Anda di halaman 1dari 12

InformationProcessing& Management,Vol.31, No. 4. lap.479-490.

1995
Copyright 1995ElsevierScienceLtd
Printedin GreatBritain.All rightsreserved
0306-4573/95$9.50+0,00

Pergamon

0306-4573(95)00010-0

TRENDS

IN...

A CRITICAL

REVIEW

THE IMPACT OF INFORMATION--II.


INFORMATION

CONCEPTS

OF

AND ITS VALUE

MICHEL J. MENOU
CIDEGI, 129 Av. P. Vaillant-Couturier,F-94250, Gentilly,France
(Received September 1994; accepted in final form January 1995)

Abstract--A renewal of interest for the theory of information seems to emerge from a series
of recent publications. A number of them advocate a shift toward a cognitive perspective. The
preliminary investigation of the concept of impact of information and the research questions
raised by its assessment suggest that the most significant impact may be found in the
transformation of knowledge structures at the deep paradigmatic level as a result of
information-as-contents. This leads to propose a revised formulation of Brookes' Fundamental equation and possible approaches for describing the attributes of the beneficiaries and
their knowledge structure.

INTRODUCTION
The previous article, "The impact of information--I. Toward a research agenda for its definition
and measurement" described the project "Impact of information on development" of the
International Development Research Centre (IDRC) and reviewed the research issues involved
in the assessment of the impact of information, further to the first report of the project (Menou,
1995). It stressed that among the different states of information, information-as-contents might
be the most critical. We now expand these considerations by revisiting the theories of
information and suggesting a model for the assessment of information-as-contents, which was
first outlined in an earlier paper (Menou, 1995).

THE NEED FOR A THEORY


The question of the impact of information inevitably brings to the fore the lasting issue of a
theory of information. Recent literature seems to evidence a renewal of interest for this question.
Dervin and Nilan (1986) reviewed the major tensions and stressed the role of "a definitional
conceptualization" as "clear premises for focusing on variables and generating research
questions" (p. 5).
Many efforts are directed at refining Shannon and Weaver's theory (e.g. Heilprin, 1989; Cole,
1993) and incorporating into it some additional parameters which, by way of simplification, may
be coined as "intelligence factors". One of the latest examples is Hayes' introduction of a
syntactic measure (Hayes, 1993). But these models are mostly geared to the analysis of discrete
transactions, either single or iterative, of single messages exchanged between a single pair of
sender and recipient. The engineering perspective which commands this form of information
theory, prevents the required distinction between "the concepts of information itself and amount
of information" as Langlois (1982, p. 395) puts it.
Even in the most simple situation, any human being, or adaptive system, is in fact exposed
to, or using, a plurality of sources and channels conveying a plurality of messages, e.g. checking
479

480

MichelJ. Menou
EX RNAL

INFORMATION I

RESOURCES,,,,,,I

I PItOBI.I~
IDENTIFICATION

E /Jt S

!
PROBLEM
FORaMULATION

PERSONALITYI

CULTURE

I FORMULATION
SOLUTION

INTERNAL
KNOWLEDGE

_1

EMOTION ]

BASE

LOGIC

CHOICE OF
ACTION

INTELLIGENCE

ACTION

Fig. 1. Informationuse,externalitiesand internalities.


the time at one's watch, the car's clock and from the radio; assuming they do not give exactly
the same time, which is quite likely, how to decide which is the correct one, and how much does
it matter, depending on the nature of one's next commitment, depending also on individual and
social concepts of punctuality? I have tentatively depicted the real information use situations in
the model presented in Fig. 1. It tries to show in first place that the steps in the problem-solving
process primarily rely upon the internal knowledge base (individual or collective) to which
external resources have to be combined. The internal knowledge base, in second place, operates
under the influence of a series of factors, or motors if we wish to use an artificial intelligence
metaphor, identified for the time being as personality, culture, emotion, logic (reasoning process)
and intelligence (quality of judgement). The latter also continuously interact one with the other.
The distinction between logic and intelligence may find a basis in Brookes' observation
(Brookes, 1980a, p. 252) that "Problems can be solved by objective calculation or by making a
judgement .... As sound judgements are of great social importance, it surprises me that so much
effort is devoted to the teaching of calculation and so little to the teaching of judgement". I was
pleased to discover, retrospectively, that the motors identified in this model are quite close to the

Concepts of informationand its value

481

attributes Brookes had mentioned for what he called the "meta-cognitive basis".
Other attractive metaphors about information are mentioned or tried from time to time. One
of them stems from the many analogies between information and energy, of which Stonier
(1991) recently reminded us. Otten and Debons (1970) also noted that "information, like,
energy, can be viewed as a fundamental phenomenon . . . . Similarly, the attributes of information
are experienced in various forms (knowledge, news, etc.)". Another metaphor, which should
have deserved more attention, is based on the similarities between information and money,
which Ribault (1993) explored with a parallel between information and capital.

REDUCING THE UNCERTAINTY OF THE CONCEPT OF INFORMATION


Thow-Yick (1994, p. 653) suggests in his first postulate that "Data, information, knowledge
and wisdom are the only basic entities of information theory". The taxonomy is however far
from established and systematically adhered to. It would be advisable, even if this is to be an
open-ended task, to establish it more firmly and avoid the shifts between these basic notions,
especially information and knowledge, which are far too common in the literature.
The word "information" encapsulates a wide range of concepts and phenomena, of which
Belkin (1978) provided a most useful review. They relate to both processes and material states
which are closely inter-related and may often come to play in any situation. That they are
inextricably interwoven is no excuse for not pursuing their differentiation and definition, even
if the latter are only tentative and temporary. Remaining alert about what type of information
one is dealing with at any stage appears to be a basic requirement for effective reasoning.
The processes refer to the transformation and transportation of information. One may
consider that they involve 6 main types of activities:
--acquisition,
--material, or physical, processing,
--intellectual processing,
--transmission,
--utilization, and
--assimilation.
Information-as-object may be differentiated according to its source, or store, as suggested by
Ruben (1992). I would rather consider 4 instead of his 3 orders:
- - t h e hidden information existing in nature but yet to be discovered,
- - t h e information existing in the environment, consisting of messages or stimuli, which are
intelligible but waiting to be perceived and interpreted (also called latent information),
- - t h e information configured by individuals, and
- - t h e information configured by groups or social systems.
There might soon be a need for a fifth order, the artificial information contained in computer
systems for collective use.
Information may further be categorized according to its various "states", which can be:
- - a product, which encompasses information-as-thing, as-object, as-resource and ascommodity;
- - w h a t is carried in a channel, including by extension the channel itself, as for instance
assumed in expressions such as "electronic information", or "the media"; and
- - t h e contents.
All processes, sources and states constantly interact and are interdependant. But they are
distinct and do not have the same "value". Protons and neutrons are both basic constituents of
nuclei, neither the higher nor the lower orders exist without the other. Likewise information
when communicated can be characterized concurrently by its source, process(es) and form(s)
under each product, channel and contents states. The primitive logic by which western thought
tries to force reality into rigid classifications and obliges to consider any entity is either in or out

482

Michel J. Menou

a given category, while it is both in and out, is more an impediment than anything else. In this
respect, the communitarian, or dialogic, ontological stereotype suggested by Dervin (1994)
might offer a most appropriate alternative.
When considering the nature and role of information, from whatever perspective, meaning, or
sense-making, becomes central. Sperber and Wilson (1986) tell us that "a fact is manifest to a
person at a given time if and only if he can understand it at that time, and accept its
representation as true or probably true". Information operates only when it "makes sense", that
is either fit with pre-existing meanings, or even can be integrated to them, and thus possibly
transform them. Asking West African mothers to report incidents such as blood in the urine of
their children, in order to monitor the spread of bilharziosis, when they instead consider the lack
of blood as being abnormal, is one among the many examples of the basic requirement for
meaning.

THE "FUNDAMENTAL EQUATION" REVISITED


If, in our quest for the impact or value of information, we "focus on how people construct
sense", as Dervin and Nilan (1986, p. 16) advocated, we should then recognize that it is only in
the contents state that the essential value of information might be discovered. This by no means
implies that the other states have no value. They clearly have one, would it be only transitory.
The perspectives which have so far prevailed were perhaps too much dependant on grossly
oversimplified types of "users" or "use" seeking an immediate solution to discrete problems.
While a value of information can be found there, I feel that its lasting value, if any, is far more
significant.
The contents state is itself made of successive layers:
--semantic,
--syntactic, and
--paradigmatic.
All three contribute to the construction of meaning. But, from the example above, which is
a typical case of what Belkin and Oddy (1978) call an "Anomalous state of knowledge", it is
clear that in the absence of a supportive paradigm, a message recognized only at the semantic
and syntactic levels will have no, or limited, effect. However, information-as-contents which is
not supported by existing paradigmatic structures, may well be assimilated, or memorized and
further used, to some extent. Most people know and can use, at least for communication
purposes, facts and concepts which they do not believe in and do not rely upon when it comes
to making serious choices or taking action. For instance, as a result of the current campaigns,
one may assume that many West-African males "know" appropriate means of protection against
AIDS. Yet, as most of those means are perceived, at the deeper paradigmatic level of their
knowledge base, as destructing their male identity, a surrogate of castration, they are not likely
to use them.
From an impact perspective, it seems to me that information is only a means to an end and
thus not the principal, or most promising, object of investigation. Machlup (1980, p. 56) already
alerted us that "information as the act of informing is designed to produce a state of knowing".
The impact of information is to be seen in the improvement of a new state of knowledge over
the previous one as a result of information-as-contents. That is in first place the reduction of the
anomaly. But the reduction of the anomaly does not only temporarily add contents to the state
of knowledge, it may also transform the contents and organization of its superficial layers
(semantic and syntactic levels) and possibly of its deep layer (paradigmatic level) as noted by
Katzer and Snyder (1990). Brookes (1980a) had already suggested that "information (is) that
which adds to or modifies a knowledge structure". He also mentioned that "Yet the subjective
provides the meta-cognitive basis---4he overview, the perspective, the judgements--which give
purpose and direction to all objective thought". The "meta-cognitive basis" is what I call the
paradigmatic structure, for lack of a better expression. This notion may also be refered to as the
stock of cognitive maps, global layers, frames etc. Cole (1994) shows the importance many

Concepts of information and its value

483

authors have recognized to the existence and role of a lower and higher, or deeper, level in the
knowledge structure. It may well be that the deeper, paradigmatic structure is itself composed
of several sub-layers. At the surface those which provide the connections necessary for
understanding and use, though not necessarily acceptance. At the core, those on which
appropriation is grounded, which contain "our theory of the world in our heads" (Smith, 1988).
That the different levels in the knowledge structure work independently seems to be a
reasonably established, but this does not exclude their interdependance in the later stages of
sense making and integration of the new elements into the knowledge structure.
Strong stressed the importance of "another kind of information to be considered in behaving
systems which is not directly treated in classical information theory--that deals with pattern,
and thus with meaning in relation to goal-directed behavior". Assuming that the process of
information reduction by which significance is established in terms of meeting goals and needs
is primarily a pattern-based selection as he suggests (Strong, 1982, p. 403), the principal benefit
of information is to be looked for in the enhancement of the paradigmatic structure of the
knowledge base.
The "fundamental equation" proposed by Brookes (1980a,b) is not altered in this statement.
He tentatively formulated (Brookes, 1980a) it as:

A I-,K(S +A S)-K(S)
where

K(S) is the knowledge structure


AI is the increment of information
S is the effect of the modification.
He also gave (Brookes, 1980b) another formulation which reads:

K(S)+AI=K(S+AS).
If the knowledge structure is defined as the paradigmatic structure, the fundamental equation
may be kept as it is. However one may wish to distinguish between the superficial knowledge
structure (semantic and syntactic) and the deep structure (paradigmatic) and thus use two
parallel equations of the same form. One may further consider that the ultimate benefit of
information is a combination of the two. I feel that it may be possible to derive from the above
another "'fundamental equation" for the effect, or value, of information, which would read:

E=K(S+f(S, AI))-K(S)
where K(S) stands for the paradigmatic structure of the knowledge base.
Brookes also mentioned that the increment of information could be measured in bits, what
may apply for the superficial structure, but not for the deep one. As a matter of fact, we do not
know which measures could be appropriate for the paradigmatic structure, the pattern content,
of information or the knowledge structure, whether the subject is an individual or, even less, if
it is a group. This is yet another exciting subject for future investigations.
The above implies that information-as-contents, in order to have a lasting value, should first
present attributes that meet the requirements resulting from the existing paradigmatic structure
of the knowledge base, in order to be acknowledged, possibly understood, memorized and
eventually used at a later stage. Information-as-contents should secondly, in order to have a
lasting impact, transform the existing knowledge structure in such a way that it becomes more
able to cope with the kind of problem at hand, or is perceived as such. These two conditions can
be seen as a result of the quality which Cole (1994, p. 473) expects from information: "being
unexpected and expected, old and new at the same time". Until further examination, I feel that
the requirement set by Cole (1994, p. 470) in his second criterion that "Information must modify
knowledge structure at the highest fundamental level of it, not the lower level of it" can apply
to the essential and lasting value of information-as-contents. But a less valuable yet significant
modification can be limited to increments to the existing structure, e.g. simply validating it
through another experience, and also occur at the superficial level.
The stock of paradigms is not static, even though it may evolve only slowly. It is also not
isolated but permanently confronted with incoming information-as-contents it has to process and

484

Michel J. Menou

organize. The more information-as-contents carries patterns that can be absorbed, the higher its
value. The more diversified, comprehensive and flexible the stock of paradigms available to
individuals and groups, the more they are able to interpret new information, or to generate
appropriate meanings, hopefully leading to adapted actions. Not only is the transformation likely
to require a two-stage process as Cole is suggesting, but it may well require a series of
successful iterations of the process associated with successful actions. The transformation of the
knowledge structure, both at the superficial and deep levels, is obviously at least a part of, if not
the key to, leaming. These statements echo one of the fundamental questions in information
science research mentioned by Kochen (1984, p. 198) "What is the process of learning, of
growth in knowledge, growth in understanding and growth in wisdom?".
The shift from information to knowledge, which represents information that has been
understood and assimilated, in first place, and more importantly, perhaps further to a maturation
period during which the new structure is trialed, information that has been appropriated, has
many important consequences. With regard to developing countries, it implies in particular that
indigenous knowledge rather than the "world's stores of information" and access to them should
be regarded as the main source of potential benefits. Machlup (1980, p. 167) told us that "from
various points of view, "living knowledge", or what living people know, may be the relevant
stock of knowledge in society".

AND WISDOM IN ALL THAT?


A rich knowledge base does not however guarantee that it will ever be used in a considerate
fashion so that the reactions, actions or adaptations it will support will result in a well balanced,
more satisfactory, reversible and so to speak, better situation. To that end, adaptive systems need
to turn knowledge into wisdom. Wisdom may depict those knowledge structures which have
been so organized and factored that their use offers minimal risks of unappropriate outcomes and
maximizes the comfort of the users with the outcomes. In other words, wisdom is what increases
harmony. Or happiness. This may be dependent in part on the ability of the paradigmatic
structure of the knowledge base. And it is certainly also dependent in part on the attributes of
the users, of which some will be discussed below.
Criteria for wisdom are certainly not universal and may not apply beyond individual ideology.
Wisdom can perhaps be witnessed only in retrospect or in comparison. The seven principles set
forth by the Foundation for human progress in its manifesto "For a jointly responsible and liable
world" (Foundation pour le progr~s humain, 1994) may offer a first approximation of wisdom.
They are: safeguard, humanity, responsibility, moderation, caution, diversity and citizenship.
Schoenhoff (1993, p. 100) reminds us that "In many Third World cultures, where formal
education is still the exception, there is a greater emphasis upon wisdom than upon knowledge,
a greater respect for the wise judge than for the scholar". By any yardstick, contemporary
societies have a long way to go before they achieve it. Societies which are the better off with
regard to their material wealth, technological capacity and richness of their knowledge base can
be predatory and ultimately self-destructive, as evidenced by so many examples in history and
contemporary affairs. Is it wise, for instance to turn thousand of acres of amazonian forests into
grasslands for future hamburgers?
In spite of its merits, Thow-Yick (1994, p.652) attempt at formalizing wisdom as "a small
increment in knowledge created by a person's intellectual deductive ability after attaining a
sufficient level of understanding of the knowledge area" and more drastically his concept that
"this creation of new knowledge is usually the activity of experts or intellectuals", sounds
somewhat short and biased toward the western-like techno-structure. For the common human
beings, rather that the originality or creativity he recognizes as a special attribute of wisdom, it
is its general trustworthiness which is critical. And the latter is more dependent on repetition
than novelty.
The conditions for the transformation of knowledge into wisdom and the attributes of wisdom
are subjects which should deserve more attention in the information science community and

Concepts of information and its value

485

elsewhere. The session on "Wisdom is . . . " organized by Karl Schneider at the 1993 Annual
Meeting of the American Society for Information Science is hopefully a positive signal.

ASSESSING ATTRIBUTES OF THE CONSTITUENCIES


It should also be emphasized that attempts at "measuring" information-as-contents, or any
other state, as a discrete entity cannot go without a parallel effort at characterizing the
individuals or groups which are using it and possibly measuring their most critical traits. Thus
the importance of the description of the Information Use Environments (Taylor, 1991), which
was stressed in the previous article, and by other authors such as Debons et al. (1993). The IUEs
specifies in particular the power balance under which information use, and thus its impact,
occur. Its importance is not limited to the dialectics of information and democracy which Dervin
(1994, p. 381) reminded, but apply for all aspects of empowerment. The range of logics upon
which people depend is another key consideration which was discussed in the previous article.
Dervin (1994, p. 382) puts it beautifully: "To remain muddled, to refuse to choose, to dream
instead of seek facts-these can be all as informative, and therefore as gap-bridging-as what
traditionally has been thought of as information seeking and use (e.g. fact finding, evidence
collecting, deciding, source connecting)". It would inded be interesting to know the relative
importance among human beings (individually and in groups) of these activities, and of the
purposeful activities and the lower order ones suggested by Ackoff (1971).
A tentative list of the attributes to be considered, and possibly included as a subset of the
measurements, would include:
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)

readiness, or willingness
propensity to change
capability, or ability
appropriation
"gestuality"
mediacy
objectification or "reification" of institutions.

Readiness and capability, according to Lamberton (1993) may be regarded as especially


important. Cole (1994, p. 467) also recognizes their role, after Pratt. Readiness can be described
as the willingness and ability to act upon the problem. This in many instances is dependent on
another trait, which Slamecka (1982) identified as propensity to change. It should not be limited
to the condition of the actual system, after all everybody expects an improvement of
his/her lot, but be concerned with the structure of the system. And clearly resistance to change
is the first and foremost protection of all adaptive systems against inconsiderate transformations.
Capability is the talent to turn action into effective results.
I would add a fourth attribute, "appropriation", which is the sense of ownership of the means
and ends, or more simply the perception that what is a stake is one's business. Kuhlthau (1988,
p. 300) noted for instance that her subjects "revealed a sense of ownership on an internal,
intellectual and private level. There was an element of possession in the process as well as the
product". Appropriation may also be expressed through the belonging to the group, or
relationship with the master, so essential in the initiation process, which is, as I once stressed
(Menou, 1969) a basic information transfer pattern.
A fifth trait, which I would call "gestuality", may be of particular importance. It relates to the
fact that the application of paradigms is evidenced in behavior. Strong (1982, p. 405) refers to
the possibility "that pattern is constructed via behavior representations--as the "glue" relating
one state to another. This particular form of organization of information may be a result of the
social adaptation of primates whereby behavioral invariance is important in the ability to pass
along group adaptations. This view would lend to a new basis for theories of the social
construction of reality in that it is much easier for organisms to copy one another's behavior than
share one another's perception". The importance of tales in the transmission of knowledge,
stressed by Bruner (1991) could be considered as an indication of the soundness of this
IPM 31-4-C

486

Michel J. Menou

assumption. Tales convey knowledge through the description of behaviors. I would not be
surprised that what we call "innate" is nothing but paradigms of the knowledge base which were
glued over so many thousands of years of behavior that they became part of our heritage, e.g.
the fascination for fire I refered to in the above quoted paper, or standard machism. How could
this be demonstrated is another story, worth attention.
Mediacy (Carbo Bearman, 1992), that is the understanding of, and ability to use modem
media, was discussed in the previous paper. The last trait was suggested by Cherry (1977,
p. 201) who saw the trust in "idealized" institutions, as opposed to personally known
individuals, as one key to the effective use of modem information.
These traits are obviously dependent on the complex interaction of a wealth of factors. But
they also suppose the existence of paradigms which can support them. In the absence of those
traits, the availability of information-as-contents would make no difference, as we have plenty
of evidence with regard to the protection of the environment, prevention of AIDS and the like.
The timely provision of accurate and relevant information to the highly educated professionals
who flourish in today's large and complex organizations seems on the contrary to breed more
and more failures.

ASSESSING THE PARADIGMATIC STRUCTURE


Should one accept that the more significant impact of information is to be looked at in the
transformation of the paradigmatic level of the knowledge structure, then comes the question of
suitable ways to identify and characterize a stock of paradigms and its evolution.
One would have to identify in first place the groups to be studied, whose IUEs should then
be described. The groups should then be segmented and sampled as appropriate. One would then
proceed with the subjects to the identification of a specific problem, which is regarded as critical,
and should be defined as precisely as possible, using when appropriate a nested hierarchy in
order to reduce it to discrete components. An example of a problem could be "Defending crops
against baboons" or "Availability of welfare programs for single mothers to support the
education of their children".
The next step would be to identify, in cooperation with the sample group, the critical factors
associated with the solution of the problem. Then, the more important ones may be selected, if
there are too many and/or some appear to be more closely connected to the problem. One would
have to further list the information which is related to each factor. At this stage, one would be
in a position to move into the analysis of the paradigms which are called upon in order to
interpret this information. This is obviously a quite difficult exercise, in which one should limit
as far as possible, if not avoid, interferences from the observer. Some methods are available for
surveying the semantic and syntactic contents of knowledge bases, such as the grids for mental
constructs developed by Kelly (1955), or the "informs" and "knows" suggested by Debons
(1994), which may provide a useful starting point.
I do not know yet of any tool for digging out the paradigmatic structures, except perhaps those
which were used by ethnologists for the structural analysis of tales, beliefs and the like and of
course psycho-analytic techniques, which may be difficult to apply in the context we are
considering. Both the process of interpretation of information-as-contents and the deepest layers
of the paradigms are in fact often "unconscious, what does not necessarily means superficial"
(Van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983, p. 34). Semi-directive interviews with neutral question leading the
respondents to state the attributes attached to each information item and sources may perhaps be
a solution. For instance a statement like "The extension agent is seldom available" may be
specified by questions like "How often is he/she in the village?", "Is he/she aware of your
problems?", "Is he/she doing some fact finding for the administration when you ask for
information?" in order to find out whether the original statement refers to such dimensions as
exogeneity, distanciation from the group, interference from the administration or any
combination of the above. The paradigms behind each information item will hopefully emerge
in the course of the fact finding with also possibly some of their attributes, or dimensions.
One would then have to run a second series of interviews in order to position the paradigms

Concepts of information and its value

487

along the axes of the dimensions, as discussed below. Here again extreme care should be taken
not to bias the answers and their interpretation. During this process, new dimensions may
become apparent. The investigator will also be in a position, either from the original replies or
from subsidiary questions, to determine the relative importance and significance of the
various dimensions. This discussion may also help single out the most critical factors
and/or a selection of the most relevant paradigms. It would then be possible to tentatively draw
the maps for single paradigms and, when appropriate and feasible, a combined map, based for
instance on mean values on each axis.
The resulting picture could be validated by constructing stories similar to those by which the
initial information was gathered, relying upon paradigms with the same dimensions as those
found, or on the contrary antithetic ones, and analyzing the feedback of the respondents when
they hear these stories. If the behaviour and information reported in the story are judged valid,
in the case when the same dimensions are implied, or else invalid, one may assume that the
mapping is correct. Group discussions of the findings or the simple observation of relevant cases
may also be considered as means of validation. One time investigations are not likely to yield
decisive results because of the risk of misinterpretation and other interferences. It would be
advisable to repeat them at regular intervals over some time, as a further means of verification.
More distant observations would probably be even more interesting by evidencing possible
changes in the paradigmatic stock and the related information use and behaviour.

MAPPING THE DIMENSIONS OF THE PARADIGMATIC STRUCTURE


Trying to impose preconceived yardsticks may be fraught with ethnocentric biases. It seems
more appropriate to derive the measures from the properties which will emerge from actual
observations and be recognized as significant and discriminant by the various groups being
investigated.
However, it may be helpful to refer from the inception to a number of dimensions which a
priori appear to be useful pointers to the characteristics of a given stock of paradigms. I have
tentatively identified 7 dimensions, on the basis of those particular aspects of appropriate
information which are often mentioned in the literature, for instance Neelameghan (1981),
and/or those I had the opportunity to observe:
--Informal-Formal, which refers to the existence, or not, of an identified formal source for
the paradigms,
----Endogenous-Exogenous, which refers to the fact that the paradigms originated, or not,
from within the group,
---Resident-Circulating, which refers to the fact that the paradigms remain unsaid or are
exposed to open discussion,
---Unconscious-Conscious, which refers to the degree to which the paradigms are
grounded in structured justifications,
--Ancient-Recent, which refers to the lasting of the paradigms,
--Stable-Changing, which refers to the possibility for the paradigms to be adapted, or not,
as a result of experience,
---Multiple purposes-Single purpose, which refers to the range of events whose
interpretation can be supported by the paradigms.
Not all these dimensions may be relevant in any particular context. Other dimensions may
also prove appropriate in particular cases. I feel nevertheless, at the present time, that the
proposed 7 dimensions are in fact most critical ones with regard to the ability of a stock of
paradigms to put a community in a better position to cope with the challenges of its existence.
These dimensions should be seen as axes along which the paradigms can be positioned.
Although it is possible that some paradigms fully fit at any one end of an axis, the more likely
situation is that they take an intermediate position, being more formal than informal, more
circulating than resident, etc. The relative position could be depicted by using, for instance, a
scale of 1 to 10, where 1 would represent a fully informal paradigmatic stock, 5 an equally

488

MichelJ. Menou

7
FONCTIONS MULTIPLES
MULTIPLE PURPOSES

1
FORMELLE
FORMAL

2
ENDOGENE
ENDOGENOUS

6
CHANGEANTE
CHANGING

3
RESlDENTE
RESIDENT

4
INCONSCIENTE
UNCOUNSClOUS

5
RECENTE
RECENT

5
ANClENNE
ANCIENT

4
CONSCIENTE
COUNSCIOUS

6
STABLE
STABLE

3
CIRCULANTE
CIRCULATING

2
EXOGENE
EXOGENOUS

1
INFORMELLE
INFORMAL

7
FONCTION UNIQUE
SINGLE PURPOSE

Fig. 2. Hypothetical maps of the dimensions of the paradigmatic core of information and knowledge.

informal and formal paradigmatic stock and 10 a fully formal paradigmatic stock, etc. A
"traditional" stock of paradigms could hypothetically be characterized as being fully informal,
endogenous, resident, unconscious, ancient, stable and multiple purposes. This implying no
value judgement. Conversely a flexible and responsive paradigmatic stock may be a priori
depicted as one which would concentrate in the area along the axes which is more congenial to
the problem at hand, or in general terms evenly distributed around the median position.
Considering, for example, the adoption of a new cultural practice, an appropriate stock of
paradigms would need to be more on the formal, exogenous, circulating, conScious, recent,
changing and single purpose side.
Using these dimensions, one may be able to draw maps similar to the chart of a visual field.
The notation of the dimensions could not be done by means of single dots on the axis. It calls
for lines to be drawn from the centre and extending toward both ends, so as to represent the
balance between the two extreme states. Half a unit on the scale would represent 10% in either
direction. An equally formal and informal and informal paradigm would be represented by a line
equally distributed from the centre, from 2,5 on one side to 2,5 on the other. A 60% formal
paradigm would thus be represented by a line of 6 units on the formal side and 4 on the informal
one. A 60 % informal paradigm by a line of 6 units on the informal side and 4 on the formal
one. A 100% formal paradigm by a 10 units line on the formal side.
The differences between the paradigmatic stocks of two groups confronted with a particular
problem, or the evolution of the paradigmatic stock of a single group over time, in conjunction
with an improved supply of information for instance, could then be visualized and assessed. A
theoretical example is shown in Fig. 2 where I tentatively mapped the paradigmatic stock of two
groups, e.g. farmers and extension workers, involved in a given problem such as the
preservation of water resources in a Sahel village.

Concepts of information and its value

489

CONCLUSION
Focusing on information-as-contents and its ability to transform the paradigmatic structure of
the knowledge bases does not invalidate the concerns which were discussed in the previous
article. Information products whose contents would be appropriate to the problem at hand could
certainly contribute to bring about a positive solution, at least in the short-term. But it adds, in
my view, an essential perspective which may well form a field of research in its own right.
If one is sensitive to a broad and universal notion of development which would be the
enhanced capability of individuals and groups to cope with their problems and achieve more
harmony both within themselves and in relation to their environment, the issue of the
transformation of the knowledge structures and especially their deep paradigmatic layers
becomes central.
The speculative nature of the above outlined concepts and models needs no emphasis. We are
nevertheless struck by the apparent convergence they seem to have with a number of recent
contributions. A renewal of interest and possibly a leap forward seems to take shape on this
front. In addition to the many questions underlying what we said in these two articles on the
impact of information, two of those mentioned by Dervin (1994, p. 383) are certainly worth
mentioning "What are the different strategies people use to construct and deconstruct their
worlds? What are the different ways in which humans conform to and resist order?"
Empirical research is required in order to determine whether:
- - t h e concepts and models I have suggested can be applied;
---objective, or non-biased, measures of these dimensions can be performed, and
- - t h e resulting assessments and maps allow for meaningful interpretations in the eyes of
both the groups under study and the investigator.
It is hoped that a first attempt could be made in the framework of a research project to be soon
initiated in Africa. Appropriate procedures and protocols for fact finding and interpretation can
only be devised on this occasion.
Kochen (1984) challenged us with one of his open and fundamental questions for information
science research: "Can a deeper conceptualization of "information" that includes the relation to
meaning, decision-relevancy, reduction of uncertainty, complexity and tension be devised that
lead to insights about its nature and properties?" In face of this questioning, most of the hightech induced subjects which monopolize the research agenda of information science seem
somewhat futile. The two types of research may be more interdependent than usually
acknowledged. As a matter of fact, the reader will have noted the convergence, though at
different levels and with different perspectives, of the above mentioned concerns and those of,
for instance, research on relevance (Froehlich & Eisenberg, 1994), or on knowledge utilization
(Lunin & Paisley, 1993).

REFERENCES
Ackoff, R. L. (1971). Toward a system of systems concepts. Management Science, 17, 661--671.
Belkin, N. J. (1978). Information concepts for information science. Journal of Documentation, 34(1), 55-85.
Belkin, N. J., & Oddy, R. N. (1978). Document retrieval based on the automatic determination of the user's information
needs. Journal oflnformatics, 2(1), 8-11.
Brookes, B. C. (1980a). Measurement in information science: Objective and subjective metrical space. Journal of the
American Society for Information Science, 3(4), 248-255.
Brookes, B. C. (1980b). The foundations of information science. Part 1. Philosophical aspects. Journal of Information
Science, 2(3/4). Quoted by Debons (1992b) op. cit.
Bruner, J. (1991). Car la culture donne forme c) resprit. De la rdvolution cognitive ~ la psychologic culturelle. Paris:
Eshel. (Translation of Acts of Meaning, Harvard University Press, 1990.)
Carbo Bearman, T. (1992). Information transforming society: challenges for the year 2000. Information Services and
Use, 12, 217-223.
Cherry, C. (1977). Worm communication: Threat or promise? A socio-technical approach. Chichester: Wiley.
Cole, C. (1993). Shannon revisited: Information in terms of uncertainty. Journal of the American Society for Information
Science, 44(4), 204-21 i.
Cole, C. (1994). Operationalizing the notion of information as a subjective construct. Journal of the American Society
for Information Science, 45(7), 465-476.

490

Michel J. Menou

Debons, A. (1992). The measurement of knowledge. In D. Shaw (Ed.), Information management on the move.
Proceedings of the 55th Annual Meeting of the American Society of Information Science (Vol. 29,
pp. 212-215 ). Medford, N J.: Learned Information.
Debons, A. et al. (1993). Knowledge counseling: The concept, the process and its application, lnfomediary, 6(2),
113-128.
Dervin, B. (1994). Information Democracy. Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 45(6),
369-385 (Special topic issue: Information resources and democracy edited by Leah A. Lievrouw.)
Dervin, B., & Nilan, M. (1986). Information needs and uses. In M. E. Williams (Ed.), Annual review of information
science and technology, (Vol. 21, pp. 3-33). White Plains, N.Y.: Knowledge Industry Publications.
Fondation pour le pregr~s humain (1994). Pour un monde responsable et solidaire. Le Monde Diplomatique, April,
16-17.
Froehlich, T. J., & Eisenberg, M., (Eds) (1994). Special topic issue: Relevance research. Journal of the American Society
for Information Science, 45(3).
Hayes, R. M. (1993). Measurement of information. Information Processing & Management, 29(1), l - l l .
Heilprin, L. B. (1989). Foundations of information science reexamined. In M. E. Williams (Ed.), Annual review of
information science and technology, (Vol. 24, pp. 343-372). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Katzer, J., & Snyder, H.(1990). Toward a more realistic assessment of information retrieval performance. In D.
Henderson (Ed.), Information in the year 2000: From research to applications. Proceedings of the 53rd Annual
Meeting of the American Society for Information Science (Vol. 27, pp. 80-85). Medford, NJ.: Learned Information.
Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. Vol. 1. A theory of personality. New York: W. W. Norton.
Kochen, M. (1984). Information science research: The search for the nature of information. Journal of the American
Society for Information Science, 35(3), 194-199.
Kuhlthau, C. C. (1988). Longitudinal case studies of the information search process of users in libraries. Library and
Information Science Research, 10, 257-304.
Lamberton, D. M. (1993). Private communication. See also his contribution "Information and organization: Questions
and clues", 3rd International Information Research Conference, Poigny-la-Foret, France.
Langlois, R. N. (1982). Systems theory and the meaning of information. Journal of the American Society for Information
Science, 33(6), 395-399.
Lunin, L., & Paisley, W. (Eds) (1993). Perspectives o n . . . Knowledge utilization. Journal of the American Society for
Information Science, 44(4).
Machlup, E (1980). Knowledge: its creation, distribution and significance. Vol. 1 Knowledge and knowledge production.
Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.
Menou, M. J. (1969). Knowledge transfer from autoconsumption to mass production. In A.I. Mikhailov A. I. Chernyi,
R. S. Gilyarevski, & V. Y. Afremov (Eds), Theoretical problems of informatics (pp. 41-63). Moscow: Viniti, FID
435.
Menou, M. J. (Ed.) (1993). Measuring the impact of information on development. Ottawa, Canada: International
Development Research Centre.
Menou, M. J. (1995). The impact of information on development: Results of a preliminary investigation. Paper
presented at the 3rd International Information Research Conference, Poigny-la-For~t, France.
Neelameghan, A. (1981). Some issues in information transfer: A Third World perspective. IFLA Journal, 7(1),
8-18.
Otten, K., & Debons, A. (1970). Towards a metascience of information: Informatology. Journal of the American Society
for Information Science, 21(1), 89-94.
Ribault, T. (1993). Economic de l'information: Approche patrimoniale. Paris: A jour.
Ruben, B. D. (1992). The communication-information relationship in system-theoric perspective. Journal of the
American Society for Information Science, 43(1), 15-27.
Schoenhoff, D. M. (1993). The barefoot expert. The interface of computerized knowledge systems and indigenous
knowledge systems. Westport, Conn.: Greenwood Press.
Slamecka, V. (1982). The inclination toward information use. In G. P. Sweeney (Ed.), Information and the
transformation of society. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Smith, E (1988). Understanding reading: A psycholinguistic analysis of reading and learning to read. Hillsdale, N.J.:
Erlbaum. Quoted by Cole (1994), op. cit., p. 471.
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University
Press, as summarized (p. 604) in Harter, S.P. Psychological relevance and information science. Journal of the
American Society for Information Science, 43(9), 602-615.
Stonier, T. (1991). Towards a new theory of information. Journal of Information Science, 17(5), 257-263.
Strong, G. W. (1982). Adaptive systems: The study of information, pattern and behavior. Journal of the American Society
for Information Science, 33(6), 400-406.
Taylor, R. S. (1991). Information use environments. In B. Dervin & M. J. Voigt (Eds), Progress in communication
sciences, (Vol. 10, pp. 217-255). Norwood, N.J.: Ablex.
Thow-Yick, L. (1994). The Basic Entity Model: A fundamental theoretical model of information and information
processing. Information Processing & Management, 30(5), 647-661.
Van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. Orlando, Fla: Academic Press. Quoted
by Cole (1994), op. cir.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai