Anda di halaman 1dari 14

SPECIAL ARTICLE

This is the eighth in a series of lO-year updates in child and adolescentpsychiatry. Topics are selected in consultation
with the MCA? Committee on Recertification. both for the importance ofnew research and its clinical or developmental
significance. The authors have been asked to place an asterisk before the five or six most seminal references.
j. McD.

Infant Development and Developmental Risk:


A Review of the Past 10 Years
CHARLES H. ZEANAH. M.D .. NEIL W . BORIS. M.D . AND JULIE A. LARRIEU. PH.D.

ABSTRACT
ObJective: To review critically the research on infant developmental risk published in the past 10 years. Method: A
brief framework on development in the first 3 years is provided. This is followed by a review of pertinent studies of
developmental risk, chosen to illustrate major risk conditions and the protective factors known to affect infant develop-
ment. Illustrative risk conditions include prematurity and serious medical illness and infant temperament, infant--earegiver
attachment, parental psychopathology, marital quality and interactions, poverty and social class, adolescent parenthood,
and family violence . Results: Risk and protective factors interact complexly . There are few examples of specific or
linear links between risk conditions and outcomes during or beyond the first 3 years of life. Infant development is best
appreciated within the context of caregiving relationships. which mediate the effects of both intrinsic and extrinsic risk
conditions . Conclusions: Complex and evolving interrelationships among risk factors are beginning to be elucidated.
Linear models of cause and effect are of little use in understanding the development of psychopathology. Refining our
markers of risk and demonstrating effective preventive interventions are the next important challenges. J. Am. Acad.
Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 1997. 36(2): 165-178. Key Words: infancy, risk and protective factors, developmental psycho-
pathology, infant-parent relationships.

Research on development and on developmental risk and the process of development recently highlighted in
increasingly inform and illuminate one another. The research on infant development.
purpose of this review is to highlight the major findings
from the research on development and developmental Content: Biobehavioral Shifts and Domains
risk in the first 3 years of life that are relevant to of Development
child psychiatry.
There are three major periods of qualitative reorgani-
DEVELOPMENT zation or discontinuity in the first 3 years: 2 to 3
months. 7 to 9 months, and 18 to 20 months (Ernde,
The first 3 years are unique in the life span for the 1984; Stern, 1985; Zeanah et aI., 1989). Table 1
rapidity and complexity of developmental changes that presents the major new developments that characterize
occur. It is important to be aware of the content these biobehavioral shifts and details the qualitative
changes in biological, cognitive. emotional, communi-
cative, and social development that arise. Development
Accepted f~bn",ry 6. / 996.
From thr Di uision of /lIpl1lt, Child and Adolescrnt Psy chiatry, Louisiana between these points consists primarily of quantitative
Statr Uniuersity S r hool ofMrdirine, Nr w Orleans. changes. while development across these points results
Reprint rrqursts to Dr. Zeanah, Department of Psychiatry. LS U Sr hool of in qualitative changes.
Medicine, /542 Tulane Arenur, Nrw Orleans, LA 701/2-2822.
0890-8567/97/3602-0165$03.00/01997 by the American Acad emy First 2 Months. Early theories of development held
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry . that the human newborn was disorganized, passive.

J. AM . ACAD . CHIl.D AD O l. ESC. P SYCHIATRY . '\(, :2 . fE li R lJA RY 1'1'17 165


ZEANAH ET AL.

TABLE 1
Biobehavioral Shifts and Domains of Infant Development
First 2 Months 2 to 7 Months 7 to 18 Months 18 to 36 Months

Cognitive Cross-modal fluency Enhanced habituation. Differentiation of means Symbolic representation as


allows translation of classical conditioning, and ends; object per- reflected in true sym-
perceptual experiences and operant manence; intersubjec- bolic play; recognition
across different modal- conditioning riviry makes it possible of gender differences;
ities; remarkable ability for infants to share ability to entertain
to detect invariant as- thoughts feelings and imaginings that are dif-
pects of various percep- desires with others and ferent from reality for
tual experiences ; to be aware of subjec- first time
habituation, operant tive experiences; visual
and classical condition- memory predicts later
ing present prenatally intelligence; enhanced
participation

Language Crying major means of Cooing becomes respon- Intentional communica- Blossoming of expressive
communication; oc- sive; bilabial "rasberry" tion appears and ges- language leads to 2-
casional cooing sounds sounds; consonant vo- tural communication and then 3-word com-
begin after several calizations appear and dominates; under- binations; expressive vo-
weeks progress to pollysyl- standing of a word as cabulaty grows from
labic babbling (e.g.. an agreed-upon symbol an average of 50 words
" gagagaga" or to designate an object; at 18 months to 500
"lalalala1a") may imitate or sponta- at 36 months; re-
neously produce ceptive language begins
speech sounds or to deconrextualize so
words without comper- that words themselves
hension. then gradually become more meaning-
begin to express word ful without other cues
sound correctly across
contexts
Emotional Distress , contentment, Distress differentiates int o Emotional expressions of "Moral" emotions appear:
and interest are d is- sadness. disgust . and smiling. pouting, and embarrassment. empa-
crete emotions detect- anger; contentment dif- anger begin to be used thy, and envy after 18
able at birth ferentiates into joy and instrumentally to help months, and guilt,
contentment; interest infants obtain desired pride, and shame after
differentiates into inter- goals; affective sharing. 24 months
est and surprise in which caregivers
match infant positive
affect through another
sensoty modality, may
be observed.infants
may be relatively im-
pervious to frustration
at this time ; social ref-
erencing to caregivers
to resolve emotional
uncertainty may be
observed

-e-Continued

reactive, or withdrawn. Research on newborn behavior cal endowment of infants at birth includes prewired
suggests a different view: biological , cognitive, commu- knowledge of the world, such as cross-modal fluency,
nicative, emotional, and social capacities, which are as well as a remarkable ability to detect and to remember
functionally integrated, enable infants to seek stimula- invariant aspects of experiences (Stern , 1985). These
tion actively and to regulate their own behavior through capacities make the infant in the first 2 months of life
interactions with the environment. The psychobiologi- a far more sophisticated social partner than many

166 J. AM . ACAD . CHILD ADOLES C. PSYCHIA TRY . 36 :2 . FEB RU A RY 199 7


INFANT DEVELOPMENTAL RISK REVIEW

TABLE 1
Continued
First 2 Months 2 to 7 Months 7 to 18 Months 18 to 36 Months

Social Physical attributes of baby- Enhanced interest and Preferred attachment to a Enhanced capacity for ex-
ishness draw adults ability to engage adults small number of care- pressing needs and ap-
into involvement and in synchronous and re- giving adults develops; preciating conflicting
interaction ciprocal social inter- stranger wariness and agendas of others leads
changes; play periods separation protests ap- to increased negotia-
alternate with time- pears; social referenc- tion with caregivers; in-
outs; affective rnis- ing to resolve creased interest in peer
matches during inrerac- uncertainty relatedness; changes
tions stimulate the from parallel play to
infant's coping fleeting contact to true
capacities interactive play; con-
cern with personal pos-
sessions and sensitive
to being included or
excluded; relationships
with others become in-
creasingly important as
referents for self-
appraisal
Sleep/Wake Alternates among 6 states Greater stability in all Night waking occurs in Increased mobility of rod-
Cycles of consciousness; quiet states; preponderance virtually all infants; sig- dlers and heightened
sleep, active sleep, of sleep states at night nalers call out for pa- separat ion protest lead
drowsy, quiet alert, ac- and awake states dur- rental intervention to intensified conflicts
rive alert, and cry: no ing day; by 6 months whereas self-soothers re- over falling asleep;
diurnal pattern detect - of age, infants sleep turn to sleep on thei r night terrors and night -
able; 16 hours/day in through the night and own; signalers may mares may appear
sleep states begin to fall asleep have no previous his-
after being put down tory of night waking

widely quoted developmental theories have recognized can be understood by another person. These changes
(Freud, 1940; Mahler et al., 1975; Piaget, 1952). continue to be refined throughout the next year, but
Two to 7 Months. Dramatic changes in develop- their appearance for the first time after the transition
mental capacities across a number of domains appear at 7 to 9 months makes infants at this age qualitatively
at 2 to 3 months after birth, changing both infant different social experiencers and agents (Stern , 1985).
behavior and the behavior of caregiving adults. All of Also after 7 to 9 months, infants have developed a
these changes enhance the infant's appeal to others as strong preference for turning to a relatively small num-
a much more responsive and enjoyable social partner. ber of caregiving adults for nurturance and comfort.
The nature of caregivers' responsiveness to infants is The overarching developments of intersubjectiviry and
associated with relationship characteristics that have far- focused attachment underlie many of the specific
reaching implications for infant development. Infants' changes described in Table 1.
efforts at adaptation within the goal-correcting system Eighteen to 36 Months. The final transition period
of interaction with their primary caregivers provide an of major reorganization in infancy occurs at age 18 to
early sense of what it is like to be with another in an 20 months. New biological developments appear to
intimate relationship. make possible significant advances in symbolic repre-
Seven to 18 Months. At 7 to 9 months, another sentation, which is in turn associated with dramatic
major developmental transition occurs, termed by cognitive, emotional, communicative, and social ad-
Emde (1984) the onset of focused attachment and by vances. Infants are substantially more verbal, both in
Stern (1985) the discovery of intersubjecriviry. After understanding others' directives to them and in making
the transition at 7 to 9 months, infants act as if they their own intentions apparent to others, and this affects
understand that their thoughts, feelings, and actions both their emotional experience and their social relat-

J. AM . ACAD . CHILD AD OLES C. PSYCHIATRY . 3 6 :2. FEBR UARY 199 7 167


ZEANAH ET AI..

edness. After all of these dramatic changes, infants viewed as an optimal time to intervene to prevent later
consolidate and enhance their new capacities during mental health problems (Mrazek and Haggerty, 1994).
the third year of life as they prepare to move into Recent research on how risk and protective factors
wider social spheres of peer and teacher influences in affect development suggests that the transmission of
the preschool years. By the time children reach their risk is neither specific nor linear (Seifer et aI., 1992).
third birthday, they have available a sophisticated reper- For instance, it is known that maternal depression
toire of skills for communicating and experiencing relates not just to an increased incidence of depression
relationships. Qualitative features of their caregiving in offspring, as might be expected by a linear genetic
context during the first 3 years of life shape their model, but also to a host of other less specific outcomes
expectations of relationship as they move into the in infancy, including insecure attachment (Lyons-Ruth
broader social world. et al., 1987; Shaw and Vondra, 1993), language and
cognitive problems (Murray, 1992), and social inter-
Process of Development: The Transactional Model active problems (Field et al., 1995b; Weinberger and
Models of development describe the process by Tronick, in press). Furthermore, multiple risk condi-
which an individual develops and changes over time. tions from different domains (e.g., biological, psycho-
The transactional model of development, described by logical, or social) may occur simultaneously and may,
Sameroff (in press), is currently the most widely ac- in turn, be exacerbated or ameliorated by the infant's
cepted model of the developmental process. In this family system (Rutter, 1987; Seifer, 1995). Thus, the
model, genetic and environmental regulators of behav- total number of risk conditions affecting an infant may
ior transact continually over time, mutually influencing be more predictive of various outcomes in later life
one another. In fact, Sameroff (in press) has posited that than exposure to any specific type of risk factor. This
much as the genotype acts as the biological regulator of is true for risk factors for insecure infant attachment
infants' behavior, the environtype acts as the social (Shaw and Vondra, 1993), for social competence in
regulator of the infants' behavior. For infants, the early childhood (Sameroff et aI., 1987), and for behav-
environ type comprises the cultural, familial, and paren- ior problems in early childhood (Sanson et aI., 1991;
tal characteristics that regulate infants' experiences and Shaw and Vondra, 1995; Shaw et al., 1994). The lack
opportunities. Individuals' genotypic and environ typic of specificity of single or combined risk factors may
regulators transact continually over time. This model explain why results involving only a small set of risk
accounts reasonably well for most developmental out- factors and a particular outcome are often equivocal.
comes that have been studied, except for those that Furthermore, each specific risk factor is likely to be
follow the extremes of biological insults, e.g., chromo- an aggregate of a series of smaller risk factors acting
somal disorders, or environmental adversities, e.g., mas- in concert. For instance, infants growing up in poverty
sive institutional deprivation. Still, the transactional are more likely both to have parents with psychiatric
model does not give predictive weight to any particular disturbances and to suffer from inadequate nutrition
set of risk or protective factors, and the search for and poor prenatal care (Halpern, 1993), although some
more precise predictive models continues. poor infants will be affected by none of these factors.
For each infant studied, the use of valid and reliable
DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY methods to generate an inventoty of total risk impacting
the infant and his or her family is important. This list
Overview of Risk and Protective Factors
always should include protective factors operating in the
The field of developmental psychopathology has infant or environment. What accounts for individual
emerged as interest in predicting maladaptive patterns differences in outcome given seemingly similar experi-
of development in children has grown (Miller and ences remains largely speculative and will likely be the
Lewis, 1990). Identifying the mechanisms by which focus of future research (Werner, 1989).
various psychosocial and biological factors influence
Biological Risk and Protective Factors
development has been a central focus of research in
this field. This research is important clinically, especially Prematurity and Serious Medical Illness. Premature
in the area of prevention, in which infancy has been birth and serious medical illness in infancy represent

168 J. AM. ACAD. CHI!.D ADO!.ESC. PSYCHIATRY, %:2, FEBRUARY 1997


IN FAN T DEV ELOPM ENT AL RI SK R E VI EW

obvious biological risk facrors that may significantly related factors (e.g., birth weight and a summary score
inAuence infant outcome. There are a variety of etiolo- of medical complications).
gies both for the different severe medical conditions In this large and diverse sample, race (black >
and for premature birth. Research suggests that the Hispanic > white) was both the most sensitive and
interplay of aggregate s of risk facrors may eventually the most specific measure of outcome, followed by
lead to a continuum of outcomes. maternal education and , finally, medical complications.
Approximately 3% of births in this country show Birth weight was neither sensitive nor specific in pre-
evidence of major malformations, and about 11% of dicting IQ at age 3 years. Black infants with poorly
children are born at less than 37 weeks' gestation educated mothers and a high medical complication
(Paneth, 1995). In total, 135,000 infants per year are rate had a 90% chance of falling in the target group
at heightened risk for major developmental problems. for the analysis (lQ ~85); white infants with well-
Ne vertheles s, these numbers do not account for many of educated parents, regardless of birth complication rates,
the children who eventually suffer from developmental had a 9% chance of having an IQ ~85. Analysis of
disorders; in fact, developmental disorders occur in 3 data from a cohort that received intervention revealed
of every 1,000 children , a majority of whom are not that the percentage of infants with IQ ~85 in the
diagnosed until after age 2 years. Most of these develop- former group dropped ro 50% with a multifaceted
mental disorders have a primarily biological basis (e.g., intervention, while that of the latter dropped to 7%.
chromosomal abnormalities, inadequate fetal blood These results, particularly in light of the fact that
supply, infections, etc.), though 10% are idiopathic intervention significantly changed outcome in the high-
(Kopp and Kaler, 1989). Understanding the develop- risk group, suggest that psychosocial factors were largely
mental pathways for children born biologically compro- responsible for compromised outcome in this cohort.
mised is complicated by the fact that there is a wide Other similar data suggest complex interrelationships
range of degree of compromise even within types between illness and outcomes, with environmental fac-
of disorders; this variability makes outcome research tors playing a central role in inAuencing those outcomes
difficult because the groups studied may not be compa- (Benedersky and Lewis, 1994; Minde, 1993).
rable. Furthermore, rates of biological compromise are Infant Temperament. The modern empirical study
not equally spread across socioeconomic strata or family of temperament in infancy has been most broadly
composition. For instance, the rate of low birth weight inAuenced by the longitudinal studies of Thomas,
(less than 2,500 g) for healthy white women aged 20 Chess, and colleagues begun in the 1950s (Rutter,
ro 30 years is less than 6% , while it is as high as 15% 1989; Thomas and Chess, 1977) . Thomas and Chess
for low socioeconomic status minority teenagers, a asserted that differences in how infants modulate their
higher proportion of whom are single (Panerh , 1995). behavioral responses to the environment reflect the
Thus, prenatal and postnatal psychosocial environ- heritable biological makeup of the infant and not the
ments must be considered when biological compromise summative effects of the environment on the infant,
is observed. a concept still emphasized today (Rothbart and Ahadi,
In fact, the etiology of medical compromise in 1994). Still, debate on the fundamental questions of
infancy is likely ro be less important than the severity how best ro define and measure temperament continues
of the compromise and the context in which it occurs . (Goldsmith et al., 1987), and this debate is fueled
For instance, recent data from the rnultisite Infant by research findings consistently demonstrating little
He alth and Development Project were analyzed using agreement between maternal and observer reports of
a statistical technique called signal detection to explore infant temperament (Seifer et al., 1994) .
which factors were most determinative of compromised Advances in both psychophysiology and genetic
outcome (Kraemer, 1995). A large cohort of premature modeling have recently enriched the study of tempera-
infants were followed and compromised outcome was ment. Research on the phenomenon of behavioral
defined as Stanford-Binet score of (85 at age 3 years inhibition provides the best example of how these
(Korner er al., 1993). A series of social indices affecting tools can provide critical evidence in support of a
postnatal environment. including maternal education temperamental construct. Behavioral inhibition,
and race, were analyzed alongside a variety of health- defined and studied by Kagan and his colleagues, refers

J. AM . ACA D . c.u u.n A D O l.E SC. P S YCIII ATR Y. -'(, :~. FE RR LJA RY 1<)') 7 169
ZEANAH ET AL.

to a moderately stable pattern of responses manifested gesting possible differences in central neural regulation
by wariness, avoidance, or fear in response to unfamiliar between these groups (Kagan and Snidman, 1991).
people or events. This pattern of responsiveness is Replication of these studies is ongoing.
reliably identifiable in about 15% of children from The next level of investigation in temperament re-
middle class samples and is usually evident by about search is in the area of genetics. In the absence of a
18 months of age (Kagan et aI., 1988). Assessment specific identifiable gene controlling behavioral inhibi-
involves analysis of a child's pattern of responsiviry tion, the study of the degree of concordance in behavior
during a set of laboratory paradigms. Since these re- patterns between monozygotic twin pairs (who share
sponses have been considered to be indicative of herita- identical genes) compared with dizygotic twin pairs
ble biological differences, study of each subject's (who share, on average, half their genetic complement)
physiological makeup has been pursued (Snidman et is informative. A series of studies, the MacArthur
al., 1995). Longitudinal Twin Studies, have provided the most
The refinement of techniques for measuring salivary comprehensive information to date. Behavioral inhibi-
cortisol and vagal tone has provided insight into physio- tion scores in this sample were only slightly stable
logical systems related to behavioral inhibition. Assays across ages 14 to 24 months, and scores for behavioral
of salivary cortisol provide a noninvasive and highly inhibition at these ages were significantly higher for
sensitive measure that captures the reactivity of the girls than for boys (Robinson et al., 1992).
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Gunnar, The behaviorally inhibited pattern of behavior was
1990). The relationship between the reactivity of the found to be determined significantly by heritability at
HPA axis and behavioral inhibition is complex (Stans- all ages tested. Furthermore, even though behavioral
bury and Gunnar, 1994) . Some studies have shown inhibition scores changed over the 10 months of evalua-
higher home and laboratory cortisol levels among in- tion, this change was due primarily to genetic contribu-
fants with behavioral inhibition (Kagan et al., 1987), tions rather than environmental factors (Plomin et al.,
whereas others have shown higher levels among children 1993). It is likely that genes may turn off and on at
classified as outgoing or uninhibited (Tennes and different ages, suggesting that simple linear models of
Kreye, 1985). These conflicting results may be ex- genetic contribution to behavior are unlikely (Cherny
plained in a recent study that looked at the relationship et aI., 1994; Plomin er al., 1993).
between attachment and behavioral inhibition. Behav- A final frontier in the study of behavioral inhibition
iorally inhibited toddlers who had insecure attachments has been the analysis of the relationship between this
to the parent accompanying them to a novel test pattern of responsiveness in infancy and the develop-
procedure showed elevated cortisol response to elicited ment of anxiety disorders. Though longitudinal studies
arousal. Those inhibited toddlers who were securely from infancy through adulthood have not been com-
attached to the accompanying parent did not show pleted, some evidence exists that children of parents
these elevations in cortisol response (Nachmias et al., with panic disorder with agoraphobia have significantly
1996). These results also indicate that different patterns higher rates of behavioral inhibition in early childhood
of attachment relationships may be associated with compared with control subjects (children of parents
different biological responses to stress in inhibited with other psychiatric disorders) (Rosenbaum et al.,
toddlers. 1988). Furthermore, follow-up studies of school-age
Another interesting technique is the calculation of children identified as behaviorally inhibited revealed
vagal tone from analysis of heart rate variability. Vagal that they have a higher incidence of psychiatric disor-
tone is thought to reflect the level of input of the ders. including anxiety disorders. when compared with
parasympathetic nervous system on the heart (Porges , noninhibited controls (Biederman et al., 1993).
1992). It can be measured while the child is engaged
Parenting Risk and Protective Factors
in a variety of tasks and provides insight regarding
regulation of the autonomic nervous system. Young Infant-Caregiver Attachments. Recent attachment re-
children with behavioral inhibition have been shown to search has identified patterns of attachment in adults
have higher and less variable heart rates than extremely that are analogous to the patterns described in infancy
uninhibited children or noninhibited controls, sug- using the Strange Situation Procedure (see Table 2).

170 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADO LE s e . PSYCHIATRY, 36:2, FEBRUARY 1997


INFANT DEVELOPMENTAL RISK REVIEW

TABLE 2
Attachment Patterns in Infanrs and Parenrs
Effect Size of
Patterns of Attachment Description of Pattern Concordance

Infanr secure Positive affect sharing when nondistressed Effect size d = 1.09,
r = .48.
Fisher's Z = 0.52
Adult autonomous Coherenr description of childhood relationship experiences in
which positive and negative aspects of relationships are acknowl-
edged; relationships valued and important
Infanr avoidanr Avoid caregiver despite high levels of inrernal distress; suppress Effect size d = 0.92,
attachment behaviors and focus on external environment r = .42,
Fisher's Z = 0.45
Adult dismissing Fail ro recall details of childhood relationships or minimize the
effects of adverse experiences; relationships neither valued nor
important
Infanr resistant Seek proximity when distressed but resist caregiver artempts ro Effect size d = 0.39.
soothe them at the same time they appeal for soothing; behave r = .19,
ambivalendy about contact, signaling for it and rejecting it Fisher's Z = O.I 9
Adult preoccupied Describe childhood relationship experiences incoherently, exhib-
iting angry preoccupation or passive thought processes
Infanr disorganized Exhibit one or more anomalous, bizarre conflict behaviors, directed Effect size d = 0.65,
toward caregiver, especially during stress; may have one of the r = .31,
other classifications as an underlying pattern Fisher's Z = 0.32
Adult unresolved Lack of resolution of mourning after a significanr loss or severely
traumatic experience as revealed in unintegrared, incoherenr
narrative in describing these experiences; especially disorienred,
confused, or emotionally uninregrated descriptions of these
experiences

Note: Data presenred in this table are drawn from a meta-analysis by van IJzendoorn (1995) summarizing results from
nine investigations that examined four-way infant-adult attachment concordance. Prinred with permission of the
American Psychological Association.

The Adult Attachment Interview (Main and Goldwyn, ents' attachment classification (van Ijzendoorn, 1995).
in press) was developed as a structured clinical interview Since conventional criteria suggest that effect sizes of
that inquires about an individual's childhood relation- 0.20 are small, of 0.50 are moderate, and 0.80 are
ship experiences and asks the individual to evaluate large (van Ijzendoorn, 1995), the effect sizes for concor-
and reflect on them. Narrative responses to these probes dances of infant-parent attachment reviewed in the
are analyzed and classified into patterns of attachment. meta-analysis and listed in Table 2 are moderate to
What is believed to be similar about the patterns in large.
infancy and adulthood is that they reflect differences Furthermore, in high- and low-risk groups, parents'
in internal representational processes involved in re- representations assessed in pregnancy predict the in-
sponding to distress and the need for comfort. Table fant's attachment pattern more than 1 year later (Benoit
2 describes the patterns and their characteristics. and Parker, 1994; Fonagy et al., 1991; Ward and
A number of investigations have demonstrated con- Carlson, 1995; Zeanah et al., 1995). In keeping with
cordances between attachment patterns in parents and the idea that attachment patterns are relationship-
in their infants at a level well beyond chance. Data specific in infancy rather than trait-like, fathers' prena-
on the concordance of infant-parent attachment are tally assessed representations of attachment predicted
summarized in a recent meta-analysis of 18 investiga- infant attachment to father but not to mother, and
tions from a number of different countries (total n = mothers' prenatally assessed representations of attach-
854 dyads) and indicate a strong tendency for infants' ment predicted infant attachment to mother but not
attachment classifications to be analogous to their par- to father (Steele et al., 1996). The mechanisms by

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY. 36:2. FEBRUARY 1997 171


ZEANAH ET AI..

which patterns of attachment are transmitted intergen- pressed women (Campbell et al., 1995) . Severity and
erationally remain to be demonstrated. chronicity of depression, and double depression (major
Psychopathology in Parents. A wealth of research has depression and dysthymia), are associated with worse
suggested that psychiatric symptomatology and disor- outcomes for infants (Campbell et al., 1993; Frankel
ders in parents are associated with specific and non- et al., 1991) . Despite the robust associations between
specific effects on infant and child development (Seifer maternal depression and infant development, mecha-
and Dickstein. 1993). Although many psychiatric disor- nisms that link maternal symptomatology and infant
ders " run in families," the nonspecific effects of psychi- development are only partially understood.
atric disorders associated with infant development are Problematic "depressed" maternal interactive behav-
of most concern. Seifer and Dickstein (1993) have iors that have been delineated in recent investigations
suggested that compromises in various domains of include negative affect expressions. less positive engage-
development in the first 3 years of life may be predictive ment, less stimulation. and less sensitivity (Cohn and
of more symptom-specific forms of dysfunction in later Tronick, 1989; Field, 1992; Lyons-Ruth et al., 1990).
childhood or adolescence. It is not yet clear whether At least three distinctive interactive patterns have been
there is any specificity between early developmental described in depressed mothers: a withdrawn, unavail-
compromises and subsequent outcomes. able style; a hostile-intrusive style; and a largely positive
There is little evidence at present to suggest that style (Cohn et al., 1986; Field et al., 1990).
specific psychiatric disorders are associated with specific Infants of depressed mothers also have been shown
proximal infant outcomes. Rather. the severity and to exhibit a number of problematic behaviors (Cohn
chronicity of a given disorder seem to be more im- and Tronick, 1989; Field et al., 1990). Infants of
portant than the specific diagnosis (Seifer and depressed mothers also match negative states more
Dickstein. 1993). Since multivariate approaches to risk often and positive states less often with their mothers
assessment suggest that the more important determi- than infants of nondepressed mothers (Field et al.,
nants of infant outcome are the number rather than 1990). Furthermore, a variety of biological abnormali-
the kind of risk factors impacting an infant. parental ties have been demonstrated in infants of depressed
psychopathology must be considered alongside other mothers. Right frontal EEG asymmetry, a possible
associated risk factors. Still, there are important reasons marker of a bias for expression of negative emotion,
for studying parental psychiatric illness. It serves as a has been demonstrated in two cohorts of depressed
convenient clinical marker of risk. it facilitates attempts mothers: those with 3- to 6-month-old infants (Field
to discover whatever degree of specificity may exist et al., 1995a) and those with 11- to 16-month-old
between parent and child symptomatology, and it infants (Dawson et al., 1992). One of these cohorts
identifies parents more likely to exhibit problematic also had significantly lower vagal tone than a compari-
parenting (Lyons-Ruth, 1995). One of the most im- son group of infants of nondepressed mothers (Field
portant areas of research in the past decade has been er al., 1995a). Newborns of depressed mothers also
the study of maternal depression. have been shown to have poorer performance on the
Maternal Depression. Although it is not clear that orientation cluster of the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral
the postpartum period uniquely increases the risk for Assessment Scale (Abrams et al., 1995) and to have
depression in women (O'Hara et al., 1990), it is clear elevated levels of epinephrine and norepinephrine
that maternal depression is associated with a variety (Field. 1995). These biological findings are suggestive
of problems in both parenting behaviors and in infants of a within-the-infant depressive "disorder."
born to depressed women (Field, 1992; Seifer and On the other hand. there is some evidence that
Dickstein, 1993). Risks to infants associated with ma- maladaptive infant behaviors associated with maternal
ternal depression have been demonstrated in lower depression may be relationship-specific. For instance,
socioeconomic status. high-risk samples using self-re- infants of depressed mothers have been shown to
port measures (Cohn and Tronick, 1989; Field et al., interact more positively with their day-care providers
1990; Lyons-Ruth et al., 1990). in women diagnosed (Pelaez-Nogueras er al., 1994) and with their nonde-
with depressive disorders seeking treatment (Teri and pressed fathers than with their mothers (Hossain et al.,
Gelfand. 1991), and in community samples of de- 1994). Such specificity indicates that depressive disor-

172 J. AM . A CAD . CHILD ADOL ESC. PSYCHIATRY , 36 :2 , FE BRU A RY 19 97


INFANT DEVELOPMENTAL RI SK REVIEW

ders may be experienced and expressed differently in among women living in poverty. especially in the inner
the context of different relationships. Further evidence cities (Halpern. 1993). There are high rates of parental
of relationship disturbances in infants of depressed psychopathology associated with substance abuse dur-
mothers comes from studies of attachment indicating ing pregnancy (Haller et al., 1993). Caregiving environ-
that insecure, especially disorganized attachments are ments for infants whose mothers abuse drugs may be
increased (Campbell et al., 1993; Demulder and Radke- marked by disorganization, with infants often being
Yarrow , 1991; Teti er al., 1995). exposed to multiple caregivers. These factors together
Maternal Substance Abuse. Recent surveys indicate may account for the high rates of insecure and disorga-
that each year between 100,000 and 375,000 women nized attachments found in infants prenatally exposed
give birth to infants prenatally exposed to illicit drugs, to drugs (Rodning et al., 1990; O'Connor et al., 1987).
not including alcohol and nicotine (US General Ac- Infants prenatally exposed to drugs and to associated
counting Office, 1990) . The rising incidence of crack risk factors are at high risk for adverse developmental
cocaine use among pregnant women has inspired re- outcome. Outcomes are best predicted by analysis of
search in this area, sparked, in part, by concern regard- the number and severity of individual risk factors
ing direct toxic effects on infants resulting in long-term affecting the infant's proximal environment and by the
developmental effects. There has been little research on infant's neurobehavioral profile.
the effects of paternal substance abuse on infant
Family and Social Risk and Protective Factors
development.
The tendency for pregnant substance abusers to use Marital Quality and Interactions. Recent research has
more than one drug and the large number of pre- and indicated that there are specific relationships between
postnatal risk factors associated with substance abuse marital quality and infant functioning. Marital conflict
has complicated research in this area. There is no has been related to intrusive infant behavior (Easter-
evidence that simple models linking prenatal drug brooks and Emde, 1986), as well as conduct problems in
exposure directly to any specific developmental out- toddlers (Jouriles et al., 1988). Overt conflict, especially
come are valid. Instead, the interplay between individ- inrerparental anger. is particularly disruptive to chil-
ual biological and psychosocial risk factors must be dren's healthy adaptation. The more toddlers observe
accounted for in determining the ultimate effect of interparental anger. the more insecure and disturbed
prenatal drug exposure on infant outcome. they behave when exposed to these conflicts. Children
The drug-using lifestyle is often associated with from conflicted and physically hostile families demon-
inadequate nutrition, which may itself affect fetal strate more distress and heightened reactivity in re-
growth. This problem may be exacerbated by the sponse to displays of inreradult anger than do peers
tendency for substance-abusing women nor to receive from less conflicrual families. Children who have been
adequate prenatal care (Lester and Tronick, 1994). exposed to high levels of marital conflict in the past
Timing. dose, and duration of drug exposure is almost show even more intense negative emotions in subse-
never controlled for in studies, though these factors quent conflict situations (Cummings et al., 1985).
may be critical in determining possible structural effects Maladjustment is greatest when infants are exposed to
on the developing CNS (see Tronick et al., in press, parents' physical conflicts as opposed to verbal anger
for an exception) . Since these issues are difficult to (Cummings et al., 1981). When the adult conflict is
account for even in the most well-designed studies, the child-related. children are even more likely to show
relative effects of drug exposure itself remain obscured. fear and dysphoria (Cummings et al., 1989; Grych
Even the effects of alcohol. long known to have direct et al.. 1991).
toxic effects on developing neurons and to be associated The content of the conflict is important. as well.
with a teratogenic syndrome (fetal alcohol syndrome), Child-rearing disagreements are related to 3-year-old
appear to be modified by factors unrelated to dosage or boys' behavior problems more strongly than are general
exposure (Abel and Sokol, 1987; Sampson et al., 1989). measures of marital satisfaction and conflict (Jouriles
Numerous postnatal factors associated with " the et al., 1991). How adults handle conflict, regardless
culture of drug abuse" may independently affect infant of its content, also is salient. Cummings et al. (1985)
outcome. Drug abuse appears to be more common demonstrated that 2-year-olds' displays of aggression

J. A M . A CAD. C H I l. D A D O I. ESc:. PSYCHIA TRY ..\ 6 :2. FE B RU A RY 19 9 7 173


ZEANAH ET AL.

and distress were reduced to baseline levels after adults on the part of adolescent mothers of children 2 months
resolved their conflicts . Infants and toddlers engaged of age.
in positive responses (e.g., smiling, laughing, playing) Poor families also are less likely to provide stimula-
when parents had constructive marital disagreements ting home environments (Duncan et al., 1994). These
(Easterbrooks er al., 1994). variables indirectly affect children in that a stimulating
In addition to lack of conflict, high parental intimacy environment and positive mother-infant interactions
also positively influences infant development. Closeness are related to secure attachment behaviors. The presence
berween marital partners has been associated with sensi- of adequate social support is associated with a more
tive parental caregiving and with secure infant attach- stimulating home environment and provision of appro-
ment (Cox er al., 1989), even when closeness is priate play materials, both of which are powerful pre-
measured prenatally (Howes and Markman, 1989). dictors of developmental outcomes.
Thus, parents who confide in and support one another, Adolescent Parenting. Adolescent parenthood has
resolve their conflicts, and remain close and connected short- and long-term biological, psychological, and
have infants who have fewer difficulties and display social consequences for parent and infant. There is an
more positive adaptation than parents who sustain increased risk of mortality associated with maternal
conflicrual, negative marital relationships. age at the extremes; neonatal mortality rates are ac-
Poverty and Social Class. Poverty and socioeconomic counted for primarily by low birth weight. Mothers
status have been found to be strongly related to a younger than age 15 are more likely to conceal their
number ofdevelopmental outcomes in infancy, exerting pregnancy and may begin antenatal care later than
indirect effects through their impact on variables such older teenagers (Brooks-Gunn and Furstenberg, 1986).
as availability of resources (i.e., food , shelter, and The perinatal outcome of infants born to adolescent
medical care) and lifestyle issues (e.g., crowding, quality mothers 15 years of age and younger continues to be
of neighborhood). There is a higher prevalence of poor, even when adequate prenatal care is obtained,
illnesses and the effects are more pervasive in poor perhaps because of the competing nutritional needs
children, who are more likely to be of low birth weight of the adolescent and the baby (Hechtman, 1989).
and to experience lead poisoning, failure to thrive, Nevertheless, when medical care is sufficient, little or
otitis media , and iron-deficiency anemia than socially no risk is found in the health of neonates born to
advantaged children (Parker et al., 1988). Infants and adolescent mothers, particularly those aged 16 and
toddlers with these difficulties score lower on develop- older.
mental and cognitive scales (Pollitt, 1994). Sudden A large body of research indicates that, compared
infant death syndrome also occurs more often among with older mothers, adolescent mothers differ in their
poor infants (Wise and Meyers , 1988). As McLoyd interactions with their infants: teenagers engage in less
(1990) points out, chronic poverty is not a unitary smiling and positive eye and physical contact with
variable, but rather a combination of pervasive stressful their infants, even when matched on socioeconomic
conditions that severely constricts choices (Halpern, and ethnic characteristics. They talk less, give more
1993). commands and authoritarian statements, and make
Poverty and economic loss increase the risk of emo- fewer elaborated, descriptive, and articulate responses
tional distress in parents and heighten their vulnerabil- (Culp et al., 1988). They are more passive in their
ity to negative life events (e.g., single parenthood, social face-to-face interactions, and they score lower than
isolation, depression, anxiety). Poor parents are more adult mothers in maternal-affectional match, rate of
likely to value obedience and use power-assertive disci- stimulation, flexibility, positivity, motivation, and over-
pline and physical punishment and are less likely to all quality of mothering (Passino et al., 1993). Adoles-
be supportive of their children (McLoyd, 1990) . Even cent mothers have been found to be less committed,
within poor, abusive families, fewer economic resources satisfied, and skilled than older mothers (Whitman
are associated with increased severity of maltreatment er al., 1987). Their children speak less and are more
(Horowitz and Wolock, 1985). McKenry et al. (1991) likely to have poorer cognitive and linguistic outcomes
found that poverty significantly predicted role reversal (Spieker and Bensley, 1994).

174 J. AM . AC AD . C H I LD ADOL ESC. PSYCHIATRY . 3 6 :2 . FE BR U ARY 199 7


INFANT DEVELOPMENTAL RISK REVIEW

Adolescent mothers are perceived as less sensinve and preschool children found major differences in
and responsive, and more restricted, physically intru- security between maltreated and control groups but
sive, and punitive, in their child-rearing practices com- less clear differences in disorganized attachment, espe-
pared with adult mothers (Coli et al., 1986). Children ciallyas children became older (Cicchetti and Barnett,
are more likely to show avoidance and less contact- 1991). At 30 months, 30% of the maltreated were
seeking when parenting is intrusive, and punitive disci- secure, compared with 65% of the controls, and 36%
pline has been related to impulsivity, aggression, social were disorganized, compared with 15% of controls.
withdrawal, and poor peer relations in children (Crock- At 36 months, 21 % of the maltreated but 71 % of the
enberg, 1987; Hart et al., 1992; Weiss et al., 1992). controls were secure, and 36% of the maltreated and
Adolescent mothers are more likely to be depressed 27% of the controls had disorganized classifications.
than older mothers, and depressed mothers are less Similar proportions and significant differences between
emotionally available (Osofsky et al., 1993). Their maltreated and comparison infants and preschool chil-
children are at higher risk for problems in affect regula- dren were reported in a third study by another team
tion, including both flattened affect and aggressive of investigators (Crittenden et al., 1991). Furthermore,
behavior (Zahn-Waxler et aI., 1990). mothers who reported high levels of partner violence
Adolescent mothers are less knowledgeable about were more likely to have toddlers who had disorganized
child development than are adult mothers; they gener- attachment relationships with them, even when there
ally underestimate social, cognitive, and language func- was no evidence that the toddlers had been abused
tioning and overestimate the attainment of (Zeanah et al., 1994). These data suggest that family
developmental milestones. Compared with adult moth- violence is strongly associated with attachment
ers, teenage mothers also have been reported to perceive disturbances.
their infant's temperament as more difficult (Osofsky Other aspects of social and emotional development
et al., 1993). have been identified as problematic in maltreated in-
Researchers have found that the behavior patterns fants (see Cicchetti and Toth, 1995). Maltreated infants
associated with adolescent mothers and their children and toddlers exhibit affective withdrawal, anhedonia,
may result in differences in attachment classifications inconsistent and unpredictable signals, indiscriminant
between infants of adolescent and adult mothers. Ado- sociability and proneness to anger and distress while
lescent mothers' infants evidenced significantly more interacting with their caregivers, and increased aggres-
avoidant behavior and were more likely to be avoidantly sion toward peers and caregivers. They also exhibit
attached and to be at high risk for developing disorga- fear and aggression in response to peer distress, although
nized attachments to their mothers. These attachment nonrnaltreared toddlers demonstrate interest, empathy,
patterns have been associated with earlier insensitive, sadness and concern.
negative, and emotionally unavailable caregiving (Osof- It is not surprising that self-perception in maltreated
sky et al., 1993). toddlers is compromised. Although maltreated toddlers
Culp et al. (1988) found that adolescent mothers exhibited self-recognition at the same time as their
exhibit more variability in behaviors than older moth- nonmaltreated peers (about 18 months), they exhibited
ers. It is important to remember that some adolescent significantly less positive affect in response to their
mothers and their children have favorable outcomes image in a mirror (Schneider-Rosen and Cicchetti,
and do very well in spite of the numerous adversities 1991). Maltreated toddlers also used fewer internal
outlined above (Osofsky et al., 1993). state words, especially fewer words describing negative
Family Violence. The effects of maltreatment in the affective states than a nonmaltreated comparison group
first 3 years of life have been explored in several who had similar levels of receptive vocabulary (Beeghly
developmental domains. In one investigation of attach- and Cicchetti, 1994). These important findings need
ment, 82% of I-year-old infants maltreated by their to be extended longitudinally.
mothers were classified as having disorganized attach-
CONCLUSIONS
ments to them, as opposed to only 19% of demographi-
cally matched control infants (Carlson et al., 1989). Infants develop rapidly, shaped by the ongoing inter-
A second study that examined attachment in toddlers dependence of biology and caregiving contexts. Com-

j. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESc:. PSYCHIATRY, 36:2, FEBRUARY 1997 175


ZEANAH ET AI..

plex and evolving interrelationships among risk factors Crockcnberg SB (1987), Predic tor s and co rrelates of anger toward and
pu n it ive contro l of to d d lers by ad ol es cent m others . Ch ild De u
at different intrinsic and extrinsic levels are onl y begin- 58 :%4-975
ning to be elucidated. Already we have learned that C ulp RE, App elbaum MI, O sofsky )D , Levy )A (1988), Ado lescent an d
o lder mothers: com pa riso n be tween prena tal. ma terna l var iables and
linear models of cau se and effect are of little use in newbo rn interaction measures. Injimt Beha De II :j5J-J62
understanding the development of psychopathology. C um mings EM. Ianot ti R), Zahn-Waxler C (19 8 5). The influence o f
co nflic t between ad ults on the emotio ns and aggressio n of young
Refining our markers of risk and demonstrating effec-
chi ldr en. Drv Psy cho! 21 :49 5- 507
tive preventive interventions are the next important C um mi ngs EM , Zahn-W axler C, Radke-Yar row M ( 198 0 , Young childre n 's
challenges facing child psychiatrists and other research- respon ses to expressions of ange r and affectio n by o thers in the famil y.
Chi ld De 52: 1274 -1 28 2
ers and clini cians concerned with infant mental health. C um mi ngs )S . Pellegr in i DS, No tarius Cl , C um mi ngs EM ( 1989), C hil-
d ren' s responses to angry adult behavior as a fun ct ion of mar ital distress
an d hist o ry of inrerparenr hostility, Child Deu 60 :lOj5-1 0 43
Daw so n G , Klinger LG , Panagitodes H , H ill D, Sp ieker S ( 1992) , Frontal
REFERENCES lobe activity and affect ive beh avior o f in fant s and mo thers with depressive
sym pto ms. Child D,,! 6 j :725 - 73 7
Abel EI.. So ko l R) ( 1')87) . Inciden ce offeta l alcohol synd ro me and econom ic Demulder EK. Radke-Yarrow M (199 1), Att achment with affectively ill
im pact of I'AS-relate d ano malies. Alcohol D rug Depend 19 :5 1- 70 and well mot hers : co ncu rrent beha vioral cor relates. Deu Psycbopathol
Abrams SM , held T. Scafidi 1', Prodromid is M (1 99 5), Newborns of .~ : 22 7- 2 4 2
depre ssed mo th ers. Infanr MOil Health J 1 6 : 2 .B -2 .~ 9 Duncan )G , Bro oks-G unn) , Klcbanov PK (1994). Economic deprivation
Beeghl y M . Cicchetti D (I ')1)4). Child maltr eatment . att achment . and the and early chi ld hoo d dev elo pme nt . Chi ld Deu 65 :296-31 8
self system : em ergence of an intern al state lexicon in toddlers at high Easte rh roo k. M A. C ummi ngs EM , Ernde RN (19 94 ), Young child ren's
socia l risk. D" , Psychop,uh oI 6:5- jO respon ses to co nstruc t ive mar ital d isputes. J Fam Psy choI 8:160-1 69
Bcncdcrsky M , l.cwis M (1<)')4), Environmental risk" bio logi cal risks. and East cr b roo ks M A. Em de RN ( 1986), M arr iage and infa nt : different systems'
develo pm ental o utcome. De Psycho! .~ 0 : 484-4,)4 linkages lo r mothers and infants . Pap er presented at the Int ernation al
Benoit D, Park er KCH (1')')4), Srabiliry and t ransm issio n of atta chment C o nferenc e on In fant Studies. Beverly Hills, CA
across three generation s, Child D", 65:14 44 -1457 Emd e RN ( 1984) , The affect ive self: continu it ies and transfo rma t ions from
Biederman ) . Rosenbaum ) F. Boldu c-Murphy EA et al. ( 19,)j ), A j-year infancy. In : Frontiers of Injimt Psychiatry II. C all) , Galenso n E. T yson
follo w-u p of child ren with and witho ut be ha viora l in hib itio n . J Am R L, eds. N ew Yor k: Basic Boo k" pp j8-54
Acad Child Adolesc I'sy chi,ttry .n: 8 14 - 821 fi eld T ( I ')1)2) . In fant s of depressed mothers. De v PsychopathoI4:4 9-66
Bro oks-Gu nn ], Fu rste n berg FF ( \ 986) , The chi ld ren o f ado lescent mothers: Field T ( 199 5). In fant s of de pr essed mothers. InJitm Beha De 18:1-1 3
ph ysical, academic and psycho logica l outcom es. f)" , Rrt.6:224 - 251 Field T , f o x N . Picken s ), Na wroc ki T (J 995a), Relativ e right frontal
Campbell SB, C nhn )F, M eyers T (1')95), Depression in firsr-rime mothers: EEG activ ation in J - to 6 -m onth-old in fant s of depressed mothers.
m o th er-infant in t er ac tio n a nd d epressi on chro ni city . D", Psychol D ru Psvcho] j 1:j58- j 6j
j \ :.H ') - j 57 Field T , H ealy B. G o ldstein S, Gutherz M ( 1990), Beh avior sta te matchi ng in
C am p bell SB, Coh n )1', Me yers T. Ross S, flanagan C ( 199j), C h ro n icity mo ther-infant int eract io ns of no n-depressed vs depr essed mother-infant
of ma tern al dep ression and mo ther-i nfant interactio n. In : Depressed d yads. Dn . Psycbol 26 :7- 14
M others and Their Children: lndiuidua] Dijfrmttrs ill Mother-Child Field T . Pickens) . f o x NA, N awrock i T , Gonzalez ) ( 199 5b ), Vaga l tone
Uu tcom e, T ed D, chai r. Sym posium pre sent ed to the bienn ial meet in g in infants of depressed mothers. De u PsychopathoI 7 :227- 231
of the Soci ety for Resear ch in C hi ld Devel opment , New Orleans Fon agy P, Steele H, Steel e M (1991 ), Matern al represe nt ations of attachment
C arlson V , Cicchetti D . Barn ell D. Braunwald KG ( 1989) , Find ing during pr egnancy pr edict the o rga nizat io n o f infant-mother att achment
order ill d isor gan izat ion : lesso ns from research o n rnalrr earcd infan ts' at o ne year of age. Child De 62 :8') 1-90 5
attach me nts to th eir ca regivers. In : ChiM Maltrratmmt: Theory and f rank el K. Maslin-Co le C, H arm on R (J 99 1), De pressed mo thers of
Resea rch on th Causes and Con srqumcrs of Child Abuse and N rglrct. pr escho oler s: wha t th ey say is not what th ey do . Paper pre sented to
Ci cc he tt i D , Carlson V, cds, Ca mb rid ge. Eng land : Cam b ridge U niver- th e b ienn ial meet ing o f [he Soci ety for Research in Ch ild De velop-
siry Press ment , Seatt le
Cherny SS, lulker DW. C o rley RP, Plom in R, De fries ) C ( 1')94 ). freud S (J 940 ), An outline of psychoanalysis. In: Sta nda rd Edition of thr
Con tin u ity an d change in infant shyness fro m 14 to 20 mont hs. Beha Comp lete Works of Sigm und Freud, Vo l. 23 , St rach ey ), ed . London :
(;enrt 24 :j65- .F ') H o garrh
Cicche rti D , Barn ell D ( 199 I ), Attach me nt orga ni zatio n in maltreated Goldsm ith HH, Buss AH, Plom in R ct al. (J 98 7 ), Roundtable : wh at is
p rescho o lers. D" . Psychopatho! 4:j,)7- 412 temperam ent ? f our ap proache s. Child D eu 58:50 5- 52 9
"C icch ett i D , Toth S ( \ ')') 5), A developmental psych ol ogy pers pec tive o n Crych ) H , Seed M . Finch am I'D (199) , C hi ldr en's co g ni t ive and afTec rive
chi ld abuse an d negle ct. J A m Acad C'h ild Adolesc Psychiatry j 4 :541 -565 resp ons es to different form s of inrerparcnral conflict. Pap er presented
Cohn ), Tronick E (1')8'), Speci ficiry of infants' resp onse to mothers' at the Society for Research and Child Development, Seattle
affective beh avior . J Am A cad O Jild Adolesc Psych iatry 28:242 -248 Gu n nar MR ( 199 0) , The psych o biology of infa nt temperam ent. In : Indi vid-
Co h n ) F, M atias R, Tronick E, Connell D , Lyons-Ru th K (1 986 ), Face- ual Dijfrrm m in InJitney: Reliability, Stability. Prediction. Colo m bo J.
to-fa ce interact ion s o f depre ssed mo thers and their infant s. In : Ma ternal Fagen ), cds. Hill sdale, N): Erlbaum, pp 387-409
Depression and IlIjimt D isturb ance. T ronick EZ , f ield T. eds, San Ha ller DL , Knisely j S, Da wso n KS. Sch no ll SH ( 199J). Perinatal substa nce
franc isco : jessey- Bass, pp j 1- 44 ab us e rs : psyc ho logica l a nd soc ia l cha racte ris tics. J N ero Ment Dis
Coil CG, Vohr BR, Hoffman) , Oh W (J 98 6 ), M atern al and environmental 181 :509 -5 U
factors affectin g devel o pmental ou tco me of infants of ad o lescent moth - Halpern R (1')') j ). Pov er ty and in fant devel opment. In : Handbook ofInfa nt
ers. J f)". Brha Pediatr 7:B O- 2j 5 M ental H ealth, Zeanah C H. cd . New York : Guilford . p p 7., - 86
Cox M) , O wen MT, Lew is JM , H end erso n VK (J 989 ), M arr iage. ad ult H an CG , DeW ol f M . W ozn iak P, BUllS DC (19 92) . M aternal and paternal
adjus tment , and ea rly pa renti ng . Child De 60: 10 15- 1024 d isciplinary sty les: relat io ns wit h pr esch ooler s' playground beha vio ral
C rit ten d en PM , Partridge Ml-, Clausen AH ( 1')9 1), Fam ily panern s of ori enta tions and peer stat us. Child Deu 63:87 9- 892
rela t io nshi p in norm at ive a nd d ysfun ct ion al fa m ili es. Dr Psy cho- H echtman L ( 1989 ). Teena ge mot hers and their chi ld ren: risks and prob-
patho! j:4,) I -S U lems. A review. Call J Psychiatry 34: 569 -575

176 J. A M . ACAD . C H ILD ADO L ESC. PSYC HI ATRY .%:2. FE B RU A RY 199 7


INFAN T D EVELOPM E NTAL RI SK R EVI EW

Ho row itz B. Woloc k I (I9 85). Ma te rial depr ivati on , child maltreat m ent . Osofsky JD, H ann DM . Peebles C (1993) , Adolescent parenthood : risks
and agency interventio ns among poor fami lies. In : The Social Context and opportun iti es for moth ers and infa nts . In : Handb ook of Injitnt
of Chi ld Abuse and Neglect, Pelton L, ed . New Yor k: Human Science M ental Health , Ze an ah C H, ed, N ew York: Guilford . PI' 106-11 9
Pr ess. PI' 137-1 84 Panerh NS (I 995). T he pr obl em of low birth we ight. In : The Future of
Hossain Z. Field T . Gonzalez '1', M alphurs J . D el Valle C (I994 ). Infants Children: Low Birthwright , Behrman RE. ed, Los Altos, CA : D avid
o f "dep ressed " m others interact better with th eir nondcpressed farh ers. and Lucill e Packard Foundation. PI' I 1-34
Inft nt M rnt Health J 15:348-3 57 Par ker S. Greer S. Z uckerma n B (1988 ). D ouble jeo pardy: the im pact of
Howes P. M arkman HJ (198 9). M ar ital qu ality and ch ild functioni ng : a p o vert y o n ea rly c h i ld d ev elo p me nt. Prdiat r C li n N orth Am
lon gitudinal invest igation. Child Drl l 60: 1044-1051 35:122 7- 1240
jouriles EN. Murphy C M. Farris AM . Smith DA . Richter J E, W ater s E Passino AW. W h it man '1'1.. Borkowski JG et al. (1993), Personal adjus tment
(199 1), Marital adjus tm ent , parental d isagreements about ch ild rearing . duri ng pre gn ancy and ado lescent parent ing. A dolescence 28 :97- 122
and behavior pro b lem s in boys: increasing the speci ficity of the mari tal Pelaez-Noguer as M . Field '1', Ci gales M . Gonzalez A, C1asky S (199 4),
assessm ent. Child De 6 2:1424- 143 3 In fants o f depressed m others show less " depressed" be havior with a
Jouriles EN. Pfiffner LJ . O'Leary SG (I98 8). Marital confl ict, parenting, fam iliar care giver . Infimt Mmt Health J 15:358 - 367
and toddler conduct problems. J Abnorm Child Psycho! 16: 197-206 Piagct J (I952 ). Tbe Origins of Intelligenc in Children. C ook M, trans.
Kagan J, Reznick JS. Sn id m an N (I987), The ph ysiology and psycho logy N ew York : International U niversities Press (o riginal work published
of behavi ora l inhi bitio n in chi ld ren . Child Deu 58 :145 9-1 473 in 1936 )
Kagan J, Rezn ick JS . Sn id man N (198 8). Biological bases of ch ildhood Plom in R. Em de RN . Braun garr JM er al, (199 3). G enetic change and
shyness. Science 240:167- 173 co nti nu ity from fourteen to twenty months: the M acArthu r Longitudinal
Kagan ] , Sni d m an N (199 1), T em peramental factors in human development . T win Study. Child Dr v 64 :1354 - 1376
A m PsychoI46:856-862 Poll itt E (1994), Poverty and chi ld de velopment: relevan ce o f research in
Kopp C B. Kaler SR (1989) . Risk in infa ncy: origi ns and impli catio ns. A m developing co unt ries ro th e Un ited States . Child Dev 6 5:283-295
Psy choI44:224-230 Porges SW (I 992 ). Vagal ton e: a physiologic marker of stre ss vuln erabil ity.
Korner AF, Stevenson DK, Kraemer HC er al. (I993), Pred icrion of the Pediatrics 90 :498-504
devel opment o f low birth weight preterm infant s by a new neonat al Ro bi nson JL . Kagan J, Rezn ick JS. Corley R (I 992) , The heritab ility of
med ical index. J De Behat. Pediatr 14: 106-1 11 inhibi ted and un inh ibited behav ior : a twin study. De Psycbol 28:1- 8
Kraem er H (1995). Ad vances in tech n ique s for data ana lysis: signal d etec tion. Rodn ing C. Beckw ith L. H oward J (1990) . C harac teristics of attac h me nt
Presentation at th e ann ual K-award ees meet ing . N IM H , Rockville. MD o rgan ization and play organi zation in pren atally drug-exposed toddlers.
Lester BM , Tronick EZ (1994). The effect s of prenat al coca ine exp osur e De Psychopathol 1:277- 289
an d chi ld o utcome . Inftnt Mmt Hralth J 15:107-1 20 Rosenbaum J F, Bied erm an J . Ge rsten M et al. ( 1988 ). Beh av ioral in hibitio n
"Lyo ns-Ru th K (1995). Broaden ing o ur co ncep t ual fram eworks : can we in children of parents w ith panic disor der and agora pho bia. Arch Grn
re- int roduce relat io nal strategies and impl icit representational system s Psychiatry 45 :463 - 470
ro th e stud y of psychopathology. Deu Psycho! 31:432-436 Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA (I 994), Tcrnperarnent and th e develop ment of
Lyon s-Ruth K. C o nn ell DB. G run eba um H . Bor ein S (1990). Infants at pe rsonality . J Abnorm Psycho! 103:55-66
social risk: maternal depre ssio n and fami ly sup po rt services as med iat ors Ru tter M ( 1987). Psychosocial resilience and protect ive mecha nis ms . Am
of infant dev elo pm ent and secu rity of attach m ent . Child D,, 6 1:85- 98 J O rth opsychia try 57:.~ 16-331
Lyon s-R uth K, Co nnell DB , Zo ll D . Stah l J (I987). In fant at social risk: Rutter M (1989). T em peram ent : con ceptual issues and im plications . In :
relat io ns among infa nt maltreatme nt , mat ern al beh avio r, and infa nt Temperammt in Childhood, Kohnsramm GA . Bate s J E, Rothbart M K.
att achment beh avio r. D" , Psychol 23:223 -232 eds , N ew Yor k: W iley, PI' 463 -479
Mahler M. Pine F. Bergman A (197 5), Th Psychological Birth oftilt Human "Sarneroff AJ (in pre ss). Understanding the social context of early psychopa -
Inftnt. New York: Basic Books thology. In : Handbook of Child and Adolescrnt Psychiatry, Noshp irz J.
Main M . Goldwyn R (in pre ss), Interview-based adult attac h ment clas- ed. New Yor k: Basic Books
sifications: relate d ro infant-mother an d infa nt - fat he r att ach ment. Samero ff AJ. Seifer R, Baro cas R, Zax M, G reens pa n S (l987). Intelligen ce
Drv Psycho] qu otient scores o f four yea r old ch ildren: social-env ironmental risk
M cKenry PC . Kot ch JB , Browne D H (199 1). Co rrelates of dysfun cti on al factors. Pediatrics 7 9:34 3- 350
parentin g attitudes amo ng low-in com e ado lescent mothers. J Adolrsc Sampson PD. Srreissgu rh AI' . Barr HM. Bookstein FL (1989), Ne u ro behav-
Rrs 6:2 12-234 io ral effect s of prenatal alco ho l expos ure. Part 11. Par tial least squares
M cLoyd VC (1990) . T he im pac t o f eco no mic hardship o n b lack fami lies analysis. Ne urobehau ToxicoITrratol l l :477-49 1
and chi ld ren : psychol og ical d istre ss. parent ing. and socio crno rional Sanson A. Oberklaid F. Pedl ow R. Pr ior M (199 1). Risk ind icators:
de velopment. Child Drv 61:311-346 assessment of infancy pred ictors ofpre school behavioural maladjustment.
Miller SM . Lewis M (1990). Handbook of Deuelopmental Psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 32 :609- 6 26
Ne w York: Plen um Sch nei der-Ros en K. Cicchetti D (I 99 1). Early self-knowledge and em oti on al
Minde K (1993). Prematuri ty an d serious med ical illness in infancy: d evelopment: visual self-recogni tio n and affective react ion s to m irror
impl ications for development and intervent ion . In: H andbook ofInjitnt self- images in m altreated and non -m altrea ted toddlers . D". Psy rhol
Mental Health , Zea na h C H . ed . New York: G u ilford 27:47 1-4 78
Mrazek PJ, Haggerry RJ (I 994) , Reducing Risks fOr Men tal D isorders. Seifer R (1995), Perils and pit falls of h igh -risk research . Dev Psycho!
W ash ingron , D C : Nat ional Academ y Press 31 :420 -424
Murray I. (199 2). The im pact of postn at al depression on infa nt development . Seifer R. D ickstein S (1993). Parental mental illness and infa nt de velop ment.
J Child Psycbol Psychiatry 33 :543-561 In : Handbo ok of lnftnt Mental Health, Z eanah C H . ed. New York:
Nachrnias M. Gunnar MR. Mangelsdorf S. Parritz RH , Buss K (1996). G uilford . PI' 120-142
Beh avioral inhibit ion and stre ss reactivity: mod erating role of attachm ent Seifer R, Sameroff AJ, An agn ostopolou R. Elias pK (1992) , Child and
secu rity . Child Drv 67 :508 -522 fami ly facto rs th at am elior ate risk betw een 4 and J3 years of age. J
O'Connor MJ. Sig man M. Brill N (198 7). D iso rgan izat ion of attach m ent Am A cad Child Adolac Prychiatry j l :893-903
in relat ion to mat ern al alco ho l co ns um ptio n . J Clin Consult Psycho! Seifer R. Samero ff AJ. Barr ell L. Krafchu k E (l 994) . Infant temperam ent
55:831-836 measured by m ultiple obse rvations an d mother report. Child Deu
O 'Hara M W . Zekoski EM. Ph illips AH , W right EJ (1990). C o ntrolled 65 : 1478- 1490
p rospecti ve st udy o f p o stp artum m o o d disord e rs: com p ar iso n of Shaw DS. Vo nd ra JI (1993). C h ro n ic family adversity and infa nt attachm ent.
chi ld bea ring and no n-child bear ing women . J Abnorm PsychoI 99:3- 15 J Child Psychol Psychiatry 34: 120 5- 12 15

J. AM . ACA D . C H I L D A DOI. ESC. PSYCH I ATR Y, .\(>: 2, FEB RUA RY 1997 177
ZEANAH ET AL.

Shaw DS, Vondra JI (1995), Infant attachment security and maternal van ljzendoorn M (l995), Adult attachment representations, parental
predictors of early behavior problems: a longirudinal study of low responsiveness. and infant anachment: a meta-analysis on the predictive
income families. J Child Psycho/ Psychiatry 23:335-357 validity of the adult attachment interview. Psycho/ Bull I 17:387-403
'Shaw DS. Vondra JI . Hommerding D. Keenan K. Dunn M (I 994}, Ward MJ , Carlson EA (1995), Associations among adult attachment
Chronic family adversity and early child behav ior problems: a longitudi- representations, maternal sensiriviry , and infant-mother attachment in
nal stud y of low in come families . J Child Psycho/ Psychiatry a sample of adolescent mothers. Child Dev 66:69-80
35 :1109-1122 Weinberger K Tronick E (in press), Maternal depression and infant malad-
Snidman N, Kagan J, Riordan L, Shannon DC (1995). Cardiac function justment: a mutual regulation model. In : Handbook of Child and
and behavioral reactivity during infancy . Psychophysiology 32 :199-207
Adolescent Psychiatry. Noshpirz J, ed. New York: Basic Books
Spieker SJ, Bensley L ( 1994), Roles ofliving arrangements and grandmother
Weiss B, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS (l992), Some consequences of early
social suppon in adolescent mothering and infant attachment. Deu
harsh discipline: child aggression and a maladaptive social information
Psycho/ 30: 102-111
processing sryle. Child Deu 63:1321-1335
Stansbury K. Gunnar MR (1994), Adrenocortical activity and emotional
regulation. Monogr Soc Res Child Deu 59:108-134 Werner E (l989) , High-risk children in young adulthood: a longitudinal
'Steele H, Steele M. Fonagy P (1996), Associations among attachment study from birth to 32 years. Am J Orthopsychiatry 59:72-81
classificarions in mothers, Fathers and their infants. Child Deu Whitman TL, Borkowski JG. Schellenbach CJ, Nath PS (1987), Predicting
67 :541-555 and understanding developmental delay of children of adolescent moth-
Stern D (1985) , Thelnterprnonal Worldoflhrlnftnt. New York: Basic Books ers: a multidimensional approach. Am J Ment Defic 92:40-56
Tennes K, Kreye M (1985), Children's adrenocortical responses to classroom Wise PH, Meyers A (l988), Poverty and child health . Pediasr C/in North
activities and tests in elementary school. Psychosom Mtd 47:451-460 Am 35:1169-1186
Teri DM. Gelfand DM (1991). Behavioral competence among mothers Zahn-Waxler C. Kochanska G, Ktupnik J, McKnew D (I 990}, Patterns
of infants in the first year: the mediational role of maternal self-efficacy. of guilt in children of depressed and well mothers. Dev Psycho/26:51-59
Child Deu 62:918-929 Zeanah CH. Anders TF, Seifer R. Stern DN (l989), Implications of
Ten DM, Gelfand DM , Messinger DS, Isabella R (1995), Maternal research on infant development for psychodynamic theory and practice .
depression and the qualiry of early attachment: an examination of JAm Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 28:657-668
infant s, preschoolers and their mothers Deu Psycho/31:364-376 Zeanah CH, Hirshberg L, Danis B, Brennan M (1994), Partner violence
Thomas A, Chess S (1977) , Temperament and Development. New York:
and infant attachment. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the
Brunner/Mazel
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, New York
Tronick EZ, Frank DA, Cabral H. Zuckerman BS (in press), A dose-
Zeanah CH, Hirshberg L, Danis B er al, (l995), The specificiry of the
response effect of in Utero cocaine exposure on infant neurobehavioral
Adult Attachment Interview in a high-risk sample . In : Sodrl).fOr Research
functioning. Pediatrics
US General Accounting Office (1990), Drug-Exposed Inftnts: A Generation in Child Development Abstracts, p 222
at Risk. Report to the Chairman, Committee on Finance. US Senate.
Washington , DC: US Government Printing Office

178 J. AM . ACAD . CHILD ADOLESC . PSYCHIATRY. 36 :2. FEBRUARY 1997

Anda mungkin juga menyukai