Anda di halaman 1dari 8

This article was downloaded by: [Tufts University]

On: 07 October 2014, At: 16:06


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Family Science
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rfsc20

Twin relationships: A comparison across monozygotic


twins, dizygotic twins, and nontwin siblings in early
childhood
a a a
Keren Fortuna , Ira Goldner & Ariel Knafo
a
Department of Psychology , The Hebrew University of Jerusalem , Israel
Published online: 04 May 2011.

To cite this article: Keren Fortuna , Ira Goldner & Ariel Knafo (2010) Twin relationships: A comparison across
monozygotic twins, dizygotic twins, and nontwin siblings in early childhood, Family Science, 1:3-4, 205-211, DOI:
10.1080/19424620.2010.569367

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19424620.2010.569367

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Family Science
Vol. 1, Nos. 34, JuneOctober 2010, 205211

RESEARCH ARTICLE
Twin relationships: A comparison across monozygotic twins, dizygotic twins, and nontwin
siblings in early childhood
Keren Fortuna*, Ira Goldner and Ariel Knafo
Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel
(Received 16 November 2010; final version received 4 March 2011)

Twin relationships remain understudied despite the increasing prevalence of twinning and the unique influence it entails
for individual development and family functioning. The current study assessed closeness, conflict, dependence, and rivalry
within 1281 pairs of 3-year-old twins via maternal reports. Relationship quality was compared based on the twins zygosity
and dyadic sex-composition. As expected, monozygotic (genetically identical) twins were perceived as closer and more
co-twin dependent relative to dizygotic (fraternal) twins. Same-sex twins were reported to have lower dependence and higher
levels of conflict and rivalry than opposite-sex dyads. As compared with a matched sample of 84 nontwin siblings, dizygotic
Downloaded by [Tufts University] at 16:06 07 October 2014

twins were described as closer and more co-dependent. Findings emphasize the need to consider the special nature of the
twin relationship and lend support for theories regarding the effects of genetic similarity and gender in shaping interpersonal
relationships.
Keywords: twins; twin relationships; sibling relationships; LIST

Starting from the womb, twins intimately develop side this end, we focused on the relationships of 1281 pairs
by side, and are constantly present for mutual compari- of monozygotic (MZ; genetically identical) and dizygotic
son and companionship (Castiello et al., 2010). Given the (DZ; fraternal) twins, as described by their mothers at
exclusive nature of their genetic relatedness, twins have age 3. By systematically comparing the quality of twins
been studied extensively in the context of behavior genetic relationships we hope to shed light on commonly held con-
research (Plomin, DeFries, McClearn, & McGuffin, 2001). victions regarding the nature of the twin bond which are
However, systematic research focusing on twin relation- likely to guide caregivers behaviors and decisions with
ships as a distinct type of sibling relationship and a unique regards to their twins (e.g., whether or not to separate them
social context is relatively scarce. Research on nontwin sib- in school).
lings clearly shows that siblings are an integral part of indi- Most of what is known about sibling relationships is
viduals lives, and a key resource in social and emotional derived from families with nontwin children. Does it gen-
development (Dunn, 2007). However, despite the impor- eralize to families with twins? Some believe that twinship
tance of sibling relationships, very little is known about constitutes a specific developmental context which alters
the more unique twin relationship. Most of the knowledge the usual circumstances governing development (Ainslie,
on this topic is based on rather small samples that despite 1985, p. 163). Yet it is not clear whether twins lives differ
providing detailed accounts and fascinating personal sto- considerably from those of the more common sibling type
ries (e.g., Ainslie, 1985) may not be representative of the (Although see Reiss, Neiderhiser, Hetherington, & Plomin,
twin population. Thus, there is a need for additional, large- 2000 for twin-sibling comparisons during adolescence).
scale, research on the development of individuals growing For example, do twins tend to more highly depend on each
up as twins, the bond between them, and ways in which the other, fight more frequently, and rival for parental atten-
presence of twins influences family functioning. tion? To address these questions, we have also sampled 84
Multiple births are becoming increasingly common, nontwin sibling pairs (1 to 5 years apart) and compared
such that in certain countries one of 33 children is a them to a matched twin sample.
twin or a higher multiple (Hay & Preedy, 2006). In the For many individuals their sibling relationships are
current study, we were interested in studying the rela- their most enduring relationships, lasting from childhood
tionships among twins and ways in which relationship through old age. During the early years, individuals spend
functioning may vary depending on twins zygosity (i.e., much of their free time with their siblings, often more than
degree of genetic similarity) and sex-composition. Toward with parents or peers (McHale & Crouter, 1996). Due to

*Corresponding author. Email: keren.fortuna@mail.huji.ac.il

ISSN 1942-4620 print/ISSN 1942-4639 online


2010 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/19424620.2010.569367
http://www.informaworld.com
206 K. Fortuna et al.

such frequency of interaction, combined with the built-in co-twin dependence was more prevalent among MZ pairs
need to compete for shared resources (e.g., parents, mate- (Penninkilampi-Kerola et al., 2005), adult MZ twins were
rial possessions) and perhaps a degree of social tolerance more likely to be attached to their co-twin (Tancredy &
for free expression of positive as well as negative feel- Fraley, 2006), and MZ twins showed higher grief inten-
ings, sibling interactions are often emotionally loaded and sity to losing their twin (Segal, Wilson, Bouchard, & Gitlin,
uninhibited (Dunn, Stocker, & Plomin, 1990). Sibling rela- 1995). In a Prisoners Dilemma game between twins, MZ
tionship quality varies markedly across dyads, and such pairs were significantly more altruistic toward each other
differences have been shown to relate to a variety of (yet not toward an unknown child; Segal, Connelly, &
developmental outcomes. Conflict (in moderation) within Topoloski, 1996), and less competitive than their DZ twin
the sibling relationship provides a context for emotional counterparts (Segal & Herschberger, 1999). Similarly, in
expression, understanding others feelings, and learning a puzzle completion task (Segal, 1984), MZ twins were
to resolve differences (Dunn, 1991, 2007). In this sense, more cooperative and likely to complete the task in contrast
siblings act as socializing agents promoting the develop- to higher competitiveness (e.g., aggression, monopolizing)
ment of theory of mind, perspective taking, and empathy that took place among DZ pairs.
(Cutting & Dunn, 2006). That said, elevated levels of hos- Given inherent differences between MZ and DZ twins
tility and coercion between siblings can serve as training in their level of genetic overlap, evolutionary-driven bio-
grounds for antisocial behaviors (Bank, Patterson, & Reid, logical effects have been suggested as driving zygosity
1996). differences in twins relationships. One such explanation
Downloaded by [Tufts University] at 16:06 07 October 2014

Twins, like other siblings, vary from extreme close- is derived from Kin Selection Theory (Hamilton, 1964),
ness to excessive conflict (Ainslie, 1985; Schave & Ciriello, which suggests that one would be more likely to act altruis-
1983). Being of the same age and constantly present makes tically toward a close relative with whom he or she shares
the co-twin a significant person in ones life, for better and genes as a way to increase ones inclusive fitness (i.e., the
for worse. For some, the ever-present twin comes to serve survival and reproduction of ones genes). Hence, higher
a comforting, soothing function, thus fostering prolonged genetic similarity should be related to higher cooperation
mutual dependence (Ainslie, 1985; Penninkilampi-Kerola, and lower competition. Based on this evolutionary logic,
Moilanen, & Kaprio, 2005). Twins often describe strong MZ twins, who share 100% of their DNA sequence, are
empathic reactions toward their co-twins and fear of los- expected to have a stronger incentive to cooperate as com-
ing them. At an extreme, twin co-dependence restricts pared to DZ twins, who share on average about 50% of
their ability to develop individual identities (Hay & Preedy, their genes. This is because genetically speaking, helping
2006). An interesting phenomenon identified in twins is ones identical twin is like helping oneself (Segal, 1999,
the use of a secret language (Rutter & Redshaw, 1991), p. 101). A more proximal explanation is that character sim-
perhaps developing when the bond is especially tight. ilarities between MZ twins (Plomin et al., 2001), bringing
Rivalry/competition is another significant aspect of about more shared interests and abilities, make it easier for
sibling relationships, which may be especially salient in them to get along.
twins, given that the same-age sibling is always present for It is important to note that MZ twins closeness was not
direct comparison (Hay & Preedy, 2006), and given lim- found to mean complete lack of mutual antagonism (Segal,
ited resources (e.g., parental attention) available for them 1984), and despite observed differences in cooperation
to share from the womb onward. Rivalry may be fueled by and competitiveness, previous reports of twin relationships
parents and others tendency to inadvertently (but almost did not identify MZ-DZ differences in amount of fighting
inevitably) compare the two, who are developing side by (Segal, 1999). Thus, while previous evidence leads to the
side, which can push twins to compete with each other. hypothesis that MZ twins have a closer, more dependent,
and less competitive relationship than DZ twins, zygosity
differences in conflict intensity are less clear. In this study
Zygosity we provide the largest to-date investigation of these aspects
A major question that arises when studying twin rela- of twin relationships.
tionships involves whether considerable differences exist
among the two twin types (MZ and DZ). Some twin
researchers emphasize the uniqueness of having a geneti- Twins and other siblings
cally identical twin (Segal, 1999), a connection described What about twin versus nontwin sibling relationships? The
as the closest tie between two individuals (Burlingham, presence of a same-age sibling, and circumstances specific
1945). Though empirical studies of twins relationships to twins early lives, significantly alter their environment
are in short supply, some support for this view is pro- as compared with that of nontwin siblings (Ainslie, 1985).
vided. For example, the existence of an exclusive language To name a few, twins need to deal with a sibling who is
was found to be more common among MZ than DZ pairs similar in terms of developmental status rather than older
(Mittler, 1970); as compared to DZ twins, adolescents or younger; there may be a need to more directly compete
Family Science 207

for attention as parents divide their time between two chil- The hypotheses were: (1) MZ twins were expected to
dren with compatible needs (Rutter & Redshaw, 1991), be higher on ratings of closeness and co-dependence, and
heightening awareness of parental differential treatment. lower on rivalry as compared to DZ twin pairs, yet no
Different-age siblings, on the other hand, are more likely zygosity differences were anticipated for conflict levels.
to seek out different forms of parental attention, and accept (2) We hypothesized that DZ twins would have higher
differential treatment based on their relative ages. Some closeness and co-dependence ratings relative to nontwin
have indeed observed heightened competition among twins siblings. We did not postulate a clear hypothesis regarding
relative to siblings, most often linked to parental attention conflict and rivalry differences given somewhat conflict-
(Lytton, 1980). Then again, the fact that limited one-on-one ing expectations and scarcity of previous research. (3)
parental attention is a given right from the start for twins, Twin girl dyads were hypothesized to have higher levels of
while not always so for nontwin siblings, may keep twin closeness/dependence, and lower levels of conflict/rivalry
rivalry at bay. than boy dyads.
On a more positive note, twins enjoy the availability
of a same-age playmate, are likely to have more common Method
interests at each developmental point, and thus to develop Participants
more intimate bonds relative to siblings who differ in age Twins. Twin pairs (N = 1337) participated in a longitudi-
and developmental stage. Adult twins are found to be more nal study of twins (Knafo, 2006). Contact details for twins
likely than nontwins to regard their sibling as an attachment
Downloaded by [Tufts University] at 16:06 07 October 2014

families were provided by the local government office,


figure, suggesting that twin relationships serve a nurturing based on information about all mothers giving birth to more
function that is non-typical among nontwins (Tancredy & than one child within 24 hours during the years 20042005.
Fraley, 2006). Many open questions remain regarding twin- Families were contacted with mail surveys at the time of the
nontwin differences, especially early in life. In this study, twins third birthday. The surveys asked mothers to report
we examine some of these questions by empirically com- about their twins characteristics and relationship with each
paring pairs of DZ twins and nontwin siblings on several other (in addition to questions which are beyond the focus
relationship indicators. of this report).
Zygosity was determined by the Zygosity Question-
Dyadic sex-composition naire for Young Twins (Goldsmith, 1991) using an algo-
Few studies have examined the potential effects of rithm which has been shown to be over 95% accurate when
dyadic sex-composition on twins relationship function- compared to DNA testing (Price et al., 2000). This zygos-
ing. Lemery and Goldsmith (2001) found twin boys to be ity questionnaire contains questions regarding genetically
higher on conflict, whereas girls were higher on initiating determined physical differences between the twins (e.g.,
cooperation. Moreover, girl-only DZ pairs were higher on height, weight, hair and eye color, timing of teeth eruption),
cooperation relative to other twin pairs. Self-reported co- likelihood of confusion between the twins by family mem-
twin dependency was more prevalent among girls, both bers, friends, and caretakers, and medical information (e.g.,
in same and opposite-sex dyads (Penninkilampi-Kerola blood type, possible medical reasons for differences). In
et al., 2005). Akerman and Suurvee (2003) have identified addition, mothers are also asked about their opinion regard-
exaggerated levels of co-dependence (symbiotic relation- ing the twins zygosity (i.e., whether they think the twins
ships) among adolescent female twins (especially MZ), but are identical or fraternal), and whether they were ever given
not in male-only dyads. The present study aimed to iden- any medical opinion regarding their twins zygosity. These
tify potential early-emerging effects of sex-constellation two questions were not included in the zygosity algorithm.
on twins relationships by examining differences between Twin pairs for which we could not determine zygosity
same versus opposite-sex dyads, and between male and (N = 27), or for whom more than three items on the
female pairs. relationship questionnaire were missing (N = 29) were
excluded from the analyses. The final twin sample con-
The current study sisted of 272 MZ pairs and 1009 DZ pairs; mean age 37.16
months (SD = 2.62).
In order to understand the characteristics of twin relation-
Nontwin siblings. Mothers whose child attended the
ships in early childhood we studied the relationships of
same daycare as one of the twins were asked to report
preschool twin dyads, with the largest twin sample to date
about the relationship between their 3-year-old child (the
to address this issue. The twin relationship questionnaire
target child) and his/her closest-age sibling. Mothers of 93
employed in this study asked mothers to report on levels of
sibling pairs returned the survey. We excluded seven pairs
inter-twin conflict, closeness, dependence, and rivalry in
for whom more than three questionnaire items were miss-
order to test ways in which relationships vary as a function
ing and two pairs because the non-target siblings were too
of zygosity, sibling type (i.e., DZ twins vs. nontwins),
old (22 years) or too young (1 month). The final sample
and sex-composition (i.e., same vs. opposite-sex, boys vs.
consisted of 84 pairs. The target childrens mean age was
girls).
41.29 months (SD = 4.17), and the mean age-difference
208 K. Fortuna et al.

between siblings was 33.65 months (SD = 14.99). The tar- his/her sibling, though correlations were weaker (mean
get child was matched to a DZ twin according to the childs r = .55).
age, childs sex, mothers country of birth, mothers edu- Multidimensional scaling was used to analyze the
cation, number of siblings, and religiosity. The matched structure of the twin relationship questionnaire. Analyses
sub-sample of DZ pairs was similar to the full DZ sam- were conducted on standardized values using the ALSCAL
ple on all relationship variables, though they were slightly routine (Figure 1; stress = .08). The items clearly mapped
older (M = 40.3, SD = 4.05 and M = 36.87, SD = 2.26, on four constructs conflict, closeness, dependence, and
respectively). rivalry. Results were further confirmed by a Principal Com-
ponent Factor Analysis. Items within each factor were
averaged. Internal reliabilities were satisfactory; Cron-
bachs alphas: closeness .74, conflict .81, dependence .78,
Measures and rivalry .73.
We constructed the Twin Relationship Questionnaire by
adopting items from several questionnaires assessing sib-
ling relationships (Volling & Blandon, 2005; Stocker,
Results
Dunn, & Plomin, 1989 described in Dunn, Deater-Deckard,
& Pickering, 1999) and adding items inspired by descrip- Twin relationships by zygosity
tions of twin relationships in Hay and Preedy (2006). The Table 1 presents means and standard deviations of the four
Downloaded by [Tufts University] at 16:06 07 October 2014

items (see Figure 1) were selected with the intention of cap- relationship dimensions by twin and sibling types. First, we
turing behaviors indicating closeness, conflict, dependence, examined whether twins relationship quality varied across
and rivalry between twins. Mothers were asked to rate the zygosity groups. In order to unconfound possible effects of
degree to which each statement is characteristic of each of sex-composition, MZ twins were compared with same-sex
her twins, using a scale of 1 = not characteristic at all to DZ (SSDZ) twins only. MZ pairs had significantly higher
5 = very characteristic. levels of closeness, t(777) = 5.36, p < .001 (d = .38) and
As we wished to capture the relationship as pertaining dependence, t(777) = 5.18, p < .001 (d = .37) as compared
to both twins, mean scores were computed between moth- with DZ pairs. The groups did not significantly differ on
ers reports of the twins behaviors. All the correlations conflict and rivalry levels.
between twin A and B were high (r > .64) and significant These results suggest an effect for zygosity on twin
(p < .001; mean r = .74), with the exception of one item relationship quality. An alternative explanation was tested.
(teases and provokes the other twin; r = .47), which It is possible that the observed differences between MZ
was dropped from subsequent analyses. For comparison and DZ twins were due to tendencies of mothers of MZ
purposes, mothers reports regarding nontwin siblings pairs to inflate closeness and dependence among their twins
relationships were also averaged across the target child and relative to mothers of DZ twins. In order to address this

Figure 1. Multidimensional scaling of the twin relationship questionnaire items.


Family Science 209

issue, we relied on motherss answer to the question about closeness and dependence. Interestingly, close-age sib-
whether they thought that their twins were identical or lings were also somewhat [t(106) = 1.87, p < .07, d = .36]
fraternal. Parents are sometimes mistakenly informed by higher on rivalry (M = 3.26, SD = .94) than their matched
hospital personnel that their twins are fraternal because twins (M = 2.93, SD = .86).
two placentas were present (which can occur with either
twin type). Also, some parents believe that their twins
are fraternal simply because they notice phenotypic (i.e., Twin relationships by sex
behavioral) differences (Goldsmith, 1991). Therefore, we Finally, we investigated the effects of sex-composition on
did not use their answers to classify the twins but as a twins relationships. Initially, SSDZ versus opposite-sex
way to control for mothers knowledge of their twins true DZ dyads were compared (MZ twins were not included
zygosity. Indeed, mothers of 94 MZ pairs (36% of MZ in these analysis). SSDZ twins had lower dependence
twins) reported thinking that their twins were non-identical. levels, t(1007) = 1.98, p < .05 (d = .12), higher rivalry,
This variation allowed us to compare between the mother- t(1007) = 1.98, p < .05, (d = .12) and conflict [a marginally
reported relationship characteristics of MZ twins whose significant effect, t(1007) = 1.66, p < .10 (d = .10)].
mothers correctly said they were identical to those whose The influence of sex was further examined by
mothers mistaken for DZ twins. There were no significant comparing same-sex male versus female dyads, sepa-
differences between the groups on any of the relationship rately for MZ and DZ twins. SSDZ girl dyads were
dimensions, indicating that zygosity differences were likely described as having higher levels of closeness than SSDZ
Downloaded by [Tufts University] at 16:06 07 October 2014

not due to maternal reporting biases. boy pairs, t(505) = 2.06, p < .05 (d = .18) and lower
conflict [approaching significance, t(505) = 1.73, p = .08
(d = .15)]. Among MZ pairs, boys were described as more
Relationships by sibling type co-dependent than girls, t(270) = 2.06, p < .05 (d = .25).
We next compared twin relationships to those of nontwin
siblings. Only DZ twins were selected for this comparison
to unconfound sibling-type from zygosity. As compared Discussion
with matched nontwin siblings, DZ twins had significantly Twin relationships remain understudied despite increased
higher levels of closeness, t(166) = 4.25, p < .001 (d = .66) commonality of twinning and their influences on individ-
and dependence, t(166) = 2.63, p < .01 (d = .41). Twins ual development and families lives. With its large sample
and siblings did not significantly differ on levels of conflict size and direct comparison between twins and nontwin
and rivalry (Table 1). siblings, the current study enabled us to empirically test
To test whether larger age-differences between sib- whether characteristics of sibling relationships, as reported
lings drive these effects we first examined whether age- by their mothers, vary systematically as a function of
difference related to relationship quality. Age-difference genetic relatedness, sibling type, and sex-composition.
between siblings was not associated with closeness, con- Though both closeness and conflict appear to be part
flict, and dependence, though it was marginally associated of twins everyday experiences, maternal ratings of close-
with less rivalry (r = -.21, p < .06). We further com- ness among twins were, on average, very high (top of
pared the matched twins to a sub-sample of 54 sibling Table 1). Thus, our findings suggest that twins at this age
pairs whose age-difference was relatively small (up to 34 indeed seek and enjoy each others company. Following
months the mean age-difference of the 84 sibling pairs; evolutionary logic (Hamilton, 1964; Segal & Herschberger,
M = 24.48, SD = 5.77), and may therefore be thought to 1999), genetically identical twin pairs were hypothesized
most closely resemble twins. Twins were still higher on to have stronger bonds than twins who are not genetically

Table 1. Mean levels (SD) of twin relationship dimensions by groups.

Sample N Closeness Conflict Dependence Rivalry

Full twin sample 1281 4.41 (.49) 2.83 (.83) 3.40 (.81) 3.10 (.93)
MZ twins 272 4.56 (.45) 2.84 (.89) 3.60 (.80) 3.13 (1.00)
Same-sex DZ twins 507 4.37 (.50) 2.87 (.82) 3.29 (.80) 3.15 (.90)
Opposite-sex DZ twins 502 4.38 (.49) 2.79 (.80) 3.39 (.81) 3.04 (.91)
Matched DZ twins 84 4.41 (.44) 2.69 (.74) 3.21 (.84) 2.99 (.83)
Nontwin siblings 84 4.10 (.51) 2.54 (.85) 2.89 (.75) 3.13 (.97)
Male MZ twins 150 4.58 (.45) 2.83 (.92) 3.69 (.75) 3.09 (1.03)
Female MZ twins 122 4.54 (.46) 2.86 (.86) 3.49 (.84) 3.18 (.96)
Male DZ twins 248 4.32 (.53) 2.94 (.80) 3.29 (.83) 3.15 (.84)
Female DZ twins 259 4.41 (.46) 2.81 (.83) 3.29 (.77) 3.15 (.84)
210 K. Fortuna et al.

identical. Our results confirmed this hypothesis; monozy- identifying sex-differences by helping overcome gender
gotic twins were depicted as sharing stronger closeness and biases in reporting dependence.
dependence relative to same-sex dizygotic pairs. Reliance on maternal reports is also a limitation in
These relationship dynamics may indeed be a func- this study. The ratings of twins relationship characteris-
tion of kin selection pressures shaping interactions between tics depend on mothers observations and interpretations
more versus less genetically alike individuals within fam- of their twins interactions, and thus at least partly reflect
ilies. Nonetheless, caution should be exerted not to con- maternal perceptions of the relationships. That said, parent-
fuse proximate and ultimate mechanisms (de Waal, 2008). reports of sibling relationships have been shown to be
Whereas closer MZ twin relationships may serve an ulti- valid, and to relate to siblings self-reported relationship
mate evolutionary goal of promoting ones genetic survival quality (Stoneman & Brody, 1993). The twin relationship
through the welfare of a genetically identical kin, individ- questionnaire did not ask mothers to evaluate twins rela-
uals cannot be expected to knowingly apply this logic in tionship quality or even to directly rate levels of twins
their relationships (e.g., Warneken & Tomasello, 2009). closeness or dependence, but rather we asked regarding
Childrens behavior toward their twin is likely motivated each twins observable behaviors toward his/her co-twin
by more proximate goals. Behavior genetic studies indicate under certain situations, making it more about the twins
that MZ twins are more similar than DZ twins on intelli- actual behaviors and somewhat less about mothers percep-
gence and basic temperamental tendencies (Plomin et al., tions. The use of surveys in the present report enabled us to
2001). Hence, it may be partly about interacting with a per- reach hundreds of families across the country, and since our
Downloaded by [Tufts University] at 16:06 07 October 2014

son who is like you which fosters the stronger bond that interest was in twins early relationships it was impossible
sets this relationship apart. to directly survey the children. Nevertheless, being aware
Dizygotic twins were depicted as closer and more of the importance of this issue, as we follow this cohort,
co-dependent than nontwin siblings. Notably, siblings who we plan to use multiple methods and informants to further
are closest in age were also higher on ratings of rivalry assess twins relationships.
than twins, whereas MZ and DZ twins were no different In addition, one may wonder whether differences
on rivalry levels. Thus, variables other than genetic similar- among zygosity groups and twins versus siblings reflect
ity appear to play a role in differentiating twins from other what mothers think their childrens relationships should
siblings, lending support for the idea that there is some- be like, with tendency to perceive twins, MZ in particular,
thing unique about the bond between twins, who go through as exceptionally close. We have partly dealt with this
development together, which promotes greater closeness, issue by showing that MZ twins whose mothers mistaken
reliance, and acceptance of the others needs, including them for fraternal twins were reported to be just as close
those that conflict with ones own. and dependent as twins whose mothers know they are
Sibling relationship research yields inconsistent find- genetically identical.
ings with regards to effects of sex-composition on relation- In the current report, we have focused on mean level
ship quality. Our findings show that mothers report lower differences in relationship characteristics across zygosity
dependence and stronger rivalry among same-sex twins as and sibling types. Future research may wish to further
compared with opposite-sex twins. It may be that same- compare the groups by looking at within-dyad differences.
sex pairs have more similar needs, making the struggle for Thus, by considering reports regarding each twins relation-
shared resources especially tight. Also, parents are tempted ship behaviors separately, differences between twins within
to draw comparisons between their children, more so when each dyad can be examined. In this fashion it would be
they are alike (e.g., same-sex twins), which may indirectly possible to test whether, for example, MZ twins are not
set the stage for stronger competitiveness. only perceived to have a tighter bond, but also as being
As hypothesized, we found girl dyads to be described more similar to each other on their need for closeness and
as closer and less conflictual than boys, but only among dependence than are DZ twins.
DZ twins. Counter to expectations, DZ boys and girls had It is our hope that this paper inspires further research
similar levels of mother-reported dependence and among on twin relationships, and that a better understanding
MZ dyads it was the boys who were described as more of this relationship will help shape informed decisions
co-dependent. Previous findings that suggested higher lev- with regards to twins development. For example, a major
els of dependence among twin girls (Penninkilampi-Kerola decision that parents face is whether or not to separate
et al., 2005) were based on adolescent twins self-reports. twins in school. Some studies show detrimental effects of
It is possible that higher dependency among girls emerges classroom separation on childrens psychological wellbe-
only later. Alternatively, boys may be more reluctant ing and school achievement, especially among MZ twins
to admit to dependency, which can explain why sex- (Tully et al., 2004). Although others find only partial sup-
differences tended to disappear (or reverse) in maternal port for this effect (Van Leeuwen, Van Den Berg, Van
reports about adolescent twins. Thus, the use of moth- Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma, 2005), it is clear that under-
ers reports in the present study may have contributed to standing the relationship between twins can aid in making
Family Science 211

decisions that best serve their needs. In conclusion, this relationships: Their causes and consequences (pp. 173195).
study emphasizes the need to consider the special nature of Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing.
twins relationships on their own right as well as a mean for Mittler, P. (1970). Biological and social aspects of language
development in twins. Developmental Medicine and Child
providing valuable information on factors that affect close Neurology, 12, 741747.
relationships more generally. Penninkilampi-Kerola, V., Moilanen, I., & Kaprio, J. (2005). Co-
twin dependence, social interactions, and academic achieve-
ment: A population-based study. Journal of Social and
References Personal Relationships, 22, 519541.
Ainslie, R.C. (1985). The psychology of twinship. Lincoln, NE: Plomin, R., Defries, J.C., McClearn, G.E., & McGuffin, P. (2001).
University of Nebraska Press. Behavioral genetics (4th ed.). New York: Worth.
Akerman, B.A., & Suurvee, E. (2003).The cognitive and identity Price, T.S., Freeman, B., Craig, I.W., Petrill, S.A., Ebersole,
development of twins at 16 years of age: A follow-up study L., & Plomin, R. (2000). Infant zygosity can be assigned
of 32 twin pairs. Twin Research, 6, 328333. by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Research, 3,
Bank, L., Patterson, G.R., & Reid, J.B. (1996). Negative sibling 129133.
interaction patterns as predictors of later adjustment prob- Reiss, D., Neiderhiser, J.M., Hetherington, E.M., & Plomin,
lems in adolescent and young adult males. In G.H Brody R. (2000). The relationship code: Deciphering genetic and
(Ed.), Sibling relationships: Their causes and consequences social influences on adolescent development. Cambridge,
(pp. 197229). Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing. MA: Harvard University Press.
Burlingham, D. (1945). The fantasy of having a twin. Rutter, M., & Redshaw, J. (1991). Annotation: Growing up as
Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1, 205210. a twin: Twin-singleton differences in psychological devel-
Downloaded by [Tufts University] at 16:06 07 October 2014

Castiello, U., Becchio, C., Zoia, S., Nelini, C., Sartori, L., et al. opment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32,
(2010). Wired to be social: The ontogeny of human interac- 885895.
tion. PLoS ONE, 5, e13199. Schave, B., & Ciriello, J. (1983). Identity and intimacy in twins.
Cutting, A.L., & Dunn, J. (2006). Conversations with siblings and New York: Praeger.
with friends: Links between relationship quality and social Segal, N.L. (1984). Cooperation, competition, and altruism within
understanding. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, twin sets: A reappraisal. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5,
24, 7387. 163177.
de Waal, F.B.M. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: Segal, N.L. (1999). Entwined lives: Twins and what they tell us
The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, about human behavior. New York: Plume Press.
279300. Segal, N.L., Connelly, S.L., & Topoloski, T.D. (1996). Twin
Dunn, J. (1991). Understanding others: Evidence from naturalis- children with unfamiliar partners: Genotypic and gender
tic studies of children. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories influences on cooperation. Journal of Child Psychology and
of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday Psychiatry, 37, 731735.
mindreading (pp. 5161). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell. Segal, N.L., & Herschberger, S.L. (1999). Cooperation and
Dunn, J. (2007). Siblings and socialization. In J.E. Grusec & competition between twins: Findings from a prisoners
P.D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and dilemma game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 29
research (pp. 309327). New York: The Guilford Press. 51.
Dunn, J., Deater-Deckard, K., & Pickering, K. (1999). Siblings, Segal, N.L., Wilson, S.M., Bouchard, T.J., & Gitlin, D.G. (1995).
parents, and partners: Family relationships within a longi- Comparative grief experiences of bereaved twins and other
tudinal community study. Journal of Child Psychology and bereaved relatives. Personality and Individual Differences,
Psychiatry, 40, 10251037. 18(4), 511524.
Dunn, J., Stocker, C., & Plomin, R. (1990). Assessing the rela- Stocker, C., Dunn, J., & Plomin, R. (1989). Sibling relationships:
tionship between young siblings: A research note. Journal of Links with child temperament, maternal behavior, and family
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 983991. structure. Child Development, 60, 715727.
Goldsmith, H.H. (1991). A zygosity questionnaire for young Stoneman, Z., & Brody, G.H. (1993). Sibling temperament, con-
twins: A research note. Behavior Genetics, 21, 257269. flict, warmth, and role asymmetry. Child Development, 64,
Hamilton, W.D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behav- 17861800.
ior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 152. Tancredy, C.M., & Fraley, C.R. (2006). The nature of adult twin
Hay, D.A., & Preedy, P. (2006). Meeting the educational needs relationships: An attachment-theoretical perspective. Journal
of multiple birth children. Early Human Development, 82, of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 7893.
397403. Tully, L.A., Moffitt, T.E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Kiernan, H.,
Knafo, A. (2006). The Longitudinal Israeli Study of Twins & Andreou, P. (2004). What effect does classroom separa-
(LIST): Childrens social development as influenced by genet- tion have on twins behavior, progress at school, and reading
ics, abilities, and socialization. Twin Research and Human abilities? Twin Research, 7, 115124.
Genetics, 9, 791798. Van Leeuwen, M., Van Den Berg, S.M., Van Beijsterveldt,
Lemery, K.S., & Goldsmith, H.H. (2001). Genetic and envi- T.C.E.M., & Boomsma, D.I. (2005). Effects of twin separa-
ronmental influences on preschool sibling cooperation and tion in primary school. Twin Research and Human Genetics,
conflict: Associations with difficult temperament and parent- 8, 384391.
ing style. In K. Deater-Decjard & S.A. Petrill (Eds.), Gene- Volling, B.L., & Blandon, A.Y. (2005). Positive indicators of sib-
environment processes in social behaviors and relationships ling relationship quality: The sibling inventory of behavior. In
(pp. 7597). Binghamton, NY: The Haworth Press. K.A. Moore & L.H. Lippman (Eds.), What do children need
Lytton, H. (1980). Parent-child interaction: The socialization pro- to flourish: Conceptualizing and measuring indicators of pos-
cess observed in twin and singleton families. New York: itive development (pp. 203219). New York: Springer Science
Plenum Press. +Business Media.
McHale, S.M., & Crouter, A. (1996). The family contexts of Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). The roots of human
childrens sibling relationships. In G.H. Brody (Ed.), Sibling altruism. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 455471.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai