Anda di halaman 1dari 16

Schema Congruity as a Basis for

Product Evaluation
JOAN MEYERS-LEVY
ALICE M. TYBOUT*

Mandler theorized that the level of congruity between a product and a more general
product category schema may influence the nature of information processing and
thus product evaluations. Products that are moderately incongruent with their as-
sociated category schemas are expected to stimulate processing that leads to a
more favorable evaluation relative to products that are either congruent or ex-
tremely incongruent. Data from three experiments conducted in new product con-
texts are consistent with Mandler's hypothesis and serve as a basis for theorizing
about the process.

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


R ecent research in psychology and in consumer
behavior provides some insight regarding how
In practice, however, the (in)congruity between a
product and its associated product category schema
the level of congruity between products and their as- may lie between the extremes of a perfect match or
sociated product category schemas affects processing mismatch. New products often claim both attributes
and evaluative judgments (e.g., Cohen and Basu congruent and attributes incongruent with a more
1987; Fiske 1982; Fiske and Pavelchak 1986; Srull general product category schema. A recent example
1981; Sujan 1985). Two extreme cases have been con- is the beverage Slice, which is positioned as a soft
trasted-either a complete match between a product drink and has attributes associated with the soft drink
and an activated product category schema (i.e., the schema-such as carbonation, slightly sweet taste,
sort of congruity represented by a "me-too" product) and packaging in cans and plastic bottles. But Slice
or a complete mismatch between multiple features of also has an attribute not included in the soft drink
a product and an activated category schema (i.e., the schema; it contains real fruit juice. The more moder-
level of incongruity that occurs when all of a prod- ate incongruity created by a partial match between a
uct's attributes contradict its claimed category mem- product and a category schema may lead to a different
bership). Typically, the category schemas examined evaluation process than will either a pure match or
in such research have been ones that carry strong mismatch. Further, the cognitive activity stimulated
affect so that the transfer of schema affect can be by various levels of congruity may have effects be-
traced. Findings suggest that evaluations for matches yond those demonstrated by Fiske (1982) and Sujan
are based on the product category schema affect, (1985). Mandler (1982) suggests that the very process
whereas for mismatches there is more elaborate pro- of responding to (in)congruity may itself produce
cessing whereby affect is derived piecemeal by aggre- affect that contributes to product evaluations.
gating the affect associated with the product's specific More specifically, Mandler (1982) argues that the
attributes (e.g., Fiske and Pavelchak 1986; Sujan affect generated by responding to moderate incongru-
1985). ity will be more favorable than that typically gener-
ated by responding to either congruity or extreme in-
congruity. This outcome is most likely to be mani-
* Joan Meyers-Levy is Visiting Assistant Professor and Alice M. fested when the affect associated with an activated
Tybout is the Harold T. Martin Professor of Marketing, both at schema or category label and/or the attributes associ-
the J.L. Kellogg Graduate School of Management, Northwestern ated with a product are not so extreme or strongly
University, Evanston, IL 60208. This research was supported by
funds from the Buchanan Research Chair and the General Foods
held as to overwhelm the more subtle contribution of
Research Chair, which were awarded to the second author. The au- the process that occurs when responding to schema
thors thank Valerie Brock for her assistance with data analysis and incongruity. Thus, it is not surprising that process
Jim Bettman, Jennifer Crocker, Reid Hastie, Laura Peracchio, effects have gone undetected in research using the
Rick Staelin, Brian Stern thai, and Mita Sujan for their comments procedures developed by Fiske.
on earlier drafts of this article. Inquiries should be directed to the
second author.
The current research explores the notion that
schema (in)congruity can affect evaluations in the
39
JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCHeVol. 16eJune 1989
40 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

more subtle manner proposed by Mandler. Because "It's a soft drink, but one that doesn't have the usual
Mandler's theorizing has received only limited atten- preservatives."), or activation of an alternative
tion in the consumer research literature (Cohen and schema (e.g., "It's not really a soft drink; it's more of
Basu 1987), we begin by elaborating his view and then a fruit juice. "). Moderate incongruities are regarded
developing a framework in which different levels of as "interesting and positively valued" (Mandler
incongruity can be operationalized. We then report 1982, p. 22), thereby leading to more positive re-
three studies that use this framework to test Mand- sponses than ones elicited by schema congruity. In-
deed, the very process of resolving the incongruity is
ler's schema (in)congruity hypothesis and to explore thought to be rewarding and thus may contribute to
the processes thought to mediate the evaluation the resulting positive affect.
effects observed. By contrast, extreme incongruity is defined as in-
congruity that cannot be resolved or can be resolved
MANDLER'S HYPOTHESIS only if fundamental changes are made in the existing
cognitive structure (e.g., redefining the basic soft
Mandler (1982) theorized that the process of re- drink schema). Such incongruities may generate cog-
sponding to different levels of schema congruity can nitive elaboration, but this elaboration may lead
itself influence the valence and extremity of affective more to frustration than resolution. Thus, extreme
responses. Incongruity, as the term is used here, refers incongruities typically elicit more negative evalua-
to the extent that structural correspondence is tions than do moderate incongruities.

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


achieved between the entire configuration of attribute In summary, Mandler predicts a nonmonotonic re-
relations associated with an object, such as a product, lationship between schema (in)congruity and evalua-
and the configuration specified by the schema. Thus, tion, such that the process of responding to moderate
"it is the structure of the object and not the presence incongruity leads to more favorable evaluation than
or absence of certain (congruent/incongruent) fea- does the process of responding to either congruity or
tures that [establishes (in)congruity and] seems to de- extreme incongruity. This notion that some moderate
termine . . . value" (Mandler 1982, p. 10, bracket level of unexpectedness or distinctiveness is valued
data added). Mandler reasons that the heightened and perhaps even sought has precedence in the psy-
arousal and cognitive effort that accompanies in- chology literature (e.g., Beriyne 1963; Fiske and
creases in incongruity between a schema and an ob- Maddi 1961; Snyder and Fromkin 1980). Moreover,
ject increases the extremity of evaluation. Whether an in reviewing this theorizing and relating it to models
evaluation is relatively more favorable or more unfa- of consumer behavior, Venkatesan (1973, p. 376)
vorable is a function of how readily the processor can concludes that "to maintain the optimal level moder-
satisfactorily resolve the incongruity. ate amounts of novelty will be sought, or even pre-
Mandler begins with the proposition that schema ferred, over nonnovel or extremely novel items."
congruity leads to a favorable response because, other Although some evidence exists for the belief that
things being equal, people like objects that conform moderate incongruity is preferred, Mandler makes it
clear that there may be limits to when support for his
to their expectations and allow predictability. How- hypothesis will be evident. When extreme affect is as-
ever, schema congruent objects are not very note- sociated with the relevant schema or the incongruity,
worthy and therefore are unlikely to prompt exten- this affect likely will dominate that which may be ob-
sive cognitive elaboration. Hence, the positive re- tained as a consequence of the processing and elabo-
sponse that they generate typically is mild rather than ration prompted by different levels of schema congru-
extreme. ity (cf. Fiske 1982). Therefore, we investigate Mand-
A different scenario occurs when schema incongru- ler's hypothesis in the context of new product
ity is encountered. The novelty of the object increases introductions where the relevant schema affect is not
arousal, and greater cognitive elaboration may occur extreme. Specifically, we examine introductions of a
in an effort to resolve the incongruity. Mandler sug- new soft drink and a new fruit juice. The soft drink
gests that moderate incongruities are those that can and fru~t juice categories are well known and yet, as
be successfully resolved. For example, suppose a con- categones, they do not generate extreme affect (al-
sumer learns of a new product that is labeled as and though brands within the category may be loved or
possesses the general characteristics of a soft drink hated). We suspect that many categories offrequently
but also is described as being "all natural," a feature purchased package goods (e.g., cereal, paper towels,
that is at odds with most preservative-laden soft laundry detergents) evoke similar consumer re-
drinks. This incongruity with the soft drink schema sponses.
would be considered moderate if it could be resolved
without prompting a fundamental change in the con- OPERATIONALIZING SCHEMA
sumer's existing cognitive structure. Such resolution CONGRUITY
might be accomplished via assimilation (e.g., "Oh, Testing Mandler's hypothesis necessitates develop-
it's really just another soft drink."), subtyping (e.g., ing an a priori means of operationalizing congruity
SCHEMA CONGRUITY 41

levels. Mandler, like Fiske, suggests that congruity is uct hierarchies established in Sujan's work with the
represented by a match between the attributes of an exception that at the subordinate level, attributes
object/product and a relevant schema, whereas in- coupled with basic level category labels are employed
congruity involves some form of mismatch. What is instead of brand names. This conceptualization of
less apparent is how the distinction between moder- subordinate level categories is consistent with that of
ate and extreme incongruity should be operational- Rosch et al. (1976) who, for example, identified green
ized. Mandler argues that the level of incongruity is seedless grapes as subordinate to the basic category
determined by the ease with which anomalies can be grapes.
resolved within the existing cognitive structure. Thus, The hierarchy of beverage categories depicted in
we turn to Rosch's work (Rosch 1978; Rosch et al. Figure 1 suggests that schema congruity occurs when
1976) for some insight regarding cognitive structure all the attributes of a product are represented within
in semantic memory. Though Rosch exemplifies such the activated schema. This situation is expected to re-
structures using natural objects, as will be discussed sult in a mild positive evaluation of the product.
later, products and their categories appear to be orga- Schema incongruity occurs when the total config-
nized in a similar manner. uration of a product's attributes is not represented in
According to Rosch, natural object categories are the activated schema. Individuals presented with
organized in a hierarchical fashion. Superordinate such mismatches or incongruity are thought to en-
categories are at the highest level. Members of super- gage in cognitive elaboration directed at resolving the

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


ordinate categories are distinguished from each other incongruity. We propose that moderate and extreme
on key attributes, but they share few features. To incongruities are distinguished by the ease with which
move from a superordinate category to the next lower an incongruity can be addressed within the activated
level category, the so-called basic level, groups having hierarchical structure. And, as suggested by the work
a greater portion of shared-within to shared-between of Collins and Quillian (1969), we assume that indi-
category attributes are sought. These categories are viduals sequentially access levels in a hierarchy.
called basic because their attributes are thought to We posit that moderate schema incongruities can
provide the greatest discrimination between catego- be resolved by moving to the next lower level in the
ries and because they tend to be most frequently used hierarchical structure, because this level should be
to categorize both natural and social objects (Lingle, linked directly to the activated schema in the associa-
Altom, and Medin 1984V Moving from the basic tive network and accessible with only moderate effort.
level to the next lower or subordinate level in a hierar- Thus, in Figure 1, a structural incongruity with the
chy requires identifying a single or small number of beverage schema (e.g., a beverage description includ-
attributes that discriminate objects sharing a large ing attributes not represented in the beverage schema,
number of other features. such as preservatives) that could be resolved by acti-
Research by Sujan and Dekleva (1987) substanti- vating an alternative schema at the next lower basic
ates that product classes, product types, and brands level (e.g., soft drink) would be a moderate incongru-
can be equated with Rosch's superordinate, basic, ity. Similarly, a structural incongruity with the soft
and subordinate categories, respectively. In a similar drink schema (e.g., a soft drink description that in-
fashion, pilot studies conducted to develop stimulus cludes an attribute not associated with that schema,
materials for the present research provided evidence such as all natural) that could be resolved by activat-
ofa Rosch-type hierarchy of beverage categories (see ing an alternative schema at the next lower subordi-
Figure 1). 2 This beverage hierarchy parallels the prod-
ment 3 and the study checking the operationalizations.) It was
See Medin (1982) for a discussion of whether basic categories found that a limited number of attributes were associated with bev-
indeed provide the greatest discrimination between alternatives in erages, and these attributes were largely functional and general in
a particular level in an object hierarchy. nature (e.g., thirst quenching, refreshing, liquid). In comparison, a
2Rosch et al. (1976) distinguished hierarchical levels from one greater number of more specific attributes were listed for soft drinks
another on the basis of the number and types of attributes associ- and fruit juices (e.g., carbonated and bubbly for soft drinks, nutri-
ated with them. Specifically, more attributes are associated with tious and healthy for fruit juices). Finally, the attributes associated
basic than with superordinate level categories; moving down the with all natural soft drinks and high preservative fruit juices tended
hierarchy, a larger number of attributes are added in the shift from to be the same ones associated with soft drinks and fruit juices,
the superordinate to the basic level than in the shift from the basic respectively, with the exception that associations implying the spe-
to the subordinate level; whereas attributes that are associated with cific attributes all natural and high preservative also were men-
superordinate level categories are largely general and functional in tioned. This pattern of data is consistent with the view that bever-
nature, those added to categories as one moves from the basic to ages are the superordinate level, soft drinks and fruit juices are basic
subordinate levels are almost exclusively adjectives. level categories, and all natural soft drinks and high preservative
Using these criteria and the procedures similar to Rosch et aI., fruit juices are subordinate level categories. Moreover, an alterna-
we conducted pilot studies to identify the hierarchical order of bev- tive hierarchy in which all natural beverages and high preservative
erages. (This and most other research reported herein was con- beverages served as basic level categories and soft drinks and fruit
ducted before the national introduction of soft drinks that contain juices served as subordinate level categories was examined and
natural fruit juices, such as Slice. The only exceptions were Experi- failed to meet Rosch's criteria for these different levels.
42 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

FIGURE 1
A HYPOTHETICAL BEVERAGE HIERARCHY

Superordinate
Level I
I
I
I
I
I
I

--------------- ----------------------
I
--~-----------------------

Basic
Level

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


-----t"----
I
I

,
I
I
I
I

Subordinate
Level

- - - moderate schema incongruity


- - - - -, extreme schema incongruity

NOTE: Moderate schema incongruity category resolution can be achieved by accessing the next lower level in the hierarchy. Extreme schema incongruity category
resolution is unlikely to be achieved because resolution requires ultimately accessing a level in the hierarchy that is nonsequential, and this is impeded by the
absence of a match at an intervening level in the hierarchy.

nate level (e.g., all natural soft drink) also would be a should produce evaluations that are unfavorable rela-
moderate incongruity. Such moderate incongruities tive to moderate incongruity.
are expected to result in more favorable evaluations The foregoing proposal rests on the assumption
than are schema congruities. that the degree of an incongruity can be determined
Finally, we propose that extreme schema incongru- by the ease with which it can be resolved in the exist-
ities are ones that cannot be resolved by invoking an ing cognitive structure. This implies that our pro-
alternative schema at the next lower level in the hier- posed operationalizations of moderate and extreme
archy. Instead, they require a more complex strategy. incongruity will produce differences on indicators of
For example, an extreme incongruity might be re- the ease of incongruity resolution. Specifically, mod-
solved by moving down multiple levels, but such tra- erate incongruities should take less time to process
versal is made effortful by the absence of a match at and categorize, induce less focus on the incongruity,
the level immediately below the activated schema and result in more specific categorizations than
(i.e., there is no ready associative pathway to traverse; should extreme incongruities. Support for these pre-
Anderson and Bower 1980). Accordingly, an incon- dictions was obtained in a small experiment con-
gruity with the beverage schema that could not be re- ducted to examine the moderate and extreme incon-
solved at a next lower basic level would represent ex- gruity manipulations involving the beverage schema
treme incongruity (e.g., a beverage description that detailed in Experiment 1.
included carbonated, slightly sweet, and all natural Thus, it appears that incongruity levels can be var-
would fit neither the soft drink nor the fruit juice basic ied within the product hierarchy as outlined. Further,
level category schema). Such an extreme incongruity these operationalizations have the advantage of using
SCHEMA CONGRUITY 43

a single attribute (in combination with a category la- H3: The nonmonotonic relationship between
bel) to create the different levels. Such a strategy has schema (in)congruity and evaluation de-
ecological validity in that manufacturers, particularly fined by Hypothesis 1 and Hypothesis 2 will
in package goo"ds categories, often introduce new be more evident for individuals scoring low
products that differ from existing alternatives only on rather than high on a scale measuring dog-
one attribute. Of course, this is not intended to imply matism.
that ours is the only or the best means of varying in-
congruity. Other procedures are possible and may be H4: The nonmonotonic relationship between
equally effective (e.g., Sujan and Tybout 1988). schema (in)congruity and evaluation de-
fined by Hypothesis 1 and Hypothesis 2 will
be more evident for individuals scoring high
HYPOTHESES rather than low on a scale measuring need
for cognition.
U sing the operationalizations of the three levels of
schema congruity outlined in the preceding section, Our hypotheses are examined in three experiments.
we sought to test Mandler's prediction of a nonmono- In Experiment 1, the predicted nonmonotonic rela-
tonic relationship between schema congruity and tionship between schema congruity and evaluation
evaluation. First, we examined the following two for a new soft drink (Hypotheses 1 and 2) is tested and
hypotheses. the moderating impact of individual differences in

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


dogmatism (Hypothesis 3) is explored. In Experiment
HI: Moderate schema incongruity will lead to 2, Experiment 1 is replicated and extended by using
more favorable product evaluations than measures of recall and thoughts to explore additional
will complete congruity between the de- hypotheses related to the process underlying the eval-
scription and the activated schema. uation effects. In Experiment 3, another related prod-
H2: Moderate schema incongruity will lead to uct category, fruit juice, is examined, and further in-
more favorable product evaluations than sight into the process underlying schema congruity
will extreme schema incongruity. effects is provided by demonstrating that the effects
can occur irrespective of the baseline favorableness of
We also consider several individual difference fac- the schema ultimately activated to resolve incongru-
tors that may qualify these hypotheses. The preceding ity. Also in Experiment 3 are (1) tests of additional
analysis assumes that people will find incongruity in- hypotheses regarding the effect of schema (in)congru-
teresting and will be motivated to undertake some ity on individuals' certainty and response latencies in
cognitive elaboration to resolve it. Such an orienta- making evaluative judgments and (2) an investigation
tion toward incongruity seems most likely among of the moderating effect of individuals' need for cog-
people who are nondogmatic (Rokeach 1960) and nition on the evaluation process (Hypothesis 4).
who have a high need for cognition (Cacioppo and
Petty 1982). Consistent with this view concerning
dogmatism, Shaffer and Hendrick (1974, p. -602) ar- EXPERIMENT 1
gue that, "The open mind is regulated by a need to Method
know and understand, whereas the closed mind is ori-
ented to defend against anxiety and threat." Like- One hundred and two men and women from an up-
wise, Jacoby (1971) reports that across a variety of per middle-class suburb of Chicago participated in re-
product categories, nondogmatics made more inno- search involving tasting and evaluating a new bever-
vative selections than did dogmatics. Thus, we specu- age. They received$5 each and participated in small
late that dogmatics simply may ignore or discount in- groups of five to 10 people.
congruent information, whereas nondogmatics will Premixed research booklets were distributed in
find moderate incongruity interesting and will engage such a way that subjects were assigned randomly to
in the cognitive work necessary to take it into ac- one of four product descriptions. Subjects began by
count. reading a beverage description that manipulated two
Need for cognition has been found to be inversely independent variables using procedures adapted
related to dogmatism (Cacioppo and Petty 1982), and from person impression tasks (e.g., Srull et al. 1985).
it has been shown to be positively related to recall of The first sentence in the description activated an ini-
incongruent information (Srull, Lichtenstein, and tial schema by informing subjects that the product
Rothbart 1985). Thus, we also anticipate that in con- was either a new beverage (superordinate category) or
trast to people low in need for cognition, those high a soft drink (basic category). To control for the total
in need for cognition will be more likely to notice and category information provided, the last sentence of
to spontaneously undertake the cognitive work neces- the description was counterbalanced with the first.
sary to account for incongruent information. Thus, subjects initially informed that the product was
44 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 1
CELL CENTROIDS AND MEANS FOR EXPERIMENT 1 AND 2

Beverage schema Soft drink schema

All natural High preservative All natural High preservative

Extreme Moderate Moderate


Dependent measures incongruity incongruity incongruity Congruity

Experiment 1:
Pretaste Evaluation"
Nondogmatics -.22 .59 .73 -.19
Dogmatics .61 -.43 -.03 .51
Posttaste Evaluation"
Nondogmatics 2.48 3.32 3.63 2.52
Dogmatics 3.15 2.61 2.47 3.28
Experiment 2:

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


Pretaste evaluation" -5.23 -4.52 -4.39 -4.26
Posttaste evaluation" -.53 .98 .39 -.49
Number of statements recalled 4.85 4.91 5.50 3.55
Mention of target attribute in recall b .31 .27 .58 .00
Mention of soft drink in recallb .15 .45 .25 .37
Number of thoughts 5.14 4.50 5.38 3.86
Number of positive thoughts 1.50 1.10 2.00 .93
Number of negative thoughts 1.29 1.60 1.15 .43
Mention of target attribute in thoughtsb .36 .40 .38 .00
Mention of soft drink in thoughtsb .30 .67 .50 .27

These numbers are the centroids from the MANOVAs performed on the evaluation scales. Higher numbers (or less negative scores) imply more favorable
evaluation.
b 0 = no mention, 1 = mention.

a beverage were apprised that it was a soft drink, and the other condition the target attribute was "all natu-
subjects initially told that the product was a new soft ral." Twenty pretest subjects, who rated these attri-
drink were informed it was a beverage. However, the butes on 1O-point scales, indicated that "high preser-
category label specified in the first sentence was antic- vative" is more descriptive of soft drinkS than is "all
ipated to guide subjects' intial processing. Evidence natural" (.Ks = 8.50 versus 2.70; t(1,19) = 8.85, p
that initial information guides processing has been re- < 0.01). But these two attributes do not differ signifi-
ported elsewhere in the literature (nontransitivity; cantly in their favorableness for soft drinks (Ks = 4.45
Nisbett and Ross 1980). versus 5.45; t(I,19) = 1.29, p > 0.20).4 Following is a
The remainder of the description contained infor- sample product description.
mation about six beverage attributes, five of which
were common to all descriptions. Common attributes As part of a study being conducted by a major food
products company, you will be asked to read a descrip-
were ones that 15 pretest subjects indicated were not tion and evaluate a new beverage (soft drink). This bev-
spontaneously associated with all beverages but were erage will soon be introduced in the marketplace. The
readily linked with the basic level category for soft manufacturer, which markets a diverse line of con-
drinks (nonalcoholic, slightly sweet, best served cold, sumer products enjoying widespread household recog-
available at food stores, and carbonated). Further, in nition, has devoted considerable financial resources to
a separate pretest, 15 out of 16 subjects labeled a
drink that had these attributes as a soft drink. The
sixth attribute was varied to alter the structural and 4Questions were worded in the following manner: "How likely is
it that a product described as being high in preservatives/all natural
descriptive congruence of the product description is a soft drink?" and "How favorable is a soft drink that is described
with the soft drink schema. 3 In one condition, this as being high in preservatives/all natural?" Responses were ob-
target attribute was "high preservative," whereas in tained on 1O-point scales anchored as 1 = extremely unlikely /unfa-
vorable and 10 = extremely likely/favorable. In addition, subjects
answered these same questions as they pertained to fruit juices.
3See Wyer and Gordon 1982 for a discussion of how descriptive These latter ratings are reported in the method section of Experi-
and evaluative congruence differ. ment 3.
SCHEMA CONGRUITY 45

the research and development effort aimed at develop- Results and Discussion
ing a competitive entry within this product category.
This new drink, produced by a company that is active
in both the national and international food products A similar pattern of effects was observed for pre-
market, is specially formulated for those people with a taste and posttaste evaluations in this experiment and
zest for life. The nonalcoholic drink is slightly sweet to in the two that follow. However, the significance of
the taste and tastes best when served at a cold tempera- the effects typically was greater following the taste ex-
ture. It will be available in a variety of convenient food
stores. The carbonation and tingle of the drink will perience despite the beverage's taste being held con-
perk up anyone's taste buds. Priced at about $.47 per stant across treatments. In view of the similar pattern
12-ounce serving, this high preservative (all natural in- for pretaste and posttaste and the need to simplify
gredients) beverage will stay fresh in your refrigerator. data presentation across the three experiments, we fo-
This new soft drink (beverage) is expected to be avail- cus on the posttaste effects, which are statistically
able in cans and bottles in the fall of 1985. stronger. However, the significance level of the pre-
After reading the product description, subjects taste effects is noted, and pretaste data appear in the
completed a series of "Global Outlook Questions" tables. Because some readers may consider pretaste
that, in actuality, were Troldahl and Powell's (1965) evaluations a more appropriate test of our hypothe-
20-item version of Rokeach's (1960) dogmatism ses, we address the relationship between pretaste and
scale. Next, subjects were administered a set of pre- posttaste effects in detail in our general discussion.

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


taste measures, then drank a small sample of the bev- Separate MANOVAs were performed on the seven
erage (a cola soft drink), and performed posttaste evaluation scales measured at pretaste and posttaste,
evaluations. The same set of seven-point semantic and the data are reported in Table 1. Analyses re-
differential scales was completed at pretaste and post- vealed three-way interactions on both pretaste
taste and assessed dimensions of appeal, taste, satisfy- (F(7,88) = 2.03, p = 0.06, n~ult = 0.02) and posttaste
ing, desirability, refreshing, quality, and interest in evaluations (F(7,88) = 2.30, p < 0.05, n~ult = 0.02).5
trial. Thus, the general two-way interaction of schema acti-
Thus, the study employed a 2 (schema activated: vated and target attribute was not significant, but
beverage, soft drink) X 2 (target attribute: high preser- rather was qualified by dogmatism (see Hypothesis 3).
vative, all natural) X 2 (dogmatism: dogmatics, non- However, a more detailed analysis of posttaste data
dogmatics) factorial between-subjects design. The showed that support for the schema activated by tar-
first two variables were experimenter controlled and get attribute interaction outlined in Hypotheses 1 and
the third was determined by a mean split on subjects' 2 was obtained for nondogmatics (F(7,88) = 10.99, p
scores on the dogmatism scale. < 0.01; see Figure 2). When the beverage schema was
In this design, the soft drink/high preservative con- activated, nondogmatics were more favorable when
dition represented schema congruity because all the the target attribute was high preservative (moderate
attributes included in the description in this condi- incongruity) than when it was all natural (extreme in-
tion were characteristic of the activated schema. congruity; F(I,86) = 4.03, p < 0.05). When the soft
Moderate incongruity was operationalized by two drink schema was activated, they were more favor-
conditions, beverage/high preservative and soft able when the target attribute was all natural (moder-
drink/all natural. In these conditions, the product de- ate incongruity) than when it was high preservative
scription was incongruous with the schema but it (congruity; F(1,86) = 6.90,p < 0.05). The schema ac-
could be reconciled by accessing a schema directly be- tivated by target attribute interaction also was sig-
low the activated one (e.g., a high preservative bever- nificant for dogmatics (F(7,88) = 4.80, p < 0.05). Al-
age could be readily classified at the basic level as a though the pattern for this subgroup was opposite
soft drink, and an all natural soft drink could be that observed for nondbgmatics, none of the contrasts
viewed as a subordinate category of the basic level soft reached conventional levels of significance (ps
drink; see Figure 1). Finally, extreme schema incon- > 0.10).
gruity was represented by the beverage/all natural In summary, the results of Experiment 1 show sup-
condition because the incongruity between the de- port for our hypotheses but only for nondogmatic in-
scription and the activated schema could not be re-
solved by evoking a single alternative schema at the 5No a priori weighting and aggregation of the scale items was nec-
next lower, basic level, making it difficult to access the essary because MANOYA performs this function as part of the
subordinate category of all natural soft drink. (The analysis. However, it was necessary to calculate subjects' dogma-
earlier mentioned experiment assessing the opera- tism scores by summing responses to these items. Dogmatism
tionalizations revealed that, in the extreme incongru- scores ranged from 28 to 75. A mean split at 51 was used to classify
individuals as either dogmatics or nondogmatics. The ~~lUlt statistic
ity condition, subjects considered the possibility that is a multivariate statistic reflecting the variance explained by the
the product was a soft drink and the possibility that significant interaction. It is similar conceptually to w 2 for univari-
the product was a fruit juice equally viable.) ate effects.
46 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

FIGURE 2
PLOTS OF MAN OVA CENTROIDS DEPICTING THE SCHEMA ACTIVATED BY TARGET ATTRIBUTE
BY DOGMATISM INTERACTION OF EXPERIMENT 1 POSTTASTE EVALUATION

Nondogmatics Dogmatics
Favorableness Favorableness
of evaluation of evaluation
4.0 4.0
Moderate Congruity
incongruity
Extreme
3.5 3.5 incongruity

3.0
"""
3.0
////

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


2.5 /'/ 2.5
Moderate
"""""
// Congruity incongruity
Moderate
Extreme
2.0 incongruity 2.0 incongruity

Beverage Soft drink Beverage Soft drink


Schema activated Schema activated
- - - high preservative
- - -, all natural

dividuals. Stronger evidence would entail obtaining Hypotheses 1 and 2 might hold for all subjects in Ex-
these effects more generally (i.e., for all individuals) periment 2. Further, if the effect of schema (in)con-
and providing further data for the assumptions that gruity on evaluations is mediated by the extent of
incongruity induces cognitive elaboration and that elaboration and the success of that elaboration in re-
moderate but not extreme incongruity can be re- solving any incongruity, then the cognitive response
solved in subjects' existing cognitive structure. To ad- and recall measures should offer evidence of such ac-
dress these issues, subjects' thoughts and recall con- tivity.
cerning the product description were collected in Ex- Specifically, we propose the following hypotheses
periment 2. regarding the underlying process.
It was anticipated that the schema activated by tar-
get attribute interaction observed for nondogmatics H5: If schema incongruity leads to increased
in Experiment 1 would be obtained on evaluations for cognitive elaboration relative to schema
all subjects. This prediction was based on the assump- congruity, then the three conditions in
tion that the inclusion of cognitive response and re- which some level of incongruity exists
call measures would alter processing and evaluation should exhibit enhanced cognitive perform-
effects. Specifically, listing cognitive responses and at- ance relative to the schema congruity condi-
tempting to recall the stimulus material seemed likely tion in terms of total number of thoughts,
to motivate subjects to engage in the cognitive elabo- overall recall of the product description,
ration regarding the task at hand irrespective of their and mention of the target attribute in recall
dogmatism. However, the nature of that elaboration and thoughts.
still would be expected to vary as a function of schema H6: If moderate but not extreme schema incon-
(in)congruity, implying that the effects predicted in gruity leads to resolution within the existing
SCHEMA CONGRUITY 47

cognitive structure, then greater.mention.of uation outlined in Hypotheses 1 and 2 received sup-
categories that can resolve the 1Ocongrulty port at posttaste.
should be made in thoughts and recall mea-
sures in the moderate than in the extreme Mediation. Hypotheses 5, 6, and 7 were tested by
incongruity condition. examining subjects' cognitive responses and recall.
Thoughts were classified into five categories: the total
87: If extreme schema incongruity leads to neg- number of thoughts generated, the proportion of
ative feelings of helplessness and frustra- thoughts that were positive, the proportion of
tion then a greater proportion of negative thoughts that were negative, the presence/absence of
and' a smaller proportion of positive any mention of the target attribute, and the presence/
thoughts should be observed in this than in absence of any mention of soft drink. 6 In coding re-
other conditions. call, statements paraphrasing those appearing in the
description were considered valid responses, and the
EXPERIMENT 2 number of such statements was summed. The pres-
ence/absence of any mention of the target attribute
Method and mention of soft drink in recall also was recorded.
Hypothesis 5, in which we posited that incongruity
Subjects were 54 middle-aged m~na~ers and t~eir leads to greater elaboration than does congruity, was
spouses who were attending a.cont!nu1Og e~~cat~on

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


tested by examining the total number of thoughts
program at a Midwestern uDlverslty. PartIcIpatIon generated, overall recall of the product description,
was motivated by entering subjects in a $50 lottery. and mention of the target attribute in thoughts and
The experimental procedure was identi~al to that in recall (cf. Wood 1982). A marginal schema activated
Experiment 1 except for several changes 10 the depen- by target attribute interaction emerged for three of
dent measures. Thus, subjects were assigned ran- the four measures (overall recall, F(1,43) = 3.70, p
domly to read one of the four product. d~~criptio~s = 0.06, w 2 = 0.05; target attribute in thoughts,
that resulted from crossing the schema IDltlally actI- F(1,47) = 3.09, p = 0.09, w2 = 0.04; target attribute
vated (beverage or soft drink) with the target attri~ute in recall F( 1,43) = 4.80, p = 0.08, w2 = 0.06). Follow-
(high preservative or all natural). The dogmatIsm up contrasts comparing the three conditions involv-
scale that had been administered following the prod- ing some level of schema incon~uity (beverag~/all
uct description in Experiment 1 was deleted, and cog- natural, beverage/high preservatIve, and s,!~ dnnk/
nitive response and recall measures were added. No all natural) with the schema congruity condItIon (soft
time limit was imposed on either task. Subjects com- drink/high preservative) revealed that inco~gruity
pleted the same pretaste scales used in Experiment 1, led to higher scores on these measures than dId con-
tasted a sample of the beverage, and then repeated gruity (overall recall, t(I,45) = 2.52, p < 0.05; target
their beverage evaluation posttaste. attribute in thoughts, t(1,45) = 2.89, p < 0.01; target
attribute in recall, t(1,45) = 2.59, p < 0.05. But, con-
Results trary to expectations, no significant effect was ob-
Evaluations. Pretaste and posttaste evaluations served on the fourth measure, total number of
displayed a pattern of effects similar to the ones ob- thoughts. This latter outcome is consistent with our
served for nondogmatics in Experiment 1. However, earlier speculation that administering cognitive re-
in this instance, MANOVA on the seven pretaste sponse and recall measures would motivate all sub-
scales did not uncover any statistically significant jects to engage in significant elaboration. Thus, only
effects (Fs < 1 see Table 1). The MANOV A on the the content of elaboration reflected the different pro-
seven posttast~ scales rev~aled that the only s~gnifi cessing.
cant effect was an interactIon ofthe schema actIvated Hypothesis 6 was tested by examining mentions of
and the target attribute (F(7,43) = 2.37, p < 0.05, soft drink for the two conditions in which the bever-
?mult = 0.09; see Table 1 for cell centroids), and this age schema was activated. We have suggested that
followed the same pattern as that depicted for non- the beverage/high preservative condition embodied
dogmatics in Figure 2. When the beverage schema moderate incongruity because the incongruity repre-
was activated, evaluations were more favorable when sented could be resolved by activating the soft drink
the target attribute was high preservative than when schema, the next lower level in the beverage hierar-
it was all natural (F(I,49) = 14.20, p < 0.01), whereas chy, whereas the beverage/all natural condition pre-
when the soft drink schema was activated, evalua-
tions were more favorable when the target attribute 6Focus centered on mentions of soft drink because this was the
basic level category that pretests demonstrated resolved the incon-
was all natural than when it was high preservative gruity in the moderate but not the extreme. condition. A rea~ing of
(F(I,49) = 5.47, p < 0.05). Th!ls, the nO!lmonotonic subjects' thoughts revealed that few mentions of other baSIC level
relationship between schema (1O)congrulty and eval- categories occurred, so no attempt was made to code them.
48 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

sum ably represented extreme incongruity because them (e.g., high preservative/fruit juice) can be inter-
the incongruity could not be resolved by activating preted in either a favorable or unfavorable manner
the soft drink (or any other) schema, the next lower depending on consumers' motivations (see studies
hierarchical level. Consistent with this logic, there demonstrating affect priming effects; Hamilton and
was a greater tendency to mention soft drink in Zanna 1974 and Higgins, Rholes, and Jones 1977).
thoughts and recall in the beverage/high preservative Second, consumers are likely to expect manufactur-
condition than in the beverage/all natural condition ers who introduce new products to offer advantages
(thoughts, F(1,41) = 2.82, p < 0.10; recall, F(1,41) (rather than disadvantages) over existing alternatives.
= 2.10, p = 0.15). However, the effects on these mea- Thus, consumers may be predisposed to interpret
sures are relatively weak. In retrospect, it seems likely new product features positively. On the basis of these
that the procedure of ultimately informing all sub- observations, we reason that consumers' expectancies
jects that the product was a soft drink may have com- combined with the positive affect produced by resolv-
promised the sensitivity of the test of Hypothesis 6. ing incongruity are likely to prime or encourage fa-
Finally, we tested Hypothesis 7, which suggests that vorable interpretation of new products that are
extreme incongruity results in frustration and, thus, moderately incongruent with a product category to
a greater proportion of negative and a smaller propor- the extent that such interpretations are plausible. The
tion of positive thoughts. This hypothesis received no implication is that under these conditions, consumers
support (ps ~ 0.20). The failure to observe any evi- might view a high preservative fruit juice as offering

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


dence of frustration in thoughts may be due to the the benefit of long shelf life and evaluate it more fa-
absence of such affect or subjects simply may have felt vorably than a new product offering all natural fruit
it inappropriate to include such affect in their cogni- juice, which is schema congruent and thus offers no
tive responses. point of difference. This prediction regarding the
robustness of Hypothesis 1 is examined in Experi-
Discussion ment 3.
Fruit juice, a beverage schema different from that
To this point, some support has been offered for the used in Experiments 1 and 2, was chosen for Experi-
hypothesized nonmonotonic relationship between ment 3 to obtain additional evidence for the non-
schema (in)congruity and evaluation and the process monotonic nature of the relationship between
underlying this relationship. However, at least two schema congruity and evaluation. In Experiments 1
questions related to the robustness of this phenome- and 2, evidence for the nonmonotonic relationship
non remain. First, will this pattern of effects be ob- was necessarily across attributes. More compelling
served if a product schema other than soft drink is evidence for this relationship would be obtained by
employed? Second, will the enhancing effect of mod- building upon the previous analysis to demonstrate
erate incongruity on evaluation that was observed in such nonmonotonicity for a single attribute, all natu-
Experiments 1 and 2 occur even when resolving the ral, across the three levels of congruity.
incongruity entails activating an ostensibly unfavor- Also in Experiment 3 is a further examination of
ably regarded schema? Mandler implies that the posi- schema (in)congruity and its effects on two new mea-
tive affect generated by the process of resolving mod- sures, certainty and accessibility of evaluations. Re-
erate incongruity may be overwhelmed by other search by Fazio and his colleagues (Fazio et al. 1982;
sources of affect, such as extreme affect that may be Powell and Fazio 1984) suggests that elaborative
associated with an activated schema or an incongru- thought allows judgments to be made with greater
ent attribute. Applying this logic to beverages, one certainty, and because accessibility appears to be pos-
might argue that resolving moderate incongruity by itively related to the amount of processing items re-
activating an unfavorable alternative schema, such as ceive (Wyler and Carlston 1979), these judgments are
high preservative fruit juice (see beverage hierarchy more rapidly retrieved from memory. Thus, on the
depicted in Figure 1), might lead to less rather than basis of our assumption that incongruity increases
more favorable evaluations than would schema con- elaboration, two additional hypotheses are advanced.
gruity (e.g., an all natural fruit juice). Yet this reason-
ing rests on the assumption that the high preservative H8: Moderate schema incongruity will lead to
fruit juice schema is extremely and reliably negative. greater certainty about evaluations than will
Although limits to the advantage of moderate in- schema congruity.
congruity undoubtedly exist, there are at least two H9: Moderate schema incongruity will lead to
reasons to suspect that features of the new product faster response in retrieving evaluations
evaluation context may make the advantage of mod- than will schema congruity.
erate incongruity more generalizable than one would
expect intuitively. First, many product features and, Finally, in Experiment 3, we examine how individ-
in turn, schemas that may be integrally related to uals' need for cognition may affect the relationship
SCHEMA CONGRUITY 49

between schema (in)congruity and evaluation. Even Procedure


when all individuals attempt to resolve schema in-
congruities (as appeared to occur in Experiment 2) A Zenith 150 microcomputer terminal was used to
the desire to cogitate may moderate the enjoyment present all stimulus materials and to record subjects'
derived from this process and, thus, may influence the responses. Response times to all dependent measures
ex~ent to which Hypotheses 1 and 2 hold (see Hypoth- ~ere assessed to the nearest second. Subjects were as-
eSIS 4 for a formal statement of this hypothesis). sIgned randomly to a treatment condition by the
computer, and they proceeded through all tasks at
EXPERIMENT 3 their own pace.
As in previous studies, subjects read a product de-
Method sc~iption that activated one of two schemas (soft
Seventy-five male graduate students served as sub- dnnk or fruit juice) and contained one of the two tar-
jects and were tested individually. Their cooperation get attributes (high preservative or all natural). After
was motivated by a chance to participate in a lottery reading this description, subjects completed the
for two prizes valued at $100 each. "Need for Cognition" scale (Cacioppo and Petty
The product description employed in the previous 1982). They then responded to pretaste evaluation
studies was modified so that one of two basic level cat- measures on the same scales used in the previous
studies, tasted a sample of the beverage, and com-

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


egory schemas in the beverage hierarchy was acti-
vated: soft drink or fruit juice. Fruit juice was chosen pleted posttaste evaluation scales. In addition sub-
because it represented a category schema at the same jects' certainty with regard to each evaluation' item
level in the beverage hierarchy as soft drinks but one w,:,-s assessed on seven-point scales (1 = not at all cer-
for which schema incongruity as a function dfthe tar- taIn, 7. = c.ompletely certain). The beverage object
get attributes was reversed. When the soft drink tasted In thIS study was a carbonated drink that con-
schema was activated, the high preservative attribute tained the juice and pulp of real oranges. Thus, the
produ~t tast~d could be viewed by subjects as compat-
represented schema congruity and the all natural at-
tribute represe~ted moderate schema incongruity, Ible wIth whIchever schema (soft drink or fruit juice)
whereas pretestIng revealed that when the fruit juice was evoked in their product description.
schema was activated, the all natural attribute repre- Once again, theorizing regarding the effects of
sented schema congruity and the high preservative at- schema (in)congruity led to the prediction of a
tr!bute represented moderate sch~ma incongruity schema activated by target attribute interaction. In
(hkelihood of being a fruit juice XS = 8.00 versus this interaction, two contrasts were of particular in-
ter~st. First, comparison of the fruit juice/high preser-
4.50; t(1,19) = 4.32, p < 0.01; see Footnote 4). Fur-
ther, and in contrast to soft drinks, the ratings for the vatIve (moderate incongruity) and the fruit juice/all
two attributes differed significantly in their favorable- natural (congruity) conditions allowed examination
ness for a fruit juice. A high preservative fruit juice of the generality of Hypothesis 1 to a new and ostensi-
was evaluated significagtly more negatively than an bly unfav:orable alternative schema (fruit juice/high
all natural fruit juice (Xs = 2.30 versus 8.55; t(1, 19) preservatIve). Second, comparison of the soft drink/
= 7.97, p < 0.01).
all natural (moderate incongruity) and the fruit juice/
This difference in attribute favorableness allows ex- all natural (congruity) conditions provides additional
amination of whether, in the new product introduc- testing of the nonmonotonic nature of schema con-
tion context, subjects develop a favorable interpreta- gruity effects on evaluation. In Experiments 1 and 2
tion for a moderately incongruous product that oth- the all natural attribute yielded more favorable evalu~
erwise would be viewed unfavorably. For example, ~tions when it ~as moderately rather than extremely
will they reason that a high preservative fruit juice Incongruent w~th the activated schema. Thus, if the
offers the benefit of a long shelf life? This interpreta- all natural attnbute also could be shown to result in
~ore favorable evaluations when it was moderately
tion would perhaps be encouraged by the claim that
the drink will "stay fresh in your refrigerator." If such Incongruent rather than congruent with the activated
an interpretation were generated, then this moder- schema, further evidence of nonmonotonicity would
ately incongruous treatment might once again result be offered.
in more favorable evaluation than would schema
congruity (i.e., all natural fruit juice). Other than the
served cold, available in food stores, and carbonated) led subjects
c.hange in .the schema activated, the product descrip- !o. rate Jhe product as more likely to be a soft drink than a fruit
tIon remaIned the same as in previous experiments. 7 JUice (Xs. = 4.88 versus 4 .. 13 on seven-point scales). However, the
observat1~n that these ratmgs were near the midpoint of the scales
and the ~lfference between them was nonsignificant suggests that
7Pretesting had show~ that, in the absence of any category label, the labeling of a beverage described in this manner as a fruit juice
the five constant attnbutes (nonalcoholic, slightly sweet, best was reasonable.
50 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 2
CELL MEANS FOR EXPERIMENT 3

Soft drink schema Fruit juice schema

All natural High preservative All natural High preservative

Moderate Moderate
Dependent measures incongruity Congruity Congruity incongruity

Pretaste evaluation 39.73 34.52 36.38 37.53


Posttaste evaluation 40.40 36.59 31.50 38.24
Certainty of pretaste evaluation 37.53 35.27 34.83 35.28
Certainty of posttaste evaluation 45.00 42.70 38.75 43.29
Reaction time for pretaste evaluation 46.27 46.83 59.67 44.07
Reaction time for posttaste evaluation 28.71 33.08 45.44 35.88
Reaction time for certainty of pretaste evaluation 32.33 26.08 26.90 30.13
Reaction time for certainty of posttaste evaluation 16.50 21.00 22.20 25.13

NOTE: Higher numbers reflect more favorable, more certain, and slower responses. Means were calculated by summing over the seven indicators measured.

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


The thought stimulated by moderate incongruity subjects' pretaste and posttaste evaluations revealed
was anticipated to lead to more certain evaluations significant and similar schema activated by target at-
and evaluations that could be more readily retrieved tribute interactions (pretaste, F(1,71) = 6.00, p
when responding to evaluation scales (Hypothesis 8
and Hypothesis 9). The effects also were expected to
be moderated by subjects' need for cognition.
Stronger effects were predicted for subjects having a FIGURE 3
high need for cognition (Hypothesis 4). PLOT OF SUMMED EVALUATION SCALES DEPICTING THE
SCHEMA ACTIVATED BY TARGET ATTRIBUTE INTERACTION
ON EXPERIMENT 3 POSTTASTE EVALUATIONS
Results and Discussion
Favorableness
The effects of the need for cognition measure were of evaluation
explored both by including it as a factor in the design
and testing it as a covariate. This variable yielded no 50
significant effects, perhaps due to the relative homo-
geneity of Jhe graduate student sample on this di-
mension (X = 234.61, sd = 22.84). As a result, need
for cognition is not discussed further, but it should be 45 Moderate
explored in future research employing more hetero- incongruity
geneous subject populations.
Analyses on the basic 2 X 2 design (schema acti- ", Moderate
incongruity
vated by target attribute) employing MANOVA re- "
",
40
vealed a skewness in the data. As a result, despite sig-
nificant univariate Fs for the interaction term on
nearly all pretaste and posttaste measures, the multi-
"
variate F for posttaste was only marginally significant
(p = 0.15). Under these conditions, analysis of a 35 Congruity
""
summed scale is recommended because it is less sensi-
tive to skewness in data. Accordingly, the sets of seven "Congruity
evaluation and certainty items measured at pretaste
and posttaste were summed, and analysis proceeded 30
on the four scales thus created. These scales were reli-
able (evaluation and certainty at pretaste a = 0.95 Soft drink Fruit juice
and 0.94 and at posttaste a = 0.80 and 0.79).
The means for the evaluation and certainty mea- Schema activated
sures appear in Table 2, and posttaste data are de- - - - high preservative
picted graphically in Figure 3. Separate ANOV As on all natural
SCHEMA CONGRUITY 51

< 0.05, 'Ii = 0.06; posttaste F(I,71) = 5.72, p < 0.05, of the soft drink schema (moderate incongruity) than
w2 = 0.06). Planned contrasts on posttaste data ex- upon activation of the fruit juice schema (congruity;
plored the two relationships of central interest. Mod- posttaste certainty, F(1,71) = 12.23, P < 0.01; post~
erate schema incongruity, represented by the fruit taste RT-EVAL, F(I,67) = 6.98,p < 0.01).
juice/high preservative condition, led to more favor- The observation that, relative to schema congruity,
able evaluations than did schema congruity, which moderate schema incongruity is associated with
was represented by the fruit juice/all natural condi- greater certainty about and accessibility of evalua-
tion (F( 1,71) = 4.33, p < 0.05). This finding occurred tions is noteworthy. As Fazio et al. (1982) have
despite the negative affect ostensibly associated with claimed, evaluations that are held with greater cer-
fruit juice/high preservative. tainty and that are more accessible are more reliable
Comparison of the two all natural treatments al- predictors of overt behavior. Further, that certainty
lowed further examination of the robustness of Hy- and RT-EVAL effects are evident at posttaste but not
pothesis 1. As expected, evaluations were more favor- pretaste implies that these effects may reflect retrieval
able when activation of the soft drink schema resulted rather than encoding processes.
in moderate incongruity than when activation of the
fruit juice schema produced schema congruity GENERAL DISCUSSION
(F( 1,71) = 7.10, p < 0.01). Further, when this finding
is combined with the results of the two previous ex- The three experiments provide support for Mand-

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


periments, there is evidence for the non monotonic ler's (1982) view that the process of responding to lev-
effect of schema (in)congruity on evaluations even els of schema congruity influences evaluations, and
when the target attribute is held constant. Such an that moderate schema incongruity enhances evalua-
outcome reduces the plausibility of any rival explana- tions. Moderate schema incongruity led to more fa-
tion that is based on attribute differences. vorable evaluations than either schema congruity
To test Hypotheses 8 and 9, the pretaste and post- (soft drink schema, all experiments; fruit juice
taste certainty scales, summed reaction times to eval- schema, Experiment 3) or extreme schema incongru-
uation measures (RT-EVAL), and summed reaction ity (beverage schema, Experiments 1 and 2). Further,
times to certainty measures (RT-CERT) were exam- the data are consistent with our contention that,
ined. 8 At pretaste, none of these measures showed any though moderate schema incongruity might be oper-
significant effect (Fs < 1). However, at posttaste sub- ationalized in other ways, one way is to view it as in-
jects' ratings of their certainty about their evaluations congruity that is resolvable by activating an alterna-
and their reaction time to the evaluation scales tive schema at the next lower level in the product hier-
yielded the same schema activated by target attribute archy. Both incongruity at a superordinate category
interaction observed for posttaste evaluations (post- schema that could be resolved at the next lower, basic
taste certainty, F(I,71) = 8.41, p < 0.01, w 2 = 0.09; level and incongruity at a basic level schema that
posttaste RT-EVAL, F(I,67) = 3.70, p < 0.01, w2 could be resolved at the next lower, subordinate level
= 0.07; (posttaste RT-CERT did not show any sig- operated in the manner expected for moderate incon-
nificant effects; however, none had been anticipated). gruity. And, in the new product context examined,
Planned contrasts for posttaste certainty and post- this operationalization of moderate schema incon-
taste RT-EVAL revealed support for Hypotheses 8 gruity produced favorable evaluations even when the
and 9. When the fruit juice schema was activated ini- alternative schema activated was itself evaluated un-
tially, subjects were more certain of their evaluations favorably (high preservative/fruit juice schema, Ex-
and responded somewhat faster if the target attribute periment 3).
was high preservative (moderate schema incongruity) One finding that may puzzle or disturb some read-
than ifit was all natural (schema congruity; posttaste ers is that evaluation effects were found to be stronger
certainty, F(1,71) = 6.88, p < 0.01; posttaste RT- for posttaste than for pretaste measures. Some may
EVAL, F(1,67) = 3.37, p = 0.07). Further, when the feel that pretaste measures offer a purer test of the
target attribute was all natural, subjects were more effects, as they rule out the possibility that evaluations
certain and responded more rapidly upon activation were contaminated by subjects' having tasted the
product and that this may somehow have produced
8In the analysis of the reaction time measures, the summed reac-
the outcomes observed. For example, one might posit
tion time to the need for cognition items was used as a covariate. that the treatment differences obtained at posttaste
This was done to control for individual differences in skill with the occurred because in some cases the product that sub-
computer keyboard, which varied considerably (X = 309.79, sd jects tasted confirmed their expectations about the
= 104.93 on summed reaction time to the need for cognition mea-
product's category and benefits (e.g., the soft drink/
sures). Before using this variable as a covariate, it was examined as
a dependent variable, and no significant effects on it were observed. all natural condition), whereas, in other cases, expec-
The reaction time to the need for cognition items was a significant tations were disconfirmed (e.g., the fruit juice/all nat-
covariate in the analysis ofposttaste RT-EVAL. ural condition because the product sampled may
52 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

have been viewed as more of a soft drink than a fruit instances where no taste expectancy exists, pretaste
juice). Though plausible at first glance, inspection of effects may be stronger (e.g., Lehtisalo 1985). Hence,
the data casts doubt on this explanation. If this pro- further research exploring the relationship between
cess were operating, the pattern of effects on subjects' pretaste and posttaste evaluations is warranted.
pretaste evaluations should reflect their expectancies Several findings provide some insight regarding the
against which the sampled product is compared. And process thought to underlie the nonmonotonic effect
if these expectancies sometimes were disconfirmed at of schema incongruity on evaluation. As proposed by
trial, the pattern of pretaste evaluations should be op- Mandler (1982), schema incongruity is associated
posite that observed at posttaste. To the contrary, the with increased cognitive elaboration, as evidenced by
pattern of evaluations at pretaste mimicked that at increased performance on recall and thoughts in Ex-
posttaste, and these pretaste effects were of marginal periment 2. However, increased cognitive activity is
significance or better in two of the three experiments. not sufficient to account for schema (in)congruity
Thus, this alternative explanation does not seem to effects on evaluation. As findings observed on recall
account for the data. and thoughts also suggest, evaluation is enhanced
What then might account for the observation that only when increased thought leads to resolution of
pretaste evaluation effects follow the same pattern as the incongruity. Such resolution appears to occur for
posttaste effects, but the latter effects are stronger moderate but not extreme schema incongruity. Fur-
even though all subjects tasted the very same prod- ther, relative to schema congruity, the increased cog-

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


uct? A possible explanation for this phenomenon, nitive activity stimulated by moderate schema incon-
and one that has some precedence, is that hypothesis gruity also led to evaluations that were made with
testing may have occurred. In a new product taste test greater certainty, and these evaluations could be
quite similar to the ones reported here, Scott and more rapidly accessed from memory (Experiment 3).
Yalch (1978, p. 180) found that the effects of their This implies that evaluations made under moderate
manipulations on product ratings did not appear incongruity may be better predictors of overt behav-
"until subjects gathered more information about the ior than evaluations made under schema congruity
object (i.e., tasted it)." Darley and Gross (1983) also (Fazio et al. 1982). Additional insight into the process
provide evidence that individuals often are reluctant can be obtained by continuing to explore thoughts
to express hypotheses that they develop on the basis and recall effects in future studies.
of ambiguous experimental stimuli prior to having an It is noteworthy that, in the new product context
opportunity to test them through direct experience. examined, the advantage of moderate schema incon-
Thus, it seems likely that subjects in our study who gruity is robust across the presumably favorable and
at the outset expected to taste the product, initially unfavorable alternative schemas evoked in response
formulated tentative hypotheses about the product to both favorable and unfavorable target attributes.
that they then tested in a biased, confirmatory man- We suggest that this outcome arose because both the
ner (Bettman, John, and Scott 1986), thereby inter- context and the positive affect presumably generated
preting the ambiguous taste experience as supportive as a consequence of resolving the moderate incongru-
of these hypotheses (Crocker 1981; Scott and Yalch ity primed subjects to access or develop positive asso-
1978). Some support for this notion was obtained on ciations concerning a schema that would otherwise
pretaste and posttaste certainty scales. Subjects were have been evaluated unfavorably. Results similar to
more confident of and willing to express their views the ones reported here also were obtained in three ex-
after having confirmed them by experiencing the periments conducted by Lehtisalo (1985). She used
product. Prior to tasting the product, subjects were our general procedure but varied price to create the
generally uncertain and certainty ratings exhibited no different levels of schema congruity. Lehtisalo reports
treatment differences; after tasting, their certainty that moderate schema incongruity, created by either
ratings were higher and followed the pattern expected an unusually high or an unusually low price for a
on the basis of different degrees of cognitive activity. product category, led to more favorable evaluation
At the same time, caution is urged in generalizing than schema congruity or extreme schema incongru-
these findings. In our studies, the product taste expe- ity. Apparently, subjects were able to make favorable
rience was relatively neutral/ambiguous, allowing it interpretations of either price (high price = high qual-
to be interpreted in a confirmatory fashion across all ity, low price = good value) when moderate incongru-
treatments. It may be that if experience is clearly at ity and the new product context motivated them to
odds with expectancies, affect may be guided more by do so. However, it seems likely that despite any moti-
the magnitude of the contrast or disconfirmation vation to activate positive associations to moderate
rather than by initial hypotheses. Moreover, subjects incongruity that may occur, no such associations may
initially were told that they would taste the product, be accessible for certain extremely negative schemas
and thus they may have hesitated to express their (e.g., a carcinogenic fruit juice). And in other situa-
views strongly prior to obtaining experiential data. In tions, favorable affect generated by the process of re-
SCHEMA CONGRUITY 53

sponding to moderate incongruity may only temper Object-Evaluation Association," Journal of Experi-
an even stronger affect associated with the category mental Social Psychology, 14 (July), 339-357.
(cf. Fiske 1982). Such limits to the advantage of mod- Fiske, Donald W. and Salvatore R. Maddi (1961), Func-
erate incongruity should be explored in future re- tions of Varied Experience, Homewood, IL: Dorsey
Press.
search.
Fiske, Susan T. (1982), "Schema-triggered Affect: Applica-
Finally, the findings reported in the current studies tions to Social Perception," Affect and Cognition: The
may be qualified by other variables of interest to con- 17th Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition, eds.
sumer researchers. Alba and Hutchinson (1987) and Margaret S. Clark and Susan T. Fiske, Hillsdale, NJ:
Sujan and Dekleva (1987) both suggest that relative Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 55-78.
to novices, experts may possess different and more - - and Mark A. Pavelchak (1986), "Category-Based
differentiated category structures. This implies that Versus Piecemeal-Based Affective Responses: Devel-
novices and experts may respond differently to incon- opments in Schema-Triggered Affect," The Handbook
gruity because what is a moderate incongruity for ofMotivation and Cognition: Foundations ofSocial Be-
novices may be relatively congruent for experts either havior, eds. Richard M. Sorrentino and E. Tory Hig-
gins, New York: Guilford, 167-203.
because of differences in the way experts organize
Hamilton, David L. and Mark P. Zanna (1974), "Context
knowledge or because experts may have greater facil- Effects in Impression Formation: Changes in Connota-
ity in accessing more distal hierarchical categories tive Meaning," Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
that may facilitate resolution of incongruity. Thus, chology, 29 (May), 649-654.

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


the extent to which our results generalize to novices Higgins, E. Tory, William S. Rholes, and Carl R. Jones
and experts, children and adults, and nonloyal versus (1977), "Category Accessibility and Impression For-
brand loyal consumers is in need of investigation. mation," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
13 (March), 141-154.
Jacoby, Jacob (1971), "Personality and Innovation Prone-
[Received May 1987. Revised October 1988.] ness," Journal of Marketing Research, 8 (May), 244-
247.
Lehtisalo, Marjo-Riita (1985), "The Influence of Price on
REFERENCES Product Evaluation: An Information Processing Per-
Alba, Joseph W. and J. Wesley Hutchinson (1987), "Di- spective," unpublished doctoral dissertation, Depart-
mensions of Consumer Expertise," Journal of Con- ment of Marketing, Northwestern University, Evans-
sumer Research, 13 (March), 411-454. ton, IL 60208.
Anderson, John R. and Gordon H. Bower (1980), Human Lingle, John H., Mark W. Altom, and Donald L. Medin
Associative Memory: A Brief Edition, Hillsdale, NJ: (1984), "Of Cabbages and Kings: Assessing the Exten-
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. dibility of Natural Object Concept Models to Social
Berlyne, Donald E. (1963), "Motivational Problems Raised Things," in Handbook ofSocia I Cognition, eds. Robert
by Exploratory and Epistemic Behavior," in Psychol- S. Wyer and Thomas K. Srull, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
ogy: A Study of Science, Vol. 5, ed. Sigmund Koch, Erlbaum Associates, 71-118.
New York: Ronald Press, 284-364. Mandler, George (1982), "The Structure of Value: Ac-
Bettman, James R., Deborah Roedder John, and Carol A. counting for Taste," in Affect and Cognition: The 17th
Scott (1986), "Covariation Assessment by Consum- Annual Carnegie Symposium, eds. Margaret S. Clark
ers," Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (December), and Susan T. Fiske, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
316-326. Associates, 3-36.
Cacioppo, John T. and Richard E. Petty (1982), "The Need Medin, Donald L. (1982), "Structural Principles in Catego-
for Cognition," Journal of Personality and Social Psy- rization," in Perception, Cognition and Development:
chology, 42 (January), 116-131. Interactional Analysis, eds. Thomas Tighe et aI., Hills-
Cohen, Joel and Kunal Basu (1987), "Alternative Models dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 203-230.
of Categorization: Toward a Contingent Processing Nisbett, Robert and Lee Ross (1980), Human Inference:
Framework," Journal of Consumer Research, 13 Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment, En-
(March),455-472. glewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Collins, Allan M. and M. Ross Quillian (1969), "Retrieval Powell, Martha C. and Russell H. Fazio (1984), "Attitude
Time From Semantic Memory," Journal of Verbal Accessibility as a Function of Repeated Attitudinal Ex-
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8 (April), 240-247. pression," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Crocker, Jennifer (1981), "Judgment of Co variation by So- 10 (March), 139-148.
cial Perceivers," Psychological Bulletin, 90 (Septem- Rokeach, Martin (1960), The Open and Closed Mind, New
ber),272-292. York: Basic Books.
Darley, John M. and Paget H. Gross (1983), "A Hypothesis- Rosch, Eleanor (1978), "Principles of Categorization," in
Confirming Bias in Labeling Effects," Journal of Per- Cognition and Categorization, eds. Eleanor Rosch and
sonality and Social Psychology, 44 (January), 20-33. Barbara B. Lloyd, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Fazio, Richard H., Jeaw-Mei Chen, Elizabeth C. McDonel, Associates, 27-48.
and Steven J. Sherman (1982), "Attitude Accessibility, - - - , Carolyn Mervis, Wayne Gray, David Johnson, and
Attitude-Behavior Consistency, and the Strength ofthe Penny Boyes-Braem (1976), "Basic Objects in Natural
54 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Categories," Cognitive Psychology, 7 (October), 573- - - and Christine Dekleva (1987), "Product Categoriza-
605. tion and Inference Making: Some Implications for
Scott, Carol A. and Richard F. Yalch (1978), "A Test of the Comparative Advertising," Journal o/Consumer Re-
Self-Perception Explanation of the Effects of Reward search, 14 (December), 372-378.
on Intrinsic Interest," Journal of Experimental Social - - and Alice M. Tybout (1988), "Applications and Ex-
Psychology, 14 (March), 180-192. tensions of Categorization Research in Consumer Be-
Shaffer, David R. and Clyde Hendrick (1974), "Dogmatism havior," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 15,
and Tolerance for Ambiguity as Determinants of ed. Michael J. Houston, Provo, UT: Association for
Differential Reactions to Cognitive Inconsistency," Consumer Research, 50-54.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29 Troldahl, Verling C. and Fredric A. Powell (1965), "A
(MaY),601-608. Short-Form Dogmatism Sc~le for Use in Field Stud-
Snyder, C.R. and Howard L. Fromkin (1980), Uniqueness: ies," Social Forces, 44 (December), 211-214.
The Human Pursuit ofDifference, New York: Plenum. Venkatesan, M. (1973), "Cognitive Consistency and Nov-
Srull, Thomas K. (1981), "Person Memory: Some Tests of elty Seeking," in Consumer Behavior: Theoretical
Associative Storage and Retrieval Models," Journal of Sources, eds. Scott Ward and Thomas S. Robertson,
Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Mem- Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 334-384.
ory, 7 (November), 440-463. Wood, Wendy (1982), "Retrieval of Attitude-Relevant In-
- - , Meryl Lichtenstein, and Myron Rothbart (1985), formation from Memory: Effects on Susceptibility to
"Associative Storage and Retrieval Processes in Person Persuasion and Intrinsic Motivation," Journal of Per-

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 30, 2016


Memory," Journal of Experimental Psychology: sonality and Social Psychology, 42 (May), 798-810.
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11 (April), 316- Wyer, Robert S. and Donald E. Carlston (1979), Social Cog-
345. nition, Inference, and Attribution, Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
Sujan, Mita (1985), "Consumer Knowledge: Effects of rence Erlbaum Associates.
Evaluation Strategies Mediating Consumer Judg- - - and Sallie Gordon (1982), "The Recall of Informa-
ments," Journal ofConsumer Research, 12 (June), 31- tion about Persons and Groups," Journal of Experi-
46. mental Social Psychology, 18 (March), 128-164.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai