Anda di halaman 1dari 2

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/7809387

Feeling the Beat: Movement Influences Infant


Rhythm Perception

Article in Science July 2005


DOI: 10.1126/science.1110922 Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

286 417

2 authors:

Jessica Phillips-Silver Laurel Trainor


Georgetown University McMaster University
21 PUBLICATIONS 771 CITATIONS 186 PUBLICATIONS 7,663 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Laurel Trainor on 09 January 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


BREVIA
a model) on every second beat during train-
Feeling the Beat: Movement ing identified the duple form as familiar at
test, whereas those who bounced on every
third beat identified the triple form. Finally, we
Influences Infant Rhythm Perception tested infants_ preferences without any move-
ment training and found no preference for
Jessica Phillips-Silver and Laurel J. Trainor* either auditory interpretation, again indicat-
ing that movement is crucial for the multi-
We hear the melody in music, but we feel the bouncing determined whether infants later sensory effect.
beat. People in all cultures move their bodies to preferred the auditory rhythm pattern con- These studies illustrate the strong multisen-
the rhythms of music, whether drumming, sing- gruent with duple or triple form. sory connection between body movement and
ing, dancing, or rocking an infant (1). Body Experiment 2 was identical to experiment 1 auditory rhythm processing when inputs from
movement involves motor, proprioceptive (per- except that infants were blindfolded during both sources are experienced concurrently. Be-
ception of body position), vestibular (percep- training. Infants still preferred to listen to the cause infants did not engage in self-movement,
tion of movement and balance), visual, and auditory stimulus that matched the metrical the observed effect likely involves the vestib-
auditory systems (2), but few studies have ex- form of their movement training Et (15) 0 2.93, ular and perhaps proprioceptive systems. The
amined auditory-vestibular interactions. P 0 0.01^ (Fig. 1C), indicating that visual in- early development of the vestibular system (5),
The ability to feel and interpret the strong formation was not necessary for the effect. and infant delight at vestibular stimulation when
and weak beats in a rhythm pattern allows Experiment 3 investigated whether per- bounced to a play song or rocked to a lullaby,
people to move and dance in time to music. sonal motion experience was necessary. Dur- suggest that we are observing a strong, early
Typically, the strong beats of a rhythm pat- ing training, infants watched without moving vestibular-auditory interaction that is critical
tern are played louder, for the development of hu-
longer, or both, and the man musical behavior. It
metrical structure (what has long been known that
you move to) is derived infants are attracted to mu-
from, and consistent with, sic and responsive to its
these physical accents emotional content (6, 7 ).
(3). However, in an am- Our findings provide evi-
biguous rhythm pattern dence that the experience
with no physical ac- of body movement plays an
cents, different move- important role in musical
ments might give rise to rhythm perception.
different metrical inter-
pretations. In other words, References and Notes
how we move may influ- 1. N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, S.
Brown, The Origins of Music
ence what we hear. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA,
We tested the hy- 2000).
pothesis that movement 2. J. R. Lackner, J. Vestib. Res. 2,
307 (1992).
influences the auditory 3. F. Lehrdal, R. Jackendoff, A
encoding of rhythm pat- Generative Theory of Tonal
terns in human infants. In Music (MIT Press, Cambridge,
MA, 1983).
experiment 1, we trained 4. Materials and methods are
7-month-olds by having available as supporting ma-
them listen to a 2-min rep- terial on Science Online.
5. D. L. Clark, Science 196, 1228
etition of an ambiguous (1977).
(without accented beats) 6. L. J. Trainor, L. A. Schmidt, in
rhythm pattern (Fig. 1A, Fig. 1. Influence of bouncing on auditory encoding of rhythm patterns. (A) Stimuli. Vertical lines The Cognitive Neuroscience of
row 1, and sound file S1). represent the snare drum sounds of the rhythm patterns, and oblique lines represent time-marking Music, I. Peretz, R. Zatorre, Eds.
slapstick sounds (4). (B to D) Results. The y axis represents listening time preference; the x axis (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford,
Half of the infants were represents congruency between bouncing (duple or triple) during training and auditory accents 2003), pp. 310324.
bounced on every second (duple or triple) during testing. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. C, congruent; I, 7. S. E. Trehub, Nat. Neurosci.
beat, and half on every incongruent. 6, 669 (2003).
8. We thank J. Tang for assistance
third beat. After training, and T. Lewis for comments.
infants_ listening preferences were tested for as the experimenter bounced either on every Supported by a grant to L.J.T. from the Natural Sciences
two auditory versions of the rhythm pattern, second or on every third beat of the ambig- and Engineering Research Council of Canada.
which included intensity accents on either uous rhythm pattern. In this case, infants Supporting Online Material
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/308/5727/1430/
every second beat (the duple form) or every showed no preference for the two auditory DC1
third beat (the triple form) (Fig. 1A, rows 2 versions Et(15) 0 0.51, P 0 0.62^ (Fig. 1D), Materials and Methods
and 3, and sound files S1 and S3). Infants indicating that movement of the infant_s own References and Notes
Sound Files S1 to S3
controlled how long they listened to each body was critical for the multisensory effect
version of the rhythm pattern in a head-turn observed in experiments 1 and 2. 10 February 2005; accepted 25 March 2005
preference procedure (4). Infants chose to In order to confirm that the movement it- 10.1126/science.1110922
listen longer to the auditory test stimulus self did not induce auditory accents due to Department of Psychology, McMaster University,
with accented beats that matched the beats on changing room acoustics as the subjects moved, Hamilton, ON L8S 4K1, Canada.
which they were bounced Et (15) 0 4.00, we trained a group of adult listeners with *To whom correspondence should be addressed.
P(two-tailed) 0 0.001^ (Fig. 1B). Thus, their headphones. Those who bounced (mimicking E-mail: LJT@mcmaster.ca

1430 3 JUNE 2005 VOL 308 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org

View publication stats

Anda mungkin juga menyukai