Anda di halaman 1dari 8

The Effects of Store Environment on Shopping

Behaviors: a Critical Review


Shun Yin Lam, City University of Hong Kong
ABSTRACT - This paper reviews previous studies about the store environmental effects on shopping behaviors
with an aim of identifying issues for future research. A conceptual framework which integrates various
environmental effects is first constructed. Using the framework, I analyze previous findings about environmental
effects and posit several propositions for future investigation. These propositions concern the multiple effects of
individual environmental elements/factors, congruence among these elements/factors, congruence between
these elements/factors and a stores merchandise, the moderating role of consumer characteristics, and the
lagged effects of store environment.
[ to cite ]:

Shun Yin Lam (2001) ,"The Effects of Store Environment on Shopping Behaviors: a Critical Review", in NA Advances in Consumer Research Volume 28, eds. Mary C. Gilly and Joan Meyers-Levy, Valdosta, GA :
Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 190-197.

Advances in Consumer Research Volume 28, 2001

Pages 190-197

THE EFFECTS OF STORE ENVIRONMENT ON SHOPPING BEHAVIORS: A CRITICAL REVIEW


Shun Yin Lam, City University of Hong Kong
ABSTRACT This paper reviews previous studies about the store environmental effects on shopping behaviors with an aim of
identifying issues for future research. A conceptual framework which integrates various environmental effects is
first constructed. Using the framework, I analyze previous findings about environmental effects and posit several
propositions for future investigation. These propositions concern the multiple effects of individual environmental
elements/factors, congruence among these elements/factors, congruence between these elements/factors and a
stores merchandise, the moderating role of consumer characteristics, and the lagged effects of store
environment.
Many retailers acknowledge the importance of store environment as a tool for market differentiation (Levy and
Weitz 1995). Store environment, the physical surroundings of a store, is made up of many elements, including
music, lighting, layout, directional signage and human elements, and can also be divided into external
environment and internal environment (that is, exterior and interior of a store). The effects of store environmental
elements could be complex. While many of these elements influence shoppers behavior through their effects on
shoppers emotion, cognition and physiological state, some of these elements could elicit more direct response
from shoppers with very little impact on their thinking, feeling or body comfort . Despite numerous studies on
store environment, their findings are not enough to provide a detailed understanding of the store environmental
effects.
By reviewing previous studies on store environment, this article attempts to identify research issues inadequately
explored or with conflicting findings, and to posit several propositions for future investigation. In this review, I
include studies that report empirical results or discuss (review) empirical results of other studies. I used the
ProQuest Direct database to search for relevant articles, employing concepts such as store environment and
store design, and environmental elements such as music, color and scent, as keywords for the search. The
search mainly covers mjor marketing journals in the 1989-1999 period as the articles published in this period
would reflect the current, more systematic approach to store environment research. The journals selected for
review mainly include the top ten marketing journals based on the survey by Hult, Neese and Bashaw (1997).
These include Journal of Marketing, Journal of Marketing Research, Journal of Consumer Research, Journal of
Retailing, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Marketing Science, Harvard Business Review, Journal
of Business Research, Journal of Advertising, and Journal of Advertising Research. As some articles selected

from these journals also cite other articles from other sources (such as Advances in Consumer Research) or
before the 1989-99 period, I also include in the review some of these articles, considering their contribution to
store environment research. In addition, I also looked into the retail patronage and service quality literature for
relevant articles as physical facility is often cited as a factor affecting patronage and service quality evaluation.
To achieve the reviews objectives, I first construct a framework that integrates the various effects of store
environment, and describe the methodology of store environment research, based on a synthesis of numerous
studies about store environment. With the help of the framework, I summarize and discuss previous findings
about the store environmental effects, identifying "gaps" not well addressed by previous studies. Based on these
gaps, I posit a number of propositions for future research.
AN INTEGRATIVE FRAMEWORK
The framework proposed here takes account of the multiple effects that store environment could have on
shopping behaviors and shopping outcomes (see Figure 1). Store environment may be studied at different levels
of aggregation. At an elementary level, one may examine individual environmental elements, such as music,
noise, color, odor and furnishing. At a more aggregated level, the factor level, one may study these elements as
groups (factors) B for example, the ambient, design and social factors defined by Baker (1986). The ambient
factor refers to background characteristics, such as temperature, lighting, noise, music and ambient scent; the
design factor includes stimuli that exist at the forefront of our awareness, such as architecture, color and
materials; and the social factor refers to social conditions represented by the number, type and behavior of
customers and employees (Baker 1986; Bitner 1992). At the factor level of analysis, researchers manipulate
several elements belonging to the same factor to project a particular store image (Baker, Levy and Grewal 1992;
Baker, Grewal and Parasuraman 1994). For instance, Baker et al. (1992) in their experiment defined a high
image by background classical music and soft lighting, and a low image by foreground music and bright lighting.
At an even more aggregated level of analysis, the global level, researchers use the environments of different
stores as manipulations. Their focal interest is on the relationship between emotions induced by a particular
environment and behaviors in this environment, rather than how the emotions or behaviors are related to the
characteristics of the environment (Donovan and Rossiter 1982; Donovan, Rossiter, Marcoolyn andNesdale
1994).
Store environment could affect shoppers behaviors in several ways (see Figure 1). Certain response of human
being to environment may be conditioned or hard-wired in the human brain. For example, for a store layout in a
racetrack form, shoppers may follow the path defined by the layout with little thought or emotion aroused by the
layout (Levy and Weitz 1997). In the environmental psychology literature, Mehrabian and Russell (1974) showed
that in a variety of settings (schools, hospitals, homes, etc.), the emotions affected by the environment can be
fully described by three states, pleasure, arousal and dominance (PAD). The majority of studies on emotional
response to store environment adopt the PAD paradigm, and provide evidence that shoppers emotional states
can be largely represented by the PAD dimensions (Donovan and Rossiter 1982; Bellizzi and Hite 1992; Babin
and Darden 1995). These studies also show that the emotional response leads to a variety of behaviors and
outcomes, such as how long the shoppers stay and how much money they spend inside a store. Some other
studies use other scales that include some emotion measures (Bellizzi et al. 1983; Crowley 1993). However,
many of these measures are similar to the measures found in the PAD dimensions.
Store environment also influences various stages of shoppers cognitive process inside a store, including
attention, perception, categorization and information processing. For example, it has been shown that perceived
waiting time varies with the valence of music and consumers categorization of a restaurant as a fast food outlet
depends largely on the external appearance of the store (Hui, Chebat and Chebat 1997; Ward, Bitner and Barnes
1992). The influence of store environment on these cognitive stages would subsequently affect evaluations of the
store, its merchandise and service, and hence on the shopping behaviors or outcomes (Hui et al. 1997; Ward et
al. 1992). Furthermore, store environment may influence these evaluations directly by providing consumers with
a peripheral cue or a tangible evidence for assessing the service and merchandise quality of a store, or by
transfer of meanings from the environment (Parasuraman, Zeithaml and Berry 1988; Bitner 1992).
FIGURE 1

AN INTEGRATIVE FRAMEWORK OF STORE ENVIRONMENTAL EFFECTS


While the foregoing discussion is mainly concerned about the immediate effects of store environment, store
environment may also have lagged or carryover effects on shopping behaviors. For example, consumers
impression of store environment can influence their retail patronage decision (store choice or choice of a
shopping area for visit) over a period of time. Store environment can provide shoppers with various kinds of
shopping value (such as convenience in locating products and recreation), and hence shoppers impression of a
stores environment in terms of shopping value that the environment delivers may affect their likelihood of
choosing the store for shopping (Darden, Erdem and Darden 1983; Babin, Darden and Griffin 1994).
Bitner (1992) postulates that environmental conditions, such as noise, temperature and air quality, affect peoples
physiological state (such as comfort) and hence influence whether people stay in or enjoy a particular
environment. Griffitt (1970) demonstrated in a lab setting that attraction and affective feelings towards strangers
are negatively related to the effective temperature of the surroundings. Birren (1997) reported that red color
increased blood pressure and pulse rate of participants in a lab setting.
METHODOLOGY
Baker et al. (1992) describe several methods of testing the effects of store environment: using a prototype store,
asking participants to respond to verbal descriptions of a store, or creating a simulated store environment. Retail
chains, such as the Limited, first develop a prototype store and determine customer acceptance before adopting
the new design throughout the chains. This approach incurs high cost and may not be feasible for small retailers.
An alternative technique is to ask subjects to respond to verbal descriptions of a store. Gardner and Siomkos
(1985) found that such descriptions systematically affect consumer perceptions of physical sensations. However,
Baker et al. (1992) comment that although this technique is good for laboratory testing, it is limited in external
validity because verbal descriptions can be value-laden. Other studies have used slides or videotapes to provide
a simulated store environment. Ecological validity of this simulation method is supported by Bosselman and Craik
(1987), and Bateson and Hui (1992). Furthermore, the method enables researchers to keep all irrelevant cues
constant across subjects. In addition to the foregoing methods, qualitative methodology has also been used by
researchers. For example, using the methods of in-depth interview with shoppers and participant observation,
McGrath (1989) recorded shoppers response to the atmospherics of a gift store.
In terms of setting, store environment research has been done in both laboratories and the field. In a laboratory
setting, subjects may be asked to imagine themselves in hypothetical situations and to respond accordingly, or be
required to respond as they believe others would in these situations. Gardner and Siomkos (1985) found that
assessments of atmosphere effects are not biased by the use of role playing or third party. For studies based on
the laboratory setting, researchers can randomly assign subjects to different treatment conditions and balance
the number of subjects in different treatment conditions. Most field studies do not have these advantages
although they may have higher external validity. The correlation between explanatory variables and the
unbalanced design commonly found in field studies reduce the power of hypothesis testing and hence the
statistical conclusion validity of the findings. For example, Donovan et al. (1994), failed to find conclusive
evidence for the interaction effect between pleasure and arousal on shopping behaviors, and attributed the lack
of strong evidence to the unbalanced design of their field study.
DISCUSSION OF PREVIOUS FINDINGS
Previous findings are summarized in Table 1. Consistent with the integrative framework, I classify the findings into
three categories: elementary level, factor level and global level. By reviewing these findings citically, I identify
several issues which have not been researched into or have not been studied adequately, and also posit several
propositions for future research.
Multiple Effects of Store Environment
As Table 1 shows, previous studies provide evidence at the elementary, factor and global levels that store
environment affects cognition (Findings 2, 3, 7, 8, 10, 17, 19 and 27), emotions (Findings 3, 4, 5, 10, 15 and 21)
and behaviors (Findings 1, 6, 9, 11 and 16). Furthermore, previous research provides support for the mediating

role of cognition and emotions in the relationship between store environment and shopping behaviors (Findings
11, 16, 18, 21 and 27). However, no study has investigated the direct effect of store environment and the
mediating role of physiological states in the relationship between store environment and shopping behaviors.
Also, no study has empirically examined the consequences of meaning transfer from store environment to a
stores merchandise.
With the exception of Donovan et al. (1994), no study has investigated the multiple effects of store environment
simultaneously, and thus our understanding about the unique contribution of each kind of effects is very limited.
Some environmental elements may have multiple impacts on shopping behaviors. Lighting, for example, could
affect visual acuity of objects in display and arousal experienced by shoppers (Areni and Kim 1993). It may also
have a direct effect on consumer movement inside a store as human being may have an innate response
towards contrast between light and dark. In contrast, some elements may primarily affect shopping behaviors
through a particular route B for example, background color may mainly affect shopping behaviors through the
"emotions" route shown in Figure 1. It is worth studying the multiple effects of store environment simultaneously.
This investigation can indicate which routes are particularly important for a particular element or factor, and
hence enable us to differentiate the elements or factors from each other. Thus, the following propositions are
posited.
P1: Individual environmental elements or factors can have multiple effects on shopping behaviors, and these
effects vary from one element/factor to another. In particular, the following propositions about specific elements
and effects would be worth investigating:
P1a: Through the process of meaning transfer, product display affects the cognitions and emotions of shoppers in
a store environment.
P1b: A store layout can affect shoppers behavior inside a store without significant impact on their emotions,
cognitions or physiological state.
P1c: Lighting condition of a store can affect shoppers emotions, attention towards and evaluation of the stores
merchandise, and hence their behaviors inside the store. However, it can also elicit direct response from
shoppers in certain circumstances.
Congruence Issues
The congruence between environmental elements/factors may greatly facilitate consumers' categorization of a
retail outlet. For example, when both the ambient and design conditions project a prestige image, consumers
may readily accept that the outlet is a high-class store. Since categorization and evaluation are often intertwined,
the congruence may thus magnify the effect of these factors on product evaluation (Cohen and Basu 1987;
Grewal and Baker 1994). This magnifying effect may also manifest itself as a positive interaction effect between
the elements/factors on shopping behaviors or the internal response variables - emotions and cognition.
However, only two studies investigated this congruence issue (Baker et al. 1992; Grewal and Baker 1994). They
reported conflicting results (compare Findings 15 and 19, Table 1). In view of the contradictory findings and the
small number of studies conducted, more research would be needed to test the congruence proposition.
P2: If store environmental elements/factors are congruent with each other, their effects on emotions, cognition
and shopping behaviors will be magnified. For example, when both the ambient and design factors project a
prestige image, their resultant impact on shoppers' evaluation of the store and its merchandise will be greater
than the sum of their individual impacts.
TABLE 1
SUMMARY OF PREVIOUS FINDINGS
The congruence between environmental elements/factors and product category has also raised some concerns.
The study by Mitchell, Kahn and Knasko (1994) on ambient scent indicates that the congruence between a
store's merchandise and environment could affect shoppers' information search and processing (refer to Finding

7). While Mitchell et al. focus on the odors of a product category, one may extend their research to other
characteristics of a category. For instance, one may look into the congruence between the image of a product
category and the image of store environment. Some categories, notably apparel, watches and jewelry, may have
specific images associated with them, such as "casual", "formal", "lively" and "prestige". Through careful selection
and combination of certain design elements (music, lighting, decorations, etc.), a store environment may project
an image similar to that of the merchandise. One may study whether this congruence in image substantially
affects consumer response, particularly information search and processing inside a store. Likewise, the effects on
shopping behaviors due to the congruence in shopping value between a product category and the type of store
design could be examined. For example, Floch (1988) found that consumers tend to emphasize the utilitarian
value (such as convenience) when they shop for household and some grocery items, but to focus on hedonic
value (such as fun) when they shop for books, perfumes and fashion. Thus, a grid layout that works well for a
household section may not be suitable for an apparel section in a store. Furthermore, ethnic products (such as
sushi) may be matched by environmental stimuli having the same origin (such as Japanese music). The match in
the cultural dimension may help shoppers retrieve from their memory information about these products, thus
facilitating them to attributes of the store. Thus, response to store environment may make their purchase
decisions.
P3: The congruence between store environment and a store's merchandise affects shoppers' information search
and processing, and hence their shopping behaviors. For example, shoppers' retrieval of purchase needs or
other information about a product category from their memory is facilitated when a store environment matches
the image of the category.
Moderating Role of Consumer Characteristics
Previous studies provide evidence that the effects of store environment on emotions, cognition and behaviors are
moderated by several consumer variables, including age, environmental dispositions, self-regulation and taskorientation, and several situational variables, such as purchase risk and time pressure (Findings 2, 12, 13, 14
and 23). However, consumer differences due to countries' conditions are yet to be examined. Consumers in
different societies may show different preferences regarding environmental elements because these elements
may produce different connotations in different cultures. For example, people in different cultures prefer particular
colors because of the meaning that they attach to these colors. Thus, the effects of store decor or store music on
shopping behaviors may depend on whether the color(s) of the decor matches the prevailing preference in a
culture. In addition, consumers in different countries may adapt to different levels of retail density (Eroglu and
Harrell 1986). Thus, shoppers' sensitivity to a change in retail density and hence their response to the change
may vary across. countries. The moderating role of culture and adaptation level on consumer response to store
environment is to be explored.
P4: The effects of store environment on emotions, cognition and shopping behaviors differ across countries
owing to differences in culture and adaptation level of environmental stimuli.
The service quality literature suggests that store environment (indicated by the tangible dimension of the
SERVQUAL scale) has weak influence on overall service quality assessment and customers' recommendation of
a store to a friend (Findings 25 and 26). Nevertheless, the importance of store environment may actually vary
with consumers' shopping experience with the store. Compared to regular customers, new customers of a store
may rely more heavily on the tangible cues provided by a store environment in evaluating the service and the
merchandise of the store as the new customers may have little knowledge or experience about the other
attributes of the store. Thus, resonse to store environment may differ by shopping experience.
P5: The effects of store environment on service quality assessment and shopping behaviors vary with
consumers' shopping experience with a store.
Immediate and Lagged Effects of Store Environment
Most of previous studies on store environment focus on the immediate effects, particularly on how consumers
react to store environment when they are inside a store. The lagged effects of store environment on patronage
decision are examined in the retail patronage literature. Overall, this literature shows that store environment is a

weak predictor of patronage. However, anecdotal evidence suggests that renovation of a store often leads to
subsequent change in shoppers' evaluation of the store and shopping behaviors (e.g., Discount Store
News, February 21, 1994; Chain Store Age, October 1995). Thus, the magnitude of the lagged effects needs to
be re-examined.
P6: Change of store environment can have substantial lagged effects on shopping behaviors, particularly
consumers' patronage behaviors.
The low level of importance of store environment reported by the patronage literature may be related to the
methodology used by previous patronage studies. These studies typically ask consumers to evaluate store
environment based on their previous shopping experience in the studied stores. As Donovan and Rossiter (1982)
caution that emotional responses by consumers are transient and not always readily recallable, the evaluation
based on recall of shopping experience may have weak predictive validity. Perhaps other methodology could
better unveil the lagged effects of store environment. For example, an intervention analysis based on time-series
behavioral data may capture these effects more precisely.
REFERENCES
Areni, Charles S. and David Mm (1993), "The Influence of In-store Lighting on Consumers' Examination of
Merchandise in a Wine Store,"International Journal of Research in Marketing, 11, 117-125.
Arnold, Stephen J., Tael H. Oum and Douglas J. Tigert (1983), "Determinant Attributes in Retail Patronage:
Seasonal, Temporal, Regional, and International Comparisons," Journal of Marketing Research, 20 (May), 14957.
Babin, Barry J., William R. Darden, and Mitch Griffin (1994), "Work and/or Fun: Measuring Hedonic and Utilitarian
Shopping Value," Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (March), 644-56.
Babin, Barry J. and William R. Darden (1995), "Consumer Self-Regulation in a Retail Environment," Journal of
Retailing, 71 (1), 47-70.
Baker, Julie (1986), "The Role of the Environment in Marketing Services: The Consumer Perspective, " in John A.
Czepeil, Carlole A. Congrarn and James Shanahan (eds.), The Service Challenge: Integratingfor Competitive
Advantage, Chicago, IL: American Marketing Association, 79-84.
Baker, Julie, Dhruv Grewal, and A. Parasuraman (1994), "The Influence of Store Environment on Quality
Inferences and Store Image,"Journal of A cadenty of Marketing Science, 22 (4), 328-39.
Baker, Julie, Michael Levy, and Dhruv Grewal (1992), "An Experimental Approach to Making Retail Store
Environmen- Griffitt, William (1970), "Environmental Effects on Interpertal Decisions," Journal of Retailing, 68 (4),
445-60.
Bellizzi, Joseph, A., Ayn E. Crowley, and Ronald W. Hasty Attraction, (1983), "The Effects of Color in Store
Design," Journal of Retailing, 59 (Spring), 21-45.
Bellizzi, Joseph, A. and Robert E. Hite (1992), "Environment Color, Consumer Feelings, and Purchase
Likelihood," Psychology and Marketing, 9 (September/October), 347-363.
Birren, Faber (1997), The Power of Color: How It Can Reduce Fatigue, Relieve Monotony, Enhance Sexuality,
and More, Secaucus, New Jersey: Carol Publishing Group.
Bitner, Mary J. (1992), "Servicescapes: The Impact of Physical Surroundings on Customer and
Employees," Journal of Marketing, 56 (April), 57-71.
Chain Store Age, October (1995), "Bob's Rolls Out New Signage Package," 71 (10), 90.

Chebat, Jean-Charles, Claire Gelinas-Chebat, and Pierre Filiatrault (1993), "Interactive Effects of Musical and
Visual Cues on Time Perception: An Application to Waiting Lines in Banks," Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77 (3),
995-1020.
Cohen, Joel B. and Kunal Basu (1987), "Alternative Models of Catcgorization: Toward a Contingent Processing
Frame work," Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (March), 455-72.
Darden, William R., Orhan Erdem, and Donna K. Darden (1983), "A Comparison And Test of Three Causal
Models of Patronage Intentions," in William R. Darden and Robert F. Lusch, eds., Patronage Behavior and Retail
Management, New York, NY: North-Holland.
Darden, William R. and Barry J. Babin (1994), "Exploring the Concept of Affective Quality: Expanding the
Concept of Retail Personality,"Journal of Business Research, 29 (February), 101-10.
Discount Store News, February 21 (1994), -HB & C Section Puts on a Good Face," 33 (4), 79.
Donovan, Robert J. and John R. Rossiter (1982), "Store Atmosphere: An Environmental Psychology
Approach," Journal of Retailing, 58 (1), 34-57.
Donovan, Robert J., John R. Rossiter, Gilian Marcoolyn, and Andrew Nesdale (1994), "Store Atmosphere and
Purchasing Behavior."Journal of Retailing, 70 (3), 283-94.
Eroglu, Sevgin and Gilbert D. Harrell (1986), "Retail Crowding: Theoretical and Strategic Implications," Journal of
Retailing, 62 (4), 346-63.
Eroglu, Sevginand Karen A. Machleit (1990), "An Empirical Study of Retail Crowding: Antecedents and
Consequences," Journal of Retailing, 66 (2), 201-21.
Floch, Jean-Marie (1988), "The Contribution of a Structural Semiotics to the Design of a
Hypermarket," International Journal of Research in Marketing, 4 (3), 233-52.
Foxall, Gordon R. and Gordon E. Greenley (1999), "Consumers' Emotional Responses to Service
Environments," Journal of Business Research, 46, 149-58.
Gardner, Meryl P. and George J. Siomkos (1985), "Toward a Methodology for Assessing the Effects of In-Store
Atmo spherics," in Richard Lutz (ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, Chicago, IL: Association for Consumer
Research.
Gentry, James W. and Alvin C. Burns (1977-78), "How 'Important' Are Evaluative Criteria in Shopping Center
Patronage," Journal of Retailing, 4 (Winter), p. 73-85.
Grewal, Dhruv and Julie Baker (1994), "Do Retail Environmen tal Factors Affect Consumers' Price Acceptability?
An Empirical Examination," International Journal of Research in Marketing, 11 (2), 107-115.
Griffitt, William (1970), "Environmental Effects on Interpersonal Affective Behavior: Ambient Temperature and
Attraction," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15 (3), 240-44.
Grossbart, Sanford, Ronald Hampton, B. Rammohan, and Richard S. Lapidus (1990), "Environmental
Dispositions and Customer Response to Store Atmospherics," Journal of Business Research, 21 (3), 225-41.
Hui, Michael K. and John E. G. Bateson (1991), "Perceived Control and the Effects of Crowding and Consumer
Choice on the Service Experiences," Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (September), 175-86.
Hui, Michael K., L. Dube and J. Chebat (1997), "The Impact of Music on Consumers' Reactions to Waiting for
Services," Journal of Retailing, 73 (1), 87-104.

Hult, G. Tomas M., William T. Neese, and R. Edward Bashaw (1997), "Faculty Perceptions of Marketing
Journals," Journal of Marketing Education, 19 (1), 37-52.
Levy, Michael and Barton A. Weitz (1998), Retailing Management, 3rd ed., Chicago: Irwin NcGraw-Hill, Chapter
18, 54069.
Malhotra, Naresh K. (1983), "A Threshold Model of Store Choice," Journal of Retailing, 59 (2), 3-21.
Milliman, Ronald E. (1982), "Using Background Music to Affect the Behavior of Supermarket Shoppers," Journal
of Marketing, 46 (2), 86-91.
Milliman, Ronald E. (1986), "The Influence of Background Music on the Behavior of Restaurant Patrons," Journal
of Consumer Research,13 (September), 286-89.
Mitchell, Deborah J., Barbara E. Kahn, and Susan C. Knasko (1995), "There's Something in the Air: Effects of
Ambient Odor on Consumer Decision Making," Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (September), 229-38.
Parasuraman, A., Leonard L. Berry, and Valerie A. Zeithaml, (1991), "Refinement and Reassessment of the
SERVQUAL Scale," Journal of Retailing, 67 (4), 420-50.
Parasuraman, A., Valerie A. Zeithaml, and Leonard L. Berry (1988), "SERVQUAL: A Multiple-Item Scale for
Measuring Consumer Perceptions of Service Quality," Journal of Retailing, 64 (1), 12-46.
Smith, Patricia and Ross Curnow (1966), "'Arousal Hypothesis' and the Effects of Music on Purchasing
Behavior," Journal ofApplied Psychology, 50 (June), 255-56.
Spangenberg, Eric R., Ayn E. Crowley, and Pamela W. Henderson (1996), "Improving the Store Environment: Do
Olfactory Cues Affect Evaluations and Behaviors," Journal of Marketing, 60 (April), 67-80.
Ward, James C., Mary Jo Bitner, and John Barnes (1992), "Measuring the Prototypicality and Meaning of Retail
Environments," Journal of Retailing, 68 (2), 194-220.
Yalch, Richard F. and Eric Spangenberg (1990), "Effects of Store Music on Shopping Behavior," Journal of
Consumer Marketing, 7(Spring), 55-63.
Yalch, Richard F. and Eric Spangenberg (1993), "Using Store Music for Retail Zoning: A Field Experiment,"
in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 20, Leigh McAlister and Michael L. Rothschild, eds., Provo, UT
Association for Consumer Research, 632-36.
----------------------------------------

Anda mungkin juga menyukai