Anda di halaman 1dari 1

14.

10 brief comms 757 MH 7/10/04 5:20 pm Page 757

brief communications

Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain


Proficiency in a second language and age at acquisition affect grey-matter density.

H
umans have a unique ability to learn a predisposition to increased density, or from
more than one language — a skill that a structural reorganization induced by expe-
is thought to be mediated by functional rience4. Early bilinguals probably acquire a
(rather than structural) plastic changes in the second language through social experience,
brain1. Here we show that learning a second rather than as a result of a genetic predisposi-
language increases the density of grey matter tion. Our findings therefore suggest that the
in the left inferior parietal cortex and that the b 0.1 structure of the human brain is altered by the
degree of structural reorganization in this experience of acquiring a second language.

Grey-matter density
0.05
region is modulated by the proficiency The inferior parietal region that is associ-
attained and the age at acquisition. This 0 ated with second-language acquisition cor-
relation between grey-matter density and responds exactly to an area that has been
performance may represent a general prin- –0.05 shown by functional imaging to become
ciple of brain organization. –0.1
activated during verbal-fluency tasks5,6.
We used a whole-brain unbiased objective 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 Whether grey-matter reorganization in this
Proficiency in second language
technique, known as voxel-based morpho- region is related to changes in neuropil, neu-
metry (VBM)2,3, to investigate structural c 0.1 ronal size, dendritic or axonal arborization
plasticity in healthy right-handed English will be revealed by methods other than
Grey-matter density

0.05
and Italian bilinguals. whole-brain magnetic resonance imaging.
To test for differences in the density of 0 These results are consistent with growing
grey and white matter between bilinguals and evidence that the human brain changes struc-
monolinguals, we recruited 25 monolinguals –0.05 turally in response to environmental demands
who had had little or no exposure to a second –0.1
— for example, structure is already known
language; 25 ‘early’ bilinguals, who had 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 to alter as a function of learning in domains
Age at acquisition of second language
learned a second European language before other than language7,8. We have shown that
the age of 5 years and who had practised it Figure 1 Structural reorganization in the bilingual brain. a, Sagit- the degree of this structural reorganization
regularly since; and 33 ‘late’ bilinguals, who tal (x45), coronal (y59) and axial (z48) view of the in bilinguals is correlated with their second-
had learned a second European language left inferior parietal region, which has increased grey-matter den- language performance. The relationship
between the ages of 10 and 15 years and prac- sity in bilinguals relative to monolinguals. b, Grey-matter density, between grey-matter density and perfor-
tised it regularly for at least 5 years.All volun- measured as cubic millimetres of grey matter per voxel in the left mance discovered here could be an example of
teers for this test were native English speakers inferior parietal region, as a function of second-language profi- a more general structure–function principle
of comparable age and level of education. ciency. Second-language proficiency was estimated for each that extends beyond the domain of language.
Voxel-based morphometry revealed that subject from a battery of standardized neuropsychological tests, Andrea Mechelli*, Jenny T. Crinion*,
grey-matter density in the inferior parietal using principal component analysis (for details, see supplemen- Uta Noppeney*, John O’Doherty*,
cortex was greater in bilinguals than mono- tary information). c, Grey-matter density, measured as for b, as a John Ashburner*, Richard S. Frackowiak*†,
linguals (Fig. 1a). This effect was significant function of age at acquisition. Cathy J. Price*
in the left hemisphere (x45, y59, *Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience,
z48; Z-score7.1; P0.05, corrected for indexed by principal component analysis, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
multiple comparisons across the whole brain) correlated negatively with age at acquisition e-mail: a.mechelli@fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk
and a trend was also evident in the right hemi- (P0.01; r0.855). Remarkably, VBM †Fondazione Santa Lucia, Roma 00179, Italy
sphere (x56,y53,z42;Z-score3.4; revealed that second-language proficiency 1. Kim, K. H., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K.-M. & Hirsch, J. Nature 388,
P0.001, uncorrected). Although increased correlated with grey-matter density in exactly 171–174 (1997).
2. Ashburner, J. & Friston, K. J. NeuroImage 11, 805–821 (2000).
grey-matter density in the inferior parietal the same left inferior parietal region that we 3. Good, C. D. et al. NeuroImage 17, 29–46 (2002).
cortex was common to both early and late had already identified (x48, y59, 4. Golestani, N., Paus, T. & Zatorre, R. J. Neuron 35, 997–1010
bilinguals, the effect was greater in the early z46; Z-score4.1; P0.05,corrected after (2002).
5. Poline, J. B. et al. NeuroImage 4, 34–54 (1996).
bilinguals in the left (x48, y62, 10-mm small-volume correction; Fig. 1b). In 6. Warburton, E. A. et al. Brain 119, 159–179 (1996).
z44; Z-score3.5; P0.001, uncorrected) addition, grey-matter density in this region 7. Maguire, E. A. et al. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 4398–4403
and right (x45, y65, z47; Z- correlated negatively with the age of acquis- (2000).
8. Draganski, B. et al. Nature 427, 311–312 (2004).
score3.5; P0.001, uncorrected) hemi- ition of the second language (x50,
Supplementary information accompanies this communication on
spheres. No other significant effects were y58, z42; Z-score3.2; P0.05, Nature’s website.
detected in either grey or white matter. corrected after 10-mm small-volume correc- Competing financial interests: declared none.
We next investigated whether there was a tion; Fig. 1c). There were no other significant
relation between brain structure and profi- effects in grey or white matter.
ciency in the second language and age at We have therefore identified an increase in brief communications arising online
acquisition.We tested 22 native Italian speak- the density of grey matter in the left inferior ➧ www.nature.com/bca
ers who had learned English as a second parietal cortex of bilinguals relative to mono-
language at an age between 2 and 34 years. linguals, which is more pronounced in early Mouse transcriptome: Neutral evolution of
Second-language reading, writing, speech rather than late bilinguals, and have also 'non-coding' complementary DNAs
comprehension and production were assessed shown that the density in this region in- J. Wang, J. Zhang, H. Zheng, J. Li, D. Liu, H. Li,
using a battery of standardized neuro- creases with second-language proficiency but R. Samudrala, J. Yu, G. K.-S. Wong
psychological tests (see supplementary infor- decreases as the age of acquisition increases. (doi:10.1038/nature03016)
mation). We found that overall proficiency, as These effects could result from a genetic Reply: Y. Hyashizaki (doi:10.1038/nature03017)

NATURE | VOL431 | 14 OCTOBER 2004 | www.nature.com/nature 757


©2004 Nature Publishing Group

Anda mungkin juga menyukai