Anda di halaman 1dari 8

American Journal of Epidemiology

The Author 2011. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of
Public Health. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.

Vol. 174, No. 5


DOI: 10.1093/aje/kwr143
Advance Access publication:
July 12, 2011

Practice of Epidemiology
Performance of Disease Risk Scores, Propensity Scores, and Traditional
Multivariable Outcome Regression in the Presence of Multiple Confounders

Patrick G. Arbogast* and Wayne A. Ray


* Correspondence to Dr. Patrick G. Arbogast, Department of Biostatistics, S-2323 Medical Center North, Vanderbilt University,
Nashville, TN 37232-2158 (e-mail: patrick.arbogast@vanderbilt.edu).

Propensity scores are widely used in cohort studies to improve performance of regression models when considering large numbers of covariates. Another type of summary score, the disease risk score (DRS), which
estimates disease probability conditional on nonexposure, has also been suggested. However, little is known
about how it compares with propensity scores. Monte Carlo simulations were conducted comparing regression
models using the DRS and the propensity score with models that directly adjust for all of the individual covariates.
The DRS was calculated in 2 ways: from the unexposed population and from the full cohort. Compared with traditional
multivariable outcome regression models, all 3 summary scores had comparable performance for moderate correlation between exposure and covariates and, for strong correlation, the full-cohort DRS and propensity score had
comparable performance. When traditional methods had model misspecication, propensity scores and the fullcohort DRS had superior performance. All 4 models were affected by the number of events per covariate, with
propensity scores and traditional multivariable outcome regression least affected. These data suggest that, for
cohort studies for which covariates are not highly correlated with exposure, the DRS, particularly that calculated
from the full cohort, is a useful tool.
confounding factors (epidemiology); epidemiologic methods; observational study; pharmacoepidemiology

Abbreviations: DRS, disease risk score; NSAID, nonsteroidal antiinammatory drug.

3 dose categories, the exposure has 30 levels (9 3 3 levels


of current use, 1 for recent use, 1 for former use, and 1 for
nonuse). Even with modern computing capacity, models with
130 or more covariates will require substantial computing
power and analyses may be cumbersome, particularly if
there are dependencies (e.g., allowing an individual to enter
the cohort multiple times) that require more complex variance estimation. This, in turn, may inhibit performance of
important sensitivity analyses.
Second is the question of model specification. Because
some of the covariates may not be confounders, variable
selection procedures may be considered to improve exposure estimate precision and to reduce computational complexity. However, this may involve subjective decisions such
as the type of variable selection procedure, whether to base
selection upon P values or change in exposure parameter
estimates, and numeric cutoffs (e.g., P 0.05, 0.10, 0.20)

In contemporary cohort studies, there frequently are a


large number of covariates that must be considered as potential confounders. The increasing use of automated databases, in which past medical encounters (e.g., a prescription
for insulin) provide surrogates for important patient comorbidities (e.g., diabetes), has contributed to this trend. Indeed,
it is not uncommon to have 100 or more covariates available.
The standard method to control for such confounding is to
fit multiple regression models with terms for the exposure
variable of primary interest and each of the potential confounders. However, this approach poses 4 important challenges for statistical analysis. The first is computational
complexity, dependent upon the number of covariates and
exposure categories. Consider a study of nonsteroidal antiinflamatory drugs (NSAIDs) with 100 covariates. If there
are 9 individual drugs of interest (6 traditional NSAIDs and
3 newer selective inhibitors of cyclooxygenase-2), each with
613

Am J Epidemiol. 2011;174(5):613620

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on February 29, 2016

Initially submitted August 19, 2010; accepted for publication April 8, 2011.

614 Arbogast and Ray

was calculated. The disease risk score also may be estimated


as an unexposed-only DRS, from a regression model fit
only for the unexposed population, with the fitted values
then computed for the entire cohort.
However, use of the disease risk score was inhibited by an
early study of its statistical properties. Pike et al. (9) examined the full-cohort DRS theoretically and in simulation
studies and demonstrated an exaggeration of the statistical
significance of the exposure estimate. Cook and Goldman
(10) reexamined the score and, in particular, revisited the
simulation study conducted by Pike et al. Cook and Goldman
agreed with the theoretical arguments of Pike et al. and
expanded their simulation study to include a propensity
score analysis. They observed an exaggeration of statistical
significance, similar to that reported by Pike et al. However,
this exaggeration was most pronounced when the squared
multiple correlation coefficient between exposure and confounders exceeded 90%, which is unlikely to occur in practice. When the correlation between exposure and confounders
was lower, there was little exaggeration of statistical significance. The propensity score analysis was more robust in the
presence of large exposure-confounder correlations.
In recent years, interest in disease risk scores has increased. We have used these scores in several pharmacoepidemiologic studies (1117), motivated by the need to
reduce computational complexity in large cohort studies
with numerous covariates, as well as to assess effect modification. In 2005, Sturmer et al. (18) compared several
approaches to controlling confounding in a real world
example that assessed NSAIDs and 1-year all-cause mortality in 103,133 elderly Medicaid beneficiaries, using 71 covariates to control for confounding. They compared several
propensity score analyses, the disease risk score, and traditional multiple regression models and observed no major
difference in any of these methods. We recently reported
simulation studies comparing regression models using the
unexposed-only DRS with models adjusting for the individual covariates (6). These were designed to resemble a recently
published study of NSAIDs and risk of acute myocardial
infarction/sudden cardiac death, with the exception that
we simulated cohort data instead of case-control samples
(16). So long as there was not a high degree of intercorrelation between confounders and exposure, the unexposedonly DRS was a reasonable approach for summarizing risk
factors from large cohort studies.
Hansen (19) recently examined the theoretical properties
of the disease risk score (termed the prognostic score), demonstrating that it is a balancing score. Hansen noted some
practical difficulties and additional assumptions required
with use of the estimated prognostic score, particularly the
possible need to estimate the score in the unexposed group
and the difficulty of assessing prognostic balance between
exposed and unexposed. He noted that, if the prognostic
score is estimated in the full cohort, with exposure in the
model, and the exposure increases the outcome, then the estimated prognostic score will tend to be a mixture of the true
propensity and prognostic scores that can obscure the exposure effect. Hansen also noted that if a covariate is
strongly associated with exposure, then applying a prognostic score to the same sample used to estimate it could result
Am J Epidemiol. 2011;174(5):613620

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on February 29, 2016

for variable inclusion. For covariates that confer relatively


modest increases in disease risk, some variable selection
procedures, such as stepwise regression, may exclude important covariates from the final model. Furthermore, such
techniques as stepwise regression have limitations that can
lead to underestimation of standard errors for exposure
estimates (1).
Third, if the number of outcome events under study is
small, parameter estimates based upon large sample theory,
such as those provided by widely used regression programs,
may be inappropriate. Peduzzi et al. (2) conducted simulation studies evaluating events per variable in logistic regression; they observed that, for fewer than 10 events per
variable, regression coefficients could be biased and standard errors could be incorrect (2). Harrell et al. (3) reported
similar findings.
A final challenge is the question of effect modification.
Investigators are nearly always interested in whether or not
the effect of the exposure varies for patients according to
baseline risk of the study outcome. However, with more than
100 variables that measure this factor, the effect modification analyses may be cumbersome.
An increasingly popular approach to these challenges is
to calculate a single summary measure from the covariates
and then to use this summary measure in subsequent statistical analysis. At present, the most common approach to
reducing the dimensionality of models is propensity scores
(4). For dichotomous exposure variables, this score is calculated as the probability that each subject is exposed, as
a function of his/her observed covariates. Rosenbaum and
Rubin (4) demonstrated that the propensity score is a balancing score, meaning that, conditional on the propensity score,
the distribution of the covariates used to derive the propensity score is the same for exposed and unexposed persons.
Cepeda et al. (5) conducted simulation studies comparing
multiple logistic regression models adjusting for the individual covariates with propensity score analyses. They reported that the propensity score was a good alternative when
there were 7 or fewer events per covariate; exposure estimates had less bias, were more robust, and were more precise. However, if there were at least 8 events per covariate,
logistic regression was preferable; in the corresponding propensity score analyses, coverage probabilities decreased and
exposure effect estimates had greater bias.
The disease risk score is an alternative approach to the
propensity score (6, 7). Like the propensity score, it is a summary measure derived from the observed values of the
covariates. However, the disease risk score estimates the
probability or rate of disease occurrence as a function of
the covariates. The disease risk score may be estimated in
2 ways. First, it can be calculated as a full-cohort disease
risk score (DRS), which is the multivariate confounder
score originally proposed by Miettinen (8) in 1976. This
score was constructed from a regression model relating
the study outcome to the exposure of interest and the covariates for the entire study population. The score was then
computed as the fitted value from that regression model
for each study subject, setting the exposure status to unexposed. The subjects were then grouped into strata according
to the score, and a stratified estimate of the exposure effect

Performance Comparisons of Four Models as Tools

in an increased type I error rate that did not occur when


estimating the prognostic score in a separate sample and
applying it.
In this paper, we present simulation studies comparing the
unexposed-only DRS, full-cohort DRS, and propensity
score analyses with traditional regression models that include
all of the individual covariates used to construct these summary scores. We sought to assess performance for 3 types of
scenarios: 1) those typical of practical applications with an
adequate number of events per confounder, 2) those in
which traditional methods might be subject to the effects
of model misspecification, and 3) those with small numbers
of events per confounder.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Scenarios and simulations

Adequate events per covariate, traditional model correctly


specied. The first scenario compared performance of the

summary scores under favorable conditions for traditional


regression methods: an adequate number of events per covariate and the traditional regression model correctly specified.
There were 10 independent binary covariates (reflecting applications in which these are indicators of medications or
comorbidities), each with a prevalence of 10%. Prior work
suggests that the performance of the disease risk score is
affected by both the correlation between exposure and confounders and the degree of confounding present (10). The
simulations thus varied both of these factors over a broad
range. In one setting, we set the covariate-exposure odds
ratios and covariate-disease rate ratios to 1.25 for all 10
covariates and simulated data with the exposure-disease rate
ratio of 1.25. We then considered exposure-disease rate
ratios of 1.5, 2.0, and 3.0. We then increased the covariateexposure rate ratio to 2.0 and simulated data for the 4 different exposure-disease rate ratios. We repeated this with the
covariate-exposure rate ratio set to 1.25 and the covariatedisease rate ratio set to 2.0. We repeated this again with both
Am J Epidemiol. 2011;174(5):613620

the covariate-exposure and covariate-disease rate ratios set


to 2.0.
Adequate events per covariate, traditional model
misspecied. The second scenario assessed the effect of

model misspecification that can occur when regression models


are constructed with traditional variable selection methods
based upon statistical significance. These simulations compared this method with the alternative approach of using
all measured covariates to calculate the summary scores.
The covariates were simulated by using the same set-up as
described in the previous section, except that the covariatedisease rate ratios were set to 1.0 for 2 covariates, 1.25 for
6 covariates, and 1.5 for 2 covariates, and the covariateexposure odds ratios were set to 1.25 for all 10 covariates.
These values reflect the commonly encountered circumstance
in which many of the covariates are modestly or not associated with the disease. We further assumed that the traditional variable selection methods resulted in models that
included only the 2 covariates with covariate-disease rate
ratios of 1.5. The summary scores were calculated from all
of the potential covariates. We then repeated this setting the
covariate-exposure odds ratio to 2.0 for all 10 covariates.
Limited number of events per covariate. The third scenario assumed limited numbers of events per covariate.
There were 10 binary covariates (prevalence of 10%), and
covariate-exposure odds ratios and covariate-disease rate
ratios were set to 1.25 for all 10 covariates (reflecting the
moderate degree of confounding typical for many applications), with an exposure-disease rate ratio of 2.0. Baseline
disease probability was varied so that the expected number
of events per covariate ranged from 12 to greater than 20.
Propensity and disease risk scores

The propensity score was estimated by using logistic regression with the exposure as the dependent variable and the
covariates as the independent variables. The estimated propensity score is then the fitted value from this regression
model, that is, the estimated probability of being exposed
conditional on each study subjects individual covariate
values.
The unexposed-only DRS, or probability of disease occurrence in the absence of the exposure, was estimated with
Poisson regression in the unexposed group. The dependent
variable was the disease, and the independent variables were
the covariates. We estimated the full-cohort DRS using the
entire cohort with exposure status set to unexposed for all
simulated subjects.
Model-tting approaches

For each simulated sample, the data were fit by using


4 different Poisson regression models. We first fit the exposure and all of the individual covariates in a single regression model, which was designated the traditional model.
The remaining models fit the exposure and propensity score,
the exposure and unexposed-only DRS, and the exposure
and full-cohort DRS. The scores were expressed as linear
terms. We also ran simulations expressing the summary
scores as deciles, and the findings were similar.

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on February 29, 2016

All simulations assume a binary exposure, a binary disease outcome, and a fixed period of follow-up with no censoring. The values of the covariates were first determined to
achieve the expected prevalence specified for each scenario.
Once the covariates were specified, the exposure status was
simulated by using a logistic model to determine exposure
probability, with the baseline exposure prevalence (all covariate values zero) specified according to that scenario.
Once both the covariates and exposure status were determined, the disease outcome was simulated via a Poisson
model, by use of the baseline disease rate per person (covariates and exposure zero) for that scenario. The reason for
using a Poisson model instead of a logistic model is due to
the noncollapsibility of the odds ratio, which is an issue for
the propensity score (20, 21). Unless otherwise specified,
the baseline exposure prevalence was 10%, and the baseline
disease rate was 0.01 per person.
For each simulation setting, 1,000 samples were generated, each consisting of 10,000 study subjects. Simulations
were conducting by using SAS, version 9.1, software (SAS
Institute, Inc., Cary, North Carolina).

615

616 Arbogast and Ray

B)
10

10

% Bias

% Bias

A)

4
2

4
2

0
1.25

1.5

1.25

1.5

Rate Ratio

Rate Ratio
D)
10

% Bias

10

4
2

4
2

0
1.25

1.5

1.25

Rate Ratio

1.5

Rate Ratio

Figure 1. Percent bias (AD) under different degrees of confounding when there are an adequate number of events per covariate and the
traditional model is correctly specied. The x-axis is the exposure-disease rate ratio. Solid curve, traditional multiple outcome regression; shortdash curve, disease risk score (DRS)unexposed only; long-dash curve, DRSfull cohort; dash-3 dots-dash curve, propensity score. In part A,
the covariate-exposure odds ratios and covariate-disease rate ratios are 1.25; in part B, the covariate-exposure odds ratios are 1.25 and the
covariate-disease rate ratios are 2.0; in part C, the covariate-exposure odds ratios are 2.0 and the covariate-disease rate ratios are 1.25; and in part
D, the covariate-exposure odds ratios and covariate-disease rate ratios are 2.0. The scenario included 10 binary covariates, each with a prevalence
of 10%. Baseline exposure prevalence was 10%, and baseline disease probability was 0.01. The mean standard errors and standard error
estimates were similar for all 4 regression models, as was empirical power.

Evaluation strategies

The primary measures of performance were the percent


bias and coverage probabilities. Percent bias was the average difference between the estimated and true regression
coefficients for the exposure divided by the true regression
coefficient times 100%. Coverage probabilities were the
proportion of the 95% confidence intervals that include
the true regression coefficient. If a method has proper coverage, then for a 95% confidence interval, the coverage
should be very close to 0.95. Secondary measures included
the mean standard error of the estimated regression coefficient for the exposure, as well as the standard error estimate
from the simulated samples. The standard error estimate is
the standard deviation of the estimated regression coefficients from the simulated samples, and the mean standard
errors should approximate the standard error estimates.
Finally, the empirical power was estimated by dividing the

exposures estimated regression coefficient by its standard


error and tabulating the proportion of simulations in which
this ratio exceeded 1.96, the upper 97.5 percentile for a
2-tailed 0.05-level test. Given that there is a true association
between the exposure and disease, values closer to 1 are
desirable.

RESULTS
Adequate events per covariate, traditional model
correctly specied

Figures 1 and 2 graphically depict percent bias and coverage probabilities, respectively, under the different degrees
of confounding likely to be encountered in practice for scenarios with an adequate number of events per confounder.
When there was a moderate association between covariates
Am J Epidemiol. 2011;174(5):613620

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on February 29, 2016

% Bias

C)

Performance Comparisons of Four Models as Tools

A)

617

B)
0.10
1 Coverage Probability

1 Coverage Probability

0.10
0.08
0.06
0.04
0.02
0.00

0.08
0.06
0.04
0.02
0.00

1.25

1.5

1.25

1.5

Rate Ratio

Rate Ratio

C)

D)

1 Coverage Probability

0.10

0.08
0.06
0.04
0.02
0.00

0.08
0.06
0.04
0.02
0.00

1.25

1.5

Rate Ratio

1.25

1.5

Rate Ratio

Figure 2. Coverage probabilities (AD) under different degrees of confounding when there are an adequate number of events per covariate and
the traditional model is correctly specied. Solid curve, traditional multiple outcome regression; short-dash curve, disease risk score (DRS)
unexposed only; long-dash curve, DRSfull cohort; dash-3 dots-dash curve, propensity score. In part A, the covariate-exposure odds ratios and
covariate-disease rate ratios are 1.25; in part B, the covariate-exposure odds ratios are 1.25 and the covariate-disease rate ratios are 2.0; in part C,
the covariate-exposure odds ratios are 2.0 and the covariate-disease rate ratios are 1.25; in part D, the covariate-exposure odds ratios and
covariate-disease rate ratios are 2.0.

and exposure (i.e., covariate-exposure odds ratio of 1.25), all


4 models had little bias. Performance was comparable except
when there was a moderate association between covariates
and disease (i.e., covariate-disease rate ratio of 1.25) in which
the unexposed-only DRS model consistently had slightly
greater bias. However, this bias never exceeded 3%. Coverage
was consistent across all 4 models. When there were strong
correlation between covariates and exposure and moderate
association between covariates and disease (i.e., covariateexposure odds ratio of 2.0 and covariate-disease rate ratio
of 1.25), all models except for the unexposed-only DRS
had little bias. The unexposed-only DRS model consistently
had greater bias with range 6%10%. All models had proper
and consistent coverage except for the unexposed-only DRS.
When the covariates were strongly associated with both
exposure and disease (i.e., rate ratios of 2.0), the traditional
regression and full-cohort DRS models had little bias. The
propensity score model had greater bias, but it was consistently less than 3%. The unexposed-only DRS model had
Am J Epidemiol. 2011;174(5):613620

the greatest bias, but it was still consistently less than 5%. All
4 models had proper coverage.
Adequate events per covariate, traditional model
misspecied

Figure 3 graphically depicts percent bias and coverage


probabilities when the traditional model was misspecified.
For both moderate and strong associations between covariates and exposure, the propensity score and full-cohort DRS
models had little bias. The unexposed-only DRS models had
greater bias, although for moderate associations between
covariates and exposure, bias was consistently less than
3%. Not surprisingly, the misspecified traditional model
had the greatest bias, although this bias was consistently
less than 5% when the covariates and exposure were moderately correlated. All 4 models had comparable coverage
for moderate associations between covariates and exposure.
For strong associations between covariates and exposure,

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on February 29, 2016

1 Coverage Probability

0.10

618 Arbogast and Ray

B)
12

12

10

10

8
% Bias

% Bias

A)

0
1.25

1.5

1.25

1.5

Rate Ratio

Rate Ratio

C)

D)

1 Coverage Probability

0.10

0.08
0.06
0.04
0.02
0.00

0.08
0.06
0.04
0.02
0.00

1.25

1.5

Rate Ratio

1.25

1.5

Rate Ratio

Figure 3. Percent bias (A and B) and coverage probabilities (C and D) under different degrees of confounding when there are an adequate
number of events per covariate and the traditional model is misspecied. Solid curve, traditional multiple outcome regression; short-dash curve,
disease risk score (DRS)unexposed only; long-dash curve, DRSfull cohort; dash-3 dots-dash curve, propensity score. In parts A and C, the
covariate-exposure odds ratios are 1.25; in parts B and D, the covariate-exposure odds ratios are 2.0. The scenario included 10 binary covariates,
each with a prevalence of 10%. The covariate-disease rate ratios were set to 1.0 for 2 covariates, 1.25 for 6 covariates, and 1.5 for 2 covariates.
Traditional variable selection methods resulted in models that included only the 2 covariates with disease rate ratios of 1.5. Disease risk scores and
the propensity score were calculated from all of the potential covariates. Baseline exposure prevalence was 10%, and baseline and disease
probability was 0.01.

the propensity score and full-cohort DRS models had proper


coverage, whereas the unexposed-only DRS and misspecified traditional models had poorer coverage.
Limited number of events per covariate

Figure 4 graphically depicts percent bias and coverage


probabilities for scenarios with limited numbers of events
per covariate. All models performed well so long as there
were more than 5 events per covariate. For smaller numbers
of events, bias increased for all 4 models, with the unexposedonly DRS model having the greatest increase in bias. The
other models were remarkably comparable. Coverage was
comparable across all 4 models.
DISCUSSION

We assessed the performance of the increasingly popular


practice of replacing individual covariates with summary

scores in regression modeling for observational cohort studies. We thus ran simulations comparing traditional models
with all of the covariates versus those that included either
the propensity score or disease risk score derived from either
the full cohort or from only unexposed patients.
When there was an adequate number of events per confounder and when the covariates were moderately associated with the exposure, all 4 models were comparable and
performed well. When covariates were strongly associated
with both exposure and disease, the full-cohort DRS and
traditional models performed best, followed by the propensity score model, and lastly by the unexposed-only DRS
model. However, the unexposed-only DRS model still had
reasonable performance. When the covariates were strongly
associated with exposure and moderately with disease, all of
the models except the unexposed-only DRS model performed
well. In this setting, the unexposed-only DRS model had
greater bias and poor coverage. The sensitivity of the performance of the unexposed-only DRS to covariates strongly
Am J Epidemiol. 2011;174(5):613620

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on February 29, 2016

1 Coverage Probability

0.10

Performance Comparisons of Four Models as Tools

A)
20

% Bias

15

10

0
12

35

610

1120

>20

Events per Confounder


B)

0.08
0.06
0.04
0.02
0.00
12

35

610

1120

>20

Events per Confounder


Figure 4. Percent bias (A) and coverage probabilities (B) for limited
numbers of events per covariate. Solid curve, traditional multiple
outcome regression; short-dash curve, disease risk score (DRS)
unexposed only; long-dash curve, DRSfull cohort; dash-3 dots-dash
curve, propensity score. The scenario included 10 binary covariates
(prevalence of 10%), with covariate-exposure odds ratios and covariatedisease rate ratios of 1.25. Baseline exposure prevalence was 10%,
and the exposure-disease rate ratio was 2.0. Baseline disease probability varied so that the expected number of events per covariate
ranged from 12 to greater than 20.

associated with the exposure is consistent with what has


been reported elsewhere (7). Hence, for adequate numbers
of events per confounder, the full-cohort DRS and traditional regression models consistently performed the best.
The propensity score models had similar performance or were
at least comparable depending on the associations among
covariates, exposure, and disease, and the unexposed-only
DRS models were comparable when the covariates were
moderately associated with exposure.
As expected, traditional models performed poorly when
the model was misspecified because of exclusion of covariates only weakly associated with the disease outcome. The
magnitude of the underperformance was affected by the
strength of the association between covariates and exposure.
Also, as expected, the performance of the unexposed-only
DRS was affected by the covariate-exposure association. In
contrast, both the propensity score and full-cohort DRS performed well, with little bias and good coverage probabilities.
Am J Epidemiol. 2011;174(5):613620

All 4 models were affected by the numbers of events per


confounder. Bias increased for all 4 models as the number of
events per confounder decreased, with the DRS models
most affected by the number of events per confounder.
The traditional regression and propensity score models were
remarkably consistent, which could reflect the robustness of
traditional regression (directly adjusting for all covariates in
the exposure-outcome model) and/or the limited advantages
of propensity score models in the scenarios we studied. Although the bias was small, 10%, for propensity scores at 12
events per confounder, this may seem counterintuitive and
differs from the findings of Cepeda et al. (5). However, that
study used logistic regression for the exposure-outcome
model. Changing our simulations to a logistic rather than a
log-linear model yielded findings similar to those of Cepeda
et al. Further work that assessed such performance for much
larger numbers of covariates, such as those utilized in highdimensional propensity scores (22), would be useful.
Our findings thus suggest that, so long as the association
between covariates and exposure is moderate, disease risk
scores are an acceptable alternative to traditional models
and propensity scores. Our simulations further suggest that,
if the assumptions underlying the full-cohort disease risk
score are met (absence of effect modifiers among the covariates), the full-cohort method is generally superior to the
unexposed-only method, with performance generally equivalent to that of the propensity score. The superiority of the
full-cohort DRS in our simulations is consistent with
findings reported by Cadarette et al. (23).
Thus, the disease risk score may be particularly useful in
circumstances in which propensity scores are not appropriate or have poor performance (5, 24, 25). These include
exposures that are rare or that have a large number of categories. Because the disease risk score directly estimates
each cohort members risk of developing the study outcome,
it also provides a single summary measure of disease risk,
particularly useful for assessing effect modification.
A limitation of simulation studies is whether they adequately cover the range of possible scenarios that are likely
to be encountered with real-life data. We designed our simulations to reflect realistic covariate-disease and covariateexposure rate ratios, as well as to reflect both previously
published simulation and epidemiologic studies in terms of
events per covariate, numbers of covariates, incidence of outcome, prevalences of exposure and covariates, measures of
association, and sample size. However, it is likely that we
have not considered some important settings; further work
for these would be useful. In most of our simulation settings,
all of the covariates were confounders. However, with real
data, some covariates may only be related to the outcome
and some only to the exposure. This has been examined for
propensity scores (26). We plan to investigate this for disease risk scores in the future.
There are many unanswered questions regarding the use
of both propensity and disease risk scores in practice. These
include the effects of model misspecification on the scores
(i.e., whether it is better to include all available variables or
to apply variable selection procedures) and the use of either
type of score for case-control studies (16). In addition,
Hansen (19) noted the potential benefit of using a separate

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on February 29, 2016

1 Coverage Probability

0.10

619

620 Arbogast and Ray

sample to estimate the disease risk score, which is a very


intriguing notion but may be difficult to implement in practice because it may be problematic to find a separate-yetcomparable cohort. Given the widespread and seemingly
growing use of summary scores for epidemiologic research,
further research to address these questions is needed.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

REFERENCES
1. Altman DG, Andersen PK. Bootstrap investigation of the
stability of a Cox regression model. Stat Med. 1989;8(7):
771783.
2. Peduzzi P, Concato J, Kemper E, et al. A simulation study of
the number of events per variable in logistic regression analysis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49(12):13731379.
3. Harrell FE Jr, Lee KL, Matchar DB, et al. Regression models
for prognostic prediction: advantages, problems, and suggested solutions. Cancer Treat Rep. 1985;69(10):10711077.
4. Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DB. The central role of the propensity
score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika.
1983;70(1):4155.
5. Cepeda MS, Boston R, Farrar JT, et al. Comparison of logistic
regression versus propensity score when the number of events
is low and there are multiple confounders. Am J Epidemiol.
2003;158(3):280287.
6. Arbogast PG, Kaltenbach L, Ding H, et al. Adjustment for
multiple cardiovascular risk factors using a summary risk
score. Epidemiology. 2008;19(1):3037.
7. Arbogast PG, Ray WA. Use of disease risk scores in pharmacoepidemiologic studies. Stat Methods Med Res. 2009;18(1):
6780.
8. Miettinen OS. Stratification by a multivariate confounder
score. Am J Epidemiol. 1976;104(6):609620.
9. Pike MC, Anderson J, Day N. Some insights into Miettinens
multivariate confounder score approach to case-control study
analysis. Epidemiol Community Health. 1979;33(1):104106.

Am J Epidemiol. 2011;174(5):613620

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on February 29, 2016

Author affiliations: Department of Biostatistics, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee (Patrick G. Arbogast);
Department of Preventive Medicine, Vanderbilt University,
Nashville, Tennessee (Patrick G. Arbogast, Wayne A. Ray);
and Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Center, Nashville Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Nashville, Tennessee
(Wayne A. Ray).
This work was supported in part by the Centers for Education and Research on Therapeutics, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (HS1-0384, HS016974).
The authors thank Dr. M. Alan Brookhart for his valuable
discussions and suggestions.
Conflict of interest: none declared.

10. Cook EF, Goldman L. Performance of tests of significance


based on stratification by a multivariate confounder score or by
a propensity score. J Clin Epidemiol. 1989;42(4):317324.
11. Ray WA, Meredith S, Thapa PB, et al. Antipsychotics and the
risk of sudden cardiac death. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;
58(12):11611167.
12. Ray WA, Stein CM, Hall K, et al. Non-steroidal antiinflammatory drugs and risk of serious coronary heart disease:
an observational cohort study. Lancet. 2002;359(9301):118123.
13. Ray WA, Stein CM, Daugherty JR, et al. COX-2 selective nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs and risk of serious coronary
heart disease. Lancet. 2002;360(9339):10711073.
14. Ray WA, Meredith S, Thapa PB, et al. Cyclic antidepressants
and the risk of sudden cardiac death. Clin Pharmacol Ther.
2004;75(3):234241.
15. Ray WA, Murray KT, Meredith S, et al. Oral erythromycin and
the risk of sudden death from cardiac causes. N Engl J Med.
2004;351(11):10891096.
16. Graham DJ, Campen D, Hui R, et al. Risk of acute myocardial
infarction and sudden cardiac death in patients treated with
cyclo-oxygenase 2 selective and non-selective non-steroidal
anti-inflammatory drugs: nested case-control study. Lancet.
2005;365(9458):475481.
17. Ray WA, Chung CP, Stein CM, et al. Risk of peptic ulcer
hospitalizations in users of NSAIDs with gastroprotective
cotherapy versus coxibs. Gastroenterology. 2007;133(3):
790798.
18. Sturmer T, Schneeweiss S, Brookhart MA, et al. Analytic
strategies to adjust confounding using exposure propensity
scores and disease risk scores: nonsteroidal antiinflammatory
drugs and short-term mortality in the elderly. Am J Epidemiol.
2005;161(9):891898.
19. Hansen BB. The prognostic analogue of the propensity score.
Biometrika. 2008;95(2):481488.
20. Greenland S, Robins JM. Identifiability, exchangeability, and
epidemiological confounding. Int J Epidemiol. 1986;15(3):
413419.
21. Gail MH, Wieand S, Piantadosi S. Biased estimates of treatment effect in randomized experiments with nonlinear regressions and omitted covariates. Biometrika. 1984;71(3):
431444.
22. Schneeweiss S, Rassen JA, Glynn RJ, et al. High-dimensional
propensity score adjustment in studies of treatment effects
using health care claims data. Epidemiology. 2009;20(4):
512522.
23. Cadarette SM, Gagne JJ, Solomon DH, et al. Confounder
summary scores when comparing the effects of multiple drug
exposures. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2010;19(1):29.
24. Austin PC, Grootendorst P, Normand SL, et al. Conditioning
on the propensity score can result in biased estimation of
common measures of treatment effect: a Monte Carlo study.
Stat Med. 2007;26(4):754768.
25. Greenland S. Invited commentary: variable selection versus
shrinkage in the control of multiple confounders. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167(5):523529.
26. Brookhart MA, Schneeweiss S, Rothman KJ, et al. Variable
selection for propensity score models. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;
163(12):11491156.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai