Anda di halaman 1dari 482

AN ANALYSIS OF THE NATURE AND FUNCTION OF

MENTAL COMPUTATION IN PRIMARY


MATHEMATICS CURRICULA

by

GEOFFREY ROBERT MORGAN


Cert. T., B.Ed.St., B.A., M.Ed. (Primary Mathematics)

A thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor


of Philosophy at the Centre for Mathematics and Science Education,
Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane.

1999

KEYWORDS

Arithmetic, Computation, Computational Estimation, Mathematics, Mathematics


Curriculum, Mathematics Teaching, Mental Arithmetic, Mental Computation,
Mental Strategies, Number, Number Sense, Queensland Educational History,
Queensland Mathematics Syllabuses, Teacher Beliefs and Practices.

ii

ABSTRACT
This study was conducted to analyse aspects of mental computation within
primary school mathematics curricula and to formulate recommendations to inform
future revisions to the Number strand of mathematics syllabuses for primary
schools. The analyses were undertaken from past, contemporary, and futures
perspectives. Although this study had syllabus development in Queensland as a
prime focus, its findings and recommendations have an international applicability.
Little has been documented in relation to the nature and role of mental
computation in mathematics curricula in Australia (McIntosh, Bana, & Farrell, 1995,
p. 2), despite an international resurgence of interest by mathematics educators.
This resurgence has arisen from a recognition that computing mentally remains a
viable computational alternative in a technological age, and that the development of
mental procedures contributes to the formation of powerful mathematical thinking
strategies (R. E. Reys, 1992, p. 63). The emphasis needs to be placed upon the
mental processes involved, and it is this which distinguishes mental computation
from mental arithmetic, as defined in this study. Traditionally, the latter has been
concerned with speed and accuracy rather than with the mental strategies used to
arrive at the correct answers.
In Australia, the place of mental computation in mathematics curricula is only
beginning to be seriously considered. Little attention has been given to teaching, as
opposed to testing, mental computation. Additionally, such attention has
predominantly been confined to those calculations needed to be performed mentally
to enable the efficient use of the conventional written algorithms. Teachers are
inclined to associate mental computation with isolated facts, most commonly the
basic ones, rather than with the interrelationships between numbers and the
methods used to calculate. To enhance the use of mental computation and to
achieve an improvement in performance levels, children need to be encouraged to
value all methods of computation, and to place a priority on mental procedures.
This requires that teachers be encouraged to change the way in which they view

iii

mental computation. An outcome of this study is to provide the background and


recommendations for this to occur.
The mathematics education literature of relevance to mental computation was
analysed, and its nature and function, together with the approaches to teaching,
under each of the Queensland mathematics syllabuses from 1860 to 1997 were
documented. Three distinct time-periods were analysed: 1860-1965, 1966-1987,
and post-1987. The first of these was characterised by syllabuses which included
specific references to calculating mentally. To provide insights into the current
status of mental computation in Queensland primary schools, a survey of a
representative sample of teachers and administrators was undertaken. The
statements in the postal, self-completion opinionnaire were based on data from the
literature review. This study, therefore, has significance for Queensland educational
history, curriculum development, and pedagogy.
The review of mental computation research indicated that the development of
flexible mental strategies is influenced by the order in which mental and written
techniques are introduced. Therefore, the traditional written-mental sequence
needs to be reevaluated. As a contribution to this reevaluation, this study presents
a mental-written sequence for introducing each of the four operations. However,
findings from the survey of Queensland school personnel revealed that a majority
disagreed with the proposition that an emphasis on written algorithms should be
delayed to allow increased attention on mental computation. Hence, for this
sequence to be successfully introduced, much professional debate and
experimentation needs to occur to demonstrate its efficacy to teachers.
Of significance to the development of efficient mental techniques is the way in
which mental computation is taught. R. E. Reys, B. J. Reys, Nohda, and Emori
(1995, p. 305) have suggested that there are two broad approaches to teaching
mental computationa behaviourist approach and a constructivist approach. The
former views mental computation as a basic skill and is considered an essential
prerequisite to written computation, with proficiency gained through direct teaching.
In contrast, the constructivist approach contends that mental computation is a
process of higher-order thinking in which the act of generating and applying mental
strategies is significant for an individual's mathematical development. Nonetheless,
this study has concluded that there may be a place for the direct teaching of
selected mental strategies. To support syllabus development, a sequence of mental

iv

strategies appropriate for focussed teaching for each of the four operations has
been delineated.
The implications for teachers with respect to these recommendations are
discussed. Their implementation has the potential to severely threaten many
teachers sense of efficacy. To support the changed approach to developing
competence with mental computation, aspects requiring further theoretical and
empirical investigation are also outlined.

TABLE OF CONTENTS

KEYWORDS................................................................................................................ i
ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................ ii
TABLE OF CONTENTS ............................................................................................. v
LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................xii
LIST OF FIGURES ..................................................................................................xiv
ABBREVIATIONS..................................................................................................... xv
STATEMENT OF ORIGINAL AUTHORSHIP ..........................................................xvi
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS .........................................................................................xvii

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY

1.1

Orientation of the Study .................................................................................... 1

1.2

Context of the Study.......................................................................................... 4


1.2.1

Mental Computation: Overview ........................................................... 4

1.2.2

Mental Computation: Reasons For The Resurgence Of Interest ........ 8

1.2.3

Mental Computation: Place In Current Mathematics Curricula ......... 12

1.2.4

Mental Computation: Student Performance ...................................... 13

1.2.5

Mental Computation: Essential Changes In Outlook......................... 16

1.2.6

Mental Computation: Needed Research ........................................... 18

1.3

Purposes and Significance of the Study ......................................................... 19

1.4

Overview of the Study ..................................................................................... 21


1.4.1

Method And Justification ................................................................... 22

1.4.2

Chapter Guidelines ........................................................................... 23

CHAPTER 2

MENTAL COMPUTATION

2.1

Introduction ..................................................................................................... 27

2.2

Research Questions........................................................................................ 29

2.3

Recent Developments in Mathematics Education of Relevance to


Mental Computation ........................................................................................ 30
2.3.1

Numeracy .......................................................................................... 31

2.3.2

Computation ...................................................................................... 32

vi

2.3.3

Number Sense .................................................................................. 34

2.3.4

Learning Mathematics....................................................................... 35

2.4

The Calculative Process ................................................................................. 37

2.5

The Nature of Mental Computation ................................................................. 40

2.6

2.5.1

Mental Computation Defined............................................................. 41

2.5.2

Mental and Oral Arithmetic................................................................ 42

2.5.3

Mental Computation and Folk Mathematics...................................... 44

2.5.4

Characteristics of Mental Procedures ............................................... 48

Mental Computation and Computational Estimation ....................................... 53


2.6.1

Components of Computational Estimation........................................ 54

2.6.2

Computational Estimation Processes ............................................... 56

2.6.3

Comparison of Mental Computation and Computational


Estimation ......................................................................................... 60

2.7

Components of Mental Computation............................................................... 63


2.7.1

Affective Components ....................................................................... 67

2.7.2

Conceptual Components................................................................... 68

2.7.3

Related Concepts and Skills ............................................................. 69

2.7.4

Strategies for Computing Mentally .................................................... 74


Models for Classifying Mental Strategies ..................................... 77
Counting strategies....................................................................... 84
Strategies Based Upon Instrumental Understanding ................... 88
Heuristic Strategies Based Upon Relational Understanding ........ 92

2.7.5

Short-term and Long-term Memory Components of Mental


Computation.................................................................................... 107

2.8

2.9

Characteristics of Proficient Mental Calculators ........................................... 112


2.8.1

Origins of the Ability to Compute Mentally ...................................... 114

2.8.2

Memory for Numerical Equivalents ................................................. 117

2.8.3

Memory for Interrupted Working ..................................................... 118

2.8.4

Memory for Calculative Method ...................................................... 120

Developing the Ability to Compute Mentally.................................................. 123


2.9.1

Approaches to Developing Skill with Mental Computation.............. 125


Traditional Approach .................................................................. 125
Alternative Approaches .............................................................. 127

2.9.2

General Pedagogical Issues ........................................................... 131

vii

2.9.3

Sequence for Introducing Computational Methods ......................... 134

2.9.4

Assessing Mental Computation....................................................... 137

2.10 Summary and Implications for Mental Computation Curricula ...................... 139
2.11 Concluding Points ......................................................................................... 150

CHAPTER 3:
3.1

MENTAL COMPUTATION IN QUEENSLAND: 1860-1965

Introduction ................................................................................................... 152


3.1.1

Method ............................................................................................ 153


Sources of Evidence................................................................... 153
Research Questions ................................................................... 155
Structure of Analysis................................................................... 156

3.2

Selected Background Issues Related to Syllabus Development and


Implementation.............................................................................................. 157
3.2.1

Focus of Syllabus Development and Implementation ..................... 158

3.2.2

Principles Underlying the Syllabuses from 1905 ............................. 163

3.2.3

Syllabus Interpretation and Overloading ......................................... 167

3.2.4

Summary of Background Issues ..................................................... 177

3.3

Terms Associated with the Calculation of Exact Answers Mentally .............. 178

3.4

Roles Ascribed to Mental Arithmetic ............................................................. 183

3.5

3.4.1

Mental Arithmetic as a Pedagogical Tool ........................................ 185

3.4.2

The Social Usefulness of Mental Arithmetic ................................... 190

3.4.3

Mental Discipline and Mental Arithmetic ......................................... 192

The Nature of Mental Arithmetic ................................................................... 198


3.5.1

Interpretations of Mental Arithmetic................................................. 199

3.5.2

The Syllabuses and Mental Arithmetic ............................................ 202

3.5.3

Mental Arithmetic as Implemented .................................................. 216

3.6

Recommended Approaches to Teaching Mental Arithmetic ......................... 231

3.7

Conclusions and Summary ........................................................................... 249

viii

CHAPTER 4
4.1

MENTAL COMPUTATION IN QUEENSLAND: 1966-1997

Introduction ................................................................................................... 250


4.1.1

Background to Research Strategy .................................................. 251

4.1.2

Research Focus ............................................................................. 251

4.2

The Syllabuses and Mental Computation in Queensland: 1966-1987 .......... 252

4.3

Survey of Queensland Primary School Personnel ........................................ 256


4.3.1

Survey Method ................................................................................ 258


Research Questions ................................................................... 258
Instrument Used ......................................................................... 259
Sample ....................................................................................... 262
Research Procedure................................................................... 265
Methods of Analysis ................................................................... 267

4.3.2 Survey Results ...................................................................................... 272


Response Rate........................................................................... 272
Analysis of Nonresponse............................................................ 275
Beliefs About Mental Computation and How It Should Be
Taught ........................................................................................ 279
Current Teaching Practices ........................................................ 287
Past Teaching Practices............................................................. 291
Inservice on Mental Computation ............................................... 398
Textbooks Used to Develop Skill with Mental Computation ....... 299
4.3.3

Discussion....................................................................................... 302
Limitations of Findings................................................................ 303
Conclusions ................................................................................ 303
Concluding Points....................................................................... 317

4.4

Mental Computation in Queensland: Recent Initiatives ................................ 317


4.4.1

Student Performance Standards and Mental Computation............. 321

4.4.2

Number Development Continuum and Mental Computation........... 325

4.4.3

Implications for Mental Computation Curricula ............................... 325

ix

CHAPTER 5

MENTAL COMPUTATION: A PROPOSED SYLLABUS


COMPONENT

5.1

5.2

Introduction ................................................................................................... 327


5.1.1

Context and Focus for Change ....................................................... 330

5.1.2

Framework for Syllabus Development ............................................ 332

Mental, Calculator, and Written Computation ............................................... 334


5.2.1

Traditional Sequence for Introducing Mental, Calculator, and


Written Computation........................................................................ 334

5.2.2

A Sequential Framework for Mental, Calculator, and


Written Computation........................................................................ 337

5.3

Mental Strategies: A syllabus Component .................................................... 342


5.3.1

Background Issues.......................................................................... 343

5.3.2

Developmental Issues ..................................................................... 345

5.3.3

Mental Strategies for Addition, Subtraction, Multiplication, and


Division............................................................................................ 348

5.4

Concluding Points ......................................................................................... 354

CHAPTER 6:

MENTAL COMPUTATION IN QUEENSLAND: CONCLUSIONS


AND IMPLICATIONS

6.1

Restatement of Background and Purpose of Study ...................................... 356

6.2

Mental Computation: Conclusions ................................................................ 358

6.3

6.2.1

The Emphasis Placed on Mental Computation. .............................. 359

6.2.2

Roles of Mental Computation .......................................................... 361

6.2.3

The Nature of Mental Computation ................................................. 364

6.2.4

Approaches to Teaching Mental Computation ................................ 366

Implications for Decision Making Concerning Syllabus Revision.................. 369


6.3.1

Fostering Debate about Computation ............................................. 369

6.4 Recommendations for Further Research ....................................................... 373

REFERENCES ....................................................................................................... 376

APPENDIX ASUMMARY OF MENTAL ARITHMETIC IN QUEENSLAND


MATHEMATICS SCHEDULES AND SYLLABUSES (1860-1964)

A.1

1860 Schedule ......................................................................................... 419

A.2

1876 Schedule ......................................................................................... 419

A.3

1891 Schedule ......................................................................................... 420

A.4

1894 Schedule ......................................................................................... 422

A.5

1897 Schedule ......................................................................................... 423

A.6

1902 Schedule ......................................................................................... 425

A.7

1904 Schedule ......................................................................................... 426

A.8

1914 Syllabus........................................................................................... 427

A.9

1930 Syllabus........................................................................................... 428

A.10

1938 Amendments ................................................................................... 432

A.11

1948 Amendments ................................................................................... 434

A.12

1952 Syllabus........................................................................................... 436

A.13

1964 Syllabus........................................................................................... 441

APPENDIX BADDITIONAL NOTES: CHAPTER 3................................................. 450

APPENDIX C

SELF-COMPLETION QUESTIONNAIRE ................................... 456

Section 1

Beliefs About Mental Computation and How It Should Be Taught ... 457

Section 2

Current Teaching Practices .............................................................. 459

Section 3

Past Teaching Practices ................................................................... 461

Section 4

Background Information.................................................................... 464

APPENDIX D

SURVEY CORRESPONDENCE

D.1

Initial Letter: One-teacher Schools ........................................................... 466

D.2

Initial Letter: to All Schools Except One-teacher Schools ........................ 468

D.3

Letter to Contact Persons Accompanying Questionnaires....................... 471

D.4

Initial Follow-up Letter to Principals of Schools Not Replying to Original


Letter ........................................................................................................ 472

D.5

Second Follow-up Letter to Schools Requesting Questionnaires From


Which Completed Forms Had Not Been Received .................................. 473

APPENDIX EMEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF


SURVEY ITEMS IN FIGURES 4.1-4.6 ....................................... 474

xi

LIST OF TABLES

2.1

Components of Mental Computation ............................................................ 65

2.2

Counting Strategies ..................................................................................... 86

2.3

Strategies Based Upon Instrumental Understanding ................................... 89

2.4

Heuristic Strategies Based Upon Relational Understanding ....................... 94

3.1

Queensland Mathematics Schedules and Syllabuses: 1860-1965............. 161

3.2

Selection of Textbooks Relevant to Mental Arithmetic Available to


Queensland Teachers From the Mid-1920s ............................................... 170

3.3

Extract From Recommended Mental Arithmetic Exercise for "Middle


Standards" for Use by Teachers of Multiple Classes.................................. 227

3.4

Examples of Written Items from the 1925 Mathematics Scholarship


Paper Given to Fifth Class Children as Mental ........................................... 230

4.1

Queensland Mathematics Schedules and Syllabuses: 1965-1987............. 254

4.2

Sample of Schools by Band Within Educational Regions........................... 263

4.3

Schools Returning Questionnaires ............................................................. 273

4.4

School Response Rate by Region and Band.............................................. 274

4.5

Analysis of Number of Questionnaires Returned........................................ 275

4.6

Questionnaire Response Rate by Region and Band .................................. 276

4.7

Items for Which Significant Differences in Response were Observed, Based


on Time of Receipt...................................................................................... 278

4.8

Percentage of Responses Related to Beliefs About the Importance of Mental


Computation ............................................................................................... 280

4.9

Percentage of Responses Related to Beliefs About the Nature of Mental


Computation ............................................................................................... 281

4.10

Percentage of Responses Related to Beliefs About the General Approach to


Teaching Mental Computation .................................................................... 283

xii

4.11

Percentage of Responses Related to Beliefs About Issues Associated with


Developing the Ability to Calculate Exact Answers Mentally ...................... 285

4.12 .. Percentage of Responses Related to Current Teaching Practices for


Developing the Ability to Compute Mentally ............................................... 288
4.13

Percentage of Responses Related to Past Beliefs About Mental Computation293

4.14

Percentage of Responses Concerning Past Teaching Practices Related to


Mental Computation.................................................................................... 294

4.15

Percentage of Responses Related to the Importance of and Participation in


Inservice Sessions on Mental Computation ............................................... 299

4.16

Source of Inservice on Mental Computation During Period 1991-1993...... 299

4.17

Categorisation of Resources Listed by Respondents in Sections 2.2 and 3.3


of the Survey Instrument ............................................................................ 301

4.18

Textbooks Specific to Mental Computation Currently Used by Middle and


Upper School Teachers .............................................................................. 301

4.19

Textbooks Specific to Mental Computation Used During the Period 19641987............................................................................................................ 302

5.1

Traditional Sequence for Introducing the Four Operations with Whole


Numbers as Presented in the Mathematics Sourcebooks for Queensland
Schools ....................................................................................................... 336

5.2

Revised Sequential Framework for Introducing Mental, Calculator and


Written Procedures for Addition, Subtraction, Multiplication, and Division . 340

5.3

Mental Strategies Component for Addition of Whole Numbers Beyond the


Basic Facts for Inclusion in the Number Strand of Future Mathematics
Syllabuses for Primary Schools .................................................................. 350

5.4

Mental Strategies Component for Subtraction of whole numbers Beyond the


basic facts for Inclusion in the Number Strand of Future Mathematics
Syllabuses for Primary Schools .................................................................. 351

5.5

Mental Strategies Component for Multiplication and Division of Whole


Numbers Beyond the Basic Facts for Inclusion in the Number Strand of
Future Mathematics Syllabuses for Primary Schools ................................. 353

xiii

LIST OF FIGURES
2.1

A model of the calculative process highlighting the central position of mental


calculation..................................................................................................... 39

2.2

Components of computational estimation .................................................... 55

2.3

A view of memory processes for computing mentally................................. 108

2.4

Traditional sequence for introducing computational procedures for each


operation..................................................................................................... 135

2.5

An alternative sequence for introducing computational procedures for each


operation..................................................................................................... 136

4.1

Position of means for items relating to the beliefs about the nature of mental
computation an a traditional-nontraditional continuum ............................... 282

4.2

Position of means for items relating to beliefs about the general approach to
teaching mental computation an a traditional-nontraditional continuum ..... 284

4.3

Position of means for items relating to beliefs about specific issues


associated with developing mental computation skills an a traditionalnontraditional continuum............................................................................. 287

4.4

Position of means for selected current teaching practices related to


developing mental computation skills on a traditional-nontraditional
continuum ................................................................................................... 291

4.5

Position of means for items relating to teaching practices used during the
periods 1964-1968, 1969-1974, 1975-1987 on traditional-nontraditional
continua. ..................................................................................................... 297

4.6

Means for selected teaching practices and the beliefs which underpin them
for middle and upper school teachers......................................................... 312

5.1

A conceptualisation of syllabus development to provide a focus on student


learning ....................................................................................................... 336

xiv

ABBREVIATIONS
AAMT

Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers

AEC

Australian Education Council

CDC

Curriculum Development Centre

MSEB

Mathematics Sciences Education Board

NCSM

National Council of Supervisors of Mathematics

NCTM

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics

NCMWG

National Curriculum: Mathematics Working Group

NRC

National Research Council

QSCO

Queensland School Curriculum Office

xv

STATEMENT OF ORIGINAL AUTHORSHIP


The work contained in this thesis has not been previously submitted for a degree or
diploma at any other higher education institution. To the best of my knowledge and
belief, the thesis contains no material previously published or written by another
person except where due reference is made.

Signed: G. R. Morgan

Date:

12 January 1998

xvi

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study would not have been completed without the advice, support and cooperation of a number of people towards whom I wish to formally express my
appreciation. Principal among these are:

Dr Calvin Irons, Senior Lecturer, Queensland University of Technology,


whose critical comments, advice and support as my supervisor were
invaluable at each stage of this study's development.

Associate Professor Tom Cooper, Head, Mathematics, Science and


Technology, Queensland University of Technology, who, as my assistant
supervisor, provided constructive criticisms and direction at various stages
of the project.

My wife, Lena, and daughter, Fiona, whose understanding and support


created an environment conducive to completing the task.

Appreciation is also extended to:

Mr Greg Logan, Ms Rosemary Mammino, and Mr Lex Brasher, History Unit,


Queensland Department of Education, for their guidance and assistance in
gathering the sources of primary data for the analysis of mental
computation in Queensland mathematics curricula.

Dr Shirley O'Neill and Mr Barry Tainton, Research and Evaluation Unit,


Department of Education, for their providing the information on which to
form the sample of Queensland state primary schools.

The staff of the Centre for Mathematics and Science Education,


Queensland University of Technology, particularly for their assistance with
the distribution of the questionnaires.

The teachers and administrators who returned completed questionnaires,


and particularly to those staff members of the Lawnton State School who
contributed to the questionnaire's development.

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY

1.1

Orientation of the Study


Several factorsgrowth of technology, increased applications, impact of
computers, and expansion of mathematics itselfhave combined in the past
quarter century to extend greatly both the scope and the application of the
mathematical sciences. Together, these forces have created a revolution in
the nature and role of mathematicsa revolution that must be reflected
in...schools if...students are to be well prepared for tomorrow's world. (National
Research Council [NRC], 1989, p. 4)

In responding to this revolution in the nature and role of mathematics, the


National Council of Supervisors of Mathematics (1989, pp. 45-46) delineates twelve
interrelated areas that it considers critical to the development of children's
mathematical competences essential for meeting the demands of the twenty-first
century. These are: problem solving, communicating mathematical ideas,
mathematical reasoning, applying mathematics to everyday situations, alertness to
the reasonableness of results, estimation, appropriate computational skills (including
mental, written, and technological procedures), algebraic thinking, measurement,
geometry, statistics, and probability. In concert with these competences, A National
Statement on Mathematics for Australian Schools (Australian Education Council
[AEC], 1991, pp. 11-13), suggests that the goals for learning mathematics involve
students in (a) developing confidence and competence in dealing with commonly
occurring situations, (b) developing positive attitudes towards their involvement in
mathematics, (c) developing their capacity to use mathematics in solving problems
individually and collaboratively, (d) learning to communicate mathematically, and (e)
learning techniques and tools which reflect modern mathematics.

These beliefs imply that computational skill per se can no longer be considered
an adequate measure of achievement in mathematics. Nonetheless, computational
competence remains an important goal of mathematics programs in primary
classrooms. This goal, however, involves more than the routine application of
memorised rules. It involves children in developing:

An expertise in problem solving and higher-order thinking.

A sound understanding of mathematical principles.

An ability to know when and how to use a variety of procedures for


calculating.

(National Council of Supervisors of Mathematics [NCSM], 1989, p. 44)

Such development is consistent with Williss (1995) advocacy for a curriculum that
reflects the learning of mathematics which is significant and of value for an
individual's success in both private and professional endeavours.
The ability to calculate exact as well as approximate answers mentally is
essential to the repertoire of skills for computational competence in the 1990s and
beyond (AEC, 1991, p. 109). However, the development of an ability to arrive at
exact answers mentally without the aid of external calculating or recording
devicesmental computation (R. E. Reys, B. J. Reys, Nohda, & Emori, 1995, p.
304)is one that has generally been neglected, or at least de-emphasised, in
classrooms during recent years, both in Australia and overseas (Koenker, 1961, p.
295; McIntosh, 1990a, p. 25; Shibata, 1994, p. 17; Trafton, 1978, p. 199; Wiebe,
1987, p. 57). French (1987) suggests that "one reason for the lack of interest [in
mental computation] is the association that [this] has with the daily mental tests
once used universally in schools, with their emphasis on recall of facts and speed"
(p. 39). This emphasis characterised the mental arithmetic programs that were
regularly conducted in classrooms as precursors to the main focus of arithmetic
lessons: the development of the standard written algorithms for the four basic
operations.
Given that it is essential that the development of an ability to calculate exact
answers mentally gains greater prominence in classroom mathematics programs
(Gough, 1993, p.2) and that little research relevant to its development has been

undertaken (McIntosh, Bana, & Farrell, 1995, p. 2; B. J. Reys, 1991, p. 1; R. E. Reys


et al., 1995, p. 324), the aims of this study were:

To analyse key aspects of mental computation within primary school


curricula from past, contemporary and futures perspectives.

To formulate recommendations concerning mental computation to inform


future revisions to the Number strand of the mathematics syllabus for
Queensland primary schools.

In planning for and guiding the implementation of suggested changes to the


nature of school mathematics, cognisance needs to be given to the nature of past
mathematics curricula, as well as those of the present. As R. E. Reys et al. (1995)
suggest: "In order to get where we want to be, it is essential to know where we are"
(p. 324); integral to which is knowing where we have been (Skager & Weinberg,
1971, p. 50). Hence, a significant aspect of this study is the analysis of the nature
and function of mental computation in past syllabuses, particularly from a
Queensland perspective. A focus such as this can assist mathematics educators to
(a) understand educational movements (their "why" and "how, their relevance to the
period in which they received prominence, and their relevance to current problems);
and (b) analyse suggested innovations to determine whether the proposals are likely
to be successful in meeting current and future needs (Best & Kahn, 1986, pp. 6162).
To complement the data from the analysis of past syllabuses (Chapter 3), a
survey of Queensland state primary school teachers' and administrators' attitudes
and teaching practices related to mental computation has been undertaken (Chapter
4). This has enabled the linking of the literature review (Chapter 2) and the
historical information to the present situation in Queensland primary classrooms,
thus providing a comprehensive summary of the state of knowledge about mental
computation, particularly from a Queensland perspective. This summary has
provided the basis for the recommendations concerning the ways in which mental
computation may be explicitly included in the Number strands of future mathematics
syllabuses (Chapter 5).

1.2

Context of the Study


To provide an understanding of the context in which this investigation has

occurred, it is necessary to give consideration to (a) the nature and role of mental
computation in past mathematics curricula; (b) the reasons for the contemporary
resurgence of interest in mental computation; (c) its place within current
mathematics programs; (d) the degree to which students show proficiency with
calculating exact answers mentally; (e) the essential changes to the ways in which
mental computation is viewed by teachers and students, changes regarded as
critical for mental computation to fulfil the roles for which it is envisaged; and (f)
issues related to mental computation in need of further clarification.

1.2.1 Mental Computation: Overview


As intimated above, "the teaching of mathematics is shifting from a
preoccupation with inculcating routine skills to developing broad-based
mathematical power" (NRC, 1989, p. 82). A key element in the repertoire of skills
that underpins the development of mathematical power is the ability to compute
exact answers mentally. In endeavouring to create curricula and learning
environments conducive to ensuring that children gain power over the mathematics
they use, an understanding of the historical context is critical to informed debate and
the decision-making process.
Little has been documented in relation to the nature and role of mental
computation in mathematics curricula in Australia (McIntosh et al., 1995, p. 2).
However, the importance placed on it by teachers and students is a function of
factors which include the availability of particular tools for calculating, the prevailing
psychological theory, and the objectives for teaching arithmetic during a particular
period (Atweh, 1982, p. 53).
With respect to the United States of America, the place of mental computation
in mathematics curricula has a "long and sporadic history" (B. J. Reys, 1985, p. 43).
The emphasis placed on mental computation has fluctuated with the prevailing
psychological and pedagogical theories during any given period. In contrast, mental
computation has received a continuing emphasis in Soviet (Russian) elementary
schools (Menchinskaya & Moro, 1975, p. 73) and in Japanese schools, particularly

prior to the introduction of a new mathematics curriculum in 1989 (Shibata, 1994, p.


17), albeit for different reasons. In the Soviet Union mental computation has been
viewed as a means for deepening mathematical knowledge (Menchinskaya & Moro,
1975, p. 74), while in Japan the focus has been on the utility it provides for day-today calculations (Shibata, 1994, p. 17).
Mental computation first gained prominence during the mid-nineteenth century
in formal mathematics curricula as part of a reaction to the perceived slowness with
which students carried out written calculations. Following Pestalozzi's work in
Europe, Warren Colburn, in the United States, encouraged an emphasis on oral
arithmetic in which problems were orally stated and computed mentally "as a protest
against the intellectual sluggishness, lack of reasoning, and slowness of operation
of the old written arithmetic" (Smith, 1909, cited in Wolf, 1966, p. 272).
The rationale for the inclusion of oral arithmetic in the curriculum was based on
the tenets of Formal Discipline which held that the mind was a muscle and therefore
in need of exercise if it was to become strong (Kolesnik, 1958, p. 4). Exercises in
oral arithmetic were used as a form of drill to improve general mental discipline.
Speaking in 1830 of arithmetic in general, but relevant to the oral aspects, Colburn
suggested that:

Arithmetic, when properly taught, is acknowledged by all to be very important


as discipline of the mind; so much so that even if it had no practical application
which should render it valuable on its own account, it would still be well worth
while to bestow a considerable portion of time on it for this purpose alone.
(Colburn, 1830, reprinted in Bidwell & Clason, 1970, p. 24)

Nonetheless, despite the importance placed on the need to develop mental


discipline, Colburn (1830, reprinted in Bidwell & Clason, 1970, p. 24) considered
that it was secondary to the practical utility of arithmetic; a view that was to be
echoed during the 1930s and 1940s with respect to mental computation, following
its decreased emphasis early in the twentieth century (B. J. Reys, 1985, p. 44).
The near total neglect of mental methods of computation in mathematics
curricula in the United States during the first quarter of the twentieth century was
due to the Theory of Mental Discipline, and by association, Formal Discipline, falling
"into such disrepute that it was difficult to maintain a place in the curriculum for any

form of mental activity, including mental arithmetic" (Reys & Barger, 1994, pp. 3233). This was despite such beliefs as those of Suzzallo who, in 1911, contended
that:

It is altogether probable that many simple calculations or analyses can be done


"silently" from the beginning; that others require visual demonstrations, but
once mastered can thereafter be done without visual aids; that still others will
always be performed, partially at least, with some written work. It is purely a
matter for concrete judgment in each special case, but the existing practice
scarcely recognises this truth. The result is that many problems are arbitrarily
done in one way, and it is too frequently the uneconomical and inefficient way
that is used. (Suzzallo, 1911, p. 78, cited in Reys & Barger, 1994, p. 33)

Hall (1954, p. 349) observed that it was unfortunate that mental arithmetic
should also have been discredited. However, the Theory of Mental Discipline's
promise of transfer of knowledge through exercising each general faculty was
questioned when it was shown that learning arithmetic (and Latin) did not facilitate
learning other subjects. Its demise was accentuated by "the rise of associationism
as a dominant psychological account of mental functioning" (Resnick, 1989a, p. 8).
Similar concerns to those of the mid-nineteenth century began to be expressed
during the 1930s in the United States, with respect to the perceived
overdependence on written methods of calculation. The rationale for a renewed
emphasis on mental (oral) methods was one of social utility, namely, that mental
arithmetic was more useful outside the classroom than were paper-and-pencil
procedures (B. J. Reys, 1985, p. 44). This advocacy for an emphasis on mental
computation coincided with attempts to improve instruction in mathematics, such as
Brownell's (1935) promotion of the meaning theory of arithmetic instruction.
Reflected in these recommendations were the beginnings of a shift in the
philosophic orientation in teaching mathematics, away from drill and practice
towards discovery learning and independent inquiry (Reys & Barger, 1994, p. 34).
During the 1940s and early 1950s there was an increased emphasis on mental
computation until the concern for developing an understanding of mathematical
structure gained prominence during the New Mathematics era in the late 1950s to
mid-1970s. During this period the issue of paper-and-pencil versus mental

computation was virtually ignored. Nevertheless, most proponents of mental


computation have always advocated that a focus on mental computation supports a
deeper understanding of numbers and the use of structural relationships when
calculating (Menchinskaya & Moro, 1975, p. 74; R. E. Reys, 1984, p. 549). As Hall
(1954) points out, the renewed emphasis on mental arithmetic, in the 1940s in the
United States, was geared to:

(a) Functional problem situations, including those requiring approximate and


exact answers; (b) an [increased] understanding of place value and of ten as
the foundation of our number system; (c) an awareness of number
relationships in the discovery of acceptable short cuts, once the conventional
procedure [was] understood; and (d) recreational exercises to motivate and
enrich number experiences. (p. 349)

The revival of interest in mental computation since the late 1970s initially
coincided with, and was strengthened by, a reevaluation of what constitutes school
mathematics as a reaction by the mathematics education community to the Back to
Basics movement, principally in the United States (NCSM, 1977; National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics [NCTM], 1980). Together with this reevaluation was "a
growing realization that many students apply written algorithms mechanically, with
little sense as to why, how, or what they are doing" (B. J. Reys, 1985, p. 44), an
echo of previous calls for a renewed interest in mental computation. However, the
relative importance and the nature of mental computation as now proposed differ
markedly from the oral arithmetic of the past that emphasised oral drill"mental
gymnastics" in Koenker's (1961, pp. 295-296) viewrather than exploration and
discussion.

1.2.2 Mental Computation: Reasons for the Resurgence of Interest


The current resurgence of interest in mental computation stems from the
recognition that computing mentally remains a viable computational alternative in
the calculator age and that the development of mental procedures contributes to the
formation of powerful mathematical thinking strategies (R. E. Reys, 1992, p. 63).

Encouraging children to develop idiosyncratic cognitive methods for carrying out


computations is compatible with the constructivist approach to learning, which
asserts that "human beings acquire knowledge by building it from the inside instead
of internalizing it directly from the environment" (Kamii, 1990, p. 22).
Except for the implied belief that mental computation skills should only be
developed "once the conventional procedure is understood," the goals outlined by
Hall (1954, p. 349) are ones echoed in the current advocacy for mental computation
to be given prominence in the mathematics curriculum. Robert Reys (1984, p. 549)
suggests that an emphasis on mental computation contributes to the development
of:

A deeper understanding of the structure of numbers and their properties.

Creative and independent thinking.

Ingenious ways of manipulating numbers.

Skills and strategies associated with problem solving and computational


estimation, the latter being an essential skill in the efficient use of electronic
calculating devices.

Despite the similarities in the goals for developing mental computational skills,
as expressed by Hall (1954) and Robert Reys (1984), there are marked differences
in the nature of mental computation as now envisaged and in the ways that such
skills should be developed. The current thrust, which had its beginnings in the late
1970s, has a broader focus than earlier movements. Besides highlighting a
recognition of the applicability of the constructivist theory of learning to the
development of mathematical abilities, it also emphasises the belief that paper-andpencil skills, particularly the traditional written algorithms, should receive decreased
attention (B. J. Reys, 1991, p. 7). Additionally, it also gives recognition to the gulf
between learning and practising school mathematics and learning and practising the
mathematics used outside the classroom, a focus that centres on the utility of school
mathematics in the society of the 1990s (Masingila, Davidenko, Prus-Wisniowska, &
Agwu, 1994, p. 3).
The present interest in mental computation coincides with a need to redefine
the way in which calculations are performed, particularly as a consequence of the
availability of calculators and computers. As McIntosh (1990a) points out, "none of

[the previous] pendulum shifts [has] suggested other than that mental computation
[be] the bridesmaid of written computation" (p. 36). This was despite such contrary
views for their time as those of Branford (cited by McIntosh, 1990a) who suggested,
in 1908, that "mental arithmetic should come first and form the solid food: written
arithmetic should be the luxury, given where and when it can be appreciated" (p.
36), a view now promoted, given the influence of technological calculating devices,
as well as for pedagogical reasons.
With respect to the latter, the Mathematical Sciences Education Board and the
National Research Council (1990, p. 19) believe that there is now sufficient
evidence to suggest that an overemphasis on paper-and-pencil skills may hinder a
child's effective use of mental techniques for calculating. Mental computation is
increasingly being considered as an essential prerequisite to the successful
development of written algorithms (R. E. Reys, 1984, p. 549). This is in marked
contrast to the traditional view of the place of mental computation within the
mathematics curriculum.
A "novel facet of [the current] revival is the interest in using mental computation
as a vehicle for promoting thinking, conjecturing and generalizing based on
conceptual understanding rather than as a set of skills which serve as an end of
instruction" (Reys & Barger, 1994, p. 31). By focusing on conceptual understanding
rather than on the memorisation of rules, the manipulation of quantities rather than
symbols, in Reed and Laves' (1981, p. 442) terms, is involved. Rathmell and
Trafton (1990) assert that:
The varied and thoughtful ways children manipulate quantities when doing
mental computation promote number sense as well as mathematical thinking.
This kind of activity brings a dynamic quality to learning mathematics because
children are actually doing mathematics rather than learning to repeat
conventional procedures. (p. 157)

During this process, children construct their own mathematical knowledge that
not only enhances learning but also encourages them to view mathematics as
meaningful, rather than as a collection of arbitrarily derived rules. Children are
compelled to seek novel ways to use numbers and number relations, methods that
are likely to increase an understanding of the structure of the number system
(Sowder, 1992, p. 15). Ironically, although the development of an understanding of

10

mathematical structure was an important goal of the new maths movement, mental
computation was virtually ignored in the syllabuses of the 1960s and 1970s, despite
such beliefs as those espoused, in the United States, by Beberman (1959, cited by
Josephina, 1960). He asserted that:

Mental arithmetic...is one of the best ways of helping children become


independent of techniques which are usually learned by strict
memorisation....Moreover, mental arithmetic encourages children to
discover computational short cuts and thus to gain deeper insight into the
number system. (p. 199)

Undertaking mental calculations is not only the simplest method of performing


many arithmetical procedures, it is also the main form of calculation used in
everyday life. Wandt and Brown (1957, pp. 152-153) found that during a 24 hour
period 75% of nonvocational uses of mathematics by college students were mental
in nature, rather than ones involving paper-and-pencil. Forty-eight percent entailed
mental computation to provide exact answers, while 27% involved computational
estimation. The ability to compute mentally is therefore a valuable skill for everyday
living (NCSM, 1989, p. 45).
That mental computation is not emphasised in classrooms is seen by Cockcroft
(Cockcroft, 1982, p. 75, para 255) as representing a failure to recognise the central
place that mental procedures occupy throughout mathematics. Flournoy, in 1957,
contended:

Because activities of everyday life require competence in mental arithmetic,


schools must provide pupils with [opportunities] to learn to think without paper
and pencil in solving problems involving simple computation, making
approximations, and interpreting quantitative data, terms and statements. (p.
147)

However, those who are proficient at mathematics in daily life, and in the
workplace, seldom make use of the standard written algorithms during mental
calculations. Rather, idiosyncratic methods are used or else the written algorithms
are adapted in unique ways (Cockcroft, 1982, p. 75, para 256). People are involved

11

with, what Maier (1980, pp. 21-23) calls, folk mathematics or, what Howson and
Wilson (1986, p. 21) term, ethnomathematics. The methods used differ with the
situation in which an arithmetical problem is to be solved (Carraher, Carraher, &
Schliemann, 1987, p. 83). Further, Lave (1985, p. 173) suggests that the
organisation of arithmetic varies qualitatively from one situation to another. It
appears that algorithms taught in schools are only likely to be used to solve schooltype problems, with little transfer to real-life problem situations (Carraher et al.,
1987, p. 95). Conversely, self-taught strategies used in every-day situations are
unlikely to be used by children in the classroom without specific encouragement by
teachers (Gracey, 1994, p. 75).
While school mathematics remains largely oriented towards paper-and-pencil
algorithms, folk mathematics predominantly involves mental calculations and
algorithms that lend themselves to mental use. Calculators and computers are used
for the more difficult and cumbersome calculations, with paper-and-pencil
procedures considered as final choices (Maier, 1980, p. 22). Therefore for school
mathematics to become more meaningful and useful in non-classroom situations an
emphasis needs to be placed on encouraging children "to develop personal mental
computational strategies, to experiment with and compare strategies used by others,
and to choose from amongst their available strategies to suit their own strengths
and the particular context" (AEC, 1991, p. 109).

1.2.3 Mental Computation: Place in Current Mathematics Curricula


Despite the on-going advocacy for increased attention to mental computation,
this has not yet been translated significantly into mathematics classrooms in the
United States (Coburn, 1989, p. 47; Koenker, 1967, p. 295; Resnick & Omanson,
1987, p. 65; Reys & Barger, 1994, p. 46; R. E. Reys, 1992, p. 69; Sachar, 1978, p.
233). However, some progress is beginning to be made with attempts by education
authorities to implement the Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School
Mathematics (NCTM, 1989); for example, the Ohio Department of Education with its
Model Competency-Based Mathematics Program in which Strand 7 focuses
explicitly on mental computation and estimation (Ohio Department of Education,
n.d., pp. 89-99).
In the United Kingdom, Cockcroft (1982, pp. 74-75, para 254) had also
expressed concern at the decline in attention given to skills associated with

12

calculating mentally (Cockcroft, 1982, pp. 74-75, para 254). One reason for this is
that the nature of mental computation, when viewed as a higher-order thinking
process, does not facilitate the delineation of a fixed scope and sequence (R. E.
Reys, 1992, p. 69). The encouragement of mental computation through textbooks,
therefore, becomes an even more difficult task. Strategies for computing mentally
are naturally developed through discussion and exploration (R. E. Reys, 1992, p.
70). This implies that a less structured approach to lesson design than that
traditionally used to develop the written algorithms is imperative, a requirement that
necessitates changes to the teaching practices of many teachers.
In Australia, the place of mental computation in mathematics curricula is only
beginning to be seriously considered (AEC, 1991, pp. 106-134). As McIntosh
(1990a, p. 25) has observed, little attention has been given to teaching mental
computation, with such attention predominantly confined to those calculations
needed to be performed mentally to enable the efficient use of the conventional
written algorithms. Teachers are inclined to associate mental computation with
isolated facts, usually the basic ones, rather than "with networks of relationships
between numbers or with methods of computation" (McIntosh, 1990a, p. 25).
Further, in Atweh's (1982, p. 57) view, teachers often discourage mental calculation
by insisting that children write down their solutions so that each individual step may
be detailed. This expectation may stem from the belief that children are not
productively engaged in mathematics unless they are writing, or, it may be a
classroom management procedure designed to enable the teacher to retain control
over the pace of a lesson.
In the Queensland context, the need for an emphasis on mental computation is
not inconsistent with many recommended teaching practices. Associated with the
implementation of the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of
Education, 1987a) emphasis is being given to issues relevant to the development of
the ability to calculate exact answers mentally. It is believed that learning is
enhanced by (a) teaching through problem solving, (b) encouraging children to
explore and discuss mathematical ideas with their peers and teachers, and (c)
accepting a range of solutions as well as various strategies for arriving at a
particular solution (Department of Education, 1987b, pp. 3-4).
Further, the need to develop a range of strategies for arriving at approximate
answers is being emphasised. Strategies for calculating exact answers mentally,

13

different to those traditionally used for paper-and-pencil calculations are given


consideration, albeit a limited one, in the year-level sourcebooks for Years 4, 6 and
7, and particularly in that for Year 5 (Department of Education, 1988, pp. 51-57).
However, due to the unavailability of research data, the degree to which these
recommendations are being implemented in classrooms is unknown. Nonetheless,
given the inadequacies of teacher inservice associated with the implementation of
the current mathematics syllabus in Years One to Seven and the abandonment, in
1996, of the implementation of Student Performance Standards in Mathematics for
Queensland Schools (1994), which supported an explicit focus on mental
computation, the extent to which these recommendations may be observed in
classrooms could be limited. This issue is a focus of the survey of Queensland
state primary school teachers and administrators reported in Chapter 4.

1.2.4 Mental Computation: Student Performance


Data related to performance levels on mental computation are limited, with
none available for large samples of Australian students. However, data provided by
McIntosh et al. (1995) do permit some insight into the abilities of West Australian
children in Years 3, 5, 7 and 9. Robert Reys (1985, p. 14) suggests that, within the
standardised achievement testing programs in the United States, the lack of
information is primarily due to the unique conditions required for testing mental
computation, namely, that the examples given need to be paced and that answers
need to be open-ended to permit children to record their individual responses,
conditions that depart significantly from those normally associated with standardised
achievement tests. Another factor is that there is insufficient information from
research to assist in the designing of performance outcomes for mental computation
(Coburn, 1989, p. 47).
The data that are available, primarily from the United States and in common
with that available from Australia (McIntosh et al., 1995) and Japan (R. E. Reys et
al., 1995), suggest that children perform rather poorly on tasks designed to measure
mental computation abilities and that children generally prefer not to calculate
mentally even where the items lend themselves to mental calculation (Carpenter,
Matthews, Lindquist, & Silver, 1984, p. 487; B. J. Reys, R. E. Reys, & Hope, 1993,
p. 314; R. E. Reys et al., 1995, p. 323). However, two Australian studies, although

14

limited in scope, suggest that Australian children may not be as biased against
mental computation, even though little instruction is undertaken. Gracey (1994, p.
113) found that mental computation was the most commonly preferred option for
calculating one-step multiplication items, 945 x 100 for example, by a class of Year
6 children. Similar findings are reported for all four basic operations by McIntosh et
al. (1995, p. 12). For children who did not prefer to calculate such items as 100 x 35
mentally, it was concluded that this was due to a lack of conceptual understanding
rather than a lack of computational skill (McIntosh et al., 1995, p. 11-12).
Results from the United States Third National Mathematics Assessment reveal
that the ability of nine year-old children to compute mentally is only beginning to
emerge, with performance levels on such items as 6 + 47, 36 + 9 and 90 x 3 being
below 50% correct (R. E. Reys, 1985, p. 15). Barbara Reys (1991, p. 3) suggests
that this may be because children of this age are primarily concerned, in present
curricula, with developing the written algorithms and that this emphasis may
interfere with their ability to compute mentally, which requires an abstract and
flexible manipulation of numbers. For the nine year-old children and the 13 yearolds, a wide range of performances was recorded: Twenty percent correct for 36 - 9
to 52% correct for 64 + 20 (nine year-olds), and 32% correct for 60 15 to 92%
correct for 700 - 600 (13 year-olds). Addition was the easiest operation, with
division being the most difficult (R. E. Reys, 1985, p. 15).
In contrast, results from Periodiek Peilings Onderzoek in The Netherlands
indicate 70% to 90% accuracy on items similar to those of the Third National
Mathematics Assessment (Treffers, 1991, p. 336). Dutch 13 year-olds scored
approximately 90% correct on 480 6 and 7 x 90, and on 600 300 and 20 x 2400
about 70%. While these scores are superior to those of the United States, Treffers
(1991) concluded that "rather than being proud of our students' achievement, [Dutch
educators] should draw the general conclusion...that mental arithmetic badly needs
improvement" (p. 336).
Barbara Reys et al. (1993) report similar results to those of the National
Assessment from research undertaken with a sample of children in the second, fifth
and seventh grades in Texas and Saskatchewan. For each of these year-levels,
performances on applied problems were also quite low. For six of the 10 items,
Grade 2 children scored below 20% correct. This may reflect their relative lack of
experience with problem solving and particularly with tests in which items are paced

15

(B. J. Reys et al., 1993, p. 309). Although a narrower range of performances was
evident for children in Grades 5 and 7, for most items a success rate of
approximately 10% was achieved. The highest percentage correct for Grade 5 was
32% for the item: "Chuck's family lives 100km from Chicago. They stop after driving
65km. How much farther do they have to go?" (B. J. Reys et al., 1993, p. 311).
Chaining of addition and subtraction was difficult for both Grade 2 and Grade 5
children who also dropped in performance where chaining of multiplication was
involved (B. J. Reys et al., 1993, p. 310). Examples such as 75 + 85 + 25 + 2000
(fifth grade) require partial values to be mentally retained for later computation, a
skill that is not developed during the traditional focus on paper-and-pencil skills.
For performance levels in mental computation to rise, children need to be
encouraged to calculate mentally and to develop a range of strategies for carrying
out such calculations. They also must develop an appreciation of when it is
appropriate to calculate mentally, in context with their abilities, and in so doing
develop the confidence to use mental procedures. Such confidence appears to be
seriously lacking at this time. R. E. Reys et al. (1995, p. 323) conclude that this is a
theme that appears to be common across various school systems. Flournoy (1959,
p. 134), following a survey of children's use of mathematics across a 7 day period,
concluded that greater use of mental arithmetic would be made if they felt confident
in solving number situations without paper-and-pencil. Of relevance to this finding is
that of Case and Sowder (1990) with respect to computational estimation. They
concluded that the lack of confidence children exhibit in their mathematical abilities
results from the "split...between the understanding of number that children glean
from their everyday quantitative activity and the school-based algorithms they learn
to execute" (Case & Sowder, 1990, p. 100). This conclusion has implications for
how mental computation is defined and for how it is to be taught.
Children need to make appropriate choices between applying paper-and-pencil
procedures, mental calculations and the use of a calculator. Data from the Third
National Mathematics Assessment indicate that on items considered appropriate for
mental computation most 13 year-old students preferred to use either paper-andpencil or a calculator. For example, for 4 x 99, although 44% indicated that they
would "Do it in their heads, 39% would use paper-and-pencil and 16% would use a
calculator (R. E. Reys, 1985, p. 15). A similar pattern of preferred methods for
calculating items whose structure encourages mental calculation is reported by B. J.

16

Reys et al. (1993, p. 310) for children in Grades 5 and 7, with the majority preferring
to employ paper-and-pencil for all except 1000 x 945, a fifth grade example. The
preference for written methods rather than calculator use is also reported by Gracey
(1994, p. 113) and reflects the emphasis on written procedures in the classroom.

1.2.5 Mental Computation: Essential Changes in Outlook


To enhance the use of mental computation and to achieve an improvement in
performance levels, Rathmell and Trafton (1990, p. 156) believe that children should
be encouraged to value all methods of computation. This requires that teachers
come to recognise that the development of mental skills is a legitimate goal for
computational programs in classrooms (B. J. Reys et al., 1993, p. 312), a goal
supported by recent curriculum reform documents in mathematics education (AEC,
1991, pp. 114-115, 120-121, 126-127; NCTM, 1989, pp. 44-49, 94-97; National
Curriculum: Mathematics Working Group [NCMWG], 1988, pp. 20-21).
These documents recommend that the emphasis on paper-and-pencil skills in
current curricula should be reduced, particularly with respect to the standard written
algorithms. Representative of these recommendations, is the belief of the
Australian Education Council (1991) that "less emphasis should be given to
standard paper-and-pencil algorithms and, to the extent that they continue to be
taught, they should be taught at later stages in schooling" (p. 109). Terezinha
Carraher et al. (1987, p. 83) suggest that school-learned algorithms may not be the
preferred ways for solving numerical problems outside the classroom. Further, by
continuing the emphasis on the written algorithms, the commonly-held but
erroneous view of arithmetic as necessarily involving linear, precise and complete
calculations is maintained. Such a view restricts the development of a mental agility
with numbersnumber sensewhich is an essential ingredient for the development
of a range of flexible strategies for calculating, whether using paper-and-pencil, a
calculator or the mind.
The procedures involved in performing the traditional written algorithms for
addition, subtraction, and multiplication, in particular, with their emphasis on right-toleft processing, conflict with the procedures commonly employed when calculating
mentally. Cooper, Haralampou, and Irons (1992) suggest that "if the pen-and-paper
algorithms interfere, deter, impede, replace or stop the development of mental

17

strategies...there may be a need to consider resequencing the teaching of


computation for each operation throughout the primary school" (p. 101). Except in
relation to the development of the basic facts, a focus on mental calculation, albeit a
limited one, has traditionally occurred after the written algorithm has been
introduced for a particular operation.
Children need to be given opportunities to explore mathematical relationships
and to invent idiosyncratic strategies for computing mentally, unencumbered by
patterns of thought developed through a premature focus on the written algorithms,
either idiosyncratic or standard. Mental computation, and computational estimation,
should receive an ongoing emphasis throughout all computational experiences in
the classroom (NCTM, 1989, p. 45). Rathmell and Trafton (1990, p. 156) advocate
an early and ongoing emphasis on mental computation, estimation, and an
appropriate use of calculators to provide a framework for developing paper-andpencil skills. Such skills should be ones that generally extend and support the use
of mental strategies (McIntosh, 1990a, p. 37), and the development of number
sense.

1.2.6 Mental Computation: Needed Research


Although much has been written about mental computation during the past
century concerning its place and importance in the classroom, little research is
available to guide either curriculum or instruction (B. J. Reys, 1991, p. 7), an
observation that is particularly relevant to the Australian context. Additionally, there
is only a limited body of knowledge, albeit one that is gradually expanding,
concerning how children think when they compute exact answers mentally. This
applies particularly to mental strategies for multiplication and division beyond the
basic facts (McIntosh, 1990b, p. 18).
Robert Reys and Nohda (1994, p. 5) suggest that the basic question of "What
is mental computation?" is one that continues to be analysed and debated, a
situation suggestive of the paucity of definitive knowledge about mental
computation. Essential to a comprehensive understanding of mental computation
are a number of issues identified in the research literature as needing clarification.
These include:

18

The nature of the interface between self-generated strategies and formally


taught written algorithms.

The extent to which children of varying abilities develop efficient mental


strategies.

The effects of direct and indirect teaching methods to encourage the


development of self-generated strategies for computing mentally.

The relationship between the development of strategies for mental


computation and those for computational estimation.

(Reys & Barger, 1994, p. 45)

The interrelationship between mental computation and number sense.

The methods for and timing of the introduction of alternative computational


procedures, namely, mental computation, computational estimation, paperand-pencil procedures and calculator use.

(Reys & Nohda, 1994, p. 5)

It is suggested that for children to successfully implement the range of


computational alternatives available to them, further knowledge, some of which may
be country specific, is required on each of these issues (Reys & Nohda, 1994, p. 6).

1.3

Purposes and Significance of the Study


Focussing on the development of personal strategies for calculating exact

answers mentally places the child at the centre of the learning process. This
highlights that the advocacy for an increased emphasis on mental computation goes
well beyond simply focussing on it as a computational method per se. While it is
important for mental computation, and the use of calculators, to receive greater
emphasis in the calculative process, with a concomitant de-emphasis on the
standard written algorithms, a focus on the relationships between the development
of idiosyncratic thinking strategies and the development of number sense and
numeracy is of equal importance.
By undertaking (a) an analysis of past and present syllabuses, from a mental
computation perspective, and (b) a survey of Queensland school personnel, issues
related to these recommendations are able to be placed in context with beliefs and

19

teaching practices, both past and current. This should lead to an enriched context
in which these issues can be debated from a Queensland perspective, issues which
encompass aspects of the areas of needed research identified by Barbara Reys and
Barger (1994, p. 45), and Robert Reys and Nohda (1994, p. 5). Consequently, the
principal purposes of this study, in accordance with the aims delineated in Section
1.1, were:

1. To formulate a mental computation strand, encapsulating key elements of


the research data, for inclusion as a core element in future mathematics
syllabuses.
2. To draw implications for decision making about mental computation, from
past, contemporary and futures perspectives. These implications may be
essential to any discussion for change within the Number strand of the
Queensland primary school mathematics syllabus.
3. To provide an in-depth analysis of key psychological, socio-anthropological
and pedagogical issues related to mental computation.
4. To document the nature and role of mental computation, and associated
pedagogical practices, under each of the Queensland mathematics
syllabuses.
5. To investigate the beliefs about mental computation currently held by
Queensland primary school teachers and administrators.
6. To gain an insight into the status of mental computation in current
mathematics programs, and to identify current pedagogical practices
related to mental computation in Queensland primary school classrooms.
7. To compare and contrast past and current beliefs and practices with those
recommended as essential for children to gain mastery of the calculative
process.

Achieving these purposes has provided a comprehensive summary of the state


of knowledge about mental computation, both from theoretical and Queensland
perspectives. This has significance for the following areas:

Psychological and pedagogical significance: The analysis of the nature of


mental computation, together with the identification of recommended

20

teaching practices, should not only be significant as a comprehensive


summary of the knowledge about mental computation, but it also should
provide guidance in selecting and implementing teaching strategies that will
enhance the development of a student's ability and confidence to calculate
exact answers mentally. Additionally, where mental strategies are to be
formally taught, suggestions as to those considered appropriate for each
operation have been highlighted.

Mathematical significance: Students' attitudes towards, and performances


on, tasks involving mental computation should be improved by the
implementation of teaching approaches and sequences based on data
relevant to the Queensland context. This may lead students to view mental
computation as the method of first resort rather than one that is to be
avoided for all but the simplest calculations.

Significance for curriculum development: This study should provide a


comprehensive source of data to support the decision making processes of
curriculum developers during any future reshaping of the Queensland
mathematics syllabus for the primary school. This applies particularly to
the degree to which mental computation should be emphasised viz-a-viz
paper-and-pencil and calculator use, as embodied in the proposed mental
computation strand.

Significance for Queensland educational history: The analysis of the nature


and role of mental computation in Queensland state primary school
classrooms since 1860 should extend and deepen the available knowledge
about the mathematics taught, the teaching methods used, and issues
which were of importance to their development and implementation.

1.4

Overview of the Study


This study has been conceived as comprising three major investigations,

which, although necessarily interrelated, are essentially distinct. Consequently, a


more detailed analysis of methodological issues is presented in the respective
chapters. The following provides an overview of the research procedures employed.

21

1.4.1 Method and Justification


In a paper prepared, in the mid-1960s, by three District Inspectors for the
Queensland Department of Education's Syllabus Committee, it was suggested that
"one of the major functions of a study of [the past] is to enable a community to avoid
future errors, and to assist them to solve problems which may arise in the future, by
a consideration of events of the past" (Schildt, Reithmuller, & Searle,

n.d., p. 1).

With respect to curricula, the preoccupation in such analyses should principally be


with the gathering of data which will assist in understanding contemporary
curriculum issues (Goodson, 1985, p. 126; Fox, 1969, p. 45), in this instance,
aspects of mental computation.
The preparation of the paper by Schildt et al. (n.d.) occurred during a period
when the mathematics syllabus for Queensland state primary schools was
undergoing major revisions due to administrative changes and altered beliefs about
how children learn mathematics. While the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus
(Department of Education, 1987a), which was introduced into Queensland schools
in 1988, is not at present scheduled for revision, there is an increasing emphasis by
mathematics educators on the need to determine an appropriate balance between
mental, written and technological calculations within the mathematics curriculum. A
National Statement on Mathematics for Australian Schools (AEC, 1991) suggests
that mental computation should be the method of "first resort" (p. 109) in many
calculative situations. This implies that the primary focus on written computation
embodied in the current Queensland syllabus will need to be revised. For this to
occur successfully, the two revolutions delineated by McIntosh (1992, pp. 131-134)
will need to occur. The first revolution, in particular, namely the need for change in
the way mental computation is viewed by teachers, should be supported by the data
gathered in this study. These provide an understanding of how mental computation
has been viewed under each of the mathematics syllabuses in Queensland from
1860, an outcome of which should be the fostering of an enriched context for
debate.
Consequently, this study is characterised by four approaches to gathering and
analysing data, and drawing conclusions, relevant to the nature and role of mental
computation within the primary school, both from a theoretical perspective and from

22

one that relates directly to its teaching in Queensland state schools. These
approaches entailed undertaking:

1. A literature review, the aim of which was to analyse the pedagogical, socioanthropological and psychological literature relevant to mental computation.
2. An analysis of Queensland syllabuses in relation to the nature and function
of mental computation within Queensland primary school curricula from
1860, with particular emphasis on the period 1860-1965 during which
Queensland mathematics syllabuses made specific references to the
mental calculation of exact answers.
3. A survey, using a postal, self-completion questionnaire, of Queensland
state primary school teachers and administrators, from a random sample of
Queensland state schools, to ascertain their beliefs and teaching practices
pertaining to mental computation.
4. A synthesis of the data from the first three approaches, the aim of which
was to highlight similarities and differences in beliefs and practices
concerning the nature and function of, and teaching methods related to,
mental computation. In so doing it was aimed to provide recommendations
for future action with respect to syllabus revision in Queensland, the key
element of which is the proposed mental computation strand for future
mathematics syllabuses.

1.4.2 Chapter Guidelines


Developed from the study's purposes and from key theoretical and practical
aspects of mental computation, each of the research strategies was guided by a
series of questions for which answers were sought. These questions are presented
below.

Chapter 2:

Mental Computation

1. What are the recent developments in mathematics education of relevance


to mental computation?

23

2. What is the place of mental computation in the calculative process and


particularly its relationships with computational estimation?
3. What is the nature of mental computation as perceived by mathematics
educators, both contemporaneously and historically?
4. What are the roles currently perceived for mental computation within and
beyond the classroom?
5. What are the affective and cognitive components, including commonly used
mental strategies, that constitute skill in computing exact answers
mentally?
6. What are the affective and cognitive characteristics exhibited by skilled
mental calculators, including the role that memory plays in the process of
calculating exact answers mentally?
7. What are the teaching approaches and sequence necessary for the
development of mental computation skills?

Chapter 3:

Mental Computation in Queensland: 1860-1965

1. What emphasis was given to mental computation in the various


mathematics syllabuses for Queensland primary schools during the period
1860-1965?
2. What was the nature of mental computation as embodied in the various
syllabuses and in the manner in which it was taught from 1860 to 1965?
3. What was the role of mental computation within the mathematics curricula
from 1860 to 1965?
4. What was the nature of the teaching practices used to develop a child's
ability to calculate exact answers mentally during the period 1860-1965?
5. What was the nature of the resources used to support the teaching of
mental computation during the period 1860-1965?

Chapter Four:

Mental Computation in Queensland: 1966-1997

1. What beliefs do teachers currently hold with respect to the nature and role
of mental computation and how it should be taught?

24

2. What emphasis was given to mental computation in the period 1966-1987,


with respect to both syllabus documents and teachers?
3. What emphasis is currently placed on developing the ability to compute
mentally?
4. What are the characteristics of the teaching approaches currently used to
develop the ability to calculate exact answers mentally?
5. What were the characteristics of the teaching approaches used to develop
the ability to calculate exact answers mentally during the period 19661987?
6. What need for inservice on mental computation is expressed by school
personnel?
7. What was the nature of the resources used to support the teaching of
mental computation during the period 1966-1987 and of those used
currently?
8. What is the relevance to mental computation of recent initiatives in
mathematics education in Queensland?

Chapter Five:

Mental Computation: A Proposed Syllabus Component

1. How should the computation strand of the primary school mathematics


syllabus be reorganised to incorporate a mental-written sequence for
introducing computational procedures?
2. Which mental strategies for each operation are appropriate to recommend
for direct teaching?

Chapter Six:

Mental Computation: Conclusions and Implications

1. What conclusions about mental computation across the range of time


periods can be drawn?
2. What implications do these conclusions hold for syllabus development?
3. What aspects of mental computation require further theoretical and
empirical investigation?

27

CHAPTER 2

MENTAL COMPUTATION

2.1

Introduction
Although "many of the fundamental ideas which underpin the school

mathematics curriculum of old'...are not necessarily old-fashioned' or out-of-date'"


(Willis, 1990, p. 12), the ways in which mathematics is produced and applied are
undergoing rapid change through the availability of technological devices for
calculating. This has resulted in reanalysing what should constitute mathematics for
primary school children. In the reappraisements to date, significant changes are
suggested for the number strand of school mathematics curricula (AEC, 1991;
NCTM, 1989; NCMWG, 1988).
In outlining principles for change, the Mathematical Sciences Education Board
and National Research Council (1990, p. 36) suggest that mathematics education
must focus on the development of mathematical power (Principle 1). Mathematical
power enables individuals to (a) understand mathematical concepts and
procedures, (b) perceive the relationships between mathematical ideas embodied in
a variety of situations, (c) reason logically and to solve both routine and nonroutine
problems, and (d) use mathematical ideas intelligently in all aspects of their lives.
With respect to an understanding of number concepts, mathematical power
flows from the development of a strong sense of number and from an ability to
confidently select from, and apply, a range of computational procedures appropriate
to particular contexts. As Plunkett (1979, p. 5) suggests, calculators are ideal tools
for difficult and laborious computations. Their acceptability and use within
classrooms are, however, yet far from ideal, necessitating a reassessment of
teaching practices (Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers [AAMT], 1996a,
p. 5). For most computational needs, the Australian Education Council (1991, p.
109) believes that mental methods, combined with informal written procedures to

28

provide memory support, are the most appropriate. Elevating the importance of
mental computation (and calculator use) in school mathematics is a recognition of
the central role of such calculation outside the classroom. As Cockcroft (1982)
comments: "In almost all jobs the ability to carry out some calculations mentally is of
value and lack of ability to do this is a frequent cause of complaint by employers" (p.
20, para 71). Therefore, there is a need for positive attitudes towards mental
computation to be fostered in classrooms (J. P. Jones, 1988, p. 42), a view
supported by the performance data referred to in Section 1.2.4.
The importance of a focus on mental computation does not merely centre on its
role as a computational procedure per se. Barbara Reys et al. (1993, p. 314)
suggest that mental computation is a vehicle for developing each of the curriculum
standards with respect to problem solving, reasoning, number sense and
communication. These standards, which are in accord with recommendations
contained in A National Statement on Mathematics for Australian Schools (AEC,
1991), were proposed by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1989, p.
2) as a means for indicating educational goals, ensuring quality of teaching, and
promoting change in education. Mental computation, as now perceived, is
considered to be a more creative activity than that embodied in what has been
traditionally called mental arithmetic (Curriculum Programmes Branch, 1989, p. 26),
where the focus was primarily on the correctness of the answer rather than on the
mental methods employed. Joy Jones (1988, p. 44) suggests that the real value of
emphasising mental procedures is the development of the soundness of number
that comes from considering a variety of methods. Indeed, mental computation
constitutes the process most readily available to assist in the understanding of how
numbers operate (McIntosh, 1990a, p. 37). Additionally, the thinking that is involved
facilitates the growth of a sense for computational routines (B. J. Reys, 1985, p. 43).
Number sense and the ability to calculate exact and approximate answers
mentally evolve concurrently, with each supporting the development of the other.
Children who are adept at mental computation can flexibly use a rich variety of
reasoning strategies (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 96). This ability is firmly grounded in
their sense of number, their drive and capacity to formulate and apply logical
connections between new and previously acquired information (B. J. Reys, 1992, p.
94). It follows, therefore, that effective mental methods cannot be acquired by rote
learning (French, 1987, p. 39). Indeed, mental computation helps prevent a reliance

29

on acquiring facts and algorithms through meaningless verbal memorisation (Atweh,


1982, p. 53).
Several benefits for both students and teachers arise from including mental
computation in the mathematics curriculum. Mental computation can be a means
for individualising learning. As Barbara Reys (1985, p. 45) suggests, through a
focus on personal strategies that make sense to the child, the brightest students can
be challenged while allowing all students to use their knowledge of basic
mathematical concepts and relationships to solve particular problems. Obtaining
answers mentally is a problem solving process. In many situations mental
algorithms do not exist for the child, or are forgotten (Atweh, 1982, p. 18). Students
are allowed to become cognitive apprentices (Collins, Brown, & Newman, 1989, p.
457). Such a situation arises where relevant conceptual and factual knowledge are
exemplified and situated in the contexts of their use, and where learning
experiences are structured to "allow skill to build up bit by bit, yet permit participation
even for the relatively unskilled, often as a result of the social sharing of tasks"
(Resnick, 1989a, p. 13). Insights are therefore provided into the ways children think
and understand as they solve problems mentally (R. E. Reys, 1992, p. 65).

2.2

Research Questions
In Robert Reys' (1992, pp. 65-66) view, a child's ability to reason

mathematically and to use a range of flexible thinking skills is enhanced by the


process of computing mentally. This process (a) requires a considered inspection of
the problem situation before computing, (b) promotes and encourages the use of
basic mathematical properties, (c) rewards adaptable approaches to manipulating
numbers, and (d) facilitates the use of visual thinking skills.
These issues are considered in later sections of this chapter, the purpose of
which is to delve more deeply into specific aspects of the nature and role of mental
computation in primary classrooms. Specifically, the following questions are
addressed:

1. What are the recent developments in mathematics education of relevance


to mental computation?

30

2. What is the place of mental computation within the calculative process and
particularly its relationships with computational estimation?
3. What is the nature of mental computation as perceived by mathematics
educators, both contemporaneously and historically?
4. What are the roles currently perceived for mental computation within and
beyond the classroom?
5. What are the affective and cognitive components, including commonly used
mental strategies, that constitute skill in computing exact answers
mentally?
6. What are the affective and cognitive characteristics exhibited by skilled
mental calculators, including the role that memory plays in the process of
calculating exact answers mentally?
7. What are the teaching approaches and sequence necessary for the
development of mental computation skills?

2.3

Recent Developments in Mathematics Education of


Relevance to Mental Computation
In proposing what was effectively Australia's first national statement on basic

mathematical skills and concepts for children to effectively participate in Australian


society, the Australian Mathematics Education Project (1982, p. 4) highlighted that
any such statement needed to be time-dependent. Contrary to the belief that
mathematics is the least dynamic of school subjects, the fundamental essential
learnings cannot be considered as an absolute. This being the case, most countries
in the western world are currently developing recommendations for school
mathematics to meet the needs of future adults in the Information Age, an age in
which "the world of work in the twenty-first century will be less manual but more
mental; less mechanical but more electronic; less routine but more verbal; and less
static but more varied" (NRC, 1989, p. 11).
The exact nature and method of delivering the recommendations are
dependent upon the unique socio-political environment in which mathematics
education in a particular country is to occur (AAMT, 1996b, p. 6). Australia's A
National Statement on Mathematics for Australian Schools constitutes "a framework

31

around which systems and schools may build their mathematics curriculum....It is
descriptive rather than prescriptive" (AEC, 1991, p. 1). This is in contrast to
England's national curriculum, which has legislative status for the teaching and
learning of mathematics (NCMWG, 1988). Whatever the nature of the statements
for the future of mathematics education, given the close contact between
mathematics educators throughout the world, together with the increasing
interdependence of nations, there are many issues of agreement and common
concern, issues that centre on the concept of numeracy, computation, number
sense and learning mathematics.

2.3.1 Numeracy
The development of numeracy (or mathematical literacy) is commonly held as
a key purpose for studying mathematics in school. Willis (1990, p. 9) considers
such development to be a service to students. It should equip them with skills and
understandings necessary for successfully dealing with other aspects of their
livesin the home, in the workforce and across the curriculum. As the technological
demands in relation to work and social interactions within a society increase, the
way in which numeracy is conceptualised also needs modification (NRC, 1989, p.
8).
This suggests that a wide definition needs to be formulated. In common with
recommendations of the Cockcroft Report (1982, pp. 10-11, paras 35-39) and the
position paper by the National Council of Supervisors of Mathematics (1989), the
Queensland Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a,
p. 2) suggests that although there is a continuing need for students to develop
competence and confidence in computational skills, they also need to develop a
much broader competency, one that includes an ability to apply understandings of
number, space and measurement in realistic situations, both familiar and unfamiliar,
to be able to use a range of technological aids, and to recognise the
reasonableness of results.

2.3.2 Computation

32

In the late 1970s Girling (1977) provocatively, for that time, defined basic
numeracy as "the ability to use a four-function electronic calculator sensibly" (p. 4).
Whereas the accepted view of numeracy is now much broader than this, as
delineated above, Girling's statement draws attention to the profound effect that the
availability of calculators (with computers and other electronic calculating devices)
has had on society and therefore on the features of school mathematics relevant to
the present technological age.
Australia's national statements on the use of calculators in schools (AAMT,
1996a; Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers & Curriculum Development
Centre [AAMT & CDC], 1987) reflect the commonly held belief that school
mathematics needs to capitalise upon the power of the calculator. The statement
not only recommends that all students at all year levels (P-12) should use
calculators, but that they should be used both as an instructional aid and as a
computational tool during the learning process across the curriculum.
The impact of electronic calculating devices on the nature of calculation within
present western societies requires the number strands of mathematics curricula to
be reassessed. This applies particularly to their goals and to the computational
procedures with which children are expected to become proficient. Rathmell and
Trafton (1990) assert that because "most complex computation is now done by
calculators and computers...paper-and-pencil procedures can no longer be the
focus of computation in the curriculum" (p. 54). Such a view is not new. Before the
calculator (and personal computer) age, Biggs (1969) cautioned that "we must be
quite clear...about out [sic] purpose in continuing to include written computational
practice as a part of the primary school curriculum, as this will determine the nature
and extent of this aspect of the work" (p. 25). To the extent that written algorithms
continue to be taught they need to be ones that are relatively easy to learn and be
ones that assist in the development of concepts and processes (Lindquist, 1984, pp.
602-603).
With the implementation of the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus
(Department of Education, 1987a) in Queensland in 1988 a greater emphasis is
given to the use of calculators, at least at the level, in Howson and Wilson's (1986,
p. 37) terms, of the intended curriculum. Some written algorithms have been
deleted from the primary school curriculummultiplication of common
fractionswhereas others have been delayeddivision by two-digit numbers.

33

However, mastery of the traditional paper-and-pencil algorithms for the four


operations remains the chief goal of the number strand. In contrast, Willis (1992, p.
4) points out that Australia's national statement takes the view that children need to
be helped to develop sensible methods for calculating, many of which may be
idiosyncratic with respect to the child or to the particular task. Further, many of
these methods are likely to be mental rather than written. The statement asserts
that "students should regard mental arithmetic as a first resort in many situations
where a calculation is needed" (AEC, 1991, p. 109), a view supported by the United
Kingdom's National Curriculum: Mathematics for Ages 5 to 16 (NCMWG, 1988, p.
9).
In reviewing and restructuring the number strands of mathematics curricula, a
shift in emphasis is required to ensure a realistic balance among the various forms
of computation (B. J. Reys & R. E. Reys, 1986, p. 4). Trafton and Suydam (1975, p.
531) have suggested that the study of computation should promote broad, longrange goals of learning. Children should develop confidence in their ability to learn
and perform mathematics, and gain insights into number ideas and relationships,
through their ability to discover patterns and form generalisations, the culmination of
which is the development of an ability to reason mathematically to solve problems of
significance to the individual.

2.3.3 Number Sense


McIntosh (1990a) stresses that the importance of being able to handle numbers
and to compute is not diminished by the availability of calculating devices:

What we all need to become, are thinking calculators with an ability to adapt
and improvise methods and to test quickly the reliability of results produced by
machines. And we need, now more than ever in this calculator age, a welldeveloped and flexible sense of number. (McIntosh, 1990a, p. 31)

Although number sense cannot be defined precisely (Hope, 1989, p. 12), and
is dependent upon the number system being used (Sowder, 1992, p. 6), Howden
(1989) suggests that it can be described "as good intuition about numbers and their

34

relationships" (p. 11). It may be elusive and difficult to pin down (Greeno, 1991, p.
36), but there are identifiable characteristics in the behaviour of those who have a
well developed feel for number. The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics
(1989, pp. 39-40) believes that children with good number sense (a) have wellunderstood number meanings, (b) have developed multiple relationships among
numbers, (c) recognise the relative magnitudes of numbers, (d) know the relative
effects of operating on numbers, and (e) have developed referents for measures of
common objects and situations within their environment. Sowder (1992, pp. 5-6)
and Resnick (1989b, p. 36) extend this list to include the ability to (a) perform mental
computations with nonstandard strategies that take advantage of the ability to
compose and decompose numbers, (b) use numbers flexibly to estimate numerical
answers to computations and to realise when as estimate is appropriate, and (c)
judge the reasonableness of solutions obtained, dependent upon their belief that
mathematics makes sense and that they are capable of finding sense in a numerical
situation.
Whereas the development of number sense is something that has always
occurred in many classrooms, the pivotal role that it plays in the ability of individuals
to respond flexibly and creatively to number situations requires its development to
be viewed as a major goal of primary school mathematics (AEC, 1991, p. 107;
MSEB & NRC, 1990, p. 46). Markovits (1989, p. 78) suggests that when students
with number sense are given a mathematical task, they are expected to have in
mind that there is not always one answer, that there is not always one algorithm,
that mathematics and real life are related, and that decisions and judgements are
expected.
Further, students with number sense tend to analyse the whole problem first,
rather than immediately applying a standard algorithm. They look for relationships
among the numbers, and with the operations and contexts involved. The
computational procedure chosen or invented takes advantage of these observed
relationships. At each step in the solution process individuals with number sense
seem to be aware of the mathematical reasonableness of what is being done and of
the answers obtained (Markovits, 1989, p. 79). Hence, Carroll (1996, p. 3) suggests
that an individuals number sense can be assessed through an analysis of the
mental strategies used when calculating.

35

2.3.4 Learning Mathematics


Current developments related to the teaching and learning of mathematics,
including the development of mental computation skills, are fairly consistent across
developed countries (Literacy and Numeracy Diagnostic Assessment Project, 1991,
p. 7). These are based on beliefs which hold that:
Learning is enhanced where children are placed in problem solving
situations.
Learning requires opportunities for active involvement and reflection.
Learning is enhanced where recognition is given to what the child already
knows.
Learning is highly tuned to the situation in which it occurs.

Such beliefs place the child at the centre of the learning process where
personally meaningful solutions can be developed. Mathematical power is gained.
This requires classroom environments to be "cultures of sense-making" (MSEB &
NRC, 1990, p. 32), environments in which the mathematics presented is seen to be
predictable, purposeful and personally relevant. That this view of mathematics was
not an outcome of the curricula of the new maths era was one factor in their failure
to meet the goals set, particularly those with respect to developing positive attitudes
towards mathematics and to being able to think purposively and effectively in
mathematical situations.
This child-centred focus is consistent with the constructivist approach to
learning that has its contemporary genesis in the work of Piaget who believed that
children learn through the assimilation and accommodation of new with existing
knowledge. Learning mathematics is an active, problem solving process in which
social interaction plays an important role. In the view of Yackel, Cobb, Wood,
Wheatley, and Merkel (1990, pp. 12-13), the problems that arise when attempting to
communicate are equally valuable learning experiences as the problem tasks
themselves. Further, providing for reflection on experiences allows children time to
link new to existing knowledge, an essential process for the expansion and
refinement of current understandings (AEC, 1991, p. 17).

36

Learning as acquiring situated knowledge is interrelated with the beliefs that


learning is knowledge-dependent and a process of knowledge construction
(Resnick, 1989a, p. 1). Lave (1985) suggests that people engage in mathematical
tasks in richly varied ways in different situations. When engaged in arithmetical
situations:

People changed problems, decomposed and recomposed them in ways


that reflected the organisation of the activity at hand as well as the
structure of the number system, and often turned the social and physical
environment into a calculating device. (p. 173)

This contrasts with school mathematics in which the procedures traditionally


taught are designed to be context free and therefore universally applicable. Howson
and Wilson (1986, p. 21) suggest that there are three types of mathematics. These
are: (a) ethnomathematics, the idiosyncratic ways in which people in particular
socio-cultural groups think about and engage in mathematical tasks; (b) school
mathematics; and (c) higher (pure) mathematics. If mathematics is to be meaningful
to the child, school mathematics needs to embody clear links among the three
types. Only then will children gain mathematical power and therefore learn "that
mathematics can help in the solution of their problems and in their own decision
making" (Howson & Wilson, 1986, p. 22).
The syllabi of the 1960s attempted to create links between school mathematics
and higher mathematics by transplanting the aims, methods and structures of the
latter onto the former with the concomitant ignoring of the needs of the student. A
task for the 1990s is to develop ways in which key features of the
ethnomathematical domain can be incorporated into school mathematics. Fostering
classroom environments in which, for example, idiosyncratic computational
strategies are accepted and valued would be a step towards this goal.

2.4

The Calculative Process


Although the demands of the technological age require that a broad view of

basic skills be taken, the development of appropriate skills for calculating provides

37

students with useful tools for everyday life, for the workplace, and for learning other
topics where calculative proficiency is required. The aim, therefore, is not to
develop calculative skill per se, but to provide children with skills useful for solving
problems, making applications and exploring new knowledge in mathematics and
other subject areas (Coburn, 1989, p. 47). With the ready availability of hand-held
calculators, as well as other electronic calculating devices, together with the need to
be able to flexibly adopt and improvise methods of calculation, "the thrust of
curriculum reform...is not to reduce the importance of computation but rather to
broaden the definition of computation and to elevate the importance of problem
solving" (Coburn, 1989, p. 47), in context with the development of number sense.
Paper-and-pencil procedures continue to be appropriate for situations requiring
a written record or in which the numbers are too complicated for mental calculation
but not so unwieldy as to require a calculator (Rathmell & Trafton, 1990, p. 155).
However, A National Statement on Mathematics for Australian Schools (AEC, 1991,
p. 109) presents the view that mental calculation should be the method of first
resort, particularly for less complex calculations where the numbers are easy to
work with, and where there is no need for recording partial answers. Further, mental
calculation is involved when computing with paper-and-pencil and with a calculator:
known facts need to be recalled, and estimates need to be mentally calculated as a
check on the reasonableness of the solutions obtained. Hence mental calculation is
central to the calculative process, and closely linked to paper-and-pencil and
technological calculation.
This position is encapsulated in Figure 2.1. This model of the calculative
process, while based on that in the Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School
Mathematics (NCTM, 1989, p.9) and on that devised by Rathmell and Trafton (1990,
p. 154), recognises that, for any individual, the computational process is firmly
embedded in their sense of number, computation being considered as a dimension
of number sense (Trafton, 1992, p. 9). Whatever the method of calculationmental,
paper-and-pencil, or technologicalits proficient use is likely to involve the
decomposition and recomposition of numbers, and is associated with the
characteristics of number sense delineated in Section 2.3.3 (NCTM, 1989, pp. 3940; Resnick, 1989b, p. 36; Sowder, 1992, pp. 5-6).
The need for computation most commonly arises from problem situations
(NCTM, 1989, p. 9). In such situations an individual needs to be able to (a)

38

recognise that computation is required, (b) formulate the calculation by deciding


what operations to use, (c) choose an appropriate method of calculation, (d) carry
out the calculation, and (c) interpret the solution obtained in terms of its
reasonableness to the characteristics of the situation. The latter may result in
changes to the nature of the answer required or to the method of calculation.
Additionally, the model (Figure 2.1) recognises that the method chosen to carry
out the calculation is dependent upon a range of factors that includes:

The nature of the operation to be performed: the specific operation, its


degree of complexity, the type and size of the numbers involved.

39

Situation Requiring
Calculation

Calculative tools
available

Operation required

Selection of Appropriate Method of


Calculation

Confidence felt

Degree of precision
required

Emotional state
Exact Answer

PAPER-AND-PENCIL
CALCULATION

Approximate Answer

MENTAL CALCULATION

TECHNOLOGICAL
CALCULATION

Precision - Critical

Precision - Noncritical

MENTAL
COMPUTATION

COMPUTATIONAL
ESTIMATION

Exact Answer

Approximate Answer

Reasonableness of Answer

Figure 2.1.

NUMBER SENSE

A model of the calculative process highlighting the central position of


mental calculation (Adapted from: NCTM, 1989, p. 9; Rathmell &
Trafton, 1990, p. 154)
The degree of precision required: whether an approximate or exact answer
is more appropriate.

40

The availability of particular calculative tools: the human brain, writing


materials, electronic calculating devices.

The degree of confidence felt with respect to the mathematics embedded in


the situation and to the use of the particular computational tools available.
The level of confidence with which a calculative task is approached is
influenced by the emotional state of the individual.

Irrespective of the approach takenmental calculation, paper-and-pencil,


technologicalthe solution may be an exact or approximate answer, dependent
upon the needs of the calculative situation. With respect to mental calculation,
Figure 2.1 suggests that mental computation and computational estimation are
interrelated. The latter involves calculating exact answers using approximate
numbers, derived from the numbers embedded in the problem task environment
(Silver, 1987, p. 41), to provide a reasonable estimate of the true answer to the
problem. Hence, during computational estimation a limited range of numbers are
manipulated as compared to that for mental computation. This and other features of
the relationship between mental computation and computational estimation are
explored in Section 2.6.

2.5

The Nature of Mental Computation


In developing an understanding of mental computation it is necessary not only

to consider the ways in which it has been defined, but also to consider other terms
which have been used to refer to the mental calculation of exact answers.
Additionally, the links between mental computation as taught in schools and the
methods used in non-classroom settings need to be analysed. Essential to this
analysis are the characteristics of mental procedures which distinguish them from
written methods.

2.5.1 Mental Computation Defined


Whereas a focus on approximate answers is a relative newcomer to the
mathematics curriculum in primary schools, mental computation is "not a stranger"
to the history of mathematics education (B. J. Reys, 1986a, p. 22). The use of the

41

term mental computation to refer to the process of producing an exact answer


mentally without resort to calculators or any external recording device, usually with
nontraditional mental procedures (Hazekamp, 1986, p. 116), based on conceptual
knowledge (R. E. Reys et al., 1995, p. 324), is, however, relatively recent. Until the
1980s the most frequently used terms to describe such calculations were oral
arithmetic and particularly mental arithmetic (or simply mental). These terms lack
the preciseness of mental computation as their use often included the calculation of
approximate as well as exact answers.
Although not discounting the role paper-and-pencil and technological
techniques may play, the process of mentally producing an answer that is not exact
but sufficiently close so that the necessary decisions in a problem situation can be
made is now commonly referred to as computational estimation. (R. E. Reys, 1984,
p. 551). Mental computation is an "important component of estimation [see Figure
2.1] in that it provides the cornerstone necessary for the diverse numeric processes
used in computational estimation" (R. E. Reys, 1984, p. 548).
By focussing on the nature of the answers provided rather than solely on their
accuracy, concern is shifted from the surface features of the computational process,
such as the modes in which computational situations are presented, to the thinking
strategies that are involved in calculating an answer. "Children's construction of
increasingly powerful thinking strategies goes hand in hand with their development
of increasingly sophisticated conceptual understandings" (Cobb & Merkel, 1989, p.
71), understandings that lead to children viewing mathematics as making sense.
Although mental computation is now the term most commonly used in the
mathematics education literature to refer to calculating exact answers mentally,
other terms remain in use. Mental calculation is used by some writers (Cockcroft,
1982; Hope, 1986a; Hope & Sherrill, 1987). However, their usage is in contrast to
that contained in the Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School Mathematics
(NCTM, 1989, p. 9), and by Hope (1986b). In the latter instances, mental
calculation is employed as a general term for representing all mental procedures,
whether producing exact or approximate answers (see Figure 2.1). Other writers
continue to use mental arithmetic, albeit redefined to refer only to the process for
calculating exact answers mentally (Allinger & Payne, 1986; McIntosh, 1990a,
1990b; Treffers, 1991). This redefinition is accompanied by a recognition that the
way skill with mental computation should be developed needs to be a departure

42

from the "series of short, low-level unrelated questions to which answers


are...calculated instantaneously and the answers written down with speed",
characteristics of conventional mental arithmetic lessons (McIntosh, 1990a, p. 40).

2.5.2 Mental and Oral Arithmetic


Traditionally, mental arithmetic has been used as a generic term for all mental
calculation, equivalent to the use of mental calculation in this study. It was typically
defined as "arithmetic done without the aid of paper and pencil" (Flournoy, 1954, p.
148), and usually without a clear initial indication whether exact or approximate
answers were being considered. Arithmetic problems presented verbally were also
called mental problems or oral problems (Hall, 1947, p. 212). The term oral
arithmetic was used in the 1952 and 1964 Queensland mathematics syllabuses to
represent all mental calculations (Department of Public Instruction, 1952b, p. 2;
Department of Education, 1964, p. 2). However, its usage referred primarily to
calculating exact answers, with very few references to finding approximate solutions.
Much discussion is contained in the professional literature from the first-half of
this century, particularly in that originating in the United States, concerning the
correctness of the terms oral arithmetic and mental arithmetic. As Hall (1954, p.
351) points out, many authorities attempted to avoid controversy by using the terms
interchangeably, whereas others referred solely to oral arithmetic or to mental
arithmetic. Some (Suzzallo, 1912; Thompson, 1917, cited in Hall, 1954; Thorndike,
1922), however, made a distinction between the two. Their concern was not simply
with the method of calculation being a mental one. Of equal importance were the
nature of presentation and the mode of response. Thompson (1917, p. 270, cited in
Hall, 1954, p. 351) contended that oral arithmetic could be classified as mental
arithmetic, but that all mental arithmetic is not necessarily oral. It could be classified
as oral if the calculation was started by a spoken question, or its result was recorded
in speech (Thompson, 1917, p. 270, cited in Hall, 1954, p. 351; Thorndike, 1922, p.
262). However, the stimulus for a mental calculation need not be oral. It could be
something that has been read or merely thought about. Further, the solution could
be recorded mentally or in writing, and not necessarily communicated orally.

43

From a somewhat different perspective, Suzzallo (1912, p. 75, cited in Hall,


1954) uses oral to refer to the situation where a "child works aloud, that is,
expresses his procedure step by step in speech" (p. 352). Mental arithmetic was, to
Suzzallo, the silent method. Thorndike (1922) believed that a devotion to oralness
or mentalness per se was simply fanatical...[as] oral, written, and inner
presentations of initial situations, oral, written, and inner announcements of final
responses, and oral, written, and inner management of intermediate processes have
varying degrees of merit according to the particular arithmetical exercise, pupil, and
context" (p. 263).
Thorndike's (1922, p. 263) concern, however, was with pedagogical issues
rather than with any real consideration of how a child may conduct the inner
management of a calculation. In his view, the merit of a particular calculative
method depended on such factors as: (a) the ease with which tasks could be
understood, (b) the ease with which work could be corrected, (c) the ways in which
cheating could be prevented, (d) the freedom from eyestrain, (e) the amount of
practice a class would receive per hour spent on a task, and (f) the cheerfulness
and sociability of the work to be undertaken.
Given the restrictions that could be placed on the mental activities that could
be classified as oral, Hall (1954) argued that mental arithmetic was the expression
that should be used exclusively, and that it should have the following meanings:

(1) Arithmetic problems which arise (a) in an oral manner, (b) in a written form,
or (c) "in the head" of the person who needs to solve the problem; (2) problems
in which pencil and paper and other mechanical devices, such as calculators,
are not used to record the intermediate steps between the statement of the
problem and its answer; (3) problems in which pencil and paper are used, and
problems in which they are not used to record the answer; and (4) problems in
which quick estimations are made which either may or may not be verified by a
written response. (pp. 352-353)

Except for point four, these criteria are ones which remain applicable to the use
of mental computation to refer to the mental calculation of exact answers, as now
used in the mathematics education literature and in this study. With concern shifting
from modes of presentation and response to the cognitive processes involved, oral

44

now has a much broader meaning than the narrow usage outlined above. This is
reflected in Cockcroft's (1982, pp. 92-93, paras 315-317) use of the expression
mental mathematics to refer to the mental and oral work that should form a major
part of the mathematics undertaken in classrooms. Its usage highlights that the
promotion of mathematical discussion is as important as a consideration of the
mental strategies employed, with both teachers and students benefiting from the
discussion of strategies that ensues (Cockcroft, 1982, p. 93, para 317)

2.5.3 Mental Computation and Folk Mathematics


The issue of school mathematics versus ethno- or folk mathematics has as its
focus the way mathematical tasks are accomplished. Central to this are the roles
paper-and-pencil, calculators and mental calculation play in the calculative process.
In particular, the concern is with the appropriate balance between, and the nature
of, written and mental methods of calculating. School mathematics continues to be
primarily concerned with standard paper-and-pencil methods of calculating
(McIntosh, 1990a, p. 25; Willis, 1990, p. 12). Nevertheless, as French (1987, p. 41)
points out, it is becoming increasingly unusual for these standard algorithms to be
used anywhere except in the classroom. Reporting findings from the Bath and
Nottingham studies which provided background information for his report, Cockcroft
(1982, p. 20, para 71) indicates that in industry a range of idiosyncratic and "back of
an envelope" methods is used, especially for long multiplication and division.
For school mathematics to be useful, it should reflect the techniques used in
everyday life (Willis, 1990, p. 9), and, in so doing, "assist children to cultivate and
enlarge their inherent affinities and abilities for folk mathematics" (Maier, 1980, p.
21). Proficient folk mathematicians are skilled at calculating exact and approximate
answers mentally ( Carraher et al., 1987, p. 94; Maier, 1980, p. 23) and in the use of
calculators (J. P. Jones, 1988, p. 44).
From an analysis of data from research undertaken in such every-day
situations as a supermarket, a commercial dairy, and home kitchens, Lave (1985)
concluded that not only are arithmetic and problem solving techniques situationally
specific, but also "there are theoretically crucial ways in which people are similar in
how they vary" (Lave, 1985, p. 172). One is that the arithmetic procedures used are

45

often more complex than those many of the users learnt in school. Secondly,
making errors is a natural part of a process for arriving at correct arithmetic
solutions. Terezinha Carraher et al. (1987) observe that in out-of-school situations,
the results obtained by young Brazilian market sellers, "even when wrong, were
sensible because there was a continuous monitoring of the quantities during the
computation procedure; in [mental] procedures, children seem to know where they
are' at any given point" (p. 94). With respect to division strategies, Murray, Olivier,
and Human (1991, pp. 50, 55) note that most children invent powerful non-standard
algorithms, some of which are mental, in parallel with those learnt in school.
Further, children are significantly more successful when they use their own
procedures rather than the standard algorithms (Murray et al., 1991, p. 50).
In school-like settings, "people [tend] to produce, without question, algorithmic,
place-holding, school-learned techniques for solving problems, even when they
could not remember them well enough to solve problems successfully" (Lave, 1985,
p. 173). Ginsburg, Posner, and Russell (1981, p. 173) indicate that when schooled
American and Dioula children added mentally, they frequently used written
algorithmic procedures and were incorrect about twenty-five percent of the time, a
finding which may reflect the lack of emphasis given to the development of personal
procedures for computing mentally in classrooms. In Lave's (1985, p. 175) view, the
standard written algorithms taught in schools de-contextualise arithmetic, are
cumbersome and inappropriately require pencil and paper to be used most of the
time.
School mathematics should therefore be organised to provide opportunities for
children to deal with mathematics in their own environments in ways similar to those
of folk mathematicians (Maier, 1980, p. 23; Masingila et al., 1994, p. 13). In such
situations people use a variety of techniques, often mental, and invent units with
which to compute (Lave, 1985, p. 173). Gladwin (1985) suggests that:

Learners need to have a strong sense of the problem and the


contradictions that must be resolved to reach a solution. Take away this
strong sense of problem and we are left listening to the
calculation...without knowing the purpose of the calculation. (p. 209)

46

This has implications for how problems are presented to children within
classrooms. Murtaugh's (1985, p. 192) analysis of how shoppers mentally solve
arithmetic problems in a supermarket suggests that, in many real-world situations
problem formation and problem solving are likely to be integral parts of a single
process. In contrast to the prepackaging of traditional school problems, with or
without a veneer of real-world characteristics, "when people are free to formulate
their own problems...the relevant inputs may be as negotiable as the eventual
solution" (Murtaugh, 1985, p. 189). In making best-buy decisions, shoppers use
qualitative features of items on the supermarket shelves to narrow the choice to two
items for which some mental arithmetic calculation is undertaken to select the item
for purchase (Murtaugh, 1985, p. 192).
As a step towards closing the gap between folk and school mathematics,
mental computation (with computational estimation and calculator usage) should
receive greater prominence in the mathematics which children undertake in
classrooms and should replace written methods as the basic computational skill of
the computer age (MSEB & NRC, 1990, p. 19). The National Research Council
(1989, p. 46) intimates that an emphasis on the development of number sense, with
which mental computation is closely entwined, should move children beyond a
narrow concern for school-certified computational algorithms.
Such algorithms have no intrinsic merit (Willis, 1992, p. 11). They were
developed to make use of the technology of paper-and-pencil and designed to be
used without the need to think about the numbers involved (Jones, 1988, p. 42;
McIntosh, 1992, p. 136). For this reason, Hope (1986a, p. 50) suggests that
standard paper-and-pencil algorithms may contribute to a fragmentary view of
numbers and number relationships and therefore not support the development of
number sense. Individuals, who are proficient with their use, work with digits and
book-keeping rules. They focus on "the written symbols, thereby losing track of both
the meaning in the transactions they are quantifying and the meaning within the
quantification system" (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 95). In contrast, meaning is
preserved during mental calculations. Hence, the major differences between mental
and written procedures arise from the degree of knowledge required of an individual
about the problem situation (Nunes, Schliemann, & Carraher, 1993, pp. 53-54). In
Skemp's (1976) terms, opportunities for the development of relational understanding
are restricted. However, with the demise of standard computational algorithms

47

opportunities arise to combine the development of number sense with the teaching
of computational procedures in useful and meaningful ways (Sowder & Schappelle,
1994, p. 344). To the degree that children continue to be required to perform paperand-pencil calculations, McIntosh (1990a, p. 37) suggests that at least one of the
following criteria should be met: (a) informal methods should be used to support and
extend the use of mental procedures, (b) their development should occur in a
problem solving context, (c) they assist in the development of number sense, and
(d) they are of intrinsic interest to children.
The calculator, in Hope's (1986a, p. 47) view, poses a greater threat to paperand-pencil procedures than do mental ones. When folk mathematicians use
calculators it is to replace the use of paper-and-pencil (Maier, 1980, p. 23). It
therefore "seems very unlikely that any children at primary school in the 1990s [sic]
will, as adults, make more than very sporadic use of [standard paper-and-pencil
methods] of calculation in a society in which calculators are commonplace"
(Curriculum Programmes Branch, Western Australia, 1989, p. 24). However, given
the propensity for individuals to take the easy way out, it is possible that calculators
could be regularly used in instances for which mental computation is more
appropriate. For this reason, Atweh (personal communication, May 13, 1992)
believes that calculators do pose a threat to mental procedures, as well as to paperand-pencil ones. It is therefore essential for students to be assisted in making
sensible choices about the planned method of calculation in particular
situationswhether to use a calculator, paper-and-pencil, or mental computation
(AAMT & CDC, 1987, p. 2).

2.5.4 Characteristics of Mental Procedures


Plunkett (1979, p. 3) proposes eleven characteristics of mental algorithms. He
suggests that such algorithms are often fleeting, variable, flexible, active, holistic,
constructive and iconic. They usually are not designed for recording, require
understanding, and often provide early approximations of correct answers.
Additionally, the range of contexts for which they are appropriate is limited. These
features are in contrast to those of the standard written algorithms which are
considered to be standardised, contracted, efficient, automatic, symbolic, general

48

and analytic. Further, they are not easily internalised and encourage cognitive
passivity (Plunkett, 1979, pp. 2-3).
Unlike written procedures which are, by definition, permanent and correctable,
mental strategies are often transitory. As Hope (1985, p. 358) reports in his analysis
of expert mental calculators, some appear incapable of explaining the processes
used. This may be due to their ability to engage in automatic processing (Shriffrin &
Schneider, 1977, cited in Jensen, 1990, p. 270) of information. Such processing is
"fast, relatively effortless, and can handle large amounts of information and perform
different operations on it simultaneously" (Jensen, 1990, p. 270). For non-expert
mental calculators, their capacity for short-term mental storage is a critical factor in
their ability to compute mentally and, by implication, in their ability to recall
procedures used. The rate of forgetting information stored in working memory is
directly proportional to the number of interpolated stages before its recall, and to the
total amount of information simultaneously being held (Hitch, 1977, p. 337). These
factors are a function of the particular procedure being employed for a mental
calculation.
Strategies for computing mentally are also characterised by their variability.
This contrasts with school-authorised paper-and-pencil procedures which are
standardised. This standardisation creates the impression that such procedures
may be easily taught and leads to the belief that their use is the correct and only
way in which calculations should be performed (Curriculum Programmes Branch,
Western Australia, 1989, p. 28), a conventional goal of school mathematics
programs. Plunkett (1979, p. 3) suggests that, for members of the public and many
non-specialist mathematics teachers, the concept of a particular operation and its
standard written algorithm are synonymous. This belief leads to the view that to
teach an operation, a method, rather than an idea, needs to be taught. It is this
assumption that confuses the issues relevant to the debate concerning the
appropriate mathematical concepts, processes and skills with which children should
become familiar.
In instances where children are permitted to use the mathematics that they
know, efficient mental procedures develop almost spontaneously (Sowder, 1990, p.
20). Such are personally meaningful and reflect the differences which exist in the
knowledge that children bring to the task (Yackel et al., 1990, p. 13). The variations
that can occur between strategies used by different children for the same task are

49

illustrated by the following finding of Cooper et al. (1992, p. 112). When calculating
the sum of 52 and 24, some Year 2 children used a counting-on procedure (count
on 24 with fingers by fives), whereas others used their knowledge of tens (50 + 20 =
70; 2 + 4 = 6). The latter strategy is based on a deeper understanding of number
than the counting strategy. Cooper et al. (1992, p. 113) report that children who
used tens in their strategies were able to progress to items beyond those which
consisted solely of basic facts or simple algorithms involving teens. These children
tended to exhibit diverse number understandings and hence were able to
manipulate numbers in sophisticated ways.
However, the knowledge, both conceptual and procedural (Hiebert & Lefevre,
1986), which an individual brings to a situation is not the only factor which governs
the nature of the strategy applied. The features of the problem context also bear on
a strategy's make-up. Mental strategies exhibit a flexibility which enables an
individual to use a particular form of a strategy, one that is matched to the nature of
the numbers involved. A general methoddistributingidentified by Hope (1987, p.
334), for calculating products involves the transformation of one or both of the
numbers to be multiplied into a series of sums or differences before calculating the
product using one of three strategies, namely, additive distribution, subtractive
distribution, and quadratic distribution. The particular strategy used is dependent
upon the numbers involved. For example, Hope (1987, p. 334) found that his
subject used additive distribution to calculate 16 x 72, namely 16 x (70 + 2) = 16 x
70 + 16 X 2 = 1120 + 32 = 1152. Whereas for 17 x 99, subtractive distribution was
used: 17 x (100 - 1) = 17 x 100 = 1700 - 17 = 1683.
In summary, the variability and flexibility of mental strategies are features that
arise naturally from the process of calculating mentally. Representative of this is
Jensen's (1990) observation of the procedures used by Shakuntala Devi, a
prodigious mental calculator. Solutions are obtained:

Through exercising different routines drawn from an immense repertoire


of numerical information and strategies, and the peculiarities of the
problem itself determine the elements that are drawn upon from this
repertoire to achieve the solution most efficiently. (Jensen, 1990, p. 270)

50

Standard paper-and-pencil algorithms discourage thinking (R. E. Reys, 1984,


p. 551), or in Williams' (cited in Plunkett, 1979, p. 3) terms, encourage cognitive
passivity, or suspended understanding. Such algorithms are designed to be
automatic, which permits their being taught to and used by students who have little
understanding of the processes involved (Plunkett, 1979, p. 2). Little, if any, thought
is given to why the procedure is being carried out in the way that it is. Irrespective
of the nature of the calculative situation, the same procedure is applied for each
operation. The recognition and use of the structural relationships embedded in the
situation is constrained, the use of alternate solution paths is deterred (R. E. Reys,
1984, p. 551).
In situations where mental computation is appropriate, the use of mental
methods allows an individual to select from a range of possible solution paths and
strategies. This leads Plunkett (1979, p. 3) to suggest that mental methods are
active ones. An individual is able to exercise some choice over the procedures
used, even though the user may not always be totally conscious of the reasoning
processes involved. Expert mental calculators give priority to selecting an
appropriate strategyone that allows an economy of effort. This is the fundamental
decision in calculating mentally and governs all that follows during the computation
process (Hunter, 1977a, pp. 36-37).
In contrast to the use of the standard written algorithms, mental computation
encourages thinking. Understanding is required to effectively arrive at a solution.
Plunkett (1979, p. 3) suggests that children who get their mental calculations correct
almost certainly understand what they are doing. However, care needs to be taken
in assuming that children's self-devised strategies will be necessarily understood
and that they will always provide correct answers. McIntosh (1991a, p. 5) found that
when children from Years 2 to 7 used a working from the left strategy, more
competent children produced incorrect answers on five occasions out of twentynine, whereas less competent children produced incorrect answers in fifteen
instances out of forty. This is indicative of "a potentially valuable strategy [being]
intuitively devised by different children, but in its embryonic form it is not yet efficient:
not all the features of the situation are understood and therefore correctly controlled
by the children" (McIntosh, 1991a, p. 5).
Standard written procedures are not easily internalised because these
methods do not match the ways in which people think about numbers (Plunkett,

51

1979, p. 3). In Reed and Lave's (1981, p. 442) terms, and supported by Carraher et
al. (1985, p. 28), the focus is on the manipulation of symbols rather than on the
manipulation of quantities as occurs during informal (folk) mental calculations. Such
an analytic approach is "divorced from reality" (Reed & Lave, 1981, p. 442), as
efficient use of the standard paper-and-pencil algorithms requires that the digits be
dealt with separately without reference to their meaning or their relationship to realworld or representational models. This view remains valid under current
approaches to developing children's skill with the written algorithms. These
approaches are based on developing an understanding of the processes involved
through relating each step to the manipulation of base-ten materials. However, the
goal continues to be the automatic processing of written calculations.
A manipulation-of-quantities approach, which characterises mental
computation procedures, allows children to make meaningful alterations to the
problems encountered and to work with quantities that can be easily manipulated
(Carraher et al., 1987, p. 94). The approach is a holistic one (Plunkett, 1979, p. 3)
in which people use convenient groups or invent units with which to calculate (Lave,
1985, p. 173). To calculate the cost of four coconuts at Cr$35.00 each, a 12 yearold street market seller in Brazil determined that "three will be 105, plus 30, that's
135...one coconut is 35...that is...140" (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 26). The calculation
involved using the cost of a coconut as the calculative unit, and the knowledge that
30 plus 5 is 35. Such a procedure is a constructive one, as the correct answer is
progressively built-up from an early approximation. This applies particularly where a
left-to-right approach is used (Plunkett, 1979, p. 3). Such mental calculations occur
in the context of complete numbers. For example, to calculate 24 x 50, a proficient
mental calculator may simply calculate 12 x 100 (Hope, 1985, p. 362). The digits
are not considered separately as occurs with the standard written algorithm, or its
mental equivalent that non-proficient mental calculators tend to use (Hope & Sherrill,
1987, p. 108).
Related to their meaningful use of the knowledge they have of numbers and
number relationships, some children are aware of their reference to, and
manipulation of, mental pictures (McIntosh, 1990c, p. 7; Olander & Brown, 1959, p.
100; R. E. Reys, B. J. Reys, Nohda, & Emori, 1995, p. 319). Plunkett (1979, p. 3)
refers to mental strategies as often being iconic. Children have an overall picture of

52

the numbers involved, often relating them to an icon such as a number-line. A


subject of McIntosh's (1990c) explains:

I just picture the numbers in my head somehow. I don't know how I do


it....When I think of sums I sort of close my eyes a little bit and picture
them all in my head...in a straight line....It's sort of like an arrow and when
I want to I can imagine it moving down the many spaces. (pp. 7-8)

Although mental methods can be recorded if the need arises, they are not
designed for doing so. This contrasts with written methods which Plunkett (1979)
describes "as contracted in the sense that they summarise several lines of
equations involving distributivity and associativity" (p. 2). Further, standard paperand-pencil algorithms are general, being applicable to situations involving all
numbers, large or small, whole number or decimal. Hence, there is a trade-off in
meaning and generalisability for both mental and written procedures (Nunes et al.,
1993, p. 54). Although mental algorithms preserve meaning, they are limited in their
usage, becoming grossly inefficient when dealing with large numbersfor example,
when multiplying through the chaining of successive additions, the preferred
strategy for multiplication identified by Carraher et al. (1985). Nevertheless, Plunkett
(1979, p. 3) has concluded that mental strategies are applicable to a greater range
of mathematical situations than a cursory analysis of classroom number work might
suggest.

2.6

Mental Computation and Computational Estimation


As illustrated in Figure 2.1, the distinction between mental computation and

computational estimation derives from the degree of precision required in arriving at


appropriate solutions. Threadgill-Sowder (1988) defines computational estimation
as "the process of converting from exact to approximate numbers and mentally
computing with those numbers to obtain an answer reasonably close to the result of
an exact computation" (p. 182). In problem solving contexts, the approximate
answer obtained needs to be sufficiently close to the exact answer to allow
appropriate decisions to be made.

53

2.6.1 Components of Computational Estimation


Computational estimation is a complex task (Sowder, 1989, p. 26). It involves
the co-ordination of two fundamental subskills: (a) converting from exact to
approximate numbers using nearness judgements and number comparison skills,
and (b) mentally computing with these numbers (Case & Sowder, 1990, p. 88).
Sowder (1989, p. 27) reports that, even though students may be skilled at both
approximation and mental computation, they may still not be able to arrive at
reasonable estimates. Case and Sowder's (1990) study provides support for this
view. In neo-Piagetian terms, it was hypothesised that children would not be
capable of coordinating the two components of computational estimation until they
were capable of vectorial thought at around age 12. During this stage children
exhibit "growth in their ability to coordinate two or more complex and qualitatively
different components of a task" (Sowder, 1989, p. 25). By around 17 years of age,
students are capable of estimating 188 + 249 + 296 + 6 using the same level of
significance for each approximation within the calculation (Sowder, 1989, p. 26).
Mental computation, computational estimation, and number comparison share
a common background characterised by factors essential to a well-developed sense
of number (see Figure 2.2). These include: (a) an understanding of place value
concepts related to whole numbers and decimals, (b) an ability to operate with
multiples and powers of ten, (c) an ability to use the properties of operations, and (d)
an understanding of the symbol systems used to represent numbers (ThreadgillSowder, 1988, p. 195). The four constructs, mental computation, computational
estimation, number comparison, and number sense do not develop in a strictly
hierarchical order. Rather, Threadgill-Sowder (1988, pp. 194-195) suggests that
each is dependent upon, while strengthening, the others in a spiral development,
with computational estimation developing slightly later than mental computation and
number comparison abilities associated with an increase in number sense.

54

Based on their study of the 1200 students in Years 7 to 12, Robert Reys,
Bestgen, Rybolt, and Wyatt (1982, pp. 196,198) reported that a common
characteristic of those who are proficient with computational estimation, is the

55

Prerequisite
Concepts and
Skills

Fundamental
Subskills

Estimation
Processes

Specific
Estimation
Concepts

Convert Exact to Approximate Numbers

Understand
symbol system

Compare
numbers by
size

Reformulation

Understand
place value
Compensation

Operate with
multiples and
powers of ten

Knowledge of
basic facts

Approximate
numbers are
used

The estimate is
an approximation

Appropriateness
of estimate
depends on
desired outcome

MENTAL
COMPUTATION

More than one


estimate is valid
Use properties
of operations

Translation

More than one


estimation
process is
permitted

Figure 2.2.

Components of computational estimation


(Adapted from Threadgill-Sowder, 1988, p. 192)

56

ability to quickly and efficiently calculate exact answers mentally, a finding


supported by Heirdsfield (1996, p. 128), particularly for addition by Year 4 children.
All demonstrated skill with operating with a limited number of digits or with multiples
of tens. The most proficient estimators displayed self confidence, used a variety of
strategies and were able to mentally compute with larger numbers, with more digits,
and with types of numbers other than whole numbers (R. E. Reys et al., 1982, pp.
198, 199). From a study of Year 8 students' performances on computational
estimation tasks, Rubenstein (1985, p. 117) concluded that the ability to multiply and
divide by powers of ten has an especially strong relationship with estimation
performance. Further, students who lack this ability have a reduced understanding
of the size of numbers.
An analysis of Figure 2.2 suggests that it may be possible to be simultaneously
competent at mental computation and very poor at computational estimation, a view
supported by Robert Reys (1984, p. 549). The ability to convert exact to
approximate numbers is one which needs to be developed before proficiency with
computational estimation is achieved. This ability depends upon being able to make
comparisons and to judge the relative size of numbers. Number comparison is
defined by Threadgill-Sowder (1988) as "the ability to order real numbers [according
to] size, such as in selecting the larger of two or more numbers, or by the ability to
compare different magnitudes, such as selecting which of two numbers is closer to a
third" (p. 183).

2.6.2 Computational Estimation Processes


Three key cognitive processesreformulation, compensation, and
translationare evident in the methods used by proficient computational estimators
(R. E. Reys et al., 1982; R. E. Reys et al., 1991). Reformulation, which is "heavily
dependent on the ability to compare numbers" (Threadgill-Sowder, 1988, p. 191), is
the one most often used to convert exact to approximate numbers. Although some
aspects of compensation and translation are dependent upon number comparison
skills and therefore inform the selection of appropriate approximate numbers, these
strategies are more directly involved with the mental computation process (see
Figure 2.2).

57

Traditionally, where the development of computational estimation skills has


been included in the curriculum, it has been confined primarily to approximating
numbers using the rounding convention, a reformulation strategy (R. E. Reys, 1984,
p. 551). Sauble (1955, p. 37) has suggested that for children to develop
competence in estimating they need to learn to round numbers and to recognise
situations in which the use of approximate numbers is more meaningful and useful
than exact numbers. This view is reflected by the finding of Schoen, Blume, and
Hart (1987, pp. 22-23) that, irrespective of grade and ability levels, middle-school
students equated estimation to a round to the closest approach, for whole numbers
and decimals. The students were observed to round numbers to the leading powers
of ten before mentally calculating. However, little use was made of compensatory
strategies or compatible numbers, with few attempts to refine initial estimates. The
estimation strategies used were not associated with conceptual understanding, even
in instances where this was encouraged by the test item.
Reformulation is defined as a "process of altering numerical data to produce a
more mentally manageable form...[while leaving] the structure of the problem intact"
(R. E. Reys et al., 1982, p. 187). It is characterised by two basic strategies. These
are the front-end use of numbers and the substitution of numbers by more
acceptable forms. The most common method for approximating numbers is
rounding, usually to the nearest five, ten, hundred, thousand, and so forth (Trafton,
1978, p. 200). Robert Reys et al. (1982, p. 197) report that this ability is evident in
all those who are proficient with computational estimation. Rounding is a variant of
a front-end use of numbers. The other common front-end approach is truncation, in
which the left-most digits of a number are used in a mental calculationfor example,
87,398 - 54,246 could be converted to 87 (thousand) - 54 (thousand).
The substitution of numbers by more acceptable forms usual occurs in one of
two ways. The first, using compatible numbers, involves rounding to more
convenient multiples of numbers within a problemfor example, substituting 5 for the
3 in 63 5. The use of this skill is less common than rounding to multiples of ten,
but is still exhibited by a majority of those who are proficient with computational
estimation (R. E. Reys et al., 1982, p. 197). A second common substitution strategy
is the use of equivalent forms of numbers to simplify the mental calculation. For
example, to find an approximate answer for 30% of $103, 30% could be replaced by

58

1/3. In this situation it is likely that $103 would be rounded to $105 to simplify the
mental calculation further. This highlights that reformulation (and translation and
compensation) strategies are not simply used in isolation. Proficient computational
estimators use a combination of strategies appropriate to the context in which a
mental calculation, designed to provide an approximate answer, is to occur.
Figure 2.2 indicates that both compensation and translation also depend on an
ability to compare numbers. This view is somewhat at variance with that of
Threadgill-Sowder (1988) who does not postulate a link between translation and
number comparison. Translation entails modifying the mathematical structure of a
problem to a form which is more easily managed mentally (R. E. Reys et al., 1982,
p. 188). Threadgill-Sowder (1988, p. 191) and Sowder and Wheeler (1989, p. 135)
categorise averaging, classified as a translation strategy by Robert Reys et al.
(1982, p. 188), as reformulation. Averaging is particularly useful for addition
problems in which the numbers cluster around a common value. An approximate
answer is calculated by multiplying the common value, which can be viewed as a
reasonable group average, by the number of values in the group (R. E. Reys, 1984,
p. 554). For example, for 6,146 + 7,200 + 5,300, an approximate answer could be
calculated by selecting 6,000 as the group average and multiplying it by 3. Although
a reformulation strategycompatible roundingis used, it can be argued that the
rounding occurs in context with a desire by the estimator to change the structure of
the problem and that this influences the particular approximations used in the
calculation, a strategy dependent upon an individual's ability to compare numbers.
Translation is also dependent upon factors relevant to the process of
calculating mentally. The structure of the problem is changed to simplify the mental
computation and this is contingent upon such factors as a child's knowledge of
number relationships and confidence with various computational methods. Robert
Reys et al. (1982) suggest that the process for translation is more flexible than that
for reformulation: "The student seems to have a panoramic view of the problem and
is less constrained by the numbers involved. In fact, very often the numbers and
operations are simultaneously altered to result in more manageable forms" (p. 189).
Trafton (1986, p. 25) suggests that an essential element of insightful estimation
is the ability to sense the relationships between approximate and exact answers. In
judging an estimate's appropriateness, it may be necessary to make adjustments to
take account of any numerical variation that may have occurred through the process

59

of translation or reformulation (R. E. Reys et al., 1982, p. 189). Compensation, as


this process is called, also relies on an ability to compare numbers, and is affected
by (a) the manageability of the numerical data, (b) the context of the problem, and
(c) the individual's tolerance for error (R. E. Reys et al., 1982, p. 189). Robert Reys
et al. (1991) report that Japanese fifth- and eighth-grade children were more likely to
adjust their estimates if compatible rounding, rather than straight rounding, had
been used. This suggests that "compatible rounding is more likely to leave the user
with a sense of the direction of the exact answer and therefore more likely to be
accompanied by compensation" (R. E. Reys et al., 1991, p. 49). As one Grade 8
student explained for 347 x 6 43: "That's about 347 x 6 42 or 347 x 1/7. This is
close to one-seventh of 350, which is 50; 350 is more than 347, so my estimate is
48" (R. E. Reys et al., 1991, p. 48).
Of the three key cognitive processes, Threadgill-Sowder (1988, p. 194)
suggests that compensation seems to be the one most closely linked to a
conceptual understanding of the computational estimation process. Nonetheless,
as Poulter and Haylock (1988, p. 28) have observed, adjustments often reflect an
intuitive feeling for number and are ones for which a rationale may not be easily
explained. Compensation need not only occur after an estimate has been
calculated. Adjustments may also be made during intermediate stages of mental
computation. Robert Reys et al. (1982, p. 190) report that some children, when
calculating an estimate for the total attendance at six Super Bowl games, selected a
group average for five of the attendance figures and ignored the sixth to
compensate for rounding upwards. Such a strategy appears to be more difficult
than compensating after an estimate has been calculated. In Robert Reys' et al.
(1982, p. 197) study, intermediate compensation is classified as a Level 3
characteristic, being evident in only 20-30% of the competent estimators
interviewed. In contrast, final compensation is classified as a Level 2 strategy and
is one that is in the repertoire of a majority of proficient computational estimators.

2.6.3 Comparison of Mental Computation and Computational Estimation


Barbara Reys (1986a, p. 22) suggests that whereas an approximate answer
can be obtained for every arithmetical problem through computational estimation,

60

the calculation of exact answers is limited to a subset of problems. The degree of


arithmetical complexity which can be mentally processed is dependent upon such
factors as an individual's (a) knowledge of useful strategies for mental calculation,
(b) ability to recall a large number of numerical equivalents, and (c) ability to
remember partial answers at various stages of a calculation (Hope, 1985, p. 358).
These factors are relevant to calculating exact as well as approximate answers
mentally. However, by definition, the degree of arithmetical complexity that has to
be processed during computational estimation is less than that when calculating an
exact answer. The first step in arriving at an approximate answer is to convert from
exact to approximate numbers so that an estimate can be more easily calculated
mentally (see Figure 2.2). It follows that the range of numbers dealt with when
mentally computing approximate answers is a subset of those that may be required
to be manipulated when calculating exact answers. Conventional rounding and
truncation usually produce an even number of tens, hundreds or thousands with
which to compute mentally. Conversion to compatible numbers typically results in
an operation for which the result is able to be retrieved from long term memory, or is
easily calculated. This applies not only to reformulation per se, but also when frontend and compatible number strategies are used in conjunction with translation and
compensation.
When calculating exact answers, all digits in each of the numbers to be
operated on must be taken into account. Some of the strategies used, which are
analysed in detail in Section 2.7.4, bear some relationship to the processes used to
derive approximate numbers in computational estimation. In contrast to written
procedures, those skilled in mental computation often proceed from left-to-right
(Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 108; Carraher et al., 1987, p. 94), a front-end strategyfor
example, 38 x 5 is calculated as: (30 x 5) + (8 x 5) = 150 + 40 = 190 (Hazekamp,
1986, p. 118). Some form of compensation is also used by some children when
calculating exact answers mentally. Olander and Brown (1959, p. 99) outline the
following method for calculating 34 - 9: 34 - 10 = 24, 24 + 1 = 25, albeit a confusing
approach for those who have difficulty in working out which way to compensate
(Sowder, 1992, p. 41). The similarity between these approaches and their
computational estimation counterparts adds weight to the belief that mental
computation, computational estimation, number comparison, and number sense
develop in a spiral fashion, as suggested by Threadgill-Sowder (1988, pp. 194-195).

61

Some methods used to arrive at more precise estimates may aid children in
their development of strategies for calculating exact answers. For example, in
calculating a closer estimate for 378 + 236 + 442, children could consider the tens
digit in each number, or the tens and ones in each, to adjust an initial estimate of
900, increasing it by 140 or 160, respectively. Addition examples such as this are
possibly ones for which primary school children may not normally be expected to
calculate exact answers mentally. Nevertheless, experiences with getting closer
strategies during computational estimation may assist children to develop flexible
approaches to calculating exact answers. Where the numbers and operations
involved are within the capability of an individual to calculate mentally, getting closer
strategies may ultimately result in turning estimates into exact answers (Irons,
1990b, p. 1).
Strategies used by those skilled in mental computation, however, go beyond
those which bear any direct relationship to those used in computational estimation
(see Figure 2.1, Tables 2.1 & 2.4). A characteristic of highly skilled mental
calculators is their ability to perceive number properties and relationships which may
be useful for calculating an exact answer (Hope, 1985, p. 358; Hunter, 1977a, p. 36;
B. J. Reys, 1986b, p. 3279-A). Numbers are decomposed and recomposed in ways
which are not necessarily based on conventional base-ten place value relationships.
For example, Hope (1987, p. 335) reports that Charlene, a highly skilled mental
calculator, calculated 87 x 23 in the following steps:

87 x 23

= (29 x 3) x 23
= 29 x (3 x 23)
= 29 x 69
= 69 x (30 - 1)
= 69 x 30 - 69
= 2070 - 69
= 2001

Such an approach requires sufficient working memory capacity to be able to coordinate and monitor many interrelated calculation activities without losing track of
the calculation (Hope, 1987, p. 335).
In summary, mental computation and computational estimation are processes
that are closely related. Each is performed mentally, taking advantage of the
structural properties and relationships among numbers. Both are used to check

62

whether or not an answer from a calculator or paper-and-pencil calculation is


reasonable (R. E. Reys, 1984, p. 551)a reflective process that Shigematsu,
Iwasaki, and Koyma (1994, p. 20) call metacomputation. As represented in Figure
2.1, some prerequisite skills are shared. As well, people proficient with mental
computation and computational estimation exhibit similar cognitive and affective
characteristics and use strategies which bear some similarity.
Nonetheless, there are essential differences between mental computation and
computational estimation. Although computational estimation produces many
different solutions, all of which are reasonable and acceptable, mental computation
is concerned with producing answers that are either correct or incorrect. Mental
computation is a vital prerequisite to computational estimation (see Figure 2.2).
However, in Robert Reys' view (1984, p. 551), the reverse does not apply, a belief
that is in conflict with that of Irons (1990a, p. 31) referred to above.
In the sections that follow a detailed analysis of a number of issues central to a
deeper understanding of mental computation is presented. These issues include:
(a) cognitive and affective components of mental computation, (b) the
characteristics of proficient mental calculators, (c) the process of computing
mentally, and (d) pedagogical issues.

2.7

Components of Mental Computation


As previously intimated, although approximate answers may be determined for

all arithmetical problems, the range of problems for which exact answers may be
found mentally is limited (B. J. Reys, 1986a, p. 22). This situation is not merely due
to the numerical complexity of many arithmetical tasks. Of equal importance is a
consideration of an individual's understanding of, and competence with, the
essential components of mental computation.
Little research is available on which to base a comprehensive analysis of the
components of mental computation. Flournoy (1957) suggests that in situations
requiring exact answers, a person must be able to:

Recognise the problem and organise the facts of the problem.

Keep the numbers in mind as s/he thinks about the problem.

63

Perform the necessary arithmetical process or processes, without paper


and pencil, and reach a decision about the problem. (p. 148)

Flournoy's (1957, p. 148) first point is one that is not peculiar to mental
computation. Whatever the mathematical task, an individual needs to be able to
recognise what the problem is and be able to organise the perceived facts so that a
solution can be determined. The second requirement depends on working memory
capacity and the way in which it is used during processingin Hunter's (1978, p.
339) terms, their memory for interrupted working. The degree to which working
memory is burdened during mental calculation is largely dependent upon the
particular strategy being used, an issue that is explored in Section 2.8.3. By
arithmetical process, Flournoy (1957, p. 148) is simply referring to the particular
operationaddition, subtraction, multiplication or divisionthat has to be applied. Of
greater importance is a consideration of the particular mental strategies that are
used to carry out these operations. In any analysis of the components of mental
computation, not only should the range of strategies used be considered but also
the factors that underpin their proficient use.
In the absence of a detailed analysis of the components of mental computation
in the literature, it is considered that Sowder and Wheeler's (1989, p. 132) model for
specifying the components of computational estimation is one that can provide a
framework for discussion. Sowder and Wheeler (1989, p. 132) classify the
components into four categories: (a) Conceptual Components, (b) Related Concepts
and Skills, (c) Skill Components, and (d) Affective Components.
Conceptual components are defined as those that relate to a basic
understanding of what the process of finding an estimate entails (Sowder &
Wheeler, 1989, p. 131). For this analysis, conceptual components of mental
computation are deemed to be those that relate to an understanding of the basis of
the process for calculating exact answers mentally. Sowder and Wheeler (1989, p.
131) define related concepts and skills as those abilities that are known, or
suspected, to indirectly influence an individual's proficiency with finding approximate
solutions. With respect to computational estimation, the ability to compute mentally
is classified as a related skill (Sowder & Wheeler, 1989, p. 132). Given the
delineation of prerequisite concepts and skills relevant to mental computation in its
relationship with computational estimation (see Figure 2.2), it is considered

64

appropriate that this category is also relevant to an analysis of the components of


mental computation per se.
Research into the categorisation of strategies for calculating exact answers
mentally has not yet resulted in such a well-defined classification as that for the
processes used to convert exact to approximate numbers. Sowder and Wheeler
(1989) define these components as those "that seemed more appropriately listed as
skills, even though they do not appear in students' work without accompanying
evidence of the conceptual understanding necessary to employ them as skills" (p.
131). The classification of the components of mental computation presented in
Table 2.1 highlights strategies classified as relying on relational understanding.
Such strategies reflect a constructivist view of mental computation which posits that
mental strategies are based on an individual's intuitive understanding of numbers
and of the manipulations required (R. E. Reys et al., 1995, p. 305).
It is hypothesised that mental computation strategies relevant to particular
contexts cannot be devised, understood or performed successfully without the

65

Table 2.1
Components of Mental Computation
1.

Affective components

2.

Display confidence in ability to calculate mentally.


Display confidence in ability to do mathematics.
Recognise that mental computation is useful.
Recognise that procedures for computing mentally can make sense.

Conceptual components
Recognise arithmetical contexts for which mental computation is
appropriate.
Accept more than one strategy for obtaining an exact answer mentally.
Recognise that the appropriateness of a strategy depends on the context of
the calculation.

3.

Related concepts and skills


An ability to:
Translate a problem into a more mentally manageable form.
Key questions:
(a) How can the numbers be expressed to obtain questions which can be
answered by recall?
(b) How will the operational sequence proceed as result of the way that
the numbers have been expressed?

Understand and apply place value concepts.


Recall basic facts related to the four operations.
Operate with multiples and powers of ten.
Compose and decompose numbers and to express them in a variety of
ways.
Link numeration, operation and relation symbols in meaningful ways.
Recall and use a wide range of relationships between numbers, including
whole numbers, fractions, decimals and percents.
Use the associative and commutative properties of addition and
multiplication.
Use the distributive properties of multiplication and division.
Recognise the need for and undertake compensations necessitated by
transformations to the numbers involved.

Table 2.1 cont.

66

Components of Mental Computation


4.

Heuristic strategies based upon relational understanding

Add or subtract parts of the first or second number

Use fives, tens and/or hundreds

add-up

decomposition

compensation

Work from the left

organisation

incorporation

Work from the right

mental analogue of standard written algorithm

place-grouping

use known facts

Use factors

general factoring

half-and-double

aliquot parts

exponential factoring

iterative factoring

Use distributive principle

additive distribution

subtractive distribution

fractional distribution

quadratic distribution

Note.

The four hypothesised categories for analysing the components of mental


computation are based on Sowder & Wheelers (1989, p. 132) model for the
components of computational estimation.

67

support of appropriate conceptual understandings, related concepts and skills, and


affective characteristics. It is therefore proposed to analyse the components of
mental computation using the following categories: (a) Affective Components, (b)
Conceptual Components, (c) Related Concepts and Skills, and (d) Strategies for
Computing Mentally.

2.7.1 Affective Components


When learning mathematics, "it is not the memorization of mathematical skills
that is particularly important...but the confidence that one knows how to find and use
mathematical tools whenever they become necessary" (NRC, 1989, p. 60). Mental
computation is one such tool, one with which students generally do not display
proficiency and prefer not to use, even in instances where mental calculation is
appropriate (Carpenter et al., 1984, p. 487; B. J. Reys et al., 1993, p. 314; R. E.
Reys, 1985, p. 15; R. E. Reys et al., 1995, p. 323). If children are to become
proficient with mental computation, they need to develop confidence in their ability to
calculate mentally (see Table 2.1 & Figure 2.1).
It has been argued previously that mental computation, computational
estimation and number sense are closely interdependent. The development of a
good sense of number requires confidence in dealing with numerical situations
(AEC, 1991, p. 107). Further, Sowder and Wheeler (1989, p. 132) suggest that
such confidence is an important affective component of computational estimation. It
is therefore reasonable to assume that the degree of confidence with which children
approach mental computation tasks is influenced by the degree of confidence felt in
their ability to perform mathematical tasks in general, particularly those related to
numerical situations (see Table 2.1).
Children gain confidence and ownership of the mental procedures they use if
they are allowed to create, construct and discover mathematics for themselves
(Payne, 1990, p. 3), particularly where the mathematics is encountered in
meaningful situations. They come to view mathematics, and procedures for
computing mentally, as making sense and useful, as evidenced in the findings of
Carraher et al. (1985). It is therefore proposed that the affective components of
mental computation should also include a recognition that (a) mental computation is

68

useful, and (b) procedures for computing mentally should make personal sense (see
Table 2.1).

2.7.2 Conceptual Components


As highlighted in Figure 2.1, in any situation requiring a calculation, children
need to be able to determine whether paper-and-pencil, mental or technological
calculation is the most appropriate method. This depends on the nature of the
operation, the degree of precision required, the availability of particular calculative
tools, and the level of confidence felt with regard to the mathematics embedded in
the situation. It is therefore hypothesised that a fundamental conceptual component
of mental computation is the ability to recognise arithmetical contexts for which
mental computation is appropriate (see Table 2.1).
Given that in any particular context children display varying levels of
confidence, with respect to their ability to compute mentally, there can be no fixed
criteria for defining contexts appropriate for mental computation. This component
needs to be operationalised with respect to particular individuals in particular
contexts. Howden (1989) suggests that "students who can make judgements about
the reasonableness of computational results and realize that more than one way
can be used to arrive at a solution gain confidence in their ability to do mathematics"
(p. 7).
In Sowder and Wheeler's (1989) delineation of the components of
computational estimation, consideration is given to multiple processesthat is, the
"acceptance of more than one process for obtaining an estimate" (p. 132). In their
classification, this component is not specifically tied to the need to convert exact to
approximate numbers nor to the mental computation component of computational
estimation. However, one of the defining characteristics of mental algorithms, as
discussed beforehand, is their variability (Plunkett, 1979, p. 3). Those skilled at
mental computation use a variety of strategies for calculating exact answers
(Carraher et al., 1987, p. 91; Hope, 1987, p. 331; Hunter, 1978, p. 340).
Accordingly, it seems reasonable to propose that an important conceptual
component of mental computation is an acceptance of more than one strategy for
obtaining an exact answer mentally (see Table 2.1).

69

Hope (1987) contends that "success in mental [computation] depends more on


the ability to select the right tool for the job' than upon the possession of some
innately superior mechanism for processing information" (p. 339). The right
toolmental computation strategydepends not only on an individual's store of
conceptual and procedural knowledge, but also on the context in which the
calculation is to be undertaken. In parallel with Sowder and Wheeler's (1989, p.
132) suggestion that appropriateness needs to be a consideration when formulating
the components of computational estimation, a third conceptual component of
mental computation is proposed, namely, people engaged in mental computation
need to recognise that the appropriateness of a strategy for computing mentally
depends on the context of the calculation (see Table 2.1).

2.7.3 Related Concepts and Skills


Barbara Reys (1989, p. 72) notes that Paul Trafton suggested during a
conference on number sense that there are possibly two levels of mental
computation, each distinguished by the nature of the strategies used to determine
the exact answers and the nature of the numbers involved in the operation. The first
of the two levels does not generally necessitate an invented strategy to be applied,
but does require a method for determining place value of the answer (B. J. Reys,
1989, p. 72). Such computational problems often involve operating with powers of
ten or multiples of ten, and are of the nature of the mental computations required
during computational estimation. At this level, the mental operations required are
essentially extended basic facts (B. J. Reys, 1989, p. 72), but can also involve
numbers other than whole numbers. With respect to operations involving simple
common fractions, + for example, Barbara Reys (1989, p. 72) suggests that,
although conceptual understanding is essential to obtaining the correct answer, it is
unlikely that an invented strategy would be requiredat least for individuals who
have a sound understanding of common fractions.
Trafton's second level of mental computation (cited by B. J. Reys, 1989, p. 72)
concerns problems for which an in-depth knowledge of the properties of the
numbers involved is generally required, as well as a self-developed procedure
understood by the userfor example for 99 x 7 (B. J. Reys, 1989, p. 72). Sowder

70

(1992, p. 5) suggests that the ability to perform mental computations using invented
strategies is a behaviour that indicates the presence of number sense. Problems
which require the invention of strategies are those more likely to be encountered
when the purpose of calculating mentally is the determination of exact answers (see
Figure 2.1). For example, one method for calculating 7 x 99 relies on the application
of the distributive law of multiplication: 7 x 99 = 7 x (100 - 1) = (7 x 100) - (7 x 1) =
700 - 7 = 693. This method also relies on a knowledge of, what Hazekamp (1986)
calls, special products"products that are easily found by multiplying by a power of
10 or a multiple of a power of 10" (pp. 117-118).
Threadgill-Sowder (1988) approached an analysis of the components of mental
computation from a somewhat different, though related, perspective to that of
Trafton. A principal component of mental computation is the ability to translate a
problem into a more mentally manageable form (Hunter, 1977a, p. 25; B. J. Reys,
1985, p. 46). Two key questions need to be asked and answered, albeit
unconsciously in many instances. These are: (a) How can the numbers be
expressed to obtain basic fact questions? and (b) How will the operational sequence
proceed as result of the way that the numbers have been expressed? (ThreadgillSowder, 1988, p. 184)
Threadgill-Sowder (1988) suggests that "because there is never just one way
to answer Question 1, that is, to choose a format for the numbers to be calculated,
mental [computation] is a very creative, inventive act" (pp. 185-186). For those who
are skilled with mental computation, it is likely that their aim is to express the
numbers in a form which can be related to elements in their store of number
knowledge and relationships that extends beyond the basic facts. Hope (1987, p.
335) reported that Charlene, an expert thirteen year-old mental calculator,
calculated 16 x 72 by reasoning 16 x (70 + 2) = (16 x 70) + (16 x 2) = 1120 + 32 =
1152. It is likely that Charlene used place value knowledge to express 72 as 70 + 2
so that she could draw on her store of products that included 16 x 70 = 1120. This
suggests that Threadgill-Sowder's (1988, p. 184) first question should be rephrased
to increase its generality, namely, How can the numbers be expressed to obtain
questions which can be answered by recall? (see Table 2.1)
"Skilled mental calculation demands that the user search for meaning' by
scanning the problem for salient number properties and relationships" (Hope,
1986a, p. 52), a view supported by Barbara Reys (1986b, p. 3279-A). Olander and

71

Brown (1959, p. 99) report a number of ways in which children calculated 51 - 34.
As suggested by Threadgill-Sowder (1988, p. 184), the operational sequence in
each case is dependent upon the way in which the numbers to be operated upon
are expressed. Included among the methods observed by Olander and Brown
(1959, p. 99), for 51 - 34, were:
34 + ?(6) = 40, 40 + ?(11) = 51; Therefore 51 - 34 = 17 (6 + 11).
34 + 10 = 44, 44 + ?(7) = 51; Therefore 51 - 34 = 17 (10 + 7).
34 + ?(1) = 35, 35 + ?(15) = 50, 50 + ?(1) = 51;
Therefore 51 - 34 = 17 (1 + 15 + 1).
50 - 30 = 20, 20 - 4 = 16; Therefore 51 - 34 = 17 (16 + 1).
51 = three 17s, 34 = two 17s; Therefore 51 - 34 = (one) 17.
During a calculation "numbers [obtained] are re-expressed in ways that lead to
basic fact [or recall] questions, and the computations carried out as a
result...present new mental calculations calling for a new cycle of questions"
(Threadgill-Sowder, 1988, p. 185). For example, in relation to the first method, 34 +
?(6) = 40 was either recalled, or calculated using basic fact knowledge (10 - 4 = 6),
and to move from 40 to 51 required question one to be reconsidered. As with the
first step, 40 + ?(11) = 51 may have merely been recalled, or it may have been
calculated using basic fact knowledge (1 - 0 = 1, 5 - 4 = 1). In each case it is
evident that the way in which the numbers and the perceived relationships between
them are expressed determines the way in which the answer is calculated.
From the above analysis, for Threadgill-Sowder's (1988) first question to be
answered successfully, children need to be able to:

Recall basic facts.

Understand place value concepts (72 is 70 + 2).

Compose and decompose numbers to express them in a variety of


waysthat is, to draw on their store of numerical equivalents (51 is 3 x 17 or
40 + 11).

Operate with multiples and powers of ten (Use 5 - 3 to solve 50 - 30, or


recognise that 40 x 800 is 4 x 10 x 8 x 100).

(Threadgill-Sowder, 1988, p. 185)

72

However, in view of the rephrasing of question one, the list of related concepts
and skills (see Table 2.1) should be extended to include the ability to recall and use
a wide range of relationships between numbers. Cockcroft (1982, p. 92, para 316)
provides support for the first two components. He suggests that efficient mental
procedures are based upon an understanding of place value in association with an
ability to recall addition and multiplication facts. The ability to compose and
decompose numbers is dependent upon well-developed part-whole relationships
(Ross, 1989, p. 47) and is closely related to place value knowledge. An
"understanding of place value and ownership of its essential features is crucially
important in opening up more efficient and more simple [mental] strategies"
(McIntosh, 1991a, p. 5). Sowder (1992, p. 4) lists the ability to compose and
decompose numbers as one which demonstrates some presence of number sense.
As their number sense grows, children demonstrate increased flexibility in the way
they think about numbers. As Ross (1989) points out, ultimately "their thinking
allows them mentally to compose wholes from their component parts, decompose
whole quantities into parts, and perhaps rearrange the parts and recompose the
whole quantity, confident all the while that the quantity of the whole has not
changed" (p. 47).
Hiebert (1989, p. 82) believes that written arithmetic symbols can function in at
least two ways: (a) as records of something already known, and (b) as tools for
thought. A well developed sense of number requires that numerals, operation and
relation signs operate in both ways. This leads Sowder (1992, p. 5) to suggest that
a key behaviour indicative of the presence of number sense and critical to mental
computation (and numeration and computational estimation) is the ability to link
numeration, operation and relation symbols in meaningful ways (see Table 2.1).
Employing symbols as tools necessitates that they be treated as objects of thought.
There is evidence to suggest that proficient mental calculators use symbols in this
manner, as reflected by their physically-oriented language. They often refer to
chopping or breaking numbers apart when describing their mental techniques
(Hiebert, 1989, p. 83).
Although the use of numbers as objects of thought contributes to the
development of number sense, through advantage being taken of the properties of
the system (Hiebert, 1989, p. 83), in Trafton's (1989) view, "number sense is more
related to intuitions and insights associated with numbers as quantities, rather than

73

numbers as abstract, formal entities" (p. 74). As discussed previously, proficient


mental calculators use a manipulation-of-quantities approach. Terezinha Carraher
et al. (1987, p. 94) reported that children, when calculating mentally, altered the
problems presented so that they were able to work with more manageable
quantities. This is reflective of the approaches observed by Olander and Brown
(1959) outlined above. Children were not operating with symbols per se, but with
symbols given meaning by their relationships to place value and part-whole
knowledge.
With respect to the Threadgill-Sowder's (1988) second question"How will the
operational sequence proceed as result of the way that the numbers have been
expressed?" (p. 184)children should be able to:
Regroup terms using associative and commutative properties of addition
and multiplication: 54 - 31 = (50 + 4) - (30 + 1) = (50 - 30) + (4 - 1).
Use the distributive properties of multiplication and division: 16 x (70 + 2) =
(16 x 70) + (16 x 2).

Multiply by powers of 10 (as for question one).

(Threadgill-Sowder, 1988, p. 185)

However, as previously described, each question is considered cyclically


during the mental computation process. Skills listed by Threadgill-Sowder (1988, p.
185) as applying to Question 2 may in fact be used to aid in answering Question 1,
as in Charlene's use of the distributive property (Hope, 1987, p. 335) to produce a
form which could be related to her store of number knowledge. Hence, in providing
a summary of essential concepts and skills in Table 2.1, each is not specifically
related to Threadgill-Sowder's (1988) two key questions.
An additional skill related to proficient mental computation is the ability to
recognise the need for and undertake compensations necessitated by modifications
to the numbers involved (see Table 2.1). One of the strategies observed by Olander
and Brown (1959, p. 99) is classified as compensatory rounding, the fourth
approach to solving 51 - 34 listed above. This approach requires that 1 be added to
the partial result (16) to compensate for initially rounding 51 to 50 before
sequentially subtracting 30 and 4. As discussed previously, this strategy is

74

conceptually similar to final compensations which may be necessary to derive closer


estimates during computational estimation (see Figure 2.2). The ability to
compensate during computational estimation and mental computation is indicative
of a well developed sense of number (Sowder, 1992, p. 6). Behr (1989, p. 85)
suggests that there are two aspects of, what he calls, variability: (a) a transformation
of one of the operands in combination with a compensatory transformation of the
answer, the case outlined above, and (b) compensatory transformations which are
applied to each of the operands prior to an answer being calculated. Although
Behr's (1989, p. 85) analysis is specifically related to number sense and
computational estimation, Sowder (1992, p. 6) suggests that the ability to
compensate for numerical transformations plays a critical role in mental
computation.

2.7.4 Strategies for Computing Mentally


Since the mid-nineteenth century recognition has been given periodically to the
importance of mental computation as a component of education, employment, and
everyday living (R. E. Reys, 1984, p. 549). Nevertheless, it is only relatively recently
that there has been any empirical interest in the mental processes that underlie the
ability to calculate exact answers mentally (Vakali, 1985, p. 106). The research that
has been undertaken focuses primarily on mental strategies associated with
determining the basic factsaddition, subtraction, multiplication, and division
operations with numbers from zero to nine.
The identification, analysis and classification of mental computation strategies
beyond the basic facts has received little attention (McIntosh, 1990b, p. 1; Vakali,
1985, p. 107). Of the studies that have been carried out, analogous to those
associated with identifying and classifying basic fact strategies, most have focussed
on addition and/or subtraction with whole numbers (Beishuizen, 1985, 1993; Cooper
et al., 1992; Cooper, Heirdsfield & Irons (1996); Ginsburg et al., 1981; Hamann &
Ashcraft, 1985; Heirdsfield, 1996; Hitch, 1978; Murray & Olivier, 1989; Olander &
Brown, 1959; Resnick, 1983; Resnick & Omanson, 1987; Vakali, 1985; van der
Heijden, 1995). Multiplication (Carraher et al., 1985; Gracey, 1994; Hope, 1985,
1987; Hope & Sherrill, 1987) and division, in particular, have received scant

75

consideration. Four studies considered all four operations (Carraher et al., 1987;
Carroll, 1996; McIntosh, 1991a; B. J. Reys, 1986b). In some studies (Cooper et al.,
1992; Ginsburg et al., 1981; Hamann & Ashcraft, 1985) operations with basic facts
were included in the examples used. However, clear distinctions between the
strategies used for these and the strategies that may have been used to mentally
calculate with the larger numbers, should they be different, have not always been
clarified (Ginsburg et al., 1981; Hamann & Ashcraft, 1985). Consequently, the
following synthesis of available data to formulate a single system of strategies for
mentally computing with numbers greater than nine (see Tables 2.1 to 2.4), using
the work by McIntosh (1991a, 1990c) as a framework, needs to be considered
preliminary, and therefore interpreted and applied with caution.
In undertaking research to identify and classify mental strategies, McIntosh
(1990b) believes that "it is very difficult to escape the need for subjective judgments
on the part of the interviewer or the analyser of the protocols as to the interpretation
of the child's description of its mental activity" (p. 13). Such identification is all the
more difficult in instances where attempts to identify strategies are based on written
reports of past performances by expert adult mental calculators. Hope (1985, p.
358) noted that many of these reports are vague, with the writer not giving a clear
indication of the methods used by the calculator. Further, many professional mental
calculators kept the strategies they used as closely guarded secrets. Others found
difficulty in explaining their computational techniques.
The analysis of data arising from the paucity of research is confused, not only
by the subjective nature of the identified strategies, but also by the variations in
sample characteristics. The samples vary with respect to age, grade, ability, and
cultural background. Although the majority of studies centre on strategies used by
randomly selected children, some focused on those used by children categorised
according to their mental computation ability (Hope, 1985, 1987; Hope & Sherrill,
1987; McIntosh, 1991a; Olander & Brown, 1959; B. J. Reys, 1986). The latter
sampling technique is the more useful approach, particularly where the aim is to
identify efficient mental strategies, ones that could become the focus when providing
learning experiences to develop children's mental computation abilities. The age of
subjects ranged from approximately six years for children in Grade 1 (Hamann &
Ashcraft, 1985) to adulthood (Hitch, 1977, 1978; Hope, 1985). Some studies
investigated strategies used by children in various year-levels (Cooper et al., 1996,

76

Grades 2-4 longitudinally; Hope & Sherrill, 1987, Grades 11-12; Hamann & Ashcraft,
1985, Grades 1, 4, 7 & 10; McIntosh, 1991a, Grades 2-7; Olander & Brown, 1959,
Grades 6-12).
A further variable to be taken into consideration when analysing the research is
the size of the numbers contained in the numerical situations. The majority of
studies focused on operations with one- and two-digit numbers. Those that involved
three-digit numbers and beyond are generally ones in which senior students or
adults formed part or all of the samples (Ginsburg et al., 1981; Hitch, 1977, 1978;
Hope, 1985; Hope & Sherrill, 1987; Olander & Brown, 1959; Petitto & Ginsburg,
1982). Exceptions to this are Hope's (1987) study of a 13 year-old skilled mental
calculator and that of Carraher et al. (1987), which investigated the strategies used
by third-grade Brazilian children, whose ages ranged from eight to thirteen years.
Few studies have included examples employing common fractions, decimal
fractions or percentages (R. E. Reys et al., 1995).
Contexts used in the studies include informal out-of-school settings and formal
school situations in which the operations are most commonly context-free. In some
studies, the numbers to be operated upon were embedded in word problems in an
endeavour to add a life-like quality to the operations (Carraher et al., 1987; Cooper
et al., 1992; Vakali, 1985). Context-free examples may be presented in a horizontal
or vertical format, thus providing an additional variable that may influence the use of
a particular mental strategy. Operations presented vertically increase the likelihood
of paper-and-pencil procedures being used to calculate mentally (Cooper et al.,
1992, p. 105). Terezinha Carraher et al. (1987, p. 89) reported that children were
more likely to use mental approaches when solving simulated store situations and
word problems, with context-free computational exercises being more likely solved
by written procedures.

Models for Classifying Mental Strategies


A number of researchers have proposed models for the classification of
strategies used to calculate mentally beyond the basic facts. McIntosh's (1991a,
1990c) system is the most comprehensive of these. This model, based on data
from children in Grades 2 to 7 who were presented with context-free examples

77

involving the four operations, classifies strategies into clusters dependent upon such
factors as whether an understanding of place value is evident, or whether it was
simply a matter of using known facts. Other classifications have been based, not
only on the particular strategies used, but also on the observed general
predispositions towards mental computation (Cooper et al., 1992). The results from
a longitudinal study, initially with Grade 2 children's approaches to addition and
subtraction, by Cooper et al. (1992), suggest that general predispositions may be
considered on two continua, namely, (a) the propensity to visualise numbers, and
(b) the way in which children perceive number situations. Although some children
do not appear to have the ability to visualise numbers, others demonstrate "a strong
ability..., particularly with arrays, and to use this visualisation in their counting"
(Cooper et al., 1992, p. 114). The strategies used are hypothesised to be directed
by three different approaches to thinking: (a) a feel for number, (b) a feel for
strategy, and (c) a feel for process.
Feel for number is characterised by verbal reports such as "eight is two less
than ten" and reflects a tendency to compose and decompose the numbers to make
the operation more manageablea related concept and skill discussed above (see
Table 2.1). Cooper et al., (1992) reported that "children with feel for strategy tend to
be fixated on their strategy and attempt to apply it seemingly without regard for
efficiency" (p. 115). Such children often rely on counting-on and counting-back by
twos, fives and tens. These two approaches are not necessarily used in isolation.
Each may be used with the other, or in association with the approach that reveals a
more global conception of their taskfeel for process (Cooper et al., 1992, p. 115).
The researchers reported that the children who consistently use a feel for process
approach are demonstrating the most marked growth in their ability to compute
exact answers mentally. Such an approach indicates a more acute sense of
number and operation, as reflected by such comments as: "I want the number to go
up" and "Adding makes things bigger" (Cooper et al., 1992, p. 115).
In subsequent research, Cooper, Heirdsfield, and Irons (1995, p. 9) considered
the approach to operation and the method of calculation, together with a subject's
attitude to numbers and approach to process, in determining the category into which
a child's strategy would be classified. In categorising a child's approach to an
operation, such factors as the method of subtractiontake-away, missingaddendwere considered. Consideration was also given to the method of

78

calculation. For example, whether a solution was achieved through recall or


counting.
Hamann and Ashcraft (1985, p. 54) distinguish between preparation strategies
and solution strategies. Preparation strategies are those that relate to the way in
which complex number situations are approached. In Hamann and Ashcraft's
(1985, p. 54) terms, complex addition occurs in examples with sums greater than
18. Their complexity stems from there being at least one two-digit addend,
sometimes requiring carrying. The most common preparation strategies are
classified as: (a) begin with the 1's column, and (b) begin with the 10's columnrightto-left and left-to-right approaches, respectively.
A solution strategy is one used to describe the way in which a sum, or
component sum, is reached. Three solution strategies are reported: "counting,
statement of a memory operation, and statement of an arithmetic fact" (Hamann &
Ashcraft, 1985, pp. 66-67). However, the distinction between the last two strategies
is unclear. Commenting on these two categories, McIntosh (1990b) asserts that "if I
know a fact, then I hold it in memory: if I remember that 3 + 3 = 6, then I know it as a
fact. The child's words may be different, but the strategy is the same" (p. 15).
Whether an answer to a calculation can be retrieved from long-term memory
depends upon an individual's store of numerical equivalents. Given that most
people have access to number relationships that extend to varying degrees beyond
the basic facts, it could be hypothesised that the strategies used to calculate beyond
the basic facts evolve in a manner comparable to those related to the development
of basic fact knowledge. Svenson and Sjoberg (1982), from a longitudinal study,
over Grades 1 to 3, which analysed children's strategies for subtraction with
numbers less than or equal to 13, reported that, in general, "the development of the
children's cognitive processes involved a gradual shift from more primitive and less
demanding memory strategies...to reconstructive memory processes...to retrieval
processes" (p. 91), that is, a shift from using external memory aids to reconstructing
basic facts in working memory, to recalling the answer from long-term memory.
In identifying the components of mental computation (Table 2.1), it is the
reconstructive memory processes that are of primary concern. These processes are
the ones that should provide the focus in any attempt to develop children's mental
computation skills. "In a reconstructive process the answer is reached through a
series of more or less conscious derivations or manipulations in working memory to

79

reach the answer" (Svenson & Sjoberg, 1982, p. 91). The complexity of the
manipulations increases proportionally to the complexity of the numerical situation
(Vakali, 1985, p. 112). An increase in complexity may be due to a change in size of
the numbers involved, a factor that influences the implementation of particular
mental strategies (R. Cooper, 1984, cited in Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 4). With
increased number size, complexity may be added by the need to carry and borrow
during addition and subtraction, respectively, procedures that are avoided by more
proficient mental calculators (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 108).
As discussed previously, Trafton (cited in B. J. Reys, 1989, p. 72) proposes
two levels of mental computation. The first level incorporates problems that
primarily require routine operations with powers of ten and multiples of powers of
ten. In contrast, the reconstructive strategies, which typify the second level, depend
on a person being able to use self-developed techniques that are dependent upon a
knowledge of the properties of numbers and operations (B. J. Reys, 1989, p. 72).
Such strategies require adaptive expertise, rather than routine expertise (Hatano,
1988, cited in Sowder, 1992, p. 19). Adaptive expertise necessitates an
understanding of how and why particular strategies work and how they can be
modified to suit the characteristics of particular numerical situations.
A key aspect of an individual's conceptual knowledge relevant to mental
computation is an understanding of place value concepts (see Table 2.1). Murray
and Olivier (1989, pp. 5-7) propose a model describing four increasingly abstract
levels of computational strategies with two-digit numbers. The type of strategy at
each level is linked to prerequisite number and numeration knowledge. It is
suggested that the distinction between pre-numerical (count-all) and numerical
(counting-on, bridging-the-ten) strategies evident in basic fact calculations remains
for two-digit numbers (Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 5). A further distinction, which may
also apply to larger numbers, can be made between the numerical strategies based
on counting and the heuristic strategies that do not entail countinga distinction
originally proposed by Carpenter (1980, p. 317) with respect to the acquisition of
addition and subtraction concepts.
Heuristic strategies are ones that "often involve the decomposition of one or
more of the numbers in a problem in order to transform the given problem to an
easier problem or series of problems" (Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 5). Carraher et al.
(1987) refer to the two types of mental strategies identified in their

80

studydecomposition and repeated grouping"as heuristics, so as to emphasize the


flexibility of the solutions" (p. 91). It is suggested by Murray and Olivier (1989, p. 4)
that when children are required to compute with numbers outside their range of
constructed numerosity, they regress to more primitive strategiesthat is, they may
rely on a counting strategy rather than on an heuristic one.
With respect to two-digit numbers, and based on research involving contextfree additions presented orally, written horizontally and written vertically to Grade 3
children, Level 1 of Murray and Olivier's (1989, pp. 5-6) model is characterised by
the pre-numerical strategy of counting-all. Children who rely on this strategy have
not yet acquired the numerosities of numbers within the range presented, although
their names and numeral forms are known. No meaning is assigned to individual
digits within a number, with, for example, "the symbol group 63...regarded as a way
of spelling' the number name" (Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 5).
At Level 2, the numerosities of numbers within the range under consideration
have been acquired. This allows the use of counting-on strategies. However, such
strategies, when used with larger two-digit addends, become laborious and prone to
error (Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 6). Based on chronometric data, and supported by
verbal reports, Resnick and Omanson (1987, p. 66) distinguish between three forms
of count-on strategies related to the addition of a two-digit number and a single-digit
number. These are: min of addends, sum of addends and min of units.
The thinking which characterises Murray and Olivier's (1989) Level 2, together
with that for Level 3, is a necessary prerequisite for understanding the intricacies of
two-digit numeration and computation (Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 9). At the third
level "the child sees a two-digit number as a composite unit, and can decompose or
partition the number into other numbers that are more convenient to compute with,
for example to replace 34 with 30 and 4" (Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 6). These
abilities provide the conceptual basis for the use of heuristic strategies and, at least
for the Grade 3 children in the sample, are usually based on place value knowledge
(see Table 2.1). Murray and Olivier's (1989, p. 6) research suggests that Level 3
understanding is sufficient for developing powerful strategies for computing
mentally. This is before a full understanding of base ten numeration has been
achieved which "necessitates the conceptualization of ten as a new abstracted
repeatable (iterable) unit which can be used as a unit to construct other numbers"
(Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 3). Resnick and Omanson (1987, p. 70) have observed

81

that few primary grade children, in the United States of America, decompose and
recompose counting numbers based on knowledge of their decimal structure.
The conceptualisation of ten as an iterable unit typifies Level 4 understanding,
as defined by Murray and Olivier (1989, pp. 6-7). Children are able to conceive of
two-digit numbers as consisting of groups of tens and some ones. Murray and
Olivier (1989, p. 6) point out that, at Level 3, children work with tens as numbers, but
at Level 4 work with ten as an iterable unit. Hence, Level 3 children view 67, for
example, as 60 and a 7 and not as 6 tens and 7 ones. Although McIntosh (1990b)
considers that "the distinction, though defended by the authors, is not entirely clear
for mental computation" (p. 17), Level 4 understanding, in Murray and Olivier's
(1989) view, allows for "a progressive schematization (shortening') and abstraction
of the Level 3 heuristic strategies" (p. 7). To illustrate this distinction, a Level 3
solution to 36 + 27 is: "Take the six and the seven away, thirty plus twenty is fifty;
now add six, then add seven. This compares to a Level 4 strategy: "Thirty plus two
tens, that's fifty. Six plus seven is thirteen, that's sixty-three" (Murray & Olivier,
1989, pp. 6-7).
Terezinha Carraher et al. (1987) have suggested that mental computation "can
no longer be treated merely as idiosyncratic procedures nor inconsequential
curiosities. It involves sophisticated heuristics that are general, revealing a
substantial amount of knowledge about the decimal system and skill in arithmetic
problem solving" (p. 96). Additionally, they should not be dismissed as inferior and
irrelevant to formal learning. Teachers should become aware of the strategies
children use and capitalise on these to enhance the development of children's
mathematical abilities.
As indicated previously, it is the reconstructive memory processes (Svenson &
Sjoberg, 1982, p. 91) that are of primary concern in the formulation of the strategy
components of mental computation. If teachers are to take advantage of the
approaches used by children and adults to compute mentally, they need to be aware
of the pre-numerical, numerical and, most importantly, the heuristic strategies
(Carraher et al., 1987, p. 91) that have been identified as being in common use.
This awareness needs to be accompanied by a realisation that mental strategies
tend to be adapted to the specific characteristics of the numerical situations,
particularly in out-of-school settings (Carraher et al., 1985, p. 95; Murtaugh, 1985, p.
192).

82

McIntosh (1990c, 1991a) provides a comprehensive analysis of mental


strategies. His classification constitutes a system that encompasses all four
operations for the basic facts and beyond. However, in identifying the breadth of
commonly used strategies, this classification needs to be supplemented with data
from other studies. In comparing the categorisations used by various researchers,
recognition needs to be given to the absence of agreed descriptors for many of the
commonly identified approaches.
The categories into which mental strategies may be clustered are described by
McIntosh (1990c, 1991a) as: (a) initial strategy, (b) elementary counting,
(c) counting in larger units, (d) used place value instrumentally, (e) used place value
relationally, (f) used other relational knowledge, (g) known fact, (h) related to known
fact, and (i) used aids. Except for the last category, McIntosh (1990c, p. 7) views
the classification as hierarchical.
The strategies classified by McIntosh (1990c, 1991) as used aidsused
fingers, and used a mental pictureare ones normally employed in conjunction with
other strategiesfor example, counting (Cooper et al., 1992, pp. 108-109). Svenson
and Sjoberg's (1982, p. 99) first two stages in the evolution of children's strategies to
solve basic subtraction facts highlight the use of fingers to serve as external
memories. McIntosh (1990c, p. 7) reported the use of a wide range of finger
techniques, some quite sophisticated. Additionally, many children appear to use
mental pictures to aid their calculations. Although some appear to be unaware of
their use, others are quite conscious of referring to, and manipulating, iconic
representations such as number lines (McIntosh, 1990c, p. 7), and mental images of
a sorobanJapanese abacus (R. E. Reys, B. J. Reys, Nohda & Emori, 1995, p.
319).
Resnick (1983) suggests that:

The earliest stage of decimal number knowledge can be thought of as an


elaboration of the number line representation so that, rather than a single
mental number line linked by the simple "next" relationship, there are now two
co-ordinated lines....Along the rows a "next-by-one" relationship links the
numbers....Along the columns a "next-by-ten" relationship links the numbers.
(p. 127)

83

This somewhat different view of decimal structure, as represented by a ninetynine board, can be applied to mental addition and subtraction. For example, 72 - 47
can be calculated by moving down the 10 string from 72 four positions to 32 and
then by moving down the ones string seven positions to 32 (Resnick, 1983, p. 133).
This view is supported by Beishuizen (1993, p. 316) who found, particularly for
Grade 2 children classified as demonstrating a lower-level ability, that the use of a
hundredsquare had relatively positive effects on developing proficiency with addition
and subtraction using an N10 strategya strategy in which the first number is kept
whole while the second is decomposed.
Although not included in Hamann and Ashcraft's (1985, p. 66) list of
preparation strategies, the strategies categorised by McIntosh (1990c, p. 2) as initial
strategies reflect ways in which complex number situations can be approached.
These are classified as: (a) change subtraction to addition and division to
multiplication, and (b) use the commutative laws of addition and multiplication. With
respect to the latter, Barbara Reys (1986b, p. 3279-A) categorises the use of the
commutative (and associative) properties of addition and multiplication as
translation strategies. Translation is identified as one of the three most common
approaches used by high and middle ability mental computers in Grades 7 and 8.
To facilitate a comprehensive summary of the categories of strategies identified
as being used to calculate exact answers mentally, a more detailed analysis is
presented in the sections which follow. This analysis is organised under the
following headings: (a) Counting Strategies, (b) Strategies Based Upon Instrumental
Understanding, and (c) Heuristic Strategies Based Upon Relational Understanding.
Counting Strategies
McIntosh (1990c, pp. 2-3, 1991a, p. 2) distinguished between counting
forwards or backwards in oneselementary countingand counting in both directions
using larger unitscounting in larger units (see Table 2.2). With respect to countingon in ones, Resnick and Omanson (1987, p. 66), following the work of Resnick
(1983), hypothesised four counting procedures for adding a one-digit number to a
two-digit number: min of the addends, sum of the units, min of the units, and mental
carry. These strategies have their origins with the work by Groen and Parkman
(1972) who investigated addition methods involving two addends less than ten.

84

Chronometric models, such as these, "[assume] a counter in the head' that [can] be
set initially at any number, then incremented a given number of times and finally
read out'" (Resnick, 1983, p. 117). Different strategies necessitate a different
number of increments. Hence, the time taken to provide an answerreaction
timeis the measure by which assumptions are made with respect to particular
strategies used.
From Resnick and Omanson's (1987) study, chronometric evidence supported
by interview data, provides an indication for the use of each of the strategies, except
sum of the units. The min of the addends strategy requires no place value
knowledge as the single-digit number is added-on in increments of one after the
mental counter is set to the two-digit numberfor 23 + 9, the mental counter is set to
23, and the child counts 24, 25, 26...32. It follows from this example that an ability
to count across the decade barrier is a prerequisite.
To be able to use the min of the units strategy, a child needs to have acquired
some ability with partitioning and recombining numbers (see Table 2.1), a Level 3
numeration skill, as proposed by Murray and Olivier (1989, p. 6). Resnick and
Omanson (1987, p. 66) described this strategy as decomposing the two-digit
number into a tens component and a ones component and then recombining the
tens component with whichever of the two unit quantities is larger. The mental
counter is then set to the reconstituted number and the smaller of the units digits is
added in increments of one. For 23 + 9, the operation is recomposed to 29 + 3, with
the mental counter set to 29 and then incremented three times to 32.
Mental carry is a strategy that mimics the carrying procedure for the written
algorithm. For addition, "mentally add the units digits, mentally carry a 1 if

85

Table 2.2
Counting Strategies
Elementary counting

counting-on in ones
min of the addends
23 + 9: 24, 25, 26...32; 32
min of the units
23 + 9: 29 + 3; 30, 31, 32; 32

counting-back in ones
24 - 6: 23, 22, 21, 20, 19, 18

Counting in larger units

Note.

counting-on in twos/fives/tens
80 + 60: 90, 100...140; 140
71 - 44: 54, 64, 74; minus 3; 27

counting-back in twos/fives/tens
28 - 15: 23, 18, 13; 13

counting-back to a second number in twos/fives/tens


140 - 60: 130, 120, 110...60; 80

repeated addition
15 x 50: 50 + 50 + 50 + 50 + 50 = 250; 2 x 250 = 500;
500 + 50 + 50 + 50 + 50 + 50 = 750

repeated subtraction
150 30: 150 - 30 - 30 - 30 - 30 - 30 = 0; 5

Adapted from: McIntosh (1990c, 1991a) and Resnick & Omanson (1987)

necessary, then mentally add the tens digit to the carry digit" (Resnick & Omanson,
1987, p. 67). The use of a mental form of a written algorithm is a strategy
categorised by McIntosh (1990c, 1991a) as one relying on the instrumental use of
place value (see next section).
In a cross-cultural study by Ginsburg et al. (1981, p. 173) it is reported that, for
both schooled and unschooled young American and Dioula children, counting was
frequently observed for addition examples involving relatively small sums12 + 7, for
example. However, no indication of the particular counting strategies used was
provided. Olander and Brown (1959, p. 99) observed that counting is a slow

86

procedure, with many low achievers forgetting what has been counted before the
process is completed, an indication of their limited capacity for interrupted working
(Hunter, 1978).
Although decrementingcounting-back from the larger numberand
incrementingcounting-onmodels for subtraction have been proposed for basic
addition and subtraction facts (Resnick, 1983, p. 119), few studies have investigated
these models for operating beyond the basic facts. Counting-on for subtraction
involves starting with the smaller number and counting on to the larger, usually by
ones, twos, fives or tens (Cooper et al., 1992, p. 108; McIntosh, 1991a, p. 2;
Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 99). Except for the first approach, McIntosh (1990c, p. 1;
1991a, p. 2) classifies these strategies as Counting in larger units (see Table 2.2).
Olander and Brown (1959) related a count-on by tens approach for subtraction: "71 44: 54, 64, 74, minus 3, the answer is 27 because there are 3 tens minus 3" (p. 99),
an approach that relies on a knowledge of basic subtraction facts, and an ability to
compensate. Cooper et al. (1992, p. 108) reported a count-on strategy for the
addition of two-digit numbers based on a knowledge of doubles. To add 23c and
12c, a child twice adds six to 23: 23 + 6 + 6.
Other strategies classified by McIntosh (1990c, p. 1) as counting in larger units
are: counted back in twos/tens, counted back to second number in two/tens,
repeated addition, repeated subtraction, multiples, and recited tables. To the first of
these can be added counting back in fives, for examples such as 28 - 15 (Cooper et
al., 1992, p. 111). Whereas counting back by twos/fives/tens entails decrementing
the larger number by a number of steps whose value is the smaller number,
counting back to the second number requires that the value of the number of steps
equals the difference between the two numbersfor 140 - 60, a child explained:
"You have 140 and you counted backwards in 10s" (McIntosh, 1990c, p. 3).
Some children have been observed to multiply using repeated addition. One of
the strategies, classified by Carraher et al. (1987) as a repeated-grouping heuristic,
involves repeatedly adding fifty to find 15 x 50: "[Jos] started by adding chunks of
50 five times to 250. He doubled this chunk, getting ten 50s, and then went back to
adding individual 50s" (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 93). This procedure, as Carraher et
al. (1985) point out, is one that "becomes grossly inefficient when large numbers are
involved" (p. 28).

87

Repeated subtraction, the quotition aspect of the division process, is one


strategy for mentally dividing one number by another that has been observed
(Carraher et al., 1987, p. 93; McIntosh, 1990c, p. 3). Carraher et al. (1987) report
an approach, classified as a repeated-grouping heuristic, that provides an example
of repeated subtraction, albeit a complex one, to solve 75 5:

The problem was solved by successively subtracting the convenient groups


distributed while keeping track of the increasing share that each child received;
10 marbles were given to 5 children, which accounted for 50 marbles, and the
remaining 25 were distributed among 5 children5 to each, totalling 15 for each
child. (p. 93)

The remaining two categories for counting in larger units, as defined by


McIntosh (1990c, p. 1; 1991a, p. 2), Multiples and Recited tables, may be
considered to apply more to basic fact calculations than to operations involving
larger numbers. McIntosh's (1990c, p. 4) examples relate to division and
multiplication basic facts, respectively.

Strategies Based Upon Instrumental Understanding


In the transition from counting strategies to the use of strategies based on
relational knowledge, McIntosh (1990c, p. 1; 1991a, p. 2) has identified a cluster of
approaches that reflect an instrumental understanding of place value. This involves
the application of "rules without reason" (Skemp, 1976, p. 20). These strategies
(see Table 2.3) entail "removing zeros without knowing why it worked, and
calculating mentally by recreating mentally the standard written algorithm"
(McIntosh, 1991a, p. 2). These techniques reflect the manipulation of symbols
approach (Reed & Lave, 1981, p. 442) discussed earlier, an approach that
embodies strategies that "are divorced from reality...[and give] no consideration [to]

88

Table 2.3
Strategies Based Upon Instrumental Understanding
Used place value instrumentally

removed zero
90 - 70: 9 - 7 = 2; add a zero; 20

used mental form of written algorithm


39 + 25: 9 + 5 = 14; carry the 1; 1 + 3 = 4 + 2 = 6; 64
For multiplication:

no partial product retrieved


(No attempt to adapt written algorithm for mental use.)

Note.

one partial product retrieved


25 x 48: 5 x 48 is 5 x 8 = 40, carry 4, 24, 240; 2 x 48 = 96, 960;
240 + 960 = 1200

two partial products retrieved


12 x 250: 2 x 250 = 500; 1 x 250 = 250, 2500; 500 + 2500 = 3000

stacking
8 x 999: 8 x 9 = 72, 72(0), 72(00)

Adapted from: Hope & Sherrill (1987) and McIntosh (1990c, 1991a)

the relative value of the symbols" (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 90). With respect to the
removed zero strategy, McIntosh (1990c) records the following dialogue between
child and interviewer, and asserts that teaching mental computation through a focus
on rules is as self-defeating as such an approach is for written computation
(McIntosh, 1991a, p. 6; 1996, p. 273):

Child:

Well 90 take away 70 ...well I just take it from 9 minus 7 equals


2 and add the zero.

Interviewer:

Right and how is it you are able to take these zeros on and off?

Child:

I always do it like that cause it seems easier for me.

Interviewer:

How did you find out how to do that. Do you remember when,
how you know...

Child:

I think the teacher taught me to away the 0. You always take


away the zero.

89

Interviewer:

Why does it work?

Child:

Don't know.

(McIntosh, 1990c, p. 4)

Ginsburg et al. (1981, pp. 173-174) have observed that as schooled American
and Dioula children grow older, they tend to abandon counting strategies in favour
of the standard paper-and-pencil algorithms applied mentally. These were found to
be used approximately 75% of the time for addition by children categorised as
middle, with respect to age. A number of researchers have observed the mental
application of the standard written algorithms for addition, subtraction and
multiplication to operations beyond the basic facts (Cooper et al., 1992; Ginsburg et
al., 1981; Hope & Sherrill, 1987; Markovits & Sowder, 1994; McIntosh, 1990c,
1991a; B. J. Reys, 1985; Vakali, 1985). In other studies (Hamann & Ashcraft, 1985;
Hitch, 1977, 1978; Resnick & Omanson, 1987), although the classifications have not
been explicitly linked to the written algorithms, some of the strategies identified can
be interpreted as being analogous to the standard procedures. One of the
preparation strategies identified by Hamann and Ashcraft (1985) was classified as
one's column, where an approach to solving 14 + 12 was recorded as "two plus four
equals six and one plus one is two" (p. 67).
It is where regroupingborrowing or carryingis required that it becomes most
apparent that children are following the written algorithm, operating, in Ginsburg's et
al. (1981, p. 171) terms, on the numbers as digits and not as tens or hundreds. One
child explained their procedure for adding 39 and 25 as: "9 plus 5 is 14, carry the 1,
1 plus 3 is 4, plus 2 is 6,...64" (Vakali, 1985, p. 111). Vakali (1985, p. 111) observed
that this ones-tens-organise strategy is the most frequently used approach for
mental addition and subtraction by Year 3 children. In comparing the performances
of children in Years 2 to 7, McIntosh (1991a, p. 5) indicated that many errors were
made by children who used a mental form of the written addition and subtraction
algorithms, particularly those classified as the least competent with mental
computation.
Similar results are reported by Markovits and Sowder (1994), and Hope and
Sherrill (1987), with respect to multiplication. Students in Grades 11 and 12,
unskilled with mental multiplication, preferred to use an analogue of the written
algorithm, "[making] little attempt to examine the calculative task for even the most

90

transparent number properties that might aid in the calculation" (Hope & Sherrill,
1987, p. 104). Although heuristic strategies, reliant on a recognition of such
properties, provide strong evidence for an understanding of the composition and
partition principles, Resnick and Omanson (1987, p. 70) caution that the tendency to
use school-taught algorithms may mask such an understanding.
Four principal variations of the standard written algorithm for multiplication
have been identified by Hope and Sherrill (1987, p. 101-105): no partial product
retrieved, one partial product retrieved, two partial products retrieved and stacking
(see Table 2.3). When using the first strategy, Grade 11 and 12 students "made no
attempt to adapt paper-and-pencil methods to a mental medium. Each partial
product was calculated digit by digit, and no numerical equivalent larger than a basic
fact was retrieved during the calculation" (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 101). This
strategy, together with the second, was often guided by gestures that reflected a
desire to write each stage of the calculation (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 105).
Markovits and Sowder (1994, p. 23) found that, following the implementation of
instructional units on mental strategies, Grade 7 students tended to remain with
standard paper-and-pencil algorithms in situations where they could easily be
applied mentally, but looked for nonstandard methods where these could not so
easily be used.
By retrieving one or two of the partial products from long-term memory, the
demands on working memory are reduced. However, Hope and Sherrill (1987, p.
105) observed difficulties with the organisation stage of the procedure not dissimilar
to those observed by Vakali (1987, p. 112) for subtraction. As defined by Vakali
(1985, p. 112), this stage involves combining partial answers into a final solution.
Hope and Sherrill (1987) record that, following determining the partial products of
500 and 2500 for 12 x 250, one student said: "5, 0, 0, and 2, 5, 0, 0,...would be...0,
5, 7, 2...2, 7, 5, 0?" (p. 105), a right-to-left additive process.
The organise stage, when using the stacking strategy relies on visualising the
partial products as they would appear if they had been recorded in writing. This
strategy was applied only in operations that involved multiplying a multiple-digit
number by a single-digit number. For example, 8 x 999 was calculated by thinking:
"8 times 72, 72, and 72, right across" (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 102). Where skilled
students used the paper-and-pencil analogue, they tended to retrieve larger

91

numerical equivalents than basic multiplication facts. In so doing, the number of


calculation steps was minimised (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 106).

Heuristic Strategies Based Upon Relational Understanding


The most powerful strategies are those that rely on relational knowledge;
knowledge that allows an individual to "[know] not only what method worked but
why" (Skemp, 1976, p. 23). This understanding depends on the nature of a person's
propositional and procedural knowledge. One view of propositional knowledge is
that it encompasses both conceptual and declarative knowledge (Sowder, 1991, p.
3). Conceptual knowledge is conjectured to be rich in relationships (Putman,
Lampert, & Peterson, 1988, p. 83), part of which is the declarative knowledge of
specific factsnumerical equivalentsstored in a long-term memory network
(Hamann & Ashcraft, 1985, p. 52). An essential part of procedural knowledge,
which consists of rules, algorithms, or procedures used to solve mathematical tasks
(Putman et al., 1988, p. 83), is an individual's store of mental strategies or, in
Hunter's (1977a) terms, calculative plans.
Given the uniqueness of the patterns of each individual's knowledge, it is not
unexpected that Vakali (1985) should comment that though "all students reported
breaking down the problems into a series of elementary steps,...there were
considerable individual differences with regard to the nature of the steps and the
order of their execution" (p. 110). It follows that these differences are likely to be
greater when heuristic strategies based on relational knowledge are employed.
McIntosh (1991a, pp. 4-5) found a preponderance of place value strategies in
his analysis of methods used to solve context-free addition and subtraction
examples comprising two- and three-digit numbers. Strategies that use a relational
understanding of place value are classified by McIntosh (1990c, p. 1) as:
added/subtracted parts of second number, bridged tens/hundreds, used
tens/hundreds, worked from left, or worked from right. In comparing this
classification to the heuristic strategies identified by other researchers (Beishuizen,
1985, 1993; Carraher et al., 1987; Cooper et al., 1992, 1996; Flournoy, 1959;
Ginsburg et al., 1981; Hamann & Ashcraft, 1985; Hazekamp, 1986; Heirdsfield,
1996; Hitch, 1978; Markovits & Sowder, 1994; Olander & Brown, 1959; Resnick,

92

1983; Sowder, 1992; Trafton, 1978; Vakali, 1985), those strategies that rely primarily
upon place value knowledge can reasonably be placed into one of the McIntosh's
(1990c, 1991a) categories without the need to use bridged tens/hundreds. Hence
this strategy cluster is not included in the following analysis of heuristic strategies for
calculating beyond the basic facts (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4).
Strategies categorised as added/subtracted parts of second number are
described by Vakali (1985), for the addition of two-digit numbers, as entailing:

The addition of the two-digit first addend to the tens of the second
addend, then addition of this sum to the ones of the second addend....For
46 + 38, "46 plus 30 is 76, 76 plus 8 is 84. (p. 111)

Beishuizen (1985, p. 252; 1993, p. 295) and Wolters, Beishuizen, Broers, and
Knoppert (1990, p. 22) refer to this approach as the N-10 procedure, whereas
Cooper et al. (1992, p. 108) classify it as a used tens strategy. Cooper et al. (1996,
p. 149) and Heirdsfield (1996, p. 133) refer to this approach as left to right
aggregation. A variation of this strategy is also reported by Cooper et al. (1992, p.
111). Rather than add the value of the tens digit first, the ones digit is used
initiallyfor example, for 36 + 29; 36 + 9 = 45, 45 + 20 = 65. This is referred to by
Beishuizen (1993, p. 295) as the u-N10 strategy, one which Cooper et al. (1996, p.
150) and Heirdsfield (1996, p. 133) term right to left aggregation.
Though it appears customary to decompose the second addend, Flournoy
(1959, p. 138) reported that, after a series of mental computation lessons, the
majority of children were using two approaches to solving 34 + 48, one of which
involved decomposing the first addend (34): 30 + 48 = 78, 78 + 4 = 82. To allow

93

Table 2.4
Heuristic Strategies Based Upon Relational Understanding
Add or subtract parts of the first or second number
34 + 48:
30 + 48 = 78, 78 + 4 = 82
46 + 38:
46 + 30 = 76, 76 + 8 = 84
33 - 16:
33 - 10 = 23, 23 - 6 = 17
Use fives, tens and/or hundreds

add-up
317 - 198:
51 - 34:

198 + 2 = 200, 200 + 100 = 300, 300 + 17 = 317; 119


34 + 10 = 44, 44 + 7 = 51, 17

decomposition
200 - 35: 200 = 100 + 100, 35 = 30 + 5, 100 - 30 = 70, 70 - 5 = 65, 165
252 - 57: 252 - 52 = 200, 200 - 5 = 195

compensation
28 + 29: 30 + 30 = 60, 60 - 2 - 1 = 57
25 + 89: 89 + 11 = 100, 25 - 11 = 14, 100 + 14 = 114
86 - 38: 88 - 40 = 48

Work from the left

organisation
58 + 34: 50 + 30 = 80, 8 + 4 = 12, 80 + 12 = 92
36 - 23: 30 - 20 = 10, 6 - 3 = 3, 10 + 3 = 13

incorporation
39 + 25: 30 + 20 = 50, 50 + 9 = 59, 59 + 5 = 64
51 - 34: 50 - 30 = 20, 20 - 4 = 16, 16 + 1 = 17
43 - 26: 40 - 20 = 20, 20 + 3 = 23, 23 - 6 = 17

Work from the right

mental analogue of standard written algorithm


58 + 34: 4 + 8 = 12, 5 + 3 = 8(0), 80 + 12 = 92
74 - 28: 14 - 8 = 6, 60 - 20 = 40, 6 + 40 = 46

place-grouping
439 - 327: 39 - 27 = 12, 4(00) - 3(00) = 1(00), 112

Use known facts


29 - 14:
2 x 14 + 1 = 29, 1 x 14 + 1 = 15

94

Table 2.4 cont.


Heuristic Strategies Based Upon Relational Understanding

Use factors

general factoring
60 x 15:
60 x 3 x 5 = 300 x 3 = 900

half-and-double
60 x 15:
30 x 30 = 900

aliquot parts
25 x 48:
48 x (100 4) = (48 4) x 100 = 12 x 100 = 1200

exponential factoring
32 x 32:
(25)2 = 210 = 1024

iterative factoring
27 x 32:
27 x 25; 27, 54, 108, 216, 432, 864

Use distributive principle

Note.

additive distribution
64 4:
(60 4) + (4 4) = 15 + 1 = 16
21 x 13:
(20 x 13) + (1 x 13) = 260 + 13 = 273

subtractive distribution
8 x 999:
8 x (1000 - 1) = (8 x 1000) - (8 x 1) = 8000 - 8 = 7992

fractional distribution
15 x 48:
(10 + 5) x 48 = 10 x 48 = 480, of 480 = 240, 480 + 240 = 720

quadratic distribution
49 x 51:
50 - 1 = 2500 - 1 = 2499

Adapted from: Carraher et al. (1987); Flournoy (1959); Hope (1987, 1985);
McIntosh (1991a, 1990c); Olander & Brown (1959)

for this variation this cluster is referred to as: added or subtracted parts of the first or
second number (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4).
When this approach is used for subtracting two-digit numbers a strategy
analogous to that described above for addition is used:

95

Subtraction of tens of the subtrahend from the minuend, subtraction of ones of


the subtrahend from the above difference....For 33 - 16, "33 minus 10 is 23, 23
minus 6 is 17. (Vakali, 1985, p. 111)

In some instances, as for addition, the ones digit of the subtrahend is


subtracted initially: "For 36 - 23, 36 minus 3 is 33, 33 minus 20 is 13" (Vakali, 1985,
p. 111). Whichever variation of the added or subtracted parts of the first or second
number approach is used, it has the advantage of incorporating the result from each
stage of the calculation into a single result, thus reducing the demands on shortterm memory. Hitch (1978, p. 306) suggests that if partial results are not
immediately used they will undergo rapid forgetting. Cooper et al. (1996, pp. 157158) found that these aggregation strategies were not as frequently used for
addition and subtraction as were strategies that involved separating both numbers
involved in the calculation.
For subtraction, the used tens and/or hundreds approach typically relies on
using the initial strategy of changing subtraction to addition so that an adding-up
method can be used. This method is akin to the way in which change is often
counted, except that the first step is to arrive at a multiple of ten or one hundred.
Whereas Cooper et al. (1992, p. 109) refer to this approach as estimation, Olander
and Brown (1959, p. 99) describe it as a rounding approach. They indicate that it
often involves multiples of five or combinations of ten and five. Hence this strategy
cluster is referred to as Use fives, tens and/or hundreds (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4).
Cooper et al. (1996, p. 151) include this strategy, which incorporates what is
described as N10 missing addend and u-N10 missing addend (Beishuizen, 1993, p.
295) approaches, within their aggregation categories referred to previouslyfor
example, 362 - 128: 128 + 2 = 130, 130 + 70 = 200, 200 + 162 = 362, 2 + 70 + 162
= 234 (Heirdsfield & Cooper, 1995, p. 3).
Sowder (1992, p. 41) indicated that the adding-upcounting upmethod is one
which works well for children: To find 317 - 198, count up 2 to 200, then 100 to get
300, then 17 more, or 119 in all. Olander and Brown (1959) referred to this strategy
as "round [add] to a multiple of 10, where two-digit numbers are being subtracted.
A second variation of the use of ten is to "round by (add) a multiple of 10, e.g. in 51 34....34 and 10, 44; 44 and 7, 51; 17'" (p. 99). These strategies, in Olander and
Brown's (1959, p. 99) terms, entail rounding upward.

96

Another such approach, similar to that for counting change, involves arriving at
a multiple of five at the first step, rather than ten as described above: For "51 34....34 plus 1, 35; 35 plus 15, 50; 1, 51; 17'" (Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 99). It is
suggested by Olander and Brown (1959, p. 99) that the advantage of these
approaches is that the subtrahend can be forgotten following the initial step,
although they require partial answers to be retained in working memory. A further
advantage is that the need to borrow or regroup is eliminated, thus reducing the
complexity of an operation.
Strategies that involve decomposing the minuend and/or the subtrahend, so
that multiples of fives, tens or hundreds may be used to simplify the calculation, are
classified by Carraher et al. (1987, p. 91) as decomposition heuristics (see Tables
2.1 & 2.4). To solve 200 - 35, presented as a word problem, the following
explanation was provided:

If it were thirty, then the result would be seventy. But it's sixty-five; one
hundred sixty-five. (The 35 was decomposed into 30 and 5, a procedure
that allows the child to operate initially with only hundreds and tens; the
units were taken into account afterward. The 200 was likewise
decomposed into 100 and 100; one 100 was stored while the other was
used in the computation procedure.) (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 91)

Carraher et al. (1987, p. 92) point out that this approach for subtraction
replaces the digits in one or more places with zero. The use of tens and hundreds
is sought because round numbers, numbers that end in zero, are more likely to be
related to known number facts100 - 30 - 5, for example. Further, this procedure
reduces the demands on short-term memory as the need to operate on hundreds,
tens and units simultaneously is avoided. Where numbers without any zeros are
involved, the search for round numbers becomes particularly apparent in some
instances. To subtract 57 from 252 a child explained:

Take away fifty-two, that's two hundred, and five to take away, that's one
hundred and ninety-five. (The children decomposed 252 into 200 and 52;
57 was decomposed into 52 + 5; removing both 52s, there remained
another five to take away from 200.) (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 92)

97

A number of researchers (Cooper et al., 1996; Heirdsfield, 1996; B. J. Reys et


al., 1993; Sowder, 1992; Trafton, 1978) have observed variations to these
approaches which involve final compensations for initial rounding (see Tables 2.1 &
2.4), a holistic strategy (Cooper et al., 1996, p. 150). Such an approach is confusing
for some children who find it difficult to determine in which direction the
compensation should occur (Sowder, 1992, p. 41). However, for children proficient
with such strategies, the complexity of the calculations is reduced by being able to
calculate initially with multiples of fives, tens or hundreds. Two compensatory
approaches to calculating 28 + 29 reported by B. J. Reys et al. (1993, p. 314) are:
30 + 30 is 60, so just take off 2 and 1 more. 60, 58, 57.
25 + 25 is 50, plus 3 more is 53 and 4 more is 57.

In contrast to these undoing approaches, adjusting each number before


calculating by levelling (Heirdsfield, 1996, p. 134) circumvents the need for final
adjustments. This approach is exemplified by the following dialogue presented by
McIntosh (1990c) to explain his used tens/hundreds classification:

Interviewer:

What's 25 add 89

Child:

114...I took 89...I knew I needed 11 more to make 100 so I took


11 away...from 25...left me with 14...then I added 14 extra up to
100.

Interviewer:

How did you know you needed 11 more for 89?

Child:

Well 90 to 100 is 10 and 1 more. (p. 5)

Unlike unskilled mental calculators who tend to use a right-to-left approach,


skilled mental calculators usually work from the left (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 106).
Although Hope and Sherrill's (1987) study involved multiplication, other researchers
(Beishuizen, 1985, 1993; Cooper et al., 1992; Flournoy, 1954, 1959; Ginsburg et al.,
1981; Heirdsfield, 1996; Hitch, 1977, 1978; McIntosh, 1990c; Olander & Brown,
1959; Vakali, 1985) report a common usage of strategies that involve working from
the left (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4) for addition and subtraction. Observations from
school practice and remedial teaching in The Netherlands indicate that a left-to-right

98

approach is one that children spontaneously use (Beishuizen, 1985, p. 253). A


common form of this approach, categorised as left to right separated place value by
Cooper et al. (1996, p. 155) and Heirdsfield (1996, p. 132), and organisation (Vakali,
1985, p. 111) in this study, is recounted by McIntosh (1990c):

Interviewer:

58 + 34

Child:

92...I added the 50 out of 58 and the 30 out of the 34 together


and came up with 80, and then I added 8 and 4 and then got 12,
so I added 12 onto [80]. (p. 57)

Beishuizen (1985, pp. 253, 256) and Wolters et al. (1990, p. 28) indicate that
this strategythe 10-10 procedureproduces more errors than the added/subtracted
parts of second number (N-10) strategy discussed previously, a finding supported by
Heirdsfield (1996, p. 133). In using the N-10 procedure fewer decisions have to be
made as only one of the numbers is decomposed, thus resulting in fewer calculative
steps. Arising out of an analysis of approaches to teaching addition in Grade 2
using concrete materials and children's spontaneous methods for addition and
subtraction, Beishuizen (1985, p. 256) suggests that the added or subtracted parts
of second number approach may be viewed as a more learned and algorithmic
procedure than working from the left which is a more invented and heuristic
procedure. However, the error rate for the latter strategy may be due, at least for
Queensland children, to its being infrequently practised in classrooms (Heirdsfield,
1996, p. 133).
Hope (1985, p. 359) indicates that there is some evidence to suggest that a
left-to-right approach is less demanding on short-term memory than one that
involves working from the right. The memory load, using a working from the left
approach, can be reduced by progressively incorporating each interim calculation
into a single result, a characteristic of an efficient mental strategy (Hope & Sherrill,
1987, p. 108). For example, "for 39 + 25, 30 plus 20 is 50, 50 + 9 is 59, 59 plus 5 is
64'" (Vakali, 1985, p. 99). This approach, designated the 10-10-N procedure by
Beishuizen (1985), regrouping by Ginsburg (1982, p. 196), group by tens and ones:
cumulating sums by R. E. Reys et al. (1995), and incorporation in this analysis, does
not require an organisation stage (see Table 2.4) in which two sums are added to
form the answer as the final step of the calculation (Vakali, 1985, p. 111).

99

With respect to subtraction, difficulties arise when the need to regroup occurs
when using an approach analogous to that of McIntosh's (1990c, p. 57) example for
addition outlined previously, which relies on subtracting the hundreds/tens then the
ones. To overcome this need, an approach similar to the incorporation procedure
for addition may be used (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4). Two variations of this method,
referred to by Olander and Brown (1959, p. 99) as compensatory rounding, have
been observed. The difference between the strategies pertains to the order in which
the ones digits are brought into the calculation. The following examples
characterise these approaches:

For 51 - 34, "50 minus 30, 20; minus 4, 16; plus 1, 17" (Olander &
Brown, 1959, p. 99)

For 43 - 26, "40 minus 20 is 20, 20 plus 3 is 23, 23 minus 6 is 17.


(Vakali, 1985, p. 111)

As discussed previously, unskilled mental calculators tend to use right-to-left


methods which are essentially mental analogues of the standard written algorithms.
Such methods are categorised as right to left separated place value by Cooper et al.
(1996, p. 155) and Heirdsfield (1996, p. 132)for example for 74 - 28: 14 - 8 = 6, 60
- 20 = 40, 46 (Heirdsfield & Cooper, 1995, p. 3). In some instances, however, work
from the right strategies (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4) reflect a relational understanding of
place value knowledge. However, in many cases they tend to lack a clear
distinction from the mental application of the standard written algorithms.
Nonetheless, an example of the use of relational place value knowledge is given by
McIntosh (1990c):

Interviewer: 58 + 34. How did you get 92 as an answer?


Child:

Well I took the 4 and the 8 and added them together which went
to 12, and then I took the 5 and the 3 which equalled 80 and
added on the 12, which equalled 92. (p. 6)

This approach, classified as transition by Markovits and Sowder (1994, p. 14), is


indicative of Murray and Olivier's (1989, pp. 6-7) Level 4 place value knowledge

100

which allows a child to use ten as an iterable unit. It can be assumed that this child
understands, implicitly at least, that 5 tens were added to 3 tens to give 8 tens (80).
A strategy, which relies on an in-depth understanding of numeration to
decompose the numbers so that the calculation is simplified, is referred to by
Olander and Brown (1959, p. 99) as a place-grouping approach (see Tables 2.1 &
2.4). This approach is reflective of Jensen's (1990) observations of the methods
used by Shakuntala Devi. Devi explained that when given a large number it
"automatically falls apart' in its own way, and the correct answer simply falls out'.
Each number uniquely dictates its own solution" (Jensen, 1990, p. 262). Five high
achievers are reported by Olander and Brown (1959) as having used a placegrouping approach:

For example, in 439 - 327, when a pupil thought "39 minus 27, 12; 4
minus 3, 1; 112," he broke up the 439 into 400 and 39, and 327 into 300
and 27. (p. 99)

A complex variation of this strategy, which involves compensating for not regrouping
during the initial subtraction, is also presented by Olander and Brown (1959): "6000
- 2249...."100 minus 49, 51; 60 minus 22, 38; minus 1, 37; 3751" (p. 99).
In proposing a classification system for mental computation strategies,
McIntosh (1990c, p. 1) distinguishes between strategies based upon the relational
use of place value concepts and the use of other relational knowledge. Such a
distinction, however, is not always clear-cut and may be considered somewhat
artificial, given that place value knowledge is simply one aspect of an individual's
conceptual knowledge. Mental computation strategies that rely on mathematical
concepts and principles other than those related to place value, or on numerical
equivalents beyond the basic facts, are likely to be ones used by those who
demonstrate greater computational skill. With respect to the mental multiplication
strategies used by skilled mental calculators identified by Hope and Sherrill (1987),
McIntosh (1990b) remarked that "many of the students in this study were clearly at a
more advanced level than those considered elsewhere in this paper" (p. 19), an
observation relevant to the strategies hitherto discussed in this analysis of heuristic
strategies.

101

When using relational knowledge, other than that primarily based on place
value, to calculate exact answers mentally, three categories are evident in the
research literature. These are: (a) a number-relations approach (Olander & Brown,
1959, p. 99), and strategies based on (b) factoring and (c) distribution (Hope, 1985,
pp. 358-366; Hope, 1987 p. 334; Hope & Sherrill, 1987, pp. 102-103). Although a
number-relations approach is one appropriate for all four operations, factoring and
distribution are ones more suited to multiplication and division. Based on an
analysis of reports of strategies used by expert mental calculators, Hope (1985, pp.
358, 362) concludes that factoring is their favourite method of calculating, with
multiplication their favourite operation.
A number-relations approach relies on relating a calculation to another to
which the answer is known (see Table 2.4)that is, to use known facts (French,
1987, p. 39; McIntosh, 1991a, p. 2; Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 99). For example, to
subtract 14 from 29, one method is to relate the operation to known multiplication
facts: "two 14s plus 1, 29; one 14 plus 1, 15" (Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 99).
The categories for classifying mental strategies are not mutually exclusive and
the classification of particular strategies can only be achieved with any certainty if
based on verbal reports. Of relevance to relating a calculation to a known fact,
French (1987, p. 39) has suggested that many mental methods depend on relating a
particular calculation to a simpler one. For example, to work out 80 x 7, it can be
related to 10 x 7. In effect, the calculation becomes 10 x 7 x 8 and hence could also
be classified as a factoring approach. Factoring involves transforming one or more
factors into a series of products or quotients (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 102). Hope
(1985, pp. 362-266; 1987, p. 334) and Hope and Sherrill (1987, pp. 102-103) have
identified five categories of factoring strategies into which strategies identified by
other researchers (Carraher et al., 1987; Flournoy, 1954; B. J. Reys, 1985) are able
to be placed. The five categories of mental strategies that involve factoring are:
general factoring, half-and-double, aliquot parts, exponential factoring and iterative
factoring (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4).
General factoring involves factoring "one or more of the factors before applying
the associative law for multiplication" (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 103). Flournoy
(1954) recounts one method used by an intermediate grade student to calculate 11
x 16: "Half of 16 is 8; 11 x 8 = 88. I doubled the 88 to get my answer of 176" (p.
149). Barbara Reys (1985), cited in a draft of B. J. Reys and Barger (1994),

102

describes a similar process for finding the product of 60 x 15: "It's the same as 60 x
3 x 5 or 300 x 3 or 900. In some instances, other strategies may be used to
calculate intermediate steps. For example, Hope and Sherrill (1987, p. 103) noted
that for 25 x 48 one senior high school student calculated "5 times 48 is 240, and 5
times 240 is 1200"; 25 had been factored into 5 x 5 and additive distribution applied
to ascertain the intermediate calculations, 5 x 48 and 5 x 240: 5 x 48 = (5 x 40) + (5
x 8 ) and 5 x 240 = (5 x 200) + 5 x 40).
This approach was classified by Hope and Sherrill (1987) "as general factoring
rather than distribution because the computation was transformed initially into a
series of products rather than sums" (p. 103). A strategy for solving 100 4,
considered by Carraher et al. (1987) to be a repeated-grouping heuristic, can also
be classified as a general factoring strategy. A child explained that:

One hundred divided by four is twenty-five. Divide by two, that's fifty.


Then divide again by two, that's twenty-five. (There was a factoring
operation, two successive divisions by 2 to replace the given division by
4.) (p. 93)

The remaining types of factoring strategies can be viewed as special cases of


general factoring as they have significant distinguishing characteristics that allow
them to be regarded as separate categories (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 103).
Although a basic fact example is provided by McIntosh (1990c, p. 6) to illustrate
doubling/halving as a use of other relational knowledge, this strategy is one of value
when calculating beyond the basic facts. Half-and-double (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4) is
particularly useful where at least one of the factors in a multiplication task is a
multiple of two. To apply this strategy, the factor that is a multiple of two is halved
and the other is doubled to compensate. This process continues until the answer
can be determined by applying another strategy or by recall (Hope, 1987, p. 334).
For example, to calculate 60 x 15 a seventh-grade child said: "It's the same as 30 x
30 (halve one factor, double the other) or 900" (B. J. Reys, 1985, cited in a draft of
Reys & Barger, 1992, p. 13).
Hope (1985, p. 334; 1987, pp. 364-365) and Hope and Sherrill (1987, p. 103)
have defined a factoring strategy, which involves transforming one factor into a
quotient, as factoring through the use of aliquot parts. "This strategy [can be]

103

applied...to those computations where one factor (f) [is] a factor of a power of 10 (p)
and the remaining factor [is] a multiple of the quotient p/f " (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p.
103). Hope (1987) reports that Charlene, an expert 13 year-old mental calculator,
multiplied 25 and 48 by reasoning "48 x 100/4 = 48/4 x 100 = 12 x 100 = 1200" (p.
334). This approach is particularly useful in instances where one of the factors is
25, 50 or 125, as the reference numbers are 100 (25 x 4), 100 (50 x 2) and 1000
(125 x 8), respectively (Hope, 1985, p. 364)a useful method to simplify mental
calculations.
Strategies that rely on a knowledge of exponential arithmetic have been
identified by Hope and Sherrill (1987, p. 103), and Hope (1985, pp. 365-368),
namely, exponential factoring and iterative factoring, respectively (see Tables 2.1 &
2.4). Although these approaches are similar, Hope and Sherrill (1987) define
exponential factoring as a strategy that relies on the application of an exponential
rule. For example, to solve "32 x 32, one student reasoned, I solved it by thinking
powers of 2. 32 is 2 to the fifth, and squaring this is 2 to the tenth, which I just know
is 1024'" (p. 103).
Although iterative factoring also involves a knowledge of exponential
arithmetic, this approach does not entail the use of the rules for operating with
exponents. For example, 27 multiplied by 32 can be calculated by doubling the
factor 27, 5 times: 54, 108, 216, 432, 864 (Hope, 1985, p. 366). However, this
approach is rather cumbersome when finding the product of larger factors, given the
number of iterations required. It is therefore useful primarily in situations where
powers of two or three are involved.
Whereas strategies that involve factoring entail the transformation of a
calculation into series of products, other strategies depend on the calculation being
transformed into a series of sums or differences. Hope and Sherrill (1987, p. 102)
have identified four approaches that are based on the distributive properties of
multiplication and division: additive distribution, subtractive distribution, fractional
distribution, and quadratic distribution (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4).
By convention, division is a worked from the left procedure. The most
significant digits are considered first. A convenient mental approach to calculating
64 4 is reported by Flournoy (1954): "I know that 60 4 is 15 and one more 4 is 64
makes 16" (p. 149). This approach can be represented as: (60 + 4) 4 = (60 4) +

104

(4 4) = 15 + 1 = 16. It relies on a knowledge, implicitly or explicitly, of the


distributive property of division over addition. Hope (1985, pp. 358-359; 1987, p.
334) and Hope and Sherrill (1987, p. 102) refer to this procedure as additive
distribution and is "considered the calculative drafthorse' of [expert] mental
calculators because it is suited to a wide range of calculation tasks" (Hope, 1985, p.
358). This strategy may involve the use of place value knowledge, as in Flournoy's
(1954, p. 149) example, or depend on an individual's knowledge of numerical
equivalents. For example, 174 3 may be calculated by converting 174 to 150 + 24
so that the calculation becomes: (150 3) + (24 3) = 50 + 8 = 58 (Flournoy, 1959,
p. 138).
Although additive distribution strategies are usually implemented in a form that
requires a final organisation stage, expert mental calculators often successively add
each partial product to produce a running total, thus reducing the load on short-term
memory. For example, 8 x 4211 is calculated by "8 times 4000 is
32 000; 8 times 200 is 1600; so it's 33 600. 8 times 11 is 88; so the answer is 33
688" (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 102).
Where one factor is near to a multiple of a power of 10, subtractive distribution
is often applied (Hope, 1985, p. 360). To use the distributive principle of
multiplication over subtraction, Hope and Sherrill (1987, p. 102) report that some
skilled senior high school students use the following method for calculating 8 x 999:
8 x (1000 - 1) = 8000 - 8 = 7992. This strategy is one of two approaches to mental
multiplication identified by Trafton (1978, p. 209) as ones that can be learned by
students, the other being an additive distribution.
Fractional distribution (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4) is an approach that is often
"applied to those tasks in which at least one factor [contains] 5 as a unit digit" (Hope
& Sherrill, 1987, p. 102). This allows partial products to be calculated from those
that have already been determined. Hope and Sherrill (1987) indicate that one
method for calculating 15 x 48 is: "10 times 48, 480, and half of 480 is 240; so it's
720" (p. 102).
A common form of quadratic distribution (see Tables 2.1 & 2.4) is to calculate
products through distributing by difference of squares. Hope (1985, p. 362) notes
that even young children of average ability can stumble onto this method as
evidenced by McGartland's (1980, cited by Hope, 1985, p. 362) finding that a fourth-

105

grade child calculated 7 x 9 by squaring 8 and subtracting 1. This method,


irrespective of number size but dependent upon an individual's store of number
facts, is most useful when the two numbers to be multiplied are of a similar
magnitude (Hope, 1985, p. 362). Hope (1987) reported that Charlene calculated 49
x 51 "by reasoning 50 - 1 = 2500 - 1 = 2499" (p. 334). Algebraically, this method is
represented by: (x + y)(x - y) = x - y, where x = 50 and y = 1.
Other forms of quadratic distribution have been reported as being used by
expert mental calculators to square large multi-digit numbers. These are based on
the algebraic equivalences (a + b) = a + 2ab + b and (a - b) = a - 2ab + b (Hope,
1985, p. 361). Scripture (1891, cited in Hope, 1985, p. 361) noted that Henri
Mondeux, a famous nineteenth-century lightning calculator, calculated the square of
2419 through an implicit use of the binomial theorem: 24 + 2 x (24 x 19) + 19. This
procedure involves an initial use of the place-grouping approach outlined above,
with mental adjustments being made to the partial products to account for the value
of the 24 being 2400. Alexander Aitken, in Gardner's (1977) view "perhaps the best
all-round mental calculator of recent times" (p. 70), used the algebraic identity a =
(a + b) x (a - b) + b to square numbers. For example, 777 was calculated by
transforming the calculation into [(777 + 23) x (777 - 23)] + 23, thus allowing him to
use his vast store numerical equivalents, particularly of squares. Gardner (1977)
points out that "the b is chosen to be fairly small and such that either (a + b) or (a b) is a number ending in one or more zeros" (p. 75).
Aitken (cited in Gardner, 1977) has pointed out that "the first step in any
complicated calculation...is to decide in a flash on the best strategy" (pp. 73-74).
This requires a knowledge of a range of computational strategies, such as those
discussed in this section, combined with an ability to recall numerical equivalents
and an ability to remember the numbers that are involved in the intermediate stages
of a calculation. Strength in these characteristics distinguishes experts from nonexperts (Hope, 1985, p. 358). These, together with other traits of proficient mental
calculators, are to be given further consideration in Section

2.7.5 Short-term and Long-Term Memory Components of Mental


Computation

106

Whatever the strategies used to undertake mental calculations, Hunter (1978,


p. 339) suggests that three types of demands on memory can be delineated. These
are: memory for calculative method, memory for numerical equivalents, and memory
for interrupted working. Although the first two place demands on an individual's
long-term memory, the latter places demands on short-term memory capacity.
People who are proficient at calculating exact answers mentally are able to draw
upon greater than average resources in long-term memory, specifically, declarative
and procedural knowledge related to stored factsnumerical equivalentsand
heuristic strategies, respectively (see Figure 2.3). This enables them to devise
efficient methods for approaching a problem and to carry out a

107

PROBLEM TASK ENVIRONMENT

SHORT-TERM MEMORY
WORKING STORAGE

EXECUTIVE PROCESSOR

Information about problem

Information from Working


Storage transformed using
knowledge stored in Long-term
Memory

Information about answer

ANSWER

LONG-TERM MEMORY

CONCEPTUAL KNOWLEDGE
Declarative Knowledge
(Network of stored facts)

PROCEDURAL KNOWLEDGE
(Network of known strategies)

METACOGNITIVE KNOWLEDGE
(Beliefs about ability to
compute mentally)

REAL WORLD KNOWLEDGE


(Prior knowledge about the
Problem Task Environment)

Figure 2.3.

A view of memory processes for computing mentally


(Adapted from: Ashcraft, 1982, p. 229; Hitch, 1978, p. 320;

Silver, 1987, p. 37)

108

calculation in a rapid, but orderly, sequence of steps (Hunter, 1978, p. 343). In


contrast, even expert mental calculators exhibit relatively slender resources with
which to meet the demands of interrupted working (Hunter, 1978, p. 343; Jensen,
1990, p. 270). They tend to "shift the burden of calculation onto long-term memory
while, at the same time, minimising their reliance on temporary memory" (Hunter,
1978, p. 340).
Information processing models of mental computation are based on the
assumption that human beings have only a limited capacity for processing
concurrent activities. Consequently many tasks need to be performed as a series of
steps rather than being accomplished all at once (Hunter, 1977a, p. 37). This
implies that information concerning the problem task environment, "the structure of
facts, concepts, and the relationships among them that constitute the problem"
(Silver, 1987, p. 41), needs to be stored with partial answers derived during
computation. Hitch's (1978, p. 320) hypothesised model for mental calculation of
exact answers suggests that short-term memory consists of a working storage
component and an executive processor (see Figure 2.3). The executive processor
is responsible for the control of mental processes (Hitch, 1978, p. 303). It is also
hypothesised to be the component that stores and retrieves information from longterm memory. Fayol, Abdi, and Gombert (1987, p. 199) observe that, as the storage
load in short-term memory increases, the space devoted to reasoning is reduced.
Consequently the number of errors made during mental computation increases.
Hence, children need to be encouraged to develop proficiency with strategies that
reduce the load on short-term memory, those in which partial results are
incorporated into a single result (see Table 2.4).
Long-term memory contains an individual's store of mathematical knowledge
(Silver, 1987, p. 42). Hiebert and Lefevre (1986) distinguish between knowledge of
mathematical conceptsconceptual knowledgeand knowledge of the formal
language of mathematics and the strategies used to undertake mathematical
tasksprocedural knowledge. Conceptual knowledge, of which declarative
knowledge can be considered a part (see Figure 2.3), is viewed as an interrelated
web of knowledge, with the links being as prominent as the distinct pieces of
information (Hiebert & Lefevre, 1986, pp. 3-4). Each piece of information is
considered to be part of conceptual knowledge only if its relationship to other pieces
of information is recognised.

109

In instances where strong relationships have been built between conceptual


and procedural knowledge, recalling and effectively using particular computational
procedures is enhanced. Recall is aided by strategies having been "stored as part
of a network of information, glued together with semantic relationships...[with] the
retrieval process...triggered by several external and internal cues" (Hiebert &
Lefevre, 1986, p. 11). Conceptual knowledge and its links to procedural knowledge
can enhance problem representations, monitor strategy operation, and promote
transfer to structurally similar problems (Hiebert & Lefevre, 1986, p. 11).
Representing a problem conceptually allows reasoning to focus on the quantities
involved rather than on their symbol representations (Greeno, 1983, p. 228), a
feature of out-of-school mental computation (Carraher et al., 1985, p. 28; Reed &
Lave, 1981, p. 442). Although the demonstrated links between understanding and
the improvement of algorithmic performance remain tenuous, Resnick (1984, cited
in Nesher, 1986, p. 6) has suggested that such links depend upon how children
represent a problem to themselveswhether they focus on manipulating the
quantities involved or whether they focus the symbols used to represent them.
Conceptual knowledge can therefore be thought of as a control structure for
procedural knowledge. Such a view implies that conceptual knowledge, if linked to
particular strategies, can be used to monitor strategy selection and execution, as
well as to judge the reasonableness of the solution obtained (Hiebert & Lefevre,
1986, p. 12). Sowder (1994, p. 143) notes that the protocols of expert mental
calculators illustrate the self-monitoring and self-regulation of the procedures
employed during computation. It is these metaprocesses that characterise proficient
mental calculators. Further, in Sowder's (1994, p. 143) view, flexibility in the
selection of mental strategies is a result of this self-monitoring process.
Aspects of both short-term memory and long-term memory exercise control
over mental computation. Metacognitive processes are believed to involve two
separate components: "(a) An awareness of the skills, strategies, and resources
needed to perform a task effectivelyknowing what to do; and (b) the ability to use
self-regulatory mechanisms to ensure the successful completion of the
taskknowing how and when to do what(Woolfolk, 1987, p. 258). However,
knowing what to do is not solely dependent upon relevant conceptual and
procedural knowledge. Equally important is the metacognitive and real-world
knowledge germane to the particular context that is also stored in long-term memory

110

(Silver, 1987, p. 37), as depicted in Figure 2.3. In view of the findings that mental
strategies tend to be adapted to the specific characteristics of the numerical
situations, particularly in out-of-school settings (Carraher et al., 1985, p. 25;
Murtaugh, 1985, p. 191; Scribner, 1984, p. 38), real-world knowledge, which can be
applied to assist in understanding the problem task environment, has particular
relevance for mental computation.
With respect to the Woolfolk's (1987, p. 258) first component, metacognitive
knowledge held in long-term memory concerns an individual's beliefs about
mathematics and opinions of their ability to perform particular mathematical
procedures in particular contexts (Silver, 1987, p. 42), affective components of
mental computation discussed previously (see Table 2.1). Metacognition also refers
to what Silver (1987, p. 37) designates meta-level processes, namely, the cognitive
monitoring processes of self-monitoring, regulation and evaluation of the cognitive
activity (Silver, 1987, p. 49), processes related to knowing how and knowing
whenWoolfolk's (1987) second component. These processes are thought to be
part of the executive control processes in short-term memory that operate on and
control the flow of information through the memory systems (Woolfolk, 1987, p.
259).
Based on chronometric evidence in relation to operations with basic facts,
Ashcraft (1982) has postulated a fact retrieval model for mental computation.
According to this model, older children and adults store an increasingly large
number of numerical equivalents in long-term memory. The growth in declarative
knowledge allows a gradual shift from "an initial reliance on procedural knowledge
and methods such as counting...to retrieval from a network representation of basic
facts" (Ashcraft, 1982, p. 213), a view in accordance with that of Svenson and
Sjoberg (1982, p. 91) discussed previously. The fact retrieval model assumes that
solution speed increases with an increased involvement of declarative knowledge
(Ashcraft, 1982, p. 233). This assumption has its parallel in the cognitive
characteristics of individuals proficient with mental computation beyond the basic
facts, people who have been found to exhibit a large store of numerical equivalents
rich in relationships.
Ashcraft's (1982) model holds that all procedural processes are slow, an
assumption that Baroody (1983) believes to be incorrect. In his view, efficient
number fact knowledge is due not only to an increase in declarative knowledge, but

111

more importantly to the development of procedural knowledge. "As the child learns
rules, heuristics, and principles, these supplant less efficient procedural processes
such as informal counting algorithms. Moreover, as these rules, heuristics, and
principles become more secure and interconnected, their use becomes more
automatic" (Baroody, 1983, p. 227). Hence, principled procedural processes are
believed to be the key factor in the efficient production of number facts, even for
adults, with the most important developmental shift being away from inefficient
strategies that rely on counting to the use of spontaneous procedural knowledge
(Baroody, 1983, p. 227).
Whether this view has legitimacy when considering mental computation
beyond the basic facts is yet to be determined. With respect to basic fact
production, Baroody (1984, p. 152) cautions that whatever the relative importance
that should be attributed to declarative and procedural knowledge it is too early to
conclude that any one model of mental computation is clearly superior.
Nonetheless, focusing on such knowledge, which these models entail, provides a
useful framework for analysing the role of long-term memory components in
mentally computing beyond the basic facts.

2.8

Characteristics of Proficient Mental Calculators


Little research has been undertaken by contemporary researchers into the

cognitive processes involved in proficient mental computation (Hope, 1985, p. 331;


McIntosh, 1990b, p. 1). That which is available indicates that significant differences
exist in the nature of the strategies used by people who are competent with mental
computation and those who are not so competent. Expert mental computers tend to
use advanced arithmetic and algebraic techniques (B. J. Reys, 1985, p. 43), as
evidenced by the heuristic strategies outlined previously. These strategies rely on a
relational understanding of the properties of numbers. This usage is in contrast to
that of low- and middle-ability mental calculators who tend to rely on an instrumental
use of mental forms of the written algorithms (B. J. Reys, 1991, p. 4). Possibly
through possessing knowledge that is richer in relationships, proficient mental
calculators are able to comprehend the mathematical structure of a situation and
rapidly classify the problem type so as to employ a strategy appropriate for that

112

situation (Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 100; R. E. Reys, 1992, p. 63). High achievers,
therefore, tend to use a variety of methods for computing mentally (Olander &
Brown, 1959, p. 99).
Based on a study of the strategies used by less and more competent children
in Years 2 to 7, McIntosh (1991a, p. 3) observed that generally the latter start to use
particular strategies earlier than the less able and in so doing make more active use
of place value strategies (see Tables 2.3 & 2.4). Additionally, they have superior
number fact knowledge. Although the non-proficient gradually increase their
repertoire of strategies, they tend to rely more on elementary counting (see Table
2.2) and the use of external aids such as fingers.
Strategies based on a relational use of place value, such as use fives, tens
and/or hundreds and work from the left (see Table 2.4), were used as much by
competent children in Years 2 to 4 as by the least competent in Years 5 to 7
(McIntosh, 1991a, p. 3). Except for children in Year 7, who often were able to
answer a question by recall, those who displayed proficiency tended "to manipulate
numbers and exploit the place value aspects of them in a dynamic way" (McIntosh,
1991a, p. 3), a finding supported by Olander and Brown (1959). Students classified
as high achievers were more likely to use a place-grouping approach (see Table
2.4) than were those classified as low achievers (Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 98).
The differences between the mental strategies used by proficient and nonproficient mental calculators reflect different uses of long-term and short-term
memory systems (Hitch, 1977, p. 337; Hunter, 1978, p. 343). With respect to
Shakuntala Devi, a skilled adult mental calculator, Jensen (1990, p. 270) suggests
that an abnormally efficient encoding and retrieval system for long-term memory
largely overcomes the basic information processing limitations of short-term
memory. However, as discussed previously, there is considerable controversy in
the chronometric literature (Ashcraft, 1982, 1983; Baroody, 1983, 1984) as to the
way in which this information is encoded, particularly with respect to the relative
importance of the role of declarative and procedural knowledge in the calculative
process.
Fayol et al. (1987, pp. 187-188) suggest that, for verbally presented arithmetic
problems, arriving at a solution implies the use of three kinds of mental operations.
These are: (a) storing of information as a whole until the task is understood, (b)
searching long-term memory for appropriate schemata to organise and solve the

113

problem, and (c) applying the problem solving process to the data as well as
controlling its execution. This implies that arriving at a solution relies on the use of
particular computational strategies or on prestored knowledge in long-term memory
(Fayol et al., 1987, p. 188). Storing information during processing, together with the
processing itself, places demands on working memory.
However, there appears to be only a weak relationship between mental
computation performance and short-term memory capacity (Hope & Sherrill, 1987,
p. 110; Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 98). Hope and Sherrill (1987) suggest that
"through the judicious selection of a calculative strategy, a skilled calculator can get
by with fewer short-term memory resources than the selection of more inefficient
strategies would necessitate" (p. 110). Individual differences in mental calculation
can therefore be conjectured to depend upon differences in the choice of
computational strategy, which are dependent upon a knowledge of useful numerical
equivalents, and on the effects that such a choice has on the capacity to process
arithmetical data (Hope, 1985, p. 358; Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 99).

2.8.1 Origins of the Ability to Compute Mentally


Most great adult mental calculators have indicated that their methods of
calculation were largely self taught and arose from playful explorations of number
patterns of interest to them. Hope (1986b, p. 73) suggests that their fascination with
such patterns is the likely key to their remarkable calculative skill. The propensity to
explore arithmetic patterns in their everyday environments characterises skilled
student and adult mental calculators. Hope (1987, p. 337) reports that Charlene, a
13 year-old skilled in mental computation, attributes her largely self-taught ability to
her practising and playing with numbers. Hunter (1962, p. 252) concludes that,
while genetic predispositions may have been an important factor, Professor Aitkens
calculative prowess was largely due to prolonged and intensive practice, begun in
his early teens. Proficient mental calculators appear to be more highly motivated
and tend to enjoy their work more than children who are less proficient (Olander &
Brown, 1959, p. 100).
Those skilled at mental calculation are "driven by a passion for numbers"
(Hope, 1985, p. 372). Shakuntala Devi attributes her career as a stage calculator to

114

her love of numbers. As a child, "numbers and arithmetic were her favorite toys'"
(Jensen, 1990, pp. 269-270). In common with other skilled mental calculators, Devi
perceives numbers in everyday situations in idiosyncratic waysfor example,
interpreting the number 720 on a car's license plate as factorial six. "Each [number]
evokes many associations and transformations, some more interesting than others"
(Jensen, 1990, p. 271).
Hunter (1977a, p. 38) has suggested that an increase in calculative ability, by
whatever means, necessitates the acquisition of a system of calculative strategies.
These are dependent upon the particular way in which numbers are viewed and
therefore on the store of declarative knowledge in long-term memory. This
knowledge "builds up, piece by piece, as a byproduct of its usefulness in pursuits
which interest the individual. There is relatively little deliberate memorization bleakly
undertaken for its own sake" (Hunter, 1978, p. 343). Different skilled mental
calculators employ markedly different calculative systems. One characteristic of
these systems is the direction in which the digits in the solution are usually
producedin a left-to-right or right-to-left order (Hunter, 1977a, p. 39), as presented
in Table 2.4. Hunter (1977a, pp. 38-39) identifies two general properties of the
systems of strategies used by proficient mental calculators. Firstly, solving
problems in terms of the system saves effort. Problems can be solved in a shorter
time. Additionally, the solving of problems that were previously too difficult can then
be achieved. Secondly, effort is required to acquire this energy efficient system.
The system of calculative plans or strategies is developed through experience
with computing mentally (Hunter, 1977a, p. 39). The same degree of constant
practice, undivided attention and knowledge, essential to the development of
proficiency in other fields of human endeavour, is required (Hope, 1985, p. 372).
With lack of practice, skill deteriorates (Hunter, 1977a, p. 40). Charlene's parents
have suggested that she "was faster and more accurate in calculating mentally
when she was younger because she used to play and practice with numbers more
[often]'" (Hope, 1987, p. 337).
Some researchers have achieved some success in improving mental
computation ability through extended periods of practice. College students, who
were given three hundred hours of systematic practice with mental multiplication
strategies used by expert mental calculators, reduced their average solution time for
multiplying five- by two-digit numbers from around 130 to 30 seconds (Staszewski,

115

1988, cited in Jensen, 1990, p. 272). However, as Jensen (1990, p. 272) notes,
such students still do not demonstrate the skill of a lightning calculator such as Devi.
For calculating prodigies, "it seems necessary to posit some initial, probably innate,
advantage on which practice can merely capitalize" (Jensen, 1990, p. 273).
Individuals with an exceptional ability for calculating exact answers mentally
are characterised by diversity and widely different accomplishments. Hunter
(1977a, p. 39) notes that such an ability has been evident in children, illiterates and
gifted mathematicians, as well in those who have mental deficiencies. Some excel
in a limited range of problems, while others are able to solve quite rapidly a wide
range of numerical problems using a variety of ingenious techniques. Others attack
reasonably simple problems by slow and conventional methods, but are remarkable
for their ability to calculate without external aids. For some, average
accomplishments may be considered exceptional, given their limited ability in other
areas.
With respect to Shakuntala Devi, "to all appearances...a perfectly normal and
charming lady" (Jensen, 1990, p. 263), her exceptional skill may be due to her being
able, as a child, to function in a "high fidelity attentional mode" (Jensen, 1990, p.
273) when calculating mentally. However, the source of such exceptional skill
remains uncertain. It may prove to have more to do with motivation than primarily
with attention or ability. As Jensen (1990, p. 273) points out, the biographies of
exceptional performers in many fields record an extreme devotion to practice. From
reports concerning skilled mental calculators, Hunter (1977a, p. 40) suggests that
two general conclusions can be drawn: (a) Individuals gradually develop distinctive
calculative systems, which arise from their experiences with numerical situations,
and which facilitate solving particular types of problems with less effort; and (b) the
limitations of short-term memory place restrictions on the accomplishments of
mental calculators.

2.8.2 Memory for Numerical Equivalents


As suggested previously, in contrast to the average adult, expert mental
calculators have an immense store of numerical equivalents which are drawn upon
with speed and accuracy (Hunter, 1978, p. 370). In 1907, Mitchell (cited in Hope,

116

1985, p. 367) concluded that an individual's vast store of declarative knowledge


could only be built up through a deliberate attempt to commit extended tables to
memory. This, however, is possibly an impossible task, given that many lightning
calculators give the impression that they know the multiplication facts to 100 by 100
and beyond (Gardner, 1977, p. 73). As Hope (1985, p. 367) points out, such a view
is in conflict with evidence suggesting that large amounts of information can be
memorised unconsciously by those who are highly interested in a subject, a
characteristic of skilled mental calculators. Further, to undertake deliberate
memorisation for its own sake would most likely result in boredom and
discouragement (Hunter, 1978, p. 343).
However, given Baroody's (1983) proposition that principled procedural
knowledge plays a key role in the production of number facts, it is possible that a
similar process may be used by expert mental calculators to produce some of the
vast store of numerical equivalents which they appear to possess. With respect to
Aitken, Hunter (1962) notes that it was sometimes difficult to distinguish between
recalling and thinking on the part of the mental calculator: "When he [attained] the
answer to a problem with rapidity, good timing and a feeling of all correct', then he
[could not] easily say whether he calculated [the] answer or recalled it" (p. 256).
This applied particularly to calculations for which Aitken was sure that he had
previously derived answers.
Nonetheless, expert mental calculators demonstrate an exceptional recall of
some large numerical equivalents, especially 2- and 3-digit squares (Hope, 1984,
cited in Hope, 1985, p. 368). Eighty-five percent of the 2-digit squares from 11 to
99, not including the squares of the multiples of 10, were successfully recalled
during Hope's (1985, p. 368) analysis of Charlene's declarative knowledge. During
computation, Charlene relied "heavily on an apparent ability to discern quickly
number properties useful for a calculation and to complete the calculation by
recalling large products from memory" (Hope, 1987, p. 335). To calculate 81 x 27,
Charlene rearranged the calculation to 27 x 3 after realising that 81 was a multiple
of 27. The answer was obtained by recalling and tripling the square of 27.
Evidence provided by Hope and Sherrill (1987, pp. 104-105) indicates that
marked differences between the abilities of skilled and unskilled mental calculators
to retrieve numerical equivalents, useful for mental multiplication, are not always
clearly apparent. In comparing the abilities of skilled and unskilled Grade 11 and 12

117

students, Hope and Sherrill (1987, p. 109) reported that although the difference in
their abilities to recall basic multiplication facts was statistically significant, it was not
substantial. Hence, at least for Grade 11 and 12 students, basic fact mastery may
not be an important factor contributing to differences in mental calculation
performance. With respect to recalling larger numerical equivalents, although the
ability of the skilled students to recall large 2-digit squares was markedly superior,
their knowledge of the extended mental multiplication tables went only slightly
beyond 10 x 10 (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 109).

2.8.3 Memory for Interrupted Working


The use of procedural knowledge during mental computation necessitates a
number of calculative steps and the temporary holding in working storage of details
of the problem task environment and the outcomes of various subproblems (see
Figure 2.3). Consequently, the capability for mentally solving arithmetic problems is
confined by an individual's capacity for storing and processing information in shortterm memory (Wolters et al., 1990, p. 21). According to this information processing
view of mental computation, skilled mental calculators "should possess a superior
mechanism for storing and processing numerical information" (Hope, 1987, p. 338).
Keeping track of the results of a calculation is supposedly the greatest burden
during mental computation (Hope, 1987, p. 338), with forgetting interim calculations
a great source of error (Hitch, 1977, p. 336; Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 110; Wolters
et al., 1990, p. 28).
Many expert mental calculators exhibit superior memory resources for storing
and retrieving numerical information from short-term memory, as measured by digitspan tests such as those in the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children used by
Hope (1987) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale used by Jensen (1990) and
Hope and Sherrill (1987). Hope (1987, p. 336) records that Charlene's digitsforward and digits-backward scores were 12 and 8, respectively, far in excess of the
average child. Some of the 19th century adult lightning calculators studied by Alfred
Binet also exhibited large digit-spans. Binet estimated Jacques Inaudi's forward
memory span to be approximately 42 digits, whereas the accepted average adult's
digit-span is seven (Hope, 1985, p. 370).

118

Nevertheless, strong evidence to support the proposition that short-term


memory capacity is a decisive factor in the superiority of the more competent mental
calculators does not exist (McIntosh, 1991a, p. 4). Olander and Brown (1959, p. 98)
report a correlation coefficient of .35 for the relationship between proficiency with
mental subtraction and short-term memory, as measured by digit-span tests.
Similar results were obtained by Hope and Sherrill (1987, p. 108) with correlations of
.33 and .30 between mental multiplication and recalling digits forwards and digits
backwards, respectively. Hope (1985, p. 371) noted that many people who
demonstrated a superior memory for strings of digits and numbers in general did not
necessarily demonstrate an above average ability for mental computation.
As Mitchell (1907, p. 83, cited in Hope, 1985, p. 371) has cautioned, a
distinction needs to be made with respect to a memory for figures per se and
memory related to an ability to undertake calculations. Hope (1987) has concluded
that "because Charlene could analyze a calculational task in terms of alternative
subproblem formulations, the model of memory function implicit in digit-span
assessments appears far too rudimentary and simplistic to have much explanatory
power" (p. 338). The study of memory processes in skilled mental calculation,
therefore, should not be limited to a model characterised by a sequential processing
of numbers. Hope (1987, p. 339) suggests that the use of tasks devised to assess
an ability to manipulate multiple pieces of information would be more appropriate for
developing an understanding of highly proficient mental computation.
One such test is Raven's Progressive Matrices, a nonverbal test of abstract
reasoning involving the mental manipulation of complex nonrepresentational figures
(Jensen, 1990, p. 265). However, with respect to Shakuntala Devi, Jensen (1990, p.
266) has concluded that her performance on this test was unexceptional, being
similar to those of older, college-educated adults tested in earlier studies and within
the range for university students. Devi, however, does exhibit a superior ability to
encode digits in short-term memory, an ability made possible by a vast knowledge of
numbers, and evident in her performance on memory-search tasks to determine the
presence or absence of single digits held in short-term memory (Jensen, 1990, p.
269). Expert mental calculators search for meaning in the data presented. Aitken
comprehended in terms of rich conceptual maps with his memory "intimately linked
to discern multiple properties that were interwoven into distinctive patterns" (Hunter,
1977b, p. 157).

119

2.8.4 Memory for Calculative Method


Although the relationship between short-term memory capacity and the ability
to compute mentally remains somewhat equivocal, it is evident that enterprising
mental calculators are able to find methods which are memory efficient. Such
methods keep the need for information processing to a minimum. "Through the
judicious selection of a calculative strategy, a skilled [mental] calculator can get by
with fewer short-term memory resources than the selection of more inefficient
strategies would necessitate" (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 110).
Expert mental computers draw upon a large repertoire of calculative methods
(Hunter, 1978, p. 340) stored as part of their procedural knowledge. However,
Owen and Sweller (1989, p. 326) caution that simply knowing and understanding a
strategy is not sufficient to allow its efficient use in a new context, a view supported
by van der Heijden (1995, p. 12). If at least elements of its use are not automated,
sufficient cognitive resources may not be available for the controlled processing of
the novel aspects of the solution process in short-term memory (Silver, 1987, p. 40).
Jensen (1990, p. 270) has observed that the majority of the basic operations used
by Devi probably became automatised during her childhood. Aitken often calculated
using a "repeated cycle of operations on which he [had] a well practised grip...[and
made] use of a strong, steady rhythm which [helped] him, as it were, to throw
forward a loose end and then catch it at an appropriate later moment (Hunter, 1978,
p. 343).
From observing the way in which Aitken approached a mental calculation,
Hunter (1977a, p. 36) concluded that the key decision was the selection of a
strategy that allowed economy of effort, to lead to the answer in the shortest
possible time and with the least difficulty (Hunter, 1977a, p. 37). Strategy selection
is based on the nature of the properties and relationships between the numbers that
are detected in the problem task environment (Hope, 1985, p. 358). This
observation is supported by Barbara Reys (1986b). High ability seventh- and
eighth-grade mental computers were found to make extensive use of number
properties and equivalent number forms to translate an existing problem into one
that is easier to compute mentally (B. J. Reys, 1986b, p. 3279-A). This enables

120

appropriate declarative knowledge of numerical equivalents to be applied, thus


reducing the load on short-term memory. Hence, in many instances, experts
undertake very little calculation. For example, Aitken gave the following explanation
for multiplying 123 by 456: "I see at once that 123 times 45 is 5535 and that 123
times 6 is 738; I hardly have to think. Then 55350 plus 738 gives 56088" (Hunter,
1978, p. 341). Numbers are thought of as distinctive entities (Hunter, 1978, p. 342),
not as individual digits with particular place values. Hunter (1977a) notes that
Bidder, a 19th century English lightning calculator, explained "that, for him, every
number up to a thousand was but one idea, and every number between a thousand
and a million was, to his regret, two ideas" (p. 43).
In Hitch's (1977) view, the most efficient mental strategies are those which
"minimize the effects of rapid forgetting, or at least localize such effects in less
important components of the final answer" (p. 337). On this basis, work from the left
strategies are preferable to work from the right strategies (see Table 2.4), as the
latter may produce the highest errors with the most significant digitsfor example,
with the thousands in a four-digit whole number computation. Additionally, when
calculating from right to left, the answer cannot be stated until the entire calculation
is completed, thus increasing the likelihood that some feature will be forgotten
(Hope, 1985, p. 359). Stage calculators have an additional reason for preferring to
calculate from left to right. Calculating from left to right allows them to begin calling
out an answer while still calculating, thus giving the impression that computing time
is much less that it really is (Gardner, 1977, p. 70).
Skilled mental calculators tend to use left-to-right approaches (Hope & Sherrill,
1987, p. 106; Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 99). In contrast, unskilled (Hope & Sherrill,
1987, p. 104; Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 99) and middle ability (B. J. Reys, 1986b,
3279-A) mental calculators make more use of mental analogues of the standard
written algorithms, which except for division, are right-to-left approaches. Such
strategies, which can involve many carry operations, are infrequently used by
proficient mental calculators (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 106). Carrying can be
avoided, not only by using a left-to-right approach, but also by retrieving rather than
calculating partial answers (Hope & Sherrill, 1987, p. 108).
As well as working from left to right, highly proficient mental calculators tend to
progressively incorporate interim results into a single solution (Hope, 1985, p. 360),
thus obviating the need for an organisation stage (Vakali, 1985) in which partial

121

answers are combined to form a final solution. As Hope and Sherrill (1987) have
observed, when using distribution strategies "the retention of a single result [is]
accomplished by continually retrieving a sum, updating by adding a newly calculated
partial product, and storing the new sum. In the case of factoring, a running product
rather than a sum [is] continually modified" (p. 109) see, for example, additive
distribution and general factoring in Table 2.4. This approach, together with the
tendency of skilled mental calculators to work rapidly, reduces short-term memory
load. When working rapidly, numbers set aside for subsequent use are brought into
the calculation before much forgetting can occur (Hunter, 1977a, p. 43). Speed is
gained by excluding all irrelevances. Writing of Professor Aitken, Hunter (1977a)
observed that "when calculating, he is physically relaxed and inattentive to
everything but the calculation....He solves a problem with concentrated and
streamlined effort" (p. 37).
In conclusion, efficient mental strategies are essentially those that (a) proceed
from left-to-right, (b) eliminate the need for carrying, and (c) allow the progressive
incorporation of partial answers into a single result (Hope & Sherrill, 1978, p. 108)
(see Table 2.4). Hence, the fostering of an ability to compute mentally should
centre on the development of strategies which enable an individual to make efficient
use of a limited capacity for mentally handling data (Hunter, 1977a, p. 43). Hence:

It appears imperative that children be given a chance to build on their own


natural skills by choosing and verbalising mental strategies, and by choosing
when mental computation is appropriate. Less emphasis should be placed on
traditional pen and paper algorithms and more emphasis on identifying and
developing children's legitimate spontaneous strategies. (Cooper et al., 1996,
p. 160)

Such an advocacy arises from the finding that teaching the standard written
algorithms for addition and subtraction to Grade 2 and 3 children may account for
the replacement of left-to-right strategies, based on out-of-school mathematical
knowledge, by inefficient right-to-left approaches to calculating mentally (Cooper et
al., 1996, p. 158).

122

2.9

Developing the Ability to Compute Mentally


McIntosh (1990a, p. 25) has observed that primary school mathematics

continues to be characterised by an emphasis on written computational procedures


which find limited use outside the classroom, while the process used by all, mental
computation, receives little consideration. For those who maintain that there has
been a decline in numeracy, the de-emphasis of mental computation is considered
by some to be a contributing factor (Ewbank, 1977, p. 28; Treffers, 1991, p. 343).
Treffers (1991) suggests that mental computation (and computational estimation) "is
a forceful means to prevent innumeracy, or better, to promote numeracy" (p. 343),
numeracy being "the ability to comprehend and recognise the power and potential of
mathematics..., and to direct that power towards solving personal and social
problems of everyday life" (Handran, Toohey, & Luxton, 1993, p. 223).
In as much as some proficiency with paper-and-pencil techniques needs to be
maintained, such proficiency will be heightened through encouraging children to
solve problems mentally. Children who become proficient with mental computation
learn to think quantitatively with numbers (Carraher et al., 1985, p. 28; Reed & Lave,
1981, p. 442), "a much neglected but greatly needed arithmetic ability" (Koenker,
1961, p. 295). People who are mathematically effective in everyday life rarely use
the standard written algorithms mentally, although such algorithms remain the focus
of classroom instruction. Proficient mental computers tend to use personal
adaptations of paper-and-pencil algorithms or idiosyncratic mental strategies
(Cockcroft, 1982, p. 75, para 256). At present, these are largely self-acquired and
are not well known by teachers (McIntosh, De Nardi, & Swan, 1994, p. 7).
However, mental computation should not become a focus in mathematics
classrooms simply for its social utility. Strategies for computing mentally are, in
essence, strategies for thinking about mathematical tasks (Cobb & Merkel, 1989, p.
80). Fostering the development of thinking strategies that extend beyond the
memorisation and recall of basic facts has a number of positive effects. Conceptual
knowledge, to which procedural knowledge is linked, is enhanced. Building blocks
for further learning are provided and children come to believe that mathematics
makes sense (Cobb & Merkel, 1989, p. 80). This is a belief essential for gaining
mathematical power and one that needs to become a goal of classroom
mathematics (MSEB & NRC, 1990, p. 5). In short, through an emphasis on mental

123

computation, ingenuity and resourcefulness in dealing with numbers are developed


(Menchinskaya & Moro, 1975, p. 74; Sauble, 1955, p. 33), and number sense is
enhanced (Markovits & Sowder, 1994, p. 23).
However, significant changes are required in many mathematics classrooms to
adjust the balance between mental and written arithmetic and to allow children to
gain competency with mental computation (Willis & Stephens, 1991, p. 7). The
climate in which mathematics is taught needs to be such that students' attempts at
problem solution are valued and explored and "where mathematical conclusions are
supported by reasoned argument rather than teachers or answer books" (Putman et
al., 1990, p. 137). In harmony with this view is that of Heirdsfield and Cooper (1995,
p. 5), which contends that the approaches taken to develop skill with mental
computation should be such that the repression of children's spontaneous strategies
should be circumvented.

2.9.1 Approaches to Developing Skill with Mental Computation


Robert Reys et al. (1995, p. 305) suggest that there are two broad ways in
which to view the approaches for developing skill with mental computation: (a) a
behaviourist approach, and (b) a constructivist approach. The former holds that
mental computation is a basic skill and a prerequisite to paper-and-pencil
computation, with proficiency gained through direct teaching and practice (Shibata,
1994, p. 17). This approach necessitates the prior determination of mental
strategies to be taught, as well as a sequence for teaching them. In contrast, the
constructivist view is that mental computation is a process of higher-order thinking in
which "more than the mental application of an algorithm [is involved]. The acts of
both generating and applying a strategy are significant" (R. E. Reys et al., 1995, p.
305). These acts constitute a personal application of idiosyncratic conceptual,
procedural and metacognitive knowledge. Both these approaches, however,
contrast with what may be considered the traditional approach to developing skill
with mental computation. Whereas the behaviourist and constructivist approaches
give recognition to the mental strategies employed, the traditional approach focuses
primarily on the correctness or otherwise of the answer.

124

Traditional Approach
Reminiscent of the role of mental arithmetic as a means of exercising the mind,
as advocated by the proponents of faculty psychology in the 19th century, Davidson
(1980) has called for a return to:

The good old days when our teachers hammered mental arithmetic into us as
students....Starting a lesson with 10 "quickies" helps to sharpen up the mental
processes and gets the students thinking about mathematics right from the
start of the lesson. Answers should be corrected quickly so that the normal
lesson can be started as soon as possible. (p. 24)

Such an approach, which tends to focus on one-step calculations or word


problems (McIntosh, 1990a, p. 41), devalues the key role that mental computation
plays in everyday life and in the development of mathematical ideas. Under this
approach mental computation is not considered as real mathematicsa view in
contrast to the demands of everyday living. It fails to consider the many important
features of mental computation. These include (a) the variety of methods able to be
used to arrive at an answer, (b) the ability of children to use an increasing network
of connections between numerical equivalents and relationships between numbers,
(c) the ability to check an answer by applying different mental strategies, and (d) the
ability to carry out a string of logical operations mentally rather than a single-step
process (McIntosh, 1988, p. 261).
Mental computation taught in this traditional way constitutes testing rather than
teaching (McIntosh, 1991b, p. 53). Ten quick questions at the commencement of a
mathematics lesson may be a source of enjoyment and challenge for more able
students, but for others with limited mathematical ability such questions are "much
more likely to lead to a loss of confidence, increasing antipathy to mathematics and
sometimes even to feelings of humiliation which [will] long be remembered"
(Cockcroft, 1982, p. 75, para 254). For these children, the emphasis on being solely
right or wrong results in mathematics lessons beginning with failure (Giles, 1986, p.
190). The reality of the classroom as a community of mixed abilities is neglected.
Such traditional methods tend to exclude those who most need reinforcement and
involvement (McIntosh, 1988, p. 261). In earlier advocacies for mental (oral)

125

arithmetic, little consideration was given to individual differences. Spitzer (1948)


contended that mental arithmetic was a useful means for keeping the class together
and maintaining class spirit: "Suppose that, at the end of...thirty minutes [of written
work], the teacher uses an oral exercise. All pupils become again one class giving
attention to the same thing. When used in this way, oral arithmetic is a unifying
experience" (p. 54).
Biggs (1967, p. 220, cited in McIntosh, 1990a, p. 41) reported that there was
no relationship between the allocation of time to this traditional approach to mental
computation and attainment. Although the computational tricks that this approach
encourages may have a place in the curriculum, they should not be emphasised at
the expense of instruction that enhances a child's sense of number (Sowder, 1992,
p. 15). It is essential that children be given opportunities to develop the ability to
observe patterns, explain, generalise and to validate (McIntosh, 1978, p. 18). These
abilities can be developed through a focus on the thinking strategies employed
during mental computation rather than being solely concerned with correct or
incorrect answers. As Colburn advocated in 1830, "if...teachers would have the
patience to listen to their scholars and examine their operations, they would
frequently discover very good ways that had never occurred to them before"
(Colburn, 1830, reprinted in Bidwell & Clason, 1970, p. 34).

Alternative Approaches
Although the traditional approach to mental computation is dismissed as
inappropriate for fostering efficient and flexible mental strategies, a preferred
methodology for their development, based on a knowledge of how children learn
mathematics, is as yet unclear (McIntosh, 1991, p. 6; Reys & Barger, 1994, p. 38;
Zepp, 1976, p. 103). Referring to mental multiplication, but applicable to the other
three operations, Hazekamp (1986, p. 117) has observed that little is known about
how children develop strategies for mental computation. Nonetheless, there is
evidence to suggest that gains are made where systematic instruction is provided
(Flournoy, 1954, p. 153; Gracey, 1994, pp. 112-116; Josephina, 1960, p. 200;
Markovits & Sowder, 1994, p. 22; Schall, 1973, pp. 365-366). Schall (1973, pp. 365366) concluded that short, frequent exercises in mental arithmetic seemed to be a

126

worthwhile addition to the traditional paper-and-pencil oriented mathematics


classroom. Further, Flournoy (1954, p. 153) has cautioned that although children in
the intermediate grades can become proficient with mental computation in everyday
situations, such will not occur unless children are provided with definite mental
arithmetic experiences in school.
Three approaches to developing the ability to compute mentally, which are
worthy of further research and analysis, are delineated by Barbara Reys (1991, pp.
8-9). These approaches are characterised by the two opposing beliefs about how
children learn mathematics referred to previously: (a) an authoritarian model based
on teaching as the transmission of knowledgea behaviourist approach; and (b) a
child centred, constructivist approach in which the teacher assumes the role of
intellectual coach (MSEB & NRC, 1990, p. 40).
As suggested previously, the first of these approaches requires mental
computation to be viewed as a topic within the mathematics curriculum. This implies
that the focus should be on specifically teaching the strategies identified as those
used by proficient mental calculators (see particularly Table 2.4). However, Hope
(1987, p. 340) cautions that further research is required to ascertain whether it is
legitimate to plan instruction on this basis. Robert Reys et al. (1995, p. 321) report
that Japanese children, taught through this approach, exhibit a narrow range of
mental strategies, with few of these strategies being idiosyncratic ones.
The second approach, in which mental computation is not viewed as a topic,
recognises that learners construct meaning from sensory and cognitive inputs by
processing it through existing memory structures. Elements of these inputs are
retained in long-term memory in formsconceptual or procedural knowledge, for
examplethat allow for later retrieval to facilitate future processing (Good & Brophy,
1986, p. 229). This approach allows students to generate thinking strategies based
on their own prior experience and knowledge (B. J. Reys, 1991, p. 8). Recognition
of children's prior knowledge and understanding is one means for bridging the gap
between in-school and out-of-school experiences (Masingila et al., 1994, p. 13), an
essential factor in making the classroom more meaningful to students. Trafton
(1989) suggests that mental computation should be viewed within a number sense
sphereto view mental computation "as an extension of children's abilities to
compose and decompose numbers in ways that make sense to them in given
situations" (p. 76). The focus is on the individual learner. Strategies are devised for

127

solving problems that arise in natural settings or as part of planned problem solving
experiences (Reys & Barger, 1994, p. 39). Children are given opportunities to build
distinctive calculative systems, a feature that characterises skilled mental
calculators (Hunter, 1977a, p. 40).
The third approach suggested by B. J. Reys (1991, p. 9), and supported by
Carroll (1996, p. 8), is an amalgam of these two approaches. The focus is on
discussion and on sharing the devised mental strategies with peers and teachers.
Such an approach leads children to think about alternative strategies as they listen
to the ideas of others, to "negotiate shared meaning" (Masingila et al., 1994, p. 13).
Although a teacher, under this approach, does not present a defined set of
strategies to the children, at times it may be appropriate to introduce different ways
of thinking about a problem and in so doing provide children with alternative paths to
the solution of a particular problem, to introduce strategies which may not be
spontaneously invented by children (Carroll, 1996, p. 8). For example, given that
low proficiency students tend to use inefficient right-to-left procedures, Cooper et al.
(1996, p. 159) suggest that such children may benefit from direct teaching of left-toright strategies (see Table 2.4). However, in so doing it is essential that children do
not gain the impression that one strategy is necessarily superior to another, merely
because it is the focus of attention (Cockcroft, 1982, pp. 75-76, para 256; Trafton,
1992, p. 92). Hence, Carroll (1996, p. 8) has suggested that the dilemma over how
to facilitate the development of number sense while providing students with powerful
mental strategies is one for which a solution is not readily apparent.
Greeno (1991, p. 173), in context with the development of number sense, has
suggested that it is a natural, though inappropriate, response to treat mental
computation as a topic in which a set of identified skills becomes the focus. Such
an approach places value only on established mathematical techniques with the
result that children "come to feel that their intuitive ideas and methods are not
related to real mathematics" (Clements & Battista, 1990, p. 35). Further, strategies
memorised without understanding do not aid the development of number sense,
including an ability to flexibly think about numbers, a characteristic of skilled mental
calculators, nor does rote memorisation aid the development of a comprehensive
view of computation (Rathmell & Trafton, 1990, p. 171). It is likely that where
standard strategies are taught, mental computation will lose many of its

128

characteristics, as defined by Plunkett (1979), characteristics that provide


mathematical power for the learner.
"Mental computation is more appropriately viewed as an ongoing emphasis in
a mathematics program than as a separate topic composed of subskills arranged in
order and taught as separate lessons" (Rathmell & Trafton, 1990, p. 160). In
situations where children are allowed to develop their own computational
procedures, as evidenced in the second and third approaches outlined above, they
gain experience in processes that are essential to doing mathematics: Decision
making, creating problem solving strategies, refining home-made algorithms
(Wheeler, 1977, cited in McIntosh, 1992, p. 133).
Such approaches to mental computation take cognisance of the state of
knowledge with respect to how children learn mathematics. Three interrelated
aspects of learning are emphasised, namely, the belief that learning is a process of
knowledge construction, that learning in knowledge dependent, and that learning is
implicitly linked to the situation in which it occurs (Resnick, 1989a, p. 1).
Constructivist instruction gives pre-eminence to the development of personal
mathematical ideas. Children are encouraged to use their own methods for solving
problems (Clements & Battista, 1990, p. 35). Whether or not particular individuals
find particular problem solving settings problematic depends upon their store of
conceptual and procedural knowledge, and their experience with the type of
situation under investigation. Giving recognition to this in the classroom facilitates
the individualisation of instruction (Yackel et al., 1990, p. 14). Children at different
conceptual levels interpret tasks in different ways and use different solution
strategies. Children are the best judges of what is a problem, what makes sense
and what is helpful (Cobb & Merkel, 1989, p. 81).
The third approach, in particular, with its emphasis on discussion, recognises
that skills and the conceptual knowledge on which they are based are not
independent of mental, physical, and social contexts (Resnick, 1989a, p. 3).
Greeno (1991) has argued that an "environment that fosters curiosity and
exploration" (p. 173) is a social construction in which students interact with the
teacher and with each other about quantities and numbers. Whether an information
processing perspective or a situated knowledge perspective of acquiring skill with
mental computation is held, discussion plays an important role. As Putman et al.
(1990, p. 138) have pointed out, it is through communication that individual

129

knowledge is refined and revised in the social setting of the classroom, and through
which children come to understand that which is implicit in particular mathematical
situations. "When children are given opportunities to talk about their mathematical
understandings, problems of genuine communication arise. These problems, as
well as the mathematical tasks themselves, constitute occasions for learning
mathematics" (Yackel et al., 1990, p. 12).
In classrooms that encourage exploration, questioning, verification, and sensemaking, the teacher's role is one of guide and moderator, rather than a dispenser of
answers, the traditional role of the teacher (B. J. Reys, 1992, p. 95), thus making
school mathematics more like folk mathematics (Maier, 1980, p. 23). The guidance
provided by the teacher is a feature that distinguishes constructivism from unguided
discovery (Clements & Battista, 1990, p. 35). Mathematical authority does not
reside solely with the teacher, but in partnership with the children as an intellectual
community (Yackel et al., 1990, p. 18).
In summary, the preferred role of the teacher is that of an intellectual coach.
The Mathematical Sciences Education Board and National Research Council (1990,
p. 40) have outlined the various roles that an intellectual coach assumes and in so
doing have described the ways in which mathematics teaching should be
approached. Equally, the role descriptions describe the ways in which teachers
should approach the task of developing skill with mental computation. At various
times the teacher is required to act as: (a) a role model for the problem solving
process, (b) a consultant, (c) a moderator who leaves much of the decision-making
to the students, (d) an interlocutor who encourages self-reflection, and (e) a
questioner who challenges students to define their strategies and conclusions.

2.9.2 General Pedagogical Issues


If children's performances with mental computation are to be improved, mental
computation abilities must be developed in a regular and systematic manner (B. J.
Reys et al., 1993, p. 314). Given that skilled mental calculators place an emphasis
on opportunities for practice and playing with numbers as a key element in the
development of their proficiency, the main object of each mental computation

130

experience should be to provide children with an opportunity to explore numbers in a


non-threatening environment.

[It] is not a drill session, not even a practice session. Still less is it a
testing session. It is simply an opportunity for playing with the numbers
and exploring the way they seem to relate to one another and the ways in
which one can think about these relationships. (C. Thornton, 1985, pp. 910)

McIntosh et al. (1994, p. 9) maintain that the aims are to help children to see
how to calculate mentally and to realise that mental computation is a creative
process, acceptive of many solution paths. Instruction needs to reflect the spirit of
computing mentallyto explore different ways of reasoning and to share and justify
solutions (Rathmell & Trafton, 1990, p. 158). Everyone needs to be involved and to
find such involvement enjoyable. This can be facilitated by small-group work where
the chance of dispiriting failure is reduced. Further, when working in small groups
difficulties can more easily be perceived and handled sensitively, with children
challenged according to their abilities (Cockcroft, 1982, p. 93, para 319). However,
"mental work will be enjoyed if it is challenging only when the pupils are ready to be
challenged" (Jones, 1988, p. 44). Children need to experience success with mental
computation. Such success is self-fulfilling (Jones, 1988, p. 44) and contributes to
the development of the affective components of mental computation (see Table 2.1),
particularly the gaining of confidence.
Although children cannot be made to think mathematically, their curiosity can
be aroused through their participation in activities that require mental
computationactivities that include games and puzzles (Ewbank, 1977, p. 28).
Maier (1980) suggests that such experiences assist in the development of a
"friendliness with numbers" (p. 23), a key aspect in the formation of a dense web of
connections between numbers stored as declarative knowledge. Whether the
problem context is one from real-life or purely mathematical, the aim is to encourage
a flexible approach and to make explicit through discussion the advantages and
insights that come from alternative strategies (French, 1987, p. 39), thus supporting
the development of appropriate mental computation strategies and the Conceptual
Components and Related Concepts and Skills that underpin them (see Table 2.1).

131

Although there is a role for a weekly series of brief focus sessions, of around
fifteen minutes each, Trafton (1978, p. 212) asserts that mental computation needs
to become an integral part of classroom learning. This view is supported by
Carraher et al. (1985) in their call for educators to "question the practice of treating
mathematical systems as formal subjects from the outset and should instead seek
ways of introducing these systems in contexts which allow them to be sustained by
human daily sense" (p. 28). Given the idiosyncrasy of mental strategies, Barbara
Reys (1985, p. 46) cautions that mental computation is not a topic that can fit into a
certain grade level or sequence of the curriculum. Integrating mental computation
into all relevant classroom activities should aid children in the realisation that mental
techniques are legitimate computational alternatives for particular calculative needs.
However, when focus sessions are conducted, it is better to discuss several
approaches to a few problems rather than focussing on one approach for each of a
large number of questions (McIntosh, 1988, p. 261). The emphasis can therefore
be placed on how answers were calculated in a range of contexts with a range of
number types and operations, rather than on whether they are simply correct or
incorrect. "By merely reinforcing right answers and correcting wrong ones,
traditional mathematics instruction unwittingly stifles children's ability to do their own
thinking" (Kamii, 1990, p. 28).
Both McIntosh et al. (1995, p. 36) and Robert Reys et al. (1995, p. 332) report,
for Australian and Japanese students, respectively, that, for some items, the effect
of mode of presentation on performance is significantfor example Year 5 children
gained more correct responses for 182 + 97 when presented visually and for +
when presented orally (McIntosh et al., 1995, p. 64). It is likely that the varied
modes of presentation lead to different strategies being employed by different
students, resulting in differential performances levels (McIntosh, et al., 1995, p. 60).
McIntosh (1988, p. 261) suggests that although many tasks may be presented
visually, the answers should be oral. By so doing, children are able to focus on the
mental strategy used rather than on writing the response, a view supported by
Spitzer (1948, p. 215). In Frenchs (1987, p. 41) view, the oral presentation of
problem contexts places these contexts closer to those faced in everyday situations,
as well as eliminating the reading difficulties which some children may experience.
The relationships between numbers more clearly become the focus (Hazekamp,
1986, p. 123).

132

Nonetheless, there is some evidence to suggest that children perform better,


as determined by the number of correct answers, in situations where written tasks
are presented. Sister Josephina (1960), based on research involving workbook
examples, has concluded that "teachers in presenting mental arithmetic problems
strictly by the so-called oral method' actually inhibit better productivity than if their
pupils had the written problems before them" (p. 200). The difficulties that some
children experience with oral presentation may stem from their lack of an ability to
visualise numbers or from their inability to retain essential elements of the problem
task environment in short-term memory while processing occurs.
Olander and Brown (1959, p. 98) reported that the greatest difficulty that Grade
6 to 12 students experienced when calculating mentally was with the visualisation of
numbers presented orally. Therefore there is a need for children to gain experience
with a variety of representational models and to develop an ability to switch
smoothly and quickly from one to the other (Giles, 1986, p. 191). Younger children
should be allowed to use aids such as cubes and number lines when computing
mentally to assist in the development of mental representations (Ewbank, 1977, p.
28).

2.9.3 Sequence for Introducing Computational Methods


Wherever practicable, children should be encouraged to perform mental
calculations in preference to those reliant on paper-and-pencil (AEC, 1991, p. 109).
Classroom experience suggests that children have difficulty with mental computation
(and computational estimation) when paper-and-pencil skills are developed prior to
an emphasis on mental procedures (Musser, 1982, p. 40). This is the sequence
traditionally employed in mathematics classrooms (Irons, 1990a, p. 20 ; Rathmell &
Trafton, 1990, p. 156), as represented in Figure 2.4. However, paper-and-pencil
procedures for addition, subtraction, and multiplication conflict with those for mental
computation. The former emphasise right-to-left processing compared to the left-toright processing evident in many heuristic mental strategies. As Cooper et al. (1992,
pp. 100-101) point out, there is a need to re-evaluate the sequence in which
procedures for mental computation, computational estimation, and paper-and-pencil
calculation are introduced.

133

Although the research evidence is limited, focussing on mental computation


strategies prior to introducing the standard paper-and-pencil algorithm for each
operation "is both powerful and effective" (Rathmell & Trafton, 1990, p. 156), a view
supported by Carroll (1996, p. 3) in context with the trial of The University of
Chicago School Mathematics Project curriculum. Such a practice would assist
children to make more appropriate use of the range of mental and written strategies
which they have available; a flexible approach to calculating would result (Thornton,
Jones, & Neal, 1995, p. 483). Biggs (1969, p. 25) suggests that mental agility with
numbers should precede written practice, and that the "crucial test of readiness for
practice in written computation with tens and units [is] the ability to add two 2-figure
numbers mentally by an efficient method" (The Schools Council,
Concept of Operation

Basic Number Facts

Paper-and-Pencil Computation

Computational
Estimation

Figure 2.4.

Mental
Computation

Traditional sequence for introducing computational procedures for


each operation (Adapted from Irons, 1990a, p. 20)

1966, cited in Ewbank, 1977, p. 29). Related to the development of this agility is a
child's proficiency with computational estimation. In Lindquist's view (1984, p. 599),
this should occur prior to the introduction of paper-and-pencil procedures,
particularly before the standard written algorithm for each operation becomes the
focus. Figure 2.5 presents an alternative view of the sequence in which
computational skills should be developed.

134

In implementing this sequence, recognition needs to be given to the role of


mental computation as a fundamental component of computational estimation (see
Figure 2.2). Hence, the development of selected strategies for mentally calculating
exact answers needs to occur prior to a focus on computational estimation
(Leutzinger, Rathmell, & Urbatsch, n.d., p. 10). Additionally, Case and Sowder
(1990, p. 95) demonstrated that until around 12 years of age, children are unable to
successfully co-ordinate the two qualitatively different procedures involved in finding
approximate answers, namely (a) converting from exact to approximate numbers
using nearness judgements, and (b) mentally computing with these numbers (Case
& Sowder, 1990, p. 88). This finding has implications for the introduction of
computational estimation. As Sowder (1992, p. 18) has observed,
Concept of Operation

Basic Number Facts

Computational
Estimation

Mental
Computation

Paper-and-Pencil Computation

Figure 2.5.

An alternative sequence for introducing computational procedures for


each operation

this finding has led to the belief that a focus on computational estimation should not
occur too soon after the introduction of a particular operation. Rather, the teachers
should focus on developing (a) number size concepts, (b) mental computation
strategies, and (c) on estimation-type problems that do not require the coordination
of complex skills.
With respect to written procedures, if standard paper-and-pencil algorithms are
introduced after an initial focus on strategies for calculating both exact and

135

approximate answers mentally, and following experiences with informal written


techniques, it is likely that the standard algorithm for each operation will come to be
viewed as one of many possible ways for calculating in particular contexts, rather
than the way to calculate (Ross, 1989, p. 51). The ability of children to select
appropriate calculative methods will be enhanced, a key computational outcome of
classroom mathematics programs (AEC, 1991, p. 115; NCTM, 1989, p. 44).
Nonetheless, "When should paper-and-pencil algorithms be introduced?" is, in
Rathmell and Trafton's (1990, p. 171) view, a key question that remains. They point
out that the placement of written algorithms for particular year-levels, the size of the
numbers to be manipulated, and the manner in which the algorithms are taught will
be fundamentally changed by the introduction of curricula in which mental
computation and computational estimation skills are introduced early in combination
with a ready-availability of electronic calculating devices.

2.9.4 Assessing Mental Computation


"What students learn and how they learn will be influenced by what they think
teachers...value. Their view of what really counts in learning and doing mathematics
will relate quite closely to what is assessed" (AEC, 1991, p. 21). For this reason
Robert Reys (1992, p. 71; 1985, p. 16) has advocated the regular testing of mental
computation skills. This would provide an overview of a student's performance with
mental computation as well as a record of progress over time. Although recognising
that such testing may take several different forms, Robert Reys (1985, pp. 15-16)
concluded that, where testing occurs, emphasis appears to be given to traditional
mental computation testing, with each test limited to between 10 and 20 context-free
examplesa summative view of assessment. Such testing may "let both child and
teacher know whether the child is good' or bad' at mental arithmetic. But does little
or nothing to help the child acquire good mental methods" (McIntosh, 1991b, p. 53).
Nevertheless, Robert Reys (1985, p. 16) does acknowledge that additional
learning may occur in instances where particular examples are discussed.
Assessment needs to be an integral part of the teaching cycle, the major purpose of
which is to improve learning (AEC, 1991, p. 21). All components of mental
computation (see Table 2.1), not merely the answers to particular items, need to be

136

focussed upon at various times during assessment. The spotlight needs to be on


the process of computing mentally.
This requires that teachers go beyond traditional testing procedures to
decisively measure mental computation skills (Sowder, 1992, p. 23). Opportunities
for assessment, using observational techniques, occur during the discussion and
sharing of strategies used. To gain deeper insights into the approaches that
children use to compute mentally, structured interviews may be required. Selfreporting techniques can also provide useful information, particularly with respect to
the level of confidence felt while computing mentally. In D. J. Clarke, D. M. Clarke,
and Lovitt's (1990, p. 123) view, assisting students to develop the skills necessary
for self-monitoring of their progress would be a useful and empowering educational
goal.
Although it is essential that teachers extend their repertoire of assessment
techniques, a limited place remains for the traditional timed mental computation
tests. To the extent that they continue to used, Robert Reys (1985, pp. 15-16) has
remarked that such tests should be kept short, start by focussing on one operation
with specific numbers, emphasise the mental nature of the tests, use oral and visual
presentations of the problem-task environment, and build in groups of examples for
which the strategy for one assists in solving anotherfor example, to be able to use
the knowledge of 6 x 100 to calculate 6 x 99.

2.10 Summary and Implications for Mental Computation Curricula


As stated in Section 1.4.1, the purpose of this literature review was to "analyse
the pedagogical, socio-anthropological and psychological literature relevant to
mental computation" to provide a comprehensive understanding of key issues. In so
doing, a framework for analysing past and contemporary beliefs and practices
related to mental computation has been developed. This framework is reflected in
the questions devised to guide the investigations discussed in Chapters 3 and 4.
With respect to those which guided the analysis presented in this chapter, the
following is a summary of principal findings.

137

1.

What are the recent developments in mathematics education of relevance to


mental computation?

The developments in mathematics education of relevance to mental


computation centre on issues related to redefined beliefs about numeracy,
computation, number sense, and how children learn mathematics. The perceived
interrelationships between these aspects are essential for children to gain
mathematical power. A wide definition of numeracy encompassing mathematics
beyond the development of competence and confidence in computational skills per
se needs to be taken. Children need to be able to interpret, understand, use, apply
and communicate numerical and spatial ideas associated with daily living skills,
work tasks and recreational activities, many of which necessitate proficiency with
mental computation.
The impact of electronic calculating devices requires that the number strand of
the mathematics syllabus be reassessed with a view to helping children develop
appropriate methods for calculating (Willis, 1991, p. 3)that is, the methods should
be relevant to the tools available and to the contexts within which the mathematics
is embodied. Many of these methods, whether mental, written or electronic, will be
idiosyncratic to the individual. Hence, a realistic balance among the various forms
of calculation needs to be encouraged (B. J. Reys & R. E. Reys, 1986, p. 4).
Sowder (1992, p. 5) believes that an ability to calculate mentally using
idiosyncratic strategies is an indicator of number sense, essential to which is an
ability to compose and decompose numbers, an ability suggested as a component
of mental computation (see Table 2.1). Number sense has been described as the
demonstration, both implicitly and explicitly, of a "good intuition about numbers and
their relationships" (Howden, 1989, p. 11). This is characterised by having an indepth understanding of numbers and their relative magnitudes, the relative effects of
operating on numbers, together with a well-developed recognition of multiple
relationships between numbers (NCTM, 1989, p. 39). Characteristics such as these
are essential to developing proficiency with mental computation (see Table 2.1).
For children to gain mathematical power, learning needs to occur in
meaningful, problem solving situations in which there is scope for active involvement
and reflection (Literacy and Numeracy Diagnostic Assessment Project, 1991, p. 7).
Such a child-centred focus allows the child to construct new knowledge from that

138

already possessed. This is enhanced through social interaction, opportunities for


which are believed to facilitate the development of multiple relationships between
numbers and computational techniques. The belief that learning necessitates
acquiring situated knowledge is interrelated with those that hold that learning is a
process of knowledge construction and is knowledge-dependent (Resnick, 1989a, p.
1). Such beliefs reflect findings which suggest that, as individuals engage in mental
computation, they modify the mathematics they use to suit the structure of a
particular situation and their perception of its mathematical elements (Lave, 1985, p.
173). A holistic view is taken, one that is characterised by a focus on the quantities
involved. Consequently, engaging children in mental computation is considered to
be a means for linking school and folk mathematics, as well as a means for
enhancing mathematical knowledge and confidence in its application.

2.

What is the place of mental computation within the calculative process and
particularly its relationships with computational estimation?

In any calculative situation, an individual needs to be able to decide what


operations to use, to choose the method of calculation, and, once the calculation
has been undertaken, to judge the reasonableness of the solution in terms of the
characteristics of the problem. In choosing a calculation method, the Australian
Education Council (1991, p. 109) suggests that mental computation should be the
method of first resort, particularly for less complex calculations. In any given
situation, the appropriateness of a method of calculation is dependent upon the
nature of the operation, the degree of precision required, the availability of particular
calculative tools, and particularly the confidence with which the situation is
approached (see Figure 2.1). Nonetheless, as represented in Figure 2.1,
irrespective of the method of calculation, some mental calculation occursnumber
relationships need to be recalled or calculated, estimates need to be mentally
determined to verify the reasonableness of solutions. Children need to be given
opportunities to develop the ability to make sensible choices between mental
computation, paper-and-pencil and technological methods (AAMT & CDC, 1987, p.
2).
As noted by Robert Reys (1984, p. 58), mental computation is the cornerstone
of the range of strategies used in computational estimation (see Figure 2.2).

139

However, the mental strategies used, and the numbers manipulated, are subsets of
those involved in situations where the mental calculation of exact answers is
undertaken. The interrelationships between mental computation and computational
estimation strategies, and their reliance on number comparison and elements of
number sense has led Threadgill-Sowder (1988, pp. 194-195) to suggest that their
development occurs in a spiral fashion, each feeding on and strengthening the
others.
Although mental computation is considered to be an essential prerequisite to
computational estimation (see Figure 2.2), Robert Reys (1984, p. 551) has
suggested that the reverse does not apply. This appears to be in conflict with Irons'
(1990b, p. 1) view that the use of getting closer strategies during computational
estimation may assist in the development of flexible approaches for calculating
exact answers. Nonetheless, it is possible that although a limited range of mental
computation strategies may be essential for initial experiences with computational
estimation, strategies for calculating exact answers may be refined by additional
experiences during the calculation of approximate answers.

3.

What is the nature of mental computation as perceived by mathematics


educators, both contemporaneously and historically?

Terms used to describe the mental calculation of exact answers during the
past century have been characterised by their impreciseness. Although a concern
for developing skill with the calculation of approximate answers is relatively new to
primary school mathematics curricula (B. J. Reys, 1986a, p. 22), the use of such
terms as mental arithmetic, oral arithmetic and mental to refer to the mental
calculation of exact answers, often included references to approximate answers.
Additionally, in using these terms, the focus was on the correctness of the answer
rather than on the strategies used. Mental arithmetic was seen as a preamble to the
real arithmetic, usually written, which was to follow.
In an endeavour to distinguish between the mental calculation of exact and
approximate answers, as well as to shift the focus from the answer to the processes
used during calculation, it has become customary in the mathematics education
literature to define mental computation as the process of producing an exact answer
mentally without resort to calculators or any external recording device, usually with

140

nontraditional mental procedures (Hazekamp, 1986, p. 116). Nonetheless, this term


continues to be used imprecisely. For example, in Student Performance Standards
in Mathematics for Queensland Schools (1994, p. 9) mental computation is used to
refer to the calculation of both exact and approximate answers.
In an endeavour to overcome the confusion in the use of the terms mental
arithmetic and oral arithmetic, Cockcroft (1982, p. 92, para 316) employed the
expression mental mathematics to refer to the mentalmental computation and
computational estimationand oral work essential to mathematics learning.
Cockcroft (1982) was giving recognition to the role that discussion and explanation
need to play in learning mathematics. Although oral arithmetic became equated
with mental arithmetic as "arithmetic done without the aid of paper and pencil"
(Flournoy, 1954, p. 148), oral has had a multiplicity of meanings during this century.
These included instances where the problem was presented orally (Thorndike,
1922, p. 262) and where the calculation was worked aloud (Suzzallo, 1912, cited in
Hall, 1954, p. 352). The concern, therefore, was not simply with the calculation
being a mental one, but also with the nature of the presentation and mode of
response. In common with the current use of the term mental computation, Hall
(1954, p. 353) suggested that mental arithmetic should refer to arithmetic problems
that arise orally, in writing or in the head, and where intermediate steps are typically
recorded mentally, irrespective of how the answer is presented.
By focusing on the approaches used to calculate mentally, the differences
between mental and written computation can be clearly perceived. Mental
strategies are considered to be fleeting, variable, flexible, active, holistic,
constructive and iconic, but are limited in the range of contexts in which they may be
applied. This contrasts with written procedures which are viewed as standardised,
contracted, efficient, automatic, symbolic, general and analytic (Plunkett, 1979, pp.
2-3). Given that mental procedures require the mathematics involved to be
understood, that thinking is encouraged, and that the focus is on the quantities
involved rather than on mathematical symbols, the variability and flexibility of mental
strategies arise naturally from the process of calculating mentally. That these are
dependent upon an individual's declarative and procedural knowledge supports the
belief that number sense and mental computation are inextricably bound.

141

4.

What are the roles currently perceived for mental computation within and
beyond the classroom?

Although recognising that mental computation constitutes a valuable skill in


everyday living, proponents for an increased classroom emphasis on mental
computation suggest that it should have a broader role. Recognition is given to the
central role that mental procedures occupy throughout mathematics (Cockcroft,
1982, p. 75, para 255). Additionally, as Reys and Barger (1994, p. 31) suggest,
mental computation is believed to provide a vehicle for promoting thinking,
conjecturing and generalising based on conceptual understanding. Such a
provision contributes to the development of an in-depth understanding of the
structure of numbers and their interrelationships, and hence to ingenious
approaches to manipulating numbers (R. E. Reys, 1984, p. 549). Mental
computation, therefore, may be viewed as a pedagogical tool which facilitates the
meaningful development of concepts and skills associated with the number strand of
the mathematics syllabus. Consequently, contrary to current practice, it is
considered to be an essential prerequisite to written methods of calculation.
Recognition is also being given to the gulf between learning and using school
mathematics and that used outside the classroom (Masingila et al., 1994, p. 3). It
has been demonstrated that the algorithms, particularly paper-and-pencil ones,
traditionally taught in schools are generally not used once children leave the
classroom (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 95; French, 1987, p. 41; Murray et al., 1991, p.
50). Willis (1990, p. 9) has asserted that the usefulness of school mathematics
would be increased if it reflected the techniques used in everyday life. Folk
mathematics is characterised by techniques that are often mental (Maier, 1980, p.
23) and frequently involve the manipulation of non-standard units using invented
strategies (Lave, 1985, p. 173). This implies that there needs to be a shift in the
way problems are presented in the classroomfrom the presentation of
prepackaged examples, with or without a veneer of real-world characteristics, to
open situations in which children are free to formulate their own problems. In such
situations the relevant inputs may be as negotiable as the solutions (Murtaugh,
1985, p. 189).

142

5.

What are the affective and cognitive components, including commonly used
mental strategies, that constitute skill in computing exact answers mentally?

Little research is available to authoritatively present a concise, but


comprehensive, model of the affective and cognitive components of mental
computation. Nonetheless, Sowder and Wheeler's (1989) model for specifying the
components of computational estimation was considered an appropriate startingpoint from which to analyse and synthesise the available data. It was concluded
that the concepts and skills, considered essential for mental computation to become
a flexible, active and constructive process, could be classified under the following
headings: Affective Components, Conceptual Components, Related Concepts and
Skills, and Heuristic Strategies Based Upon Relational Understanding (see Table
2.1).
Although other categories of mental strategies were identified, only those
based on relational understanding were included in the proposed model. Such
strategies are based on relational knowledge, as proposed by Skemp (1976),
knowledge that allows an individual to understand why particular strategies are the
most appropriate for particular situations. The thinking which occurs, a key element
of mental computation, is based on an individual's declarative, conceptual and
procedural knowledge (Sowder, 1991, p. 3), knowledge that is rich in relationships.
The model recognises the role of affective components as facilitators of mental
computationhaving the confidence with which to select and apply particular
strategies in particular contexts (see Table 2.1). Underpinning this process are what
Sowder and Wheeler (1989, p. 131) have defined as conceptual components.
These have been interpreted to refer to those elements that relate to an
understanding of the factors on which the calculation of exact answers mentally is
based (see Table 2.1). Critical to being able to engage efficiently and creatively in
mental computation is the ability to recognise when it is appropriate to calculate
mentally. Additionally, an individual needs to be able to recognise that the
appropriateness of a chosen strategy is dependent upon the context of the
calculation.
The components classified as Related Concepts and Skills, in accordance with
Sowder and Wheeler's (1989, p. 132) definition, are those that may be considered to
indirectly influence an individual's proficiency with calculating exact answers

143

mentally (see Table 2.1). Essential to this is an ability to translate a problem into a
more mentally manageable form so that the demands on memory may be shifted
from short- to long-term memory. Based on a proposal of Threadgill-Sowder (1988,
p. 184), two key questions have been hypothesised, questions which are considered
cyclically during the process of mental computation. These relate to the
modification of the numbers embodied in the mathematical situation so that the
resulting operations may be answered by recall, and to a consideration of how the
operational sequence will proceed as a consequence of the way in which the
numbers have been expressed (see Table 2.1). Underpinning these processes is
an individual's conceptual knowledge related to, for example, place value, basic
facts, numeration, arithmetical principles and their relationships.
The classification of mental strategies for calculating exact answers beyond the
basic facts was made difficult by a number of characteristics of the available
research. Not only has such research been limited (McIntosh et al., 1995, p. 2), but
also it has primarily focussed on addition and subtraction. In instances where
studies have focussed on all four operations, clear distinctions have not always
been adequately drawn between the strategies used for basic facts and those used
to calculate with larger numbers (see Section 2.7.4). An added difficulty was that
the data was obtained from a range of samples, varying with respect to age, grade,
ability, cultural background, and contexts. Further, as McIntosh (1990b, p. 13)
points out, the classification of strategies has often relied on subjective
interpretations of interview protocols, and on written reports of past performances by
expert mental calculators (Hope, 1985, p. 358), thus preventing certainty, on the part
of the researcher, with respect to the use of particular strategies in particular
contexts. Finally, these difficulties have been compounded by the absence of
agreed descriptors for many of the commonly identified approaches.
Nonetheless, the analysis presented in this chapter, centred on a model
derived from one proposed by McIntosh (1990c, p. 1), provides a comprehensive
summary of identified strategies. The following categories were used as organisers:
Counting strategies (see Table 2.2), Strategies based upon instrumental
understanding (see Table 2.3), and Heuristic strategies based upon relational
understanding (see Table 2.4). Counting strategies, which primarily rely on
counting-on or counting-back, do not entail the manipulation of complex numerical
relationships, a skill indicative of a well-developed sense of number. The category

144

of strategies that depends on instrumental understanding is viewed as an


intermediary stage. These strategies, which include the use of the mental form of a
written algorithm, are characterised by the application of, in Skemp's (1976) terms,
"rules without reason" (p. 20).
As noted previously, the use of strategies which rely on relational
knowledgefor example: Working from the left, Using factorsare indicative of the
view of mental computation as a process that involves the application of selfdeveloped techniques which are dependent upon a knowledge of the properties of
numbers and arithmetical operations (B. J. Reys, 1989, p. 72). Users of these
strategies tend to take a holistic view of the numerical situation, modifying the
numbers and operations involved to capitalise upon perceived relationships and
known numerical equivalents, thus reducing the demands on short-term memory.

6.

What are the affective and cognitive characteristics exhibited by skilled mental
calculators, including the role that memory plays in the process of calculating
exact answers mentally?

Individuals proficient at mental computation appear to enjoy mentally


manipulating numbers and are more highly motivated than those who are less
proficient (Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 100). They exhibit a passion for numbers,
which is reflected in the degree to which they practice calculating (Hope, 1985, p.
372). Further, skilled mental calculators display a proclivity for exploring number
patterns in their everyday environments, often in idiosyncratic ways. Such
behaviours result in the largely unconscious build-up of systems of mental strategies
based upon their understanding of numbers and number relationships which are
stored as declarative knowledge (Hunter, 1978, p. 343). Nonetheless, the basis for
the exceptional skill of expert mental calculators remains unclear. As Jensen (1990,
p. 273) has observed, it may be motivational, or it may be attributable to superior
powers of concentration, rather than to an ability variable, although it is likely to be a
combination of all three.
Hunter (1977a, p. 40) has concluded that the success of those engaged in
mental computation is limited by their short-term memory capacities. However,
strong evidence to support this capacity as a decisive factor in determining
superiority in mental computation does not exist (McIntosh, 1991a, p. 4). In

145

considering the demands placed on memory during mental computation, Hunter


(1978, p. 339) has proposed three forms, namely, Memory for calculative method,
Memory for numerical equivalents and Memory for interrupted working. Although
the latter places demands on short-term memory, the first two draw on long-term
memory components (see Figure 2.3).
Given the limitations of short-term memory, proficient mental calculators tend
to shift the burden of calculation onto long-term memory by accessing their superior
store of declarative and procedural knowledge to devise efficient methods for
approaching a problem (Hunter, 1978, p. 343). The key is to select a strategy from
long-term memory that maximises the use of the properties and relationships
between the numbers that are detected in the problem task environment (Hope,
1985, p. 358), thus reducing the number of partial answers that need to be
determined. Further, the demands on short-term memory are reduced by their
tendency to work quickly, thus allowing stored partial answers to be used before
forgetting occurs.

7.

What are the teaching approaches and sequence necessary for the
development of mental computation skills?

The approaches recommended for developing skill with mental computation


stem from the belief that the focus in classrooms should not be solely on its social
utility, but should recognise that mental strategies are, in essence, ways of thinking
about mathematics (Cobb & Merkel, 1989, p. 80). The focus needs to be on
developing resourceful and ingenious approaches to working with numbersto
promote number sense. Hence, children's learning experiences should be such that
their spontaneous calculative plans are recognised and valued (Heirdsfield &
Cooper, 1995, p. 5), so that mental computation does not lose its essential
characteristics, as described by Plunkett (1979, pp. 2-3).
In contrast to what has been defined as the traditional approach to mental
computation in this study, where the focus has been on the correctness of the
answer, Robert Reys et al. (1995, p. 305) have identified two broad approaches in
which mental strategies are of primary concern. The first, which may be classified
as a behaviourist approach, involves the direct teaching and practice of a
predetermined set of mental strategies in preparation for paper-and-pencil

146

computation, whereas the second is a constructivist approach. This approach


reflects the belief that mental computation is a process of higher-order thinking in
which the act of generating a mental strategy is of equal importance to its
application (R. E. Reys et al., 1995, p. 305). In the former, the teacher takes an
authoritarian stance, whereas with the implementation of a constructivist approach
the teacher assumes the role of an intellectual coach, assisting students to develop
distinctive calculative systems, a characteristic of skilled mental calculators (Hunter,
1977a, p. 40).
However, as Hazekamp (1986, p. 117) has observed, little is known about how
children develop strategies for mental computation. Further, there is some evidence
(Flournoy, 1954, p. 153; Gracey, 1994, pp. 112-116; Josephina, 1960, p. 200;
Markovits & Sowder, 1994, p. 22; Schall, 1973, pp. 365-366) to suggest that gains
are made, at least with respect to the correctness of the answer, where systematic
instruction has been provided. Hence, a third approach suggested by Barbara Reys
(1991, p. 9) and Carroll (1996, p. 8) is worthy of consideration. This approach,
which contains elements of the first two, has as its focus discussion and the sharing
of devised mental strategies with teachers and other students. Although the
teacher, under this essentially constructivist approach, does not present a
predetermined set of strategies to children, alternative solution paths may be
presented to children in instances where, for example, the efficiency of particular
procedures is of primary concern.
More generally, as Rathmell and Trafton (1990) assert, "mental computation is
more appropriately viewed as an ongoing emphasis in a mathematics program than
as a separate topic composed of subskills arranged in order and taught as separate
lessons" (p. 160). Children need to be given opportunities to play with numbers (C.
Thornton, 1985, p. 9), using games and puzzles, for example. The aim is to assist
children to see how to calculate mentally and to come to understand that mental
computation is a creative process with many legitimate solution paths for particular
problems. This understanding will be enhanced where opportunities are provided
for children to share and justify their answers and methods of solution (Rathmell &
Trafton, 1990, p. 58). Where focussed teaching occurs, it is recommended that
several approaches to a few problems be considered, rather than one approach to
each of a large number of questions (McIntosh, 1988, p. 261). It is essential that
children find mental computation enjoyable and that they experience success.

147

However, children should be challenged only when they are ready to be challenged
(J. P. Jones, 1988, p. 44). Although this may be difficult for a teacher to determine,
the critical indicator is the perceived confidence with which a child approaches
mental computation in particular situations.
The overriding emphasis needs to be on integrating mental computation into all
relevant classroom activities in an endeavour to maximise the links to the real-world
of the child, thus capitalising on their idiosyncratic mathematical knowledge.
Nevertheless, consideration needs to be given to the emphasis on mental viz--viz
written methods of computation at particular stages within a class mathematics
program. There is some evidence to suggest that children have difficulty with
mental computation when paper-and-pencil skills, particularly the standard written
algorithms, are developed prior to a focus on mental strategies (Cooper et al., 1992,
p. 100; Musser, 1982, p. 40). This has resulted in the suggestion that, for each
operation, the focus should be on mental computation prior to the introduction of
written procedures (see Figure 2.5). Carol Thornton et al. (1995, p. 40) have
suggested that the consequences of such an approach would be the development
of more flexible computational strategies, both mental and written. This contrasts
with the inflexibility of the mental strategies which result from the traditional
approach to teaching mental computation.

2.11 Concluding Points


For children to develop powerful mental computation strategies a second
revolution, as proposed by McIntosh (1992, p. 134), is required. Although
McIntosh's (1992, pp. 131-133) first revolution centres on changing the way in which
mental computation is viewed by teachers, the second revolution is concerned not
simply with the provision of opportunities for children to develop skill with mental
computation, but, more importantly, that these opportunities should occur in
association with teachers finding numbers a source of enjoyment for themselves.
Should this occur, McIntosh (1992) asserts that "a window onto a wonderful,
enjoyable and fascinating worldthe world of numbers[will be opened for children]"
(p. 134). However, as Barbara Reys (1991) cautions, the changes in teacher
attitudes and "in the content and methodology of teaching computation...will not

148

occur without considerable teacher education, and [these] will take both time and a
concerted effort by the entire mathematics education community" (p. 12).
The nature of the teacher education required is not only dependent upon the
current state of teacher knowledge and attitudes towards mental computation, but
also on an understanding of the context within which these have developed. Hence,
prior to the development of a proposed syllabus strand for mental computation
(Chapter 5) and a consideration of the implications for professional development for
Queensland teachers (Chapter 6), an understanding of their current beliefs and
practices needs to be gained (Chapter 4). Additionally, an understanding of the
nature of mental computation and how it has been taught under each of the
Queensland syllabuses provides the background and context for current beliefs and
practices. An analysis of mental computation in the mathematics syllabuses in
Queensland from 1860 to 1965 is the focus of the next chapter (Chapter 3).

152

CHAPTER 3

MENTAL COMPUTATION IN QUEENSLAND:


1860-1965

3.1

Introduction
As stated in Chapter 1, little has been documented about mental computation

within Australian mathematics curricula. In an attempt to redress this situation, this


study aimed to document the nature and role of mental computation, and associated
pedagogical practices, under each of the Queensland mathematics syllabusesthe
fourth principal purpose of this study outlined in Section 1.3. Further, by
undertaking an analysis of mental computation prior to the introduction of the current
Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a), issues
related to the refocussing of mental computation, as outlined in Chapter 2, can be
placed in context with past beliefs and teaching practices. Such knowledge is
essential to informed debate about the features of arithmetic teaching necessary for
meeting the future needs of Queensland primary school children. As noted earlier,
in order to introduce changes to an educational program, it is essential to know what
has gone before (Skager & Weinberg, 1971, p. 50)to be able to capitalise on past
successes while avoiding the pitfalls.
The analysis of mental computation and Queensland mathematics syllabuses
has been divided into three time-periods: (a) 1860-1965, (b) 1966-1987, and (c)
post-1987. These periods were selected on the basis of the perceived emphasis on
mental computation, as embodied in syllabus documents and their implementation.
The first of these, which is the focus of this chapter, is characterised by syllabuses
which contained specific references to the mental calculation of exact answers
beyond the basic facts. Hence, of the three periods, it is the one for which a depth
of understanding of the nature and role of mental computation in Queensland
primary school classrooms may be gained from primary documents.

153

3.1.1 Method
In contrast to empirical research, there is no single, definable method of
historical inquiry (Edson, 1986, cited by Wiersma, 1995, p. 234). Such inquiry is
primarily qualitative, with the process being essentially a holistic one. Although the
steps in undertaking historical research can be delineated, there is considerable
overlap of the activities associated with eachfor example, analysis and
interpretation occurs during the identification of sources of evidence, during data
collection, as well as during the synthesising and interpretation phases.
Subsequent to the identification of Mental Arithmetic in Queensland: 1860-1965 as
the topic to be investigated in this study, the following steps were undertaken:

The identification of relevant sources of evidence and collection of data.

The evaluation of the data for their authenticity and validity.

The synthesis of the data into a meaningful thematic pattern.

The reporting of the analysis and interpretation of the data, together with
the conclusions drawnthis chapter.

(Borg & Gall, 1989, p. 805; Wiersma, 1995, p. 235)

Sources of Evidence

During this study, the basic rule of historical research was followed, namely to
place an emphasis on primary sources of evidence (Wiersma, 1995, p. 238). The
primary data, upon which the issues of significance to mental computation in
Queensland were analysed, were obtained from:
Queensland mathematics schedules and syllabuses (1860-1964) published
in the Queensland Government Gazette (1860-1904), The Education Office
Gazette (1904), and as separate documents (1914-1964).
Annual reports of General Inspectors (1876-1904) and District Inspectors of
Schools (1869-1965) published as part of the annual reports of the Board of
General Education (1865-1875), Department of Public Instruction (18761939), or as archival material (1940-1965).

154

The Education Office Gazette (1899-1965), the official publication to schools


by the Department of Public Instruction, and from 1952 by the Department of
Education in Queensland.
Queensland Education Journal (1895-1922) and Queensland Teachers
Journal (1923-1965), published by the Queensland Teachers Union.
Text-books referred to in official publicationsfor example, Gladman (1904)
and ones identified as being used by teachers to support their teaching of
mental arithmeticLarcombe (n.d.), for example.
First-hand accounts of experiences in Queensland primary schoolsfor
example that of Hanger (1963).

Data were also obtained from such secondary sources as (a) the reports of
studies into aspects of Queensland educationfor example, Greenhalghs (1957)
study of the 1952 syllabus; (b) books and articles on the history of Queensland
educationLogan & Clarke (1984), for example; and (c) newspaper reports and
articles of relevance to the teaching of mental arithmetic in Queensland primary
classroomsfor example, Scientific & Useful (1882). Wherever possible, the data
from these sources were cross-checked with the original references, or more
generally against such primary sources of data as the reports of District Inspectors.
The major sources of historical evidence were obtained from (a) the
Queensland Department of Educations History Unit, (b) the Queensland State
Archives, and (c) the library of the Queensland Teachers Union. Data were also
obtained from documents, books, and articles held in (a) the Fryer and Main
Libraries, University of Queensland; (b) John Oxley Library and State Library of
Queensland; (c) Mitchell Library, Sydney; and (d) private collections.
During historical research sources of data need to be subjected to both external
and internal criticism to determine their authenticity and validity with respect to the
topic being investigated (Rodwell, 1992, p. 97). Given the nature of this study and
its sources of information, determining the authenticity and validity of the primary
documents was not as significant an issue as for those studies not primarily based
on official documents and reports. The authenticity of the syllabus documents,
District Inspectors reports and articles in the Queensland Teachers Journalthe
documents on which this study was largely basedwas without question.

155

The validity of the beliefs espoused in the reports and articles was substantiated
primarily through the completeness of the historical record and the consistency with
which the various District Inspectors and writers expressed their views. All the
reports of District Inspectors were accessed, either directly as archival material from
1940, or in the Annual Reports of the Board of General Education and Department
of Public Instruction prior to 1940. Although only a summary of each District
Inspectors report was published in the Annual Reports from 1920 to 1939, it was
evident that the majority of District Inspectors commented on mental arithmetic, as
part of their reports on Arithmetic teaching and performance in the schools within
their inspectorial districts. Nonetheless, the detail in the reports varied, with some
District InspectorsMacgroarty during the period 1879 to 1902 and Mutch from 1906
to 1925, for examplebeing particularly insightful.
During the analysis of the data gathered and during the preparation of this
studys findings, cognisance was given to the inherent biases within the various
reports and articles. It was recognised that key sources of data were often written
from inherently conflicting perspectives about what occurred or should have
occurred in classrooms. The impact of particular reports of District Inspectors and
the articles in the journals of the Queensland Teachers Union was also unclear.
However, a consideration of the opinions and recommendations expressed by
different authors over time has enabled a clear understanding of the issues
surrounding mental arithmetic to be ascertained and reported.

Research Questions

In historical research, a set of questions rather than an hypothesis is often


devised to guide and focus the data collection and analysis (Cohen & Manion, 1994,
p. 36). Such questions are effective for enhancing the continuity of the information
presented (Wiersma, 1995, p. 236). In this study, to guide the analysis of past
syllabuses, and to facilitate the identification of the major similarities and differences
between current and past beliefs and practices, and those recommended as
essential for gaining mastery of the calculative process, the following research
questions were posed:

156

1.

What emphasis was given to mental computation in the various


mathematics syllabuses for Queensland primary schools during the period
1860-1965?

2.

What was the nature of mental computation as embodied in the various


syllabuses and in the manner in which it was taught from 1860 to 1965?

3.

What was the role of mental computation within the mathematics curricula
from 1860 to 1965?

4.

What was the nature of the teaching practices used to develop a child's
ability to calculate exact answers mentally during the period 1860-1965?

5.

What was the nature of the resources used to support the teaching of
mental computation during the period 1860-1965?

These questions were derived from the fourth principal purpose of this research
project, which was stated in Section 1.3, and emphasised in the introduction to this
chapter.

Structure of Analysis

Reports in historical research do not have a standard format. They may be


organised chronologically or thematically, with the format influenced by the nature of
the questions posed (Wiersma, 1995, p. 832). In this study, the report of the
research into past beliefs and practices also needed to be linked to the analysis of
issues related to mental computation presented in the previous chapter. Such a link
facilitated the formulation of the proposals for the future of mental computation in
primary classrooms, as presented in Chapters 5 and 6. This chapter, therefore, has
been structured thematically, on themes suggested by the analysis of the
pedagogical, socio-anthropological, and psychological literature reported in Chapter
2, namely:

Background issues related to syllabus development and implementation


which may have influenced the role and nature of mental arithmetic within
the classroom.

The terms used to describe the ability to calculate exact answers mentally.

157

The roles ascribed to mental arithmetic.

The nature of mental arithmetic from the perspectives of syllabus content,


syllabus notes, textbooks, and psychological and pedagogical beliefs, and
the constraints which impinged on syllabus implementation.

The teaching methods used to develop skill with calculating exact answers
mentally.

3.2

Selected Background Issues Related to Syllabus


Development and Implementation
It is evident that the importance of mental calculation has long been recognised

by Queensland educators. District Inspector Mutch (1906), in his annual report for
1905, reiterating Fish's (1874, p. vii) position, rhetorically asked: "Should not all
arithmetic be mental, and the pencil called into requisition only when the numbers
are large? Are not slate and paper used merely to lessen the strain on the
memory?" (Mutch, 1906, p. 63). From another perspective, it can be considered
that all computation, including written and technological, involves a mental
component.
However, from a review of the primary sources of historical data, there appears
to be little documentary evidence to link mental arithmetic specifically to issues
which surrounded and underpinned the syllabuses formulated during the period
being investigated. Nevertheless, it is reasonable to assume that such issues would
have had some influence, at least indirectly, on the nature of mental arithmetic, as
embodied in each of the syllabuses, and as presented to children. Among the key
issues which impinged on the development and implementation of syllabuses were:
(a) the function of primary education, including its role as a preparation for the afterlife of the child; (b) the focus of teachingthe subject or the child; (c) the principles
on which the syllabuses from 1905 were based; (d) syllabus interpretation and
overloading; and (e) teacher freedom versus syllabus specificity. These issues and
the links between them, which constitute the context within which mental arithmetic
was taught, are to be explored using the following organisers: (a) Focus of Syllabus
Development and Implementation, (b) Principles Underlying the Syllabuses from
1905, and (c) Syllabus Interpretation and Overloading.

158

3.2.1 Focus of Syllabus Development and Implementation


Syllabus development and particularly its implementation were characterised by
conservatism between 1860 and 1965 (Department of Education, 1978, p. 5; "Der
Tag, 1936, p. 3; Lawry, 1968, p. 568). This conservatism had its origins in the
colonial period in which educational authority was excessively centralised
(Creighton, 1993, p. 77), initially with the Board of General Education and
subsequently with the Department of Public Instruction, following the passing of the
Education Act of 1875. Furthermore, the Department was dominated in its early
years by Under Secretary Anderson and General Inspector Ewart1 "whose
administration was authoritarian and whose philosophy of educationbased on
mental discipline theory[became] increasingly antiquated" (Creighton, 1993, p. 77).
These officers were not greatly interested in educational innovations external to
Queensland2, nor did they encourage teachers or District Inspectors to participate in
the decision making processes (Barcan, 1980, p. 185).
"So completely was the spirit of teacher-participation in the framing of school
activities suppressed that the medieval idea of education, as the imparting of
correct knowledge,' almost completely dominated the curriculum" (Professional
Standards, 1936, p. 1). The attitudes of Anderson and Ewart gave little scope for
hope of rapid progress in state education, no matter how urgent the needs (Lawson,
1970, p. 228), needs that included syllabus revision to reflect the changing nature of
Queensland's society and economy (Lawson, 1970, p. 215), and improved teacher
training (The Queensland State Department of Education, 1966, p. 330; Wyeth,
1955, p. 156). Their resistance to change also occurred in an environment
characterised by public apathy and opposition towards education (Lawson, 1970, p.
216), coupled with satisfaction with the state of education as felt by politically
influential groups (Tyrrell, 1968, p. 154)to such an extent that the systematic
transformation of Queensland's education system was not urged by any influential
group prior to the outbreak of World War 1 (Tyrrell, 1968, p. 155).
The debate surrounding the passing of the State Education Act of 1875 resulted
in the Department of Public Instruction, for reasons of political feasibility, being given
the responsibility for controlling a narrow system of primary education (Lawry, 1975,
p. 60). Sir Samuel Griffith, the architect of the 1875 Bill and the first Minister for
Public Instruction, considered that "the duty of the state...was merely to give that

159

rudimentary instruction which would enable the child to become a good member of
society" (Griffiths, 1874, p. 395). This view reflected that of "Omega" (1871) who
suggested that:

The...object [of education was] to develope (sic) and improve to the utmost all
the powers and faculties, and to give instruction in all those branches of
knowledge which are considered suitable and necessary to our probable
circumstances and condition in after-life....[Education also included] the
formation of good, and what is much more important, the eradication of bad
habits [particularly for the poorer and working classes]. (p. 8)

The main focus of the 1876 Syllabus was therefore on reading, writing and
arithmetic, although object lessons, drill and gymnastics, vocal music and
needlework for girls were also included. In keeping with the rudimentary nature of
the desired instruction, the higher mathematics subjects, Euclid and algebra, which
were part of the 1860 Syllabus for the Fifth Class, were not considered necessary
for a primary education.
The frequency of syllabus change between 1876 and 1905 (see Table 3.1)
reflected the indecision surrounding the nature of the curriculum for children in
upper primary classes, particularly for those who were not able to win scholarships
for a secondary education3 at one of the private fee-charging Grammar Schools
(Dagg, 1971, p. 14). The modifications that were made were undertaken to
accommodate pressures for the inclusion of practical subjects, drawing in 1894, for
example, but without disturbing the predominance of the mental disciplinary
approach to education (Lawry, 1968, pp. 635-636). Reports from District Inspectors
indicating poor results in formal subjects, which included mental arithmetic, were
ignored by Anderson (Lawry, 1968, p. 570).
Despite the progressive outlook embodied in the 1904 Syllabus (Wyeth, 1955,
p. 157), in essence it maintained a belief in a narrowly conceived primary school
curriculum (Lawry, 1968, pp. 604, 606). As Ewart (1905) pointed out in his report for
1904: "What is needed...is to show how little new' there is in it. Stand the three R's'
where they did? Undoubtedly" (p. 161). The Education Conferences4 of 1903 and
1904, attended by departmental officials, District Inspectors and teachers, thus
avoided the task of a full curriculum revision, although reports were prepared by a

160

range of sub-committees, including one which focussed on arithmetic5 (Lawry, 1968,


p. 602).
The 1930 Syllabus was considered by Chief-Inspector Edwards (1933) to be
one that, in content and aims, embodied "a compromise between the extreme
English theory of former yearsknowledge for its own sake, and the extreme
American preference for utility and efficiency" (p. 27). Edwards (1933) noted that
whereas the English view tended to dismiss the value of practical work, a view
previously espoused by Anderson and Ewart, the American "attitude tended to
dismiss as mere mental lumber' anything of which the practical value was not
apparent" (p. 27). The philosophy that underpinned this syllabus, as well as
subsequent ones, was encapsulated in the Report of the Secretary of Public

161

Table 3.1
Queensland Mathematics Schedules and Syllabuses: 1860-1965
1860

Regulations for the establishment and management of primary schools in


Queensland. (1860). Brisbane: Fairfax and Belbridge.

1876

Schedule V: Table of the minimum amount of attainments required from pupils for
admission into each class in primary schools. (1876). Queensland Government
Gazette, XVII(36), 825.

1892

Para 143: The course of instruction for each class. (1891). In State Education Act
of 1875; together with the regulations of the Department (pp. 23-24). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Para 144: Standards of proficiency. (1891). In State Education Act of 1875;
together with the regulations of the Department (pp. 24-28). Brisbane: Government
Printer.

1894

Schedule V: Course of instruction for each class. (1894). Queensland Government


Gazette, LXII(34), 320-321.
Schedule VI: Standards of proficiency. (1894). Queensland Government Gazette,
LXII(34), 321-323.

1897

Schedule V: Course of instruction for each class. (1897). Queensland Government


Gazette, LXVII(62), 798-799.
Schedule VI: Standards of proficiency. (1897). Queensland Government Gazette,
LXVII(62), 799-801.

1902

Schedule XIV: Course of instruction for each class. (1902). Queensland


Government Gazette, LXXIX(18), 169-170.
Schedule XV: Standards of proficiency. (1902). Queensland Government Gazette,
LXXIX(18), 170-174.

1905

Schedule XIV: Course of instruction for each class. (1904). Education Office
Gazette, VI(11), 204-211.

1915

Department of Public Instruction. (1914). The syllabus or course of instruction in


primary schools with notes for the guidance of teachers. Brisbane: The
Department.

1930

Department of Public Instruction. (1930). The syllabus or course of instruction in


primary and intermediate schools. Brisbane: The Department.

1938

Department of Public Instruction. (1938). Amendments to course of instruction in


primary and intermediate schools. Brisbane: The Department.

1948

Department of Public Instruction. (1948a). Course of instruction in primary and


intermediate schools. Brisbane: The Department.

1952

Department of Public Instruction. (1952b). The Syllabus or course of instruction in


primary and intermediate schools, Book 3: Mathematics. Brisbane: The
Department.

1964

Department of Education. (1964). The syllabus or course of instruction in primary


schools: Mathematics. Brisbane: The Department.

162

Instruction for 1920: "Education authorities have come to recognise that education
has a life-long relationship with the various aspects of citizenshipphysical, social,
industrial, and politicaland this conception steadily widens" (Huxham, 1921, p. 21).
Education had come to be "considered in terms of life rather than of purely
intellectual equipment....[It was recognised] that the purpose of any educational
scheme is to develop the full powers of [the] personality [of the child]" (J. A.
Robinson, 1945, p. 12)the "principle of social living" outlined in the 1930 Syllabus
(Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. vi) and the 1952 Syllabus (Department of
Public Instruction, 1952a, p. 2).
The centre of interest, therefore, had shifted from the subject to the child.
Nonetheless, as an indicator of the conservative implementation of syllabuses,
particularly from 1905, is the proposition in the 1952 Syllabus that "modern thought
regards the child as the focal point in the educational process" (Department of
Public Instruction, 1952a, p. 1). Influenced by the beliefs of John Dewey6, such a
focus was first proposed in the 1904 Syllabus. The preface to the schedule
indicated that the course of instruction was designed "to increase the influence of
the school as an agent in the intellectual, moral, and social development of the
child" (Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 200), a principle of utmost importance and potency
(Roe, 1915, p. 29). Ewart (1907) stressed in his report for 1906 that:

[Teachers] must learn without loss of time that there is before them in the
profession they have joined a new study they must tackle seriously, and that it
will take them all their lives to learn. That subject is the CHILD. Hitherto, it has
been arithmetic, languages, and the rest of ithow to know these, understand
them, explain them; now it must be how to bring all these in turn or together to
bear upon the life and wellbeing, the growth and development of the children
placed in their charge. (p. 39)

163

3.2.2

Principles Underlying the Syllabuses from 1905

The development of the 1904 Syllabus occurred during the early stages of a
period, 1900 to 1914, noted for its considerable reform and progress7 in Queensland
education (Parliamentary Select Committee, 1980, p. 4). This period signalled, in
theory at least, an end to the emphasis on the theory of mental discipline, with its
focus on teacher and subject, and heralded a concern for the child (Creighton, 1993,
p. 77; Logan & E. Clarke, 1984, p. 3). Moreover, the 1904 Syllabus could be
considered to be the ideological turning point for Queensland State school curricula
(Creighton, 1993, p. 78), "an epoch in [Queensland's] educational history" (The
Revised Syllabus, 1914, p. 81). However, the high ideals expressed in the preface
to this syllabus"an excellent New Charter' for...primary schools" (The New
Syllabus, 1904, p. 143)were doomed to lie dormant, even following the passing of
Anderson's influence in mid-1904 and that of Ewart in 1909.
The preface to the 1904 Syllabus, the first to give teachers assistance in
syllabus implementation, not only provided some guidance as to how mathematics
should be taught, but, possibly more importantly, delineated the principles on which
the schedule was based. In addition to indicating that the school needed to
increase its influence on the pupils' social, moral and intellectual development, it
was stated that:

[The syllabus was] designed to give practical application in the teaching work of
schools to the principle of the correlation of the subjects of study, to make "the
self-activity of the pupil the basis of school instruction," [and] to bring the work of
the pupil into closer touch with his home and social surroundings. (Schedule
XIV, 1904, p. 200)

These principles were espoused by advocates of the New Education Movement


which, in the late nineteenth century, constituted an international reaction against
the narrowness and formality of the prevailing subject-based pedagogy (Turney,
1972, p. 32). The nature knowledge introduced into the syllabus was to be the main
vehicle for the implementation of these principles (Creighton, 1993, p. 79).
However, it was also recognised that, in teaching mathematics:

164

From the earliest stages the child [should be] led to deal with quantities of actual
things and with the measurement of quantities, using various units....The
youngest children should not only see but handle the quantities with which they
deal, and actually make measurements on which they are to operate....It is
valuable education for the pupil to be made to do things for himself, instead of
merely seeing the teacher do them. (Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 201)

Proponents of aspects of the New Education believed that teaching through


chalk-and-talk was anathema to the essence of the development of the young child
who wanted to be active and to investigate material for himself (Ensor8, reported in
Wasted Time at School, 1937, p. 15). The focus in Queensland, particularly prior
to World War 1, however, was on extending basic education throughout the state,
with the result that the impact of the implied reforms to teaching was tentative and
incomplete (Tyrrell, 1968, p. 78), at least until the late 1970s when renewed efforts
to reform the way mathematics was taught began to be reflected in classroom
practice. Greenhalgh (1949a) observed that classrooms continued to be
characterised by passivity rather than activity with "children [being] required to sit
still and listen far too frequently" (p. 5). This, in Greenhalgh's (1949a, p. 5) view,
was due to teachers trying to cover too much work with its concomitant cursory
consideration of many aspects of the mathematics curriculum, including mental
arithmetic.
Nonetheless, Edwards (1929, p. 31) believed that, by 1928, teachers were
making links between subjects to an extent not considered possible in 1905 when
the revised schedule was introduced. In Roe's (1915) view, a focus on the
correlation of subjects and the practical application of school knowledge to the
features of daily life were "merely improvements of method by which a child's
interest in his lessons [were] awakened and maintained" (p. 29). It was recognised
that interest is the "mainspring in educational progress" (Roe, 1915, p. 29) and
leads to a desire to become involved in the learning process. The General
Memorandum on 1948 Amendments to the [1930] Syllabus (1948) gave recognition
to this:

By making a child desire to solve a certain problem gets him to put his Heart
into it; it gets him to use his Head in the collection and arrangement of the

165

relevant facts; it gets him to use his Hand in the construction of apparatus to be
used in the solution of the problem. (Department of Public Instruction, 1948b, p.
16)

This statement was presented in context with an advocacy for the Project
Method, the principal manifestation of an emphasis on self-activity in the upper
grades. As Greenhalgh (1957, p. 76) pointed out, the 1930 Syllabus virtually
instructed teachers in small-schools and of upper grades in larger schools to use
this method which allowed the teacher to "set problems going, so that the pupils, in
order to solve them, must arrive at the knowledge of the [mathematical] principle or
rule" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. vii)an emphasis on self-education
was therefore advocated.
The acceptance and implementation of the principles and teaching approaches
outlined in the 1904 Syllabus relied on the training of a whole new generation of
teachers (The Queensland State Department of Education, 1966, p. 330).
Although the long awaited Teachers' College was opened in 1914, many teachers
continued to be trained under "the old hard system" (Wyeth, 1955, p. 157), the pupilteacher system not being phased out until 1935. Hence, teachers were generally
not prepared for the changed expectations. Representatives of the Queensland
Teachers' Union, during a deputation to the Minister of Public Instruction in 1946,
asserted that few teachers were undertaking an activity approach to learning due to
a lack of equipment and materials, a lack of time as well as not having an
understanding of how to incorporate activity based learning into their teaching
programs (Deputation to Minister, 1946, p. 4).
Wyeth (1955, p. 158) asserted that it would be reasonable to state that the
aspirations crystallised into specific statements of ideals in the 1904 Syllabus had
not been realised by the mid-1950s. This applied particularly to the principles on
which the syllabus was based, even though they continued to be emphasised in
subsequent syllabuses (Department of Public Instruction, 1914, p. 6; 1930, p. vi;
Edwards, 1951, p. 25). Further, their implementation was not supported by all
senior officers of the Department. Director-General Watkin voiced his official
antagonism to such education in his 1956 Report:

166

In emphasising "Learning by Doing" [teachers] concentrated on activity, any


activity provided it was not intellectual; self-activity developed into licence; selfdiscipline degenerated into lack of discipline, and objective standards were
reduced if not entirely neglected. (Watkin, 1957, cited by Barcan, 1980, p. 308)

That the principles of correlation of subjects, self-activity and closer links


between home and school were not being implemented was given recognition at
various times during the 1920s and 1930s. Edwards (1922), on retiring as the
president of the Queensland Teachers' Union, noted that although "the educational
work was throbbing with new ideas, having as their objects the freedom of the child,
the greater elasticity of the curriculum, and the closer connection of education with
reality, ...the influence of these movements has scarcely been felt in Queensland"
(p. 1). This observation was reiterated in 1939 in an editorial in the Queensland
Teachers' Journal. While noting that the function of the school was to provide more
than a grounding in the three Rs, it was concluded that: "Queensland [had] been in
an educational backwater where the flood tide of this reform movement [had]
scarcely rippled the surface" (Educational Reform, 1939, p. 1). District Inspector
Pestorius (1940) noted that "the complacency and self-satisfaction of many teachers
in their methods [should be undermined, and that]...for complete success...we must
devise our methods or base them upon our own understanding of the individual
children" (p. 15).
In reality, it has only been since the 1970s that gains have been made in placing
the child in his/her environment (Creighton, 1993, p. 78), the prime focus of the
1904 Syllabus principles. This has been facilitated by such factors as the cessation
of the external Scholarship examination from 1963, the discontinuation of annual
inspections for teachers in 1970, and improvements in the education of teachers,
the influence of which on mental arithmetic is to be analysed later in this chapter.
Cramer9 (1936) noted that "just as external examinations may make teachers think
they are teaching arithmetic...rather than boys and girls, so inspectors may forget
boys and girls and think of the machinery of the system and the teachers that they
are visiting" (p. 8).

3.2.3 Syllabus Interpretation and Overloading

167

The 1904 Syllabus was the first to be accompanied not only by the principles on
which the syllabus was based, but also by suggestions concerning its
implementation. These were intended to be suggestive rather than specific, namely,
"to indicate the scope of treatment, and to lay down fundamental principles and lines
of direction at various stages of the pupils' progress; but it [did] not prescribe
detailed methods for teaching individual subjects" (Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 203).
St. Ledger (1905) regarded the 1904 Syllabus as the "Teachers' Magna Carta" (p.
218). It gave freedom to the teacher. Importantly, it was accompanied by changes
to the system of inspection. No longer were District Inspectors required to examine
every child in every subject. This double change was considered to be "the most
important advance...since the passing of the Education Act of 1875" (St. Ledger,
1905, p. 218). The Queensland Teachers' Union interpreted the 1904 Syllabus as
being based on the principle of thought, whereas previous syllabuses were
considered to have been based on pressure (The New Syllabus, 1904, p. 143),
pressure from "the blighting influence of percentages" (Shirley, 1905a, p. 189).
Recognition was given to the belief that "there is no absolutely best method of
treating any subject; one method is best suited to one type of teacher, another is
best for others10" (Department of Public Instruction, 1914, p. 10)methods which
were surrounded in controversy (Teaching Hints, 1908, p. 15). However, teacher
freedom was not extended to the selection of course content. Director of Education
McKenna (1936, p. 12) reported that he could not agree with the absence of definite
schemes of work for teachers, as occurred in England. His disagreement, however,
appears to have arisen primarily from administrative rather than from educational
considerations. He stated that he would be disinclined to advocate the extension of
freedom of this type for teachers as it "would demand a much more highly trained
type of teacher than we have at present, and would necessitate the retention of
head teachers in the same schools for considerable periods of time" (McKenna,
1936, p. 12). Nonetheless, Edwards (1937b, p. 7), on replacing McKenna as
Director of Education, suggested that the greatest hope for educational progress lay
with improving the quality of teachers. Cramer (1937, p. 8) observed that in the
Australian states which he visited, of which Queensland was one, the preparation of
teachers was referred to as teacher training rather than teacher educating. The
focus of teacher training, Cramer (1937) observed, was the production of "a
competent school-room technician whose training [was] largely restricted to the

168

syllabus he [was] expected to teach" (p. 8), rather than the development of a welleducated individual able to adapt to various situations. Such a view was not in
conflict with teacher beliefs about education, which held that education was the act
of training, although recognition was given to the development of the various
intellectual, physical, aesthetic and moral faculties (Bensted, 1924, p. 34).
Paradoxically, although scope for teacher freedom in planning for syllabus
implementation remained a feature of syllabuses subsequent to 1905, syllabus
outlines, particularly from 1930, increased in detail. This potentially incompatible
tendency arose from a belief that inexperienced teachers needed greater guidance.
Edwards (1938a), in his memorandum to schools concerning the 1938
Amendments, emphasised that "such specification [was]...not [intended] as a
restriction of the freedom of the more experienced. Scope [was] still afforded for
originality and initiative in the inclusion of matter and the selection of method" (p. 2).
However, in his reports for 1936 and 1947, Edwards (1937a, p. 25; 1948, p. 20)
noted that the spirit of professional freedom was not always grasped by teachers.
He suggested that this, together with the calls for a greater prescription of work, was
perhaps due to:

A form of inertiaa disinclination to get off the beaten track and to depart from
methods to which they have become accustomed. [Further,] it [was] more likely
due to timidity. Many teachers [were] not prepared to accept the responsibility
of embarking on some original line of thought or action. (Edwards, 1937a, p.
25)

The Queensland Teachers' Union believed that the conundrum of professional


freedom in association with calls for greater prescription, particularly with respect to
the 1930 Syllabus and its 1938 Amendments, was due to the system of teacher
inspection (The Dead Hand of Inspection, 1939, p. 1). Most teachers interpreted
the detailed statements as prescriptive, believing that District Inspectors would use
them as their guide for assessing teacher performance (Turney, 1972, p. 45). In
what could be interpreted as an admonishment of District Inspectors, Edwards
(1948) emphasised in his report for 1947 that high marks on a teacher's "yellow
card' should not depend upon rigid conformity with standards or curricula set up by
the central authority" (p. 20). Calls for greater prescription, and for standardised

169

work books, were designed to place limits on the ways in which the syllabus had
been interpreted. However, the suggestions for standardised workbooks were
frowned upon by the Queensland Teachers' Union as it believed that this would
make the individualisation of learning even less likely (Burge, 1946, p. 13).
Although the 1930 Syllabus in mathematics was a very exacting one, with little
to be added to meet the requirements of the secondary school Junior Public
Examination (Greenhalgh, 1957, p. 285), it was not uncommon to find that the
examinations of District Inspectors and Head Teachers exceeded the requirements
specified for particular grades (Syllabus Debate, 1938, p. 15). As "The
Commentator" (1947) observed in the Queensland Teachers' Journal, "teachers and
inspectors went into operation when the 1930 Syllabus was published; [and this] is
not the same thing as putting the 1930 Syllabus into operation" (p. 21), a
consideration that was often overlooked by teachers who advocated syllabus
change.
A multiplicity of textbooks (see Table 3.2), all more or less unofficial, became
available to teachers, many of which were criticised for not being trialed before

170

Table 3.2
Selection of Textbooks Relevant to Mental Arithmetic Available to Queensland
Teachers From the Mid-1920s
Bevington, W. F. (Ed.). (n.d.). Queensland syllabus mental arithmetic. Brisbane: H. Pole.
Class teacher's manual of oral arithmetic for grades VII & VIII in Queensland schools. (n.d.).
Warwick: Premier Educational Publishers.
Department of Public Instruction/Education. Queensland. (1946-). Arithmetic for Queensland
schools. Brisbane: Government Printer.
Henderson, T., Irish, C. A., Bowden, L. T., & Watkin, H. G. (1932-). New syllabus arithmetic
(Brooks' Queensland School Series). Brisbane: William Brooks.
Henderson, T., Irish, C. A., Bowden, L. T., & Watkin, H. G. (1932-). New syllabus mental
arithmetic (Brooks' Queensland School Series). Brisbane: William Brooks.
a
Larcombe, H. J. (n.d.). Speed tests in mental arithmetic: Senior book 1 (The Minute a sum
series). London: Evans Brothers.

Mental arithmetic: Senior grades (The Moreton Series). (1926-). Brisbane: Moreton Printing
Company.
Mutual Aid Society, Ipswich. (n.d.). Queensland syllabus mental arithmetic: Grades VI-VII.
Sydney: Philip.
Olsen, F. J. (1953). Oral arithmetic (McLeod's School Series). Brisbane: McLeod.
b
Potter. (n.d.). Mental and intelligence tests in common-sense arithmetic. London: Pitman.

Test papers based on model work books: Grades III to VI. (n.d.). Brisbane: Pole.
Thompson, F. C. (Ed.). (1930-). The Queensland mathematics. Brisbane: Ferguson.
Wisdom, A. (1932). Arithmetical dictation.a London: University of London Press.

Note.

Identified as a text used in Queensland by the presence of a Queensland School's


identification stamp in a copy held in the Oxley Library. bA text listed as a valuable
guide to the intelligent treatment of the arithmetic in the 1930 Syllabus (Department
of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 31).

publication ("Debunker, 1940, p. 14). Some were compiled or, at least, supported
by District Inspectorsfor example, those prepared by the South Coast Syllabus
Notes Committee under the direction of ex-District Inspector Bevington. It was
asserted by "Debunker" (1940, p. 14) that some inspectors had insisted upon the
use of particular texts. Some years prior to this, in an endeavour to promote the
teaching of mental arithmetic, District Inspector Mutch had given the majority of
schools in his district a copy of Burt's Mental Tests in Arithmetic11, which he

171

regarded as exemplary (Mutch, 1924, p. 40). Model work books were also prepared
by District Inspectors for commercial publication. For example, District Inspectors
Router and Pascoe, assisted by District Inspectors Baker and Inglis, prepared a
series of Programmes of Work (1932), which included suggestions for oral
arithmetic for each term of the school year.
These texts tended to provide exhaustive treatments, with their maximums
becoming teachers' minimums (Darling Downs Branch, 1946, p. 21). The fear of
teachers was that if the children in their classes did not achieve high percentages on
the inspectors' test, they would be marked down (Syllabus Debate, 1938, p. 15).
The Queensland Teachers' Union believed that "all concerned, with the exception of
those most vitally concerned, the children, brushed aside the important General
Introduction [to the 1930 Syllabus] in their eagerness to get at the actual
requirements....The Syllabus was ruined by zealots with their fanatical shibboleth of
a bit ahead'" (Editorial, 1947, p. 2), an opinion shared by the Department in 1937
(Department of Public Instruction, 1937, p. 26). Pestorius (1942, p. 2) noted that
some head teachers prescribed work from textbooks rather than planning
independently from the syllabus. This was a criticism also levelled at the examiners
for the annual Scholarship examination during the 1930s and 1940s"What was
found in a Scholarship Class text book could surely be used as part of the
examination!" (Dagg, 1971, p. 57). Pestorius (1942) believed that "reliance upon the
subject matter in textbooks rather than individual planning and initiative based upon
Syllabus requirements [tended] to make the work stereotyped, and [was] likely to
[have resulted in] cramming. As Dagg (1971, p. 57) concludes, the textbooks which
often went beyond the scope of the syllabus therefore set the standard.
The General Introduction to the 1930 Syllabus recommended that "arithmetic,
while emphasizing speed and accuracy, [should] not entail long and useless
calculations" (p. vi). Further, the syllabus stressed that "the very practical purposes
that mathematical work has to serve in the child's future life should regulate the
character of its treatment in school" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 30).
Nonetheless, "Der Tag" (1936) felt compelled to write that "nothing is left out [of the
textbooks] even when ruled out by the Syllabus itself" (p. 3). In "Green Ant's" (1942,
p. 17) view, the extensions of syllabus requirements were most flagrant for mental
arithmetic. For example, in a textbook prepared by the Head Master of the Ascot
Practising School as the main author, the mental work for Grade VI included such

172

examples as: (a) Increase 83 by .37 of its value; and (b) Express

15 32
100

as a decimal

(Henderson, Irish, Bowden, & Watkin, 1935, p. 10). This, in spite of the syllabus
indicating that the oral work for Grade VI should be preparatory to the written work,
with the notes accompanying the latter indicating that "the exercises [should] deal
only with simpler fractions" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, pp. 42, 43).
Extensions to the syllabus were also encouraged by non-Queensland texts.
Larcombe's (n.d., p. 2) Speed Tests in Mental Arithmetic, for example, a text
referred to by Router and Pascoe (1932, p. ii), presented items such as the following
for pupils aged 10-12 years: 23,765 x 9 and 70,186 7.
Such instances prompted calls for the Department to publish official textbooks,
the first of which was published for Grade 3 in 1946, as a means for gaining realistic
interpretations of the syllabus (Darling Downs Branch, 1946, p. 21). With a layout
similar to that of the commercial textbooks, these texts included sets of ten
mechanical and problem arithmetic examples. However, it is unlikely that these
textbooks proved to be a panacea. As the General Secretary of the Queensland
Teachers' Union pointed out in 1951: "A teacher, a professional man, might be
expected to do his work without a text book; a coach or a crammer is useless
without one" (cited by Dagg, 1971, p. 33), a sentiment which echoed that of
Edwards in his 1930 report (Edwards, 1931, pp. 28-29).
Teacher discontent with the way in which the 1930 Syllabus had been
interpreted, together with its perceived overloading, resulted in increased teacher
input into syllabus development, to such an extent that, by 1952, the initiative for a
new syllabus "came [right through], not from the administration, but from the
teachers" (Greenhalgh, 1957, p. 84). Overloading was a traditional feature of
Queensland mathematics syllabuses, and one that reached its climax in 1930
(Dagg, 1971, p. 32). Until 1915, overloading was, in Lawry's (1968) view, primarily
due to the "absence of an articulated system of education from primary school to
university" (p. 600). Nonetheless, mathematics tended to be "marked by the logical
completeness of the schedule, based on an adult notion of what should be taught,
as opposed to what the capacity of the average child could assimilate" (Greenhalgh,
1957, p. 77).
Syllabus development to 1930 had been characterised by additions, such that,
by 1928, the primary school pupil was regarded "as a polymath in comparison with
his predecessor of twenty years [previously]" (Edwards, 1929, p. 31). However,

173

Edwards (1929), the architect of the 1930 Syllabus, believed that no subjects could
be omitted during the revision of the syllabus as "they [were] calculated to widen the
outlook and to develop those qualities of body, heart, and mind that lead to the
efficient and true fulfilment of function in life" (p. 31). It was emphasised in the
General Introduction to the 1930 Syllabus that:

Complaints in regard to an overloaded curriculum usually come from those who


make instruction and information the chief aims of school work, and who fail to
regard added subjects, or branches of subjects, merely as further means of
training and developing the mind of the child. (Department of Public Instruction,
1930, p. vi)

By 1933, the Queensland Teachers' Journal had cause to editorialise that the
1930 Syllabus was in urgent need of a thorough overhauling (The Primary School
Syllabus, 1933, p. 1), a belief often repeated during the mid-1930s. Following a
European study tour, Director of Education McKenna (1936, p. 13) gave recognition
to the need for the syllabus to be revised. He noted that, in comparing
Queensland's requirements with those expected overseas, the syllabus, particularly
for the upper grades, demanded too high an academic standard. He doubted that
one pupil in a hundred could have passed Queensland's Scholarship Examination at
the age of 13 years (McKenna, 1936, p. 13). Chief-Inspector Edwards also
recognised the excessively high demands of the Queensland syllabus following his
visit to the United States of America in 1935. He noted that:

The main fault of our educational system in Queensland is that we seem to be


in a great hurry to cover the course. Children are required to gulp down rich
material in a short space of time. They do not have sufficient time for
mastication and digestion. They take this rather rich food too quickly and they
suffer to some extent from mental indigestion. (Edwards, 1936, p. 17)

A study by Cunningham and Price (1934, p. 81) demonstrated that the majority
of mathematics topics were introduced at lower ages in Queensland than in any
other Australian state, as well as being below the age recommendations of the
United States Committee of Seven12. This, and the length of time spent on

174

mathematics13, particularly in the lower grades, were judged to be the chief


contributing factors to the consistently high performances of Queensland primary
school pupils in written arithmetic (Cunningham & Price, 1934, p. 93). However, as
was noted in the press at the time of the release of the findings of a 1931 survey on
arithmetic by the Australian Council for Educational Research, "thoughtful teachers
see [this pre-eminence] as a menace rather than [as] an occasion for
congratulations" (cited in The Primary School Syllabus, 1933, p. 1). Not only was
this rush to impart knowledge "a negation of real teaching" (The Primary School
Syllabus, 1933, p. 1), but as revealed by a curriculum survey of Australian schools
published in 1950 by the Australian Council for Educational Research, Queensland
children occupied the lowest position of all the states for each of the two reading
tests (cited by Dagg, 1971, p. 26). In Queensland, the concentration had been on
arithmetic, primarily written arithmetic, at the expense of reading.
Edwards (1936, p. 17) also gave recognition to Thorndike's belief that
individuals learn many things very laboriously when young that could be learned
more easily and readily when older. This undoubtedly contributed to Edwards'
giving the committee of departmental and union representatives "a definite mandate
to secure relief" (Greenhalgh, 1957, p. 286) through the 1938 syllabus revision. It
was characterised by omissions rather than additions. For example, for Grade V,
mental exercises involving simple proportions were deleted from the syllabus, and
"Ratio expressed as fractions and decimals" was moved to Grade VI (see Appendix
A.10). However, the changes which were undertaken were not seen as final. It was
stated during debate at the 1938 Annual Teachers' Conference that "the Syllabus is
now in the transition stage and further amendments must come" (Syllabus Debate,
1938, p. 16).
Nevertheless, there would appear to have been uncertainty in the late 1940s
surrounding the development of a new syllabus. This uncertainty possibly arose
from a lack of confidence on the part of Queensland syllabus designers to prepare a
syllabus to match the needs of Queensland children without significantly relying on
overseas trends, particularly since educational research across all curriculum areas
specific to Queensland was limited (Creighton, 1993, p. 83). In a discussion paper,
submitted to the Syllabus Committee in 1947, it was argued that a new syllabus
should not be developed in the short term as there was:

175

A need to await a clearer definition of the post-war international pattern and of


the way of life which will be the lot of individuals therein....[Also] there was the
need to await the further crystallization of general world education ideas and
especially those within the Empire; [particularly there was a need to] await for a
little more clarity concerning the innovations implied in the series of reports of
the Consultative Committee on the English Board of Education and especially
those suggested by the very next report of the Advisory Council in Scotland
upon the curriculum of the Primary School. (Mathematics Subcommittee, 1947,
p. 1)

The report of the Advisory Council on Education in Scotland supported the


emphasis on the social utility of arithmetic. It recognised that arithmetic was
required by all citizens in their daily lives, while stressing that an ability to undertake
complicated problems was not one essential for all pupils. The Council also
believed that children should be required to have an automatic recall of tables, and
an efficiency and accuracy in simple calculations. Most importantly, it was deemed
necessary for individuals to be able to correlate these skills with the needs of
everyday life (Advisory Council on Education in Scotland, cited by Burge, 1947, p.
6). The latter constituted a reaffirmation of the third principle underlying each of the
syllabuses since 1905, namely, that school work should be closely linked to
activities outside the classroom.
In accordance with this view, the Syllabus Committee received
recommendations that calculation should be the core mathematics subject, and that
speed and mechanical accuracy tests should receive greater prominence,
particularly during Grade VI. Somewhat paradoxically, given the emphasis placed
on social utility during this period, it was suggested that oral arithmetic had been
given greater prominence than it deserved (Mathematics Subcommittee, 1947, p. 2).
It was also proposed that:

Teachers should rest content mainly with teaching the rules listed in the
Syllabus and applying them to simple mechanical operations of the same rule,
stressing at all times accuracy and speed. The old type of "brain teasers"
cannot justify its place in the modern Arithmetic Syllabus. The only "problems"

176

to be taught will be those arising in Class Activities. (Mathematics


Subcommittee, 1947, p. 3)

Given these beliefs, and the apparent hesitancy in drafting a new syllabus, it is
not inconsistent that the goals for 1952 Syllabus were adopted from a dated
publication of the Board of Education in England, namely, its Handbook of
Suggestions (1937, pp. 499-500). Director-General Edwards (1951, p. 25) noted, in
his report for 1950, that the Syllabus Committee did not find any fault with the 1930
educational objectives14. As a consequence, the general introductions to the 1930
and 1952 Syllabuses were essentially identical. With respect to mathematics, these
objectives included an assertion that the role of the primary school was to "give
[children] practice in the art of calculation" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930,
p. v; 1952a, p. 1). In keeping with this belief, the 1952 Syllabus stated that the
generally accepted goals for mathematics were: (a) "To help the child to form clear
ideas about certain relations of number, time and space; (b) to make the more
useful of these ideas firm and precise in his mind through practice in the appropriate
calculations; and (c) to enable him to apply the resulting mechanical skill
intelligently, speedily and accurately in the solution of every-day problems."
(Department of Education, 1952b, p. 1)
The response to the overloading and interpretations placed on the 1930
syllabus and its amendments, by District Inspectors, Head Teachers and teachers,
constituted by the 1952 Syllabus, was therefore a re-emphasis on calculation, albeit
in context with number, time and spatial concepts, as the principal focus in the
primary grades. This occurred in conjunction with a suggested time allocation of
five hours per week15 being placed on mathematics (Department of Public
Instruction, 1952a, p. 6), the first occasion that such a specification had been made
in a Queensland syllabus. Following the introduction of the 1952 Syllabus, District
Inspector Hendy (1953, p. 2) hoped that mathematics had been deposed as the
subject that dominated the curriculum and that no longer would classes be judged
primarily on their arithmetical performances.
Although the 1964 Syllabus did not include a precise set of goals, the emphasis
on computational competency in relevant social situations remained. However, preempting the focus of subsequent syllabuses, the importance of the discovery and
understanding of mathematical principles was highlighted for the first time in a

177

Queensland mathematics syllabus. It was held that this "should prepare the child
for more advanced studies beyond the primary school" (Department of Education,
1964, p. 1), this syllabus being introduced following the abolition of the Scholarship
examination, but before discussions on the New Maths were sufficient to provide a
basis for further syllabus development.

3.2.4 Summary of Background Issues


Although the historical record does not contain many specific links between the
background issues discussed and mental arithmetic, the foregoing analysis does
suggest that a number of the aspects highlighted are relevant to an understanding
of mental arithmetic during the period 1860-1965. These include:

The authoritarian atmosphere of the Department of Public Instruction in


which students, teachers, Head Teachers and District Inspectors of Schools
worked, albeit one that decreased as the twentieth century progressed.

The pervasiveness of elements of the theory of mental discipline into the


1950s, despite its tenets being discredited around the turn of the century.

The attempted shift from a focus on the subject to a focus on the child from
1905.

The tension between viewing arithmetic as essential knowledge for its own
sake and as knowledge that is socially useful, with the ascendancy of the
latter culminating with the 1952 Syllabus.

The advocacy for closer links between what is taught in schools and the life
of children outside the classroomthe third principle espoused in the 1904
Syllabus.

The general passivity of children in classrooms, despite the various


interpretations placed on the need for schools to make "the self-activity of
the pupil the basis of school instruction" (Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 200).

The effect on classroom practice of the ways in which children and teachers
were examined by Head Teachers and District Inspectors of Schools.

The influence of the Scholarship Examination on what and how children


were taught.

178

The effect of the methods of teacher training on the ways in which children
were taught.

The impact of textbooks and journal articles on syllabus implementation


and teaching practices, particularly during the 1930s and 1940s.

These issues, although not of equal importance, are given further consideration
in subsequent sections of this chapter, the first of which focuses on the terminology
associated with the calculation of exact answers mentally during the period being
investigated.

3.3

Terms Associated with the Calculation of Exact Answers


Mentally
As discussed in Chapter 2, the practice of using the term mental computation

to refer to the calculation of exact answers mentally, where the focus is on selfdeveloped strategies based on conceptual knowledge (R. E. Reys et al., 1995, p.
324), is a relatively recent one. Its use has arisen from a perceived need to
distinguish more clearly between the various aspects of mental calculation,
particularly calculating exact answers vis--vis approximate answers, as well as to
provide a distinction from the traditional approaches to teaching mental calculation.
Based on an analysis of mathematics syllabus documents, textbooks, articles in the
Queensland Education Journal, Queensland Teachers' Journal, and Education
Office Gazette, and from reports of District Inspectors of Schools, it is evident that a
number of terms were used, seemingly interchangeably, during the period 18601965. These primarily were: mental arithmetic, oral arithmetic, mental exercises,
mental, mental work, oral work, mental and oral work, and oral and mental work.
Negligible recognition was given to computational estimation in Queensland
syllabuses prior to 1966-1968.
Although the period being investigated is characterised by a lack of preciseness
in terminology, attempts were made to clarify the situation as early as 1881. Joyce
(1881), in his Handbook of School Management and Methods of Teaching16, argued
that:

179

[Mental arithmetic] is commonly understood to mean merely a number of short


rules (often called short accounts'), which are usually found at the end of
treatises....But this is only a portion of mental arithmetic; the term has wider
significance, and means not only these technical rules but all kinds of numerical
combinations performed mentally, from the common addition table up to the
most complicated operations. (p. 210)

This usage is similar in meaning to the broad definition given to mental by the Board
of Education (1937) in its Handbook of Suggestions. In the Board's view, "Mental'
Arithmetic [included] all exercises in which pen or pencil [were] not used, except
perhaps to record the answer" (Board of Education, 1937, p. 513).
For all mathematics syllabuses except those of 1952 and 1964, in which the
heading Oral Arithmetic was used for Grades II to VIII to indicate work related to
mentally calculating exact answers, little consistency in terminology within each
syllabus is evident. For example, in the Mathematics Schedule published in 1904
the following terminology was used: Mental Exercises (First Class), Mental Work
(Second Class), Mental and Oral Work (Third and Fourth Classes), Oral and Mental
Work (Fifth and Sixth Classes) (see Appendix A.7).
This schedule marked the initial use of the term oral. In a general sense, oral
referred to the explanation and discussion of arithmetic processes, as reflected in
the notes on arithmetic accompanying the 1914 Syllabus. In these it was stated
that, for the First Class (First Half-year), "the work should be exclusively oral, except
that the children should learn to know and to make the digits including 0"
(Department of Public Instruction, 1914, p. 62). The importance of oral questioning
had been stressed previously by District Inspector McIntyre in his 1875 report. In
decrying the "absurd habit, on the part of junior teachers, of writing down [on the
blackboard] all arithmetic exercises...ready manufactured into the shape of sums',
he advocated that "the method of dictating sums should be frequently practised"
(McIntyre, 1876, p. 30). A narrower meaning of oral, one more closely related to
mental arithmetic, is suggested by a statement in the 1904 Schedule with respect to
the Fourth Class. This required the "oral statement of processes employed in
written work of [the] class", a declaration that was omitted from the 1914 Syllabus as
it was considered to be a valuable practice for all pupils, not simply for those in the
Fourth Class17 (Department of Public Instruction, 1914, p. 71).

180

Following the implementation of the 1904 Schedule from January 1905, District
Inspector Shirley observed that teachers were placing an increased emphasis on
the oral explanation of problems. He noted that "it is now quite common to find
pupils able to state rules for working a problem, to make special application of the
rule to the question in hand, and to work out the steps mentally" (Shirley, 1905b, p.
117). Hence mental arithmetic, slate arithmetic and the discussion of the processes
involved were seen to be inextricably bound. However, the focus on the latter by
teachers was not widespread. District Inspector Gripp noted that, in his inspectoral
district in 1908, "oral work, particularly the explanation of the successive steps of a
solution, deserves still more practice than it is at present receiving" (Gripp, 1909, p.
60).
Although oral was initially used to highlight the need for teachers above the First
Class to encourage explanations of arithmetic processes, usage of the term in
syllabuses subsequent to that of 1904 made no apparent distinction between oral
and mental arithmetic. In the notes on arithmetic accompanying the 1914 Syllabus,
explanations of work required for each class concerning the calculation of exact
answers mentally were presented under the headings Oral Arithmetic and Oral Work
(Department of Public Instruction, 1914, pp. 64-70). This practice was continued in
later syllabuses (see Appendix A.8). For example, in the 1948 amendments, which
retained the headings used in the 1930 Syllabus and the 1938 Amendments, buying
and selling came under the heading of Mental for Grade I and Oral Work for Grade
II, whereas shopping transactions for Grade IV were outlined under Oral Arithmetic.
Oral Work for Grade VI included a list of short methods of calculation with which
children were to become familiar, reminiscent of Joyce's (1881, p. 210) reference to
mental arithmetic. The blurring of the distinction between oral and mental was also
encouraged by articles in the Queensland Teachers Journal. For example, in an
article which provided sets of examples for mental arithmetic, although the heading
was Mental Arithmetic, the subheading on the same page was Seventh grade oral
mathematics ("J.R.D., 1931, p. 9), the implication being that the set of examples
was to be presented orally, rather than by writing on the blackboard or on cards.
Although oral arithmetic did not appear as a syllabus heading until 1930, it was
used as early as 1906 by a district inspector to refer to the calculation of exact
answers mentally. Canny (1907) observed in his report that "Oral arithmetic [italics
added] is yet, as of old, mostly a failing subject" (p. 55). Nevertheless, mental

181

arithmetic was the term most commonly used by District Inspectors in their reports
prior to the introduction of the 1952 syllabus, by the authors of text books used in
Queensland classrooms, and by the writers of curriculum articles in the journals of
the Queensland Teachers' Union and in the Education Office Gazette. It therefore
seems reasonable to assume that, despite the various descriptions used in the
syllabuses from 1860 to 1965, mental arithmetic was the term habitually used by
school personnel, at least until the 1950s when oral arithmetic and mental (personal
knowledge) became the commonly used terms. This, despite the fact that the work
associated with the calculation of exact answers mentally was rarely referred to as
mental arithmetic in the various syllabuses. This occurred only for the Third and
Fourth Classes in the 1860 Schedule and for the Fifth and Sixth Classes in the 1897
and 1902 Schedules (see Appendices A.1, A.5 & A.6).
The inconsistency with which the terms were used undoubtedly contributed to
the blurring of the distinction between oral and mental arithmetic, with an attendant
loss of the intended meaning of oral work. In his report for the 1958 school year,
District Inspector Searle (1959) noted that "the lack of practice in oral [italics added]
arithmetic [that is, mental arithmetic] problems is reflected in the results in written
work of this type, which is very seldom satisfactory" (p. 14). In contrast, in his report
of the same year, District Inspector Costin (1959) suggested that there was "an
undue reduction in oral [italics added] teaching and explanation" (p. 14), a view with
which District Inspector Pyle (1959) concurred: "There [was] a tendency [for some
teachers] to resort to the old mental' types of examples rather than [use] the oral
lessons as aids to mechanical accuracy on the one hand and processes on the
other" (p. 9).
The apparent confusion in the use of these terms reflects that referred to by Hall
(1954), with respect to usage in the United Statesan issue discussed in Chapter 2.
Hall's (1954, p. 351) conclusion that many authorities attempted to avoid
controversy by using a range of terms interchangeably may also be appropriate for
the Queensland situation, at least prior to 1952. From an analysis of primary
historical sources, there appears to have been little serious discussion in
Queensland concerning the use of these terms. What did occur was largely based
on overseas debate, particularly that from the United Kingdom, the most influential
of which is likely to have been contained in Primary Education: A Report of the
Advisory Council on Education in Scotland (1949). Greenhalgh (1949b), writing in

182

the Queensland Teachers Journal, highlighted the following in his presentation of


key aspects of this Scottish Report to Queensland teachers: "All arithmetic is
mental; the kind so called would more correctly be termed oral.' The artificial
distinction thus made...encourages the giving of unnecessarily difficult and elaborate
examples" (p. 11). Given that Greenhalgh was a member of the Department of
Public Instruction's Syllabus Committee, which was responsible for overseeing the
development of the 1952 Syllabus, it is not surprising that oral arithmetic was the
term consistently used in reference to what was colloquially called mental arithmetic.
In the forward to his text, oral arithmetic, to support the 1952 syllabus, Olsen
(1953) stated that "mental arithmetic need not necessarily be oral arithmetic with its
attendant air of hurry and consequent tension for some children. It is apparent from
this statement that Olsen attributed to oral arithmetic the characteristics traditionally
associated with mental arithmetic, namely, a "series of short, low-level unrelated
questions to which answers [were]...calculated instantaneously and the answers
written down with speed" (McIntosh, 1990a, p. 40). Nevertheless, despite the
concern expressed, his text consisted of daily sets of ten questions on which "a time
limit [should] usually...[be] placed" (Olsen, 1953, Foreword).
It is likely that the inconsistency in the use and interpretation of the various
terms to describe the mental calculation of exact answers, and its attendant
confusion with the oral aspects of a mathematics lesson, contributed to mental
arithmetic being a facet of the mathematics curriculum that "never [seemed] to reach
a standard above fair in many schools" (Lidgate, 1958, p. 7). This was a conclusion
consistently reached by District Inspectors during the period under investigation.

3.4

Roles Ascribed to Mental Arithmetic


District Inspectors consistently reported that children did not demonstrate

proficiency with the ability to calculate mentally, despite mental arithmetic, in its
various guises, being considered an important aspect of all mathematics schedules
and syllabuses from 1860 to 1965. This may be attributed to the attitudes towards
mental arithmetic engendered by teachers. District Inspector Lidgate (1957)
observed in his 1956 report that "too many teachers...caused their pupils to dislike
Oral Arithmetic because they have treated the subject as one apart, or because they
[have made] the examples too difficult" (p. 8). Such practices can, at least in part,

183

be ascribed to the roles designated for mental arithmetic during this period. Mental
arithmetic was seen to provide:

1.

A tool for developing mechanical skill with the various arithmetic


processes18.

2.

A skill that was socially useful.

3.

A means for "quickening the intelligence" (Bevington, 1926, p. 80;


Robinson, 1882, p. 179).

Although the first two roles have an enduring validity, little concern for mental
arithmetic as a means for developing a deeper understanding of the structure of
numbers and their properties was evident. In contrast, such an outcome is essential
to the current vision for mental computation (R. E. Reys, 1984, p. 549). Reflecting a
utilitarian view of mental arithmetic, Cowham (1895) suggested that:

[Mental arithmetic] is of high value during the acquisition of a new rule, and is
not less valuable in the practical application of any rule to calculations of everyday life. The scholar who has had large experience in mental calculations
acquires thereby a facility in dealing with numbers which no amount of slatework can yield. (p. 169)

Nonetheless, in instances where children mentally applied the operations learnt


to the calculations of every-day life, this may, as an unforeseen consequence, have
resulted in a deeper understanding of numbers. Such an outcome was dependent
upon the nature of mental arithmetic as implementedan issue investigated in the
Section 3.5.3.
However, it was the third role, albeit a fallacious one, that was largely
responsible for mental arithmetic being an aspect of the curriculum that was not well
taught nor well liked by children. This role originated with a belief in the educational
theory of formal discipline, one form of the psychological theory of mental discipline,
which held that "education consists in strengthening or developing the powers of
mind by exercising them, preferably on difficult, abstract material, such as Latin,
Greek and mathematics" (Kolesnik, 1958, p. 4). Lawry (1968, pp. 635-636)
contends that there was general support for teaching methods based on this theory,

184

particularly prior to 1905 while the Department of Public Instruction was dominated
by Under Secretary Anderson and General Inspector Ewart, both of whom were
strong proponents of formal discipline. In directing that the teaching methods used
by teachers at the Central School in Brisbane were to provide the prototypes for all
other vested schools, it was stated in the Regulations for the Establishment and
Management of Primary Schools (1861) that "these methods...have for their object,
not the mere cramming of a child's memory, but the training and development of his
intellectual faculties" (p. 84). Hence, the primary school teacher was expected to
teach his pupils to think, with a minimal reliance on learning by rote, albeit in context
with providing exercises to strengthen the child's mind.
Despite such beliefs, none of the syllabus documents, nor accompanying notes,
made reference to this aspect of the role of mental arithmetic (see Appendix A).
Additionally, the theory of formal discipline was discredited early this century. This
was recognised by District Inspector Baker (1929) who stressed "that facility
acquired in any particular form of intellectual exercise produces a general
competence in all exercises that involve the same faculty is no longer accepted" (p.
273). Nonetheless, the belief in the value of mental gymnastics persisted to such an
extent that, although not referring to mental arithmetic specifically, the Syllabus
Committee overseeing the 1948 Amendments to the mathematics syllabus saw the
need to assert that teachers "should be instructed that all school-subjects should
now be approached from the stand-point of realism and practical utility and that the
old idea of including subjects to train the muscles of the mind' [was] now
discredited" (Mathematics Subcommittee, 1947, p. 1).

3.4.1 Mental Arithmetic as a Pedagogical Tool


As discussed in Chapter 2, the usual sequence for introducing computational
procedures entails mental calculation beyond the basic facts becoming a focus
subsequent to the introduction of the standard written algorithm for each operation
(see Figure 2.4). Musser (1982, p. 40) has suggested that such an approach results
in children having difficulty with mental calculations. An aim of the current vision for
mental computation is the fostering of a flexible approach to viewing numbers and
their interrelationships. Hence it has been argued (Cooper et al., 1992, pp. 100101) that the sequence for introducing computational procedures needs to be

185

revised so that mental computation (and computational estimation) becomes a focus


prior to, rather than subsequent to, the development of written procedures (see
Figure 2.5).
This view of mental computation was foreshadowed early this century in an
article published in The Education Office Gazette in 1908, an article adapted from
the England Board of Education's Suggestions for the Consideration of Teachers
published in 1905. In presenting hints for teaching arithmetic it was declared that:

It is important that arithmetic should be treated not merely as the art of


performing certain numerical operations; it should be taught with a view of
making the scholars think clearly and systematically about number. It is thus
clear that written work should be an appendage to mental work rather than the
reverse. (Teaching Hints: Arithmetic, 1908, p. 15)

The written-mental sequence for teaching mental and written computational


procedures has arisen in the Queensland context despite statements such as the
above. In various schedules and syllabuses prior to those of 1966-1968, and in
their accompanying notes, it was suggested that a prime role of mental arithmetic
was to provide an introduction to the written mechanical and problem work to be
undertaken. Although the 1904 Schedule was the first to specify that "written work
[should] be supplementary to the mental and oral work" (Schedule XIV, 1904, p.
206), this practice was one previously advocated by some of the recommended
texts for teachers on school method19, and by District Inspectors. Gladman (n.d.), in
School Method, a text authorised by the Department for use by pupil-teachers,
indicated that "mental exercises, generally involving concrete examples, should be
employed in introducing a new rule in arithmetic, and should increase in difficulty
until the learner finds it necessary to resort to the use of slate and pencil" (p. 75).
This view was supported by Robinson (1882): "Mental arithmetic should be taught
with each of the rules, and from the very first" (p. 178).
In his report for 1884, District Inspector Ross (1885) concluded that one reason
for arithmetic not being successfully taught in schools was that teachers did not fully
realise "the importance of mental arithmetic as initiatory and subsidiary to slate
work" (p. 70), a comment reiterated in his 1889 report (Ross, 1890, p. 109), and
supported by District Inspector Radcliffe in 1898 (p. 73). The preface to Schedule

186

XIV of the 1904 Regulations of the Department of Public Instruction stated that
"mental calculations should be the basis of all the instruction, and [that] the pupils
should be made familiar by mental exercises with the principles underlying every
process before the written work is undertaken" (Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 200). It
was stressed to District Inspectors that "the value of mental arithmetic in
familiarizing the pupils with the principles of new rules cannot be too strongly
insisted upon" (Circular to District Inspectors, 1904, p. 1).
This belief was a theme flowing through each syllabus from 1904 to 1964. The
1930 Syllabus, for example, stated that "the teacher should introduce...new [rules]
through appropriate and simple mental exercises devised by himself, not simply
extracted from available textbooks (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 31).
However, only occasionally did District Inspectors comment favourably on the
effective use of this approach by teachersfor example, Pyle (1954, p. 2). Annual
reports were used regularly to reemphasise their belief in the importance of using
mental arithmetic as preliminary to written methods. Representative of these were
the comments of Bevington (1925, p. 83), Fewtrell (1914, p. 70), and Lidgate (1953)
who asserted, "If children are sound at oral arithmetic there will be little to fear with
written arithmetic" (p. 2). Such comments suggest that the use of mental arithmetic
as an introduction to written work was not widely practised by Queensland teachers,
even though it seems reasonable to assume that issues highlighted in their annual
reports were ones that District Inspectors would have emphasised during their
inspections of State schools. Factors which may have contributed to the
development of this mismatch between the recommended teaching practices and
their implementation are analysed later in this chapter, particularly those factors
which influenced the teaching processfor example, large classes, poor teacher
education, the inspectorial and examination systems.
Echoing Ross's 1885 comment referred to previously, District Inspector Farrell,
in 1929, noted that:

Failure to teach mental arithmetic so as to make it a definite preparation for the


written work to follow and to aid the child generally in his Arithmetical
calculations is another cause of the poor results obtained in [arithmetic].
Frequently it is found that...mental exercises [and tables and notation] are
taught on the one hand with very little reference to the written work, and classes

187

appear to waste time in working out a series of mental gymnastics in figures


which lead nowhere. (p. 289)

These comments were reiterated by those of District Inspector Moorhouse


(1939, p. 76), and by District Inspector Crampton (1954) who noted that:

Many teachers [were] slow to realize that the main use of oral arithmetic is to
teach the various mathematics processes, and thus it becomes a lead-in to all
written arithmetic. Oral arithmetic is a most valuable teaching medium, whereas
it [was] too often confined to testing. Graded teaching exercises should be first
used and, when the processes taught have been mastered, miscellaneous
exercises based on the processes taught should be given to consolidate the
work taught. (p. 3)

The importance of typing the mental arithmetic to be taught on the written


arithmetic that was to follow, was stressed in various curriculum articles in the
Queensland Teachers' Journal, particularly during the late 1920s and 1930s.
"J.R.D." (1930, p. 13) suggested that if the written work was to include the example
48 plus 69 plus 77 plus 89 then the oral work should focus on examples such as 9
plus 7, 16 plus 9 and 25 plus 8. However, it is likely that such an analytic approach
may have contributed to an emphasis being placed on gaining the correct answers,
rather than on the methods used by children to arrive at those answers, in effect,
testing rather than teaching. In comparison, this teaching principle was interpreted
in Mental Arithmetic: A Few Suggestions (1910) at a global levelthat is, the focus
was on the structure of the written examples. It was suggested that a written
Standard I problem such as "A man has 94 apples. He gives away 30, and shares
the rest equally between 4 boys. How many does each boy get?" becomes for
mental arithmetic: "A boy has 18 apples. Gives away 6. Shares rest equally
between 4 boys. How many each?" (p. 176). However, neither of these
approaches would have assisted children to think creatively and systematically
about number.
The mental-written sequence for teaching arithmetic operations was embodied
in the 1930 Syllabus as a spiralled introduction to new processes and rules. For
example, the oral (mental) work for Grade II included the "application of

188

multiplication to easy one-step reduction of money" (Department of Public


Instruction, 1930, p. 36), and in Grade III, "MoneyFour rules and reduction of sums
to 20" was specified as written work (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p.
37). This approach was foreshadowed in the text recommended for teachers from
1912 as a replacement for Gladman's School Work, a "faithful servant to
Queensland schools" (Roe, 1913, p. 33), namely, Cox and MacDonald's (n.d.)
Practical School Method. In this it was emphasised that "in the last quarter of the
school year [mental arithmetic (oral practice)] should deal chiefly with the work of
the next highest standard" (p. 217), a practice included in the Instructions to His
Majesty's Inspectorate in England (cited by Cox & MacDonald, n.d., p. 217).
Besides the role of mental arithmetic as a preparation for particular written work,
District Inspector Cochran (1960, p. 12) suggested that oral work could also serve a
number of other purposes: (a) to give brisk drill in basic number facts, especially
when they are presented in a wide variety of forms; (b) to cultivate speed and
accuracy in new work; and (c) to revise work essential for sound progress.
These were legitimate goals for an arithmetic lesson, previously highlighted in
the Board of Education's Handbook of Suggestions (1937, p. 513), and by District
Inspectors in their annual reportsfor example, Mutch (1916, p. 62). With respect to
revision work, it was recommended in the Education Office Gazette in 1927 that a
distinction needed to be made between the revision of rules and the revision of
mechanical operations for the purposes of speed and accuracy, with the former
being "undertaken through mental problems involving different processes"
(Teaching of Arithmetic, 1927, p. 292). Additionally, Farrell (1929) suggested that,
through oral exercises in revision work, "at least four times the ground could be
covered, the knowledge gained becomes conversational which is the knowledge
mostly required in every-day life, and it also becomes habitual, thus adding greater
facility to the written work" (p. 295). However, these goals, in combination with the
nature of the textbooks used by teachers, may have contributed to the belief that
mental arithmetic primarily involves the giving of a series of examples, often
involving one-step, with little or no reference to the strategies used by the children.
Of relevance to strategies for calculating mentally, was the recommended use of
mental work to introduce short methods of calculation (Board of Education, 1937, p.
513; Jeffrey, 1923, p. 68), both mental and written. In the Introductory Notes to the
1952 Syllabus it was emphasised that the application of oral arithmetic should not

189

be limited to such methods of calculation (Department of Public Instruction, 1952b,


p. 2), a point also made by Cox and MacDonald (n.d., p. 217). Nevertheless, in
contrast to the range of strategies used to calculate exact answers mentally, which
were discussed in Chapter 2 (see Tables 2.1 to 2.4), it was also stated in the 1952
Syllabus that "the processes which are applied orally are the same as those used in
written operations" (Department of Public Instruction, 1952b, p. 2), further
emphasising the perceived role of mental exercises as an introduction to the written
work, albeit one that would have restricted the development of a range of flexible
idiosyncratic strategies for children to use.
The narrowness of this statement on the role of mental strategies in the 1952
Syllabus, which does not appear in the 1964 Syllabus, may have been influenced by
the Board of Education's (1937) Handbook of Suggestions, the text from which the
syllabus aims were drawn. It stated, in reference to the mechanical rules of written
arithmetic, that "[they]...are best regarded as forms of mental technique or as
complex habits to be formed" (p. 506). Such does not necessarily imply that mental
and written techniques are synonymous. However, it was held that where the goal
was to develop skill with written computation, "the purpose [of mechanical work, as
opposed to problem work, was]...to help the child to form the mental habits in which
skill in computation...[was believed to have been] rooted" (Board of Education, 1937,
p. 506). Such a view reflects that of Thorndike (1922), who stated that "[children]
learn the method of manipulating numbers by seeing them employed, and by more
or less blindly acquiring them as associative habits" (p. 71).

3.4.2 The Social Usefulness of Mental Arithmetic


Although the mathematics schedules and syllabuses prior to that of 1952 did not
specifically refer to the social utility of mental arithmetic, it can be argued that this
was a constant theme during the period under investigation, given statements within
recommended textbooks and by District Inspectors. In defining "Mental' Arithmetic
[as including] all exercises in which pen or pencil is not used, except perhaps to
record the answer, the Board of Education (1937) concluded that, in this sense,
"much of the Arithmetic of everyday life is mental'" (p. 513). The 1952 Syllabus
asserted that "the fact that oral arithmetic is more commonly used in after-school life
than written arithmetic [indicated] to teachers the importance attached to this section

190

of the work" (Department of Public Instruction, 1952b, p. 2), a sentiment reaffirmed


in the syllabus of 1964 (Department of Public Instruction, 1964, p. 2). This
statement reflected those in textbooks authorised for use by school personnel
during the early years of the Department of Public Instruction: Gladman (n.d., p. 74);
Park (1879, p. 42); Joyce (1881, p. 209); Robinson (1882, p. 179); and Gladman
(1904, p. 206).
In stressing that mental arithmetic was a valuable preparation for the business
of life, Robinson (1882) highlighted two aspects, namely those required during
industrial endeavours and "those little domestic and mercantile transactions that are
continually occurring to all of us" (p. 179). Whereas Robinson (1882) focussed the
former on the usefulness of mental arithmetic in agricultural communities, Gladman
(n.d.) emphasised its commercial usefulness:

The practical value of [mental arithmetic] in ordinary business is universally


allowed, and those who have watched the employs (sic) in our London
warehouses as they "extend" their invoices, hardly know which to admire most,
the wonderful accuracy and rapidity of the calculations, or the extent to which
business is facilitated by their skill. (p. 74)

Consequently mental arithmetic was considered a "subject of great practical


importance" (Park, 1879, p. 42) within the primary school curriculum, a belief often
reiterated by District Inspectors in their annual reports. District Inspector Macgroarty
(1891) wrote in his report for 1890 that "the importance of mental arithmetic...in
rendering instruction in [arithmetic] intelligent and interesting...can scarcely be
overrated, entering as it does so largely into the everyday transactions of most
people in all walks of life" (p. 75). A similar view was expressed by Caine (1878, p.
97), Bevington (1926, p. 80) and Router (1941, p. 2) in their annual reports, and by
Farrell (1929, p. 283).
That the social utility of mental arithmetic was a consistent concern of those
who devised and supervised the mathematics schedules and syllabuses is a
distinguishing feature of the mathematics taught in Queensland schools. In
contrast, in the United States of America, as discussed in Chapter 1, the renewed
emphasis on the social utility of mathematics in the 1930s and 1940s resulted in a
revival of mental arithmetic, following its de-emphasis in American classrooms,

191

which had occurred following the discrediting of the theory of formal discipline some
twenty years previously.
A concern for the social usefulness of mental arithmetic was declared within the
wider context of the usefulness of arithmetic generally in the after-life of children, a
view expressed by Cox and MacDonald (n.d., p. 180), and Gladman (n.d., p. 72).
Both the 1914 and 1930 Syllabuses suggested that "the very practical purposes that
mathematical work has to serve in the child's future life should regulate the
character of its treatment in school" (Department of Education, 1914, p. 60; 1930, p.
30). It was suggested, in part, that "the teaching of...[arithmetic] should aim...at
imparting facility in the working of concrete examples dealing with matters pertaining
to the everyday life of the pupil" (Department of Education, 1914, p. 60; 1930, p. 30).
However, these syllabuses also assisted in perpetuating the belief in the
disciplinary powers of arithmetic by stating that another aim of teaching arithmetic
was to "[develop] the intelligence" (Department of Education, 1914, p. 60; 1930, p.
30). This aim, although increasingly open to question, was not inconsistent with
such beliefs as those expressed by Macgroarty (1886) in his 1885 report, who,
although recognising arithmetic's social utility, was also concerned with its
disciplinary powers:

Arithmetic, which comes into play in the ordinary transactions of business life, is
of very great importance in its practical bearings, but the part it takes in
developing the pupil's intellectual and reasoning faculties, when properly and
intelligently taught, is hardly of less importance. (p. 63)

3.4.3 Mental Discipline and Mental Arithmetic


Writing in the Queensland Teachers' Journal in 1945, "X+Y=Z" asserted that
there was a widespread belief among both educators and laymen "that a study of
mathematics improves [a child's] all round reasoning powers irrespective of what he
reasons about" (p. 14). This belief had its origins in the theory of formal discipline,
the most pervasive educational theory of the nineteenth century (Burns, 1973, p. 1).
This belief persisted throughout the first half of the twentieth century, despite its
tenets having been seriously questioned, both philosophically and scientifically. The
theory of formal discipline provided the basis for maintaining that "[mental arithmetic]

192

improves the tenacity of the mind, strengthens the memory, cultivates the power of
abstraction, and tends to promote clearness of conception....It developes [sic] such
moral qualities as patience, readiness, activity, and presence of mind" (Wilkins,
1886, p. 40). Further,

If a pupil is induced, first to give close attention to what he is expected to do,


secondly to keep to the terms of his question, and thirdly to give a result which
is in accordance with the truthit can not but react favourably on his moral
character. (Martin, 1920a, p. 82)

The consideration of moral, in addition to the intellectual aspects of behaviour,


distinguished formal discipline from the theory from which it originatedmental
discipline (Kolesnik, 1958, p. 7). Mental discipline, Kolesnik (1958) contends,
"signifies nothing more than the psychological view that man's mental capacities can
somehow be trained to operate more efficiently in general,' and the philosophical
conviction that such training constitutes one of the chief purposes of schooling" (p.
3). Formal discipline, however, involved developing the powers of the mind by
exercising them on difficult, abstract material such as Latin, Greek and Mathematics
(Kemp, 1944, p. 29). In so doing transfer of learning was thought to be facilitatedto
produce a mental gymnast of a general kind from undertaking mental gymnastics of
a particular kind (Ballard, 1928a, p. 3). However, the terms mental discipline and
formal discipline were not used precisely, particularly as now defined, during the
19th and early 20th centuries. Mental discipline was commonly used to refer to the
characteristics of the three related, but essentially separate, theories.
Underpinning formal discipline was a belief that the mind was composed of a
number of distinct powers or faculties. These included memory, attention,
observation, reasoning and will (Kolesnik, 1958, p. 6). Robinson (1882) suggested
that the foremost value of such calculations as 78, 654, 931 multiplied by 6 was not
arithmetical but educational:

Practically, a child will never be called upon to solve mentally so long a


question....Its utility consists in the formation of a power of concentrating all the
faculties on the performances of an allotted task; and the mind that can do so

193

will soon prove capable of any amount of labour upon other tasks as well. (p.
178)

Although a similar view was taken by Park (1879, p. 43) and Gladman (1904, p.
200), they did not propose that children should operate with numbers as large as
those suggested by Robinson (1882, p. 178), a recommendation in keeping with the
Queensland mathematics schedules of that time. Park (1879, p. 43) advocated that,
once a class had a fairly intelligent grasp of an operation, a few questions should be
framed for the children to work mentally, with such questions being related to the
business of commercial and every-day life. Under the 1904 Schedule, where a
focus was on the application of mathematical principles, mental arithmetic ideally
should have discarded large numbers, and consisted of exercises which did not
require any great mechanical working, so much as an intelligent grasp of principles
(Mental Arithmetic: A Few Suggestions, 1910, p. 176).
Many colonial teachers, however, went beyond the requirements of the
schedules. In their hands "mental arithmetic consisted principally in working certain
hard numbers in the shortest time by the shortest method" (Mental Arithmetic: A
Few Suggestions, 1910, p. 176). Nonetheless, Burns (1973, p. 4) questions
whether such teachers fully implemented their espoused beliefs in the use of
arithmetic to, in the words of one Queensland State school teacher, "help sharpen
the wits and strengthen the mind's grasp" (Scientific and Useful, 1882, p. 301).
The latter was a view in accordance with that held by Ewart (1890) who stated, in
his report for 1889, that from the "intelligent teaching [of arithmetic accrues] the
intellectual gymnastics necessary for bracing the mind to logical and continuous
thought" (p. 67). Burns (1973) suggested that:

Despite their apparent lip service to the aim of mental discipline and to its basis
in faculty psychology the practices of many colonial teachers implied a different
kind of aim from the one ostensibly guiding their teaching. The real aim of most
teachers centred on the inculcation and retention of information and if they
concerned themselves at all with faculty development then it was almost
exclusively with the faculty of memory. (p. 4)

194

Although research had cast doubt on the validity of formal discipline and the
concomitant theory of transfer of training, beliefs in these theories persisted well into
the twentieth century in Queensland (Kemp, 1944, p. 29; Schonell, cited in
Teachers and the Syllabus, 1949, p. 1), albeit in a modified form. Greenhalgh
(1949a, p. 3) made use of Bassett's (1949) arguments in a further attempt to
convince Queensland teachers to forgo their conservatism in the late 1940s.
Bassett (1949, p. 110) had argued that the fall-back position of the proponents of
formal discipline, which held that mental arithmetic, in the form in which it had been
taught, had a special disciplinary value, was also questioned by research evidence.
Prior to this, in 1947, Greenhalgh had asked:

How...may we justify many thousands of the mathematics exercises that have


been administered to children in enormous doses...; and above all that mental
arithmetic, the sole effect of which [was] to develop in both teachers and pupils
that nervous strain more often associated with hospitals to which the same
adjective is applied? (p. 11)

Only in the United States of America did educators take cognisance of these
experimental findings ("X+Y=Z, 1945, p. 14), the most influential of which were
those of Thorndike obtained during the first quarter of the twentieth century (Kemp,
1944, p. 31). These findings were accepted to such a degree that mental arithmetic
ceased to be an essential part of the primary curriculum (Reys & Barger, 1994, p.
33). However, with the discrediting of formal discipline, many psychologists and
educators assumed that transfer of training and mental discipline had also been
discredited. "The mind could not be trained in general'. [Hence] whatever was to
be learned had to be taught specifically, and the only things worth teaching were
those for which there was some obvious and immediate use" (Kolesnik, 1958, p. 5).
To use Greenhalgh's (1947, p. 11) term, the predominant educational philosophy in
the 1930s and 1940s was realism. The only "justification [for] the inclusion of any
subject rests in its usefulness to the child, the child as he now is and the man he is
to be" (Greenhalgh, 1947, p. 11), a belief that permeated the 1948 Amendments
and the 1952 Syllabus.
From his 1901 study, Thorndike concluded that "it [was] misleading to speak in
terms of sense discrimination, attention, memory, observations and quickness, since

195

what these words refer to are multitudinous, separate, individual functions' which
may have very little in common" (Kolesnik, 1958, p. 33). Chief-Inspector Edwards
(1936), reporting to Queensland teachers following his visit to the United States of
America, indicated that "the Progressivist does not believe that there is any such
thing as general mental training. Mental benefit is specific. The results obtained
from one subject cannot be transferred to any other subject" (p. 16).
Prior to the work of Thorndike, belief in the tenets of formal discipline and faculty
psychology were shaken by the advocates of Herbartian psychology (Ballard,
1928a, p. 3). Johann Herbart, a German philosopher, "contemptuously [cast] aside
the doctrine of inborn faculties or capacities for acquiring knowledge" (Fennell,
1902, p. v) when he declared, as cited by Kolesnik (1958), that those who "cherish
the obsolete opinion that there resides in the human soul such certain powers or
faculties which have to be trained have no psychological insight" (p. 24). The
Herbartian, therefore, did not set out to train or exercise the mind, but rather aimed
to present new information in a form that could be selected and assimilated with the
old (Fennell, 1902, p. v). Herbart asserted that masses of ideas needed to be
trained, not abstract powers or faculties.
The continued belief by Queensland teachers in aspects of formal discipline,
faculty psychology and transfer of training was attributable, in Burns' (1973, p. 4)
view, to the teacher training received under the pupil-teacher system which tended
to perpetuate the traditional beliefs and practices espoused by senior teachers, and
to the system of examinations and inspections which placed a premium on
knowledge and learning by rote. Hence the concern was for developing the
memory, as Burns (1973, p. 4) has suggested. That teachers and District
Inspectors retained a belief in the role of arithmetic, in general, and mental
arithmetic, in particular, as a means for quickening the intelligence, developing
judgement, improving reasoning (Baker, 1929, p. 281; Bevington, 1923, p. 64;
Mutch, 1924, p. 40) and the "power of concentrating the mind upon the solution of a
problem" (Martin, 1916, p. 135), may also be ascribed to their beliefs about the
nature of mathematics. Cox and MacDonald (n.d.), the recommended text on
school management from 1912, asserted that:

As a means of mental training, arithmetic is the most important subject of the


elementary school curriculum. Nothing need be taken for granted; every truth is

196

capable of demonstration, and each new truth is seen to grow out of what has
preceded. Thus the child is trained to investigate, to think in logical sequence,
and to advance step by step along a chain of reasoning, until the desired truth is
demonstrated. In this process the child learns the value of methodical
arrangement and clearness of statement; he is taught to discern the essential
from the non-essential, and to seize on that which is useful for his purpose; he
is trained to habits of close attention and fixity of purpose, knowing no rest till
the end is attained. Each successful effort tends to make him more and more
conscious of his powers, and implants a spirit of self-reliance and perseverance.
(pp. 180-181)

Ballard (1928a, p. xi) pointed out that this English view of arithmetic represented
arithmetic as logic, whereas the American view was that of arithmetic as habit, a
view based primarily on Thorndike's (1922) associationist beliefs about how
arithmetic should be taught. However, it is the English view that was most influential
on Queensland mathematics education beliefs and practices (Clements, Grimison, &
Ellerton, 1989, p. 51; Schildt, Reithmuller, & Searle, n.d., p. 8). Arithmetic as logic
was also espoused by Gladman (n.d.): "Mathematics are studied, not so much for
the practical worth of their facts as for the logical processes through which the mind
must pass in learning them" (p. 73). Cox and MacDonald (n.d., p. 217) argued that
problems worked mentally share the same disciplinary value as those worked with
paper and pencil, but have the advantage of being worked quickly. Hence mental
arithmetic was judged to be the means for training children to think and to reason.
"Intelligence in Arithmetic should be secured through the medium of mental
exercises...taken for a few minutes at the commencement of [each] arithmetic
lesson" (Bevington, 1925, p. 83). Accuracy in thinking and reasoning was
paramount, as was accuracy in each step taken in working towards a solution
(Martin, 1920a, p. 81).
Following the implementation of the 1904 Schedule, with its emphasis on the
correlation of subjects and the self-activity of the pupils, Mutch (1907) commented
that "now teachers regard arithmetic not only as a practical art, but also as an
excellent means of intellectual discipline....With...more stress laid on oral arithmetic,
there has been a distinct gain in thinking power" (p. 70). It is possible that Mutch
was reflecting a modified view of mental discipline, one that discounted the beliefs in

197

direct transfer of training and in faculties of the mind, as defined by the proponents
of faculty psychology. This revised view of mental discipline held that a study of
arithmetic, including mental arithmetic, enhances concentration and the ability to
think critically:

By mathematics a power has been gained that is universally applicable in all


mental operations. The activity of attention is fundamental to all intellectual life,
and the power of concentration and application developed to a habit by
mathematical work will lead a pupil to attack a new subject and to progress with
it more effectively than if no such habit had been acquired. (Welton, 1924, pp.
409-410)

3.5

The Nature of Mental Arithmetic


In coming to an understanding of the characteristics of mental arithmetic during

the period under investigation, cognisance needs to be taken of issues beyond the
background issues related to syllabus development and implementation, and the
roles of mental arithmetic discussed previously. Aspects which need to be analysed
are: (a) the various interpretations placed on the term mental arithmetic, (b) the
nature of mental arithmetic as embodied in the syllabus documents, and (c) the
characteristics of mental arithmetic as implemented, given that the nature of mental
arithmetic as reflected in the learning experiences of children, does not necessarily
mirror syllabus content nor the recommended ways in which skill with mental
calculation should be taught.

3.5.1 Interpretations of Mental Arithmetic


Reflecting the social usefulness of arithmetic skills, Joyce (1881) defined a good
practical arithmetician as:

[One who] can perform mentally, with readiness, and with little danger of error,
all those innumerable short computations that are met with in everyday life;

198

and...who can execute on paper all sorts of elementary calculations, even when
considerably extended with rapidity and certainty. (p. 203)

In keeping with this view, Ross (1905) suggested that "the desiderata of mental
arithmetic are speed' and accuracy'" (p. 33). Additionally, the introduction to a
mental arithmetic textbook20 commonly used by Queensland teachers early in the
twentieth century highlighted that mental arithmetic questions should exercise the
mind of the child so as to encourage dexterity with numbers ("An Inspector of
Schools, 1914, p. 3). This consideration alludes to current beliefs about mental
computation, which stress a need for children to be able to perform mental
calculations with nonstandard strategies by taking advantage of an ability to
compose and decompose numbers (Resnick, 1989b, p. 36; Sowder, 1992, p. 4).
Although little recognition was given in the various schedules and syllabuses to
the need for children to be encouraged to invent short methods for themselves, as
advocated by Joyce (1881, p. 215), such was occasionally given, in an ad hoc
manner, by articles in the Queensland Teachers' Journal and The Education Office
Gazette, and by District Inspectors. In Teaching Hints: Arithmetic (1908), it was
stated that "the teacher [should] have no difficulty in devising questions in mental
arithmetic which are easy by special methods but too difficult for mental work by
ordinary rules" (p. 16). In providing advice to teachers, District Inspector Bevington
(1925) did not necessarily interpret all short cuts as being based on the rote
application of rulesthat is, on instrumental understanding, as defined in Chapter 2.
Bevington (1925, p. 83) advocated the use of a compensatory approach (see Table
2.4) for examples such as 99 + 87 and 100 books at 19s 11d.
A similar approach was recommended by District Inspector Kennedy (1903) for
mentally calculating the cost of 24 articles at 19s.11d. eachby taking 24 pence
from 24 pounds. Nevertheless, he did consider that his example was "a
suppositious and extreme one" (p. 69), when placed in context with the usual
method of calculating such examples using aliquot parts. A more detailed outline of
flexible approaches to mental addition and subtraction was presented by Cox and
MacDonald (n.d., p. 224). A range of work from the left strategies (see Table 2.4)
was advocated for addition: For example, 25 + 37 = 2 tens + 3 tens = 5 tens 5; 5
tens 5 + 7 = 6 tens 2 = 62. For subtraction, a decomposition strategy (see Table

199

2.4) was the approach suggested. It was pointed out that 45 - 18, for example,
could be calculated as 45 - 10 = 35; 35 - 8 = 27 (Cox & MacDonald, n.d., p. 231).
However, such advocacies for flexibility were counteracted not only by an
emphasis on the application of the written methods of computation, but also by such
rigid views, with respect to process, as those expressed in Mental Arithmetic: A Few
Suggestions (1910):

Great stress should be paid to the correct method of obtaining answers. When
asked how the first answer (12) is obtained [for "A boy has 18 apples. Gives
away 6. Shares equally between 4 boys. How many each?"] such an answer
as 12 and 6 make 18 is incorrect. The child must see that from 18 six has been
subtracted. The rest of the sum should be solved by the use of correct
operationsi.e., 12 is divided by 4, not that 4 x 3 = 12. This is important. (p. 176)

Effective mental methods cannot be acquired by rote learning (French, 1987, p.


39). This was recognised by District Inspectors Kennedy (1887, p. 82) and Caine
(1878, p. 97) in the late nineteenth century. The former concluded that the
unsatisfactory results in mental arithmetic were due to "this branch [appearing] to be
looked upon as merely a matter of fixed rules, applicable to particular kinds of
worksuch as finding the price of a dozen, score or gross" (Kennedy, 1887, p. 82).
Such a focus on rule of thumb methods, applied without an understanding of the
principles involved, was one which Priestley (1912, p. 222) believed would be
eradicated by the New Education movement, which at that time was rapidly gaining
strength. However, although an emphasis on short cuts diminished as the century
progressed (see Appendix A), the widespread implementation of methods which
encouraged children's understanding of mathematical processes did not occur until
late-1960s, a period during which mental arithmetic did not receive explicit
recognition in syllabus documents (see Section 4.2).
The poor performances were also attributed to "[mistaken efforts] to apply to
mental calculation processes proper enough in slate arithmetic but quite out of place
in [mental arithmetic]" (Kennedy, 1903, p. 69). Nonetheless, if this practice did
receive reduced emphasis during the first half of the twentieth century, it gained
renewed prominence in the 1952 Syllabus which emphasised that the procedures

200

used in written arithmetic should be the ones applied orally (Department of Public
Instruction, 1952b, p. 2).
Robinson (1882), in advocating that mental arithmetic was simply a collection of
rules, suggested that mental arithmetic was "not so much opposed to slate
arithmetic as to mechanical arithmetic, though it is opposed to both" (p. 177).
Computations were considered to be mechanical when a class of questions was
solved using one fixed rule. Such an approach reduced the practical application of
mental arithmetic as the rules were easily forgotten (Robinson, 1882, p. 177). In
contrast,

When mental arithmetic is opposed to mechanical, it consists of a judicious


modification of the common rules, or in the framing of rules entirely new....This
sort of mental arithmetic, therefore, requires a quick intelligence, and active and
retentive memory, a thorough acquaintance with each step in the ordinary rules,
and a perfect knowledge of the principles upon which they are founded.
(Robinson, 1882, pp. 178-179)

Others, during the era under investigation, took a more simplistic view of mental
arithmetic. Lidgate (1954) suggested that "oral arithmetic [would] improve if children
[were] induced to realise that it is merely the application of tables" (p. 2). Whereas
this may be true for children in lower grades, as was asserted by Joyce (1881, p.
210) and in the notes accompanying the 1914 Syllabus (Department of Public
Instruction, 1914, p. 64), for example, the beliefs of Robinson (1882, pp. 178-179)
suggest that mental arithmetic involves much more than the mere application of
tables. Such beliefs are consistent with the characteristics of mental computation
discussed in Chapter 2.
Nonetheless, Queensland teachers appear to have consistently taken a narrow
view of mental arithmetic, a view that cannot be fully explained merely by reference
to the apparent on-going belief in aspects of the theory of formal discipline.
Essential to this analysis is a consideration of more general factors that impinged on
teacher beliefs and practices. These include: (a) the beliefs about mental arithmetic
embodied in the syllabuses, (b) the influence of the systems of examination and
inspection, (c) the level of teacher training, and (d) the size and structure of classes
taught.

201

3.5.2 The Syllabuses and Mental Arithmetic


Following the separation of Queensland from New South Wales on 10
December 1859, the establishment of the Board of General Education, through the
Education Act of 1860, provided the foundation for the development of primary
education in Queensland. The system framed by the board was largely that of the
national system of education in Ireland (Our First Half-Century, 1909, p. 78), this
being the system on which the New South Wales National Schools had previously
been based. The Education Act of 1860, however, did not specify the curriculum.
This was published in the regulations adopted by the Board of General Education on
14 December 1860 (Regulations, 1860, pp. 15-17) as a Table of the Minimum
Amount of Attainments Required From Each Class in Primary Schools (see
Appendix A.1). This table outlined the attainments expected of children enrolled for
one quarter of the school year in each of the classes.
The expected attainments were identical to those established by the National
Board in New South Wales (Statement. Explanatory, n.d., p. 1). In both of these
syllabuses, children in First Class were expected "to perform mentally all the
Elementary Arithmetical operations with numbers, not involving a higher result than
30" (Regulations, 1860, p. 15)that is, to be able to add, subtract, multiply and
divide whole numbers. Children in third and fourth classes were expected to
become familiar with the "Rules of Mental Arithmetic" (Regulations, 1860, pp. 1617)for example, with the rules for finding the cost of a dozen, score or gross of
articles21. These rules, predominantly related to money calculations which were
essential for "the business and duties of every-day life" (Park, 1879, p. 43), were
usually listed in arithmetic and mental arithmetic texts, with the number of rules
presented varying from text to text.
With the passing of the Education Act of 1875, which established the
Department of Public Instruction, education in Queensland passed from being
controlled by a subordinate board under the control of Executive Council to being
overseen by a responsible minister, the first of whom was Sir Samuel Griffith, whose
beliefs about education prevailed as "they were administratively feasible and
politically acceptable at the time" (Lawry, 1975, p. 58). The curriculum outlined in
Schedule V of the Regulations of the Department of Public Instruction (Schedule

202

V, 1876, pp. 825-826) was narrower in scope than that in the 1860 Schedule. This
restriction was designed to prevent a clash between the curriculum offered by the
free primary schools and that of the Grammar Schools (Lawry, 1975, p. 59).
Schedule V presented a Table of the Minimum Amount of Attainments Required
From Pupils for Admission Into Each Class in Primary Schools (Schedule V, 1876,
p. 825) (see Appendix A.2). General Inspector Anderson noted in his report for
1876 that the publication of the standards of attainments ensured "that no teacher
who [read the schedule] ...carefully [could] unintentionally commit the grave fault of
promoting his scholars prematurely" (Anderson, 1877, p. 21). The implementation
of this table was clarified in a revision to Schedule V published in 1885. It explained
that "the work to be gone through in any class (the Fifth Class excepted) [was to] be
found detailed in the column with the name of the class next above it" (Schedule V,
1885, p. 490).
With respect to mental arithmetic, the changes contained in the 1876 Schedule
set more realistic goals for each class than those embodied in that of 1860.
Children were required "to perform mentally easy operations in the simple rules"for
addition, subtraction, multiplication and division, by the end of the Upper Second
Class. In the First Class, instead of being required to master all four operations with
results to 30, children were required under the 1876 Schedule "to perform mental
addition up to a result not higher than thirty. Subtraction was included for the
Lower Second Class (Schedule V, 1876, p. 825). Although no specific reference
was given to the rules of mental arithmetic in the 1876 Schedule, among the
arithmetic texts furnished to schools (List of Books, 1880, p. 23) were ones that
contained expositions of these rulesfor example, that presented in Colenso's
(1874) A Shilling Arithmetic. The importance for children to be able to calculate
mentally in a range of contexts beyond whole numbers and money was also
recognised in the 1876 Schedule. Work for the Third and Fourth Classes, and by
implication for the Fifth Class, included easy mental operations in the "compound
rules and reduction, including bills of parcels, rectangular areas, ...proportion,
practice, vulgar fractions and simple interest, including miscellaneous problems"22
(Schedule V, 1876, p. 825) (see Appendix A.2).
By the 1890s almost all children in Queensland were receiving some primary
education, albeit to a standard "hardly appropriate for a developing urban society"
(Lawson, 1970, p. 215). However, the compulsory attendance provision of the

203

Education Act of 1875 could not be enforced until the early 1900s as attendance
was irregular, and education was not held in high regard. It was Hanger's (1963)
view that:

More than sixty per cent of parents of 1890 were suspicious of education for
their children or hostile to it: they regarded it as useless and teachers as a
nuisance, and felt that the sooner the children were at work and helping to
support the home, the better for themselves and none the worse for the
children. (p. 89)

With the publication of a revised schedule (Para 143, 1891, pp. 23-24),
effective from January 1892, school head teachers gained additional guidance for
the placement of children into classes. Not only were the six classes redesignated
as First Class to Sixth Class, with a specified duration for each, but also the
schedule was restructured to include the expected standard of proficiency for each
class (Para 144, 1891, pp. 24-28) (see Appendix A.3). The main changes in
mathematics concerned the extension into elementary geometry for boys, whereas
girls were to devote more time to needlework (Lawry, 1968, p. 583). The standards
of proficiency provided greater guidance for the treatment of the various aspects of
arithmetic in each class. The work was delineated for each half-year of enrolment
and provided a sequence for introducing particular learnings. For example, children
in First Class were required "to add mentally numbers applied to objects to a result
not greater than 10" during the first half-year, to 20 during the second six months, to
30 during the third half-year and "to a result not greater than 40" by the end of the
fourth half-year (Para 144, 1891, pp. 24-25).
Nonetheless, few changes were made to mental arithmetic. As previously,
children were expected "to perform mentally operations in [the four] rules" by the
end the second class with the standards of proficiency for the third half-year
indicating mental addition and subtraction of money to one pound (Para 144, 1891,
p. 25). Mental subtraction was to be introduced during the third half-year of First
Class, so that by the end of that grade children were required "to add mentally
numbers to a result not greater than 40; [and] to lessen mentally any number of two
figures by any number of one digit" (Para 144, 1891, p. 25). Mental work for the
Third Class included shopping transactions based on measurement tables and easy

204

bills of parcel, as well as the application of the dozen and score rules. Mental
arithmetic in the Fourth and Fifth classes was extended to include the unitary
method23, decimals, percentages, and square and cube roots (see Appendix A.3).
Although some changes to the syllabus were introduced in 1894, 1897 and
1902, their minor nature induced Senior District Inspector Platt (1905) to observe in
his report for 1904 that "the course of instruction in our schools has been practically
the same for the last thirteen yearsthat is, since 1st January, 1892" (p. 30).
Although Platt was referring to the syllabus as a whole, his comments were
particularly relevant to mental arithmetic. In 1894, minor changes were made to the
standards for the First Class due to its length being reduced from two to 1 years
(see Appendix A.4): The end-of-class expectations remained the same, but the size
of the numbers to be manipulated in the each half-year were increasedto 20, 30
and 40 for each of the three half-years, respectively (Schedule VI, 1894, p. 321).
The State Education Act Amendment Act of 1897, effective from 1 July 1898,
not only introduced algebra and Euclid to the Fifth and Sixth Classes, but also
extended the First Class course to two years and reduced its requirements (see
Appendix A.5) to lessen "the field of work in schools taught by one teacher"
(Dalrymple, 1899, p. 5), an outcome of which Macgroarty (1900, p. 63) was not
convinced. Mental arithmetic for the first class focussed on developing the ability "to
add mentally numbers of one figure to a result not greater than fifty" (Schedule V,
1897, p. 798). During the Second Class the ability to mentally subtract, multiply and
divide with abstract numbers was to be developed. Additionally, mental operations
with the weights and measures considered to be of more use were designated for
the Fourth, rather than the Third Class.
The 1902 revised schedule (Schedule XIV, 1902, pp. 169-170), the first to
provide an indication of the expected age ranges of children in each of the classes,
did not introduce any explicit changes to mental arithmetic (see Appendix A.6). As
in the 1897 Schedule V, Schedule XIV (1902) and Schedule XV (1902), the latter
presenting the standards of proficiency, did not specifically specify the mental
arithmetic for the Fifth and Sixth Classes. For example, the arithmetic for the third
half-year of enrolment in the Sixth Class was listed as: "Commercial arithmetic;
mensurationLongmans' Chapters I to XI.; mental arithmetic" (Schedule XV, 1902,
p. 173).

205

However, some insights may be gained from the textbooks used in conjunction
with the 1897 and 1902 schedules. The mental arithmetic text supplied to schools
for the instruction of pupils was The Practical Mental Arithmetic, an English
publication written by "An Inspector of Schools" (1914). One of the four arithmetic
texts was A Shilling Arithmetic by Pendlebury and Beard, first published in 1899
(Appendix B, 1902, p. 95). This text contained a section devoted to the mental
rules for "the calculation of prices" (Pendlebury & Beard, 1899, pp. 174-175), which
constituted the only reference to mental arithmetic within the text. Unlike A Shilling
Arithmetic, which was arranged by topics, The Practical Mental Arithmetic was
organised into sets of questions for each of the classes in the schools in England
(Standards I to VII). It included the advice that teachers should consider the work
for Standards V to VII together, given that the numbers of children in upper
standards were usually small24 ("An Inspector of Schools, 1914, p. 103). The 195
questions for Standard V and the 160 for Standards VI and VII were relevant to the
work set for the Fifth and Sixth Classes in Queensland schools in the 1897 and
1902 schedules, given their focus on vulgar and decimal fractions, percentages and
proportion, in context with money, weights and measures.
In implementing the prescribed course of instruction for each class there is
evidence to suggest that teachers interpreted the order in which the learnings were
presented in the schedule as the order in which they were to be taught. This may
help to explain why mental arithmetic, "except in a few rare cases, [gave] poor
results" (Radcliffe, 1898, p. 73), an observation typical of those made by District
Inspectors during the period under investigation. In an analysis of the reports for
1897 in the Queensland Education Journal, it was noted that the schedule
"[prescribed] mental arithmetic last in the order of the arithmetic work, [whereas] it
ought to [have been]...first" (District Inspectors' Reports for 1897, 1898, p. 6). It
was also observed that Radcliffe "[had] put his finger on the key to the [poor results].
He [suggested that] the teaching of arithmetic [should be based] on a thorough
mental grasp of simple operations in the rules" (District Inspectors' Reports for
1897, 1898, p. 6), an affirmation of the importance of mental arithmetic as initiatory
to written work.
In accordance with this belief, stated officially for the first time in the preface to
the 1904 Schedule (Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 201), the mental work for each class
was listed separately and prior to the listing of the written work in mathematics for

206

Classes 2 to 6. Although the mental work remained essentially the same for each
class as that in the previous schedule, the presentation of the mental and oral work
reflected beliefs about how mathematics should be taught (see Appendix A.7).
Teachers were encouraged to take every opportunity to allow for the self-activity of
the pupils, thus allowing them to interact with actual objects and to measure
quantities using various unitssticks, bundles, tens, dozens, feet, pence, ounces
(Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 201).
The mental work for the Second Class, for example, included "concrete
exercises involving the four simple operations, and falling within the range of the
pupils' experience" (Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 206), a child-centred focus for the
operations dealt with in Second Class under previous schedules. However, the
syllabus for this class became more exacting with the inclusion of money tables,
previously in Third Class, and halves, fourths, and eighths of an inch which were
previously taught in Fourth Class. Proportion, however, was moved from Fourth
Class to the Fifth Class during which cubic measure, previously a Sixth Class
requirement, was also to be taught. Ratio was specifically included for the first time
in a schedule, as a topic for the Fourth Class (see Appendix A.7).
The syllabus which came into force from January 1915 was the first to be
published as a separate document, and the first to include detailed notes on the
teaching of mathematics delineated for each grade. In was noted in the preface to
this syllabus that the changes in the spirit of teaching, which were associated with
the 1904 Syllabus, "were so extensive that some years of practical experience as to
its working were required by teachers and inspectors before its full advantages
could be reaped or its deficiencies detected" (Department of Public Instruction,
1914, p. 5). As the departmental view of the 1904 Syllabus was that it was a "good
Syllabus" (Department of Public Instruction, 1914, p. 5), and one that was an
improvement on previous schedules, drastic changes were not made to the syllabus
in its 1914 form. With respect to mental arithmetic, no changes were made to the
requirements for the mental and oral work for any of the six primary classes (see
Appendix A.8).
In association with the introduction of the 1930 Syllabus, classes were
reclassified into seven yearly grades preceded by a preparatory grade of 1 years
for children enrolled in July, the time for enrolment considered most appropriate for
those born in the first half or early in the second half of the year (Department of

207

Public Instruction, 1930, p. x). It was expected that most children would enter Grade
II at approximately 7 years of age, an age comparable to that for the Second Class
under the 1914 Syllabus. Grades VI and VII, Forms I and II, respectively, of the
Intermediate Schools, were designed to cater for the interests, capacities and
attitudes of the over-twelves, with a study of algebra and geometry intended to
provide an opportunity for determining those children who would benefit from a high
school education (Greenhalgh, 1957, p. 75). The syllabus recognised, however,
that many children would not receive a secondary education. It was stated that "it is
generally agreed that the Primary School [is] the only school that some children will
know" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. v). The changes embodied in the
1930 Syllabus were formulated to:

Fulfil the demands for foundation work, [to apply] school Arithmetic to the
solution of problems connected with ordinary business transactions and
operations with numbers and quantities within the children's experience, and to
give more definitely the standards required in the different grades of the course.
(Farrell, 1929, p. 291)

The importance of mental and oral work was stressed in the belief that such
work had a definite place on the daily time-table and was the means by which new
work would be introduced (Farrell, 1929, pp. 294-295). Although major changes
were made to the written work25, the spiral nature of the 1930 Syllabus resulted in
the mental work under this syllabus being essentially the same as that for the 1914
Syllabus (see Appendix A.8), when allowances were made for the restructuring of
classes (New Syllabus, 1929, pp. 460-462). "A new rule [was to be] introduced in
one grade by means of mental exercises followed by easy mechanical exercises
and easy problems in the next grade, and by harder mechanical work and problems
in the succeeding grade" (Farrell, 1929, p. 292). For example, the mental and oral
work with vulgar fractions in Grades I to III were followed in Grade IV by written work
involving "simple exercises in finding fractional parts of quantities or numbers, and
in Grade V by the addition, subtraction, multiplication and division of vulgar fractions
(Department of Public Instruction, 1930, pp. 34, 35, 37, 39, 41).
In combination with the syllabus notes, which were set out in columns opposite
the requirements for each grade, the mental work in the 1930 Syllabus was

208

presented with greater specificity than that presented in previous syllabuses.


Mental calculations were to be applied to the range of mathematics topics to be
encountered during the seven years of primary school. These calculations included
mechanical and easy problem work with whole numbers, vulgar fractions, decimals,
money, weights and measures, ratio, proportion, percentages, simple interest, and
mensuration (see Appendix A.9). An emphasis was placed on social utility: For
Grade III, it was recommended that "the material for [simple exercises based on the
four rules in money was to] be supplied by ordinary household accounts, as for
example, the butcher's, the baker's, the grocer's or the draper's bills" (Department of
Public Instruction, 1930, p. 37). In accordance with this approach, and in context
with the deletion of obsolete matter from the syllabus, simple practice using aliquot
parts of a pound was retained only for mental exercisesexercises such as finding
the cost of 18 books at 2s. 6d each (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 42),
a Grade VI example.
A study of short methods of calculation was recommended for Grades V to VII,
"after the pupils [had] been thoroughly exercised in any rule" (Department of Public
Instruction, 1930, p. 41). However, the short methods to be applied were not
stipulated, except for the suggestion for Grade V that "the dozens' and scores'
rules might be applied" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 41). Such a
focus was designed to encourage initiative, particularly where different methods of
solution to the same problem were required of children, methods that did not entail
the rote application of rules.
In some respects the 1930 Syllabus was a more exacting one than that
introduced in 1915. Although the mental arithmetic for First Class in the 1914
Syllabus was limited to addition and subtraction to 50, this was extended to 99 in
1930, together with multiplication involving multipliers to 6. Multiplication was
applied "to easy one-step reduction of money and of the weights and measures
dealt with in the Tables" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 36) in the oral
work for Grade II, in preparation for the written reduction of money to 20 in Grade
III. Children in Grade I were also expected to find one-half, one-quarter and threequarters of numbers and quantities, albeit following "practical exercises in dividing
and measuring things" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 34).
Nonetheless, the 1930 Syllabus was characterised by more clearly defined
limits for mental operations with money, particularly for the lower grades. Exercises

209

in buying, selling and giving change were limited to 1s. in Grade I, and 1 in Grades
II and III. This contrasts with "exercises in domestic accounts and simple business
transactions" for the Third Class under the 1914 Syllabus (Department of Public
Instruction, 1914, p. 17), the first two terms of which equated with Grade III from
1930. From Grade IV, however, the requirements were not expressed so precisely,
with work for this class being delineated as "mental exercises based on the
compound rules, including household and shopping transactions familiar to the
pupils" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 39). Such wording opened the
1930 Syllabus to criticisms similar to those of the 1914 Syllabus, which centred on
its being "too vague and open to many constructions, even by common-sense
persons" (Arithmetic, 1927, p. 17), with the result that there were always wide
differences of opinion particularly regarding standards in mental work ("J.R.D.,
1931, p. 9).
The inappropriate interpretations placed on the 1930 Syllabus, together with
complaints of its being overloaded, resulted in amendments being introduced,
initially in 1938, and subsequently in 1948, as previously noted. Edwards (1938a, p.
2) pointed out that the 1938 amendments were more extensive for mathematics
than for other subjects within the primary school curriculum. Many of the changes
affected mental arithmetic, particularly that for the lower school where "greater
stress than formerly [was]...placed...on the mental and oral Arithmetic prescribed"
(Edwards, 1938a, p. 2). Whereas no specific reference was made to mental
arithmetic for the Preparatory Grade in the 1930 Syllabus, limits were placed on the
mental exercises for the preparatory grades26 under the 1938 amendments (see
Appendix A.10). These exercises, including easy problems, were limited to
numbers to 10 for Preparatory 1 and 2, to 19 for Preparatory 3 and to 99 for the
fourth preparatory grade (Department of Public Instruction, 1938, pp. 10-12).
Although some changes reduced the difficulty of the work to be undertaken,
others centred on re-allocating requirements to higher grades as a consequence of
changes to the written work, in context with the spiral nature of the syllabus. The
difficulty of fraction work for young children was given some recognition by
restricting a study of vulgar fractions in Grade I to one-half and one-quarter of
numbers and quantities. Additionally "resolving easy numbers such as 24 and 36 to
prime factors as preparation for the work in fractions and cancelling" was moved
from Grade III to Grade IV (Department of Public Instruction, 1938, p. 15),

210

concomitant with the work in vulgar fractions for Grade V altered to include "the four
simple rules with easy practical application to concrete quantities" (Department of
Public Instruction, 1938, p. 16). Similarly, expressing ratios as fractions and
decimals, and simple proportion were moved from Grade V to Grade VI. As a
consequence, Grade VI children were also required to undertake preparatory
exercises to simple proportion which entailed expressing ratios of measures as
vulgar and decimal fractionsfor example, "1 rood to 1 acre = or .25" (Department
of Public Instruction, 1938, p. 17).
Even though the 1938 amendments were considered to be "better adapted to
the mental ages of the children" (Edwards, 1939, p. 31), teachers continued to
advocate for further reductions in the requirements, as hitherto discussed. The
amendments introduced in January 1948 were designed to "give some relief where
such [was] necessary [and were to]...remain in force until a projected New Syllabus
[had] been drawn up" (Department of Public Instruction, 1948, p. 1), a project that
was not completed until 1952. With respect to mental arithmetic, the most
substantial reduction in requirements related to the preparatory grades (see
Appendix A.11). Mental exercises in addition and subtraction, with easy problems,
were limited to numbers to eight in Preparatory 2, to 13 in Preparatory 3 and to 19 in
Preparatory 4. For the latter, mental exercises related to counting in ones and twos
were restricted to 19, in contrast to counting in ones, twos, fives, and tens to 100
under the 1938 amendments. Halving and doubling were also reduced from
numbers to 100 to numbers to 18, with the former limit being moved to Grade I (see
Appendix A.11). Multiplication and division of whole numbers within the pupils'
range, and finding halves and quarters of numbers and quantities became part of
the oral work for Grade II, rather than for Grade I (Department of Public Instruction,
1948, pp. 5-7).
During the discussions, initiated by District Inspectors of Schools during the
mid- to late-1940s, concerning proposed syllabus amendments, the requirements for
short methods of calculation were frequently discussed, although it is apparent that
unanimity of opinion was not achieved. Based on recommendations to the Syllabus
Committee (Mathematics Subcommittee of Syllabus Committee, 1947, p. 1), a
number of short methods of calculation were specified for Grades VI and VII. In
addition to the dozens and scores rules stipulated in the 1930 Syllabus, from 1948
children were also expected to apply the rules for: (a) finding the value of 240 and

211

480 articles27, (b) calculating using the aliquot parts of one pound, (c) finding the
squares of numbers with and of those ending in 5, (d) dividing and multiplying by
25, and (e) finding the differences between pairs of square numbers (Department of
Public Instruction, 1948, p. 18). It had been recommended to the Syllabus
Committee that calculating the value of 960 articles, finding the square root by
factors and determining the difference between two squares should be deleted from
the prepared list (Mathematics Subcommittee of Syllabus Committee, 1947, p. 1).
That the deletion of the scores, 240, 480 and 960 rules was also recommended by
some teachers (McCormack, 1947, p. 1) suggests that short methods of calculation
was an area of mathematics that exceeded the intent of the 1930 Syllabus and its
1938 amendments in their implementation.
In association with the introduction of the 1952 Syllabus, changes were made to
the classification of children to more closely align the structure of primary school
classes with those of the other Australian states. The classes were restructured into
Grades I to VIII preceded by a Preparatory Grade28 of one year, the purpose of
which was to "be a real preparatory or settling-in grade" (Devries, 1951, p. 10).
Focussing on kindergarten methods, this grade was designed to provide children
with informal experiences in reading and number. Consequently, mental arithmetic
was not specifically mentioned in the requirements for the Preparatory Grade in the
1952 Syllabus.
The changes embodied in the 1948 amendments and in the 1952 mathematics
syllabus aimed "at developing skill in those calculations which men and women
have to make in daily life...[so that] children should come to see in [mathematics]
one of the indispensable tools used in all crafts and trades" (Department of Public
Instruction, 1948b, p. 17). Although additional assistance, through the notes
accompanying the content for each grade, was given to teachers concerning how
the various requirements were to be taught, few substantial alterations were made
to the syllabus from that presented in the 1948 amendments (Dagg, 1971, p. 33).
Those that were made centred on a further reduction of the level of difficulty of
the mathematics for particular grades. Although the children entering Grade I in
1952 were of a comparable age to those of previous years, the oral exercises
involving addition and subtraction were limited to numbers to 10, compared to 99
previously, the same limits as those placed on written addition and subtraction (see
Appendix A.12). Such a reduction also gave recognition to the one- rather than two-

212

year period in preparatory grades, fortuitously, given this grade's abandonment in


December 1952.
Mental multiplication and division were moved from Grade II to Grade III, as
were shopping exercises involving the use of component parts of 3d., 6d. and 1s.
Work with fractions previously undertaken in Grades I and II was deferred to Grade
III, where finding a half and a quarter of numbers and quantities were to be first
considered. The addition and subtraction of two vulgar fractions was to be first
encountered in Grade VI, with multiplication and division in Grade VII, rather than in
Grade V. A limit of 16 was also placed on the denominator, in an attempt to further
reduce the complexity and artificiality of the mental calculations with vulgar fractions.
One-step reductions of money to one pound were also moved from Grade III to
Grade IV, as were the addition and subtraction of money on which definite limits
were placedthe addition of two amounts in pence and halfpence, for example, was
not to exceed 1s, and giving change was limited to 3s. The aim was to provide
examples within the pupils' range of experience and capabilities (Department of
Public Instruction, 1952b, pp. 12-25).
Ratio, simple proportion and percentages were moved from Grade VI to Grade
VII, with the former becoming the class in which exercises involving the use of finite
decimal fractions were to be first introduced. The support given to teachers in the
accompanying notes which outlined the limits for each of the four operations with
decimals was characteristic of the additional specificity contained in the 1952
Syllabus (see Appendix A.12). A focus on mental mensuration calculations was
included for Grades V to VIII. These were designed to reflect life-like activities, with
Grade VII students, for example, required to find the areas and perimeters of paths,
borders, and walls so that construction and painting costs could be calculated
(Department of Education, 1952b, pp. 24, 26). Except for the suggestions that the
multiplication and division of decimal fractions by 10, 100, 1000 should be
undertaken by inspection (Department of Education, 1952b, p. 24; 1964, p. 25),
neither the 1952 Syllabus nor the 1964 Syllabus made specific reference to short
methods for calculating mentally. This was a position with which Crampton (1956,
p. 5) did not concur. He recommended in his report for 1955 that some practical
short methods should be prescribed in the syllabus to provide oral practice prior to
the related written work.

213

Although a revised syllabus was introduced in 1964, in essence, the 1952


Syllabus remained in use until the publication, during the late-1960s, of the Program
in Mathematics for Primary Schools (Department of Education, 1966-1968). The
1964 Syllabus was an interim document which was necessitated by the transfer of
Grade 8 to the secondary school and the planned introduction of decimal currency
in February 1966, an event which was to have a major influence on the nature of
arithmetic in the primary school. Consequently, these changes, together with
revised beliefs about how children learn and what constitutes mathematics
appropriate for primary school children, were to be of greater ultimate consequence
than the limited changes introduced in 1964 (Department of Education, 1978, p. 3).
However, in the syllabuses published during the New Maths era, mental arithmetic
was not emphasised to the extent that it had been in the syllabuses from 1860 to
1964.
The mental arithmetic for Grades 1 and 2 in the 1964 Syllabus was essentially
the same as that in the previous syllabus, except that the limit for oral addition and
subtraction exercises in Grade 1 was lowered from 10 to 9. Additional specificity
was also provided for Grades 3 to 5 for extending addition and subtraction beyond
the first extension, and for multiplication and division (see Appendix A.13). An
emphasis was placed on combined multiplication and addition to "teach [the]
processes that arise in actual multiplication and division sums" (Department of
Education, 1964, p. 12). Work with easy factors was also to be introduced in Grade
3, while factors and multiples, squares and square roots were to receive attention in
Grade 6, the former being a requirement for Grade VII under the 1952 Syllabus. In
recognition of the impending obsolescence of calculating with pounds, shillings and
pence, the mental multiplication and division of money, which had been limited to
one step reductions for Grade 5 students, was included in the syllabus only for 1964
(Department of Education, 1964, p.17).
In summary, it is evident that mental arithmetic, as embodied in Queensland
mathematics syllabuses from 1860 to 1964, was characterised by:

A gradual reduction in the complexity of the mental calculations required for


most children, in concert with the various changes introduced to the written
requirements.

214

Increased specificity as new syllabuses were developed, primarily to


provide assistance to inexperienced teachers, but also to provide limits on
the mental work to be undertaken.

A gradual lessening of an emphasis on mental arithmetic as the application


short methods of calculation, despite the importance given to them during
the 1930s and 1940s.

Little emphasis on children devising their own strategies, although the 1930
Syllabus, for example, did suggest that Grade VII children "should be given
practice in devising short cuts and easy methods" (Department of Public
Instruction, 1930, p. 45).

An emphasis on mental calculations involving the breadth of mathematical


concepts dealt with in particular grades.

The application of these concepts to situations relevant to the childrenfor


example, the "daily experience of children in the home and school"
(Department of Public Instruction, 1938, p. 14).

When compared to the size of the numbers that children were required to
operate with in their written work29, the limits that were placed on mental calculations
could be considered to have been reasonable. This, despite the gradual increase in
the limit for First Grade mental addition, for example, from 30 in the 1860 Syllabus to
99 in 1930, prior to its reduction to 10 in the 1952 Syllabus. Teachers advocated a
limit of 20 for Class I mental addition during the 1920s (The Teachers Revised
Syllabus, 1927, p. 17). However, this was opposed by some departmental officers.
Representative of their opinions was that of Farrell (1929) who asserted that "tables
and mental exercises [appeared] to have become old-fashioned. [Further,] the too
frequent use of the concrete [had] made [teachers] sacrifice the substance for the
shadow and forget also the end in the means employed" (p. 284). This was a view
indicative of the resistance exhibited towards shifting the teaching focus from the
subject to the child, a shift that was essential if teachers were to acknowledge the
need for placing mental calculations in contexts of relevance to children.

3.5.3 Mental Arithmetic as Implemented

215

The General Introduction to the 1930 Syllabus recognised that any curriculum
may be viewed as (a) a course of study, and (b) as a program of experiences and
activities. Further, it was recognised that "while it is possible to prescribe a definite
course of study, it is not possible to specify what activities the teachers should
devise for the educational benefit of the children" (Department of Public Instruction,
1930, p. vii). The use which teachers make of a prescribed syllabus varies from
teacher to teacher (Edwards, 1937a, p. 25) and is dependent upon such factors as
their understanding of syllabus aims and content, their use of appropriate teaching
strategies, and on the constraints placed on teacher interpretation of these factors.
This was perceived by "Scipio" (1943, pp. 13-14) in his attack on those teachers
who were critical of the 1930 Syllabus in its failure to foster sustained changes in
teaching. Their criticisms, which were anchored in the assumption that positive
changes could be effected by merely adjusting Syllabus content, failed to appreciate
"that the printed programme of work followed by a school is but one of the factors
which determine the quality of work done therein" ("Scipio, 1943, p. 13). Turney
(1972, p. 45) suggested that, by the early 1930s, the syllabus and classroom
practice as well as practice and syllabus ideals were characterised by considerable
schisms, the former dichotomy being particularly applicable to mental arithmetic.
Except for rare commendationsfor example, those of Hendy (1953, p. 2),
Moorhouse (1941, p. 1), Mutch (1907, p. 70), and Pestorius (1939, p. 64)District
Inspectors of Schools were consistently highly critical of the standard of mental
calculation and of the teaching methods used during the period 1860-1965. In the
first District Inspector's report to be published, it was noted that "mental
arithmetic...[was] not generally advanced, [and that] this subject [cannot] be
considered satisfactory till children [could] solve, mentally, a questioninvolving
small numerical operationsin every rule through which they...passed" (Anderson,
1870, p. 14). District Inspector Benbow (1911, p. 67) considered that the absence
of success in mental operations was rather remarkable, particularly considering that
the examples given during his inspections were predominantly easy shopping
transactions, examples in accordance with the third principle delineated in the 1904
Schedule: "The work of the pupil [should be brought] into closer touch with his home
and social surroundings" (Department of Public Instruction, 1904, p. 52).
This principle, reemphasised in the 1914 and 1930 Syllabuses, was one that
District Inspector Hendren (1939, p. 55) would have liked to have seen more

216

evidence of its implementation, a view foreshadowed by District Inspector Canny


(1893), when noting that "the efforts to deal with the easy shopping transactions'
were feeble in the extreme" (p. 102). He stated that he had "little admiration for
what were known as calculation tricks,' and would [have preferred] to see pupils
trained in the more solid mental work of dealing with practical problems, such as
they [would] meet with in their everyday lives" (Canny, 1893, p. 102). Although it
was not uncommon for inspectors to report that children lacked facility with the
calculation of every-day transactions, it was more often noted that a greater
proficiency was demonstrated with the difficult mechanical operations, involving
fractions, for example (Fewtrell, 1914, p. 70). This observation was supported by
Crampton, who, in 1955, noted that there was a "tendency on the part of some
[teachers] to limit Oral Arithmetic to purely mechanical processes, [with] too few
exercises...given of the problem type" (Crampton, 1955, p. 3), thus leading to poor
performances on such examples (Searle, 1958, p. 15).
In his report for 1927, District Inspector Palfrey (1928) observed that "mental
arithmetic [was] the outstandingly weak subject of schools. Even where it [was]
regularly practised, and where thought, care, and attention [were] bestowed upon
the teaching, results [were] frequently disappointing to all concerned" (p. 106), a
view supported by District Inspector Taylor (1928, p. 43). Although the effectiveness
of the 1930 Syllabus and its amendments depended on the spiral nature of the
arithmetic content, with new topics being introduced mentally in one class before
preceding to the written in the next, Hendren (1939) was to conclude from his
inspections that mental arithmetic had "not yet assumed the new (sic) significance
envisaged in the amended syllabus" (p. 55), namely, as a preparation for written
work, a significance previously emphasised in the 1904 and 1914 Syllabuses. That
Hendren (1939, p. 55) was to word his conclusion in this manner suggests that this
use of mental arithmetic was one with which teachers were not readily cognisant.
Ross (1894, p. 75) and Benbow (1905, p. 7) had previously stressed that mental
arithmetic should not be taken as a separate lesson, except for the purposes of
testing, but should be taught in conjunction with the written arithmetic lesson.
When reporting on mental arithmetic, it was not uncommon for District
Inspectors to note that it was a difficult topic. Acting District Inspector Kemp (1913)
explained that "when valuing results in this branch, one does not expect to find the
same percentage of correct answers as in written work. A 50 per cent. result would

217

be valued at more than moderate'" (p. 59). A similar view was held by Benbow
(1926, p. 52), who, for one-teacher schools, regarded a proficiency of forty percent
as being quite satisfactory. District Inspector Kemp (1929, p. 47) believed that a fair
or moderate result in mental arithmetic may actually be considered as being good or
very good. In highlighting characteristics of mental computation, similar to those
discussed in Chapter 2, he suggested that:

Teachers should not expect results in mental arithmetic to equal those obtained
in written work; the two branches vary considerably. The former demanding not
only method of working, but a good memory and a visualisation of the figures
representing the quantities involved. (C. Kemp, 1929, p. 47)

In response to the renewed emphasis on developing the ability to calculate


speedily and accurately that was embodied with the 1952 Syllabus, District
Inspector Thistlethwaite (1954) reported:

Oral work was often weak [with] many children [taking] far too long to work
simple types in mechanical addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division of
numbers, money, and weights and measures....There was a tendency to neglect
the achievement of speed and accuracy in the use of the three extensions and,
often, little use was made of them in oral work after the children had passed
through Grade III. (p. 2)

Prior to the introduction of the 1930 Syllabus, District Inspectors Trudgian (1929, p.
109) and Farrell (1929, p. 290) had decried that speed and accuracy in mental work
were being neglected. In their view, this neglect arose from too much time being
spent on trying to understand problems rather than focussing on achieving correct
answers, and from the practice of doing unnecessary work on paper.
Although children of Grades 3 to 7 in 1946-47 were spending approximately
22% of their mathematics time on mental arithmetic30 (ACER, 1949, pp. 20-21), it
was neglect to which District Inspectors regularly attributed the poor results in
mental arithmetic during the period under investigation (Bevington, 1926, p. 80;
Farrell, 1927, p. 104; Fox, 1905, p. 9; Gutekunst, 1958, p. 8; Platt, 1901, p. 59;
Scott, 1892, p. 92). District Inspector Johnston (1920, p. 98) considered the efforts

218

of many teachers to be perfunctory, a view shared by Farrell (1927, p. 104) who


deemed the work in mental arithmetic in some schools to be valueless. That mental
arithmetic received little, if any attention, in some schools during the mid-1920s was
evidenced, in Bevington's (1927) opinion, by the children's response to his saying,
"Now we'll have a few tests in mental." He noted that "often the children [seemed]
bewildered, [picked] up [their] slates, [attempted] to work on their slates, [and didn't]
wait to be told to write answers down" (p. 77). Bevington (1927, p. 77) noted further
that it was quite uncommon to hear a mental arithmetic lesson in progress on
entering a school.
A contributing factor, however, to the response reported by Bevington (1927)
may have been the view of inspectors as "strict, dour, humourless characters of
whom the teacher lived in dread because their salaries depended on [their reports]"
(Connors, 1984, p. 95). Representative of their approach to inspection, at least
during the early years of the Department, is the observation that usually there was:
No cheery word [to encourage] the children when they [displayed] proficiency,
accuracy, and readiness in...useful and practical calculations. Mark, on the
contrary, the inspector's disparaging grimace, when they fail to solve some
intricate problem dictatedonce onlyin a vague and puzzling form. ("Z, 1879,
p. 768)

For children to become proficient in mental arithmetic, "regular and systematic


treatment" (Crampton, 1955, p. 6) was required, a view supported by District
Inspectors George (1930, p. 56), Ross (1894, p. 75), and Anderson (1872, p. 11),
the latter reporting that mental arithmetic was not sufficiently practised, particularly
in the girls' school31. District Inspector Fox (1918, p. 61) noted that many teachers
seemed to dislike the mental arithmetic lesson, their bte noir (Mental Arithmetic,
1927, p. 18), and that this was a reason for its not receiving the full time allotted to it
in the timetables. Teachers were often criticised for not appreciating the usefulness
of mental arithmetic (Canny, 1893, p. 102; Smith, 1914, p. 78)its social utility as
well as its "teaching value, when and if scientifically applied" (Woodgate, 1955, p.
2). Further, Fox (1908) had concluded in his report for 1907 that "it [would] evidently
take a good deal of time to convince some teachers that a child's proficiency in
arithmetic [was] not measured solely by his ability to add five lines of five figures'"
(p. 74).

219

It was also recognised by some District Inspectors that mental arithmetic made
larger demands on a teacher's time, energy, and ability than written work (Canny,
1910, p. 58), particularly in small schools where multiple grades had to be taught. In
many instances, teachers were admonished in their annual reports, and by
implication during their inspections of schools, for not sufficiently preparing for
mental arithmetic lessons (Benbow, 1911, p. 67; Benbow, 1925, p. 51; Harrap,
1908, pp. 46-47; Inglis, 1926, p. 97; Inglis, 1929, p. 86; Kemp, 1917, p. 96;
Radcliffe, 1898, p. 73). Representative of these comments is that of Searle (1956)
who emphasised that:

In very few cases would teachers produce lists of examples that had been
definitely taught to the class when they were asked to do so. [Such] teachers
[imagined] that they [could give] mental exercises extempore, and [did] not stop
to consider that the questions asked [were] of a very stereotyped nature calling
for little reasoning. (p. 7)

Kemp (1917, p. 96) considered the preparation of examples of such importance that
he gave consideration, in his report for 1916, to recommending that failure to do so
should lead to a teacher being officially reprimanded.
That examples were not "properly graded out of school hours" (Benbow, 1911,
p. 67) and matched to the average mental ages of the children in the class (Mutch,
1925, p. 48) often resulted in the examples used being considered as "too
promiscuous...[with] the aims of the lessons too indefinite or too imperfectly
realised" (George, 1930, p. 56). This was a judgement in accordance with the
position of the 1930 Syllabus, which considered that "promiscuous work [was] of
little value" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 33). Given the lack of
preparation considered appropriate, many teachers relied on questions taken
directly from textbooks, "without plan or purpose" (Kemp, 1915, p. 98), a practice
regularly criticised by district inspectors (Cochran, 1955, p. 2; Denniss, 1933, p. 31;
Farrell, 1929, p. 286; Jeffrey, 1930, p. 68). Such a reliance precluded a teacher
from making the mental work "a live part" of the curriculum (Denniss, 1927, p. 62).
Searle (1958, p. 15) expressed concern for the brighter children in the classes of
teachers who indicated that were working through the textbook, albeit the one
supplied by the Department. In Searles (1958, p. 15) view, such children, society's

220

future scientists and engineers, required a different approach to mathematics in the


primary school, an approach which the New Maths curricula of the 1960s and 1970s
had hoped to provide.
The slavish use of the departmental written arithmetic cards32 was also seen as
a reason for mental arithmetic being of a low standard in schools (Somers, 1928, p.
84). Moorhouse (1941) commented that "the best work was [observed] in schools
which regularly [took mental arithmetic and were] working along planned lines with
specified types of work daily taken" (p. 3). Such was the perceived need for
carefully planned examples that District Inspector Fowler (1923) called for a series
of "[practical] exercises in logical sequence" (p. 54) to be arranged for each of the
classes. However, this suggestion was not contained in the model syllabus
compiled in 1924 by the Queensland Teachers' Union, nor in the departmental
syllabus introduced in 1930.
Nonetheless, in Lidgate's (1959) view, it was the teaching approaches used
which "may to a degree [have been] the cause of...very few schools [reaching] a
standard above fair in oral arithmetic" (p. 5). It was held that teachers did not
sufficiently recognise that each day's mental arithmetic should have been based on
the written work to followto teach and test the types and processes involved.
Further, many teachers wrote the examples on the blackboard allowing the children
an unlimited amount of time to obtain the answers (Crampton, 1959, p. 9), thus
ignoring the intended focus on the development of speed and accuracy (Department
of Public Instruction, 1952b, p. 1). In contrast to this view, Kemp (1929) had
advocated that "questions in mental arithmetic should be put on the blackboard
much more freely than [was] usually done [as] in after life the child [would] nearly
always be called upon to work mentally what is written" (p. 47).
In instances where examples were presented orally to the class, Farrell (1929),
reiterating the comments he made in his 1926 report (Farrell, 1927, p. 104),
described the typical approach thus:

The usual practice [was] for the teacher to give a sum. In many cases the
question [was] repeated. The bright pupils put up hands and shake them up
and down, thereby distracting the attention of many earnest pupils who [were]
trying faithfully to work the sum. The lazy children simply [did] nothing. After a
time the answer [was] written. Then the teacher [started] to do all the work. He

221

generally [worked] it on a board, which [was] little help for the visual process
required. It [was] also the bright pupils who [did] the work and who already
[had] the correct answer. The backward pupils again [did] nothing. After all
this, the teacher [went] cheerfully on with another type of problem, and so the
lesson [proceeded]. There [was] no attempt at individual work or at individual
diagnosis to discover the causes of the failures, and the backward pupils [were]
hardly ever called upon to do any of the work. (pp. 289-290)

By so doing, it was considered that teachers were not encouraging children to


think clearly and systematically about number (Smith, 1917, p. 82), which was
probably one of the causes for pupils simply taking it for granted that they could not
do mental arithmetic (Lidgate, 1954, p. 2). In consequence of the way in which
mental arithmetic lessons were taken, "many teachers, especially young ones,
[were] apt to [have made] mental arithmetic an examination lesson instead of
teaching the subject. It [was] thus made obnoxious, instead of being a pleasant
effort" ("Howard, 1899, p. 79). Despite the changes made to the syllabus, this view
of mental arithmetic continued to be a common one during the period under
investigation (Crampton, 1954, p. 3; George, 1934, p. 29; Kehoe, 1955, p. 2; Pyle,
1959, p. 9), to which previous references have been made.
Instrumental in the maintenance of this view were factors primarily beyond the
control of the teacher, namely, their level of training, and the size and structure of
classes taught, in context with the systems of pupil examination and teacher
inspection. It was not until the 1970s that the Department of Education recognised
that changes in curriculum content and method could not be successfully imposed
unilaterally and that their adoption by teachers requires the provision of adequate
support (Department of Education, 1978, p. 4). Whenever a new syllabus is to be
introduced, a large percentage of teachers and senior officers are in need of reeducation (Greenhalgh, 1957, p. 327). With respect to the 1930 Syllabus,
Greenhalgh (1957) noted that "little was done to assist teachers, other than to hold
group meetings33, resided over by District Inspectors" (p. 39), at which the more
highly qualified teachers assisted the less experienced (Edwards, 1931, p. 28), a
questionable process for improving mental arithmetic, given the reports of District
Inspectors as to its neglect in schools. Connor (1954, p. 19) suggested that the
spirit of the 1952 Syllabus was not universally adopted as the majority of senior

222

teachers had received their training up to 40 years earlier and had neither the
opportunity nor the time to keep abreast of modern developments in mathematics
education.
Further, it was from this group of teachers that District Inspectors and Head
Teachers were drawn, many of whom would have been trained under the pupilteacher system. Although they may have become competent teachers, the early
years of their training at least, would have been attained at the expense of the
scholars (Department of Public Instruction, 1928, p. 41). Some were as young as
14 years of age, receiving their teacher training before and following the school day
(Logan & Clarke, 1984, p. 2), thus providing an added difficulty to meeting the
advocacy by District Inspectors for out-of-school preparation of mental arithmetic
examples34. Many of the teachers employed were untrained, particularly in
Provisional Schools where their disproportionate number prior to 190935 was
instrumental in minimising the overall standard of teaching (Logan & Clarke, 1984,
p. 2). As the Queensland Teachers' Journal editorialised in 1936, "it [was] still
possible for Queensland children to be placed in charge of a teacher who...had no
previous teaching experience, no technical training, nor been given any opportunity
to study the theory of modern educational practice (Professional Standards, 1936,
p. 1).
Additionally, little professional development was gained from inspectors during
their visits, in contrast to the hopes expressed following the changes in inspection
procedures introduced in 1904: "[The inspector] can [now] be what he is intended to
be, the professional adviser and assistant to the teacher. He can go where he is
best needed, and can also leave quickly where he is not needed" (The New
Syllabus, 1904, p. 142). Such did not occur until the compulsory inspection of
teachers was discontinued from 1970. As noted by the staff of Albert State School
in 1947: "The present hurried methods make it difficult for the inspector to do
anything but inspect; he has little time to instruct teachers in new methods nor has
he sufficient opportunity to listen to the teacher expressing his ideas" (p. 1).
Not only were Queensland teachers faced with the inadequacy of their training,
but also with the physical lay-out of the school, and the size36 and structure of the
classes that they had to teach. The size of the classes was often exacerbated by
the organisation of classes designed to reduce the numbers in the Scholarship
Class (Fletcher, 1931, p. 51; Pizzy, 1950, p. 17). Mutch (1916) suggested that

223

where a large school consisted of comparatively few classrooms, as was frequently


the case, the quietness required for mental arithmetic was difficult to find: "The
subject appeals through hearing, and [with] the majority of pupils being eye-minded
and ear-minded, extra quietness [was] needed" (p. 62).
One consequence of large classes was that teachers tended to maintain order
by keeping the children "quiet and occupied in sedentary work, and to treat them in
the mass rather than individually" (Connor, 1954, p. 19), factors which may have
contributed to mental arithmetic either being neglected or being treated as a topic to
be tested rather than taught. Such was particularly true for teachers of multiple
grades, most notably those in one-teacher schools37 who tended to be the less
experienced (Cramer, 1936, p. 8). In relation to such teachers, Edwards (1931)
commented:

The inexperienced teacher [found] it difficult to organize his school properly, to


limit the number of drafts, to distribute his time in such a way that all of the
classes [received] a fair share of his attention, and to ensure that while he [was]
engaged with one class, the others [were] profitably employed. (p. 27)

Benbow (1925) declared in his report for 1924 that it was impossible for
teachers in one-teacher schools to devote time to mental arithmetic because "during
the slate arithmetic lesson of one class the teacher usually is actively employed in
the actual teaching of another subject to one or more of his remaining classes" (p.
51). One method suggested to teachers to enable children to practice mental
arithmetic by themselves, was for an exercise of the form presented in Table 3.3 to
be written on the blackboard, preferably prior to the commencement of class. It was
maintained that such an approach allowed quick workers to work more examples
and to proceed at their own pace; the advantage being that they did not have to wait
for the slow children as in an oral lesson. Further, as the exercises "[were] not
wordy they [could] be read more quickly than mental' sums printed on cards or in
books" (Mental Arithmetic, 1927, p. 18). However, although such exercises could
focus on a range of number types, their use would not necessarily have contributed
to the use of mental arithmetic as an introduction to the written work to follow,
particularly where that work involved problems, nor would it have significantly
contributed to developing a facility with everyday calculations.

224

225

Table 3.3
Extract From Recommended Mental Arithmetic Exercise for "Middle Standards" for
Use by Teachers of Multiple Classes

This figure is not available online.


Please consult the hardcopy thesis
available from the QUT Library

The uncompromising nature of mental arithmetic as implementedgiven the


uncertainty that it provoked in the minds of children, with its emphasis on
testingwas possibly enhanced by the generally authoritarian atmosphere of the
classroom during the period being investigated.

Inspectors and teachers were subject to a long tradition which stamped the
school as a heavily authoritarian institution. The accumulated experience of
teachers in the system led them to believe that strict order, the threat of
sanctions, repetition, drill and cramming were likely to achieve results in
examinations and during the inspector's visit. (England, 1971, p. 193)

Contributing to this atmosphere was the rigid view of arithmetic and how it
should be taught, characteristic of educators during the period being investigated.
As previously discussed, children needed to be trained to think in logical sequence
and to learn the value of setting out mathematical processes in precise, ordered
steps (Cox & MacDonald, n.d., pp. 180-181). Such a conviction was reflected in the
recommendations that the teacher of arithmetic needed to be a strong disciplinarian
(Cox & MacDonald, n.d., p. 214; Gladman, n.d., p. 77).

226

This authoritarian atmosphere was enhanced in Queensland primary schools


by the emphasis placed on the Scholarship Examination (Schonell, 1955, p. 5), at
least from the late 1920s, as the "be-all and end-all of education at the primary level"
(Dagg, 1971, p. 47). Despite protestations to the contrary (Edwards, 1929, p. 32), it
was a belief held by class teachers, Head Teachers, parents and by some senior
departmental officers, including District Inspectors (Dagg, 1971, p. 47). To ensure
that children passed Scholarship, they, in Pizzy's (1950) view, were "driven hard,
bellowed at, scolded, caned, detained, overloaded with homework and crammed full
with a host of useless facts, forgotten almost as soon as they were learnt, and
denied a full and broad education" (p. 17).
Although the effects of the need for children to pass the Scholarship
Examinationan outcome on which both teachers and schools were judged
(Connor, 1954, p. 19; Dagg, 1971, p. 47; "The Syllabus, 1934, p. 1)impacted
primarily on the Scholarship Class, it indirectly affected the learning of children in
lower classes, in ways other than its impact on class size. This, despite Director of
Education Edwards' (1937a) proposition that the "examination [would] become an
evil" (p. 6) if the examination were to dominate a school's outlook. One of its
consequences, in association with the regular testing of classes by head teachers,
was the concentration on skills which were readily testable (ACER, 1964, Annexure
2, p. 2), skills that were able to be tested by written rather than oral methods.
Further, it is likely that this may have been a contributing factor to the interpretation
of mental arithmetic as entailing testing rather than teaching, an interpretation
supported by the nature of available text-books and Teachers' Notes contained in
the Queensland Teachers' Journal in which the exercises presented were
predominantly in the form of sets of questions38, often context-free.
Representative of the recommendations to teachers was a method for
stimulating mental arithmetic outlined by "J.R.D." (1928, p. 7), a regular contributor
to the Queensland Teacher's Journal during the late 1920s and early 1930s. This
method involved giving mental a few minutes before dismissal time and allowing the
pupils to leave as they gained correct answers to questions. The exercises
presented (see Table 3.4) were "those excellent examples which [had] been given
of late years in the scholarship examinations [for the Sixth Class], differing from
mental arithmetic only in the use of paper or slate in working" ("J.R.D., 1928, p. 7).
Although some of the examples could be considered within the scope of the 1914

227

Syllabus for the Fifth Class (see Appendix A.8), manyfor example: .142857 times a
certain number is 18.83, what is the number?went beyond the spirit of its
requirements, and were ones which would have proved very difficult. As "J.R.D."
(1928) records, "The slower ones seldom completed them" (p. 8). The focus, at
least with the decimal examples (see Table 3.4), was on mental calculation by the
rote application of rules, as evidenced by his exhortation for teachers to "get from
the pupils that these are division sums and [that] points must be moved accordingly"
("J.R.D., 1928, p. 8).
The use of such procedures suggests that teachers went beyond the
requirements of the syllabus, as previously discussed, and that the scholarship
mathematics papers influenced the work undertaken in lower grades. The papers
were considered by some to be the official interpretation of the Syllabus (The Real
Issue, 1949, p. 2), at least for the Scholarship Class, and more generally exerted a
firm control over the curriculum in the primary school (Barcan, 1980, p. 9; G. A.
Jones, 1979, p. 20). "The...Commentator" (1947) concluded that "the curriculum for
[grade seven] in our schools [was] the Scholarship examination. The curriculum for
the other grades [was] that part of the 1930 Curriculum which the inspectors deal
with in their annual examination of the school" (p. 21).
Although notation was listed as a separate topic in syllabuses from 1930, its
teaching could be categorised under the intended definition of oral arithmeticas
that in which explanation and discussion were of paramount importance. In some
mental arithmetic texts available to teachers, some of the sets of questions
contained notation items. For example, in a text designed to meet the requirements
of the 1952 Syllabus, Grade VII children were asked to "Write 11 million" and to
"Make 101.03 ten times greater" (Class Teacher's Manual, n.d., p. 12). With the
introduction of the 1948 Amendments it had been stressed that place value
questions should be kept reasonably simple (Greenhalgh, 1947, p. 12).

228

Table 3.4
Examples of Written Items from the 1925 Mathematics Scholarship Paper Given to
Fifth Class Children as Mental

This figure is not available online.


Please consult the hardcopy thesis
available from the QUT Library

Such a statement had become necessary as notation had "become a mental


gymnastics exercise" (Radford, 1947, p. 1) with "some inspectors, head teachers
and compilers of text-books [appearing] to delight in devising an almost infinite
number of questions on place value and relationships'" (Comments, 1947, p. 1)
that went beyond the scope of the syllabus. For example, the Fassifern Branch of
the Queensland Teachers' Union (1947, p. 1) noted that some inspectors were
using such examples as:

101,101,101.00001
b

What decimal of b is a?
(Presented orally. Time allowed: two seconds)

"Twenty-Keep List" (1942, p. 14) had protested against the practice of problemising
place values. He suggested that it had been intensified and perpetuated by the
monthly examinations set by head teachers. These examinations often "resulted in
the disappointment and mystification of the pupils, [and] chagrin at failure to the
teacher who...[had] not taken the types' so cleverly and secretly worked out"
(Twenty-Keep List, 1942, p. 14). Nonetheless, although the complexity of the
calculations may have been beyond the 1930 Syllabus and may not have been

229

appropriate as mental arithmetic, the nature of such examples was not in conflict
with the syllabus. For Grade V, it had been suggested that children could be asked
"How often is the least one' contained in the greatest one' [in 101.011]?"
(Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 40), a question designed to assist
teachers in "developing the intelligence [of children]" (Department of Public
Instruction, 1930, p. 30).

3.6

Recommended Approaches to Teaching Mental Arithmetic


From the foregoing analyses, it is evident that there were three essential factors

for effectively teaching mental arithmetic during the period 1860-1965. These were:
(a) regular and systematic treatment, (b) prior preparation of graded examples, and
(c) basing the mental examples on the written work which was to follow. In
providing for mental arithmetic that was regular and systematic, teachers were
advised that such work should form part of every arithmetic lesson, in contrast to
being taught in isolation (Benbow, 1925, p. 52; Department of Public Instruction,
1914, p. 61; Joyce, 1881, p. 208; Kennedy, 1887, p. 82; "Successful Mental
Arithmetic, 1899, p. 79). The latter, however, was encouraged by the nature of
many of the commercial texts. In one typical of those published from the 1930s,
teachers were advised that the textbook provided "daily sets [of examples] for four
days a week for nine months" (Olsen, 1953, p. i). Such a format did not encourage
nor facilitate the linking of mental and written work, as recommended in the
syllabuses. Nevertheless, some textbooksThe Queensland Arithmetic (Thompson,
1930), for examplerecognised that "no text-book [could] take the place of a skilful
teacher" (p. 1). Through the notes accompanying the 1914 Syllabus, teachers had
been advised not to "trust to any text-book, but should prepare their own series of
questions, adapted to local conditions and needs" (Department of Public Instruction,
1914, p. 70). Somewhat paradoxically, although the 1930 Syllabus did not prescribe
any particular reference, it did recommend Pitman's Mental and Intelligence Tests in
Common-Sense Arithmetic (Potter, n.d.)39 as an appropriate text for teachers to use.
Under the 1860 Regulations for Queensland primary schools, teachers were
permitted only to use the books40 sanctioned by the Board, unless its approval had
been specially obtained (Regulations, 1860, p. 8). In practice, however, other
books were used by teachers for lesson preparation. Margaret Berry, Head

230

Mistress of the Central School for Girls, informed the 1874 Royal Commission on
Education that although her teachers did not have children use any textbook not
authorised by the Board, other texts were used for lesson preparation (Minutes of
Evidence, 1875, p. 96). The list of authorised books published in the 1875
Regulations, which included Moffats Mental Arithmetic, was "intended to show what
books teachers [were] empowered to place, when necessary, in the hands of their
pupils and pupil-teachers" (List of Books, 1880, p. 23). It was further stated that
although they were not confined to the listed books in preparing for their teaching,
they would "be held responsible for the character of [their] lessons" (List of Books,
1880, p. 23).
Effective mental arithmetic lessons were believed to be ones that were relatively
short and conducted in the mornings when children's minds were fresh (Baker,
1929, p. 274; Drain, 1941, p. 2; Teaching Hints, 1908, p. 15). Mental calculation
was considered to be taxing and therefore inadvisable for the strain to be
maintained for long (Gladman, 1904, p. 207; Robinson, 1882, p. 180). Although
some (Joyce, 1881, p. 212; Mental Arithmetic, 1910, p. 176) recommended five to
ten minutes at a maximum, "Howard" (1899, p. 79) suggested that at least fifteen
minutes should be spent early each morning on mental work, with children working
ten or twelve sums. "The profit of the lesson [was judged to be] in proportion to the
number of questions that [had] been answered correctly" (Joyce, 1881, p. 207;
Gladman, 1904, p. 215), thus suggesting an emphasis on the answer rather than on
the strategy used. Supporting this view, Taylor (1928, p. 43) reported that a method
used by a successful teacher involved regular practice for short periods in which the
examples were presented in constantly changing forms and where explanations
were characterised by their brevity. Such was required if children were to develop
the power of concentrating the mind, the development of which required a degree of
effort which few children found easy to make (Martin, 1916, p. 135).
Nonetheless, Gladman (1904), somewhat contradictory to his earlier statement,
also promoted the cultivating of ingenuity through a focus on the strategy used:
Children should be encouraged "to work by different methods. Get them to explain
how they work; their explanation, with [the teacher's] comments, [would] do great
good, especially if [the teacher could] show a readier method" (p. 208). Such an
approach was alluded to in the 1930 Syllabus for Grade I, whereby pupils were
required to give oral statements of the various steps involved in solving mental

231

problemssteps that entailed stating the problem, providing reasons for successive
operations, specifying the rules used for calculating the answer and describing their
working (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 33). Prior to this, it had been
recommended in The Practical Mental Arithmetic, initially issued to schools in
1902, that the "composition of numbers and easy methods of dealing with numbers
should be taught"for example, in mentally calculating 99 + 67, children "should at
once observe the facility of taking 1 from 67 to add to 99" ("An Inspector of Schools,
1914, p. 5).
As recognised by "J.R.D." (1928, p. 7), with the syllabuses from 1905 instructing
teachers to use mental exercises as a preparation for written work, both mechanical
and problem, it was expected that mental arithmetic lessons would be thoroughly
planned. The 1930 Syllabus suggested, albeit for Grade I but applicable to other
grades, as evidenced by inspectors' comments, that "exercises should be well
graded and suited to the average intelligence of the class." (Department of Public
Instruction, 1930, p. 33). Teachers were advised to record the planned examples in
special notebooks for future reference and revision (Fewtrell, 1917, p. 74; Harrap,
1910, p. 56; Shirley, 1913, p. 37). Gladman (n.d., p. 75) had recommended that the
difficulty of the examples should increase until the child needed to use slate and
pencil, the implementation of which would have required an individual or group
approach, one that was not seriously experimented with above Grade II in
Queensland primary schools until the 1960s (Pyle, 1965, p.3). Acting District
Inspector Martin (1916, p. 135) asserted that the use of carefully graded exercises
that lead in steps from the easy to the more difficult would assist children to develop
confidence and determination, key factors in being able to calculate mentally. Such
gradations applied to problem as well as to mechanical work. It was considered
important for children to master problems through their own working and in so doing
gain the self-confidence necessary for attempting further problems even of unlike
types (Farrell, 1929, p. 286).
In consequence of the opinion that the imagination played an important part in
mental arithmetic, it was suggested that questions should be framed so as to "fire
the imagination of the children" (Grade III Mental Arithmetic, 1936, p. 14). This
involved placing the arithmetic into detailed contexts. The long questions41 which
resulted and the reduced number of questions treated during a session were not
seen as difficulties. It was considered more important to exercise the imaginations

232

of the children than "to give them a greater number and have [the pupils] guessing
the answers" (Grade III Mental Arithmetic, 1936, p. 14). A countervailing view
("J.R.D., 1928, p. 7) was that teachers needed to be watchful of the wordiness of a
problem so that the children were not distracted from focusing on the arithmetic
within the setting provided. Nonetheless, it was considered important to place the
numbers used within a context so as to familiarise pupils with the working of
problems (Kennedy, 1889, p. 92).
Significantly, the 1930 Syllabus, in accordance with Ballard's (1927) belief that
"the most profitable form of oral arithmetic...is not that which consisted [of] casual
questions" (p. 18), stated that promiscuous work was considered to be inappropriate
(Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 33). Such work:

Nearly always [meant] asking the first questions that [came] into [a teacher's
head]questions that falsify the importance of tea or sugar, dozens or scores, in
the general scheme of things, and that bear but little relationship either to the
body of mathematical knowledge which the children have already acquired, or to
the new material which they are about to study. As a mental exercise it [was]
casual and fitful, and less closely related to the pupils' needs than to the
grooves in the questioner's mind. (Ballard, 1928b, p. i)

It was necessary for every lesson to have some point to teach: a special rule to
be learned, a short method of calculation or the revision of difficulties (C. Kemp,
1917, p. 96). Only one type, in contrast to the range of types generally presented in
each of the sets of examples in textbooks, was to be the focus in any lesson
(Bevington, 1922, p. 58; "J.R.D., 1928, p. 7; Moorhouse, 1927, p. 96; "Successful
Mental Arithmetic, 1899, p. 79), a recommendation that was made prior to the
introduction of the 1930 Syllabus and the influence of a multiplicity of quasi-official
textbooks. When teaching short methods of mental calculation, Park (1879, p. 13)
stressed that rules developed through rote learning should be avoided. It was
essential that:

Instruction in mental arithmetic should be imparted in such a way that it might


be helpful to the practical business of life, and to prevent the possibility of the
subject being taught in that worthless manner of which "exhibitions" are often

233

given in public examinations. Every true teacher who knows "these tricks of the
trade" gives the practice his unhesitating condemnation" (Park, 1879, p. 43)

Gladman (1904) believed that, when teaching short methods, oral exercises
should not be limited to the "strict rule, [but] to go a little way on each side" (p.
208)for example, when focusing on the rule for finding the cost of 100 articles it
was considered useful to have children find the price of such quantities as 98 or 102
articles. Nonetheless, Macgroarty (1879, p. 71) cautioned that any focus on short
methods of calculation should not be at the expense of suitable examples on the
procedures necessary for slate work.
As already implied, mental arithmetic was seen to have had a role in each of the
three clearly marked stages in teaching new arithmetic, namely, (a) the theoretical
or introductory phase in which children were to come to understand the processes
involved in a new rule, (b) the practical or mechanical stage during which neatness,
speed and accuracy were developed, and (c) the application stage during which the
skills acquired were applied to problems (Board of Education, 1937, p. 504;
Teaching Hints, 1908, p. 16). The role of mental examples during the first stage
was to teach the "principle of the [operation],...worked out on the blackboard in such
a way that the method of working [was] clearly seen" (Cox & MacDonald, n.d., 219).
Following the introduction of the 1904 Syllabus, such teaching was to occur in
conjunction with tangible objects, particularly for easier types of addition and
subtraction, using examples involving subject-matter within the children's
experiences (Baker, 1929, p. 274; Teaching of Arithmetic, 1927, p. 292). From this
oral procedure, the method of working was to be determined before proceeding to
the written (Gladman, n.d., p. 74). For each step in the process, teaching and test
exercises were to be prepared. It was considered essential that such mental work
should be based on that related to the application of tables, especially "the extended
addition table, and the practical use of the Multiplication, Money, and Weights and
Measures Tables" (Denniss, 1927, p. 62).
In practice, however, the introduction of new work "largely centred on
demonstration followed by the pupils' working of [written] examples" (Dagg, 1971, p.
75). Martin (1920b, p. 98) pointed out that this method was reasonable for the mere
application of rules and formulae. However, he considered that such an approach
provided for little mental development, which should have been the main aim of

234

every lesson, besides providing for any additions to a pupil's knowledge. Farrell
(1939) noted that there had been a tendency to test new work immediately after its
presentation, at a time when "pupils have hazy ideas, and imperfect knowledge, and
while they lack confidence" (p. 44). Although Farrell (1939) was referring to
teaching in general, his recommendations to teachers during staff talks that
discussion should be allowed for between presentation and testing is reminiscent of
the intended meaning of oral arithmetic.
Recommendations for the use of aids in mental calculation, which came with the
espoused concern for the child from the 1890s, contrasts with Macgroarty's (1879,
p. 71; 1886, p. 63; 1902, p. 67) abhorrence of the use of finger counting, in
particular, and with Joyce's (1881, p. 213) rule for calculating mentally, another
source of the authoritarian nature of mental arithmetic lessons. Joyce (1881)
believed that:

The children, while calculating, should not be allowed to mutter audibly, or even
to move the lips or distort the face; and remember not to let them count on their
fingers. There should be, in fact, no exterior manifestation of the interior
intellectual exertion; the first thing heard should be the answer. (p. 213)

As a method for catering for differing abilities, Olsen (1953, p. i) suggested that
children could occasionally record partial answers as a prop during mental
calculation, a procedure supported by Park (1881, p. 43) and Robinson (1882, p.
180). Farrell (1929), however, advised that the "practice of doing unnecessary work
on paper [resulted] in loss of speed and deterioration in mental work generally" (p.
289). He also suggested that, on occasions, children could be compelled to perform
all the calculations mentally. The Board of Education (1937) stressed that the rule
"Never do work on paper that can be done mentally' often needs to be emphasised"
(p. 514), a procedure with which District Inspector Brown (1901) would have
concurred: The "best results in mental arithmetic [were] secured in schools in which
the pupils [were] trained to perform mentally many of the operations for which the
slate or blackboard [was] commonly used" (p. 85). Alternatively, Kemp (1914, p.
101) advocated using written arithmetic lessons for practice in mental arithmetic by
not requiring children to record every elementary step in the working of a written
calculation, a suggestion with which Gladman (n.d., p. 78) would have concurred,

235

given his advocating an explicit connection between mental and written work
through allowing children to use more efficient mental methods for particular
examples set as written workfor example, by expecting the "elder boys to multiply
by 25 and 125 in one line, and by 2,884,816 in three lines, exclusive of the answer"
(Gladman, n.d., p. 78).
The 1952 and 1964 Syllabuses made particular reference to the belief that
effective oral arithmetic depended on the use of the blackboard and other graphical
aids, particularly for more difficult examples (see Appendices A.12 & A.13).
However, it was considered by some that the blackboard needed to be used
judiciously. "Domas" (1952, p. 11) cautioned that, in instances where mental
examples had been written on the board, mental arithmetic had a tendency to
develop into written tests, a conclusion which may also be attached to the use of
written examples on cards. Nonetheless, in recognising that some children had
difficulty remembering significant details from examples presented orally, it was
suggested in Grade III Mental Arithmetic (1936, p. 15) that cards, with five questions
on each, could be used once a week. It was suggested that it was appropriate for
slower children to do fewer than the five as was "it not better that they should
successfully do these [few] than do none at all under the dictation system?" (Grade
III Mental Arithmetic, 1936, p. 15).
"An Inspector of Schools" (1914, p. 3) maintained that the tendency for mental
arithmetic to become a written test was also manifested in instances where pupils
recorded their answers to a series of oral questions in writing. Although such a
procedure allowed the teacher to identify the lazy children, it did not facilitate the
explanation of the operations necessary to calculate an answer. However, "the evil
to be guarded against...[during the oral presentation of answers was] that it
[afforded] much opportunity for lazy pupils to neglect making calculations, relying on
the sharper and more industrious to satisfy the teacher with answers" ("An Inspector
of Schools, 1914, p. 3). Rapid question delivery was one of the means by which
listlessness and inattention could be prevented (Robinson, 1882, p. 180). Ross
(1893) believed that such exercises as "2s. 3d + 6d + 9d + 2s. 6d...with a long
pause between each item, [were] neither instructive nor amusing. Speed and
accuracy should both be aimed at" (p. 83). In instances where children were
required to provide written answers, "J.R.D." (1928) cautioned that much valuable
teaching time could be lost marking the slates of those who claimed to have the

236

correct answer. Time was better spent focusing on those who were incorrect and
"teaching them without delay" (p. 7).
Given that the intention under each of the syllabuses was for children principally
to become proficient in written arithmetic, little support was shown for Acting District
Inspector Papi's (1912, p. 51) suggestion that teachers should devote the time spent
on written arithmetic to mental. However, he believed that once proficiency with
mental arithmetic was attained accuracy and speed in written work would
necessarily follow. Nonetheless, with the general neglect by teachers of the
recommendation that mental arithmetic should be a precursor to the written, it is not
unexpected, in light of the analyses in Chapter 2, that Farrell (1929, p. 283) should
have reported that children fail to calculate mentally on their leaving primary school.
He noted further that "professional men and tradesmen [complained] that...the
Arithmetical exercises [which could be interpreted as including those for mental
arithmetic]...had no bearing on the problems met with in the affairs of every-day life"
(Farrell, 1929, p. 283). Recognising that individuals relate to particular calculative
situations in idiosyncratic ways, "Vigilate" (1950) asserted that:

The whole trouble seems to be that though in many ways arithmetic may be an
exact science with regard to its truths, formulae and fundamental methods, it is
not an exact science concerning its application, where the sky's the limit, without
defined horizons and a comprehensible ceiling. (p. 11)

3.7

Conclusions and Summary


The foregoing analysis was designed to provide an understanding of nature and

role of mental arithmetic during the period 1860 to 1965. It is evident that the
historical record is substantially limited to the beliefs and opinions of two groups of
stakeholders, namely, those of District Inspectors of Schools, as recorded in their
annual reports, and of teachers, as expressed through publications of the
Queensland Teachers' Union. Although these two groups of departmental officers
often expressed differing opinions about issues which impinged on classroom
practice, taken together, their recorded views, in conjunction with syllabus
documents, have enabled a clear picture of mental arithmetic to emerge.

237

Although generally there was unanimity in the views of teachers, albeit of those
involved in the affairs of the Union, the inspectorate was not noted for its
concordance on many issues. As the Director of Primary Education noted in his
report for 1960, there tended to be "little unanimity of outlook among inspectors"
(Guymer, 1961, p. 8). Nevertheless, as had been cautioned earlier, these
differences of opinion were not to be taken as a house divided against itself
(Edwards, 1931, p. 28; Ewart, 1901, p. 55), but as evidence of the inherent elasticity
of the Department and of the Syllabus (Edwards, 1931, p. 28), albeit an elasticity
that had resulted in teacher confusion with respect to syllabus implementation.
However, in contrast to the differences of opinion which members of the
inspectorate may have held on many educational issues, those associated with
mental arithmetic were ones on which there was general agreement. Inspectors
consistently reported that children lacked the ability to efficiently calculate mentally,
that teachers did not plan their teaching effectively and that mental arithmetic did not
receive the regular and systematic treatment which was required for meeting
syllabus expectations. Although not always disagreeing with inspectors, teachers,
particularly through their union and its publications, championed beliefs about
mental arithmetic and associated Departmental procedures which they saw as
essential to the improvement of teachingnot the least of which was the abolition of
the Scholarship examination and the system of teacher inspection, which occurred
in 1963 and 1970, respectively.
To provide a focus for the presentation and analysis of the historical data, a
number of research questions were posed. The following provides a summary of
key points related to each.

1.

What emphasis was given to mental computation in the various mathematics


syllabuses for Queensland primary schools during the period 1860-1965?

In contrast to mathematics syllabuses in the United States of America during


this period, each of the Queensland syllabuses contained specific references to the
calculation of exact answers mentally beyond the basic facts (see Appendix A), the
most common term for which was mental arithmetic, even though it was not often
used in syllabus documents. As previously noted, the mental arithmetic embodied
in the syllabus documents was characterised by a gradual reduction in the

238

complexity of the mental calculations required, parallelling the greater specificity of


syllabus requirements, requirements that covered the breadth of the mathematics
curriculum. Mental arithmetic as the application of short methods of calculation
decreased in emphasis during the early-20th century. However, short methods
received increased attention during the 1930s and 1940s. This reversal was a
reaction to the practice of many Head Teachers and District Inspectors of Schools
presenting mental examples beyond syllabus requirements during their inspections
of teachers and children. Nonetheless, little emphasis was given to children
devising their own strategies for calculating mentally.
Mental arithmetic retained its place in the syllabuses taught in Queensland
schools for two key reasons. First, despite the doubt cast on the validity of the
theories of formal discipline and the transfer of training, Queensland teachers and
District Inspectors generally retained their belief in mental arithmetic as a means for
providing "the intellectual gymnastics necessary for bracing the mind to logical and
continuous thought" (Ewart, 1890, p. 67). However, although their belief in the mind
as a complex of faculties may have diminished, Burns (1973, p. 4) questioned
whether any faculty other than memory was ever developed as a consequence of
the way in which mental arithmetic was taughtthe focus in teaching was on the
inculcation and retention of information, and the production of correct answers.
Although recognising that the mind could not be trained in general, and believing
that whatever was to be learned had to be specifically taught (Kolesnik, 1958, p. 5),
a belief in arithmetic, in general, and mental arithmetic, in particular, as a means for
concentrating the mind (Martin, 1916, p. 135) and developing the ability to think
critically (Baker, 1929, p. 281; Bevington, 1923, p. 64; Mutch, 1924, p. 40) continued
to be held. This, to such an extent that Greenhalgh (1947, p. 11; 1949b, p. 11) felt
the need, during the late 1940s, to argue for teachers to forgo their conservatism
and to reduce the complexity of the mental arithmetic examples given to children.
Second, mental arithmetic retained its place in each of the syllabuses because
of its recognised usefulness in the after-life of the child, even though prior to the
1952 Syllabus this was not specifically mentioned. However, textbooks issued to
schools (Cox & MacDonald, n.d.; Gladman, n.d., 1904; Joyce, 1881; Park, 1879;
Robinson, 1882) to support arithmetic teaching during the period investigated,
stressed the social usefulness of mental arithmetic, a belief often reiterated by

239

District Inspectors in their annual reports (Bevington, 1926, p. 80; Caine, 1878, p.
97; Macgroarty, 1891, p. 75; Router, 1941, p. 2).
Although the degree to which the belief in the disciplinary powers of mental
arithmetic remained fairly constant during this period, it is evident that the belief in
its social usefulness gained ascendancy. The changes made to the mathematics
syllabus in 1948, and continued in 1952, were aimed at ensuring that children
developed skill in the calculations that needed to be made in daily life (Department
of Public Instruction, 1948b, p. 17). The predominant educational philosophy from
the 1930s was realism (Greenhalgh, 1947, p. 11). With the perceived discrediting of
the transfer of training, it was believed that "the only things worth teaching were
those for which there was some obvious and immediate use" (Kolesnik, 1958, p. 5).

2.

What was the nature of mental computation as embodied in the various


syllabuses and in the manner in which it was taught from 1860 to 1965?

The period 1860-1965 is characterised by a lack of preciseness in the way in


which the mental calculation of exact answers was described, with each syllabus,
prior to that of 1952, lacking consistency in the terminology used. Such calculation
was variously called, in the 1904 Syllabus, for example, Mental exercises, Mental
work, Mental and oral work and Oral and mental work (see Appendix A). Oral
arithmetic was the term used in the 1952 and 1964 Syllabuses. However, mental
arithmetic was the expression most commonly employed by District Inspectors,
textbook authors, and contributors to the journals of the Queensland Teachers
Union and the Education Office Gazette during the period being investigated.
In practice, Queensland teachers took a narrow view of what constituted mental
arithmetic. Encouraged by syllabus statements, comments by District Inspectors
and the way in which mental examples were presented in textbooks, teachers
tended to view mental arithmetic as the presentation of a series of oral questions,
the aim of which was to obtain correct answers, speedily and accurately (Cochran,
1960, p. 12; Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. vi, 1952, p. 1; Farrell, 1929,
p. 289; "J.R.D., 1928, p. 7; Ross, 1893, p. 83, 1905, p. 33; Trudgian, 1929, p. 109).
Given the impreciseness of the terminology used, and the poor quality of teacher
training (Professional Standards, 1936, p. 1), the distinction between oral
arithmetic, as encouraging explanation and discussion, and mental arithmetic was

240

generally not exhibited in classrooms, nor in syllabus documents. The terms came
to be used interchangeably. In the 1914 Syllabus, with respect to the First Class,
oral arithmetic was viewed as "nothing more that a ready application of the tables"
(Department of Public Instruction, 1914, p. 64), a view echoed by Lidgate (1954, p.
2) with respect to the oral arithmetic for all grades.
Occasionally, textbook authors and District Inspectors of Schools attempted to
broaden the view of mental arithmetic. Joyce (1881, p. 210), while defining an
effective practical arithmetician as one who could mentally perform the short
computations of everyday living, recognised that these required more than the rote
application of short methods of calculation. In his view, individuals needed to be
able to mentally perform "all kinds of numerical combinations..., from the common
addition table up to the most complicated operations" (Joyce, 1881, p. 210).
Recognition was also periodically given to a need for encouraging children to
develop a "dexterity with numbers" ("An Inspector of Schools, 1914, p. 3; Cox &
MacDonald, n.d., p. 217; Joyce, 1881, p. 210; Mutch, 1924, p. 40).
However, only Bevington (1925, p. 83) and Cox and MacDonald (n.d., pp. 224,
231) specifically referred to strategies similar to those identified in Chapter 2 as
being based on instrumental understandingones that could be defined as
compensatory and worked from the left. District Inspector Kennedy (1903) argued
against the use of written methods for mental calculation, methods he considered as
"quite out of place" (p. 69). Nonetheless, by 1952, as a consequence of the reaction
to the misinterpretations of syllabus requirements by teachers, Head Teachers and
District Inspectors during the 1930s and 1940s, coupled with the long-held view of
mental arithmetic as initiatory to written arithmetic, this became the recommended
approach to mental calculation"The processes which are applied orally [that is,
mentally] are the same as those used in written operations" (Department of Public
Instruction, 1952b, p. 2). This view is one reason for the poor performances on
mental arithmetic which were regularly criticised by District Inspectors in their
reports, criticisms that continued into the 1950s and 1960s, even though there was
increased specificity following criticisms of syllabus vagueness (Arithmetic, 1927,
p. 17), and despite adjustments being made to grade placements of mental
arithmetic tasks, in light of research findings concerning the written mathematical
expectations of Queensland children (Cunningham & Price, 1934).

241

District Inspectors of Schools identified a number of reasons for the poor


performances of children on mental calculations, the most common of which was
that mental arithmetic was often neglected in classrooms (Bevington, 1926, p. 80;
Farrell, 1927, p. 104; Fox, 1905, p. 9; Gutekunst, 1957, p. 8; Platt, 1901, p. 59;
Scott, 1892, p. 92). Where it was taught, teachers were often criticised for the
perfunctory manner in which it was taken, often stemming from their failure to
prepare sets of graded examples before the lessons (Benbow, 1911, p. 67, 1925, p.
51; Harrap, 1908, pp. 46-47; Inglis, 1926, p. 97, 1929, p. 86; Kemp, 1917, p. 96;
Radcliffe, 1898, p. 73), or for using textbook examples "without plan or purpose"
(Kemp, 1915, p. 98). Given the promiscuous manner in which examples were often
presented, teachers tended not to base the mental work on the written work that
was to follow (Hendren, 1939, p. 55; Lidgate, 1959, p. 5), despite syllabus
recommendations for this to occur. Further, teachers were also castigated for
limiting the work to mechanical processes to the neglect of problem types
(Crampton, 1955, p. 2). Trudgian (1929, p. 109) had earlier complained that in
aiming to assist children to understand problems speed and accuracy had been
sacrificed. As a consequence of the way in which mental arithmetic lessons were
usually conducted, with the teacher and bright children doing all the work (Farrell,
1929, pp. 289-290), many children came to believe that they could not calculate
mentally. They felt that they were examined not taught during each lesson
(Crampton, 1954, p. 3; George, 1934, p. 29; "Howard, 1899, p. 79; Kehoe, 1955, p.
2; Pyle, 1959, p. 9).
That teachers did not effectively implement mental arithmetic in the spirit of the
syllabus nor as advocated by the inspectorate, was not only due to the nature of
their pre-service and lack of inservice training, but also to such factors as: (a) the
physical lay-out of the school and the size and structure of the classes taught,
particularly in one-teacher schools (Benbow, 1925, p. 51), (b) the authoritarian
nature of schools with their emphasis on "strict order, the threat of sanctions,
repetition, drill and cramming...[as means for achieving] results in examinations and
during the inspector's visit" (England, 1971, p. 193), and (c) the rigid view of
arithmetic and how it should be taught. Cox and MacDonald (n.d.) encapsulated a
linear view of arithmetic by declaring that "nothing need be taken for granted; every
truth is capable of demonstration, and each new truth is seen to grow out of what
has preceded" (p. 180). In this way, the child was trained to think logicallya form of

242

mental training. Further, teachers of arithmetic needed to be strong disciplinarians


(Cox & MacDonald, n.d., p. 214; Gladman, n.d., p. 77), not the least to ensure that
copying did not occur (Cox & MacDonald, n.d., p. 214).

3.

What was the role of mental computation within the mathematics curricula from
1860 to 1965?

The roles ascribed to mental arithmetic centred on its usefulness as: (a) a
pedagogical tool, (b) a skill that was socially useful, and (c) a means for "quickening
the intelligence" (Bevington, 1926, p. 80; Robinson, 1882, p. 179); issues that have
been mentioned previously with respect to the first two research questions. As a
pedagogical tool, mental arithmetic was seen to have had a role in each of the three
stages for teaching new arithmetical ideas: (a) to assist teaching the processes
involved, (b) developing speed and accuracy, and (c) applying the new skills to
problems (Board of Education, 1937, p. 504; Teaching Hints, 1908, p. 16). Each
syllabus from 1904 to 1964 highlighted that mental arithmetic should be initiatory to
written work"Mental calculations should be the basis of all the instruction, and the
pupils should be made familiar by mental exercises with the principles underlying
every [operation] before the written work is undertaken" (Schedule XIV, 1904, p.
201); a view previously expressed by Ross (1885, p. 70).
The spiral nature of the syllabuses from 1930 embodied this belief in their
allocation of requirements across the range of topics in mental and written arithmetic
for each grade. Taking an associationist view, the Board of Education's (1937)
Handbook of Suggestions, which influenced the preparation of the 1930 and 1952
Syllabuses, stated that the mechanical rules of written arithmetic are "forms of
mental technique or...complex habits to be formed" (p. 506), thus implying the rote
nature of written work; a belief transposed to mental calculation in the 1952 Syllabus
by its statement that the "processes applied orally are the same as those used in
written operations" (Department of Public Instruction, 1952b, p. 2).
Mental work was also seen as a means for drilling basic facts, cultivating speed
and accuracy in new work and revising work essential for sound progress (Board of
Education, 1937, p. 513; Cochran, 1960, p.12; Mutch, 1916, p. 62). This approach
enabled "at least four times the ground to be covered" (Farrell, 1929, p. 295) in
revision work. However, it is likely that this role was one that contributed to the

243

belief that mental arithmetic constituted the presentation of a series of examples, the
focus of which was the gaining of correct answers.
Although not specifically mentioned in a Queensland syllabus prior to that of
1952, the recognition given to the social usefulness of mental arithmetic was a
distinguishing feature of the arithmetic recommended for Queensland schools,
particularly through the reports of District Inspectors, and through textbooks and
journal articles, as referred to under the first research question. This recognition
was given in context with the social usefulness of arithmetic generally, a factor
emphasised in the 1914 and 1930 Syllabuses. Nonetheless, teachers were often
criticised for not giving sufficient recognition to its practical importance (Canny,
1893, p. 102; W. H. Smith, 1914, p. 78; Woodgate, 1955, p. 2), a criticism
inextricably bound to District Inspectors' adverse reports on the quality of mental
arithmetic teaching.
As noted above, Queensland teachers maintained a belief in the role of mental
arithmetic as a means for developing thinking power (Mutch, 1907, p. 70),
particularly with respect to its role in promoting powers of concentration (Welton,
1924, p. 409). The development of an ability to concentrate on a task required
educational effort (Martin, 1916, p. 135) and this gave "strength and activity to the
mind" (Park, 1879, p. 43). However, not all children were willing to give the
necessary mental effort, the consequence of which was to attribute the poor
performances on mental arithmetic "simply to the lack of concentration and [the]
ability to visualize" (Baker, 1953, p. 2). As recognised by the English Board of
Education (1959) in the late 1950s, the use of mathematics, including mental
arithmetic, as a means for developing concentration, accuracy and logical thinking,
which in its crudest form encouraged the belief that such skills were automatically
transferred to non-mathematical activities, had been discredited. Nonetheless, it
was believed "that mathematics, well taught, may have an influence on children's
general attitude to learning, and that ways of tackling problems in other situations
are influenced for good by sound mathematical training" (Board of Education, 1959,
p. 180).

4.

What was the nature of the teaching practices used to develop a child's ability to
calculate exact answers mentally during the period 1860-1965?

244

During the period being investigated three tenets for teaching mental arithmetic
were consistently advocated. These were: (a) regular and systematic treatment, (b)
prior preparation of graded examples, and (c) the need to base the mental examples
on the written work which was to follow. The profit of a lesson was believed to be in
the number of questions answered correctly (Gladman, 1904, p. 215) in a period of
five to ten minutes. Rapid question delivery was essential to prevent listlessness
and inattention (Robinson, 1882, p. 180). Taylor (1928, p. 43), in contrast to
Bevington (1922, p. 58) and Moorhouse (1927, p. 96), suggested that the examples
given should constitute a range of forms and that explanations should be kept to a
minimum. Such an approach was considered to have the effect of "concentrating
the mind" (Martin, 1916, p. 135), with the examples selected having relevance for
the goal of a lessona particular operation, a short method or revision (Kemp, 1917,
p. 96).
The Board of Education (1937, p. 514) recommended that children should never
be required to work in writing those calculations that could be undertaken mentally.
Prior to this, Gladman (n.d., p. 75) had suggested that children should work the
prepared graded examples mentally to a point where pencil and slate were required.
To encourage mental calculation, it was held that there was a need to "fire the
imagination of the children" (Grade III Mental Arithmetic, 1936, p. 14) by placing
the examples into contexts familiar to the child, a recommendation in keeping with
the third principle espoused in the 1904 and subsequent syllabuses"to bring the
work of the pupil into closer touch with his home and social surroundings"
(Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 200). However, given the tendency of some who followed
this suggestion to present wordy examples, "J.R.D." (1928, p. 7) cautioned that it
was essential for the arithmetic not to get misplaced in the complexity of the
situation presented.
The use of the blackboard was considered necessary for effective oral
arithmetic, particularly for difficult examples (Department of Public Instruction,
1952b, p. 20; Department of Education, 1964, p. 11). However, as Farrell (1929, p.
289) noted, at least during the 1920s, there was a tendency for teachers to do all
the working on the blackboard to the detriment of children's understanding of the
processes involved. Additionally, it was argued that, where examples were written
on the board prior to calculation, the propensity for mental arithmetic to become a
written test was increased ("Domas, 1952, p. 11). This added to the view that

245

mental arithmetic constituted testing rather than teaching, an outcome that resulted
in both teachers and students expressing apprehension with respect to mental
arithmetic (Greenhalgh, 1947, p. 11).

5.

What was the nature of the resources used to support the teaching of mental
computation during the period 1860-1965?

With the introduction of the 1904 Syllabus in January 1905, teachers were
encouraged to make "the self-activity of the pupil the basis of school instruction"
(Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 200), a recommendation specifically contained in the 1914
and 1930 Syllabuses, and implied in those subsequently introduced. Teachers were
encouraged to allow children to see and handle quantities of actual thingssticks in
tens and hundreds, the foot-rule, money, weights. It was recommended that a
child's introduction to number should be through the senses (Department of Public
Instruction, 1930, p. 30). However, besides the use of the blackboard, little use
appears to have been made of such aids to support a child's mental calculations.
When observing that mental exercises were being neglected in schools, Farrell
(1929, p. 284) suggested that the use of the concrete had diverted teachers and
children from the all-important abstract manipulation of numbers.
In instances where District Inspectors did refer to aids to support mental
arithmetic, it was usually to the use of printed sheets and textbooks, the ad hoc use
of which was identified as a reason for the poor performances on the mental
arithmetic tests given during their inspections. The format of each of these texts
(see Table 3.2) was predominantly in the form of sets of mechanical examples,
which, although often referring to money and measures, did not sufficiently provide
clear links to the "things by which the child is surrounded and [to] things in which he
was interested" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. 30). The format and
content of the sets of questions supported the view that mental arithmetic lessons
should involve the working of a number of questions in a relatively short period of
time, the success of which was measured by the number of correct answers.
Similar criticisms may be made of the teaching notes published in the
Queensland Educational Journal and Queensland Teachers' Journal. Further, many
of these articles were designated as mental arithmetic testsfor example, Mental
Arithmetic Tests (1910), Mental Tests (1930)thus reinforcing the view that the

246

mental arithmetic lesson be equated with testing procedures. Further, even where
testing was the legitimate goal of a lesson, the use of such examples did not allow
teachers to implement the direction, as contained in the 1914 Syllabus, that
"teachers should not trust to any text-book, but should prepare their own series of
questions, adapted to local conditions and needs" (Department of Public Instruction,
1914, p. 70).
However, given the conditions under which they had to operatetheir
inadequate training, their desire for greater specificity in grade requirements, the
pressures exerted by Head Teachers, District Inspectors and Scholarship
Examinersit is not inconsistent that teachers should have embraced the examples
provided as the published materials became available. Paradoxically, the
consequence of this was for teachers, particularly during the 1930s and 1940s, to
exacerbate the basis of their criticisms of the syllabus as being overloaded. As
"Green Ant" (1942, p. 17) observed, the extensions to the syllabus requirements by
the textbooks were most flagrant for mental arithmetic. Their requirements set the
standard of work expected for each grade (Dagg, 1971, p. 57). Whether or not this
remained a consequence of textbook use post-1965 was an avenue of investigation
discussed in the next chapter, the purpose of which was to extend the
understanding of mental arithmetic in Queensland schools beyond the era in which
it was included as a specific branch of the mathematics syllabus.

250

CHAPTER 4

MENTAL COMPUTATION IN QUEENSLAND:


1966-1997
4.1

Introduction
In contrast to the mathematics syllabuses introduced into Queensland primary

schools during the period 1860-1965, a focus on mental computation was not a
feature of those introduced during the New Maths era, which effectively occurred in
Queensland between 1966 and 1987; nor is it considered explicitly in the current
Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a).
Nonetheless, as discussed in Chapter 1, there has been a resurgence of interest in
mental computation from the late 1980s, as a consequence of which is an emphasis
on the calculation of exact answers mentally in national documents designed to
guide syllabus development in AustraliaA National Statement on Mathematics for
Australian Schools (AEC, 1991), for example. Such a development is one that
parallels that which has occurred in the United States, where educational authorities
are attempting to implement the Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School
Mathematics (NCTM, 1989).
Hence, to extend an understanding of mental computation within Queensland
primary schools from 1966 to the present, it is not only necessary to analyse
syllabus documents of the period, but also to consider the nature and effects of
recent Queensland curriculum initiatives which have relevance for mental
computation. Further, for recommendations to be formulated to enhance the
teaching of mental computation, it is essential to gain an understanding of teachers
current beliefs and teaching practices, the origins of which may be embedded in
those of the past. This chapter, therefore, is structured around (a) an analysis of
mental computation under the syllabuses from 1966-1987, (b) a survey of
Queensland primary school personnel, and (c) an analysis of recent curriculum
initiatives in Queensland state primary schools relevant to mental computation.

251

4.1.1 Background to the Research Strategy


In contrast to the period 1860-1965, documentary evidence, similar to that
accessed during the research reported in Chapter 3, was unavailable for the post1964 period. The Queensland Teachers' Union had discontinued its practice of
including articles on mental arithmetic in its journal, not only due to the changes to
the mathematics incorporated in the Program in Mathematics (Department of
Education, 1966-1968, 1975) but, more importantly, due to its emphasis on
industrial rather than curriculum issues. The annual reports of the Queensland
Department of Education also ceased to include those prepared by District
Inspectors of Schools. Further, the thirty-year rule for the public release of
government documents precluded direct access to the latter.
Consequently, the use of a questionnaire to survey teacher beliefs and
practices (see Section 4.3) was considered to be the most effective research
method for this stage of the study. A postal, self-completion questionnaire allowed
the opinions of a representative sample of school personnel to be obtained. Also,
given that many teachers would have had experience teaching under each of the
syllabuses from 1964, this was considered an appropriate method for extending the
analysis of past beliefs and practices beyond the mid-1960s. The survey data
support and extend issues raised in the discussion of the 1966 to 1987 Queensland
Syllabuses (Section 4.2), and provides a background against which recent initiatives
in mathematics education in Queensland (Section 4.4) may be considered for their
relevance to mental computation.

4.1.2 Research Focus


Specific questions, which guided the construction of the questionnaire and the
analysis of the data obtained, are delineated in Section 4.3.1. However, each of
these is related to a broader question which provides a focus for all aspects of the
analyses presented in this chapter, namely analyses related to the (a) 1966-1987
Syllabuses, (b) survey of school personnel, and (c) recent curriculum initiatives.
These broader questions are:

252

1. What beliefs do teachers currently hold with respect to the nature and role
of mental computation and how it should be taught?
2. What emphasis was given to mental computation in the period 1966-1987,
with respect to both syllabus documents and teachers?
3. What emphasis is currently placed on developing the ability to compute
mentally?
4. What are the characteristics of the teaching approaches currently used to
develop the ability to calculate exact answers mentally?
5. What were the characteristics of the teaching approaches used to develop
the ability to calculate exact answers mentally during the period 19661987?
6. What need for inservice on mental computation is expressed by school
personnel?
7. What was the nature of the resources used to support the teaching of
mental computation during the period 1966-1987 and of those used
currently?
8. What is the relevance to mental computation of recent initiatives in
mathematics education in Queensland?

4.2

The Syllabuses and Mental Computation in Queensland:


1966-1987
As discussed in Chapter 3, the 1964 Syllabus was essentially an interim

document necessitated by the introduction of decimal currency planned for February


1966 and the transfer of Year 8 to secondary schools from 1964. Although a new
syllabus for Grades 1 to 3 was introduced in 1966, that of 1964 continued to be the
syllabus for Grades 4 and 5 until 1967, and for Years 6 and 7 until 1969. The
introduction of decimal currency in February 1966 was to have a major impact on
primary school arithmetic, not least with the removal of the need for children to
manipulate pounds, shillings, and pence in concert with an increased importance
being attached to the ability to calculate with decimal numbers. Consequently,
these changes, in addition to revised beliefs about how children learn mathematics,

253

were ultimately of greater consequence than the modifications made to the syllabus
in 1964 (Department of Education, 1978, p. 3).
Primarily through the influence of Piaget, "probably the most outstanding
psychologist of the day" (Department of Education, 1966, p. i), it was recognised
that an over-simplified behavioural theory of learning, using the stimulus-response
model, was no longer adequate to describe mathematics learning (Hughes, 1965, p.
32). This recognition was also influenced by the work of European and English
mathematics educatorsthat of Dienes, Gattegno, and Fletcher, for examplein
which an emphasis was placed on the needs and interests of the child and how
mathematical ideas grow in the minds of children (Keeves, 1965, p. 6). An
emphasis was therefore placed on the discovery approach to teaching and learning
(Department of Education, 1967, p. i), the rediscovery of which was, in Gordon
Jones' (1967) view, "probably the most important development in pedagogy
associated with modern mathematics" (p. 23). Following a series of conferences
sponsored by the Australian Mathematical Society and the Australian Council of
Educational Research in the early-1960s, Blakers (1978) was later to observe:

Leading educators and mathematicians accepted the rhetoric which stressed


the need for precision of language and symbolism, the use of integrating
concepts such as sets, function, algebraic structure and transformation
geometry, and the need for children to justify each line of their mathematical
reasoning by reference to an axiom or rule. (pp. 149-150).

In implementing these ideas, commencing in 1966 with the Program in


Mathematics: Stages 1-4 (Department of Education, 1966), the Queensland
mathematics syllabuses introduced during the New Mathematics era of the late1960s to mid-1970s (see Table 4.1) did not specifically refer to the development of
an ability to calculate exact answers mentally beyond the basic facts, except with
respect to: (a) the "application of higher decade addition and related subtraction to

254

Table 4.1
Queensland Mathematics Schedules and Syllabuses: 1965-1987

Department of Education. (1966). Program in mathematics for primary schools:


Stages 1-4. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1967) Program in mathematics for primary schools:
Stages 5-6. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1968). Program in mathematics for primary schools:
Stages 7-8. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1975). Program in mathematics. Brisbane: The
Department.
Department of Education. (1987). Years 1 to 10 mathematics syllabus. Brisbane:
The Department.

numbers up to 100, (b) "combined multiplication and addition to 100, and (c)
"division with remainders within 100" (Department of Education, 1967, p. 13). This
is reflected in the findings presented later in this chapter which indicate that the
teachers surveyed believed that the Program in Mathematics (Department of
Education, 1966-1968, 1975) placed considerably less emphasis on mental
arithmetic than the 1964 Syllabus. Nonetheless, it is likely that, during
implementation of the 1967, 1968, and 1975 Syllabuses, teachers continued to
place an emphasis on mental arithmetic as a means for drilling basic facts,
cultivating speed and accuracy, and for revision. Data from the survey reveals that,
in contrast to their views about the importance of mental arithmetic embodied in the
syllabuses, many teachers continued to place great importance on mental
arithmetic, at least as traditionally defined (see Table 4.13).
The various editions of the Program in Mathematics (Department of Education,
1966-1968, 1975) embodied a concern for developing an understanding of
"underlying properties inherent in number systems and the discovery of underlying
principles that enable...[the investigation of] many different areas of inquiry" (G. J.
Jones, 1967, p, 20)that is, the syllabuses embodied a concern for structure of the
number system. However, stemming from an inadequate understanding of the
intent of the syllabuses, teachers became focused on developing the standard

255

written algorithms through an over-emphasis on step-by-step proof, mathematical


symbolism, and on the use of mathematical principles as ends rather than means
(Boxall, 1981, p. 2). It is in this context that mental arithmetic was likely to have
been devalued, although, in teaching the concepts outlined under the various
syllabus topics, oral presentation and mental calculation would have been involved.
However, it is likely that the focus would have remained on the correctness or
otherwise of the answer, rather than with the mental strategies employed.
As prophesied by G. J. Jones (1967), "it [was] all too possible for the content of
[the new mathematics] course to receive such a formal, sterile and rigorous
treatment, that it [became] far less appealing and meaningful than most traditional
courses" (p. 63). Teachers were unable to cope with the "lofty ideas" contained in
the syllabuses of this era (Clements, Grimison, & Ellerton, 1989, p. 70), due to a
large extent to the inadequate inservice provided (Izard, 1969, p. 66). Powell (1968,
p. 1) had noted that the Department of Education had not given any clear directives
on how or when the programme was to be implemented. Further, Powell (1968)
indicated that three days of inservice had been conducted for some teachers, but
"no expert [had been] used to show us how to teach or intergrate (sic)" (p. 1), a
situation in accordance with this researcher's experience.
Ironically, mental computation, as now conceived, allows children to explore
numbers and their interrelationships in ways that increase a child's awareness of the
structure of the number system (Sowder, 1992, p. 15) in a learning environment
similar to that recommended in the syllabuses of the New Maths era: "There should
be frequent opportunities for the child to discuss his mathematical experiences and
discoveries both with his teacher and with his classmates. Every opportunity should
be allowed for expression, both oral and written" (Department of Education, 1975, p.
iii).
During a review of the Queensland mathematics syllabus during the early1980s, Murray (1981, p. 7) suggested that there was a need to investigate the
implications of hand-held calculators and problem solving for the curriculum. In
taking account of these recommendations and while redressing the causes of the
rigid approach to teaching mathematics fostered by the 1966-1975 Syllabuses,
although partially redressed in the 1975 Syllabus (G. A. Jones, 1979, pp. 24-25), the
Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a) placed an
emphasis on how children learn mathematics, and, significantly for mental

256

computation, placed an emphasis on the thinking processes involved when using


mathematics.
Although it retains the development of the standard written algorithms as the
major goal of the Number work, and makes no specific reference to mental
computation, the 1987 Syllabus implicitly supports such a focus. Calculating is
included as one of the general processes, and reference is made to "estimating and
calculating...using mental and calculator procedures and written algorithms"
(Department of Education, 1987a, pp. 15, 16). The development of a proficiency
with mental strategies to extend the basic facts for each operation is recommended
for "as many students as possible" in the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Teaching,
Curriculum and Assessment Guidelines (Department of Education, 1987b, p. 14).
However, it is only in some of the accompanying sourcebooks, particularly in that for
Year 5 (Department of Education, 1988, pp. 51-57), that it is clearly evident that
some consideration should be given the development of flexible strategies for
calculating exact answers mentally beyond the basic facts. Hence, the extent to
which teachers were aware of these recommendations was one consideration for
the survey of Queensland teachers and administrators reported next in this chapter.

4.3

Survey of Queensland Primary School Personnel


The procedures associated with undertaking the survey and the analysis of the

data obtained have been presented using the following major headings: (a) Survey
Method, (b) Survey Results, and (c) Discussion. The latter includes the conclusions
drawn with respect to each of the specific research questions outlined in Section
4.3.1. The data from the survey reported in this chapter are designed to provide an
understanding of:

The beliefs about mental computation held by Queensland state primary


school teachers and administrators.

The current status of mental computation in classroom mathematics


programs.

The pedagogical practices related to mental computation presently


employed, as well as those used during the period 1964-1987.

257

(Although this period provides an overlap with those previously discussed,


it was considered that data from the questionnaire could inform the
analyses relevant to the 1964-1987 Syllabuses, given that many current
teachers began their careers during the early- to mid-1960s.)

The need for teacher inservice related to mental computation.

The resources used to support the development of the ability to calculate


exact answers mentally beyond the basic facts.

An additional consideration for gaining insights into the status of mental


computation at the time of the survey was the proposed implementation of Student
Performance Standards in mathematics (Student Performance Standards, 1994)
into Queensland primary schools (see Section 4.4). The introduction of these
Standards required that mental computation be given a higher profile than that
embodied in the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education,
1987a). Mental computation was one of the seven Number sub-strands within the
Standards, whereas it is not specifically mentioned in the 1987 Syllabus.
If teachers are to effectively implement the recommended changes to the way
in which mental computation should be taught, as discussed in Chapter 2,
McIntosh's (1992, p. 134) two revolutions need to occur. Firstly, teachers need to
enjoy manipulating numbers, but more importantly, teachers' understanding of the
nature of mental computation, and of appropriate methods of teaching and
assessing, needs to reflect those beliefs about computing mentally which are
embodied in A National Statement on Mathematics for Australian Schools (AEC,
1991), the document on which the now discarded Student Performance Standards
were based. The survey was therefore designed to determine whether the beliefs
and practices of Queensland primary school teachers' and administrators' match
with those currently espoused by mathematics educatorsbeliefs and practices
previously analysed in the review of the mathematics education literature presented
in Chapter 2.

4.3.1 Survey Method


In outlining the procedures used to undertake the survey, and their rationales,
consideration has been given to: (a) the research questions used to guide the

258

development of the survey instrument and its analysis, (b) the instrument itself, (c)
the sample of Queensland state school teachers and administrators, (d) the method
of implementation, and (e) the procedures used to analyse the data.

Research Questions
To gain an understanding of the issues associated with the beliefs and teaching
practices related to mental computation, as delineated above, questions were posed
in relation to each. These are:

1.

The beliefs about mental computation held by Queensland state primary


school teachers and administrators.
(a)

What beliefs do teachers currently hold with respect to the various


aspects of the nature of mental computation and how it should be
taught?
(I)

Is skill in mental computation considered an important goal in


mathematics education?

(ii)

Do the beliefs about the nature of mental computation reflect a


nontraditional view?

(iii)

Do the beliefs about how mental computation should be taught


reflect a nontraditional view?

(b)

What importance was placed on mental computation in the period


1964-1987, with respect to both syllabus documents and teachers?

2.

The current status of mental computation in classroom mathematics


programs.
(a)

What emphasis is currently placed on developing the ability to


compute mentally?

3.

The pedagogical practices related to mental computation presently


employed, as well as those used during the period 1964-1987.
(a)

What are the characteristics of the teaching approaches currently


used to develop the ability to calculate exact answers mentally?
(i)

Do teachers take a nontraditional approach to developing skill


with mental computation?

259

(ii)

Are the teaching approaches employed by middle- and upperschool teachers consistent with their stated beliefs?

(b)

What were the characteristics of the teaching approaches used to


develop the ability to calculate exact answers mentally during the
period 1964-1987?

4.

The need for teacher inservice related to mental computation.


(a)

What need for inservice on mental computation is expressed by


school personnel?

5.

The resources used to support the teaching of mental computation.


(a)

What is the nature of the resources currently used to support the


teaching of mental computation?

(b)

What was the nature of the resources used to support the teaching of
mental computation during the period 1964-1987?

Instrument Used
The instrument used to undertake the survey of Queensland state primary
school teachers and administrators was a postal, self-completion
opinionnaire/questionnaire (Appendix C). In descriptive research, instrument items
concentrate on the phenomenon to be described and on background characteristics,
rather than on the identification of dependent and independent variables essential to
explanatory research (de Vaus, 1991, p. 81). Hence, in keeping with the research
purposes and questions, the survey instrument was divided into four sections:
Section 1

Beliefs About Mental Computation and How it Should Be


Taught.

Section 2

Current Teaching Practices.

Section 3

Past Teaching Practices.

Section 4

Background Information.

The items in Sections 1 to 3, to which school personnel were asked to respond,


are relevant to issues discussed in Chapters 1, 2 and 3, issues that are related to
traditional and nontraditional beliefs about the role and nature of mental computation
and how the ability to compute exact answers mentally should be developed. The
traditional approach to teaching mental computation is characterised by an

260

emphasis on the teacher delivering a series of one-step questions. Speed of


calculation and correctness of the answers are of paramount importance. In
contrast, approaches which are defined as nontraditional are characterised by a
concern for the mental strategies used to arrive at solutions. Recognition is given to
the constructivist nature of mathematical development. Children are allowed to
develop their own strategies for calculating mentally, and are provided with
opportunities for their explanation and discussion.
In Section 1, Items 1 to 9 are related to the nature of mental computation and its
place within the curriculum, with Items 3, 5 and 8 reflecting a traditional view of
mental computation. Items 10 to 23 concern issues related to the teaching of
mental computation. Of these Items 15, 17, 20, and 21 concern teaching
approaches advocated by educators who hold traditional beliefs about teaching
mental computation (see Appendix C). Respondents were asked to record their
beliefs on a four point Likert scale: strongly disagree, disagree, agree and strongly
agree. To force respondents into expressing their beliefs, the scale did not include
a neutral position.
Section 2 of the survey instrument required respondents, who were currently
teaching a class, to indicate how often particular pedagogical techniques were used
in developing the ability to calculate exact answers mentally beyond the basic facts.
It is recognised that the year level taught influences the nature of the responses to
the items in this section. This confounding factor was taken into account in the data
analysis. Of Items 24 to 38, traditional approaches to mental computation are
represented by Items 29, 30, 33, 34, and 36. A four-point Likert scale was also
used in this section: never, seldom, sometimes and often. Although a fifth point on
the scale (always) would have provided a direct opposite to never and created a
more balanced scale, always was omitted as it was considered that it is unrealistic
to suggest that a particular approach would always be used when teaching a
particular topic. In contrast, it is realistic to indicate that a particular approach is not
within the repertoire of a particular teacher.
As a means for gaining some insight into the teaching practices employed under
the three syllabuses that do not specifically refer to mental computation, Items 39 to
46 in Section 3 required those teachers who were teaching at any time during the
period 1964-1987 to respond to statements, similar to those in Section 2, in relation
to each of the syllabuses. Items 39 and 40 relate to the degree of importance

261

placed on the ability to compute exact answers mentally by each of the three
syllabuses, and by the respondents themselves during these periods. Items 43 to
46 reflect the traditional approach to developing mental computation skills. Given
that responding to items in this section of the survey instrument required teachers to
recall past teaching practices, an unsure category was added to the four-point Likert
scale used in Section 2, and to that related to the importance of mental computation
(see Appendix C).
The items in the survey instrument were developed in association with Dr Calvin
Irons, the researcher's supervisor, as well as with school personnel who have some
expertise in educational research. Further revisions to the instrument resulted from
its piloting with 10 teachers of Years 1 to 7 from an outer-Brisbane school which
was not part of the sample of schools to be surveyed. Item analyses using
correlational techniques were not undertaken in finalising the instrument. Such is in
accordance with Tuckmans (1988) view that these procedures are "not as critical
for the refinement of questionnaires as they are for the refinement of tests.
Questionnaire items are usually reviewed for clarity and distribution of responses
without necessarily running an item analysis" (p. 226).
During the revision of items based on trial data, an attempt was made to
address the four assumptions on which survey research depends. Karweit (1982, p.
1837) describes these as:

The respondent should understand the question in the same way as the
researcher.

The respondent should be able to understand the question and to respond


to it appropriately.

The responses should be able to be coded systematically.

Asking a particular question should not lead to a change in response.

Sample
It was intended that the data collected should be representative of the views of
the 11,970 Queensland state primary school teachers and administrators. However,
given the size of the population, direct sampling of school personnel would have
been an impractical task. Further, data are not readily available to undertake the

262

identification of each teacher in advance by such factors as length of service, year


level/s taught, location within the state and inservice undertaken. Hence the school
was used as the sampling unit. To maximise the representativeness of the sample,
a sampling frame (see Table 4.2), which categorised schools by Education Region
and Band, was developedRegion classifies schools by geographic location, and
Band classifies schools by student enrolment in combination with factors that
impinge on the degree of complexity of school organisation and management. This
procedure ensured that factors that influence the nature of educational programs at
the school level, and their implementation, were considered, at least implicitly.
These factors include school location, access to inservice, degree of teacher
experience, socio-economic factors, and ethnicity of pupils.
Although there is no fixed percentage of a population that determines optimum
sample size (Best & Kahn, 1986, p. 16), a sample of approximately 10% of the 1075
state primary schools was considered adequate to produce a sample of teachers of
sufficient size to permit meaningful conclusions to be drawn. It was also considered
to be of sufficient size to allow for school personnel who chose not to participate in
the survey. A sample of 10% of state schools was used by Warner (1981) to
investigate the perceptions of Queensland state primary school teachers

263

Table 4.2
Sample of Schools by Band Within Educational Regions
Band of school

Region

10

Total

9
1b

6
1c

10
1

27
3

18
2

6
1

77
9
11.7

Metropolitan
West

11
1

33
3

13
1

25
3

39
4

21
2c

143
14
9.8

Metropolitan
East

5
1c

10
1

33
3

45
5

10
1

3
1

108
12
11.1

Darling
Downs

19
2

37
4

26
3

12
1

20
2

3
1c

119
13
10.9

South
Western

23
2

10
1

9
1

8
1

6
1c

3
1

19
2

33
3

24
2

14
1

29
3c

Wide Bay

Capricornia

22
2

45
5c

24
2

18
2

29
3

12
1

150
15
10.0

Northern

16
2

16
2c

15
2

7
1

15
2

11
1

80
10
12.5

North
Western

10
1

7
1

5
1

8
1c

3
1

36
5
13.8

11
1

21
2

27
3

12
1

23
2

15
2c

Peninsula

111
11
9.9

2
1c

6
1

9
1

19
2

13
1

20
2

70
8
11.4

153
16
10.4

260
28
10.8

111
13
11.7

35
4
11.4

1075
115
10.7

Sunshine
Coast

South Coast

135
13
9.6

Total

Note.

218
24
11.0

163
17
10.4

59
7
11.9
1

122
11
9.0

Top line in each row: Number of schools in Region/Band. bBottom line in each row:
Number of schools in sample. cOne school in sample was a trial school for SPS in
1993

264

with respect to aspects of the 1975 edition of the Program in Mathematics for
Queensland Primary Schools (Department of Education, 1975).
Alreck and Settle (1985) suggest that:

It is seldom necessary to sample more than 10 percent of the population to


obtain adequate confidence, providing that the resulting sample is less than
about 1000 and larger than [100]... For populations of 10,000 or more,
most experienced researchers would probably consider a sample size
between about 200 and 1000 respondents [appropriate]. (p. 89)

For each cell in Table 4.2 with 10 or more schools, 10% of the number of
schools, rounded to the nearest 10, were randomly selected. To ensure that the
percentage selected from each region and band was approximately 10%, further
random selections were drawn from cells with fewer than ten schools. Prior to the
random selection from each cell, given that the experiences of teachers trialing the
Student Performance Standards in mathematics may have had an impact on their
views of mental computation, one trial school from each region was randomly
selected. These schools formed part of the 10% of schools selected from the cell in
which they were categorised. On average, four primary schools were involved in the
trial of Student Performance Standards in mathematics within each Educational
Region.
From the Directory of Queensland State Schools (Department of Education,
1991) it was determined that the 115 schools selected would provide a potential
sample of approximately 1400 school personnel. With a potential sample of this
magnitude, even allowing for a high nonresponse rate, 50% for example, it was
projected that the actual sample of school personnel would fall comfortably within
the range suggested by Alreck and Settle (1985, p. 89) referred to previously.
It was recognised that in conducting a postal, self-completion survey control
over who completes the questionnaire would be limited. The sample of teachers
and administrators who participated within each of the schools was therefore largely
self-selected. This introduced bias into the resultant sample (Karweit, 1982, p.
1839) of school personnel. However unavoidable this source of bias may be, it
does necessitate cautious generalisation of the conclusions that are drawn from this
study.

265

Research Procedure
The research was undertaken during Term 4 (October-December) of the 1993
school year. Three mailings to schools in the sample were undertaken. The initial
mailing occurred on 4 October 1993, with follow-up correspondence being sent on
25 October and 28 November 1993 (see Appendix D). The initial mailing was timed
so that the initial letter would not be received as part of the volume of mail usually
received by schools at the commencement of a school term.
Gay (1987) suggests that "it is sometimes worth the effort to do a preliminary
check of potential respondents to determine their receptivity...and it is more
productive to send the questionnaire to a person in authority" (p. 196), the latter also
being the protocol where the purpose is to gain access to school personnel. As
discussed below, this study was to be undertaken in a climate within schools not
conducive to the willing involvement of teachers and administrators in the
completion of survey instruments. This climate had arisen from the number of
surveys, usually with short time-frames, that had been sent to schools from various
sources during a period of rapid structural and curriculum change, change
particularly associated with the management of schools and the concomitant
increased expectations placed on teachers.
Following Gay's (1987, p. 196) suggestion, in an endeavour to maximise
participation by teachers and administrators, a letter was sent to each school
principal explaining the purpose of the survey (see Appendices C.1 & C.2). The
letter invited the principal, or another staff member, to act as Contact Person for the
receipt and management of questionnaires within their school, and to indicate how
many questionnaires they wished to receive. Cavanagh and Rodwell (1992, p. 286)
suggest that obtaining contact persons is one method by which the response rate
may be maximised. In the case of one-teacher schools, the principal was invited to
participate in the study. This approach was also designed to reduce the cost of
printing, packaging, and mailing questionnaires that would not have been returned.
The initial mailing also contained a letter of authority from The Executive Director,
Review and Evaluation Section of the Queensland Department of Education. This
letter provided approval for the study to be conducted in the 115 schools nominated
as the sample.
Following the initial mailing on 4 October 1993, 52 schools agreed to participate
in the survey and 27 schools declined the invitation to be involved. No response

266

was received from 36 of the 115 schools. The number of questionnaires requested
by each school was dispatched in three batches as the participation forms were
received. A deadline of three weeks from the date of dispatch was given for the
return of the survey forms. Three hundred and ninety-five (395) questionnaires
were sent to the 52 schools that had agreed to participate.
Annotations on some participation forms returned by schools not wishing to
receive the survey instrument provided reasons for their non-involvement. The
annotations included:

"This is an inordinately busy and taxing time for our staff."

"We do not have any time to complete any more surveys this year."

"The school is currently involved in too many surveys and projects to be


able to accommodate yet another one."

"We are committed with LOTE [Language Other Than English] immersion
and inclusive curriculum project."

These comments are reflective of the responses from the Queensland


Department of Education's Senior Executive Forum to a request by the Minister for
Education in August 1993 for comments related to the pace and scope of change
occurring within the Department. The Forum indicated that schools were advocating
that they should be left alone for some time to consolidate. With respect to the
receipt of surveys, the Forum recommended that the number sent to schools should
be reduced and that the distribution of surveys to schools had been too frequent
(Senior Executive Forum, 1993, p. 9). It was in this environment that this study was
undertaken and partly explains the relatively low initial positive response rate of
45%52 of the 115 schools in the sample.
As suggested by de Vaus (1991, p. 119), a follow-up mailing should be sent to
schools that have not responded after three weeks. This mailing should contain a
new letter, a replacement questionnaire and stamped return envelope. An
estimation of the number of teachers in each of the 36 schools that had not
responded was made from the Directory of Queensland State Schools (Department
of Education, 1991). A maximum of 10 questionnaires was sent to each school with
an accompanying follow-up letter (Appendix D.3) and reply-paid envelope. It was
intended that there would be at least one questionnaire for teachers of each year
level and one for each administrator. The identification of nonresponse schools was

267

enabled by the inclusion of a school identification number on each address label on


the reply envelopes sent with the initial mailing.
Two hundred and three (203) questionnaires were sent to schools in this
mailing, resulting in a total of 598 questionnaires distributed. This represents survey
instruments being sent to approximately 43% of the estimated 1387 teachers and
administrators in the 115 schools in the sample.
To further enhance the response-rate, a second follow-up letter was sent to the
15 Contact Persons at schools from which survey instruments had not been
received by 25 November 1993 (Appendix D.4). Although agreeing to participate in
the survey through a staff member nominating to be a Contact Person and
requesting questionnaires, none were received from 12 of the 52 schools that had
agreed to participate in response to the initial letter.

Methods of Analysis
The data from each respondent was coded in the form indicated below, and
analysed using various subprograms of the Studentware version of the Statistical
Package for the Social Sciences, SPSS/PC+ Studentware Plus (Norusis, 1991):

1.

Identification number (001 through 201).

2.

Beliefs about mental computation and how it should be taught:


(a)

Items 1 to 23Strongly Disagree (1), Disagree (2), Agree (3), Strongly


Agree (4), Missing (9).

3.

Current teaching practices:


(a)

Items 24 to 38Never (1), Seldom (2), Sometimes (3), Often (4)


Missing (9).

4.

Past teaching practices:


(a)

Items 39a to 40cNo Importance (1), Little Importance (2), Some


Importance (3), Great Importance (4), Unsure (5), Missing (9).

(b)

Items 41a to 46cNever (1), Seldom (2), Sometimes (3), Often (4),
Unsure (5), Missing (9).

5.

Background Information:
(a)

Item 47 (Educational Region)Sunshine Coast (01), Metropolitan


West (02), Metropolitan East (03), Darling Downs (04), South West

268

(05), Wide Bay (06), Capricornia (07), Northern (08), North West (09),
Peninsula (10). South Coast (11), Missing (99).
(b)

Item 48 (Size of school)Band 4 (1), Band 5 (2), Band 6 (3), Band 7


(4), Band 8 (5), Band 9 (6), Band 10 (7), Missing (9)

(c)

Item 49 (Years of teaching experience)< 1 yr (1), 1-5 yrs (2), 6-10 yrs
(3), 11-15 yrs (4), 16-20 yrs (5), 21-25 yrs (6), 26-30 yrs (7), 30+ yrs
(8), Missing (9).

(d)

Item 50 (Teaching role)Class Teacher (1), Teaching Principal (2),


Principal (3), Deputy Principal (4), Missing (9).

(e)

Item 51 (Year Level/s taught)For each of Years 1 to 7: Teaching a


particular year level (1), not teaching a particular year level (0),
Missing (9).
(Data from the None box associated with this item were not coded.
Using the highest class taught, the year-level data were used to
categorise teachers as lower-, middle- or upper-school teachers.)

(f)

Items 52 to 53 (Student Performance Standards)Yes (1), No (0),


Missing (9).

(g)

Items 54 to 55 (Inservice)Yes (1), No (0), Missing (9).

(h)

Item 56 (Inservice source)Teaching colleague (1), Administrator (2),


Mathematics Adviser (3), Tertiary Lecturer (4), Other (5), Missing (9).

Resources listed by respondents in Sections 2.2, 3.3, and 4 of the questionnaire


were collated for inclusion in the results of the survey. Those used by middle- and
upper-school teachers, as defined below, are the textbooks referred to in Table
4.18. Comments recorded by respondents in each of the above sections of the
questionnaire were also collated. Where relevant, these are included in the
presentation of data and their analysis.
As indicated previously, the survey was conceptualised as descriptive research,
for which univariate analysis provided appropriate statistical techniques.
Nonetheless, given the non-randomness of the sample of school personnel (see
below), the conclusions presented in Section 4.3.3 may only be considered
suggestive of the beliefs and practices of Queensland teachers and administrators
in general. The surveys purpose was to gain insights into the beliefs and teaching
practices of school personnel; to describe the status of mental computation, present
and past, in Queensland state primary school classrooms. Hence, it was

269

considered sufficient to present the data for each item as a frequency distribution
(see Tables 4.7 to 4.14).
To facilitate an understanding of the degree to which school personnel espouse
nontraditional beliefs and teaching practices concerning mental computationthose
that are reflective of beliefs and practices currently advocated by mathematics
educatorsthe mean ratings for selected items have been presented on continua
(Figures 4.1 to 4.5). Although the theoretical measure of central tendency for
ordinal data is the median, a scan of the data revealed that the means of item
ratings would better reflect the spread of opinion in such graphical representations.
For example, for the items represented in Figure 4.1 the median for each variable is
3, whereas the means range from 2.63 to 3.24, with standard deviations of .74 and
.55, respectively.
To present the percentage distribution for each item in Section 1 of the survey
instrument (Beliefs), the items were grouped into four categories. These were:

1.

Beliefs about the importance of mental computation


(Table 4.8: Items 1, 2, and 7).

2.

Beliefs about the nature of mental computation


(Table 4.9: Items 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, and 9).

3.

Beliefs about the general approach to teaching mental computation


(Table 4.10: Items 10, 12, 13, 14, 18, and 23).

4.

Beliefs about specific issues related to developing the ability to calculate


exact answers mentally
(Table 4.11: Items 11, 15, 16, 17, 19, 20, 21, and 22).

The mean rating was calculated for each item in Categories 2, 3, and 4 (see
Figures 4.1 to 4.3), except for Items 12, 18, and 23 in Category 3. It was considered
inappropriate to interpret these items in terms of their reflecting traditional or
nontraditional beliefs, as, for example, conducting a series of mental computation
sessions each week (Item 23) has relevance to both orientations. In calculating the
means, the scoring for items that reflect a traditional approach was reversed to
facilitate their placement on traditional-nontraditional continua. Hence, Items 3, 5, 8,
15, 16, 17, 20, and 21 (see Appendix C) were recoded as: Strongly Disagree (4),
Disagree (3), Agree (2), and Strongly Agree (1). Appendix E presents the standard
deviation of each mean.

270

The purpose of Section 2.1 of the survey instrument was to determine how
frequently present classroom teachers use each of a range of teaching techniques
to develop the ability to mentally calculate exact answers beyond the basic facts.
The Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Teaching, Curriculum and Assessment Guidelines
(Department of Education, 1987b, pp. 16-17) suggests that children should begin to
experience mental strategies to extend the basic facts from Year 2 for addition, Year
3 for subtraction, Year 4 for multiplication, and from Year 5 for division. For each
operation, it is expected that as many children as possible will develop proficiency
by Year 7. The focus during Years 1, 2, and 3 is primarily on the development of
basic fact knowledge. Hence, in analysing the responses to the items in Section
2.1, it was considered appropriate that the data to be analysed should be restricted
to those from teachers of Years 4 to 7.
It was anticipated that many teachers would be responsible for multi-agemultigradeclasses. Additionally, it was assumed that, in responding to items related to
developing the ability to calculate exact answers mentally beyond the basic facts,
respondents would be likely to indicate their teaching practices with respect to the
highest year level taught. Classroom teachers and teaching principals of multi-age
classes were therefore classified by the highest year level taught and categorised as
a lower-school (Years 1 to 3), middle-school (Years 4 and 5) or upper-school (Years
6 and 7) teacher. Teachers of single classes were classified similarly. For the
purposes of the presentation of data from Section 2.1 (see Table 4.12) and their
analysis, only data from middle- and upper-school teachers, with respect to current
teaching practices, were considered.
To gain insights into the degree to which current teaching practices of middleand upper-school teachers reflect a nontraditional approach, means for items in
Section 2 that referred to specific teaching practices were calculated and presented
on a continuum (Figure 4.4). The coding for items reflecting traditional teaching
practices was reversed, namely Never (4), Seldom (3), Sometimes (2), and Often
(1). Items 25, 26, 29, 30, 31, 33, 34, and 36 (see Appendix C) were selected for
analysis, with Items 29, 30, 33, 34, and 36 being recoded (see Appendix E).
Section 3 of the survey instrument was designed to elicit opinions and past
teaching practices related to mental computation within the context of Queensland
mathematics syllabuses in use between 1964 and 1987 (see Table 4.1). As
previously intimated, the Program in Mathematics for Primary Schools (Department
of Education, 1966-1968) progressively replaced the 1964 Syllabus during the late

271

1960s, with the program for Grades 6 and 7 being introduced in 1969. This syllabus
was revised in 1975 and remained in use until 1987 when the Years 1 to 10
Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a) became the basis on
which mathematics teaching was to be planned for Queensland primary school
pupils. Hence, the time periods used were: 1964-1968, 1969-1974, and 1975-1987.
As the purpose of this section was to gain insights from school personnel who
had teaching experience in Queensland classrooms during each of these three
periods, responses were selected for analysis on the basis of their length of service
(Item 49): (a) for the 1964 Syllabus, teaching for more than 25 years; (b) for the first
edition of the Program in Mathematics (Department of Education, 1966-1968),
teaching for more than 20 years; and (c) for the 1975 edition of the program,
teaching for six years or more. It is recognised that this procedure may have
resulted in some loss of data. It is possible that school personnel, with noncontiguous teaching careers, may have taught during one or more of these periods
but not have a length of service matching the selection criteria. However, an
advantage of applying these criteria is the exclusion of responses from teachers
who did not teach during any of the periods under investigation. An analysis of the
data revealed that at least two class teachers with less than one year's experience
responded to this section of the survey.
The mean for each item in Section 3.2 of the questionnaire was calculated to
provide a procedure to gain insights into the degree to which teaching approaches
classified as nontraditional were in use during each of the periods under
investigation, namely 1964-1968, 1969-1974, and 1975-1987. Items 43 to 46, which
reflect traditional teaching methods, were recoded as: Never (4), Seldom (3),
Sometimes (2) and Often (1). The means for Items 41 to 46 were plotted on
continua (Figure 4.5) to provide graphical representations of the data (see Appendix
E).

4.3.2 Survey Results


Prior to presenting the data related to each of the sections of the questionnaire,
analyses of the patterns of response and non-response have been undertaken.
Such analyses are critical to the validity of the study's findings and to their
generalisability.

272

Response Rate
Survey forms were received from 49 of the 115 schools in the sample, which
constitutes a 42.6% response rate (see Table 4.3). Thirty-nine (39) of the 52
schools nominating Contact Persons, and 10 schools which did not reply to the
initial letter, returned questionnaires. As described previously, copies of the survey
instrument were sent to the 36 schools which did not reply to the initial invitation to
participate in the survey.
An analysis of the response rate of schools by band of school and educational
region is provided in Table 4.4. Two (2) of the 11 trial schools for Student
Performance Standards in mathematics, one from each of the Northern and North
Western regions, returned questionnaires; three questionnaires (5.1% of those
dispatched) were returned for analysis. In response to the initial letter, five trial
schools indicated that they did not wish to receive the survey instrument. Fifty-nine
(59) questionnaires were sent to the remaining six schools in the sample. This low
response rate precludes a meaningful analysis of data with respect to school
personnel who were involved with the trial of the Student Performance Standards.
Table 4.3
Schools Returning Questionnaires
Number of schools in sample:
Schools refusing to participate in survey:

115
27

Schools requesting questionnaire after initial letter:


Schools returning questionnaires:

52
39

75.0%

Schools sent questionnaires in second mailing:


Schools returning questionnaires:

36
10

27.7%

TOTAL:

49

42.6%

As revealed in Table 4.5, 201 questionnaires were returneda response rate of


33.6%. One hundred and seventy-one (171) questionnaires were returned from
schools with Contact Persons and 29 from schools sent questionnaires in the
second mailing. One (1) questionnaire remains uncategorised as the school from
which it was returned was not able to be identified.

273

The response rate for questionnaires by educational region and band of school
is presented in Table 4.6. Although the size of the sample of school personnel was
potentially about 1400, a two-stage reduction in sample size was in operation.
Firstly, not all schools in the sample chose to participateeither declining in
response to the initial letter or by not returning any of the questionnaires sent.
Secondly, not all teachers and administrators from each participating school chose
to complete and return the questionnaire. As de Vaus (1991) comments: "Although
we can ask the person who receives the mail questionnaire to pass it on to the
appropriate person, we cannot be sure that this happens" (p. 108); nor can we be
sure that the potential respondent is sufficiently interested to complete and return
the survey form.
Asterisks in Table 4.6 indicate cells from which no questionnaires were
received from schools selected as part of the sample26 of the 64 cells. However
an analysis of Table 4.6 reveals that the spread of questionnaires received does
encompass a range of geographic locations and sizes of schools. Despite this, an
analysis of the data by educational region or band of school is precluded, given the
magnitude of the row and column totals displayed in Table 4.6. Hoinville (1977,

274

Table 4.4
School Response Rate by Region and Band
Band of school

Region

Sunshine
Coast
Metropolitan
West

1
1

Metropolitan
East

10

1a
1b

3
3

2
2

3
1

3
1

4
3

3
1

5
2

Total
9
6
66.7
14
6
42.9

1
1

12
4
33.3

Darling
Downs

2
1

4
1

3
1

13
3
23.1

South
Western

2
2

1
1

7
3
42.9

2
1

3
3

2
1

1
1

Wide Bay

2
2

5
1

2
1

2
1

3
2

Capricornia

15
7
46.7

2
1

2
1c

2
1

Northern

10
3
30.0

North
Western

1
1

1
1c

5
2
40.0

Peninsula

1
1

2
1

3
1

1
1

2
1

11
5
45.5

2
1

1
1

2
2

24
8
33.3

17
5
29.4

16
6
37.5

28
13
46.4

13
4
30.8

4
3
75.0

South Coast

Total: Sent
Returned
Percent

Note.

13
10
76.9

11
6
54.5

8
4
56.0
115
49
42.6

Top line in each row: Number of schools in sample. bBottom line in each row:
Number of schools returning questionnaires. cTrial school for Student Performance
Standards in 1993.

275

Table 4.5
Analysis of Number of Questionnaires Returned
TOTAL questionnaires sent:

598

Number of questionnaires sent after initial mailing:


Number returned:

395
171

43.3%

Number of questionnaires sent in second mailing:


Number returned:

203
29

14.3%

Number returned of unknown origin:

TOTAL questionnaires returned:

201

33.6%

cited by de Vaus, 1991, p. 73), suggests that the smallest subgroup should have at
least 50 to 100 cases for a meaningful subgroup analysis.

Analysis of Nonresponse
From Table 4.5 it is apparent that the nonresponse rate for the return of
questionnaires was high (66.4%). Nonresponse may lead to an unacceptable
reduction in sample size, particularly when missing values are accounted for during
analysis, and to bias (de Vaus, 1991, p. 73). With respect to the former, the number
of valid cases for the belief Items ranged from a low of 186 for Item 15 to 201 for
Items 4 and 11 (see Appendix C). The mean number of valid cases was 196.4. For
Current Teaching Practices, the mean number of valid cases was 173.4, a lower
mean as a consequence of this section not being relevant to all respondents. Nine
non-teaching principals and six deputy principals returned questionnaires.
Additionally, some respondents who identified themselves as class teachers may
not have had responsibility for a class at the time of the survey's completion. The
number of valid cases relating to Current Teaching Practices ranged from 170 for
Item 38 to 176 for Items 25 and 26.
Except for Items 4 and 11, the number of valid cases for each item was less
than the optimum range200 to 1000 cases for populations of 10,000 or
moresuggested by Alreck and Settle (1985, p. 89). However, "the size of the
population from which...the sample [was drawn] is largely irrelevant for the

276

Table 4.6
Questionnaire Response Rate by Region and Band
Band of school
Region

Sunshine Coast
Metropolitan
West

1
1

Metropolitan
East

10

*a

4
*

14b
7c

51
16

31
26

9
*

6
1

19
4

48
28

19
*

8
*

16
2

51
12

Total
109
49
45.0
93
34
36.6

8
8

83
22
26.5

Darling
Downs

1
1

2
2

4
4

24
*

30
*

61
7
11.5

South
Western

2
2

2
*

2
*

5
*

8
*

8
1

27
3
11.1

Wide Bay

2
1

7
6

8
4

16
9

16
*

Capricornia

2
2

6
1

9
4

11
3

16
8

8
*

52
18
34.6

2
1

3
1

8
5

8
*

9
*

Northern

30
7
23.3

North
Western

1
1

2
*

5
2

Peninsula

1
1

3
3

6
1

6
5

16
3

9
*

17
8

9
4

South Coast

49
20
40.8

8
3
37.5
41
13
31.7
19
12

Unknown
Total:
Sent
Returned
Percent

Note.

45
24
53.3
1

12
10
83.3

31
14
45.2

49
18
36.7

95
30
31.6

261
77
29.5

114
31
27.2

36
20
55.6

598
201
33.6

Asterisk indicates cell in which no school in sample accepted/returned


questionnaire. bTop line in each row: Number of questionnaires sent. cBottom line
in each row: Number of questionnaires returned.

277

accuracy of the sample. It is the absolute size of the sample that is important" (de
Vaus, 1991, p. 71). A perusal of Tables 4.7 to 4.11 suggests that, for most items,
opinions and practices are skewed towards one end of the four-point Likert scale.
Assuming representativeness of the sample (see below), this suggests a measure
of homogeneity in the population of school personnel with respect to their beliefs
and practices. In such instances, smaller sample sizes produce similar degrees of
accuracy as compared to those required for samples from heterogeneous
populations. It is therefore considered reasonable to conclude that the numbers of
valid cases obtained are sufficient to allow meaningful conclusions to be drawn.
Henry (1990) suggests that "nonresponse creates a potential for nonsampling
bias that cannot be overlooked after the data has been collected" (p. 131).
However, in Alreck's and Settle's view (1985, p. 76), it is not possible to completely
eliminate such bias, and therefore some nonresponse bias needs to be tolerated.
One method for determining the potential impact of nonresponse on the conclusions
drawn involves detecting differences in the data obtained from questionnaires
received at different times. As Henry (1990) intimates, "no differences in the waves'
of responses can indicate that response bias is less likely. This assumes that late
responders may share characteristics with nonresponders" (p. 132).
A search for differences in response was restricted to items related to Beliefs
(Items 1 to 23) and Current Practices (Items 24 to 38). The receipt of
questionnaires approximated four waves of responses, namely survey instruments
identified as numbers 1 to 83 (Group 1), 84 to 129 (Group 2), 130 to 164 (Group 3)
and 165 to 201 (Group 4). A one-way analysis of variance, together with Bonferroni
multiple comparisons (Norusis, 1991, pp. 178-180), was undertaken to detect
differences among the grouped responses for each of Items 1 to 38. This method is
conservative in its approach to reducing the likelihood of a Type 1 error (Agresti &
Finlay, 1997, p. 447; Myers & Well, 1995, p. 181). For each pairwise comparison,
given that the survey responses were allocated to four groups, the observed
significance level was set at .05 6, or .008 approximately, for the difference to be
considered significant at the .05 significance level.
Table 4.7 reveals that statistically significant differences between pairs of
group means at the .05 level were detected for Items 13, 18 and 28, the first two

278

Table 4.7
Items for Which Significant Differences in Response were Observed, Based on Time
of Receipt
Item

Group Mean

F Ratio

F Prob

13. Strategies for calculating exact


answers mentally are best
developed through discussion
and explanation.

Grp 1: 3.07a
Grp 2: 3.16
Grp 3: 3.23
Grp 4: 3.41a

4.02

.01

18. Opportunities for children to


calculate exact answers
mentally need to be provided in
all relevant classroom activities.

Grp 1: 3.07b
Grp 2: 2.73b
Grp 3: 2.94
Grp 4: 3.06

2.94

.03

28. Teach particular mental


strategies and follow up with
practice examples.

Grp 1: 3.31
Grp 2: 3.56c
Grp 3: 3.13c
Grp 4: 3.34

2.47

.06

Note.

a,b,c

Pair of groups significantly different at the .05 level.

being belief statements and the latter being one of the Current Teaching Practices
to which teachers were asked to respond. For Item 13, statistically significant
differences at the .05 level were detected between Groups 1 and 4. Given the
progressive increase in group means (see Table 4.7), together with Henry's (1990,
p. 132) assumption that late responders may share characteristics with
nonresponders, it is possible that the latter may more strongly agree with this
statement. For Items 18 and 28, the means for Groups 1 and 2, and Groups 2 and
3, respectively, were significantly different at the .05 level. Given this pattern, the
opinion of nonresponders may not be markedly different to that of those who
responded to the questionnaire.
As statistically significant differences were detected for only 3 of the 38 items
tested, it seems reasonable to assume that the opinion of nonresponders is likely
not to be greatly different from that of respondents. Hence, although the
nonresponse rate was 66.4%, the data collected may be cautiously considered
indicative of the beliefs and practices of Queensland state primary school teachers.

279

Beliefs About Mental Computation and How it Should Be Taught


Queensland state primary school teachers and administrators agree that mental
computation is a legitimate goal of mathematics education, with only 6% disagreeing
and 0.5% strongly disagreeing with Item 1 (see Table 4.8). Complementing this
finding, is the view that the ability to calculate exact answers with paper-and-pencil
is not of more use outside the classroom than the ability to calculate mentally.
Approximately 87% of respondents did not agree with Item 7 (see Table 4.8).
However, with respect to the degree of importance on mental computation
embodied in the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Item 2), the opinion of school
personnel is divided. Approximately 51% of respondents believe that the syllabus
places little importance on mental computation, whereas 46.8% hold the opposing
view. This pattern was reversed for middle- and upper-school teachers, with
more52.8% believing that the current syllabus places little importance on mental
computation.
The responses to Items 4, 6, and 9 (see Table 4.9) reveal an agreement with
current beliefs about the role of mental computation with the respect to the
development of number sense and ingenious methods for manipulating numbers.
This is in contrast to the responses to Item 5 which indicate a relatively high
percentage of respondentsapproximately 37%believing that mentally calculating
exact answers involves applying rules by rote.
Only 8.5% of respondents disagreed with the proposition that mental
computation encourages children to devise ingenious computational short cuts (Item
4). Approximately 89% either agreed or strongly agreed that mental computation
helps children to gain an understanding of the relationships between numbers (Item
6). Most respondents93.1%believe that children who are proficient with mental
computation use non-standard mental strategies (Item 9). The responses for Item 8,
however, reveal some inconsistency with the data for Item 9. Although the
majority72.6%either disagree or strongly disagree with the

280

Table 4.8
Percentage of Responses Related to Beliefs About the Importance of Mental
Computation
Item
1.

2.

7.

Strongly
Disagree

Disagree

Agree

Strongly
Agree

Missing

The development of the


ability to calculate exact
answers mentally is a
legitimate goal of
mathematics education.

0.5

6.0

37.8

55.2

0.5

The Years 1 to 10
Mathematics Syllabus
places little importance
on the development of
the ability to calculate
exact answers mentally.

6.5
8.3a

44.8
38.0a

43.3
49.1a

3.5
3.7a

2.0
0.9a

24.4

62.7

9.5

1.5

2.0

The ability to calculate


exact answers with
paper-and-pencil is
more useful outside the
classroom than the
ability to calculate
mentally.

Note.

N = 201. aPercentage related to middle- and upper-school teachers (n = 108).

proposition that children should use the written algorithms for mental calculations
(Item 8), approximately 23% of respondents believe that children should use paperand-pencil algorithms when computing mentally (see Table 4.9).
In comparing the pattern of responses to Item 7 (see Table 4.8) and Item 3 (see
Table 4.9), the overall pattern of responses is similar. However, a greater
percentage of respondentsapproximately 23%consider written methods for
calculating to be superior to mental methods (Item 3). This, despite the strong
disagreement with the proposition that written methods are more useful outside the
classroom than are mental procedures (Item 7).
The mean responses for items relating to the beliefs of school personnel about
the nature of mental computation were: Item 32.89, Item 43.17, Item 52.63,
Item 63.17, Item 82.89, and Item 93.24 (see Appendix E for the standard
deviations of these means). The coding for Items 3, 5, and 8 was

281

Table 4.9
Percentage of Responses Related to Beliefs About the Nature of Mental
Computation

Item
3.

4.

5.

6.

8.

9.

Note.

Written methods of
calculating exact answers
are superior to mental
procedures.

Strongly
Disagree

Disagree

Agree

Strongly
Agree

Missing

16.4

56.7

18.9

4.0

4.0

8.5

65.7

25.9

50.2
52.8a

30.8
33.3a

6.5
5.6a

3.5
0.9a

10.0

61.2

27.4

1.5

58.2
57.4a

21.9
24.1a

1.5
1.9a

4.0
1.9a

6.0

63.2

29.9

1.0

Mental computation
encourages children to
devise ingenious
computational short cuts.
Calculating exact answers
mentally involves applying
rules by rote.

9.0
7.4a

Mental computation helps


children gain an
understanding of the
relationships between
numbers.
Children should use the
algorithms for written
computation when
calculating exact answers
mentally.
Children who are proficient
at mentally calculating exact
answers use personal
adaptations of written
algorithms and idiosyncratic
mental strategies.

14.4
14.8a

N = 201. aPercentage related to middle- and upper-school teachers (n = 108).

reversed to facilitate their placement on a traditional-nontraditional continuum


(Figure 4.1). The data suggest that Queensland state school personnel tend
towards holding nontraditional beliefs about the nature of mental computation.
As discussed in Chapter 2, mathematics educators believe that consideration
should be given to focussing on mental computation prior to written computation
(Biggs, 1969, p. 25; Carroll, 1996, p. 35; Cooper et al., 1992, pp. 100-101; Musser,
1982, p. 40; Rathmell & Trafton, 1990, p. 156). However, approximately 65% of

282

Traditional
Strongly
Disagree
1

Nontraditional

Disagree

Strongly
Agree

Agree

2
3
4
||XX|XX|

Items:

3
8

4
6
9

Recoded items: 3, 5 and 8

Figure 4.1.

Position of means for items relating to the beliefs about the nature of
mental computation on a Traditional-Nontraditional continuum

Queensland teachers and administrators either disagreed or strongly disagreed


with the proposition that the written algorithms should be delayed so that mental
strategies could be given increased attention (see Table 4.10, Item 10). The
responses to Items 12 and 13 reveal that there is general agreement with the view
that mental strategies need to be specifically taught83.1%and that such
strategies are best developed through discussion and explanation93.5%.
However, approximately 14% of respondents did not believe that specific teaching of
strategies was necessary (Item 12).
This finding is complemented by data for Item 18 (see Table 4.10).
Approximately 20% of respondents believe that opportunities for developing the
ability to compute mentally should not be provided in all relevant classroom
activities. However, 78.1% do support the integration of mental computation with
other classroom experiences. A similar pattern of data is observed for Item 23 (see
Table 4.10). The majority of respondents (77.6%) support the use of a series of
focus lessons each week, whereas 19.4% do not agree with this approach.
Paralleling the finding for Item 9 (see Table 4.9), which suggests a belief that
proficient mental calculators use personal mental strategies, the data for Item 14
(see Table 4.10) indicate that school personnel believe that children should be
allowed to develop and use their own mental strategies63.2% agreed and 29.9%
strongly agreed with this proposition.

283

Table 4.10
Percentage of Responses Related to Beliefs About the General Approach to
Teaching Mental Computation

Item
10. Emphasis on written
algorithms needs to be
delayed so that mental
computation can be given
increased attention.

Strongly
Disagree

Disagree

Agree

Strongly
Agree

Missing

5.5

59.7

25.9

5.5

3.5

14.4

57.2

25.9

2.5

5.0

70.6

22.9

1.5

6.0
a
3.7

63.2
63.9a

29.9
30.6a

1.0
1.9a

17.9

60.2

17.9

2.5

19.4

60.2

17.4

3.0

12. Strategies for calculating


exact answers mentally
need to be specifically
taught.
13. Strategies for calculating
exact answers mentally are
best developed through
discussion and
explanation.
14. Children need to be
allowed to develop and use
their own strategies for
calculating exact answers
mentally.
18. Opportunities for children
to calculate exact answers
mentally need to be
provided in all relevant
classroom activities.
23. A series of session that
focuses on developing
strategies for computing
exact answers mentally
needs to be conducted
each week.
Note.

1.5

N = 201. aPercentage related to middle- and upper-school teachers (n = 108).

The mean responses for three of the items listed in Table 4.10 that may be
considered on a Traditional-Nontraditional continuum were: Item 102.32, Item
133.18, and Item 143.24 (See Appendix E). These are presented in Figure 4.2
and indicate that, except in relation to delaying the focus on written algorithms,

284

Traditional

Nontraditional

Strongly
Disagree
1

Disagree

Agree

Strongly
Agree

2
3
4
||X|XX|

Items:

Figure 4.2:

10

13
14

Position of means for items relating to beliefs about the general


approach to teaching mental computation on a TraditionalNontraditional continuum.

school personnel generally agree with propositions that reflect a nontraditional


approach to developing the ability to calculate exact answers mentally.
Table 4.11 reveals some inconsistencies in the beliefs of teachers and
administrators about how mental computation should be taught. Although no
respondent disagreed with the proposition that children should be given
opportunities to discuss, compare, and refine their mental strategies (Item 19), there
was sizeable support for key aspects of the traditional approach to calculating exact
answers mentally in which the focus is on testing rather than teaching.
Approximately 67% of respondents believe that children's mental processes are
sharpened by starting a mathematics lesson with 10 quick questions (Item 15). This
finding is supported by data for Item 16 which reveal that 57.2% agree and 7.5%
strongly agree with the belief that children are encouraged to think about
mathematics right from the start of a lesson when given 10 quick questions to solve
mentally. Further, 47.7% of respondents believe that answers to the 10 quick
questions need to be corrected quickly so that the mathematics lesson can begin
(Item 17). Somewhat contrary to the latter finding, the responses to Item 20 indicate
that the majority of respondents85.1%believe that the focus should not be on the
correctness of the answer during mental computation sessions, but on the mental
strategies used.
There is general agreement that teachers need to be aware of the strategies
used by those proficient at calculating exact answers mentally (Item 11)59.7% of

285

Table 4.11
Percentage of Responses Related to Beliefs About Issues Associated with
Developing the Ability to Calculate Exact Answers Mentally

Item

Strongly
Disagree

Disagree

Agree

Strongly
Agree

4.5

59.7

35.8

2.5

22.9

54.7

12.4

7.5

3.5

24.9

57.2

7.5

7.0

6.0
a
6.5

42.8
41.7a

37.3
40.7a

10.4
9.3a

3.5
1.9a

49.8
a
53.7

49.8
46.3a

0.5

10.0
9.3a

3.0
3.7a

2.0
2.8a

11. Teachers need to be aware


of the strategies used by
those who are proficient at
calculating exact answers
mentally.
15. Children's mental
processes are sharpened
by starting a mathematics
lesson with ten quick
questions to be solved
mentally.
16. Children are encouraged to
think about mathematics
right from the start of a
lesson when given ten
quick questions to solve
mentally.
17. Answers obtained for the
"ten quick questions" need
to be corrected quickly so
that the mathematics
lesson can begin.
19. Children should be given
opportunities to discuss,
compare and refine their
mental strategies for
solving particular mental
problems.
20. During mental computation
sessions, the focus should
be on the correctness of
the answer rather than on
the mental strategies used.
Note.

21.9
14.8a

63.2
69.4a

Missing

N = 201 aPercentage related to middle- and upper-school teachers (n = 108)

286

Table 4.11 cont.


Percentage of Responses Related to Beliefs About Issues Associated with
Developing the Ability to Calculate Exact Answers Mentally

Item
21. During mental
computation sessions,
one approach to each of a
number of problems
should be the focus,
rather than on several
approaches to each of a
few problems.
22. Children should be
encouraged to build on
the thinking strategies
used to develop the basic
facts.

Note.

Strongly
Disagree

Disagree

Agree

Strongly
Agree

Missing

18.9

52.7

23.4

1.5

3.5

60.7
71.3a

38.3
27.8a

0.5

0.5
0.9a

N = 201. aPercentage related to middle- and upper-school teachers. (n = 108)

respondents agreed and 35.8% strongly agreed (see Table 4.11). Ninety-nine
percent (99%) of respondents believe that children should be encouraged to build
on the thinking strategies used to develop the basic facts (Item 22). Complementing
the findings for Item 23 (see Table 4.10), 52.7% disagreed and 18.9% strongly
disagreed with the suggestion that it was better to focus on one approach to a
number of problems rather than focus on several approaches to each of a few
problems (see Table 4.11, Item 21).
Figure 4.3 presents a graphical representation of the means for the items listed
in Table 4.11, with the right-hand side of the continuumStrongly Agreereflecting a
nontraditional approach to the issues raised. The means for each of the items were:
Item 113.31, Item 152.16, Item 162.26, Item 172.45, Item 193.50, Item
203.06, Item 212.92, and Item 223.37 (see Appendix E). Items 15, 16, 17, 20,
and 21 were recoded to facilitate their placement on the continuum. The data
suggest that, except for issues related to introducing a mathematics lesson by giving
10 quick questions (Items 15 to 17), there is some agreement with constructivist
approaches to developing skill with mental computationthat is, children should be
given opportunities for discussing, comparing, and refining their strategies (Item 19),

287

and that their idiosyncratic strategies should be built upon those used to develop
basic fact knowledge (Item 22).

Current Teaching Practices


Of the 201 respondents, 164 were class teachers and 19 were teaching
principals. Fifty-seven (57) of these 183 teachers were teaching multi-age classes
at the time of the survey. One hundred and eight (108) respondents were teachers
of Years 4 to 7 47 middle-school and 61 upper-school teachers.
The data presented in Table 4.12 indicate that mental computation is focussed upon
to a varying degree in the classrooms of the majority of the middle- and upperschool teachers surveyed. Approximately 34% of teachers indicated that they often
focus on developing the ability to calculate exact answers beyond the basic facts. A
further 54.6% sometimes focus specifically on developing this ability (Item 24).
Thirty-eight percent (38%) of teachers often teach particular mental strategies and
follow up with practice examples (Item 28).

Traditional
Strongly
Disagree
1

Nontraditional

Disagree

Strongly
Agree

Agree

||XXXX|XXX|
Items:

15

17
16

20
21

11 19
22

Recoded items: 15, 16, 17, 20, 21

Figure 4.3.

Position of means for items relating to beliefs about specific issues


associated with developing mental computation skills on a TraditionalNontraditional continuum.

288

Table 4.12
Percentage of Responses Related to Current Teaching Practices for Developing the
Ability to Compute Mentally for Middle- and Upper School Teachers
Item

Never

24. Focus specifically on


developing the ability to
calculate exact answers
mentally beyond the basic
facts.
25. Allow children to decide
the method to be used to
arrive at an exact answer
mentally.
26. Allow children to explain
and discuss their mental
strategies for solving a
problem.
27. Allow children to work
mentally during practice of
written computation.

0.9

28. Teach particular mental


strategies and follow up
with practice examples.
29. Give several one-step
questions and simply mark
answers as correct or
incorrect.

4.6

30. Require answers to


problems solved mentally
to be recorded on paper.
31. Relate methods for
calculating beyond the
basic facts to the thinking
strategies used to develop
the basic facts.
32. Use mental computation
for revising and practising
arithmetic facts and
procedures.
33. Emphasise speed when
calculating exact answers
mentally.

Note. N = 108.

0.9

Seldom

Sometimes

Often

Missing

6.5

54.6

34.4

4.6

2.8

37.0

56.5

3.7

5.6

37.0

53.7

3.7

3.7

50.0

39.8

5.6

11.1

46.3

38.0

4.6

32.4

47.2

12.0

3.7

10.2

56.5

29.6

3.7

7.4

50.0

38.0

4.6

3.7

48.1

43.5

4.6

27.8

50.0

16.7

4.6

289

Table 4.12 cont.


Percentage of Responses Related to Current Teaching Practices for Developing the
Ability to Compute Mentally for Middle- and Upper School Teachers
Item

Never

Seldom

Sometimes

Often

Missing

34. Insist on children using the


procedures for the written
algorithms when
calculating exact answers
mentally.

38.0

38.9

18.5

0.9

3.7

0.9

14.8

49.1

30.6

4.6

1.9

5.6

40.7

47.2

4.6

17.6

26.9

35.2

15.7

4.6

12.0

47.2

34.3

6.5

35. Provide opportunities for


children to appreciate how
often they and adults use
mental computation.
36. Teach rules for calculating
exact answers mentally
(e.g. divide by ten by
removing a zero).
37. Have children commit to
memory number facts
beyond the basic facts.
38. Use examples involving
measures or spatial
concepts in problems to be
calculated mentally.

Note. N = 108.

A further 46.3% sometimes employ this approach to develop the ability to calculate
exact answers mentally.
Approximately equal percentages of teachers allow children to decide the
method of calculation (Item 25) and to explain and discuss their mental strategies
(Item 26). Thirty-seven percent (37%) of teachers indicated that they sometimes
use both of these teaching approaches, whereas 56.5% and 53.7%, respectively,
often use these two approaches. The data for Item 25 is complemented by that for
Item 34 (see Table 12). These reveal that, whereas 18.5% of teachers of middleand upper year levels sometimes insist on children using the written algorithm for
calculating exact answers mentally, the majority rarely place this requirement on
their pupils38.9% seldom have children use the written algorithm and 38% never
insist on the use of this strategy. Nonetheless, the data for Item 36 (see Table 12)
indicate that many teachers still teach rules as a means for developing the ability to
calculate exact answers mentally. Only 1.9% of teachers indicated that this

290

approach was never used, with an additional 5.6% indicating that teaching rules for
mental computation is undertaken infrequently.
Approximately 90% of teachers allow children to work mentally during practice
of written computation (see Table 12, Item 27). This unexpectedly high percentage
suggests that many respondents may not have interpreted this statement as
meaning that written answers may not always be required for children who are able
to mentally calculate the operations planned as practice for the paper-and-pencil
algorithms. The prime focus of the number strand of the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics
Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a) is on developing the ability of pupils to
calculate using the standard written algorithm for each operation.
Although 32.4% of teachers seldom give several one-step questions and simply
mark the answers as correct or incorrect (Item 29), 47.2% sometimes, and 12%
often, use this approach. Analogous to these findings, 27.8% of teachers of middleand upper-school classes seldom emphasise speed during mental computation,
whereas 16.7% often do so and 50% sometimes place this emphasis during mental
computation sessions (Item 33). Most teachers require answers to problems solved
mentally to be recorded on paper (Item 30)56.5% sometimes require written
answers, and 29.6% often emphasise this requirement.
The responses to Item 31 reveal that an approach regularly used to develop
mental proficiency is to ensure that the thinking strategies used to support basic fact
development are used as a basis for calculating beyond the basic facts (Item 31).
Fifty percent (50%) sometimes use this approach and 38% often relate strategies for
calculating beyond the basic facts to the thinking strategies used to support their
development. Many teachersapproximately 16% often encourage children to
commit to memory number facts beyond the basic facts (Item 37). This occurs
sometimes in the classrooms of 35.2% of middle- and upper-school teachers.
However, 17.6% of teachers never require children to commit such facts to memory.
When encouraging the development of the ability to calculate mentally, teachers
often integrate the development of strategies for calculating exact answers mentally
with other areas of mathematics (Items 32 & 38). Approximately 48% of teachers
sometimes use mental computation as a means for revising and practising
arithmetic facts and procedures (Item 32). No teacher indicated that this was a
procedure that they never used. Further, the data for Item 38 reveal that 47.2% of
teachers sometimes incorporate spatial and measurement concepts in problems to
be calculated mentally. Opportunities are also provided for children to appreciate
how often they and adults use mental computation (Item 35), with approximately

291

31% of teachers often endeavouring to lead children to an appreciation of the


usefulness of mental computation.
Of the items listed in Table 4.12, Items 25, 26, 29, 30, 31, 33, 34, and 36 relate
to specific teaching practices which reflect either a traditional or nontraditional
approach. The means for middle- and upper-school teachers for these items, after
recoding Items 29, 30, 33, 34, and 36 to provide a nontraditional orientation, were:
Item 253.55, Item 263.50, Item 292.30, Item 301.79, Item 313.32, Item
332.13, Item 343.18, and Item 361.60 (see Appendix E). Figure 4.4 presents
these means on a traditional-nontraditional continuum. This graphical
representation suggests that both traditional and nontraditional teaching strategies
for developing the ability to calculate exact answers mentally are currently being
used by middle- and upper-school teachers.

Traditional

Nontraditional

Never

Seldom

Sometimes

Often

|XX|XX|XXXX|
Items:

36 30

33

34
29

25
31

26

Recoded Items: 29, 30, 33, 34, 36

Figure 4.4.

Position of means for selected current teaching practices related to


developing mental computation skills on a traditional-nontraditional
continuum.

Past Teaching Practices


The application of the selection criteria outlined in the Methods of Analysis
section resulted in data being obtained for Section 3 of the survey instrument (see
Appendix C) from 32 teachers who had experience teaching the 1964 Syllabus.
Fifty-three (53) taught during the period 1969-1975 and 161 teachers taught using
the 1975 edition of the Program in Mathematics (Department of Education, 1975).
Table 4.13 presents the percentage of responses with respect to the
importance placed on mental computationmental arithmeticby each syllabus and
by teachers who taught during each of the syllabus periods. Section 3 of the survey

292

instrument required that respondents be able to respond to particular items based


on their recollections. Hence, for this analysis, responses classified as unsure have
been recoded as missing.
The data for Item 39 (see Table 4.13) reveal that teachers perceive that the
1964 syllabus placed a greater emphasis on mental arithmetic that did either edition
of the Program in Mathematics (Department of Education, 1966-1968, 1975). The
majority, 88.5% of valid responses, view the 1964 Syllabus as having placed great
importance on the ability to calculate exact answers mentally. This compares with
72.2% and 70.8% of valid responses believing that the first and second editions of
the Program in Mathematics (Department of Education, 1966-1968, 1975),
respectively, placed some importance on mental arithmetic. In contrast to Item 39a,
the responses to Items 39b and 39c reveal that some teachers considered that both
editions of the Program in Mathematics (Department of Education, 1966-1968,
1975) placed little importance on mental arithmetic13.9% and 13.5% of valid
responses, respectively.
The pattern of results for Item 40a (see Table 4.13) is similar to that for Item
39a. The majority of teachers81.5% of valid casesindicated that they placed
great importance on developing the ability to calculate exact answers mentally
during the period 1964-1968. In contrast to the data for Items 39b and 39c, many
teachers continued to place great importance on mental arithmetic during each of
the periods 1969-1974 and 1975-1987. Sixty-one percent (61%) of valid responses
indicated great importance during 1969-1974 and 47.7% during 1975-1987.
However, 45.8% placed some importance during 1975-1987 compared to

293

Table 4.13
Percentage of Responses Related to Past Beliefs About Mental Computation
Degree of Importance
Item

None

Little

Some

Great

Unsure

Missing

39. For each period, how important did the syllabus consider the ability to
calculate exact answers mentally?
(a) 1964a - 1968 (1964 Syllabus)
(b) 1969b - 1974 (PIM: 1st ed)
(c) 1975c - 1987 (PIM: 2nd ed)

1.2
2.2

9.4d
11.5e

71.9
88.5

6.3

18.8

9.4
13.9

49.1
72.2

9.4
13.9

13.2

18.9

7.5
13.5

39.1
70.8

7.5
13.5

10.6

34.2

40. For each period, how important did you consider the ability to calculate exact
answers mentally?
(a) 1964a - 1968 (1964 Syllabus)

15.6
18.5

68.8
81.5

3.1

12.5

(b) 1969b - 1974 (PIM: 1st ed)

1.9
2.4

28.3
36.6

47.2
61.0

1.9

20.8

(c) 1975c - 1987 (PIM: 2nd ed)

4.3
6.5

30.4
45.8

31.7
47.7

.6

32.9

Note.

1964 Syllabus: N = 32. b1968 Syllabus: N = 53. c1975 Syllabus: N = 161. dTop
line in each row: Percentage of number of cases. eBottom line in each row:
Percentage of valid cases, excluding unsure and missing categories.

36.6% during 1969-1974. Taking the percentage of teachers who placed some or
great importance on mental arithmetic, the data suggest that teachers may have
gradually placed less importance on this topic during these periods.
The data for Item 41 (see Table 4.14) reveal greater percentages of
respondents allowing children to decide the method for calculating exact answers
mentally during the periods 1969-1974 and 1975-1987 than during 1964-1968,
when the percentages for sometimes and often are considered together. This
teaching practice was never used under the 1964 syllabus by 16% of valid cases
compared to 5.3% and 1.9% under the first and second editions, respectively, of the
Program in Mathematics (Department of Education, 1966-1968, 1975).

294

Table 4.14
Percentage of Responses Concerning Past Teaching Practices Related to Mental
Computation
Item

Never

Seldom

Sometimes

Often

Unsure

Missing

41. Allowed children to decide the method to be used to arrive at an exact answer.
(a) 1964a - 1968 (1964 Syllabus)

12.5d
16.0e

15.6
20.0

18.8
24.0

31.3
40.0

3.1

18.8

(b) 1969b - 1974 (PIM: 1st ed)

3.8
5.3

3.8
5.3

39.6
55.3

24.5
34.2

7.5

20.8

(c) 1975c - 1987 (PIM: 2nd ed)

1.2
1.9

6.2
9.5

28.6
43.8

29.2
44.8

3.1

31.7

42. Allowed children to explain and discuss their mental strategies for solving a
problem.
(a) 1964a - 1968 (1964 Syllabus)

9.4
12.0

18.8
24.0

18.8
24.0

31.3
40.0

21.9

(b) 1969b - 1974 (PIM: 1st ed)

3.8
5.0

7.5
10.0

35.8
47.5

28.3
37.5

3.8

20.8

(c) 1975c - 1987 (PIM: 2nd ed)

1.9
2.7

8.1
11.7

28.0
40.5

31.1
45.0

.6

30.4

43. Gave several one-step questions and simply marked the answers as correct or
incorrect.
(a) 1964a - 1968 (1964 Syllabus)

3.1
4.0

15.6
20.0

15.6
20.0

43.8
56.0

21.9

(b) 1969b - 1974 (PIM: 1st ed)

17.0
22.5

35.8
47.5

22.6
30.0

3.7

20.8

(c) 1975c - 1987 (PIM: 2nd ed)

13.7
19.8

34.2
49.5

21.1
30.6

.5

30.6

44. Emphasised speed when calculating exact answers mentally.


6.3
8.3

18.8
25.0

46.9
62.5

25.0

(b) 1969b - 1974 (PIM: 1st ed)

7.5
10.3

35.8
48.7

30.2
41.0

3.8

22.6

(c) 1975c - 1987 (PIM: 2nd ed)

13.0
19.6

31.1
46.7

22.4
33.6

.6

32.9

(a) 1964a - 1968 (1964 Syllabus)

Note.

3.1
4.2

1964 Syllabus: N = 32. b1968 Syllabus: N = 53. c1975 Syllabus: N = 161. dTop
line in each row: Percentage of number of cases. eBottom line in each row:
Percentage of valid cases, excluding unsure and missing categories.

295

Table 4.14 cont.


Percentage of Responses Concerning Past Teaching Practices Related to Mental
Computation
Item

Never

Seldom

Sometimes

Often

Unsure

Missing

45. Insisted that children use the procedures for the written algorithms when calculating exact
answers mentally.
(a) 1964a - 1968 (1964 Syllabus)

18.8
25.0e

21.9
29.2

18.8
25.0

15.6
20.8

3.1

18.8

b
(b) 1969 - 1974 (PIM: 1st ed)

15.1
20.5

30.2
41.0

22.6
30.8

5.7
7.7

5.7

20.8

c
(c) 1975 - 1987 (PIM: 2nd ed)

14.3
21.9

24.8
38.1

18.0
27.6

8.1
12.4

3.7

31.1

46. Placed an emphasis on teaching rules for calculating exact answers mentally
(e.g. divide by ten by removing a zero).
a
(a) 1964 - 1968 (1964 Syllabus)

3.1
4.2

28.1
37.5

43.8
58.3

3.1

21.9

b
(b) 1969 - 1974 (PIM: 1st ed)

1.9
2.5

37.7
50.0

35.8
47.5

3.8

20.8

4.3
6.6

36.0
54.7

24.2
36.8

1.9

32.3

c
(c) 1975 - 1987 (PIM: 2nd ed)

Note.

1.2
1.9

1964 Syllabus: N = 32. b1968 Syllabus: N = 53. c1975 Syllabus: N = 161. dTop
line in each row: Percentage of number of cases. eBottom line in each row:
Percentage of valid cases, excluding unsure and missing categories.

However, the difference between the percentage of respondents who often used
this approach during 1964-1968 and 1975-1987 is not marked, being 40% and
44.8% of valid responses, respectively.
The data for Item 42 follows a similar pattern to those for Item 41. Most
respondents, for each period, indicated that they sometimes or often allowed
children to explain and discuss their mental strategies for solving a problem (see
Table 4.14). However, the percentage of respondents indicating that they never or
seldom used this approach decreased under both editions of the Program in
Mathematics (Department of Education, 1966-1968, 1975). The percentage of
teachers who never used this approach decreased from 12% for the period 19641968 to 2.7% of valid responses under the 1975 edition.
Items 43 to 46 (see Table 4.14) reflect teaching practices classified as
traditional in this study. Although 56% of valid responses indicate that teachers
often gave several one-step questions and simply marked the answers as correct or

296

incorrect (Item 43) during the period 1964-1968, this had decreased to 30% and
30.6% for the periods 1968-1974 and 1975-1987, respectively. This decrease was
accompanied by a concomitant increase in the number of respondents who
sometimes used this approachfrom 20% of valid cases under the 1964 syllabus to
49.5% for the 1975 Syllabus.
The emphasis on speed when calculating exact answers mentally (Item 44)
decreased during the periods under investigation. An analysis of the percentage of
valid cases indicates that 62.5% of respondents often emphasised speed under the
1964 syllabus (see Table 4.14). This had decreased to 33.6% during the period
1975-1987 and was accompanied by an increase, from 8.3% (Item 44a) to 19.6%
(Item 44c) of valid cases, in the number of respondents who indicated that this
emphasis was seldom applied.
Although the data for Item 45 (see Table 4.14) reveal that many teachers
insisted on the use of the procedures for written algorithms, the majority either never
or seldom placed such an expectation on their pupils in each of the three periods of
interest. Whereas the percentage of valid cases indicating that they sometimes
insisted on the use of written procedures remained relatively constant25%, 30.8%,
and 27.6%, respectivelythe percentage who often placed this expectation on
children decreased from 20.8% under the 1964 syllabus to 12.4% during the period
of the second edition of the Program in Mathematics (Department of Education,
1975).
The emphasis placed on teaching rules for calculating exact answers mentally
decreased slightly during the three periods under investigation. However, it
remained an approach used by the majority of teachers. An analysis of data for
Item 46 (see Table 4.14) indicates that whereas the percentage of teachers who
often used this approach decreased from 58.3% to 36.8%, the percentage who
sometimes taught rules to calculate mentally increased from 37.5% to 54.7% for the
periods 1964-1968 and 1975-1987 respectively.
Figure 4.5 presents a graphical representation, on a traditional-nontraditional
continuum, of the means for the teaching practices focussed upon in Section 3.2 of

297

Traditional

Nontraditional

Never

Seldom

Sometimes

Often

1964 - 1968
1

2
3
4
|XXX|XX||

Items:

46a

45a

43a
44a

41a
42a

1969 - 1974
1

2
3
4
|XXX|X|XX|

Items

46b
44b

43b

45b

41b
42b

1975 - 1987
1

2
3
4
|XXX|X|XX|

Items:

46c

43c
44c

45c

41c
42c

Recoded items: 43a-46a, 43b-46b, 43c-46c

Figure 4.5.

Position of means for items relating to teaching practices used during


the periods 1964-1968, 1969-1974, 1975-1987 on TraditionalNontraditional continua.

the survey instrument (see Appendix C). Following the recoding of Items 43 to 46 to
reflect a nontraditional orientation, the means for the period 1964-1968 were: Item
41a2.83, Item 42a2.87, Item 43a1.72, Item 44a1.56, Item 45a2.58, and Item
46a1.46. Those for 1969-1974 were: Item 41b3.18, Item 42b3.17, Item
43b1.92, Item 44b1.69, Item 45b2.74, and Item 46b1.55. For the third period
investigated, 1975-1987, the means were: Item 41c3.31, Item 42c3.27, Item
43c1.89, Item 44c1.86, Item 45c2.69, and Item 46c1.73 (see Appendix E).
The data suggest that during 1969-1974 there was a slight increase in the use
of teaching strategies that allowed children to decide the method of calculation (Item
41b), and to discuss and explain the mental strategies used (Item 42b). This trend

298

appears to have continued from 1975 to 1987 (Items 41c & 42c, respectively) (see
Figure 4.5). Over the three periods investigated, there is also a suggestion that
there was a slight decrease, represented by movement of item means towards the
nontraditional end of the continuum, in the use of three of the teaching approaches
classified as traditional, namely giving several one-step questions (Item 43),
emphasising speed (Item 44), and teaching rules (Item 46). With respect to the
remaining traditional approachusing the written algorithm to calculate mentally
(Item 45)little change in its use appears to have occurred across the three time
periods.

Inservice on Mental Computation


Items 54 to 56 relate to teacher inservice on mental computation. An analysis
of Table 4.15, which presents data with respect to the need for inservice (Item 54)
and recent inservice attendance (Item 55), reveals a high percentage (57.7%) of
missing data for the Item 54. This occurred due to respondents, who were at
schools which were not trialing the Student Performance Standards in mathematics,
being incorrectly directed to jump from Item 52 to Item 55 when completing the
questionnaire (see Appendix C). However, of the 42.3% of school personnel who
responded to Item 54, 88.2% indicated that inservice should be made available to
teachers.
Only 14.4% of the respondents had attended inservice in which mental
computation was a specific topic for discussion during the three years prior to
survey (see Table 4.15). The majority of these 29 teachers (65.5%) had received
inservice from Departmental mathematics advisory teachers (see Table 4.16), the
number of whom has now been greatly reduced in all educational districts. A further
24.1% had attended inservice sessions conducted by tertiary lecturers. These
findings complement those for Item 2 (see Table 4.8). The divided opinion of school
personnel concerning the degree of importance placed on mental computation by
the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a) may be a
consequence of the lack of inservice opportunities and/or participation.

Table 4.15
Percentage of Responses Related to the Importance of and Participation in
Inservice Sessions on Mental Computation

299

Item
54. Do you consider it important
that inservice sessions on
mental computation be made
available to teachers.
55. Have you attended, during the
last three years, inservice
sessions in which Mental
Computation was a specific
topic for discussion.
Note.

Yes

No

Missing

37.3a
88.2b

5.0a
11.8b

57.7

14.4

84.1

1.5

N = 201 aPercentage of total number of cases. bPercentage of valid cases.

Table 4.16
Sourcea of Inservice on Mental Computation During Period 1991-1993

Colleague

Note.

Administrator

Mathematics
Adviser

Tertiary
Lecturer

Other

3.4%

65.5%

24.1%

6.9%

N = 29 aQuestion 56: If you have attended inservice on mental computation, who


conducted the inservice?

Textbooks Used to Develop Skill with Mental Computation


Fifty-six (56) of the 108 respondents classified as middle- and upper-school
teachers listed resources that they use to support current teaching practices related
to mental computation (Section 2.2 of the survey instrument). Some of these were
not able to be clearly identified owing to the inadequacy of bibliographic information.
For example, five respondents identified the resource simply as "Mental arithmetic,
a title used by a number of different authors. Another respondent indicated that
"various small Mental Arithmetic' books" were used.
Allowing for this ambiguity, 71 different resources were listed. An analysis of
Table 4.17 reveals that 18.3% of these resources related specifically to mental
computation (see Table 4.18 for a list of these resources). Resources to support
basic fact development accounted for 11.3% of those listed. These included Basic
Maths Facts kits (Department of Education, 1984), Nothing but the Facts (Baturo,

300

1988), and Maths in the Mind (Baker & Baker, 1991). The last text, however, is one
which allows for the extension of its activities to mental calculation beyond the basic
facts.
Of the remaining resources, 25.3% were categorised as supporting the
development of Problem Solving strategies and 24.0% were classified General
Mathematics Texts. A range of resources relating to problem solving were listed,
with no item being cited by more than one respondent. Those categorised as
resources to support problem solving included Lateral Thinking Puzzlers (Sloane,
1991) and Creative Problem Solving in School Mathematics (Lenchner, 1983).
Sixteen (16) respondents28.6%indicated that they used the Years 1 to 10
Mathematics Sourcebook (Department of Education, 1987-1990) relevant to the
year level being taught. Other general mathematics resources commonly used were
Rigby Moving Into Maths (Irons & Scales, 1982-1986) by 10.7% of respondents and
Sunshine Mathematics (Baturo & English, 1983-1985) by 12.5%.
Resources which did not fit into the four main categories presented in Table
4.17 were classified as Other (21.1%). This category included inservice that had
been attended, calculator activities, concrete materialsmultilink, for exampleand
more general resources such as Making the Most of 20 Minutes (Cain, 1989).
Twenty-nine (29) of the 161 school personnel who responded to Sections 3.1
and/or 3.2 of the survey instrument (see Appendix C), concerning past teaching
practices, listed resources that they had used during the period 1964-1987. Twentythree (23) different resources were identified. Thirteen percent (13%) of these were
classified as referring specifically to Basic Fact development (see Table 4.17), and
17.4% were classified as relating to mental computation (see Table 4.19). However,
the majority47.8%were General Mathematics Texts. The resources categorised
as Other21.7%were primarily items of mathematics equipment a bead frame, for
example. The basic facts resources listed included

301

Table 4.17
Categorisation of Resources Listed by Respondents in Sections 2.2 and 3.3 of the
Survey Instrument
Resource Categories
Teaching
Practices

Mental
Computation

Basic Facts

Problem
Solving

General
Texts

Other

Currenta

18.3%

11.3%

25.3%

24.0%

21.1%

Pastb

17.4%

13.0%

47.8%

21.7%

Note.

N(Current) = 72. bN(Past) = 23

Table 4.18
Textbooks Specific to Mental Computation Currently Used by Middle- and UpperSchool Teachers
Couchman, K. E., Jones, S. B., & Nay, W. (1992). Quick practice maths 2000 (Years 3-6).
Melbourne: Longman Cheshire.
Lewis, B. (1991). Mental arithmetic and problem solving (Years 3-7) (2nd ed.). Melbourne:
Longman Cheshire.
Nash, B., & Nightingale, P. (1992). Kookaburra mental activities (Books 1-6). Sydney:
Nightingale Press.
Parkes, A. A., Couchman, K. E., Jones, S. B., & Green, K. N. (1982). Betty and Jim mental
arithmetic (4th ed.). Sydney: Shakespeare Head Press.
Perrett, K., & Donlan, R. (1985). Breakthrough mental arithmetic (Years 3-6). Melbourne:
Methuen.
Perrett, K., & Whiting, E. (1972). The "Dux" series mental arithmetic (Books 1-6) (2nd ed.).
Melbourne: School Projects.
Petchell, D. L., & McDonald, M. K. (1980). Progress in mental arithmetic (Years 3-6).
Sydney: Primary Education Publications.

Basic Maths Facts (Oostenbroek, 1976) and an audio tape simply referred to as a
"tables tape. With respect to general texts, of the 29 respondents, 65.5% indicated
that they used texts published by William Brooks (Brisbane), particularly the
Mathematics Guide (Willadsen, 1970-1976). As one respondent commented, "Were
there any except Brooks [during this era]?"

302

Of the resources for which full bibliographic details were able to be determined
from the information provided by respondents, Table 4.18 presents those that are
currently being used to teach mental computation by middle- and upper-school
teachers. Six (6) texts relating to mental computation, listed by 10 respondents,
were not able to be definitively identified. The most commonly used text, by four
respondents, is that by Bob Lewis, Mental Arithmetic and Problem Solving (1991).
Except for two respondents, none indicated that they made use of more than one of
the resources identified.
Eight (8) of the 29 respondents (27.6%) who listed resources used under
previous syllabuses listed resources specific to mental computation (see Table
4.19). None of these respondents indicated that they used more than one of the
texts listed. Two listed Breakthrough Mental Arithmetic (Perrett & Donlan, 1985).

Table 4.19
Textbooks Specific to Mental Computation Used During the Period 1964-1987
Lewis, B. (1986). Mental arithmetic and problem solving (Years 3-7). Melbourne: Longman
Cheshire.
Perrett, K. & Donlan, R. (1985). Breakthrough mental arithmetic (Years 3-6). Melbourne:
Methuen.
Perrett, K., & Whiting, E. (1972). The "Dux" series mental arithmetic (Books 1-6) (2nd ed.).
Melbourne: School Projects.

4.3.3 Discussion
As stated in Section 4.3, the survey of Queensland state primary school
personnel was designed to provide insights into:

Their beliefs about mental computation.

The current status of mental computation within the classroom curriculum.

The pedagogical practices used to develop skill with mental computation,


both currently as well as under previous syllabuses.

Their inservice needs with respect to mental computation.

The resources used to support the teaching of mental computation.

303

Such insights enable the data from the analysis of the nature and role of mental
computation within Queensland state school mathematics syllabuses, as presented
in Chapter 3 and Section 4.2, to be extended to the present. The integration of the
survey data with that from Chapters 2 and 3 is specifically undertaken in Chapter 6.
The purpose of this section is to draw conclusions with respect to the survey
questions, as delineated in Section 4.3.1. These conclusions form the basis for the
extension of the historical analysis to the present.

Limitations of Findings
It was concluded previously in this chapter, that although the nonresponse rate
was 66.4%, the data collected may be cautiously considered indicative of the beliefs
and current practices of Queensland state primary school teachers (see Analysis of
Nonresponse). This conclusion was drawn from an analysis of data obtained from
the four waves of questionnaire return. It was also demonstrated that the numbers
of valid cases obtained were sufficient to allow meaningful conclusions to be drawn,
at least with respect to the beliefs and current practices of Queensland state primary
school teachers.
The data obtained from Section 3 of the survey instrumentPast Teaching
Practicescan only be considered suggestive of the beliefs and practices which
prevailed during the period 1964-1987, particularly given the relatively few
respondents (32) for the 1964-1968 era. Although it was intended that the data be
representative of the views of teachers who taught at any time during 1964-1987, it
was not possible, prior to the sample being drawn, to identify those teachers who
had taught under the three syllabuses of interest.
It is recognised that self-selection introduces bias into the resultant sample of
school personnel. Although this source of bias may have been unavoidable, it does
necessitate cautious generalisation of the conclusions drawn from the survey.
However, this caution should be viewed in context with the intent of the studythat
is, to describe and to gain insights, rather than to explain.

Conclusions
The contemporary beliefs of, and the teaching practices employed by,
Queensland state school personnel reflect aspects of traditional and nontraditional

304

approaches to mental computation. The view that mental computation helps to


sharpen the mind appears to be one that may be commonly held. As one upperschool teacher expounded, "It is my experience that by sharpening' the wit by
Mental Arithmetic practice the children are more prepared and focussed on the
tasks ahead. Often I use this strategy as an awakener during lessons."
Nonetheless, recognition is given to the importance of allowing children to develop
their own strategies for computing mentally: "To emphasise the process rather than
getting the correct answer" (Teaching Principal).
Insights into the relative balance between traditional and nontraditional beliefs
currently held by school personnel may be determined by considering research
Questions 1(a)(i), 1(a)(ii), and 1(a)(iii) (see Section 4.3.1). As Question 2(a) has
relevance to Question 1(a)(i), these two questions are analysed together.

Question 1(a)(i):

Is skill in mental computation considered an important goal of


mathematics education?

Question 2(a):

What emphasis is currently placed on developing the ability to


compute mentally?

The development of the ability to calculate exact answers mentally is seen as a


legitimate goal of school mathematics by 93% of school personnel who responded
to the survey (see Table 4.8, Item 1). That 89% of middle- and upper-school
teachers at least occasionally focus on mental computation skills (Item 24, Table
4.12) provides additional support for the conclusion that school personnel do
consider mental computation as a goal of some importance for school mathematics.
However, this conclusion needs to be tempered by the recognition that only
34.4% of middle- and upper-school teachers often focus specifically on developing
mental strategies for calculating exact answers beyond the basic facts (Item 24,
Table 4.12). This finding may be a consequence of the belief, by approximately
50% of school personnel, that the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department
of Education, 1987a) places little importance on the development of the ability to
compute exact answers mentally (Item 2, Table 4.8).
Some respondents provided anecdotal evidence of a belief in the need for an
increased emphasis on mental computation: "I think this is an area that needs
sharpening up on, and teaching in more depth and frequency" (Year 2 teacher).
However, some teachers appear to conceptualise mental computation as a separate
topic within the curriculum, rather than one which may be used as a means to

305

enhance and capitalise upon the links between various number, space and
measurement concepts. Representative of this view is the following comment by a
Year 6 teacher: "Although mental computation is important, it is only one of many
mathematical strategies [sic] within the Queensland syllabus. Too many topics
mean essential topics are not emphasised as much as they probably should."

Question 1(a)(ii): Do the beliefs about the nature of mental computation reflect a
nontraditional view?

Figure 4.1 suggests that Queensland state primary school personnel tend
towards holding a nontraditional view of mental computation, albeit one that does
not appear to be strongly held. Traditional elements remain in their beliefs,
particularly with respect to the use of rules for calculating mentally (Item 5).
Although 91.6% of school personnel support the view that mental computation
encourages children to devise ingenious computational short cuts (Item 4, Table
4.9), 37.5% believe that calculating exact answers mentally involves applying rules
by rote (Item 5, Table 4.9). Approximately 92% of respondents supported the belief
that proficient mental calculators use personal adaptations of written algorithms and
idiosyncratic strategies (Item 9, Table 4.9). Nevertheless, approximately 23% of
teachers agreed with the proposition that children should use the algorithm for
written computation when calculating exact answers mentally (Item 8, Table 4.9).

Question 1(a)(iii): Do the beliefs about how mental computation should be taught
reflect a nontraditional view?

Two categories of items are relevant to this question. The first consists of items
concerned with the general approach to teaching mental computation (Figure 4.2,
Table 4.10), whereas the second involves more specific issues (Figure 4.3, Table
4.11). With respect to the first category, it can be concluded that school personnel
tend towards holding nontraditional views (see Figure 4.2). Table 4.10 indicates
that most respondents agreed with all items in this category, except for that relating
to the need for an emphasis on the written algorithms to be delayed so that mental
computation can be given increased attention (Item 10). The focus of the number
strand of primary school mathematics under the 1987 Syllabus is the development
of the written algorithms. As discussed in Chapter 2, the traditional sequence for
introducing computational procedures involves a focus on mental computation

306

following, rather than preceding, the introduction of the paper-and-pencil algorithm


for each operation (Figure 2.3).
Approximately 93% of school personnel indicated support for the proposition
that strategies for calculating exact answers mentally are best developed through
discussion and explanation (Item 13, Table 4.10). A similar percentage of support is
evident for Item 14. This agreement with the belief that children should be allowed
to develop and use their own strategies for calculating exact answers mentally is
consistent with that expressed for Item 9 (see Table 4.9) concerning the use of
idiosyncratic strategies by proficient mental calculators.
The data displayed in Figure 4.3, concerning the second category of items,
which relates to more specific teaching practices, provides additional evidence for
the coexistence of traditional and nontraditional beliefs about mental computation.
The traditional beliefs relate to the practice of introducing a mathematics lesson with
10 quick questions. Approximately 65% of respondents believe that such a practice
sharpens children's mental processes and encourages them to think about
mathematics right from the start of a lesson (Items 15 & 16, Table 4.11). The
opinion of school personnel with respect to the need for the questions to be
corrected quickly so that the mathematics lesson can begin was evenly divided
(Item 17, Table 4.11).
This division of opinion is conceivably related to the finding that 81.5% of
respondents disagreed with the traditional view that the focus should be on the
correctness of the answer rather than on the mental strategies used (Item 20, Table
4.11). One Year 5 teacher commented that, although sets of questions are given to
children, they "are encouraged to discuss their methods to analyse either why they
messed up' or why they were right when everyone else messed up'. Item 19, the
teaching practice that received greatest support (99.5%), pertained to the
importance of providing opportunities for children to discuss, compare and refine
their mental strategies for solving particular mental problems (see Table 4.11).
However, some respondents expressed the opinion that the advantages of
discussion and explanation are limited to the child whose strategy is being
considered:

Yes, it's great fun listening to how some students calculate their answers to
larger sums beyond the basic facts. The other students enjoy listening to
them as well but I'm not sure [that] they then adopt other students'

307

strategies. When the pressure is on, you tend to go with what feels natural.
(Year 4 teacher)

A somewhat contrasting view, but consistent with the reservations expressed


concerning the use of the ideas of others to modify a strategy, is the comment from
another respondent: "I find kids are interested in and willing to tell me how they
calculated mentally, but the other 29 kids are really not interested in how that person
solved the problem" (Year 7 teacher).
Strong support was given to the practice of building on the thinking strategies
used to develop the basic facts (Item 22) to generate mental strategies for
calculating exact answers beyond the basic facts (see Figure 4.3). The degree of
support for this nontraditional approach to mental computation may be confounded
by the apparent focus on basic fact development, rather than on mental computation
as defined for the survey, by some respondents. This assumption is supported by
11.3% of those who listed resources nominating texts that support basic fact
development rather than mental computation per se (see Table 4.17). Further
support for this assumption stems from the comments made by some respondents.
For example, a Year 5 teacher stated: "I approach the teaching of basic facts
[italics added] in as many ways as possible as I see it as an important component of
the Year 5 curriculum in preparation for upper-school.
Whereas opinion with respect to the need to focus on several approaches to
each of a few problems (Item 21) reflected a nontraditional view (see Figure 4.3),
approximately 25% of teachers and administrators indicated a belief in the
traditional view that one approach to each of a number of similar problems should
be the focusa focus on the teaching of types of problems. This belief is
reminiscent of the advice given by "J.R.D." (1928) who suggested that teachers
should plan to cover "one type only in one lesson" (p.7).
In summary, from Figures 4.1, 4.2, and 4.3, it can be concluded that
Queensland state school personnel tend towards holding nontraditional views about
the nature of mental computation and its development. However, it is possible that
such beliefs have arisen not so much from a depth of understanding of issues
related to mental computation per se, but more from an acceptance of the
recommended teaching practices that accompany the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics
Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a), particularly with respect to the
development of the basic number facts and problem solving. As revealed in Table
4.15, the majority of school personnel have not recently undertaken inservice in

308

which mental computation was a specific topic for discussion (Item 55). "Current
methods emphasise problem-solving strategies, estimation. Mental computation for
exact answers is not a high priority" (Year 4 teacher).
Many of the issues raised in the survey are not necessarily specific to mental
computation, but relevant to mathematics teaching in general. As discussed in
Chapter 2, current developments in mathematics education suggest, inter alia, that
learning is enhanced where children are placed in problem solving situations, and
where active involvement and reflection are encourageda focus on the construction
of mathematical knowledge.
The Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Teaching, Curriculum and Assessment
Guidelines (Department of Education, 1987b, p. 4) advocates teaching mathematics
through problem solving. This approach uses problem solving as a focal point for
planning learning experiences in which children are required to apply their
mathematical knowledge to resolve the problems encountered. Associated with this
approach is the encouragement for children to discuss and reflect upon solutions
and the strategies used. Teachers and children have been encouraged to accept a
range of solutions and a range of strategies for arriving at particular solutions. Many
respondents listed resources more specifically related to problem solving in general,
rather than ones directly related to the development of the ability to mentally
calculate exact answers beyond the basic facts (see Table 4.17).
In commercial mathematics textbooks published during 1980s an emphasis has
been given to the role of thinking strategies for developing number fact knowledge.
This emphasis is also evident in the sourcebooks published by the Queensland
Department of Education, texts that operationalise the beliefs about how children
learn mathematics that are embodied in the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus
(Department of Education, 1987a). A focus on thinking strategies for obtaining the
basic facts, before introducing drill and practice, is believed to make the facts more
meaningful as well as easier for children to learn (Booker, n.d., p.11).

Question 2(b):

What importance was placed on mental computation in the period


1964-1987, with respect to both syllabus documents and teachers?

From Table 4.13 it is apparent that the perceived emphasis placed on mental
computation by the mathematics syllabuses diminished sharply with the introduction
of the first edition of the Program in Mathematics for Primary Schools (Department
of Education, 1966-1968). Whereas 88.5% of valid cases considered that the 1964

309

Mathematics Syllabus placed great emphasis on mental arithmetic (Item 39a), this
had fallen to 13.9% for the first edition of the Program in Mathematics (Department
of Education, 1966-1968) (Item 39b). Of the three syllabuses in operation during
the period 1964-1987, only the 1964 syllabus made specific reference to mental
arithmetic, as discussed in Section 4.2.
A similar pattern of results was obtained for Item 40 (see Table 4.13)
concerning the importance that teachers placed on mental computation during this
period. However, the level of importance that teachers gave to mental computation
remained relatively high under both the 1969 and 1975 editions of the Program in
Mathematics. Sixty-one percent (61%) and 47.7%, respectively, indicated that they
continued to place great importance on mental computation, even though the
syllabuses made no specific reference to the need to develop the ability to calculate
exact answers beyond the basic facts.

Question 3(a)(i): Do teachers take a nontraditional approach to developing skill with


mental computation?

From Figure 4.4 it is evident that middle- and upper-school teachers employ
both traditional and nontraditional teaching practices when developing the ability to
calculate exact answers mentally. This finding parallels those that relate to the
beliefs about mental computation expressed by all respondents as well as by
middle- and upper-school teachers (see Tables 4.8, 4.9, & 4.10).
Children taught by middle- and upper-school teachers are permitted to decide
the method of calculation (Item 25, Table 4.12), and to explain and discuss the
strategies used (Item 26), teaching practices classified as nontraditional. Links are
made to the thinking strategies used to develop the basic facts. Thirty-eight percent
(38%) of middle- and upper-school teachers indicated that they often use this
approach (Item 31). Whereas 38% of middle- and upper-school teachers stated that
they never insist on children using the procedures for the written algorithms (Item
34), 18.5% indicated that this requirement was sometimes placed on children (see
Table 4.12).
Teaching practices classified as traditional that remain in the repertoire of
middle- and upper-school teachers include the practice of giving several one-step
questions and simply marking the answers as correct or incorrect (Item 29). Twelve
percent (12%) of respondents indicated that they use this strategy often, and 47.2%
revealed that they use it sometimes. In excess of half of the middle- and upper-

310

school teachers continue to emphasise speed when calculating exact answers


mentally (Item 33), with 16.7% of teachers often doing so.
Most middle- and upper-school teachers require answers to problems solved
mentally to be recorded on paper (Item 30). This practice and the teaching of rules
for calculating exact answers mentally (Item 36) are the traditional practices most
commonly identified by middle- and upper-school teachers as ones that are
sometimes or often employed.

Question 3(a)(ii): Are the teaching approaches employed by middle- and upperschool teachers consistent with their stated beliefs?

Six of the current teaching practices delineated in Section 2 of the survey


instrumentItems 25, 26, 29, 31, 34, and 36may be directly related to particular
belief statements in Section 1 of the questionnaire, namely Items 14, 19, 20, 22, 8,
and 5, respectively (see Figure 4.6). Following the recoding of items representing
beliefs and practices classified as traditional, namely Items 29, 34, and 36 (current
teaching practices) and Items 5, 8, and 20 (beliefs), to reflect a nontraditional
orientation, the means for each of the matched items, for middle- and upper-school
teachers, were: Item 253.55 and Item 143.27, Item 263.50 and Item 193.46,
Item 292.30 and Item 202.98, Item 313.32 and Item 223.25, Item 343.18 and
Item 82.86, and Item 361.60 and Item 52.63 (see Appendix E for the standard
deviations of these means).
From Figure 4.6, it is apparent that, except for Items 29 and 36, there would
appear to be some consistency between particular teaching practices and the
beliefs that underpin them. Approximately 84% of middle- and upper-school
teachers disagreed with the belief that the focus should be on the correctness of the
answer rather than on the mental strategies used (Table 4.11, Item 20). However,
most60% approximatelycontinue to place an emphasis on giving several one-step
questions and simply marking the answers as correct or incorrect (Table 4.12, Item
29). The continued use of this traditional teaching practice may be influenced by the
ambivalence of middle- and upper-school teachers towards the belief that answers
obtained for the 10 quick questions need to be corrected quickly so that the
mathematics lesson can begin (Table 4.11, Item 17). A similar disparity between
belief and practice is evident for Item 36 (see Table 4.12) and Item 5 (see Table
4.9). Although 60.2% of middle- and upper-school teachers do not support the

311

belief that calculating exact answers mentally involves applying rules by rote (Item
5), 87.9% continue to teach rules for calculating exact answers mentally (Item 36).
The dissimilarity between some teaching practices and the beliefs that underpin
them may stem, at least in part, from the nature of teacher inservice which
accompanied the introduction of the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus
(Department of Education, 1987a). The inservice model was founded on Hall,
Teaching Practices
Traditional

Nontraditional

Never
1

Seldom

Sometimes

Often

2
3
4
|X|X|XXXX|

Items:

36

29

34
31

25
26

Beliefs
Traditional

Nontraditional

Strongly
Disagree
1

Disagree

Strongly
Agree

Agree

2
3
4
||XXX|XXX|

Items:

20
8

Recoded Items:
Matched Items:

Figure 4.6.

14
22

19

5, 8, 20, 29, 34, 36


25-14, 26-19, 29-20, 31-22, 34-8, 36-5

Means for selected teaching practices and the beliefs that underpin
them for middle- and upper-school teachers on a traditionalnontraditional continuum.

Wallace, and Dossett's (1973) Concerns-Based Adoption Model which posits that
teachers implementing an innovation move through a series of levels before fully
incorporating a particular teaching strategy, for example, into their repertoire of
pedagogical techniques (Dunlop, 1990a, p. 4). For this to occur, teachers not only
require knowledge about particular topics and associated teaching approaches, but
collegiate support during its implementation.

312

Mental computation was not a specific focus during the inservice which
occurred, nor was adequate in-classroom support available for teachers. Hence, it
may be that, whereas teachers became convinced of the need for certain changes
to occurfor example, that children should understand the processes involved when
calculating, rather than simply applying rules by rotethe implementation of these
beliefs has not occurred to any significant level, given the lack of knowledge and
support during the change process. Traditional approaches to mental computation
therefore remain in classroom practice. As one Year 4 teacher commented: "I enjoy
mathematics, know a lot of short cuts and teach these to my class."

Question 3(b):

What were the characteristics of the teaching approaches used to


develop the ability to calculate exact answers mentally during the
period 1964-1987?

As discussed previously, the findings that relate to this question should be


interpreted with caution, given the nonrepresentativeness of the sample for each
syllabus period between 1964 and 1987. Additionally, as one respondent pointed
out, It is difficult to recall exactly what strategies were used when; changes evolve
gradually so cannot classify easily by year.
The data (see Table 4.14) that were obtained suggest that some teaching
practices classified in this study as nontraditional may have been used during the
period 1964-1987. Many teachers indicated that they allowed children to decide the
method to be used to arrive at an exact answer (Item 41) and allowed children to
explain and discuss their mental strategies (Item 42). The use of these strategies
became more frequent during this period, particularly between 1975 and 1987
(Figure 4.5).
This period was marked by a gradual disillusionment with the approaches
recommended in the 1975 edition of the Program in Mathematics (Department of
Education, 1975), particularly with those that related to the development of the
written algorithms for the four operations. In Boxall's (1981, p. 2) view, there were
errors in application of the intent of the syllabus that were partially exemplified by an
over-emphasis on step-by-step proof in the development of the algorithm for each
operation.
The disillusionment that was being experienced was counterbalanced during the
early- to mid-1980s by recommendations to place an emphasis on problem solving
(Hickling, 1983; Salmon & Grace, 1984), and to reconsider how the four operations

313

might be taught meaningfully (Irons, Jones, Dunphy, & Booker, 1980). The
recommended approaches placed an emphasis on understanding, discussion, and
the need to explore alternative ways for deriving solutions. It is possible that the
recent emphasis given to these approaches may have influenced respondents'
recollections of the methods used to teach mental computation under earlier
syllabuses.
Although respondents indicated that nontraditional approaches were employed,
strategies classified as traditional in this study were more commonly used. There
was an emphasis on speed (Item 44), teaching rules (Item 46), and giving several
one-step questions and simply marking the answers as correct or incorrect (Item 43)
(see Table 4.14 & Figure 4.5). However, the use of these approaches decreased
slightly under each edition of the Program in Mathematics (Department of
Education, 1966-1968, 1975) (Figure 4.5), which paralleled the increased use of
methods classified as nontraditional discussed previously.

Question 4(a):

What need for inservice on mental computation is expressed by


school personnel?

Queensland state primary school teachers88.2% of valid casesconsider that


it is important for inservice on mental computation to be made available to teachers
(Item 54, Table 4.15). As one Year 5 teacher commented: "I feel mental
computation is very important and I probably don't put enough emphasis on it in my
teaching. I feel I need inservice on how to teach it. Different strategies etc."

Question 5(a):

What is the nature of the resources currently used to support the


teaching of mental computation?

The characteristics of the resources used to develop mental computation skills


(see Table 4.18) are consistent with the finding that traditional teaching practices
continue to be employed by middle- and upper-school teachers (Figure 4.4). Each
of these texts is characterised by sets of predominantly one-step examples, many of
which simply require the recall of factsfor example, "How many days in April?".
Where teachers' notes are provided, no references are made to strategies for
calculating mentally. Typical of the teachers' notes is that in the most commonly
used resource, Mental Arithmetic and Problem Solving (Lewis, 1991). The "Note to
the teacher" indicates, inter alia, that:

314

This series is designed to be of practical use to the teacher who wishes to


reintroduce [italics added] "Mental" to the Maths programme, with exercises
related to the main course. Each book [in the series] is set out in forty
weekly units, a unit consisting of five sets of ten exercises. (p. i)

Each of the resources contains exercises from across the range of number,
space and measurement topics within the syllabus, although none is specifically
written to match the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education,
1987a). Various structures for the sets of questions are evident in the texts. For
example, Mental Arithmetic and Problem Solving: Year 6 (Lewis, 1991) employs a
range of topics which includes number facts, extended facts, money, factors,
measurement, and problems. Prominence is also given to the use of short
methodsfor example, to subtract by nine, subtract 10 and add one. In contrast,
Progress in Mental Arithmetic: Year 6 (Petchell & McDonald, 1980) presents sets of
examples based on a range of concepts grouped into four levels of difficulty within
each unit. For example, Levels 3 and 4, for "more competent children, include
items such as: "48c x 4" and "How many m tiles are required to tile an area 17m
by 8m?" (p. 21).
Indicative of the dominance of traditional texts to support mental computation
are the comments of two teachers. A Year 7 teacher commented: "It is almost
impossible to purchase books for this purpose. I rely on my own resources built up
for many years. Such a view gained support from a Year 6 teacher who noted that
"the older the book the better" as a resource to assist in teaching mental
computation. In contrast, another Year 6 teacher suggested that "any maths
resource can be used. [The] teacher modifies the resource to meet current needs.
Teachers should use common sense and make maths as everyday' as possible.
Support for nontraditional teaching approaches to mental computation are to be
found in some of the resources categorised as General Texts and in some
supporting the development of the basic facts. Nonetheless, the majority of the
resources classified as Other Than Specifically Relating to Mental Computation bear
no direct relevance to developing the ability to calculate exact answers mentally.
Sixteen (16) middle- and upper-school teachers indicated that they made use of
the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Sourcebook (Department of Education, 1987-1991)
relevant to the year level taught. The sourcebook for Year 5 provides
comprehensive support for a focus on developing thinking strategies for calculating
mentally beyond the basic facts (Department of Education, 1988, pp. 51-57). Ideas

315

for assisting children to extend basic fact strategies to mental operations with larger
numbers are included. However, the degree to which teachers actually apply these
ideas is not able to be determined from this study. As discussed previously, opinion
is divided (Item 2, Table 4.8) as to the importance placed on mental computation by
the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a), and, by
implication, by the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Sourcebooks (Department of
Education, 1987-1991).
Maths in the Mind: A Process Approach to Mental Strategies (Baker & Baker,
1991), although primarily concerned with basic fact development, provides ideas for
developing mental strategies for calculating beyond the basic facts. In approach, it
is similar to the ideas presented by McIntosh (1988) in Volume 1 of the Mathematics
Curriculum and Teaching Program (Lovitt & Clarke, 1988), nominated by one Year 7
teacher as a resource used to support mental computation. The nontraditional
approach advocated by Baker and Baker (1991) emphasises that mental strategies
need not be specifically taught. Rather teachers should:

Begin to understand the strategies that children invent themselves and


encourage [their] use...in the classroom by demonstrating that it is not
"wrong" to find a quick way of working out a number fact [mentally]; rather
that it is valid and desirable. (p. 8)

Of relevance to developing the ability to calculate beyond the basic facts, A.


Baker and J. Baker (1991) present games such as "Target numbers" in which
children are required to find, for example, "How many times does five go into 1990,
the target number?" (p. 95). Children are encouraged to discuss and compare the
strategies, and to investigate ones that they think may be helpful in arriving at a
solution.

Question 5(b):

What was the nature of the resources used to support the

teaching of mental computation during the period 1964-1987?

The degree to which conclusions can be drawn with respect to the resources
teachers used between 1964 and 1987 is limited by information having been
provided by only 29 of the 161 school personnel who responded to Section 3 of the
survey instrument. As revealed in Table 4.17, 17.4% of the 23 resources listed for
the period 1964-1987 related specifically to mental computation. Each of the texts

316

that was able to be identified from the bibliographic information provided (see Table
4.19) support traditional teaching approaches. These texts are ones that are
currently being used by middle- and upper-school teachers (see Table 4.18).
The text most commonly cited by respondents55.2%was a general
mathematics text, the Mathematics Guide (Willadsen, 1970-1976), initially published
to correspond to each stage of the Program in Mathematics for Primary Schools
(Department of Education, 1966-1968). This finding is consistent with that by
Warner (1981, pp. 76-77) who found that the Mathematics Guide (Willadsen, n.d.)
was the text most widely used by teachers of Years 5-7. Although it made no
specific reference to mental computation, its structure facilitated giving 10 quick
questions in such areas as the four operations to 100 and numeration.

Concluding Points
In conclusion, although the findings from this survey may not authoritatively
extend our understanding of mental computation in Queensland classrooms
between 1964 and 1987, it is reasonable to conclude that those relating to the
present, albeit late-1993, are representative of contemporary beliefs and practices.
Given that many teachers are yet to fully embrace mental computation as currently
envisaged, both in belief and practice, there is an urgent need for (a) significant
documentary guidance for teaching mental computation, and (b) teacher inservice
and classroom support, as recognised by the majority of respondents to the
questionnaire. Of relevance to meeting these needs was the abortive introduction
during the mid-1990s of the Student Performance Standards in Mathematics for
Queensland Schools (1994), one of the initiatives discussed in the next section.

4.4

Mental Computation in Queensland: Recent Initiatives


Although the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education,

1987a), supported by the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Teaching, Curriculum and


Assessment Guidelines (Department of Education, 1987b), continues as the
syllabus for mathematics teaching in Queensland primary schools, two innovations,
which were introduced in 1995, have impacted upon its continued implementation,
innovations with some relevance for mental computation. These are: (a) Student
Performance Standards in Mathematics for Queensland Schools (1994), which

317

introduced Queensland primary teachers to the concept of outcomes-based


assessment, and (b) the Number Developmental Continuum (Queensland School
Curriculum Office [QSCO], 1996b). Associated with the latter is the Year 2
diagnostic net, a performance-based mechanism proposed in the Report of the
Review of the Queensland School Curriculum (Wiltshire, 1994, R7.1, p. xiv), for
identifying children who have inadequate numeracy levels and for whom intensive
intervention should be implemented.
These innovations occurred in context with national trends which [saw]
students, parents and educators across Australia claiming for more uniformity,
portability, accessibility, universality, and even higher standards to ensure Australia
[remained] internationally competitive (Wiltshire, 1994, p. 4). Entwined with the
latter, was the belief, expressed particularly by employers, that many students were
leaving school without reaching appropriate numeracy standards. Concern was
expressed over the perceived inability of students to add, subtract, multiply and
divide, or to handle number facts, as well as an over-reliance on calculators to their
detriment of mental calculations (Wiltshire, 1994, p. 153). Within this atmosphere,
reflective of that in other western democracies, and in context with a paucity of data
to inform public opinion, has been a call for greater accountability on the part of
schools, an exhortation which tends to gain prominence in times of economic crises
(Boomer, 1989, cited by Clements, 1996, p. 6).
In common with the implementation of the national curriculum in England and
Wales in 1989, it was assumed that education [could] play a major economic role,
and, to do so, that the direction of education [could not] be left in the hands of
educators (Hughes, 1993, p. 143). Hence, as the New South Wales DirectorGeneral of Education observed in 1988, the determination of educational policy has
slipped largely from the hands of professionals...to reside firmly with governments,
political parties with their educational policy committees, economists, management
experts and their major advisers from business and large employee organizations
(Sharpe, 1988, p. 16, cited by Barcan, 1996, p. 20).
The move towards a collaborative approach to curriculum development between
the Commonwealth, State and Territory governments, with its attendant overt
politicisation of curriculum issues (Ellerton & Clements, 1994, p. 49), was first given
formal expression at the June 1986 meeting of the Australian Education Council.
This collaboration, made difficult by political mistrust, by the intransigence of states
interests (Baker, 1993, p. 1, cited in Wiltshire, 1994, p. 94), and by a perceived lack
of consultation with tertiary mathematics educators, resulted in the publication, in

318

1990, of A National Statement on Mathematics for Australian Schools (AEC, 1991)


and, in 1994, Mathematics: A Curriculum Profile for Australian Schools (AEC, 1994).
Whereas the former is an input document designed as a framework for curriculum
development, the latter focuses on outputs describing what it is expected of
students at each competency level (Willis, n.d., p. 8).
The decision by the Australian Education Council in July 1990, and supported
by state Directors-General of Education, to endorse the concept of national profiles
constituted, in Speedys (1992, p. 6) view, a significant move towards encouraging
curriculum reform through a focus on outcome statements rather than on syllabus
content. The Mathematics Profile was designed to provide the proposed national
curriculum package with the leverage thought necessary to encourage curriculum
reform (Clements, 1996, p. 9). Nonetheless, for the reasons outlined above, the
Australian Education Council at its meetings in July and December 1993 chose not
to adopt a national curriculum. Instead it was expected that the States and
Territories would develop their own mathematics curricula based on the National
Statement and Profile, a decision in accordance with preferences expressed to the
Report of the Review of the Queensland School Curriculum (Wiltshire, 1994, p. 96).
In Queensland, this decision confirmed the move towards a focus on outcomesbased education through the development of the Student Performance Standards in
Mathematics for Queensland Schools (1994), a document that was based upon
Mathematics: A Curriculum Profile for Australian Schools (AEC, 1994). The Student
Performance Standards provided a means for responding to the increasing clamour
from parents and the community for greater accountability for what is taught, and for
how it is taught and reported (Department of Education, 1995, p. 1). In parallel with
this development was the implementation of recommendations contained in the
report on the school curriculum in Queensland (Wiltshire, 1994) particularly with
respect to the introduction of a Year 2 early-age net whereby diagnosis
(suggested by running records) of the...numeracy levels of all students after 18
months in the compulsory school system and the introduction of Year 6 test in
numeracy (Wiltshire, 1994, R7.1, p. xiv).
Although the Year 6 test has had little direct positive relevance to mental
computation, being a traditional paper-and-pencil test, it does represent an
embracing of a statewide testing approach for gathering accountability data, as well
as for providing information to teachers to support their judgements about student
learning outcomes. Included among its aims is the provision of data to inform the
Minister of Education on numeracy trends, and to provide school and system level

319

information to guide decisions about learning and teaching and resource


management (Queensland School Curriculum Council, 1997, p. 1). The
procedures used to develop the Year 6 test provides support for Clements (1996, p.
12) suggestion that the nationally-developed profiles provide an obvious basis for
constructing statewide and national tests, with their inherent tendency to overvalue
those components of the curriculum which can be easily tested (AAMT, 1997, p. 2).
The Student Performance Standards continued to be used to inform the
development of Year 6 test items during 1996 and 1997. This, despite their use
being effectively discontinued in Queensland state schools from the end of the 1995
school year.
Although earlier attempts at national collaboration to develop a national
curriculum failed, a renewed effort was agreed to at the July 1996 and March 1997
meetings of the Ministerial Council of Education Employment Training and Youth
Affairs. At the latter meeting it was agreed that a new national goal of schooling
should indicate that the achievement of numeracy for every child leaving primary
school is a national priority. In context with this goal, the state Directors-General of
Education approved the development of benchmarks in numeracy, initially for Years
3 and 5. These are designed to: (a) improve student learning in numeracy, and
school performance, and (b) inform Australian governments and the community
about student achievement in...numeracy (Draft Numeracy Standards, 1997, p. 1).
For this task, numeracy has been defined as the effective use of mathematics to
meet the general demands of life at school and at home, in paid work, and for
participation in community and civic life (Literacy & Numeracy Benchmarks, 1997
p. 1).
Given the importance of mental computation in these contexts, as discussed in
Chapters 1 and 2, it is expected that it will receive some prominence in the
benchmarks being developed. However, what impact they, and the associated Year
3 and 5 tests to be introduced in 1998 (Peach, 1997b, p. 2), will have on the
development of mental computation in Queensland state primary school classrooms
remains to be determined. Nonetheless, as outlined above, the recent innovations
in Queensland, arising from the concern for accountability and the desire to provide
effective early intervention, that have had some relevance for mental computation,
are the Student Performance Standards in Mathematics for Queensland Schools
(1994), and, to a lesser extent, the Number Developmental Continuum (QSCO,
1996a).

320

4.4.1 Student Performance Standards and Mental Computation


As discussed in Section 4.3.1, teachers in approximately 80 schools in
Queensland were involved in trialing the Student Performance Standards during
1993, an occurrence that was taken into consideration when designing the sample
of schools for the survey of teachers and administrators. However, as a
consequence of a range of concerns expressed by school personnel, and given
voice particularly through the Queensland Teachers' Union, the Standards were not
formally introduced until 1995 when teachers of Years 3 to 7 were required to report
to parents on the Number, Space, and Measurement strands. Performances on the
remaining strands, Chance and Data, and Working Mathematically, were to be
reported from 1996. However, with the change of government in Queensland in late
February 1996, the Director-General of Education, at the Minister of Education's
direction, advised schools that the implementation of the Student Performance
Standards in state schools would be suspended until advice was received from the
Queensland School Curriculum Office with respect to their future use and
development (Peach, 1996, p. 1), a direct outcome of the concerns which had been
expressed by teachers. Consequently, the potential for the Standards, which made
specific references to mental computation, to enhance the teaching of mental
computation was short-lived.
One of the misgivings held by teachers and administrators centred on perceived
mismatches between the 1987 Syllabus and the Standardsa concern of relevance
to mental computation. Included in the terms of reference for the Queensland
School Curriculum Office project team, which was established to "accurately identify
and analyse the concerns of teachers and school administrators about the
implementation of SPS" (Quinn, 1996, p. 1), was the requirement to provide advice
on: (a) "the extent to which the current SPS match the Years 1-10 Mathematics
Syllabus, and (b) "any changes that may need to be made to either SPS or the
syllabus to improve the match" (Quinn, 1996, p. 1). However, the interim
adjustments to the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education,
1987a) recommended by the review did not refer to mental computation.
The recommended adjustments were restricted to aspects of probability and
statistics, algebra, and two- and three-dimensional shapes (QSCO, Interim
Adjustments, 1996b. pp. 1-11). The intention of these recommendations was to

321

strengthen the link between A National Statement on Mathematics for Australian


Schools (AEC, 1991) and the 1987 syllabus, and consequently, indirectly, its link
with the Student Performance Standards. Significantly for mental computation, but
to its detriment, it was considered that there was a reasonable match between key
aspects of the Number strands in the national statement and the syllabus (QSCO,
Interim Adjustments, 1996b, p. 1). Although there were differences in structure,
there was an essential commonality among the documents which set out the broad
directions for mathematics education in Queensland (Student Performance
Standards, 1994, p. iii). These documents were: (a) the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics
Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a), (b) Years 1 to 10 Mathematics
Teaching, Curriculum and Assessment Guidelines (Department of Education,
1987b), and (c) Student Performance Standards in Mathematics for Queensland
Schools (1994), at least prior to the Minister for Education declaring, in November
1996, that Student Performance Standards, as we know them are finished (Office
of the Minister for Education, 1996, p. 1),
As discussed in Section 4.2, although no specific mention is made of mental
computation in the 1987 Syllabus, mental computation was included as one of the
seven sub-strands within the Number Strand of the Student Performance Standards.
The Standards for Levels 2 to 5 were designed to cover Years 3 to 8, with the
outcomes for Levels 3 and 4 of particular relevance for the majority of children in
Years 4 to 7. These Standards were expressed as follows:

Level 2

Estimates and calculates mentally, including adding and subtracting


numbers to 10 and making extensions based on place value.

Level 3

Estimates and calculates mentally, including adding (sum to 100) and


subtracting two-digit numbers and multiplying numbers to 10.

Level 4

Estimates and calculates mentally, including adding and subtracting


most two-digit numbers and multiplying and dividing multiples of 10 by
one-digit numbers.

Level 5

Estimates and calculates mentally with whole numbers, money and


simple fractions, including multiplying and dividing some two-digit
numbers by one-digit numbers.

(Student Performance Standards, 1994, pp. 42, 58, 72, 88)

It is apparent from these outcomes that both computational estimation and


mental computation, as defined in this study and the mathematics education

322

literature, were encompassed by the term mental computation in the Standards.


This, despite distinctions being made between the calculation of approximate and
exact answers in A National Statement on Mathematics for Australian Schools
(AEC, 1991, pp. 108-109), the document to which the Standards were linked.
Although the Standards primarily constituted a reporting framework, rather than a
framework for planning, Willis (n.d.) suggests that one of their purposes was to
support improved teaching and learning through the identification of agreed
desirable outcomes of learning (p. 3)that is, to supply the leverage for classroom
change referred to previously. Therefore, it can be argued that if both mental
computation and computational estimation are to receive the emphasis required for
effective mathematical functioning, consideration needs to be given to distinguishing
more clearly between the two forms of mental calculation.
To some extent, this has been achieved in the holistic Strand Level Statements
presented in the revised version of Student Performance StandardsQueensland
Levels of Student Performance (QSCO, 1996b). Significantly, the descriptive nature
of the statements for Number implicitly embeds the various computational
procedures for each level with the development of number sense. Nonetheless, the
term number sense, the construct on which the development and application of
flexible mental strategies rely, is used only in the Level 7 statement which suggests
that students demonstrating Level 7 outcomes show a well developed number
sense (QSCO, Strand Level Statements, 1996b, p. 10).
Of significance to mental computation is the elimination of all substrands and that
no specific use is made of the term mental computation. However, the level
statements for the Number Strand include clear references to both calculating exact
and approximate answers. For Levels 2 to 5, which are comparable with the
Student Performance Standards levels outlined above, both in grade comparability
and content, the references to calculating exact answers mentally are:

Level 2:

Students make and then test conjectures about operations on


numbers, especially where mental calculations support their
observations of relationships between sets of numbers. They work
out that 48-30 is 18, suggesting that 49-31 would have the same
answer (18)....Students calculate solutions using mental procedures.

Level 3:

[Students] use effective strategies when calculating mentally beyond


the basic facts in addition, subtraction and multiplication to deal with:

323

(a) adding and subtracting two-digit numbers, and (b) multiplication


of two-digit numbers by single digit numbers.
Level 4:

[Students] try mental arithmetic initially for most one-off


calculations....They use a variety of strategies for calculating in their
head in order to give exact values or approximate values.

Level 5:

[Students] use a range of mental and written methods to add,


subtract, multiply and divide whole numbers, common fractions and
decimal fractions, although for divisors with more than one digit they
may use a calculator.

(QSCO, Strand Level Statements, 1996b, pp. 1, 5)

Irrespective of the future of the Queensland Levels of Student Performance


(QSCO, 1996b), as major revisions to the mathematics syllabus for Queensland
schools are not scheduled to occur until after 2000, it is likely that teachers will not
be encouraged to place an increased emphasis on mental computation in the shortterm. This situation strengthens the conclusion that recent curriculum initiatives will
have had little enduring relevance for mental computation (see Question 8, Section
4.1.2).
The need for the syllabus to explicitly emphasise mental computation is
essential for children to gain mastery of mental strategies for calculating exact
answers, particularly given the finding that approximately 53% of middle- and upperschool teachers believe that the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of
Education, 1987a) places little importance on the development of the ability to
calculate exact answers mentally (see Table 4.8). This situation is one that is
unlikely to have altered since the survey was undertaken in light of the paucity of
inservice related directly to mental computation, a conclusion based on anecdotal
evidence obtained from some Education Advisers (Mathematics) during 1996.
Further, opportunities for inservice were significantly reduced by the number of
these advisers being cut by approximately two-thirds across the state from the
commencement of the 1997 school year.

4.4.2 Number Developmental Continuum and Mental Computation


Although the Student Performance Standards and the Queensland Levels of
Student Performance have been embroiled in disputes with both educational and

324

industrial connotations, the implementation of the Number Developmental


Continuum (QSCO, 1996a) has continued. However, there are indications that Year
2 teachers, in particular, are finding that the performance-based validation tasks
impact significantly on teaching time (Personal Observations, 1996-1997), an
observation in accordance with that of Silvernail (1996, pp. 51, 59) concerning the
impact of the original format of the Standard Assessment Tasks used in the national
assessment program in primary schools in England in the early 1990s.
Given that the number continuum applies to children in Years 1 to 3, its
relevance to the mental calculation of exact answers beyond the basic facts is
limited. Only two of the indicators provide an overlap with the mental computation
statements for Levels 2 and 3 of the Student Performance Standards, namely:

Phase E

Calculates mentally extended addition facts.

Phase F

Calculates mentally extended subtraction facts.

(QSCO, 1996a, Indicators 8.3 & 12.3)

Nonetheless, it is while moving through the six phases of the continuumPhases A


to Fthat children develop mental strategies, critical to basic fact development,
strategies from which efficient mental procedures for calculating with larger numbers
may be constructed.

4.4.3 Implications for Mental Computation Curricula


In summary, although the beliefs about mathematics education underlying the
1987 Syllabus are supportive of a focus on mental computation, the suspension of
the implementation of Student Performance Standards in February 1996 is likely to
have negated any momentum that may have been building towards a consideration
of mental computation by Queensland primary school teachers. Nevertheless,
irrespective of the future of the Standards in their modified formQueensland Levels
of Student Performance (QSCO, 1996b)a renewed focus on mental computation is
essential, not only for its social usefulness but also so that its role as a vehicle for
promoting thinking, conjecturing and generalising (Reys & Barger, 1994, p. 31) may
be fully realised. The mental strategies which are developed during the exploration
of basic factsa focus in the Number Developmental Continuum (QSCO, 1996a)

325

and in the Sourcebooks (Department of Education, 1987-1991) which accompany


the 1987 Syllabusneed to be capitalised upon.
However, as concluded with respect to the analysis of the survey data, the
degree to which teachers are able to focus on mental computation is dependent
upon their (a) gaining access to a syllabus that explicitly places an emphasis on
mental computation, and (b) becoming aware of relevant issues, particularly with
respect to the range of strategies used to calculate mentally, and the teaching
practices considered essential for the development of flexible mental strategies.
The former is addressed in the next chapter in which a syllabus component for
mental computation is proposed, one that may inform the future revision of the 1987
Syllabus, and provide a basis for teacher inservice. Issues pertaining to the latter
are explored in the final chapter.
The professional development that needs to occur, should be planned in context
with current teacher attitudes and practices. Although the survey data presented in
this chapter provide relevant insights, the broader question as to the degree to
which the philosophy and teaching practices embodied in the 1987 Syllabus have
gained ownership by teachers is unknown. However, anecdotal evidence from
Queensland Department of Education mathematics advisers (Personal
communications, 1996-1997) suggests that many teachers, particularly those in the
middle- and upper-schools, have yet to fully implement the spirit of the Years 1 to 10
Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a). One reason for this is that
the 1987 Syllabus and Guidelines provide only very brief statements about content.
The more detailed messages are contained in the year-level sourcebooks,
resources that were never promoted as being prescriptive. Hence, the more
detailed messages about content coverage and teaching strategies were lost on
many teachers, especially in those sectors of schooling where textbooks have been
the prime engines of syllabus interpretation (Grace, 1996, p. 11).

327

CHAPTER 5

MENTAL COMPUTATION:
A PROPOSED SYLLABUS COMPONENT

5.1

Introduction
One conclusion that may be drawn from the analyses in preceding chapters is

that a focus on mental computation is critical to a revitalisation of school


mathematics. For school mathematics to be useful, it needs to reflect the
computational techniques used in everyday life (Maier, 1980, p. 21; MSEB & NRC,
1990, p. 19; Willis, 1990, p. 9). Whereas school mathematics continues to be
oriented towards paper-and-pencil techniques (McIntosh, 1990a, p. 25; Willis, 1990,
p. 12), those used outside the classroom are predominantly mental (Carraher et al.,
1987, p. 94; Wandt & Brown, 1957, pp. 152-157). Such methods, those used by folk
mathematicians (Maier, 1980, pp. 21-23), differ with the context in which an
arithmetical problem is to be solved (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 83; Lave, 1985, p.
172).
Bridging the gap between computational techniques used within the classroom
and those used beyond is critical to students developing confidence in their
mathematical abilities (Case & Sowder, 1990, p. 100). Those who are proficient at
mathematics in daily life, including the workplace, seldom make use of the standard
computational techniques, particular the written algorithms, taught in schools
(Carraher et al., 1987, p. 95; Murray et al., 1991, p. 50). Rather, idiosyncratic
methods tend to be used or else unique adaptations of the written algorithms are
developed (Cockcroft, 1982, p. 75 para 256; Lave, 1985, p. 172). Hence, it is
concluded that the current emphasis on the standard paper-and-pencil algorithms
needs to be reduced. Such a reduction is essential to dispelling the erroneous view
of arithmetic as essentially involving linear, precise, and complete calculations.
However, the impact of an emphasis on mental computation is not limited to its
social utility. The research evidence suggests that such an emphasis significantly

328

contributes to the development of number sense through the fostering of ingenious


ways in which to manipulate numbers. This depends upon, and contributes to, the
development of a deeper understanding of the structure of numbers and their
properties. Further, mental computation is now viewed as an essential prerequisite
to the successful development of written algorithms (J. Jones, 1988, p. 44;
McIntosh, 1990a, p. 37; Rathmell & Trafton, 1990, p. 157; R. E. Reys, 1984, p. 549).
It is the concern for these aspects of mental computation that B. J. Reys and Barger
(1994, p. 31) believe to be the novel facet of the resurgence of interest in mental
computation, one which highlights mental computation as a means for promoting
thinking, conjecturing, and generalising based on conceptual understanding.
Nevertheless, despite the on-going advocacy for an increased emphasis on
teaching rather than testing mental computation, this has yet to significantly
translate into classroom practice (McIntosh, 1990a, p. 25; Reys & Barger, 1994, p.
46). In common with England and the United States, in particular, the place of
mental computation in Australian mathematics curricula is only beginning to be
seriously considered. To overcome the unwillingness on the part of many students
to attempt to calculate mentally, and the concomitant low standard of mental
computation (Carpenter et al., 1984, p. 487; McIntosh et al., 1995, pp. 36-37; B. J.
Reys et al., 1993, p. 314; R. E. Reys et al., 1995, p. 323), requires school
mathematics to become more meaningful to students, and more useful in nonclassroom settings. Children need to be encouraged to value all methods of
computation (Rathmell & Trafton, 1990, p. 156), and particularly to develop personal
strategies for calculating mentally (AEC, 1991, p. 109).
As emphasised by Barbara Reys et al. (1993, p. 312), teachers need to come
to recognise the legitimacy of the development of mental skills as a major goal for
school mathematics, and in so doing change the way in which mental computation is
viewedMcIntoshs (1992, pp. 131-134) first revolution. The second is of equal
importance. This advocates that the opportunities provided for children to develop
the understandings essential for effective mental computation should occur in
association with teachers finding numbers a source of enjoyment for themselves
(McIntosh, 1992, p. 134).
However, teacher and curriculum change occur in association with such factors
as recommendations of mathematics educators, historical context, and, significantly,
teacher beliefs and classroom practices (Weissglass, 1994, p. 78). A consideration

329

of these factors in this chapter, and in Chapter 6, has been facilitated by the analysis
of Queensland syllabuses from 1860 (Chapter 3) and the data from the survey of
Queensland State school personnel (Chapter 4). Of relevance to the proposals
presented in this chapter, these analyses reveal that Queensland teachers have
tended to ignore syllabus recommendations with respect to calculating mentally,
partly as a consequence of the impreciseness in the terminology used, the poor
quality of teacher training, and the lack of appropriate professional development to
accompany the introduction of new syllabuses. Further, arising from the nature of
mental arithmetic embodied in the syllabuses prior to 1966, coupled with teacher
understanding of the 1987 Syllabus, and a commitment to teaching the standard
written algorithms, a tradition for encouraging students to devise their own mental
and written strategies remains to be developed. These factors suggest that ongoing professional development and support will be required to empower teachers
to emphasise mental computation, despite the inclusion of some nontraditional
techniques such as encouraging discussion in their teaching repertoires (see Table
4.12),.
This need, and that for significant documentary guidance, was revealed by the
analyses of survey data (Section 4.3) and recent curriculum initiatives (Section 4.4)
of relevance to mental computation in Queensland. Although aspects of the first of
these needs are analysed in this chapter, the primary focus is on the formulation of
proposals that may form a basis for incorporating specific references to mental
computation into future mathematics syllabuses. The proposals centre upon (a) a
sequential framework, embodying a mental-written sequence, for introducing
mental, calculator and written procedures, and (b) mental strategies appropriate for
focussed teaching at various year-levels. As a consequence of the critical nature of
the environment in which curriculum change occurs, these proposals have been
developed within a Queensland context. Nonetheless, given the similarities in
school mathematics across national boundaries, as reflected in the research
literature (see Chapters 1 & 2), it is considered that the proposals, and the
contextual features, have relevance for all teachers of mathematics.

5.1.1 Context and Focus for Change

330

From a Queensland perspective, the need for professional development and


documentary guidance is embedded in an environment characterised by a stalling of
the potential for a renewed focus on mental computation through the discontinuance
of the Student Performance Standards in Queensland State schools in 1996 (Office
of the Minister for Education, 1996, p. 1). Not since the 1964 Syllabus has mental
computation been specifically included. Further, major revisions to the mathematics
syllabus are not scheduled to occur until after 2000, leaving the 1987 Syllabus to
continue as the basis for mathematics learning in Years 1 to 10, a syllabus which, in
the opinion of approximately 65% of the State primary school personnel surveyed,
places little importance on the development of the ability to calculate exact answers
mentally (Table 4.8, Item 2).
Nonetheless, in common with syllabus development elsewhere, given that
these revisions could be expected to address issues central to teachers beliefs
about the nature of school mathematics and mathematics teaching, it is essential
that debate, both professional and community, occurs well in advance of the
preparation of a new syllabus. Such would not only provide an understanding of,
and assist in the development of a commitment to, the recommended changes, it
would, most importantly, serve to preserve the sense of efficacy teachers exhibit in
their role as teachers of mathematics in the primary school. It is believed that the
development of a syllabus component for mental computationthe focus for this
chaptercould provide a stimulus for such debate.
Key aspects of this debate centre on the sequence in which mental, written,
and technological methods of calculation are introduced to students, the emphasis
on self-generated mental and written computation strategies, and the legitimacy of a
continued role for standard written algorithms. Of relevance to these issues are
findings from the analysis of survey data. These indicate that, although Queensland
State school personnel may tend towards holding nontraditional views about the
nature of mental computation and its development (see Figures 4.1-4.3), aspects of
the current view of mental computation (Chapter 2) are likely to threaten their
confidence in their effectiveness as teachers of mathematics. Central to this threat
is their disagreement with the proposition that an emphasis on the written algorithms
should be delayed to facilitate increased attention to mental computation (Table
4.10, Item 10). To this can be added the proposal that students should be allowed
to use self-generated written strategies, and, particularly, the questioning of the

331

place of standard written algorithms within the curriculum (Cockcroft, 1982, p. 75,
para 256; Lave, 1985, p. 175; Lindquist, 1984, pp. 602).
Additionally, in conflict with the proposition that mental computation involves
the development of flexible mental strategies (R. Reys, 1992, pp. 65-66), is the
belief of many teachers that calculating exact answers mentally involves applying
rules by rote (Table 4.9, Item 5). Such a belief, however, is not inconsistent with that
traditionally held by Queensland teachers, one that originated during the period
1860-1965 when mental arithmetic was viewed as the presentation of a series of
one-step oral questions. The focus was on obtaining correct answers, speedily and
accurately (see Section 3.7), rather than on the mental strategies employed. The
success of a lesson was dependent upon the number of questions correctly
answered (Gladman, 1904, p. 235).
If students are to develop confidence with mental computation, the change
process for school communities needs to be initiated without delay. Critical to this
process is the need for teachers to gain a clear practical understanding of how any
proposed changes might impact upon their beliefs and teaching practices (Lovitt,
Stephens, Clarke, & Romberg, n.d., p. 2). The development of a syllabus
component focussing on mental computation should assist teachers to ascertain the
potential personal impact that may ensue from the recommended changes to the
nature of computation within the primary mathematics syllabus.
In formulating and implementing a mental computation syllabus component,
mental computation needs to be considered from various perspectives. First, from a
global perspective, mental computation needs to be viewed as an integral part of the
whole school program, where the intent is the maximisation of links to the real-world
of the children so as to capitalise upon their idiosyncratic knowledge (see Section
2.10, Conclusion 7). Second, from a mathematics learning perspective, a focus on
mental computation needs to recognise that mental strategies are essentially ways
of thinking about mathematics (Cobb & Merkel, 1989, p. 80). The development of
flexible and resourceful approaches to manipulating numbers both reflects and
develops number sense, a key goal for school mathematics.
Third, from an operations curriculum perspective, the development of
strategies for calculating mentally is implicitly enmeshed with the development of
written and technological methods of calculation. Finally, from a calculation
perspective, mental computation needs to be considered in terms of the

332

effectiveness, efficiency and accuracy of the various mental solution strategies. It is


principally from the last two perspectives that the following analyses and proposals
have been developed. However, in responding to the focus for this chapter (see
Section 1.4.2), which particularly concerned proposals for restructuring the
computation strand, and identifying mental strategies suitable for direct teaching,
cognisance also has been taken of issues relevant to the second perspective.

5.1.2 Framework for Syllabus Development


From the analysis of recent issues in mathematics education in Queensland
(Section 4.4), it is apparent that, for an outcomes-based approach to education to
have a positive impact, outcome statements must be in accordance with a syllabus
that emphasises the desired learnings. Merely including these in assessment and
reporting documents such as Mathematics: A Curriculum Profile for Australian
Schools (AEC, 1994) or Student Performance Standards in Mathematics for
Queensland Schools (1994) with the intention of forcing curriculum change
(Clements, 1996, p. 9; Speedy, 1992, p. 6) is ineffective, particularly where
perceived mismatches between these documents and the syllabus are apparent.
The alignment of outcome statements and curriculum content requires modifications
to the curriculum development process. If the major focus is to be on what children
are to do, rather than on what teachers are to do, the desired learning outcomes
need to be clearly determined prior to, or at least in conjunction with, the design of
appropriate mathematics syllabuses.
Consequently, consideration needs to be given to (a) the desired learning
outcomes, and (b) how their development can be effectively encapsulated in a
syllabus, in context with relevant research data, particularly about the role of mental
computation and how it should be taught. Hence, a principal purpose of this study
has been to formulate a mental computation component, encapsulating key
elements of the research data, for inclusion as a core element in future mathematics
syllabuses (see Section 1.3).
Recognition needs to be given to the nature of current mathematics syllabuses
and that of syllabuses necessary to meet future needs. In primary schools,
syllabuses, such as the Queenslands 1987 mathematics syllabus, essentially
provide outlines of scope and sequence. Whereas some recommendations about

333

teaching and assessment strategies are provided, little is included about expected
student outcomes. Schools and teachers have been left to formulate their own
standards for judging student progress (Grace, 1996, p. 11), with variability between
teachers, both within and across schools, as a consequence. An approach to
syllabus design which may assist in providing a focus on what students should be
able to do as a result of their learning is presented in Figure 5.1. However, an
analysis of the structure and organisation of future mathematics syllabuses (Stage
4) is considered beyond the scope of this study. Hence, this chapter is primarily
concerned with the relationships between Stages 1 to 3 and mental computation.

Goal/s
re student
learning

Stage 1

Figure 5.1.

5.2

Sequential
Framework
for learning

Stage 2

Outcomes
for each
aspect of
sequential
framework

Stage 3

Syllabus
Content
and
organisation

Stage 4

A conceptualisation of syllabus development to provide a focus on


student learning

Mental, Calculator, and Written Computation


The discussion presented in Section 2.9.1 revealed that the research literature

relating to the most appropriate ways to incorporate mental computation into the
curriculum (Carroll, 1996, p. 8; McIntosh, 1991a, p. 6, 1996, p. 273; Rathmell &
Trafton, 1990, p. 160; B. Reys, 1991, p. 9; R. Reys, 1995, p. 305) is characterised
by a degree of equivocalness (see Section 2.9.1). Nonetheless, irrespective of the
approach employed, teaching to promote the desired outcomes occurs within an
overall sequence for introducing computational procedures. This sequence may be
considered at two levels: (a) the order in which mental, technological, and paperand-pencil techniques are taught to children; and (b) the order in which particular

334

procedures associated with each of these methods are learned. Of these, the first
may have greater significance for mental computation.

5.2.1 Traditional Sequence for Introducing Mental, Calculator and Written


Computation
The sequence in which computational procedures have traditionally been
introduced (Figure 2.4) is inextricably bound with the purposes for learning school
mathematics. Although current syllabuses exhibit broader emphases than those
introduced prior to the 1960s, the prime focus of primary school mathematics
continues to be written computation and the development of standard written
algorithms. As concluded in Section 2.5.4, the goal continues to be the automatic
processing of paper-and-pencil calculations, despite the concern for developing
students understanding of the processes involved. Written algorithms are products
of the needs of an industrial age which necessitated minimum competencies in
arithmetic for all students, with higher mathematical training reserved for a few. The
continued emphasis on written procedures has its origins in two outdated, but
steadfastly held, assumptions, namely that (a) mathematics is a fixed and
unchanging body of facts and procedures, and (b) to do mathematics is to calculate
answers to set problems using a specific catalogue of rehearsed techniques (NSEB
& NRC, 1990, p. 4).
Although the sequence represented in Figure 2.4 has come to be considered
as traditional, it was not one that was recommended in Queensland mathematics
syllabuses prior to the New Maths era, syllabuses that explicitly referred to mental
calculations. As discussed in Section 3.5.2, the focus in these syllabuses was on
using mental arithmetic as an introduction to the written work that was to follow
(Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 201). Nevertheless, in contrast to the current emphasis
on mental strategies as a means for promoting number sense, the mental work
under the syllabuses prior to 1966 was designed primarily as a means for
rehearsing the steps to be undertaken in applying the standard written algorithms
(see Section 3.5.2).
However, it is evident, as reflected by the observations of District Inspectors of
Schools (see Section 3.5.3), that teachers rarely satisfactorily achieved this syllabus
requirement. Mental arithmetic tended to be thought of as a separate topic within

335

the mathematics curriculum, a view not inconsistent with that of many current
Queensland primary school teachers (see Table 4.10). In the minds of teachers
were syllabus goals that stressed written calculationscalculations that were to be
achieved with speed and accuracy (Department of Public Instruction, 1952, p. 1).
This belief has persisted to the present, despite the focus on teaching methods
designed to promote an understanding of the concepts and processes being
learned. To support this development, current mathematics syllabuses, of which the
Queensland Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987) is
representative, are characterised by: (a) a delay in the age at which particular
aspects of the four operations are learneda process first stimulated in 1930s in
Queensland (see Section 3.2.3) by reports such as that of Cunningham and Price
(1934), (b) clear distinctions being drawn between the concept of an operation and
the written algorithm, (c) the memorisation of basic facts being supported by insights
into the relationships between numbers gained from a focus on thinking strategies,
and (d) the standard algorithms taught being ones which may be logically and
mathematically linked to the use of appropriate materials, supported by the use of
consistent language.
Table 5.1 presents the operationalisation of these considerations, as embodied
in Queenslands 1987 Syllabus and its supporting documents. This

336

Table 5.1
Traditional Sequencea for Introducing the Four Operations with Whole Numbers as
Presented in the Mathematics Sourcebooks for Queensland Schools
Yearb
1

ADDITION
1. Addition concept
2. Recording addition
3. Addition factsThinking strategies
4. The tens facts
5. Standard written algorithmc
2-digit numbers, no regrouping
2-digit numbers, regrouping
3-digit numbers, regrouping
4-digit numbers, regrouping
SUBTRACTION
1. Subtraction concept
2. Recording subtraction
3. Subtraction factsThinking strategies
4. The tens facts
5. Standard written algorithmc
2- & 3-digit numbers, no regrouping
3-digit numbers, regrouping
4-digit numbers, regrouping
MULTIPLICATION
1. Multiplication concept
2. Recording multiplication
3. Multiplication factsThinking strategies
4. The tens facts
5. Standard written algorithmc
2-digit by 1-digit number, regrouping
2-digit by 2-digit numbers, regrouping
DIVISION
1. Division concept
2. Recording division
3. Division factsThinking strategies
4. Standard written algorithmc
2- & 3-digit 1-digit number, no regrouping
2- & 3-digit 1-digit number, regrouping
Note.

Adapted from Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Sourcebook: Year 5 (Department of


Education, 1988, pp. 68-69, 72-73, 76-77, 82). bThe operations for Years 2 to 5
equate with Phases C to F of the Number Developmental Continuum (QSCO,
1996a, pp. 23-25, 27-28, 30-31, 33). cSelected levels of complexity of the standard
written algorithm.

337

framework encapsulates the view expressed by the National Council of Teachers of


Mathematics (1989) that the long-standing preoccupation with [written]
computation...has dominated what mathematics is taught and the way mathematics
is taught (p. 15). One consequence of this has been that a students view of
mathematics has generally not reflected the subjects vitality, an essential element in
promoting the development of flexible mathematical thinking.

5.2.2 A Sequential Framework for Mental, Calculator, and Written


Computation
Critical to flexible mathematical thinking is an individuals sense of number (see
Section 2.3.3), the development of which is considered by the Australian Education
Council (1991, p. 107) to be an essential goal for primary school mathematics.
Number sense is, in part, characterised by an ability to perform mental computations
with nonstandard strategies that take advantage of an ability to compose and
decompose numbers (Resnick, 1989b, p. 36; Sowder, 1992, p. 5). In so doing,
students with number sense tend to analyse the whole problem first to ascertain and
capitalise upon the relationships among the numbers, and the operations and
contexts involved, rather than merely apply a standard algorithm (Lave, 1985, p.
173). Hence, it was concluded in Section 2.10 that engaging children in mental
computation can not only be considered as a means for linking school and folk
mathematics, but also as a means for enhancing mathematical knowledge, and
confidence in its application.
The development of flexible mental strategies is influenced by the order in which
mental and written techniques are introduced. Musser (1982, p. 40) has concluded
that classroom experience indicates that children have difficulty with mental
methods when written algorithms are taught prior to a focus on mental computation.
Such a focus places an emphasis on symbols rather than on the quantities
embodied in calculative situations (Reed & Lave, 1981, p. 442), thus reducing the
opportunities for the development of number sense. Further, the right-to-left
characteristics of the standard written algorithms for addition, subtraction, and
multiplication contradict the holistic, left-to-right strategies frequently used by
proficient mental calculators (see Table 2.4). Hence, as Cooper et al. (1992, p. 104)
assert, the conventional written before mental sequence (Figure 2.4, Table 5.1)

338

needs to be reevaluated. One consequence of such a reevaluation would be the


development of more flexible computational strategies, both mental and written
(Thornton et al., 1995, p. 40).
An additional consideration is the necessity to reassess the balance between
the various forms of calculationmental, calculator, and writtento calculate
approximate and exact answers. Children need to be encouraged to select
computational techniques appropriate to particular tasks in accordance with their
store of declarative and procedural knowledge (see Figure 2.3). In so doing,
recognition needs to be given to the profound effect that electronic calculating
devices have had on society, and therefore on the features of school mathematics
relevant to the present technological age (see Section 2.3.2). Consequently, less
emphasis should be placed on developing paper-and-pencil algorithms (Rathmell &
Trafton, 1990, p. 54), particularly the standard ones, which is the continuing principal
concern of primary school mathematics, including that in Queensland classrooms
(see Table 5.1).
The continuing place for standard written algorithms, even those designed to
enhance an understanding of the procedures involved, needs to be questioned,
given that the focus for learning mathematics should be on the development of
techniques and tools which reflect modern mathematics (AEC, 1991, p. 13). R. E.
Reys (1984, p. 551) asserts that standard written algorithms discourage thinking as
they are designed to be used automatically by students who require only a limited
understanding of the processes involved (Plunkett, 1979, p. 2). Hence, they
contribute little to the development of number sense, particularly where
decontextualised examples are presented to students (Lave, 1985, p. 175). Further,
as discussed in Section 2.5.3, there is evidence to suggest that the standard written
algorithms are not used widely outside the classroom (Carraher et al., 1987, p. 95;
Cockcroft, 1982, p. 20; French, 1987, p. 41; Murray et al., 1991, p. 50).
As discussed in Section 2.5.4, when calculation occurs in settings in which the
method of calculation is not imposed, the elements of problem situations are often
changed to reflect the perceived numerical relationships. This facilitates the
modification of calculation techniques to match the physical environment (Lave,
1985, p. 173). Consequently, school mathematics needs to be reorganised so that
opportunities are provided for students to deal with mathematics in their own

339

environments in ways similar to those of folk mathematicians (Maier, 1980, p. 23;


Masingila et al., 1994, p. 13).
Nonetheless, a structured approach to developing operational proficiency,
which a focus on standard written algorithms can provide, may be beneficial for
some children. The language and representational models used may assist in the
development of conceptual, procedural, metacognitive, and real-world knowledge
(see Figure 2.3) of use when calculating in non-teacher-directed situations. This is
an area for further research to inform the professional and community debate which
needs to occur to support the recommended changes to the computation curriculum,
research that could be modelled on that by Gracey (1994) and Shuard (1989), which
focussed on teaching mental strategies and the impact of calculator use,
respectively.
As intimated previously, based on the model of the calculative process
presented in Figure 2.1 and on statements in documents to guide syllabus
development (AEC, 1991, pp. 115, 121; NCTM, 1989, p. 94), it is apparent that
individuals need to be able to choose computational methods matched to context
and personal strengths. Hence, a global outcome (Figure 5.1: Stage 1) for
computational learning in the primary school could be expressed as: Select and
apply a range of mental, written, and technological strategies for each
operationstrategies appropriate to the context and the student.
To operationalise a mental-written approach for introducing computational
procedures, as represented in Figure 2.5, a sequential framework (Figure 5.1: Stage
2) encompassing each operation is presented in Table 5.2. An emphasis is placed
on the use of mental and calculator procedures for each operation beyond the basic
facts prior to the introduction of paper-and-pencil techniques. It assumes the
availability of calculators for computations beyond those capable of being worked
mentally, thus maximising opportunities for their becoming real computational tools,
particularly for low-attaining children, an outcome supported by the PrIME Project in
England (Shuard, 1989, p. 73).

340

Table 5.2
Revised Sequential Framework for Introducing Mental, Calculator and Written
Procedures for Addition, Subtraction, Multiplication and Division
Year
1 -3

3-5

5-7

ADDITION
1. Addition concept
2. Non-written procedures
(a) Addition factsThinking strategies
(b) Addition beyond basic facts
Self-generated/shared mental strategiesa
Calculator strategies
3. Written procedures
Self-generated/shared strategies
Standard written algorithmb
SUBTRACTION
1. Subtraction concept
2. Non-written procedures
(a) Subtraction factsThinking strategies
(b) Subtraction beyond basic facts
Self-generated/shared mental strategiesa
Calculator strategies
3. Written procedures
Self-generated/shared strategies
Standard written algorithmb
MULTIPLICATI0N
1. Multiplication concept
2. Non-written procedures
(a) Multiplication factsThinking strategies
(b) Multiplication beyond basic facts
Self-generated/shared mental strategiesa
Calculator strategies
3. Written procedures
Self-generated/shared strategies
Standard written algorithmb
DIVISION
1. Division concept
2. Non-written procedures
(a) Division factsThinking strategies
(b) Division beyond basic facts
Self-generated/shared mental strategiesa
Calculator strategies
3. Written procedures
Self-generated/shared strategies
Standard written algorithmb
Note.

Mental strategies for calculating approximate as well as exact answers. bWhere


appropriate, introduced using the traditional sequence (see Table 5.1).

341

Reflecting the place of mental calculation within the calculative process (see
Figure 2.1), mental strategies refers to strategies for the calculation of both
approximate and exact answers. Such strategies take advantage of the structural
properties of numbers and the relationships between them. Their development and
that of number comparison and number sense occurs spirally, each "feeding on and
strengthening the others" (Threadgill-Sowder, 1988, p. 195). The ability to mentally
compute with truncated and rounded numbers is a prerequisite for computational
estimation (see Figure 2.2). Additionally, mental strategies used to refine estimates
may assist in the development of flexible approaches for calculating exact
answersgetting closer may ultimately result in turning approximate answers into
exact answers (Irons, 1990b, p. 1).
In concert with the analysis presented above and that in Section 2.5.3, the
emphasis for written procedures is placed on self-generated strategies. However,
reference to the standard written algorithm for each operation has been retained.
Despite the arguments for such algorithms not to be included, from personal
observations of previous syllabus changes, the reality of the classroom dictates that
they will continue to be taught, even should a revised syllabus advise otherwise.
The degree to which standard algorithms will continue to be taught is dependent
upon the effectiveness of the professional debate, supported by further research into
managing mathematics classrooms in which the focus is on self-generated mental,
technological, and written strategies.
By highlighting the position of the standard written algorithms within the
sequential framework (see Table 5.2), the standard algorithm for each operation
may come to be viewed as one of many possible ways for calculating in particular
contexts, rather than the way (Ross, 1989, p. 51), thus enhancing a child's capacity
to select appropriate methods for calculating in particular contexts. Most
importantly, the focus on non-written and non-standard written procedures may
assist in overcoming the belief of many adults and students, as identified by Plunkett
(1979, p. 3), that the concept of a particular operation and its standard paper-andpencil algorithm are synonymous. To support this focus, the placement of the
standard written algorithms within the sequence accords with the recommendation
of the Australian Education Council (1991, p. 109) that, in instances where these
algorithms continue to be taught, such teaching should occur later in a childs
schooling (see Tables 5.1 & 5.2).

342

The framework presented in Table 5.2 recognises that schools need to (a)
acknowledge that all students can learn, and (b) focus on differences with respect to
the way they learn and the rate at which learning occurs (Peach, 1997a, p. 1). Such
a focus requires a flexible approach to the allocation of particular learnings to
particular time periods. Hence, the elements of the sequential framework are not
year-level specific, an approach embodied in the Queensland 1987 Syllabus
documents (Department of Education, 1987a, pp. 14-30; 1987b, pp. 12-40). This is
in contrast to the traditional approach to syllabus design, which is characterised by
estimating the range of topics that can be learned during a particular school year.
However, it is an approach that is encapsulated in the Sourcebooks that accompany
the 1987 Syllabus. The structure of the year-level bands (see Table 5.2) is based on
recommendations contained in the Report of the Review of the Delivery of
Curriculum in Primary Schools (Avenell, 1996, pp. 8, 10). It is intended that the
overlapping bands provide for a smooth progression through the learnings related to
computation, while recognising that each class is characterised by students who
exhibit a range of achievement levels.

5.3

Mental Strategies: A Syllabus Component


It has been suggested that in formulating and implementing a mental

computation syllabus strand, mental computation needs to be considered from a


range of perspectives. The fourth of these has mental strategies that may be used
to calculate mentally as its focus. The mental strategies included in the sequential
framework for introducing mental, calculator, and written computational procedures
(Table 5.2) refer to those for arriving at approximate as well as exact answers. The
analysis presented in Section 2.6.1 indicated that mental computation is one of the
two fundamental skills of computational estimation (see Figure 2.2). However, given
that the second of these is the ability to convert exact to approximate numbers
(Case & Sowder, 1990, p. 88), often a multiple of ten or hundred, mental
computation as it applies to computational estimation involves the manipulation of a
limited range of numbers. Nonetheless, students need to have developed some
proficiency with the mental calculation of exact answers before they are able to find
approximate ones.

343

It was concluded in Section 4.4.1 that if both mental computation and


computational estimation are to receive the emphasis required for effective
mathematical functioning, consideration needs to be given to distinguishing more
clearly between the two forms of mental calculation. Whereas computational
estimation is specifically featured in Queenslands 1987 Syllabus, mental
computation is not (see Section 4.2). The intent of this section, therefore, is to
provide an outline of mental strategies for calculating exact answers which could
form an essential component of the Number strand in future mathematics
syllabusesone aspect of Stage 3 of the proposed syllabus development process
(Figure 5.1).
However, the formulation of a complete mental computation substrand (Stage 4,
Figure 5.1) is considered beyond the scope of this study. Such a strand would need
to consider mental strategies, particularly the heuristic strategies based on relational
understanding (Table 2.4), in context with the other components of mental
computationthe Affective and Conceptual Components, and the Related Concepts
and Skills, with the latter including those that mental computation shares with
computational estimation (Figure 2.2, Table 2.1). The intent is for children to
develop a range of holistic, flexible and constructive mental strategies embedded in
a strong sense of number. The gaining of mathematical power is the desired
outcome (see Section 2.1).

5.3.1 Background Issues


As revealed in Section 2.9.1, the delineation of a mental strategies component
for inclusion in future mathematics syllabuses occurs in context with the recognition
of the equivocal research evidence for such an undertaking. Hope (1987, p. 340)
cautions that further research is required to ascertain whether it is legitimate to
teach the strategies identified as those used by proficient mental calculators (Table
2.4). This caution is amplified by such findings as those of R. E. Reys et al. (1995,
p. 321) that Japanese children taught using the behaviourist approach to mental
computation exhibited a narrow range of non-idiosyncratic mental strategies.
Nonetheless, the recommendations presented may provide a basis for the research
which Hope (1987, p. 340) considers necessary, some of which has already been
undertakenfor example that by Gracey (1994).

344

Additionally, as Carroll (1996, pp. 4, 8) suggests, the spontaneous invention of


mental strategies may be difficult for some children. Hence, at times it may be
appropriate for teachers to introduce particular strategies to students while
recognising that strategies meaningful to one person may not be so for another
(Heirdsfield, 1996, p. 146). Cooper et al. (1996, p. 159) suggest that low-proficiency
children may benefit from direct teaching. The focus for these children tends to be
on right-to-left mental strategies, which are often inefficient. However, the strategies
taught need to be those matched to their level of mathematical development,
particularly that related to place value (Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 9).
Further, there is some evidence (Flournoy, 1954, p. 153; Gracey, 1994, pp. 112116; Josephina, 1960, p. 200; Markovits & Sowder, 1994, p. 22; Schall, 1973, pp.
365-366) to suggest that gains can be made with respect to strategy use, and the
correctness of the answer, where systematic instruction is provided. Markovits and
Sowder (1994, p. 23) have reported that, following instruction, Grade 7 students
exhibited a propensity for employing nonstandard strategies in instances where
standard paper-and-pencil algorithms could not be applied easily to mental
calculations. The strategies children construct have been found to relate to the
nature of classroom experiences (Carpenter, 1980, p. 321), a finding not in conflict
with the reasons provided for the tendency for many Queensland Years 2 and 3
students to use strategies based upon the use of the standard written algorithms for
addition and subtraction (Cooper et al., 1996, p. 158; Heirdsfield, 1996, p. 132).
Hence, it is considered legitimate to propose which mental strategies may
become a focus at different stages of a childs primary schooling. Nonetheless,
when incorporating these suggestions into a mathematics syllabus (Figure 5.1:
Stage 4), it is essential that this occurs in a way that encourages teachers to view
the development of number sense as paramount. This may be assisted by
highlighting B. J. Reys (1991, p. 9) approach to developing computational skillan
approach which is an amalgam of the behaviourist and constructivist approaches,
one that focuses on discussion and on sharing the devised mental strategies with
peers and teachers. Such an approach may be supported by the delineation of
outcomes related to particular mental strategies. However, it is essential that
teachers have an understanding of the range of mental strategies (Tables 2.2 - 2.4)
that may be used by children (Carroll, 1996, p. 8). As McIntosh et al. (1994, p. 7)
point out, these are not currently well known by teachers. Therefore, they constitute

345

one aspect of the professional development for teachers which needs to be


conducted urgently.

5.3.2 Development Issues


In proposing a syllabus component which may form the basis of a mental
computation strand, recognition needs to be given to the multiplicity of strategy
variations arising from each individuals unique long-term memory components (see
Figure 2.3). Vakali (1985, p. 110) has noted that there are considerable individual
differences in the way in which calculative plans are formulated and in the order in
which their inherent steps are executed. In accordance with this view, Heirdsfield
(1996, p. 135) has concluded that there is a diversity of mental strategies in use that
are not prevalent in the literature.
The variations in the mental strategies that have been observed are
characterised by differences in the type of strategy used to solve particular
problems, and by differences within particular strategy classifications. The more
general the classification, the greater diversity of strategies within each category.
Carroll (1996, p. 6) reports four different approaches used by Grade 5 students to
correctly solve 426 + 75, namely

426 + 5 = 431 + 70 = 501

70 + 20 = 490 + (6 + 5) = 501

70 + 30 = 100 + 1 = 101 + 400 = 501

425 + 75 + 1 = 501

From the classification of strategies based on relational understanding presented in


Table 2.4, the first of these may be classified as add (or subtract) parts of the (first
or) second number, the second as incorporation, and the third and fourth strategies
as compensation. However, given their idiosyncrasy, the second and third variants,
in particular, are ones which may not be appropriate for inclusion in any
recommendations for focussed teaching.
The diversity of strategies also depends on the operation. Heirdsfield (1996, p.
131) observed a greater range of strategies for subtraction than for addition. For
subtraction, the complexity of the mental strategies increases where carrying or

346

regrouping is required, particularly where an organisation10-10strategy is used


(Beishuizen, 1993, p. 296). For example, one approach to solving 42 - 15 is: 40 10 = 30 e 20, 2 - 5 = ? e 12 - 5 = 7, 20 + 7 = 27. This contrasts with an (add or)
subtract parts of the (first or) second number approachN10 in Beishuizens (1993,
p. 297) terms: 42 - 10 = 32, 32 - 2 - 3 = 27, an approach that reduces the load on
working memory through the incorporation of partial differences.
This diversity of mental strategies, both across and within categories, presents
some difficulty for selecting strategies for inclusion in a proposed syllabus
component. Nevertheless, the classification of heuristic strategies based on
relational understanding, in particular, discussed in Section 2.7.4, provides an
appropriate framework on which to base the recommendations (Table 2.4). Also
included are selected counting strategies (Table 2.2) which are indicative of children
who have at least gained an understanding of the numerosities of numbers (Murray
& Olivier, 1989, p. 5).
Strategies classified as relying on an instrumental understanding of place value
(Table 2.3) have not been considered. These transitional strategies (McIntosh,
1991a, p. 2) rely on the rote application of rules and procedures, akin to the
approach to mental arithmetic embodied in the pre-1966 syllabuses (see Section
3.5.2). Hence, the transitional strategies are not ones which significantly contribute
to the development of flexible mental strategies.
In formulating syllabus outcomes relating to mental computation (Figure 5.1:
Stage 3) consideration needs to be given to the most effective way in which they
may be expressed. Elements to be considered include the operation involved, the
mental strategy, and the type and size of the numbers to be manipulated. This
suggests a number of alternatives to structuring the outcomesfor example:
Specify the operation and mental strategy.
Use left-to-right addition to refine estimates obtained by using front-end digits
(Ohio Department of Education, n.d., p. 91).
Specify the operation, and the type and size of the numbers.
Add two 2-digit [whole] numbers mentally (Michigan State Board of
Education, 1989, p. 26).
Specify the operation, the mental strategy, and the type and size of the
numbers.

347

Use incorporation to add two 2-digit whole numbers.

Of these alternatives, the third is the preferred. However, it is essential that the
selection of strategies for focussed teaching arises from the research literature,
rather than from a theoretical analysis of possible number combinations that may
appear in numerical situations, which appears to have occurred in the Framework
for Numeracy: Years 1 to 6 (National Numeracy Project, 1997) for primary schools in
England. The suggested mental strategies (Tables 5.3-5.5) are categorised by
operation to provide a direct link to the revised sequential framework (Table 5.2),
elements of which relate to self-generated and shared mental strategies for each
operation. Although teachers should place an emphasis on self-generated mental
strategies (Hunter, 1977a, p. 40), as implied by the second alternative, the purpose
of the proposed syllabus component is to provide some structure and guidance for
teachers to assist students to extend their repertoire of mental strategies (Carroll,
1996, p. 4; Cooper et al., 1996, p. 159; Heirdsfield, 1996, p. 146).
By placing limits on the size of the numbers involved, teachers should be
persuaded to allow students to extend their range of mental strategies through
encouraging them to devise alternative methods, rather than principally maintaining
the focus on a particular mental strategy to calculate with numbers of increasing size
or complexity, an approach not inconsistent with that embodied in Queenslands
1952 Syllabus (see Section 3.5.2). Although the intent of a syllabus Number strand
should be to promote proficiency with a range of computational strategies with a
range of number types, by limiting the proposed mental strategies to operations with
whole numbers it is intended to encourage proficiency with particular mental
strategies before they are applied to decimals, money, measures, or percentages.
The essence of a mental strategy is not dependent upon the size or complexity of
the numbers being manipulated. For example, the use of the strategy add parts of
the second number to calculate 548 + 332 (548 + 300 + 30 + 2) or 5.48 + 3.32 (5.48
+ 3 + .3 + .02) is not intrinsically different to its use when calculating 48 + 32 (48 +
30 + 2). Moreover, other aspects of an individuals store of numerical equivalents
and calculative plans (see Section 2.8.1) are developed and applied when
alternative strategies are employed. When using a compensation strategy, for
example, to find the sum of 48 and 32 this could be calculated as 50 + 30 = 80.

348

Such an approach has links to compensatory strategies used to find approximate


answers.

5.3.3 Mental Strategies for Addition, Subtraction, Multiplication and Division


Although Tables 5.3-5.5 identify specific mental strategies which may be
appropriate for focussed teaching, given that the variety used by students exceeds
those which may be specifically taught (Heirdsfield, 1996, p. 131; Murray, Olivier, &
Human, 1991, pp. 50, 55), and that a desired outcome of mathematics teaching is
the development of an ability to respond flexibly and creatively to number situations
(AEC, 1991, p. 107), it is appropriate for a learning outcome for mental computation
to be expressed in general terms: Select and apply a variety of mental strategies in
a range of numerical situationsstrategies appropriate to the context and the
student.
The aim is for students to become proficient with a variety of heuristic
strategies (Carpenter, 1980, p. 317), both self-generated and taught. As discussed
in Section 2.7.4, such strategies are ones that rely on a well-developed sense of
number. Hence the main focus for teaching mental strategies for each operation is
the development of a proficiency with those that require one or more of the numbers
to be decomposed in order to transform a problem into one that is more
manageable, either through the use of a series of steps or through the application of
known facts.
Of the heuristic mental strategies discussed in Section 2.7.4, and summarised
in Table 2.4, four which have been frequently reported in the literature in relation to
addition and subtraction (see Tables 5.3 and 5.4) have been recommended for
focussed teachingadd or subtract parts of the first or second number,
incorporation, organisation, and compensation (Beishuizen, 1993, pp. 296-297;
Cooper et al., 1996, p. 159; Heirdsfield, 1996, pp. 133-134; Olander & Brown, 1959,
p. 99; Vakali, 1985, p. 111). Although Heirdsfield (1996, p. 132) found that using
mental analogues of the standard written algorithms for addition and subtraction
(see Table 2.4) were the most common strategies used by Year 4 students, these
have not been included in the recommendations. The emphasis is on the more
efficient left-to-right strategies. Add or subtract parts of the first or second number
and incorporation have the advantage of producing a single result at each step, thus

349

reducing the load on working memory (see Sections 2.8.3 & 2.8.4). The former,
together with organisation, is considered by Beishuizen (1993, p. 320) to be a basic
mental strategy for calculating with 2-digit numbers.
Cooper et al. (1996, p. 159) suggest that explicit exploration of holistic
strategies should be undertaken to reduce the emphasis on paper-and-pencil
procedures. The fourth heuristic strategy included for addition and subtraction,
compensation (see Tables 5.3 & 5.4), is classified by Heirdsfield (1996, p. 134) as a
holistic strategyone that requires the manipulation of numbers as single entities.
This strategy requires students to have a strong sense of the direction in which the
answer needs to be compensated if an undoing approach is employed. Markovits &
Sowder (1994, p. 15) report that this is particularly difficult for some Year 7 students
when using a compensation approach for subtraction.
The recommendations for addition and subtraction also include counting
strategies selected from those discussed in Section 2.7.4min of addends and min
of units (Resnick & Omanson, 1987, p. 66) for addition, and counting-on in twos,
tens and fives (Olander & Brown, 1959, p. 99) for subtraction. Except for min of
units, these strategies (see Tables 2.2, 5.3, & 5.4) require students to be operating
at least at Murray and Oliviers (1989, p. 6) second level of understanding 2-digit

350

Table 5.3
Mental Strategies Component for Addition of whole numbers beyond the basic facts
for Inclusion in the Number Strand of Future Mathematics Syllabuses for Primary
Schools
Year
1 -3

3-5

5-7

Students should be able to use:

Counting strategies
min of addends
add 2-digit and 1-digit number
23 + 9: 24, 25, 26...32; 32
min of units
add 2-digit and 1-digit number
23 + 9: 29 + 3, 30, 31, 32; 32

Heuristic strategies
add parts of the first or second number
add two 2-digit numbers
46 + 38: 46 + 30 = 76, 76 + 8 = 84
46 + 38: 40 + 38 = 78, 78 + 6 = 84
incorporation
add two 2-digit numbers
39 + 25: 30 + 20 = 50, 50 + 9 + 5 = 64
organisation
add two 2-digit numbers
58 + 34: 50 + 30 = 80, 8 + 4 = 12, 80 + 12 = 92
compensation
add two 2-digit numbers
28 + 29: 30 + 30 = 60, 60 -2 -1 = 57
28 + 29: 30 + 27 = 57

numbers. Each builds upon strategies used to develop basic fact knowledge for
addition and subtraction, particularly count-on, and on number knowledge
developed through counting in twos, tens and fives. Min of addends, classified as
an elementary counting strategy, constitutes one from which the various strategies
classified as counting in larger units may be developed (see Table 2.2). It is a

351

Table 5.4
Mental Strategies Component for Subtraction of whole numbers beyond the basic
facts for Inclusion in the Number Strand of Future Mathematics Syllabuses for
Primary Schools
Year
1 -3

3-5

5-7

Students should be able to use:

Counting strategy
counting-on in twos/tens/fives
subtract 2-digit number from 2-digit number
38 - 34: 36, 38; 4
71 - 44: 54, 64, 74; minus 3; 27
57 - 35: 40, 45, 50, 55; plus 2; 22

Heuristic strategies
subtract parts of the second number
subtract 2-digit number from 2-digit number
33 - 16: 33 - 10 = 23, 23 - 6 = 17
incorporation
subtract 2-digit number from 2-digit number
51 - 34: 50 - 30 = 20, 20 + 1 = 21, 21 - 4 = 17
organisation
subtract 2-digit number from 2-digit number
46 - 23: 40 - 20 = 20, 6 - 3 = 3, 20 + 3 = 13
46 - 28: 40 - 20 = 20, 10; 16 - 8 = 8, 10 + 8 = 18
compensation
subtract 2-digit number from 2-digit number
86 - 38: 88 - 40 = 48

strategy to which many students are likely to regress when required to compute with
unfamiliar numbers (Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 4).
Min of units is a strategy that relies on an ability to recognise the magnitude of
the numbers of units involved, and to exchange these as necessary, a strategy that
requires an understanding of numeration characteristic of Murray and Oliviers
(1989, p. 6) third level. It is this level which provides the conceptual basis for the
use of heuristic strategies (Murray & Olivier, 1989, p. 6). Hence, this strategy may
be difficult for some students in Years 1 to 3 (see Table 5.3). Further, using a counton in twos, tens or fives strategy for subtraction may also be difficult for many

352

children, particularly where compensation is involved (see Table 5.4). This


approach places greater demands on working memory, as not only does the number
of iterations need to be tracked, but also the differential between the minuend and
partial answer needs to be calculated. However, the variants of this strategy that
require compensatory procedures to be applied may assist in developing a feel for
the processes involved, processes essential to effectively using heuristic
compensatory strategies for calculating exact (see Table 5.4) and approximate
answers (see Section 2.6.2).
Counting strategies for multiplication and divisionrepeated addition and
repeated subtraction, respectively (see Table 2.2)have not been recommended for
focussed teaching. Although these strategies will be used by students as their
understanding of multiplication and division is developed, they become inefficient
with increased number size (Carraher et al., 1985, p. 28). More importantly, the
models on which these strategies are based are not those commonly employed to
develop basic fact knowledge for multiplication and division. Such knowledge is
developed through thinking strategies based on array and partition models,
respectively.
As discussed in Section 2.7.4, the strategies recommended for multiplication
and division (Table 5.5) do not have as wide a research base as those for addition
and subtraction. Little research has been undertaken into mental strategies for
division, and that which has been reported, in common with that for multiplication,
has tended to focus on students in Year 7 at least (Hope, 1987; Hope & Sherrill,
1987; Markovits & Sowder, 1994; B. J. Reys, 1986b). Nonetheless, Gracey (1994,
pp. 84, 90, 101) has successfully taught multiplication strategies, which may be
classified as general factoring and half-and-double (Hope, 1987, p. 334), to Year 6
students. In common with the heuristic strategies recommended for addition and
subtraction, factoring strategies, as discussed in Section 2.7.4, are based on a
relational understanding of number. Consequently, their use depends upon, and
contributes to, the development of numerical equivalents and calculative plans,
which are part of an individuals declarative and procedural knowledge, respectively
(Hamann & Ashcraft, 1985, p. 52; Hunter, 1978, p. 339; Putman et al., 1988, p. 83).

353

Table 5.5
Mental Strategies Component for Multiplication and Division of whole numbers
beyond the basic facts for Inclusion in the Number Strand of Future Mathematics
Syllabuses for Primary Schools
Year
1 -3
Students should be able to use:
MULTIPLICATION
general factoring
multiply 2-digit number by 1- or 2-digit number
15 x 8: 5 x 3 x 2 x 4, 10 x 12 = 120
60 x 15: 60 x 5 x 3, 300 x 3 = 900
additive distribution
multiply 2-digit number by 1- or 2-digit number
23 x 4: 20 x 4 + 3 x 4, 80 + 12 = 92
21 x 13: 20 x 13 + 1 x 13, 260 + 13 = 273
subtractive distribution
multiply 2-digit number by 1- or 2-digit number
98 x 8: 100 x 8 - 2 x 8, 800 - 16 = 784
19 x 13: 20 x 13 - 1 x 13, 260 - 13 = 247
half-and-double
multiply 2-digit number by 1- or 2-digit number
42 x 5: 21 x 10 = 210
60 x 15: 30 x 30 = 900
aliquot parts
multiply 2-digit number by 1- or 2-digit number
14 x 5: 14 x (10 2), (14 x 10) 2, 140 2 = 70
48 x 25: 48 x (100 4) = (48 4) x 100 = 12 x 100 = 1200

DIVISION
general factoring
divide 2-digit number by 1- or 2-digit divisor
90 15: 90 3 5, 30 5 = 6
additive distribution
divide 2-digit number by 1- or 2-digit divisor
64 4: 60 4 + 4 4 = 15 + 1 = 16
78 15: 60 15 + 18 15 = 4 + 1 rem 3 = 5 rem 3
subtractive distribution
divide 2-digit number by 1- or 2-digit divisor
76 4: 80 4 - 4 4 = 20 - 1 = 19

3-5

5-7

354

A third factoring strategy, aliquot parts, has also been included in the
recommendations (see Table 5.5). This strategy draws upon an individuals
understanding of special products, an essential component of mental multiplication,
which refers to those products that are easily found by multiplying by a power of 10
or a multiple of a power of 10 (Hazekamp, 1986, p. 117). This interpretation of
aliquot parts contrasts with that expressed in the 1930 and 1952 Syllabuses (see
Section 3.5.2) where, as traditionally included as one of the essential short methods
of mental calculation, the emphasis was on aliquot parts of a pound, thus reflecting
mental arithmetics use as a book-keeping tool.
Aliquot parts, together with additive and subtractive distribution, was found to
be the most difficult strategy for multiplication for Year 5 students by B. J. Reys et al.
(1993, p. 310). Nonetheless, a form of the latter was one frequently used by Year 2
and 3 students who were encouraged to explore division through a constructivist
approach which emphasised experimentation and discussion (Murray et al., 1991, p.
54). Both of these distribution strategies have been successfully taught to Year 6
students for mental multiplication (Gracey, 1994, pp. 68, 73).
Despite the lack of research support, it is considered reasonable to propose
that general factoring and additive distribution should also be a focus for the
development of mental strategies for division (see Table 5.5). General factoring is
closely linked to number fact knowledge for multiplication and division, and additive
distribution is considered to be the calculative drafthorse of expert mental
calculators (Hope, 1985, p. 358). Nonetheless, exponential and iterative factoring,
and fractional and quadratic distribution (see Table 2.4) have not been included in
the recommendations for focused teaching. Although these strategies are also used
by exceptional mental calculators (Hope, 1985, pp. 361-362, 365-368; Hope &
Sherrill, 1987, pp. 102-103), they are not widely reported in the literature. More
importantly, their complexity removes them from those easily understood by primary
school students.

5.4

Concluding Points
Prior to the implementation of the suggestions contained in the revised

sequential framework (Table 5.2), and in the proposed mental strategies component
(Tables 5.3-5.5), a number of points need to be taken into consideration. With
respect to the former, as previously intimated, the implementation of a mental-

355

written sequence for introducing computational methods has the potential to


severely threaten the sense of efficacy most teachers now hold towards written
computation. It is imperative that teachers are informed about the proposed
changes, and assisted in the actuation of the revised sequenceissues explored
more deeply in the next chapter.
Also, the mental strategies relating to each of the four operations proposed for
direct teaching need to be validated through further research prior to their
incorporation in a future mathematics syllabusvalidation with respect to their
appropriateness and year-level placement. Consideration needs to be given to the
range of variants of a particular strategy, some of which may be inappropriate for
focussed teaching for many children. However, the range of possible strategies
should allow teachers to maintain a focus on student-generated strategies during
focussed teaching, particularly where students are given opportunities to explore
strategies using their intuitive knowledge, and are actively involved in the learning
process through social discourse (Gracey, 1994, pp. 129-130).
Data from such research would assist in the provision of specific guidance for
teachers. Further information would be gained concerning factors which impinge on
the use of the mental strategies recommended for focussed teaching. Such
information could include: (a) the demands of particular strategies on working
memory; (b) the effects arising from the need to regroup or carry, and the size and
type of numbers being manipulated; (c) the level of numeration understanding
required for particular strategies, one consequence of which would be to extend the
work of Murray and Olivier (1989); (d) the nature of any difficulties experienced in
balancing strategy development through using elements of the behaviourist and
constructivist approaches; (e) the consequences arising from using visual and oral
stimuli, and examples presented in and without context; (f) the appropriateness of
various models which may be used to represent the calculative situation; and (g) the
effectiveness of discussion as a means for encouraging strategy growth, and how it
should be managed within the classroom, issues questioned by some respondents
to the survey of Queensland school personnel (see Section 4.3.3).

356

CHAPTER 6

MENTAL COMPUTATION:
CONCLUSIONS and IMPLICATIONS

6.1

Restatement of Background and Purpose of Study


From the analyses presented, it is evident that, in contrast to the American

experience, in particular, a continued emphasis has been placed on the calculation


of exact answers mentally in Queensland State primary schools from 1860, albeit
one that has fluctuated over time, due particularly to variations in teacher
interpretations of the syllabus. Although the mathematics syllabuses from 1966 in
Queensland have not explicitly given such calculation a high profile, mathematics
educators, from the mid-1980s, have been taking a renewed interest in mental
computation. While not neglecting the correctness of the answer, it is now
recommended that the emphasis be placed on the mental processes employed. It is
this which distinguishes mental computation from earlier considerations in which the
correctness of the answer was of prime concernthat is, to distinguish mental
computation from mental arithmetic.
The resurgence of interest in mental calculation, as outlined in Chapter 1, has
its origins in a number of sources. Calculating mentally remains a viable alternative,
despite the availability of various electronic calculating devices. It continues to be
the major form of calculation used in every-day life. Such mental calculations are
typically undertaken using methods adapted to the particular characteristics of the
situation in which they occur (Lave, 1985, p. 173). Coupled with this recognition is
the realisation that the standard written algorithmsa major focus of primary school
mathematicsare seldom used outside the classroom. School-taught algorithms are
predominantly employed only in the solution of school-type problems (Carraher et
al., 1987, p. 95). Further, as the Mathematical Sciences Education Board and the
National Research Council (1990, p. 19) have observed, there is sufficient research

357

evidence now available to suggest that an overemphasis on written methods may


reduce an individual's development of flexible mental strategies. Hence it is argued
that paper-and-pencil skills should receive decreased attention in schools.
Mental computation is no longer viewed as an end in itself, but rather as a
means for promoting an individual's ability to think mathematically (R. E. Reys,
1992, p. 63). This, in Reys and Barger's (1994, p. 31) view, is the novel facet of the
current resurgence of interest. However, if this facet is to be realised, teachers
need to be persuaded to place an emphasis on encouraging children to devise
personal mental strategies and to compare their strategies with those of others, so
that they may be able to select the most appropriate method in accordance with
their strengths and the particular mathematical context (AEC, 1990, p. 109).
However, as McIntosh (1990a, p. 25) has pointed out, little attention is currently
being given to actually teaching mental computation in Australian primary
classrooms. The relevance of this observation to Queensland was one focus of the
survey reported in Chapter 4. It is only in some of the sourcebooks, which
operationalise the current mathematics syllabus in Queensland, particularly in that
for Year 5, that specific references to mental computation have been included.
However, the philosophy of the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of
Education, 1987a) is not in conflict with the recommendations of mathematics
educators concerning how mental computation should be taught. Nevertheless, the
significant debate necessary for curriculum change in Australian, and particularly
Queensland, schools has not yet occurred. For mental procedures to be given preeminence over written procedures, a revolution in the way teachers and parents
view mathematics will need to be achieved.
In consequence, this study provides a comprehensive summary of the state of
knowledge about mental computation. Key aspects of mental computation within
primary school curricula have been analysed from past, contemporary, and futures
perspectives. Based on these analyses, recommendations concerning how mental
computation could be incorporated into the Number strand of future mathematics
syllabuses have been delineated (Chapter 5).
It is believed that by having undertaken this research from various
perspectives, contributions have been made towards creating an enriched context
for debate about future modifications to the mathematics taught in Queensland
primary schools. Consequently, this chapter provides (a) conclusions about mental

358

computation across the range of time periods considered, (b) an outline of the
implications these conclusions hold for syllabus development, and (c) an analysis of
the aspects of mental computation that require further theoretical and empirical
investigation (see Section 1.4.2). The data on which these conclusions,
recommendations, and implications have been based were obtained from the review
of the pedagogical, socio-anthropological and psychological literature relevant to
mental computation (Chapter 2), and analyses of mental computation in Queensland
primary schools from past and contemporary perspectivesthrough an examination
of archival material (Chapter 3), and the analysis of a postal, self-completion
questionnaire (Chapter 4).

6.2

Mental Computation: Conclusions


This study has analysed the beliefs and practices pertaining to the nature,

function, and teaching methods associated with the mental calculation of exact
answers beyond the basic facts, from theoretical and Queensland perspectives.
The conclusions which have been drawn need to be interpreted within the confines
of the historical and survey data obtained. The former were derived from syllabus
documents, textbooks, and the recorded beliefs and opinions of Departmental
personnel, primarily the annual reports of senior Departmental officers and
publications of the Queensland Teachers' Union. Taken together, these documents
have provided a comprehensive source from which an understanding of both the
intent and practice related to mental arithmetic has been obtained for the period
1860-1965.
As noted in Chapter 4, the data applying to 1965-1987 may only be considered
suggestive of the beliefs and practices which prevailed during that period, as few
respondents had taught under the 1964 Syllabus or the first edition of the Program
in Mathematics (Department of Education, 1966-1968). However, with respect to
the beliefs and practices related to the current syllabus, which was formally
implemented in 1988, it was concluded that the numbers of valid cases were
sufficient to permit meaningful conclusions to be drawn, despite the non-response
rate being relatively high (see Section 4.3.2). Further, based on anecdotal evidence
obtained from some Departmental Education Advisers (Mathematics) during 1996, it

359

is probable that the pattern of beliefs and practices in evidence during 1993 largely
remains unchanged. It was their opinion that mental computation had received
negligible attention during inservice related to the Student Performance Standards
conducted in 1994 and 1995, despite such calculation being emphasised in the
Standards for Number (Student Performance Standards, 1994, pp. 28, 42, 58, 72).
In presenting the conclusions which may be drawn from this study, it is
intended to provide a summary of key similarities and differences between the
beliefs and teaching practices related to mental computation currently advocated by
mathematics educators, and those of Queensland teachers across the eras
investigated. Consideration has been given to: (a) the emphasis placed on the
mental calculation of exact answers, (b) the roles ascribed to such calculation, (c)
the nature of these calculations, and (d) the approaches to teaching mental
computation.

6.2.1 The Emphasis Placed on Mental Computation.


Computational competence is now believed to involve more than the routine
application of memorised rules. It encompasses an expertise in higher-order
thinking, a sound understanding of mathematical principles, and an ability to know
when and how to use a variety of procedures for calculating (NCSM, 1989, p. 44), a
belief supported by the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of
Education, 1987a, pp. 7-11, 15) and one pre-empted by the 1964 and 1966-1968
Syllabuses. Nonetheless, paper-and-pencil calculation using standard algorithms
continues to be the major focus of the number work undertaken by Queensland
children. Although the 1987 Syllabus explicitly emphasises computational
estimation, recognition of the ability to calculate exact answers mentally is implicit.
Estimating is specified as one of the mathematical processes to be developed
(Department of Education, 1987a, p. 8), whereas procedures for calculating exact
answers mentally are incorporated under Calculating"calculating with addition,
subtraction and multiplication using mental and calculator procedures, and written
algorithms...as appropriate for the context" (Department of Education, 1987a, p. 15).
This implies that for mental computation to be considered as the method of first
resort (AEC, 1990, p. 109), it will require greater prominence, both in syllabus

360

documents and in the manner in which calculative methods are taught. The
recommendations outlined in Chapter 5 are designed to aid in the achievement of
this goal.
Prominence was given to the calculation of exact answers mentally in all
Queensland syllabuses from 1860 to 1964, albeit under various headings, which
included mental exercises, mental, mental work, oral work, mental and oral work,
oral arithmetic and mental arithmetic. From that of 1904, each syllabus emphasised
that "mental calculations should be the basis of all instruction" ("Schedule XIV,
1904, p. 201). The 1914 and 1930 Syllabuses suggested that "new types of
problems should invariably be introduced in this way" (Department of Public
Instruction, 1914, p. 61; 1930, p. 31), whereas the 1952 Syllabus stressed that "all
written work should be preceded by introductory oral exercises" (Department of
Public Instruction, 1952b, p. 2).
Despite the prominence given to mental arithmetic in the syllabus documents
and the exhortations of District Inspectors of Schools for teachers to implement their
intent, the generally low standard of mental calculation was most commonly
attributed to its not receiving sufficient regular and systematic treatment during the
period 1860-1965. Mental arithmetic appeared to be the bte noir of many teachers
("Mental Arithmetic, 1927, p. 18), an outcome of the characteristics ascribed to it
and of the ways in which it was taught. Nonetheless, it can be concluded that
mental arithmetic received some emphasis by teachers during the period 18601965.
Somewhat paradoxically, although the Program in Mathematics (Department of
Education, 1966-68, 1975) did not give mental arithmetic the prominence of earlier
syllabuses, the survey data suggest that middle- and upper-school teachers
continued to place importance on such calculation. However, this emphasis
progressively decreased from 1969 to 1987 (see Table 4.13). Additionally, whereas
approximately 53% of middle- and upper-school teachers agreed that the 1987
Syllabus places little emphasis on the development of the ability to calculate exact
answers mentally (see Table 4.8), 89% indicated that they at least sometimes
focused specifically on developing the ability to calculate exact answers mentally
beyond the basic facts (see Table 4.12). However, in so doing, teaching practices
classified as traditional in this study continue to be integral to the range employed

361

(see Figure 4.4), thus placing limits on the degree to which children are developing
flexible, idiosyncratic mental strategies.

6.2.2 Roles of Mental Computation


The various roles attributed to mental computation during the period being
investigated are associated with its perceived usefulness for strengthening the mind,
and with its social and pedagogical usefulness. Undertaking mental calculations is
not only the simplest method for performing many arithmetical procedures, it is also
the main form of calculation used in everyday life. However, specific recognition
was given to this only in the 1952 and 1964 Syllabuses, with the former
acknowledging that "oral arithmetic is more commonly used in after-school life than
written arithmetic" (Department of Education, 1952b, p. 2), a view with which
present-day school personnel concur (see Table 4.8). Nonetheless, the social
usefulness of mental computation was recognised by the authors of textbooks used
in Queensland schools from 1860. Park (1879), for example, stressed that mental
arithmetic was a "subject of great practical importance" (p. 42), a conviction often
reiterated by District Inspectors in their annual reports. Representative of these was
that of Bevington (1926) who commented that "in domestic and mercantile
transactions, in calculating about farms, &c., mental exercises are so frequent that it
seems to be absolutely essential that children [should] be trained to calculate
quickly and accurately" (p. 80).
However, it is in relation to the pedagogical usefulness of mental computation,
that distinctions can more clearly be drawn between the beliefs presently held by
mathematics educators and those of the past. In contrast to past beliefs and
practices, which focused on gaining answers speedily and accurately, it is now
recommended by mathematics educators that the focus should be on the mental
processes involved. Such a focus would allow mental computation to be used as a
tool to facilitate the meaningful development of mathematical concepts and skillsto
promote thinking, conjecturing and generalising based upon conceptual
understanding (Reys & Barger, 1994, p. 31). Hence, mental computation is closely
linked to the development of number sense, to gaining mathematical power, a power
that would be enhanced where the gap between learning and using school

362

mathematics and that used outside the classroom is bridged (Willis, 1990, p. 9). An
emphasis on the processes involved in mental computation could provide that
bridge. Folk mathematics is essentially mental (Maier, 1980) and often involves the
manipulation of non-standard units using invented strategies (Lave, 1985, p. 173).
Hence, in McIntosh's (1990a, p. 37) view, mental computation is the most readily
available means by which an understanding of how numbers generally behave may
be gained, a belief supported by many current school personnel (see Table 4.9).
While occasional recognition was given to mental arithmetic as a means for
"making scholars think clearly and systematically about number" ("Teaching Hints:
Arithmetic, 1908, p. 15), the primary pedagogical function of mental work during the
period 1860-1965 was to familiarise students with the arithmetical operations prior to
an emphasis on paper-and-pencil calculation. This mental-written sequence was
encapsulated in the 1904 Syllabus by its stressing that "the pupils should be made
familiar by mental exercises with the principles underlying every process before the
written work is undertaken" ("Schedule XIV, 1904, p. 201), a sequence that was
embodied in the spiral nature of the 1930 Syllabus. However, the failure on the part
of teachers to sufficiently model their mental arithmetic examples on the written work
that was to follow was a regular criticism of District Inspectors in their annual
reports.
Mental work was also considered to be an effective means for cultivating speed
and accuracy in new work and for the revision of the arithmetic procedures (Board of
Education, 1937, p. 513; Cochran, 1960, p. 12; Mutch, 1916, p. 62). However, such
a focus possibly contributed to the belief that mental arithmetic entailed the
presentation of a series of often one-step examples, the focus of which was the
gaining of correct answers, the traditional view of mental computation, as defined in
this study.
It is with respect to the role of mental arithmetic as a means for "improving the
tenacity of the mind" (Wilkins, 1886, p. 40) that sharp distinctions may be drawn
between past beliefs and those currently advocated by mathematics educators.
Nonetheless, aspects of this traditional view would appear to remain in the minds of
many Queensland teachers and administrators. Approximately 67% of respondents
supported the view that "children's mental processes are sharpened by starting a
mathematics lesson with ten quick questions to be solved mentally" (see Table
4.10).

363

Such a view has as its origins the tenets of formal discipline which were
espoused particularly in the 19th century. This theory held that the mind was
composed of a number of distinct powers or faculties, including memory, attention,
observation, reasoning and will (Kolesnik, 1958, p. 6). Late last century, Robinson
(1882) had maintained that the value of giving complex calculations, beyond the
requirements of the various syllabuses of the period, was "the formation of a power
of concentrating all the faculties on the performances of an allotted task...[so that]
the mind... [would] prove capable of any amount of labour upon other tasks" (p.
178). Such a belief resulted in mental arithmetic being considered essentially as the
"working [of] certain hard numbers in the shortest time by the shortest method"
("Mental Arithmetic: A Few Suggestions, 1910, p. 176).
Despite the discrediting of these beliefs early in the 20th century, and
statements by senior Departmental officers affirming their belief that there is no such
thing as general mental training, and that learning in one subject cannot be
transferred to another (Edwards, 1936, p. 16), teachers and inspectors retained their
beliefs in the role of mental arithmetic as a means for quickening the intelligence,
developing judgement, improving reasoning (Baker, 1929, p. 281; Mutch, 1924, p.
40) and for enhancing an individual's ability to concentrate on mathematical tasks
(Martin, 1916, p. 135). The maintenance of these beliefs was supported by the
influence of the English view of arithmetic, which represented arithmetic as logic
(Ballard, 1928a, p. xi). Hence, mental arithmetic was judged to be the means by
which children were trained to think and reason: "Intelligence in Arithmetic should be
secured through the medium of mental exercises" (Bevington, 1925, p. 83), with
accuracy in thinking and reasoning of paramount importance (Martin, 1920a, p. 81).
Nevertheless, Burns (1973, p. 4) concluded that most teachers were primarily
concerned with imparting factual knowledge. Hence, their concern was almost
exclusively with memory.

6.2.3 The Nature of Mental Computation


Essential to the nature of mental computation, as now conceived, is Plunkett's
(1979, p. 2) notion of mental strategies as being fleeting, variable, flexible, active,
holistic, constructive, and iconic. Such strategies are not designed for recording, and

364

require an understanding of the mathematical relationships embodied in the problem


task environment. These features contrast with those of the standard paper-andpencil algorithms. Such procedures are believed to be standardised, contracted,
efficient, automatic, symbolic, general, and analytic. Further, they are not easily
internalised and encourage cognitive passivity (Plunkett, 1979, p. 3). The use of
mental strategies facilitates a focus on quantities rather than on symbols, thus
permitting an individual to make meaningful alterations to the problems encountered
and to work with quantities that can be manipulated more easily (Carraher et al.,
1987, p. 94), the latter being those that are dependent upon an individual's store of
conceptual knowledge in long-term memory (see Figure 2.3).
The survey data indicated that Queensland teachers supported the notion that
mental computation encourages children to devise ingenious computational short
cuts and helps children to gain an understanding of the relationships between
numbers (see Table 4.9). Such a notion supports the belief that mental computation
bears a reciprocal relationship with conceptual and procedural knowledge (see
Figure 2.3). It was also recognised that children who are proficient at mentally
calculating exact answers use personal adaptations of written algorithms and
idiosyncratic mental strategies (see Table 4.9). While it can be concluded that
Queensland State school personnel tend towards holding nontraditional views about
the nature of mental computation (see Figure 4.1), this conclusion needs to be
tempered by the finding that approximately 40% of teachers believe that calculating
exact answers mentally involves applying rules by rote (see Table 4.9).
As early as 1887, District Inspector Kennedy had argued against mental
arithmetic being viewed as the mere application of fixed rules (Kennedy, 1887, p.
82), rules that were generally listed at the end of the treatises on mathematics used
by teachers. Park (1879) had previously stressed that where rules were to be
taught they should not be "got by rote" (p. 43). Nonetheless, the period 1860-1965
was characterised by an importance placed upon the rote application of short
methods of mental calculation, although it was only in the 1930 Syllabus, and its
amendments in 1938 and 1948, that specific references were made to such
calculations in schedules and syllabuses. That the 1952 Syllabus did not refer to
such methods was a situation with which District Inspector Crampton (1956, p. 5)
did not approve, maintaining that practical short methods should have been
prescribed.

365

Little emphasis appears to have been placed on permitting children to devise


their own mental strategies. Nevertheless, teachers were occasionally encouraged
in articles in the Queensland Teachers' Journal and the Education Office Gazette to
permit children to invent short methods for themselves. Some encouragement was
also contained in the 1930 Syllabus and the 1948 Amendments. Complementing
practice in the use of short methods, Grade V children were to be encouraged to
devise different solutions for particular problems, but only "after the pupils [had]
been thoroughly exercised in any rule" (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p.
41; 1948, p. 13).
The 1952 Syllabus appears somewhat equivocal about the nature of oral
arithmetic. While stating that "its application should not be limited to short methods
or similar devices, it also maintained that "the processes applied orally [that is,
mentally,] are the same as those used in written operations" (Department of Public
Instruction, 1952b, p. 2). It is the latter view of mental arithmetic that was
emphasised for all grades in all schedules and syllabuses from that of 1904, albeit a
view that was not often realised, as recorded by District Inspectors in their reports.
Rather, teachers tended to view mental arithmetic as the presentation of a series of
often unrelated oral questions, the aim of which was to obtain correct answers,
speedily and accurately.
Such a conviction was supported by the format of textbooks and articles on
mental arithmetic in the Queensland Teachers' Journal. As Lidgate (1954) noted,
"There [was] a general tendency to test rather than to teach Mental (p. 2). This,
combined with the practice of providing examples beyond the requirements of
particular grades by teachers, District Inspectors and Head Teachers, eventuated in
Greenhalgh (1947, p. 11) condemning the way in which mental arithmetic was
taught for the nervous strain that was being placed on children. However, this was
a practice that continued at least into the late-1950s. District Inspector Kehoe
(1957) recorded in his 1956 Annual Report that "some teachers were puzzling their
pupils in tables and giving them mental gymnastics by ingenious and complicated
methods they have evolved for working, for example, extensions tables and
measurement tables" (p. 2).

366

6.2.4 Approaches to Teaching Mental Computation


Robert Reys et al. (1995, p. 305) have suggested that there are two broad
approaches to teaching mental computationa behaviourist approach and a
constructivist approach. The former views mental computation as a basic skill and
is considered to be an essential prerequisite to written computation, with proficiency
gained through direct teaching. However, the constructivist approach contends that
mental computation is a process of higher-order thinking in which the act of
generating and applying mental strategies is significant for an individual's
mathematical development. In both approaches, the emphasis is placed on mental
strategies. This contrasts with the traditional approach to the mental computation.
Although also behaviourist in nature, it focuses on the speed and accuracy with
which answers are obtainedmental arithmetic, as defined in this study.
From 1860 to 1965 teachers were impelled by District Inspectors and syllabus
documents to make mental arithmetic a part of every lesson. The aim was for it not
to be taught in isolation (Department of Public Instruction, 1914, p. 61), but as an
introduction to all written workfor teaching the principle of an operation, for
promoting speed and accuracy at the mechanical stage, and for applying the
particular operation in problem situations. Hence, mental examples were to be
based on the written work that was to follow, with these carefully graded prior to a
lesson. Although mathematics educators would agree that mental computation
needs to be a part of every mathematics lesson, Rathmell and Trafton (1990, p.
160) assert that it should not be considered as a separate topic with a set of ordered
skills. Rather, a focus on mental calculation should receive an on-going emphasis
throughout all situations requiring computation, thus leading children to view mental
methods as legitimate computational alternatives (B. J. Reys, 1985, p. 46).
Nonetheless, it was argued in Section 5.4.1 that, in context with developing
proficiency with mental computation in a mental-written sequence for each operation
(Table 5.2), it was legitimate to highlight particular mental strategies for direct
teaching (Tables 5.3-5.5). This is not to suggest that mental computation should be
considered as a discrete topic. Rather it recognises that, for some children, the
spontaneous invention of mental strategies may be difficult (Carroll, 1996, p. 4).
Further, it also recognises that those who demonstrate low proficiency with mental
computation may benefit from a teacher-directed focus on selected mental

367

strategies (Cooper et al., 1996, p. 159). The strategies highlighted are those
counting and heuristic strategies (Tables 2.2 & 2.4) considered accessible for most
children in each of the three year-level clusters (Tables 5.3-5.5). These strategies
contribute to and draw upon a students sense of number.
As B. J. Reys et al. (1993, p. 314) have observed, mental computation can
provide numerous opportunities for the development of mathematical thinking.
However, it requires regular and systematic practice. District Inspectors of Schools
prior to 1965 often exhorted teachers to provide additional opportunities for practice
in mental arithmetic. Preferably this was to be undertaken for short periods in the
mornings when children's minds were fresh (Baker, 1929; Drain, 1941, p. 2;
Teaching Hints, 1908, p. 15). Lessons were characterised by rapid question
delivery, with the profit generally considered to be in the number of questions
answered correctly (Gladman, 1904, p. 215). Martin (1916) advocated that
explanations should be kept to a minimum as a means for ensuring a "concentrating
of the mind" (p. 135).
This contrasts with current beliefs about teaching mental computation, the
essence of which is the focus on assisting children to see how to calculate mentally.
The object is to play with numbers, to explore their relationships (C. Thornton,
1985, p. 10). Hope (1985, p. 372) and Olander and Brown (1959, p. 109) have
observed that proficient mental calculators exhibit a passion for numbers, which is
reflected in the degree to which calculating mentally is practised.
Elements of the traditional approach remained in teachers' repertoires following
the introduction of the Program in Mathematics (Department of Education, 1967a,
1968) in Grades 4 to 7 during 1968 and 1969, albeit with decreasing emphasis
during the use of this syllabus and its 1975 revision. The survey data analysed in
Chapter 4 revealed that middle- and upper-school teachers continued to emphasise
speed and the use of rules to calculate mentally during the periods 1969-1974 and
1975-1987 (see Table 4.14). Similar findings were found for those who were
classroom teachers in 1993, with the majority also indicating that they, at least
sometimes, gave several one-step questions and simply marked the answers as
correct or incorrect (see Table 4.12). Essential to the retention of these approaches
was the nature of the textbooks identified by teachers as ones used to support
mental computation (see Table 4.18).

368

These findings with respect to traditional practices were tempered by middleand upper-school teachers indicating that they also used constructivist approaches
to developing skill with mental computation. The majority disclosed that they, at
least sometimes, allowed children to decide on which mental strategy to employ,
explain and discuss their strategies, and relate methods for calculating beyond the
basic facts to the thinking strategies used in their development (see Table 4.12).
Overall, the teaching practices of contemporary classroom teachers tend to reflect
nontraditional approaches to developing mental computation skills (see Figure 4.4).
Nonetheless, this conclusion needs to be considered cautiously. Although there
was some consistency between their beliefs about how mental computation should
be taught and the teaching practices that they employed, many respondents
appeared to be focused on basic facts rather than on more complex mental
calculations (see Table 4.17). Further, their apparent acceptance of constructivist
approaches may derive, not from an understanding of mental computation per se,
but from an acceptance of the principles of teaching mathematics embodied in the
1987 Syllabus and its supporting documents. "Discussion between the teacher and
students and between the students themselves, for example, is listed as one of six
recommended approaches for teaching mathematics (Department of Education,
1987b, p. 3).

6.3

Implications for Decision Making Concerning Syllabus


Revision
This study, as described in Section 1.3, has significance for (a) pedagogy, (b)

the use of mathematics, (c) curriculum development, and (d) Queensland


educational history. Key issues related to the nature and role of mental computation
have been identified, together with an analysis of suggested teaching approaches.
Essential to this analysis is the recommended sequence for introducing mental,
written and technological methods for calculating (Table 5.2), and the proposed
sequence for focusing on particular mental strategies during particular year-level
periods (Tables 5.3-5.5). Their implementation, in context with teachers gaining an
understanding of the breadth of mental strategies used by proficient mental

369

calculators, should assist children to come to view mental computation as the


method of first resort.
More generally, this study provides a comprehensive source of data for
mathematics educators, mathematics teachers and curriculum developers. By
analysing mental computation from an historical perspective, cognisance may be
given to the nature of past curricula and their implementation during future revisions
to mathematics syllabuses. Syllabus development and implementation, however,
need to be guided by research, in context with informed debate within society and
within schools, in particular. Critical to the latter is adequate professional
development for teachers of mathematics.

6.3.1 Fostering Debate about Computation


For children to become proficient calculators using a range of mental,
technological, and paper-and-pencil strategies requires that syllabus developers,
teachers, and parents become familiar with the issues surrounding computational
methods. The decision by the Minister for Education (Office of the Minister for
Education, 1996, p. 1) not to reintroduce Student Performance Standards into
Queensland schools in 1997 has removed a potentially potent platform from which a
focus on mental computation, and computational procedures in general, could have
been fostered. Whereas an emphasis on mental strategies, and appropriate
methods for teaching, can be incorporated within the approach to teaching
mathematics embodied in the Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of
Education, 1987a), the implementation of a revised sequence for introducing
computational procedures, as proposed in Table 5.2, requires a long period of
professional and public debate. Teachers, parents and employers will need to be
convinced that a reduced emphasis on the standard written algorithms accompanied
by an increased emphasis on non-standard mental, calculator and paper-and-pencil
strategies should result in increased computational proficiency.
Teachers will require extensive inservice to support the implementation of a
mental-written computational sequence. Managing learning while allowing children
to calculate using self-generated written strategies will require many teachers to
broaden their repertoire of procedures for organising for mathematics learning.

370

Conferencing techniques used to guide the development of children's writing may


need to be adapted for the mathematics classroom. The use of self-generated
strategies, whether mental, technological or written, individualises learning and
therefore instruction, particularly where the goal of instruction is to promote an
individual's ability to think creatively and independently.
As revealed in Chapter 4, Queensland teachers believe that it is important for
inservice on mental computation to be made available (see Table 4.15). It is
through such professional development that teachers could become acquainted with
the range of issues surrounding the nature, sequence, and role of mental,
technological, and written computational procedures. Queensland teachers, while
retaining many traditional beliefs about mental computation also hold some
classified as nontraditional in this study (see Figure 4.1), an orientation conducive to
their accepting many of the beliefs about the nature and role of mental computation
currently espoused by mathematics educators. However, this acceptance does not
at present extend to a change in the traditional sequence for introducing
computational proceduresapproximately 65% of teachers disagreed with the
proposition that an emphasis on written algorithms needs to be delayed so that
mental computation can be given increased attention (see Table 4.10).
Skager and Weinberg (1971) suggest that an analysis of past syllabuses and
their implementation enables curriculum developers to "know where [they] have
been" (p. 50). The analysis presented in this study (Chapter 3) revealed that a
frequent criticism of the various syllabuses from 1860 was that their implementation
was usually accompanied by limited inservice opportunities for teachers, a key
factor in teachers not teaching mathematics in the spirit intended. Teacher
inservice, therefore, is crucial to effective syllabus implementation. However, as
Blakers (1978) has pessimistically cautioned: "With virtually every primary school
teacher teaching mathematics, the problems of adequate inservice preparation for
significant changes at the primary level are well nigh insoluble" (p. 153).
Nonetheless, if children are to gain mastery over of a range of flexible computational
techniques and to learn mathematics that is significant and of value to their
individual success, in both private and professional endeavours, it is essential that
an emphasis is placed on facilitating teacher change with respect to their beliefs and
practices related to the calculations of both exact and approximate answers. This

371

needs to occur in the wider context of reanalysing their beliefs about mathematics
and mathematics teaching.
In common with the United States' Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for
School Mathematics (NCTM, 1989, p. 9), a concern to encourage teachers to adopt
conceptual rather than a calculational orientation (Thompson, Philipp, P. W.
Thompson, & Boyd, 1994, p. 86) for teaching mathematics is embodied in the Years
1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus (Department of Education, 1987a), the Years 1 to 10
Mathematics Teaching, Curriculum and Assessment Guidelines (Department of
Education, 1987b) and in A National Statement on Mathematics for Australian
Schools (AEC, 1990). Such an orientation is in harmony with the current view of
mental computation and how it should be taught. Thompson et al. (1994) contend
that "conceptually oriented teachers focus children toward a rich conception of
situations, ideas and relationships among ideas" (p. 86)key factors in developing
and being able to apply flexible mental strategies.
Making this shift requires a depth of reflection by teachers on the images and
outcomes of mathematics and mathematics teaching. The Professional Standards
for Teaching Mathematics (NCTM, 1991, p. 160) emphasises that opportunities
need to be provided for teachers to "examine and revise their assumptions about
the nature of mathematics, how it should be taught, and how students learn
mathematics" (p. 160). Of significance for the implementation of new syllabuses, is
the recognition that any recommended changes to teaching practices will not
substantially impact upon what occurs in schools should the examination and
revision of these assumptions not occur (D'Arcy, 1996, p. 1). This is particularly
relevant during periods of system initiated change, which is usually perceived as
being imposed upon teachers rather than teacher initiated.
Opportunities for teachers to become familiar with the issues related to mental
computation need to occur in context with viewing teacher change as a process of
individual growth or learning (Clarke & Hollingsworth, 1994, pp. 158-159), a process
encapsulated by Clarke and Peters (1993, p. 170) Dynamic Model of Professional
Growth. This multidimensional model recognises, in accordance with that of Guskey
(1985, p. 58), that significant changes to teacher beliefs and attitudes only occur
after valued learning outcomes have arisen from experimentation with changed
classroom practices. Of significance to gaining teacher acceptance of the
recommended changes to teaching computation is the recognition that the desired

372

learning outcomes, particularly the emphasis on self-generated mental and paperand-pencil strategies, are in sharp contrast to the current emphasis on standard
written algorithms.
Although teachers adapt rather than adopt new teaching practices (Clarke,
1994, p. 46; Fennema & Franke, 1992, p. 162), Doyle and Ponder (1977, p. 8)
caution that classroom practices radically different from common practice are
unlikely to be successfully implemented. Additionally, it needs to be recognised that
teachers have passions about their classroom practices and about the expectations
for change placed upon them (Weissglass, 1994, p. 78). Hence, for the desired
changes to occur, it is essential that teachers are assisted in the development of a
clear vision (Lovitt et al., n.d., p. 2), in practical terms, of how a focus on mental and
written idiosyncratic strategies within a mental-written sequence may impact upon
their current teaching practices. This implies that an essential component of any
professional development program is the facilitation of teacher reflection, a process
for which time is typically limited during the normal school day (Clarke, 1994, p. 40).
On-going support, including that in the classroom, is critical to the change
process (Guskey & Sparks, 1991, p. 73; Joyce & Showers, 1980, p. 384). As
Thompson et al. (1994, p. 90) note, once committed to a conceptual orientation,
many teachers lose their reliance on resources such as worksheets and textbooks
as well as on their repertories of familiar teaching practices. Such an experience is
threatening due to their loss of a sense of efficacy, and consequently is a major
obstacle to change. The design of professional development programs, therefore,
needs to have as a key component procedures which recognise that success in
making the desired changes of itself brings about a reconceptualisation of a
teacher's sense of efficacy (Smith, 1996, p. 394). This is a view supportive of the
belief that improvements in student learning arising from changed practices are
critical to the adoption of the beliefs and attitudes which underpin those practices.
For teachers to become committed to the recommended changes to the
approach to developing computational skill, these beliefs and attitudes must become
an essential part of a teachers professional belief system (Clarke & Peter, 1993, p.
174). This necessitates ongoing classroom assistance (Guskey, 1985, p. 59), and
enhanced collegial relationships to provide emotional support during the change
process. Change is not only a source of anxiety for teachers arising from the threat
to their sense of efficacy, but also may occur in context with other sources of anxiety

373

with which they may be faced (Weissglass, 1994, p. 71)sources such as those
related to an increased emphasis on school-based management, and using
technology in classrooms, the latter being a source that has implications for what
mathematics is to be learned in school, and how it is to be taught.

6.4

Recommendations for Further Research


The analyses of the various aspects of mental computation presented in this

study were structured around a number of hypothesised models. Each of these


requires refinement and validation, processes that are critical to changing teacher
beliefs and attitudes about mental computation within the wider context of how
computation is viewed. Hence, research needs to be directed towards:
The model of the components of mental computation (Table 2.1). This
model, based on that for computational estimation delineated by Sowder and
Wheeler (1989, p. 192), was developed to provide a mechanism for
discussing the concepts and skills essential to the mental calculation of exact
answers.
The categorisation of mental strategies presented in Tables 2.2 to 2.4. This
categorisation needs further consideration to determine its appropriateness
for providing a framework for synthesising the disparate data from the range
of studies relevant to identifying and defining mental strategies. Of relevance
to their categorisation is the need for researchers to standardise the labels
ascribed to particular approaches to facilitate the analysis of the data and the
drawing of meaningful conclusions.
The alternative sequence for introducing computational procedures, as
represented by Figure 2.5 and Table 5.2. Although it is believed that an
enhanced understanding of arithmetic can be developed through a
curriculum that focuses on mental computation, computational estimation,
and calculators, with a concomitant reduction in the emphasis on written
computation (MSEB & NRC, 1990, p. 19), Barbara Reys (1991, p. 11) points
out that it is essential that the effects of introducing such changes are

374

identified and analysedeffects not only related to student learning, but also
those related to teacher and parent attitudes.
The hypothesised mental strategies syllabus components for the four
operations beyond the basic facts (Tables 5.3-5.5) that were based on the
above models. Prior to the incorporation of these components in any future
mathematical syllabus or professional development programs, research,
similar to that by Gracey (1994), for example, needs to be undertaken to
determine their appropriatenessto the child, to the development of
proficiency with mental computation, and to the models, and data which
underpin them.

By undertaking such research, insights would be gained into issues that


impinge on the mental strategies that have been recommended for focussed
teaching (see Chapter 5). These issues include:
The strategies used by proficient mental calculators to divide mentally.
The nature of difficulties experienced in balancing strategy development
through using elements of the behaviourist and constructivist approaches.
The way in which self-generated mental strategies mesh with formally taught
algorithms.
The level of place value understanding required for particular strategies.
The demands of particular strategies on working memory.
The effects arising from the need to regroup or carry, and the size, and type
of numbers being manipulated.
The consequences arising from using visual and oral stimuli, and examples
presented in and without context.
The appropriateness of various models which may be used to represent the
calculative situation.
The effectiveness of discussion as a means for encouraging strategy growth,
and how it should be managed within the classroom.

A consequence of this research would be an enhancement of the aims of this


study, namely to analyse key aspects of mental computation and to formulate

375

recommendations to inform future revisions of the Number strand in mathematics


syllabuses for primary schools. The implementation of the outcomes of this
research should therefore strengthen the possibility that mental computation will
receive priority in mathematics classrooms, and become the method of first resort in
situations requiring calculations to be made.

376

REFERENCES
Agresti, A., & Finlay, B. (1997). Statistical methods for the social sciences (3rd ed.).
Upper Saddle River: Prentice-Hall.
Albert State School Staff. (1947, August 8). Notes on staff discussion concerning
recommendations for syllabus revision. Queensland State Archives. A/18581.
Allinger, G. D., & Payne, J. N. (1986). Estimation and mental arithmetic with percent.
In H. L. Schoen & M. J. Zweng (Eds.), Estimation and mental computation (pp. 141155). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Alreck, P. L., & Settle, R. B. (1985). The survey research handbook. Homewood:
Illinios.
"An Inspector of Schools". (1914). The "practical" mental arithmetic. London:
Longmans, Green & Co.
Anderson, J. G. (1870). Mr. Anderson's general report of inspection of schools
during the year 1869. In Report of the Board of General Education, for the Year
1869 (pp. 13-14). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Anderson, J. G. (1872). Annual report of inspection. In Report of Board of General
Education, for the year 1871 (pp. 9-11). Brisbane: Government.
Anderson, J. G. (1877). General Inspector's Report. In First report of the Secretary
of Public Instruction, for the year 1876 (pp. 19-22). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Appendix B: Books supplied to schools for the instruction of pupils. (1902). The
State Education Acts, 1875 to 1900; together with the regulations of the department,
general instructions for the guidance of teachers and others, and appendices (pp.
95-96). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Arithmetic. (1927). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 33(2), pp. 14-17.
Ashcraft, M. H. (1982). The development of mental arithmetic: A chronometric
approach. Developmental Review, 2, 213-236.
Ashcraft, M. H. (1983). Procedural knowledge versus fact retrieval in mental
arithmetic: A reply to Baroody. Developmental Review, 3, 231-235.
Atweh, B. (1982). Developing mental arithmetic. In L. Silvey (Ed.), Mathematics for
the middle grades (5-9) (pp. 50-58). Reston: National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics.

377

Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers. (1996a). Statement on the use of


calculators and computers for mathematics in Australian schools. Adelaide: The
Association.
Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers. (1996b). Mathematical knowledge
and understanding for effective participation in Australian society. Adelaide: The
Association.
Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers. (1997). AAMT position paper on
statewide testing (Draft). Adelaide: The Association.
Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers & Curriculum Development Centre.
(1987). A national statement on the use of calculators for mathematics in Australian
schools. Canberra: Curriculum Development Centre.
Australian Council for Educational Research. (1949). Curriculum survey: Report on
aspects of Arithmetic and English in Australian Schools. Summary and general
conclusions to April 15, 1949. (Paper presented to 1949 Directors of Education
Conference). Queensland State Archives. A/18579.
Australian Council for Educational Research. (1964). Primary school mathematics.
Report of a conference of curriculum officers of State education departments.
Melbourne: The Council.
Australian Council for Educational Research. (1965). Background in mathematics.
Melbourne: Education Department of Victoria & The Council.
Australian Education Council. (1991). A national statement on mathematics for
Australian schools. Carlton: Curriculum Corporation.
Australian Education Council. (1994). Mathematics: A curriculum profile for
Australian Schools. (A joint project of the States, Territories and the Commonwealth
of Australia initiated by the Australian Education Council). Carlton: Curriculum
Corporation.
Australian Mathematics Education Project. (1982). Basic mathematical skills and
concepts for effective participation in Australian society. Canberra: Curriculum
Development Centre.
Avenell, K. (1997). Report of the review of the delivery of curriculum in primary
schools. Brisbane: Education Queensland.
Baker, A., & Baker, J. (1991) Maths in the mind: A process approach to mental
strategies. Melbourne: Eleanor Curtin.
Baker, F. P. (1929). Arithmetic-The teaching of the problem. Education Office
Gazette, 31(7), 272-282.
Baker, W. (1953). Report as district inspector. 1952. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A738, File No. 66544/53.

378

Ballard, P. B. (1927). Fundamental arithmetic: Teacher's Book VI. London:


University of London Press.
Ballard, P. B. (1928a). Teaching the essentials of arithmetic. London: University of
London Press.
Ballard, P. B. (1928b). Foreword. In A. Wisdom, Arithmetical dictation: A systematic
series of exercises in mental arithmetic (Book V, p. ii). London: University of London
Press.
Barcan, A. (1980). A history of Australian education. Melbourne: Oxford University
Press.
Barcan, A. (1996). Attempts to reform Australian state schools, 1979-1996.
Educational Research and Perspectives, 23(1). Retrieved September 13, 1997 from
the World Wide Web: http://www.ecel.uwa.edu.au/gse/erp/Vol32no1/
barcan.htm.
Baroody, A. J. (1983). The development of procedural knowledge: An alternative
explanation for chronometric trends of mental arithmetic. Developmental Review, 3,
225-230.
Baroody, A. J. (1984). A reexamination of mental arithmetic models and data: A
reply to Ashcraft. Developmental Review, 4, 148-156.
Bassett, G. W. (1949). Reform in secondary education. The Forum of Eduction, 7(3),
109-123.
Baturo, A. (1988). Nothing but the facts . Brisbane: Frangipanni.
Baturo, A., & English, L. (1983-1985) Sunshine Mathematics (Years 1-7).
Melbourne: Longman Cheshire.
Behr, M. (1989). Reflections on the conference. In J. T. Sowder & B. P. Schappelle
(Eds.), Establishing foundations for research on number sense and related topics:
Report of a conference (pp. 85-88). San Diego: San Diego State University for
Research in Mathematics and Science Education.
Beishuizen, M. (1985). Evaluation of the use of structured materials in the teaching
of primary mathematics. In B. S. Alloway & G. M. Mills (Eds.), New directions in
education and training technology (pp. 246-258). London: Kogan Page.
Beishuizen, M. (1993). Mental strategies and materials or models for addition and
subtraction up to 100 in Dutch second classes. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 24(4), 194-323.
Benbow, W. E. (1905). Reports of District Inspectors. The Education Office Gazette,
7(1), 6-8.

379

Benbow, W. E. (1911). Report of Mr. District Inspector Benbow. In Thirty-fifth report


of the Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1910 (pp. 64-68). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Benbow, W. E. (1925). From Mr. Benbow's Report. In Forty-ninth report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1924 (pp. 49-52). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Benbow, W. E. (1926). From the late Mr. Benbow's Report. In Fiftieth report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1925 (pp. 50-54). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Bensted, R. J. (1924). President's address to conference: Thoughts and reflections
upon our Queensland primary system of education. Queensland Teachers' Journal,
29(9), 34-36.
Best, J., & Kahn, J. (1986). Research in education (5th ed.). Englewood Cliffs:
Prentice-Hall.
Bevington, W. F. (1922). From Mr. Bevington's Report. In Forty-sixth report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1921 (pp. 56-58). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Bevington, W. F. (1923). From Mr. Bevington's Report. In Forty-seventh report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1922 (pp. 60-65). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Bevington, W. F. (1925). From Mr. Bevington's Report. In Forty-ninth report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1924 (pp. 78-84). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Bevington, W. F. (1926). From Mr. Bevington's Report. In Fiftieth report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1925 (pp. 76-82). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Bevington, W. F. (1927). From Mr. Bevington's Report. In Fifty-first report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1926 (pp. 71-80). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Bidwell, J. K., & Clason, R. G. (Eds.). (1970). Readings in the history of
mathematics education. Washington: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Biggs, E. (1969). Mathematics in primary school: Curriculum Bulletin No. 1 (3rd ed.).
London: Her Majesty's Stationery Office.
Blakers, A. L. (1978). Change in mathematics education since the late 1950s: Ideas
and realisation Australia. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 9, 147-158.
Board of Education. (1937). Handbook of suggestions: For the consideration of
teachers and others concerned in the work of public elementary education. London:
His Majesty's Stationery Office.

380

Board of Education. (1959). Primary Education: Suggestions for the considerations


of teachers and others concerned with the work of primary schools. London: Her
Majesty's Stationery Office.
Board of General Education (1866). Report of the Board of General Education, for
the year 1865. Brisbane: Government Printer.
Booker, G. (Ed.). (n. d.). Teaching mathematics in the primary school: Operations.
Brisbane: Mathematics Department, Brisbane College of Advanced Education.
Borg, W. R., & Gall, M. D. (1989). Educational research: An introduction (5th ed.).
New York: Longman.
Boxall, R. (1981, May). Perspectives on the "Program in mathematics" and
possibilities for its revision. Paper presented at a conference of mathematics
educators, Bardon, Qld.
Brown, T. R. (1901). Report of Mr. District Inspector Brown. In Twenty-fifth report of
the Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1900 (pp. 83-86). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Brownell, W. A. (1935). Psychological considerations in the learning and the
teaching of arithmetic. In National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (Ed.).The
teaching of arithmetic. Tenth yearbook of the National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics (pp. 1-31). New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.
Burge, E. N. J. (1946). A chat from the chair. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 51(9),
13.
Burge, E. N. J. (1947). A chat from the chair. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 52(6),
6-7.
Burns, R. J. (1973). Mental discipline and the colonial curriculum. ANZHES Journal,
2(1), 1-5.
Cain, M. (1989). Making the most of 20 minutes: Sydney: Delastra.
Caine, J. J. (1878). Annual report of inspection. In Second report of the Secretary
for Public Instruction, for the year 1877 (pp. 96-98). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Canny, J. A. (1893). Report of Mr. District Inspector Canny. In Seventeenth report of
the Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1892 (pp. 100-104). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Canny, J. A. (1907). Report of Mr. District Inspector Canny. In Thirty-first report of
the Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1906 (pp. 54-56). Brisbane:
Government Printer.

381

Canny, J. A. (1910). Report of Mr. District Inspector Canny. In Thirty-fourth report of


the Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1909 (pp. 57-60). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Carpenter, T. P. (1980). Heuristic strategies used to solve addition and subtraction
problems. In R. Karplus (Ed.), Proceedings of the fourth international conference for
the psychology of mathematics education (pp. 317-321). Berkeley: University of
California.
Carpenter, T. P., Matthews, W., Lindquist, M. M., & Silver, E. A. (1984).
Achievement in mathematics: Results from the National Assessment. Elementary
School Journal, 84, 485-495.
Carraher, T. N., Carraher, D. W., & Schliemann, A. D. (1985). Mathematics in the
streets and in schools. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 21-29.
Carraher, T. N., Carraher, D. W., & Schliemann, A. D. (1987). Written and oral
mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18, 83-97.
Carroll, W. M. (1996, April). Mental computational skills of students in a reform
mathematics curriculum. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, New York. (Educational Resources Information
Centre Document, Microfiche Edition, ED 395 830).
Case, R., & Sowder, J. T. (1990). The development of computational estimation: A
neo-Piagetian analysis. Cognition and Instruction, 7(2), 79-104.
Cavanagh, D., & Rodwell, G. (1992). Dialogues in educational research. Darwin:
William Michael Press.
Circular to District Inspectors (Draft). (1904). Queensland State Archives,
EDU/A787.
Clarke, D. J., Clarke, D. M., & Lovitt, C. J. (1990). Changes in mathematics teaching
call for assessment alternatives. In T. J. Cooney (Ed.), Teaching and learning
mathematics in the 1990s (pp. 118-129). Reston: National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics.
Clarke, D. J., & Hollingsworth, H. (1994). Reconceptualising teacher change. In G.
Bell, B. Wright, N. Leeson, & J. Geeke (Eds.), Challenges in mathematics education:
Constraints on Construction. proceedings of the seventeenth Annual Conference of
the Mathematics Education research Group of Australasia (MERGA) (pp. 153-163).
Lismore: MERGA.
Clarke, D. J., & Peter, A. (1993). Modelling teacher change. In B. Atweh, C. Kanes,
M. Carss, & G. Booker (Eds.), Contexts in mathematics education. Proceedings of
the Sixteenth Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia (MERGA) (pp. 167-175). Brisbane: MERGA.

382

Clarke, D. M. (1994). Ten key principles from research for the professional
development of mathematics teachers. In D. B. Aichele (Ed.), Professional
development for teachers of mathematics (pp. 37-48). Reston: National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics.
Class teacher's manual of oral arithmetic for Grades VII & VIII in Queensland
schools. (n. d.). Warwick: Premier Educational Publishers.
Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1990). Constructivist learning and teaching.
Arithmetic Teacher, 38(1), 34-5.
Clements, M. A. (1988). Looking to the past as a guide to the future. Vinculum,
25(4), 3-6.
Clements, M. A. (1996). The national curriculum in Australia. Educational Research
and Perspectives, 23(1). Retrieved September 13, 1997 from the World Wide Web:
http://www.ecel.uwa.edu.au/gse/erp/Vol32no1/clements.htm
Clements, M. A., Grimison, L., & Ellerton, N. (1989). Colonialism and school
mathematics in Australia 1788-1988. In N. F. Ellerton & M. A. Clements (Eds.),
School mathematics: The challenge to change (pp. 50-78). Geelong: Deakin
University Press.
Cobb, P., & Merkel, G. (1989). Thinking strategies: Teaching arithmetic through
problem solving. In P. R. Trafton & A. P. Shulte (Eds.), New directions for
elementary school mathematics (pp. 70-81). Reston: National Council of Teachers
of Mathematics.
Coburn, T. (1989). The role of computation in the changing mathematics curriculum.
In P. R. Trafton & A. P. Shulte (Eds.), New directions for elementary school
mathematics (pp. 43-56). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Cochran, D. (1955). Report as district inspector. 1954. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A738, File No. 52837/54.
Cochran, D. (1960). Report as district inspector. 1959. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A740, File No. 23511/60.
Cockcroft, W. H. (Chairman). (1982). Mathematics counts: Report of the committee
of inquiry into the teaching of mathematics in schools. London: HMSO.
Cohen, L., & Mannion, L. (1994). Research methods in education (2nd ed.). London:
Croom Helm.
Colburn, W. (1970). Teaching of arithmetic: An address delivered to the American
Institute of Instruction, August 1830. Reprinted in J. K. Bidwell & R. G. Clason,
(Eds.), Readings in the history of mathematics education (pp. 24-37). Washington:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Colenso, J. W. (1874). A shilling arithmetic designed for the use of elementary
schools (New Edition). London: Longmans, Green & Co.

383

Collins, A., Brown, J. C., & Newman, S. E. (1989). Cognitive apprenticeship:


Teaching the crafts of reading, writing, and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.),
Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 453-494).
Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Comments concerning suggested amendments to Syllabus. (1947). Queensland
State Archives. A/18581.
Connor, D. W. (1954). A blueprint of Queensland: Progressive teaching method
(Part V). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 56(6), 18-20.
Connors, L. (1984). The formation of character: Queensland schooling in the
interwar period. Unpublished Bachelor of Arts (Honours) dissertation, University of
Queensland, Brisbane.
Cooper, T. J., Haralampou, C., & Irons, C. J. (1992). Mental computation: What can
children do? What should teachers do? In C. J. Irons (Ed.), Challenging children to
think when they compute: Conference papers (pp. 99-118). Brisbane: The Centre for
Mathematics and Science Education.
Cooper, T. J., Heirdsfield, A., & Irons, C. J. (1995). Year 2 and 3 childrens mental
strategies for addition and subtraction word problems. Unpublished manuscript,
Queensland University of Technology, Centre for Mathematics and Science
Education, Brisbane.
Cooper, T. J., Heirdsfield, A., & Irons, C. J. (1996). Children's mental strategies for
addition and subtraction word problems. In J. Mulligan & M. Mitchelmore (Eds.),
Children's number learning (A Research Monograph of MERGA/AAMT) (pp. 147162). Adelaide: The Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers.
Costin, R. H. (1959). Report as district inspector. 1958. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A740, File No. 36701/59.
Cowham, J. H. (1895). A new school method (complete) for pupil teachers and
students (2nd ed.). London: Westminster School Book Depot.
Cox, T. A., & MacDonald, R. F. (n. d.). The suggestive handbook of practical school
method: A guide to the school room and examination room (28th ed.). London:
Blackie & Son.
Cramer, J. F. (1936). Australian schools through American eyes. Melbourne:
Australian Council for Educational Research.
Cramer, J. F. (1936). Queensland's inspection methods through American eyes.
Queensland Teachers' Journal, 42(4), 7-8.
Cramer, J. F. (1937). Queensland teachers through American eyes. Queensland
Teachers' Journal, 42(7), 8-9.

384

Crampton, A. V. (1954). Report as district inspector. 1953. Queensland State


Archives. EDU/A738, File No. 52837/54.
Crampton, A. V. (1955). Report as district inspector. 1954. Queensland State
Archives. EDU/A738, File No. 46554/55.
Crampton, A. V. (1956). Report as district inspector. 1955. Queensland State
Archives. EDU/A739, File No. 16634/56.
Crampton, A. V. (1959). Report as district inspector. 1958. Queensland State
Archives. EDU/A740, File No. 36701/59.
Creighton, I. (1993). A history of curriculum development in Queensland. In K.
Wiltshire (Chair), Report of the review of Queensland school curriculum: Volume 3
(pp. 75-132). Brisbane: The State of Queensland.
Cunningham, K. S., & Price, W. T. (1934). The standardization of an Australian
Arithmetic Test (Education Research Series, No. 21). Melbourne: Melbourne
University Press.
Curriculum Programmes Branch, Ministry of Education, Western Australia. (1989).
Learning mathematics handbook: Pre-primary to stage seven mathematics syllabus.
Perth: The Branch.
Dagg, E. M. (1971). An enquiry into the teaching and learning of mathematics in
Queensland primary schools with special reference to the period 1930 to 1971.
Unpublished Bachelor of Education (Honours) dissertation, University of
Queensland, Brisbane.
Dalrymple, D. H. (1899). Twenty-third report of the Secretary for Public Instruction in
Queensland, being the report for the year ending 31st December, 1898. Brisbane:
Government Printer.
D'Arcy, J. (1996). Schools as learning organisations: Could this be one of the
answers? Perspectives on Educational Leadership, 6(5), 1.
Darling Downs Branch. (1946). Reduction of syllabus requirements (Part II).
Queensland Teachers' Journal, 51(6), 21.
Davidson, D. (1980). What's happened to mental arithmetic? The Australian
Mathematics Teacher, 36(3), 24.
"Debunker". (1940). The scholarship question. Queensland Teachers' Journal,
55(10), 14-15.
Denniss, H. (1927). From Mr. Denniss's Report. In Fifty-first report of the Secretary
for Public Instruction, for the year 1926 (pp. 60-62). Brisbane: Government Printer.

385

Denniss, H. (1933). From Mr. Denniss's Report. In Fifty-seventh report of the


Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1932 (pp. 30-33). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Department of Education. (1964). The syllabus or course of instruction in primary
schools: Mathematics. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1966). Program in mathematics for primary schools:
Stages 1-4. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1967a) Program in mathematics for primary schools:
Stages 5-6. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1967b). Education Office Gazette. 69(3), 270.
Department of Education. (1968). Program in mathematics for primary schools:
Stages 7-8. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1975). Program in mathematics. Brisbane: The
Department.
Department of Education. (1978). The preparation and authorisation of syllabuses
for primary schools: A background paper prepared for the Parliamentary Select
Committee on Education in Queensland. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1984). Basic maths facts kits. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1987a). Years 1 to 10 mathematics syllabus. Brisbane:
The Department.
Department of Education. (1987b). Years 1 to 10 mathematics teaching, curriculum
and assessment guidelines. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1987-1991). Years 1 to 10 mathematics sourcebook
(Years 1-7). Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Education. (1989). Using questionnaires to survey community
opinions. Chermside: Brisbane North Region, Queensland Department of Education.
Department of Education. (1991). Directory of Queensland state schools. Brisbane:
The Department.
Department of Education. (1995). Reporting achievement in mathematics:
Information for parents. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Public Instruction. (1879). General instructions for the guidance of
teachers. Brisbane: Government Printer. Queensland State Archives, EDU/A602.
Department of Public Instruction. (1902). The state education acts, 1875 to 1900;
together with the regulations of the Department, general instructions for the
guidance of teachers and others, and appendices. Brisbane: Government Printer.

386

Department of Public Instruction. (1908). The state education acts, 1875 to 1900;
together with the regulations of the Department, general instructions for the
guidance of teachers and others, and appendices. Brisbane: Acting Government
Printer.
Department of Public Instruction. (1914). The syllabus or course of instruction in
primary schools with notes for the guidance of teachers. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Public Instruction. (1928). State education in Queensland. Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Department of Public Instruction. (1930). The syllabus or course of instruction in
primary and intermediate schools. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Public Instruction. (1937). State education in Queensland. Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Department of Public Instruction. (1938). Amendments to course of instruction in
primary and intermediate schools. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Public Instruction. (1946). Arithmetic for Queensland Schools.
Brisbane: Government Printer.
Department of Public Instruction. (1948a). Course of instruction in primary and
intermediate schools. Brisbane: The Department.
Department of Public Instruction. (1948b). General memorandum on 1948
amendments to the syllabus. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 53(9), 16-18.
Department of Public Instruction. (1952a). The Syllabus or course of instruction in
primary and intermediate schools, Book 1. Brisbane: Government Printer.
Department of Public Instruction. (1952b). The Syllabus or course of instruction in
primary and intermediate schools, Book 3: Mathematics. Brisbane: The Department.

Deputation to Minister: Curricula. (1946). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 51(6), 4-6.


"Der Tag". (1936). The mathematical morass. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 42(2),
3.
De Vaus, D. A. (1991). Surveys in social research (3rd ed). North Sydney: Allen &
Unwin.
Devries, G. H. (1951). Teacher assessment and new syllabus for primary schools
[Broadcast made by the Minister of Public Instruction on 4KQ on 9 September
1951]. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 56(9), 9-11.

387

Dewey, J. (1915). The school and society (2nd ed.). Chicago: The University of
Chicago Press.
District Inspectors' Reports for 1897. (1898). Queensland Education Journal, 4(6), 67.
Domas. (1952). Queensland Teachers Journal, 57(10), 10-11.
Doyle, W., & Ponder, G. (1977). The practical ethic and teacher decision-making.
Interchange, 8(3), 1-12.
Draft numeracy standardsYears 3 & 5Professional elaboration. (20 June 1997).
Canberra: Curriculum Corporation.
Drain, D. J. A. (1941). Report as district inspector. 1940. Queensland State
Archives. EDU/A737.
Dunlop, R. (1990a). Curriculum area evaluation: Mathematics education. Brisbane:
Department of Education.
Dunlop, R. (1990b). Professional development: A review of contemporary literature.
Brisbane: Department of Education.
Editorial. (1888, August 18). The Brisbane Courier, p. 6.
Editorial. (1947). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 52(10), 1-2.
Educational Reform. (1939). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 44(4), 1.
Edwards, L. D. (1922, December 30). Teachers Conference. The Queenslander. p.
13.
Edwards, L. D. (1929). Report of the Chief Inspector of Schools. In Fifty-third report
of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1928 (pp. 30-34). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Edwards, L. D. (1931). Report of the Chief Inspector of Schools. In Fifty-fifth report
of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1930 (pp. 27-30). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Edwards, L. D. (1933). Report of the Chief Inspector of Schools. In Fifty-seventh
report of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1932 (pp. 24-28). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Edwards, L. D. (1936). Impressions gained abroad. Queensland Teachers' Journal,
41(8), 11-18.
Edwards, L. D. (1937a). Report of the Director of Education. In Sixty-first report of
the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1936 (pp. 22-25). Brisbane:
Government Printer.

388

Edwards, L. D. (1937b). The problem of education ("Substance of the Report of the


Chief Inspector of Schools", 1936). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 42(8), 6-8.
Edwards, L. D. (1938a). Amendments to the syllabus of instruction in primary and
intermediate schools (Memorandum to teachers from Director of Education. 26
January 1938).
Edwards, L. D. (1938b). Report of the Director of Education. In Sixty-second report
of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1937 (pp. 23-26). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Edwards, L. D. (1939). Report of the Director of Education. In Sixty-third report of
the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1938 (pp. 31-35). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Edwards, L. D. (1948). Report of the Director of Education. In Seventy-second
report of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1947 (pp. 20-29). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Edwards, L. D. (1951). Report of the Director-General of Education. In Seventy-fifth
report of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1950 (pp. 20-37). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Ellerton, N., & Clements, M. A. (1988). Reshaping school mathematics in Australia
1788-1988. Australian Journal of Education, 32(3), 387-405.
Ellerton, N., & Clements, M. A. (1994). The national curriculum debacle. West Perth:
Meridian Press.
Elsom, A. (n. d.). Brown's suggestive mental arithmetic for teachers. London: Brown
& Sons.
England, G. C. (1971). The impact of the inspectorial system: A profession
demeaned? Queensland Teachers' Journal, 76(5), 189-196.
Ewart, D. (1890). Report of the General Inspector. In Fourteenth report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1889 (pp. 66-69). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Ewart, D. (1901). Report of the General Inspector. In Twenty-fifth report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1900 (pp. 55-57). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Ewart, D. (1905). Report of the General Inspector (1904). The Education Office
Gazette, 7(10), 159-168.
Ewart, D. (1907). Report of the Director of Education. In Thirty-first report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1906 (pp. 36-40). Brisbane:
Government Printer.

389

Ewbank, W. A. (1977). Mental arithmetic: A neglected topic. Mathematics in School,


6, 28-31.
Farrell, J. (1927). From Mr. Farrell's Report. In Fifty-first report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1926 (pp. 100-105). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Farrell, J. (1929). Arithmetic. Education Office Gazette, 31(7), 282-295.
Farrell, J. (1939). From Mr. Farrell's Report. In Sixty-third report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1938 (pp. 42-45). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Fassifern Branch, Queensland Teacher's Union. (1947, July 26). Letter to
Queensland Teachers' Union Curriculum Committee re recommendations for
syllabus revision. Queensland State Archives. A/18581.
Fayol, M., Abdi, H., & Gombert, J.-E. (1987). Arithmetic problems formulation and
working memory load. Cognition and Instruction, 4(3), 187-202.
Fennell, M. (1902). Notes of lessons on the Herbartian method. London: Longmans,
Green.
Fennema, E., & Franke, M. L. (1991). Teachers' knowledge and its impact. In D. A.
Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp.
147-164). New York: MacMillan.
Fewtrell, J. (1914). Mr. Fewtrell's report. In Thirty-eighth report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1913 (pp. 66-71). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Fewtrell, J. (1917). Mr. Fewtrell's report. In Forty-first report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1916 (pp. 71-76). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Fish, D. W. (1874). The complete arithmetic: Oral and written. New York: American
Book Company.
Fletcher, F. H. (1931). From Mr. Fletcher's report. In Fifty-sixth report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1930 (pp. 50-53). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Flournoy, F. (1954). The effectiveness of instruction in mental arithmetic.
Elementary School Journal. 55, 148-153.
Flournoy, F. (1957). Developing ability in mental arithmetic. Arithmetic Teacher, 4,
147-150.
Flournoy, F. (1959). Providing mental arithmetic experiences. Arithmetic Teacher, 6,
133-159.
Fowler, S. L. (1923). From Mr. Fowler's Report. In Forty-seventh report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1922 (pp. 52-54). Brisbane: Government
Printer.

390

Fox, C. L. (1905). Reports of District Inspectors. The Education Office Gazette, 7(1),
8-9.
Fox, C. L. (1908). Report of Mr. District Inspector Fox. In Thirty-second report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1907 (pp. 72-74). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Fox, C. L. (1918). Mr. Fox's report. In Forty-second report of the Secretary of Public
Instruction, for the year 1917 (pp. 58-62). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Fox, D. J. (1969). The research process in education. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
French, D. (1987). Mental methods in mathematics. Mathematics in School, 16(2),
39-41.
Gardner, M. (1977). Mathematical carnival. New York: Random House.
Gay, L. R. (1987). Educational research: Competencies for analysis and application.
(3rd Ed.). Columbus: Merrill.
George, J. (1930). From Mr. George's Report. In Fifty-fourth report of the Secretary
of Public Instruction, for the year 1929 (pp. 52-57). Brisbane: Government Printer.
George, J. (1934). From Mr. George's Report. In Fifty-eighth report of the Secretary
of Public Instruction, for the year 1933 (pp. 28-29). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Giles, E. (1986). Mental as anything. In N. F. Ellerton (Ed.), Mathematics who needs
what? (pp. 188-192). Parkville: The Mathematical Association of Victoria.
Ginsburg, H. P. (1982). The development of addition in the contexts of culture,
social class, and race. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.),
Addition and subtraction: A cognitive perspective (pp. 191-210). Hillsdale: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Ginsburg, H. P., Posner, J. K., & Russell, R. L. (1981). The development of mental
addition as a function of schooling and culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 12(2), 163-178.
Girling, M. (1977). Towards a definition of basic numeracy. Mathematics Teaching,
81, 4-5.
Gladman, F. J. (n. d.). School method: Notes and hints from lectures delivered at the
Borough Road Training College, London (Revised ed.). London: Jarrold & Sons.
Gladman, F. J. (1904). School work (7th ed.). London: Jarrold & Sons.
Gladwin, H. (1985). In conclusion: Abstraction versus 'How it is'. Anthropology and
Education Quarterly, 16(3), 207-209.

391

Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (1986). Educational psychology: A realistic approach


(3rd ed.). New York: Longman.
Goodson, I. (1985). History, content and qualitative methods in the study of
curriculum. In R. G. Burgess (Ed.), Strategies of educational research: Qualitative
methods (pp. 121-151). London: The Falmer Press.
Gough, J. (1993). Just tools for minds. Directions in Education, 2(10), p. 2.
Grace, N. (1996). The changing face of syllabus development. Teaching
Mathematics, 21(3), 10-14.
Gracey, C. (1994). Analysis of a teaching sequence to develop mental computation
involving multiplication with year six children. Unpublished Master of Education
Thesis, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane.
Grade III mental arithmetic. (1936). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 42(2), 14-15.
"Green Ant". (1942). About maths. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 47(10), 17-18.
Greenhalgh, J. C. (1947). The syllabus revision. Queensland Teachers' Journal,
52(10), 7-13.
Greenhalgh, J. C. (1949a). The curriculum and methods of the primary school
should be thought out afresh. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 54(5). 2-5.
Greenhalgh, J. C. (1949b). Scottish proposals for syllabus reform. Queensland
Teachers' Journal, 54(7), 10-11.
Greenhalgh, J. C. (1957). Experimental investigation of the 1952 syllabus for
Queensland Primary Schools. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Queensland, Brisbane.
Greeno, J. G. (1983). Conceptual entities. In D. Gentner & A. Stevens (Eds.), Mental
models (pp. 227-252). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Greeno, J. G. (1991). Number sense as situated knowing in a conceptual domain.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 22(3), 170-218.
Griffiths, S. W. (1874). Queensland Parliamentary Debates, 16, 395.
Gripp, W. (1909). Report of Mr. District Inspector Gripp. In Thirty-third report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1908 (pp. 57-60). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Gripp, W. (Chair), Mutch, A., & McKenna, B. (1904). Report of the Sub-Committee
on Arithmetic: Proceedings of the Education Conference which sat at parliament
House, 25th to 29th January. Queensland Parliamentary Papers, p. 297.

392

Groen, G. J., & Parkman, J. M. (1972). A chronometric analysis of simple addition.


Psychological Review, 79(4), 329-343.
Guskey, T. R. (1985). Staff Development and teacher change. Educational
Leadership, 42(7), 57-60.
Guskey, T. R., & Sparks, D. (1991). What to consider when evaluation staff
development. Educational Leadership, 49(3), 73-76.
Gutekunst, E. F. (1958). Report as district inspector. 1957. Queensland State
Archives. EDU/A739, File No. 33437/58.
Guymer, A. E. (1961). Annual report of the Director of Primary Education. 1960.
Queensland State Archives. EDU/A740, File No. 60295/61.
Hall, G. E., Wallace, B., & Dossett, B. (1973). A developmental conceptualisation of
the adoptive process within educational institutions. Austin: University of Texas.
Hall, J. V. (1947). Solving verbal arithmetic problems without pencil and paper. The
Elementary School Journal, 12, 212-217.
Hall, J. V. (1954). Mental arithmetic: Misunderstood terms and meanings. The
Elementary School Journal, 54, 349-353.
Hamann, M. S., & Ashcraft, M. H. (1985). Simple and complex mental addition
across development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 40, 49-72.
Handran, Y., Toohey, M., & Luxton, P. (1993). Literacy and numeracy: Equal
partners in learning. In Shaping the future: Review of the Queensland school
curriculum (Vol. 3, pp. 205-231). Brisbane: The State of Queensland.
Hanger, T. (1963). Sixty years in Queensland schools. Sydney: Wentworth Books.
Harrap, G. (1908). Report of Mr. District Inspector Harrap. In Thirty-second report of
the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1907 (pp. 45-47). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Harrap, G. (1910). Report of Mr. District Inspector Harrap. In Thirty-fourth report of
the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1909 (pp. 54-56). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Hazekamp, D. W. (1986). Components of mental multiplying. In H. L. Schoen & M.
J. Zweng (Eds.), Estimation and mental computation (pp. 116-126). Reston:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Heirdsfield, A. (1996). Mental computation, computational estimation, and number
fact knowledge for addition and subtraction in year four children. Unpublished
Master of Education Thesis, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane.

393

Heirdsfield, A., & Cooper, T. J. (1995, July). Teaching implications from research on
children's mental computation for addition and subtraction. Paper presented at the
fifteenth national conference of the Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers,
Darwin, Northern Territory.
Henderson, T., Irish, C. A., Bowden, L. T., & Watkin, H. G. (1935). New syllabus
mental arithmetic for Sixth Grade. Brisbane: William Brooks.
Hendren, G. R. (1939). From Mr. Hendren's Report. In Sixty-third report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1938 (pp. 53-56). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Hendy, A. V. (1953). Report as district inspector. 1952. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A738, File No. 66544/53.
Henry, G. T. (1990). Practical sampling. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.
Hickling, R. W. (1983, April). Problem-solving in Queensland primary schools. Paper
presented at Brisbane North Region Principals Meeting, Brisbane.
Hiebert, J. (1989). Reflections after the conference on number sense. In J. T.
Sowder & B. P. Schappelle (Eds.), Establishing foundations for research on number
sense and related topics: Report of a conference (pp. 82-84). San Diego: San Diego
State University for Research in Mathematics and Science Education.
Hiebert, J., & Lefevre, P. (1986). Conceptual and procedural knowledge in
mathematics: An introductory analysis. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and
procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics (pp. 1-27). Hillsdale: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Hill, S. (1983). An agenda for action: Status and impact. In G. Shufelt (Ed.), The
agenda in action (pp. 1-7). Reston: National Council of Mathematics Teachers.
Hitch, G. J. (1977). Mental arithmetic: Short-term storage and information
processing in a cognitive skill. In A. M. Lesgold, J. W. Pellegrino, S. Fokkema, & R.
Glaser (Eds.), Cognitive psychology and instruction (pp. 331-338). New York:
Plenum Press.
Hitch, G. J. (1978). The role of short-term memory in mental arithmetic. Cognitive
Psychology, 10, 302-323.
Hope, J. A. (1985). Unravelling the mysteries of expert mental calculation.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 16(4), 355-374.
Hope, J. A. (1986a). Mental calculation: Anachronism or basic skill? In H. L. Schoen
& M. J. Zweng (Eds.), Estimation and mental computation (pp. 45-54). Reston:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

394

Hope, J. A. (1986b). Benchmark: Number patterns and the development of expert


mental calculation. In H. L. Schoen & M. J. Zweng (Eds.), Estimation and mental
computation (pp. 72-73). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Hope, J. A. (1987). A case study of a highly skilled mental calculator. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 18(5), 331-342.
Hope, J. A. (1989). Promoting number sense in school. Arithmetic Teacher, 36(6),
12-16.
Hope, J. A., & Sherrill, J. (1987). Characteristics of unskilled and skilled mental
calculators. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18(2), 98-111.
"Howard". (1899). Mental arithmetic. The Education Office Gazette, 1(6), 79-80.
Howden, H. (1989). Teaching number sense. Arithmetic Teacher, 36(6), 6-11.
Howson, G., & Wilson, B. (1986). School mathematics in the 1990s. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Hughes, P. (1993). Similar but different: The curriculum scene in Australia.
Educational Review, 45(2), 143-153.
Hughes, P. W. (1965). New ideas in the teaching of number in the primary school. In
Modern mathematics for the primary school (pp. 31-41). Brisbane: Queensland
Institute for Educational Research.
Hunter, I. M. L. (1962). An exceptional talent for calculative thinking. British Journal
of Psychology, 53(3), 243-258.
Hunter, I. M. L. (1977a). Mental calculation: Two additional comments. In P. N.
Johnson-Laird & P. C. Watson (Eds.), Thinking: Readings in cognitive science (pp.
35-42). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hunter, I. M. L. (1977b). An exceptional memory. British Journal of Psychology, 68,
155-164.
Hunter, I. M. L. (1978). The role of memory in expert mental calculation. In M. M.
Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory. (pp.
339-345). London: Academic Press.
Huxham, J. (1921). Forty-fifth annual report of the Secretary of Public Instruction in
Queensland, being report for the year ending 31st December, 1920. Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Inglis, T. (1926). From Mr. Inglis's report. In Fiftieth report of the Secretary of Public
Instruction, for the year 1925 (pp. 94-98). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Inglis, T. (1929). From Mr. Inglis's report. In Fifty-third report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1928 (pp. 82-87). Brisbane: Government Printer.

395

Instructions to inspectors: Are they carried out?. (1935). Queensland Teachers'


Journal, 40(6), 6-7.
Irons, C. J. (1990a). Teaching computation skills. Workshop presented at Centre
2000, Mitchelton, Brisbane.
Irons, C. J. (1990b). How can we develop skills in computational estimation and
mental arithmetic? Unpublished paper.
Irons, C. J., Jones, G. A., Dunphy, M., & Booker, G. (1980). Fostering arithmetic in
the primary school. Canberra: Curriculum Development Centre.
Irons C. J., & Scales, D. (1982-1986). Rigby moving into maths (Levels 3-7).
Adelaide: Rigby.
Izard, J. F. (1969). New learning materials for mathematics. Chronicle of Australian
Education, 2(2), 66-68.
Jeffrey. J. (1923). From Mr. Jeffrey's report. In Forty-seventh report of the Secretary
of Public Instruction, for the year 1922 (pp. 67-69). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Jeffrey. J. (1930). From Mr. Jeffrey's report. In Fifty-fourth report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1929 (pp. 65-69). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Jensen, A. R. (1990). Speed of information processing in a calculating prodigy.
Intelligence, 14(3), 259-274.
Johnston. J. (1920). Mr. Johnston's Report. In Forty-fourth report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1919 (pp. 93-99). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Jones, G. A. (1979). Changes in mathematics education in Queensland since the
late 1950s. Teaching Mathematics, 4(1), 20-25.
Jones, G. A., Lubinski, C. A., Swafford, J. O., & Thornton, C. A. (1994). A framework
for the professional development of K-12 mathematics teachers. In D. B. Aichele
(Ed.). Professional development for teachers of mathematics (pp. 23-36). Reston:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Jones, G. J. (1967). Modern mathematics: Its implications for mathematics teaching
in Queensland. Quest, 1(1), 19-24.
Jones, J. P. (1988). Mental mathematics moves ahead. Mathematics in School,
17(3), 42-44.
Josephina, Sister. (1960). Mental arithmetic in today's classroom. Arithmetic
Teacher, 7, 199-200, 207.
Joyce, B., & Showers, B. (1980). Improving inservice training: The message of
research. Educational Leadership, 37(5), 379-385.

396

Joyce, P. W. (1881). A hand-book of school management and methods of teaching


(7th ed.). Dublin: M. H. Gill & Son.
"J.R.D." (1928). Mental arithmetic and quick methods. Queensland Teachers'
Journal, 34(7), 7-8.
"J.R.D." (1930). Home made arithmetic. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 35(7), 1314.
"J.R.D." (1931). Mental arithmetic. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 36(8), 9-10, 13.
Kamii, C. (1990). Constructivism and beginning arithmetic (K-2). In T. J. Cooney
(Ed.), Teaching and learning mathematics in the 1990s (pp. 22-30). Reston:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Karweit, N. L. (1982). Survey research methods. In H. E. Mitzel (Ed.), Encyclopedia
of education research (5th ed.) (pp. 1834-1841). New York: The Free Press.
Keeves, J. P. (1965). Overseas contributions to Australian classroom practice in
primary school mathematics. In Modern mathematics for the primary school (pp. 530). Brisbane: Queensland Institute for Educational Research.
Kehoe, M. (1955). Report as district inspector. 1954. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A738, File No. 46554/55.
Kehoe, M. (1957) Report as district inspector. 1956. Queensland State Archives,
EDU/A739, File No. 53322/57.
Kemp, C. (1913). Mr. Kemp's Report. In Thirty-seventh report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1912 (pp. 55-60). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Kemp, C. (1914). Mr. Kemp's Report. In Thirty-eighth report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1913 (pp. 98-102). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Kemp, C. (1915). Mr. Kemp's Report. In Thirty-ninth report of the Secretary of Public
Instruction, for the year 1914 (pp. 95-98). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Kemp, C. (1917). Mr. Kemp's Report. In Forty-first report of the Secretary of Public
Instruction, for the year 1916 (pp. 94-96). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Kemp, C. (1929). From Mr. Kemp's report. In Fifty-third report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1928 (pp. 44-48). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Kemp, L. (1944). Mental discipline. The Forum of Education, 3(2), 28-32.
Kennedy, A. S. (1887). Report of Mr. District Inspector Kennedy. In Eleventh report
of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1886 (pp. 71-85). Brisbane:
Government Printer.

397

Kennedy, A. S. (1889). Report of Mr. District Inspector Kennedy. In Thirteenth report


of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1888 (pp. 88-96). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Kennedy, A. S. (1903). Report of Mr. District Inspector Kennedy. In Twenty-seventh
report of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1902 (pp. 68-69). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Koenker, R. H. (1961). Mental arithmetic. Arithmetic Teacher, 8, 295-296.
Kolesnik, W. B. (1958). Mental discipline in modern education. Madison: The
University of Wisconsin Press.
Larcombe, H. J. (n. d.). Speed tests in mental arithmetic: Senior Book 1 (The Minute
a sum series). London: Evans Brothers.
Lave, J. (1985). Introduction: Situationally specific practice. Anthropology and
Education Quarterly, 16(3), 171-176.
Lawry, J. R. (1968). Some aspects of education in Queensland: 1859-1904.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Monash University, Melbourne.
Lawry, J. R. (1972). Understanding Australian education: 1901-1914. In J. Cleverly
& J. R. Lawry (Eds.), Australian education in the twentieth century (pp. 1-31).
Melbourne: Longman.
Lawry, J. R. (1975). State education - Which centenary? Quest, 16, 57-61.
Lawson, R. L. (1970). An analysis of Brisbane society in the 1890s. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Queensland, Brisbane.
Lenchner, G. (1983). Creative problem solving in school mathematics. Boston:
Houghton Mifflin.
Leutzinger, L. P., Rathmell, E. C., & Urbatsch, T. D. (n. d.). Primary estimation.
Unpublished paper.
Lewis, B. (1991). Mental arithmetic and problem solving (Years 3-7). Melbourne:
Longman Cheshire.
Lidgate, R. (1953). Report as district inspector. 1952. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A738, File No. 66544/53.
Lidgate, R. (1954). Report as district inspector. 1953. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A738, File No. 52837/54.
Lidgate, R. (1955). Report as district inspector. 1954. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A738, File No. 46554/55.

398

Lidgate, R. (1957). Report as district inspector. 1956. Queensland State Archives.


EDU/A739, File No. 53322/57.
Lidgate, R. (1958). Report as district inspector. 1957. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A739, File No. 33437/58.
Lidgate, R. (1959). Report as district inspector. 1958. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A740, File No. 36701/59.
Lindquist, M. M. (1984). The elementary school curriculum: Issues for today. The
Elementary School Journal, 84(3), 595-609.
List of Books. (1880). The State Education Act of 1875, together with the
Regulations of the Department and General Instructions for the guidance of
teachers and others. Brisbane: Government Printer.
Literacy and numeracy benchmarks: Background for information. (June 1997).
Canberra: Curriculum Corporation.
Literacy and Numeracy Diagnostic Assessment [LANDA] Project. (1991). Managing
mathematics: Assessing for planning, learning and teaching. Brisbane: Department
of Education.
Logan, G., & Clarke, E. (1984). State education in Queensland: A brief history.
Monographs on the history of education in Queensland, 2. Brisbane: Department of
Education.
Lovitt C, & Clarke, D. M. (Eds.). (1988). The Mathematics curriculum and teaching
program activity book (Vol. 1) (pp. 239-297). Canberra: Curriculum Development
Centre.
Lovitt, C., Stephens, M., Clarke, D. M., & Romberg, T. A. (n. d.). Mathematics
teachers reconceptualising their roles. Madison: National Center for Research in
Mathematical Sciences Education.
Macgroarty, D. C. (1879). Annual report of inspection. In Third report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1878 (pp. 70-72). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Macgroarty, D. C. (1886). Report of Mr. District Inspector Macgroarty. In Tenth
report of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1885 (pp. 61-65). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Macgroarty, D. C. (1891). Report of Mr. District Inspector Macgroarty. In Fifteenth
report of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1890 (pp. 72-77). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Macgroarty, D. C. (1900). Report of Mr. District Inspector Macgroarty. In Twentyfourth report of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1899 (pp. 62-63).
Brisbane: Government Printer.

399

Macgroarty, D. C. (1902). Report of Mr. District Inspector Macgroarty. In Twentysixth report of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1901 (pp. 66-67).
Brisbane: Government Printer.
Maier, E. (1980). Folk mathematics. Mathematics Teaching, 93, 21-23.
Markovits, Z. (1989). Reactions to the number sense conference. In J. T. Sowder &
B. P. Schappelle (Eds.). Establishing foundations for research on number sense and
related topics: Report of a conference (pp. 78-81). San Diego: San Diego State
University for Research in Mathematics and Science Education.
Markovits, Z., & Sowder, J. (1994). Developing number sense: An intervention study
in Grade 7. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 25(1), 4-29.
Martin, F. J. (1916). Mr. Martin's Report. In Fortieth report of the Secretary of Public
Instruction, for the year 1915 (pp. 133-136). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Martin, G. (1920a). Talks to young teachers - Arithmetic. Queensland Education
Journal, 25(12), 81-82.
Martin, G. (1920b). Talks to young teachers - Arithmetic. Queensland Education
Journal, 25(13), 98-100.
Masingila, J. O., Davidenko, S., Prus-Wisniowska, E., & Agwu, N. (1994, April).
Mathematics learning and practice in and out of schools: A framework for making
these experiences complementary. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Indianapolis. (Educational Resources
Information Centre Document, Microfiche Edition, ED 373 960).
Mathematical Sciences Education Board and National Research Council. (1990).
Reshaping school mathematics: A philosophy and framework for curriculum.
Washington: National Academy Press.
Mathematics Subcommittee of Syllabus Committee. (1947). Discussion paper:
Changes to Grade VI mathematics. Queensland State Archives. A/18581.
McCormack, J. (1947, 8 August). Letter to Mr. District Inspector Radford containing
comments concerning syllabus [Attached to letter to D. I. Radford from Granville
State School]. Queensland State Archives. A/18581.
McIntosh, A. J. (1978). Some subtractions: What do you think you are doing?
Mathematics Teaching, 83, 17-18.
McIntosh, A. (1988). Mental arithmetic. In C. Lovitt & D. M. Clarke (Eds.). The
mathematics curriculum and teaching program activity book (Vol. 1) (pp. 239-297).
Canberra: Curriculum Development Centre.

400

McIntosh, A. (1990a). Becoming numerate: developing number sense. In S. Willis


(Ed.), Being numerate: What counts? (pp. 24-43). Melbourne: Australian Council for
Educational Research.
McIntosh, A. (1990b, July). Analysing and classifying children's mental arithmetic
strategies. Paper presented at the thirteenth annual Conference of Mathematics
Education Research Group of Australasia, Hobart, Tasmania.
McIntosh, A. (1990c, July). A classification system for mental computation
strategies. Paper presented at the thirteenth annual Conference of Mathematics
Education Research Group of Australasia, Hobart, Tasmania.
McIntosh, A. (1991a, July). Less and more competent primary school mental
calculators. Paper presented at the fourteenth Annual Conference of the
Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia, Perth, Western Australia.
McIntosh, A. (1991b). The changing emphasis in arithmetic in the primary school:
From written computation to mental computation and calculator use. Journal of
Science and Mathematics in S. E. Asia, 14(1), 45-55.
McIntosh, A. (1992). Two needed revolutions. In C. J. Irons (Ed.), Challenging
children to think when they compute: Conference papers (pp. 127-137). Brisbane:
The Centre for Mathematics and Science Education.
McIntosh, A., Bana, J, & Farrell, B. (1995). Mental computation in school
mathematics. Perth: Mathematics, Science & Technology Centre, Edith Cowan
University.
McIntosh, A., De Nardi, E., & Swan, P. (1994). Think mathematically! How to teach
mental maths in the primary classroom. Melbourne: Longman Cheshire.
McIntyre, T. (1876). Annual report of inspection. In Report of the Board of General
Education for the year 1875 (pp. 28-30). Brisbane: Government Printer.
McKenna, B. J. (1936). Director's report on educational matters abroad: Queensland
schools compared. Queensland Education Journal, 41(6), 11-16.
Menchinskaya, N. A., & Moro, M. I. (1975). Instruction in mental and written
calculation. In J. Kilpatrick, I. Wirszup, E. Begle, & J. Wilson (Eds.), Soviet studies in
the psychology of learning and teaching mathematics (Vol. 14, pp. 73-88).
Standford: School Mathematics Study Group.
Mental arithmetic. (1927). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 32(11), 18.
Mental arithmetic: A few suggestions. (1910). Queensland Education Journal, 16(9),
176.
Mental arithmetic: Book II. Senior grades. (1926). Brisbane: Moreton Printing
Company.

401

Mental arithmetic tests: Intermediate. (1910). Queensland Education Journal, 16(2),


30-31.
Mental tests: Grades VI. and VII. (1930). Queensland Education Journal, 36(1), 23.
Michigan State Board of Education. (1989). An interpretation of: The Michigan
essential goals and objectives for mathematics education. Lansing: The Board.
Minutes of evidence taken before The Royal Commission appointed to inquire into
the working of the educational institutions of the Colony. (1875). Votes &
Proceedings. Queensland Legislative Assembly (Vol. 2, pp. 83-110). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Moorhouse, E. J. (1927). From Mr. Moorhouse's Report. In Fifty-first report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1926 (pp. 94-97). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Moorhouse, E. J. (1939). From Mr. Moorhouse's Report. In Sixty-third report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1938 (pp. 74-76). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Moorhouse, E. J. (1941). Report as district inspector. 1940. Queensland State
Archives. EDU/A737.
Murray, H., & Olivier, A. (1989). A model of understanding two-digit numeration and
computation. In G. Vergnaud, J. Rogalski & M. Artique (Eds.), Proceedings of the
thirteenth annual conference of the International Group for the Psychology of
Mathematics Education (Vol. 3) (pp. 3-10). Paris, France.
Murray, H., Olivier, A., & Human, P. (1991). Young children's division strategies. In
F. Furinghetti (Ed.), Proceedings: Fifteenth PME Conference (Vol. III) (pp. 49-56).
Assisi: Program Committee.
Murray, S. (1981, January). The changing primary mathematics curriculum. Paper
prepared for the Primary Mathematics Syllabus Committee, Department of
Education.
Murtaugh, M. (1985). The practice of arithmetic by American grocery shoppers.
Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 16(3), 186-192.
Musser, G. L. (1982). Let's teach mental algorithms for addition and subtraction.
Arithmetic Teacher, 29(8), 40-42.
Mutch, A. (1906). Report of Mr. District Inspector Mutch. In Thirtieth report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1905 (pp. 61-64). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Mutch, A. (1907). Report of Mr. District Inspector Mutch. In Thirty-first report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1906 (pp. 68-71). Brisbane: Government
Printer.

402

Mutch, A. (1916). Mr. Mutch's report. In Fortieth report of the Secretary of Public
Instruction, for the year 1915 (pp. 59-63). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Mutch, A. (1924). From Mr. Mutch's report. In Forty-eighth report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1923 (pp. 38-41). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Mutch, A. (1925). From Mr. Mutch's report. In Forty-ninth report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1924 (pp. 46-49). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Myers, J. L., & Well, A. D. (1995). Research design and statistical analysis.
Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
National Council of Supervisors of Mathematics. (1977). Position paper on basic
skills. Arithmetic Teacher, 25(1), 19-22.
National Council of Supervisors of Mathematics. (1989). Essential mathematics for
the twenty-first century: The position of the National Council of Supervisors of
Mathematics. Arithmetic Teacher, 37(1), 44-46.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1980). An agenda for action:
Recommendations for school mathematics of the 1980s. Reston: The Council.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1989). Curriculum and evaluation
standards for school mathematics. Reston: The Council.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1991). Professional standards for
teaching mathematics. Reston: The Council.
National Curriculum: Mathematics Working Group. (1988). National curriculum:
Mathematics for ages 5 to 16. London: Department of Education and Science and
the Welsh Office.
National Numeracy Project. (April 1997). Draft framework for numeracy: Years 1 to
6. London: Department of Education and Science and the Welsh Office.
National Research Council. (1989). Everybody counts: A report to the nation on the
future of mathematics education. Washington: National Academy Press.
Nesher, P. (1986). Are mathematical understanding and algorithmic performance
related? For the Learning of Mathematics, 6, 2-9.
New syllabus. (1929). The Education Office Gazette, 31(10), 460-465.
New syllabus mental arithmetic for third grade. (1932). Brisbane: William Brooks.
Norusis, M. N. (1991). SPSS/PC+ Studentware plus [Computer program manual].
Chicago: SPSS Inc.

403

Nunes, T., Schliemann, A. D., & Carraher, D. W. (1993). Street mathematics and
school mathematics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Office of the Minister for Education. (1996). Media statement: Quinn cans Student
Performance Standards. Brisbane: The State of Queensland.
Ohio Department of Education. (n. d.). Model competency-based mathematics
program. Cleveland: The Department.
Olander, H. T., & Brown, B. I. (1959). Journal of Educational Research, 53(3), 97102.
Olsen, F. J. (1953). Oral arithmetic: Grade VIII. Brisbane: A. McLeod.
"Omega". (1871, January 14). Short papers on popular education, No. 1. The
Queenslander, p. 8.
Oostenbroek, P. (1976). Basic Maths Facts. Brisbane: Jacaranda.
Our first half-century: A review of Queensland progress. (1909). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Owen, E., & Sweller, J. (1989). Should problem solving be used as a learning device
in mathematics? Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20, 322-328.
Palfrey A. E. (1928). From Mr. Palfrey's Report. In Fifty-second report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1927 (pp. 102-108). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Papi, F. (1912). Report of Mr. Acting District Inspector Papi. In Thirty-sixth report of
the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1911 (pp. 49-52). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Para 143: The course of instruction for each class. (1891). In State Education Act of
1875; together with the regulations of the Department, general instructions for the
guidance of teachers and others (pp. 23-24). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Para 144: Standards of proficiency. (1891). In State Education Act of 1875; together
with the regulations of the Department , general instructions for the guidance of
teachers and others (pp. 24-28). Brisbane: Government Printer
Park, A. (1879). Manual of method for pupil-teachers and assistant masters.
London: Blackie & Son.
Parliamentary Select Committee on Education in Queensland (M. Ahern, Chair).
(1980). Final report: Section 2 - A brief history of education in Queensland and the
background to the present inquiry. Brisbane: Government Printer.

404

Payne, J. N. (1990). New directions in mathematics education. In J. N. Payne (Ed.),


Mathematics for the young child (pp. 1-15). Reston: National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics.
Peach, F. J. (1996). Deferment of Student Performance Standards during 1996.
Memorandum to Principals and teachers of all State schools from Director General
of Education, 27 February 1996.
Peach, F. J. (1997a, September). School-based management. Paper presented at
the Sunshine Coast Regional Principals Conference, Marcoola Beach, Queensland.
Peach, F. J. (1997b, November 14). From the DGs desk: National literacy plan
commitment. Education Views, 6(21), 2.
Pendlebury, C., & Beard, W. S. (1899). A shilling arithmetic. London: Bell & Sons.
Perrett, K., & Donlan, R. (1985). Breakthrough mental arithmetic (Years 3-6).
Melbourne: Methuen.
Pestorius, G. C. (1939). From Mr. Pestorius's Report. In Sixty-third report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1938 (pp. 62-65), Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Pestorius, G. C. (1940). From Mr. Pestorius's Report. In Sixty-fourth report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1939 (p. 15), Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Pestorius, G. C. (1942). Report as district inspector. 1941. Queensland State
Archives. EDU/A737.
Petchell, D. L., & McDonald, M. K. (1980). Progress in mental arithmetic (Years 3-6).
Sydney: Primary Education Publications.
Petitto, A. L., & Ginsburg, H. P. (1982). Mental arithmetic in Africa and America:
Strategies, principles, and explanations. International Journal of Psychology, 17, 81102.
Pizzy, J. (1950). Reports from parliamentary debates. Queensland Teachers'
Journal, 55(8), 17.
Platt, J. (1901). Report of Mr. Senior District Inspector Platt. In Twenty-fifth report of
the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1900 (pp. 58-59). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Platt, J. (1905). Report of Mr. Senior District Inspector Platt. In Twenty-ninth report
of the Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1904 (pp. 29-30). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Plunkett, S. (1979). Decomposition and all that rot. Mathematics in School, 8(3), 2-5.

405

Potter, F. F (n. d.). Mental and intelligence tests in common-sense arithmetic.


London: Pitman.
Poulter, J. G., & Haylock, D. W. (1988). Teaching computational estimation.
Mathematics in School, 17(2), 27-29.
Powell, L. W. (1968, June 20). Letter to Acting Minister of Education
[Implementation of Program in Mathematics]. Oxley Memorial Library. OM 72.071.
Priestley, H. J. (1912). The teaching of elementary mathematics. Queensland
Teachers' Journal, 17(11), 219-224.
Professional standards. (1936). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 42(2), 1.
Putman, R. T., Lampert, M., & Peterson, P. L. (1990). Alternative perspectives on
knowing mathematics in elementary schools. In C. B. Cazden (Ed.), Review of
research in education (No. 16, pp. 57-150). Washington: American Educational
Research Association.
Pyle, N. E. T. (1954). Report as district inspector. 1953. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A738, File No. 52837/54.
Pyle, N. E. T. (1959). Report as district inspector. 1958. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A740, File No. 36701/59.
Pyle, N. E. T. (1965). Annual Report for the year ending December 1964.
Queensland State Archives. TR1530/1, Box 90.
Queensland School Curriculum Council. (1997). 1997 Queensland year 6 test
information guide. Brisbane: The State of Queensland.
Queensland School Curriculum Office. (1996a). Number developmental continuum.
(Revised ed.). Brisbane: The State of Queensland.
Queensland School Curriculum Office. (1996b) Queensland levels of student
performance: Report to the Honourable the Minister for Education on Student
Performance Standards in Queensland Schools. Brisbane: The State of
Queensland.
Quinn, B. (1996). Future developments and use of student performance standards.
Memo from Minister of Education to Director-General of Education, 29 February
1996.
Radcliffe, O. (1898). Report of Mr. District Inspector Radcliffe. In Eighteenth report
of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1897 (pp. 72-74). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Radford, R. W. (1947, August 18) Memo to District Inspector Farrell: Comments on
syllabus from teachers in the Pialba District. Queensland State Archives. A/18581.

406

Rathmell, E., & Trafton, P. R. (1990). Whole number computation. In J. N. Payne


(Ed.), Mathematics for the young child (pp. 153-172). Reston: National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics.
Reed, H. J., & Lave, J. (1981). Arithmetic as a tool for investigating relations
between culture and cognition. In R. W. Casson (Ed.), Language, culture, and
cognition: Anthropological perspectives (pp. 437-455). New York: MacMillan.
Regulations for the establishment and management of primary schools in
Queensland. (1860). Brisbane: Fairfax & Belbridge.
Regulations for the establishment and management of primary schools. (1861).
Queensland Government Gazette, II(9), 77-86.
Resnick, L. B. (1983). A developmental theory of number understanding. In H. P.
Ginsburg (Ed.), The development of mathematical thinking (pp. 109-151). New York:
Academic Press.
Resnick, L. B. (1989a). Introduction. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and
instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 1-16). Hillsdale: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Resnick, L. B. (1989b). Defining, assessing, and teaching number sense. In J. T.
Sowder, & B. P. Schappelle (Eds.), Establishing foundations for research on number
sense and related topics: Report of a conference (pp. 35-39). San Diego: San Diego
Sate University for Research in Mathematics and Science Education.
Resnick, L. B. (1992). Defining, assessing and teaching number sense. In C. J.
Irons (Ed.), Challenging children to think when they compute: Conference papers
(pp. 35-39). Brisbane: The Centre for Mathematics and Science Education.
Resnick, L. B., & Omanson, S. F. (1987). Learning to understand arithmetic. In R.
Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 41-95). Hillsdale:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Reys, B. J. (1985). Mental computation. Arithmetic Teacher, 32(6), 43-46.
Reys, B. J. (1986a). Estimation and mental computation: It's "about" time. Arithmetic
Teacher, 34(1), 22-23.
Reys, B. J. (1986b). Identification and characterization of mental computation
algorithms used by seventh and eighth grade students on visually and orally
presented mental computation exercises. (Doctoral Dissertation, University of
Missouri, 1985). Dissertation Abstracts International, 46(11), 3279-A.
Reys, B. J. (1989). Conference on number sense: Reflections. In J. T. Sowder & B.
J. Schappelle (Eds.), Establishing foundations for research on number sense and
related topics: Report of a conference (pp. 70-73). San Diego: San Diego State
University for Research in Mathematics and Science Education.

407

Reys, B. J. (1991, August). Mental computation: The role of self-initiated, derived


and executed strategies for computing. Paper presented at Gwinganna
Computation Conference, Gold Coast Hinterland, Australia.
Reys, B. J. (1992). Number Sense: Building on student's intuitive knowledge. In C.
J. Irons (Ed.), Challenging children to think when they compute: Conference papers
(pp. 93-98). Brisbane: The Centre for Mathematics and Science Education.
Reys. B. J., & Barger, R. S. (1994). Mental computation: Issues from a United
States perspective. In R. E. Reys & N. Nohda (Eds.), Computational alternatives for
the twenty-first century: Cross-cultural perspectives from Japan and the United
States (pp. 31-47). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Reys, B. J., & Reys, R. E. (1986). Mental computation and computational estimation
- Their time has come. Arithmetic Teacher, 33(7), 4-5.
Reys, B. J., Reys, R. E., & Hope, J. A. (1993). Mental computation: A snapshot of
second, fifth and seventh grade student performance. School Science and
Mathematics, 93(6), 306-315.
Reys, R. E. (1984). Mental computation and estimation: Past, present and future.
The Elementary School Journal, 84(5), 547-557.
Reys, R. E. (1985). Testing mental-computation skills. Arithmetic Teacher, 33(3),
14-16.
Reys, R. E. (1992). Mental computation: Some ideas for teachers and directions for
teaching. In C. J. Irons (Ed.), Challenging children to think when they compute:
Conference papers (pp. 63-72). Brisbane: The Centre for Mathematics and Science
Education.
Reys, R. E., Bestgen, B. J., Rybolt, J., & Wyatt, J. W. (1982). Processes used by
good computational estimators. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
13(3), 183-201.
Reys, R. E., & Nohda, N. (1994). Computation and the need for change. In R. E.
Reys & N. Nohda (Eds.), Computational alternatives for the twenty-first century:
Cross-cultural perspectives from Japan and the United States (pp. 1-11). Reston:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Reys, R. E., Reys, B. J., Nohda, N., & Emori, H. (1995). Mental computation
performance and strategy use of Japanese students in Grades 2, 4, 6, and 8.
Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 26(4), 304-326.
Reys, R. E., Reys, B. J., Nohda, N., Ishida, J., Shigeo, Y, & Shimizy, K. (1991).
Computational estimation performance and strategies used by fifth- and eight-grade
Japanese students. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 22(1), 39-58.
Robinson, J. A. (1945). Curricula. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 50(8), 11-16.

408

Robinson, R. (1882). Teacher's manual of method and organisation: Adapted to the


primary schools of Great Britain, Ireland, and the colonies (7th ed.). London:
Longmans, Green.
Rodwell, G. W. (1992). Historical research in education. In D. Cavanagh & G.
Rodwell (Eds.), Dialogues in educational research (pp. 89-111). Darwin: William
Michael Press.
Roe, R. H. (1913). Report of the Inspector-General of Schools. In Thirty-seventh
report of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1912 (pp. 30-35). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Roe, R. H. (1915). Report of the Inspector-General of Schools. In Thirty-ninth report
of the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1914 (pp. 29-36). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Ross, R. N. (1884). Report of Mr. District Inspector Ross. In Eighth report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1883 (pp. 67-70). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Ross, R. N. (1885). Report of Mr. District Inspector Ross. In Ninth report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1884 (pp. 68-71). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Ross, R. N. (1890). Report of Mr. District Inspector Ross. In Fourteenth report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1889 (pp. 104-111). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Ross, R. N. (1893). Report of Mr. District Inspector Ross. In Seventeenth report of
the Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1892 (pp. 79-83). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Ross, R. N. (1894). Report of Mr. District Inspector Ross. In Eighteenth report of the
Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1893 (pp. 72-77). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Ross, R. N. (1905). Report of Mr. District Inspector Ross. In Twenty-ninth report of
the Secretary for Public Instruction, for the year 1904 (pp. 31-33). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Ross, S. H. (1989). Parts, wholes, and place value. Arithmetic Teacher, 36(6), 4751.
Router, E. F. (1941). Report as district inspector. 1940. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A737.
Router, E. F., & Pascoe, V. E. (1932). Programmes of work. Toowoomba:
Robertson & Provan.
Rubenstein, R. N. (1985). Computational estimation and related mathematical skills.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 16(2), 106-119.

409

Sachar, J. (1978). An instrument for evaluating mental arithmetic skills. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 9, 233-237.
Salmon, B., & Grace, N. (1984). Problem solving: Some issues concerning the
teaching and learning of skills and strategies during Years 1-10. Brisbane:
Curriculum Services Branch, Department of Education.
Sauble, I. (1955). Development of ability to estimate and to compute mentally.
Arithmetic Teacher, 2, 33-39.
Schall, W. (1973). Comparing mental arithmetic modes of presentation in
elementary school mathematics. School Science and Mathematics, 73, 359-366.
Schedule V: Table of the minimum amount of attainments required from pupils for
admission into each class in primary schools. (1876). Queensland Government
Gazette, XVII(36), 825.
Schedule V: Standards of the classification of pupils; or, table of the minimum
amount of attainments required for admission into each class in primary schools.
(1885). Queensland Government Gazette, XXXVI(30), 490-491.
Schedule V: Course of instruction for each class. (1894). Queensland Government
Gazette, LXII(34), 320-321.
Schedule V: Course of instruction for each class. (1897). Queensland Government
Gazette, LXVII(62), 798-799.
Schedule VI: Standards of proficiency. (1894). Queensland Government Gazette,
LXII(34), 321-323.
Schedule VI: Standards of proficiency. (1897). Queensland Government Gazette,
LXVII(62), 799-801
Schedule XIV: Course of instruction for each class. (1902). Queensland
Government Gazette, LXXIX(18), 169-170.
Schedule XIV: Preface & Course of instruction for each class. (1904). Education
Office Gazette, VI(11), 200-211.
Schedule XV: Standards of proficiency. (1902). Queensland Government Gazette,
LXXIX(18), 170-174.
Schildt, A. M., Reithmuller, S. E., & Searle, C. L. (n. d.) Towards our emerging
philosophy of education in Australia. Queensland State Archives: A/18580.
Miscellaneous correspondence, notes and papers of Syllabus Committee.
Schoen, H. L., Blume, G., & Hart, E. W. (1987, April). Measuring computational
estimation processes. Paper presented at the Annual meeting of the American

410

Educational Research Association. (Educational Resource Information Centre


document, Microfiche edition, ED 286 929).
Schonell, F. J. (1955). Seven priorities in Queensland education. Queensland
Teachers' Journal, 60(4), 4-7.
Scientific and useful: Arithmetic for the bush. (1882, March 11). The Queenslander,
p. 301.
"Scipio". (1943). Syllabus soliloquy. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 48(1), 13-14.
Scott, W. (1892). Report of Mr. District Inspector Scott, 1891. In Sixteenth report of
the Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1991 (pp. 87-94). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Scribner, S. (1984). Studying working intelligence. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.),
Everyday cognition: Its development in social context (pp. 9-40). Cambridge:
Harvard University Press.
Searle, C. L. (1956). Report as district inspector. 1955. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A739, File No. 16634/56.
Searle, C. L. (1958). Report as district inspector. 1957. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A739, File No. 33437/58.
Searle, C. L. (1959). Report as district inspector. 1958. Queensland State Archives.
EDU/A740, File No. 36701/59.
Second progress report of the Royal Commission appointed to inquire into the
general working of the civil service and the mode of keeping the public accounts of
the colony. (1889). In Votes & Proceedings of the Legislative Assembly of
Queensland. (Vol. 1, pp. 607-974). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Senior Executive Forum, Queensland Department of Education. (1993). Managing
the pace of change (A report to the Minister of Education). Brisbane: The
Department.
Shibata, R. (1994). Computation in Japan from the Edo era to today: Historical
reflection on the teaching of mental computation. In R. E. Reys & N. Nohda (Eds.),
Computational alternatives for the twenty-first century: Cross-cultural perspectives
from Japan and the United States (pp. 14-18). Reston: National Council of Teachers
of Mathematics.
Shigematsu, K., Iwasaki, H., & Koyma, M. (1994). Mental computation: Evaluation,
curriculum, and instructional issues from the Japanese perspective. In R. E. Reys &
N. Nohda (Eds.), Computational alternatives for the twenty-first century: Crosscultural perspectives from Japan and the United States (pp. 19-30). Reston:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

411

Shirley, J. (1905a). The new syllabus of instruction. The scheme explained.


Queensland Education Journal, 10(10), 189-191.
Shirley, J. (1905b). The working of the new schedule. Queensland Education
Journal, 11(6), 116-118.
Shirley, J. (1906). Report of Mr. District Inspector Shirley. In Thirtieth report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1905 (pp. 36-38). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Shirley, J. (1913). Dr Shirley's Report. In Thirty-eighth report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1912 (pp. 36-38). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Shuard, H. (1989). The PrIME project. British Journal of Special Education, 16(2),
71-73.
Silver, E. A. (1987). Foundations of cognitive theory and research for mathematics
problem-solving instruction. In A. H. Schoenfeld (Ed.), Cognitive science and
mathematics education (pp. 33-60). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Silvernail, D. L. (1996). The impact of Englands national curriculum and
assessment system on classroom practice: Potential lessons for American
reformers. Educational Policy, 10(1), 46-62.
Skager, R., & Weinberg, C. (1971). Fundamentals of educational research: An
introductory approach. Glenview: Scott-Foresman.
Skelton, R. G. (Chair). (1912). Report of the revising committee of the schedule.
27.01.1912. Queensland State Archives. EDU/A788.
Skemp, R. R. (1976). Relational understanding and instrumental understanding.
Mathematics Teaching, 77, 20-26.
Sloane P. (1991). Lateral Thinking Puzzlers. New York: Sterling.
Smith, J. P. (1996). Efficacy and teaching mathematics by telling: A challenge for
reform. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27(4), 387-402.
Smith, W. H. (1914). Mr. Smith's Report. In Thirty-eighth report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1913 (pp. 72-80). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Smith, W. H. (1917). Mr. Smith's Report. In Forty-first report of the Secretary of
Public Instruction, for the year 1916 (pp. 77-87). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Somers, K. A. (1928). From Mr. Somers's Report. In Fifty-second report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1927 (pp. 81-85). Brisbane: Government
Printer.
Sowder, J. T. (1989). Research into practice: Developing understanding of
computational estimation. Arithmetic Teacher, 36(3), 25-27.

412

Sowder, J. T. (1990). Mental computation and number sense. Arithmetic Teacher,


37(7), 18-20.
Sowder, J. T. (1991, August). Considerations for research on computation:
Borrowing from others. Paper presented at Gwinganna Computation Conference,
Gold Coast Hinterland, Australia.
Sowder, J. T. (1992). Making sense of number in school mathematics. In G.
Leinhardt, R. Putnam, & R. Hattrup (Eds.), Analysis of arithmetic for mathematics
(pp. 1-51). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Sowder, J. T. (1994). Cognitive and metacognitive processes in mental computation
and computational estimation. In R. E. Reys & N. Nohda (Eds.), Computational
alternatives for the twenty-first century: Cross-cultural perspectives from Japan and
the United States (pp. 139-146). Reston: National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics.
Sowder, J. T., & Schappelle, B. (1994). Number sense-making. Arithmetic Teacher,
41(6), 342-345.
Sowder, J. T., & Wheeler, M. M. (1989). The development of concepts and
strategies used in computational estimation. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 20(2), 130-146.
Speedy, G. (1992). Curriculum initiatives (Commissioned Report No. 12). Canberra:
Australian Government Publishing Service.
Spitzer, H. F. (1948). The teaching of arithmetic. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
St. Ledger. (1905). Points of contrast between the present and the former syllabus.
Queensland Education Journal, 10(11), 218-223.
Statement. Explanatory of the system of education administered by the National
Board in New South Wales (n. d.). Sydney: The Board.
Student performance standards in mathematics for Queensland schools. (1994).
Melbourne: Curriculum Corporation.
Successful mental arithmetic. (1899). The Education Office Gazette, 1(6), 79.
Svenson, O., & Sjoberg, K. (1982). Solving simple subtractions during the first three
school years. Journal of Experimental Education, 50(2), 91-100.
Syllabus debate: 50th Annual Conference. (1938). Queensland Teachers' Journal,
44(1), 15-17.
Taylor, W. (1928). From Mr. Taylor's Report. In Fifty-second report of the Secretary
of Public Instruction, for the year 1927 (pp. 41-46). Brisbane: Government Printer.
Teachers and the Syllabus. (1949). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 54(2), 1-2.

413

Teaching hints: Arithmetic. (1908). The Education Office Gazette, 10(1), 15-18.
Teaching of arithmetic. (1927). The Education Office Gazette, 29(8), 292.
"The...Commentator". (1947). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 52(4), 21.
The "dead hand" of inspection. (1939). Queensland Teachers Journal, 44(10), 1.
The new syllabus. (1904). Queensland Education Journal, 10(8), 142-143.
The primary school syllabus. (1933). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 38(7), 1.
The Queensland State Department of Education: 1875-1904. (1966). Queensland
Teacher's Journal, 71(10), 324-331.
The real issue in the 1948 scholarship papers. (1949). Queensland Teachers'
Journal, 54(3), 1-2.
The syllabus. (1934). Queensland Teachers' Journal, 39(8), 1.
The teacher's revised syllabus. (1927). Queensland Teacher's Journal, 32(10), 1119.
Thistlethwaite, J. K. (1954). Report as district inspector. 1953. Queensland State
Archives. EDU/A738, File. No. 52837/54.
Thompson, A. G., Philipp, R. A., Thompson, P. W., & Boyd, B. A. (1994).
Calculational and conceptual orientations in teaching mathematics. In D. B. Aichele
(Ed.). Professional development for teachers of mathematics (pp. 79-92). Reston:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Thompson, F. C. (1930). The Queensland arithmetic. Grade V. Brisbane: Aitken.
Thorndike, E. (1922). The psychology of arithmetic. New York: Macmillan.
Thornton, C. (1985). Mental. Rhombus, 13(4), 9-12.
Thornton, C. A., Jones, G. A., & Neal, J. L. (1995). The 100s chart: A stepping stone
to mental mathematics. Teaching Children Mathematics, 1(8), 480-483.
Trafton, P. R. (1978). Estimation and mental arithmetic: Important components of
computation. In M. Suydam & R. E. Reys (Eds.), Developing computational skills
(pp. 196-213). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Trafton, P. R. (1986). Teaching computational estimation: Establishing an estimation
mind-set. In H. L. Schoen (Ed.), Estimation and mental computation (pp. 16-30).
Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

414

Trafton, P. R. (1989). Reflections on the number sense conference. In J. T. Sowder


& B. P. Schappelee (Eds.), Establishing foundations for research on number sense
and related topics: Report of a conference (pp. 74-77). San Diego: San Diego State
University for Research in Mathematics and Science Education.
Trafton, P. R. (1992). Using number sense to develop mental computation and
computational estimation. In C. J. Irons (Ed.), Challenging children to think when
they compute: Conference papers (pp. 78-92). Brisbane: The Centre for
Mathematics and Science Education.
Trafton, P. R., & Suydam, M. N. (1975). Computational skills: A point of view.
Arithmetic Teacher, 22(7), 528-537.
Threadgill-Sowder, J. T. (1988). Mental computation and number comparison: Their
roles in the development of number sense and computational estimation. In J.
Hiebert & M. Behr (Eds.), Number concepts and operations in the middle grades
(pp. 182-197). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum & National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics.
Treffers, A. (1991). Meeting innumeracy at primary school. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 22, 333-352.
Trudgian, W. J. (1929). From Mr. Trudgian's report. In Fifty-fifth report of the
Secretary of Public Instruction, for the year 1928 (pp. 104-110). Brisbane:
Government Printer.
Tuckman, B. W. (1988). Conducting educational research (3rd ed.). San Diego:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Turney, C. (1972). Continuity and change in the public primary schools 1914-1932.
In J. Cleverly & J. R. Lawry (Eds.), Australian education in the twentieth century (pp.
32-76). Melbourne: Longman.
"Twenty-Keep List". (1942). The "Notation-ists". Queensland Teachers' Journal,
47(8), 14.
Tyrrell, I. R. (1968). The reform of State education in Queensland, 1900-1914.
Unpublished Bachelor of Arts (Honours in History) thesis, University of Queensland,
Brisbane.
Vakali, M. (1985). Children's thinking in arithmetic word problem solving. Journal of
Experimental Education, 53(2), 106-113.
van der Heijden, M. K. (1995). A holistic Vygotskian operational definition of
approach behaviour for the study of personality and learning. Retrieved June 18,
1997 from the World Wide Web: http://psych.hanover.edu/vygotsky/heijden.html.
"Vigilate". (1950). Arithmetic in a hurry. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 55(6), 10-11.

415

Wandt, E., & Brown, G. W. (1957). Non-occupational uses of mathematics.


Arithmetic Teacher, 4, 151-154.
Warner, I. (1981). Program in mathematics for Queensland primary schools;
teachers' perceptions. Brisbane: Queensland Department of Education, Research
Branch.
Wasted time at school: Importance of individual teaching. (1937, August 7). The
Courier Mail, p. 15.
Weissglass, J. (1994). Changing mathematics teaching means changing ourselves:
Implications for professional development. In D. B. Aichele (Ed.), Professional
development for teachers of mathematics (pp. 67-78). Reston: National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics.
Welton, J. (1924). Principles and methods of teaching (3rd ed.). London: University
Tutorial Press.
Wiebe, J. H. (1987). Calculators and the mathematics curriculum. Arithmetic
Teacher, 34(6), 57-60.
Wiersma, W. (1995). Research methods in education (6th ed.). Needham Heights:
Allyn & Bacon.
Wilkins, W. (1886). The principles that underlie the art of teaching: Six lectures,
delivered at the Technical College, Sydney, under the auspices of the Board of
Technical Education - Lecture IV (pp. 33-43) . Sydney: Government Printer.
Willadsen, E. J. (1970-1976). Mathematics guide (Grades 4-7). Brisbane: William
Brooks.
Willis, S. (1990). Numeracy and society: The shifting ground. In Willis, S. (Ed.),
Being numerate: What counts? (pp. 1-23). Melbourne: Australian Council for
Educational Research.
Willis, S. (n. d.). From national statements and profiles to student performance
standards: Fitting the pieces together. Unpublished paper.
Willis, S. (1992). The national statement on mathematics for Australian schools: A
perspective on computation. In C. J. Irons (Ed.), Challenging children to think when
they compute: Conference papers (pp. 1-13). Brisbane: The Centre for Mathematics
and Science Education.
Willis, S. (1995, September). Keynote address. Queensland Association of
Mathematics Teachers Annual Conference, Surfers Paradise.
Willis, S., & Stephens, M. (1991). A national statement on mathematics for
Australian schools. Curriculum Perspectives - Newsletter Edition, 11(1), 3-9.

416

Wisdom, A. (1932). Arithmetical dictation: A systematic series of exercises in mental


arithmetic. London: University of London Press.
Wiltshire, K. (Chair). (1994), Report of the review of the Queensland school
curriculum (Vol. 1-3). Brisbane: The State of Queensland.
Wolf, W. C. (1966). What about non-written figuring? Peabody Journal of Education,
43(5), 271-274.
Wolters, G., Beishuizen, M., Broers, G., & Knoppert, W. (1990). Mental arithmetic:
Effects of calculation procedure and problem difficulty on solution latency. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 20-30.
Woodgate, W. J. L. (1955). Report as district inspector. 1954. Queensland State
Archives. EDU/A738, File. No. 52837/55.
Woolfolk, A. E. (1987). Educational psychology (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs:
Prentice-Hall.
Wyeth, E. R. (1955). Education in Queensland: A history of education in
Queensland and in the Moreton Bay District of New South Wales (Australian Council
for Educational Research Series No. 67). Melbourne: Australian Council for
Educational Research.
"X+Y=Z". (1945). The myth of mathematics. Queensland Teachers' Journal, 50(5),
14-15.
Yackel, E., Cobb, P., Wood, T., Wheatley, G., & Merkel, G. (1990). The importance
of social interaction in children's construction of mathematical knowledge. In T. J.
Cooney (Ed.), Teaching mathematics in the 1990s (pp. 12-21). Reston: National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
"Z". (1879, June 7). Letter to the editor. The Australian, p. 768.
Zepp, R. (1976). Algorithms and mental computation. In M. Suydam & A. R.
Osborne (Eds.), Algorithmic learning (pp. 97-104). (Educational Resources
Information Centre Document, Microfiche Edition, ED 373 960).

419

APPENDIX A

Summary of Mental Arithmetic in Queensland


Mathematics Schedules and Syllabuses (1860-1964)
A.1

1860 Schedule

Minimum attainments for children enrolled one Quarter in each class:


First class:
Perform mentally all the elementary arithmetical operations with numbers, not
involving a higher result than 30.
Second class:
Know the multiplication tables.
Sequel to second class:
Know the most useful arithmetical tables.
Third class:
Know the easier rules of mental arithmetic.
Fourth class:
Know the rules of mental arithmetic.
Fifth class:
(Be acquainted with the whole Theory of Arithmetic, and its application, to the
Mensuration of Superficies1.)

A.2

1876 Schedule

Minimum attainments2 required from pupils for admission into each class.
(Specific details were not provided for the First class in 1876. The following were
issued by Under Secretary Anderson in April 1879):

Mensuration of surfaces.

2 The minimum attainments for admission into any class are the maximum attainments
expected from the class below it. The schedule can therefore be regarded as a
Programme of Instruction for pupils attending Primary Schools. The work to be gone
through in any class (the Fifth Class excepted) will be found detailed in the column with
the name of the class next above it. (Regulations of the Department of Public instruction
in Queensland. (1985). Queensland Government Gazette, XXXVI(30), p. 490).

420

First class (Division I):


To add mentally two figures to a result not greater than 10.
First class (Division II):
To add mentally three numbers to a result not greater than 20.
First class (Division III):
To add mentally any numbers to a result not greater than 30.
(General Instructions for the guidance of teachers. April 1879, Queensland State
Archives, EDU/A602.)
Lower second class:
Perform mental addition up to a result not higher than thirty.
Upper second class:
Perform easy mental operations in addition and subtraction.
Third class:
Perform mentally easy operations in the simple rules.
Fourth class:
Perform mentally easy operations on the compound rules and reduction, including
bills of parcels, rectangular areas, and other practical problems.
Fifth class:
Perform mentally simple operations in proportion, practice, vulgar fractions and
simple interest, including miscellaneous problems.

A.3

1891 Schedule (Effective 1 January 1892)

First class (Course 2 years) (In infant schools 3 years):


To add mentally numbers of one figure to a result not greater than 40, and to
subtract any number of one figure from any number of two figures.
Standards of proficiency:
1st half-year of enrolment (9 months - Infant Schools):
To add mentally numbers applied to objects to a result not greater than 10.
2nd half-year of enrolment:
To add mentally numbers applied to objects to a result not greater than 20.
3rd half-year of enrolment:
To add mentally numbers to a result not greater than 30, and to separate
numbers not greater than 30 into two component parts.

421

4th half-year of enrolment:


To add mentally numbers to a result not greater than 40; to lessen mentally
any number of two figures by any number of one digit.
Second class (Course 1 years):
To perform mentally operations in the following rules: Addition, subtraction,
multiplication and division of abstract numbers, and addition and subtraction of
money.
Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
Mental - applications of addition, subtraction, and multiplication.
Second half-year:
Mental - applications of addition, subtraction, multiplication and division.
Third half-year:
Mental - to add any two sums of money (excluding parts of a penny) to a result
not greater than 1; to subtract any sum of money as before from 1; to
express 1 by combinations of current coins; to subtract any sum of money as
before from 1; to express 1 by combinations of current coins.
Third class (Course 1 years):
To perform mentally operations in the following rules: The "more useful" weights
and measures; Addition, subtraction, multiplication, division, and reduction of
money; Bills of parcels.
Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
Mental - to find the price of a dozen the price of one being given, and
conversely; easy shopping transactions.
Second half-year:
Mental - to find the price of a score or a gross, the price of one being given,
and conversely; easy shopping transactions.
Third half-year:
Mental - shopping transactions.
Fourth class (Course 1 years):
To perform mentally operations in the following rules: The unitary method, vulgar
fractions, practice, decimals, proportion.
Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
Mental - exercises in easy vulgar fractions and the unitary method.
Second half-year:
Mental - exercises in easy decimals and practice.

422

Third half-year:
Mental - exercises in vulgar fractions, decimals and proportion.
Fifth class (Course 1 years):
To perform mentally operations in the following rules: Interest; discount; square
root; percentages. Revision of the course for first, second, and third classes.
Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
Mental - easy exercises in interest, discount, and square root;
Second half-year:
Mental - easy exercises in percentages.
Third half-year:
Mental - easy exercises in the preceding rules.
Sixth class (Course 2 years):
Revision of the course for fourth and fifth classes, with mental exercises.

A.4

1894 Schedule

Minor changes in standards of proficiency from 1892 resulting from the First and
Sixth Class being reduced from 2 years to 1 years.
First class (Course 1 years)
Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
To add mentally numbers applied to objects to a result not greater than 20.
Second half-year:
To add mentally numbers applied to objects to a result not greater than 30,
and to separate numbers not greater than 30 into two component parts.
Third half-year:
To add mentally numbers to a result not greater than 40, and to lessen
mentally any number of two figures by any number on one figure.

423

A.5

1897 Schedule

First class (2 years):


To add mentally numbers of one figure to a result not greater than fifty.
Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
To add mentally numbers applied to objects to a result not greater than 10.
Second half-year:
To add mentally numbers applied to objects to a result not greater than 30.
Third half-year:
To add mentally to a result not greater than 40.
Fourth half-year:
To add mentally numbers to a result not greater than 50.'
Second class (1 years):
To perform mentally operations in addition, subtraction, multiplication, and
division of abstract numbers.
Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
Mental - applications of addition and subtraction.
Second half-year:
Mental - application of addition, subtraction, and multiplication.
Third half-year:
Mental - application of the four simple rules.
Third class (1 years):
Mental operations in the rules for reduction, addition, subtraction, multiplication,
and division of money and easy bills of parcels.
Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
Mental - easy practical applications of reduction, addition, and subtraction of
money (mechanical operations only).
Second half-year:
Mental - easy practical applications of reduction, addition, subtraction,
multiplication, and division of money (mechanical operations only).

Third half-year:
Easy problems in money, mentally.

424

Fourth class (1 years):


Mental operations in the rules for reduction, addition, subtraction, multiplication,
and division of the more useful weights and measures; easy vulgar and decimal
fractions; simple practice and simple proportion.
Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
Mental - exercises in reduction, addition, subtraction, multiplication, and
division of the more useful weights and measures.
Second half-year:
Mental - exercises in easy vulgar fractions.
Third half-year:
Mental - exercises in simple practice, and simple proportion.
Fifth class (1 years):
Mental arithmetic3. (Compound practice and compound proportion; vulgar and
decimal fractions; interest and discount; square root; mensuration of the
parallelogram, triangle, and circle)
Standards of proficiency (Fifth Class):
First half-year:
Mental arithmetic. (Compound practice, and compound proportion; square
root; mensuration of the parallelogram)
Second half-year:
Mental arithmetic. (Vulgar and decimal fractions, with applications to concrete
quantities; mensuration of the triangle)
Third half-year:
Mental arithmetic. (Simple and compound interest; discount; mensuration of
he parallelogram, triangle and circle)
Sixth class (1 years):
Mental arithmetic. (Percentages; miscellaneous problems; cube root; mensuration
of plane surfaces and solids)

For the fifth and sixth classes, the mental arithmetic to be taught is not directly
specified. Following the specification of other work, which is presented here in
brackets, the schedule simply states "mental arithmetic".

425

Standards of proficiency:
First half-year:
Mental arithmetic. (Profit and loss; miscellaneous problems, with special
reference to vulgar fractions;, cube root; mensuration of the polygon, prism,
and cylinder)
Second half-year:
Mental arithmetic. (Stocks; miscellaneous problems, with special reference to
decimal fractions; mensuration of the cone and pyramid)
Third half-year:
Mental arithmetic. (Misc. problems; mensuration of planes and solids)

A.6

1902 Schedule (Minor changes from 1897)

Fifth class (Course 1 years. Age 11 - 13 years):


Mental arithmetic4 (Commercial arithmetic; compound practice and compound
proportion; vulgar and decimal fractions; interest and discount; square root;
Longman's Junior School Mensuration, Part I.)
First half-year:
Mental arithmetic. (Commercial arithmetic; compound practice, and compound
proportion; square root; mensuration - Longman's Junior School Mensuration,
Chapters I to III)
Second half-year:
Mental arithmetic (Commercial arithmetic; vulgar arithmetic and decimal
fractions, with applications to concrete quantities; mensuration, Longmans,
Chapters I to V)
Third half-year:
Mental arithmetic. (Commercial arithmetic; simple and compound interest;
discount; mensuration, Longmans', Chapters I to VII)
Sixth class (Course 1 years. Age 13 - 14 years):
Mental arithmetic (Commercial arithmetic; percentage; cube root; Longman's
Junior School Mensuration, Parts I and II)
First half-year:
Mental arithmetic (Commercial arithmetic; profit and loss; cube root;
mensuration - Longman's, Chapters I to IX)

For the fifth and sixth classes, the mental arithmetic to be taught is not directly
specified. Following the specification of other work, which is presented here in
brackets, the schedules simply states "mental arithmetic".

426

Second half-year:
Mental arithmetic (Commercial arithmetic; stocks; mensuration - Longman's,
Chapters I to X)
Third half-year:
Mental arithmetic (Commercial arithmetic; mensuration - Longman's,
Chapters I to XI)

A.7

1904 Schedule (Effective 1 January 1905)

First class (Course, 2 years. Age 5-7 years):


Mental exercises in addition and subtraction to 50.
Second class (Course 1 years. Age 7-8 years):
Mathematics - Mental Work. Concrete exercises involving the four simple
operations, and falling within the range of the pupils' experience. Simple factors.
Exercises in finding , , 1/8 of given quantities and numbers. Construction by
pupils of multiplication table to 12 times 12. The money tables. Measurement with
foot-rule in yards, feet, inches, and halves, fourths, and eighths of an inch. The
symbols of operations required in addition, subtraction, multiplication, and
division, and their use in representing the processes employed in concrete
exercises.'
Third class (Course 1 years. Age 8-10 years):
Mathematics. - Mental and Oral Work. measures of length (yard, feet, and
inches), weight (avoirdupois), capacity (gallons, quarts, pints), and time. Concrete
applications of the fractions , 2/3, 3/4, 11/12. Concrete exercises in these and in
domestic accounts and simple business transactions. Measurements of objects
and distances about the school premises. Symbols of operation - their use
continued
Fourth class (Course 1 years. Age 10 to 11 years)
Mathematics - Mental and Oral Work. Tables of length, area, weight, capacity,
and time. Mental operations in the practical use of these and in the extended use
of fractions, including decimal fractions. Measurements of furniture, rooms, school
buildings, and play ground continued. Oral statement of processes employed in
written work of class. Ratio. Symbols of operation - their use continued.
Fifth class (Course 1 years. Age 11 - 13 years):
Mathematics - Oral and Mental Work. Mental operations in common business
transactions of wider range than in the Fourth Class. Easy calculations in area.
Exercises involving the use of simple fractions, decimals, and easy percentages.
percentages applied to interest, ordinary retail discount, profit and loss, and
proportion. Cubic measure.

Sixth class (Course 1 years. Age 13 - 14 years)

427

Mathematics - Oral and Mental Work. As before, with the addition of easy
transactions in stocks. Easy operations in algebra and geometry, at the discretion
of the teacher.

A.8

1914 Syllabus (Effective 1 January 1915)

First class (Course, 2 years. Age, 5-7 years):


Mental exercises in addition and subtraction to 50.
First half-year:
"Work should be exclusively oral, except that children should learn to know
and to make the digits including 0."
Second half-year:
Add mentally any numbers to a result not greater than 19, and to reduce any
number not greater than 19 by any smaller number.
Third half-year:
Mental addition and subtraction should involve, answers included, numbers no
larger than 30.
Fourth half-year:
Mental exercises in addition and subtraction to 50.
Second class (Course, 1 years. Age, 7-8 years):
Mental Work. Concrete exercises involving the four simple operations, and falling
within the range of the pupils' experience. Simple factors. Exercises in finding ,
, 1/8 of given quantities and numbers. Construction by pupils of multiplication
table to 12 times 12. The money tables and to know the value of current coins.
Measurement with foot-rule in yards, feet, inches, and halves, fourths, and
eighths of an inch. The symbols of operations required in addition, subtraction,
multiplication, and division, and their use in representing the processes employed
in concrete exercises.'
Third class (Course, 1 years. Age, 8-10 years):
Mental Work. Measures of length (yard, feet, and inches), weight (avoirdupois),
capacity (bushels, pecks, gallons, quarts, pints), and time. Concrete applications
of halves, thirds, fourths, eights, twelfths. Exercises in domestic accounts and
simple business transactions. Measurements of objects and distances about the
school premises; weighing common objects and measuring liquids, if practicable.
Fourth class (Course, 1 years. Age, 10 to 11 years):
Mental Work. Tables of length, area, weight, capacity, and time. Mental
operations in the practical use of these and in the extended use of fractions,
including decimal fractions. Measurements of furniture, rooms, school buildings,
and playground. Ratio as expressed by fractions and decimals.
Fifth class (Course, 1 years. Age, 11 - 13 years)

428

Oral Work. Mental operations in common business transactions of wider range


than in the Fourth Class. Easy calculations in areas. Exercises involving the use
of simple fractions, decimals, and easy percentages, interest, ordinary retail
discount, profit and loss, and proportion. Cubic measure.
Sixth class (Course, 1 years. Age, 13 - 14 years)
Oral Work. As before, with the addition of easy transactions in stocks. Easy
operations in algebra and geometry, at the discretion of the teacher.

A.9

1930 Syllabus

Preparatory Grade: Mental arithmetic not mentioned specifically.


Grade I: Mental.
(a) Exercises in addition, subtraction, and multiplication with numbers and
quantities within the pupil's range. Very simple examples in division.
Notes:
Work in mental should precede and be preparation for written. The
maximum amount of work should be thrown upon the pupils. An oral
statement of the various steps to be followed in the solving of each mental
problem should be given by the pupils
Suggested steps: (1) Statement of problem, (2) reasons for successive
operations (3) rules employed, and (4) actual working. By this method
teachers able to discover individual weaknesses.
The exercises should be well graded and suited to the average
intelligence of class. Promiscuous work is of little value. One difficulty at a
time should be mastered and constant revision is essential.
Exercises in addition and subtraction should involve numbers to 99. In
multiplication, deal with multipliers to 6.
(b) To find , and 3/4 of numbers and quantities
Notes:
Applied to things, measurement of size or value; to numbers.
Recognition of parts should follow practical exercises in dividing and
measuring things, and exercises in finding fractions of numbers should
follow after practice in tables, thus: 4 times 6 = 24; of 24 = 6; 2/4 or
of 24 = 12; 3/4 of 24 = 18.
(c) Further practice in buying, selling, and giving change with sums to 1s.
Component parts of 1s. taken two at a time, as 5d. and 7d., etc. Coin
equivalents of 1s. in current coins.
Notes:
Tender a shilling for a pound of sugar at 5d. and loaf of bread at 6d.
Purchase a notebook at 3d., a 1d. stamp, and a lead pencil at 2d. out of a
shilling, &c.

429

Grade II: Oral work.


(a) Finding half, quarter, and eighth of numbers and quantities
Notes:
Introductory exercises in paper-folding.
Fractional parts could be taught in connection with multiplication and
division tables, e.g. 8 times 12 = 96, then 12 = 1/8 of 96
Halves and fourths of an inchtaught and applied to measurements
"Half" and "quarter" should also be applied to "Time".
(b) Application of multiplication to easy one-step reduction of money and of the
weights and measures dealt with in the Tables (quarts, pints, pounds,
minute, hour day week, inches, feet, yard).
Notes:
For example, 1 quart = 2 pints; how many pints in 3 quarts?
Shillings to pence and pence to shillings (whole numbers of shillings only)
(c) Component parts of 1s. taken two at a time, such as 2d. + 9d., and
1s. - 10d., etc.
Notes:
Shopping transactions as suggested in previous grades.
(d) Simple exercises in buying and selling with current coins up to 1.
Notes:
Spending a florin, a shilling, and a sixpence = total. Spending a two
shilling piece and a shilling out of 1. (Actual or imitation coins could be
used.)
(e) Easy problems in the four simple rules, with numbers within the pupils'
range.
Notes:
These exercises should be a preparation for the written work.
Grade III: Oral Work.
(a) Fractions; applications of fractions (one-half, one-third, one-quarter, etc) to
numbers, money, and quantities.

430

Notes:
The exercises outlined in the previous grade could be continued and
extended to harder examples in their application. These exercises are
intended only as an introduction to the study of vulgar fractions at a later
stage.
Resolving easy numbers such as 24 and 36 to prime factors as
preparation for the work in fractions and cancelling.
(b) Simple mental problems based on the tables.
Notes:
These exercises are intended to show the practical application of the
tables, and to serve as an introduction to the compound rules.
(c) Buying and selling, saving and spending, with sums to 1. Simple exercises
based on the four rules in money.
Notes:
The material for these exercises will be supplied by ordinary household
accounts, as for example, the butcher's, the baker's, the grocer's, or the
draper's bills.
Grade IV: Oral Arithmetic.
(a) Exercises involving the use of vulgar fractions (denominators not to exceed
12) and of decimals to tenths.
Notes:
Fractional parts will now include fractions such as 3/8, 5/6, 11,12, 4/9, 6/7,
3/5 applied to numbers, money, and to tables - e.g. 3/8 of 1; 7/10 of 1;
3/7 of 1 quarter (in lb);
Decimals - .7 of 1; .9 of 1 ton, etc.
(b) Mental exercises based on the compound rules, including household and
shopping transactions familiar to the pupils.
Notes:
Shopping transactions continued, and in addition, exercises as
preparation for the written work in weights and measures.
Grade V: Oral Arithmetic.
(a) Exercises involving the use of vulgar and decimal fractions
Notes:
Simple exercises involving the four rules as introduction to the written
work, e.g. + 1/3; 3/8 + 1/4; of 8/9; .2 + .3 + .4; .8 2; 2 .5.
The conversion of easy vulgar fractions to decimals and vice versa:
= .5; = .25; 7/8 = .875; 3/5 = .6, etc.

(b) Ratio expressed as fractions and decimals

431

Notes:
Preparatory exercises as introduction to Simple Proportion, e.g. 1 florin to
1 = 1/10 or .1; 1 furlong to 1 mile = 1/8 or .125; 1 rood to 1 acre = or
.25; 1 ton 10 cwts. to 2 tons 10 cwts = 3/5 or .6. etc. (See Practical School
Method, Cox & MacDonald, 269-171)
(c) Aliquot parts of 1 represented by either fractions or decimals
Notes:
Variety of mental exercises involving use of simple aliquot parts is
essential.
(d) Mental exercises dealing with money, wights and measures, vulgar and
decimal fractions (applied to concrete quantities), simple proportion. Finding
areas of squares and rectangles.
Notes:
Preliminary work in mensuration should be confined entirely to oral and
mental exercises.
(e) Practice in working by short methods (e.g. "dozens" and "scores" rules might
be applied).
Notes:
After the pupils have been thoroughly exercises in any rule, short
methods of calculation could be introduced. To encourage initiative,
different solutions of the same problem might be required from the pupils.
The 'dozens' and 'scores' rules might be applied.
Grade VI (1st Form, Intermediate Schools): Oral Work.
(a) Exercises preparatory to the written work, including operations in simple
business transactions of a wider range than those taken in Grade V.
Notes:
The work in this Grade will extend that of Grade V., and will include
exercises in simple proportion, percentages, and simple interest; squaring
and taking the square root; exercises preliminary to written work in
Mensuration. While written work in Practice is not required, simple
exercises, formerly regarded as questions in this rule, should be given
and worked by appropriate methods. These will include problems in use
of aliquot parts in such examples as - 18 books at 2s 6d each; 40 sheep
at 1 12s. 6d. each.
Grade VII (2nd Form, Intermediate Schools): Oral Work.
(a) Mental exercises preparatory to the written work in arithmetic and
mensuration.
(b) Practice in short methods of calculation.
Notes:

432

Pupils should be given practice in devising short cuts and easy methods.

A.10 1938 Amendments


Preparatory 1 and 2 (One Year):
Mental exercises to 10. Easy problems.
Preparatory 3 (Six months):
Mental exercises, including easy problems in numbers of 19.
Preparatory 4 (Six months):
Mental exercises to cover: Count to 100 in ones, twos, fives, tens; Notation and
numeration to 99; Addition tables to 99; Subtraction tables to 99; Halving and
doubling to 100; Recognise coins - half-penny, penny, threepence, sixpence,
shilling; The foot rule (inches only); Symbols +, -, and = ; Meaning of terms: pair,
couple, dozen, score. Easy problems.
Grade I: Mental.
(a) Exercises in addition, subtraction, and multiplication with numbers and
quantities within the pupils' range. Very simple examples in division.
Notes:
Work in mental should precede and be a preparation for the written work.
The maximum amount of work should be thrown upon the pupils.
The exercises should be well graded and suited to the average
intelligence of the class. one difficulty at a time should be mastered, and
constant revision is essential. Exercises in addition and subtractions
should not involves numbers beyond 99.
At every stage the mechanical work should be applied to easy mental
problems. teachers should not depend exclusively on text-books, but
should prepare their own examples adapted to local conditions and
needs.
(b) Fractions: To find one-half and one-quarter.
Notes:
Applied to things, measurement of size or value, and to numbers.
Recognition of parts should follow from practical exercises in dividing and
measuring things, and exercises in finding fractions of numbers should
follow after practice in tables - thus, 4 times 6 = 24; of 24 = 6.
(c) Further practice in buying, selling, and giving change with sums to 1s.;
Component parts of 1s. taken two at a time as 5d. and 7d. etc.; Coin
equivalents of 1s. in current coins. No farthings.
Grade II: Oral Work.
As for 1930 syllabus.
Notes (Additional):

433

Material for mental problems will be found in daily experience of children


in home and school. In framing questions, teachers should be careful that
the price they place on commodities should approximate those which
obtain locally.
Examples should not exceed in difficulty those given in the syllabus.
Grade III: Oral Work.
(a) Fractions; applications of fractions (one-half, one-third, one-quarter, etc. to
one-twelfth) to numbers, money, and quantities.
Notes:
Reference to "Resolving easy numbers such as 24 and 36 to prime
factors as preparation for the work in fractions and cancelling" was
deleted.
(b) Simple mental problems based on the tables.
(c) Buying and selling, saving and spending, with sums to 1. Simple exercises
based on the four rules in money.
Grade IV: Oral Arithmetic.
As for 1930 syllabus
Notes:
"Resolving easy numbers such as 24 and 36 in to prime factors as a
preparation for the work in fractions and cancelling" moved from Grade III.
Grade V: Oral Arithmetic.
As for 1930 Syllabus except that "Ratio expressed as fractions and decimals" and
"Simple proportion" were deleted.
Grade VI: Oral Arithmetic.
(a) Exercises preparatory to the written work, including operations in simple
business transactions of a wider range than those taken in Grade V.
(b) Ratio expressed as fractions and decimals (From Grade V in 1930 Syllabus).
Grade VII: Oral Work. As for 1930 Syllabus.

434

A.11 1948 Amendments


Preparatory 1 (Six months)
No mental exercises specified.
Preparatory 2 (Six months)
Mental exercises to 8. Terms - pair. Easy problems.
Preparatory 3 (Six months)
Mental exercises including easy problems in number to 13. One operation only.
Preparatory 4 (Six months)
Mental exercises to cover: Count to 19 in ones, twos; Notation and numeration to
19; Addition tables (including first extension) to 19; Subtraction tables to 19
(based on addition tables); Halving and doubling to 18; Recognise coins - halfpenny, penny, three pence, six pence, shilling; Symbols +, -, and = ; Terms: pair,
couple, dozen.
Grade I: Mental.
(a) Exercises in addition and subtraction with numbers and quantities within the
pupil's range
Notes:
Exercises in addition and subtraction should not involve numbers beyond
99.
(b) Fractions: Doubling and halving to 100
Notes:
Examples should not involve carrying or remainder, e.g. Double 42 or 21.
Halve 84, 68
(c) Further practice in buying, selling and giving change with sums to 1s.
Notes:
Continue shopping practice as suggested in previous grade. Tender 1s.
Grade II: Oral Work.
(a) Easy problems on the four simple rules, with numbers within the pupils'
range.
Notes:
Children should have extensive practice in addition.
(b) Component parts of 1s. taken two at a time, such as 2d. 9d.; 1s.-10d.
etc.
(c) Simple exercises in buying and selling with current coins to 1.
(d) Terms - Pair, couple, dozen, score, gross.

435

(e) Finding halves and quarters of numbers and quantities


Grade III: Oral Work.
(a) Simple exercises based on the four simple rules, and on the tables taught,
(limited to two steps)
Notes:
These exercises are intended to show the practical application of the
tables, and to serve as an introduction to compound rules
(b) Fractions; applications of fractions (one-half, one-third, one-quarter, etc. to
one-twelfth) to numbers, money and quantities. (Multiples of denominators
only)
Notes:
The exercises outlined in previous grade could be continued and
extended to harder examples in their applications. The exercises are
intended only as an introduction to the study of vulgar fractions at a later
stage.
(c) Buying and selling, saving and spending with sums to 1. Very simple
exercises based on reduction and the four rules in money.
Notes:
The material for these exercises supplied by the ordinary household
accounts, as for example, the butcher's, the baker's, the grocer's, or the
draper's bills.
Grade IV: Oral Arithmetic.
(a) Mental exercises based on compound rules, including household and
shopping transactions familiar to the pupils.
Notes:
Shopping transactions continued and, in addition, exercises as a
preparation for the written work in weights and measures.
Grade V: Oral Arithmetic.
(a) Exercises involving the use of vulgar and decimal fractions
Notes:
Simple exercises involving the four rules as introduction to the written
work, e.g. + 1/3; 3/8 + 1/4; 3/4 - 1/8; 11/12 - 1/6; 3/4 x 2/3; of 8/9; 4/5
1/10; 8/9 2/3; .2 + .3+ .4; .9 - .1 - .3; 7 times .5; 6 times .9; .8 2; 2
.5; 2 - .7; .3 x .2; .6 .2; The conversion of easy vulgar fractions to
decimals and vice versa: e.g. = .5; = .25; 3/4 = .75; 1/8 = .125;
7/8 = .875
(b) Prime factors of numbers to 100
(c) Aliquot parts of 1 represented either by fractions or by decimals

436

Notes:
Variety of mental exercises involving the use of simple aliquot parts is
essential.
(d) Exercises dealing with money, weights, and measures, and vulgar and
decimal fractions (applied to concrete quantities)
(e) Practice in working by short methods.
Notes:
After the pupils have been thoroughly exercises in any rule, short
methods of calculation could be introduced. To encourage initiative,
different solutions of the same problem might bed required from the
pupils. The 'dozens' and 'scores' rules might be applied.
Grade VI: Oral Work.
(a) Exercises preparatory to written work
(b) Exercises of practical value in teaching principles involved in written
arithmetic and those used in every day commercial transactions
(c) Short methods of calculation.
Notes:
The dozen rule; The score rule; The value of 240 articles; The value of
480 articles; Aliquot parts of one pound; The square of numbers with ;
also square of numbers ending in 5; Division and multiplication by 25.
Grade VII: Oral Work.
(a) Mental exercises preparatory to the written work in Arithmetic and
Mensuration
(b) Continued practice in short methods of calculation

A.12 1952 Syllabus


Preparatory Grade: Mental arithmetic not mentioned specifically.
Grade I:
Oral exercises to 10.
Notes:
These must involve one operation only. Correlate with number facts,
games, songs, number experiences, projects, &c.

437

Grade II: Oral arithmetic.


(a) Addition and subtraction to 18.
Notes:
Problems should be limited to two operations
(b) First extension to 99.
Notes:
One operation only is required.
(c) Other activities:
Notes:
Practical elementary measuring with foot rule and tape measure, e.g.
length of reader, slate, desk; heights of seat, table, &c. (Approximate
measurements to an inch are sufficient.) Estimating lengths to 1 foot.
Reading clock faces - hours only.
Grade III: Oral Arithmetic.
(a) Exercises on the four simple rules with numbers within the pupils' range.
Notes:
These exercises should be a preparation for the written work.
(b) Shopping exercises involving use of component parts of 3d, 6d, and 1s (to
meet shopping needs within the pupils' experience).
Notes:
Highest amount from which change will be required is one shilling. (Only
one operation: 1d. out of 3d.; 4d. out of 6d.; 8d. out of 1s.) (Change
from recognised current coins only.)
(c) Finding half and quarter of numbers and quantities.
Notes:
Introductory exercises in paper-folding. Fractional parts should be based
on multiplication and division tables: 4 times 9 = 36, then of 36 = 9.
'Half' and 'quarter' should be applied to 'Time': half-hour and quarter-hour.
(d) Terms: Pair, couple, dozen, score.
(e) Problems involving not more than two operations.
Grade IV: Oral Arithmetic.
(a) Exercises based on the four simple rules and on the tables taught (limited to
two steps)

438

Notes:
Give abundant practice in such exercises as: 26+17; 43-17; 34+19; 41-16
(b) Fractions: Application of fractions (,1/3,, &c. to 1/12) to numbers, money,
and quantities (multiplies of denominators only)
Notes:
The exercises are intended only as introduction to the study of vulgar
fractions at a later stage. 1/6 of 72; 1/9 of 54; 1/5 of 1 = ?s.; 1/7 of 1s.
2d. = ?d.; lb = ?ozs.; of a gallon = ? pts.
(c) Money: Exercises based on reduction, addition, and subtraction.
Notes:
Reduction - one step - to s.; s. to ; s. to d.; d. to s.; d. to d.; d. to d.;
s. to 6d. or 3d. and vice versa.
Addition: Any two amounts in pence and halfpence not to exceed 1s., e.g.
3d. and 5d. Any three amounts in pence only, not to exceed 2s, e.g.,
2d. + 8d. + 9d. (Answer in s.d.). Any two amounts each composed of an
exact number of shillings, the sum not to exceed 2, e.g. 18s. + 19s.; 27s.
+ 9s. (Answer in and s.)
Subtraction: Exercises based on giving change from sums of money not
exceeding 3s.
(d) Problems involving not more than two operations.
Grade V: Oral Arithmetic.
(a) Exercises based on the four simple rules.
(b) Money: Reduction and four rules.
Notes:
Reduction: One operation with amounts to 1, e.g. 16s.4d. to pence. Two
operations: shillings, pence, and halfpence to halfpence and viced versa;
limit of 4s., e.g. 3s.9d. to halfpence
Addition: Add three amounts expressed in pence and halfpence e.g. 9d.
+ 10d. + 8d. Add two amounts expressed in shillings and pence, e.g.
3s. 6d. + 5s. 6d.; 2s 11d + 8d.
Subtraction: Practical problems in giving change, e.g. Docket, 2s.7d.;
coins offered 2 florins; what change?
Multiplication: Limited to one reduction, e.g. 9d. x 12; 2s. 9d. x 6;
1 7s. x 5.
Division: Sum to be divided not to exceed 2, and not exceed one step in
reduction: 3s.4d. 8; 15s.7d. 5 (No remainders)
(c) Practical applications of tables of weights and measures
Notes: Exercises introductory to written work.
(d) Exercises in finding perimeters of rectangular figures.

439

Notes:
Exercises to be regarded as practical applications of Long measure table.
Dimensions of one denomination only; answer may be expressed in two
denominations.
(e) Fractions: Application of fractions (,1/3,, &c. to 1/12) to numbers, money,
and quantities
Notes:
Exercises are intended as introduction to the study of fractions at a later
stage.
Grade VI: Oral Arithmetic
(a) Exercises preparatory to written work
Notes:
In the initial stages use blackboard freely in setting examples
(b) Exercises involving the use of vulgar fractions. Denominators not to exceed
12.
Notes:
Easy exercises on fractional equivalents up to 12ths. e.g. 3/4 = 1/8;
4/6 = 2/?; 4 = 4 ?/10
Exercises in reduction to lowest terms.
Reduction of improper fractions to mixed numbers and vice versa.
Reduce one number or amount to the fraction of another, e.g. What
fraction is 6 of 8? Reduce 1s.3d. to the fraction of 2s. What fraction of 1
mile is 20 chains?
Addition and subtraction of two fractions - LCD not to exceed 12.
Multiply a proper fraction or a mixed number by whole number to 12.
Multiply two fractions.
Divide proper fractions by a whole number.
Divide whole number by a fraction.
Divide a proper fraction by a proper fraction.
(c) Exercises involving the use of finite decimal fractions.
Notes:
Conversion of vulgar fractions to decimals and vice versa: involving
halves, quarters, fifths, eights, and tenths, e.g. = .5; = .25
Addition and subtraction: Limited to two decimal places, e.g. .2+.3; .8+.5;
.08+.2; .9-.2; 3-.6; 2-.05
Multiplication: Product not to exceed two decimal places, e.g. 7 x .5; .3x.2;
8x1.2
Division: Quotient not to exceed two decimal places, e.g. 34; 68; .25.
Easy decimalization of money and quantities, e.g. 6 8s. = 6.4; 3 tons 16
cwt = 3.8 tons

440

(d) Aliquot parts of 1 and 1s. - both vulgar and decimal fractions.
Notes:
Denominators of vulgar fractions not to exceed 12.
(e) Practical exercises dealing with money, weights and measures, vulgar and
decimal fractions
(f)

Practical application of tables to find: perimeters of squares and rectangles;


Areas of squares and rectangles.
Notes:
Dimensions limited to whole numbers. Reduction limited to one step.

Grade VII: Oral Arithmetic. (Exercises preparatory to written work)


Notes:
Teachers should make use of the blackboard for the more difficult
exercises which cannot be readily visualized without this aid.
(a) Factors and multiplies
(b) Vulgar fractions: Addition, subtraction, multiplication and division (Limited to
two terms; Denominators to 12)
(c) Decimal fractions: (Finite decimals only; limited to thousandths)
Notes:
Addition or subtraction; two terms to hundredths. Multiply or divide by 10,
100, 1000 (by inspection). In other examples of multiplication and division
one of the terms should be limited to one figure and the other to two
figures.
(d) Vulgar and decimal fractions applied to concrete quantities.
(e) Exercises involving everyday commercial and domestic transactions,
including examples bearing on local industries.
Notes:
Exercises should be practical.
(f)

Ratio.

441

Notes:
Give much oral practice in expressing ratios as vulgar fractions and vice
versa.
(g) Simple proportion.
(h) Percentages and their application to Profit and Loss and Simple Interest.
Notes: (See written work):
Express percentages as vulgar and decimal fractions.
Express fractions and percentages.
Express money as percentage of a , and vice versa.
Find percentages of numbers and quantities.
With percentages given, find full values of numbers and quantities.
Find what percentage one number or quantity is of another.
Increase or decrease a number or quantity by a given percentage
Find what percentage numbers or quantities are increased/decreased.
(i)

Mensuration
Notes:
Exercises preparatory to written work.
Area and perimeter of square and rectangle.
Areas and cost of paths and borders.
Areas of walls and costs of painting.
No reverse processes.

Grade VIII: Oral Arithmetic.


Exercises preparatory to the written work in Arithmetic and Mensuration.
Notes:
Teachers should make use of the blackboard for the more difficult
exercises which cannot be readily visualized without this aid.

A.13 1964 Syllabus


Grade 1: Oral exercises to 9.
Notes:
Correlate with number facts, games, songs, number experiences,
projects, &c. Children should be given practice in putting mechanical
exercises into problem form and vice versa. The expression 5 - 3 = ?
might produce he response - I had 5 oranges and I ate 3. How many
oranges have I left?

442

Grade 2: Oral Arithmetic.


Notes:
Teachers should make use of the blackboard for the more difficult
exercises. Jotting should be permitted where children cannot readily
visualize without this aid.
(a) Exercises based on number facts to 18
Notes:
Problems should be limited to two operations.
(b) First extension to 99.
Notes:
Frequent practice should be given in working examples such as:
16+2, 21+4, 60+1, 90+4; 14+5, 24+5, 63+5, 96+3;
18-16, 25-21, 61-60, 94-90; 19-14, 29-24, 68-63, 99-96.
One operation only
(c) Symbols: +,-,=
Notes:
Use extensively in various number situations, e.g. 13-7=, 6+8-7=.
Continue giving practice in putting such mechanical exercises into
problem form (and vice versa).
Grade 3: Oral Arithmetic.
Notes:
Teachers should make use of the blackboard for the more difficult
exercises. Jotting should be permitted where children cannot readily
visualize without this aid.
(a) Extended addition and subtraction to 99.
Notes:
Frequent practice should be given in working examples such as:
Second extension - completing the 10; Third extension - bridging the ten
(b) Easy factors.
Notes:
A knowledge of simple factors follows from an intelligent acquaintance
with the multiplication table, and, when pupils learn a table, exercises in
finding the factors of a product should be given.
(c) Combined multiplication and addition. Division (with remainder)

443

Notes:
These exercises teach processes that arise in actual multiplication and
division sums: 7 x 3 + 2, 23 3; 12 x 4 + 3, 51 4.
(d) Practical activities:
Reading the clock (In divisions of five minutes);
Measuring length (In feet and inches).
Notes:
The necessity for practice for each member of class is stressed.
(e) Exercises on the four simple rules with numbers within the pupils' range.
Notes:
Exercises should be a preparation for written work.
(f)

Shopping exercises involving use of component parts of 3d., 6d., and 1s. (to
meet shopping needs within the pupils' experience)
Notes:
Highest amount from which change will be required is one shilling.

(g) Finding half and quarter of numbers and quantities.


Notes:
Introductory exercises in paper-folding.
Fractional parts should be based on multiplication and division tables: 4
times 9 = 36, then of 36 = 9.
"Half" and "Quarter" should be applied to "time": half-hour and quarterhour.
(h) Problems involving not more than two operations.
Notes:
Mechanical work involved in these problems should be simple.
(i)

Terms:
Pair, couple, dozen, score, half, double, plus, minus, equals.
Pound, pint, minute, hour, day, week.

Grade 4: Oral Arithmetic.


Notes:
Teachers should make use of the blackboard for the more difficult
exercises. Jotting should be permitted where children cannot readily
visualize without this aid.
(a) Extended addition and subtraction.

444

Notes:
Types such as 28+7; 49+6; 35-28; 55-49 need special attention.
(b) Combined multiplication and addition. Division with remainder.
Notes:
These applied tables teach processes that arise in multiplication and
division sums: 8 x 7 + 3, 59 7; 5 x 9 + 6, 51 9.
(c) Easy factors and multiples.
(d) Exercises on the four simple rules on tables taught (limited to two steps)
Notes:
Give practice in such exercises as: 26+17; 34+19; 43-17; 61-16.
(e) Fractions: Application of fractions (, 1/3, , &c. to 1/12) to numbers, money
and quantities (multiples of denominators only).
Notes:
The exercises are intended only as an introduction to the study of vulgar
fractions at a later stage: 1/6 of 72; 1/9 of 54; 1/5 of 1 = ?s.; 1/7 of 1s.2s.
= ?d.; lb. = ?ozs; of a gallon = ?pts.
(f)

Money: Exercises based on addition, subtraction, reduction.


Notes:
Addition: Any three amounts in pence only, not to exceed 2s., e.g.
2d.+8d.+9d. (answer in s.d.)
Any two amounts each composed of an exact number of shillings, the
sum not to exceed 2, e.g. 18s.+19s.; 27s.+9s. (answer in and s.)
Subtraction: Exercises based on giving change from sums of money not
exceeding 3s.
Reduction: One step - Pounds to shillings and reverse; shillings to pence
and reverse; shilling to sixpences and threepences and reverse;

(g) Problems involving not more that two operations.


Notes:
Mechanical work involved in these problems should be simple.
Grade 5: Oral Arithmetic.
Notes:
Teachers should make use of the blackboard for the more difficult
exercises. Jotting should be permitted where children cannot readily
visualize without this aid.
(a) Extended addition and subtraction.

445

(b) Combined multiplication and addition. Division with remainder.


(c) Factors and multiplies to 144.
Notes:
To follow directly from multiplication tables.
(d) Money to 144 pence and 200 shillings.
Notes:
Applied to intermediate numbers (1964 only)
(e) Weights and measures.
Notes:
Exercises in estimating weights and measures and checking estimates by
practical weighing and measuring are required.
(f)

Exercises based on the four simple rules.

(g) Money: Reduction and four rules.


Notes:
Reduction: One operation with amounts to 10s., e.g. 7s.9d. to pence;
2 17s. to shillings.
Addition: Add three amounts expressed in pence and halfpence, e.g.
9d.+10d.+8d.
Add two amounts expressed in shillings and pence, e.g. 3s. 6d. + 5s. 9d.;
2s 11d + 8d.
Subtraction: Practical problems in giving change, e.g. Docket, 2s.7d.;
coins offered 2 florins; what change?
Multiplication: Limited to one reduction, e.g. 9d. x 12; 2s. 9d. x 6;
1 7s. x 5. (1964 only.)
Division: Sum to be divided not to exceed 2, and not exceed one step in
reduction: 1 15s 5; 3s.4d. 8; 15s.6d. 3. (1964 only) (No remainders)
(h) Practical application of tables of weights and measures.
Notes:
Simple exercises in reduction, addition, subtraction. Reduction limited to
one step. Addition and subtraction limited to two denominations.
(i)

Fractions: Application of fractions (, 1/3, , &c. to 1/12) to numbers, money


and quantities.
Notes:
The exercises are intended only as an introduction to the study of
fractions at a later stage.

446

(j)

Mensuration (Square and rectangle):


Notes:
Exercises in finding perimeters. Teach reverse processes. (Exercises to
be regarded as practical applications of Long Measure table. Dimensions
of one denomination only. Limited to whole numbers.)

(k) Problems involving not more than two operations.


Notes:
Mechanical work involved in these problems should be simple.
Grade 6: Oral Arithmetic.
Notes:
Teachers should make use of the blackboard for the more difficult
exercises. Jotting should be permitted where children cannot readily
visualize without this aid.
(a) Extended addition and subtraction.
(b) Combined multiplication and addition. Division with remainder.
(c) Money to 144 pence and 200 shillings.
Notes:
Applied to intermediate numbers.
(d) Weights and measures.
Notes:
Exercises in estimating weights and measures and checking estimates by
practical weighing and measuring are required.
(e) Exercises preparatory to written work.
(f)

Exercises involving the use of vulgar fractions. Denominators not to exceed


12.
Notes:
Easy exercises on fractional equivalents up to 12ths; e.g. 3/4 = ?/8; 4/6 =
2/?; 4 = 4 ?/10.
Exercises in reduction in lowest terms.
Reduction of improper fractions to mixed numbers and vice versa.
Reduce one number or amount to the fraction of another, e.g. What
fraction is 6 of 8? Reduce 1s.3d. to fraction of 2s. What fraction of 1 mile
is 20 chains?
Addition and subtraction of two fractions - LCM not to exceed 12.
Multiply a proper fraction or a mixed number by any whole number to 12.
Multiply two fractions.
Divide a proper fraction by a whole number.

447

Divide a whole number by a fraction.


Divide a proper fraction by a proper fraction.
(g) Factors and multiples.
Notes:
Introduce terms:- Prime number, prime factor, common multiple, lowest
common multiple.
Frequent practice should be given in finding prime factors of numbers to
144, and in building multiples to 144 from two or more numbers.
(h) Squares and square roots.
Notes:
Introduce terms square and square root. Square whole numbers to 12
and reverse.
(i)

Exercises involving the use of finite decimal fractions.


Notes:
Conversion of vulgar fractions to decimals and vice versa: involving
halves, quarters, fifths, eights, and tenths, e.g. = .5; = .25; 3/4=.75;
1/8=.875; 3/5=.6
Addition and subtraction: Limited to two decimal places, e.g. .2+.3; .8+.5;
.08+2; .9-.2; 3-.6; 2-.05
Multiplication: Product not to exceed two decimal places, e.g. 7x.5; .3x.2;
8x1.2
Division: Quotient not to exceed two decimal places, e.g. 3 4; 6 8;
2 5.
Easy decimalization of money and quantities, e.g. 6 8s. = 6.4;
3 tons 16 cwt = 3.8 tons

(j)

Aliquot parts of 1 and 1s. - both vulgar and decimal fractions.


Notes:
Aliquot parts of 1s. Limited to: 6d. = s.; 3d. = s.
Aliquot parts of 1. Limited to: 10s. = ; 5/0d. = ; 4/0d. = 1/5;
2/0d. = 1/10

(k) Practical exercises dealing with money, weights and measures, vulgar and
decimal fractions.
(i)

Mensuration (Square and rectangle).


Notes:
Dimensions limited to whole numbers)
Reduction limited to one step
Find perimeter. Teach reverse process.
Find area. No reverse process.

448

Grade 7: Oral Arithmetic.


Notes:
Teachers should make use of the blackboard for the more difficult
exercises. Jotting should be permitted where children cannot readily
visualize without this aid.
(a) Extended addition and subtraction.
(b) Combined multiplication and division. Division with remainder.
(c) Money to 144 pence and 200 shillings.
Notes:
Applied to intermediate numbers.
(d) Weights and measures.
Notes:
Exercises in estimating weights and measures and checking estimates by
practical weighing and measuring are required.
(e) Exercises preparatory to written work.
(f)

Factors and multiples. (See note for Grade VI.)

(g) Squares and square roots. (See note for Grade VI.)
(h) Vulgar fractions: Addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division.
Notes:
Limited to two termsdenominators not to exceed 12.
(i)

Decimal fractions; (Finite decimals only; limited to thousandths)


Notes:
Addition or subtraction; two terms to hundredths.
Multiply or divide by 10, 100, 1000 (by inspection).
In other examples of multiplication and division one of the terms should be
limited to one figure and the other to two figures.

(j)

Vulgar and decimal fractions applied to concrete quantities.


Notes:
To include simple exercises in multiplication and division of weights and
measures.

(k) Exercises involving everyday commercial and domestic transactions,


including examples bearing on local industries.
Notes:

449

Exercises should be practical.


(l)

Ratio.
Notes:
Give much oral practice in expressing ratios as vulgar fractions and vice
versa.

(m) Simple proportion


(n) Percentages.
Notes:
Express percentages as vulgar and decimal fractions.
Express fractions and percentages.
Express money as percentage of a , and vice versa.
Find percentages of numbers and quantities.
With percentages given, find full values of numbers and quantities.
Find what percentage one number or quantity is of another.
Increase or decrease a number or quantity by a given percentage.
Find by what percentage numbers or quantities are increased or
decreased.
(o) Mensuration.
Notes:
Exercises preparatory to written work.
(p) Problems.
Notes:
Mechanical work...should be simple.

450

APPENDIX B

Additional Notes: Chapter 3


1. Following the 1887 Civil Service Commission's interim report in 1888, The
Brisbane Courier (Editorial, 18 August 1888) suggested that the Department of
Public Instruction was little more that "a euphonious synonym for Messrs.
Anderson and Ewart..., the almighty Siamese twins of the Education
Department". Real authority lay with them rather than with the responsible
minister (Barcan, 1980, p. 184).
2. After a tour of European schools in 1897, Ewart reported that "nothing in the
professional working of the schools struck me as note-worthy compared with the
ordinary conditions of our own schools; but I was somewhat impressed with the
buildings and their equipment" (Second Progress Report, 1889, p. 959).
3. The Department of Public Instruction did not enter the field of secondary
education until 1912. From 1913 the State provided free secondary education
for all who qualified; "a dramatic reversal of the longstanding concept of the
state's responsibility being limited to the provision of primary education" (Lawry,
1972, p. 25).
4. These conferences were effectively forced on the Department by Minister
Barlow who was himself under political pressure, particularly from the
Queensland Teachers' Union: "The conferences continued to press the position,
and assisted by professional opinion outside, the work of revision was forced on
the Department" (St. Ledger, 1905, p. 218).
5. The Sub-committee on Arithmetic recommended two minor changes to the
mental arithmetic requirements: "In Class I.1. - To add and subtract mentally
numbers applied to objects to a result not greater than 10. In Class II.1, 2, 3. Easy practical applications of the Rules." These recommendations constituted
an earlier introduction of mental subtraction and division (see Appendix A), and
were made under a belief that "the...course of instruction in [Arithmetic was]
calculated to give [the] pupils a fair knowledge of all that may be required of
them in the ordinary business of life" (Gripp, Mutch, & McKenna, 1904, p. 20).
6. At a lecture given in 1899, and reprinted in 1915, Dewey stated that "the child
[is] the sun about which the appliances of education revolve; he is the center
about which they are organized" (p. 35).
7. The period 1909 to 1914 was, in Wyeth's (1955, p. 178) view, one of the most
progressive periods for education in Queensland, which, probably not
coincidently, coincided with Ewart's retirement in 1909. The University of
Queensland opened in 1911, State High schools from 1912, and a Teachers'
College in 1914.

451

8. Mrs Beatrice Ensor, founder of the New Education Fellowship, visited Australia
in 1937 to attend conferences of the Fellowship, the first of which was held in
Brisbane.
9. Cramer (Superintendent of Schools, The Dalles, Oregon) toured Australian
schools in 1934 and published his observations in an Australian Council of
Educational Research publication, Australian Schools Through American Eyes
(1936).
10. This belief is reiterated in the General Introduction to the 1930 Syllabus:
Teachers will be expected to adapt the curriculum to levels of growth and to
individual differences, and to organise activities that, while realising the
purposes of the curriculum, will suit local circumstances...Where teachers
probe below the letter to catch the spirit of the Syllabus, we can expect the
most permanent results. (Department of Public Instruction, 1930, p. xi)
11. A copy of Burt's Mental Tests in Arithmetic was not found during the research
for this study.
12. The Committee of Seven, under the Direction of Carleton Washburne
(Superintendent of Schools, Winnetka, Illinois), set out to determine the stage of
mental development at which specific mathematics topics could be most
effectively mastered. Its report was published in the 29th Year Book of the
National Society for the Study of Education (1932).
13. Prior to the 1952 syllabus, times for each subject were not specified. Arithmetic
was considered by many to be "a subject that [would] poach time from every
other subject in the syllabus" (Darling Downs Branch, 1946, p. 21), particularly
in the scholarship class. Cunningham and Price (1934, p. 61) found that
Queensland teachers of Grades II to VII devoted an average of approximately 5
hr per week on Arithmetic, compared to 4hr 23min in South Australian schools,
for example. By 1949 the average time spent in Queensland was 5hr per
week (ACER, 1949, p. 12).
14. In the General Introduction to the 1930 Syllabus it was stated that:
It is generally agreed that the Primary School - the only kind of school some
children will know - should teach its pupils to speak, to read, and to write;
introduce them to what men think and do, and to what men have thought
and done; enable them to gain some general ideas of the natural world;
give them practice in the art of calculation; train them in habits of
observation; develop their manual dexterity; introduce them to the beauty of
form, colour, and sound; and improve their physique - in short, it should
develop a sound mind in a sound body. (Department of Public Instruction,
1930, p. v; 1952a, p.1)
15. Five hours per week for mathematics was suggested as an appropriate time
allocation for Grades III to VII in the General Memorandum issued to schools

452

concerning the 1948 Amendments (Department of Public Instruction, 1948b, p.


17). Advocating a further reduction on the time spent on mathematics, Primary
Education: A Report of the Advisory Council on Education in Scotland
suggested that 5 periods of 40 minutes (3 hours 20 minutes) per week were
ample (cited by Greenhalgh, 1949a, p. 11).
16. An earlier version was a text recommended for pupil-teachers in The State
Education Act of 1875 Together With the Regulations of the Department and
General Instructions for the Guidance of Teachers and Others. (List of Books,
1880). Brisbane: Government Printer.
17. The committee, under District Inspector Skelton's chairmanship, constituted to
make recommendations for the revision of the 1904 syllabus, considered the
oral statement of processes of such importance that it recommended that the
following be inserted in the syllabus for the First Class: "Attention is directed to
the following extract from the preface of the Syllabus: `Pupils should be made
familiar by mental exercises with the principles underlying every process before
the written work is undertaken'". However, this recommendation was not
accepted by the District Inspectors responsible for writing the 1914 Syllabus. It
was deemed unnecessary as the matter would be dealt with in the explanatory
memorandum. (Skelton, 1912, p. 1).
18. Process, in this historical context, refers to an arithmetical procedure or
algorithm (e.g. for addition); a much narrower definition than that used in the
Years 1 to 10 Mathematics Syllabus in which a process is defined as a "[way] of
operating, both cognitively and physically, with or on mathematical concepts"
(Department of Education, 1987a, p.10)
19. Gladman's School Method, Park's Manual of Method and Joyce's School
Management were three of the texts listed in The State Education Act of 1875.
Together With the Regulations of the Department and General Instructions for
the Guidance of Teachers and Others (List of Books, 1880, p. 24). It was
noted for head teachers that "this list of authorised books is chiefly intended to
show what books teachers are empowered to place, when necessary, in the
hands of...pupil-teachers" (p. 23). Gladman's School Work was included as one
of the recommended texts for pupil-teachers in List II of the 1895 publication of
the State Education Act, as was Robinson's Teacher's Manual of Method and
Organisation.
20. The Practical Mental Arithmetic, written by "An Inspector of Schools" (1914).
An earlier impression of this book was the mental arithmetic text "supplied to
schools for the instruction of pupils" (Department of Public Instruction, 1902, p.
95).
21. To find the cost of:
(a) a dozen articles. Calculate the given cost of one article in pence, and call
the pence shillings: 1 dozen @ 1s. 4d. = 16s.

453

(b) a score of articles. Calculate the given cost of one article in shillings, and
7 = 22. 11s. 8d.
call the shillings pounds: 1 score @ 1. 2s. 7d. = 22 12
(c)

a gross of articles. As a gross is a dozen times a dozen, the cost of 1 gross


@ 1s. 4d. = cost of 12 at 16s. = 9. 12s.
(Pendlebury & Beard, 1899, p.174)

22. (a) Compound rules involved the addition, subtraction, multiplication or division
of compound quantities; that is, of quantities that are expressed in terms of
different denominations: e.g. 2s. 6d. x 4. (Pendlebury & Beard, 1922, p. 29)
(b) Reduction was the process of converting a quantity from one denomination
to another: e.g. inches to feet or vice versa. (Pendlebury & Beard, 1922, p.
27)
(c) "Practice [was] a method of calculating by the addition of aliquot parts' the
value of a simple or compound quantity, when the value of a unit of one
denomination [was] given. The method [was] simple' or compound',
according as the quantity considered [was] simple or compound".
(Pendlebury & Beard, 1922, p. 105)
For example: (i) to calculate the cost of 18 articles at 10s. each, using
simple practice, knowing that 10s. is one-half of one pound, the cost of the
18 articles would be of 18, which is 9; (ii) to calculate the cost of 60
yards of material at 5d. per yard, find the cost if it was 1d. per yard (5s.) and
multiply this result by 5 (0 5s. 0d. x 5 = 1 5s. 0d.).
23. The unitary method for manipulating proportions involved finding the cost of one
unit from given information from which the value of the required number of units
could be calculated. For example, to calculate the cost of 7 yards of material,
knowing that 5 yards cost 15s: The cost of 1 yard is 3s. Therefore, the cost of 7
yards is 21s. or 1 1s.
24. While this advice was directed at the situation in England, it may have been
relevant to Queensland, given that the Report of the Secretary of Public
Instruction for 1898 indicated that the number of Sixth Class children in
Queensland was 1,668 (Dalrymple, 1899, p. 5).
25. The changes to the written work contained in the 1930 Syllabus, for children of
comparable ages given the changes in class structure, included:
Grade I: "Simple introductory exercises in division" rather than working with
divisors to 6.
Grade II: Divisors limited to one-digit numbers rather than to 100.
Grade III: Four rules and reduction of money with sums limited to 20 rather than
100.
Grade VI: Complex fractions, previously studied during the second term of Fifth
Class were removed from syllabus.
("New Syllabus, 1929, pp. 460-462)

454

26. In association with the implementation of the 1938 Amendments, the


Preparatory Grade was extended to two years, with Preparatory 1 and 2
occupying one year and Preparatory 3 and 4 each occupying six months. The
work for Preparatory 1 and 2 was presented as a combined syllabus in the 1938
Amendments and separately in the 1948 Amendments.
27. To calculate the cost of:
(a) 240 articles. Change the price of one article to pence and call these
pounds. The cost of 240 at 1s. 5d. = 17.
(b) 480 articles: Change the price of one article to half-pence and call these
pounds. 480 articles at 3s. 2d. = 480 at 76 half-pence each = 76.
(c) 960 articles: Change the price of one to farthings and call these pounds.
960 articles at 6d. each = 960 at 25 farthings each = 25.
(Mental Arithmetic: Book II, 1926, p. 7)
28. An acute shortage of teachers resulted in this grade being omitted from January
1953 (Education Office Gazette, 1952, p. 251, cited by Dagg, 1971, p. 35).
29. The first explicit direction concerning the limit of written addition in First Class
was given in 1879: "To add three numbers of three figures" (Department of
Public Instruction, 1879, p. 14). With the increase in the length of the First
Class from 1 to 2 years in 1892, the limit was extended to: "To add six
numbers of five figures on slates" ("Para 143, 1891, p. 23). In the 1904
Syllabus, addition was limited to sums to 200, but was extended to 999 in 1930.
30. The percentage of time spent on oral/mental work included work done during
time tabled periods in arithmetic, homework, and in incidental and indirect
instruction in other subjects (ACER, 1949, p. 10).
31. Under the 1860 Syllabus, mental arithmetic was to be conducted on Thursdays
between 3:15pm and 4:00pm for girls, and for boys on Mondays, Wednesdays
and Fridays between 2:30pm and 3:00pm ("Board of General Education, 1866,
p. 5).
32. The sums on the Departmental arithmetic cards were arranged, in sequence,
into specific rules and types for each half-year, which tended "to divide the
teaching of Arithmetic into watertight compartments" (Somers, 1928, p. 84).
33. Edwards (1931) reported, with apparent satisfaction, that the costs for the
meetings organised by District Inspectors were "borne by the teachers
themselves" (p. 28).
34. District Inspector Ross (1884) concluded that pupil-teachers "as pupils...[were]
burdened with lessons and exercises of preposterous length; and as
teachers...[were] left to blunder through their wearisome duties with little of
either direction or encouragement" (p. 68).

455

35. In 1909 the minimum number of children in attendance required for


classification as a State School was reduced from 30 to 12. This created 1059
State Schools with only 79 classified as Provisional (Logan & Clarke, 1984, p.
3).
36. In 1899, 11 pupil-teachers at the Central Brisbane State School taught classes
from 28 to 76 pupils (Lawry, 1968, p. 607).
37. During the 1930s, in excess of 70% of schools were one-teacher schools
(Edwards, 1930; p. 27; 1937a, p. 23; "Instructions to Inspectors, 1935, p. 7).
38. The following items are from the Grade III text in the seriesBrooks's
Queensland School Seriesmost commonly used by teachers in the late 1940s
(ACER, 1949, Appendix 1, p. 3):
(a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
(e)

(13 x 8) - (14 x 7)
12 x AB = 204. Find the missing figures A and B.
(32 - 17 + 15) 15
The dividend is 288 and the divisor is 12. What is the quotient?
A man had four bags. The first held 19 pennies and each of the others 15
pennies. How many pennies were there in the four bags?
(New Syllabus Mental Arithmetic for Third Grade, 1932, p. 5)

39. A copy of Pitman's Mental and Intelligence Tests in Common-Sense Arithmetic


was not found during the research for this study.
40. The books sanctioned for arithmetic in the 1860 Regulations were listed simply
as: (a) Arithmetic, (b) Arithmetic in Theory and Practice, and (c) Set Tablet
Lessons, Arithmetic, 60 sheets ("Regulations, 1860, p. 7).
41. "May, Nellie and Joan were three sisters, all of whom had been promised a new
beret by their Aunt. Last Thursday she took them to town to buy the berets.
The ones they liked were 1/6 each, so their Aunt allowed them to have the ones
they chose. When paying for them she found that she had to give a ten-shilling
note. Of course, they had to wait for change; how much was it?" ("Grade III
Mental Arithmetic, 1936, p. 14).

456

APPENDIX C

Self-Completion Questionnaire
COVER PAGE
Focus:

The focus for this survey is Mental Computation (Mental/Oral Arithmetic).


Mental computation is considered to be the calculation of exact answers
mentally. Of particular interest is calculating beyond the basic facts. For the
purposes of this study, Mental Computation needs to be considered as
something distinct from Computational Estimation.

Purpose:

This survey seeks to gain an understanding of the beliefs about Mental


Computation held by Queensland State primary school teachers and
administrators. It also aims to gather information about teaching practices.
The data gathered will supplement that gained from archival material to
provide a history of mental computation in Queensland State Primary Schools
since 1859.

Instructions: Please do not put your name or that of your school on the questionnaire as
there is no valid use for this information in the analysis and reporting of the
data supplied.
Sections 1, 2 and 3 of the survey contain statements about Mental
Computation to which you are asked to respond. Section 4 obtains data about
you, your school, and inservice with respect to Mental Computation.
Section 1 is for all respondents. Please circle a number to show the extent to
which you Agree or Disagree with each of the statements.
Section 2 is for those currently responsible for a particular class. Please circle
a number to show the frequency over a week that you use each of the
teaching techniques. Space is also provided for you to list resources that you
have found useful.
Section 3 is for those who had the responsibility of a class at anytime in the
period 1964 - 1987. Please circle a number to show how important you
considered mental computation to be, and how frequently each of the listed
teaching techniques was used. Space is also provided for you to list
resources that you have found useful during this period.
Section 4 is for all respondents. Please tick the boxes that are appropriate for
you and your school.
On completion, please return this questionnaire to the contact person in your
school. This will enable all questionnaires from your school to be returned in
the stamped, addressed packet which has been sent to your school's contact
person.
Thanking you
Geoff Morgan
Lawnton State School

457

SECTION 1
BELIEFS ABOUT MENTAL COMPUTATION AND HOW IT SHOULD BE TAUGHT
Circle a number on each line to show the extent to which you Agree or Disagree with
each statement. Please remember that this part of the survey is interested in the
mental calculation of exact answers beyond the basic facts.

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

8.

9.

10.

11.

Strongly
Disagree

Disagree

The development of the ability to calculate


exact answers mentally is a legitimate goal of
mathematics education.

The Years 1-10 Mathematics Syllabus places


little importance on the development of the
ability to calculate exact answers mentally.

Written methods of calculating exact


answers are superior to mental procedures.

Mental computation encourages children to


devise ingenious computational short cuts.

Calculating exact answers mentally involves


applying rules by rote.

Emphasis on written algorithms needs to be


delayed so that mental computation can be
given increased attention.

Teachers need to be aware of the strategies


used by those who are proficient at
calculating exact answers mentally.

Mental computation helps children gain an


understanding of the relationships between
numbers.
The ability to calculate exact answers with
paper-and-pencil is more useful outside the
classroom than the ability to calculate
mentally.
Children should use the algorithms for written
computation when calculating exact answers
mentally.
Children who are proficient at mentally
calculating exact answers use personal
adaptations of written algorithms and
idiosyncratic mental strategies.

Agree

Strongly
Agree

458
SECTION 1 (BELIEFS) cont.

Strongly
Disagree

12.

13.

14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

22.

23.

Disagree

Agree

Strongly
Agree

Strategies for calculating exact answers


mentally are best developed through discussion
and explanation.

Children need to be allowed to develop and


use their own strategies for calculating
exact answers mentally.

Children's mental processes are sharpened by


starting a mathematics lesson with ten quick
questions to be solved mentally.

Children are encouraged to think about


mathematics right from the start of a lesson
when given ten quick questions to solve mentally.

Answers obtained for the "ten quick questions"


need to be corrected quickly so that the
mathematics lesson can begin.

Opportunities for children to calculate exact


answers mentally need to be provided in all
relevant classroom activities.

Children should be given opportunities to


discuss, compare and refine their mental
strategies for solving particular mental problems.

During mental computation sessions, the focus


should be on the correctness of the answer
rather than on the mental strategies used.

Children should be encouraged to build on the


thinking strategies used to develop the basic
facts.

A series of sessions that focuses on developing


strategies for computing exact answers mentally
needs to be conducted each week.

Strategies for calculating exact answers


mentally need to be specifically taught.

During mental computation sessions, one


approach to each of a number of problems
should be the focus, rather than on several
approaches to each of a few problems.

Please continue to Section 2 over page.

459

SECTION 2
CURRENT TEACHING PRACTICES

If you're not a class teacher at this time, please go to Section 3 on Page 5.


SECTION 2.1
Please circle a number on each line to show how frequently you use each teaching technique to develop the
ability to mentally calculate exact answers beyond the basic facts.

Never

24.

25.

26.

27.

28.

29.

30.

31.

32.

33.

34.

35.

Focus specifically on developing the ability


to calculate exact answers mentally beyond
the basic facts.

Seldom Sometimes Often

Allow children to decide the method to be


used to arrive at an exact answer mentally.

Allow children to explain and discuss their


mental strategies for solving a problem.

Allow children to work mentally during


practice of written computation.

Teach particular mental strategies and


follow up with practice examples.

Give several one-step questions and simply


mark answers as correct or incorrect.

Require answers to problems solved mentally


to be recorded on paper.

Use mental computation for revising and


practising arithmetic facts and procedures.

Emphasise speed when calculating exact


answers mentally.

Insist on children using the procedures for


the written algorithms when calculating
exact answers mentally.

Provide opportunities for children to


appreciate how often they and adults use
mental computation.

Relate methods for calculating beyond the


basic facts to the thinking strategies used
to develop the basic facts.

460
SECTION 2 (CURRENT PRACTICES) cont.

Never

36.

37.

38.

Seldom Sometimes Often

Teach rules for calculating exact answers


mentally (e.g. divide by ten by removing a zero)

Have children commit to memory number facts


beyond the basic facts.

Use examples involving measures or spatial


concepts in problems to be calculated mentally.

SECTION 2.2
Resources:
Please list any resources (e.g. texts) currently used for developing the ability to
calculate exact answers mentally.

Comments:
Please write any comments you may care to make:

Please continue to Section 3 over page.

461

SECTION 3
PAST TEACHING PRACTICES
If you were not a class teacher at any time during the period 1964 - 1987, please go to
Section 4 on Page 8.
SECTION 3.1
If you taught during a particular period, please circle a number to show the level of importance placed on mental
computation during each period. If you are unsure, please place a tick on the line provided.

No
Little
Some
Great
Importance Importance Importance Importance Unsure

39.

40.

For each period, how important


did the syllabus consider the ability
to calculate exact answers mentally
to be?
(a) 1964 - 1968 (1964 Syllabus)

___

(b) 1969 - 1974 (PIM: 1st ed)

___

(c) 1975 - 1987 (PIM: 2nd ed)

___

(a) 1964 - 1968

___

(b) 1969 - 1974

___

(c) 1975 - 1987

___

For each period, how important


did you consider the ability to
calculate exact answers mentally
to be?

SECTION 3.2
If you taught during a particular period, please circle a number on each line to show how frequently you used each
teaching technique to develop the ability to mentally calculate exact answers beyond the basic facts. If you are
unsure, please place a tick on the line provided.

Never

41.

Seldom

Sometimes Often

Unsure

Allowed children to decide the method to be


used to arrive at an exact answer mentally.
(a) 1964 - 1968

___

(b) 1969 - 1974

___

(c) 1975 - 1987

___

462
SECTION 3 (PAST PRACTICES) cont.

Never

42.

43.

44.

45.

46.

Seldom

Sometimes

Often

Unsure

Allowed children to explain and discuss their


mental strategies for solving a problem:
(a) 1964 - 1968

___

(b) 1969 - 1974

___

(c) 1975 - 1987

___

(a) 1964 - 1968

___

(b) 1969 - 1974

___

(c) 1975 - 1987

___

(a) 1964 - 1968

___

(b) 1969 - 1974

___

(c) 1975 - 1987

___

(a) 1964 - 1968

___

(b) 1969 - 1974

___

(c) 1975 - 1987

___

(a) 1964 - 1968

___

(b) 1969 - 1974

___

(c) 1975 - 1987

___

Gave several one-step questions and simply


marked the answers as correct or incorrect:

Emphasised speed when calculating exact


answers mentally.

Insisted that children use the procedures


for the written algorithms when calculating
exact answers mentally.

Placed an emphasis on teaching rules for


calculating exact answers mentally
(e.g. divide by ten by removing a zero).

463
SECTION 3 (PAST PRACTICES) cont.

SECTION 3.3
Resources:
Please list any resources (e.g. texts) that were used to develop the ability to
calculate exact answers mentally. If any were used for a limited period only,
please indicate.

Comments:
Please write below any comments you may care to make:

Please continue to Section 4 over page.

464

SECTION 4
BACKGROUND INFORMATION

For each question, please place a tick () in the appropriate box.

47. In which Educational Region is your school located?


Sunshine Coast

South West

North West

Metropolitan West

Wide Bay

Peninsula

Metropolitan East Darling Downs

Capricornia
Northern

South Coast

48. What is the size of your school?


Band:

49.

4
5
6
7
8
9
10

How many years teaching experience have you had?


< 1 yr

1-5 yrs

6-10 yrs 11-15 yrs

16-20 yrs 21-25 yrs 26-30 yrs 30+ yrs

50. Are you a: Class Teacher

Principal

Teaching Principal?

Deputy Principal?

(Go to next question)

(Go to Question 53)

51. If a class teacher, which year level/s are you currently teaching?
1
2
3
4
5
6
7 None


52. Is your school a trial school for Student Performance Standards in
mathematics?
Yes

No

(If NO, go to Question 55)

465
SECTION 4 (BACKGROUND INFORMATION) cont.

53. If your school is a trial school and you are currently a class teacher,
are you trialing the standards in your classroom?
Yes

No

54. Do you consider it important that inservice sessions on mental Computation


be made available to teachers?
Yes

No

55. Have you attended, during the last three years, inservice sessions in which
Mental Computation was a specific topic for discussion?
Yes

No

(If NO, skip the next question)

56. If you have attended inservice on Mental Computation, who conducted the
inservice?
Teaching colleague

Tertiary Lecturer

Administrator

Mathematics Adviser

Other

If Other, please specify:

Comments:
Please write any further comments you may care to make:

THANK YOU for completing this survey. Please return it, in the envelope provided, to the
contact person in your school.

466

APPENDIX D

SURVEY CORRESPONDENCE
D.1

Initial Letter: One-teacher Schools

Dear Colleague
As detailed in the attached "Memorandum to Principals" from the Executive
Director, Review and Evaluation, approval has been granted for me (Geoff
Morgan, Deputy Principal, Lawnton State School) to approach you to invite
your participation in a survey of beliefs and teaching practices related to
Mental Computation (Mental Arithmetic). Your school has been randomly
selected as part of a sample of 115 State Primary Schools from all Education
Regions and bands of schools.
The attached Research Outline indicates that the survey is designed as a
culmination to historical research currently being undertaken. The aim of the
project is to document the nature and place of Mental Computation in
Queensland primary classrooms from 1860 to the present. The research is
part of a doctoral program supervised by Dr Calvin Irons of the Queensland
University of Technology's School of Mathematics, Science and Technology.
Rather than simply send a copy of the questionnaire to you in this mailing,
the purpose of this letter is to determine whether you would be prepared to
participate in the survey. The questionnaire should take about 15-20 minutes
to complete, with not all respondents having to complete all sections. The
information to be gathered concerns: Current beliefs about Mental
Computation and how it should be taught; Present teaching practices; and
Past teaching practices related to the 1964 Syllabus, and to the 1966-68 and
1975 "Programs in Mathematics". I am also interested in identifying the
resources used by teachers to support the development of Mental
Computation skills.
While some personal background information is asked for, all information
obtained will be treated confidentially. In fact, you are asked not to identify
yourself or your school on the questionnaire itself as this information does not
have any valid use during data analysis. Some questions relate to recent
inservice opportunities with regard to Mental Computation. Besides being
able to document the status of Mental Computation, it is also hoped to be
able to provide recommendations to the Department on any inservice needs
which may be identified.
/2

467

Your participation in this survey will be greatly appreciated.


Please tear off the form below and return in the envelope provided,
preferably by Monday, 18 October. A questionnaire will then be dispatched
should a preparedness to be involved be expressed.
Thanking you,
Yours faithfully

MENTAL COMPUTATION SURVEY

Please complete the appropriate section below and return in the envelope
provided (by Monday, 18 October) to:
Geoff Morgan
Lawnton State School
P.O. Box 2
LAWNTON 4501

Phone:
Fax:

(07) 285 2968


(07) 285 6506

Please SEND the questionnaire on Mental Computation.

Name of school: _____________________ State School

Your name (optional): _____________________________________


(For addressing envelope to send questionnaire.)

OR

Please DO NOT SEND the questionnaire on Mental Computation.

468

D.2

Initial Letter: to All Schools Except One-teacher Schools

Dear Colleague
As detailed in the attached "Memorandum to Principals" from the Executive
Director, Review and Evaluation, approval has been granted for me (Geoff
Morgan, Deputy Principal, Lawnton State School) to approach you to invite
your participation, and that of your staff, in a survey of beliefs and teaching
practices related to Mental Computation (Mental Arithmetic). Your school has
been randomly selected as part of a sample of 115 State Primary Schools
from all Education Regions and bands of schools.
The attached Research Outline indicates that the survey is designed as a
culmination to historical research currently being undertaken. The aim of the
project is to document the nature and place of Mental Computation in
Queensland primary classrooms from 1860 to the present. The research is
part of a doctoral program supervised by Dr Calvin Irons of the Queensland
University of Technology's School of Mathematics, Science and Technology.
Rather than simply send copies of the questionnaire to you in this mailing,
the purpose of this letter is to determine whether you and/or members of your
staff would be prepared to participate in the survey. The questionnaire should
take about 15-20 minutes to complete, with not all respondents having to
complete all sections. The information to be gathered concerns: Current
beliefs about Mental Computation and how it should be taught; Present
teaching practices; and Past teaching practices related to the 1964 Syllabus,
and to the 1966-68 and 1975 "Programs in Mathematics". I am also
interested in identifying the resources used by teachers to support the
development of Mental Computation skills.
While some personal background information is asked for, all information
obtained will be treated confidentially. In fact, respondents are asked not to
identify themselves or their school on the questionnaire as this information
does not have any valid use during data analysis. Some questions relate to
recent inservice opportunities with regard to Mental Computation. Besides
being able to document the status of Mental Computation, it is also hoped to
be able to provide recommendations to the Department on any inservice
needs which may be identified.

/2

469

Your school's participation in this survey will be greatly appreciated. To


facilitate the distribution of the questionnaire to staff members, I am
wondering if you (or one of your staff) would be prepared to act as Contact
Person for the receipt of the questionnaires, their distribution to other staff
members, and their return after completion for which a postage-paid
envelope will be provided.
It is hoped that at least one teacher from each year level, as well as
school administrators, will be prepared to complete the questionnaire.
Could the form attached please be returned in the envelope provided,
preferably by Monday, 18 October. Questionnaires will then be dispatched
should a preparedness to be involved be expressed.
Thanking you,
Yours faithfully

470

MENTAL COMPUTATION SURVEY

Please complete the appropriate section below and return in the envelope
provided (by Monday, 18 October) to:
Geoff Morgan
Lawnton State School
P.O. Box 2
LAWNTON 4501

Phone:
Fax:

(07) 285 2968


(07) 285 6506

Please SEND the questionnaire on Mental Computation.

(a) Name of School: ______________________________ State School

(b) Name of Contact Person: _________________________________

(c) Position in school: ______________________________________

(d) Number of Questionnaires required:

_____

(e) Information to assist data analysis:


Number of class teachers on staff:

_____

Number of Deputy Principals:

_____

OR
Please DO NOT SEND the questionnaire on Mental
Computation.

471

D.3

Letter to Contact Persons Accompanying Questionnaires

Dear < Contact Person/Principal >


Thank you for your offer to act as Contact Person for the survey on Mental
Computation. As requested, I have enclosed < number > copies of the
questionnaire. Also enclosed is a Reply Paid envelope in which the
completed questionnaires may be returned.
Hopefully the survey will not take too long to complete. For those who have
been teaching since the mid-1960s, to which all sections may apply, it is
anticipated that it should take around 15-20 minutes of their time.
As indicated in my original letter, all information received will be treated
confidentially. It will constitute a valuable contribution to the knowledge about
the teaching of Mental Computation in Queensland during the last quarter
century.
Your assistance, and that of the other members of your school's staff who
complete the survey, is greatly appreciated. Could the questionnaires please
be returned by Friday, < Date > November.
Thanking you

Yours sincerely

472

D.4

Initial Follow-up Letter to Principals of Schools Not Replying to


Original Letter

Dear Colleague
I am writing to you with respect to the survey of Queensland state primary
school teachers and administrators on Mental Computation that I am
currently undertaking, to which documentation sent earlier this term referred.
A couple of schools have indicated that they wish to participate in the survey,
but have mislaid my initial letter. On the off-chance that your school may be
in a similar situation, I have enclosed 6 copies of the questionnaire. If you, or
any of your staff members, are prepared to complete the questionnaire it
would be greatly appreciated. I have enclosed a Reply Paid envelope in
which any completed questionnaires may be returned.
Hopefully the survey will not take too long to complete. For those who have
been teaching since the mid-1960s, to which all sections may apply, it is
anticipated that it should take around 15-20 minutes of their time.
As indicated in my original letter, all information received will be treated
confidentially. It will constitute a valuable contribution to the knowledge about
the teaching of Mental Computation in Queensland during the last quartercentury.
Once again, your assistance, and that of the other members of your school's
staff who complete the survey, is greatly appreciated. Could any completed
questionnaires please be returned by Friday, 26 November.

Thanking you,
Yours faithfully
Geoff Morgan

P.S.

Should you have already posted the form, which was included with
my previous letter, indicating that your school did not want to receive
any questionnaires, please disregard this mailing.

473

D.5

Second Follow-up Letter to Schools Requesting Questionnaires


From Which Completed Forms Had Not Been Received

Dear < Contact Person/Principal >


With this school term fast drawing to a close, I'm writing to once again
express my thanks to you for agreeing to act as contact person for the
mental computation survey which I'm currently undertaking. Could you please
pass on my thanks to those at your school who completed the questionnaire.
A preliminary look at the information so far received suggests that it will prove
very useful in mapping the status of mental computation in Queensland
primary classrooms.
I hope to begin analysing the data during the forthcoming vacation. To this
end, it would be appreciated if the questionnaires that have been completed
could be sent to me at your earliest convenience. Recognising how busy we
all have been during this term, any that have been completed will be
gratefully accepted.
Thanks once again for your assistance in conducting this survey. Wishing
you an enjoyable vacation.

Yours sincerely
Geoff Morgan

474

APPENDIX E

MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR SURVEY


ITEMS IN FIGURES 4.1 - 4.6
Figure 4.1: Beliefs about the nature
of mental computation.

Item
3a
4
5a
6
8a
9

Mean
2.89
3.17
2.63
3.17
2.89
3.24

Std Dev.
.72
.56
.74
.59
.65
.55

Figure 4.2: Beliefs about the general


approach to teaching mental
computation.
Item
10
13
14

Mean
2.32
3.18
3.24

Std Dev.
.67
.50
.55

Figure 4.3: Beliefs about specific


issues associated with developing
mental computation skills.
Item
11
15a
16a
17a
19
20a
21a
22

Mean
3.31
2.16
2.26
2.45
3.50
3.06
2.92
3.37

Std Dev.

.55
.68
.65
.76
.50
.66
.70
.51

Figure 4.4: Selected teaching


practices of middle- and upper-school
teachers.
Item
25
26
29a
30a
31
33a
34a
36a

Mean
3.55
3.50
2.30
1.79
3.32
2.13
3.18
1.60

Std Dev.

.55
.60
.75
.61
.61
.70
.77
.69

Figure 4.5: Teaching practices used


during periods 1964-1968, 1969-1974,
and 1975-1987.
Item
41a
42a
43aa
44aa
45aa
46aa

Mean
2.78
2.78
1.64
1.58
2.62
1.47

Std Dev.

41b
42b
43ba
44ba
45ba
46ba

3.14
3.09
1.81
1.64
2.65
1.53

.76
.81

.73
.65
.88
.55

41c
42c
43ca
44ca
45ca
46ca

3.31
3.27
1.89
1.86
2.69
1.73

.72
.77
.70
.72
.95
.66

1.08
1.02
.87
.80
1.05
.62

475

Figure 4.6: Selected teaching practices and beliefs of middle- and upperschool teachers.
Current Teaching Practices
Item

Mean

Std Dev.

25

3.55
3.50

26
29

31
34

36

Note.

Current Beliefs
Item

Mean

Std Dev.

.55

14

3.27

.52

.60

19

3.46

.50

2.98

.63

2.30

.75

20

3.32

.61

22

3.18
1.60

.77
.69

3.26

.50

2.87

.67

2.63

.70

Items representing traditional beliefs and teaching practices recoded to


reflect a nontraditional orientation to facilitate placement of item means on
traditional-nontraditional continua.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai