Anda di halaman 1dari 10

Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Accident Analysis and Prevention


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aap

Inuence of trafc enforcement on the attitudes and behavior of drivers


Predrag Stanojevic a , Dragan Jovanovic b, , Timo Lajunen c,d
a
Polytechnic School of Vocational Studies, Zvecan, Serbia
b
Faculty of Technical Sciences, Department of Transport, Novi Sad, Serbia
c
Department of Psychology, Middle East Technical University, Ankara, Turkey
d
Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Traditionally, trafc enforcement has been an important means of improving trafc safety. Many studies
Received 16 April 2012 have provided evidence of connections between the level of police enforcement and both driving behavior
Received in revised form and the number of trafc accidents. In Northern Kosovo, there has been little, if any, trafc enforcement
14 December 2012
during the last 13 years. This situation has created a very rare research opportunity it is now possible to
Accepted 17 December 2012
directly study the inuence of trafc enforcement on the attitudes and behavior of drivers by comparing
two regions, one with trafc enforcement and one without it (Serbia vs. Northern Kosovo). The sample
Keywords:
in the present study consisted of 424 drivers (204 from Serbia and 220 from Northern Kosovo). Ques-
Enforcement
Behavior
tionnaires included items about the behaviors of the drivers (e.g., speeding, seat belt, drunk driving) and
Attitudes their attitudes. We also conducted eld observations of driving behavior. The results indicated that the
Accidents lack of enforcement affects almost every type of behavior that we examined. Compared with drivers in
Serbia, drivers in Northern Kosovo drive faster, exceed speed limits more frequently, use seat belts less
frequently, drive after exceeding the legal limit for alcohol more often, commit aggressive and ordinary
violations more frequently and are generally involved in more risky situations.
2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction and always using protective devices (e.g., seatbelts) could result
in avoiding as many as 38% of fatalities and 17% of other traf-
Trafc accidents are a leading cause of death in many countries. c injuries. Evans (1991) has estimated that the number of trafc
It is a well-established fact that the vast majority of all trafc acci- fatalities in the United States could be reduced by approximately
dents are caused by various types of human factors (Lewin, 1982; 40% if drunk driving alone was eliminated. Estimates based on sta-
Rumar, 1985). Trafc law violations are usually recognized as an tistical analyses of data from empirical studies suggest that these
important component of the set of human factors that contributes gures are much lower; a 10% reduction in trafc fatalities is at
to an increased risk of causing an accident. Previous research has the upper end of the range of possible improvements. However,
established the links between risky driving behaviors and accident some well-implemented and sustained enforcement efforts have
liability (e.g., Parker et al., 1995; Taylor et al., 2000). The most sig- led to signicant safety gains resulting to an improvement of 25%
nicant behavioral problems that are associated with accidents are (according to Zaidel, 2002).
also the ones that have the most serious consequences: speeding, The trafc laws and regulations that dene socially accept-
drunk driving and failing to wear seat belts (Peden et al., 2004; able road user behavior are important elements in developing a
Schick, 2007). secure road environment and when applied, they are generally con-
Traditionally, trafc enforcement has appeared to be an impor- sidered one of the most effective ways of improving road users
tant means of improving trafc safety. It has been suggested that behaviors (Zaal, 1994). Police enforcement has proven effective in
adequate police enforcement that encourages full respect for and reducing both the mean speeds and the variance in speed on var-
adherence to trafc laws and regulations could decrease the num- ious roadways (Holland and Conner, 1996; Vaa, 1997; De Waard
ber of accidents by as much as 50% (ETSC, 1999). In the Trafc and Rooijers, 1994). In their recent literature review, Elliott and
Safety Handbook, Elvik et al. (1997) estimate that full compli- Broughton (2005) concluded that increasing the level of police con-
ance with speed regulations, avoiding driving while intoxicated trol results in both a decrease in average road speed (approximately
3 mph) and a decrease in the percentage of drivers who exceed the
speed limit (by between 3% and 64%). Recent research conrms
Corresponding author. Tel.: +381 21 4852494; fax: +381 21 450644. that enforcement has effects on speeding. Walter et al. (2011) sum-

E-mail address: draganj@uns.ac.rs (D. Jovanovic). marized the achieved effects of intensifying enforcement over a

0001-4575/$ see front matter 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.12.019
30 P. Stanojevic et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938

period of four weeks. The speed of vehicles decreased systemati- The level of police enforcement, driving behavior and the num-
cally during the implementation of enforcement along a road and ber of trafc accidents have been investigated in several studies.
in surrounding areas, and some of the effects could still be observed However, the results of these studies seem to be mixed; some stud-
two weeks after the enforcement campaign ended. In a study of the ies have shown some effects of trafc enforcement, whereas others
choice of driving speed on roads in rural areas, Ryeng (2011) found did not nd any signicant relation between enforcement and traf-
that increasing the amount of police enforcement resulted in speed c accidents and behavior. Weiss and Freels (1996) showed that
reductions, whereas stricter sanctions affected a drivers choice of increasing the amount of trafc enforcement had no effect on trafc
speed only marginally. accidents. Similarly, Tarko (2008) found that increased amount of
In addition to speeding, previous research shows that police police enforcement had only a minor effect on speed. A statistically
enforcement has a signicant effect on driving while intoxicated. signicant reduction in number of trafc accidents was reported
Wechsler et al. (2003) showed that the occurrence of driving while in a study conducted in Israel (Hakkert et al., 2001). It should be
intoxicated differed signicantly according to both the policy envi- noted that the increased enforcement applied in this study covered
ronments at local and regional levels and the degree to which those 700 km of interurban roads and lasted a year.
policies were enforced. Elliott and Broughton (2005) analyzed the The conicting results about enforcement, behavior and acci-
results of eleven studies about driving under the inuence of alco- dents may be due to several methodological differences. First, the
hol and found that enforcement had resulted in a large reduction in data regarding the degree to which trafc enforcement affects driv-
the incidence of drunk driving offenses. A study in Greece (Yannis ing behavior, the number of trafc accidents and the number of
et al., 2008) showed that an increase in the number of police con- fatalities vary widely. Second, many studies have not presented
trols led to a reduction in the number of incidents of driving while the degree to which enforcement was increased. This lack of data
under the inuence of alcohol. The authors also illustrated this makes it difcult to interpret the real effects of enforcement and
change of behavior with data that were obtained from the SARTRE makes dening the optimal level of enforcement impossible. Third,
2 and 3 surveys: the number of drunk driving infringements was the research literature seems to suggest that the effects of increas-
reduced by approximately 50%, and the positive attitudes toward ing the amount of trafc law enforcement can only be achieved
controlling alcohol usage increased by 10.2%. The report of the within restricted areas that are in the foci of enforcement experi-
ESCAPE (2003) consortium concluded that the countries that have ments, but not at a national level (e.g., Elvik, 2001; Goldenbeld et al.,
long traditions of enforcing drunk driving laws, low legally permit- 1999; Zaal, 1994).
ted blood alcohol concentrations, and a relatively high objective The effects of enforcement are usually very short-lived. Road
risk of detection and enforcement that is supported by the mass users tend to modify their behavior only in enforcement areas or in
media have the lowest numbers of cases of driving under the inu- areas in which they anticipate the risk of being punished. In their
ence of alcohol. review of a large number of speed enforcement studies, stvik and
It is known that the best results regarding seat belt usage are Elvik (1991) concluded that unless the intensication of enforce-
obtained by means of establishing laws on the mandatory use of ment more than tripled the initial enforcement levels, it had little
seat belts that make failing to use one a punishable offense. For or no effect on the subjective risk of getting caught. Therefore, one
example, in Finland and Germany, measures to the enforce seat of the main problems with intensifying enforcement could be the
belt laws and legal sanctions for increasing the level of seat belt lack of police manpower that is sufcient to cover the entire net-
use occurred many years after the laws that required the mandatory work of roads, and as a result, drivers judge the risk of punishment
use of seat belts had been enacted. The effect of law enforcement on to be small. For example, trafc rules are observed very closely in
seat belt usage in both countries was drastic; the rates of seat belt the immediate vicinity of a surveillance unit; even speeds that are
usage increased from 40% to 90% (Valtonen, 1991; Heinrich, 1991). below the posted limit may decrease in these circumstances. How-
In his report on the effects of implementing legislation that requires ever, as soon as drivers have passed the surveillance site, they begin
drivers and front seat passengers to use seat belts in Saudi Arabia in increasing their speeds again. Thus, surveillance effects last short
the year 2000, Bendak (2005) reported a large increase in the num- time, whether they are measured in time or space (Goldenbeld et al.,
ber of drivers who wore seat belts. Prior to the introduction of new 1999; Oei, 1998; Zaal, 1994). Based on review of a large number of
legislation, only 2.9% of drivers reported wearing seat belts and, studies, Elliott and Broughton (2005) argued that the halo effect of
during the course of the study, this number increased by 60%. How- police presence on driving speeds may last as little as 1 h or as long
ever, it should be noted that the highest usage rates are typically as 8 weeks after the police activity has ceased. Some studies seem
achieved immediately following the introduction of new legisla- to indicate an association between larger temporal halo effects and
tion. This is followed by a steady decline in usage after the rst few longer periods of police presence. However, Elliott and Broughton
months, but usage does not generally fall to pre-legislation levels concluded that even when police presence lasts for long periods
(Williams and Wells, 2004). This effect is most often seen when of time, the temporal halo effect can only be expected to last for
the new legislation is not complemented by increases in police a limited period of time. This suggests that sustained enforcement
enforcement and by mass media publicity campaigns. In Canada, efforts are needed to inuence drivers behavior.
seat belt usage was measured at 75% immediately after the intro- The aforementioned researchers have argued that the intensi-
duction of mandatory seat belt laws, and it dropped to 50% six cation of trafc enforcement affects risky driving behavior and the
months later (Robertson, 1978). A similar situation was reported number of accidents. The question about enforcement and behav-
in Germany (Heinrich, 1991), in which the rates of wearing seat ior can be addressed also in opposite way: what would happen
belts increased dramatically with the introduction of the manda- if no trafc enforcement at all were present within a given area.
tory use legislation, then declined in the subsequent absence of law To our knowledge, only two studies have measured the effects of
enforcement. The observed decline in usage rates was most pro- trafc enforcement on trafc safety in a situation in which there
nounced during the rst 12 months after the new legislation had was no police enforcement whatsoever. During 1978, the Nashville
been introduced. These results indicate that the best improvements trafc police force had intensied enforcement activity for two
in seat belt usage levels are not achieved merely by changing the months, but the members of it subsequently went on a short-
law. It is equally important to show drivers that compliance with term slow-down strike. Carr et al. (1980) report the results from
the law is an important aspect of safety-oriented road user behav- that natural experiment. Examination of trafc accident data that
ior by making non-compliance (failing to use seat belts) punishable were collected throughout these periods found that there were no
(Mkinen et al., 1991). changes in either the frequency of accidents that were reported
P. Stanojevic et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938 31

or in the distribution of accident types involving property dam- Table 1


Demographic variables.
age, personal injury, or fatalities. Hence, temporary lapses in trafc
enforcement do not seem to have documented detrimental effects. Northern Kosovo Serbia
However, in February of 1976, the Finnish police went on strike Response rate 73.3% 68%
for approximately two weeks, during which police enforcement Gender
was completely suspended. Summala and Roine (1980) conducted Male (%) 148 (67.3) 130 (63.7)
a small observation study during the strike and found that there Female (%) 72 (32.7) 74 (36.3)
Mean age (S.D.) 35.13 (10.47) 33.92 (10.85)
was only a slight increase in the mean speed, but the percentage
Mean driving experience in years (S.D.) 12.72 (8.73) 11.32 (9.51)
of gross speed violations (>10 km/h) increased by 50 to 100%, and Mean annual mileage in km (S.D.) 12,987 (15,432) 8663 (15,909)
the standard deviation of the recorded speeds increased by approx- Number of accidents
imately 20%, which suggests that the risk of trafc accidents was Range 05 07
Mean (S.D.) 0.40 (0.84) 0.26 (0.80)
also elevated.
After the NATO intervention in the Federal Republic of
Yugoslavia in 1999 that resulted in the establishment of the inter-
their answers would remain anonymous and condential and that
national protectorate of Kosovo and Metohija, special conditions
any data that they provided would be used exclusively for scientic
were created in which government institutions do not have their
purposes.
normal functions. Northern Kosovo refers to a territory in the north-
ern part of the disputed region of Kosovo, with an ethnic Serb
majority that functions independently from the remainder of the 2.2. Measures
region, which has an ethnic Albanian majority. Northern Kosovo has
a direct border with Central Serbia. Serbian institutions, as part of 2.2.1. Perceptions of trafc enforcement
political, health care, cultural and educational system of Serbia still We rst examined each drivers perception of trafc enforce-
operate in the region of Northern Kosovo in the same degree as in ment. Respondents were asked questions about their perception of
Serbia. Law enforcement is carried out by International forces and the possibility to be punished by police due to committing trafc
Kosovo Police. In Northern Kosovo, trafc enforcement has essen- violations while driving (speeding, failing to wear a seat belt and
tially been absent for the last 13 years. This situation created a very drunk driving). Respondents answered by rating the frequency that
rare opportunity to conduct research about the inuence of trafc each item applied to them on a ve-point Likert scale that ranged
enforcement on the attitudes and behavior of drivers by comparing from 0 to 4 (0 = never; 4 = almost always).
the behavior and attitudes of drivers in two regions, one of which
has trafc enforcement, and one of which does not. In the current 2.2.2. Driver behavior
study, we compared drivers from Northern Kosovo (where trafc Driving behavior was measured in three ways. First, 27 items
laws are not enforced) with drivers from Serbia (where trafc law from the SARTRE 3 project (2004) were used to survey the drivers
enforcement has been implemented). about their speeding habits, use of seat belts, and alcohol consump-
It is worth noting that the situation in Northern Kosovo differs tion before driving. The set of items that were used and the possible
from the aforementioned situation in Nashville (Tennessee) and responses for each item are shown in Table 2.
Finland in which the police force went on strike. In both Finland and Second, we used Driver Behavior Questionnaire (DBQ) (Lawton
Nashville, the special conditions with lacking trafc law enforce- et al., 1997; Parker et al., 1998) to measure each drivers frequency
ment lasted for relatively short periods of time and, thus, could of violations, lapses and errors while driving. This questionnaire
not have effect on drivers values and trafc culture. In Northern consisted of a total of 27 items. Three of these items were designed
Kosovo, however, the long-term absence of law enforcement may to measure aggressive violations ((N. Kos. sample) = 0.54; (Ser.
truly change the manner in which people drive, and even their sample) = 0.58) (e.g., sounding the horn to indicate annoyance), 8
attitudes toward road safety. items were used to measure general violations ((N. Kos. sam-
The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects ple) = 0.74; (Ser. sample) = 0.76) (e.g., pulling so far into a junction
of (lacking) trafc enforcement on driver behavior by comparing or intersection that the driver with right of way must stop and let
non-enforcement region (Northern Kosovo) to a region in which the respondent pass), 8 items were used to measure omissions ((N.
enforcement is applied (Serbia). Kos. sample) = 0.72; (Ser. sample) = 0.77) (e.g., underestimating
the speed of an oncoming vehicle when overtaking another vehicle)
and 8 items were used to measure errors ((N. Kos. sample) = 0.74;
2. Methods (Ser. sample) = 0.70) (e.g., forgetfulness about where in a parking
lot a car had been left). Respondents were asked to indicate how
2.1. Sample often they themselves committed each of the violations and made
each of the errors when driving. Responses were recorded on a six-
The data for this study were collected by mail. The names point Likert scale that ranged from 0 to 5 (0 = never; 5 = almost all
and addresses of 600 individuals (300 from Serbia and 300 from the time). Cronbachs for aggressive violations was small (0.54
Northern Kosovo) with valid driving licenses were obtained from and 0.58), but could be acceptable given the small number of items
a registry of owners of motor vehicles. A set of research mate- that assessed aggressive violations.
rials that contained the questionnaires and a cover letter that Third, we conducted eld observations of driving behavior
explained the purpose and objectives of the research was sent (speeding, using of seat belts and headlights on daytime). Trained
to each member of the aforementioned sample group. We also observers (one per observation point) recorded if drivers were
included a prepaid envelope that could be used to return the com- using a seatbelt in built-up or non built-up areas. We observed the
pleted questionnaires. We received 424 completed questionnaires cars in places where drivers had to slow down because of trafc
(204 from individuals who were living in Serbia and 220 from indi- signs, so there was enough time to observe and record if driver
viduals who were living in Northern Kosovo). The demographic was using a seat belt. Driving speeds and headlight use were mea-
characteristics of the sample can be found in Table 1. The partici- sured simultaneously by two observers, seated in the same car on
pants were instructed to read all of the questions carefully and to roadside. One observer used radar to measure driving speed on
reply each item honestly. The participants were also ensured that roads with speed limits of 60 km/h and 80 km/h. The other observer
32 P. Stanojevic et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938

Table 2
SARTRE Project items that were used in the present study.

Variable name Symbol Description

Drivers perceptions of their own speeding behavior and driving safety


Question 6 Speed 1a Compared to other drivers, do you think your driving is. . .dangerous?
Question 8 Speed 2b Compared with other drivers, do you generally drive. . .than average speed?

Perceptions of other drivers speeding behavior


Question 7 Speed 3c How often do you think other drivers break speed limits?

Self-reported speeding behavior


Question 9b Speed 4c How often do you drive faster than the speed limit on main roads between towns?
Question 9c Speed 5c How often do you drive faster than the speed limit on country roads?
Question 9d Speed 6c How often do you drive faster than the speed limit in built-up areas?

Reported behaviors related to speed


Question 13c Speed 7c How often do you drive through a trafc light that is yellow?
Question 13d Speed 8c How often do you overtake when you think you can barely manage it?

Attitudes toward enforcement and countermeasures


Question 10b Speed 9d Compared to the present limits, what do you think the speed limit on main roads between towns should be?
Question 10c Speed 10d Compared to the present limits, what do you think the speed limit on country roads should be?
d
Question 10d Speed 11 Compared to the present limits, what do you think the speed limit in built-up areas should be?
e
Question 31a Speed 12 How much would you be in favor of afxing speed-limiting devices to cars that prevented the drivers of them from
exceeding the speed limit?
Question 31c Speed 13e How much would you be in favor of using a black box to record behavior from a driver that the police could then use
as evidence to prove speeding/dangerous driving?
Question 34b Speed 14e How much would you be in favor of the use of automated cameras in the surveillance of speed excesses?

Seat belt use in different environments


Question 15b Belts 1c How often do you wear a seat belt when making a journey on main roads between towns?
Question 15c Belts 2c How often do you wear a seat belt when making a journey on country roads?
Question 15d Belts 3c How often do you wear a seat belt when making a journey in a built-up area?

Attitudes toward wearing seat belts


Question 17a Belts 4e If you drive carefully, seat belts are not really necessary.
Question 17b Belts 5e In most accidents, seat belts reduce the risk of serious injury to both drivers and passengers.
Question 17c Belts 6e When I am not wearing my seat belt, I feel less comfortable; I feel as though something is missing.
Question 17d Belts 7e There is a risk of being trapped by a seat belt in the event of an emergency.

Drinking behavior and drunk driving behavior


Question 19 Alcohol 1f In general how many days per week do you drink alcoholic beverages?
Question 20 Alcohol 2f How many days per week do you drive after drinking even a small amount of alcohol?
Question 21 Alcohol 3f Over the last week, how many days did you drive, when you may have been over the legal limit for drinking and
driving?

Opinions about alcohol legislation and countermeasures


Question 22 Alcohol 4g Do you think that drivers should be allowed to drink?
Question 28e Alcohol 5e How much would you be in favor of not allowing new drivers to drink any alcohol before driving?
Question 30d Alcohol 6e Would you nd it useful to have a device on your car, like an alcohol-meter, that could be used to determine whether
you had been drinking and that could prevent you from driving if you were over the legal blood alcohol limit?
a
Possible responses for these questions were much more, a bit more, about the same, a bit less and a lot less.
b
Possible responses for these questions were much faster, a little faster, about average, a little slower and much slower.
c
Possible responses for these questions were never, rarely, sometimes, often, very often and always.
d
Possible responses for these questions were lower, same, higher and no limit.
e
Possible responses for these questions were very, fairly, not much and not at all.
f
Possible responses for these questions were every day, 5 to 6, 3 to 4, 1 or 2, <1 and never.
g
Possible responses for these questions were no alcohol at all, less alcohol than at present, as much alcohol as at present, more alcohol than at present and as much as
they want.

recorded if passing vehicles had lights switched on. The observation attitude toward speed (7 items, (N. Kos. sample) = 0.84; (Ser.
whether the drivers had switched on the headlights was performed sample) = 0.86) (e.g., If you have good skills, speeding is OK, or
because headlight use is mandatory by law in both regions. These Driving 5 or 10 miles above the speed limit is OK because every-
data were used as an additional indicator of adherence to traf- one does it) and the attitude toward drinking and driving (5 items,
c laws. All measuring and observations were conducted during (N. Kos. sample) = 0.77; (Ser. sample) = 0.74) (e.g., People can
daylight (08.0017.00). drive safely even when they are under the inuence of alcohol, or
Ride with a person you know has been drinking too much alco-
hol). The attitude scales were based on questionnaire developed
2.2.3. Drivers attitudes by Ulleberg and Rundmo (2003) and Iversen and Rundmo (2004).
Drivers attitudes were measured using a questionnaire that
consisted of 18 items. Their responses were given on a 5-point
scale from completely false (1) to completely true (5). We used this 2.2.4. Demographic variables
questionnaire to test the following attitudes: the attitude toward Respondents answered questions about their age, sex, involve-
violation of trafc regulations (6 items, (N. Kos. sample) = 0.71; ment in risky situations (situations in which they had nearly had
(Ser. sample) = 0.79) (e.g., There are many trafc rules which accidents) over the previous 3 months, accident involvement dur-
cannot be obeyed in order to keep up the trafc ow, or Some- ing the previous 3 years, the number of years that they had been
times it is necessary to bend the rules to keep trafc going), the licensed to drive and estimated their annual mileage.
P. Stanojevic et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938 33

Table 3 drove after exceeding the legal limit for alcohol (Alcohol 3) more
Numbers and percentages of drivers who perceived the risk of being punished (fre-
often than drivers from Serbia. However, there were no differences
quently or almost always) for specic trafc violation and mean score of possibility
of being punished by police. in responses at question if drivers should be allowed to drink or
attitudes toward various counter measures.
Northern Kosovo Serbia
t-Test statistics were used to compare data from Northern
n % Mean n % Mean Kosovo and Serbia regarding committing violations while driving
Speeding 4 1.81 0.16 44 21.56 1.91 that was obtained from answers to the DBQ, attitudes toward traf-
Alcohol 3 1.36 0.13 24 11.76 1.32 c safety, and self-reported involvement in both trafc accidents
Seat belts 3 1.36 0.12 94 46.08 1.96 and risky situations. The results of this are shown in Table 5.
The t-test results showed that drivers in Northern Kosovo com-
mitted signicantly more aggressive and ordinary violations than
3. Results drivers in Serbia did and their attitudes toward trafc rules viola-
tions and speed were worse than those of Serbian drivers. Drivers
3.1. Perception of trafc enforcement from Northern Kosovo reported more involvement in risky situa-
tions, but no signicant differences in the number of self-reported
Table 3 shows the numbers and percentages of drivers who trafc accidents were reported between drivers from Northern
responded that they believed there was a possibility they would Kosovo and other drivers.
be frequently or almost always punished for committing driving In addition to analyzing the self-reported behavior of the drivers
violations (speeding, driving under the inuence alcohol and not who we surveyed, we conducted some observations of driving
wearing seat belts). The arithmetic averages of all of the responses behavior in the eld. We used radar to measure the speeds of vehi-
(04 averaged) in the regions that were compared are also shown cles that were traveling on roads with speed limit of 60 km/h and
in Table 3. 80 km/h. We also observed the use of seat belt and low beam lights
In the territory of Northern Kosovo, where there has not been during the day in these vehicles.
any trafc enforcement over a long period of time, the perception The driving speeds that were measured by radar varied between
of trafc enforcement is approximately equal to zero, which is evi- Serbian drivers and drivers from Northern Kosovo. In the zone that
dent from the mean values of all of the responses. Several of the had a speed limit of 60 km/h, the average speed in Northern Kosovo
respondents who reported that there was a possibility that those was 3.4 km/h higher than average speed in Serbia, and the number
who committed trafc violations would be punished frequently or of drivers who exceeded the speed limit was 10% higher. In the
very often can be drivers who frequently travel to Serbia. These zone in which the speed limit was 80 km/h, the differences were
drivers may have given these kinds of responses because they were even larger. Average speed in Northern Kosovo was approximately
answering the questionnaire in reference to their perceptions of 4.8 km/h higher than average speed in Serbia, and the number of
trafc enforcement in Serbia. drivers who exceeded the speed limit was 11% higher. We found
surprisingly large differences in seat belt usage between drivers
3.2. Comparison of the self-reported behavior and attitudes of the from Northern Kosovo and drivers from Serbia. Only 6% of the
drivers drivers wore seat belts in an urban area in Northern Kosovo, com-
pared with 51% of the drivers in a similar setting in Serbia. Outside
First, respondents were asked several questions regarding of urban areas in Northern Kosovo, 24% of the drivers wore seat
speeding, seat belt usage and alcohol consumption prior to driving belts, whereas 60% of the drivers in Serbia wore them. We mea-
that had been taken from the SARTRE 3 project (SARTRE, 2004). The sured the use of lights during the day as an additional indicator of
levels of signicance of the differences in the percentages of drives adherence to trafc laws. The results of this measure were similar
that reported various behaviors and perceptions were examined to the results of our observations of seat belt use. Only a small num-
using z-tests (two-tailed test) (Montgomery and Runger, 1994). The ber of drivers in Northern Kosovo (approximately 17%) switched on
results of this analysis are shown in Table 4. their lights during the day compared with the large percentage of
Signicant difference between the self-reported responses of drivers in Serbia who did so (90%).
drivers from Northern Kosovo and Serbia regarding questions about
speeding can only be seen for one question. The drivers from North-
4. Discussion
ern Kosovo feel that the speed limit should be higher on roads
between cities (Speed 9) to a lesser extent than drivers from Serbia.
The main aim of this study was to determine the extent to which
The greatest differences among the sample populations that we
the presence or absence of trafc enforcement affects the behavior
studied can be observed in the responses to questions regarding
and attitudes of drivers. A comparison was made between regions
seat belt usage. The drivers from Northern Kosovo reported a sig-
in which trafc laws were enforced and regions in which these laws
nicantly lower level of seat belt usage in various environments
were not enforced. In the following sections, we will consider the
in comparison to drivers from Serbia (Belts 1, Belts 2 and Belts
results that we obtained regarding differences in driver behavior
3). Furthermore, major differences in the opinions of these two
and attitudes (toward speeding, seat belt usage and alcohol con-
groups of drivers can be observed, particularly regarding whether
sumption prior to driving), the commission of trafc violations and
seat belts are necessary in cases of careful driving. More than 50% of
driver involvement in risky situations and trafc accidents.
the drivers from Northern Kosovo who we surveyed feel that seat
belts are not really necessary as long as one drives carefully (Belts
4). Also, drivers from Serbia reported to greater extent that they 4.1. Speed
feel less comfortable and something is missing if they do not that
wear seat belts than drivers from Northern Kosovo (Belts 6). Finaly, Although there were no differences in self-reported behaviors
drivers from Northern Kosovo are more convinsed that there is a between Northern Kosovo and Serbia regarding driving faster than
risk of being trapped by seat belt in case of emergency (Belts 7). the speed limit, the eld measurements of driving speed that were
Although it is evident that drivers from Northern Kosovo and taken by radar on main roads between towns with limits of 60 km/h
Serbia consume approximately equal amounts of alcohol each week and 80 km/h showed that the average speeds of drivers in North-
(Alcohol 1), the drivers from Northern Kosovo reported that they ern Kosovo were 3.4 km/h and 4.8 km/h faster than that of drivers
34 P. Stanojevic et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938

Table 4
Responses to the SARTRE Project items that were used in the study.

Nort. Kosovo Serbia z-test N.K. vs. Ser.

Drivers perceptions of their own safety and speeding behavior


Speed 1 Compared to other drivers, do you think your driving is. . .dangerousa 6.4 8.3 0.75
Speed 2 Compared with other drivers, do you generally drive. . .than average speedb 30.9 24.5 1.47

Perceptions of other drivers speeding behavior


Speed 3 How often do you think other drivers break speed limitsc 79.6 79.9 0.08

Self-reported speeding behavior


Speed 4 How often do you drive faster than the speed limit on main roads between townsc 27.3 23.5 0.90
Speed 5 How often do you drive faster than the speed limit on country roadsc 15.9 10.8 1.54
Speed 6 How often do you drive faster than the speed limit in built-up areasc 9.1 8.8 0.11

Reported behaviors that are related to speed


Speed 7 How often do you drive through a trafc light that is yellowc 15.9 14.7 0.34
Speed 8 How often do you overtake when you think you can barely manage itc 5.0 3.9 0.55

Attitudes toward enforcement and countermeasures


Speed 9 Compared to the present limits, what do you think the speed limit on main roads between towns 30 41.7 2.51*
should bed
Speed 10 Compared to the present limits, what do you think the speed limit on country roads should bed 12.7 14.2 0.45
Speed 11 Compared to the present limits, what do you think the speed limit in built-up areas should bed 8.2 11.8 1.24
Speed 12 How much would you be in favor of afxing speed-limiting devices to cars that prevented the 72.7 80.4 1.87
drivers of them from exceeding the speed limite
Speed 13 How much would you be in favor of using a black box to record behavior from a driver that the 77.7 81.9 1.07
police could then use as evidence to prove speeding/dangerous drivinge
Speed 14 How much would you be in favor of the use of automated cameras in the surveillance of speed 75.5 80.3 1.19
excessese

Seat belt use in different environments


Belts 1 How often do you wear a seat belt when making a journey on main roads between townsf 22.7 83.8 12.58***
Belts 2 How often do you wear a seat belt when making a journey on country roadsf 7.3 64.2 12.30***
Belts 3 How often do you wear a seat belt when making a journey in a built-up areaf 7.3 68.6 13.08***

Attitudes toward seat belt wearing


Belts 4 If you drive carefully, seat belts are not really necessarye 51.8 29.9 4.58***
Belts 5 In most accidents, seat belts reduce the risk of serious injury to both drivers and passengerse 93.6 91.2 0.93
Belts 6 When I am not wearing my seat belt, I feel less comfortable; I feel as though something is missinge 38.2 51.5 2.75**
Belts 7 There is a risk of being trapped by a seat belt in the event of an emergencye 44.6 26.5 3.88***

Drinking behavior and drunk driving behavior


Alcohol 1 In general how many days per week do you drink alcoholic beveragesg 44.5 38.8 1.18
Alcohol 2 How many days per week do you drive after drinking even a small amount of alcoholg 32.3 24.0 1.90
Alcohol 3 Over the last week, how many days did you drive, when you may have been over the legal limit for 17.7 7.8 3.03**
drinking and drivingg

Opinions about alcohol legislation and countermeasures


Alcohol 4 Do you think that drivers should be allowed to drinkh 55.9 61.8 1.23
Alcohol 5 How much would you be in favor of not allowing new drivers to drink any alcohol before drivinge 80.0 85.3 1.44
Alcohol 6 - Would you nd it useful to have a device on your car, like an alcohol-meter, that could be used to 71.4 77.5 1.44
determine whether you had been drinking and that could prevent you from driving if you were over the
legal blood alcohol limite
a
Percentage of drivers who marked much more or a bit more
b
Percentage of drivers who marked much faster or a little faster
c
Percentage of drivers who marked often, very often or always
d
Percentage of drivers who marked higher
e
Percentage of drivers who marked very or fairly
f
Percentage of drivers who marked always
g
Percentage of drivers who marked one day or more
h
Percentage of drivers who marked no alcohol at all
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

in Serbia, respectively, and the number of drivers who were violat- fully aware of the degree to which they violate the speed limits.
ing the speed limit increased by 10% and 11%, respectively, on the Drivers in Northern Kosovo may think that in most cases they are
roads with the two different speed limit. Taking into account the not actually violating speed limits when driving at speeds which
estimates that a 1 km/h increase in the average speed of trafc leads they perceive safe. Hence, drivers in North Kosovo are not aware of
to an approximately 3% increase in the number of accidents (e.g., the speed limits simply because the speed limits are not enforced
Baruya, 1998; Finch et al., 1994), we can assume that the lack of traf- and, thus, being aware of the speed limit is not important.
c enforcement has serious consequences. Interestingly, greater The reported speeding-related behaviors are very similar
differences between the two driver groups concerned were found among drivers in Northern Kosovo and Serbia, which suggests
when measuring the speed in the eld than in self-reports. Since that the root causes of speeding problems are the same. Most
the surveys were conducted equally in Serbia and North Kosovo, importantly, this study conrmed that drivers do not believe that
this difference cannot be due to social desirability bias. Instead, speeding is associated with danger when it comes to their own
it is more likely that many drivers in Northern Kosovo are not driving habits, i.e., drivers claimed that they drove faster than other
P. Stanojevic et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938 35

Table 5
Independent t-tests for differences in violations while driving, attitudes toward trafc safety and involvement in accidents and risky situations in the two groups of drivers.

Mean Std. deviation t df p

N. Kosovo Serbia N. Kosovo Serbia

DBQ
Aggressive violations 6.65 5.56 2.44 1.85 5.137 422 .000
Ordinary violations 14.10 13.01 4.50 3.80 2.684 422 .008
Errors 13.62 13.41 4.50 3.80 .492 422 .623
Lapses 13.74 13.44 3.98 3.78 .795 422 .427

Attitudes
Attitude toward violation of trafc laws and regulations 14.60 13.04 4.46 4.20 3.69 422 .000
Attitude toward speed 17.88 15.48 6.21 5.42 4.23 422 .000
Attitude toward drinking and driving 7.76 7.56 3.50 2.73 .65 422 .514

Risky situations 1.08 .64 2.70 1.17 2.147 422 .032

Trafc accidents .40 .26 .84 .80 1.702 422 .090

drivers signicantly more often than they claimed that they drove peoples attitudes toward behavior, their subjective norms, and
more dangerously, which demonstrates that many drivers believe their perceived behavioral control determine their intention to
that they are better drivers than the average driver. Furthermore, perform the behavior. Thus, the situation that has been created in
they believe that other drivers exceed the speed limit frequently, Northern Kosovo is likely to affect the formation of different norms
which may be one of the factors that inuence general driver that determine attitudes toward speeding. Furthermore, speeding
behavior regarding speeding. In addition, drivers in both groups behavior is inuenced by some of the more negative attitudes, and
reported exceeding the speed limit on fast roads to a greater extent these attitudes, in conjunction with a lack of perceived control
than on slower roads. Interestingly, many respondents expressed of behavior, possibly result in a situation in which drivers in the
some agreement with the introduction of specic measures against territory of Northern Kosovo drive faster and violate the speed limit
speeding violations, such as having speed-limiting devices tted more frequently than drivers in other areas. It should be noted,
to cars, using black boxes to record the speed of a vehicle that however, that in this study the TPB was not empirically used.
could then be used in proceedings against an offender, and the use Like many others, the present study found that the enforcement
of speed cameras. of trafc laws can have a strong effect on driver behavior, but it
Although there is a driver consensus about measures against should be noted that different types of law enforcement also result
speeding, the results of the present study showed that drivers in in different results. Many studies have showed the clear advan-
Northern Kosovo reported negative attitudes about speeding more tages of automated enforcement. The meta-analysis conducted by
often than the drivers from Serbia, in the sense that drivers in Elvik et al. (1997) shows that the automatic enforcement of speed-
Northern Kosovo believed that it is okay to drive faster than the ing laws resulted as 19% decrease in the number of accidents and
speed limit as long as the driver is skilled and the road condi- a 17% reduction in the number of people injured in car accidents.
tions allow it. This indicates that a longer period without trafc law Moreover, speed cameras have been shown to have a greater effect
enforcement has led to changes in the attitudes toward speeding in urban areas (28% decrease) than in rural areas (4% decrease).
in addition to the obvious changes in fast driving. Speeding- Newstead and Cameron (2003) investigated the effects of a speed
related behaviors are widespread, deeply entrenched and generally camera program on the number of trafc accidents in Queensland
socially condoned (Croft, 1993). In addition, the media sometimes during a 4-year period. According to their data, the total number
treat speeding and the commission of speeding offenses erro- of trafc accidents decreased by 29%, and the number of serious
neously by arguing that drivers are unjustly punished and that accidents (those that had fatal outcomes or that resulted in injuries
the police should devote themselves to more important problems requiring hospital and medical treatment) decreased by approxi-
(Slack and Massey, 2005; Day, 2004; Parsons, 2003, according to mately 36%.
Poulter and McKenna, 2007). Many studies have shown that most In the studies about the outcomes of various kinds of speed limit
people think that they have driving skills that are superior to enforcement, the perceived risk of being caught has been identi-
those of other drivers, and they believe that they should be able to ed (Shinar and Mcknight, 1985) as the essential factor inuencing
speed because they do it safely (Walton and Bathurst, 1998; Elliott, the speed choice. The installation of automated speed enforce-
2001). It has also been found that the speed of other drivers can ment devices is regarded as the most effective means of increasing
be an important factor in speed choice (Connolly and berg, 1993; apprehension rates and deterring speeding behavior. Combining
Haglund and berg, 2000) and that some drivers can also speed due this form of enforcement with traditional methods can signicantly
to the inuence of habit (De Pelsmacker and Janssens, 2007). reduce speeding behavior and can therefore lead to reductions in
In Northern Kosovo where the current study was conducted, both the number and severity of trafc accidents caused by speed-
there is no punishment for committing a speeding violation, and ing.
thus there are no feed-back mechanisms (punishments and/or
rewards) which would enforce the norm that speeding, not using 4.2. Seat belts
seat belt, or drinking and driving is wrong and deserves to be
punished. Thus, both driver behavior and attitudes are more The greatest differences between drivers in Northern Kosovo
susceptible to the inuence of the above described situational and drivers in Serbia in terms of the behaviors investigated were
and driver related factors than trafc code. Speeding-related the differences in seat belt use. Namely, the drivers from North-
attitudes and behavior can be explained also by using the Theory ern Kosovo reported using seat belts to a lesser extent than Serbian
of Planned Behavior (Ajzen, 1991), which had already been applied drivers on the main roads between towns, country roads and in
on prediction of speeding behavior (e.g. berg, 1997; Parker built-up areas. Even eld observations in Northern Kosovo and
et al., 1992; Stradling and Parker, 1997; Wallen Warner and Serbia conrmed that there were large differences in seat belt
Aberg, 2006). According to the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB), use between the regions compared. However, the general attitude
36 P. Stanojevic et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938

toward seat belt usage is positive; i.e., the vast majority of drivers own speeding behaviors than drinking and driving, especially in
agree that wearing a seat belt reduces the risk of serious injury to light of their perception that speeding is not dangerous.
a driver or a passenger in most trafc accidents. However, posi- However, it is important that enforcement does play a signi-
tive attitudes, beliefs and intentions about wearing seat belts do cant role in the incidence of drunk driving. In the case of Northern
not strongly predict whether drivers will actually wear seat belts Kosovo, it is evident that drivers drive under the inuence of alcohol
at all times (Chliaoutakis et al., 2000; Loo, 1984). This is probably more often than they otherwise might do because they are aware of
due to the fact that a large number of drivers underestimate the the absence of law enforcement, and thus know that their offense
necessity of seat belt use when driving carefully, and they overes- will go unpunished. The differences between attitudes toward and
timate the seat belt-related risk of becoming trapped in a vehicle frequency of drunk driving can be explained in terms of the role
in an emergency situation, which is evident from the results that of trafc enforcement. For example, if certain drivers both like to
were obtained in the present study. The concerns listed above may drink and believe that a certain amount of alcohol (e.g., 23 glasses
only be some of the reasons that drivers choose not to wear seat of wine or 23 bottles of beer) does not affect their driving skills,
belts. One of the most important factors inuencing drivers deci- then in the absence of law enforcement, they are likely to consume
sion to use a seat belt is the habit (Begg and Langley, 2000; Calisir alcohol and drive. If enforcement is present, however, these drivers
and Lehto, 2002; Simsekoglu and Lajunen, 2008). Mkinen and will always have a reason to refrain from driving while under the
Hagenzieker (1991) suggested that once a habit of using a seat belt inuence of alcohol. Therefore, without enforcement, there is no
has been established, it is relatively easy to continue to use one rel- reason for them to refrain from driving while intoxicated (according
atively frequently. In the present study, the habit of using seat belt to Shinar, 2007).
was measured with a question if the drivers felt less comfortable One of the largest problems regarding drunk driving is that most
or something is missing when not wearing a seat belt. We found of the models that we currently have for explaining, predicting, and
that drivers in Northern Kosovo had lesser prominent habit of using changing driver behavior are difcult to apply in the case of drunk
a seat belt then Serbian drivers. driving. For example, if we consider driver behavior in terms of the
The results of the current study indicate that the enforcement TPB, the behavior of a driver may be rational and reasonable before
of trafc laws plays an important role in forming the habit of using he or she begins consuming alcohol but may change when the per-
a seat belt. However, it is worth of noting that when it comes to son gets intoxicated. Because alcohol has a detrimental effect on our
seat belt use, trafc enforcement can only be effective to a certain decision-making capabilities, once a driver who had been rational
extent. For example, studies of seat belt usage among sample pop- when sober becomes drunk, he or she may easily change his or
ulations from the United States and Canada show that it is difcult her plans to be more consistent with his or her subjective feelings
to raise the usage rate beyond 80%. Jonah and Grant (1985) suggest as they are right after the last drink and just before sitting down
that an 80% usage rate is the maximum level that can be achieved at the wheel of the car (Shinar, 2007). It is therefore not surpris-
by trafc enforcement alone, and it is necessary to employ edu- ing that the implementation of legislative measures that prohibit
cation and media campaigns that support the trafc enforcement drunk driving is generally accepted as the most effective means
strategy to break this 80% barrier. The high percentages of drivers of preventing such behaviors. In a recent report, the WHO (Peden
who reported that they both underestimated the necessity of using et al., 2004) states that . . .the only consistently effective strategy
a seat belt and overestimated the risk of becoming trapped in their for dealing with the problem of excess alcohol is to increase the
vehicle in emergency situations show that there is room for edu- perceived risk of being caught. Such a perception is considered a
cational and information campaigns to improve the understanding better deterrent than the severity or swiftness of the penalty.
of the signicant benets of seat belt usage among drivers. While education and information campaigns should be used to
support legal measures targeted at preventing drunk driving, they
should not be implemented independently from the implementa-
4.3. Drunk driving tion of the legal measures for suppressing drunk driving violations.
It has been found that their combined use has much stronger and
The results indicate that trafc law enforcement has an impor- longer-term effects than the use of either single deterrent (Elliott,
tant role in modulating drunk driving behavior. Drivers from 1993).
Northern Kosovo have reported driving after exceeding the legal
limit for alcohol more frequently than drivers in Serbia. 4.4. Driving violations, risky situations and trafc accidents
However, there was no difference between drivers in Northern
Kosovo and Serbia in terms of their attitudes toward drunk driving. DBQ based on a four-factor survey structure (errors, lapses,
This was conrmed by the fact that drivers in Northern Kosovo aggressive and ordinary violations) has been widely used in a large
expressed strong support to various measures that would prevent number of international studies (De Winter and Dodou, 2010) and
driving while under the inuence of alcohol, which was either at the DBQ can be considered as one of the most used and valid
the same or at an even greater level than among drivers in Serbia. instruments for measuring aberrant driving (Lajunen and zkan,
Unlike the case of speeding, trafc enforcement does not play an 2011). The results obtained through the use of this survey showed
important role in dening attitudes toward drunk driving. There that drivers from Northern Kosovo committed both ordinary and
are several reasons for this. First, the society in general condemns aggressive violations more often than drivers in Serbia. However,
drinking and driving (Homel et al., 1988; Loxley et al., 1990) while there were no differences between these two groups in terms
speeding is a much more socially accepted behavior. Second, there of either errors or lapses while driving, which shows that both
are clear material and psychological gains to speeding (arriving groups of drivers possess similar capabilities and that drivers in
at the nal destination sooner and a sense of progress), whereas both groups have similar tendencies toward making mistakes. This
drivers who are driving under the inuence of alcohol are most is an expected result, because trafc rule enforcement should not
often either returning home or looking for another place for improve driver performance (or skills) but behavior (driving style),
entertainment and, thus, not in hurry. Third, the risks of drunk and because the key component in safe driving style is the respect
driving and speeding are perceived differently; drivers consider for rules. In addition to the previous results (speed, failing to wear
speeding to be less dangerous than driving under the inuence of a seat belt, and driving under the inuence of alcohol), the ndings
alcohol, so they are less likely to drive while intoxicated (SARTRE, of this analysis once again conrm the fact that drivers in North-
2004). In short, drivers have much more ways of justifying their ern Kosovo take advantage of the situation in which there is a lack
P. Stanojevic et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938 37

of trafc law enforcement and, thus, they willingly violate trafc Ajzen, I., 1991. The theory of planned behavior. Organisational Behavior and Human
rules. Decision Processes 50, 179211.
Baruya, B., 1998. Speed-accident relationships on European roads. In: Proceedings of
Drivers in Northern Kosovo also reported having been in a the Conference Road Safety in Europe, Bergisch Gladbach, Germany, September
greater number of risky situations on the road (near misses 2123, 1998, VTI Konferens No. 10A, Part 10, pp. 117.
in which an accident almost occurred) compared to drivers in Begg, D.J., Langley, D.J., 2000. Seat-belt use and related behavior among young adults.
Journal of Safety Research 31, 211220.
Serbia. The difference in the number of self-reported trafc acci- Bendak, S., 2005. Seat belt utilisation in Saudi Arabia and its impact on road accident
dents between drivers in Northern Kosovo and Serbia was in the injuries. Accident Analysis and Prevention 37 (2), 367371.
expected direction but did not reach statistical signicance because Calisir, F., Lehto, M.R., 2002. Young drivers decision making and safety belt use.
Accident Analysis and Prevention 34, 793805.
of small sample size. While near misses are relatively frequent, acci-
Carr, A.F., Schnelle, J.F., Kirchner, R.E., 1980. Police crackdowns and slowdowns:
dents are rare occurrences which do not follow normal distribution a naturalistic evaluation of changes in police trafc. Behavioral Assessment 2,
and, therefore, require large samples to gain statistically signicant 3341.
Chliaoutakis, E.J., Gnardellis, C., Drakou, I., Darviri, C., Sboukis, V., 2000. Modelling
results.
the factors related to the seatbelt use by the young drivers of Athens. Accident
Analysis and Prevention 32, 815825.
5. Limitations Connolly, T., berg, L., 1993. Some contagion models of speeding. Accident Analysis
and Prevention 25 (1), 5766.
Croft, P.G., 1993. Speed management in New South Wales. In: Fildes, B.N., Lee, S.J.
This study has some limitations. First, the vast majority of (Eds.), The Speed Review: Appendix of Speed Workshop Papers. Monash Uni-
the results were obtained based on self-reported behavior. This versity Accident Research Centre, Clayton, Victoria.
Day, E., 2004. Prosecute motorway lane hogs, RAC says. http://www.telegraph.
method of collecting data may lead to the possibility of collecting co.uk/news/
data that are distorted due to receiving socially desirable answers De Pelsmacker, P., Janssens, W., 2007. The effect of norms, attitudes and habits
from the respondents. Although the subjects were ensured of the on speeding behavior: scale development and model building and estimation.
Accident Analysis and Prevention 39, 615.
anonymity and condentiality of their data and the same data col- De Waard, D., Rooijers, T., 1994. An experimental study to evaluate the effectiveness
lection method was applied in both populations, they may still of different methods and intensities of law enforcement on driving speed on
have been wary to fully disclose personal information. Second, this motorways. Accident Analysis and Prevention 26 (6), 751765.
De Winter, J.C.F., Dodou, D., 2010. The driver behaviour questionnaire as a predictor
study was focused on the existence/absence of supervision in two of accidents: a meta-analysis. Journal of Safety Research 41 (6), 463470.
geographical areas and, thus, was not aimed at investigating all Elliott, B., 1993. Road Safety Mass Media Campaigns: A Meta Analysis. Report CR
possible factors related to risky driving. While Northern Kosovo 118. Federal Ofce of Road Safety, Canberra.
Elliott B.J., 2001. The Application of the Theorists Workshop Model of Behaviour
and Serbia are very similar in terms of such factors as culture, eth-
Change to Motorists Speeding Behaviour in Western Australia. Ofce of Road
nical background, and institutions (e.g. education, administration) Safety, Department of Transport, Western Australia.
but opposite in terms of police supervision on the roads, there still Elliott, M., Broughton, J., 2005. How Methods and Levels of Policing Affect Road
might be some unidentied factors inuencing driver behavior and Casualty Rates. TRL Report TRL637. TRL Limited, Wokingham.
Elvik, R., 2001. Cost-Benet Analysis of Police Enforcement. Report ESCAPE Project,
possibly causing the differences between these two regions. Tak- Working Paper, VTT, Helsinki.
ing into account the similarities and differences between these two Elvik, R., Mysen, A.B., Vaa, T., 1997. Trakksikkerhetshndbok (Trafc Safety Hand-
regions, it can be hypothesize that trafc enforcement is a factor book). Institute of Transport Economics, Oslo.
ESCAPE Consortium, 2003. Trafc enforcement in Europe: effects, measures, needs
which most inuences the differences in the behavior and attitudes and future. The Escape Project Final Report, Contract No.: RO-98-RS.3047, 4th
of the drivers. RTD Framework Programme.
European Transport Safety Council, 1999. Police Enforcement Strategies to Reduce
Trafc Casualties in Europe. ETSC, Brussels.
6. Conclusion Evans, L., 1991. Trafc Safety and the Driver. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York.
Finch, D.J., Kompfner, P., Lockwood, C.R., Maycock, G., 1994. Speed, Speed Limits and
Accidents. TRL Report 58. Transport Research Laboratory, Crowthorne.
In the present study, the comparisons based on self-reported Goldenbeld, C., Jayet, M.C., Fuller, R., Mkinen, T., 1999. Enforcement of trafc laws.
behavior and eld observations indicated that drivers in North- Review of the literature on enforcement of trafc rules in the framework of
ern Kosovo engage in signicantly riskier driving behaviors than GADGET Work Package 5. SWOV, INRETS, TCD, VTT, Leidschendam, Netherlands.
Haglund, M., berg, L., 2000. Speed choice in relation to speed limit and inuences
drivers in Serbia. Also, drivers in Northern Kosovo drive faster,
from other drivers. Transportation Research Part F 3, 3951.
exceed speed limits more frequently, use seat belts less frequently, Hakkert, A.S., Gitelman, V., Cohen, A., Doveh, E., Umansky, T., 2001. The evalu-
drive after exceeding the legal limit for alcohol more often, commit ation of effects on driver behaviour and accidents of concentrated general
both aggressive and ordinary violations more frequently and are enforcement on interurban roads in Israel. Accident Analysis and Prevention 33,
4363.
more involved in risky situations. The lack of enforcement seems Heinrich, H., 1991. Strategies to increase the use of restraint systems: report about
to inuence the formation of attitudes toward speeding and the Germany. In: Proceedings of the Conference Strategic Highway Research Pro-
violation of trafc regulations too. gram and Trafc Safety on Two Continents, Gothenburg, pp. 113119.
Holland, C.A., Conner, M.T., 1996. Exceeding the speed limit: an evaluation of the
Several types of risky driver behaviors were included into the effectiveness of a police intervention. Accident Analysis and Prevention 28 (5),
present study as dependent variables. These behaviors have differ- 587597.
ent causes and are based on different psychological mechanisms, Homel, R., Carseldine, D., Kearns, I., 1988. Drink-driving countermeasures in
Australia. Alcohol, Drugs and Driving 4 (2), 113144.
and therefore require different control mechanisms. Differences in Iversen, H., Rundmo, T., 2004. Attitudes towards trafc safety, driving behaviour
etiology of these behaviors must be taken into account in imple- and accident involvement among the Norwegian public. Ergonomics 47 (4),
mentation of the measures that are designed to change the behavior 555572.
Jonah, B.A., Grant, B.A., 1985. Long-term effectiveness of selective trafc enforce-
and attitudes of the drivers. Drivers seemed to support in great ment programs for increasing seat belt use. Journal of Applied Psychology 70,
degree the application of newer technologies in enforcement traf- 257263.
c laws (e.g. speed cameras, speed limitation devices and devices Lajunen, T., zkan, T., 2011. Self-report instruments and methods. In: Porter, B.E.
(Ed.), Handbook of Trafc Psychology. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 4359.
to prevent driving while under the inuence of alcohol). Thus, it
Lawton, R., Parker, D., Manstead, A.S.R., Stradling, S.G., 1997. The role of affect in
is likely that these devices would be well received and result in predicting social behaviours: the case of road trafc violations. Journal of Applied
signicant improvement in trafc safety in Northern Kosovo. Social Psychology 27, 12581276.
Lewin, I., 1982. Driver training: a perceptual-motor skill approach. Ergonomics 25,
917924.
References Loo, R., 1984. Correlates of reported attitude towards and use of seat belts. Accident
Analysis and Prevention 16 (5/6), 417421.
berg, L., 1997. The role of perceived risk of detection. In: Rothengatter, T., Car- Loxley, W., Saunders, B., Blaze-Temple, D., Binns, C., 1990. Drinking and driving
bonell Vaya, E. (Eds.), Trafc and Transport Psychology: Theory and Application. in Western Australia: perceptions of practices, priorities and preventives. Aus-
Elsevier, Oxford, UK, pp. 395401. tralian Journal of Social Issues 25 (2), 120136.
38 P. Stanojevic et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 52 (2013) 2938

Mkinen, T., Hagenzieker, M., 1991. Strategies to increase the use of restraint sys- Simsekoglu, ., Lajunen, T., 2008. Why Turks do not use seat belts? An interview
tems. In: Proceedings of the Conference Strategic Highway Research Program study. Accident Analysis and Prevention 40, 470478.
and Trafc Safety on Two Continents, Gothenburg, pp. 6167. Slack, J., Massey, R., 2005. Soft Target motoristshalf of all court cases are driving
Mkinen, T., Wittink, R.D., Hagenzieker, M.P., 1991. The Use of Seat Belts and Con- offences. Daily Mail.
tributing Factors: An International Comparison. Report No. R-91-30. SWOV Stradling, S.G., Parker, D., 1997. Extending the theory of planned behaviour: the
Institute for Road Safety Research, Leidschendam, Netherlands, p. 57. role of personal norm, instrumental beliefs and affective beliefs in predict-
Montgomery, D.C., Runger, G.C., 1994. Applied Probability and Statistics for Engi- ing driving violations. In: Rothengatter, T., Carbonell Vaya, E. (Eds.), Trafc
neers. Wiley, New York. and Transport Psychology: Theory and Application. Elsevier, Oxford, UK,
Newstead, S., Cameron, M., 2003. Evaluation of the Crash Effects of the Queens- pp. 367374.
land Speed Camera Program. Report 204. Monash University Accident Research Summala, H., Roine, M., 1980. Exceptional conditions of police enforcement during
Centre, Melbourne. the police strike. Accident Analysis and Prevention 12, 179184.
Oei, H., 1998. The Effect of Enforcement on Speed Behaviour: A Literature Study. Tarko, A., 2008. Can enforcement and education affect speed on county roads? Paper
MASTER Deliverable 3. SWOV Institute for Road Safety Research, Leidschendam, presented at Transportation Research Board 87th Annual Meeting, January 2008.
Netherlands. Transportation Research Board, Washington, DC.
stvik, E., Elvik, R., 1991. The effects of speed enforcement on individual road user Taylor, M.C., Lynam, D.A., Baruya, A., 2000. The Effect of Drivers Speed on the Fre-
behavior and accidents. In: Koornstra, M.J., Christensen, J. (Eds.), Enforcement quency of Road Accidents. TRL Report TRL421. TRL Limited, Wokingham.
and Rewarding: Strategies and Effects. SWOV, Leidschendam, Netherlands, pp. Ulleberg, P., Rundmo, T., 2003. Personality, attitudes and risk perception as pre-
5659. dictors of risky driving behavior among young drivers. Safety Science 41,
Parker, D., Lajunen, T., Stradling, S., 1998. Attitudinal predictors of aggressive driving 427443.
violations. Transportation Research Part F 1, 1124. Vaa, T., 1997. Increased police enforcement: effects on speed. Accident Analysis and
Parker, D., Manstead, A.S.R., Stradling, S.G., Reason, J.T., Baxter, J.S., 1992. Intention Prevention 29 (3), 373385.
to commit driving violations: an application of the theory of planned behaviour. Valtonen, J., 1991. Safety belt usage in Finland and in the other nordic countries. In:
Journal of Applied Psychology 77, 94101. Proceedings of the Conference Strategic Highway Research Program and Trafc
Parker, D., Reason, J.T., Manstead, A.S.R., Stradling, S., 1995. Driving errors, driving Safety on Two Continents, Gothenburg, pp. 8996.
violations and accident involvement. Ergonomics 39, 10361048. Wallen Warner, H., Aberg, L., 2006. Drivers decision to speed: a study inspired
Parsons, T., 2003. Why trafc cops play silly burglars. http://mirror.co.uk/ by the theory of planned behavior. Transportation Research Part F 9,
news/tonyparsons/tm column date=14072003-name index.html 427433.
Peden, M., Scureld, R., Sleet, D., Mohan, D., Hyder, A.A., Jarawan, E., Mathers, C., 2004. Walter, L., Broughton, J., Knowles, J., 2011. The effects of increased police
World Report on Road Trafc Injury Prevention. World Health Organization, enforcement along a route in London. Accident Analysis and Prevention 43,
Geneva. 12191227.
Poulter, R.D., McKenna, P.F., 2007. Is speeding a real antisocial behavior? A com- Walton, D., Bathurst, J., 1998. An exploration of the perceptions of the average
parison with other antisocial behaviors. Accident Analysis and Prevention 39, drivers speed compared to perceived driver safety and driving skills. Accident
384389. Analysis and Prevention 30 (6), 821830.
Robertson, L.S., 1978. The seat belt use law in Ontario: effects on actual use. Canadian Wechsler, H., Eun Lee, J., Nelson, T.F., Lee, H., 2003. Drinking and driving among
Journal of Public Health 69, 154157. college students: the inuence of alcohol-control policies. American Journal of
Rumar, K., 1985. The role of perceptual and cognitive lters in observed behaviour. Preventive Medicine 25 (3), 212218.
In: Evans, L., Schwing, R.C. (Eds.), Human Behavior and Trafc Safety. Plenum Weiss, A., Freels, S., 1996. The effects of aggressive policing: the Dayton trafc
Press, New York, pp. 151165. enforcement experiment. American Journal of Police 15, 4564.
Ryeng, O.E., 2011. The effect of sanctions and police enforcement on drivers choice Williams, A.F., Wells, J.K., 2004. The role of enforcement programs in increasing seat
of speed. Accident Analysis and Prevention 45, 446454. belt use. Journal of Safety Research 35, 175180.
SARTRE Consortium, 2004. European drivers and road risk. Part 1. Report on principal Yannis, G., Papadimitriou, E., Antoniou, C., 2008. Impact of enforcement on trafc
results. Part 2: Report on in-depth analyses. INRETS, Arcueil. accidents and fatalities: a multivariate multilevel analysis. Safety Science 46,
Schick, S., 2007. Accident Related Factors. Project No. 027763. Deliverable 3.1, WP3 738750.
Type of Factors. European TRACE Project. Zaal D., 1994. Trafc Law Enforcement: A Review of the Literature. Report No. 53.
Shinar, D., 2007. Trafc Safety and Human Behavior. Elsevier, Oxford, UK. Monash University, Accident Research Centre, Clayton, Victoria.
Shinar, D., Mcknight, A.J., 1985. The effects of enforcement and public information Zaidel, D., 2002. The impact of enforcement on accidents. Deliverable 3
on compliance. In: Evans, L., Schwing, R.C. (Eds.), Human Behavior and Trafc (Work Package 2) of the ESCAPE project. Contract N: RO-98-RS.3047.
Safety. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 385419. http://www.vtt./rte/projects/escape/escape d3.pdf

Anda mungkin juga menyukai