heat
i
DP V
V P
P A
up
i
Hale & Keyser (2000) put forward the proposal that the examples in (21a, b)
involve a cognation process that is similar to the one found in (17a, b): that is,
according to them, there appears to be an interesting parallelism between the
prepositional-like particle that appears in (21a, b) and the hyponymous objects
in (17a, b).
(21) a. We heated the soup up. (cf. We heated the soup)
b. The soup heated up. (cf. The soup heated)
Hale & Keysers (2000) analysis of P-cognation, modulated by Haugens
(2009) Late Insertion account to avoid cyclicity problems, can also be claimed
to account for the formation of Romance phrasal verbs like those in (22), i.e.,
they can all be analyzed as cases where the directional particle species the ab-
stract P(ath) that has been incorporated (i.e., copied) into the verb. Following
Haugen (2009), we assume that it is possible to spell-out two different roots for
the purpose of expressing the same syntactic features: i.e., the P(ath) feature in
our present case. This proposal can then be claimed to account for our main
descriptive generalization: i.e., the list of verbs that enter into the Romance
verb-particle constructions is reduced to those verbs encoding or involving di-
rectionality (e.g., see 22).
(22) It. entrare dentro, lit. enter in; uscire fuori, lit. exit out; mettere gi
put down; buttare via throw away; lavare via wash away, etc.
We exemplify the l-syntactic analysis of lavare via wash away in (23),
16
which is intended to capture Masinis (2005) observation that the verbal basis
16. See Hale & Keyser (2000; 2002), for arguments distinguishing causative constructions (e.g.,
[15d] and [21a]) from transitive ones (e.g., [15b,c] and [23]): for example, only the former
enter into the causative alternation, a fact that is presumably related to its having a double
verbal shell.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
254 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
of lavare wash involves a directional meaning. We argue that its occurrence
in a Romance language like Italian would otherwise be impossible. The anal-
ysis of (23) also captures Svenoniuss (1996) proposal, assumed by Hale &
Keyser (2002: 229230), that bare particles like via away can be analyzed as
prepositions that incorporate a complement (i.e., the Ground): such a proposal
is coherent with maintaining the birelational nature of P.
17
(23) V
V
[+P(ath)]
i
P

lavare DP P
P
[+P(ath)]
i
X

via
In the next section, we deal with Mateu & Rigaus (2002, 2009) l-syntactic
reformulation of Talmys (1991, 2000) typology and the differences between
the Romance and Germanic patterns of phrasal verbs.
4. Romance phrasal verbs do not involve the Germanic co-event pattern
In this section we show to what extent it is mistaken to claim (i) that Italian
phrasal verbs do not conform to Talmys (1985, 1991, 2000) generalization
with respect to Romance languages and (ii) that they are similar to Germanic
phrasal verbs (cf. Grcia 1988; Simone 1996; Masini 2005; Iacobini & Masini
2007; Iacobini 2009, among others).
As is well-known, Talmy points out that the Germanic family belongs to
the class of so-called satellite-framed languages, whereas the Romance one
belongs to that of verb-framed languages. Consider some paradigmatic ex-
amples of his typology in (24), where Path is a prepositional satellite around
17. For the time being, we do not have a specic proposal on why the particle is typically adjacent
to the verb in Italian (see Masini 2008): cf. (i). See also Vinka (1999) and den Dikken (2003),
for similar facts in Swedish.
(i) a.
*
Gianni
Gianni
ha
has
lavato
washed
la
the
macchia
stain
via.
away
Gianni washed the stain away.
b. Gianni
Gianni
ha
has
lavato
washed
via
away
la
the
macchia.
stain
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 255
the verb in (24a), but is incorporated into the verb in the Spanish example in
(24b):
(24) a. The bottle oated into the cave.
b. La
the
botella
bottle
entr
entered
en
in
la
the
cueva
cave
otando.
oating
The bottle entered the cave oating.
As argued by Talmy, English can typically be taken as an example of satellite-
framed language, whereas Spanish can typically be regarded as an example
of verb-framed language. To put it in Talmys (1985) terms, (24a) involves
conation of Motion with Manner, or alternatively, in Talmys (1991) terms,
(24a) involves conation of MOVE with a
SUPPORTING
[EVENT]. In contrast,
the corresponding counterpart of (24a) in a Romance language like Spanish
(see 24b) involves a different lexicalization pattern, i.e., Motion is fused with
Path, the Manner component (or the Co-event) being expressed as adjunct.
Before dealing with the question as to whether Romance phrasal verbs are
counterexamples to Talmys typology, it will be useful to showhowthe relevant
canonical patterns exemplied in (24) are analyzed in our syntactic framework.
In particular, our proposal is that the relevant l-syntactic analyses of (24a) and
(24b) can be depicted as in (25) and (26), respectively.
18
(25) The Co-event pattern
V
DP
The bottle
V
V P
dir

