Anda di halaman 1dari 9

doi:10.

1093/brain/awu395 BRAIN 2015: 138; 812820 | 812

DORSAL COLUMN
Occasional Paper
Pictures of pain: their contribution to the
neuroscience of empathy
G. D. Schott

The study of empathy, a translation of the term Einfuhlung, originated in 19th century Germany in the sphere of aesthetics, and

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016


was followed by studies in psychology and then neuroscience. During the past decade the links between empathy and art have
started to be investigated, but now from the neuroscientic perspective, and two different approaches have emerged. Recently, the
primacy of the mirror neuron system and its association with automaticity and imitative, simulated movement has been envisaged.
But earlier, a number of eminent art historians had pointed to the importance of cognitive responses to art; these responses might
plausibly be subserved by alternative neural networks. Focusing here mainly on pictures depicting pain and evoking empathy, both
approaches are considered by summarizing the evidence that either supports the involvement of the mirror neuron system, or
alternatively suggests other neural networks are likely to be implicated. The use of such pictures in experimental studies exploring
the underlying neural processes, however, raises a number of concerns, and suggests caution is exercised in drawing conclusions
concerning the networks that might be engaged. These various networks are discussed next, taking into account the affective and
sensory components of the pain experience, before concluding that both mirror neuron and alternative neural networks are
likely to be enlisted in the empathetic response to images of pain. A somewhat similar duality of spontaneous and cognitive
processes may perhaps also be paralleled in the creation of such images. While noting that some have repudiated the neuroscientic
approach to the subject, pictures are nevertheless shown here to represent an unusual but invaluable tool in the study of pain and
empathy.

Correspondence to: Dr G. D. Schott, MD, FRCP,


The National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square,
London WC1N 3BG, UK
E-mail: g.schott@ucl.ac.uk

Keywords: history of neurology; pain; pictures; empathy; networks

fuhlen, feeling into, the Germans happily put it)as the


Introduction word sympathy is intended to suggest, this enlivening . . . is
In 1873, the German philosopher Robert Vischer introduced exercised only when our feelings enter, and are absorbed
the term Einfuhling in his book Uber das optische into, the form we perceive. A few years later, in 1909, and
Formgefuhl: Ein Beitrag zu Aesthetik (On the Optical Sense when the subject was starting to be studied by Theodor Lipps
of Form: A Contribution to Aesthetics), a brief treatise and other psychologists, the British psychologist Edward
concerned with the psychology of aesthetics and form percep- Titchener rst translated into English the term Einfuhlung
tion. Soon after, the underlying principles behind Einfuhlung as empathy. The concept and studies of empathy thus origi-
were discussed in a rather rambling fashion by the English art nated in the realms of aesthetics and then psychology, several
historian, aesthetician and novelist, Vernon Lee (pseudonym decades before neuroscientic studies began (for references
for Violet Paget): the word sympathy, with-feeling(ein- and historical review, see Wispe, 1987).

Received September 18, 2014. Revised November 15, 2014. Accepted November 22, 2014. Advance Access publication January 22, 2015
The Author (2015). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved.
For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oup.com
Pictures of pain BRAIN 2015: 138; 812820 | 813

Only fairly recently have investigations begun on the These authors also went further, claiming: . . . no esthetic
neural processes underlying empathy, and intriguingly the judgment is possible without a consideration of the role of
historical wheel has turned full circle, as it is the links be- mirroring mechanisms in the forms of simulated embodi-
tween empathy and aesthetics and art that have been of ment and empathetic engagement that follow upon visual
renewed interest. But now, however, the subject has at- observation, and no form of esthetic appreciation . . . can
tracted the attention of neuroscientists, notably following be fully envisaged without considering mirror systems and
Freedberg and Galleses paper in Trends in Cognitive their role in embodied and empathetic responses . . .
Sciences entitled Motion, Emotion and Empathy in (Gallese and Freedberg, 2007).
Esthetic Experience (Freedberg and Gallese, 2007). The Although these authors thus clearly envisaged a major role
present paper aims to explore a number of issues concern- for mirror neurons in the response to art, there had also been
ing the empathetic response to much of art; focuses par- another view that they briey acknowledged but argued
ticularly on pictures depicting pain; and takes into account against (Gallese, 2011). This view, which comprised a
both the nature of the images themselves and the contribu- fully cognitive and disembodied approach to esthetics, had
tions of art historians to the neuroscientic discussion. been held by several eminent 20th century art historians, not-
ably Ernst Gombrich and others in the eld such as R.G.
Collingwood, for whom Art came to be thought of as a
matter of pure cognition (Freedberg and Gallese, 2007),
Art and empathy: two and Nelson Goodman who, while acknowledging that he
might invite hot denunciation for cold over-intellectualiza-

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016


different perspectives tion, had claimed in aesthetic experience the emotions func-
tion cognitively (Goodman, 1969).
As a preface, it should be noted that a single denition of Thus there have been two different approaches to how art
empathy has never been agreed, and Leiberg and Anders might be processed in the brain: one that implicates simula-
(2006) cited nine different denitions, one of their own tion, emotion and empathy, and the other that implicates
being the ability to perceive, share, and understand pure cognition. A cognitive approach to art can nevertheless
others emotions. In the more specic context of pain, elicit an empathetic response, an experience perhaps revealed
again a number of denitions have emerged, one such def- most strikingly on viewing abstract or other non-gurative
inition simply comprising a sense of knowing the experi- art. These two approachesone implicating mirror neurons,
ence of another person with cognitive, affective and the other nottend to be studied differently, by means of
behavioural components (Goubert et al., 2005). neuro-imaging and psychological techniques, respectively
When considering the links between art and empathy, (see de Vignemont and Singer, 2006). Freedberg and
Freedberg and Gallese (2007) maintained that a crucial elem- Gallese (2007) favoured the former approach; they con-
ent of esthetic response consists of the activation of embodied sidered that empathetic responses to art have a precise and
mechanisms encompassing the simulation of actions, emo- denable material basis in the brain, with mirror and canon-
tions and corporeal sensation . . .. The embodied mechan- ical neurons playing a crucial role, and there is now extensive
isms comprise the mirror neuron systemin humans evidence that mirror neurons are also implicated in numerous
sometimes termed the action-observation network (Shaw areas beyond their original, and perhaps particular, involve-
and Czekoova, 2013). Mirror neurons, as rst described 20 ment in movement (Keysers and Gazzola, 2009).
years ago in macaques, comprise neurons in the ventral pre- Discussed below, however, many images appear to
motor cortex that not only re during execution of goal-dir- engage the viewer empathetically without engaging
ected activity, but also vicariously during observation of the mirror neurons, from which it follows that other pro-
same action (for review, see Cattaneo and Rizzolatti, 2009). cesses must be enlisted. Whether visual and other inputs
Subsequently mirror neurons were found in brain regions into the mirror neuron system then elicit empathy directly,
other than the premotor cortex, including the auditory or indirectly through intermediary processes, remains un-
cortex in songbirds, and evidence emerged for similarly vic- known. As discussed below, however, many images
arious activation in somatosensory and emotional systems appear to engage the viewer empathetically without enga-
(for review, see Keysers and Gazzola, 2009). ging mirror neurons, from which it follows that other
processes must be enlisted. The nature of these other pro-
The discovery of this system
cessesand whether they indeed subserve the cognitive
illuminates the neural underpinnings of the frequent but hitherto
approach of the art historiansremain unknown too,
unexplained feeling of physical reaction, often in apparent imita- but both indirect involvement of the mirror neuron
tion of the actions represented within a work of art or suggested by system, and alternative, non-mirror neuron pathways
the implied movements involved in its making; mirror neurons also that lead directly or indirectly to the eliciting of empathy,
offer the possibility of a clearer understanding of the relationship remain possibilities. These issues are of more than theor-
between responses to the perception of movement within paint- etical concern, nowhere more evident than in studies of
ings, sculpture and architecture . . . and the emotions such works pain which, in the light of pains entirely subjective
provoke (Freedberg and Gallese, 2007). nature, unpleasant and pervasive features, and ability to
814 | BRAIN 2015: 138; 812820 G. D. Schott

evoke an empathetic response, attain particular import-


ance. Thus, because teasing out these different approaches
is both revealing and has practical implications, the dis-
cussion that follows mainly focuses on pictures relating to
the depiction of pain.

Pictures of pain: the


involvement of mirror
neurons?
Figure 1 Photograph showing facial expression of an in-
Images of pain have been known to trigger emotional re- tensely suffering World War II soldier with causalgia. From
sponses for millennia. For instance, classical sculptors were Mayfield and Devine, 1945, reproduced with permission of the
extraordinarily adept at depicting pain amongst other emo- American College of Surgeons.
tions, witness famous sculptures such as the Laocoon or
the Flaying of Marsyas. There is no doubt that in these
sculptures we are seeing pain and suffering on an epic
scale, and we can empathize with the protagonists. (Warburg, 1999). Again this is particularly likely when

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016


Another example from two centuries ago was reproduced those images suggest movement and evoke empathy, and
by Freedberg and Gallese, who included in their paper a striking evidence once more dating from the Renaissance
picture from Goyas Los Desastres de la Guerra (The supports this possibility: viewers of the Laocoon statue
Disasters of War) with the legend Embodied simulation . . . cant help but writhe and be moved to pity those sta-
in esthetic experience: empathy for pain (Freedberg and tues as though they were alive (Doni, 1549).
Gallese, 2007)although the justication for this legend At least partly underpinning movement in pictures is the
is assumed rather than established. And at the other end phenomenon of Movement without Motion, i.e. the per-
of the historical timeline, there are endless contemporary ception of movement in the absence of actual motion,
paintings, photographs and moving images depicting pain which long before its investigation by neuroscientists was
and evoking empathy, even in the medical literature; who studied by art historians, notably Rudolf Arnheim,
could not be affected by the grainy image of the grimacing, Professor of the Psychology of Art at Harvard.
suffering soldier in World War II clutching his causalgic
limb (Mayeld and Devine, 1945) (Fig. 1)? There is neither physical motion nor the illusion of it . . . We
Are mirror neurons in the viewers brain enlisted? have learned to associate motion with the visual images of a
Remarkably, that these neurons might indeed sometimes running man or a waterfall. When we see an image commonly
be implicated in the empathetic responses to art in general connected with motion, we supply the element of displacement
receives support dating back at least 500 years. Leonardo where it is absent in the perceptual experience itself (Arnheim,
da Vinci recorded that An artist painted a picture that 1967).
whoever saw it at once yawned, and went on doing so as
long as he kept his eyes on the picture, which represented a The particular engagement of the mirror neuron system
person who was also yawning (Richter, 1970). This ac- associated such movement may well result from processes
count chimes with the observation by the art historian variously termed imitation (Losin et al., 2009) or mimicry
Bernhard Berenson in respect of the Renaissance nude, (Hoffman, 2010), which are automatic and preverbal
whose taughtnesses of muscle and those stretchings and (Hoffman, 2010), and which lead to simulationthe
relaxings and ripplings of skin which, translated into simi- reenactment of perceptual motor, and introspective states
lar strains on our own persons, make us fully realise move- acquired during experience . . ., and which provides a gen-
ment (Berenson, 1896). At present, however, the possibility eral mechanism for establishing empathy (Barsalou, 2008).
that mirror neurons are indeed involved when such images
are viewed remains conjectural rather than established.
There seems little reason to doubt that images specically
depicting pain and evoking an empathetic response might Pictures of pain: alternative
similarly engage the viewer by enlisting the mirror neuron
circuitry. Indeed, according to another distinguished art his-
processes mediating the
torian, Aby Warburg, Renaissance artists such as Durer empathetic response
conveyed expressions of pain and other emotions in their
pictures by drawing on those methods devised by the clas- There is evidence, however, that images depicting pain can
sical sculptors, a method which a century ago he termed also engage neural processes that do not involve these neu-
Pathosformel (translated now as emotive formula) rons, and, crucially, it is predominantly the artist and the
Pictures of pain BRAIN 2015: 138; 812820 | 815

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016


Figure 3 The patients back with shattered glass.
Photograph co-created by Deborah Padfield with Nell Keddie from
the series Perceptions of Pain Deborah Padfield. Reproduced by
kind permission of Deborah Padfield and Dewi Lewis Publishing.

Yet, different from these images, depictions of pain, or


relating to pain, need not be gurative at all. Instead they
can be abstract or symbolic, but, like abstract art in general
(Melcher and Bacci, 2013), can be created to evoke an
empathetic response. For example, the contemporary
Figure 2 Pedro de Mayorga, Master of Palanquinos. artist Deborah Padeld, working closely with the patients
Flagellation of Saint Marina, from the Altarpiece of Saint Marina, c. themselves, represented their pain through illustration. But
1500, oil on board. Reproduced with permission, Museo de Bellas now The resulting images do not so much depict pain as
Artes de Asturias. Coleccion Pedro Masaveu. express it; they help, thereby, to objectify pain (Hurwitz,
2003), and several were reproduced in a photographic col-
art historian who have revealed the importance of what lection entitled Perceptions of Pain (Padeld, 2003). Such
appear to be alternative, cognitive processes. images, for instance one that shows fractured glass super-
Thus, as long ago as the late Middle Ages, religious pic- imposed on the sufferers back (Fig. 3), or anotheren-
tures designed to produce emotional effects sometimes tirely disengaged from any suffererwhich features a
showed sufferers experiencing pain without the expected piece of burning barbed wire (Fig. 4), cannot generate a
expression. To cite just one example discussed by the cul- visually mediated impression of pain through processes
tural historian Javier Moscoso, the Flagellation of Saint involving mirroring, any more than can the Renaissance
Marina depicted in the Spanish altarpiece in Valladolid, altarpiece referred to above. Rather, such images suggest
Spain, and dated around 1500, shows the martyr suffering pain by means of metaphor. Just as patients so often talk
extreme violence but without any hint of distress (Fig. 2). or write about their pain by invoking analogy or metaphor
Typical of such images, . . . nothing in their peaceful faces (Schott, 2004), so too visual analogy implicates intellectual
or their calm gestures allows us to infer any presence of or cognitive processes, rather than evoking the simulated
harm (Moscoso, 2012). It is evident that the terrible pain actions, postures, emotions or sensations that engage
and suffering shown in these pictures are unlikely to be mirror neurons. And surely the most extreme forms of pic-
mediated by cerebral processes involving mirror neurons; tures with pain, which is suggested rather than revealed,
rather, it is learned, cognitive processes experienced must be cartographic: the markings, symbols and colours
within the prevailing culture that mediate the impression that patients sometimes add to body maps (Schott, 2010).
of suffering. Similar processes may well underlie the re- Here visual pain metaphor is at its most abstract, but these
sponse to those countless images of both sacred and secular pictorial adornments nevertheless remain a graphic method
themes depicted over the centuries, which seem to suggest for communicating painful experiences and evoking em-
or allude to rather than explicitly reveal excruciating pain. pathy. In all these examples, mirror neuron involvement
816 | BRAIN 2015: 138; 812820 G. D. Schott

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016


Figure 5 Peter Paul Rubens. Prometheus Bound, c. 16111618,
oil on canvas. Reproduced by permission of the Philadelphia
Museum of Art.

Figure 4 Burning barbed wire. Photograph by Deborah gods chest being torn open by an eagles beak? Recalling
Padfield with Linda Sinfield from the series Perceptions of Pain the examples cited in the preceding paragraph, imagination
Deborah Padfield. Reproduced by kind permission of Deborah and appreciation of metaphor, but not memory, must be
Padfield and Dewi Lewis Publishing. implicated, and Scarrys account conrms these cognitive
processes are already well-developed in even young
children.

can surely be excluded; whether empathy evoked


through such non-mirror neuron systems might be less in-
tense than that evoked through the mirror neuron system is Pictures of pain as
unknown.
A particularly revealing anecdote illustrating the seeming
experimental stimuli
lack of involvement of mirror neurons is that recently re- The majority of studies on the processes involved in em-
ported by Elaine Scarry, Professor of Aesthetics and the pathy and the perception of art in general, but here pictures
General Theory of Value at Harvard (Scarry, 2007). She of pain in particular, have used functional MRI, and less
described a crowd of 7-year-old school children who, visit- often transcranial magnetic stimulation and other neuro-
ing a museum, were asked to . . . sit in front of the paint- physiological techniques. Apart from the mention below
ing that has the most physical pain in it. . A number of relating to temporal factors and mirror neuron responsive-
the children sat beneath Rubenss Prometheus Bound, and ness, these studies not only shed little light on whether
Scarry observed that the children seemed to empathize with mirror neuron or other systems are involved in eliciting
Prometheus, whose chest is being torn open by an eagles empathy, but also raise a number of other issues.
beak (Fig. 5). In some way the children had associated the First, pictures used as experimental visual stimuli have
drama with pain, in keeping with the known ability of not been made by a creative artist. They are thus inherently
children to appreciate the facial signs of pain in others by different from pictures with the emotional impact aimed for
the age of 5 or 6 (Deyo et al., 2004). Thus arguing against by an artist and can appear out of any context and dis-
the view that empathy may be based on mirror- embodied, and their selection sometimes seems arbitrary.
matching simulation of others state (Avenanti et al., Furthermore, just as viewing artworks compared with
2005), very different cognitive processes must be involved; images from everyday life may engage different neural
what mirroring could occur in respect of a Greek demi- networks (Cupchik et al., 2009), so the emotional impact
Pictures of pain BRAIN 2015: 138; 812820 | 817

and empathy achieved by an artist might engage different


neural networks compared with the images used in the ex-
periments. Both the length of time during which a picture is
viewed and contemplated, and the order in which its com-
ponent features are scanned, may also be factors which
inuence the neural networks engaged and the empathetic
response. Thus, in the example of Flagellation of Saint
Marina, observing rst the peaceful Saints expression, or
alternatively the surrounding torturers and their weapons,
might result in inconsistent if not conicting responses.
Another issue concerns the different temporal presenta-
tions of the visual stimuli. While some studies have
involved the viewing of in vivo pain-inducing procedures
rather than images (Singer at al., 2004), many studies have
used brief video clips (Avenanti et al., 2005; Botvinick
et al., 2005; Benuzzi et al., 2008), which are sometimes
edited (Lamm et al., 2007), or still photographs Figure 6 Saint Marinas face. Detail from Fig. 2.
(Jackson et al., 2005; Saarela et al., 2007). The different
temporal presentations may be important in relation to the

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016


different responsiveness of mirror neurons: observing real
picture could simply be seen as a nger being icked, and
actions evokes the strongest responses in the mirror neuron
the gure on the upper right a diver. It appears it is the
system compared with video images, but even observation
context in which the picture appears or is presented, rather
of static images of actions evokes responses in that system
than solely the picture itself, which is sometimes crucial.
(Gallese, 2011)and responses could be even greater when
The diverse issues raised by these various studies suggest
viewing artists creations, as these tend to be viewed for far
caution when drawing rm conclusions concerning the
longer than those presented in experiments (Smith and
neural networks involved, and these are discussed next.
Smith, 2001).
A further issue is the different nature or character of the
experimental visual image itself. Some have been of images
of painful events such as a soccer player breaking a leg or a Some comments on neural
rider falling from a bicycle (Osborn and Derbyshire, 2010),
whereas others have used images of painful insults inicted networks subserving
on a hand, a foot, or a neutral object (Avenanti et al.,
2005; Jackson et al., 2005; Benuzzi et al., 2008), and yet
empathy on viewing
others have depicted faces (Botvinick et al., 2005)and pictures of pain
expression is obviously a highly salient feature of a
person in pain. Some of these studies have used profes- At least 15 brain regions seem to be enlisted in the percep-
sional actors or pantomime players (Lamm et al., 2007), tive, cognitive and affective engagement when experiencing
and while there are differences between pictures of faces art, and as these engage parallel neural networks that are
that show deceptive compared with genuine pain (Hill interrelated and there is considerable feedback, it is impos-
and Craig, 2002), this difference does not exist in pictures sible to describe any meaningful sequence of events
created by artists whose skill lies in revealing the very ex- (Nadal, 2013). Nevertheless, there is now hard evidence
perience of pain. And as an aside, while the artist some- that mirror neurons are present in humans in at least ve
times shows the pain sufferer to be serene and perhaps even regions: the ventral and dorsal premotor cortex, supple-
smiling, as in the example of the Saints face in the Spanish mentary motor cortex, and inferior parietal and temporal
altarpiece discussed above (Fig. 6), there are no functional lobes, and vicarious activity can be detected in not only
MRI studies of pain and empathy that have used smiling motor but also somatosensory and emotional cortical re-
faces although, counter-intuitively, smiles often accompany gions (Keysers and Gazzola, 2009). The discussion above
pain (Kunz et al., 2009). suggests that, at least sometimes, this mirror neuron net-
Perhaps the most critical issue is whether the visual image work mediates the response to pictures of pain and the
necessarily provides an image of pain at all. Rather, images empathy evoked. But what of other candidate networks
sometimes appear ambiguous. For instance, Fig. 7 displays that might subserve the alternative, cognitive approach
the images used to elicit pain for the study of pain sensa- envisaged by the art historians?
tion evoked by observing injury in others (Osborn and In the case of responses to specically pain-related pic-
Derbyshire, 2010), a study that is discussed further torial stimuli, functional MRI and transcranial magnetic
below. On viewing these images without the clarication stimulation studies (Avenanti et al., 2005) again demon-
that appeared in the accompanying text, the upper left strate activation of many different areas of the brain, and
818 | BRAIN 2015: 138; 812820 G. D. Schott

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016


Figure 7 Images used to elicit pain for the behavioural and functional MRI studies of Osborn and Derbyshire, 2010. Reproduced
with permission of the International Association for the Study of Pain.

at least some of the differences may relate to whether the and inhibiting ones own perspective if necessary (Leiberg
response to these pictures engages with the affective or the and Anders, 2006)seem to be important particularly in
sensory components of the pain experience. Thus the af- complex or ambiguous situations, and probably engage the
fective component has been shown to be mediated by struc- temporoparietal regions, but particularly the prefrontal
tures which include the anterior insula, anterior cingulate cortex (Hynes et al., 2006; Leiberg and Anders, 2006).
cortex, brainstem and cerebellum when viewing actual Which networks are engaged may depend on the extent
painful stimulation (Singer et al., 2004), or pictures of affective versus sensory processing of pain, and it has
(Jackson et al., 2005) or video clips depicting pain been suggested that The key variable is likely to be the
(Morrison et al., 2004; Botvinick et al., 2005); the sensory mental attitude of the participants when thinking about
component is mediated by the sensorimotor structures of the pain of others (Singer and Frith, 2005).
the pain matrix (Avenanti et al., 2005), particularly the left
parietal lobe including the parietal operculum, the postcen-
tral gyrus and adjacent parts of the posterior parietal
cortex (Benuzzi et al., 2008); and when both affective
The parallel spectrum of
and sensory components of pain are engagedas in the creativity
unusual study of individuals themselves experiencing pain
Accepting there are numerous different neural networks
when observing images of another person in painthe an-
that respond to viewing works of art and which elicit
terior midcingulate cortex, anterior insula, prefrontal
empathy, so too there seem to be different processes under-
cortex and S1 and S2 regions are implicated (Osborn and
pinning the creation of art, although it is acknowledged
Derbyshire, 2010).
that creativity can hardly be reduced to the functional
Hardly surprisingly, and using somewhat different
properties of specic populations of neurons, mirror neu-
terminology, Leiberg and Anders (2006) concede:
rons included (Gallese, 2011). The inuential art historian
Disagreement subsists regarding the implementation of em-
Max Friedlander claimed that
pathy and whether it occurs in a contagion-like fashion or
depends on higher level cognitive processes. The former
A work of art comes into being in two ways either spontan-
the tendency to automatically mimic and synchronize facial eously, organic, or by deliberate effort . . . Strictly speaking, all
expressions, vocalizations, postures, and movements with art falls somewhere between these two extremes . . . The two
those of another person and, consequently, to converge ends of the scale are marked by the nave genius and the
emotionally (Hateld et al., 1994)would indeed be con- cold-blooded contriver (Friedlander, 1974).
sistent with involvement of the mirror neuron system. On
the other hand, the higher level cognitive processes that This suggests a certain parallelism: just as both mirror
include perspective-takingintegrating information from processes and those subserving cognition may be implicated
different sources when inferring the others mental state when viewing pictures depicting pain, so too there may be
Pictures of pain BRAIN 2015: 138; 812820 | 819

both spontaneous and more deliberate, cognitive processes Avenanti A, Bueti D, Galati G, Aglioti SM. Transcranial magnetic
stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain.
involved in the creation of those pictures.
Nat Neurosci 2005; 8: 95560.
Barsalou LW. Grounded cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 2008; 59:
61745.
Benuzzi F, Lui F, Nichelli PF, Porro CA. Does it look painful or dis-
Conclusion gusting? Ask your parietal and cingulate cortex. J Neurosci 2008;
28: 92331.
It is almost 150 years since Robert Vischer, when discuss- Berenson B. The Florentine painters of the Renaissance. New York
ing aesthetics, introduced the term Einfuhlung that was and London: G.P. Putnams Sons; 1896. p. 867.
soon to become synonymous with the English term em- Botvinick M, Jha AP, Bylsma LM, Fabian SA, Solomon PE,
pathy. The philosopher could not have envisaged todays Prkachin KM. Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical
areas involved in the direct experience of pain. Neuroimage 2005;
neuroscientic studies of empathy and the roles of mirror
25: 31219.
neuron and alternative networks, nor that these studies Casati R, Pignocchi A. Mirror and canonical neurons are not consti-
would also return to investigations of art, including depic- tutive of aesthetic response. Trend Cogn Sci 2007; 11: 410.
tions of pain. He would surely be intrigued, however, that Cattaneo L, Rizzolatti G. The mirror neuron system. Arch Neurol
art historians too have now become interested in the neural 2009; 66: 55760.
Cupchik GC, Vartanian O, Crawley A, Mikulis DJ. Viewing artworks:
processes underpinning the empathetic response to art, as contributions of cognitive control and perceptual facilitation to aes-
discussed in the case of the works of the 16th century thetic experience. Brain Cogn 2009; 70: 8491.
Flemish master Jan Gossart (Kavaler, 2013). de Vignemont F, Singer T. The empathic brain: how, when and why?

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016


The evidence provided here suggests that Freedberg and Trends Cogn Sci 2006; 10: 43541.
Galleses view, which envisaged the primacy of the mirror Deyo KS, Prkachin KM, Mercer SR. Development of sensitivity to
facial expression of pain. Pain 2004; 107: 1621.
neuron network, is too narrow, and other networks, in Doni AF. Disegno del doni, partito in piu ragionamenti, ne quali si
particular those subserving cognition, are also likely to be tratta della scoltura et pittura. . . (Pugliese O, translator; translation
implicated. In summary, both mirror neuron and alterna- in Kavaler, 2013). Venice: Gabriel Giolito di Ferrarii; 1549. p. 36r
tive networks are likely to be enlisted in the empathetic and v.
Freedberg D, Gallese V. Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic
response to images of pain.
experience. Trends Cogn Sci 2007; 11: 197203.
But at the same time it is only fair to record that this Friedlander MJ. Early Netherlandish painting, vol. xi. The Antwerp
entire approach to the subject has been found unconvin- Mannerists. Adriaen Ysenbrant. Nordeen H, translator. Leiden:
cing, if not condemnable, by some scientists (Casati and A.W. Sijthoff; 1974. p. 11.
Pignocchi, 2007; Tallis, 2008, 2012) and philosophers Gallese V. Mirror neurons and art. In: Bacci F, Melcher D, editors.
Art and the senses. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. p. 458;
(Sheets-Johnstone, 2012). Thus for Tallis Works of art
461.
are not merely sources of stimuli that act on some bits of Gallese V, Freedberg D. Mirror and canonical neurons are
the brain. More than anything else, they engage us as crucial elements in esthetic response. Trends Cogn Sci 2007; 11:
human beings (Tallis, 2008). Such a dissenting opinion 411.
unfortunately does not explain how pictures might Goodman N. Languages of art. An approach to a theory of symbols.
London: Oxford University Press; 1969. p. 248.
engage us as human beings, an issue addressed by neuro-
Goubert L, Craig KD, Vervoort T, Morley S, Sullivan MJL,
scientists whoas reasoned abovehave found that pic- Williams AC de C, et al. Facing others in pain: the effects of em-
tures may indeed reveal important insights into cerebral pathy. Pain 2005; 118: 28588.
processes mediating empathy. And in respect of pain, Hateld E, Cacioppo JT, Rapson RL. Emotional contagion.
Accepting the denition of pain as a subjective experience Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1994. p. 5.
Hill ML, Craig KD. Detecting deception in pain expressions: the struc-
requires that we take a unique approach to the study [of]
ture of genuine and deceptive facial displays. Pain 2002; 98:
the neurobiological basis of pain phenomena (Rainville, 13544.
2002). The invaluable but often neglected contributions Hoffman ML. Empathy and prosocial behavior. In: Lewis M,
of pictures and of artists and art historians to the neuros- Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF, editors. Handbook of emotions.
cientic study of pain represent one such an approach. 3rd edn. New York and London: Guilford Press; 2010. p. 441.
Hurwitz B. Looking at pain. In: Padeld D, editor. Perceptions of pain.
Stockport: Dewi Lewis Publishing; 2003. p. 10.
Hynes CA, Baird AA, Grafton ST. Differential role of the orbital
frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking.
Acknowledgements Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 37483.
Jackson PL, Meltzoff AN, Decety J. How do we perceive the pain of
I am most grateful to Professor Javier Moscoso for help in others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy.
obtaining Figure 2. Neuroimage 2005; 24: 7719.
Kavaler EM. Gossarts bodies and empathy. J Historians
Netherlandish Art 2013; 5: 111.
Keysers C, Gazzola V. Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for
References action, emotions, and sensations. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2009; 19:
66671.
Arnheim R. Art and visual perception. A psychology of the creative Kunz M, Prkachin K, Lautenbacher S. The smile of pain. Pain 2009;
eye. London: Faber & Faber; 1967. p. 3967. 145: 2735.
820 | BRAIN 2015: 138; 812820 G. D. Schott

Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J. The neural substrate of human em- transformations: the interface of biology and culture. Cambridge,
pathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J Cogn Massachusetts and London: Harvard University Press; 2007.
Neurosci 2007; 19: 4258. p. 279316.
Leiberg S, Anders S. The multiple facets of empathy: a survey of Schott GD. Communicating the experience of pain: the role of ana-
theory and evidence. Prog Brain Res 2006; 156: 41940. logy. Pain 2004; 108: 20912.
Losin EAR, Dapretto M, Iacoboni M. Culture in the minds eye: how Schott GD. The cartography of pain: the evolving contribution of pain
anthropology and neuroscience can inform a model of the neural maps. Eur J Pain 2010; 14: 78491.
substrate for cultural imitative learning. Prog Brain Res 2009; 178: Shaw DJ, Czekoova K. Exploring the development of the mirror
17590. neuron system: nding the right paradigm. Dev Neuropsychol
Mayeld FH, Devine JW. Causalgia. Surg Gynecol Obstet 1945; 80: 2013; 38: 25671.
6315. Sheets-Johnstone M. Movement and mirror neurons: a challenging and
Melcher D, Bacci F. Perception of emotion in abstract artworks: choice conversation. Phenom Cogn Sci 2012; 11: 385401.
a multidisciplinary approach. Prog Brain Res 2013; 204: 191216. Singer T, Seymour B, ODoherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD.
Morrison I, Lloyd D, di Pellegrino G, Roberts N. Vicarious responses Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components
to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: is empathy a multisensory issue? of pain. Science 2004; 303: 115762.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2004; 4: 2708. Singer T, Frith C. The painful side of empathy. Nat Neurosci 2005; 8:
Moscoso J. Pain: a cultural history. Houndmills, Basingstoke: Palgrave 8456.
Macmillan; 2012. p. 10. Smith JK, Smith LF. Spending time on art. Empirical Studies of the
Nadal M. The experience of art: insights from neuroimaging. Prog Arts 2001; 19: 22936.
Brain Res 2013; 204: 13558. Tallis R. The limitations of a neurological approach to art. Lancet
Osborn J, Derbyshire SWG. Pain sensation evoked by observing injury 2008; 372: 1920.
in others. Pain 2010; 148: 26874. Tallis R. Aping mankind. Neuromania, Darwinitis and the misrepre-

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016


Padeld D. Perceptions of pain. Stockport: Dewi Lewis Publishing; sentation of humanity. Durham: Acumen Publishing; 2012.
2003. p. 7980; 18991; 28491.
Rainville P. Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation. Warburg A. Durer and Italian antiquity. In: The renewal of
Curr Opin Neurobiol 2002; 12: 195204. pagan antiquity: Contributions to the cultural history of the
Richter JP. The literary works of Leonardo da Vinci, vol. 1. 3rd edn. European Renaissance. David Britt, translator. Los Angeles: Getty
New York: Phaidon; 1970. p. 64.
Research Institute for the History of Art and the Humanities; 1999.
Saarela MV, Hlushchuk Y, Williams AC de C, Schurmann M,
p. 555.
Kalso E, Hari R. The compassionate brain: humans detect intensity
Wispe L. History of the concept of empathy In: Eisenberg N, Strayer J,
of pain from anothers face. Cereb Cortex 2007; 17: 2307.
editors. Empathy and its development. Cambridge: Cambridge
Scarry E. Among schoolchildren. The use of body damage to express
University Press; 1987. p. 1737.
physical pain. In: Coakley S, Shelemay KK, editors. Pain and its

Anda mungkin juga menyukai