Anda di halaman 1dari 3

Current Biology

Dispatches
nonaggregating cells can explain the 14. Ridley, A.R., Raihani, N.J., and Bell, M.B.V. 18. Brown, S.P., West, S.A., Diggle, S.P., and
coexistence of diverse genotypes in cellular (2010). Experimental evidence that sentinel Griffin, A.S. (2009). Social evolution in
slime molds. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, behaviour is affected by risk. Biol. Lett. 6, microorganisms and a trojan horse
27762781. 445448. approach to medical intervention strategies.
Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B. 364, 3157
11. Stearns, S. (1992). The Evolution of Life 15. Ridley, A.R., Nelson-Flower, M.J., and 3168.
Histories (Oxford: Oxford University Press). Thompson, A.M. (2013). Is sentinel behaviour
safe? An experimental investigation. Anim. 19. Garca-Contreras, R., Perez-Eretza, B., Lira-
Behav. 85, 137142.
12. Jousset, A., Rochat, L., Pechy-Tarr, M., Keel, Silva, E., Jasso-Chavez, R., Coria-Jimenez, R.,
C., Scheu, S., and Bonkowski, M. (2009). Rangel-Vega, A., Maeda, T., and Wood, T.K.
16. Dumas, Z., and Kummerli, R. (2012). Cost of
Predators promote defence of rhizosphere cooperation rules selection for cheats in (2014). Gallium induces the production of
bacterial populations by selective feeding on virulence factors in Pseudomonas aeruginosa.
bacterial metapopulations. J. Evol. Biol. 25,
non-toxic cheaters. ISME J. 3, 666674. 473484. Pathog. Dis. 70, 307320.

13. Clutton-Brock, T.H., ORiain, M.J., Brotherton, 17. Christian, M.S., and Ellis, A.P.J. (2011). 20. Ross-Gillespie, A., Weigert, M., Brown, S.P.,
P.N.M., Gaynor, D., Kansky, R., Griffin, A.S., Examining the effects of sleep deprivation and Kummerli, R. (2014). Gallium-mediated
and Manser, M. (1999). Selfish sentinels in on workplace deviance: A self-regulatory siderophore quenching as an evolutionarily
cooperative mammals. Science 284, 1640 perspective. Acad. Manage. J. 54, robust antibacterial treatment. Evol. Med.
1644. 913934. Public Health 2, 1829.

Human Gustation: When the Brain Has Taste


Ulrike Toepel and Micah M. Murray*
The Laboratory for Investigative Neurophysiology (The LINE), Department of Radiology & The Electroencephalography Brain Mapping Core,
Center for Biomedical Imaging (CIBM), University Hospital Center and University of Lausanne, Lausanne 1011, Switzerland
*Correspondence: micah.murray@chuv.ch
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.002

What we put into our mouths can nourish or kill us. A new study uses state-of-the-art electroencephalogram
decoding to detail how we and our brains know what we taste.

Imagine you get lost in the forest and Crouzet et al. [3] were able to without some drawbacks. On the one
become hungry. You find some rather accomplish this by capitalizing on the hand, spraying tastes onto the tongue is
nice looking berries. Their colouring and confluence of two recent methodological ethologically artificial and is thus far
fragrance make you think they are ripe advances. The first concerns how to removed from how we normally ingest
and probably tasty. You have also seen a deliver tastants within a laboratory foods. Still, this is an important step
rabbit eating them, with no apparent setting. This is no small feat when one forward, as the majority of clinical
detriment [1]. So, you eat a handful. You is trying to control when, what and for research on taste still electrically
may have just unknowingly poisoned how long a stimulus is delivered. The stimulates the tongue [5]. On the other
yourself with deadly nightshade (Atropa improvement of experimental procedures hand, a spray gustometer is in many
belladonna). Poisons are readily found in has been dramatically helped by the regards cumbersome and impractical
plants (e.g. the alkaloids atropine (found in commercial availability of a device that for any but the most motivated of
nightshade), arsenic, curare, strychnine, controls the delivery of liquid tastants, participants. Experiments are typically
and hemlock) and have historically been called a spray gustometer. The crucial quite lengthy because there is a long
particularly effective for both nefarious as innovations of this device are its ability to wash-out period between trials, and
well as medicinal purposes (Figure 1). control the timing of stimulus presentation participants need to keep their mouths
Given the vital importance of our sense of (this is essential for eliciting time-locked open and tongue immobilized (which can
taste, it is perhaps surprising that our brain activity as described below), to make speaking quite a challenge). Such
understanding of the neural basis of taste provide rapid rise-time in terms of notwithstanding, spray gustometers allow
perception in humans remains rather stimulus intensity and to minimize for both precision in stimulation as well
rudimentary, particularly when compared confounding effects of changes in as controlled trial-to-trial variability in the
with other senses, such as vision, hearing somatosensory and temperature inputs qualities of tastants hitherto unavailable
and touch [2]. In a recent issue of Current in the mouth (there is a constant flow of to scientists.
Biology, Crouzet et al. [3] provide water interspersed with tastant delivery) The second methodological advance
evidence for just how quickly information [4]. These controls are of critical used by Crouzet et al. [3] concerns how to
about tastes is decoded by the brain and, importance when trying to isolate and analyze electroencephalographic (EEG)
moreover, how this neural signature characterize the brain response to taste recordings. They applied a multivariate
relates to perceptual outcome. per se. However, this method is not pattern analysis (MVPA) framework to

Current Biology 25, R362R383, May 4, 2015 2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved R381
Current Biology

Dispatches

By combining a spray gustometer for


stimulus delivery and MVPA for the
analysis of EEG signals, Crouzet et al. [3]
show that within the initial 175 ms after
a tastant is delivered to the tongue the
brains responses differ according to
whether the tastant was salty, sweet,
sour or bitter. Because these distinctions
were in terms of the topography of the
EEG at the scalp [68], it means that
each tastant activates distinct
configurations of brain networks inside
the brain. Moreover, the MVPA analyses
revealed that sufficient information was
contained within the instantaneous
single-subject and single-trial responses
to decipher which tastant had been
presented on any given trial. This is a
remarkable result because it obfuscates
the need for signal averaging [8] and
thus circumvents some of the
Figure 1. A taste of death. aforementioned paradigmatic
This painting by Jacques Louis David depicts Socrates, who was sentenced to poison himself with
hemlock after being convicted of heresy. One cruelty of this manner of execution, particularly in the shortcomings imposed by taste research
case of a philosopher like Socrates, is that hemlock has limited effect on the central nervous system in general and by using a spray
(death is ultimately the consequence of respiratory failure). Socrates was aware both of what he was gustometer specifically.
drinking and what it was doing to him as he was dying. (Image: Catharine Lorillard Wolfe Collection,
Wolfe Fund, 1931; http://www.metmuseum.org/collection/the-collection-online/search/436105).
In a further analysis, the authors
demonstrate the specificity of the EEG
topography for a given tastant as well
their data. MVPA is not particularly novel clinical EEG systems [67]. A related as for a specific moment in stimulus
from a statistical standpoint. However, issue thus concerns the distillation of processing. MVPA-based classification
it is quite revolutionary when it comes to signals for analysis in EEG research. failed when using data from one
its application to EEG (or its magnetic Typical EEG analyses apply signal moment in time to decode later points
counterpart magnetoencephalography; averaging to obtain event-related in time of responses to the same
MEG) [68], though its origins can be potentials [68] and entail selection of tastant. This classification likewise
traced to the pioneering works of one or a few scalp electrodes (presuming failed when using data from one tastant
individuals like Dietrich Lehmann in the that recordings were made from to classify responses to another
1970s [9]. The basic idea is to use the multiple locations on the head) as well tastant. In other words, topographic
added information that is available from as one or a few time periods information is unique in time and to a
recording EEG from multiple scalp of interest that bracket established specific tastant. Next, Crouzet et al. [3]
locations simultaneously to in turn components or other archetypical directly linked the single-trial brain
better distinguish between responses to signals [10]. This type of approach for responses with perception. The errors
different experimental conditions. It is a EEG analysis can have important made by the MVPA-based classifier
bit like geographic surveys one gets a shortcomings with regard to both were significantly correlated with those
more detailed picture by collecting data statistical rigor as well as made by the participants themselves.
all across the length of a mountain range neurophysiological interpretability [68]. As above, this correlation was apparent
rather than just from the base and peak. Researchers studying taste, however, starting at 175 ms after the delivery of
The basic approach with MVPA is to often are severely limited when it the tastant. No such correlations were
train an algorithm with regard to the comes to the numbers of trials that can observed with subjective reports
differentiating features of responses to a be reasonably acquired during an concerning either the pleasantness or
given set of stimuli (e.g. a set of tastants experiment [2,4,11]. It may therefore the intensity of the tastant. Critically,
or a set of mountain ranges). Once not be quite so surprising that the none of this information could be
trained, MVPA then tests to what extent human brains response to taste gleaned from the univariate
the algorithm can successfully label had hitherto remained so poorly data from a single electrode. This
previously unseen data that were not characterized. MVPA can dramatically finding underscores not only the added
used during the training. Therefore, improve this situation by allowing value of MVPA based on high-density
MVPA is most effective when recordings researchers to use the data from all of EEG montages in basic, clinical and
and analyses involve a large number of the recorded channels and to perform applied research, but also how
scalp electrodes something readily analyses at the level of groups, important it is to consider the dynamics
feasible with current research and individual subjects or single trials. of widely distributed brain networks

R382 Current Biology 25, R362R383, May 4, 2015 2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved
Current Biology

Dispatches

when characterising sensation and developmental trajectory of this ability, event-related potentials. Neuroreport 22,
299303.
perception [68]. but also to be implemented in challenging
Given the relative infancy of our paediatric, geriatric and clinical 5. Stillman, J.A., Morton, R.P., Hay, K.D., Ahmad,
understanding of the neural bases of taste populations [68]. Because the methods Z., and Goldsmith, D. (2003).
Electrogustometry: strengths, weaknesses,
perception in humans, the results of are sufficiently powered to study
and clinical evidence of stimulus boundaries.
Crouzet et al. [3] should in many respects responses to single-trial events in Clin. Otolaryngol. Allied Sci. 28, 406410.
be taken as a rallying cry to show that individual participants, it would be
researchers should be undaunted in the feasible to conjoin these methods 6. Murray, M.M., Brunet, D., and Michel, C.M.
(2008). Topographic ERP analyses: a
face of a challenging sensory modality with genetic assays [12], clinical step-by-step tutorial review. Brain Topogr. 20,
such as gustation. Several questions will populations with impaired taste (e.g. as 249264.
require continued, extensive research, a consequence of neurodegeneration or
7. Michel, C.M., and Murray, M.M. (2012).
only a few of which are elaborated here. chemotherapy) or neuropharmacological Towards the utilization of EEG as a brain
First, if brain signals provide sufficient interventions. Third, it will be essential to imaging tool. Neuroimage 61, 371385.
information at one instant in time apply these methods to understand the
8. Tzovara, A., Murray, M.M., Michel, C.M., and
to accurately decode which of four perception of more complex tastes and De Lucia, M. (2012). A tutorial review of
tastants was delivered, then why should flavours as well as the multisensory nature electrical neuroimaging from group-average to
discriminant signals persist over time? It of food perception in general [13]. single-trial event-related potentials. Dev.
Neuropsychol. 37, 518544.
will likewise be important to ascertain how Collectively, these kinds of efforts and
information about taste is accrued over more specifically the approach taken by 9. Lehmann, D., and Skrandies, W. (1980).
time. The MVPA was applied here on each Crouzet et al. [3] may provide a better Reference-free identification of components of
checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential
data point independently. While Crouzet understanding as to why my 4-year-old fields. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.
et al. [3] indeed show that the topography refuses to eat asparagus, while my 48, 609621.
of the response sufficiently varies over 7-year-old adores it and more generally
10. Picton, T.W., Bentin, S., Berg, P., Donchin, E.,
time so that decoding of test data from why certain foods may truly be an Hillyard, S.A., Johnson, R., Jr., Miller, G.A.,
time x is unsuccessful if based on training acquired taste. Ritter, W., Ruchkin, D.S., Rugg, M.D., and
the classifier with data from time x-y, they Taylor, M.J. (2000). Guidelines for using human
event-related potentials to study cognition:
do not at present provide insights on the REFERENCES recording standards and publication criteria.
quantity or quality of information that Psychophysiology 37, 127152.
would be available by accumulating 1. Lee, M.R. (2007). Solanaceae IV: Atropa
11. Iannilli, E., Noennig, N., Hummel, T., and
information across time. Resolving this belladonna, deadly nightshade. J. R. Coll.
Schoenfeld, A.M. (2014). Spatio-temporal
Physicians Edinb. 37, 7784.
question is likely to also provide insights correlates of taste processing in the human
into the information content of these brain primary gustatory cortex. Neuroscience 273,
2. Ohla, K., Busch, N.A., and Lundstrom, J.N.
9299.
signals. (2012). Time for taste-a review of the early
cerebral processing of gustatory perception.
Second, it is undoubtedly the case that Chemosens. Percept. 5, 8799. 12. Negri, R., Di Feola, M., Di Domenico, S., Scala,
the brains perceptual discrimination M.G., Artesi, G., Valente, S., Smarrazzo, A.,
3. Crouzet, S.M., Busch, N.A., and Ohla, K. Turco, F., Morini, G., and Greco, L. (2012).
capacity documented in this study is in (2015). Taste quality decoding parallels taste Taste perception and food choices. J. Pediatr.
large part the consequence of accrued sensations. Curr. Biol. 25, 890896. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 54, 624629.
experience. The brain decoding methods
4. Singh, P.B., Iannilli, E., and Hummel, T. (2011). 13. Spence, C., and Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2014).
applied by Crouzet and colleagues are Segregation of gustatory cortex in response to The Perfect Meal: The Multisensory Science of
suited not only to characterize the salt and umami taste studied through Food and Dining (Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell).

Current Biology 25, R362R383, May 4, 2015 2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved R383

Anda mungkin juga menyukai