float V
[ / 0]
P
dir
in
loc
-to
P
loc
P
loc
in-
DP
the cave
18. For the syntactic distinction between directional vs. locative prepositions, see Koopman
(2000), Hale & Keyser (2002), den Dikken (2006), Zubizarreta & Oh (2007), and Mateu
(2008), among others. Such a distinction could be claimed to be read off the mere syntactic
structure: cf. Hale & Keyser (2002: 221224).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
256 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
(26) The Path pattern
V
DP
La botella
V
V P
dir
P
dir
V
entr
P
dir
P
loc
P
loc
en
DP
la cueva
The so-called Co-event pattern of Germanic languages is to be related to
the fact that, for example, the complex P element into in (25) is not incorpo-
rated in the verb, this null verb being allowed to be merged or compounded
with the root

float .
19
As a result, this unaccusative V(erb), whose cong-
urational/constructional meaning is that of Change (see Hale & Keyser 2002;
Zubizarreta & Oh 2007), turns out to be associated with an additional embed-
ded meaning, that of oating, where the root encodes what Talmy (1991, 2000)
refers to as Co-event. Concerning (26), the incorporation of the complex di-
rectional P element into a null unaccusative verb in a Romance language like
Spanish gives a Path verb (entrar enter), the adjunct otando (oating) be-
ing merged outside the main argument structure.
20
Assuming this Talmian background,
21
we can now show why it makes more
sense to claim that Romance phrasal verbs belong to the (otherwise typolog-
ically expected) Path pattern rather than to the (Germanic) Co-event pattern.
19. See also Mateu & Rigau (2002), McIntyre (2004), and Zubizarreta & Oh (2007), for similar
analyses.
20. As can be inferred from the English translation of the Spanish example in (24b), a similar
analysis holds for the Romance pattern involved in enter the cave oating (versus cf. the Ger-
manic pattern: oat into the cave). Accordingly, the Path lexicalization pattern is also found
in (the Romance lexicon of) English: for additional discussion of other apparent counterex-
amples, see Talmy (1985, 2000).
21. See also Zubizarreta & Oh (2007), for an insightful account of these crosslinguistic dif-
ferences, which is based on Snyders (2001) Compounding Parameter (cf. also McIntyre
[2004]): according to them, Romance cannot use the relevant Compound Rule (Merge two
lexical categories of the same categorical type) to compose Manner and Directed Motion in
the way Germanic does; see also Mateu and Rigau (2002), for the claim that the Co-event
pattern in (25) involves a V-V compound. Notice the happy coincidence between these l-
syntactic proposals and Haugens (2009) (re)denition of Conation as Compounding (pace
Hale & Keyser 2002). Since the Talmian Co-event pattern always involves Conation (i.e.,
Compounding), we have a very interesting connection between Talmys (1991, 2000) typol-
ogy and Snyders (2001) Compounding Parameter (see Mateu [2010], for an elaboration of
this connection).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 257
Indeed, such a claim is coherent with the main descriptive generalization intro-
duced above: Romance languages (and more generally, so-called verb-framed
languages) lack complex resultative(like) constructions where the verb is cru-
cially non-directional, i.e., the verb does not involve directionality (e.g., see the
examples in [27]). Basically, our structural account of this descriptive fact is
that Talmys verb-framed languages lack the l-syntactic pattern in (25) since in
these languages it is the embedded/inner Path (and not an independent/external
root) what typically provides the relevant null verb with phonological content
(see Mateu & Rigau [2002, 2009], for additional discussion).
(27) a. John danced into the room.
b. John danced away.
c. John danced the puppet across the stage.
d. John danced the night away.
e. John outdanced Mary.
f. John danced his debts off.
g. John danced his feet sore.
h. John danced his way into a wonderful world.
Despite their surface similarity, our claim is then that the l-syntax of Italian
verbi sintagmatici is different from the one of those English phrasal verbs that
involve the Talmian Co-event pattern. As pointed out in the previous section,
the verbal basis of Romance verb-particle constructions encodes or involves
a directional meaning, which is further specied through a prepositional-like
element. In contrast, as can be shown by the English examples in (28), this
restriction does not hold in Germanic. Such an observation is crucial in or-
der to properly understand the truly relevant differences between Italian and
English.
22
See also Horrocks & Stavrou (2007), for an alternative explanation of the parametric dif-
ferences which is mainly based on the interesting observation that those languages that
grammaticalize viewpoint aspect in their verb morphology (e.g., Greek and Romance) reject
resultative-like constructions like those in (27).
Finally, it would take us too far aeld to review the vast literature where Talmys typology
has been criticized and/or rened (see Beavers et al. [2009], for a recent attempt). As pointed
out above, our present goal is much more modest: i.e., to work out to what extent Romance
phrasal verbs are similar to Germanic ones. Unlike other authors (e.g., Beavers et al. [2009]),
we conclude that they are not similar in an important sense related to Talmys typology.
22. Unfortunately, Talmy often grounds his typology on surface facts, whereby he mistakenly
admits that Northern Italian dialects are an exception to his typological classication of Ro-
mance languages. See Talmy (2000:145; Fn. 60): Southern Italian dialects have a Path con-
ation pattern, Northern dialects have a Co-event conation pattern, and central dialects, in-
cluding standard Italian, have both patterns in parallel, with discourse factors determining the
pattern used.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
258 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
(28) a. John danced the night away.
b. John outdanced Mary.
c. John worked the night away.
d. John outworked Mary.
As noted, examples like those in (28) are not to be found in Romance: this
should not be surprising since in these cases the verbal basis does not lexically
involve a directional meaning. Rather the Germanic constructions exemplied
in (28) involve a complex l-syntactic structure like the one in (29) where the
phonological matrix of the relevant null verb has been saturated by the full
one of an independent root via Conation/Compounding, the P(ath) being a
complete satellite.
23
(29) The Co-event pattern
V
V P

dance V
[ / 0]
DP
the night
P
P X
away
We think that Talmys (1991, 2000) descriptive term satellite is quite mis-
leading when dealing with the differences between Germanic and Romance
phrasal verbs. Since the particle is a prepositional-like satellite in both linguis-
tic families, both patterns of phrasal verbs could in principle be classied as
satellite-framed. Given this, we prefer to use Talmys expression Co-event
pattern rather than the more usual satellite-framed pattern when referring
to the Germanic pattern. Accordingly, we propose that the relevant typological
difference is not the one exemplied by light verbs plus a directional satellite
However, it is the case that even Northern Italian dialects can be claimed to lack the Germanic
Co-event pattern (cf. Masini [2005] and Iacobini [2009] for interesting arguments against
the so-called Germanic hypothesis according to which the origin of Italian phrasal verbs
depends on a calque from the German language). As expected, Northern Italian dialects lack
examples like the ones in (27) or (28), which shows the requirement that Talmys Co-event
pattern (and, more generally, all his descriptive lexicalization patterns) be formulated within a
more explanatory theory. In our present case, we assume that Hale & Keysers (2002) theory
of l-syntax can provide us with a more adequate level of abstraction to deal with Talmys
typology (see Mateu & Rigau 2002; 2009).
23. See Svenonius (1996), Hale & Keyser (2002), and Mateu (2008), among others, for the claim
that intransitive particles like the one exemplied in (29) incorporate their Ground comple-
ment (X).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 259
(both families have this type: e.g., go away / It. andare via), but the one ex-
emplied by pure (i.e., non-directional) manner verbs plus a satellite, the latter
being present in Germanic but not in Romance (e.g., oat/dance/. . . away vs.
It. *galleggiare/ballare/. . . via). Following this trend, consider the clear con-
trast in (2), repeated below in (30), which, as argued by Folli & Ramchand
(2005), can be accounted for by positing that correre to run (unlike danzare
to dance) optionally encodes a Result feature: according to their dual classi-
cation of Italian manner of motion verbs in (31), the verbs in (31a) optionally
encode a R(esult) feature, while the ones in (31b) do not.
(30) a. Gianni
Gianni

is
corso
run
via.
away
Gianni ran away. (cf. It. Gianni ha corso Gianni ran)
b.
*
Gianni
Gianni

is
danzato
danced
via.
away
Gianni danced away.
(31) a. [+V, (+Result)] verbs b. [+v, +V] verbs
correre run galleggiare oat
rotolare roll camminare walk
rimbalzare bounce galoppare gallop
scivolare lide, slide danzare dance
gattonare crawl nuotare swim
saltae jump sciare ski
volare y passeggiare walk around
saltellare hop vagabondare wander
(ex. from Folli and Ramchand 2005)
Our present proposal is that Folli & Ramchands (2005) [+R(esult)] feature
can in fact be related to our more general Talmian P(ath) component.
24
As pre-
dicted by Talmys typology, Italian pure manner verbs (e.g., verbs like It. dan-
zare to dance or camminare to walk) do not involve directionality, whereby
they are excluded fromcomplex telic path of motion constructions: cf. It. *dan-
zare via vs.
ok
dance away; It. *camminare via vs.
ok
walk away, etc. On the
other hand, the existence of complex verbs like It. correre via run away (see
[32a]) must not be taken as a true counterexample to Talmys typology (at least
as we reformulate it here) since correre in the unaccusative structure can also
be claimed to involve P(ath) (or R(esult), in Folli and Ramchands terms: see
[31a]). In other words, dance away falls under the Co-event pattern (cf. the
24. Following the localist hypothesis, which is assumed by Talmy (1991, 2000), it is interesting
to point out that Result can be understood as an abstract variant of telic Path (for an l-
syntactic reinterpretation of this hypothesis, see Mateu [2008]).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
260 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
l-syntactic analysis in [29]), while It. correre via run away can be claimed
to fall under the Path pattern in (32b). That is to say, our present analysis of
unaccusative verb-particle constructions like It. correre via is not so different
from the analysis of transitive ones like It. lavare via wash away (see Section
3):
25
in both cases the directional particle species the abstract P(ath) feature
that has been incorporated (i.e., copied) into the verb. As noted, this analysis
can be claimed to account for our main descriptive generalization: i.e., the list
of verbs that enter into Romance verb-particle constructions is reduced to those
verbs that encode or involve Path.
26
(32) a. Gianni
Gianni

is
corso
run
via.
away
Gianni ran away.
25. To be sure, there is an important difference between the transitive argument structure in (23)
and the unaccusative one in (32b): i.e., the subject is external in the rst case, but internal in
the second one (see Hale and Keyser [2002] and Zubizarreta & Oh [2007], for more discussion
of this structural difference within the l-syntactic theory).
26. Our claim is then that there is a similar explanation for the existence of unaccusative verb-
particle constructions like It. correre via run away and transitive ones like It. lavare via
wash away: both constructions involve directional manner verbs that acquire the P(ath)
component in l-syntax. However, for an alternative analysis of the unaccusative use of those
Italian verbs in (31a), see Zubizarreta and Oh (2007: Chap. 3), who explore the following
hypothesis in (i):
(i) Italian recruits the auxiliary position designated for a class of restructuring verbs in
order to compose directed motion and manner (in some lexically restricted cases
[cf. 31a])
For reasons of space, we will not review their analysis here: we will limit ourselves to pointing
out that in Zubizarreta and Ohs (2007: chap. 3) analysis the parallelism between transitive
phrasal verbs like lavare via and unaccusative ones like correre via remains unaccounted
for. By contrast, Folli and Ramchands (2005) analysis could account for this parallelism by
positing that both correre-type verbs and lavare-type verbs optionally encode the [+R(esult)]
feature in their lexical entry:
(ii) a. Gianni ha lavato la camicia. ([+Result] is not involved in the activity use)
Gianni washed the shirt.
b. Gianni ha lavato via la macchia. ([+Result] is involved in the accomplishment
use)
Gianni washed the stain away.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 261
b. V
DP
Gianni
V
V
[+P(ath)]
i
P

run P
[+P(ath)]
i
X

via
Accordingly, the Italian verbs in (31a) have two different l-syntactic structures
available: the unaccusative one in (32b), where the verb acquires a P(ath) fea-
ture, and the less complex unergative one in (33b), where the verb does not
encode Path. In contrast, the Italian verbs in (31b) only allow the unergative
l-syntactic structure in (33b), which corresponds to Hale and Keysers creation
structure in (16a): e.g., [
V
do dance].
27
(33) a. Gianni ha corso.
Gianni ran.
b. V
V X
Interestingly, the following data in (34) drawn from Sorace (2000: 875876)
seem to point to the fact that the verb correre in its unaccusative use encodes
Path. The unaccusative use of correre is not fully excluded in cases where
there is a locative or adverbial phrase: according to Sorace, the auxiliary essere
is marginally possible in (34ab). We take this fact as evidence that the verb
correre in its unaccusative use acquires the Path feature in l-syntax. In contrast,
those pure (i.e., non-directional) manner verbs like It. nuotare to swim or
danzare to dance do not follow this pattern (see [35]): i.e., the verbs in (31b)
only have an unergative/transitive use, whereby the auxiliary essere is excluded
in all contexts.
27. Our Italian consultants have told us that it is not the case that all verbs in the list in (31a)
(e.g., gattonare crawl) can be found in unaccusative contexts (contra Folli & Ramchand
2005). For those speakers, gattonare would then be included in the list in (31b). Since we are
dealing with lexical-syntactic facts, this kind of variation is expected. See also Zubizarreta &
Oh (2007: 196; Fn. 37), for similar remarks.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
262 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
(34) a. Gli
the
atleti
athletes
svedesi
Swedish
hanno
have
corso
run
/
/
?
sono
are
corsi
run
alle
at.the
Olimpiadi.
Olympic-Games
The Swedish athletes ran at the Olympic Games.
b. Maria
Maria
ha
has
corso
run
/
/

is
corsa
run
velocemente.
fast
Maria ran fast.
(ex. from Sorace 2000: 875876)
(35) a. Gli
the
atleti
athletes
{hanno
have
nuotato
swum
/
/
*sono
are
nuotati}
swum
alle
at.the
Olimpiadi.
Olympic-Games
The athletes swam at the Olympic Games.
b. Gianni
Gianni
{ha
has
/
/
*}
is
nuotato
swum
velocemente.
quickly
Gianni swam quickly.
Furthermore, if our analysis of the Germanic vs. Romance differences is on the
right track, the interesting contrasts in (36) through (39) can also be explained
on the basis that the Romance verbal bases in these examples do involve a
directional component, while the English corresponding ones do not: indeed,
this difference would account for why the directional phrase cannot be omitted
in the English examples.
(36) a. John washed the stain
??
(away).
b. (Italian) Gianni
Gianni
ha
has
lavato
washed
(via)
away
la
the
macchia.
stain
Gianni washed the stain away.
(37) a. John wiped the ngerprints *(from the table/away . . . ).
b. (Spanish) Juan
Juan
freg
wiped
las
the
huellas
ngerprints
(de
(of
la
the
mesa).
table)
Juan wiped the ngerprints from the table.
(38) a. John wiped the dust *(from the table).
b. (French) Jean a essuy la poussire (de la table).
Jean has wiped the dust (of the table)
Jean wiped the dust from the table.
(39) a. John wiped the stains *(from the door).
b. (Catalan) En
the
Joan
Joan
freg
wiped
les
the
taques
stains
(de
(of
la
the
porta).
door)
Joan wiped the stains from the door.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 263
The ungrammaticality of the English examples in (36a) to (39a) would then run
parallel to that of (40). As shown by Hoekstra (1988), the resultative PP/AP is
compulsory in (40) since it is the Small Clause Result predicate (and not the
verb) that licenses the direct object as its argument.
(40) a. John danced the night *(away).
b. John *(out)danced Mary.
c. He talked us *(into a stupor).
d. The dog barked the chickens *(awake).
Mutatis mutandis, we argue that the English PPs in (36a) to (39a) have the
same function the resultative PP/AP has in (40): the presence of PP/PartP is
compulsory in (36a) through (39a) in order to license the direct object, which
expresses the stuff that is removed.
28
In contrast, the Romance verb in (36b)
through (39b) can be argued to incorporate the abstract predicative head of the
SC-like resultative structure which encodes the Path. Given this, the Romance
counterpart of wipe in (36b) through (39b) means remove/get out: cf. John
[V+P
i
[
SC/PP
{the stain/the ngerprints/the dust} P
i
]]. No further PP is then
necessary in (36b) through (39b) to license the inner SC-like predicate, since
such a licensing is carried out via the incorporation of the Path head of the SC-
like PP into the verb. We claim that the Small Clause Result-based account just
presented can also be nicely translated into Hale & Keysers (2002) theory of l-
syntax: given this, the English examples in (36a) to (39a) involve the l-syntactic
pattern depicted in (41).
28. For metonymy reasons, wash the stain without the directional PP (see 36a) could be coerced
to have an additional reading where the internal argument is not the removed stuff but the
surface, i.e., it could have a reading similar to the one involved in wash the shirt, the latter
corresponding to a different pattern (i.e., [PROVIDE [the shirt WITH a wash]]; see Hale &
Keyser [2002] and Mateu [2002]).
See also Rappaport Hovav & Levin (1998: 118122), for an alternative semantic explanation
of the ill-formedness of examples like the one in (ia):
(i) a. John swept the crumbs *(off the table).
(Cf. John wiped the ngerprints *(away/from the table))
b. John swept the oor.
(Cf. John wiped the table)
Interestingly, John swept the crumbs is grammatical in Romance: e.g., Sp. John barri las
migas, John swept the crumbs. Since sweep in (ia) lacks a directional component, the Path
PP is obligatory in English. In contrast, Sp. barrer to sweep is a directional manner verb in
barrer las migas, whereby the Path PP is not necessary in Spanish. As predicted by Talmys
typology, Sp. barrer, but not Engl. sweep, is allowed to acquire a Path component in the
directional context of barrer las migas (cf. Sp. quitar las migas get+out the crumbs) but not
in the activity context of barrer el suelo sweep the oor (see ib).
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
264 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
(41) V
V P

wash

scrape
V DP
the stain
the dirt
P
P
??/*(away/off/. . . )
X
The Germanic l-syntactic pattern in (41) should be distinguished from the Ro-
mance one in (23), where the Italian verb lavare wash can be claimed to
acquire an abstract directionality (see Section 3): the Co-event pattern is in-
volved in (41) and is not found in Romance, as predicted by Talmys typology.
Despite appearances, no continuum but a discrete structural difference is in-
volved here.
Another similar piece of empirical evidence that we are dealing with two
different patterns is provided by the contrast in (42). The particle in the English
construction in (42a) is compulsory (see Folli & Harley [2005]), while this is
not the case in Italian: examples like the ones in (42c) and (42d), which were
found via a Google search, are judged as well-formed by our native consultants
of Italian (pace Folli &Harley 2005). Accordingly, the l-syntactic structures for
these constructions can be claimed to be different: i.e., the Co-event pattern is
involved in (43), while the Path pattern is the relevant one in (44).
(42) a. The sea ate the beach *(away).
b. Il
the
mare
sea
si
refl

is
mangiato
eaten
via
away
enormi
big
quantit
quantity
di
of
sabbia.
sand
The sea ate a big quantity of sand away.
c.
ok
Il
the
mare
sea
ha
has
mangiato
eaten
una
a
buona
big
parte
part
della
of.the
costa.
shore
The sea ate a big part of the shore away.
d.
ok
Il
the
mare
sea
ha
has
mangiato
eaten
gran
big
parte
part
di
of
spiaggia
beach
sabbiosa
sandy
ed
and
ha
has
lasciato . . .
left . . .
The sea ate a big part of the sandy beach away and left. . .
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 265
(43) V
V P

eat V DP P
P X
*(away)
(44) V
V
[+P(ath)]
i
P

mangiare DP P
P
[+P(ath)]
i
X
(

via)
To conclude, it is worth emphasizing that Hale & Keysers (1998, 2002) syn-
tactic theory of argument structure has allowed us to provide an abstract single
explanation for the crosslinguistic contrasts in (36) to (39) and (42), which in-
volve apparently different lexical-semantic classes (i.e., removal and consump-
tion).
5. Concluding remarks
Verb-particle constructions are not a quirk of Italian but can also be found
in other Romance languages (see also Iacobini [2009] and Mateu & Rigau
[2009]). We agree with Iacobinis (2009: 25) claim that [Italian] differs from
the other major Romance languages not in the type of structures used to express
motion events but in their frequency and usage. However, there is an important
methodological difference between Iacobini and Masinis (2007) Construction
Grammar approach and our l-syntactic account: frequency and usage are not
essential factors that guide our present structural account. In other words, what
is crucial for us is not the actuation fact that phrasal verbs are more frequent
and productive in Italian (dialects) rather than in other Romance languages, but
the competence fact that Romance phrasal verbs are not of the Germanic Co-
event type: Romance lacks the Germanic type that involves a pure (i.e., non-
directional) manner verb plus a non-adjunct Path (e.g., see [3]; cf. [28]). Quite
importantly, we argue that this systematicity is not due to frequency, usage or
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
266 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
other performance factors but to a structural one, which is captured by our l-
syntactic reformulation of Talmys (1991, 2000) typology of motion events. We
therefore conclude that it is mistaken to claim that Romance verb-particle con-
structions are exceptional with respect to Talmys (1985, 1991, 2000) typolog-
ical classication of Romance languages (pace Masini [2005, 2006], Iacobini
[2009] and Masini & Iacobini [2007]).
Following Haugens (2009) syntactic distinction between Incorporation and
Conation, we have drawn some relevant syntactic differences between Ro-
mance and Germanic verb-particle constructions: phrasal verbs of the Talmian
Co-event type have been argued to involve Conation (i.e., Compounding).
Following syntactic approaches to so-called Manner Conation (cf. Mateu &
Rigau 2002; McIntyre 2004; Harley 2005; Zubizarreta & Oh 2007), phrasal
verbs like those in (3) and (28) are expected to be possible in Germanic but not
in Romance; only those ones formed via P(ath) Incorporation (i.e., Copy) are
expected to be possible in Romance.
To conclude, we hope that the present abstract (i.e., non-surface-based) l-
syntactic explanations will contribute to show the usefulness of syntactic ap-
proaches to argument structure (see Hale & Keyser 2002; Ramchand & Sveno-
nius 2002; Folli & Ramchand 2005; Harley 2005; Zubizarreta & Oh 2007;
Mateu & Rigau 2002, among others).
Universitat Autnoma de Barcelona (Centre de Lingstica Terica)
jaume.mateu@uab.cat
gemma.rigau@uab.cat
References
Acedo-Matelln, Vctor & Jaume Mateu. 2008. The path from satellite-framed Indoeuropean to
verb-framed Romance: A lexical-syntactic account. Paper presented at X Diachronic Gener-
ative Syntax Conference, Cornell University, 78 August.
Amenta, Luisa. 2008. Esistono i verbi sintagmatici nel dialetto e nellitaliano di Sicilia? In Monica
Cini (ed.), I verbi sintagmatici in italiano e nelle variet dialettali. Stato dellarte e prospet-
tive di ricerca, 159174. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.
Baker, Mark. 1988. Incorporation: A theory of grammatical function changing. Chicago: Univer-
sity of Chicago Press.
Beavers, John, Tham Shiao Wei & Beth Levin. 2009. The typology of motion expressions revisited.
To appear in Journal of linguistics 46. 331377.
Beninc, Paola & Cecilia Poletto. 2006. Phrasal verbs in Venetan and regional Italian. In Frans
Hinskens (ed.), Language variation European perspectives, 922. Amsterdam: John Ben-
jamins.
Borer, Hagit. 2005. Structuring sense II: The normal course of events. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Burnett, Heather & Mireille Tremblay. 2007. Variable-behavior Ps in Old French. Paper presented
at Going Romance, Amsterdam.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 267
Burnett, Heather & Mireille Tremblay. 2009. Variable-behavior Ps and the location of PATHin Old
French. In Enoch O. Aboh, Elisabeth van der Linden, Josep Quer & Petra Sleeman (eds.),
Romance languages and linguistic theory 2007, 2550. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John
Benjamins.
Calvo Rigaul, Cesreo. 2008. I verbi sintagmatici italiani, con appunti contrastivi con lo spagnolo
e il catalano. In Carmen Gonzlez Royo & Pedro Mogorrn Huerta (eds.), Estudios y anli-
sis de fraseologa contrastiva: lexicografa y traduccin, 4766. Alicante: Universidad de
Alicante.
Cini, Monica. (ed.). 2008. I verbi sintagmatici in italiano e nelle variet dialettali. Stato dellarte
e prospettive di ricerca. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.
Clark, Eve V. & Herbert H. Clark. 1979. When nouns surface as verbs. Language 55. 767810.
Deh, Nicole, Ray Jackendoff, Andrew McIntyre & Silke Urban (eds.). 2002. Verb-particle explo-
rations. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Dikken, Marcel den. 1995. Particles. On the syntax of verb-particle, triadic, and causative con-
structions. Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press.
Dikken, Marcel den. 2003. When particles wont part. Ms. CUNY.
Dikken, Marcel den. 2006. On the functional structure of locative and directional PPs. Ms., CUNY
Graduate Center (To appear in Guglielmo Cinque and Luigi Rizzi (eds.), The cartography of
prepositional phrases. Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press.)
Folli, Raffaella. 2002. Constructing telicity in English and Italian. Oxford: University of Oxford
dissertation.
Folli, Raffaella & Heidi Harley. 2005. Flavors of v: Consuming results in Italian and English. In
Paula Kempchinksy & Roumyana Slabakova (eds.), Aspectual inquiries, 95120. Dordrecht:
Springer.
Folli, Raffaella & Gillian Ramchand. 2005. Prepositions and results in Italian and English: An
analysis from event decomposition. In Henk Verkuyl, Henriette De Swart & Angeliek Van
Hout (eds.), Perspectives on aspect, 81105. Dordrecht: Springer.
Goldberg, Adele. 1995. Constructions. A construction grammar approach to argument structure.
Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Grcia, Llusa. 1988. Pi lo mandi gi, pi ti tira su: costruzioni italiane con particella e predicati
complessi. ATI Journal 53. 3151.
Hale, Kenneth L. & Samuel Jay Keyser. 1993. On argument structure and the lexical expression of
syntactic relations. In Kenneth L. Hale & Samuel Jay Keyser (eds.), The view from building
20: Essays in linguistics in honor of Sylvain Bromberger, 53109. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Hale, Kenneth L. & Samuel Jay Keyser. 1997. On the complex nature of simple predicators. In
lex Alsina, Joan Bresnan & Peter Sells (eds.), Complex predicates, 2965. Stanford, CA:
CSLI Publications.
Hale, Kenneth L. & Samuel Jay Keyser. 1998. The basic elements of argument structure. Ms. MIT.
Downloadable at: http://web.mit.edu/linguistics/events/tributes/hale/index.html
Hale, Hale, Kenneth L. & Samuel Jay Keyser. 2000. Aspect and the syntax of argument structure.
Ms. MIT. Downloadable at: http://web.mit.edu/linguistics/events/tributes/hale/index.html
Hale, Kenneth L. & Samuel Jay Keyser. 2002. Prolegomenon to a theory of argument structure.
Cambridge. MA: MIT Press.
Halle, Morris & Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed morphology and the pieces of inection. In Ken-
neth L. Hale & Samuel Jay Keyser (eds.), The view from building 20: Essays in linguistics in
honor of Sylvain Bromberger, 111176. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Harley, Heidi. 2005. How do verbs get their names? Denominal verbs, manner incorporation, and
the ontology of verb roots in English. In Nomi Erteschik-Shir & Tova Rapoport (eds.), The
syntax of aspect. Deriving thematic and aspectual interpretation, 4264. Oxford & New
York: Oxford University Press.
Haugen, Jason D. 2009. Hyponymous objects and late insertion. Lingua 119. 242262.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
268 Jaume Mateu and Gemma Rigau
Hoekstra, Teun. 1988. Small clause results. Lingua 74. 101139.
Horrocks, Geoffrey & Melita Stavrou. 2007. Grammaticalized aspect and spatio-temporal culmi-
nation. Lingua 117. 605644.
Iacobini, Claudio. 2009. The role of dialects in the emergence of Italian phrasal verbs. Morphology
19. 1544.
Iacobini, Claudio & Francesca Masini. 2007. The emergence of verb-particle constructions in Ital-
ian: locative and actional meanings. Morphology 16. 155188.
Johnson, Kyle. 1991. Object positions. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 9. 577636.
Kayne, Richard S. 1985. Principles of particle constructions. In Jacqueline Guron, Hans-Georg
Obenauer & Jean-Yves Pollock (eds.), Grammatical representation, 101140. Dordrecht:
Foris.
Koopman, Hilda. 2000. Prepositions, postpositions, circumpositions and particles: The structure of
Dutch PPs. In Hilda Koopman (ed.), The syntax of speciers and heads. 204260. London:
Routledge.
Masini, Francesca. 2005. Multi-word expressions between syntax and the lexicon: The case of
Italian verb-particle constructions. SKY Journal of Linguistics 18. 145173.
Masini, Francesca. 2006. Diacronia dei verbi sintagmatici in Italiano. Archivio Glottologico Ital-
iano 91(1). 67105.
Masini, Francesca. 2008. Verbi sintagmatici e ordine delle parole. In Monica Cini (ed.). I verbi
sintagmatici in italiano e nelle variet dialettali. Statto dellarte e prospettive di ricerca,
83102. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.
Mateu, Jaume. 2002. Argument structure. Relational construal at the syntaxsemantics interface.
Bellaterra: Universitat Autnoma de Barcelona dissertation. Downloadable at http://www.
tesisenxarxa.net/TDX-1021103--173806/
Mateu, Jaume. 2008. On the l-syntax of directionality/resultativity: The case of Germanic preverbs.
In Anna Asbury, Jakub Dotlacil, Berit Gehrke, & Rick Nouwen (eds.), Syntax and Semantics
of Spatial P, 221250. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Mateu, Jaume. 2010. Rening the typology of motion events: A syntactic approach to some corre-
lations between Japanese and Italian and between Chinese and English. Ms. UAB.
Mateu, Jaume & Gemma Rigau. 2002. A minimalist account of conation processes: Parametric
variation at the lexicon-syntax interface. In Artemis Alexiadou (ed.), Theoretical approaches
to universals, 211236. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Mateu, Jaume & Gemma Rigau. 2009. Romance paths as cognate complements: a lexical-syntactic
account. In Pascual Jos Masullo, Erin ORourke & Chia-Hui Huang (eds.), Romance Lin-
guistics 2007, 227242. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
McIntyre, Andrew. 2004. Event paths, conation, argument structure, and VP shells. Linguistics
42(3). 523571.
McIntyre, Andrew. 2007. Particle verbs and argument structure. Language and Linguistics Com-
pass 1(4). 350367.
Neeleman, Ad. 1994. Complex predicates. Utrecht University doctoral dissertation.
Ramchand, Gillian & Peter Svenonius. 2002. The lexical syntax and lexical semantics of the verb-
particle construction. In Line Mikkelsen & Christopher Potts (eds.), Proceedings of WCCFL
21, 387400. Somerville, MA: Casacadilla Press.
Rappaport Hovav, Malka & Beth Levin. 1998. Building verb meanings. In Miriam Butt & William
Geuder (eds.), The projection of arguments. Lexical and compositional factors, 97134. Stan-
ford: CSLI Publications.
Real Academia Espaola. Banco de datos (CORDE) [on line]. Corpus diacrnico del espaol.
http://www.rae.es
Rouget, Franois & Mireille Tremblay. 2003. Variation in 16th century French: a quantitative anal-
ysis of Rabelaisstyle and aesthetics. Paper presented at the 16th international conference on
historical linguistics, Copenhagen, 1115 August.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM
Verb-particle constructions in Romance 269
Simone, Raffaele. 1996. Esistono verbi sintagmatici in italiano? Cuadernos de Filologa Italiana
3. 4761.
Snyder, William. 2001. On the nature of syntactic variation: Evidence from complex predicates
and complex word-formation. Language 77. 324342.
Sorace, Antonella. 2000. Gradients in auxiliary selection with intransitive verbs. Language 76(4).
859890.
Stiebels, Barbara & Dieter Wunderlich. 1994. Morphology feeds syntax: The case of particle verbs.
Linguistics 32. 913968.
Svenonius, Peter. 1996. The verb-particle alternation in the Scandinavian languages. Ms. Univer-
sity of Troms.
Talmy, Leonard. 1985. Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Timothy
Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description (vol. 3), 57149. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Talmy, Leonard. 1991. Path to realization: a typology of event conation. In Laurel A. Sutton,
Christopher Johnson & Ruth Shields (eds.), Proceedings of the 17th Anuual Meeting of the
Berkeley Linguistics Society, 480519. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Talmy, Leonard. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics. Vol. 2: Typology and process in concept
structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vicario, Federico. 1997. I verbi analitici in friulano. Milano: Franco Angeli.
Vinka, Mikael. 1999. On Swedish verb particle constructions. Ms. McGill University.
Zeller, Jochen. 2001. Particle verbs and local domains. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Zubizarreta, Mara Luisa & Eunjeong Oh. 2007. On the syntactic composition of manner and
motion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Brought to you by | University of Illinois Chicago (University of Illinois Chicago)
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 5/9/12 12:32 PM

Anda mungkin juga menyukai