Anda di halaman 1dari 9

Personality and Individual Dierences 40 (2006) 331339

www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The dark triad and normal personality traits


Sharon Jakobwitz, Vincent Egan

Department of Psychology, Glasgow Caledonian University, Cowcaddens Road, Glasgow G4 0BA, Scotland
Received 18 April 2005; received in revised form 1 July 2005; accepted 26 July 2005
Available online 8 September 2005

Abstract
Machiavellianism, Narcissism and Psychopathy are often referred to as the dark triad of personality.
We examined the degree to which these constructs could be identied in 82 persons recruited from the general population, predicting that the dark triad would emerge as a single dimension denoting the cardinal
interpersonal elements of primary psychopathy. We expected the primary psychopathy dimension to correlate negatively with Agreeableness (A) and Conscientiousness (C), whereas secondary psychopathy would
be associated with Neuroticism (N). The negative correlation was found between primary psychopathy and
A, but not with C. While the predicted correlation between secondary psychopathy and N was found,
N was also positively associated with primary psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Factor analysis revealed
that all measures of the dark triad loaded positively on the same factor, upon which A loaded negatively.
Secondary psychopathy loaded positively on a second factor, together with N and (negatively) with C.
These ndings reiterate the distinguishing properties of secondary psychopathy, impulsivity and anti-social
behaviour relative to primary psychopathy. Thus, even in the general population, the dark dimension of
personality can be described in terms of low A, whereas much of the anti-social behaviour in normal persons appears underpinned by high N and low C.
2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Psychopathy; Narcissism; Machiavellianism; Dark triad; Personality; Big ve; Agreeableness; Conscientiousness; Neuroticism

Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 141 331 3037; fax: +44 141 331 3636.
E-mail address: v.egan@gcal.ac.uk (V. Egan).

0191-8869/$ - see front matter 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.07.006

332

S. Jakobwitz, V. Egan / Personality and Individual Dierences 40 (2006) 331339

1. Introduction
The term Dark Triad of Personality refers to three interrelated higher-order personality constructspsychopathy, narcissism and Machiavellianism (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). The majority of work conducted on psychopathy builds upon observations by Cleckley (1941/1988),
operationalised in Hares revised Psychopathy Checklist (PCL-R; Hare, 1991). The PCL-R and
similar measures (e.g., Levensons self-report measure of psychopathy (LSRP; Levenson, Kiehl,
& Fitzpatrick, 1995)) measure two facets of psychopathy. Factor 1 reects primary psychopathy
(e.g., selshness, callousness, lack of interpersonal aect, supercial charm and remorselessness),
factor 2 measures anti-social lifestyle and behaviours, and is akin to secondary psychopathy. It
should be noted that researchers now propose three facets to core psychopathy; an arrogant
and deceitful interpersonal style, decient aective experience, and an impulsive and irresponsible
behavioural style (Cooke & Michie, 2001).
Most research into psychopathy involves forensic populations such as prisoners and mentally
disordered oenders. However, not all persons with primary and secondary psychopathy are
in custody, and a literature has gradually emerged examining psychopathy-like traits in the
general population (Board & Fritzon, 2005; Ross, Lutz, & Bailley, 2004). This suggests that if psychopathy is a trait, it should be apparent in non-oenders, and that it may even confer some kind
of social advantage (Levenson, 1992). Examining psychopathy in the general population overcomes the sample bias of only seeing persons from prison settings, who are essentially homogenous regarding socio-economic background and who are competitively disadvantaged
intellectually, socially, and interpersonally. It is probable that the majority of institutionalised
oenders are more inclined to secondary than primary psychopathy, with dierent dispositional
mechanisms driving their transgressive behaviour (Lykken, 1995; McHoskey, Worzel, & Szyarto,
1998).
Machiavellianism (MACH) refers to interpersonal strategies that advocate self-interest, deception and manipulation. Christie and Geis (1970) examined the extent to which people use qualities
such as deceit, attery and emotional detachment to manipulate social and interpersonal interactions. While high MACHS are perceived to be more intelligent and attractive by their peers (Cherulnik, Way, Ames, & Hutto, 1981), MACH does not correlate with intelligence or measures of
success in modern life such as income or status (Ames & Kidd, 1979; Hunt & Chonto, 1984).
In experimental settings high MACHS frequently outperform low MACHS, whether this be bargaining and alliance forming (Christie & Geis, 1970), or assuming leadership in group situations
(Cherulnik et al., 1981). As persons high in MACH are likely to exploit others and less likely to be
concerned about other people beyond their own self-interest, MACH is predictably negatively
correlated with empathy (Barnett & Thompson, 1985). Given these ndings, one would expect
a relationship between MACH and primary psychopathy.
The concept of narcissism derives from the psychodynamic formulations such as a pathological
form of self-love (Freud, 1914), or personality development, whereby narcissistic wounds sustained in childhood may lead to an arrest in development and increased shame-driven rage
(Kohut, 1977). It has been argued that the construct of narcissism is compromised by the contrast
between vague psychoanalytic terminology and theory, and more observable elements of the concept (Bradlee & Emmons, 1991; Watson & Morris, 1991). However as a means of encapsulating

S. Jakobwitz, V. Egan / Personality and Individual Dierences 40 (2006) 331339

333

the behavioural grandiosity and perceived entitlement of an individual, the concept of narcissism
is a very useful concept. One commonly used scale to assess narcissism is the Narcisssistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Hall, 1979). The NPI measures persistent attention seeking,
extreme vanity, excessive self-focus, and exploitativeness in interpersonal relationships (Millon
& Davis, 1996), and comprises four factors: Exploitativeness/Entitlement, Leadership/Authority,
Superiority/Arrogance and Self-Absorption/Self-Admiration (Emmons, 1984).
The constructs of the dark triad correlate with each another singularly and in combination;
(Hare, 1991; Skinner, 1988; Smith & Grith, 1978; McHoskey, 1995). Such ndings led McHoskey et al. (1998) to argue that for the general population MACH is a global measure of psychopathy comparable to primary and secondary psychopathy, this comparability being confounded
due to the dierent paradigms and theoretical orientations of clinical and dierential psychology.
McHoskey et al. found that when secondary psychopathy was controlled for, primary psychopathy remained associated with narcissism (r = 0.46). Paulhus and Williams (2002) also investigated the dark triad in the general population and found considerable overlap between the
constructs, although the scale used to measure psychopathy (the SRP-III; Hare, 1985) did not
distinguish between primary and secondary psychopathy. The scales of the dark triad were
correlated with Agreeableness, and revealed negative correlations of 0.36, 0.47 and 0.25
for narcissism, psychopathy and MACH, respectively. No other dimension of the Big Five
captured the constructs of the dark triad. This is perhaps surprising, as MACH is typically negatively correlated with anxiety (Wiggins & Pincus, 1989), as is primary psychopathy (Fehr,
Samson, & Paulhus, 1992), so one would expect Neuroticism (N) to be a negative associate of
the construct.
The current study investigated to what extent MACH, primary psychopathy, secondary
psychopathy and narcissism reect the same underlying construct, and to examine the extent to
which normal dimensions of personality indexed by a brief measure of the Big Five could capture
the constructs of the dark triad. We expected to nd low A and low C associated with higher
scores on each of the dark triad dimensions, and to load on a single dark triad factor. We expected
N to be unrelated to the core traits of psychopathy, but to be associated with secondary
psychopathy.

2. Method
2.1. Sample and procedure
The sample in this study were recruited opportunistically from the general population using a
snowball system, whereby a starter sample were further asked recruit people from their environment who would be willing to take part in this study. Although this meant that not all participants
were in direct contact with the researcher, this form of recruitment ensured that a diverse selection
of the general population, was enlisted. Eighty-two persons were recruited, their mean age being
29; of the cohort 37% were men (N = 30), 63% (N = 52) women. Questionnaires were answered
anonymously and participants lled out a consent form before taking part in the study. The study
was a correlational within-subjects design.

334

S. Jakobwitz, V. Egan / Personality and Individual Dierences 40 (2006) 331339

3. Measures
3.1. The revised NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI-R; McCrae & Costa, 2004)
The NEO-FFI-R is a revised version of the NEO-FFI, in which 14 items of the NEO-FFI were
changed. This was done to increase the correlation between the shortened version of the scale and
overall NEO-PI-R scores, diversify item content by selecting items from underrepresented facets,
and to increase the intelligibility of the items. This modication was required following the discovery that not all scales of the NEO-FFI had equally stable structures (e.g. Egan, Deary, & Austin,
2000). The NEO-FFI-R consists of 60 items that yield scores on ve personality dimensions: Neuroticism (N), Extraversion (E), Openness (O), Agreeableness (A), and Conscientiousness (C). In
responding to the NEO, subjects report the extent to which they agree or disagree in regard to
how each item applies to them by rating themselves on a 5-point Likert scale. Internal reliability
coecients of the NEO-FFI-R scales range from 0.75 to 0.82.
3.2. The MACH-IV (Christie & Geis, 1970)
The original MACH scale consisted of 71 items in three categories: (1) interpersonal tactics; (2)
views of human nature; and (3) abstract or generalised morality. The scale was subsequently reduced to 60 meaningful items, from which the 10 highest-related items worded in the Machiavellian direction were selected into the scale along with the 10 highest-related items worded in the
opposite direction to produce the MACH-IV. Responses are given to items on a 6-point Likert
scale, ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Although the reliability of the scale
has been questioned (Ray, 1983) more recent studies have found good reliabilities, with split-half
reliabilities based on several samples averaging 0.79 (Hansen & Hansen, 1991; Wrightsman, 1991)
and the MACH-IV is now the most widely used tool to measure the construct (McHoskey et al.,
1998).
3.3. The Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (LSRP; Levenson et al., 1995)
The LSRP is based on the two-factor interpretation of the PCL-R structure and is designed to
measure psychopathy in the general population. Responses are given on a 4-point Likert scale.
The 16-item primary psychopathy scale measures callous, selsh and manipulative interpersonal
attitudes, while the 10-item secondary psychopathy scale assesses impulsivity and a self-defeating
lifestyle. Rather than examine criminal activity typical of the person high on the second dimension
of the PCL-R, the LSRP elicits information about behaviours more typical of community life
which may be morally oensive but are not illegal. Cronbachs a is 0.82 for primary psychopathy,
0.63 for secondary psychopathy. The scale is valid and reliable (Brinkley, Schmitt, Smith, & Newman, 2001; Lynam, Whiteside, & Jones, 1999; McHoskey et al., 1998).
3.4. Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Hall, 1979)
The NPI is a 40-item forced choice instrument, used to measure narcissism in non-clinical populations. Responses are scored positively; thus, the higher the score, the greater the narcissism

S. Jakobwitz, V. Egan / Personality and Individual Dierences 40 (2006) 331339

335

shown by the subject. Of the various instruments that measure narcissism, the NPI has received
the most rigorous scrutiny for its psychometric properties. Construct validity has been demonstrated for the instrument with other measures of narcissism (Emmons, 1984; Raskin & Terry,
1988), and the inventory has high internal consistency, with alphas ranging from 0.82 to 0.84.

4. Statistical analysis
Correlations were calculated between scores on the dark triad measures and the NEO-FFI-R.
The reliability of these measures was calculated using Cronbachs alpha. To simplify the relationships between measures of personality and the dark triad, and to examine whether all three scales
of the dark triad reected the same underlying construct, principal components analysis with
Varimax rotation of the derived factors was calculated.

5. Results
Table 1 presents means, standard deviations and internal reliabilities for the measures used in
the study. Reliabilities were adequate to good, but some measures clearly had greater internal reliability than others (e.g. N and narcissism vs. secondary psychopathy). While not a main hypothesis, independent sample t-tests examined whether there were any sex dierences for the measures.
Results showed that males were higher than women for C (t(79) = 2.79; p < 0.05), whereas females
were signicantly higher than males for E (t(79) = 3.37; p < 0.05), and A (t(79) = 2.56;
p < 0.05).
Table 2 presents the correlations between all measures in the study. The subscales of the NEOFFI-R were much less correlated when the NEO-FFI was used as a short-form of the Big Five.
Even though the negative correlations between O and A, and N and C were statistically signicant, they were modest in magnitude. The MACH-IV correlated strongly with primary psychopathy and secondary psychopathy, and moderately with narcissism. Primary psychopathy
and narcissism also correlated positively. To test the claim that MACH is a global measure
of psychopathy (McHoskey et al., 1998), a partial correlation between MACH and secondary
Table 1
Descriptive statistics and reliabilities of scales used in study
Neuroticism
Extraversion
Openness
Agreeableness
Conscientiousness
Primary psychopathy
Secondary psychopathy
Machiavellianism
Narcissism

Mean

SD

Cronbachs a

24.8
28.6
32.3
27.6
29.5
31.4
22.0
55.9
16.4

8.4
5.1
6.3
5.7
6.5
6.7
3.8
7.7
6.8

.85
.60
.71
.65
.79
.80
.60
.69
.85

336

S. Jakobwitz, V. Egan / Personality and Individual Dierences 40 (2006) 331339

Table 2
Correlations for the dark triad and NEO-FFI-R subscales (n = 82)
E
Neuroticism
Extraversion (E)
Openness (O)
Agreeableness (A)
Conscientiousness (C)

O
.04

A
.11
.01

C
.02
.15
.23*

PP
.27
.19
.15
.08

P. Psychopathy (PP)
S. Psychopathy (SP)
Machiavellianism (M)
Narcissism (Nar)
*
**

SP
**

.30
.08
.21
.43**
.21

Nar

**

.47
.04
.21
.23*
.19

**

.38
.13
.17
.41**
.27*

.10
.10
.10
.43**
.24*

.49**

.70**
.52**

.40**
.18
.36**

Correlation is signicant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).


Correlation is signicant at the 0.05 level (two-tailed).

psychopathy was calculated, controlling for the eects of primary psychopathy. This correlation
was found to be only .28, suggesting that the correlation of 0.52 found between the MACH-IV
and secondary psychopathy is largely attributable to shared variance between primary and secondary psychopathy. Finally, signicant negative correlations were found between the MACHIV, primary psychopathy, secondary psychopathy, narcissism and A, and signicant positive
correlations between primary psychopathy, secondary psychopathy, the MACH-IV and N. Lower
C was associated with higher scores on the MACH-IV and narcissism. In contrast to previous
ndings (e.g., Paulhus & Williams, 2002), none of the dark triad constructs was signicantly correlated to either O or E.
Finally, in order to clarify the relationship patterns found in the correlations between the NEOFFI-R and the measures of the dark triad and to identify possible latent constructs total scores of
the nine scales were subjected to a factor analysis, using varimax rotation. Four factors with an
Eigenvalue greater than 1 were extracted, with convergence occurring in nine iterations. Table 3
Table 3
Principal components analysis (with Varimax rotation; loadings less than 0.4 suppressed)
F1
Agreeableness
Primary Psychopathy
Secondary Psychopathy
Machiavellianism
Narcissism
Neuroticism
Conscientiousness
Openness
Extraversion
Eigenvalue
Variance (%)
Total variance = 74.7%

.69
.80
.54
.77
.76

F2

F3

F4

.52

.80
.71
.96
.98
2.6
29.9

2.6
19.9

1.2
13.7

1.1
12.2

S. Jakobwitz, V. Egan / Personality and Individual Dierences 40 (2006) 331339

337

shows loadings for each scale on the relevant factor as well as the variance explained by the factor.
Together, these factors account for 74.7% of the variance. The rst rotated factor contained all the
measures of the dark triad, as well as A, conrming our hypothesis. The second factor contrasted
a high positive loading for secondary psychopathy (which also had a split loading on the rst factor) and N, and a high negative loading for C. O and E both loaded on separate factors and were
found to be entirely unrelated to any of the constructs underlying the measures of the dark triad.
These ndings suggest that the dark triad is essentially unitary and associated with low A, whereas
secondary psychopathy has unique variance unrelated to the constructs underlying the dark triad,
and is associated with high N and low C.

6. Discussion
The current study examined the relationship between the constructs of the dark triad and how
they tted into the ve factor space of personality. Previous studies indicated that there is considerable overlap between MACH-IV, primary psychopathy, secondary psychopathy and narcissism,
and that more specically that MACH-IV is a global measure of psychopathy (McHoskey et al.,
1998). This could reect the varied nature of the MACH-IV, which contains items measuring dispositions as well as behaviours, thus perhaps over-integrating the two factors of psychopathy.
While McHoskey et al. found support for their argument their ndings were not replicated by
the current study which had a more dierentiated measure of psychopathy. A correlation between
the MACH-IV and secondary psychopathy, controlling for the eects of primary psychopathy
produced a much weaker correlation (0.28), suggesting that the initial correlation could be attributed to the shared variance between primary and secondary psychopathy, rather than that shared
between MACH and secondary psychopathy.
As predicted, we found the dark triad reduces to a large general factor and a secondary factor
associated with secondary psychopathy, high N and low C. Also as predicted, there were signicant and systematic correlations between the scales of the dark triad and the A dimension of
the NEO-FFI-R. Comparable with conceptions of secondary psychopaths as essentially criminal,
neurotic and disorganised (Blackburn, 1975; Levenson et al., 1995), we found secondary psychopathy correlated positively with N and negatively with C. Predictions made about a negative correlation between C and the dark triad were not upheld, perhaps because, at least in normal
samples, C is more associated with secondary psychopathy. These ndings are comparable with
those found by Ross et al. (2004).
It must be emphasised that these ndings reect participants recruited from the general community, and not from criminal or psychiatric settings. As such, the results suggest that patterns
of association seen in more exclusively forensic or mentally disordered populations can also be
found in normal samples, vindicating the view that pathology associated with personality is
dimensional, and the extreme of normal characteristics, with no obvious discontinuity (Egan,
Austin, Elliot, Patel, & Charlesworth, 2003).
Moreover it suggests that studies into psychopathy are valid for non-oender samples, expediting researchers who do not have access to specialist samples. We found neither O or E contributed signicantly to any aspect of the dark triad, and provide further negative evidence for the
long-hypothesised but often erratic contributory inuence of E on anti-social behaviour.

338

S. Jakobwitz, V. Egan / Personality and Individual Dierences 40 (2006) 331339

Secondary psychopathy had a split loading with the other variables in the study, and this may
reect the fact that although it ts into the dark dimension of personality, it indexes anti-social
attitudes and lifestyle at a behavioural rather than philosophical or dispositional level. High N
and low C tap into this behaviour, as they reect a more changeable and impulsive nature. We
speculate that anti-social behaviourlegal or illegalresults from a lack of impulse control
and a lack of planning as well as a sense of entitlement, which demands immediate gratication
of needs, no matter what the consequences are, and that low A is a contributory (but not cardinal)
dimension directing secondary psychopathy.
Our sample was relatively small, and from the general population. While these can be raised as
criticisms of the study, our ndings are quite unambiguous and reect, replicate and extend other
ndings using similar paradigms. One interesting and novel nding is that our mean scores on the
MACH-IV were higher than for studies 2025 years ago (e.g. Nigro & Galli, 1985; Smith & Grifth, 1978). Recent scores were consistently high and uninuenced by sample size. We speculate
that modern western society is much more competitive and materialistic than even 20 years
ago, and some degree of apparent psychopathy may be necessary to succeed in this type of society.
Anonymity leads to a diminished sense of communal responsibility and as long as the concept of
hurting another is abstract (i.e. society) vs. specic (i.e. my neighbour or colleague) persons might
more readily be prepared to behave in such a way (Gupta, 1986; Okanes & Murray, 1982).
In sum, the current study shows that the dark triad of personalitypsychopathy, Machiavellianism and narcissismreect an essentially unitary construct, and that the division of psychopathy into primary and secondary psychopathy usefully dierentiates normal personality traits
associated with the more unpleasant features of the self. This dierentiation could be made in
an unselected sample of persons from the general population. Our study contributes to the view
that it is perhaps unhelpful to overly dierentiate the elements for the dark triad when they so
closely overlap with one-another.
References
Ames, M., & Kidd, A. H. (1979). Machiavellianism and womens grade point average. Psychological Reports, 44,
223228.
Barnett, M. A., & Thompson, S. (1985). The role of perspective taking and empathy in childrens Machiavellianism,
prosocial behaviour and motive for helping. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 146, 295305.
Blackburn, R. (1975). An empirical classication of psychopathic personality. British Journal of Psychiatry, 127,
456460.
Board, B. J., & Fritzon, K. (2005). Disordered personalities at work. Psychology, Crime and Law, 11, 1732.
Bradlee, P. M., & Emmons, R. A. (1991). Locating narcissism within the personal circumplex and the ve factor model.
Personality and Individual Dierences, 13, 821830.
Brinkley, C. A., Schmitt, W. A., Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (2001). Construct validation of a self-report
psychopathy scale: Does Levensons self-report psychopathy scale measure the same constructs as Hares
Psychopathy Checklist-Revised?. Personality and Individual Dierences 31, 10211038.
Cherulnik, P. D., Way, J. H., Ames, S., & Hutto, D. B. (1981). Impressions of high and low Machiavellian men. Journal
of Personality, 49, 388400.
Christie, R., & Geis, F. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press.
Cleckley, H. (1988). The mask of sanity (5th ed.). Augusta, GA: Emily S. Cleckley (Original work published 1941).
Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (2001). Rening the construct of psychopath: towards a hierarchical model. Psychological
Assessment, 13, 171188.

S. Jakobwitz, V. Egan / Personality and Individual Dierences 40 (2006) 331339

339

Egan, V., Austin, E., Elliot, D., Patel, D., & Charlesworth, P. (2003). Personality traits, personality disorders and
sensational interests in mentally disordered oenders. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 8, 5162.
Egan, V., Deary, I., & Austin, E. (2000). The NEO-FFI: emerging British norms and an item-level analysis suggest N, A
and C are more reliable than E and O. Personality and Individual Dierences, 29, 907920.
Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the Narcissistic personality inventory. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 48, 291305.
Fehr, B., Samson, D., & Paulhus, D. L. (1992). The construct of Machiavellianism: twenty years later. In C. D.
Spielberger & J. M. Butcher (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (pp. 77116). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Freud, S. (1914). On Narcissism: an introduction. In Complete psychological works (pp. 3059). London: Hogarth Press.
Gupta, M. D. (1986). Eects of age and family structure on Machiavellianism. Indian Journal of Current Psychological
Research, 32, 95100.
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1991). Constructing personality and social reality through music: individual
dierences among fans. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 35, 335350.
Hare, R. D. (1985). Comparison of procedures for the assessment of psychopathy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 53, 716.
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare psychopathy checklist revised. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
Hunt, S. D., & Chonto, L. B. (1984). Marketing and Machiavellianism. Journal of Marketing, 48, 3042.
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International Universities Press.
Levenson, M. R. (1992). Rethinking psychopathy. Theory and Psychology, 2, 5171.
Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalised
population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 151158.
Lykken, D. T. (1995). Antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lynam, D. R., Whiteside, S., & Jones, S. (1999). Self-reported psychopathy: a validation study. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 73, 110132.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2004). A contemplated revision of the NEO ve factor inventory. Personality and
Individual Dierences, 36, 587596.
McHoskey, J. (1995). Narcissism and Machiavellianism. Psychological Reports, 77, 757759.
McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 74, 192210.
Millon, T., & Davis, R. D. (1996). Disorders of personality: DSM-IV and beyond. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Nigro, G., & Galli, I. (1985). On the relationship between Machiavellianism and anxiety among Italian undergraduates.
Psychological Reports, 56, 10811082.
Okanes, M. M., & Murray, L. W. (1982). Machiavellianism and achievement orientations among foreign and American
masters students of business administration. Psychological Reports, 46, 783785.
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: narcissism, Machiavellianism and
psychopathy. Journal of Research in personality, 36, 556563.
Raskin, R., & Hall, T. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological Reports, 45, 590.
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal components analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory and further
evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890902.
Ray, J. J. (1983). Defective validity of the Machiavelianism scale. The Journal of Social Psychology, 119, 291292.
Ross, S. R., Lutz, C. J., & Bailley, S. E. (2004). Psychopathy and the ve factor model in a noninstitutionalized sample:
a domain and facet level analysis. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 213223.
Skinner, N. F. (1988). Personality correlates of Machiavellianism and psychopaths. Social Behavior and Personality, 16,
3337.
Smith, R. J., & Grith, J. E. (1978). Psychopathy, the Machiavellian and Anomie. Psychological Reports, 42, 258.
Watson, P. J., & Morris, R. J. (1991). Narcissism, empathy and social desirability. Personality and Individual
Dierences, 12, 575579.
Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, L. A. (1989). Conceptions of personality disorders and dimensions of personality.
Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Assessment, 1, 305316.
Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Interpersonal trust and attitudes toward human nature. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L.
S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 373412). San Diego: Academic
Press.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai