Anda di halaman 1dari 42

February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.

docx page 1 of 42

CHAPTER 07
SELF-PRESENTATION
I. THE NATURE OF SELF-PRESENTATION.............................................................................................. 3
A. WHY DO PEOPLE ENGAGE IN SELF-PRESENTATION? ..................................................................................3
B. WHEN AND HOW DO PEOPLE MANAGE IMPRESSIONS? .............................................................................5
C. INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN SELF-PRESENTATION .....................................................................................7
II. CREATING DESIRED IMPRESSIONS ................................................................................................. 11
A. WHAT IMPRESSIONS DO PEOPLE TRY TO CREATE? ..................................................................................11
B. WHAT CONSTITUTES A DESIRABLE IMPRESSION? ....................................................................................13
C. IDENTITY PREDICAMENTS ...................................................................................................................17
D. IDENTITY-REPAIR TACTICS ..................................................................................................................20
III. SELF-PRESENTATIONS AND PRIVATE SELF-CONCEPTIONS .............................................................. 20
A. ROLE INTERNALIZATION .....................................................................................................................21
B. CARRY-OVER EFFECTS IN SELF-PRESENTATION .......................................................................................22
C. SYMBOLIC SELF-COMPLETION THEORY .................................................................................................24
IV. SELF-PRESENTATION AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR ................................................................................ 26
A. SELF-PRESENTATION AND SELF-ENHANCEMENT......................................................................................26
B. SINCERITY AND AUTHENTICITY VERSUS PRETENSE AND DECEIT ...................................................................30
V. CHAPTER SUMMARY ..................................................................................................................... 32
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 2 of 42

CHAPTER 07
SELF-PRESENTATION
There will be time, there will be time, to prepare a face to meet the faces that
you meet.
T.S. Eliot, The Lovesong of J. Alfred Prufrock
The self-concept seems like a very private phenomenon. After all, peoples thoughts
about themselves are hidden and are often highly personal. Yet the self-concept is also
very much a social phenomenon. It has social roots (e.g., reflected appraisals, social
comparison), it includes social identities and roles, and it guides our perception of others
and our behavior in social settings.
In this chapter we will explore the social side of the self in the context of self -
presentational behavior. Self-presentational behavior is any behavior intended to
create, modify, or maintain an impression of ourselves in the minds of others. 1
According to this definition, whenever we are attempting to lead people to think of us
in a particular way, we are engaging in self-presentation.
Because much of our time is spent in the company of other people, self-presentation is a
pervasive feature of social life. We even engage in self-presentation when we are alone; for
example, we rehearse what we are going to say or do in public, molding our behavior to an
imaginary or anticipated audience. Sometimes this rehearsal is deliberate and noticeable
(as when we prepare for a job interview or a public speaking engagement); other times it is
automatic and almost imperceptible (as when we mindlessly check our hair in the mirror
before stepping out the front door).
Self-presentation is not only a prevalent aspect of our lives, it is also a very
important one. Our success at leading others to believe we possess various characteristics
has a profound influence on our outcomes in life (Hogan & Briggs, 1986). Who we marry,
who our friends are, whether we get ahead at work, and many other outcomes depend, to a
great extent, on our ability to convince people that we are worthy of their love, their
friendship, their trust, and their respect. Undoubtedly, this need to create a positive
impression is one reason that people spend billions of dollars a year on cosmetics and
other personal-appearance products. Self-presentational concerns also lead people to
engage in behaviors that enhance their appearance to others but simultaneously jeopardize
their own physical well-being (e.g., overexposure to the sun; excessive dieting) (Leary,
Tchividijian, & Kraxberger, 1994). Self-presentational concerns can even underlie self-
destructive behaviors, such as cigarette smoking and substance abuse (Sharp & Getz,
1996).
The chapter begins by considering the nature of self-presentational behavior. Why
do people engage in self-presentation, and when and how do they go about creating
impressions of themselves in the minds of other people? In the second section of the

1The term impression management has also been used to describe peoples efforts to manage the
impressions others form of them. Although the two terms (self-presentation and impression management)
differ in certain respects (see Schlenker, 1980), I use them interchangeably throughout this chapter.
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 3 of 42

chapter, we will look at the kinds of images people create and the obstacles they face when
trying to create these images. Here, we will also examine some of the things people do
when they fail to make a desired impression. The third part of the chapter explores the
close connection between public behavior and private self-conceptions. We will see that
people are very often audiences for their own behavior, and in the course of trying to
convince others that they possess particular qualities, they end up convincing themselves.
Finally, we will consider the extent to which public behavior is a faithful reflection of what
people really think about themselves.
I. The Nature of Self-Presentation
A. Why Do People Engage in Self-Presentation?
We begin our discussion by considering why people engage in self-presentation. Why
do we bother to lead people to see us in one way or another?
1. Facilitate Social Interaction
The most basic function of self-presentation is to define the nature of a social
situation (Goffman, 1959). Most social interactions are very role governed. Each person
has a role to play, and the interaction proceeds smoothly when these roles are enacted
effectively. For example, airline pilots are expected to be poised and dignified. As long as
they convince their passengers that they possess these qualities, their passengers remain
calm and behave in an orderly fashion. (Imagine, for example, how unsettling it would be if
your airline pilot acted like the character Kramer on the television show Seinfeld!)
This function of self-presentation was first highlighted by Erving Goffman (1959).
Goffman noted that social life is highly structured. In some cases, this structure is
formalized (e.g., state dinners at the White House are characterized by strict rules of
protocol), but most often it is informal and tacitly understood (e.g., norms of politeness and
etiquette guide our social interactions).
Among these norms is one that mandates that people support, rather than
undermine, one anothers public identities. Goffman refers to these efforts as face work.
Each participant in an interaction is obliged to honor and uphold the other persons public
persona. Toward this end, people may misrepresent themselves or otherwise refrain from
saying what they really think or feel. For example, people publicly claim to like the
presents they receive, find another persons new clothes or hairstyle attractive, or make
excuses for why they cannot get together for some social encounter. This kind of self-
presentational behavior seems to be primarily driven by a desire to avoid social conflict
and reduce tension (DePaulo, Kashy, Kirkendol, Wyer, & Epstein, 1996).
2. Gain Material and Social Rewards
People also strive to create impressions of themselves in the minds of others in
order to gain material and social rewards (or avoid material and social punishments). As
discussed earlier, it is usually in our best interests to have others view us in a particular
way. Employees generally have a material interest in being perceived as bright, committed,
and promising. To the extent that they are successful in inducing these impressions in the
minds of their employers, they are apt to be promoted and given raises. Social rewards
also depend on our ability to convince others that we possess particular qualities. Being
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 4 of 42

liked entails convincing others that we are likable; being a leader involves convincing
others that we are capable of leading.
Jones (1990; see also, Tedeschi & Norman, 1985) notes that this type of strategic
self-presentation represents a form of social influence in which one person (the self-
presenter) attempts to gain power over another (the audience). This approach assumes
that we are in a better position to influence the nature of social interaction in a manner that
suits our purposes if we are able to control how others see us. This emphasis is apparent in
many popular books, that carry titles like How to Win Friends and Influence People
(Carnegie, 1936) and Winning through Intimidation (Ringer, 1973).
To some, the idea that people actively strive to manipulate how they are viewed by
others conjures up images of duplicity and Machiavellianism. This need not be the case,
however. Strategic self-presentation does not necessarily mean that we are trying to deceive
others (though sometimes we are). It can also involve genuine attempts to bring our (self-
perceived) positive qualities to the attention of others. In fact, for reasons to be discussed later,
misrepresentation and lying tend to be the exception rather than the rule. Most of the time,
strategic self-presentation involves selective disclosures and omissions, or matters of emphasis
and timing, rather than blatant deceit or dissimulation (Jones, 1990, p.175).
3. Self-Construction
Another reason we try to create impressions of ourselves in the minds of others is to
construct a particular identity for ourselves (Baumeister, 1982b; Rosenberg, 1979;
Schlenker, 1980). This type of self-presentational behavior serves a more private, personal
function. Convincing others that we possess some quality or attribute is a means of
convincing ourselves.
Sometimes, self-construction is initiated in order to create an identity. Rosenberg
(1979) notes that this is particularly prevalent during adolescence. Adolescents routinely
try out different identities. They adopt the dress and mannerisms of various social types
(e.g., the sophisticate; the rebel), and studiously note peoples reactions to these displays in
an attempt to fashion an identity that fits. Other times, self-construction is undertaken to
confirm an already established self-view. The successful Wall Street banker may wear
suspenders, carry a beeper, and drive a Lexus to signal to others that he is indeed a man of
wealth and taste. Swann (1990) calls this form of self-construction self-verification, and
Wicklund and Gollwitzer (1982) refer to such behavior as self-symbolizing.
Self-enhancement needs also underlie self-construction. Most people like to think of
themselves as being competent, likable, talented, and so forth. By convincing others that
they possess these positive attributes, people are better able to convince themselves. This,
in turn, makes people feel better about themselves. In this sense, we can say that people
seek to create impressions in the minds of others because it makes them feel good about
themselves to do so.
Finally, self-construction can serve a motivational function. People are expected to
be who they claim to be (Goffman, 1959; Schlenker, 1980). When they publicly announce
an intention or otherwise stake a claim to an identity, people experience additional
pressures to make good on their claims. The reformed alcoholic who proclaims his
sobriety is utilizing this function. By publicly renouncing the use of alcohol, he increases
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 5 of 42

his commitment to stay sober. We also see this in the world of sports. Before the 1968
Super Bowl, Joe Namath boldly predicted that his New York Jets would beat the Baltimore
Colts (which they did). The great boxer, Muhammad Ali, also routinely predicted the
outcomes of his fights. Under some circumstances, this kind of public boasting can serve to
make the idea a reality.
4. Summary
In this section, we distinguished three functions of self-presentation. Although
conceptually distinct, the three functions often operate simultaneously in the real world.
For example, airline pilots project an air of dignity because doing so (1) makes the plane
ride go smoother; (2) helps them retain their jobs; and (3) leads them to think of
themselves as dignified people, which in turn makes them feel good about themselves.
B. When and How Do People Manage Impressions?
People form impressions of us whenever we are in public, but we are not always
actively monitoring or regulating those impressions. In many situations, our self-
presentations are automatic or habitual, and we are devoting little conscious attention to
how we are being perceived by others. In other situations, we become acutely aware of the
impressions we are creating, and we actively strive to take control of these impressions
(Leary, 1993; Schlenker & Weigold, 1992). In this section, we will discuss factors that
influence when we are most likely to actively engage in self-presentation and what it takes
to successfully present ourselves to others.
1. Situational Variables That Influence Impression Motivation
The first component of self-presentation is a motivational one. Before we can create
a desired impression, we have to be motivated to do so. Several factors can arouse this
motive. One of the most important occurs when desired external rewards depend on the
judgments of others (Buss & Briggs, 1984; Leary & Kowalski, 1990; Schlenker, 1980). Job
interviews and first dates are two prototypic examples. In these situations, we are highly
concerned with making a positive impression and we try to put our best foot forward.
The motivation to engage in self-presentation also tends to increase when we are
the focus of other peoples attention. For some people, speaking in front of a group or
audience is an aversive experience, in part because it causes them to become highly aware
of their public identities. Certain stimuli, such as cameras and tape recorders, can also
make us aware of our public appearance, as they remind us of how we are seen by others
(Carver & Scheier, 1985; Scheier & Carver, 1982b).
Paradoxically, perhaps, being ignored or shunned by others can also increase self-
presentational concerns (Buss, 1980). Think of how you would feel if you were being
ignored by others at a party. Chances are this would make you feel acutely aware of
yourself and motivate you to make a positive impression. This occurs, in large part,
because being alone at a party is not a desired identity. In more general terms, we can say
that a motive to actively engage in self-presentation increases whenever we encounter
obstacles to creating a desired impression (Schlenker, 1985, 1986).
Familiarity with an audience is another factor that influences the nature of self-
presentational behavior (Leary, Nezlek, Downs, Radford-Davenport, Martin, & McMullen,
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 6 of 42

1994; Tice, Butler, Muraven, & Stillwell, 1995). Although there are exceptions, people are
generally more attentive to the impressions they are creating when they are interacting
with casual acquaintances and business associates than when they are interacting with
close friends, family members, and loved ones. Many people, for example, walk around the
house with their hair uncombed, but they wouldnt go out to a business meeting that way.
People also tend to be more modest and authentic when interacting with those they feel
close to (particularly those of the same sex) than when interacting with people they dont
know well. In the vernacular of the 1960s, people are more apt to let their hair down and
be themselves when they are in the company of people they feel comfortable with and
know well.
2. Social Acuity
Once we are motivated to create a particular impression, we need to possess an
awareness of how that impression can best be created. This cognitive ability is called social
acuity (Hogan & Briggs, 1986). Social acuity refers to our ability to know what we would
need to do in order to successfully create a desired impression. Usually this involves
adopting the perspective of other people and inferring what particular behaviors will give
rise to a particular impression in their minds. Imagine, for example, that I want to convince
you that I am witty. In order to do so, I must figure out what is required. I need to know
what behaviors I must execute in order to create the desired impression. This perspective-
taking ability is what we mean by social acuity.
Meads influence is apparent here. As discussed in Chapter 4, Mead (1934) argued
that in order to communicate effectively, people must be able to anticipate how their own
symbolic gestures will be interpreted by others. The same is true for successful self-
presentation. To create a desired impression, we must put ourselves in other peoples
shoes and discern what behaviors would produce a given impression. If we are inept at
adopting the other persons perspective, we are unlikely to create the impressions we
desire.
3. Behavioral Skills
Behavioral skills are the third component of successful self-presentation. People
need to be capable of performing the behaviors they believe will create a desired
impression. To return to an earlier example, I may be motivated to create the impression
that I have the wit of Noel Coward, and I may recognize that in order to create this
impression I need to toss out one bon mot after another. But wanting to create a particular
impression and knowing what it would take do not guarantee that I can pull it off. I also
need to be able to enact the desired behavior.
Numerous tactics are used to create a desired impression. Verbal claims are
perhaps the most common strategy. People selectively disclose, accidentally mention, or
overtly boast as a means of creating a particular impression. Like actors, we also use props
to establish our characters. Our hair, physique, figure, and clothing all serve to create
particular impressions of us in the minds of others. Although we may deny that self-
presentation is the most important consideration that guides our decisions in such matters,
few people claim that such decisions are made without any regard for the social
consequences. Those that do make such claims are typically trying to create an impression
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 7 of 42

of nonconformity or independence (Schlenker & Weigold, 1990)!


Even our movements signal to others what we are like. People draw inferences
about what we are like from observing our mannerisms and gestures and the way we stand
and walk (McArthur & Baron, 1983). Aware of this, people actively regulate their
movements to control the impressions others form of them. For example, unattached
people at a party or bar typically carry themselves differently than do those who are
accompanied by someone. Their behavior signals to others that they are available.
4. Summary
To summarize, successful self-presentation involves a mix of motivation and ability.
People can be motivated to make a particular impression, but they may fail to do so
because they are unaware of what behaviors are needed or because they arent able to
perform the appropriate behaviors. Viewed in this way, it can be seen that successful self-
presentation is a complicated affair. It requires a good deal of skill and sophistication.
Recognizing this complexity, Schlenker and Leary (1982a) theorized that social anxiety
arises when individuals are motivated to make a positive impression but they see little
likelihood that they will do so. In extreme cases, these doubts can be paralyzing and lead to
social phobias.
C. Individual Differences in Self-Presentation
Although everyone engages in self-presentation, people vary with respect to how
concerned they are with their public image and with the kinds of impressions they try to
convey. Before reading further about these differences, complete the scale shown in Table
7.1. When you are through, return to the text and learn more about this issue.
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 8 of 42

Table 7.1. The Self-Monitoring Scale


Please answer each of the following items True or False by circling T or F.
1. T F I find it hard to imitate the behavior of other people.

2. T F My behavior is usually an expression of my true inner feelings, attitudes, and beliefs.

3. T F At parties and social gatherings, I do not attempt to do or say things that others will like.

4. T F I can only argue for ideas which I already believe.

5. T F I can make impromptu speeches even on topics about which I have almost no information.

6. T F I guess I put on a show to impress or entertain people.

7. T F When I am uncertain how to act in a social situation, I look to the behavior of others for cues.

8. T F I would probably make a good actor.

9. T F I rarely seek advice of my friends to choose movies, books, or music.

10. T F I sometimes appear to others to be experiencing deeper emotions than I actually am.

11. T F I laugh more when I watch a comedy with others than when alone.

12. T F In a group of people I am rarely the center of attention.

13. T F In different situations and with different people, I often act like very different persons.

14. T F I am not particularly good at making other people like me.

15. T F Even if I am not enjoying myself, I often pretend to be having a good time.

16. T F I'm not always the person I appear to be.


I would not change my opinions (or the way I do things) in order to please someone else or win
17. T F
their favor.
18. T F I have considered being an entertainer.
In order to get along and be liked, I tend to be what people expect me to be rather than anything
19. T F
else.
20. T F I have never been good at games like charades or improvisational acting.

21. T F I have trouble changing my behavior to suit different people and different situations.

22. T F At a party I let others keep the jokes and stories going.

23. T F I feel a bit awkward in company and do not show up quite so well as I should.

24. T F I can look anyone in the eye and tell a lie with a straight face (if for a right end).

25. T F I may deceive people by being friendly when I really dislike them.
Note: To determine your score, give yourself 1 point if you answered true to items 5, 6, 7, 8, 10, 11, 13,
15, 16, 18, 19, 24, and 25, and 1 point if you answered false to items 1, 2, 3, 4, 9, 12, 14, 17, 20, 21, 22,
and 23. Add up your total score. Scores of 12 or less are characteristic of a low self-monitor; scores of
13 or more are characteristic of a high self-monitor. (Source: Snyder, 1974, Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 30, 526537.)

1. Self-Monitoring
Mark Snyder (1974) developed the scale shown in Table 7.1 to measure the degree
to which people monitor and control their behavior in public situations. High self-monitors
regard themselves as highly pragmatic and flexible people who strive to be the right person
for every occasion. When entering a social situation, they try to discern what the model or
prototypic person would do in that situation. They then use this knowledge to guide their
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 9 of 42

own behavior. Low self-monitors adopt a different orientation. They regard themselves as
highly principled people who value consistency between who they are and what they do.
When entering a social situation, they look inward and use their attitudes, beliefs, and
feelings to guide their behavior. Instead of striving to be the right person for the situation,
they strive to be themselves in social settings.
Individual differences in self-monitoring influence a wide range of social behaviors
(see Snyder, 1979, 1987, for reviews). In comparison with low self-monitors, high self-
monitors (1) pay more attention to the behavior of others in social situations; (2) prefer to
enter situations that provide clear guidelines for behavior; and (3) are more attracted to
careers that emphasize the importance of public behavior, such as acting, sales, and public
relations. High self-monitors also (4) are more adept at reading other peoples facial
expressions, and (5) are better at communicating a wider variety of emotions than are low
self-monitors.
High self-monitors also exhibit less congruence between their underlying attitudes
and their public behavior. They might, for example, say or do things they dont believe in if
doing so seems like the appropriate thing to do. This is less true for low self-monitors.
These individuals value congruence between their attitudes and their behavior. What they
say and do is more often a faithful reflection of what they truly believe. To illustrate these
differences, imagine that you and another person are discussing recent movies, and that
the person tells you she liked a particular movie that you did not like. What will you do?
Basically, you have three choices. You can (1) say you also liked the movie, even though
you didnt; (2) voice your true opinion and admit that you didnt like the movie; or (3)
avoid taking a stand, perhaps by changing the subject. All else being equal, high self-
monitors are more apt to choose the first option than are low self-monitors.
Friendship patterns are also influenced by differences in self-monitoring. High self-
monitors tend to have many different friends, each suitable for a different activity. For
example, they play sports with one friend, go to the theater with another, and talk politics
with yet another. This pattern allows them to express their characteristic orientation to be
a different person in different situations. In contrast, low self-monitors have relatively few
friends, and they engage in multiple activities with each one. They are more inclined to
play sports, go to the theater, and talk politics with the same friend. This pattern is
conducive to being the same person in all situations.
Table 7.2 summarizes the different orientations of high self-monitors and low self-
monitors with reference to the three components of self-presentation we discussed earlier.
High self-monitors are social chameleons. They enjoy being different people in different
situations, and they possess the cognitive and behavioral skills needed to play many roles.
In contrast, low self-monitors think of themselves as highly principled individuals who
cherish being true to themselves in various situations. They are also somewhat less
adept at reading the character of the social situation and their acting skills are not as well
developed.2

2Important questions have been raised about the self-monitoring construct and the scale Snyder (1974)
developed to measure self-monitoring. A thorough discussion of these issues can be found in Briggs, Cheek,
and Buss (1980) and Gangestad and Snyder (1985).
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 10 of 42

Table 7.2. Comparing high self-monitors and low self-monitors.

Component
High Self-Monitor Low Self-Monitor
Processes

Goals Be the right person for the situation. Be me in this situation.

Social Acuity Highly adept at reading the character of the Less adept at reading the character of the
situation and the behavior of others, and situation and the behavior of others. They
able and willing to use this knowledge to based their behavior on personal attitudes,
construct a prototype of the model person values, and dispositions.
for the situation.

Acting Ability Superior acting ability allows them to Limited acting skills lead them to play
modify their behavior to match the similar roles in various situations.
requirements of the situation.

2. Public Self-Consciousness
Related to differences in self-monitoring are differences in public self-
consciousness. In Chapter 6, we noted that people differ in the degree to which they focus
on their private, internal states. Fenigstein, Scheier, and Buss (1975) used the term private
self-consciousness to refer to these differences, and they developed a scale to measure
them. Fenigstein et al. also developed a scale to assess the degree to which people focus on
the public, observable aspects of themselves (see Table 7.3). People who score high in
public self-consciousness are very aware of themselves as a social object and think a lot
about their public appearance. Those who score low in public self-consciousness are less
aware of themselves as a social object and do not think as much about their public
appearance (Buss, 1980; Carver & Scheier, 1985; Scheier & Carver, 1982b).
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 11 of 42

Table 7.3. The Public Self-Consciousness Scale

Please indicate the extent to which each of the following items describes you by choosing one number on the
rating scale next to each item.
0 1 2 3 4
extremely extremely
uncharacteristic characteristic

1. Im concerned about my style of doing things. 0 1 2 3 4


2. Im concerned about the way I present myself. 0 1 2 3 4
3. Im self-conscious about the way I look. 0 1 2 3 4
4. I usually worry about making a good impression. 0 1 2 3 4
5. One of the last things I do before I leave my house
is look in the mirror. 0 1 2 3 4
6. Im concerned about what other people think of me. 0 1 2 3 4
7. Im usually aware of my appearance. 0 1 2 3 4
Public self-consciousness and self-monitoring share some similarities, and people who
score high in self-monitoring tend to also score high in public self-consciousness (Tomarelli &
Shaffer, 1985). The two constructs are not identical, however. Self-monitoring is a motivational
orientation. High self-monitors strive to be the right person for the situation. Public self-
consciousness is not a motivational orientation. People who score high in public self-
consciousness dont necessarily try to be the right person for the situation; they simply are
highly aware of themselves in social situations. Another key difference is that high self-
monitors enjoy opportunities for social interactions that allow them to display their (self-
perceived) acting skills, but people who are high in public self-consciousness do not necessarily
seek out opportunities to put on a show.
II. Creating Desired Impressions
A. What Impressions Do People Try to Create?
The number of impressions people try to create of themselves in the minds of others
is almost limitless. At the same time, these impressions tend to fall into a smaller number
of classes. Jones (1990; Jones & Pittman, 1982) distinguished five common self-
presentational strategies (see Table 7.4).
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 12 of 42

Table 7.4. Five Common Self-Presentational Strategies

Self-Presentational Impression Sought Prototypic Behaviors Self-Presentational Risks


Strategy

Ingratiation Likable Compliments, favors Insincere, deceitful

Self-Promotion Competent Boasting, showing off Conceited, fraudulent

Intimidation Powerful, ruthless Threats Reviled, ineffectual

Exemplification Virtuous, moral Self-denial, martyrdom Hypocritical, sanctimonious

Supplication Helpless Self-deprecation Manipulative, demanding

1. Ingratiation
Ingratiation is probably the most familiar impression management strategy. The
goal of ingratiation is to get the other person to like you. Since we tend to like people who
agree with us, say nice things about us, do favors for us, and possess positive interpersonal
qualities (e.g., warmth and kindness), it should come as no surprise that ingratiation can be
accomplished through imitation, flattery, doing favors for someone, and displaying positive
personal characteristics (Jones, 1990).
Ingratiation may backfire if it is too blatant. If your audience knows you are trying
to manipulate them, they may come to distrust or dislike you. This problem is rarely acute.
People want to believe they are likable and are liked by others. Consequently, they are
disinclined to believe that a show of admiration or affection from another person is
inauthentic or derives from an ulterior motive, even when such a motive is obvious to an
impartial observer (Jones & Wortman, 1973). For this reason, ingratiation (if it is at least
somewhat subtle) is often a highly successful self-presentational ploy.
2. Self-Promotion
Self-promotion is another common self-presentational strategy. Here we seek to
convince people of our competence. This is not the same as ingratiation. With ingratiation,
we are trying to get people to like us. With self-promotion, we are trying to get people to
think we are capable, intelligent, or talented.
In many situations, it is beneficial to be seen as both likable and competent. In academia,
for example, job offers are extended to applicants who are perceived as highly competent and
pleasant to be around. Unfortunately, it is not always easy to simultaneously display both of
these qualities. For example, modesty can be an effective form of ingratiation, but it rarely
instills a perception of competence. Conversely, blowing ones own horn may convince people
that you are competent, but it rarely leads to strong feelings of liking. For this reason, people
are often forced to blend or balance these two self-presentational strategies. Many braggarts do
not seem to understand this point, or else they are willing to sacrifice being liked for being
considered competent.
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 13 of 42

3. Intimidation
Ingratiation and self-promotion are the most common self-presentational strategies.
But there are others. Sometimes people want to be feared. This is intimidation. An
employer, for example, might want to be viewed as tough, powerful, or ruthless. These
views might serve to increase her workers productivity and soften their demands for
salary increases and other benefits. Former White House aide John Sununu once remarked
that he didnt care if he was disliked as long as he was respected and feared.
4. Exemplification
Another form of self-presentation is exemplification. With exemplification, people
attempt to create the impression that they are morally superior, virtuous, or righteous.
Exemplification is often portrayed by exaggerating the degree to which one has suffered
poor treatment at the hands of others or has endured excessive hardships.
5. Supplication
A final form of self-presentation is supplication. Supplication occurs when
people publicly exaggerate their weaknesses and deficiencies. For example, in earlier
times, women were expected to play helpless (rather than appear competent) in order
to attract a mate. Men do this as well, of course, as when a husband claims to not know
how to use the dishwasher or washing machine. The more general point is that people
will sometimes exaggerate their incompetence and frailties if doing so gets them what
they want. In extreme cases, these tendencies may underlie depression and other
psychological difficulties (Gove, Hughes, & Geerken, 1980; Leary & Miller, 1986).
B. What Constitutes a Desirable Impression?
No matter which impressions people try to convey, these impressions will be
effective only if they are accepted by others. Schlenker (1980, 1985; Schlenker & Weigold,
1989) has proposed that successful self-presentation always involves a trade-off between
two considerations: (1) beneficiality (presenting the most advantageous image possible)
and (2) believability (making sure the image you present is believable). To return to an
earlier example, job candidates strive to be seen as competent and diligent. But if they go
overboard and describe themselves in almost superhuman terms, they risk arousing
suspicion and inadvertently creating a bad impression.
1. The Role of Accountability
Several factors influence the believability of a self-presentational claim. These
factors include the acting ability of the self-presenter (highly skilled actors can make more
self-aggrandizing claims) and the ambiguity of the performance domain (the more
ambiguous the domain, the more self-aggrandizing a person can be). Accountability is
another important factor. People are accountable when their claims can be checked against
relevant facts. It is one thing to boast that you are an expert at chess if no one is around to
challenge you to a game; its quite another if there is a chess board handy and another
person waiting to test your claims. The broader point is that peoples self-presentational
claims should be more truthful when audiences are able to assess the veracity of these
proclamations.
Schlenker (1975) tested this hypothesis. In this study, participants were led to
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 14 of 42

believe that they would do very well or very poorly on an upcoming test. They were then
given the chance to present themselves to other people who either would or would not
learn how they did on the upcoming test.
Figure 7.1 displays some of the findings from this investigation. The figure shows
that the only condition in which participants did not present themselves in highly positive
terms (e.g., as having high ability at the task) was when they expected to do poorly and
believed the audience would learn how they did. When participants believed the audience
would not know how they performed, they publicly claimed high ability even when they
privately doubted that their ability was high.

High Private Expectancy


Low Private Expectancy
Self-Presentation of Ability

0.6

0.4 0.30
0.20 0.17
0.2

0.0

-0.2

-0.4 -0.49

-0.6
Audience Audience Aware
Unaware of of Upcoming
Upcoming Performance
Performance

Figure 7.1. Accountability and self-presentation. The only condition in which participants did not
present themselves as competent was when they expected to do poorly and believed the audience
would learn how they did. If they thought they could get away with claiming high competence, they
did. (Source: Schlenker, 1975, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 10301037)

2. Audience Reactions to Self-Presentational Claims


Presumably, the participants in Schlenkers (1975) study modified their claims in
the public performance condition because they feared being evaluated negatively if their
positive claims were shown to be nothing more than hollow boasts. Schlenker and Leary
(1982b) conducted a follow-up investigation to see whether such a pattern does, in fact,
evoke condemnation. Using a simulational methodology, they had participants evaluate
students whose claims about a test varied in their positivity. In some conditions, the
participants were told whether these claims were consistent with the students actual
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 15 of 42

performance (e.g., the student said he had done well and participants knew he had or
hadnt); in other conditions, the participants were given no information about how the
student had actually performed (e.g., the student said he had done well and participants
didnt know whether he had done well or not).
The upper portion of Figure 7.2 shows participants evaluations (judgment of liking)
for the student under the various conditions. It can be seen that participants generally
admired congruence between public claims and performance. Students whose
performance claims matched their performance were evaluated more positively than were
students whose performance claims did not match their performance. Participants
particularly disliked students who falsely claimed to have high ability.
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 16 of 42

Figure 7.2. Accountability and Evaluations. The top half of the figure shows that audiences generally
admire congruence between claims and performance. The bottom half of the figure shows that
audiences base their judgments of competence on test performance information if it is available, but
on self-presentational claims of ability if test performance data are not available. (Source: Schlenker
& Leary, 1982, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 89104)

High Ability Claim Medium Ability Claim Low Ability Claim

8
7.0
7 6.5
6.0 6.0
6 5.6
5.3 5.2 5.3
5 4.4 4.4
Liking

4 3.7

3 2.7

0
High Medium Low Control

Actual task Performance

High Ability Claim Medium Ability Claim Low Ability Claim

9
8.1 8.1 8.1
8
7.2
7
5.7 5.8 5.6
6
Competence

5.1
5
3.9 3.9
4
3.1
3 2.4
2

0
High Medium Low Control

Actual task Performance

The situation is different when it comes to competence ratings (see the bottom
portion of Figure 7.2). Performance claims had little effect here. Instead, students who did
well were judged to be more competent than students who did poorly, and it really didnt
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 17 of 42

make much difference whether the students claimed that their ability was high or low.
There is one interesting qualification to this conclusion, however. Notice that in the
unknown performance condition, students who claimed to have high ability were
considered more competent than students who claimed to have medium or low ability.
Apparently, in the absence of any contradictory information, people take a persons self-
presentational claims at face value: If you say you have high ability, people assume you do
have high ability. From a purely self-presentational point of view, then, the data suggest
that it will often be to your advantage to present yourself as reasonably competent and
able. Unless the audience knows you are deliberately misrepresenting yourself, they will
give you the benefit of the doubt and believe your claims.
3. Self-Promotion versus Self-Protection
The preceding analysis assumes that the benefits of making a positive impression
outweigh the costs of making a negative impression. This is not always the case, however.
In politics, for example, the candidate who wins an election is often the one with the lowest
negative rating rather than the one with the highest positive rating. This is why politicians
often equivocate or refrain from taking stands; it is often more important to avoid
alienating a block of voters than it is to favorably impress one.
Arkin (1981) has noted that individuals also differ in this regard. Some individuals
are especially motivated to avoid making a negative public impression. Such individuals
tend to play it safe in social situations, presenting themselves in relatively neutral,
modest, or noncommittal terms. This protective self-presentational style rarely leads to
highly favorable interpersonal reactions, but it also rarely leads to highly negative ones.
Several personality variables are thought to incorporate a heightened concern for
public self-protection. These personality variables include shyness (Arkin, 1987), low self-
esteem (Baumeister, Tice, & Hutton, 1989; Schlenker, Weigold, & Hallam, 1990),
depression (Hill, Weary, & Williams, 1986), and social anxiety (Baumgardner & Brownlee,
1987). In rare cases, fear of negative public evaluations can lead to social isolation (e.g.,
agoraphobia) or excessively self-defeating behavior patterns (Baumeister & Scher, 1988;
Schlenker, Weigold, & Doherty, 1991).
C. Identity Predicaments
Try as they might to avoid them, most people confront situations in which their
public identity is threatened. Some of these situations are relatively minorwhat the
French call a contretemps: We slurp our soup, get caught singing aloud in the car, or
otherwise embarrass ourselves by making a gaffe or faux pas. (Judging from how many
terms they have, it seems that this happens a lot in France!) Ordinarily, people extricate
themselves from such situations with only minimal effort.
Other identity predicaments are more consequential. One such situation occurs
when people are seen as responsible for bringing about a negative event or outcome
(Schlenker, 1982; Snyder, 1985; Weiner, Amirkhan, Folkes, & Verette, 1987). You might,
for example, keep people waiting by coming late to an important meeting; forget your
spouses birthday; or get caught in a white lie regarding your absence from a social
engagement.
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 18 of 42

1. Account Giving
People call upon a number of ingenious artifices to mitigate the negative
implications of these identity predicaments (Schlenker, 1980, 1982; Scott & Lyman, 1960).
Table 7.5 shows five such strategies in the context of an all-too-common situation that
arises in my housethe case where one brother accuses the other of hitting him.
Table 7.5. Account Giving

Actors Account Underlying Aim Example

Claim of innocence Deny responsibility I didnt hit him. He fell on his face
reaching for the remote control.

Reinterpretation Alter the meaning of the I didnt hit him. I was patting him on the
event face for being such a good brother.

Justification Legitimize behavior Yes, I hit him, but he hit me first.

Excuse Excuse behavior by reducing Yes, I hit him, but I have been under a lot
intentionality of stress lately and havent had time to
take my medication.

Apology Accept responsibility and Yes, I hit him. Im sorry. I wont do it


seek forgiveness again.

The first line of defense people use when confronting a threat to their identity is
often a claim of innocence. Here, people completely deny responsibility for a negative
outcome by asserting that they played no role in producing the event. If that doesnt work,
they might attempt to reinterpret the event in positive terms. This is a common ploy
among politicians, who habitually try to put a positive spin on a seemingly negative
outcome. (Yes, we came in third, but thats a victory for us. We were vastly outspent and
had hoped only to finish fifth. So actually, we won.)
In the next two accounts (justification and excuse), people admit responsibility for a
negative event, but they attempt to escape punishment by altering the audiences perception of
the circumstances that surrounded the event. With justification, the person attempts to
legitimize the negative behavior by making it seem warranted; with excuses, the person
attempts to rationalize the behavior by making it seem accidental or inadvertent. Finally, when
all else fails, one can admit full responsibility, apologize, and throw oneself on the mercy of the
court.
The saga of former Senator Bob Packwood provides a real-world example of these
strategies. Numerous women accused the senator of making unwanted sexual advances
over a period of many years. Initially, Packwood denied the events took place (or at least
remembering that they ever took place). He then attempted to reinterpret the events.
These were not lascivious sexual advances, he argued, but friendly kisses, no different
than when two men shake hands with one another. After it became evident that these
kisses were not platonic in nature, Packwood attempted to justify his behavior. I come
from a different generation, he said, one in which men were expected to be macho and
force themselves on women. When this account failed to appease the nation, Packwood
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 19 of 42

tried to excuse his behavior, claiming he was an alcoholic and wasnt responsible for his
actions. Finally, in a last ditch attempt to salvage his career, Packwood accepted
responsibility and apologized to any of the women he may have offended. Alas, even this
(belated) apology failed to mollify his critics, and Packwood was forced to resign from the
U.S. Senate in disgrace. (But dont feel too sorry for him. Hes now a well-paid lobbyist,
peddling influence in the very Senate corridors he once walked.)
2. Preemptive Excuse Making
In the examples just discussed, account giving occurred after the behavior in
question had taken place. People also engage in preemptive excuse making. (I should
probably warn you, Im not very good at this sort of thing.). Sometimes this excuse making
involves pointing out to others that we possess a characteristic that negatively affects
performance. Consider, for example, a study by Baumgardner, Lake, and Arkin (1985). The
participants in this study were led to believe that they would do poorly on an upcoming
memory test. Half of the participants (those in the experimental condition) were further
told that a bad mood could negatively affect their performance; other participants (those in
the control condition) were given no information about whether mood affected
performance. All participants then rated their moods before taking the test. The results
showed that participants in the experimental group reported being in a worse mood than
did control participants. Presumably, publicly claiming a bad mood allowed them to
preemptively excuse their expected poor performance on the upcoming task.
Snyder and Smith (1982) suggested that many clinically relevant conditions, such as
depression, substance abuse, and anxiety, serve a similar function (see also, Hill, Weary, &
Williams, 1986). An investigation by Smith, Snyder, and Perkins (1983) illustrates this
effect. These investigators hypothesized that hypochondriacs use self-reports of physical
illness and symptoms as a preemptive excuse-making strategy. To test this hypothesis,
they led participants to believe that poor health either could or could not negatively affect
ones performance on a test. All participants then filled out a questionnaire that assessed
their health. Hypochondriacal participants complained more than nonhypochondriacal
participants only when they believed that poor health could explain a poor performance.
These and similar findings (e.g., Smith, Snyder, & Handelsman, 1982; Snyder, Smith, Augelli,
& Ingram, 1985) suggest that some people use chronic symptoms to escape responsibility
for an anticipated negative performance or outcome.
3. Self-Handicapping Behavior
In some situations, people may even go so far as to actively create an excuse for
failure. This is self-handicapping behavior (Berglas & Jones, 1978; Jones & Berglas, 1978).
As discussed in Chapter 3, self-handicapping behavior occurs when people create obstacles
to their own success. Consider, for example, an athlete who fails to prepare properly for an
upcoming competition. Lack of preparation makes success less likely, but it also gives the
person a ready-made excuse for failure.
Self-handicapping was originally thought to be a private phenomenon that occurred
independent of the social context (Berglas & Jones, 1978; Jones & Berglas, 1978). More
recent research suggests, however, that self-handicapping is most apt to occur under public
performance conditions (Arkin & Baumgardner, 1985). For example, Kolditz and Arkin
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 20 of 42

(1982) found that only participants who believed their performance would be known to
others actively handicapped themselves on an upcoming task.
As a self-presentational tactic, self-handicapping is a double-edged sword. On the
one hand, creating an impediment to success makes it less likely that people will assume
you have low ability if your performance is poor. At the same time, people disapprove of
those who fail to maximize their potential by creating obstacles to their own success
(Luginbuhl & Palmer, 1991; Rhodewalt, Sanbonmatsu, Tschanz, Feick, & Waller, 1995;
Weiner, 1993). For these reasons, public self-handicapping would seem to be a last-ditch
impression-management tactic, useful only when success is highly unlikely and being
disliked is less aversive than being seen as incompetent.
D. Identity-Repair Tactics
Active attempts to repair a tarnished public identity are a final way of dealing with
an identity predicament. An investigation by Baumeister and Jones (1978) illustrates this
effect. The participants in this study were told that another person had negative
information about some of their personality characteristics (e.g., the other person had
learned the participant was somewhat insensitive or immature on the basis of some
personality scales the participant had completed earlier). The participants were then given
the opportunity to describe themselves on a questionnaire that was to be shown to the
other person. Some of the items on the questionnaire were relevant to information the
other person allegedly had; other items were new ones. Baumeister and Jones found that
participants tried to compensate for an initially bad impression by elevating their ratings
on the unrelated items. For example, if the other person thought the participant was
insensitive, the participant described herself as very mature; conversely, if the other
person thought the participant was immature, the participant described herself as very
sensitive. In this manner, participants attempted to rebuild a favorable public image
without contradicting the information the other person had about them (see also,
Baumeister, 1982a).
An additional way to restore a negative public image is to point out a connection you
share with another person who is somehow special or exemplary. For example, I might tell
you I once had dinner with Bob Dylans mother (now, arent you impressed?!). Cialdini and
his colleagues (Cialdini & De Nicholas, 1989; Cialdini & Richardson, 1980; Cialdini et al.,
1976) call this strategy basking in reflected glory. In an early demonstration of this
tendency, Cialdini et al. (1976) found that students were more likely to use the pronoun
we when discussing a football game their university team had won (We won!) than when
talking about a game their team had lost (They lost). Moreover, this tendency was most
apparent after students had first experienced a public failure, suggesting that it
represented an attempt to restore a positive social identity.
III. Self-Presentations and Private Self-Conceptions
To this point we have focused exclusively on the public side of self-presentational
behavior. But self-presentational behavior also has a very private side. Most obviously,
how people think about themselves influences how they present themselves to others. For
example, the person who thinks he is a connoisseur of fine wines is the one who is most apt
to try to impress his friends with his knowledge of a fine Bordeaux. Although theorists
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 21 of 42

disagree about just how strong this relation is, none deny that peoples ideas about
themselves are one factor that determines the public impressions they try to create.
More interesting, perhaps, is the manner in which public behavior influences private
self-conceptions. People are audiences for their own behavior. Just as our behavior might
convince others that we possess a given quality or attribute, so, too, might we convince
ourselves. Research from a variety of different quarters supports this claim.
A. Role Internalization
Everyone is always and everywhere, more or less consciously, playing a role.
. . . It is in these roles that we know each other; it is in these roles that we
know ourselves. (Park, 1926, p.137)
We all play many roles in social life. We are children, siblings, and parents; students,
friends, and employees; and so on. These roles figure prominently in the way people think
about themselves. When asked to describe themselves, people often respond with
reference to the social roles they play (e.g., I am a professor, a father, a husband).
This tendency to define ourselves in terms of our social roles is not the only link
between social roles and self-conceptions, however. Each role we play carries with it a set
of behavioral expectations (e.g., judges are expected to uphold the law) and assumptions
about personal characteristics (firefighters are expected to be brave). These personal
characteristics are of concern here. In the course of playing social roles, people often come
to internalize role-relevant personal characteristics. They come to see themselves as
possessing the qualities suggested by the roles they play (McCall & Simmons, 1966; Sarbin
& Allen, 1968; Stryker & Statham, 1985).
Role internalization occurs for many reasons. According to the reflected appraisal
process (see Chapter 3), social interaction is essential to the development of self-
conceptions. People form views of themselves based on the (perceived) reactions of
others. Before too long, the police officer who sees fear in the eyes of those she arrests
comes to regard herself as strong and powerful. Self-perception processes (Bem, 1972)
also operate here. People are often passive observers of their own behavior. A teacher
who helps struggling students with their homework might reasonably infer that he is a
caring, helpful person.
Of course, people gravitate toward roles that allow them to express their self-
perceived qualities, so the road between self-conceptions and roles goes in both directions.
But sometimes peoples self-views are initially at odds with the roles they adopt. Imagine
an employee who regards herself as a shy and deferent person. What becomes of such self-
views when she is promoted to a managerial position? Very often, she comes to adopt the
attitudes and beliefs that accompany her new title. She comes to think of herself as tough
and demandingbecause this is how she thinks others perceive her (reflected appraisals),
because this is how she is behaving (self-perception), or because to do otherwise would
create psychological inconsistency (or cognitive dissonance) between her actions and self-
relevant beliefs.
In summary, people thrust into new social roles often come to view themselves as
having the very qualities that the role demands. In effect, by playing the role, they become
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 22 of 42

the part. This does not mean, however, that individuals passively adopt the labels implied
by their social roles. Although the expectations of some roles are rigid and unyielding,
most are pliant and allow room for interpretation. This allows people to bring their own
distinctive stamp to the roles they play. The same is true in the theater. All actors who play
Hamlet are obliged to recite his famous To be or not to be soliloquy, but each actor is
allowed to emphasize different facets of the character. In more general terms, we can say
that people both create and are created by the social roles they play (Backman & Secord,
1968; Stryker & Statham, 1985). This is why expectations for social roles can change over
time. With every new role occupant, the role is reinvented (although the core expectations
tend to remain relatively constant).
B. Carry-Over Effects in Self-Presentation
Further evidence that public behavior alters private self-conceptions comes from
experimental research on carry-over effects in self-presentation. In these studies, participants
are asked to present themselves in specified ways to an audience. For example, some
participants might be asked to convince an interviewer that they are extraverted and sociable;
other participants are asked to convince an interviewer that they are introverted and reserved.
Later, as part of an ostensibly unrelated investigation, participants are asked to evaluate
themselves along this dimension. The typical finding is that self-presentational behavior carries
over to affect private self-conceptions. People who present themselves to others as outgoing
and gregarious subsequently regard themselves as more sociable than do those who present
themselves to others as shy and retiring (Fazio, Effrein, & Falender, 1981; Jones, Rhodewalt,
Berglas, & Skelton, 1981; Rhodewalt & Agustsdottir, 1986; Schlenker, Dlugolecki, & Doherty,
1994; Tice, 1992).
1. Theoretical Explanations
At least two explanations for this carry-over effect can be distinguished. One
possibility, which well call the cognitive accessibility model, traces the effect to a private,
intrapsychic process. According to this account, presenting oneself to others in a certain
manner (e.g., as sociable) activates or makes accessible certain behaviors and beliefs
consistent with that presentation (e.g., times we have behaved sociably). When people are
later asked whether they possess these qualities, the greater accessibility of these recently
enacted behaviors leads them to believe that they do. Note how this explanation views
public behavior as largely incidental to the carry-over effect. The effect reflects greater
cognitive accessibility, which is a private, intrapersonal process, not a public, interpersonal
one. Presumably, privately reviewing our past behaviors would produce a comparable
effect.
Another explanation, which well call the reflected appraisal model, views public
behavior as essential to the carry-over effect. This account builds on the notion that
peoples ideas about themselves are forged in the crucible of social life; that is, we come to
know ourselves by imagining how we appear in the eyes of others. According to this view,
public behavior is more consequential than private behavior. Publicly presenting ourselves
to another and seeing ourselves through that persons eyes has a greater impact on the way
we see ourselves than does private behavior.
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 23 of 42

2. Empirical Findings
In a test of these competing explanations, Tice (1992, Study 2) had participants
portray themselves as either extraverted or introverted. Half of the participants did this
publicly, in the context of a one-on-one interview; the other half did this privately, by
anonymously completing a questionnaire while seated alone in a room. The accessibility
model predicts carry-over effects in both conditions, whereas the reflected appraisal model
predicts that carry-over effects will be especially strong in the public condition.
Figure 7.3 presents the results of this investigation. In support of the reflected
appraisal model, it can be seen that carry-over effects were much stronger among
participants who presented themselves to another person than among participants who
merely completed a questionnaire under private and anonymous conditions (see also Kelly
& Rodriguez, 2006; Schlenker, Dlugolecki, & Doherty, 1994). These results suggest that
public behavior is uniquely potent when it comes to producing self-concept change.
Perhaps this is why many self-help therapies (such as Alcoholic Anonymous) operate in a
group setting, where people are asked to stand and publicly commit themselves to a course
of action.

Extraverted Presentation
Introverted Presentation
Post-task ratings of extraversion

80 77.60

70 67.55

61.36
60

50.75
50

40
Public Portrayal Private Portrayal

Figure 7.3. Carry over effects in self-presentation. Carry-over effects were stronger when self-
presentations were made in public than when they were made in private. (Source: Tice, 1992,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 435451)

This does not mean, however, that public behavior drastically changes the way
people think about themselves. Those who regard themselves as outgoing do not suddenly
claim to be wallflowers after presenting themselves as socially awkward. Instead, the
persons self-view simply shifts a bit in the direction implied by the behavior. Moreover, to
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 24 of 42

be effective, the self-presentational behavior must be at least somewhat consistent with


how the person already thinks of herself. Finally, the more certain you are of where you
stand on a given trait, the less likely it is that public behavior will alter your self-view
(Rhodewalt & Agustsdottir, 1986; Schlenker & Trudeau, 1990).
C. Symbolic Self-Completion Theory
The link between public behavior and private self-conceptions is also the focus of
Wicklund and Gollwitzers (1982) symbolic self-completion theory. Before turning to the
specifics of this theory, lets review a point we made earlier in this chapter. When
discussing the various functions of self-presentational behavior, we noted that people
sometimes engage in self-presentational behavior in order to privately establish an identity
for themselves (Schlenker, 1980). We called such behavior a form of self-construction
(Baumeister, 1982b). With self-construction, people use audiences as a means to an end.
They seek to convince others that they possess particular qualities because doing so helps
them convince themselves.
1. Social Validation of Identities
Symbolic self-completion theory builds on this function of self-presentational
behavior. The theory assumes that most important identities require social validation.
Before individuals can fully lay claim to possessing an identity (and experience a sense of
self-completion in the words of the theory), other people must recognize and acknowledge
their right to claim the identity. As an example, consider what it means to be a physician.
To be a physician in the truest sense of the word, other people need to acknowledge your
expertise and consult you in matters of personal health. Otherwise, you are a physician in
name only. This type of social validation is most obviously needed for the various social
roles we play, but it is also true for many personal identities. It is difficult for a person to
think he is popular if no one ever seeks his company.
Because most identities require social validation, people actively try to convince
others that they are entitled to claim the identity. According to symbolic self-completion
theory, one way they do this is by displaying socially defined, identity-relevant symbols
(hence the name, symbolic self-completion). In the case of a physician, this might entail
wearing a white lab coat, draping a stethoscope around ones neck, and learning to write a
prescription in an unintelligible scrawl! When recognized by others, these identity-relevant
symbols give people a sense of self-completion; they give people the sense that they truly
possess an identity or characteristic.
Gollwitzer (1986) tested the hypothesis that symbolic activities are more effective
in furnishing people with a sense of self-completion when these symbols are noticed and
recognized by others. Medical students were given a number of problems frequently
confronted by physicians and were asked to suggest some possible solutions (e.g., How
would you deal with a diabetic patient who refuses to follow the diet you prescribe?).
There were 45 such problems, and the students were told they could work on as many or
as few as they wanted. Shortly after the students had begun working on the problems, a
confederate appeared. In the social recognition condition, the confederate read through
the problems the student had completed and then addressed the student as a physician. In
the control condition, the confederate did not take notice of the completed problems and
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 25 of 42

did not address the student as a physician. The results revealed that students in the social
recognition condition chose to work on fewer problems than did those in the control
condition. Presumably, this occurred because being addressed as a physician by another
person provided these medical students with a sense of self-completion.
2. Seeking Symbolic Self-Completion
Symbolic self-completion theory makes another interesting prediction. Because
self-completion is most apt to occur when self-symbolizing efforts are recognized by
others, people committed to attaining an identity should be especially interested in finding
an audience for their symbolic activities. This should occur, according to the theory, even
though doing so might not be an effective self-presentational strategy at the time. Perhaps
you have encountered such a situation when chatting with another person at a party. The
person seems bent on convincing you that he possesses a certain characteristic (e.g., he is
rich, scholarly, dashing, or athletic), even though doing so is making a bad impression.
According to symbolic self-completion theory, this occurs because being liked is not the
persons goal. Instead, the person is seeking social validation for a desired identity and is
using the audience in a self-centered fashion to achieve this goal, without regard for what
the social costs might be.
Self-symbolizing individuals are not at all selective with respect to the people
they address. Nor are they interested in engaging in meaningful interactions
with the audience at their disposal. Rather, [they] appear to see in audiences
nothing more than passive witnesses of identity-related goal striving.
(Gollwitzer, 1986, p.149)
[They] focus only on demonstrating to others that they are in possession of
an intended identity, irrespective of others wishes, needs, or potential
responses. (Gollwitzer, 1986, p.154)
Gollwitzer (1986) reports a study relevant to this issue. The participants were
students committed to various academic identities (e.g., mathematician, biologist). During
the first part of the experiment, some of the students were led to focus on experiences that
suggested they were not well suited for these professions; other students were led to focus
on experiences that suggested they were well suited for these professions. These
manipulations were intended to arouse different needs for self-completion in the two
groups, with those in the negative feedback condition being more in need of self-
completion than those in the positive feedback condition.
In the second part of the experiment, the students were told they would engage in a
get-acquainted conversation with another student in which they would discuss various
topics. Allegedly, the other student had already indicated a preference for certain topics by
completing a questionnaire. These preferences revealed a strong disinterest in talking
about topics related to the participants interests (e.g., if the participant was a biology
major, the other student indicated that he or she was not interested in discussing biology).
Finally, the participants indicated which topics they wished to discuss. The results showed
that participants whose needs for self-completion had been raised by the negative feedback
were more eager to discuss topics related to their major, even though doing so risked
making a bad impression with the other person. One explanation for this finding is that
needs for self-completion were more powerful in this situation than were needs for social
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 26 of 42

approval.3
IV. Self-Presentation and Social Behavior
The central theme of this chapterthat people actively monitor and regulate their
public identitiesis relevant to understanding a good deal of social behavior. It also has
broad implications for psychological research. Most psychological research occurs in a
social context. Experimenters and other participants are often present in the situation, and
participants are almost always aware that their responses will be known to others. The
public nature of the experimental setting raises the possibility that participants behavior is
driven by a deliberate (or unconscious) desire to manage a particular social identity, rather
than by more spontaneous or private psychological processes.
Although this possibility has been known for some time (see, for example, Crowne &
Marlowe, 1964; Edwards, 1957; Erdelyi, 1974), interest in this topic blossomed in the 1970s. At
that time, many social psychological phenomena that had previously been understood in terms
of intrapsychic processes were reinterpreted in terms of self-presentational behavior
(Alexander & Knight, 1971). A paper by Tedeschi, Schlenker, and Bonoma (1971) illustrates
this perspective. These theorists were interested in the nature of cognitive dissonance effects
(see Chapter 5). Numerous studies had shown that participants change their attitudes when
they behave in ways that contradict their values or beliefs. According to cognitive dissonance
theory, this occurs because people find inconsistency between beliefs and behavior to be
aversive (Festinger, 1957). As a result, they change their attitudes in order to reduce this
internal state of distress.
Tedeschi et al. (1971) offered a different explanation (see also, Schlenker, 1982).
Instead of tracing attitude change to internal inconsistency, these theorists suggested that
people publicly change their attitudes in order to avoid looking like a hypocrite to others. This
perspective shifts the emphasis away from the minds inability to tolerate psychological
inconsistency toward a concern with maintaining a positive social identity. Baumeister (1982b)
subsequently applied a similar analysis to a wide range of social behavior, including conformity,
helping, aggression, and social facilitation effects. In each case, concerns with creating,
maintaining, or restoring a positive public identity were invoked to explain effects previously
attributed to more private, internal processes.
A. Self-Presentation and Self-Enhancement
Self-enhancement biases were another topic reinterpreted in self-presentational
terms (Tetlock & Manstead, 1985). As noted throughout this book (see especially Chapter
3), people evaluate themselves in very positive terms (Taylor & Brown, 1988). If, for

3Symbolic self-completion theory shares some similarities with Swanns (1990) self-verification theory. As
discussed earlier (e.g., Chapter 3), self-verification theory assumes that people seek social validation for their
self-conceptions and strive to get others to see them as they see themselves. The key difference between
Swanns approach and symbolic self-completion theory centers around whether the identity in question is
desired from the persons point of view. Swann argues that people seek to verify desired and undesired
identities, whereas symbolic self-completion theory maintains that people seek social validation only for
desired identities.
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 27 of 42

example, you ask people How kind, thoughtful, intelligent, and attractive are you? the
majority will tell you that they are kinder, more thoughtful, more intelligent, and more
attractive than most other people. People also accept greater responsibility for their
successes than for their failures, a phenomenon known as the self-serving bias in causal
attribution.
Originally, these biases were thought to stem from a motivated desire to feel good about
oneself (a desire we have called the self-enhancement motive). In the mid-1970s, alternative
explanations emphasizing cognitive processes were developed (see Chapter 5). Toward the end
of the 1970s, self-presentational explanations were developed to explain these effects.
Weary Bradley (1978) was one of the first psychologists to articulate this point of view.
Weary Bradley noted that in most investigations in which participants accepted greater
responsibility for success than for failure, their performance and attributions were known to
others. This raised the possibility that participants were merely claiming greater responsibility
for success than for failure in a self-presentational attempt to convince the experimenter of their
competence.
Despite its plausibility, research testing this claim has generally not found that
attributions are more self-serving in public than in private. In fact, just the opposite is true: Self-
serving attributions are generally stronger in private than in public (Greenberg, Pyszczynski, &
Solomon, 1982; Smith & Whitehead, 1988; Weary, Harvey, Schweiger, Olson, Perloff, &
Pritchard, 1982). This also occurs for other self-serving biases, such as the tendency to view
oneself in more positive terms than one views most other people (Brown & Gallagher, 1992)
and the tendency to preferentially seek positive self-relevant information (Brown, 1990).
Along with related work (Riess, Rosenfeld, Melburg, & Tedeschi, 1981; Greenwald &
Breckler, 1985), these findings are inconsistent with the claim that self-serving biases
reflect nothing more than public posturing. People genuinely believe their self-serving
claims. At the same time, people are not oblivious to the impressions they are creating.
They modify their public claims, but this modification is generally in the direction of
greater public modesty, not greater self-aggrandizement.
An investigation by Frey (1978) reveals the nature of these effects. The participants
in this study experienced either success or failure on an alleged intelligence test. Half the
participants believed the experimenter was aware of how they had performed (public
performance condition); the other half believed the experimenter was unaware of how
they had performed (private performance condition). Participants then rated the tests
validity under public conditions (they thought the experimenter would see their
responses) or under private conditions (they thought the experimenter would not see their
responses). In this fashion, Frey independently varied (1) whether the participants
performance was public or private, and (2) whether the participants assessment of the
tests validity was public or private.
The results of this investigation are shown in Figure 7.4. The data show that the
self-serving tendency to disparage the validity of the test after failure was strongest when
performances were public and evaluations were made in private. This corresponds to the
situation in which others know how you have performed on a test and you privately
explain your performance to yourself. Under such circumstances, peoples judgments are
February 19, 2013 at 9:44 PM 452_chapter_07.docx page 28 of 42

most self-serving (see also, Greenberg & Pyszczynski, 1985).


452_chapter_07.docx page 29 of 42

Public Performance Private Performance


Success Failure Success Failure
4.5 4.5
4.14
Rating of Test's Validity

Rating of Test's Validity


4.11

4.0 3.89 4.0


3.74
3.63

3.5 3.5 3.34


3.28

3.0 3.0
2.75

2.5 2.5
Public Evaluation Private Evaluation Public Evaluation Private Evaluation

Figure 7.4. Public Performance and Private Reactions. Evaluation of a tests validity under public performance conditions (panel 1) and private
performance conditions (panel 2). The data show that the self-serving tendency to belittle the validity of a test after failure is most apparent
when the performance is public and the evaluation is private. (Source: Frey, 1978, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 172179)
452_chapter_07.docx page 30 of 42

The pattern displayed in Figure 7.4 is quite consistent with other evidence
we have reviewed in this chapter. It shows again that public behavior is more
compelling than is private behavior. In the privacy of their own homes or minds,
people are free to try out different behaviors, opinions, and beliefs with little regard
for the consequences. This is much less true in public. When we publicly state an
opinion or behave in a given fashion, we are expected to be the person we claim to
be (Goffman, 1959); and when we display our qualities to others or others are
aware of our performance in some domain, we see ourselves reflected in their eyes
and these qualities and outcomes assume greater psychological reality. For these
reasons, social consensus and public validation bind and strengthen our self-views
(Gollwitzer, 1986; Hardin & Higgins, 1996; Schlenker, 1986; Swann, 1990).
B. Sincerity and Authenticity versus Pretense and Deceit
The final topic we will consider in this chapter is the extent to which peoples
self-presentations faithfully reflect how they think about themselves. It is obvious that
people sometimes misrepresent themselves (and some people do this more than
others). But how prevalent is this dissimulation? Do the faces people wear in public
generally mask how they privately see themselves, or is pretense and deceit the
exception rather than the rule?
Trilling (1971, cited in Baumeister, 1986) notes that interest in this issue was
particularly acute during the sixteenth century. At that time, Europeans became
virtually obsessed with the disparity between public behavior and private beliefs.
This is the era of Machiavelli and of Shakespeare, when the theater abounded with
characters whose evil intentions were known to the audience but not to the other
actors.
The notion that people cannot be trusted to be who they claim to be persists
today. Avon ladies flatter, used car salesmen wheedle, and con men and bunco
artists cheat and swindle by hiding their true intentions behind a facade of civility
and sincerity. Clearly, people are not always what they seem to be.
But whether most people typically present a false front to others is
questionable. After carefully considering this issue, several theorists (e.g., Buss &
Briggs, 1984; Leary, 1993; Schlenker, 1986; Tesser & Moore, 1986) have concluded
that, in their day-to-day lives, most people present a public identity that
corresponds with how they privately view themselves. There are at least two
reasons that this is so.
1. The Risks of Presenting a False Impression
Accountability is one relevant factor. Individuals who present an insincere
or inauthentic impression to others run the risk of being exposed as a fake or liar if
their attempts to create a false impression are discovered. Being regarded as a fake
is not a valued identity. Consequently, this concern keeps peoples self-
presentations in line with their private self-views.
This concern is particularly acute when future interaction is anticipated or
when audiences have knowledge of our past (Schlenker, 1980). These conditions
452_chapter_07.docx page 31 of 42

generally exist with people with whom we spend most of our timeour friends,
colleagues, and loved ones. It therefore follows that most of our self-presentations
are genuine and authentic.
2. A Desire for Social Validation
The need for social validation is another factor that yields convergence
between public behavior and private beliefs. People generally believe positive
things about themselves (e.g., that they are intelligent, kind, and generous), and they
want others to think they possess these qualities as well. Often, their public
behavior is in service of this goal. They actively strive to bring their (self-perceived)
positive qualities to the attention of others. For both of these reasons, then, it may
generally be the case that appearances made in the world are not veils but guides
to the authentic self of the wearer (Sennett, 1978, p. 153; cited in Tedeschi, 1986,
p.6).
452_chapter_07.docx page 32 of 42

V. Chapter Summary
All the worlds a stage, Shakespeare tells us, and all the men and women
merely players. They have their exits and their entrances, and one man in his time plays
many parts (As You Like It, act II, scene VII). And so it is. Much of our time is spent in
the company of others, and much of this time we are playing a role.
In this chapter we explored the connection between these public displays
and private self-conceptions. We began by considering the nature of self-
presentationthe functions it serves and the manner in which people go about
trying to create an impression of themselves in the minds of other people. We also
noted that people differ in regard to how actively they monitor and regulate their
public behavior.
We then looked more closely at the kinds of impressions people typically try
to convey. Here we noted that, in order to be successful, self-presentations must be
believed. We then discussed various tactics individuals use when their self-
presentational attempts go awry.
In the third part of the chapter, we examined the association between public
behavior and the way people privately think about themselves. For a variety of
reasons, people often come to view themselves in ways that are consistent with the
persona they publicly display.
Finally, we explored the extent to which behavior in social situations can be
accounted for in self-presentational terms, paying particular attention to self-
enhancement biases and the extent to which people are genuine or insincere in their
public behavior. We noted that peoples positive self-presentations are generally
believed, and that outright dissimulation and deceit are the exceptions rather than
the rule in peoples day-to-day lives.
Self-presentation is any behavior that is intended to create, modify, or
maintain an impression of ourselves in the minds of other people.
Self-presentation serves three important functions: (1)it helps facilitate
social interaction; (2)it enables individuals to attain material and social
rewards; and (3)it helps people privately construct desired identities.
There are three components of successful self-presentation. First,
individuals must be motivated to create a particular impression in the
minds of other people. Second, they must possess the cognitive ability to
know what particular behaviors will give rise to that impression. Finally,
they must be able (and willing) to enact the desired behaviors.
People differ in their self-presentational styles. High self-monitors enjoy
being different people in different situations, and they possess the cognitive
and behavioral skills needed to adapt their behavior to match the
requirements of the situation. In contrast, low self-monitors value
congruence between their actions and their underlying attitudes and are
less apt to tailor their behavior to match the requirements of the situation.
452_chapter_07.docx page 33 of 42

Five common self-presentational strategies were identified: (1)ingratiation


(we strive to get other people to like us); (2)self-promotion (we attempt to
convince other people of our competence); (3)intimidation (we try to lead
others to believe we are tough and ruthless); (4)exemplification (we aim to
create the impression that we are morally virtuous and righteous); and
(5)supplication (we seek to convince others that we are weak and helpless).
Successful self-presentation involves a balance between beneficiality
(presenting the most beneficial image for the situation) and believability
(making sure the image is believed by others). Aware of these factors,
individuals generally modify their behavior to match an audiences
knowledge and expectations.
Accountability is one factor that strongly influences believability. The more
accountable people are for their actions, the more likely they are to present
themselves in ways that match relevant facts.
People call upon a variety of strategies when they fail to make a desired
impression. These include accounts (people attempt to reinterpret, excuse,
or justify their actions) and image repair tactics, such as compensating for a
bad impression in one area by inflating the positivity of alternative
qualities.
People are often audiences for their own behavior. In seeking to convince
others that they possess particular qualities, they often wind up convincing
themselves. Sometimes this process is a rather passive one (as when the roles
we play filter down to affect the way we see ourselves), and sometimes it is a
very active one (as when we purposefully set out to create a private identity by
publicly behaving in a particular fashion).
People often seek to convince others that they possess positive qualities.
For the most part, these attempts genuinely reflect the way people privately
think about themselves, rather than representing insincere attempts to
deceive or mislead.
452_chapter_07.docx page 34 of 42

For Further Reading


Baumeister, R.F. (1986). (Ed.). Public self and private life. New York: Springer-
Verlag.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Anchor
Books.
Schlenker, B.R. (1980). Impression management: The self-concept, social identity,
and interpersonal relationships. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Schlenker, B.R. (1985). (Ed.). The self and social life. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Snyder, M. (1987). Public appearances/private realities: The psychology of self-
monitoring. New York: W. H. Freeman and Co.
452_chapter_07.docx page 35 of 42

References
Alexander, N. C., & Knight, G. W. (1971). Situated identities and social psychological
experimentation. Sociometry, 34, 65-82.
Arkin, R. M. (1981). Self-presentation styles. In J. T. Tedeschi (Ed.), Impression
management theory and social psychological research (pp. 311-333). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press
Arkin, R. M. (1987). Shyness and self-presentation. In K. Yardley & T. Honess
(Eds.), Self and identity: Psychosocial perspectives (pp. 187-195). New York:
John Wiley & Sons.
Arkin, R. M. & Baumgardner, A. H. (1985). Self-handicapping. In J. H. Harvey & G.
Weary (Eds.), Basic issues in attribution theory and research (pp. 169-202).
New York: Academic Press.
Backman, C. W., & Secord, P. F. (1968). The self and role selection. In. C. Gordon & K.
J. Gergen (Eds.), The self in social interaction (pp. 289-296). New York: Wiley.
Baumeister, R. F. (1982a). Self-esteem, self-presentation, and future interaction: A
dilemma of reputation. Journal of Personality, 50, 29-45.
Baumeister, R. F. (1982b). A self-presentational view of social phenomena.
Psychological Bulletin, 91, 3-26.
Baumeister, R. F. (1986). Identity: Cultural change and the struggle for self. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Baumeister, R. F., & Jones, E. E. (1978). When self-presentation is constrained by
the targets knowledge: Consistency and compensation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 608-618.
Baumeister, R. F., & Scher, S. J. (1988). Self-defeating behavior patterns among
normal individuals: Review and analysis of common self-destructive
tendencies. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 3-22.
Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Hutton, D. G. (1989). Self-presentational
motivations and personality differences in self-esteem. Journal of
Personality, 57, 547-579.
Baumgardner, A. H., & Brownlee, E. A. (1987). Strategic failure in social interaction:
Evidence for expectancy disconfirmation processes. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 52, 525-535.
Baumgardner, A. H., Lake, E. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1985). Claiming mood as a self-
handicap: The influence of spoiled and unspoiled social identities.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 349-358.
Berglas, S., & Jones, E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-handicapping strategy in
response to noncontingent success. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 36, 405-417.
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
452_chapter_07.docx page 36 of 42

experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1-63). New York: Academic Press.
Briggs, S. R., Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1980). An analysis of the self-monitoring
scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 679-686.
Brown, J. D. (1990). Evaluating ones abilities: Shortcuts and stumbling blocks on
the road to self-knowledge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26,
149-167.
Brown, J. D., & Gallagher, F. M. (1992). Coming to terms with failure: Private self-
enhancement and public self-effacement. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 28, 3-22.
Buss, A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Francisco: W. H.
Freeman & Co.
Buss, A. H. & Briggs, S. R. (1984). Drama and the self in social interaction. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1310-1324.
Carnegie, D. (1936). How to win friends and influence people. New York: Simon &
Schuster.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1985). Aspects of self, and the control of behavior. In
B. R. Schlenker (Ed.), The self and social life (pp. 146-174). New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Cialdini, R. B., & De Nicholas, M. E. (1989). Self-presentation by association. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 626-631.
Cialdini, R. B., & Richardson, K. D. (1980). Two indirect tactics of image
management: Basking and blasting. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 39, 406-415.
Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thorne, A., Walker, M. R., Freeman, S., & Sloan, L. R.
(1976). Basking in reflected glory: Three (football) field studies. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 366-375.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive: Studies in evaluative
dependence. New York: Wiley.
DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., & Epstein, J. A. (1996).
Lying in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 979-
995.
Edwards, A. L. (1957). The social desirability variable in personality assessment and
research. New York: Wiley.
Erdelyi, M. H. (1974). A new look at the new look: Perceptual defense and vigilance.
Psychological Review, 81, 1-25.
Fazio, R. H. Effrein, E. A., & Falender, V. J. (1981). Self-perception following social
interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 232-242.
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-
consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
452_chapter_07.docx page 37 of 42

Psychology, 43, 522-528.


Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.
Frey, D. (1978). Reactions to success and failure in public and private conditions.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 172-179.
Gangestad, S., & Snyder, M. (1985). To carve nature at its joints: On the existence
of discrete classes in personality. Psychological Review, 92, 317-349.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1986). Striving for specific identities: The social reality of self-
symbolizing. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private life (pp. 143-
159). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Gove, W. R., Hughes, M., & Geerken, M. R. (1980). Playing dumb: A form of
impression management with undesirable side effects. Social Psychology
Quarterly, 43, 89-102.
Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1985). Compensatory self-inflation: A response to
the threat to self-regard of public failure. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 49, 273-280.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1982). The self-serving attributional
bias: Beyond self-presentation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18,
56-67.
Greenwald, A. G., & Breckler, S. J. (1985). To whom is the self presented? In B. R.
Schlenker (Eds.), The self and social life (pp. 126-145). New York: McGraw-
Hill.
Hardin, C., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Shared reality: How social verification makes the
subjective objective. To appear in R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.,)
Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol., 3,
pp. xxx-xxx). New York: Guilford.
Hill, M. G., Weary, G., & Williams, J. (1986). Depression: A self-presentation
formulation. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private life (pp. 213-
240). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Hogan, R., & Briggs, S. R. (1986). A socioanalytic interpretation of the public and the
private selves. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private life (pp. 179-
188). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Jones, E. E. (1990). Interpersonal perception. New York: W.H. Freeman and Co.
Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. (1978). Control of attributions about the self through self-
handicapping strategies: The appeal of alcohol and the role of under-
achievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 200-206.
Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic self-
presentation. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 1, pp.
231-262). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
452_chapter_07.docx page 38 of 42

Jones, E. E., Rhodewalt, F., Berglas, S., & Skelton, J. A. (1981). Effects of strategic self-
presentation on subsequent self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 41, 407-421.
Jones, E. E., & Wortman, C. (1973). Ingratiation: An attributional approach.
Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Kelly, A. E., & Rodriguez, R. R. (2006). Publicly committing oneself to an identity.
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 185-191.
Kolditz, T. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1982). An impression management interpretation of
the self-handicapping strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
43, 492-502.
Leary, M. R. (1993). The interplay of private self-processes and interpersonal
factors in self-presentation. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the
self (Vol. 4, pp. 127-155). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Leary, M. R., & Kowalski, R. M. (1990). Impression management: A literature review
and two-component model. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 34-47.
Leary, M. R., & Miller, R. S. (1986). Social psychology and dysfunctional behavior.
New York: Springer-Verlag.
Leary, M. R., Nezlek, J. B., Downs, D., Radford-Davenport, J., Martin, J., & McMullen, A.
(1994). Self-presentation in everyday interactions: Effects of target
familiarity and gender composition. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 67, 664-673.
Leary, M. R., Tchividjian, L. R., & Kraxberger, B.E. (1994). Self-presentation can be
hazardous to your health: Impression management and health risk. Health
Psychology, 13, 461-470.
Luginbuhl, J., & Palmer, R. (1991). Impression management aspects of self-
handicapping: Positive and negative effects. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 17, 655-662.
McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Toward an ecological theory of social
perception. Psychological Review, 90, 215-238.
McCall, G. J., & Simmons, J. L. (1966). Identities and interactions. New York: The
Free Press.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Park, R. E. (1927). Human nature and collective behaviour. American Journal of
Sociology, 32, 733-741.
Rhodewalt, F., & Agustsdottir, S. (1986). Effects of self-presentation on the
phenomenal self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 47-55.
Rhodewalt, F., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Tschanz, B., Feick, D. L., & Waller, A. (1995).
Self-handicapping and interpersonal trade-offs: The effects of claimed self-
handicaps on observers performance evaluations and feedback. Personality
452_chapter_07.docx page 39 of 42

and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 1042-1050.


Riess, M., Rosenfeld, P., Melburg, V., & Tedeschi, J. T. (1981). Self-serving
attributions: Biased private perceptions and distorted public descriptions.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 224-231.
Ringer, R. J. (1973). Winning through intimidation. Los Angeles, CA: Los Angeles
Publishing.
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.
Sarbin, T. R., & Allen, V. L. (1968). Role theory. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.),
The handbook of social psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 488-567). Reading,
MA: Addison-Wesley.
Scheier, M. F. & Carver, C. S. (1982b). Two sides of the self: One for you and one for
me. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self
(Vol.. 2, pp. 123-157). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schlenker, B. R. (1975). Self-presentation: Managing the impression of consistency
when reality interferes with self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 32, 1030-1037.
Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management: The self-concept, social identity,
and interpersonal relationships. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Schlenker, B. R. (1982). Translating actions into attitudes: An identity-analytic
approach to the explanation of social conduct. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 15, pp. 193-247 ). New York:
Academic Press.
Schlenker, B. R. (1985). Identity and self-identification. In B. R. Schlenker (Ed.), The
self and social life (pp. 65-99). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Schlenker, B. R. (1986). Self-identification: Toward an integration of the private
and public self. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private life (pp. 21-
62). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Schlenker, B. R., Dlugolecki, D. W., & Doherty, K. (1994). The impact of self-
presentations on self-appraisals and behavior: The power of public
commitment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 20-33.
Schlenker, B. R. & Leary, M. R. (1982a). Social anxiety and self-presentation: A
conceptualization and model. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 641-669.
Schlenker, B. R. & Leary, M. R. (1982b). Audiences reactions to self-enhancing, self-
denigrating, and accurate self-presentations. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 18, 89-104.
Schlenker, B. R. & Trudeau, J. V. (1990). The impact of self-presentations on private
self beliefs: Effects of prior self-beliefs and misattribution. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 22-32.
Schlenker, B. R., & Weigold, M. F. (1989). Goals and the self-identification process:
452_chapter_07.docx page 40 of 42

Constructing desired identities. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Goal concepts in


personality and social psychology (pp. 243-290). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (pp.
243-271 only)
Schlenker, B. R., & Weigold, M. F. (1990). Self-consciousness and self-presentation:
Being autonomous versus appearing autonomous. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 59, 820-828.
Schlenker, B. R., & Weigold, M. F. (1992). Interpersonal processes involving
impression regulation and management. Annual Review of Psychology, 43,
133-168.
Schlenker, B. R., Weigold, M. F., & Doherty, K. (1991). Coping with accountability:
Self-identification and evaluative reckonings. In C. R. Snyder & D. F. Forsyth
(Eds.), Handbook of social and clinical psychology: The health perspective (pp.
96-115). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.
Schlenker, B. R., Weigold, M. F., & Hallam, J. R. (1990). Self-serving attributions in
social context: Effects of self-esteem and social pressure. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 855-863.
Scott, M. B., & Lyman, S. M. (1960). Accounts. American Sociological Review, 33, 46-
62.
Sennett, R. (1978). The fall of public man: On the social psychology of capitalism.
New York: Vintage.
Sharp, M. J., & Getz, J. G. (1996). Substance use as impression management.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 60-67.
Smith, S. H., & Whitehead, G. I., III. (1988). The public and private use of consensus-
raising excuses. Journal of Personality, 56, 355-371.
Smith, T. W., Snyder, C. R., & Handelsman, M. M. (1982). On the self-serving function
of an academic wooden leg: Test anxiety as a self-handicapping strategy.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 314-321.
Smith, T. W., Snyder, C. R., & Perkins, S. C. (1983). The self-serving function of
hypochondriacal complaints: Physical symptoms as self-handicapping
strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 787-797.
Snyder, C. R. (1985). Collaborative companions: The relationship of self-deception
and excuse making. In M. W. Martin (Ed.), Self-deception and self-
understanding: New essays in philosophy and psychology (pp. 35-51).
Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.
Snyder, C. R., & Smith, T. W. (1982). Symptoms as self-handicapping strategies: The
virtues of old wine in a new bottle. In G. Weary & H. R. Mirels (Eds.),
Integration of clinical and social psychology (pp. 104-127). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Snyder, C. R., Smith, T. W., Augelli, R. W., & Ingram, R. E. (1985). On the self-serving
function of social anxiety: Shyness as a self-handicapping strategy. Journal of
452_chapter_07.docx page 41 of 42

Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 970-980.


Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 30, 526-537.
Snyder, M. (1979). Self-monitoring processes. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 85-128). New York: Academic
Press.
Snyder, M. (1987). Public appearances/private realities: The psychology of self-
monitoring. New York: W.H. Freeman and Co.
Stryker, S. & Statham, A. (1985). Symbolic interaction and role theory. In G.
Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (3rd ed., Vol.
1, pp. 311-378). New York: Random House.
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1990) To be adored or to be known? The interplay of self-
enhancement and self-verification. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.),
Motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 408-448). New York: Guilford Press.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological
perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210.
Tedeschi, J. T. (1986). Private and public experiences and the self. In R. F.
Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private life (pp. 1-20). New York: Springer-
Verlag.
Tedeschi, J. T. & Norman, N. (1985). Social power, self-presentation, and the self. In
B. R. Schlenker (Ed.), The self and social life (pp. 293-322). New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Tedeschi, J. T., Schlenker, B. R., & Bonoma, T. V. (1971). Cognitive dissonance:
Private ratiocination or public spectacle? American Psychologist, 26, 685-
695.
Tesser, A., & Moore, J. (1986). On the convergence of public and private aspects of
self. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private life (pp. 99-116). New
York: Springer-Verlag.
Tetlock, P. E., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1985). Impression management versus
intrapsychic explanations in social psychology. Psychological Review, 92, 59-
77.
Tice, D. M. (1992). Self-concept change and self-presentation: The looking glass self
is also a magnifying glass. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
435-451.
Tice, D. M., Butler, J. L., Muraven, M. B., & Stillwell, A. M. (1995). When modesty
prevails: Differential favorability of self-presentation to friends and
strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1120-1138.
Tomarelli, M. M., & Shaffer, D. R. (1985). What aspects of self do self-monitors
monitor? Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 135-138.
452_chapter_07.docx page 42 of 42

Trilling, L. (1971). Sincerity and authenticity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University


Press.
Weary, G., Harvey, J. H., Schwieger, P., Olson, C. T., Perloff, R., & Pritchard, S. (1982).
Self-presentation and the moderation of self-serving attributional biases.
Social Cognition, 2, 140-159.
Weary Bradley, G. (1978). Self-serving biases in the attribution process: A
reexamination of the fact or fiction question. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 36, 56-71.
Weiner, B. (1993). On sin versus sickness: A theory of perceived responsibility and
social motivation. American Psychologist, 48, 957-965.
Weiner, B., Amirkhan, J., Folkes, V. S., & Verette, J. A. (1987). An attributional
analysis of excuse giving: Studies of a nave theory of emotion. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 316-324.
Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion. Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai