Anda di halaman 1dari 17

Child Development, xxxx 2014, Volume 00, Number 0, Pages 117

An Integrative View of School Functioning: Transactions Between


Self-Regulation, School Engagement, and TeacherChild Relationship Quality
Ximena A. Portilla Parissa J. Ballard
Stanford Graduate School of Education University of California, San Francisco and
University of California, Berkeley

Nancy E. Adler and W. Thomas Boyce Jelena Obradovic


University of California, San Francisco Stanford Graduate School of Education

This study investigates the dynamic interplay between teacherchild relationship quality and childrens behav-
iors across kindergarten and rst grade to predict academic competence in rst grade. Using a sample of 338
ethnically diverse 5-year-old children, nested path analytic models were conducted to examine bidirectional
pathways between childrens behaviors and teacherchild relationship quality. Low self-regulation in kinder-
garten fall, as indexed by inattention and impulsive behaviors, predicted more conict with teachers in kin-
dergarten spring and this effect persisted into rst grade. Conict and low self-regulation jointly predicted
decreases in school engagement which in turn predicted rst-grade academic competence. Findings illustrate
the importance of considering transactions between self-regulation, teacherchild relationship quality, and
school engagement in predicting academic competence.

The transition into formal schooling entails a period school readiness. While many studies have exam-
when children shift from predominately interacting ined relations among some of these elements of
with parents and begin interacting with other chil- childrens early schooling to predict future aca-
dren and teachers. As such, children are exposed to demic achievement, few have investigated these
new inuences and settings that shape later experi- together both concurrently and over time. This
ences, marking this transition a sensitive period for study aims to ll this gap by rigorously investigat-
later school success (Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta, ing the dynamic interplay between teacherchild
2000). These complex social settings place consider- relationship quality and childrens behaviors across
able demands on young children: Kindergarteners the kindergarten and rst-grade years to predict
need to form new relationships, control their academic competence in rst grade.
impulses, focus and pay attention, communicate
their needs appropriately, and engage with learning
TeacherChild Relationships
material. The dynamic interplay among all these
key ingredients is critical in determining childrens For many young children, kindergarten presents
a time for developing bonds with other adults.
Although teachers may appear to be transient g-
This research was supported by Grant R01 MH62320 from the ures in childrens lives as they progress from grade
National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH). Preparation of this
manuscript by Ximena A. Portilla was supported in part by the
to grade, teachers play an important role in shaping
Institute of Education Sciences (IES), U.S. Department of Educa- childrens adjustment to the school context.
tion, through Grant R305B090016 to Stanford University and a Teacherchild relationships that exhibit high close-
research grant from the Canadian Institute for Advanced
Research (CIFAR) to Jelena Obradovic. The authors acknowledge
ness are characterized by warmth and respect, with
the substantive contributions made by Juliet Stamperdahl and children seeing their teachers as a source for secu-
Nicole R. Bush in collecting and processing the data. The authors rity. Conversely, negative teacherchild relation-
also thank the teachers, children, and families who participated
and made this research possible. The ndings, conclusions, and
ships that are characterized by high conict appear
opinions here are those of the authors and do not represent to pose risks to childrens school success (Pianta,
views of the NIMH, IES, the U.S. Department of Education, or
CIFAR.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to 2014 The Authors
Ximena A. Portilla, Stanford Graduate School of Education, 520 Child Development 2014 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
Galvez Mall, #407, Stanford, CA 94305. Electronic mail may be All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2014/xxxx-xxxx
sent to ximena.portilla@stanford.edu. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12259
2 Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce, and Obradovic

1999). While teacherchild closeness and conict are clingy are more likely to be referred for special ser-
related constructs, they are only moderately corre- vices or be retained (Pianta, Steinberg, & Rollins,
lated, assessing unique aspects of relationship qual- 1995); this provides more evidence of the deleteri-
ity as opposed to falling along an underlying ous consequences for children who experience
continuum. conictual relationships with their teachers.
It is increasingly evident that the quality of the Furthermore, kindergarten teacherchild relation-
teacherchild relationship matters for childrens ships characterized by relational negativity pre-
social and academic performance in school. Chil- dicted lower student grades, standardized test
dren who are able to successfully navigate early scores and work habits through elementary school,
social environments in school and form close bonds and continued to uniquely predict behavioral dif-
with teachers can be set on positive developmental culty through middle school (Hamre & Pianta,
trajectories. Close teacherchild relationships have 2001). Hamre and Piantas (2001) ndings highlight
been positively linked to childrens school engage- the long reach early teacherchild relationship
ment (Birch & Ladd, 1997), academic performance, conict may have on childrens future academic
and good work habits (Baker, 2006; Birch & Ladd, success.
1997; Graziano, Reavis, Keane, & Calkins, 2007;
Hamre & Pianta, 2001), and these associations are
Bidirectional Transactions Between TeacherChild
shown to persist across the elementary school
Relationship Quality and Child Functioning
grades (Baker, 2006). Teachers who exhibit strong
emotional support in their classrooms have been Extending a transactional model of development
shown to improve childrens reading achievement (Sameroff & MacKenzie, 2003) to a school context,
from preschool to fth grade (Pianta, Belsky, Van- it is theorized that childrens behaviors and the
dergrift, Houts, & Morrison, 2008) and increase classroom environment, indexed in this study by
phonological awareness from kindergarten to rst relationship quality with teachers, interact through
grade (Curby, Rimm-Kaufman, & Ponitz, 2009). At bidirectional processes. Over time, the interplay
school entry, highly sensitive teachers have been between teacherchild relationship quality and
found to buffer the effects of a negative family con- childrens behaviors may form patterns that serve
text for children who have insecure attachments as both inputs and outcomes to childrens develop-
with their mothers by reducing childrens risk for ment (Arnold, McWilliams, & Arnold, 1998;
aggressive behavior (Buyse, Verschueren, & Dou- Downer, Sabol, & Hamre, 2010). To illustrate,
men, 2011). Furthermore, positive interactions with Doumen et al. (2008) found empirical evidence that
teachers may benet children who exhibit the high- childrens aggressive behavior displayed at kinder-
est levels of problematic behaviors at the start of garten onset led to greater teacherchild conict by
kindergarten (Silver, Measelle, Armstrong, & Essex, the middle of the school year, which in turn led to
2005). These benets appear to extend to other more aggressive behavior by those children at the
domains of adaptive functioning. When comparing end of the year. Some researchers argue that nega-
a group of children who displayed high levels of tive child characteristics largely drive conict with
aggression, those who experienced warm relation- teachers as conict tends to be measured by
ships with their teachers performed better in read- teachers perceptions of relationship quality and is
ing achievement than those who did not (Baker, composed of reactive teacher behavior resulting
Grant, & Morlock, 2008). from dealing with challenging behavior (Silver
However, children differ in their ability to con- et al., 2005).
nect with teachers and capitalize on these experi- Children who perceive their teachers to be
ences that promote school success. In particular, accepting and caring are more likely to internalize
conict with teachers may negatively impact chil- learning and prosocial goals valued by their teach-
drens sense of belonging and perception of ers (Wentzel, 1999). By displaying expected
academic competence, as well as the motivation or behavior in the classroom, positive interactions with
engagement necessary to excel in school (Spilt, teachers are theorized to further reinforce
Hughes, Wu, & Kwok, 2012). In fact, relationships acceptable behavior. However, empirical evidence
characterized by conict have been associated with suggests that teacherchild closeness is only moder-
greater school avoidance, lower school engagement, ately associated with child characteristics (Jerome,
less self-directedness, and less cooperative partici- Hamre, & Pianta, 2009). The degree to which chil-
pation (Birch & Ladd, 1997). Teachers who perceive dren and teachers can connect may be more indica-
young children to be aggressive, argumentative, or tive of a dynamic pattern building on strengths of
Integrative View of School Functioning 3

both teacher and child, rather than a reactive pat- of inattention and impulsivity in young children
tern to child characteristics as is conceptualized for (Olson, Sameroff, Kerr, Lopez, & Wellman, 2005).
teacherchild conict (Spilt et al., 2012). Moreover, children who lack the attentional and
inhibitory control processes necessary to focus on
educational material tend to exhibit challenges
Predictors of TeacherChild Relationship Quality and
learning and engaging with classroom activities.
School Readiness
These challenges potentially place them at risk for
There is general consensus that early experiences reduced academic achievement as they progress
in school are critical for shaping childrens future through school (Blair, 2002). Such an association
academic careers. Children who achieve academi- was evident from six longitudinal studies suggest-
cally early on continue to show achievement gains; ing that childrens attentional skills in kindergarten,
those who encounter learning problems face contin- such as task persistence, predicted math and read-
uing negative consequences that persist over time ing achievement in third grade (Duncan et al.,
(Perry, Donohue, & Weinstein, 2007). To enhance 2007).
childrens early school experiences, it is essential to However, inattention and impulsive behaviors
better understand competencies that promote learn- do not only reect low levels of self-regulation
ing and positive relationship quality with teachers. skills. These behaviors are multiply determined and
In particular, two areas of childrens functioning can be socially constructed. Indeed, research shows
that are thought to predict school readiness and that relationship quality is important and childrens
academic achievement are childrens self-regulation ability to self-regulate contributes to how they are
skills (Blair & Razza, 2007; Duncan et al., 2007; viewed by others, particularly teachers (Myers &
McClelland et al., 2007) and school engagement Pianta, 2008). Recent empirical studies provide evi-
(Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004; Ladd & dence that both parent and teacher survey mea-
Dinella, 2009). sures of self-regulation skills predict greater
teacherchild closeness (Liew, Chen, & Hughes,
2010; Rudasill & Rimm-Kaufman, 2009; Valiente,
Self-Regulation
Swanson, & Lemery-Chalfant, 2012) and parent-
Self-regulation is a broad, multidimensional con- reported self-regulation skills predict less teacher
struct consisting of cognitive and behavioral pro- child conict (Myers & Morris, 2009). Conversely,
cesses that allow individuals to maintain optimal children who display inattention and impulsivity
levels of emotional, motivational, and cognitive may experience difculties engaging in positive
arousal for positive adjustment and adaptation relationships with teachers (Barkley, 1998). Teachers
(Blair & Diamond, 2008). Self-regulatory capacities may view children who lack self-regulatory capaci-
are implicated in the ability to control impulses and ties as intentionally misbehaving, causing teachers to
pay attention, behaviors that are relevant for school react in a disciplinary fashion and engage in more
success. Upon entering kindergarten, children are conict with these children. Beyond disciplining,
faced with a new set of challenges in the classroom: teachers may only engage with these children in an
They need to learn how to be independent from instructional format, affording fewer opportunities
their caregivers, navigate social interactions with for mutual exchange and positive interaction (Silva
other children, pay attention for longer periods of et al., 2011). Furthermore, the presence of challeng-
time, and adhere to a classroom routine (Rimm- ing behaviors may be more salient and consuming
Kaufman & Pianta, 2000). to teachers, disrupting learning opportunities for all
Difculties with self-regulation may be most eas- children within the classroom.
ily observed as impulsive and inattentive behavior
in the classroom setting. Both of these behaviors
School Engagement
may be seen as markers of low inhibitory control,
particularly response inhibition in the context of School engagement has often been studied as a
impulsive behaviors and interference suppression in possible antecedent of academic achievement. This
the context of inattention. Furthermore, Barkley construct has been broadly conceptualized in three
(1997) theorized that inattention and impulsivity domains: behavioral (i.e., participation in extracur-
emerge when children face challenges with emo- ricular activities), emotional (i.e., positive and nega-
tional self-regulation and working memory. Low tive feelings and reactions toward school, teachers,
performance on laboratory tasks measuring self-reg- peers), or cognitive (i.e., willingness to invest in
ulation have been associated with higher incidence learning difcult skills and comprehension of
4 Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce, and Obradovic

complex ideas; Fredricks et al., 2004). Young chil- with children: Teachers report more closeness with
drens school engagement may be most manifested girls and more conict with boys (Birch & Ladd,
through an examination of emotional school 1998; Hamre & Pianta, 2001). Boys have also been
engagement. A signicant body of evidence sup- found to be more distractible and active (Mendez,
ports the idea that emotional school engagement is McDermott, & Fantuzzo, 2002; Walker, Berthelsen,
an important predictor for academic functioning & Irving, 2001) and less persistent on tasks than
(Ladd, Buhs, & Seid, 2000; Ladd & Dinella, 2009). girls (Walker et al., 2001), indicating that, on aver-
When children exhibit positive attitudes toward age, boys tend to experience more issues with self-
school, they are more likely to engage in classroom regulatory skills. Research has also shown that girls
activities that are designed to promote academic exhibit more school engagement and boys exhibit
and social competencies (Ladd et al., 2000). Simi- more school avoidance (Roorda, Koomen, Spilt, &
larly, children who demonstrate an orientation Oort, 2011; Silva et al., 2011). Taken together, this
toward learning and respond to classroom chal- evidence suggests the importance of controlling for
lenges in a mastery-oriented fashion tend to display gender differences at kindergarten entry.
patterns of motivation that predict positive school
adjustment (Heyman & Dweck, 1992).
An Integrative View of Childrens School Functioning
Teachers play a role in enhancing childrens
school enjoyment. Within a context of positive While making signicant conceptual and empiri-
teacherchild relationships, children likely feel more cal advancements to our understanding of how
condent in their abilities and motivated to partici- teacherchild conict and closeness predict chil-
pate in classroom activities (Silva et al., 2011). Fur- drens concurrent and prospective behavioral and
thermore, school engagement has been seen as academic functioning, much of the extant literature
mediating the association between teacherchild has not examined the unique contribution of con-
relationships and academic success in young chil- ict over and above teacherchild closeness (Arnold
dren (Hughes, Luo, Kwok, & Loyd, 2008). Applying et al., 1998; Doumen et al., 2008; Hamre & Pianta,
a transactional framework, childrens school engage- 2001) or vice versa (Buyse et al., 2011). Some stud-
ment may also predict subsequent teacherchild ies have examined the joint contributions of both
relationship quality. However, in a recent large-scale teacherchild closeness and conict by aggregating
study, school engagement in rst grade was unre- both constructs into one measure of overall relation-
lated to teacherchild relationship quality in fourth ship quality (Baker, 2006; Hughes et al., 2008;
grade (Archambault, Pagani, & Fitzpatrick, 2013). OConnor & McCartney, 2007), but this methodol-
Childrens school engagement is thought to be ogy does not allow for an examination of whether
supported by self-regulation. Children who are able underachievement or behavior problems are a
to control their emotions and behaviors tend to feel result of conict, deterioration of closeness, or both
more comfortable in school (Valiente, Lemery-Chal- (Spilt et al., 2012). Notably, Silver et al. (2005)
fant, & Swanson, 2010). In contrast, children who accounted for both when predicting growth in
lack self-regulation may feel socially alienated and behavior problems from kindergarten to third
withdraw from classroom participation (Valiente grade, but did not control for concurrent relations
et al., 2012). Thus, children who display better self- between child behavior and relationship quality in
regulation skills may elicit more positive interac- rst and third grades. This study addressed these
tions with teachers, which in turn promote their limitations by accounting for bidirectional inu-
enjoyment in school and other learning-related ences of both conict and closeness on childrens
activities, indicating that teacherchild relationship school adjustment over time.
quality may serve as a mediator in this association Given the interplay between teachers and chil-
(Silva et al., 2011). Examining how teacherchild dren during this rich period of development, studies
relationship quality and childrens self-regulation examining the effect of teacherchild relationship
may promote school engagement is therefore critical quality on childrens functioning must also consider
in understanding possible antecedents to academic the individual attributes that a child brings into the
competence and is a central aim of this study. school environment and how these attributes shape
transactions between teacherchild relationship
quality and the child. Recent literature has acknowl-
Gender Differences
edged this, but the majority of studies have only
A large body of literature has documented gen- investigated childrens externalizing behavior as an
der differences in how teachers perceive relations outcome (Leot, van Lier, Verschueren, Onghena, &
Integrative View of School Functioning 5

Colpin, 2011; Stipek & Miles, 2008), whereas others (see Figure 1). Model 1 depicts a continuity model
have only examined transactions within the course that examines the stability of each construct across
of one school year (Doumen et al., 2008). Building time, with gender predicting childrens behavior
on this research, this study examined indices of both and teacherchild relationship quality at kindergar-
positive and negative behavior longitudinally, ten entry. We hypothesized that gender at the start
investigating the importance of childrens self- of kindergarten would relate to teacherchild rela-
regulation and school engagement in predicting tionship quality and childrens inattention and
teacherchild relationship quality and later aca- impulsive behaviors and school engagement in the
demic competence. classroom. Specically, it was expected that boys
Relevant for this study, Eisenberg, Valiente, and might experience more conict with teachers,
Eggum (2010) proposed a conceptual model linking whereas girls might experience more closeness. We
childrens self-regulation and academic achieve- also hypothesized that girls would be more
ment. They posit that self-regulatory skills predict engaged in school and that boys would exhibit
relationship quality with others (e.g., teachers, more inattention and impulsive behaviors.
peers) and adjustment (e.g., problem behaviors, Across three time points, each construct was
social competence). These two pathways are theo- assessed using the same measures. However, chil-
rized to affect childrens school engagement, such dren were exposed to different teachers in kinder-
that issues with relationship quality or maladjust- garten and rst grade. Despite changes in
ment will lead to reduced school engagement. This, informants for teacher-reported domains, it was
in turn, predicts lower academic achievement. Thus, expected that signicant longitudinal stability
relationship quality, appropriate behavior, and would emerge. Considering recent work by Jerome
school engagement are conceptualized as account- et al. (2009), we hypothesized that the stability of
ing for indirect pathways between self-regulation teacherchild conict would be stronger than close-
and academic functioning. This study provided an ness. As extant research indicates that childrens
opportunity to empirically test these theorized link- inattention and impulsive behaviors, school engage-
ages and to expand beyond this conceptual frame- ment, and teacherchild closeness and conict are
work by examining relations among self-regulation, all related concurrently, this model also accounted
teacherchild relationship quality, school engage- for withintime-point covariation to detect longitu-
ment, and academic competence concurrently and dinal spillover effects. Cross-sectional research
over time to tease apart dynamic processes during reviewed earlier provides robust evidence for sig-
this sensitive period of development. nicant within-time covariation among these differ-
ent domains of adaptation.
Model 2 examined cross-domain transactions
The Current Study
across kindergarten to investigate whether there are
Extending prior work investigating transactional bidirectional transactions between teacherchild
processes between children and teacherchild rela- relationship quality and childrens behaviors. Given
tionship quality (Doumen et al., 2008; Leot et al., theoretical and empirical evidence suggesting that
2011; Rudasill & Rimm-Kaufman, 2009), this study teacherchild relationship quality and childrens
had three goals: (a) to examine how child gender behavior inuence each other to predict later child
predicts initial levels of childrens functioning (as outcomes, we expected to nd reciprocally inuen-
indexed by inattention and impulsive behaviors, tial relations from teacherchild relationship quality
school engagement, and teacherchild relationship to childrens functioning and vice versa. In particu-
quality) at school entry, (b) to investigate whether lar, we hypothesized bidirectional transactions
children and the teacherchild relationship quality between childrens school engagement and teacher
engage in bidirectional transactions across the kinder- child closeness, and bidirectional transactions
garten year to inuence later relationship quality between teacherchild conict and childrens inat-
and childrens functioning, and (c) to explore how tention and impulsive behaviors.
kindergarten childrens functioning and relation- Finally, Model 3 examined cross-domain transac-
ships with their teachers predict later behavior, tions to understand whether kindergarten processes
teacherchild relationship quality, and academic affected childrens functioning and teacherchild
competence in rst grade. relationship quality in rst grade. We hypothesized
Central hypotheses were systematically that school engagement at the end of kindergarten
evaluated using a series of nested path analytic would predict academic competence in rst grade
models and successive nested model comparisons given the mediating pathways that Eisenberg et al.
6 Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce, and Obradovic

Figure 1. A summary of the freely estimated paths in hierarchically nested path analytic models. Numbers denote time point of data
collection (1 = kindergarten fall, 2 = kindergarten spring, 3 = rst-grade spring). Model 1 represents a baseline comparison model of
longitudinal stability with gender predicting this stability, after accounting for withintime-point, cross-domain covariation. Model 2
represents diagonal pathways from T1 to T2. Model 3 adds diagonal pathways from T2 to T3. All models include paths from prior
models in sequence. CLO = closeness; CON = conict; SCE = school engagement; INAT/IMP = inattention/impulsivity; ACC = aca-
demic competence; T1 = kindergarten fall; T2 = kindergarten spring; T3 = rst-grade spring.

(2010) have posited. Also, given the empirical evi- attributed to longitudinal processes because the
dence linking both teacherchild relationship qual- cross-sectional cross-domain associations are already
ity and behavior problems with academic controlled for, allowing us to select a model that
achievement, we expected to nd a relation from most parsimoniously t the data. This extends prior
closeness and conict, as well as inattention and research that nds a signicant association between
impulsivity, to academic competence. earlier teacherchild relationship quality and later
This investigation extended the current literature academic achievement, but fails to examine whether
on school readiness and teacherchild relationship this association is due to longitudinal processes or
quality. Importantly, it combined research in the the processes that are happening concurrently at the
areas of self-regulation, teacherchild interactions, beginning or the end of the study period.
and school engagement, and examined how these
domains predict child academic competence in an
empirically rigorous way. Prior work has examined Method
different elements of this model; this study
Participants
included variables that were theoretically linked to
provide a more holistic picture of childrens school The sample was composed of 338 kindergarten
functioning at the transition to elementary school. children (M age at kindergarten entry = 5.31 years,
Nested path analytic comparisons offered the best SD = 0.32, range = 4.756.28 years; 175 males, 163
available method to empirically evaluate whether or females) who participated in a longitudinal study
not there were bidirectional transactions of child examining child mental and physical health, school
behaviors and teacherchild relationship quality functioning, biological responses to adversity, and
across the kindergarten year (Model 2) and into rst social dominance. Participants were recruited from
grade (Model 3). The rigor of this methodology is 29 classrooms in six public schools in the Bay Area,
due to the fact that these pathways are examined California, and data were collected across three
after accounting for longitudinal stability and waves in 2004, 2005, and 2006. The sample of children
withintime-point covariation (Model 1). As such, was highly diverse, with 43% being identied as Cau-
pathways linking domains across time can be casian, 19% as African American, 11% as Asian, 4%
Integrative View of School Functioning 7

as Latino, 22% as multiethnic, and 2% being TeacherChild Relationship Quality


described as other. Ten percent of primary caregiv-
Teacherchild relationship quality was reported at
ers described themselves as single parents and 28% of
all three time points by teachers using a shortened
children had at least one immigrant parent. Total
version of the StudentTeacher Relationship Scale
annual household income varied greatly, ranging
(STRS; Pianta, 1996). The 10-item version of the STRS
from 4% of the sample earning less than $10,000 to
assessed teachers perceptions of the level of close-
0.3% earning more than $400,000 (M = $60,000
ness and conict they experienced with individual
$79,000, Mdn = $80,000$99,999), which is represen-
children using a 5-point Likert-style rating scale
tative of the population in the Bay Area. Primary
(1 = denitely does not apply, 5 = denitely applies).
caregivers education level also varied greatly, with
The 5-item closeness subscale included items such as,
8% obtaining a high school degree or less and 45%
You share an affectionate, warm relationship with
having earned a graduate or professional degree.
this child and This child openly shares his/her feel-
Participating teachers from the 29 classrooms
ings and experiences with you. The 5-item Conict
were predominately female (76%) and Caucasian
subscale included items such as, You and this child
(82%). Teachers identied themselves as 11% Asian,
always seem to be struggling with each other and
4% African American, and 4% self-described as
This child easily becomes angry with you.
other. Mean age was 52 years (SD = 10.5) and
most of the teachers were veteran teachers with
62% having taught kindergarten for more than Child Functioning
3 years. Fifty-ve percent had a bachelors degree,
Childrens functioning was assessed in three
31% had a masters degree, and 14% had some
domains: inattention and impulsivity, school engage-
other type of credentialing.
ment, and academic competence. Parents and teach-
ers reported on these domains using scales from the
Procedures MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire
Data were collected from participants at three (HBQ; Armstrong & Goldstein, 2003). The HBQ
time points: T1kindergarten fall; T2kindergarten contains parallel measures to assess children in a
spring; and T3rst-grade spring. Before collecting multi-informant, multidomain approach. Parent and
any data, parents provided informed consent to par- teacher scores were rescaled and averaged to create
ticipate in the study. Parents were asked to provide composites for each domain at each time point to
information concerning their demographics, family capture assessments of childrens behaviors both at
functioning, and child functioning through a series home and in school and equally weight both
of questionnaires that were mailed to their home. contexts.
Compensation consisted of $50 per completed parent Inattention and impulsive behaviors were reported
survey. Teachers were asked to ll out question- at all three time points by parents and teachers via
naires about each participating childs functioning a composite of the mean of the Inattention and
and were compensated $15 per child for each com- Impulsivity subscales from the HBQ (Armstrong &
pleted survey. At T1, teachers completed responses Goldstein, 2003). Both subscales were rated using
between October and December, providing sufcient the following categories (0 = never/not true,
time to get to know the children. 1 = sometimes or somewhat true, 2 = often or very
true). Examples of items for the Inattention subscale
included, Distractible, has trouble sticking to one
Measures
activity, Cant concentrate, cant pay attention for
For this study, both parents and teachers reported long, and Has difculty following directions or
on childrens levels of impulsivity and inattention, instructions. The Impulsivity subscale included
school engagement, and academic competence to items such as, Cant stay seated when required to
capture multiple perspectives of childrens behavior do so, Has difculty awaiting turn in games or
across two different contexts (i.e., home and school; groups, and Interrupts, blurts out answers to
Kraemer et al., 2003) and teachers reported on the questions too soon.
teacherchild relationship quality. Table 1 provides School engagement was reported at all three time
descriptive and reliability statistics for the current points by parents and teachers using the School
sample (e.g., means, standard deviations, number of Engagement subscale from the HBQ (Armstrong &
items, Cronbachs alpha) for all measures by infor- Goldstein, 2003), which assessed both intrinsic
mant and across all time periods. motivation and school liking. Parents reported on
8

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for Predictors and Outcomes

Kindergarten fall Kindergarten spring First-grade spring


No. of
Domains/scales Scale items M SD % missing a r M SD % missing a r M SD % missing a r

Teacherchild
relationship quality
Closeness 15 5 4.17 0.85 0.0 0.89 4.28 0.81 3.0 0.91 4.18 0.81 12.4 0.90
Conict 15 5 1.51 0.84 0.0 0.89 1.59 0.92 3.0 0.91 1.62 0.88 12.7 0.88
Self-regulation 0.39*** 0.49*** 0.37***
Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce, and Obradovic

PHBQ
Inattention 02 6 0.52 0.40 13.3 0.81 0.56 0.41 16.9 0.80 0.54 0.42 35.8 0.82
Impulsivity 02 9 0.58 0.36 13.3 0.79 0.60 0.38 16.9 0.83 0.60 0.38 35.8 0.82
THBQ
Inattention 02 6 0.32 0.44 0.0 0.88 0.34 0.46 3.0 0.88 0.40 0.53 12.4 0.92
Impulsivity 02 9 0.40 0.47 0.0 0.91 0.45 0.50 3.0 0.91 0.41 0.49 12.4 0.92
School engagement 0.17** 0.27*** 0.37***
PHBQ 14 8 3.73 0.37 13.6 0.83 3.66 0.44 16.9 0.87 3.59 0.48 35.5 0.87
THBQ 02 8 1.80 0.28 0.0 0.81 1.79 0.30 3.0 0.84 1.75 0.33 12.4 0.86
Academic competence 0.66***
PHBQ 17 8 5.44 1.07 35.8 0.91
THBQ 15 5 3.49 0.97 12.7 0.96

Note. a = Cronbachs alpha; r = correlation between scales used to create parent and teacher composite ratings; P-HBQ = parent report on the MacArthur Health and Behavior
Questionnaire; T-HBQ = teacher report on the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire.
**p < .01. ***p < .001.
Integrative View of School Functioning 9

an eight-item scale that was rated using the follow- ten to rst grade (see Table 1 for percent of missing
ing categories (1 = not at all, 2 = a little, 3 = some- cases by measure and informant). Missing values in
what, 4 = quite a bit). Parent items captured these data were considered to be ignorable (i.e.,
attitudes and emotions toward school and included missing at random). As such, path analyses were
items such as, Is excited about school, Is frus- conducted using maximum likelihood estimation,
trated about school, and Is interested in school. which permitted statistical inference to use all avail-
Teachers reported on a slightly different eight- able data for the 338 participants (Schafer, 1997;
item scale that was rated using the following Schafer & Graham, 2002).
categories (0 = doesnt apply, 1 = sometimes applies,
2 = certainly applies). In addition to school liking, tea-
Analytic Plan
cher items included behaviors toward school speci-
cally observed in the classroom. Examples of items Analyses were conducted using nested path
included, Is cheerful at school and Seems bored analysis models in Mplus 5.2 (Muthen & Muthen,
at school. 2007). In the conceptual model, gender was proposed
Academic competence was assessed by parents and as an exogeneous, observed variable predicting child
teachers using the Academic Competence subscale functioning and teacherchild relationship quality in
from the HBQ used to assess school functioning. This the fall of kindergarten and was included as a con-
scale taps into childrens abilities in math and read- trol. Child gender was reported by parents as part of
ing. Research suggests that parents utilize relative a demographic questionnaire completed in the fall of
comparisons to evaluate childrens ability levels the kindergarten year. To account for nonnormality
(Miller, 1995); therefore, parents reported on an of some variables, we used a robust maximum likeli-
eight-item scale using a Likert-style rating scale that hood estimator to account for this (MLR option
compared their childrens abilities with other chil- in Mplus). All models accounted for clustering of
dren (1 = not at all/much worse than other children, children within classrooms by specifying the
7 = very good/much better than other children). Example TYPE = COMPLEX analysis command in Mplus.
of items included, In comparison to other children, To evaluate absolute model t between the data
how difcult is it for your child to read? and In and the models, we used the following t indices:
comparison to other children, how would you rate comparative t index (CFI; values .95 indicate
your child in math? Teachers reported on a slightly good model t), TuckerLewis index (TLI; values
different ve-item scale that read more like a report .95), and root mean square error of approxima-
card asking teachers to rate children in different sub- tion (RMSEA; values 0.06). Failure to meet one of
ject areas, using a Likert-style rating scale (1 = poor/ these t indices indicates poor model t, although
well below grade level, 5 = excellent, well above grade there is considerable debate in the eld as to how
level). Teacher-rated academic competence was to evaluate model t (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Marsh,
assessed with items such as, How would you Hau, & Wen, 2004) with the above guidelines
evaluate this childs current school performance in perhaps being too stringent to strictly evaluate
math-related skills/reading-related skills/spelling/ goodness of t.
overall? Given that academic competence is an Relative model t was evaluated using chi-
emerging domain in early schooling with childrens square difference tests between sequential models.
skills becoming more differentiated once the class- A scaling constant, c coefcient, was applied to
room activities become more academic, measurement determine whether a model with more estimated
of academic competence was included only at the parameters (i.e., less parsimonious) resulted in a
end of rst grade to both detect variability on this better relative t to the data than the more parsi-
global measure of childrens ability and maintain a monious one. If the chi-square difference test was
more parsimonious model. signicant, indicating that additional pathways
improved the model t, the more complex model
was selected (Satorra, 1999).
Data Analytic Plan
Data Preparation
Results
The rate of missing cases varied across variables
Bivariate Relations
(range = 3%35.8%). Due to the longitudinal nature
of the study, response rates decreased over time, Bivariate relations revealed associations between
particularly as children transitioned from kindergar- the gender predictor at kindergarten entry and
10 Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce, and Obradovic

domains across time (see Table 2). At kindergarten

1.00

Note. N = 338. Numbers denote time point of data collection (1 = kindergarten fall, 2 = kindergarten spring, 3 = rst-grade spring). Gender is coded: 1 = girls, 0 = boys.
14
entry, girls experienced closer relationships with
teachers and more school engagement across all

.37***
three time points; boys experienced more conict
13

1.00
with teachers and more impulsive behavior and
inattention. Gender was not correlated with rst-
grade academic competence.

.45***
.30***

CLO = teacherchild closeness; CON = teacherchild conict; INAT/IMP = inattention/impulsivity; SCE = school engagement; ACC = academic competence.
12

1.00
Teacherchild closeness was negatively corre-
lated with conict at all three time points, albeit
moderately, signifying that this measure was tap-

.62***
.53***
ping into different aspects of the relationship. Those
11

1.00

.12
children who experienced more closeness with
teachers in kindergarten also demonstrated more
school engagement across all waves, fewer issues
.27***

.41***
10

with impulsivity and inattention across the kinder-


1.00

.11

.13

garten year, and more academic competence in rst


grade. Interestingly, more closeness with rst-grade
.18***
.41***
.32***
.50***
.27***

teachers was positively correlated with school


1.00
9

engagement in rst grade but not correlated with


inattention and impulsivity or academic compe-
tence in rst grade. More conict in rst grade was
.46***

.54***
.74***
.23***
.16*

related to greater levels of inattention and impulsiv-


1.00

.11
8

ity, and less school engagement. More conict with


teachers was associated with less school engage-
.61***
.57***

.61***
.49***
.27***

ment and more inattention and impulsivity across


1.00

.03

.11
7

all waves of data collection, but was not associated


with academic competence in rst grade.
.32***
.20***
.44***
.25***
.19***

.17**
.14*

.15*
1.00
6

Nested Path Analyses


Fit statistics and model comparisons for hierar-
chically nested path analysis models are presented
.22***
.31***
.33***
.57***
.17***
.27***
.22***
.31***
.26***
1.00

in Table 3. The continuity and within-time covaria-


5

tion model (Model 1), which included gender as a


Zero-Order Correlations Between Variables Included in the Path Analyses

predictor at the start of kindergarten, evidenced rel-


.38***

.52***
.84***
.46***

.48***
.69***
.24***

atively good t to the data (CFI = .933, TLI = .893,


.12*

.17*
1.00

.10
4

RMSEA = 0.069). The addition of cross-domain


pathways from kindergarten fall to kindergarten
spring (Model 2) further improved the overall
.57***
.41***

.72***
.52***
.43***

.59***
.48***
.28***
.14**

.17**

model t (CFI = .953, TLI = .905, RMSEA = 0.064).


1.00

.09
3

The difference test of relative t indicated that this


was a signicant difference, v2(45) = 108.05,
p < .001. Model 3 added cross-domain pathways to
.23***

.33***
.63***
.18***

.28***
.29***
.16**
.14*

.14*

.15*
.13*
1.00

.12
2

predict outcomes in rst grade and indicated even


better overall model t (CFI = .974, TLI = .919,
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

RMSEA = 0.060) indicating excellent model t on


.17***
.17***
.37***
.19***
.19***
.22***
.38***
.20***
.28***
.24***
.28***
.24***

two of the three t indices and good model t on


1.00

.01
1

the TLI. As this was signicantly better than Model


2, v2(29) = 63.84, p < .001, we deemed Model 3 to
be the best t model to this data.
12 INAT/IMP3
4 INAT/IMP1

8 INAT/IMP2

Standardized path coefcients for the best tting


11 CON3
1 Gender

14 ACC3

model (Model 3) are presented in Figure 2. Stan-


10 CLO3

13 SCE3
3 CON1

7 CON2
2 CLO1

6 CLO2
Table 2

5 SCE1

9 SCE2

dardized, as well as unstandardized, path coef-


cients are shown below, with the p values
Integrative View of School Functioning 11

RMSEA

points, within-time correlations at each time point, with gender predicting T1. Model 2: Adds diagonal pathways from T1 to T2, with gender predicting T1. Model 3: Adds diagonal
presented from the standardized model results.

Note. c = weighting constant for computing the chi-square statistic using robust estimation method; cd = weighting constant for the difference between two chi-square values using
robust estimation method; CFI = comparative x index; TLI = TuckerLewis index; RMSEA = root-mean square error of approximation. Model 1: Continuity across three time
0.069
0.064
0.060
Results show that child gender predicted both child

Absolute t statistics
behaviors and teacherchild relationship quality.
Boys experienced more conict with teachers
(B = .29, b = .17, p < .001) and more inattention

0.893
0.905
0.919
and impulsivity (B = .24, b = .37, p < .001). Girls

TLI
experienced closer relationships with teachers
(B = .29, b = .17, p < .001) and more school engage-
ment (B = .13, b = .17, p < .01). The stability coef-
0.933
0.953
0.974
CFI

cients were consistently positive and signicant


across all domains.
There was signicant within-time covariation
between all domains (standardized coefcients are
.000
.000
p

presented in Table 4). At all three time points, sig-


nicant covariation emerged across domains in the
same pattern with one exception: Closeness was
(Diff)

12
16

positively related to school engagement at all three


df

time points (kindergarten fall: B = .10, b = .32,


p < .001; kindergarten spring: B = .10, b = .42,
Difference test of relative t

p < .001; rst grade: B = .14, b = .38, p < .001), but


Chi-square

negatively related to inattention and impulsivity


(Diff)

38.43
43.28

only at the end of rst grade (B = .02, b = .11,


p < .05). Conict was negatively related to school
engagement (kindergarten fall: B = .13, b = .40,
p < .001; kindergarten spring: B = .10, b = .42,
p < .001; rst grade: B = .15, b = .47, p < .001)
1.328
1.296
cd

and positively related to inattention and impulsiv-


ity (kindergarten fall: B = .15, b = .60, p < .001; kin-
dergarten spring: B = .05, b = .40, p < .001; rst
comparison

grade: B = .06, b = .42, p < .001). School engage-


1 vs. 2
2 vs. 3
Model

ment was negatively related to inattention and


impulsivity, but not at the end of kindergarten (kin-
dergarten fall: B = .04, b = .36, p < .001; rst
Fit Statistics and Model Comparisons for Hierarchically Nested Path Analyses

grade: B = .05, b = .45, p < .001). As expected,


teacherchild closeness and conict were negatively
Chi-square

147.535
108.054

related (kindergarten fall: B = .14, b = .21,


63.844

p < .001; kindergarten spring: B = .16, b = .40,


p < .01; rst grade: B = .15, b = .29, p < .001).
Academic competence in rst grade was positively
pathways from T2 to T3, with gender predicting T1.

related to school engagement (B = .09, b = .27,


1.197
1.162
1.088

p < .001) and negatively related to inattention and


c

impulsivity (B = .05, b = .28, p < .001). Notably,


Path analyses

there was no signicant within-time covariation


between rst-grade teacherchild relationship qual-
57
45
29
df

ity and academic competence.


Examining transactional patterns across the kinder-
garten year, key signicant ndings emerged after
No. cross-
domain

controlling for continuity and within-time correla-


paths

16
32
4

tion. Those children who exhibited more inattention


and impulsivity in the fall had lower levels of school
engagement by the end of the school year (B = .27,
b = .19, p < .01), as well as greater teacherchild
Table 3

Model

conict (B = .56, b = .20, p < .01). Furthermore,


greater conict between teacher and child in the fall
1
2
3
12 Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce, and Obradovic

Figure 2. Standardized path coefcients for signicant paths of nal model (Model 3). R2 values are denoted in parentheses. Numbers
denote time point of data collection. Within-time associations are represented by  (please refer to Table 4 for coefcients).
CLO = closeness; CON = conict; SCE = school engagement; INAT/IMP = inattention/impulsivity; ACC = academic competence.
v2(29) = 63.84; CFI = 0.97; TLI = 0.92; RMSEA = 0.06.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001

was associated with a similar reduction in school Table 4


engagement by spring (B = .08, b = .14, p < .05). Within-Time Associations for Model 3
The pattern between inattention and impulsivity,
Time Estimated path b SE p
and teacher conict, persisted from kindergarten to
rst grade: Children who had higher levels of inat- K-fall Clo1Scel 0.316 (0.074) .000
tention and impulsivity at the end of kindergarten Clo1Inat/Imp1 0.060 (0.067) .373
experienced more conict with their rst-grade Clo1Con1 0.205 (0.047) .000
teachers (B = .70, b = .28, p < .001). These children Sce1Inat/Imp1 0.358 (0.067) .000
also experienced reduced closeness with their rst- Sce1Con1 0.401 (0.055) .000
grade teachers (B = .30, b = .13, p < .05). School Inat/Imp1Con1 0.602 (0.052) .000
engagement at the end of kindergarten positively K-spring Clo2Sce2 0.415 (0.086) .000
Clo2Inat/Imp2 0.239 (0.128) .061
predicted teacherchild closeness (B = .25, b = .14,
Clo2Con2 0.396 (0.137) .004
p < .05) and academic competence in rst grade
Sce2Inat/Imp2 0.217 (0.136) .109
(B = .48, b = .29, p < .01). Sce2Con2 0.420 (0.093) .000
Given that the pathways from inattention/impul- Inat/Imp2Con2 0.399 (0.092) .000
sivity and conict in fall were negatively related to First-grade spring Clo3Sce3 0.378 (0.064) .000
school engagement in spring of kindergarten, which Clo3Inat/Imp3 0.105 (0.052) .044
in turn was positively related to academic compe- Clo3Acc3 0.046 (0.060) .445
tence in the rst grade, we conducted post hoc Clo3Con3 0.294 (0.059) .000
analyses to examine the strength of the indirect Sce3Inat/Imp3 0.453 (0.079) .000
effect of these variables across time using MODEL Sce3Acc3 0.273 (0.062) .000
INDIRECT in Mplus. These supplemental analyses Sce3Con3 0.466 (0.057) .000
Inat/Imp3Acc3 0.278 (0.064) .000
found a signicant indirect effect (B = .13,
Inat/Imp3Con3 0.424 (0.061) .000
b = .06, p < .01) from inattention/impulsivity to
Con2Acc3 0.023 (0.057) .689
academic competence through school engagement.
The indirect effect from conict to academic compe- Note. b = standardized path coefcient. Standard errors are in
tence was only marginally signicant (B = .04, parentheses. Numbers denote time point of data collection
b = .04, p < .10). (1 = kindergarten fall, 2 = kindergarten spring, 3 = rst-grade
spring). Clo = closeness; Sce = school engagement; Inat/
Surprisingly, teacherchild conict at the end of Imp = inattention/impulsivity; Acc = academic competence;
kindergarten was positively related to teacherchild Con = conict.
Integrative View of School Functioning 13

closeness in rst grade (B = .18, b = .20, p < .001), self-regulation skills have a unidirectional longitudi-
although these variables were not associated in the nal effect on teacherchild conict.
bivariate correlations. To probe this counterintuitive Although we did not nd bidirectional transac-
nding, we ran partial pairwise correlations tions between self-regulation and teacherchild con-
between these variables, controlling for conict in ict, this study provides evidence that bidirectional
rst grade. We examined how these correlations transactions may work along different, yet inter-
differ in children who experience high versus low related, domains of childrens functioning. Speci-
conict at the end of kindergarten, splitting the cally, initial levels of conict with teachers predicted
sample at the mean. The results showed that a posi- decreases in school engagement at the end of
tive correlation emerged for children who experi- kindergarten after accounting for a strong negative
enced high conict at kindergarten spring (r = .25, correlation within each time point. In addition, the
p < .05), whereas the association was nonsignicant theorized benets of teacherchild closeness did not
for children who experienced low conict. spill over to other domains of childrens functioning
beyond the concurrent benets at each time point.
Prior evidence showing that closeness predicts pro-
spective child functioning may be due to not con-
Discussion
trolling for the covariation between teacherchild
Using nested path analytic models, this study closeness and conict, which this study did account
examined transactional relations among teacher for. This study is one of the rst to empirically
child relationship quality, childrens behavior, and examine transactional effects across multiple aspects
academic functioning. Importantly, this study of both teacherchild relationships and childrens
employed independent measures across multiple functioning; thus, future research should continue
informants and time points, and examined bidirec- to tease apart these relations.
tional pathways after accounting for longitudinal The nal model also resulted in a counterintui-
stability of each construct and within-time covaria- tive positive association between teacherchild con-
tion between constructs. ict at the end of kindergarten and closeness
After controlling for strong continuity across reported at rst-grade spring. It appears that this
domains of childrens functioning and teacherchild result is being driven by children who experience
relationship quality, children who exhibited inatten- high conict at kindergarten spring. This indicates
tion and impulsivity at school entry experienced an that some children who experience conict with
increase in conict with teachers at the end of kin- teachers in kindergarten may have an opportunity
dergarten, and notably, this effect persisted into to establish new relationships with their teachers in
rst grade. This relation was signicant after rst grade, over and above their concurrent conict
accounting for the fact that inattention and impul- at each time point, highlighting a possible point of
sivity were strongly positively correlated with con- intervention. This is a noteworthy nding consider-
ict at each time point. The strong effect of ing that both closeness and conict exhibited strong
inattention and impulsive behaviors on teacher stability across the three time points, particularly
child relationship quality began at the transition to teacherchild conict, consistent with work by
elementary school, pointing to a need for strategies Jerome et al. (2009).
to help young children develop strong self-regula- In addition, inattention and impulsive behaviors
tory skills in preschool and earlier. Furthermore, were longitudinally associated with less school
inattention and impulsivity at the end of kindergar- engagement at the end of kindergarten, over and
ten predicted less closeness with rst-grade teach- above their strong, negative concurrent association.
ers, highlighting that these children may not receive Thus, children who had more conict with teachers
the benets of positive relationship quality due to or exhibited lower self-regulation experienced lower
their behavioral issues. enthusiasm for school-related activities, which sub-
In contrast, conict with teachers did not predict sequently undermined academic competence and
increases in inattention and impulsive behaviors closeness with teachers in rst grade. This evidence
across time. This ran counter to our original is consistent with ideas put forth by Eisenberg et al.
hypothesis that we would observe bidirectional (2010) that school engagement mediates the relation
transactions between teacherchild relationship between self-regulation and relationship quality
quality and childrens behaviors, as has been found with academic competence. Indeed, post hoc analy-
in other studies (Arnold et al., 1998; Doumen et al., ses found a signicant indirect effect of self-regula-
2008; Leot et al., 2011). Instead, it appears that tion on academic competence through school
14 Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce, and Obradovic

engagement, highlighting that future empirical This study could also be strengthened by includ-
studies should continue to test for mediation ing other measures of the classroom context. Analy-
among these constructs. ses were limited by only using teacher-rated
Taken together, these ndings suggest that chal- perspectives on the degree of closeness and conict
lenges with self-regulation place young children at with any particular child. Classroom observations
risk for negative relationships with teachers and would provide an unbiased perspective of overall
lower academic functioning, particularly for boys. classroom climate yet may not capture the degree
However, we do see some evidence of children get- of closeness and conict between individual
ting a fresh start to develop new relationships with teacherchild dyads. Incorporating childrens report
their later teachers, signifying the need to further on the relationship would provide another perspec-
examine characteristics of teachers who are able to tive, but questions remain whether 5-year-olds can
competently manage and connect with children validly report on the relationship particularly if
who display challenging behaviors. adult researchers interview them.
This study did not include any measure of
teachers own social-emotional competence or psy-
Limitations and Future Directions
chological attributes, qualities that may permit
Although this study provided a rigorous exami- teachers to be better at reading childrens cues and
nation of childrens behavior and classroom supporting their social-emotional and cognitive
processes to understand the link between self-regu- needs (Downer et al., 2010). Future research must
lation and academic competence, it is not without incorporate both child and teacher characteristics to
its limitations. First, this study could be strength- further illuminate how these interpersonal bonds
ened by including measures of inattention and develop in early schooling. Finally, while this study
impulsivity through direct child assessment or implemented multiple measures and multiple infor-
observational methods to provide unbiased assess- mants across time and examined transactional path-
ments of childrens self-regulatory behaviors. Fur- ways in a stringent manner, ascertaining causality
thermore, direct observation of childrens school is not possible due to omitted variable bias, such as
engagement or surveying children directly about variables related to teachers own beliefs and
their motivation and school liking would also trian- competencies.
gulate the construct of school engagement with par-
ent and teacher reports. However, it is notable that
Conclusion
the current survey measure is composed of both
parents and teachers perspectives, strengthening This study provides evidence of the key role that
the claims that can be made, particularly as new self-regulation plays in childrens early schooling.
teachers were introduced in rst grade. While some Findings highlight the need to provide teachers of
constructs have low correlation between parent and young children with professional development to
teacher informants in fall of kindergarten, poten- improve overall classroom climate and engage with
tially limiting the interpretation of the ndings, it is dysregulated children in nonreactive ways as conic-
important to note that orthogonal perspectives on tual relationships can persist over time and impact
behavior in the home and school contexts can cor- these childrens academic trajectories. While it is eas-
rect for deciencies in each informants data ier to nurture closeness with children who do not
(Kraemer et al., 2003). exhibit problematic behaviors, supportive classroom
We focused our research questions on measures environments may afford opportunities for all chil-
of inattention and impulsivity because they are dren, providing the maximum benet for children
markers of low self-regulation and children who experiencing higher degrees of behavior problems at
exhibit these behaviors tend to experience difculty school entry (Silver et al., 2005). These children are
with learning and engagement, issues that may precisely the ones who desperately need to have
place them at risk for reduced academic achieve- positive, supportive relationships with teachers to
ment as they progress through school (Blair, 2002). encourage their engagement with educational con-
However, these behaviors are multiply determined tent. Interventions that promote emotionally sup-
and are also related to externalizing problems. Due portive classrooms and learner-centered practices
to model complexity, we were not able to control may allow these children to feel safe, reduce the risk
for concurrent aggressivedisruptive behaviors and for mental health problems (Boyce et al., 2012), and
isolate the unique effect of inattention and impul- allow for academic risk taking necessary to foster
sivity; future studies should aim to do so. stronger academic skills (Downer et al., 2010). Fur-
Integrative View of School Functioning 15

thermore, supportive classrooms that increase the Blair, C., & Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in
quality of experience for all children may have long- prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-reg-
term effects on individual earnings and educational ulation as a means of preventing school failure. Devel-
outcomes (Chetty et al., 2011), highlighting the opment and Psychopathology, 20, 899911. doi:10.1017/
S0954579408000436
importance of early learning environments for socie-
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control,
tal savings. Finally, identifying strategies for promot-
executive function, and false belief understanding to
ing self-regulatory skills before children enter emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten.
kindergarten is crucial to ensure that children start Child Development, 78, 647663. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.
school on a positive academic trajectory. 2007.01019.x
Boyce, W. T., Obradovic, J., Bush, N. R., Stamperdahl, J.,
Kim, Y. S., & Adler, N. (2012). Social stratication,
References classroom climate, and the behavioral adaptation of
kindergarten children. Proceedings of the National Acad-
Archambault, I., Pagani, L., & Fitzpatrick, C. (2013). emy of Sciences USA, 109, 1716817173. doi:10.1073/
Transactional associations between classroom engage- pnas.1201730109
ment and relations with teachers from rst through Buyse, E., Verschueren, K., & Doumen, S. (2011). Pre-
fourth grade. Learning and Instruction, 23, 19. doi:10. schoolers attachment to mother and risk for adjust-
1016/j.learninstruc.2012.09.003 ment problems in kindergarten: Can teachers make a
Armstrong, J. M., Goldstein, L. H., & The MacArthur difference? Social Development, 20, 3350. doi:10.1111/j.
Working Group on Outcome Assessment (2003). Man- 1467-9507.2009.00555.x
ual for the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire Chetty, R., Friedman, J. N., Hilger, N., Saez, E., Schanzen-
(HBQ 1.0). University of Pittsburgh: MacArthur Foun- bach, D. W., & Yagan, D. (2011). How does your kin-
dation Research Network on Psychopathology and dergarten classroom affect your earnings? Evidence
Development, Pitsburgh, PA. from project STAR. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 126,
Arnold, D. H., McWilliams, L., & Arnold, E. H. (1998). 15931660. doi:10.1093/qje/qjr041
Teacher discipline and child misbehavior in day care: Curby, T. W., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Ponitz, C. C.
Untangling causality with correlational data. Develop- (2009). Teacher-child interactions and childrens
mental Psychology, 34, 276287. doi:10.1037/0012-1649. achievement trajectories across kindergarten and rst
34.2.276 grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 912925.
Baker, J. A. (2006). Contributions of teacherchild rela- doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00301
tionships to positive school adjustment during elemen- Doumen, S., Verschueren, K., Buyse, E., Germeijs, V., Lu-
tary school. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 211229. yckx, K., & Soenens, B. (2008). Reciprocal relations
doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2006.02.002 between teacher-child conict and aggressive behavior
Baker, J. A., Grant, S., & Morlock, L. (2008). The teacher- in kindergarten: A three-wave longitudinal study. Jour-
student relationship as a developmental context for nal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37, 588599.
children with internalizing or externalizing behavior doi:10.1080/15374410802148079
problems. School Psychology Quarterly, 23, 315. doi:10. Downer, J. T., Sabol, T. J., & Hamre, B. (2010). Teacher-
1037/1045-3830.23.1.3 child interactions in the classroom: Toward a theory of
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained within- and cross-domain links to childrens develop-
attention, and executive functions: Constructing a uni- mental outcomes. Early Education & Development, 21,
fying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65 699723. doi:10.1080/10409289.2010.497453
94. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65 Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson,
Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention-decit/hyperactivity dis- K., Huston, A. C., & Klebanov, P., . . . Brooks-Gunn, J.
order. In E. J. Mash & L. G. Terdal (Eds.), Assessment of (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Develop-
childhood disorders (3rd ed., pp. 71129). New York, NY: mental Psychology, 43, 14281446. doi:10.1037/
Guilford Press. 0012-1649.43.6.1428
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher-child rela- Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Self-
tionship and childrens early school adjustment. Journal regulation and school readiness. Early Education & Devel-
of School Psychology, 35, 6179. doi:10.1016/S0022-4405 opment, 21, 681698. doi:10.1080/10409289.2010.497451
(96)00029-5 Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Childrens interper- School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
sonal behaviors and the teacherchild relationship. the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, 59109.
Developmental Psychology, 34, 934946. doi:10.1037/ doi:10.3102/00346543074001059
0012-1649.34.5.934 Graziano, P. A., Reavis, R. D., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S.
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition D. (2007). The role of emotion regulation in childrens
and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of early academic success. Journal of School Psychology, 45,
childrens functioning at school entry. American Psychol- 319. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2006.09.002
ogist, 57, 111127. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111
16 Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce, and Obradovic

Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher-child Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers
relationships and the trajectory of childrens school out- literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psy-
comes through eighth grade. Child Development, 72, chology, 43, 947959. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947
625638. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00301 Mendez, J. L., McDermott, P., & Fantuzzo, J. (2002). Iden-
Heyman, G. D., & Dweck, C. S. (1992). Achievement tifying and promoting social competence with African
goals and intrinsic motivation: Their relation and their American preschool children: Developmental and con-
role in adaptive motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 16, textual considerations. Psychology in the Schools, 39,
231247. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00301 111123. doi:10.1002/pits.10039
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for t Miller, S. A. (1995). Parents attributions for their chil-
indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional drens behavior. Child Development, 66, 15571584.
criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00952.x
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 155. doi:10. Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2007). Mplus users guide
1080/10705519909540118 (5th ed.) Los Angeles, CA: Authors.
Hughes, J. N., Luo, W., Kwok, O.-M., & Loyd, L. K. Myers, S. S., & Morris, A. S. (2009). Examining associa-
(2008). Teacher-student support, effortful engagement, tions between effortful control and teacher-child rela-
and achievement: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal tionships in relation to Head Start childrens
of Educational Psychology, 100, 114. doi:10.1037/ socioemotional adjustment. Early Education & Develop-
0022-0663.100.1.1 ment, 20, 756774. doi:10.1080/10409280802571244
Jerome, E. M., Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2009). Tea- Myers, S. S., & Pianta, R. C. (2008). Developmental com-
cher-child relationships from kindergarten to sixth mentary: Individual and contextual inuences on stu-
grade: Early childhood predictors of teacher-perceived dent-teacher relationships and childrens early problem
conict and closeness. Social Development, 18, 915945. behaviors. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychol-
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00508.x ogy, 37, 600608. doi:10.1080/15374410802148160
Kraemer, H. C., Measelle, J. R., Ablow, J. C., Essex, M. J., OConnor, E., & McCartney, K. (2007). Examining teacher-
Boyce, W. T., & Kupfer, D. J. (2003). A new approach child relationships and achievement as part of an
to integrating data from multiple informants in psychi- ecological model of development. American Educational
atric assessment and research: Mixing and matching Research Journal, 44, 340369. doi:10.3102/000283120
contexts and perspectives. American Journal of Psychia- 7302172
try, 160, 15661577. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.9.1566 Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C. R., Lopez, N. L.,
Ladd, G. W., Buhs, E. S., & Seid, M. (2000). Childrens & Wellman, H. M. (2005). Developmental foundations
initial sentiments about kindergarten: Is school liking of externalizing problems in young children: The role
an antecedent of early classroom participation and of effortful control. Development and Psychopathology, 17,
achievement? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 255279. 2545. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050029
http://www.jstor.org/stable/23093716 Perry, K. E., Donohue, K. M., & Weinstein, R. S. (2007).
Ladd, G. W., & Dinella, L. M. (2009). Continuity and Teaching practices and the promotion of achievement
change in early school engagement: Predictive of chil- and adjustment in rst grade. Journal of School Psychol-
drens achievement trajectories from rst to eighth ogy, 45, 269292. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2007.02.005
grade? Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 190206. Pianta, R. (1996). Manual and scoring guide for the Student
doi:10.1037/a0013153 Teacher Relationship Scale. Charlottesville: University of
Leot, G., van Lier, P. A. C., Verschueren, K., Onghena, Virginia.
P., & Colpin, H. (2011). Transactional associations Pianta, R. C. (1999). Assessing child-teacher relationships
among teacher support, peer social preference, and Enhancing relationships between children and teachers.
child externalizing behavior: A four-wave longitudinal Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
study. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, Pianta, R. C., Belsky, J., Vandergrift, N., Houts, R., &
40, 8799. doi:10.1080/15374416.2011.533409 Morrison, F. J. (2008). Classroom effects on childrens
Liew, J., Chen, Q., & Hughes, J. N. (2010). Child effortful achievement trajectories in elementary school. American
control, teacherstudent relationships, and achievement Educational Research Journal, 45, 365397. doi:10.3102/
in academically at-risk children: Additive and interac- 0002831207308230
tive effects. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 51 Pianta, R. C., Steinberg, M. S., & Rollins, K. B. (1995). The
64. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.07.005 rst two years of school: Teacher-child relationships
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of and deections in childrens classroom adjustment.
golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing Development and Psychopathology, 7, 295312. doi:10.
approaches to setting cutoff values for t indexes and 1017/S0954579400006519
dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentlers (1999) Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Pianta, R. C. (2000). An ecologi-
ndings. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary cal perspective on the transition to kindergarten: A the-
Journal, 11, 320341. doi:10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2 oretical framework to guide empirical research. Journal
McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 491511. doi:
Farris, C. L., Jewkes, A. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2007). 10.1016/S0193-3973(00)00051-4
Integrative View of School Functioning 17

Roorda, D. L., Koomen, H. M. Y., Spilt, J. L., & Oort, F. J. characteristics, and the teacherchild relationship dur-
(2011). The inuence of affective teacher-student rela- ing the school transition. Journal of School Psychology,
tionships on students school engagement and achieve- 43, 3960. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2004.11.003
ment: A meta-analytic approach. Review of Educational Spilt, J. L., Hughes, J. N., Wu, J.-Y., & Kwok, O.-M. (2012).
Research, 81, 493529. doi:10.3102/0034654311421793 Dynamics of teacher-student relationships: Stability and
Rudasill, K. M., & Rimm-Kaufman, S. E. (2009). Teacher change across elementary school and the inuence on
child relationship quality: The roles of child tempera- childrens academic success. Child Development, 83, 1180
ment and teacherchild interactions. Early Childhood 1195. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01761.x
Research Quarterly, 24, 107120. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq. Stipek, D., & Miles, S. (2008). Effects of aggression on
2008.12.003 achievement: Does conict with the teacher make it
Sameroff, A. J., & MacKenzie, M. J. (2003). Research strate- worse? Child Development, 79, 17211735. doi:10.1111/j.
gies for capturing transactional models of development: 1467-8624.2008.01221.x
The limits of the possible. Development and Psychopathol- Valiente, C., Lemery-Chalfant, K., & Swanson, J. (2010).
ogy, 15, 613640. doi:10.1017/S0954579403000312 Prediction of kindergartners academic achievement
Satorra, A. (1999). Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in from their effortful control and emotionality: Evidence
multi sample analysis of moment structures (UPF Econom- for direct and moderated relations. Journal of Educational
ics & Business Working Paper No. 395). Psychology, 102, 550560. doi:10.1037/a0018992
Schafer, J. L. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data Valiente, C., Swanson, J., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2012).
(Vol. 72). Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC Press. Kindergartners temperament, classroom engagement,
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our and studentteacher relationship: moderation by effort-
view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, ful control. Social Development, 21, 558576. doi:
147177. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00640.x
Silva, K. M., Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Sulik, M. J., Walker, S., Berthelsen, D. C., & Irving, K. A. (2001). Tem-
Valiente, C., Huerta, S. . . . School Readiness Consor- perament and peer acceptance in early childhood: Sex
tium. (2011). Relations of childrens effortful control and social status differences. Child Study Journal, 31,
and teacher-child relationship quality to school atti- 177192. http://eprints.qut.edu.au/1239/
tudes in a low-income sample. Early Education & Devel- Wentzel, K. R. (1999). Social-motivational processes and
opment, 22, 434460. doi:10.1080/10409289.2011.578046 interpersonal relationships: Implications for under-
Silver, R. B., Measelle, J. R., Armstrong, J. M., & Essex, standing motivation at school. Journal of Educational
M. J. (2005). Trajectories of classroom externalizing Psychology, 91, 7697. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.76
behavior: Contributions of child characteristics, family

Anda mungkin juga menyukai