Anda di halaman 1dari 27

524015

research-article2014
ASMXXX10.1177/1073191114524015AssessmentMorizot

Article
Assessment

Construct Validity of Adolescents


2014, Vol. 21(5) 580606
The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
Self-Reported Big Five Personality Traits: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1073191114524015

Importance of Conceptual Breadth and asm.sagepub.com

Initial Validation of a Short Measure

Julien Morizot1

Abstract
While there are a number of short personality trait measures that have been validated for use with adults, few are
specifically validated for use with adolescents. To trust such measures, it must be demonstrated that they have adequate
construct validity. According to the view of construct validity as a unifying form of validity requiring the integration of
different complementary sources of information, this article reports the evaluation of content, factor, convergent, and
criterion validities as well as reliability of adolescents self-reported personality traits. Moreover, this study sought to
address an inherent potential limitation of short personality trait measures, namely their limited conceptual breadth. In
this study, starting with items from a known measure, after the language-level was adjusted for use with adolescents, items
tapping fundamental primary traits were added to determine the impact of added conceptual breadth on the psychometric
properties of the scales. The resulting new measure was named the Big Five Personality Trait Short Questionnaire
(BFPTSQ). A group of expert judges considered the items to have adequate content validity. Using data from a community
sample of early adolescents, the results confirmed the factor validity of the Big Five structure in adolescence as well as
its measurement invariance across genders. More important, the added items did improve the convergent and criterion
validities of the scales, but did not negatively affect their reliability. This study supports the construct validity of adolescents
self-reported personality traits and points to the importance of conceptual breadth in short personality measures.

Keywords
personality trait, Big Five, psychopathology, assessment, psychometric properties, adolescence

Understanding personality trait development across the life personality trait measures, namely their limited conceptual
course has overarching theoretical and practical implica- breadth.
tions (Caspi & Shiner, 2006). Whereas most adults have
the cognitive capabilities to reflect on the various aspects
of their self and report their thoughts, values, motivations,
A Developmental Perspective on Big
feelings, and behaviors, is it the same for adolescents? Five Personality Traits
While there has been growing interest in the validity and There appears to be a current consensus that a taxonomy of
reliability of youths self-reported personality traits (Soto, five broad or higher-order personality traits can help clas-
John, Gosling, & Potter, 2008; Tackett, Slobodskaya, etal., sify and account for the variation in most of the numerous
2012), there are still few short assessment instruments spe- existing primary traits (which are also sometimes called
cifically validated for this developmental period. To trust facets, subdomains, or lower-order traits; Goldberg, 1990,
these self-reported assessments by adolescents, it should 1993; John, Neumann, & Soto, 2008; Markon, Krueger, &
be demonstrated that they have adequate construct validity. Watson, 2005; McCrae & Costa, 1997). These Big Five
This article reports the evaluation of construct validity of broad traits are Openness to Experience, Extraversion,
adolescents personality traits. According to the view of Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Neuroticism (or its
construct validity as a unifying form of validity requiring
the integration of different complementary sources of 1
University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
information (Messick, 1995; Simms & Watson, 2007),
Corresponding Author:
content validity, factor validity, convergent validity, crite- Julien Morizot, School of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, C.P.
rion validity, and reliability were evaluated. This study also 6128, succ. Centre-Ville, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, H3C 3J7.
sought to address an inherent potential limitation of short Email: julien.morizot@umontreal.ca

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 581

positive pole, Emotional Stability). Openness represents Third, a number of studies using item-level factor
individual differences in intellectual curiosity, imagination, analyses of self-reports from instruments constructed for
appreciation of different ideas and artistic expressions, and adults tend to recover reasonably well the Big Five factor
different social and political values; Extraversion reflects structure with early adolescents samples (e.g., Allik,
individual differences in sociability, assertiveness, activity Laidra, Realo, & Pullmann, 2004; McCrae etal., 2002;
level, appreciation of exciting activities, and the propensity W. D. Parker & Stumpf, 1998; Scholte, van Aken, & van
to express positive emotions; Agreeableness reveals indi- Lieshout, 1997; Soto etal., 2008). In general, these stud-
vidual differences in prosociality, empathy, collaboration, ies suggest that by the age of 10 to 12, there is systematic
and helpfulness with others; Conscientiousness represents and meaningful covariation between Big Five trait indica-
individual differences in the propensity to be organized, to tors (items tapping different primary traits), even though
plan things ahead, to control impulses, and to respect and within-trait coherence (internal consistency) and between-
abide conventional social norms and rules; and Neuroticism trait differentiation (low factor correlations) tend to
refers to individual differences in the propensity to experi- increase throughout the entire adolescence (Soto etal.,
ence negative emotions such as anxiety, fear, depressed 2008). In addition to self-reports, studies using item-level
mood, and irritability and to have low self-worth. factor analyses of instruments not a priori designed to
The early evidence for the existence of the Big Five traits measure the Big Five traits and using informant reports
originated mainly from factor analytic studies with adult such as parents and teachers tend to recover well the Big
samples (Goldberg, 1990, 1993; John etal., 2008). Still, Five trait structure in late childhood and early adoles-
there is a growing body of research supporting the notion cents samples (e.g., Goldberg, 2001; Tackett, Krueger,
that the Big Five traits can be recognized and measured in Iacono, & McGue, 2008; Tackett, Slobodskaya, etal.,
childhood and adolescence (Caspi & Shiner, 2006; Shiner, 2012).
2010). On one hand, research on temperament or personal- Based on this evidence, it is reasonable to argue for the
ity traits in infancy and early childhood suggests that many construction of a developmentally sensitive personality trait
primary traits identifiable within each of the broad Big Five measure for adolescents assessing the same primary traits
traits in adults are not yet developed at this early age. that are typically assessed in adulthood. Of course, the
Indeed, most models of infant and early childhood items of such instrument need to be either developmentally
personality suggest that there are three rather than five neutral or sensitive to this developmental period (e.g., refer-
broad dimensions, namely Positive Emotionality/Surgency, ring to school rather than work) and have a language level
Negative Emotionality, and Effortful Control, which are appropriate for young adolescents.
conceptually similar to Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Apart from the factor structure replicated across differ-
Conscientiousness, respectively (Rothbart & Bates, 2006; ent developmental periods, there is also evidence that Big
Shiner, 2010). On the other hand, differing from these early Five personality traits have adequate predictive validity in
stages of life, there is cumulating evidence from different childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. There is mounting
areas of research suggesting that arguably all of the primary evidence that the Big Five traits are related, both concur-
traits identified in adults are also identifiable by early ado- rently and prospectively, to various positive and negative
lescence, at least in an emerging form. consequential life outcomes in adulthood (Ozer & Benet-
First, individual differences in the physiological, endo- Martnez, 2006), even beyond classical constructs such as
crinal, and neural systems hypothesized to underlie adult socioeconomic status and intelligence (Roberts, Kuncel,
personality traits (DeYoung & Gray, 2009) can be measured Shiner, Caspi, & Goldberg, 2007). Clinical and develop-
by early adolescence, though various brain circuits related mental psychopathology research in recent years has led to
to these systems actually change or mature during the mounting evidence that personality traits are meaningful
entire second decade of life (Spear, 2010). Second, helped correlates or predictors of various psychopathologies
by their growing capacity to think abstractly, early adoles- (Malouff, Thorsteinsson, & Schutte, 2005; Widiger &
cents can create an abstraction of their self by combining Smith, 2008), notably mood and anxiety disorders (Klein,
several of their attributes (Harter, 2006). Moreover, the self- Dyson, Kujawa, & Kotov, 2012; Klein, Kotov, & Bufferd,
concept starts becoming more differentiated in early adoles- 2011; Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, 2010), antisocial
cence, which means that early adolescents are more able behavior (Jones, Miller, & Lynam, 2011; Miller & Lynam,
than children to describe their self-attributes differently 2001), and substance use and abuse (Ball, 2005; Terracciano,
according to different social situations or roles (Harter, Lockenhoff, Crum, Bienvenu, & Costa, 2008). Research on
2006). In the same vein, there is evidence that by early ado- children and adolescents also supports the validity of per-
lescence, around age 12, autobiographical life narratives are sonality traits for predicting psychopathology (Caspi, 2000;
more globally coherent than at earlier ages (Habermas & de De Pauw & Mervielde, 2010; Krueger, Caspi, Moffitt,
Silveira, 2008). This suggests that a coherent sense of self is Silva, & McGee, 1996; Tackett, 2006; Tackett, Martel, &
manifested at least by early adolescence. Kushner, 2012).

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
582 Assessment 21(5)

Short Personality Trait Measures and (Loevinger, 1957; Smith, Fischer, & Fister, 2003). In other
Conceptual Breadth words, such a measure does not have adequate content
validity (Haynes, Richards, & Kubany, 1995).
Initial evidence for the existence of the Big Five originated As argued by different scholars, the main reason why
from early factor analytic studies of hundreds of personality short personality measures have reduced conceptual breadth
trait descriptors or adjectives (Goldberg, 1990, 1993; John has to do with the common ways items are retained (Marsh
etal., 2008). Each of these five broad traits thus encom- etal., 2005; Saucier & Goldberg, 2002; Smith etal., 2000;
passes several primary personality traits. In other words, the Stanton etal., 2002). These authors point to three common
Big Five traits are constructs with large conceptual breadth empirical methods to develop short personality trait scales:
or bandwidth (Cattell, 1966). A number of integrative factor saturation (selecting items with the highest factor
instruments measuring these broad traits and some of their loadings), factor discrimination (selecting univocal items
corresponding primary traits have been developed (see without cross-loadings), and internal consistency maximi-
chapters in Boyle, Matthews, & Saklofske, 2008). However, zation (selecting items contributing the most to internal
because users of personality measures are often faced with consistency). These methods are useful, but they all tend to
limited testing time, particularly in research situations, lead to the same shortcoming: scales with limited concep-
complete integrative personality trait measures are often tual breadth in which items tapping some fundamental pri-
considered too lengthy. For example, in large community- mary personality traits are absent (see Saucier & Goldberg,
based longitudinal studies, in which personality traits are 2002). However, it is critical to preserve an adequate con-
only one of several constructs being assessed, using shorter ceptual breadth in a personality measure for two related rea-
measures becomes practically inevitable. Not surprisingly, sons. First, when representative sampling of all the primary
short measures of the Big Five traits are currently quite traits embedded in a broad trait is not maintained, the mea-
popular. A number of such measures exists, most of them sures content validity is compromised (Haynes etal., 1995;
having satisfactory basic psychometric properties (e.g., Big Smith etal., 2000; Smith etal., 2003). When content valid-
Five QuestionnaireChildren Version [BFQ-C] ity is not adequate, it affects almost all other psychometrics
Barbaranelli, Caprara, Rabasca, & Pastorelli, 2003; Mini- properties (Haynes etal., 1995). Second, preserving con-
International Personality Item Pool Big Five Measure ceptual breadth is important when personality trait mea-
[Mini-IPIP]Donnellan, Oswald, Baird, & Lucas, 2006; sures are used to predict external criteria (i.e., criterion
International Personality Item Pool Big Five Measure validity). As pointed out by different scholars, all things
[IPIP50]Goldberg, 1999; Ten-Item Personality Inventory being equal, a conceptually broader scale will tend to be
[TIPI]Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003; Big Five related to a broader array of criteria (Loevinger, 1957;
Inventory [BFI]John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991; John Saucier & Goldberg, 2002). This is particularly important
etal., 2008; NEO Five-Factor Inventory [NEO-FFI-3] for short Big Five measures because they are often used in
McCrae & Costa, 2010; Five Factor Model Rating Form community and longitudinal studies to predict a multitude
[FFMRF]Mullins-Sweatt, Jamerson, Samuel, Olson, & of criterion variables. Of course, measures of conceptually
Widiger, 2006; Big Five Inventory10 [BFI-10] broad predictors generally tend to provide optimal predic-
Rammstedt & John, 2007; Big Five Mini Markers tion of equally complex and heterogeneous criteria, whereas
[BFMM]Saucier, 1994; Mini Modular Markers [3M40] measures of narrower predictors are most efficient in pre-
Saucier, 2002). dicting specific criteria (Cronbach & Gleser, 1965;
Various authors have reviewed issues pertaining to the Loevinger, 1957). As noted by Hogan and Roberts (1996),
validity and usefulness of short measures in general (e.g., psychologists and other social scientists generally study
Silverstein, 1990; Smith, McCarthy, & Anderson, 2000), conceptually complex or broad constructs or syndromes. In
and conceptual and statistical strategies have been proposed this context, short measures of broad constructs like Big
to shorten long versions (Marsh, Ellis, Parada, Richards, & Five personality traits preserving conceptual breadth are
Heubeck, 2005; Stanton, Sinar, Balzer, & Smith, 2002). important.
Most of these authors have pointed to some nontrivial short- An examination of the content of most existing Big Five
comings of short measures of conceptually complex or short measures indicates that they suffer from a limited con-
broad constructs. One central shortcoming is the limited ceptual breadth. Indeed, a number of theoretically impor-
conceptual breadth. Indeed, conceptually broad constructs tant primary traits are usually absent, which ipso facto
typically consist of several primary traits. In principle, for a limits content validity, but may also impact other psycho-
short measure to be considered a valid alternative to the metric properties. The point is not to argue that existing
full-length one, each of its primary traits should be repre- short personality trait measures are inappropriate or not
sented. A short measure of a broad construct has limited useful, but merely to notice that most, if not all, of them
conceptual breadth when some primary traits are propor- have limited conceptual breadth. Yet evidence is still lack-
tionally less represented, or worse, not represented at all ing regarding the impact of this limitation on other

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 583

psychometric properties, such as convergent validity with were made until a consensus was reached. Afterward, the
independent measures of related constructs or criterion items were reviewed for language level. The criterion was
validity with consequential outcomes. that adolescents starting high school (at ages 12 or 13 in the
province of Quebec) be able to understand the items. This
led to minor modifications to most of the items. A number of
The Present Study
items were slightly simplified, and for a few of them an
Rationale for Items Modification and Addition additional descriptor or stem connected by a comma was
added. Even if it is generally not recommended to have mul-
In this study, the impact of adding conceptual breadth in a tiple stems in an item, the objective was merely to provide a
short Big Five personality trait measure was evaluated. synonym or to elaborate.
While existing studies typically aimed at reducing the num- Before adding new items, one Openness item from the
ber of items, the reverse strategy was used in this study. original item pool was deleted because it was judged less
Indeed, starting with an existing measure, a number of relevant for adolescents and not central to the target con-
items tapping missing fundamental primary traits were struct (prefer work that is routine). Moreover, an
added to the scales. Extraversion item that was judged equivocal was removed
The BFI (John etal., 1991; John etal., 2008) was selected (generates a lot of enthusiasm) and replaced with an item
as an appropriate measure to provide an initial item pool tapping social dominance or leadership (17).
because of its adequate basic psychometric properties and, The second step was to add new items representing
more specifically, its satisfactory content validity as com- important primary personality traits. To keep the measure
pared with other noncommercial short personality trait short, with a testing time of 10 to 15 minutes for adolescents
measures. Moreover, this measure contains short verbal (5 to 10 minutes for adults), a maximum of 50 items overall,
statement items, which are preferable to single-trait adjec- with 10 items per scale, was decided on. The goal was not
tive items because the former are more likely to be easily to add items for all missing primary traits in the original
and unequivocally understood by early adolescents item pool, but to add only a few of the most fundamental.
(Barbaranelli etal., 2003). Also, single-trait adjectives are Scholarly reviews on trait structure and empirical
often at a higher level of abstraction than short verbal state- research with adolescents guided the choice of which pri-
ments, making them potentially harder to translate precisely mary traits were candidates for adding items. For Openness,
in different languages (Saucier & Goldberg, 2002). The the original BFI scale has good content coverage for the
items measuring the BFI traits posted on the public domain primary traits of Intellectual Inquisitiveness, Creativity, and
International Personality Item Pool website were used as Aesthetic and Artistic Appreciation. It was decided that
the initial items pool (http://ipip.ori.org/newBFITable.htm). only one item tapping Openness to Cultural Diversity (31r)
The items are also available in John etal. (2008). As will be would be added. This primary trait is part of some compre-
explained below, because of all the modifications made to hensive Openness models (McCrae & Sutin, 2009). For
the original item pool, the resulting new measure was instance, it would be represented by the facet Openness to
named the Big Five Personality Trait Short Questionnaire Values in the NEO-PI-3 (McCrae & Costa, 2010). As dis-
(BFPTSQ), to avoid confusion with the original question- cussed by McCrae and Sutin (2009), this social attitude can
naire. The BFPTSQ items are presented in the appendix. be considered part of Openness because open persons are
Back translation from the original French version (with all generally lower in ethnocentrism and thus, more open to
the modifications and additions described next) was used to different cultures and lifestyles. Assessing individual differ-
make this English version. ences in this social attitude is important because it is associ-
Because this study was conducted with French Canadian ated with behavioral and emotional adjustment of both
early adolescents, it was important to ensure that all items adolescents and teachers attending multicultural high
would be well understood by the participants. A number of schools in large metropolitan areas (Garca-Coll &
researchers have reported that adolescents have difficulty Magnuson, 1997). Among existing short Big Five mea-
reading or understanding many items in personality ques- sures, there are no items tapping Openness to Cultural
tionnaires (e.g., Allik etal., 2004; De Fruyt, Mervielde, Diversity in the BFI, the IPIP50, the NEO-FFI-3, the
Hoekstra, & Rolland, 2000; McCrae, Costa, & Martin, BFMM, or the 3M40.
2005), arguably because these measures were constructed For Extraversion, the original BFI scale has good content
and validated for use with adults. Therefore, the first step coverage for the primary traits of Sociability, Expressiveness,
was to translate and revise the items. Three researchers and Assertiveness, and Activity. It was decided that an item tap-
three graduate students familiar with personality theory (and ping Sensation Seeking (42) and one tapping Joyfulness or
the Big Five model more specifically), experienced in psy- Positive Emotions (47) would be added. These two primary
chometric test construction, as well as in research with ado- traits are part of various comprehensive models of Extraversion
lescents first translated the items into French. Modifications (Wilt & Revelle, 2009). For instance, they would correspond

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
584 Assessment 21(5)

to Excitement Seeking and Positive Emotions in the NEO-PI-3 such as ADHD and antisocial behavior (Crews &
(McCrae & Costa, 2010). Sensation Seeking is certainly a Boettiger, 2009; Jones etal., 2011; Newman & Wallace,
fundamental personality trait that has proven useful for 1993; Nigg, 2006). Among existing short Big Five mea-
understanding the development of negative life outcomes sures, there are no items tapping Impulsiveness in the
(Zuckerman, 2009). For instance, it is related to antisocial BFI, the BFQ-C, the IPIP50, the NEO-FFI-3, the BFMM,
behavior (Jones etal., 2011). Assessing individual differences or the 3M40.
in Sensation Seeking in adolescents can be useful for predict- Finally, for Emotional Stability, the original BFI scale
ing substance use and to decide which adolescents should has good content coverage for the primary traits of
participate in a preventive intervention (Sargant, Tanski, Anxiousness, Fearfulness, Worry, and Depressed Mood. It
Stoolmiller, & Hanewinkel, 2010). Joyfulness is also an was decided that an item tapping Low Self-Worth (45r) and
important primary trait, as some scholars consider it to be the another one tapping Irritability (50r) would be added. It is
core aspect of Extraversion (Tellegen & Waller, 2008; Watson clear that Self-Worth (or Self-Esteem) is a crucial trait of
& Clark, 1997). Assessing individual differences in Joyfulness the human experience (Bosson & Swann, 2009). Irritability
is important because it is related to various life outcomes, (or Anger) is also a central construct in the human personal-
such as attachment styles (Shiota, Keltner, & John, 2006), ity (Barefoot & Boyle, 2009; Snaith & Taylor, 1985). Both
stress recovery, and resilience (Ong, Bergeman, Bisconti, & of these primary traits are part of various comprehensive
Wallace, 2006). Among existing short Big Five measures, models of Neuroticism (Clark & Watson, 2008; Tellegen &
there are no items tapping Sensation Seeking in the BFI, the Waller, 2008). For instance, in the NEO-PI-3 (McCrae &
BFQ-C, the IPIP50, the BFMM, or the 3M40, while there are Costa, 2010), low Self-Worth would be represented by the
no items tapping Joyfulness in the BFI, the IPIP50, the facet Vulnerability and, to a lesser extent, by Depression,
BFMM, or the 3M40. while Irritability would be represented by the facet Angry
For Agreeableness, the original BFI scale has good Hostility. Assessing individual differences in Self-Worth is
content coverage for the primary traits of Altruism, important because it is related to psychopathology develop-
Compassion, Forgiveness, and Cooperation. It was ment, particularly internalizing psychopathologies (Mann,
decided that an item tapping Machiavellianism (48r) Hosman, Schaalma, & de Vries, 2004) and aggression and
would be added. This constitutes a ubiquitous primary antisocial behavior (Donnellan, Trzesniewski, Robins,
personality trait in the human experience (Jones & Moffitt, & Caspi, 2005). Assessing individual differences in
Paulhus, 2009; Wilson, Near, & Miller, 1996) and is argu- Irritability is also important because it is related to the
ably a central construct for studying antisocial behavior development of internalizing psychopathologies (Stringaris,
development and psychopathy (Barry, Kerig, Stellwagen, Cohen, Pine, & Leibenluft, 2009), ADHD (Leibenluft,
& Barry, 2010). This primary trait is part of various com- Cohen, Gorrindo, Brook, & Pine, 2006), reactive aggres-
prehensive Agreeableness models and would be repre- sion (Scarpa & Raine, 1997), antisocial behavior (Jones
sented by the facet Straightforwardness and, to a lesser etal., 2011), and substance use (Tarter, Blackson, Brigham,
extent, Altruism in the NEO-PI-3 (McCrae & Costa, Moss, & Caprara, 1995). Among existing short Big Five
2010).1 Assessing individual differences in this construct measures, there are no items tapping Irritability in the BFI
is important because it is clearly associated with serious and the NEO-FFI-3, while there are no items tapping Low
conduct problems and antisocial behavior in children, Self-Worth in the BFI, the BFQ-C, the IPIP50, the BFMM,
adolescents, and adults (Frick etal., 2003; Jones etal., or the 3M40.
2011). Among existing short Big Five measures, there are
no items tapping Machiavellianism in the BFI, the BFQ- Evaluation of Construct Validity and Impact of
C, the IPIP50, the BFMM, or the 3M40.
For Conscientiousness, the original BFI scale has
Added Conceptual Breadth
good content coverage for the primary traits of To ensure that the modified items as well as the newly added
Dutifulness, Self-Discipline, Planfulness, and Order. It items were valid, a group of experts in personality trait the-
was decided that an item tapping Impulsiveness (49r) ory and test construction were first asked to rate the rele-
would be added. Impulsiveness constitutes without a vance or representativeness of the items with regard to their
doubt a fundamental primary personality trait (Madden target Big Five trait. Factor validity was then assessed in two
& Bickel, 2010) and is part of most comprehensive mod- ways. First, to ensure that all items were associated with
els of Conscientiousness (Roberts, Jackson, Fayard, their target broad trait, factor analyses were conducted.
Edmonds, & Meints, 2008). In the NEO-PI-3, it would be Second, given the well-documented gender differences in
represented by the facet Deliberation (McCrae & Costa, personality trait means (Else-Quest, Hyde, Goldsmith, &
2010).2 Assessing individual differences in impulse con- Van Hulle, 2006; Schmitt, Realo, Voracek, & Allik, 2008),
trol is important because it is related to executive func- measurement invariance across genders was also evaluated.
tions and plays a key role in various psychopathologies, Indeed, before testing mean differences, there is a need to

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 585

test whether the constructs of interest are the same in both (Quebec, Canada). These schools were a priori targeted to
groups, and few studies have fully tested measurement sample adolescents from various socioeconomic statuses
invariance of personality traits across genders (Marsh etal., and ethnic backgrounds. After the project was presented to
2010). Reliability was also estimated. Given the secondary all school administrations, one school declined to partici-
objective of this study, reliability estimates from the scales pate. In total, 41 classes participated in the first assessment:
with original content (i.e., without the new items) and those 29 regular classes, 8 international curriculum classes (i.e.,
with added items were compared statistically. It was hypoth- enriched curriculum), and 4 specialized classes for special-
esized that given their small number, the added items would needs students (i.e., with behavior problems and/or learning
have no or marginal impact on reliability estimates. Indeed, difficulties).
the conceptual breadth of the scales is increased, which Before the data collection, the project was sanctioned by
could reduce their reliability; but they also become longer, the University of Montreals ethical review board as well as
which should slightly increase reliability. Convergent valid- all the school boards ethical committees. All the adolescents
ity was also assessed using the NEO-PI-3 scales (McCrae & signed a consent form, completed a form in which they pro-
Costa, 2010). This questionnaire can arguably be considered vided their contact information (email, home address, etc.),
a gold standard for convergent validity given its widespread and were given an envelope to bring to their parents. The
use in personality research. Again, to determine whether the envelop contained a letter for the parents explaining the
BFPTSQ scales with added items provided enhanced con- study, a consent form, a questionnaire about their child, a
vergent validity, the correlations derived from the original- questionnaire about themselves, and a preaddressed and
content scales and those derived from the added-item scales prepaid envelop to return all the material. All parents who
were compared using statistical tests. It was hypothesized did not return the signed consent form were contacted by
that the scales with added items would have significantly phone. Only six parents refused to allow their child to par-
higher correlations with each of the broad Big Five traits, as ticipate in the study.
well as with primary traits from the NEO-PI-3 correspond- For the data collection, two trained research assistants
ing to specific content of the newly added items. Finally, visited each group between January and June 2009.
criterion validity was assessed by correlating the personality Because the initial questionnaire in this longitudinal study
scales with consequential outcomes. Specifically, because was extensive, the adolescents were given two regular
several studies showed that they are related to personality class periods (75 minutes each) to fill it out. All those who
traits, measures of externalizing and internalizing psychopa- were absent from school the day of the assessment were
thology (e.g., De Pauw & Mervielde, 2010; Klein etal., contacted by phone to set a new date for the data collec-
2012; Tackett, 2006; Tackett, Martel etal., 2012; Widiger & tion. Moreover, to minimize missing data, all adolescents
Smith, 2008) as well as academic achievement (e.g., Noftle who were unable to fully complete the questionnaire dur-
& Robins, 2007; Poropat, 2009) were used. Based on these ing the two periods were contacted and asked to return the
studies, it was hypothesized that the externalizing psychopa- missing responses by email. Two $20 gift certificates were
thology and substance use scales would be negatively related randomly drawn in each group among the participating
to Agreeableness and Conscientiousness and positively adolescents.
related to Extraversion. For internalizing psychopathology The questionnaires of 1,036 adolescents were gathered
scales, it was expected that they would be negatively related in the first assessment of this study. Personality data were
to Emotional Stability and Extraversion. It was also hypoth- available for 1,028 of them (only a subsample of 598 filed
esized that academic achievement would be negatively the NEO-PI-3). Their mean age was 12.71 (SD = 0.59), with
related to Conscientiousness and Openness. Finally, because most aged 12 (35.5%) or 13 (59.1%) and only a few aged 14
of their larger conceptual breadth, it was hypothesized that (4.4%) or 15 (1.0%). The sample was equally proportioned
the added-item scales would provide significantly higher by gender (females, n = 520, 50.2%). Consistent with the
correlations to each relevant outcome. province of Quebec population, most of them were
Caucasian (74%), while Blacks (4.9%), Hispanics (3.3%),
Asians (3.2%), Arabs (7.4%), Canadian First Nations
Method (2.8%), and multiethnic persons (4.4%) made up the rest of
the sample. The large majority were born in Canada
Procedure and Participants (90.8%). Most of them lived with their two biological par-
The data come from the first assessment of an ongoing pro- ents (68.1%), while 16.2% lived in a shared custody situa-
spective longitudinal study on personality, antisocial behav- tion with their biological mother and father, 5.0% with their
ior, and psychopathology development during adolescence. biological mother and her new partner, 1.7% with their bio-
Eight French-language high schools from four different logical father and his new partner, 6.1% with their biologi-
school boards of the larger Montreal metropolitan and cal mother alone, 1.2% with their biological father alone,
Quebec City areas were initially targeted to participate and the rest with another family member or an adoptive

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
586 Assessment 21(5)

family or in foster care (1.7%). With regard to siblings, Outcomes


65.5% (M = 0.98, SD = 0.98) of these adolescents lived with Psychopathology. To measure psychopathology, seven
at least one brother, while 61.6% lived with at least one sis- scales from the Youth Inventory Version 4 (Gadow &
ter (M = 0.90, SD = 0.93). Sprafkin, 1999) were used. This inventory is a self-report
measure of the most prevalent DSM-IV mental disorders
during adolescence. All items were written to correspond
Measures directly to the symptoms of DSM-IV disorders. The ado-
Personality lescents rated the last 12-month frequency of all items on
Big Five Personality Trait Short Questionnaire (BFPTSQ).Even a 4-point scale (never = 0, sometimes = 1, often = 2, and
though the questionnaire resulting from this study is simi- very often = 3). While the scales retained the DSM disor-
lar in content to the BFI developed by John and his col- der labels, they can all be computed as either categorical
leagues (1991, 2008), it is a different measure. First, most (i.e., a symptom count threshold allowing identification
items were modified in two ways: First, the language was of the presence of a disorder) or dimensional (i.e., a sum
simplified for use with early adolescents, and, second, of the items, which represents a frequency or severity
new items corresponding to important primary traits were score). The dimensional scales were used in this study.
added. Because of these differences and to avoid confu- Three scales of externalizing psychopathology and four
sion with the original measure, the new one was given a scales of internalizing psychopathology were selected:
different name: the Big Five Personality Trait Short Ques- For externalizing the scales are Attention Deficit Hyper-
tionnaire (BFPTSQ). The BFPTSQ has 50 items, 10 for activity Disorder (ADHD, 18 items), Conduct Disor-
each trait. Respondents use a 5-point Likert-type response der (CD, 15 items), and Oppositional Defiant Disorder
format (totally disagree = 0, disagree a little = 1, neutral (ODD, 8 items); and for internalizing, the scales are
opinion = 2, agree a little = 3, totally agree = 4). Just like Major Depression Disorder (MDD, 12 items), Bipolar
the original instrument, the introduction sentence, I see Disorder (BD, 10 items), Generalized Anxiety Disorder
myself as someone who, is presented at the top of each (GAD, 8 items), and Social Phobia (SOP, 4 items). The
page. The French and English versions of the BFPTSQ are internal consistency of the scales is satisfactory in this
available from the author. sample, with coefficients ranging from .78 to .91.
NEO-Personality Inventory-3 (NEO-PI-3).The NEO- Substance Use. Substance use was assessed with modi-
PI-3 (McCrae & Costa, 2010) assesses the broad and fied items from the Measures of Quebec Adolescents Social
primary personality traits of the five-factor model. The and Personal Adjustment (Le Blanc, 1996). This instrument
broad traits are Openness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, assesses several dimensions of adolescent adjustment and
Conscientiousness, and Neuroticism. There is a total of was validated on a large representative sample of the Que-
30 primary-trait scales, six for each of the broad trait. bec adolescent population. Adolescents were asked whether
The major difference between the NEO-PI-3 and its pre- they had used eight substances during the past 12 months.
vious edition (NEO-PI-R) is that 37 items were replaced The items correspond to major psychoactive substance cat-
or modified to be easier to read for adolescents, as well egories, namely (a) drank alcohol to get drunk (not just for
as to be better indicators of their target primary trait tasting or for a dinner), (b) smoked cannabis (marijuana,
(McCrae etal., 2005). This inventory includes 240 items pot, hash, weed), (c) inhaled substances such as glue, gas,
(8 per primary trait) with a 5-point Likert-type response paint stripper, Dust-Off or other chemical substances, (d)
scale (strongly disagree = 0 to strongly agree = 4). In used stimulants such as Ecstasy, MDMA, XTC, etc., (e)
the present sample, internal consistency coefficients used other stimulants such as methamphetamines (meth),
are satisfactory for almost all scales, except for a few cocaine, crack, or crystal meth (Ice), (f) used halluci-
primary-trait scales. For the broad traits, internal con- nogens such as LSD, acids, PCP, mescaline, angel dust,
sistency estimates are all high, with coefficients ranging magic mushrooms, etc., (g) used tranquilizers such as
from .79 (Openness) to .92 (Conscientiousness). For the GHB, Valium, Librium, Ativan, Xanax, etc., and (h) used
primary traits, they range from .65 to .79, but fall below analgesics such as heroin, morphine, opium, codeine, Oxy-
.60 for one primary trait of Extraversion (Activity, .55), contin, Demerol, etc. All items were rated on a 4-point fre-
one of Agreeableness (Compliance, .54), two of Neu- quency scale (never = 0, rarely (1 or 2 times) = 1, often = 2,
roticism (Angry Hostility, .58; Impulsiveness, .56), and and very often = 3). Internal consistency is adequate in this
three of Openness (Feelings, .56; Actions, .37; Values, sample with a coefficient of .86.
.43). Even though internal consistency is substandard Grade Point Average (GPA).The GPA scale was com-
for some primary traits, particularly for Openness, other puted using grades from official final report cards that were
researchers have observed the same phenomenon using transmitted by the schools. The courses included in the cur-
the same instrument with adolescents (e.g., De Fruyt riculum are French (native language), English (secondary
etal., 2000). language), sciences, mathematics, history, geography, arts,

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 587

physical education, and ethics and religions. Scores can such as those included in this study, are theoretically or
vary from 0 to 100, with 60 as the passing grade, as per the conceptually defensible.
norms in effect in the Canadian province of Quebec. Inter- Factor validity was assessed using two types of models.
nal consistency is .91 in this sample. The a priori five-factor structure was first tested with inde-
pendent clusters model confirmatory factor analysis (ICM-
CFA). The initial ICM-CFA model (M1) was specified so
Statistical Analyses
that answers to the 50 items would be explained by five
All analyses were conducted using Mplus Version 6.12 correlated factors, with each item having a nonzero loading
(Muthn & Muthn, 2010). Unless otherwise noted, all on its target factor and zero loadings on all other factors,
analyses were conducted using the robust maximum likeli- while item uniquenesses are uncorrelated. However, a
hood estimator (MLR), which provides adjusted standard number of researchers have noted that a good-fitting ICM-
errors and statistical fit tests that are robust to nonnormality CFA model is rarely, if ever, obtained with personality trait
in the data. Confidence intervals (95%) were calculated and structures measured by several items (e.g., Church &
reported to provide information regarding the precision of Burke, 1994; Marsh etal., 2010; McCrae, Zonderman,
all relevant parameter estimates (Cumming, 2012). Costa, Bond, & Paunonen, 1996; J. D. A. Parker, Bagby, &
Content validity. To assess content validity, six expert Summerfeldt, 1993). One possible explanation is that in an
judges on both personality theory and construction of ICM-CFA model, a simple structure is specified (i.e., non-
personality measures were solicited to rate all items for zero loading on the target factor, and all cross-loadings
their relevance or representativeness with regard to their fixed to 0). Yet, the Big Five personality trait structure was
target Big Five scale.3 The raters were asked to estimate uncovered using exploratory factor analysis (EFA), which
the relevance of each item using a 4-point response scale: estimates all possible factor loadings. In other words, an
not at all representative = 0, somewhat representative EFA is closer to the true model. In consequence, the factor
= 1, very representative = 2, extremely representative = validity was also tested using exploratory structural equa-
3. With these ratings, a Content Validity Index (Polit & tion modeling (ESEM; Asparouhov & Muthn, 2009;
Beck, 2006) can be computed: one for individual items Morin, Marsh, & Nagengast, 2013). An ESEM model
(I-CVI) and one for scales (S-CVI). The I-CVI is com- closely resembles EFA, but with some important advan-
puted by adding up the number of raters who gave a rating tages. For instance, in addition to allowing all loadings to
of 2 or 3 and dividing the result by the number of raters. be estimated on all factors, it also (a) provides standard
When there are six raters or more, an item is considered errors for all parameters so that their statistical significance
relevant if the I-CVI is .78 or higher (Polit & Beck, 2006). can be tested, (b) allows correlated item uniqueness to be
The S-CVI is computed by adding up the I-CVI values included, (c) allows multiple-group measurement invari-
of the items forming a scale and dividing the result by ance tests to be conducted, and (d) provides fit indices and
the number of raters. When there are six raters or more, statistical tests routinely available in CFA. The a priori
a scale is considered to have adequate content validity ESEM five-factor model (M2) was tested so that all target
if the S-CVI is at least .80, but preferably .90 or higher loadings and cross-loadings are estimated, pending mini-
(Polit & Beck, 2006). mal constraints imposed on the unrotated solution for iden-
Factor validity. For all factor analyses presented here- tification purposes (for technical details on identification,
after, the items were treated as continuous. This was see Asparouhov & Muthn, 2009; Morin etal., 2013). One
justified by recent simulation studies which suggest that potential drawback of ESEM is rotational indeterminacy.
Likert-type scales with five or more response categories Indeed, a model could converge on different solutions
can be treated as continuous without resulting in biased depending on the chosen rotation method (for technical
parameters (Beauducel & Herzberg, 2006; Rhemtulla, details on rotation issues, see Asparouhov & Muthn,
Brosseau-Liard, & Savalei, 2012). Moreover, following 2009; Morin etal., 2013; Morin & Maano, 2011).
Marsh etal. (2010), all factor models were estimated Consequently, different factor loading patterns can be
with and without a priori correlated uniquenesses (CUs), found, which influence the size of factor correlations, even
which are used to reflect the fact that some items relate though models with different rotation methods have equal
to the same primary trait (or subdomain), share similar implication for the covariance matrix, and thus provide the
content (but reversed scoring), or share the same word.4 same fit to the data. In this study, six rotations available in
This was necessary because CUs are common in person- Mplus were tested: Geomin (Mplus default), Geomin with
ality trait measures and failure to include them can lead an epsilon value of .5 (a value recommended in previous
to biased parameter estimates (Marsh etal., 2010; Marsh ESEM studies of Big Five measures; Marsh etal., 2010),
& Hau, 1996). As noted by Marsh etal. (2010), using ex CF-varimax, CF-quartimax, CF-equamax, and finally tar-
post facto CUs should generally be avoided, but in the get loading rotation (all loadings on the nontarget factors
case of Big Five personality trait measures, many CUs, are given a start value of 0).

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
588 Assessment 21(5)

Once the best factor model was identified, the measure- 2002; Vandenberg & Lance, 2000). Chen (2007) suggested
ment invariance across genders of this model was evaluated using changes in RMSEA, where values below .015 indi-
using a series of increasingly stringent multiple-group mod- cate that the invariance hypothesis should not be rejected.
els (see Meredith, 1993; Morin & Maano, 2011; Vandenberg Despite all these suggested criteria, it is important to
& Lance, 2000): configural invariance (MG1; all loadings, note that since there are still very few applications of ESEM
intercepts, and uniquenesses are freely estimated, with the testing complex factor structures with 50 items or more and
latent variances constrained to 1 and latent means con- given that the adequacy of the aforementioned fit indices
strained to 0), metric invariance (MG2; loadings constrained and proposed cutoff scores have not been rigorously tested
to invariance, which allows free estimation of the factor with these models, their adequacy for assessing model fit in
variances in one group), scalar invariance (MG3; intercepts ESEM still needs to be thoroughly evaluated (Marsh etal.,
constrained to invariance, which allows free estimation of 2010; Morin etal., 2013). These proposed cutoff values
the factor means in one group), strict invariance (MG4; should thus be considered as useful, but rough guidelines in
uniquenesses constrained to equality), CU invariance an ESEM context.
(MG5), variance/covariance invariance (MG6; all of which Reliability. Reliability of the scales was estimated using
must be done simultaneously in ESEM), and latent means different statistical indices. First the traditional Cronbachs
invariance (MG7). For each model in this sequence, the alpha coefficient was computed. However, this coefficient
imposed constraints are additive and the preceding model is recognized as a limited estimator of reliability because
serves as a reference. it is adequate only if three conditions are met: the items
The assessment of model fit was based on various indi- are strongly unidimensional, the items are essentially tau-
ces (West, Taylor, & Wu, 2012). The chi-square test was equivalent (i.e., they all have high loadings), and there
estimated for all models. A nonsignificant chi-square sug- are no correlated errors or uniquenesses (Raykov, 1998).
gests a good fitting model. However, because this test is Therefore, the latent variable model composite reliability,
known to be overly sensitive to increasing sample size, to denoted by Rho (), suggested by Raykov (1997, 2012)
minor departure from multivariate normality and to minor was also computed. Composite reliability is essentially
(substantively irrelevant) model misspecifications, addi- the total amount of true score variance in relation to the
tional fit indices were considered more explicitly: the com- total scale score variance and thus corresponds to the tra-
parative fit index (CFI), the TuckerLewis index (TLI), the ditional concept of reliability in classical test theory. The
root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and its latent variable approach to reliability tends to provide less
90% confidence interval, and the standardized root mean biased estimates than Cronbachs alpha (Raykov, 2012).
square residual (SRMR). Values of .95 or above for the CFI Moreover, given that failure to account for correlated item
and TLI, of .06 or below for the RMSEA, and of .08 or uniquenesses can lead to systematic overestimation of
below for the SRMR have been suggested as being indica- reliability, the conditional on CUs was also computed
tive of a good model fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). However, (Raykov, 2001, 2012). Given their importance in deter-
these criteria are often considered too restrictive for many mining estimate precision, 95% confidence intervals were
applications; therefore, values of .90 or above for the CFI computed for all estimates. Finally, because the main
and TLI, of .08 or below for the RMSEA, and of .10 or objective of this study was to assess whether scales with
below for the SRMR suggest an acceptable fit of the model added items provide different reliability estimates, the
(Bentler, 1990; Marsh, Hau, & Wen, 2004). For the RMSEA method proposed by Raykov (2007b, 2012) to calculate
90% CI, values below .05 for the lower bound and below the difference between two composite reliability estimates
.08 for the upper bound suggest acceptable fit (MacCallum, was used. Using the MODEL CONSTRAINT option in
Browne, & Sugawara, 1996). Mplus, composite reliability was specified for both scales,
For the assessment of change in model fit in invariance and a change parameter (Delta) was then computed along
tests, the SatorraBentler scaled chi-square test (Satorra, with a formal statistical significance test. This method also
2000) was computed for all multiple-group models. This allows computation of a 95% confidence interval for the
test takes into account the scaling correction in MLR esti- delta parameter.
mation. However, because the chi-square test tends to be Convergent and criterion validities. For convergent valid-
overly sensitive to sample size and minor departure from ity, the scales were correlated with their corresponding
multivariate normality, researchers examine changes more scales from the NEO-PI-3 (McCrae & Costa, 2010), while
closely in other fit indices. Cheung and Rensvold (2002) for criterion validity, the scales were correlated with dif-
suggested using change in CFI, where values below .01 ferent consequential outcome scales: three scales of exter-
indicate that the invariance hypothesis should not be nalizing psychopathology, four scales of internalizing
rejected, values between .01 and .02 suggest the possibility psychopathology, one of substance use, and one of aca-
of non-invariance, and values above .02 support the rejec- demic achievement. To determine whether the added-item
tion of the invariance hypothesis (Cheung & Rensvold, scales provide higher correlations than the original-content

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 589

scales (i.e., improved convergent and criterion validity), items are substantial and are clearly statistically related to
the structural equation modeling approach suggested by their expected factor. Still, among the target loadings, 10
Raykov (2007a, 2012) was used to compare the correla- out of 50 have a value below .30, though they are statisti-
tions of scales with differing number of items. Using the cally related to their expected factor. The standard errors
MODEL CONSTRAINT option in Mplus, both correlations tend to be small for all items, including those with low load-
were specified and a correlation change parameter (Delta) ings. Examination of the confidence intervals suggests that
was then computed, along with a formal statistical signifi- all target loadings are relevant as none includes a value of 0.
cance test. This method also allows computation of a 95% Apart from the 50 target loadings, there is a total of 105
confidence interval for the delta parameter. statistically significant cross-loadings, with 58 clearly sig-
nificant at .001 and 47 of lesser significance at .05.
Examination of each factor shows that for Openness, the
Results anchor item (21) taps ingenuity and creativity. The new
Openness to Cultural Diversity item (31r), although it is
Content Validity clearly statistically related to Openness, has a somewhat low
In general, the six experts ratings all point to adequate standardized loading. As expected, this item has a significant
content validity. No item received a rating of 0 (not rele- cross-loading on Agreeableness. For Extraversion, the
vant). For individual items, 76% (38 out of 50) have a anchor item (22r) taps talkativeness and liveliness. The new
perfect agreement with an I-CVI = 1. All the other items Sensation Seeking (42) and Joyfulness (47) items are clearly
have an I-CVI above the recommended criterion of .78, statistically related to Extraversion, but also tend to have
except three items, which have an I-CVI of .666: Items 42 small loadings. As expected, Item 47 has a significant cross-
(Sensation Seeking), 45 (Low Self-Worth), and 48 loading on Emotional Stability. For Agreeableness, the
(Machiavellianism). Nonetheless, four out of six raters anchor item (28r) taps warmth and kindness. The new
identified these three items, which are all newly added Machiavellianism item (48r) is strongly related to
ones, as very representative. Regarding content validity Agreeableness and, in fact, has one of the highest loadings
of the scales, all received highly favorable ratings from the on that factor. For Conscientiousness, the anchor item (4)
experts, with all estimates above the recommended crite- taps thoroughness or diligence. The new Impulsiveness item
rion. The S-CVI is .966 for Conscientiousness, .949 for (49r) is statistically related to Conscientiousness, but has a
Openness, Extraversion, and Emotional Stability, and .933 somewhat low loading. Moreover, this is a complex item
for Agreeableness. because it has significant cross-loadings on all other factors,
particularly on Agreeableness. Finally, for Emotional
Stability, the anchor item (40r) taps nervousness. The new
Factor Validity
Low Self-Worth (45r) and Irritability (50r) items are clearly
The goodness-of-fit statistics from the different factor ana- related to Emotional Stability and are in fact items with
lytic models are presented in Table 1. All indices suggest some of the highest loadings on that factor. As expected,
that ICM-CFA clearly does not fit the data (M1). Adding a Item 45r has a significant cross-loading on Extraversion, as
priori CUs (M1b) significantly improved the fit, but it was does Item 50r on Agreeableness.
still a poor-fitting model. Fitting an ESEM model (M2) Table 3 presents the latent factor correlations and their
largely improved fit over the ICM-CFA model as suggested 95% confidence intervals from the ICM-CFA and ESEM
by the large 2, CFI, and RMSEA. After the results models. As expected, the factor correlations from ESEM
from all the tested rotations had been examined, the target are substantially smaller than those from ICM-CFA. While
loading rotation was selected because it provided a some- the average absolute factor correlation for ICM-CFA is .27
what clearer factor loading pattern (i.e., the target loadings (SD = .17), it is .13 (SD = .08) for ESEM. For example, in
tended to be slightly larger, while the cross-loadings tended both models, the largest correlation is between Agreeableness
to be smaller than with other rotation methods) and the fac- and Conscientious; it is .605 in ICM-CFA, but decreases to
tor correlations were only slightly higher.5 The fit of this .348 in ESEM. The second largest correlation in both mod-
model, however, remains unacceptable because the CFI and els is between Openness and Extraversion; it is .447 in
TLI values were below the acceptable criterion. A model ICM-CFA, but decreases to .269 in ESEM. Interestingly,
adding a priori CUs (M2b) again significantly improved the the lowest and only nonsignificant correlation in ICM-CFA
fit to the data. In contrast to the preceding models, this is between Openness and Emotional Stability, but it
ESEM with CUs shows fit indices all in the satisfactory becomes negative and significant in ESEM.
range, with CFI and TLI above .90, as well as RMSEA and The goodness-of-fit statistics from the gender invariance
SRMR below .06. tests are presented in Table 1. Fitting the ESEM model with
Table 2 presents the standardized factor loadings from all freely estimated parameters for males and females sepa-
the ESEM model with CUs (M2b). Most target loading rately provides acceptable fit to the data (MG1). Constraining

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
590 Assessment 21(5)

Table 1. Goodness-of-Fit Statistics From the Confirmatory Factor Analytic and Exploratory Structural Equation Models.

Model 2 (df) CFI TLI RMSEA 90% CI SRMR Ref S2 (df) CFI RMSEA
M1: ICM-CFA 5178.65* .680 .663 .058 [.056, .060] .081
(1165)
M1b: ICM-CFA with CUs 4285.04* .750 .732 .052 [.050, .053] .079 M1 782.43* +.070 .006
(1144) (21)
M2: ESEM 2604.68* .871 .839 .040 [.038, .042] .034 M1 2419.94* +.191 .018
(985) (180)
M2b: ESEM with CUs 1811.17* .932 .914 .029 [.027, .031] .028 M2 780.65* +.061 .011
(964) (21)
Gender invariance
MG1: Configural invariance 2895.64* .922 .901 .031 [.029, .034] .033
(1928)
MG2: invariant 3141.31* .921 .910 .030 [.028, .032] .042 MG1
255.03* .001 .001
(2153) (225)
MG3: , invariant 3431.88* .901 .890 .033 [.031, .035] .045 MG2 301.80* .020 +.003
(2198) (45)
MG3b: , partially invariant 3307.64* .911 .900 .031 [.029, .034] .044 MG2 138.13* .010 +.001
(2194) (41)
MG4: , , invariant 3481.55* .901 .892 .033 [.031, .035] .051 MG3b 171.17* .010 +.002
(2244) (50)
MG5: , , , CUs invariant 3518.22* .899 .891 .033 [.031, .035] .051 MG4 35.77 .002 +.000
(2265) (21)
MG6: , , , CUs, / invariant 3555.29* .898 .890 .033 [.031, .035] .054 MG5 36.63 .001 +.000
(2280) (15)
MG7: , , , CUs, /, invariant 3757.23* .882 .873 .035 [.033, .037] .064 MG6 220.36* .016 +.002
(2285) (5)

Note. ICM-CFA = independent clusters model confirmatory factor analysis; ESEM = exploratory structural equation modeling; 2 = chi square;
df = degrees of freedom; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = TuckerLewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; 90% CI = 90%
confidence interval of the RMSEA; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual; Ref = reference model; S2 = SatorraBentler scaled chi-square
difference test; df = change in degrees of freedom; CFI = change in CFI; RMSEA = change in RMSEA; = factor loadings; = intercepts;
= uniquenesses; = factor variances; = factor covariances; = factor means.
*p < .01.

all factor loadings to equality did not significantly worsen not lead to a significant worsening of fit (MG5), suggesting
the fit (MG2). This result is noteworthy, because such a test that these parameters are not gender-specific. Overall, even
involves many more parameters (i.e., all target loadings and if the fit indices tended to gradually decrease with the
cross-loadings are constrained to equality) than in ICM- increases in equality constraints (CFI and TLI), the RMSEA
CFA. Constraining intercepts to equality across genders, remained in the acceptable range for all models, even con-
however, did seem to suggest the possibility of non- sidering its confidence intervals. All these results reason-
invariance (MG3). Indeed, the scaled chi-square difference ably suggest that the BFPTSQ factor structure show full
test is significant and the change in CFI is at the upper limit measurement invariance across genders.
of the cutoff criterion suggesting the possibility of non- Constraining the variances/covariances across genders
invariance. Therefore, a model with partial invariance of did not lead to a significant worsening of fit (MG6). As
intercepts (MG3b) was estimated (Byrne, Shavelson, & expected, however, constraining the latent factor means to
Muthn, 1989). Based on modification indices, four items equality across genders did lead to a significant worsening
were freed across groups: females had higher item inter- in fit (MG7). Examination of the modification indices and
cepts for Items 26, 41, and 47 and a lower item intercept for the results of the preceding model (MG6) revealed a num-
Item 40. This model provided a better fit than the model ber of significant mean differences between males and
with fully invariant intercepts. Constraining item unique- females. With the males means fixed to 0 and the variances
nesses across genders did not lead to a significant worsen- fixed to 1 in both groupswhich allows interpretation of
ing in model fit (MG4). Indeed, even though the scaled these differences in terms of traditional effect sizethe
chi-square test is significant, the change in CFI is not sig- females latent means were significantly lower in Emotional
nificant and the change in RMSEA is again trivial. Another Stability (.755, p < .0001), significantly higher in
model constraining the CUs to equality across genders did Agreeableness (.578, p < .0001) and Conscientiousness

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Table 2. Standardized Factor Loadings From the Exploratory Structural Equation Model of the BFPTSQ Items.

Openness Extraversion Agreeableness Conscientiousness Emotional Stability

Item 95% CI 95% CI 95% CI 95% CI 95% CI


1 .504** [.373, .634] .219** [.146, .292] .133** [.068, .198] .047 [.129, .035] .051 [.009, .112] .640
6 .304** [.215, .393] .255** [.178, .332] .065 [.034, .163] .061 [.144, .022] .135** [.207, .064] .792
11 .471** [.365, .576] .074 [.154, .006] .044 [.114, .027] .242** [.155, .329] .089* [.027, .151] .687
16 .534** [.427, .640] .067 [.003, .137] .107* .038, .176] .156** [.235, .077] .005 [.054, .064] .711
21 .577** [.446, .708] .115* [.039, .191] .103* [.040, .166] .032 [.117, .053] .080* [.021, .140] .622
26 .306** [.205, .407] .049 [.030, .129] .137* [.060, .215] .063 [.020, .146] .105* [.179, .030] .850
31r .269** [.144, .388] .077 [.161, .008] .218** [.133, .303] .044 [.046, .134] .009 [.068, .087] .898
36 .408** [.325, .490] .091* [.160, .023] .041 [.120, .039] .196** [.122, .270] .063 [.002, .128] .778
41 .214** [.107, .321] .070 [.154, .014] .076 [.010, .162] .033 [.055, .122] .007 [.068, .083] .947
46 .363** [.271, .456] .131* [.206, .056] .066 [.014, .147] .011 [.072, .095] .003 [.072, .065] .871

2 .180** [.105, .255] .525** [.454, .595] .036 [.035, .107] .028 [.035, .091] .078* [.139, .016] .642
7r .239** [.307, .170] .642** [.581, .704] .005 [.059, .069] .011 [.068, .046] .192** [.124, .260] .521
12 .079 [.007, .164] .440** [.369, .512] .110* [.029, .192] .107* [.032, .181] .007 [.056, .071] .740
17 .240** [.167, .313] .468** [.401, .535] .142** [.211, .074] .101* [.036, .166] .093* [.034, .153] .601
22r .193** [.270, .115] .726** [.656, .796] .021 [.091, .050] .109* [.173, .044] .021 [.094, .053] .504
27 .209** [.138, .280] .448** [.382, .513] .167** [.232, .102] .228** [.166, .291] .191** [.130, .252] .539
32r .151* [.240, .062] .668** [.594, .742] .021 [.093, .050] .002 [.067, .064] .234** [.156, .313] .467
37 .053 [.035, .142] .628** [.553, .703] .144** [.066, .221] .011 [.057, .078] .043 [.094, .008] .576
42 .116* [.018, .215] .289** [.205, .373] .068 [.155, .019] .051 [.135, .033] .039 [.034, .112] .882
47 .104* [.001, .206] .296** [.214, .379] .149** [.061, .237] .091* [.175, .007] .226** [.300, .152] .846

3r .022 [.101, .057] .156** [.229, .083] .387** [.308, .466] .050 [.025, .126] .070* [.004, .136] .795
8 .201** [.116, .386] .053 [.018, .124] .264** [.164, .365] .180** [.094, .266] .064 [.137, .009] .770
13r .021 [.057, .098] .078* [.138, .017] .420** [.350, .489] .206** [.137, .274] .112* [.048, .175] .686

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
18 .228** [.144, .313] .083* [.157, .008] .223** [.120, .326] .053 [.140, .035] .051 [.029, .132] .873
23 .192** [.082, .302] .161** [.068, .253] .269** [.166, .373] .031 [.059, .120] .034 [.115, .048] .845
28r .078 [.180, .025] .114** [.050, .177] .550** [.478, .621] .047 [.116, .022] .110* [.046, .173] .665
33 .208** [.105, .312] .144** [.064, .224] .391** [.280, .502] .188** [.099, .278] .105* [.178, .033] .620
38r .004 [.125, .116] .153** [.222, .085] .542** [.466, .617] .167** [.097, .238] .088* [.018, .158] .570
43 .219** [.116, .322] .306** [.221, .392] .326** [.223, .429] .029 [.059, .117] .039 [.114, .037] .718
48r .060 [.143, .024] .143** [.205, .081] .504** [.435, .573] .045 [.027, .118] .044 [.018, .106] .690

(continued)

591
592
Table 2.(continued)

Openness Extraversion Agreeableness Conscientiousness Emotional Stability

Item 95% CI 95% CI 95% CI 95% CI 95% CI



4 .057 [.030, .145] .018 [.071, .034] .009 [.057, .075] .716** [.649, .792] .089* [.143, .036] .474
9r .155** [.239, .070] .048 [.023, .118] .308** [.228, .387] .213** [.135, .291] .024 [.046, .095] .747
14 .045 [.042, .133] .052 [.003, .108] .031 [.095, .033] .681** [.611, .750] .088* [.142, .034] .536
19r .244** [.317, .171] .002 [.061, .065] .017 [.067, .102] .587** [.520, .655] .035 [.031, .101] .643
24r .123* [.211, .034] .062 [.008, .133] .194** [.115, .274] .416** [.337, .494] .032 [.040, .105] .728
29 .093* [.022, .163] .058 [.003, .113] .009 [.058, .077] .618** [.557, .679] .048 [.105, .009] .574
34 .100* [.021, .179] .074* [.019, .129] .089* [.152, .025] .713** [.654, .773] .029 [.084, .025] .472
39 .141* [.046, .237] .047 [.019, .112] .012 [.092, .069] .441** [.359, .522] .096* [.164, .028] .754
44r .096* [.187, .005] .088* [.158, .017] .005 [.082, .092] .472** [.393, .552] .236** [.164, .307] .692
49r .125* [.220, .030] .223** [.288, .158] .317** [.240, .395] .278** [.206, .351] .165** [.098, .232] .607

5r .195** [.276, .113] .217** [.147, .287] .152** [.073, .230] .164** [.090, .237] .259** [.183, .335] .756
10 .211** [.119, .303] .072 [.007, .150] .086* [.170, .003] .032 [.107, .043] .589** [.508, .669] .622
15r .001 [.063, .061] .053 [.003, .110] .011 [.070, .048] .118** [.174, .062] .678** [.623, .739] .538
20r .154** [.224, .084] .033 [.029, .095] .022 [.049, .094] .012 [.079, .056] .537** [.475, .598] .661
25 .121* [.030, .211] .024 [.099, .051] .125* [.207, .043] .020 [.059, .099] .468** [.394, .543] .782
30r .115* [.213, .017] .003 [.065, .072] .250** [.169, .331] .014 [.089, .061] .379** [.306, .452] .744
35 .260** [.164, .357] .030 [.103, .044] .093* [.180, .006] .003 [.070, .077] .614** [.535, .693] .608
40r .057 [.007, .120] .065* [.012, .118] .020 [.080, .040] .096* [.151, .041] .773** [.728, .819] .404

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
45r .081 [.162, .001] .210** [.144, .276] .035 [.113, .043] .144** [.072, .216] .459** [.387, .532] .670
50r .076 [.177, .025] .085* [.152, .017] .354** [.279, .430] .080* [.008, .151] .350** [.279, .421] .644

Note. Shaded entries are the target loading items. Item numbers with an r are reverse scored. = factor loadings; = uniquenesses; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval.
*p < .05. **p < .001.
Morizot 593

Table 3. Point and Interval Estimate of Factor Correlations of the BFPTSQ.

1. Openness 2. Extraversion 3. Agreeableness 4. Conscientiousness 5. Emotional Stability

95% CI 95% CI 95% CI 95% CI 95% CI


1. .269** [.211, .327] .032 [.092, .028] .227** [.130, .324] .119** [.181, .058]
2. .447** [.349, .545] .025 [.100, .050] .078* [.009, .146] .179** [.102, .256]
3. .259** [.144, .373] .059 [.064, .181] .348** [.279, .418] .141** [.081, .201]
4. .318** [.221, .416] .187** [.095, .278] .605** [.527, .682] .155** [.087, .223]
5. .041 [.045, .127] .367** [.281, .453] .235** [.141, .328] .137** [.048, .226]

Note. Latent factor correlations from the final exploratory structural equation model (ESEM) are presented above the diagonal, while latent
correlations from the independent clusters model confirmatory factor analysis (ICM-CFA) are presented below the diagonal. = factor covariance/
correlation; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval.
*p < .05. **p < .001.

Table 4. Point and Interval Reliability Estimates of the BFPTSQ Scales.

Original-Content Scale Added-Items Scale

95% CI 95% CI 95% CI


Openness
No CU .716 .712** [.685, .740] .712 .702** [.672, .732] .011* [.020, .002]
With CUs .648** [.613, .683] .646** [.609, .682] .002 [.013, .009]
Extraversion
No CU .819 .828** [.811, .846] .802 .813** [.795, .832] .015** [.022, .008]
With CUs .783** [.758, .808] .777** [.754, .801] .006 [.014, .003]
Agreeableness
No CU .701 .705** [.677, .734] .722 .723** [.694, .752] .018* [.007, .029]
With CUs .655** [.618, .693] .690** [.658, .723] .035** [.024, .047]
Conscientiousness
No CU .795 .796** [.777, .815] .796 .792** [.772, .812] .004 [.009, .002]
With CUs .778** [.756, .800] .776** [.753, .799] .002 [.008, .004]
Emotional Stability
No CU .792 .802** [.782, .821] .808 .812** [.793, .830] .010* [.002, .018]
With CUs .764** [.741, .788] .786** [.765, .808] .022 [.013, .031]

Note. = Cronbachs alpha internal consistency coefficient; = latent variable model composite reliability estimate; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval;
= delta parameter (composite reliability difference estimate); CU = correlated uniqueness.
*p < .05. **p < .001.

(.344, p < .001), and marginally higher in Openness (.158, Stability, while they tend to be the lowest for Openness
p < .10). There was no significant gender difference in and Agreeableness.
Extraversion. Overall, the delta parameters () resulting from the
comparison tests between the original-content scales and
the added-item scales tend to suggest that both scales pro-
Reliability vide similar reliability estimates. This is supported by the
The point and interval estimates of reliability and the fact that the confidence intervals of all composite reliabil-
comparison tests between reliability estimates () are ity estimates are overlapping. This demonstrates that the
presented in Table 4. In general, all estimates suggest newly added items are not detrimental to scale reliability,
that the BFPTSQ scales have adequate reliability. The despite the fact that a number of them show low factor
traditional and without CUs tend to be similar across loadings. In fact, when considering without CUs, the
all scales. As expected, however, conditional on CUs results showed that the added items significantly decrease
are all noticeably lower, decreasing below .70 for reliability for Openness and Extraversion, while they sig-
Openness and Agreeableness. The estimates tend to be nificantly increase reliability for Agreeableness and
somewhat higher for Extraversion and Emotional Emotional Stability. However, when the conditional on

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
594 Assessment 21(5)

Table 5. Point and Interval Correlation Estimates Between BFPTSQ and NEO-PI-3 Scales.

Original-Content Scale Added-Items Scale

NEO-PI-3 Scales r 95% CI r 95% CI r 95% CI


Openness
Openness .591** [.540, .642] .629** [.583, .675] .038** [.020, .056]
Fantasy .377** [.308, .446] .385** [.315, .455] .008 [.014, .031]
Aesthetics .459** [.394, .524] .509** [.448, .570] .050** [.030, .071]
Feelings .317** [.244, .389] .331** [.260, .401] .014 [.010, .038]
Actions .186** [.108, .265] .171** [.092, .249] .016 [.039, .008]
Ideas .465** [.406, .524] .512** [.459, .566] .047** [.026, .068]
Values .196** [.116, .276] .204** [.128, .280] .008 [.015, .031]
Extraversion
Extraversion .650** [.602, .698] .694** [.650, .737] .043** [.030, .057]
Warmth .476** [.407, .545] .494** [.427, .562] .019* [.002, .036]
Gregariousness .453** [.387, .518] .477** [.413, .542] .025** [.010, .040]
Assertiveness .662** [.617, .708] .653** [.607, .699] .010 [.022, .003]
Activity .481** [.417, .544] .490** [.427, .553] .010 [.006, .025]
Excitement Seeking .347** [.282, .412] .435** [.375, .495] .088** [.072, .104]
Positive Emotions .340** [.262, .418] .390** [.314, .465] .050** [.032, .067]
Agreeableness
Agreeableness .612** [.565, .659] .671** [.628, .713] .059** [.047, .070]
Trust .472** [.408, .536] .477** [.413, .541] .005 [.010, .020]
Straightforwardness .432** [.373, .492] .532** [.479, .586] .100** [.086, .114]
Altruism .593** [.545, .640] .598** [.549, .646] .005 [.008, .018]
Compliance .436** [.376, .497] .471** [.410, .531] .034** [.020, .048]
Modesty .076 [.000, .162] .114* [.024, .204] .039** [.023, .054]
Tender-Mindedness .322** [.248, .396] .337** [.264, .409] .015* [.001, .030]
Conscientiousness
Conscientiousness .789** [.760, .818] .804** [.776, .832] .015** [.006, .024]
Competence .580** [.528, .632] .598** [.547, .649] .018* [.006, .029]
Order .616** [.568, .664] .616** [.568, .664] .000 [.011, .012]
Dutifulness .674** [.629, .719] .681** [.635, .726] .007 [.005, .018]
Achievement Striving .614** [.566, .662] .604** [.555, .652] .010 [.022, .001]
Self-Discipline .723** [.685, .761] .727** [.689, .764] .004 [.006, .014]
Deliberation .467** [.408, .527] .519** [.462, .576] .052** [.039, .064]
Emotional Stability
Neuroticisma .733** [.695, .771] .777** [.745, .809] .044** [.030, .058]
Anxiety .692** [.650, .734] .680** [.638, .722] .012 [.027, .004]
Angry Hostility .469** [.407, .532] .510** [.451, .569] .040** [.024, .057]
Depression .560** [.504, .616] .619** [.569, .670] .059** [.044, .075]
Self-Conscientiousness .486** [.427, .545] .534** [.480, .588] .048** [.032, .065]
Impulsiveness .285** [.205, .366] .319** [.242, .396] .034** [.015, .053]
Vulnerability .639** [.590, .687] .662** [.617, .706] .023* [.009, .037]

Note. In this table, each Big Five personality trait is correlated with its corresponding higher-order and primary-trait scales from the NEO-PI-3. 95%
CI = 95% confidence interval; r = delta parameter (correlation difference estimate).
a.Because Emotional Stability is simply the opposite pole, all correlations with Neuroticism and its primary traits from the NEO-PI-3 are actually
negative and are presented in absolute value to simplify the table.
*p < .05. **p < .001.

CUs are consideredwhich are arguably closer to the true Convergent Validity
modelsall differences between reliability estimates are
nonsignificant, except for Agreeableness, which continues The point and interval correlation estimates between the
to suggest that the added item increases reliability. BFPTSQ and NEO-PI-3 scales as well as the comparison
tests between correlations (r) are presented in Table 5. The

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 595

overall pattern of correlations suggests adequate convergent Stability is related to lower scores on all psychopathology
validity. All correlations between the broad-trait scales scales, except CD and substance use. Emotional Stability is
are high, mostly around .70. For the scales with added not related to GPA. As expected, Emotional Stability is
items, they range from .629 for Openness to .804 for most strongly related to the four internalizing psychopa-
Conscientiousness. The correlations between the BFPTSQ thology scales, particularly MDD, GAD, and SOP.6
scales and the corresponding NEO-PI-3 primary-trait scales Overall, the delta parameters (r) resulting from the
are generally lower, but all are clearly significant. The only comparison tests between the original-content scales and
exception is the correlation between BFPTSQ Agreeableness those with added items suggest that that the new scales sig-
and Modesty, which is quite low and even nonsignificant nificantly improved criterion validity. For Openness, the
for the original-content scale. added-item scale increases the negative relations with CD
Overall, the delta parameters (r) resulting from the (i.e., it becomes more negative), but reduces the positive
comparison tests between the original-content scales and relation with BD. For Extraversion, the added-item scale
those with added items suggest that the new scales signifi- increases the positive correlations with ADHD, ODD, and
cantly improved convergent validity. Indeed, all correla- BD, but reduces the negative relations with MDD and SOP.
tions between the broad Big Five trait scales are significantly For Agreeableness, the added-item scale increases the nega-
higher for the added-item scales. However, the confidence tive relation with ADHD, CD, ODD, and BD, but does not
intervals of all correlation pairs overlap somewhat. With change the relations with internalizing psychopathology
regard to the primary-trait scales, the expected results are scales or GPA. For Conscientiousness, the added-item scale
observed. Indeed, for Extraversion, the increase in correla- increases the negative relations with all psychopathology
tion is largest for Excitement Seeking and Positive scales, but does not change the relations with GPA. Finally,
Emotions. For Agreeableness, the increase is largest for for Emotional Stability, the added-item scale increases the
Straightforwardness, while for Conscientiousness, it is larg- negative relations with ADHD, ODD, MDD, BD, and SOP.
est for Deliberation. For Emotional Stability, the increase in However, the confidence intervals of all correlation pairs
correlation is largest for Depression, Self-Consciousness, overlap somewhat.
and Angry Hostility. In addition to these expected improved
correlations, several other correlations with primary-trait
scales also significantly increased, which is to be expected
Discussion
since all primary traits have a common variance associated The general objective of this study was to evaluate the con-
with their corresponding broad Big Five trait. Again, the struct validity of adolescents self-reported personality
confidence intervals of all correlation pairs overlap traits. In accordance with the view of construct validity as a
somewhat. unifying form of validity requiring the integration of differ-
ent complementary sources of information (Messick, 1995;
Simms & Watson, 2007), content validity, factor validity,
Criterion Validity convergent validity, criterion validity, and reliability were
The point and interval correlation estimates between the evaluated. A secondary objective was to evaluate the poten-
BFPTSQ and outcome scales as well as the comparison tially beneficial impact of increasing the conceptual breadth
tests between correlations (r) are presented in Table 6. The of scales from a short personality measure. Starting with an
pattern of correlations between the Big Five and outcome item pool from an existing measure, the language level was
scales generally suggests adequate criterion validity. first adjusted for use with adolescents, and items tapping
Openness is related to lower scores in CD and to higher fundamental primary personality traits that were missing
scores in BD and GAD. As expected, Openness is most from this pool were added to each of the Big Five scales.
strongly related to higher scores in GPA. Extraversion is This led to a new measure which was named the Big Five
related to higher scores in ADHD, CD, BD, and substance Personality Trait Short Questionnaire (BFPTSQ). Overall,
use, but to lower scores in MDD and SOP. As anticipated, the results of this study supported the construct validity of
Extraversion is most strongly related to lower scores in SOP adolescents self-reported Big Five personality traits. The
and to higher scores in ADHD. Agreeableness is related to results also support the idea that adding conceptual breadth
lower scores on all psychopathology scales and substance in a short personality trait measure can have a significant
use, but to higher scores in GPA. As projected, Agreeableness positive impact on some of its psychometric properties.
is most strongly related to the three externalizing psychopa-
thology scales, particularly CD and ODD. Conscientiousness
Construct Validity of the BFPTSQ
is also related to lower scores on all psychopathology scales
and substance use, but to higher scores in GPA. As hypoth- For the evaluation of content validity, experts in personality
esized, Conscientiousness is most strongly related to higher theory and questionnaire construction were asked to rate the
scores in GPA and to lower scores in ADHD. Emotional adequacy or representativeness of the items. All items were

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
596 Assessment 21(5)

Table 6. Point and Interval Estimates of Correlations Between BFPTSQ and Outcomes Scales.

Original-Content Scale Added-Items Scale

Outcome Scales r 95% CI r 95% CI r 95% CI


Openness
ADHD .057 [.006, .120] .000 [.064, .064] .057** [.075, .038]
CD .094* [.161, .027] .134** [.198, .071] .040** [.060, .020]
ODD .003 [.065, .059] .042 [.104, .020] .039** [.059, .019]
MDD .063* [.001, .127] .051 [.012, .114] .012 [.031, .007]
BD .090* [.027, .153] .071* [.007, .134] .019* [.038, .000]
GAD .093* [.029, .156] .095* [.032, .157] .002 [.018, .021]
SOP .030 [.095, .035] .016 [.080, .048] .014 [.006, .033]
SUBS .017 [.094, .061] .037 [.105, .031] .020 [.046, .006]
GPA .262** [.192, .333] .279** [.210, .348] .017 [.003, .037]
Extraversion
ADHD .178** [.111, .244] .219** [.155, .283] .041** [.026, .057]
CD .105** [.041, .168] .094* [.028, .160] .011 [.028, .007]
ODD .056 [.011, .123] .074* [.007, .141] .018* [.003, .033]
MDD .140** [.208, .072] .107** [.176, .038] .034** [.020, .048]
BD .153** [.087, .220] .194** [.129, .259] .040** [.027, .054]
GAD .088* [.157, .020] .057 [.126, .011] .031** [.017, .045]
SOP .341** [.399, .282] .324** [.383, .265] .016* [.002, .031]
SUBS .121** [.064, .179] .112** [.056, .168] .009 [.038, .020]
GPA .061 [.015, .137] .075* [.002, .153] .014 [.003, .032]
Agreeableness
ADHD .308** [.371, .244] .338** [.399, .277] .030** [.044, .017]
CD .393** [.458, .328] .429** [.490, .369] .036** [.052, .021]
ODD .442** [.497, .387] .480** [.532, .427] .037** [.050, .024]
MDD .200** [.261, .138] .207** [.270, .145] .007 [.020, .005]
BD .169** [.233, .106] .210** [.272, .147] .040** [.053, .027]
GAD .136** [.200, .072] .143** [.208, .079] .007 [.020, .006]
SOP .164** [.228, .101] .157** [.222, .093] .007 [.007, .020]
SUBS .124** [.197, .052] .140** [.214, .066] .016 [.003, .001]
GPA .323** [.255, .391] .314** [.246, .382] .009 [.024, .005]
Conscientiousness
ADHD .512** [.564, .461] .560** [.607, .512] .047** [.058, .037]
CD .361** [.425, .298] .401** [.460, .341] .039** [.052, .027]
ODD .384** [.442, .327] .439** [.492, .386] .055** [.066, .043]
MDD .264** [.324, .204] .297** [.356, .237] .033** [.044, .021]
BD .252** [.316, .188] .299** [.360, .237] .047** [.058, .035]
GAD .133** [.198, .068] .171** [.235, .106] .038** [.050, .027]
SOP .126** [.191, .061] .141** [.207, .076] .015* [.027, .004]
SUBS .155** [.249, .062] .178** [.268, .087] .022** [.035, .010]
GPA .404** [.340, .467] .401** [.338, .463] .003 [.015, .010]
Emotional Stability
ADHD .223** [.291, .154] .264** [.332, .196] .041** [.057, .026]
CD .001 [.069, .071] .036 [.107, .036] .037** [.051, .022]
ODD .282** [.346, .217] .327** [.390, .264] .045** [.061, .030]
MDD .527** [.576, .479] .574** [.619, .530] .047** [.060, .034]
BD .236** [.301, .171] .274** [.336, .211] .037** [.053, .022]
GAD .606** [.651, .560] .616** [.661, .570] .010 [.022, .002]
SOP .428** [.483, .373] .467** [.520, .414] .040** [.053, .026]
SUBS .013 [.065, .090] .010 [.087, .067] .023** [.036, .010]
GPA .036 [.039, .111] .069 [.005, .144] .033** [.016, .051]

Note. All psychopathology scales are dimensional scores representing the sum of the frequency for all items. ADHD = Attention Deficit Hyperactivity
Disorder; CD = Conduct Disorder; ODD = Oppositional Defiant Disorder; MDD = Major Depression Disorder; BP = Bipolar Disorder; GAD =
Generalized Anxiety Disorder; SOP = Social Phobia; SUBS = Substance Use; GPA = Grade Point Average; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval; r = delta
parameter (correlation difference estimate).

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .001.

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 597

identified as valid indicators of their target trait according to Big Five when it is measured by several items. Indeed, as
the usual criteria (Polit & Beck, 2006), which suggests recently demonstrated by a number of researchers, it is
that the BFPTSQ scales have adequate content validity. more appropriate to use ESEM to model all possible factor
However, three items, all newly added ones, were identified loadings because it is closer than ICM-CFA to the true
as somewhat less representative, namely Items 42 (Sensation model of the Big Five structure (e.g., Furnham, Guenole,
Seeking), 45 (Low Self-Worth), and 48 (Machiavellianism). Levine, & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2013; Marsh etal., 2010;
This suggests that even though most scholars would agree Rosellini & Brown, 2011). In this study, out of a total of 250
that these items represent important personality traits, there possible factor loadings (number of items times the number
appears to be no clear consensus among experts as to their of factors), there was a total of 105 (42%) significant cross-
place within the Big Five taxonomy. loadings. It should be noted, however, that the fit of the
The tests of the BFPTSQ factor validity revealed five ESEM model is acceptable, but far from excellent accord-
notable results. First, the Big Five factor structure was well ing to typical criteria suggested for practical fit indices (i.e.,
recovered in a sample of French Canadian adolescents. Hu & Bentler, 1999). This is perhaps not surprising because
There are still few published item-level factor analyses of as Marsh, Hau, Balla, and Grayson (1998) noted, as the
the Big Five structure with adolescents self-reports, but the number of indicators in a factor model increases, there tends
results of this study tend to replicate those from other stud- to be a decrease in fit, even for properly specified models.
ies with adolescent samples (e.g., Allik etal., 2004; McCrae A fourth interesting result from the factor analyses is that
etal., 2002; W.D. Parker & Stumpf, 1998; Soto etal., 2008). factor correlations are considerably lower with ESEM than
Second, all the newly added items were significantly asso- with ICM-CFA. As shown by Marsh etal. (2010), when
ciated with their target Big Five trait. However, some of researchers use ICM-CFA, thereby fixing to 0 the numerous
these added items tended to show low factor loadings. This significant cross-loadings that are actually expected in a
is perhaps not so surprising because, in accordance with the Big Five personality trait structure, factor correlations are
classic bandwidth-fidelity dilemma (Cronbach & Gleser, vastly inflated because this is the way these cross-loadings
1965), if there is an increase in the conceptual breadth of a can be represented (see also Asparouhov & Muthn, 2009).
scale composed of repeated items measuring only a few pri- Using ESEM provides factor correlations that are probably
mary traits, these new items will inevitably have somewhat closer to the true population parameters and supports the
lower loadings. Even though factor loadings are commonly discriminant validity among the Big Five traits as measured
considered meaningful when they exceed .30 or .40, this by the BFPTSQ.
popular rule of thumb is generally not recommended for A fifth interesting finding from the factor analyses is the
deciding whether an item is part of a factor. Indeed, as measurement invariance across genders. This is important
pointed out by Preacher and MacCallum (2003), now that because to make valid factor mean comparisons, research-
some statistical programs calculate the standard error asso- ers must demonstrate at least that the loadings and inter-
ciated with each factor loading, it is advisable to use statisti- cepts are invariant across groups (Millsap & Olivera-Aguilar,
cal significance and confidence intervals. Also be taken into 2012). Unfortunately, these verifications are still rarely
account is the fact that including CUs in an ESEM model done in personality research. The results of this study sug-
typically results in lower loading estimates for some items. gest that it is reasonable to assume that the BFPTSQ factor
In factor analyses of the NEO-FFI items, Marsh etal. (2010) structure shows measurement invariance across genders.
also observed that a number of items show standardized That is, factor loadings, item uniquenesses, CUs, and factor
loadings below the common recommendation of .30. Apart variances/covariances (or correlations) were all fairly
from the low loadings, another characteristic of the factor invariant across males and females. However, the item
solution observed in this study is that there were a number intercepts were not fully invariant and four of them were
of complex items (i.e., items with significant loadings on freed across groups. Two of these items were from Openness
two or more factors). For instance, the Impulsiveness item and were significantly higher for females, and, interest-
(48r) added to the Conscientiousness scale is one such item. ingly, both tap artistic interests (26 and 41r). This suggests
Even though it would be conceptually preferable to have no that females are significantly more disposed than males to
such complex items (Donnellan etal., 2006), given that have artistic interests during adolescence. Another intercept
Impulsiveness is such a fundamental personality trait, tapping joyfulness (47) was significantly higher for females,
researchers must deal with the trade-off between a full rep- which suggests they are more prone than males to experi-
resentation of primary traits within a Big Five measure and ence positive emotions during adolescence. Finally, the
the increased complexity of interpreting scale scores with intercept of a reversed item tapping nervousness (40r) was
complex items. significantly lower for females, which suggests that females
A third notable result from factor validity tests is that it are more predisposed than males to experience anxiety.
is difficultif even possibleto achieve good model fit to Non-invariant item intercepts suggest that the mean differ-
the data by using ICM-CFA for a complex structure like the ences observed across groups in the corresponding factors

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
598 Assessment 21(5)

are not uniquely due to latent factor mean differences, but Concerning convergent validity, overall, the correla-
also in part to differences in these invariant item intercepts tions with the NEO-PI-3 (McCrae & Costa, 2010)
(Millsap & Olivera-Aguilar, 2012). However, even though scales suggest adequate validity of the BFPTSQ scales.
researchers would want all intercepts to be invariant across All the correlations between the broad-trait scales
groups, partial invariance is certainly acceptable for making are high, ranging from .629 for Openness to .804 for
valid mean comparisons when there are only a few invariant Conscientiousness. The comparison tests confirmed
items (Byrne etal., 1989). that, compared with those with original content,
Backed up with this measurement invariance, the the BFPTSQ scales with added items significantly
results showed that there are sizable mean gender differ- increased these correlations for all Big Five traits.
ences in adolescents personality traits. In order of mag- Moreover, the correlations between the BFPTSQ scales
nitude, females have significantly lower levels of and their target NEO-PI-3 primary-trait scales were
Emotional Stability, significantly higher levels of generally moderate to high, and all were significant.
Agreeableness and Conscientiousness, and marginally Because nearly 43% (13 out of 30) of the correlations
higher levels of Openness. There was no significant gen- with NEO-PI-3 primary-trait scales did not signifi-
der difference in Extraversion in this sample. These cantly increase with the added items, this suggests that
results are consistent with those from a recent meta-anal- the correlation increases are not merely a matter of
ysis with adult samples, except for the absence of differ- increased general variance. Interestingly, the compari-
ences in Extraversion (Schmitt etal., 2008). However, son tests confirmed that the BFPTSQ scales with new
when gender differences in Extraversion are observed in items showed significantly higher correlations with a
adolescents, they are typically rather small (Allik etal., number of NEO-PI-3 primary-trait scales. Critically,
2004). A study by Costa, Terracciano, and McCrae (2001) the comparison tests generally confirmed expectations
could help in interpreting these results. These authors regarding the BFPTSQ scales as compared with those
observed that while mean gender differences tended to be with the original content: for Extraversion, the increase
observed across all primary traits subsumed by in correlation was larger for Excitement Seeking and
Neuroticism and Agreeableness, gender differences were Positive Emotions; for Agreeableness, it was for
not consistent across primary traits for Openness and Straightforwardness; for Conscientiousness, it was for
Extraversion. For these two broad traits, some primary Deliberation (which could also be called Impulsiveness
traits favored women, while others favored men. based on its content); and for Emotional Stability, it
Differences within broad trait thus explained that mean was for Depression and Self-Consciousness. Contrary
gender differences were rather small for Openness and to expectations, however, the BFPTSQ Openness scale
Extraversion. It is thus possible that the particular items with a new item tapping Openness to Cultural Diversity
included in the BFPTSQ scales partly explain the absence did not significantly increase the relation with the
of mean difference in Extraversion. NEO-PI-3 Openness to Values scale, which is surpris-
With regard to the reliability of the BFPTSQ scales, ing since it contains similar items. It should be noted
overall, the results suggest that the estimates are all accept- that for comparison tests, when the confidence inter-
able. Of importance, the comparison tests (i.e., delta param- vals are considered, all overlap somewhat. Even though
eters) between the original-content scales and the BFPTSQ nonoverlapping 95% confidence intervals are rather
ones with added items revealed that the new items did not stringent tests of the difference between two estimates
improve reliability, nor did they have a detrimental impact. (Cumming, 2012), this suggest that the correlation
Of course, only one or two items were added per scale, so increase flagged as statistically significant as per the
the absence of significant influence on reliability estimates delta parameters tends to be small.
is not unexpected. Still, it is interesting that reliability esti- As for criterion validity, overall the correlations with
mates were not significantly decreased, considering that the outcome measures suggest adequate concurrent
some of these new items tended to have somewhat low fac- validity of the BFPTSQ scales. As expected based on
tor loadings. Another noteworthy observation concerning scholarly reviews (Klein etal., 2012; Tackett, Martel,
reliability is that the latent variable model composite reli- etal., 2012; Widiger & Smith, 2008) and meta-analytic
ability () estimates were systematically lower when mod- findings derived with adult samples (Malouff etal.,
els including CUs were considered. It is known that the 2005), externalizing psychopathology scales tended
traditional Cronbachs alpha coefficient will be accurate to be related to low levels of Agreeableness and
only when the items are clearly unidimensional and essen- Conscientiousness and, to a lesser extent, to low levels
tially tau-equivalent and the uniquenesses are uncorrelated of Emotional Stability and high levels of Extraversion.
(Raykov, 2001, 2012). The results of this study confirmed However, the BFPTSQ Extraversion scale did not show
that CUs that are not accounted for tend to inflate reliability a significantly higher correlation for substance use. A
estimates of personality trait scales. higher correlation was expected because of the added

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 599

Sensation Seeking item, which is typically related to load strongly (but significantly) on their target factor,
substance use (Sargant etal., 2010; Zuckerman, 2009). they should not be excluded a priori for this reason. In
Internalizing psychopathology scales, meanwhile, other words, results of factor or reliability analyses
tended to be related to low levels of Emotional Stability, should not supersede issues of conceptual breadth and
Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness and, to a lesser validity in questionnaire construction (Cizek, Rosenberg,
extent, to low levels of Extraversion. An exception is & Koos, 2008; Loevinger, 1957). This should rather be
bipolar disorder, which was related to high levels of taken as a challenge for personality researchers to find
Extraversion, even though it is considered a mood disor- the place of these primary personality traits in the com-
der. As expected based on Malouff etal.s (2005) meta- plex multivariate space of the Big Five taxonomy. This
analytic findings, Openness showed small but significant challenge is difficult, however, because even though
relations to some psychopathology scales. Indeed, this personality psychologists have reached a reasonable
meta-analysis found that for self-reports, Openness is consensus on the hierarchical nature and number of
positively related to psychopathology, while for observer broad personality traits (Markon etal., 2005), they are
ratings, there is a negative relation. In the present study, not even close to a consensus concerning the nature and
Openness was negatively related to CD and positively number of fundamental primary traits.
related to MDD, BD and GAD. However, compared
with the other correlations, they tended to be small.
Moreover, as expected based on meta-analytic findings
Limitations and Future Research
(Poropat, 2009), Conscientiousness and Openness were The BFPTSQ psychometric properties evaluated in this
both positively related to GPA. In this sample of adoles- study are generally satisfactory, but a few limitations
cents, Agreeableness was also clearly related to GPA, should be mentioned. First, all the psychometric proper-
much more strongly than is typically observed in adult ties of the BFPTSQ were evaluated for one sample of
samples (Noftle & Robins, 2007). The comparison tests adolescents. Even though efforts were made to gather a
generally confirmed that, compared with those with the large, very roughly representative sample of adolescents,
original content, the BFPTSQ scales with added items the results need to be replicated. Second, because study-
provide several significantly higher correlations with ing personality trait development is important, a cross-
outcome scales. Because 33% (15 out of 45) of the cor- validation should be carried out with an adult sample.
relations with outcome scales did not increase signifi- Third, the French-language version of the BFPTSQ was
cantly with the added items, this again suggests that the validated, so it would be important to replicate these
correlation increases are not simply a matter of increased results using the English- or other-language versions.
general variance. When the 95% confidence intervals Fourth, it would be interesting to evaluate whether the
are considered for comparison tests, all overlap some- factor structure and measurement invariance across gen-
what, which suggests that the correlation increase ders can be replicated with reports from informants, such
flagged as statistically significant as per the delta param- as parents and teachers. Fifth, there is a potential shared
eters tends to be small. method effect in the criterion validity tests because self-
report measures were used for the assessment of both
personality traits and outcomes. It would be important to
Impact of Added Conceptual Breadth replicate these results using different informants or meth-
This study showed that by adding only a few items tapping ods. Sixth, the evaluation of criterion validity was con-
important primary personality traits, factor validity is ade- ducted with a limited number of outcomes (i.e.,
quate, reliability is not affected, but convergent and crite- psychopathology and achievement). Clearly, personality
rion validities are somewhat improved. Even if the BFPTSQ traits have been shown to be associated with a wide range
is far from being optimal in terms of content coverage and of other positive and negative life outcomes, so addi-
psychometric properties, the results of this study underscore tional predictive studies using these new scales are
the significance of its added conceptual breadth. needed. Seventh, this study provided only a preliminary
Because of the limited time available in many assess- evaluation of incremental validity. Indeed, before mak-
ment situations, short personality measures are popular. ing any strong claims about the incremental validity of
In principle, to be valid, short measures should have the BFPTSQ, it would be important to demonstrate that
items tapping all of the most important primary traits its scales significantly add to the prediction of conse-
subsumed in a given Big Five trait (Haynes etal., 1995; quential outcomes beyond what can be predicted by
Smith etal., 2003). Therefore, the minimal number of scales from other known short Big Five measures.
items in a short scale should arguably be the number of Despite these limitations, the results of this study suggest
primary traits encompassed in a given Big Five trait. If that the BFPTSQ appears to be a potentially useful alter-
some items tapping fundamental primary traits do not native to existing Big Five short measures.

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
600 Assessment 21(5)

Appendix
Big Five Personality Trait Short Questionnaire (BFPTSQ) Items
I see myself as someone who . . .
Openness
1 Is original, often has new ideas.
6 Is curious about many different things.
11 Is ingenious, reflects a lot.
16 Has a lot of imagination.
21 Is inventive, creative.
26 Likes artistic or aesthetic experiences.
31 R Is not really interested in different cultures, their customs and values.
36 Likes to reflect, tries to understand complex things.
41 R Has few artistic interests.
46 Is sophisticated when it comes to art, music or literature.
Extraversion
2 Likes to talk, expresses his/her opinion.
7 R Is reserved or shy, has difficulty approaching others.
12 Is full of energy, likes to always be active.
17 Is a leader, capable of convincing others.
22 R Is rather quiet, does not talk a lot.
27 Shows self-confidence, is able to assert himself/herself.
32 R Is timid, shy.
37 Is extraverted, sociable.
42 Likes exciting activities, which provide thrills.
47 Has a tendency to laugh and have fun easily.
Agreeableness
3 R Has a tendency to criticize others.
8 Is helpful and generous with others.
13 R Provokes quarrels or arguments with others.
18 Is lenient, forgives easily.
23 Generally trusts others.
28 R Can be distant and cold towards others.
33 Is considerate and kind to almost everyone.
38 R Can sometimes be rude or mean towards others.
43 Likes to cooperate with others.
48 R Can deceive and manipulate people to get what he/she want.
Conscientiousness
4 Works conscientiously, does the things he/she has to do well.
9 R Can be a little careless and negligent.
14 Is a reliable student/worker, who can be counted on.
19 R Has a tendency to be disorganized, messy.
24 R Has a tendency to be lazy.
29 Perseveres until the task at hand is completed.
34 Does things efficiently, works well and quickly.
39 Plans things that need to be done and follows through the plans.
44 R Is easily distracted, has difficulty remaining attentive.
49 R Can do things impulsively without thinking about the consequences.
Emotional Stability
5 R Has a tendency to be easily depressed, sad.
10 Is generally relaxed, handles stress well.
15 R Can be tense, stressed out.
20 R Worries a lot about many things.
25 Is emotionally stable, not easily upset.
30 R Can be moody.
35 Stays calm in tense or stressful situations.
40 R Can easily become nervous.
45 R Has a tendency to feel inferior to others.
50 R Has a tendency to be easily irritated.

Note. R = reversed-score item. Boldface item numbers represent newly added items.

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 601

Acknowledgments provide some support for the criterion validity of the


BFPTSQ scales, a potential problem is content overlap
I want to thank all the school board members, school principals, as
between constructs. Indeed, there is a persistent concern
well as all the adolescents, parents, and teachers, who participated
among scholars that the relations between personality and
in the study. Thank you also to Alexandre Morin and Dave
psychopathology could be fallacious because some personal-
Miranda for their comments on an early draft of this article.
ity trait measures actually include items clearly tapping dis-
order-specific psychopathology (Lengua, West, & Sandler,
1998; Uliaszek etal., 2009). To ensure that the correlations
Declaration of Conflicting Interests observed in this study are not solely explained by overlap-
The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect ping items between constructs, trimmed personality scales
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. with potential overlapping items removed were computed
and correlations with some of the psychopathology scales
Funding were reestimated. For Agreeableness, deleting Items 13 and
48 from the scale provided a significantly lower correlation
The author disclosed receipt of the following financial support for with CD, but it remained significant and of similar magnitude
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This (r = .076, SE = .011, p < .001, 95% CI = .097, .055).
research was made possible by a grant from the Fonds qubcois For Conscientiousness, deleting Item 44 from the scale pro-
pour la recherche sur la socit et la culture (FQRSC). vided a significantly lower correlation with ADHD, but it
remained significant and of similar magnitude (r = .030,
Notes SE = .005, p < .001, 95% CI = .039, .020). For Emotional
1. From the perspective of the HEXACO model, the Stability, deleting Items 20 and 40 from the scale provided
Machiavellianism item would be associated with the sixth a significantly lower correlation with GAD, but it remained
factor, namely Honesty-Humility (Ashton, Lee, & Goldberg, significant and of similar magnitude (r = .028, SE = .006,
2004). p < .001, 95% CI = .040, .015). Finally, for Emotional
2. It should be noted that the Impulsiveness facet in the Stability, deleting Items 5 and 45 from the scale provided a
NEO-PI-3 is associated with Neuroticism, but given its con- significantly lower correlation with MDD, but it remained
tent this facet should arguably be called Compulsiveness. It significant and of similar magnitude (r = .071, SE = .007,
should also be noted that Impulsiveness can be considered p < .001, 95% CI = .086, .057). Overall, these results are
a multidimensional construct. In the model identified by consistent with Lengua etal.s (1998) results and suggest that
Whiteside and Lynam (2001), this added item would be con- there are robust and meaningful individual differences in per-
sidered part of the Lack of Premeditation subscale. sonality traits that are related to adolescent psychopathology.
3. Brent Donnellan, Lewis Goldberg, Dave Miranda, Julie
Pozzebon, Brent Roberts, and Antonio Terracciano rated the
References
relevance of the items. These researchers were considered
experts because they worked and published on topics related Allik, J., Laidra, K., Realo, A., & Pullmann, H. (2004). Personality
to personality trait structure and test construction and valida- development from 12 to 18 years of age: Changes in mean lev-
tion. Note that these researchers neither participated in nor els and structures of traits. European Journal of Personality,
endorsed this research and the resulting new measure. 18, 445-462.
4. A total of 21 a priori correlated uniquenesses (CUs) were Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., & Goldberg, L. R. (2004). A hierarchical
posited: for Openness, 11-36, 16-21, 26-41r, 26-46, 1-16; for analysis of 1,710 English personality-descriptive adjectives.
Extraversion, 7r-32r, 2-22r, 12-42, 2-27; for Agreeableness, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 707-721.
18-23, 8-33, 23-33, 23-43; for Conscientiousness, 29-39, 19r- Asparouhov, T., & Muthn, B. O. (2009). Exploratory structural
24r, 19r-39, 29-44r; and for Emotional Stability, 10-35, 10-15r, equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 16, 397-
5r-25, 5r-45r. Most of these CUs are from a same primary trait 438.
or subdomain and have similar content but in reversed scoring Ball, S. A. (2005). Personality traits, problems, and disorders:
or share the same word. Note that seven additional a priori Clinical applications to substance use disorders. Journal of
CUs were expected but were not statistically significant in the Research in Personality, 39, 84-102.
sample used in this study, so they were not retained in the Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., Rabasca, A., & Pastorelli, C.
models: for Openness, 1-21, 41r-46; for Extraversion, 17-27; (2003). A questionnaire for measuring the Big Five in late
for Agreeableness, 18-43; for Conscientiousness 9r-19r, 4-14; childhood. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 645-
and for Emotional Stability, 30r-50r. 664.
5. It should be noted that, compared with other rotation methods Barefoot, J. C., & Boyle, S. H. (2009). Hostility and proneness
that are fully exploratory, the Target Rotation can be thought to anger. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of
of as a more confirmatory method guided by theory where individual differences in social behavior (pp. 210-226). New
small cross-loadings are expected even if the main factor York, NY: Guilford.
structure can be defined a priori. Barry, C. T., Kerig, P. K., Stellwagen, K. K., & Barry, T.
6. Even though several moderate and significant correlations (Eds.). (2010). Narcissism and Machiavellianism in
between the Big Five traits and consequential outcomes youth: Implications for the development of adaptive

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
602 Assessment 21(5)

and maladaptive behavior. Washington, DC: American De Fruyt, F., Mervielde, I., Hoekstra, H. A., & Rolland, J.-P.
Psychological Association Press. (2000). Assessing adolescents personality with the NEO-
Beauducel, A., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2006). On the performance PI-R. Assessment, 7, 329-345.
of maximum likelihood versus means and variance adjusted De Pauw, S. S. W., & Mervielde, I. (2010). Temperament, person-
weighted least squares estimation in CFA. Structural Equation ality, and developmental psychopathology: A review based
Modeling, 13, 186-203. on the conceptual dimensions underlying childhood traits.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural mod- Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 41, 313-329.
els. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238-246. DeYoung, C. G., & Gray, J. R. (2009). Personality neuroscience:
Bosson, J. K., & Swann, W. B. (2009). Self-esteem. In M. R. Leary Explaining individual differences in affect, behavior, and
& R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in cognition. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), Cambridge
social behavior (pp. 527-546). New York, NY: Guilford. handbook of personality psychology (pp. 323-346). New
Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G., & Saklofske, D. (Eds.). (2008). York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Handbook of personality theory and testing, Volume 2: Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., & Lucas, R. E.
Personality measurement and assessment. London, UK: Sage. (2006). The Mini-IPIP Scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of
Byrne, B. M., Shavelson, R. J., & Muthn, B. (1989). Testing for the Big Five factors of personality. Psychological Assessment,
the equivalence of factor covariance and mean structures: 18, 192-203.
The issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychological Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., Robins, R. W., Moffitt, T.
Bulletin, 105, 456-466. E., & Caspi, A. (2005). Low self-esteem is related to aggres-
Caspi, A. (2000). The child is father of the man: Personality con- sion, antisocial behavior, and delinquency. Psychological
tinuities from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Personality Science, 16, 328-335.
and Social Psychology, 78, 158-172. Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle,
Caspi, A., & Shiner, R. L. (2006). Personality development. In W. C. A. (2006). Gender differences in temperament: A meta-
Damon & R. M. Lerner (Ser. Eds.), N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 33-72.
Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and Frick, P. J., Cornell, A., Bodin, D., Dane, H., Barry, C. T., &
personality development (6th ed., pp. 300-365). New York, Loney, B. R. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits and develop-
NY: John Wiley. mental pathways to severe conduct problems. Developmental
Cattell, R. B. (1966). Psychological theory and scientific method. Psychology, 39, 246-260.
In R. B. Cattell (Ed.), Handbook of multivariate experimental Furnham, A., Guenole, N., Levine, S. Z., & Chamorro-Premuzic,
psychology (pp. 1-18). Chicago, IL: Rand McNally. T. (2013). The NEO Personality InventoryRevised: Factor
Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of mea- structure and gender invariance from exploratory struc-
surement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 464-504. tural equation modeling analyses in a high-stakes setting.
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness- Assessment, 20, 14-23.
of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (1999). Youths Inventory 4 (YI-4):
Equation Modeling, 9, 233-255. Manual. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus.
Church, A. T., & Burke, P. J. (1994). Exploratory and confirmatory Garca-Coll, C. T., & Magnuson, K. (1997). The psychological
tests of the Big-Five and Tellegens three- and four- dimen- experience of immigration: A developmental perspective.
sional models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, In A. Booth (Ed.), Immigration and the family: Research
66, 93-114. and policy on US immigrants (pp. 91-131). Hillsdale, NJ:
Cizek, G. J., Rosenberg, S. L., & Koos, H. H. (2008). Sources Lawrence Erlbaum.
of validity evidence for educational and psychological tests. Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative description of personal-
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 68, 397-412. ity: The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (2008). Temperament: An organizing and Social Psychology, 59, 1216-1229.
paradigm for trait psychology. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality
& L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and traits. American Psychologist, 48, 26-34.
research (3rd ed., pp. 265-286). New York, NY: Guilford. Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public-domain,
Costa, P. T., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of
differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust several five-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F.
and surprising findings. Journal of Personality and Social De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology
Psychology, 81, 322-331. in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 7-28). Tilburg, Netherlands: Tilburg
Crews, F. T., & Boettiger, C. A. (2009). Impulsivity, frontal lobes University Press.
and risk for addiction. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Goldberg, L. R. (2001). Analyses of Digmans child-personality
Behavior, 93, 237-247. data: Derivation of Big-Five factor scores from each of six
Cronbach, L. J., & Gleser, G. C. (1965). Psychological tests and samples. Journal of Personality, 69, 710-743.
personnel decisions (2nd ed.). Urbana: University of Illinois Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. (2003). A very
Press. brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of
Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the new statistics: Effect Research in Personality, 37, 504-528.
sizes, confidence intervals, and meta-analysis. New York, Habermas, T., & de Silveira, C. (2008). The development of global
NY: Routledge. coherence in life narratives across adolescence: Temporal,

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 603

causal, and thematic aspects. Developmental Psychology, 44, Lengua, L. J., West, S. G., & Sandler, I. N. (1998). Temperament
707-721. as a predictor of symptomatology in children: Addressing the
Harter, S. (2006). The self. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Ser. contamination of measures. Child Development, 69, 164-181.
Eds.), N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychol- Loevinger, J. (1957). Objective tests as instruments of psychologi-
ogy, vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development cal theory. Psychological Reports, 3, 635-694.
(6th ed., pp. 505-570). New York, NY: John Wiley. MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996).
Haynes, S. N., Richards, D. C. S., & Kubany, E. S. (1995). Content Power analysis and determination of sample size for covari-
validity in psychological assessment: A functional approach ance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1, 130-149.
to concepts and methods. Psychological Assessment, 7, 238- Madden, G. J., & Bickel, W. K. (Eds.). (2010). Impulsivity:
247. The behavioral and neurological science of discounting.
Hogan, J., & Roberts, B. W. (1996). Issues and non-issues in Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
the fidelity/bandwidth tradeoff. Journal of Organizational Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., & Schutte, N. S. (2005). The
Behavior, 17, 627-637. relationship between the Five-Factor model of personality
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cut-off criteria for fit indexes and symptoms of clinical disorders: A meta-analysis. Journal
in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 27, 101-114.
new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55. Mann, M., Hosman, C. M. H., Schaalma, H. P., & de Vries, N. K.
John, O., Donahue, E., & Kentle, R. (1991). The Big Five (2004). Self-esteem in a broad-spectrum approach for mental
InventoryVersions 4a and 54 (Tech. Rep., Institute of health promotion. Health Education Research, 19, 357-372.
Personality Assessment and Research). Berkeley: University Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating
of California, Berkeley. the structure of normal and abnormal personality: An integra-
John, O. P., Neumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift tive hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social
to the integrative Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measure- Psychology, 88, 139-157.
ment, and conceptual issues. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, Marsh, H. W., Ellis, L., Parada, R., Richards, G. E., & Heubeck, B.
& L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and (2005). A short version of the Self-Description Questionnaire
research (3rd ed., pp. 114-158). New York, NY: Guilford. II: Operationalizing criteria for short-form evaluation
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2009). Machiavellianism. In M. with new applications of confirmatory factor analyses.
R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual dif- Psychological Assessment, 17, 81-102.
ferences in social behavior (pp. 93-108). New York, NY: Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K. T. (1996). Assessing goodness of fit:
Guilford. Is parsimony always desirable? Journal of Experimental
Jones, S. E., Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2011). Personality, Education, 64, 364-390.
antisocial behavior, and aggression: A meta-analytic review. Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., Balla, J. R., & Grayson, D. (1998).
Journal of Criminal Justice, 39, 329-337. Is more ever too much: The number of indictors per factor
Klein, D. N., Dyson, M. W., Kujawa, A. J., & Kotov, R. (2012). in confirmatory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral
Temperament and internalizing disorders. In M. Zentner & R. Research, 33, 181-220.
L. Shiner (Eds.), Handbook of temperament (pp. 541-561). Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden
New York, NY: Guilford. rules: comment on hypothesis testing approaches to setting
Klein, D. N., Kotov, R., & Bufferd, S. J. (2011). Personality and cutoff values for fit indices and dangers in overgeneral-
depression: Explanatory models and review of the evidence. izing Hu & Bentlers (1999) findings. Structural Equation
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 269-295. Modeling, 11, 320-341.
Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking Marsh, H. W., Ldtke, O., Muthn, B. O., Asparouhov, T., Morin,
big personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use A. J. S., Trautwein, U., & Nagengast, B. (2010). A new look
disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 768- at the Big-Five factor structure through exploratory structural
821. equation modeling. Psychological Assessment, 22, 471-491.
Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., & McGee, McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure
R. (1996). Personality traits are differentially linked to mental as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509-516.
disorders: A multitrait-multidiagnosis study of an adolescent McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2010). NEO Inventories for the
birth cohort. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 299-312. NEO Personality Inventory-3 (NEO-PI-3), NEO Five-Factor
Le Blanc, M. (1996). MASPAQ: Mesures de ladaptation per- Inventory-3 (NEO-FFI-3), NEO Personality Inventory-
sonnelle et sociale pour les adolescents qubcois: Manuel Revised (NEO-PI-R): Professional manual. Lutz, FL:
et guide dutilisation [Measures of Quebec Adolescents Psychological Assessment Resources.
Social and Personal Adjustment (3rd ed.)]. Montreal, McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., & Martin, T. A. (2005). The NEO-PI-3:
Canada: Research Group on Problem Adolescents, School of A more readable revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal
Psychoeducation, University of Montreal. of Personality Assessment, 84, 261-270.
Leibenluft, E., Cohen, P., Gorrindo, T., Brook, J. S., & Pine, McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Terracciano, A., Parker, W. D., Mills,
D. S. (2006). Chronic versus episodic irritability in youth: C. J., De Fruyt, F., & Mervielde, I. (2002). Personality trait
A community-based, longitudinal study of clinical and development from age 12 to age 18: Longitudinal, cross-sec-
diagnostic Associations. Journal of Child and Adolescent tional and cross-cultural analyses. Journal of Personality and
Psychopharmacology, 16, 456-466. Social Psychology, 83, 1456-1468.

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
604 Assessment 21(5)

McCrae, R. R., & Sutin, A. R. (2009). Openness to experience. In Parker, W. D., & Stumpf, H. (1998). A validation of the five-
M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual factor model of personality in academically talented youth
differences in social behavior (pp. 257-273). New York, NY: across observers and instruments. Personality and Individual
Guilford. Differences, 25, 1005-1025.
McCrae, R. R., Zonderman, A. B., Costa, P. T., Bond, M. H., & Polit, D. F., & Beck, C. T. (2006). The Content Validity Index:
Paunonen, S. (1996). Evaluating the replicability of factors in Are you sure you know whats being reported? Critique and
the revised NEO Personality Inventory: Confirmatory factor recommendations. Research in Nursing and Health, 29, 489-
analysis versus procrustes rotation. Journal of Personality & 497.
Social Psychology, 70, 552-566. Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model
Meredith, W. (1993). Measurement invariance, factor analysis and of personality and academic performance. Psychological
factorial invariance. Psychometrika, 58, 525-543. Bulletin, 135, 322-338.
Messick, S. (1995). Validity of psychological assessment: Preacher, K. J., & MacCallum, R. C. (2003). Repairing Tom
Validation of inferences from persons responses and perfor- Swifts electric factor analysis machine. Understanding
mances as scientific inquiry into score meaning. American Statistics, 2, 13-43.
Psychologist, 50, 741-749. Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one
Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2001). Structural models of personal- minute or less: A 10-item version of the Big Five Inventory
ity and their relation to antisocial behavior: A meta-analytic in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality,
review. Criminology, 39, 765-798. 41, 203-212.
Millsap, R. E., & Olivera-Aguilar, M. (2012). Investigating mea- Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of composite reliability for con-
surement invariance using confirmatory factor analysis. In R. generic measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21,
H. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of structural equation modeling 173-184.
(pp. 209-231). New York, NY: Guilford. Raykov, T. (1998). Coefficient alpha and composite reliability with
Morin, A. J. S., & Maano, C. (2011). Cross-validation of the short interrelated nonhomogeneous items. Applied Psychological
form of the Physical Self-Inventory (PSI-S) using exploratory Measurement, 22, 375-385.
structural equation modeling (ESEM). Psychology of Sport Raykov, T. (2001). Bias of coefficient alpha for fixed conge-
and Exercise, 12, 540-554. neric measures with correlated errors. Applied Psychological
Morin, A. J. S., Marsh, H. W., & Nagengast, B. (2013). Exploratory Measurement, 25, 69-76.
structural equation modeling. In G. R. Hancock & R. O. Raykov, T. (2007a). Evaluation of revision effect on criterion
Mueller (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second validity of multiple-component measuring instruments.
course (2nd ed., pp. 395-436). Charlotte, NC: Information Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 415-434.
Age. Raykov, T. (2007b). Reliability if deleted, not alpha if deleted:
Mullins-Sweatt, S. N., Jamerson, J. E., Samuel, D. B., Olson, D. Evaluation of scale reliability following component deletion.
R., & Widiger, T. A. (2006). Psychometric properties of an British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology,
abbreviated instrument of the five-factor model. Assessment, 60, 201-206.
13, 119-137. Raykov, T. (2012). Scale construction and development using
Muthn, L. K., & Muthn, B. O. (2010). Mplus users guide structural equation modeling. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook
[Version 6.12, computer software]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthn of structural equation modeling (pp. 472-492). New York,
& Muthn. NY: Guilford.
Newman, J. P., & Wallace, J. F. (1993). Diverse pathways to defi- Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Savalei, V. (2012). When
cient self-regulation: Implications for disinhibitory psychopa- can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A com-
thology in children. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 690-720. parison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estima-
Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and psychopathology. Journal of tion methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 395-422. Methods, 17, 354-373.
Noftle, E. E., & Robins, R. W. (2007). Personality predictors of Roberts, B. W., Jackson, J. J., Fayard, J. V., Edmonds, G., &
academic outcomes: Big Five correlates of GPA and SAT Meints, J. (2009). Conscientiousness. In M. R. Leary & R.
scores. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social
116-130. behavior (pp. 369-381). New York, NY: Guilford.
Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., Bisconti, T. L., & Wallace, K. A. Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N., Shiner, R. N., Caspi, A., & Goldberg,
(2006). Psychological resilience: Positive emotions, and suc- L. R. (2007). The power of personality: The comparative
cessful adaptation to stress in later life. Journal of Personality validity of personality traits, socio-economic status, and
and Social Psychology, 91, 730-749. cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes.
Ozer, D., & Benet-Martnez, V. (2006). Personality and the Perspectives in Psychological Science, 2, 313-345.
prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Rosellini, A. J., & Brown, T. A. (2011). The NEO Five-Factor
Psychology, 57, 401-421. Inventory: Latent structure and relationships with dimensions
Parker, J. D. A., Bagby, R. M., & Summerfeldt, L. J. of anxiety and depressive disorders in a large clinical sample.
(1993). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Revised Assessment, 18, 27-38.
Neo-Personality Inventory. Personality and Individual Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W.
Differences, 15, 463-466. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Ser. Eds.), N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.),

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
Morizot 605

Handbook of child psychology, vol. 3: Social, emotional, and Acquiescence, factor structure, coherence, and differentia-
personality development (6th ed., pp. 99-166). New York, tion from ages 10 to 20. Journal of Personality and Social
NY: John Wiley. Psychology, 94, 718-737.
Sargant, J. D., Tanski, S., Stoolmiller, M., & Hanewinkel, R. Spear, L. P. (2010). The behavioral neuroscience of adolescence.
(2010). Using sensation seeking to target adolescents for sub- New York, NY: Norton.
stance use interventions. Addiction, 105, 506-514. Stanton, J. M., Sinar, E. F., Balzer, W. K., & Smith, P. C. (2002).
Satorra, A. (2000). Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in multi- Issues and strategies for reducing the length of self-report
sample analysis of moment structures. In R. D. H. Heijmans, scales. Personnel Psychology, 55, 167-194.
D. S. G. Pollock, & A. Satorra (Eds.), Innovations in multi- Stringaris, A., Cohen, P., Pine, D. S., & Leibenluft, E. (2009).
variate statistical analysis: A Festschrift for Heinz Neudecker Adult outcomes of youth irritability: A 20-year prospective
(pp. 233-247). London, UK: Kluwer. community-based study. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Saucier, G. (1994). Mini-Markers: A brief version of Goldbergs 166, 1048-1054.
unipolar Big-Five markers. Journal of Personality Assessment, Tackett, J. L. (2006). Evaluating model of the personality-psycho-
63, 506-516. pathology relationship in children and adolescents. Clinical
Saucier, G. (2002). Orthogonal markers for orthogonal factors: Psychology Review, 26, 584-599.
The case of the Big Five. Journal of Personality Research, Tackett, J. L., Krueger, R. F., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2008).
36, 1-31. Personality in middle childhood: A hierarchical structure and
Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (2002). Assessing the Big Five: longitudinal connections with personality in late adolescence.
Applications of 10 psychometric criteria to the development Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1456-1462.
of marker scales. In B. De Raad & M. Perugini (Eds.), Big Tackett, J. L., Martel, M. M., & Kushner, S. C. (2012).
Five assessment (pp. 29-58). Gottingen, Germany: Hogrefe Temperament, externalizing disorders, and attention-deficit/
& Huber. hyperactivity disorder. In M. Zentner & R. L. Shiner (Eds.),
Scarpa, A., & Raine, A. (1997). Psychophysiology of anger and Handbook of temperament (pp. 562-580). New York, NY:
violent behavior. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 20, Guilford.
375-394. Tackett, J. L., Slobodskaya, H. R., Mar, R. A., Deal, J., Halverson,
Schmitt, P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why cant C. F., Baker, S. R., Pavlopoulos, V., & Besevegis, E. (2012).
a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five The hierarchical structure of childhood personality in five
personality traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality countries: Continuity from early childhood to early adoles-
and Social Psychology, 94, 168-182. cence. Journal of Personality, 80, 847-879.
Scholte, R. H. J., van Aken, M. A. G., & van Lieshout, C. F. M. Tarter, R. E., Blackson, T., Brigham, J., Moss, H., & Caprara,
(1997). Adolescent personality factors in self-ratings and peer G. V. (1995). The association between childhood irritability
nomination and their prediction of peer acceptance and peer and liability to substance use in early adolescence: A 2-year
rejection. Journal of Personality Assessment, 69, 534-554. follow-up study of boys at risk for substance use. Drug and
Shiner, R. L. (2010). Mapping the landscape of personal- Alcohol Dependence, 39, 253-261.
ity in childhood and adolescence. Social and Personality Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (2008). Exploring personality
Psychology Compass, 4, 1084-1097. through test construction: Development of the multidimen-
Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion sional personality questionnaire. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews,
dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personal- & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The Sage handbook of personality
ity and attachment style. Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, theory and assessment, vol. 2: Personality measurement and
61-71. testing (pp. 261-292). London, UK: Sage.
Silverstein, A. B. (1990). Short forms of individual intelligence Terracciano, A., Lockenhoff, C. E., Crum, R. M., Bienvenu, O. J.,
tests. Psychological Assessment, 2, 3-11. & Costa, P. T. (2008). Five-Factor Model personality profiles
Simms, L. J., & Watson, D. (2007). The construct validation of drug users. BMC Psychiatry, 8, 22.
approach to personality scale construction. In R. W. Robins, Uliaszek, A. A., Hauner, K. K. Y., Ziberg, R. E., Craske, M. G.,
R. C. Fraley, & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research Mineka, S., Griffith, J. W., & Rose, R. D. (2009). An exami-
methods in personality psychology (pp. 240-258). New York, nation of content overlap and disorder-specific predictions in
NY: Guilford. the associations of neuroticism with anxiety and depression.
Smith, G. T., Fischer, S., & Fister, S. M. (2003). Incremental valid- Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 785-794.
ity principles in test construction. Psychological Assessment, Vandenberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthe-
15, 467-477. sis of the measurement invariance literature: suggestions,
Smith, G. T., McCarthy, D. M., & Anderson, K. G. (2000). On the practices, and recommendations for organizational research.
sins of short-form development. Psychological Assessment, Organizational Research Methods, 3, 4-70.
12, 102-111. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Extraversion and its positive
Snaith, R. P., & Taylor, C. M. (1985). Irritability: Definition, emotional core. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.),
assessment, and associated factors. British Journal of Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 767-793). San
Psychiatry, 147, 127-136. Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Soto, C. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The West, S. G., Taylor, A. B., & Wu, W. (2012). Model fit and model
developmental psychometrics of Big Five self-reports: selection in structural equation modeling. In R. H. Hoyle

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015
606 Assessment 21(5)

(Ed.), Handbook of structural equation modeling (pp. 209- Wilson, D. S., Near, D., & Miller, R. R. (1996). Machiavellianism:
231). New York, NY: Guilford. A synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures.
Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and Psychological Bulletin, 119, 285-299.
impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand Wilt, J., & Revelle, W. (2009). Extraversion. In M. Leary & R.
impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669-689. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social
Widiger, T. A., & Smith, G. T. (2008). Personality and psycho- behavior (pp. 27-45). New York, NY: Guilford.
pathology. In O. P. John, R. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Zuckerman, M. (2009). Sensation seeking. In M. R. Leary & R.
Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social
pp. 743-769). New York, NY: Guilford. behavior (pp. 455-465). New York, NY: Guilford.

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on November 28, 2015

Anda mungkin juga menyukai