Anda di halaman 1dari 31

Journal of

Memory and
Language
Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445
www.elsevier.com/locate/jml

Rethinking interference theory: Executive control


and the mechanisms of forgetting
Michael C. Anderson*
Department of Psychology, 1227 University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403-1227, USA
Received 21 April 2003; revision received 13 August 2003

Abstract

Interference provides an account of one of the most basic problems in the science of memory: forgetting. Histori-
cally, theories of this process were shaped by models of associative learning prevalent when interference research began.
In this article, I argue that we should reconsider the long-standing conceptualization of interference as a learning
phenomenon and reframe interference as arising from systems that achieve mental and behavioral control. Specically,
it is argued that forgetting is not a passive side eect of storing new memories, but results from inhibitory control
mechanisms recruited to override prepotent responses. In support of this idea, I discuss two control situations in which
response override is necessaryselection and stoppingand show how these situations have direct parallels in retrieval.
I then review evidence that in both of these situations, the need to override prepotent, distracting memories is supported
by inhibitory mechanisms that ultimately cause forgetting. The theoretical properties of these inhibitory eects are
outlined, along with critical factors known to modulate or mask inhibition. The relation between this executive control
theory of forgetting and classical accounts of interference is discussed.
2003 Published by Elsevier Inc.

Over a century ago, G. E. Mueller and Pilzecker re- tendency of recent memories to pop into consciousness
ported one of the rst empirical demonstrations of for- unbidden by any particular cue. Perseveration was
getting due to interference (M uller & Pilzecker, 1900). In thought to be necessary to more rmly x a trace into
this classic work, Mueller and Pilzecker found that long-term storage. If another eortful activity intervened
people were less likely to recall a memory item if in the (such as learning a second list of items), the persevera-
interim the retrieval cue that was used to test that item tive process for the earlier memories was thought to be
had become associated to another memory. They named dampened, ultimately preventing the traces from being
this eect retroactive inhibition, highlighting the manner woven into the fabric of memory.
in which the storage of new experiences interferes with Although the disrupted consolidation theory was
memories encoded earlier in time. Mueller and Pilzecker largely abandoned as an account of retroactive inter-
believed that this memory impairment occurred because ference (see McGeoch & Irion, 1952, for arguments), the
the process of storing new memories disrupted the phenomenon itself and the method Mueller and Pilzec-
consolidation process that would have ordinarily ker introduced to study it have played a central role in
strengthened the traces that subjects had acquired ear- shaping the history of memory research. Their work set
lier. By this view, all would-be memories perseverate for o the classical interference era (19001970) in memory
a brief period after they are encoded, as evidenced by the research. In this era, considerable energy was devoted to
unraveling the mechanisms of interferencea focus
deemed worthy because it addressed the fundamental
*
Fax: 1-541-346-4911. problem of forgetting. How is it possible for an experi-
E-mail address: mcanders@darkwing.uoregon.edu. ence that is vivid and lively in our memories today to

0749-596X/$ - see front matter 2003 Published by Elsevier Inc.


doi:10.1016/j.jml.2003.08.006
416 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

ultimately fade? Why do memories grow less accessible ferenceinhibitionthat causes us to forget, not the
over time? According to classical interference theory, competition itself. This view departs from the common
Mueller and Pilzeckers discovery provided an answer: it assumption that forgetting is a passive side eect of the
was not the passage of time that caused forgetting, as ever-changing structure of memory. The mere storage of
might seem intuitive, but rather, changes correlated with interfering traces is not what causes memories to grow
time, such as the storage of new experiences into mem- less accessible with time. Rather, forgetting, whether
oryin particular, highly similar experiencesthat were incidental or intentional, is produced as a response to
at the root of memory failures. You can remember what interference caused by activated competitors in memory.
you had for dinner yesterday evening now, yet in a few I review the evidence supporting this functional view.
months you will not be able to, not because time has This review focuses on retrieval-induced forgetting
passed, but because the routine nature of our lives ulti- and more recent work with the think/no-think paradigm
mately clutters memory with many highly similar dinner and provides a specication of the theoretical properties
events. This clutter makes any particular memory very of those phenomena, some of their boundary conditions,
dicult to retrieve. Thus, when we forget, it is not be- and empirical challenges to measuring inhibition. In
cause memories decay, but because we are victims of the the nal section, I contrast the proposed view with
ever-changing structure of our memory and of basic classical theories of interference. Before beginning,
limitations in our ability to dierentiate similar traces. however, I elaborate on the theoretical perspective ad-
This view has stood the test of time: after 70 years of vanced here.
research and after tens of thousands of papers on the
topic, there can be little doubt that interference is a
powerful cause of forgetting. Executive control and the mechanisms of retrieval
What can be doubted, however, is the manner in
which interference causes forgetting. On one hand, for- The current perspective begins with a simple obser-
getting may be a direct consequence of adding new vation about human behavior: Actions, once started,
traces into memory. Both classical and modern theories can usually be stopped. This simple fact was impressed
have emphasized this approach. For instance, McGe- upon me one evening while opening the kitchen window.
ochs inuential response competition theory (McGeoch, As the window slid along its track, it pushed a small
1942) attributed interference eects to heightened com- cactus o the edge of the sill. My hand darted reexively
petition arising from the association of additional traces to catch the falling cactus. Mere centimeters from it, I
to a retrieval cue (or to the strengthening of an existing stopped my hand from clutching the cactuss needle-
competitor); in his framework, forgetting was a conse- dense body. The plant dropped to the oor and was
quence of adding new associative structure. Modern ruined, but I was happy to have avoided piercing my
theories such as those embodied in relative strength or hand with thousands of little red needles. This last
ratio-rule models of retrieval (Anderson, 1983; Mensink minute save was made possible by my ability to termi-
& Raajimakers, 1988) are the conceptual descendants of nate physical actionan ability so pervasive that it goes
this view in their emphasis on how retrieval of a given nearly unnoticed in daily life.
item is impeded by competing associations (see Ander- The preceding case is a classic example of a situation
son & Bjork, 1994, for a review). Structure-based theo- in which we need to overcome a strong habitual re-
ries such as these do not require special mechanisms of sponsea situation widely regarded as requiring execu-
forgetting and have the virtue of parsimony. On the tive control. This is sometimes referred to as response
other hand, they de-emphasize a basic problem in how override, and is illustrated in Fig. 1. In response over-
we use our memory: how do we overcome interference ride, one must stop a prepotent response to a stimulus
between competing traces to retrieve the memory we (such as a falling object). This may either be because the
want? What are the repercussions, if any, of resolving circumstance requires that the response be withheld, or
competition for the traces that interfered? Given that because a less common response is more contextually
our cognitive goals often require the recall of specic appropriate. For example, it is more contextually ap-
events in long-term memory, some process must exist for propriate to say Hola when someone waves to you
resolving interference. while you are in Spain, even though your habitual re-
In this article, I present a view of how interference leads sponse may be to say Hello. The capacity to either
to forgetting which emphasizes how interference gets re- stop or redirect action in this way is crucial to daily life.
solved. I argue that a theory of interference should be Without it, we would lose essential exibility to adapt
framed in the larger context of how organisms control the behavior according to changes in our goals, or to
direction of their actions and thoughts. By this view, changes in the environment itself. We would be slaves to
memory retrieval presents a special case of a broad class habit or reex.
of situations that recruit executive control processes; it is A key theoretical question that this problem raises is
the executive control mechanism that overcomes inter- How do we keep from being automatically controlled
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 417

The retrieval of associated memories is not always de-


sirable; sometimes, we may wish to retrieve another
memory item that is associated to the cue driving re-
trieval, but that is more weakly associated to that cue;
other times, we may wish to avoid retrieval altogether
either because the associated memory is unpleasant or
simply because we wish to maintain conceptual focus on
the concept that is acting as a cue. Although we some-
times retrieve things that we do not intend, we often are
able to exercise control over this tendency; we can re-
collect the particular event we are seeking despite in-
terference from stronger competitors, and we can stop
ourselves from thinking about unwanted memories.
Fig. 1. Graphical depiction of a typical response override sit- Given these functional parallels between motor behavior
uation. Circles correspond to representations in memory, with and memory retrieval, it is possible that response over-
lines representing associations between these representations.
ride mechanisms are recruited to control unwanted
The stimulus is associated to two responses, one of which is
stronger (prepotent), and the other of which is weaker (depicted memories that intrude by virtue of spreading activation
by a dotted line). Response override must occur when either the (for related arguments, see Shimamura, 1995). If so, we
organism needs to: (1) emit the weaker response, when it is should nd evidence for inhibitory control in memory
more contextually appropriate, despite the stronger association situations likely to involve response overridesituations
to the prepotent response, or (2) stop any response from oc- such as the need to select a weaker, yet more contextu-
curring. Inhibitory control is thought to suppress activation of ally appropriate response, given interference from one or
the prepotent response to permit response override. The re- more prepotent competitors, or the need to stop a re-
sponse override situation characterizes many paradigms in sponse altogether.
work on executive function, including the Stroop and go/no-go A core claim of this article is that strong evidence for
tasks.
these parallels exists, and that inhibitory processes re-
cruited during the control of memory retrieval precipi-
tate the forgetting associated with interference. In
by the habitual action? One widely discussed answer to support of this executive control perspective, I review
this question is that response override is accomplished evidence for a role of inhibitory processes in memory
by inhibiting the undesired action. According to this selection and more briey, in memory stopping. Mem-
view, the presentation of a stimulus activates a repre- ory selection is required during retrieval when our goal
sentation corresponding to that stimulus in long-term is to recall an event or fact from long-term memory in
memory. Activation then spreads from that representa- the face of interference from related traces that become
tion to associated responses in proportion to how activated by cues guiding retrieval. The need to stop
strongly associated they are to the stimulus. When a retrieval arises when we confront a cue or reminder and
response becomes suciently activated, it will be emit- we wish to prevent an associated memory from entering
ted. If a stimulus is associated to multiple responses, the awareness. In both situations, attempts to limit the in-
one that achieves threshold most quickly will generally uence of activated and distracting memories have been
be emitted, pre-empting other responses. However, if a found to impair their later accessibility, highlighting an
weaker response is more contextually appropriate, in- important link between forgetting and the control of
hibition can be recruited to suppress the stronger one. retrieval. In both cases, the memory impairment is better
Inhibition is thought to reduce the level of activation for explained by inhibition than by conventional associative
a given response, preventing it from achieving threshold. interference mechanisms. The forgetting induced by in-
In so doing, this process permits weaker, but more hibition is often adaptive, limiting the tendency for
contextually appropriate responses to be expressed, en- outdated or intrusive memories to disrupt performance
abling exible, context-sensitive behavior. This is known (Bjork, 1989; see also Anderson, 2001; Anderson &
as inhibitory control. Green, 2001).
Given the putative importance of inhibitory control
in directing overt behavior, it is reasonable to ask whe- Inhibitory control in selective memory retrieval
ther internal actions might also be the target of such
mechanisms. Clear parallels exist between the control of The need to select a weaker response to a stimulus in
action and the control of memory. Just as a stimulus the face of interference from a prepotent competitor
may spread activation to a prepotent motor response, nds a natural parallel in memory in the situation of
a retrieval cue may spread activation to a strongly selective retrieval. Here, the aim is to recall a particular
associated item in memory, leading it to be retrieved. target event or fact when provided with one or more
418 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

retrieval cues. Typically, a given retrieval cue will be


associated with other memories as welland some may
be more strongly associated to the cue than the target
item. Since the classical interference era, we have known
that when multiple traces are associated to the same cue,
they tend to compete for access to conscious awareness
(see Postman, 1971; see Anderson & Neely, 1996 for
reviews). This form of competition presents a problem of
control because the retrieval cue by itself cannot be re-
lied upon to access the target itemin fact, the presence
of a strong competitor could in principle perpetually
divert us from that target memory. If inhibitory control
mechanisms are recruited to override prepotent re-
sponses, it seems reasonable that they might also be used
to override prepotent memories. To the extent that the
eects of inhibitory control persist, then situations de-
manding the selective retrieval of a target item should
cause long-lasting memory impairment for suppressed
competitors. Thus, the very act of remembering should
cause forgetting of related memories.
Over the last decade, we have explored the foregoing
prediction with a paradigm we developed to examine the
eects of retrieval on related memories: the retrieval
practice paradigm (Anderson, Bjork, & Bjork, 1994). In
the typical experiment, subjects study lists of category
exemplar pairs (e.g., fruitorange, drinksscotch,
fruitbanana). They then perform retrieval practice on
half of the exemplars from half of the categories by
completing cued stem recall tests (e.g., fruit-or_____).
Each practiced item is tested three times during the re-
Fig. 2. Stimulus structures in two typical retrieval-induced
trieval practice phase to increase the magnitude of the
forgetting experiments with representative results. (A) A typical
eect on related items. After a 20-min retention interval, within-category retrieval-induced forgetting study, as done by
subjects are given a nal cued recall test for all the ex- Anderson et al. (1994). The example illustrates two items from
emplars. Performance on this test can be measured for each of two categories that subjects have studied (six items are
three item types: practiced items (e.g., orange), unprac- usually studied in eight categories), for purposes of illustration.
ticed items from the practiced categories (e.g., banana), In this example, subjects have performed retrieval practice on
and unpracticed baseline items from unpracticed cate- Fruits Orange, but not on Fruits Banana (unpracticed com-
gories (e.g., scotch). Fig. 2A illustrates our initial nd- petitor) or any members from the Drinks category (an un-
ings with this paradigm, which are quite typical. As can practiced baseline category). As shown here, practice typically
be seen, recall of the practiced exemplars was improved facilitates recall of the practiced item, and impairs recall of the
unpracticed competitor, relative to performance in baseline
on the nal test relative to performance on baseline
categories. (B) Stimulus structure and results from a typical
categories, demonstrating the well documented benets cross category inhibition experiment, as performed by Ander-
of retrieval-practice on the practiced items themselves son and Spellman (1995). In the related condition (top half of
(Allen, Mahler, & Estes, 1969; Bjork, 1975; Carrier & (B)), subjects study two related categories (Red Things and
Pashler, 1992; Gardiner, Craik, & Bleasdale, 1973). Foods) and then perform retrieval practice on some of the
However, recall for the unpracticed exemplars from members of one of them (e.g., Red Blood), but not the other
the practiced categories (e.g., banana) was signicantly (Foods). As shown in (B), this not only impairs the delayed
worse than for the items from baseline categories (e.g, recall of unpracticed competitors that are explicitly studied
drinks). Thus, remembering some items during the re- under the Red category (e.g., Red-tomato), but also those
trieval practice phase caused subjects to forget other competitors (i.e., other Red things) that are studied and tested
under a separate category (e.g., Food Radish). This can be seen
things that were related to them on a delayed retention
by comparing performance to items in the corresponding cat-
test 20 min later. We have referred to this nding as egory (i.e., Food) when the red category is not studied or
retrieval induced forgetting (Anderson et al., 1994), to practiced (i.e., the Unrelated condition; see dotted box for
highlight the central role that retrieval is believed to play the appropriate comparison). The impairment of items in a
in generating the eect. Research on retrieval-induced separate category is an example of the cue-independence of
forgetting builds on classic work on the phenomenon of inhibition.
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 419

output interference (Arbuckle, 1966; Dong, 1972; been proposed as theories of interference (for a review of
Roediger, 1973; Roediger & Schmidt, 1980; Smith, 1971, non-inhibitory sources of memory impairment, see An-
1973; Smith, DAgostino, & Reid, 1970; Tulving & Ar- derson & Bjork, 1994; see description of a subset of these
buckle, 1963, 1966) in which it was shown that the in the later discussion). Although it might seem dicult
probability of recalling a previously studied item de- to distinguish these alternatives, focused empirical re-
clines with the serial position of that item in the testing search has yielded evidence favoring the inhibitory
sequence. However, work on retrieval-induced forget- control view. I discuss this next.
ting establishes that retrieval-related memory impair-
ments can be long-lasting, and are not limited to Properties of retrieval-induced forgetting
dynamics occurring in a single output session. Cru-
cially, retrieval-induced forgetting is consistent with the Work on retrieval-induced forgetting has revealed
view that inhibitory control mechanisms are recruited to properties that uniquely support the inhibitory control
overcome interference during retrieval practice, with hypothesis, and that suggest that alternative strength-
inhibition manifesting as recall impairment for com- based models may not be correct. These include cue-inde-
petitors on the nal retention test. pendence, retrieval-specicity, interference-dependence,
Although retrieval-induced forgetting could be pro- and strength-independence. I discuss these properties
duced by inhibition, the preceding results do not clearly next, along with other ndings that are of theoretical
establish inhibition as the mechanism. The basic nding interest.
of retrieval-induced forgetting is compatible with
McGeochs (1942) classical response competition theory Cue-independence
of interference. According to McGeochs theory, the Many theories of interference predict that forgetting
likelihood of recalling a target response should go down should be strongly cue-dependentthat is, observations
either when a new response gets associated to the cue of forgetting should be tied to a particular cue. For
normally used to retrieve it, or when an existing alter- example, the blocking theory asserts that strengthening
native response is strengthened. In either case, the target some exemplars through retrieval practice (e.g., Fruit
item will suer increased competition from the alterna- Orange) impairs the recall of other exemplars (e.g., Fruit
tive responsecompetition that will block access to that Banana) on a delayed test because the presentation of
target. These competitive dynamics have become for- their shared cue at test leads the stronger response (or-
malized in several modern memory architectures that ange) to intrude persistently and block the weaker item.
posit relative strength theories of retrieval (e.g., Ander- If, however, one were to try to recall the weaker item
son, 1983; Raaijmakers & Shirin, 1981). In these through an independent test cue not associated to the
models, the probability of recalling a target is deter- practiced item (e.g., Monkey B___), associative compe-
mined by that items strength of association to a cue, tition should be circumvented. Thus, whether one ob-
relative to the strengths of association of all items re- serves forgetting of Banana should depend on whether
lated to that cue. Thus, when an alternative response is one uses the original retrieval practice cue to test the
strengthened, say by retrieval practice, the relative critical item or an independent cue. Theories that pro-
strength of all other nonpracticed items declines on pose that interference derives from unlearning of the
subsequent tests. Later, when the subject tries to recall cue-target association between the practiced category
the target, the strengthened competitor will have a re- and the critical item, diversion of activational resources,
trieval advantage that will lead it to intrude so persis- or biases in the meaning of the retrieval practice cue all
tently that subjects will abandon their eorts to recall share this same feature of predicting cue-dependent
the unpracticed exemplars (see also, Rundus, 1973). forgetting (see Anderson & Bjork, 1994; Anderson &
Importantly, this approach does not require inhibition; Spellman, 1995, for discussion).
rather, practiced items become so strongly linked to the The inhibitory control perspective, by contrast, pre-
practice cue that they block the retrieval of other ex- dicts that retrieval induced forgetting should exhibit cue-
emplars. This blocking account is plausible, given the independencethat is, a tendency for the impairment to
substantial strengthening that practiced items typically generalize to novel test cues not involved in the retrieval
enjoy (however see later section on strength indepen- practice events that caused impairment. This prediction
dence). Other noninhibitory mechanisms may also con- follows because impairment is thought to arise from
tribute to retrieval induced forgetting. For example, suppression of the competing memory itself, rather than
retrieval practice may damage the association linking the from damage to any particular association. Thus, per-
category to the aected exemplar or alter instead the forming retrieval practice on Fruit-Orange should re-
meaning of the practiced category cue (e.g., by biasing duce activation for the item Banana. If Banana is less
Fruits towards Citrus fruits ) so that the category active, it should not matter whether the item is tested
label is no longer a functional cue for retrieving the from the original retrieval practice cue (Fruit) or from a
unpracticed competitor. All of these mechanisms have novel test cue (e.g., Monkey B__). To test this, Anderson
420 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

and Spellman (1995) modied the retrieval practice mixture of blocking and inhibition eects. As can be
paradigm for use with new materials (Fig. 2B). As in the seen in Fig. 2B, however, this seems unlikely. If two
original retrieval-induced forgetting experiment, subjects sources of forgetting contributed to impairment on items
studied categories containing six exemplars each, but such as Red-Tomato, but only one source (inhibition)
unlike in that study, the categories were related. For on Food-Radish, we would expect to see more retrieval-
example, although subjects studied Blood and Tomato induced forgetting in the former instance than in the
under the Red category, Tomato is also a Food; and latter. The failure to nd such a dierence casts doubt on
although Radish and Crackers were studied as Foods, the role of blocking in retrieval-induced forgetting even
Radish is also a Red thing. The key question was whe- when the practiced category is used as a test cue. Nev-
ther retrieval practice on items such as Red-Blood would ertheless, a more direct test of the role of blocking in
not only impair competitors explicitly studied under the retrieval-induced forgetting would be desirable.
same category cue, like Red-Tomato, but also red things According to the blocking hypothesis, presenting the
like Radish that were studied and tested under a sepa- retrieval practice category on the nal test leads prac-
rate category cue. ticed items to intrude perseveratively, blocking recall of
According to the response competition view, retrieval the unpracticed competitors. If so, then strengthening
practice on Red-Blood should not impair delayed recall practiced items in any way should impair related com-
for Food Radish, even if retrieval practice strengthens petitors. Impairment should be found, for example, even
the Red-Blood association and weakens the Red-Radish if items are strengthened with repeated study exposures
association. Radish should remain unimpaired because instead of retrieval practice. Several studies have ad-
it is tested with the Food categorya dierent retrieval dressed this possibility. For example, using Anderson
cue that circumvents those factors. However, if retrieval and Spellmans cross-category inhibition paradigm,
practice on Red-Blood initially activates all of the Red Anderson and Shivde (in preparation a) manipulated
items, both Tomato and Radish should become acti- whether the to-be-practiced items were strengthened by
vated, causing interference that triggers inhibitory con- retrieval practice or repeated study exposures. The re-
trol. The resulting suppression of Radish should be trieval-practice condition replicated both the within and
observable later when it is tested with Food. As Fig. 2B cross-category impairment observed by Anderson and
shows, the recall of FoodRadish was impaired. Spellman (1995). Extra study exposures, however, failed
These data show that inhibitory processes contribute to impair related items. No inhibition was found despite
to retrieval-induced forgetting, rendering competing the fact that both strengthening methods facilitated the
memories less accessible regardless of which cue is used practiced items to the same degree, as evidenced by the
to test them. Evidence for cue-independent forgetting substantial increase in their recall on the nal test.
has now been found many times with stimuli varying in Several investigators have found this pattern, using a
both type and complexity (Anderson & Bell, 2001; An- variety of dierent types of materials and dierent par-
derson & Green, 2001; Anderson, Green, & McCulloch, adigms (Anderson & Bell, 2001; Anderson, Bjork, &
2000; Anderson et al., submitted; Anderson & Shivde, in Bjork, 2000; B auml, 1996, 1997, 2002; Blaxton & Neely,
preparation a; Anderson & Shivde, in preparation b; 1983; Ciranni & Shimamura, 1999; Shivde & Anderson,
Anderson & Spellman, 1995; Johnson & Anderson, in 2001). Taken together, these ndings argue that asso-
press; Levy, Reinholz, & Anderson, in preparation; ciative blocking does not contribute substantially to
Miyamoto & Anderson, in preparation; Radvansky, within-category retrieval-induced forgetting, nor per-
1999; Shivde & Anderson, 2001; however, see Williams haps to interference eects more broadly. Rather, inhi-
& Zacks, 2001). Taken as a whole, these ndings show bition is driven by the need to override interference from
that cue-independence is a general property of retrieval- competing memories during the selective retrieval of
induced forgetting and that cue dependent mechanisms target items.
such as blocking are not adequate to account for the
eect. Interference dependence
Retrieval may be necessary to induce inhibition, but
Retrieval specicity it is not sucient. According to the executive control
Although cue-independence argues that inhibition theory, retrieval induced forgetting should only arise
causes retrieval-induced forgetting, cue-dependent for- whenever a related memory interferes with the retrieval
getting mechanisms may nevertheless contribute in some of a target item and triggers inhibitory control. If a re-
cases. In particular, whenever the retrieval practice cue is lated item does not interfere, it should not be inhibited
used during later tests of subjects memory, both inhib- even when a target has been retrieved.
itory and noninhibitory mechanisms may cause forget- Several studies favor the view that retrieval-induced
ting. For instance, the impairment of Red-Tomato in the forgetting is moderated by the amount of interference
preceding example (an item that was both studied and caused by a competing item. For instance, Anderson
tested with the retrieval practice cue) may reect a et al. (1994) found that retrieval practice did not always
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 421

impair the later recall of related exemplars. Retrieval to be the cause of dierential impairment. However, one
practice primarily caused impairment when related cat- might still be concerned that the degree of strengthening
egory exemplars were high in taxonomic frequency (e.g., was not manipulated strongly enough to reveal impair-
Fruit Banana). Low frequency competitors (e.g., Fruit ment. To address this, Shivde and Anderson (2001)
Guava) were always less impaired and often exhibited performed a parametric manipulation of the number of
no measurable impairment at all, even when subjects retrieval practice trials given to a practiced item, to see
performed retrieval practice on exactly the same items. whether impairment might emerge for weaker competi-
Anderson et al. (1994) found that the main determinant tors. To manipulate the degree of interference, Anderson
of the amount of retrieval-induced forgetting was nei- and Shivde used asymmetric homographs, pairing each
ther the taxonomic frequency of the practiced items, nor one with one word related to its dominant sense (e.g.,
the degree to which practiced items were strengthened Arm Shoulder) and another related to its subordinate
on the nal test, but rather the frequency of the com- sense (e.g., Arm Missile). Subjects were then asked to
petitors. The more strongly associated to the category an perform retrieval practice either 0, 1, 5, or 20 times on
unpracticed competitor was, the more impairment was either the dominant or the subordinate word associate.
found. The same pattern has been found in an output Following retrieval practice, subjects were tested on the
interference design: high, but not low taxonomic fre- alternate associate that they did not practice, with either
quency exemplars exhibit within category output inter- the originally trained cue or an independent cue that was
ference (Bauml, 1998; see also, Anderson et al., 1994, also encoded previously. The results were clear: Per-
Experiment 2; however, see Anderson et al., 1994, Ex- forming retrieval practice on the dominant sense (e.g.,
periment 3). These ndings are consistent with the idea Arm Shoulder) did not impair the later recall of the
that inhibitory control is most needed when a related subordinate sense (e.g., Arm Missile) at all, even though
item interferes during retrieval, as might be expected of retrieval practice yielded substantial retrieval-based
the most dominant exemplars. strengthening for the practiced item (see Fig. 3). Practice
Interference dependence has been demonstrated in on the subordinate sense, however, caused retrieval in-
other ways, as well. For example, retrieval induced duced forgetting of the dominant sense. Similar results
forgetting can be eliminated simply by manipulating the were obtained, regardless of whether subjects were tested
interference demands of the retrieval practice task. This on the unpracticed competitor with the homograph
was demonstrated by Anderson et al. (2000). In their (Arm M___) or the independent test cue (e.g., Target-
competitive retrieval practice group, subjects were given M___ for missile). Thus, even when subjects performed
the category and the rst two letters of an exemplar as as many as 20 retrieval practice trials on the dominant
cues (e.g., Fruit Or___ for Orange) during each practice sense, little retrieval-induced forgetting was observed.
trial (as is typically done), and subjects were asked to Taken together these results argue against associative
recall the item they had studied. In the non-competitive blocking accounts of retrieval-induced forgetting, but
practice condition, subjects also performed retrieval are consistent with idea that this phenomenon depends
practice, but on the category name. Specically, subjects on the need to override prepotent memories, as would be
were given the rst two letters of the category name, expected if inhibitory processes are recruited to suppress
with an exemplar (e.g., Fr___ Orange for Fruit Orange), those memories (see Conway & Engle, 1994 for a related
and were asked to recall the category name. Anderson discussion of the role of inhibitory processes in resolving
et al. (2000) argued that related exemplars were unlikely interference in memory span tasks; see also, Lustig,
to interfere with the retrieval of the category name be- Hasher, & Toney, 2001, for a recent review of work on
cause a practiced exemplar itself, which was associated inhibitory processes in cognitive aging).
to the category and not with the other exemplars, served
as a retrieval cue. As predicted, Anderson et al. (2000) Strength independence
found inhibition in the competitive, but not in the Our early work on retrieval-induced forgetting was
noncompetitive condition. This dierence was found initially premised on the classical view that strengthen-
despite the presence of retrieval in both conditions and ing some items would impair later retrieval of other
despite signicant and comparable strengthening of associates (Anderson et al., 1994). However, we quickly
practiced items. Thus, when the retrieval task itself does discovered that the degree to which practiced items are
not require interference to be resolved, little retrieval- strengthened does not predict how much retrieval in-
induced forgetting is found, even when the nature of the duced forgetting was observed. In fact, as highlighted
competitor is held constant. in the preceding sections, practiced items can be sig-
In the foregoing studies, the degree to which prac- nicantly strengthened without causing impairment:
ticed items were strengthened was nearly identical in Retrieval practice on target items does not impair
both the conditions that showed and did not show re- low taxonomic frequency competitors, subordinate
trieval-induced forgetting. These results suggest that meanings of ambiguous words, nor even high fre-
inadequate strengthening of practiced items is unlikely quency exemplars, provided that retrieval practice is
422 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

Fig. 3. Results of a study by Shivde and Anderson (2001): (A) plots the percentage of practiced items (e.g., Arm Shoulder) that
subjects recalled on the nal test as a function of the number of retrieval practices it received; (B) shows that even after extensive
practice on the practiced items, recall of weaker competitors (e.g., Arm Missile) is not impaired on the nal recall test (when tested with
Arm M___); (C) shows the recall data when the unpracticed competitor was tested with an independent probe (e.g., Target M___ for
Missile), again revealing no impairment as a result of practice.

noncompetitive. Nor does inhibition occur if practiced control for output interference biases (overt or covert)
items are strengthened by repeated study exposures in- created by providing part-set cues (see Anderson &
stead of retrieval practice. In all of these cases, Neely, 1996; Nickerson, 1984; Roediger & Neely, 1982,
strengthening occurred, with no resultant inhibition, for reviews of part-set cuing and related research). Thus,
even when the amount of strengthening was identical to although strengthening some items (even without re-
or even greater than that observed in other conditions trieval practice) appears to impair nonstrengthened
in which retrieval-induced forgetting was found. When competitors, such eects may be better explained in
analyses are restricted to cases in which retrieval- terms of retrieval-induced forgetting.
induced forgetting is found, the magnitude of the im- The dierence between the foregoing studies and re-
pairment bears little quantitative relationship to the cent demonstrations of strength independence lies pri-
degree of facilitation on practiced items. Together, these marily in the attempt to separate the process of
ndings argue that impairment is independent of the strengthening from retrieval-induced forgetting. For
strength of the practiced item. example, by using extra study exposures, we have been
The property of strength independence is surprising, able to strengthen practiced items without retrieval
given the historical emphasis on the role of competition practice, so that we can see whether the added strength
in producing interference (e.g., McGeoch, 1942; Melton for those items would impair the delayed recall of
& Irwin, 1940; see also, Anderson, 1983; Mensink & competitors. Also important has been our eort to
Raajimakers, 1988). However, given the frequent co- control the order in which subjects recall studied items,
occurrence of strengthening and impairment across a so as to prevent biases in output order typically created
variety of paradigms (e.g., retroactive and proactive in- by strengthening manipulations. Towards this end, we
terference, part-set cuing, list-strength eects), the em- have used letter stem cued recall tasks (e.g., Fruit B___)
phasis on strength as a cause of forgetting makes sense. to force subjects to recall nonstrengthened items before
Anderson et al. (1994) noted however that nearly all strengthened items, and reduce test-time retrieval in-
paradigms that appear to provide evidence for strength- duced forgetting. By controlling these factors, we have
dependent competition have confounded strengthening found that strengthening does not cause forgetting of
with some form of retrieval-induced forgetting. In competitors. In a similar vein, Bauml and colleagues
studies of retroactive interference, for example, one (Bauml, 1996, 1997, 1998) have also attempted to sep-
typically cannot disentangle the eects of strengthening arate the inuences of strengthening and inhibition in
word pairs from the second list (e.g., Dog-Sky) from the other experimental procedures such as the retroactive
suppression of rst list responses (e.g., Dog-Rock). This interference and list strength eect paradigms. B auml
ambiguity arises because word pairs from the second list (1996) found that strengthening a second list of words
are typically strengthened by repeated study/test cycles, by increasing study time did not increase retroactive
a procedure which conates strengthening of those pairs interference on the rst study list. B
auml (1997) showed
with retrieval practice. In list-strength eect studies, one that the list strength eect virtually disappears if biases
cannot disentangle the eects of strengthening one half in output order are eliminated: strengthening half of a
of the list of words from the heightened output inter- study list through extra study does not impair the later
ference that those strengthened items cause for the re- recall of the other list half as long as those non-
maining nonstrengthened words on later free recall tests. strengthened items are tested rst in the recall sequence.
If left to recall items in any order, subjects typically Thus, a variety of interference eects that have been
begin with the strengthened items, which is likely to attributed to strength-dependent competition may arise
inhibit the remainder of the list. Similar problems occur from the recruitment of inhibitory control processes
in part-set cuing studies, which often do not adequately during retrieval.
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 423

Delay dependence? forgetting from the one that sustains it. For example,
There is some evidence that retrieval-induced forget- inhibitory processes may initially deactivate the units
ting may recover over time. In one study, MacLeod and involved in representing a competing memory trace, and
Macrae (2001) had subjects perform retrieval practice this deactivation may be short-lived. However, this de-
immediately after encoding had ended. After retrieval activation may itself cause a structural change that
practice, subjects were tested either immediately or on the persists well beyond the initial period of inhibition. For
following day. Interestingly, retrieval induced forgetting instance, the constituent features of the trace may be-
was observed on the immediate test, but not for those come less tightly bound or a consolidation process that
subjects tested after 24 h. In a follow-up study, MacLeod might have otherwise been ongoing may be terminated
and Macrae (2001) replicated this nding, but also (e.g., Muller & Pilzecker, 1900). Inhibition of a subset of
showed that when retrieval practice was performed after a features in a trace may lead to reductions in the asso-
24 h delay, signicant retrieval-induced forgetting was ciations of those features to others that remain active,
observed on a test given immediately afterwards. Thus, via the mechanisms of hebbian learning. In an entirely
although the impairment may dissipate within 24 h, in- dierent approach, inhibition might be sustained toni-
troducing a long delay between study and retrieval prac- cally, even after retrieval practice has ended. The per-
tice did not insulate subjects from retrieval-induced sisting strength of practiced items might sustain
forgetting. In at least some circumstances with some types inhibition on competitors, via lateral inhibition.
of materials, the inhibitory eects of retrieval practice The foregoing theoretical possibilities are not in-
recover over time, a nding similar to spontaneous re- tended to be strong claims about the mechanisms un-
covery observed in retroactive interference (e.g., Post- derlying retrieval induced forgetting. They are described
man, Stark, & Fraser, 1968), directed forgetting (Wheeler, merely to illustrate a theoretically crucial point: not
1995), and the verbal overshadowing paradigm (Finger & enough is presently known about how inhibition is
Pezdek, 1999). Interestingly, this recovery from inhibition manifest mechanistically to strongly constrain predic-
occurs even though practiced items still exhibit signicant tions about whether inhibition should recover over time.
facilitation after the same delay, again suggesting that Inhibitory theories exist that can are consistent with
dierential strength does not cause impairment. short and long-lasting inhibition (see Anderson &
It is not clear, however, whether MacLeod and Mac- Spellman, 1995 for similar arguments). Which of these
raes particular delay is needed for people to recover from mechanistic approaches to retrieval-induced forgetting
retrieval-induced forgetting, or even whether recovery provides the best account remains to be established.
always occurs. Although MacLeod and Macraes ndings
suggest a particular recovery interval, this nding may Generality
not generalize to other materials or training protocols. Many of the studies discussed so far have used verbal
For instance, retrieval induced forgetting may be quite categories to study retrieval-induced forgetting. How-
long lasting given dierent parameters for retrieval ever, this phenomenon has now been observed with a
practice. Consider learning the new telephone number of variety of stimulus classes. For example, Ciranni and
a friend whom you call frequently. Initially, their old Shimamura (1999) found that when subjects learned the
number will intrude into consciousness when you want locations of colored objects (e.g., squares, circles, trian-
to dial their new number. But after dialing the new gles or odd, dicult to name shapes), recalling infor-
number enough times over a protracted period, the old mation about one of the objects (e.g., its color or shape)
number eventually stops intruding. Given enough prac- led subjects to forget properties of other objects with the
tice with the new number (over months or a year), one same shape. Using variations of this procedure, they in-
may become completely unable to recall the old tele- duced subjects to forget the color, location, and shape of
phone number. This inability will likely persist inde- the other objects, and found that this impairment only
nitely, even when you have periods during which you do occurred with retrieval-practice and not with extra study
not call your friend (can you remember your old phone exposures. Studies of fact learning have found that re-
number 3 residences ago?). This suggests that if retrieval trieving some facts about a topic impairs recall for other
practice occurs frequently and is distributed over facts about that topic (e.g., Anderson & Bell, 2001;
long time periods, inhibition eects may be long-lasting, Macrae & MacLeod, 1999; Radvansky, 1999). In fact,
although this at present remains an empirical issue. retrieving some facts about a topic (e.g., The actor is
Theoretically, recovery need not occur at all, how- looking at the tulip) not only impairs other facts that
ever, even if inhibitory processes produce retrieval-in- directly compete with it (e.g., The actor is looking at the
duced forgetting. In fact, impairment of nearly any violin), but also facts that share concepts with the com-
duration may be possible, depending on the mechanisms peting facts (e.g., The teacher is lifting the violin), repli-
by which inhibitory eects produce memory failure cating and generalizing the cue-independent impairment
(Anderson & Spellman, 1995). One can separate the observed by Anderson and Spellman (1995). Similar cue-
theoretical mechanism that induces retrieval-induced independent impairment occurs in the fan interference
424 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

paradigm (Radvansky, 1999; however, see Anderson & verbal overshadowing has shown that describing a re-
Reder, 1999). Koutstaal, Schacter, Johnson, and Gal- cently viewed face impairs later recognition of that face
luccio (1999) found that reviewing photographs of novel (Schooler & Engstler-Schooler, 1990). Similarly, de-
actions that subjects had performed two days earlier scribing the avor of a wine impairs its later recognition
(e.g., trace the outline of this boomerang), impaired (Melcher & Schooler, 1996). Schooler, Fiore, and
their later recall of the other actions they had performed. Brandimonte (1997) suggested that describing a per-
Thus, memory can be impaired for ones own physical ceptual memory may be a form of retrieval practice.
actions. Using an eyewitness memory paradigm, studies Subjects may retrieve those aspects of the percept easiest
have shown that interrogating subjects about some de- to verbalize, eschewing other information that, while not
tails of a mock crime scene impairs memory for other as easy to describe, is crucial to recognition. For ex-
related details (MacLeod, 2002; Shaw, Bjork, & Handal, ample, subjects might focus on verbalizable character-
1995), a nding that may have signicant practical ap- istics of a face, such as basic features (nose, mouth),
plications. In a related vein, recent work has shown that rather than congural information about the spacing of
the tendency for peoples memory for an eyewitness event features in relation to each other. Selectively retrieving
to be distorted by misleading post-event information features may suppress congural information. Although
may rely to some degree on retrieval-induced forgetting verbal overshadowing has also been found when the
(Saunders & MacLeod, 2002). Saunders and MacLeod need for retrieval is eliminated (e.g., when subjects re-
found that people were far more likely to inappropriately ceive a description generated by another person), those
remember misinformation on a later test when they had eects go away if subjects are asked to base their rec-
earlier performed retrieval practice on other aspects of ognition judgments solely on memory for the photo-
the event for which the misinformation was introduced. graph and not the verbal description. In contrast,
This suggests that vulnerability to misinformation ac- subjects generating their own descriptions are not helped
ceptance is heightened when access to the original by such instructions (Dodson, Johnson, & Schooler,
memory is weakened by inhibition. 1997; Meissner & Brigham, 2001). These ndings sug-
Implications of retrieval induced forgetting for social gest that active retrieval is important to generating a
psychological phenomena have also been explored. For robust eect, just as with retrieval-induced forgetting.
instance, Macrae and MacLeod (1999) demonstrated Dodson et al. also found that describing another face
that recalling some traits of a person impairs the re- impaired recognition for the one originally studied, in-
trieval of their other personality traits later. Dunn and dicating a generalized suppression of face memories (see
Spellman (2003) recently demonstrated that when people Anderson & Spellman, 1995, for a potentially related
repeatedly retrieve individuating traits of a person about nding termed second-order inhibition). Thus, re-
whom they recently learned, stereotypic traits of that trieving specic features of a perceptual memory while
person were inhibited. Interestingly, the tendency for describing it may inhibit other aspects of the memory.
stereotypic traits to be inhibited was moderated by If inhibitory control mechanisms resolve interference
subjects prior belief in the stereotype: subjects who were in memory retrieval generally, we would also expect
more prone to believe in the stereotype showed greater them to be at work in semantic memory. Consistent with
resistance to inhibition. Macrae and Roseveare (2002) this, B aumls (2002) found that episodic memory for
found that self-relevant encoding may also render in- several studied exemplars of a category was impaired if
formation resistant to inhibitory eects. Subjects were subjects generated new exemplars of the same category
presented with a list of words and told that the items on from semantic memory during the interval between
the list were gifts that were purchased. Some subjects study and test. However, episodic recall was unimpaired
were asked to imagine that these were gifts that they had when this semantic generation practice was replaced
purchased themselves; other subjects were asked to by study exposures of the same novel exemplars, show-
imagine that the gifts were purchased by a best friend or ing that impairment derived specically from semantic
by an unspecied other. Following this encoding phase, retrieval. In a related study, Blaxton and Neely (1983)
the standard phases of the retrieval-practice paradigm found that subjects were slower to generate a critical
were employed. Macrae and Roseveare found that when target exemplar (Fruit A___) from semantic memory
subjects imaged purchasing the gifts themselves (self after they had generated four other prime exemplars
relevant encoding), retrieval-induced forgetting was from that same category. In contrast, subjects were
completely eliminated, whereas the inhibition eect re- faster to generate the same target when the prime items
mained robust in the other encoding conditions. The were presented intact to subjects for speeded naming. In
protective eect of self-relevant encoding may be an recent work, Johnson and Anderson (in press) have
instance of the protective eects of integration (see later shown that repeatedly generating associates to the
section Integration as a moderating factor). subordinate verb meaning of a homograph from general
Some evidence suggests that retrieval impairs recog- knowledge (e.g., Prune T_ _M for Prune Trim) re-
nition memory for perceptual experiences. Research on duced the availability of its dominant noun meaning, as
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 425

measured by an independent probe test in which subjects cannot consider the characteristics of the inhibitory
free associated to novel test cues (e.g., Yogurt F___ for mechanism in isolation. Inhibitory mechanisms act on
fruit, which is related to the noun sense of Prune). memory representations. These representations may
This nding builds on work on lexical ambiguity reso- vary in type, structure, content, or strength, and these
lution suggesting that the contextually inappropriate variations may moderate the impact of inhibitory pro-
sense of a homograph may be suppressed (Gernsbacher cesses or even the necessity of inhibition. Concern over
& Faust, 1991; Simpson & Kang, 1994), by establishing this possibility has a long history in research on inter-
that impaired access to homograph meanings is cue- ference, and is reected in classic work on verbal me-
independent. Parallel ndings have been observed in diation (see Horton & Kjeldergaard, 1961; Jenkins,
episodic memory experiments using homographs, which 1963; Kjeldergaard, 1968; Postman, 1971, for reviews)
establish that inhibitory eects are recall specic, con- and similarity eects (see Osgood, 1949, for a review) on
sistent with properties of retrieval-induced forgetting retroactive interference, integration eects on fan inter-
more generally (Shivde & Anderson, 2001). Finally, re- ference (see, e.g., Radvansky, 1999; Radvansky &
search using the rare-word paradigm has found that Zacks, 1991; Smith, Adams, & Schorr, 1978), and the
dicult semantic retrievals recruit inhibitory processes: eects of level of learning on the magnitude of retro-
When subjects struggle to recall the meaning of an un- active interference or fan eects that are observed (see,
usual, infrequently encountered word that is weakly e.g., Postman, 1971 for a review for retroactive inter-
represented in memory, related concepts appear to be ference; see Hayes-Roth, 1977, for a review concerning
impaired (Barnhardt, Glisky, Polster, & Elam, 1996; fan interference). It is thus not surprising that these
Dagenbach, Carr, & Barnhardt, 1990; see also Thomp- factors are also involved in moderating retrieval induced
son-Schill, 1997 for an interesting discussion of the role forgetting as well. I review the evidence for two such
of the left prefrontal cortex in controlling selective re- moderating factors: integration and similarity. I also
trieval from semantic memory). Taken together, these describe a representational factor that masks inhibi-
results argue that retrieval induced forgetting is not tionbaseline deation.
limited to episodic retrieval, or to taxonomic categories; Integration as a moderating factor. The amount of
rather, it is a general consequence arising whenever in- retrieval-induced forgetting depends strongly on how
hibitory mechanisms are recruited to guide selection in well integrated the to-be-retrieved memories are with the
the face of competition from distracting memories. practiced competitors. Although there is some variation
in how the term integration has been used in the litera-
Moderating and masking factors in retrieval-induced ture, we have used it to refer to the existence of inter-
forgetting connections between items sharing a common retrieval
cueconnections formed either on the basis of pre-ex-
The preceding review describes evidence showing that perimental relationships, or novel interrelationships
whenever we try to selectively retrieve a target item from discovered during the course of an experiment. For in-
long-term memory, other competing memories associ- stance, suppose subjects studied Animals such as
ated to the cue guiding retrieval will be suppressed. Al- Deer, Dog, Bear, Canary, Goat, and Cow. In addition
though this is generally true, it is perhaps not surprising to studying these items in relation to their shared cate-
that there are factors that can either moderate or mask gory label, subjects might form inter-relationships be-
the eects of inhibition. Moderating factors are those tween items such as Deer and Bear (Wild things that you
that genuinely alter the magnitude of inhibition that the hunt), Goat and Cow (farm animals), Dog and Canary
competitors of main interest suer during retrieval (pets), or Dog and Deer (an image of a dog chasing a
practice; masking factors are those that alter the later deer). These inter-relationships could be based on se-
behavioral measure of inhibition without aecting the mantic similarity (e.g., Dog, Wolf), associative related-
magnitude of inhibition that actually transpired during ness (Dog Bone), or even on more elaborate encoding of
retrieval practice. Appreciating these factors is a fun- relations (e.g., interactive imagery).
damental part of understanding the behavioral condi- In general, when subjects integrate the associates of a
tions under which inhibitory control leads to forgetting. cue, it insulates nonpracticed exemplars from retrieval-
These factors can be divided broadly into those con- induced forgetting (Anderson & McCulloch, 1999).
cerning how memories are represented, how retrieval Anderson and McCulloch demonstrated this using the
practice is performed, and how memory is ultimately retrieval-induced forgetting design of Anderson et al.
assessed after inhibition has been induced. We discuss (1994), but with one change: at the time of encoding,
these in turn. subjects either were or were not encouraged to nd inter-
relationships among the exemplars of a category. Sub-
Representational factors that moderate or mask inhibition jects who were asked to integrate exemplars showed
When predicting how much inhibition will occur in a signicant reduction in retrieval induced forgetting
a given population or in a particular condition, one (and in some cases, it was completely eliminated).
426 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

Interestingly, even some subjects who were not asked to subcategory (e.g., Citrus) as the to-be-practiced items
integrate reported that they had done so on their own, as (e.g., Fruit Orange), or a dierent subcategory (e.g.,
measured by a post-experimental questionnaire. These Fruit Cherry). After the encoding phase, subjects en-
subjects showed the same reduction in retrieval-induced gaged in retrieval practice in the usual fashion, and then
forgetting as the group instructed to use integration. The were given a nal category-plus-letter stem cued recall
more study time subjects were allowed, the higher the test. In contrast to Smith and Hunt (2000), Bauml and
reported use of spontaneous integration strategies, and Hartinger found that increasing interitem similarity re-
the lower the amount of retrieval induced forgetting. duced retrieval-induced forgetting. Bauml and Hartinger
Similar integration eects were observed in retrieval-in- replicated this pattern using an output interference
duced forgetting experiments using propositional mate- paradigm instead of retrieval practice, and argued that
rials (Anderson & Bell, 2001): when subjects reported similar mechanisms underlie these two phenomena.
integrating multiple facts about a topic (e.g., The ant The Smith and Hunt (2000) and B auml and Hartin-
crawled on the rock, The ant crawled on the table), re- ger (2002) ndings are not necessarily contradictory. In
trieval-induced forgetting was either reduced or elimi- recent work, Anderson et al. (2000) explored whether
nated. These latter eects were even found in an semantic similarity might have dierent eects on re-
incidental encoding task in which subjects were asked to trieval induced forgetting depending on whether one is
form vivid mental images of the situations represented concerned with what they termed targetcompetitor
by the sentence; when subjects reported incorporating similarity or competitorcompetitor similarity. As illus-
multiple facts into a single image, inhibition was sig- trated in Figs. 4A and B, the unpracticed competitors in
nicantly reduced. Thus, although retrieving some as- a category undergoing retrieval practice can either vary
sociates of a retrieval cue generally impairs other in (a) how similar they are to the target items receiving
associates that become activated in the process, inte- retrieval practice (i.e., targetcompetitor similarity), or
gration poses a strong boundary condition on when this (b) how similar they are to each other, independent of
impairment occurs. how similar they may be to the retrieval practice targets
Similarity as a moderating factor. The amount of (i.e., competitorcompetitor similarity). Anderson et al.
inhibition that retrieval will cause also depends on se- (2000) argued that these two dimensions should have
mantic similarity between the associates of a cue. The very dierent eects on retrieval-induced forgetting,
nature of this relationship is complex, however, as il- based on the distributed representation approach pro-
lustrated by the studies of Smith and Hunt (2000) and posed by Anderson and Spellman (1995). According to
Bauml and Hartinger (2002). Smith and Hunt (2000) this approach, increasing targetcompetitor similarity
adapted the retrieval practice procedure so that the de- from a moderate level (top of Fig. 4A) to a very high
gree of within-category similarity might be varied. Spe- level (bottom of Fig. 4A) should diminish retrieval in-
cically, they altered the study phase to encourage the duced forgetting. Less impairment should be observed
encoding of either similarities or dierences between because, according to the model, the recall probability of
exemplars of a category. For the similarity encoding an item reects the summed activation of all of its fea-
group, subjects viewed all six exemplars of the category tures. Because high targetcompetitor similarity leads
at once and were asked to nd a way that the item at the many of a competitors features to overlap with the re-
top of the list was similar to all of the remaining items. trieval practice target, the facilitatory eects of retrieval
Shared features were then generated in turn for the other practice on shared features will compensate for or pos-
ve exemplars. After encoding the categories in this way, sibly even outweigh the inhibition suered by the com-
subjects went through the remaining phases of the re- petitors distinctive features. On the other hand,
trieval practice procedure. The dierence encoding increasing competitorcompetitor similarity from a
group followed the same steps, but was asked instead to moderate level (top of Fig. 4B) to a high level (bottom of
nd one feature that made the top item dierent from all Fig. 4B) should magnify the amount of retrieval-induced
of the remaining items. Smith and Hunt found that en- forgetting. More impairment should be observed be-
coding dierences between exemplars abolished retrieval cause in the high similarity condition, the impact of
induced forgetting, but encoding similarities yielded suppressing a single feature that overlaps two dierent
robust impairment. They concluded that inter-item exemplars will be realized through the impairment of
similarity increases retrieval-induced forgetting. two items, not just one; thus, the behavioral eect of
However, Bauml and Hartinger (2002) found a pat- applying the inhibition to highly overlapping represen-
tern that appears to directly contradict that observed by tations will be exaggerated, even if the same amount of
Smith and Hunt. These authors also sought to manip- inhibition is applied.
ulate the inter-item similarity between the exemplars of a Anderson et al. (2000) tested these hypotheses by
category, but they manipulated similarity by varying separately manipulating the degree of targetcompetitor
whether or not the unpracticed competitors in a cate- and competitorcompetitor similarity. Following Smith
gory (e.g., Fruit Lemon) were drawn from the same and Hunt, they held the study materials constant and
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 427

Fig. 4. Illustration of two dierent dimensions of similarity, as distinguished in the two-factor model of Anderson, Green, and
McCulloch (2000). Memory items (larger circles) are represented here as sets of semantic features (small circles). Similar items overlap
in feature space (as represented by overlapping larger circles). Retrieval practice is assumed to increase the activation of practiced
features (darkened circles) and to inhibit some of the features of the competing, similar pattern (lighter circles with Xes in them), but
not other features of the competitor (small white circles). (A) An illustration of how targetcompetitor similarity (similarity between
the practiced item and an unpracticed competitor) can be low (top half) or high (bottom half) and how this inuences inhibition. With
high targetcompetitor similarity, a greater proportion of a competitors features overlap with the practiced item and are strengthened,
compensating for inhibition on the remaining features. (B) An illustration of how competitorcompetitor similarity (similarity amongst
the competitors themselves) can be low (top) or high (bottom), and how this may inuence inhibition. With high competitorcom-
petitor similarity, inhibiting the same number of units has a greater impact on the two competitors, because the inhibition aects
features shared by the two items.

manipulated similarity by asking subjects to identify conicting ndings of Bauml and Hartinger and Smith
similarities or dierences between exemplars. However, and Hunt, as well as analogous inconsistencies in the
instead of having subjects do this for all pairwise com- literature on the role of similarity in classical interfer-
parisons within a category, subjects were presented with ence studies (see Anderson et al., 2000 for a discussion).
either targetcompetitor or competitorcompetitor
pairings, to control the dimension of similarity that was Baseline deation as a masking factor
manipulated. Following this similarity (or dierence) When considering how representational variables
encoding phase, the remaining steps of the retrieval that might aect inhibition, it is also important to attend
practice procedure were done in the typical fashion. The to the representation of baseline items. Retrieval-in-
results were striking: in the targetcompetitor condition, duced forgetting may be masked if the baseline used to
signicantly less inhibition was found when subjects assess inhibition is also aected by retrieval practice.
were asked to nd similarities than when they were Such baseline deation may arise in two ways. First,
asked to nd dierences between items during encoding. as Anderson et al. (1994) noted, practiced and baseline
In fact, subjects who were asked to nd targetcom- categories are represented in a common episodic con-
petitor similarities showed signicant retrieval-induced text. Retrieval practice may therefore suppress items in
facilitation of competing items, not inhibition. In the baseline categories because they share contextual fea-
competitorcompetitor condition, however, more inhi- tures with items undergoing retrieval practice. To the
bition was found when subjects were asked to encode extent that baseline categories are also suppressed by
similarities than when they were asked to encode dif- inhibitory processes, the ability to determine how much
ferences. Indeed, the dierence encoding condition yiel- inhibition has taken place on within-category competing
ded no signicant inhibition. These ndings strongly exemplars is compromised. This possibility is arguably
support the idea that competitorcompetitor similarity consistent with several ndings in the output interfer-
has an opposite eect on inhibition than targetcom- ence and retrieval practice literatures. For example,
petitor similarity, as suggested by the Anderson and recall probability declines for categories or paired
Spellman (1995) distributed approach. Anderson et al. associates that are tested later in a testing sequence,
(2000) argued that these ndings help to reconcile the even when those categories or paired associates are not
428 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

apparently similar and do not explicitly share any cues Retrieval-practice factors that moderate inhibition
(e.g., Roediger & Schmidt, 1980; Smith, 1971). Fur- The amount of inhibition that occurs is also likely to
thermore, Tsukimoto and Kawaguchi (2001) found that depend on the amount of attention given to the dierent
baseline categories can be impaired by retrieval practice, cues provided for retrieval practice. In particular, any
as compared to performance on those same baseline type of retrieval practice that minimizes the need to re-
categories in a control group who did not perform re- solve interference between competing items is unlikely to
trieval practice. These ndings suggest that shared con- produce inhibition. Consider, for example, the study by
textual features may precipitate baseline suppression, Anderson et al. (2000) reviewed earlier. When subjects
reducing measured inhibition. This factor thus masks were given the category and asked to recall the exemplar
inhibition because it aects estimates of the amount of based on stem cues (e.g., Fruit Or___), signicant re-
inhibition on competing items without altering the true trieval-induced forgetting was observed; however, when
level of inhibition that actually took place for those subjects were given the exemplar, and asked to recall the
items. category (e.g., Fr___ Orange), there was no impairment.
A second source of baseline deation can arise when This pattern is likely to have arisen because the cue in
baseline and practiced categories are similar. To the the latter caseOrangewas associated to the category,
extent that baseline categories share semantic features but not to other exemplars in the category, eliminating
with items that are inhibited by retrieval practice, re- competition that would lead to impairment. Similarly, if
trieval-induced forgetting may generalize to those cate- subjects were asked to perform retrieval practice without
gories. Anderson and Spellmans (1995) cross category the category label (e.g., Or_n_e for Orange), other ex-
inhibition ndings provide a case in point: Practicing red emplars in the category are unlikely to interfere and thus
items such as Red-Blood not only suppressed items ex- may not be impaired. Subtler cases may also be possible.
plicitly studied under the Red category (e.g., Red To- For example, even when subjects are given the category
mato), but also other red items that were studied and and a fragment cue for retrieval practice, subjects might
tested under a separate category (e.g., Food Radish). focus their attention on the fragment cuethat is, they
Even non-red Food items (e.g., Food Bread) were in- may solve the retrieval practice task by circumventing
hibited after subjects practiced Red-Blood, suggesting interference caused by the shared category. This seems
that the inhibition of items that directly competed with especially likely when the fragment cue is highly infor-
the retrieval practice target (e.g., Red-Tomato) seman- mative or draws attention. For example, if multiple
tically generalized to other items that overlapped with letters are provided (e.g., Fruit B_n_n_), subjects might
them in semantic features. In a similar vein, Anderson spend more of their time focusing on the distinguishing
and Bell (2001) found that practicing some facts about a letter features, trying to solve the fragment by sounding
topic (e.g., The actor is playing the guitar) impaired not the word out. In general, any factor that reduces at-
only other facts sharing that topic (e.g., The actor is tention given to the shared cue and focuses it on the
playing the oboe), but also facts studied under a dierent distinguishing cue is likely to reduce activation of com-
topic but sharing the same relation and category (e.g., petitors and therefore reduce inhibition.
The teacher is playing the drum). Thus, impairment
generalized across topics, based on semantic similarity. Test factors that moderate, mask, or exaggerate inhibition
Anderson and Bell (2001) were able to measure the in- In our initial studies of retrieval-induced forgetting,
hibition of the latter items because they included addi- we measured subjects nal memory performance with a
tional baseline topics that did not share the same category cued recall test. Subjects were provided with
relation and category with practiced items (e.g., The box each studied category name in turn, and asked to recall
is in the warehouse, The mop is in the pub). The gen- all of the studied exemplars in any order. Inhibition has
eralized suppression was circumscribed to items with been found consistently with this type of test (Anderson
specic overlap in sematic relations with items studied & Bell, 2001; Anderson et al., 1994; Anderson &
with the practiced topic, and could not have been pro- McCulloch, 1999; Anderson & Spellman, 1995; Butler,
duced by a global contextual similarity. These ndings Williams, Zacks, & Maki, 2001; Macrae & MacLeod,
strongly suggest that studies of retrieval induced for- 1999; Nader, Coles, Brigidi, & Foa, 2001; Smith &
getting need to take appropriate measures to ensure that Hunt, 1999), even when the shared cue is not categorical
within-subjects baseline conditions are as dissimilar in nature (e.g., Anderson & Bell, 2001; Macrae & Ma-
from practiced categories as possible. Baseline suppres- cLeod, 1999). Other tests have also been used, however,
sion eects such as these might be one reason why not only to characterize the range of conditions under
Anderson and Reder (1999) failed to nd evidence for which retrieval-induced forgetting occurs, but also to
cue-independent impairment in their fan eect para- infer various properties of the eect. In this section, I
digm: All of their propositions were constructed using discuss some of the work that has been done with al-
the same semantic relation and object class (all were is ternative testing formats, with an emphasis on factors
in facts, such as The lawyer was in the park). that may moderate or mask inhibitory eects.
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 429

Output interference eects. Depending on the test does. Finally, when one wants to compare the relative
type that one uses, the amount of retrieval-induced amount of inhibition across two conditions or groups, it
forgetting may reect at least two sources: impairment is essential to disentangle dierent sources of inhibition.
arising from the earlier retrieval practice phase, and Greater inhibition may occur in one condition, for ex-
impairment produced by the nal recall test. The nal ample, merely because greater associative strengthening
test contributes a second source of impairment because for practiced items produced greater output-based ef-
the strengthening of practiced items during the earlier fects in that condition.
retrieval practice phase leads those items to be recalled The most straightforward way to distinguish the
earlier in the nal test sequence. Because this will delay practice and test-based contributions to retrieval-in-
unpracticed competitors until later in the output se- duced forgetting is to use a test such as category-plus-
quence, these items are subject to additional retrieval stem cued recall that enables one to control recall order
induced forgettingthat is, they are subject to exagger- (Anderson et al., 1994). In a typical study, the nal test
ated output interference, relative to baseline categories. is composed of trials in which each exemplar is cued
This bias in the retrieval of practiced items is interesting with its category name and a one letter stem for the
because it may provide one mechanism by which the exemplar. Importantly, subjects are rst cued to recall
inhibitory eects of retrieval can be reinstated on a re- all of the unpracticed items from a category, then all of
curring basis, even when initial inhibitory eects have the practiced items, or vice versa. Comparisons are then
dissipated (Anderson & Bell, 2001). However, the con- made to baseline items tested in the corresponding
tribution of test-based sources of impairment can impair halves of their respective categories. It is typically as-
clear theoretical inferences about the conditions pro- sumed that recall impairment observed when all un-
ducing inhibition, and so it is necessary to consider this practiced exemplars are tested before practiced items
factor in assessing inhibition. Such inferential diculties must reect the lingering eects of the retrieval practice
are most likely to arise in test formats that allow subjects phase, for the simple reason that practiced items have
to report items in any order they wish, although they are yet to be recalled. When unpracticed exemplars are
not restricted to those types of test. Theoretically, ex- tested rst in this way, signicant retrieval-induced
aggerated output interference is neither a moderating forgetting is typically found (Anderson & Bell, 2001;
nor a masking factor, because it does not alter the Anderson et al., 1994; Anderson et al., 2000; Anderson
amount of inhibition that actually took place during et al., 2000; Anderson & McCulloch, 1999; B auml, 2002;
retrieval practice, nor does it prevent us from seeing this Bauml & Hartinger, 2002), although sometimes it is re-
eect; it does, however, alter the measured estimate of duced in magnitude from the eects observed with cat-
retrieval-practice based inhibition. egory cued recall without letter stems. This nding
The contribution of output interference is of greatest makes sense given the elimination of output interference
concern in two varieties of experiment: when one wants from the eect. In comparing the recall of items tested in
to establish the retrieval practice phase as the primary the rst half of their categories to those tested in the
source of impairment, and when one is concerned with second half, output interference is typically observed on
variations in the amount of inhibition that have oc- this kind of test, reinforcing the importance of isolating
curred across dierent conditions or groups. Knowing the two sources of impairment. By using this type of
whether inhibition primarily reects events in the prac- testing procedure, several studies have found that
tice phase is important, for example, in determining strengthening competitors does not reliably impair re-
whether extra study exposures cause inhibition. Re- lated items when output interference is controlled (for
trieval practice and extra study exposures both retrieval-induced forgetting, see Anderson et al., 2000;
strengthen the practiced items, so that on a delayed re- for list-strength eects, see B
auml, 1997; see also B
auml,
call test, those items are likely to be recalled early in the 1996 for a conceptually similar nding for retroactive
recall sequence. If subjects are free to recall items in any interference).
order, unpracticed competitors in both of these condi- However, using category-plus-stem cued recall is not
tions will be subject to greater output interference (test sucient to ensure that output interference has been
based retrieval-induced forgetting) than corresponding adequately matched across baseline and practiced cate-
items in baseline categories. Thus, even if extra study gories. There are cases in which output interference
exposures produced no inhibition during the practice dierences can arise even when recall order is xed. In
phase, signicant impairment might be observed on the particular, category-plus stem cued recall tests in which
nal test, leading one to conclude that extra study ex- the practiced and unpracticed exemplars of a category
posures caused inhibition. Similarly, if one is concerned are randomly interspersed in the recall order do not
with how long retrieval-induced forgetting lasts, one adequately control for output interference. For example,
must be sure that test-based sources of impairment do suppose that subjects study the items Orange, Banana,
not contribute to the measure of inhibition, or one might Lemon, Cherry, Apple, and Grape as members of the
be led to believe that inhibition lasts longer than it truly Fruits category, and then perform retrieval practice on
430 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

Orange Banana and Lemon. On the nal recall test, the for the practiced, competitor, and baseline conditions.
recall sequence Grape, Cherry and Apple, Banana Or- As might be expected, retrieval practice facilitated the
ange Lemon would control for output interference bi- delayed recall of practiced items; more surprising,
ases, whereas Apple, Orange, Cherry, Banana, Grape, however, retrieval practice also facilitated unpracticed
and Lemon would not. Although the latter format tests competitors, relative to items in unpracticed categories.
items in a xed order that is constant across practiced A comparable result can be seen in the right side of
and baseline categories, output interference is not mat- Fig. 5A, which depicts the ndings of a highly similar
ched. This is because when the category is practiced, but independent experiment by Smith and Hunt (2000).
there is a much greater likelihood of recalling the prac- The ndings of Anderson (1989) and Hunt and Smith
ticed items (Orange, Banana, and Lemon) than the items (1998) appear inconsistent with the notion that retrieval
in the corresponding positions for the baseline category. practice suppresses related items, as has been argued
Thus, more test-based output interference will be exerted throughout. Indeed, from a behavioral standpoint, these
on unpracticed competitors in the practiced category results indicate that under some testing conditions, re-
when practiced and unpracticed items are interspersed trieval practice enhances the recall of related items.
(at least on those that follow practiced items). If it is However, to conclude that no inhibition occurred in
important to ensure that inhibition eects are not being these experiments would be a mistake. Both experiments
produced at the time of output, weaker items should be used a testing format (free recall) that permits cue
tested before strengthened items. priming to inuence how much inhibition is measured.
Cue priming as a masking factor. When subjects Specically, with categorized word lists (and organized
perform retrieval practice, they are typically presented lists in general), it is widely believed that subjects adopt
with a category name and the rst two letters of the a hierarchical retrieval scheme for recalling study items;
exemplar that they are to retrieve. If retrieval is suc- retrieval progresses rst from a representation of the
cessful, the practiced item is facilitated, and competing episodic context in which items are studied, to the cat-
items are suppressed. However, retrieval practice intro- egories on the list, and next from the category repre-
duces another factor as well. Given that the practiced sentations to the particular exemplars (see, e.g., Rundus,
category is typically presented nine times in the standard 1973). Given this multi-stage process, the likelihood of
retrieval practice session (3 exemplars are practiced three recalling an exemplar is inuenced by two probabilities:
times each), the category name enjoys a substantial the probability of recalling the category label, given the
boost in accessibility. In some circumstances, this cue context as a cue, and the probability of recalling the
priming can reduce the amount of inhibition that is exemplar, given that the category label has been recalled.
measured, without actually inuencing the level of in- The combination of these factors determines how well
hibition that takes place. practiced items, unpracticed competitors, and baseline
The eect of cue priming on measures of inhibition items will be recalled. Ordinarily when category cued
can be seen in our rst experiment on retrieval-induced recall is used, the probability of recalling the category
forgetting (Anderson, 1989). This experiment employed labels is constant at 1.0, because the labels are provided.
the basic retrieval practice paradigm, except that we However, on free recall tests, biases in category recall
used free recall as our nal test instead of category cued across conditions become an issue, particularly when
recall. The results can be seen in the left side of Fig. 5A more than just a few categories are used and subjects

Fig. 5. Examples of cue priming eects in free recall in studies by Anderson (1989) and Hunt and Smith (1998). Subjects underwent the
standard retrieval practice procedure of Anderson et al. (1994) and were tested with free recall instead of category-cued recall. (A)
Percentage of practiced items, unpracticed competitors and baseline items recalled on the nal free recall test. Practice facilitated the
practiced items as well as the unpracticed competitors. (B) The same data as in (A), counting only those items from categories for which
subjects recalled at least one item (ensuring category access). Conditionalizing recall in this way reveals a signicant retrieval induced
forgetting eect that had been masked by primed access to category labels.
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 431

may thus forget categories. Given that practiced cate- relationships for the practiced topic would have been far
gory labels (e.g., Fruits) are primed, they are quite sa- more accessible than the semantic relationship used
lient to subjects, leaving baseline categories at a recall in the baseline topics. This suggests that even if the object
disadvantage. The end result is that subjects are more of the unpracticed competitor The ant crawled on the
likely to forget whole baseline categories, and miss the chair was suppressed during the practice of the ant
opportunity to recall exemplars from those categories. crawled on the rock, this suppression would be masked
Thus, suppression of unpracticed competitors by re- by heightened availability of the shared relational con-
trieval practice may be masked by the overall advantage cept unless it was provided as a nal test cue.
in the accessibility of practiced categories. Thus, the complexities introduced by cue priming are
To illustrate how cue priming masked a true decit in not limited to categorized word lists or to free recall.
exemplar access in the Anderson (1989) and Hunt and Indeed, cue priming may even inuence simple paired
Smith (1998) studies, the free recall data were reanalyzed associates tests to the extent that subjects link the
to focus on only those categories from which subjects stimulus and response members with a relation (e.g.,
recalled at least one exemplar. Our assumption was that when encoding the pair Bird Worm, the relation eats
subjects who recalled at least one exemplar from a cat- is likely to be inferred) that may be forgotten indepen-
egory had accessed the category. Restricting the analysis dently of memory for the response. This factor makes it
to those categories would allow us to examine exemplar crucial to consider whether the structure of the materials
access for the practiced and baseline conditions, un- used in a given paradigm, when coupled with the test
contaminated by dierences in category access. As can format, might mask inhibition through cue priming.
be seen in Fig. 5B, this analysis revealed a pattern of Masking through transfer-inappropriate testing eects.
retrieval induced forgetting quite similar to what is or- Whether inhibitory eects will be observed should de-
dinarily observed, with unpracticed competitors being pend on the degree to which the memory trace tapped by
recalled more poorly than baseline items. Additional the retrieval test matches the trace that was inhibited by
analyses conrmed that the probability of forgetting retrieval practice. To illustrate this, suppose that a
whole categories (i.e., category dropout) was much subject encodes the pairs Tree-Prune, Tree-Rock, and
higher for baseline categories (20%) than it was for Trim Prune and then does retrieval practice on Tree-
practiced categories (1%). Hunt and Smith (1998) found Rock. Later on, suppose that subjects memory for
a very similar pattern, as can be seen in the right side of Prune is tested either by cuing with Tree-P___ or Trim
Fig. 5B. These ndings illustrate how cue priming can P___. If retrieval-induced forgetting is found with Tree-
mask inhibition eects when a multi-stage recall process P__, but not Trim P___, would it mean that impairment
is likely, as it is on free recall tests. is cue-dependent? If so, would it mean that Prune was
However, cue priming eects are not limited to free never inhibited? At rst glance, it might seem so, to the
recall, nor to categorized word lists. Consider the study extent that cue-independence is an essential feature of
of propositional retrieval-induced forgetting by Ander- inhibition. After all, these tests vary in the cues that they
son and Bell (2001). When subjects performed retrieval present to subjects, so if impairment depends on which
practice on previously learned facts such as The ant cues are used, it must obviously be cue dependent. This
crawled on the rock, the later recall of other facts would appear to contradict the property of cue-inde-
sharing that topic such as The ant crawled on the ta- pendence. However, this conclusion does not necessarily
ble, was impaired relative to baseline facts such as The follow.
actor looked at the painting. However, Anderson and The problem is that the foregoing argument fails to
Bell cued subjects on their nal test with the topic consider the distinction between the nominal form of a
and the relation (e.g., The ant crawled on the ____, stimulus, and its functional representation by subjects.
and The actor looked at the _______ ), sometimes Although from the standpoint of the experimenter, the
together with a letter stem. If we had instead simply given word Prune is identical when presented in the pairs Tree-
subjects the cue The ant and The actor, the nal test Prune and Trim-Prune, the underlying representations
would likely have become a multi-stage recall test, even formed by subjects may not be. When studying Tree-
though free recall was not used. This is because we used Prune, subjects might have encoded prunes fruit sense,
many dierent topics with dierent semantic relation- but when studying trim-prune, they certainly would
ships (e.g., is crawling on, is looking at, is in, is eating, encode its verb meaning instead. When retrieval practice
etc), most of which could be paired with any topic and so was performed on Tree-Rock, an episodic representa-
could not be easily guessed. Given only the topic as a cue, tion including the fruit sense of prune may have been
subjects would have had to recall the activity or rela- suppressed, making it less accessible when tested with
tionship that the topic was engaged in rst, followed by Tree P___. Such inhibition would not be expected to
the objects of that activity. Because subjects practiced materialize on the test Trim P____, however, because
three facts for each practiced topic three times each this test taps subjects episodic memory for an entirely
(e.g., three things that the ant crawled on), the semantic unrelated concept that was never inhibited (a better
432 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

independent probe, in this circumstance, would have review). If orthographic or phonological tests are less
been Fruit P___). In essence, the independent probe sensitive to increases in the accessibility of conceptually
Trim P___ is simply not testing the same episodic rep- coded information about a word (as is shown by levels
resentation that was inhibited, even though it may seem of processing dissociations), it seems possible that they
the same from the experimenters standpoint. This ex- might also be less sensitive to decreases in the accessi-
ample illustrates how the cue-independence property of bility of those codes produced by inhibition.
inhibition pertains to the particular functional represen- The idea that categorically driven retrieval practice
tation that is formed by the subject: given that a repre- primarily inhibits conceptual as opposed to ortho-
sentation is inhibited, its recall should be impaired, and graphic or phonological levels of analysis receives some
this impairment should be observable from a variety of support from a recent study by Butler et al. (2001).
cues that tap that particular representation. For these These investigators employed the retrieval practice par-
reasons, when designing tests to determine whether or adigm, but varied the nature of the nal recall test.
not inhibition is present, it is essential to ensure that the Dierent groups were tested with the standard category
test might reasonably be expected to tap the represen- cued recall test (e.g., presentation of the category
tation that was inhibited by the subject. If not, transfer- Bird), or with one of several lexically oriented implicit
inappropriate testing may mask the inhibition that ac- and explicit recall tests such as word fragment comple-
tually occurred. tion (e.g., cuing subjects with _p_r_ow for the word
Transfer-inappropriate testing eects may not be Sparrow with a free completion instruction), word
limited to stimuli that have dierent meanings, or to the fragment cued recall (e.g., _p_r_ow with an explicit re-
use of the independent probe method. These eects may call instruction), category-plus-fragment cued recall
also arise when multiple levels of representation are (e.g., Bird, _p_r_ow ) or category-plus stem cued recall
possible. For instance, during word encoding, ortho- (e.g., Bird Sp_____). With the exception of category
graphic, phonological, and conceptual representations cued recall, these tests focus subjects attention to
may each be formed (depending on the orienting task), varying degrees on the orthographic and phonological
and these representations may be functionally and an- features of the cued words. Subjects are likely to com-
atomically distinct (see Balota, 1994, for a review; see plete the fragment _p_r_ow not primarily through con-
also Roskies, Fiez, Balota, Raichle, & Petersen, 2001 for ceptually driven episodic recall, but by sounding out
a discussion of anatomical localization of these dierent the answer based on general knowledge of word forms.
linguistic codes). If dierent levels of representation are If so, retrieval-induced forgetting may be attenuated
formed for the same nominal verbal stimulus, there is because the test weights a level of representation dier-
potential for transfer-inappropriate testing to attenuate ent from the one that is inhibited. Consistent with this,
or mask inhibition. To see this, suppose that performing Butler et al. found no retrieval-induced forgetting on
retrieval practice using a categorically driven cued-recall any tests involving letter cuing. These results are com-
test such as Fruits Or___ (for Fruits Orange), inhibits patible with the idea that retrieval-induced forgetting
conceptually based episodic representations of compet- primarily aects conceptually based representations.
ing fruits such as Banana. If this conceptually based Unfortunately, it is dicult to attribute these eects to
representation is structurally distinct from the phono- transfer-inappropriate testing because Butler et al.s ex-
logical and orthographic representations formed during periments are likely to be contaminated by integration
the initial processing of Banana, little inhibition would strategies during encoding. Subjects were given 8 s to
be expected for Banana on orthographic or phonologi- study each exemplar instead of the usual 45 s, a pro-
cally oriented tests. Retrieval may simply fail to make cedure likely to increase integration (Anderson & Bell,
contact with the representation that was inhibited. The 2001; Anderson & McCulloch, 1999). This seems espe-
underlying principle behind this idea receives some cially plausible, given the unusually small amount of
support from ndings in the levels of processing litera- retrieval-induced forgetting that they found in their
ture: Manipulating the level of processing of words at category cued recall condition (5%, compared to the
encoding has dramatic eects on later recall and recog- typical 920%).
nition tests, but, these eects can disappear or even re- The notion of transfer inappropriate testing is par-
verse when the nal explicit memory test focuses subjects ticularly important to consider in connection with
on the lexical and phonemic properties of words (e.g., experiments examining whether retrieval-induced for-
Fisher & Craik, 1977; McDaniel, Friedman, & Bourne, getting aects performance on implicit memory tests.
1978; Morris, Bransford, & Franks, 1977). On percep- One might argue that if retrieval practice truly inhibits
tually driven implicit memory tasks such as word frag- competitors, eects should be observed on indirect
ment completion, word stem completion, and perceptual memory tests. Caution is warranted here, however, be-
identication, levels of processing has little eect (e.g., cause not all indirect memory tests are the same. Many
Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; Roediger, Weldon, Stadler, & of the most common tests are perceptually oriented,
Rieger, 1992; see Roediger & McDermott, 1993, for a such as word fragment completion, lexical decision
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 433

(some varieties) and perceptual identication. To the retrieval practice may only reveal inhibition on con-
extent that these tests tap perceptually based represen- ceptually driven tests because retrieval practice is con-
tations, they would not be expected to yield evidence for ceptually oriented.
inhibition, regardless of their implicit/explicit status. A Even if conceptually driven implicit tests did not
better strategy would be to use conceptually driven in- show inhibition, however, it wouldnt by itself imply that
direct tests such as free association, semantic uency and inhibition eects did not occur. Here again, it remains
perhaps category verication, which would be sensitive possible that the lack of impairment on the implicit test
to variations in the accessibility of a semantic repre- may be due to transfer-inappropriate testing eects.
sentation. Consistent with this possibility, recent studies Theoretically, it seems reasonable to distinguish between
have found retrieval-induced forgetting on conceptually a general semantic representation of an item (e.g., Ba-
driven, but not perceptually driven implicit tests (Per- nana) and an episodic representation of that item as it
fect, Moulin, Conway, & Perry, 2002; see also Moulin appeared on a study list. The episodic representation of
et al., 2002 for further evidence of impairment on a the item may be composed not only of distinctive con-
conceptual implicit tests). textual features, but also instantiations of semantic
The foregoing discussion does not imply that per- features generally used to represent the item in semantic
ceptually oriented memory representations cannot be memory. To the extent that such an episodic represen-
inhibited by retrieval practice. Indeed, the type of rep- tation is at least partially structurally distinct from the
resentation aected by inhibition should be driven by general semantic representation of the item (the episode-
which representations cause competition during re- specic component residing perhaps as a bound set of
trieval. This should be determined in part by the nature features in the hippocampus, as opposed to neocortex;
of the cues guiding retrieval practice, and by subjects see, e.g, Norman & OReilly, in press), we must consider
retrieval goals. If the subject is asked to retrieve a the possibility that the episode can be suppressed with-
studied word that begins with the letters Ac, ortho- out aecting the general concept of Banana (Anderson
graphically similar competitors may be more inhibited & Bell, 2001). This form of episode-specic inhibition
than semantically related competitors. Although this has may be particularly likely when episodic retrieval prac-
not been tested, related research on implicit memory is tice is performed, as in most studies of retrieval-induced
consistent with this possibility. For instance, Rajaram, forgetting; because retrieval practice is guided not only
Srinivas, and Travers (2001) found that the amount of by a category and a letter stem, but also by a contextual
repetition priming exhibited for a word on either a word representation of the study list, the episodic represen-
fragment or stem completion test was signicantly re- tation of a competing item may be the primary source of
duced when subjects had to ignore that words identity competition, not the semantic representation of an item.
during encoding. When subjects were presented with a In fact, research has demonstrated that episodic repre-
word colored in red, blue, green, or yellow, and asked to sentations can indeed be inhibited: Ciranni and Shi-
quickly identify the color of the word, subjects exhibited mamura (1999) found evidence that novel visuo-spatial
less priming than when they simply had to name the representations can be inhibited by retrieval practice,
word itself. Although one might attribute reduced even though these representations clearly do not have
priming to reduced encoding in the color naming con- well learned semantic counterparts. It may therefore be
dition, Rajaram et al. established that the words had possible to observe episode specic inhibition in more
been identied suciently to cause competition with traditional retrieval-induced forgetting experiments in
color naming; the reaction time to name the color of a which the materials also happen to have a corresponding
word was signicantly slower than the time to name a representation in semantic memory. If episode specic
neutral stimulus (e.g., a row of Xes). Rajaram et al. inhibition is possible, such inhibition eects should
argued that the diminished repetition priming reects generalize to independent retrieval cues used to test ac-
the inhibition of the word itself, driven by the need to cessibility of that episode (on an explicit test), even when
focus attention on the color attribute of the word during eects do not appear on implicit tests.
the color naming triala process they refer to as dese- Although the Ciranni and Shimamura ndings indi-
lection. If correct, this view suggests that retrieval driven cate that episode-specic inhibition may occur, a num-
by one perceptual attribute of a stimulus (e.g., color) ber of considerations suggest that this may not provide a
may suppress other perceptual aspects of that stimulus general account of retrieval-induced forgetting. First,
that cause interference (e.g., visual word form). This there is evidence that semantic and episodic retrieval
eect may later be observed on a perceptually driven competition are not so cleanly separable, at least in
implicit test that relies on the rejected attribute. Analo- studies of inhibition. For instance, semantic retrieval
gous dynamics may be partially responsible for certain practice has been shown to impair episodic representa-
cases of implicit memory blocks driven by orthography tions of similar items (B auml, 2002), and part-set cuing
of a word (Smith & Tindell, 1997). The standard of episodically presented items appears to impair access
retrieval-practice experiment with categorically driven to semantically related competitors (Kimball & Bjork,
434 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

2002; see later section on recognition testing for further augment their memory search by recalling earlier-stud-
discussion). Second, to adopt episode-specic inhibition ied categories, some subjects reported using this covert
as a theory for all varieties of episodic retrieval-induced cuing strategy (the average rating was 2.68 on a 5 point
forgetting ignores a very plausible feature of retrieval: scale). Thus, when given the extralist category cue Food
subjects can weight the dierent cues that they use T___, some subjects may have covertly recalled the
exibly, depending on the task. In some tasks, episodic category Red Things, and used these two categories
context may be the most important cue to weight, jointly to recall items. Subjects who reported using this
whereas in others, the semantic category may be more strategy showed modestly reduced inhibition eects,
diagnostic in guiding retreival. If so, whether one ob- compared to subjects who did not use this strategy (a
serves episode-specic inhibition, or joint eects of in- reduction of the inhibition eect by 3% in Experiment 1,
hibition on both episodic and semantic representations and by 7% in Experiment 2). Given that the usefulness of
may hinge on the relative weighting of attention on covert cuing may have been limited by the timing con-
contextual versus categorical cues. Finally, the rela- straints used in the test of that experiment (4 s per cue),
tionship between episodic and semantic representations these ndings suggest that covert cuing may act to re-
is at present not theoretically resolved: episodes may or duce inhibition under less constrained conditions.
may not be structurally distinguishable from their se- The foregoing ndings may be understood by con-
mantic counterparts. These issues remain to be explored sidering the eects of cue priming discussed earlier. To
in greater depth. Nevertheless, in any study looking at the extent that practiced categories are made highly ac-
whether episodically induced inhibition may be observed cessible by retrieval practice, subjects who engage in
on conceptual implicit memory tests, it would be pru- covert cuing are more likely to covertly generate the
dent to entertain episode-specic inhibition as a theo- practiced categories than they are the baseline catego-
retical mechanism that may contribute to performance. ries. As a result, when trying to recall inhibited items,
The foregoing examples illustrate the central impor- subjects using this strategy should be more likely to have
tance of considering the nature of the representation not one, but two category cues at their disposal, con-
that is likely to be tapped by a particular variety of test, ferring a cuing advantage to those items, relative to
and how this representation may relate to the one likely baseline items. Thus, inhibition may be compensated by
to be subject to inhibition. Failure to nd evidence of the dierential availability of compound cuing. Such
inhibition on a given test may not indicate a lack of compensation would lead to an inaccurate measure of
inhibition in general; it may simply reect a mismatch in the amount of inhibition that had initially taken place
the type of representation tapped by the test and that (masking), and perhaps even undo that inhibition for the
aected by inhibition. Nevertheless, although indirect items retrieved by compound cues.
tests may not be diagnostic of inhibition, such experi- To reduce the likelihood of covert cuing contami-
ments do serve to dene the scope of inhibitory eects nating recall performance in studies using the indepen-
induced by episodic retrieval practice, and the nature of dent probe method, several strategies appear eective.
the representations aected. First, subjects are less likely to use covert cuing when the
Masking through covert cuing eects. As described extralist cues are, in general, strongly related to the
earlier, inhibition tends to generalize to novel test cues target item; if most cues are poorly related, subjects may
that are unrelated to the items receiving retrieval prac- look for additional information to supplement their re-
tice or to the practiced cues themselvesa property call. Second, providing an item specic cue such as a
known as cue-independence. However, whether cue-in- letter stem focuses subjects on recalling a particular
dependent forgetting will be observed may depend on item. Third, limiting the amount of time that subjects
whether subjects use covert cuing strategies to augment have to recall the critical item discourages the use of
their recall on the nal memory test. Consider, for ex- complex search strategies such as covert cuing. Fourth,
ample, a study by Anderson et al. (2000). In this study, using a large number of studied categories makes it
subjects studied items such as Red-Blood and Red-To- unlikely that subjects will be able to recall the relevant
mato, and later did retrieval practice on Red-Blood. On studied category, even if they try. Finally, administering
a delayed recall test, subjects were cued to recall Tomato post-experimental questionnaires to obtain subjective
with an extra-list category label and a letter stem (e.g., reports of covert cuing can help to assess whether the
Food-T___) to see whether or not any inhibition that foregoing strategies were eective. In using the inde-
was induced by retrieval practice would generalize to the pendent probe method to establish the theoretical
novel extralist test cue (see Anderson & Green, 2001; property of cue independence, it is vital to consider how
Johnson & Anderson, in press; Levy & Anderson, 2002; such strategies may aect performance.
for other studies using extralist cuing). As predicted, Special issues in recognition testing. Initially, we be-
signicant inhibition was found, suggesting cue- lieved that retrieval-induced forgetting would not occur
independent impairment. However, when asked, on a on recognition memory tests (Anderson & Bjork, 1994).
post-experimental questionnaire, whether they tried to This expectation was based on analogies to other in-
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 435

hibitory phenomena such as directed forgetting and reported in two recent experiments by Hicks and Starns
retroactive interference, which exhibit little impairment (in press) that used an item recognition test. (see Dop-
on recognition tests, and on the idea that presentation of kins & Ngo, 2002, for a potentially related inhibition
the item itself would release it from its inhibited state eect induced by incidental retrieval of an earlier pre-
(Anderson & Bjork, 1994). This perspective has proven sentation of an item during its repetition). Radvansky
to be mistaken. Signicant retrieval induced forgetting (1999) also found evidence for inhibition on a speeded
has been found on recognition memory measures, both recognition memory test using the fan eect procedure.
in the retrieval practice paradigm and in closely related In addition to generalizing these eects beyond cate-
procedures. gorical materials, Radvanskys study demonstrated that
The rst demonstration of retrieval-induced forget- impairment is cue-independent, as predicted by the in-
ting on a recognition test using the retrieval practice hibition view.
paradigm was reported by Anderson, De Kok, and Given the evidence for impairment on recognition
Child (1997). Subjects participated in the standard re- tests, the question arises as to why such eects would
trieval practice procedure except that after the 20 min occur for retrieval-induced forgetting and not other
delay, subjects were given a yes/no recognition memory phenomena such as directed forgetting and retroactive
test for all of the exemplars they had studied instead of interference. Although it is possible that retrieval-
cued recall. In Experiment 1, subjects were tested with induced forgetting may be produced by qualitatively
categoryexemplar pairs, one at a time, and exemplars dierent mechanisms, other explanations exist. One
of a given category were tested in blocks of 12 (six tar- possibility is that recognition tests might be most sen-
gets and six highly similar distractors intermixed). As sitive to retrieval-induced forgetting when the recogni-
can be seen in Fig. 6, signicant retrieval-induced for- tion judgments require active recollection rather than a
getting was observed, regardless of whether the un- mere assessment of familiarity. In the Anderson et al.
practiced competitors were tested before practiced items study just discussed, subjects were asked to claim that
in their category (tested 1st) or after them (tested 2nd). they recognized an item only if they were very condent
Subsequent experiments provided evidence that this that it had occurred in the earlier study phase. These
impairment also occurred when exemplars were pre- instructions should have encouraged a greater weight on
sented without their category labels, and in randomized recollective processes. If so, perhaps directed forgetting
tests instead of tests using category blocks. Thus, re- might also be found on recognition tests if tests required
trieval-induced forgetting can be observed even when active recollection. Consistent with this idea, directed
subjects are tested with the inhibited item presented in- forgetting does cause impairment on recognition tests
tact, and do not have to generate the item from in- requiring subjects to make source memory judgments
complete cues. Anderson et al. (1997) also observed (e.g., Geiselman, Bjork, & Fishman, 1983). Thus, al-
within-category output interference on their recognition though simple yes/no recognition tasks appear to be
tests (to see this, compare tested rst to tested second in insensitive to directed forgetting, judgments that require
Fig. 6 for each condition), consistent with other studies active retrieval of a particular episodic trace show the
that have reported output interference on recognition eect, as in recall paradigms. Although this account
tests (Smith, 1971). Similar inhibition eects have been reconciles the patterns of inhibitory eects on recogni-
tion tests across the two paradigms, it leaves unex-
plained why familiarity-based judgments might fail to
exhibit inhibition.
Another diculty that may arise is the potential for
the distractor items on a recognition test to be sup-
pressed. For example, practicing Fruit Orange may
suppress not only other studied items such as Fruit
Banana, but also nonstudied items such as Fruit
Strawberry. Because nonstudied exemplars are the very
items that would be most useful to employ as distrac-
tors, both targets and distractor items may be impaired.
Such eects ought to make it dicult to use signal de-
tection methodology to measure inhibition. Consider the
Fig. 6. Retrieval-induced forgetting in recognition memory
idealized familiarity distributions in Figs. 7AC. Fig. 7A
(Anderson et al., 1997). On a nal categoryexemplar pair
represents the situation before retrieval practice has ta-
recognition test, subjects were impaired in their ability to rec-
ognize unpracticed competitors, as measured by corrected rec- ken place and shows familiarity distributions for base-
ognition (hits-false alarms). This eect occurred regardless of line items and their distractors. Baseline items are
whether unpracticed competitors were tested in the rst half of assumed to be more familiar than distractors due to
their respective categories, or in the second half. their recent presentation on the study list, and so the
436 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

Fig. 7. An illustration of why it may sometimes be dicult to detect inhibition on recognition memory tests, in terms of signal de-
tection theory. Each gure represents a continuum of familiarity values for items stored in memory, with distributions for studied
targets and non-studied distractor items presented on the recognition test. (A) Before retrieval practice, all studied items are presumed
to be more familiar to subjects than are distractors. (B) Familiarity distributions for unpracticed competitors and their distractors, after
retrieval practice has been performed, according to Model A. In Model A, retrieval practice is presumed to selectively suppress the
unpracticed competitors and NOT their corresponding, highly similar distractors in semantic memory. This leads target items to be less
familiar, shifting the overall familiarity distribution for those items to the left, closer to the distribution for distractors, reducing d0 . (C)
The same familiarity distributions as plotted in (B), but plotted according to Model B. In Model B, retrieval practice is presumed to
suppress both the unpracticed competitors and the highly similar distractors. This leads both target and distractor items to be less
familiar, shifting the distributions for both to the left, leaving d0 unaected by suppression. Thus, if retrieval practice suppresses both
unpracticed competitors and their distractors, impairment may not be observed on a recognition memory test, because d0 will remain
constant for baseline items and unpracticed competitors.

baseline distribution is shifted to the right. Figs. 7B and Marsh, 1998). Thus, inhibition may be dicult to detect
C represent the situation after retrieval practice, ac- on recognition tests not because inhibition has been re-
cording to the view that retrieval practice: (1) suppresses leased or does not aect familiarity, but because the
only other episodically studied competitors and not di- nature of the test requires the use of foils that are
stractors, or (2) suppresses both episodic and semanti- themselves suppressed. Here again, the way in which the
cally related competitors that serve as distractors. If test is administered yields an inaccurate measure of how
inhibition is restricted to episodically related competi- much inhibition truly took place, masking those eects.
tors (7B), impairment should be measurable using d0 There is good reason to suspect that inhibitory pro-
because retrieval practice selectively shifts the distribu- cesses recruited during episodic retrieval suppress com-
tion for unpracticed competitors, but not those of their peting items in semantic memory. First, retrieval
distractors (note the leftward shift of the target distri- induced forgetting is a general phenomenon that occurs
bution in Figs. 7B and C). No such shift occurs for on both semantic and episodic retrieval tests (e.g.,
baseline categories (Fig. 7A), so a dierence in d0 should Blaxton & Neely, 1983; Johnson & Anderson, in press),
emerge. However, if inhibition also aects unstudied showing that semantic representations are susceptible to
semantically related competitors, both distributions will inhibition. Second, inhibition eects have been previ-
be shifted (Fig. 7C). Because d0 only provides a measure ously shown to span episodic and semantic memory.
of the relative discriminability of targets and distractors, Retrieving an exemplar of a category from semantic
inhibition may be quite dicult to measure, relative to memory can suppress episodic memory for other ex-
baseline categories that have not shifted (see Samuel, emplars that were studied previously (B auml, 2002). If
1996, for an analogous signal detection analysis in the semantic retrieval can suppress episodic memory, it
context of speech perception; a similar point was also seems likely that episodic retrieval might also suppress
made in the context of the revelation eect by Hicks & semantically related competitors that are not studied.
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 437

Finally, in a recent study, Kimball and Bjork (2002) Andrew, Zelaya, Williams, & Dumanoir, 2000; Gara-
found that presenting part-set cues during a recall test van, Ross, & Stein, 1999) and monkeys (e.g., Sakagami
not only impaired remaining items that were studied in & Niki, 1994). In one version of the Go/No-Go task,
that set (as expected), but also reduced the intrusion rate letters are presented one at a time and subjects must
for critical nonstudied semantic items that tend to be press a button as quickly as possible whenever they see a
mistakenly recalled with those same materials. Taken letter, except when the letter is an X. When they see an
together, these results suggest that the sensitivity of X, they are supposed to avoid pressing the button. The
recognition tests to inhibitory eects may be masked by majority of trials are designed to require a button press,
suppression of related semantic distractors. so that when an X occurs, subjects have diculty
withholding their motor response. The ability to with-
Summary hold the response is taken as a measure of inhibitory
The foregoing review highlights the core properties of control.
retrieval-induced forgetting and some of its boundary To explore whether people can stop retrieval, An-
conditions. Taken together, these properties argue for a derson and Green (2001) adapted the go/no-go task to
strong parallel between selective retrieval and the more create the think/ no-think paradigm. In this procedure,
general situation of response override. In particular, the subjects studied pairs of weakly related words (e.g.,
need to selectively retrieve a target item in the face of agsword, ordealroach) and were then trained to
interference from one or more prepotent memories leads provide the second word (e.g., roach; hereinafter re-
to the suppression of those memories, and this sup- ferred to as the response word) whenever they were gi-
pression underlies later forgetting of those items. Al- ven the rst word as a cue (e.g., ordeal). Subjects then
though inhibitory eects are sometimes moderated or entered the think/no-think phase, which required them
masked by representational or testing factors, the basic to exert executive control over the retrieval process. For
nding is quite general and likely to underlie many cases most of the trials in this phase, the task was the same as
of forgetting associated with interference. The experi- it had been during trainingto recall and say aloud the
ence of forgetting is more likely to be caused by inhib- corresponding word as quickly as possible at the sight of
itory control processes that help to focus retrieval than its retrieval cue. For certain cues, however, subjects were
by the strengthening of competing associations in admonished to avoid thinking of the response word. It
memory. was emphasized that it was not enough to avoid saying
the response wordit was crucial on those trials to
Stopping retrieval through inhibitory control prevent the associated memory from entering conscious
awareness at all. Thus, subjects had to override not only
In the preceding review, we discussed evidence for a vocal motor response, but also the cognitive act of
inhibitory processes in selective retrieval situations, retrieval. Could subjects recruit inhibitory control
which we argued are likely to require response override. mechanisms to stop the memory from entering con-
However, response override is involved in other situa- sciousness?
tions as well, such as when we need to stop a response Of course, Anderson and Green could not directly
from occurring at all. In memory retrieval, this ability measure whether subjects stopped the memory from
could prove useful to prevent a particular memory from entering consciousness, but if inhibitory mechanisms
coming into consciousness. Indeed, we sometimes con- were recruited, later recall of the excluded memory
front reminders of things that we would prefer not to should be impaired. To examine this, immediately after
think about: the sight of a car may remind us of an the think/no-think phase, subjects were given the cues
accident we had, or of a former signicant other who for all of the pairs, but they were now asked to recall the
drove that type of car; or the sight of the world trade response for each of them. As expected, forgetting oc-
center in an old movie may lead us to terminate the curred: response words that subjects tried to keep out of
natural progression from cues to memories. Other times, awareness were impaired compared to baseline pairs
we may wish to focus on a particular thought or idea they had studied initially but had not seen during the
without letting the mind wander. Can inhibitory control think/ no-think phase. The more often subjects tried to
mechanisms be engaged to serve these goals? Can inhi- stop retrieval, the worse recall for the excluded memory
bition halt the retrieval process? If so, how? Anderson became (see Fig. 8A). Interestingly, avoided words were
and Green (2001) recently looked at this issue by ex- harder to recall even though subjects had encountered as
amining how stopping retrieval aected the memories many as 16 reminders (i.e., cues) during the think/
that were to be retrieved. To study this, they developed a no-think phase. Under normal circumstances, remind-
new procedure modeled after the widely used Go/No-Go ers would be expected to facilitate the reminded mem-
task, which has been used to measure the ability to stop ory, much as it did for the items to which subjects
a prepotent motor response and to study its neural basis continued to respond (Fig. 8A). Anderson and Green
in both humans (e.g., Casey et al., 1997; de Zubicaray, (2001) further established that this impairment was cue
438 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

independent, echoing the results of Anderson and


Spellman (1995): forgetting occurred regardless of whe-
ther subjects were tested with the originally studied cue
word (e.g., ordeal) or with a novel independent cue
never studied in the experiment (e.g., insect r____ for
roach; Fig. 8B). This cue-independence argues that the
forgetting is not caused solely by associative interfer-
ence; rather, impairment reects active suppression of
the excluded memory itself.
Anderson and Green (2001) ruled out the possibility
that subjects might have deliberately withheld answers
on the nal test due to confusion or to expectations
about the purpose of the experiment. In one experiment,
subjects were told that they would be paid for all correct
answers and were urged to respond to every cue, even if
they were guessing. Another group was misled to believe
that the experimenters expected that their memory
would be better for words they had avoided thinking
about. Both manipulations left the inhibition pattern
unchanged (see Figs. 8CD for recall performance in the
original cue and independent cue conditions respectively
for the monetary incentives experiment; see Figs. 8E and
F, for the same conditions for performance by misled
subjects), demonstrating that subjects were neither
confused nor purposefully withholding responses. In a
nal experiment, subjects were merely asked to avoid
saying the response out loud and all mention of pre-
venting it from entering awareness was eliminated. No
inhibition was observed (Fig. 8G), indicating that the
recall decits in the preceding experiments were not
merely due to suppression of the vocal response for
avoided words. These results isolate forgetting in the
think/no-think paradigm to processes directed at keep-
ing the unwanted declarative memory out of awareness
and demonstrate that this cognitive act has persisting
consequences for the avoided memories.
The impaired memory observed by Anderson and
Green (2001) suggests that inhibitory control mecha-
nisms may be recruited when we seek to regulate
Fig. 8. Final recall performance in four experiments reported
awareness of unpleasant or intrusive memories. In par-
by Anderson and Green (2001) using the think/no-think pro-
cedure. Each plot represents the percentage of items that sub-
ticular, whenever the environment is such that it presents
jects recalled on the nal recall test as a function of the number unavoidable reminders to something that we would
of times that they suppressed the item (suppression condition), prefer not to think about, people may resort to con-
or tried to recall it (respond). The left panel in each row rep- trolling their memories instead. The end result may be
resents nal recall performance when tested with the originally impaired memory for the things that people avoid
trained retrieval cue (i.e., the Same probe), whereas the right thinking about. This suggests that the think/no-think
panel in each row represents nal recall performance when paradigm of Anderson and Green (2001) may provide a
tested with a novel, independent, extralist category cue. (A and useful laboratory model of the voluntary form of re-
B) depicts performance in Experiment 1; (C and D) depicts pression (suppression) proposed by Freud (Freud,
performance in an experiment oering monetary incentives,
1966). If so, results from this paradigm and other related
and encouraging guessing on the nal test; (E and F) depicts
performance when subjects were misled regarding the expected
paradigms such as the directed forgetting procedure may
outcome of the study just before the test; (G) depicts nal have implications for understanding clinical phenome-
test performance when subjects are simply asked, during the non relating to motivated forgetting (Anderson, 2001;
think/no-think phase to simply not say the response word Anderson & Green, 2001; Bjork, Bjork, & Anderson,
(withhold) instead of to not think about it; nal memory is not 1998; Conway, Harries, Noyes, Racsmany, & Frankish,
impaired. 2000; Deprince & Freyd, 2001; Myers, Brewin, & Power,
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 439

1998; see Golding & MacCleod, 1998 for a review of to the executive control approach, the probability of
directed forgetting). recalling a target item does not automatically decrease
as a consequence of adding new associations, or as a
consequence of strengthening a competing association.
Relationship to classical interference theories of forgetting Structural changes may impair the later recall of a target
item if they increase the chances that nontarget items
Although the executive control view is a relatively will occasionally be retrieved before the critical target.
new approach to interference, many of its aspects re- To the extent that competitors are retrieved earlier, the
semble components of classical interference theory. In target will be suppressed at output. The probability that
this section, I discuss some of the specic relations this suppression will impair target performance should
between this view and four mechanisms discussed in go up with the number of competitors because this will
classical interference theory: response competition, un- lead more competitors to be recalled before the target,
learning, reciprocal inhibition, and response-set sup- on average. By this view then, strengthening a compet-
pression. The executive control approach validates many itor should not impair target recall provided that the
of the intuitions behind these classical proposals, while target can be ensured to be tested before the competitor,
at the same time questioning the historical emphasis that a nding that has been observed many times now in
has been placed on associative learning as a source of experiments evaluating the hypothesis of strength-de-
forgetting. pendent forgetting. Thus, it is not the addition of new
associations, nor their strengthening that impairs mem-
McGeochs response competition theory ory, but rather the increased likelihood of suppression
correlated with those structural changes.
According to McGeochs classical response compe- There are several circumstances, however, in which
tition theory, attaching more than one response to a response competition might impair memory. First,
retrieval cue leads those responses to compete with one whenever a cue is presented that is associated to a
another when the cue is presented later on. The more stronger and a weaker response and the subject is told to
competing responses, or the stronger a competing re- only report the rst thing that comes to mind, response
sponse becomes, the more dicult it should be to recall a competition might underlie interference eects. Natu-
given item. McGeochs emphasis on the importance of rally, if the subject is to report the rst thing that comes
sharing a retrieval cue as a condition of interference was to mind, the stronger response will typically prevail over
inherited from M uller and Pilzecker (1900), and con- the weaker one, causing the omission of the latter. This
tinues today in the form of relative strength or ratio-rule will lend the appearance of inaccessibility of the weaker
models of retrieval (e.g., Anderson, 1983; Mensink & response when it may not be inaccessible at all. Second,
Raajimakers, 1988). In essence, these theories posit that when the subject is given a very short time to make
the addition of new structure into memory leads to the memory responses to a cue, interference may be pro-
occlusion or blocking of a target event. duced by blocking. Here again, stronger responses will
Many of the basic assumptions of McGeochs re- leap to mind most readily and potentially use up all the
sponse competition theory are accepted in the current time that the subject has to express their knowledge of
executive control approach. For instance, the presenta- the associated memories. Even if all responses are of
tion of a retrieval cue is presumed to activate all as- equal strength, the addition of new responses might in-
sociated responses according to their strengths of crease the chances that some nontarget item will be re-
association to the cue and these responses are thought ported to the exclusion of a target in a limited time
to compete with one another for access to conscious window. In both of these cases, interference eects may
awareness. It is this retrieval competition that precipi- reect some combination of suppression arising from the
tates the need for executive control. According to the prior output of nontarget items and blocking produced
executive control approach, however, this competition is by insucient time to express available knowledge. In
usually not enough by itself to impair memory recall for fact, much of the early work on interference theory up
a target because inhibitory processes may be deployed to until the late 1950s employed the modied free-recall test
overcome the competition. Furthermore, the empirical (i.e., the MFR test), which required that the subject
relationship between the number of competing responses provide only a single response in a limited time window.
and the probability of recalling a target item is also ac- With the advent of the modied-modied-free recall test
cepted by the theory, along with the notion that (MMFR), subjects were asked to recall all available re-
strengthening a competing response is empirically asso- sponses and were given a longer period to recall them
ciated with a decrement in recall for a target. (Barnes & Underwood, 1959), a procedure that was
Where the executive control approach advanced here thought to provide a better test of the true availability of
diers from McGeochs theory is in the underlying responses in memory. Third, when the measure of in-
mechanism that produces these relationships. According terference is reaction time, the presence of multiple
440 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

competitors or a single strong competitor should slow paired: whereas unlearning posits a decrement in the
the recall of a target; again, this retrieval interference is associative bond linking a cue to a target, the executive
thought to be an essential step in triggering inhibitory control approach attributes impairment to a suppression
control. Finally, special populations with decits in ex- of the target itself. Thus, the current approach predicts
ecutive function (e.g., older adults, children, frontal-lobe cue-independent impairment, whereas unlearning does
damaged patients) may be suciently challenged in in- not. The existence of cue-independent impairment of
hibiting competitors so that retrieval competition cannot course does not rule out the possibility that associative
be eectively managed. Under these circumstances, unlearning might also occur and contribute to the im-
competitors may block the retrieval of a target and cause pairment observed in both retrieval-induced forgetting
impairment that does not reect inhibitory processes. and classical interference paradigms.

Melton and Irwins unlearning theory Osgoods reciprocal inhibition hypothesis

According to the unlearning hypothesis, interference An often overlooked theory is Osgoods reciprocal
eects are caused in part by the unlearning of associative inhibition approach to interference. According to this
connections linking a retrieval cue to a response. Spe- theory, strengthening the association between a stimulus
cically, when a person is trying to recall a newly learned and a response also strengthens an inhibitory associa-
response (e.g., the new phone number for a friend), pre- tion between the stimulus and semantically antagonistic
viously learned responses to that same cue (e.g., the old responses that are attached to it (Osgood, 1946, 1948).
phone number for that friend) may sometimes be elicited For instance, if subjects learn the pair Tree-Elated, a
accidentally. Elicitation could take the form of an overt positive association is formed between the two words,
or covert intrusion of the unwanted item. To the extent but an inhibitory one is also established between Tree
that the older response is incorrect, it was thought to go and the antagonistic response Dejected. In essence,
unreinforced, and therefore suer extinction eects subjects not only learn to make the correct response, but
analogous to those exhibited by animals in conditioning also to NOT make the opposite responsea notion
experiments. Associative unlearning was a critical com- borrowed from Hulls behavioral theory (Hull, 1943).
ponent of Melton and Irwins classical two-factor theory Both the excitatory and inhibitory associations were
of interference (Melton & Irwin, 1940), which also in- thought to generalize semantically, so that intermediate
corporated response competition. The modern descen- responses such as Low, would also suer inhibition, by
dants of this view include the many connectionist virtue of its similarity to Dejected. Osgood provided
learning systems that might attribute forgetting in part to some support for this theory, showing gradually in-
the alteration of weights between representational units. creasing retroactive interference across similar, neutral,
The current approach shares much with the un- and antagonistic responses to stimuli, as a result of in-
learning hypothesis: it focuses on the intrusion of terpolated associative learning.
unwanted memory responses during retrieval as a con- Osgoods theory is perhaps the rst theory of retro-
dition leading to the forgetting of the intruding items; it active interference that attributed impairment to an
posits a process that responds to intrusions in such a inhibitory mechanism. In Osgoods framework, impair-
way as to render them less likely in the futurechanging ment was thought to be a direct result of inhibiting the
some aspect of the intrusions representation. Thus, a potentially intrusive response, and so the theory can
special forgetting process is proposed. It diers, how- explain cue independent impairment. Here again, the
ever, both in its theoretical orientation, and in the nature hypothesis bears some resemblance to the current exec-
of the forgetting mechanism. The unlearning idea was a utive control theory. However, Osgoods assertion that
theoretical analogy inspired by the behaviorist learning inhibition is a direct function of semantic antagonism
approach. Simple, automatic processes were proposed: between two responses is not a feature of the current
learning was the positive adjustment of associations, approach, nor is there any commitment to the devel-
forgetting, the negative adjustment. The executive con- opment of an inhibitory association between a stimulus
trol approach, however, is concerned with the moment- and an unwanted response, as Osgood proposed. In the
by-moment control of behavior with respect to exible current perspective, if a cue activates a memory that is
goals. It assumes mechanisms by which mental repre- unwantedeither because it interferes with a retrieval
sentations are adjusted dynamically in contexts in which attempt, or because it is distracting or unpleasantin-
their ongoing accessibility might disrupt our aims. The hibitory control mechanisms can be recruited to sup-
mechanisms that achieve this adjustment are not press the item. Consistent attempts to suppress a
thought of as general learning processes, but as pro- memory may or may not result in the formation of an
cesses that control the operational state of a system. inhibitory habit for a given item, as Osgood proposes,
These dierent orientations lead to dierent concep- but this possibility is beyond the scope of the present
tualizations of how intruding memories become im- theory.
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 441

Postmans response-set suppression hypothesis view drew a tight connection between the need to facil-
itate a new response set in order to suppress a preceding
Near the end of the classical interference era, Postman set. The current approach entertains the idea that sup-
and colleagues (Postman et al., 1968) proposed a theory pression can be directly applied to an unwanted memory
of interference that departed substantially from ap- without the need to facilitate a competing response or
proaches previously proposed. As highlighted in the response set. Work with the think/no-think paradigm,
preceding sections, most classical accounts of interference for example, suggests that suppression is directly applied
were embedded within larger scale theories of associative to distracting memories. However, further work needs
learning that had their conceptual roots in behaviorist to be done to determine whether such direct suppression
learning theory. Forgetting was assumed to reect the is truly possible. Finally, Postmans theory made a
eects of competition between alternate responses, or the variety of specic assumptions intended to explain the
degradation of associations by general learning mecha- conditions under which spontaneous recovery from
nisms. However, Postman proposed mechanisms that retroactive interference should occur. Although these
went well beyond the somewhat limited conceptual assumptions may be correct, they are not an intrinsic
arsenal of most learning frameworks. According to his part of the current theory, as it is presently specied.
response-set suppression hypothesis, retroactive inter- Despite these dierences, the present theory might be
ference was caused by the active suppression of response regarded as a modern cousin to response-set suppression
members from the initial list. Suppression was thought to that decouples it from the particular paradigm within
occur during the acquisition of the second list of pairs by which the theory was developed. The response-set sup-
what Postman referred to as a selector mechanism. The pression view has been overlooked as an approach to in-
function of the selector mechanism was to both enhance terference in part because the theory was developed
the representations of responses that were intended to be towards the end of the classical interference era, when the
part of the current response set and to suppress outdated eld became captivated by cognitive theory. The shift
response sets. The suppression process helped to reduce away from interference research led to the abandonment
proactive interference caused by the initial list, and to of the theory, and of research on interference generally.
eectively shift into a response set more appropriate Ironically, to the extent that interference was discussed
to the current task. after the cognitive revolution, theories became far more
The response-set suppression hypothesis can be seen associationistic than Postmansmore in the vein of
an early example of the executive control approach. Like McGeochs response competition theory (e.g., Anderson,
the executive control theory, this hypothesis attributed 1983; Mensink & Raajimakers, 1988; Rundus, 1973). The
forgetting to a mechanism that directly suppressed the developing interest in executive control functions in the
response representations of items from the rst list of last 15 years, and work on inhibitory processes has made it
pairs. This mechanism was clearly linked to response possible to view Postmans theory in a dierent light.
override: it helped the organism to select current,
more contextually appropriate response sets in the face
of interference from preceding response sets. Thus, this Concluding remarks
hypothesis acknowledged the need to control memory in
accordance with current goals, and advocated a special Research on interference has occupied a central role
process to achieve that control. Nevertheless, the current in the science of memory since the beginnings of ex-
hypothesis diers from Postmans theory in several re- perimental psychology. Throughout much of this long
spects. First, according to the response-set suppression history, theoretical discussions of interference have been
view, the selector mechanism was thought to act on dominated by ideas that were either directly borrowed
entire response repertoires and not at the level of in- from, or inspired by classical associative learning theo-
dividual responses. So, if a subject learned a list of ten ries. In many ways, this conceptual inuence pervades
pairs, followed by a second list of ten pairs, all responses thinking about interference even today not only in how
from the initial list would be suppressed, irrespective of this phenomenon is described in modern textbooks, but
whether or not the stimulus member for a given rst-list also in how it is explained within current theoretical
item was also used in the second list. The set of rst list frameworks. In modern textbooks, retroactive interfer-
responses was suppressed as a whole, and the set of ence is often dened, for example, as the forgetting that
second list responses, facilitated. The current approach arises as a result of new learning, and proactive inter-
is more exible, permitting for suppression of specic ference, as the forgetting that arises as a result of pre-
competing responses. Accordingly, it should be (and is) vious learning. If a theory is described at all, it is often
possible to suppress only select items from a list, based the classical two-factor theory of Melton and Irwin
on how much interference they cause during retrieval of (1940). Despite many dierences in terminology and
second list items, as is evident in studies of retrieval-in- constructs particular to cognitive psychology, current
duced forgetting. Second, the response-set suppression theoretical accounts of interference have essentially
442 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

returned to McGeochs associative interference theory. the present author and B.A. Spellman. Preparation of
There are excellent reasons for the continuing inuence this paper was supported by a grant from the National
of these classical ideas about learning: interference ef- Institute of Mental Health.
fects are highly correlated with the storage of new traces
into memory, and with the modication of existing ones.
The act of learning a new list does impair memory for a References
previous one, and strengthening a competing association
is often associated with impaired recall of related traces. Allen, G. A., Mahler, W. A., & Estes, W. K. (1969). Eects of
These empirical relationships lend force to the idea that recall tests on long-term retention of paired associates.
forgetting ultimately derives from the ever changing Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 463470.
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cam-
contents of memory, and our inability to cope with the
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
competition created by those changes.
Anderson, J. R., & Reder, L. M. (1999). The fan eect: New
In this article, I have argued that despite these em- results and new theories. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
pirical relationships, we should rethink our view of how ogy: General, 128, 186197.
interference leads to forgetting. I have argued that a Anderson, M. C. (1989). Similarity-based retrieval inhibition and
theory of interference should be framed in the larger mnemonic techniques. Unpublished Masters Thesis, Univer-
context of how organisms control their own thoughts sity of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles.
and actions. Memory retrieval is just a special case of a Anderson, M. C. (2001). Active forgetting: Evidence for
broad class of situations that recruit executive control functional inhibition as a source of memory failure. Journal
processes, and it is these processesparticularly inhibi- of Aggression, Maltreatment, and Trauma, 4, 185210.
Anderson, M. C., & Bell, T. (2001). Forgetting our facts: The
tionthat cause forgetting. By this view, the empirical
role of inhibitory processes in the loss of propositional
relationship between associative learning and forgetting
knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology, General,
that emerged with M uller and Pilzecker (1900) and that 130, 544570.
drives theorizing today, should not be construed as Anderson, M. C., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (1994).
proof that new learning impairs memory, as posited in Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in
many classical and modern models. New learning sets long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
the stage for the mechanism that actually causes for- Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 10631087.
getting: inhibition. Inhibition is triggered as a direct Anderson, M. C., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2000).
response to the competition caused by related traces and Retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specic
the goal to selectively retrieve a target, orin the case of mechanism. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 522530.
Anderson, M. C., & Bjork, R. A. (1994). Mechanisms of
motivated forgettingin response to the goal to prevent
inhibition in long-term memory: A new taxonomy. In D.
awareness of a distracting memory. The forgetting that
Dagenbach, & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in
results is not a passive side eect of the new learning, but attention, memory and language (pp. 265326). San Diego:
a consequence of the mechanisms that have evolved to Academic Press.
allow organisms to override prepotent responses. These Anderson, M. C., De Kok, D., & Child, C. (1997). Retrieval-
mechanisms are essential to our ability to behave in a induced forgetting on a test of recognition memory
exible, context-appropriate manner. [Abstract]. Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society, 2, 2.
I have argued that this view validates many of the Anderson, M. C., & Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted
insights oered by classical theories, while questioning memories by executive control. Nature, 410, 131134.
the widespread assumption that forgetting is tied in a Anderson, M. C., Green, C., & McCulloch, K. C. (2000).
Similarity and inhibition in long-term memory: Evidence for
direct way to new associative learning. Rather, our ex-
a two-factor model. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
periences of forgettingof past experiences, of our
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 11411159.
friends names, or of ideas with which we were once Anderson, M. C., & McCulloch, K. C. (1999). Integration as a
adept, are seen as costs of the very mechanisms that general boundary condition on retrieval-induced forgetting.
enable us to direct cognition to internal thoughts and to Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
the external environment. Cognition, 25, 608629.
Anderson, M. C., & Neely, J. H. (1996). Interference and
inhibition in memory retrieval. In E. L. Bjork, & R. A.
Acknowledgments Bjork (Eds.), Memory. Handbook of perception and cognition
(2nd ed., pp. 237313). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robert-
I would like to thank Douglas Hintzman, Larry
son, E., Gabrieli, S. W., Glover, G., & Gabrieli, J. D. E.
Jacoby, Ben Levy, Brice Kuhl, Sarah Johnson, and (submitted). Neural systems underlying the suppression of
two anonymous reviewers for comments on an earlier unwanted memories.
version of the manuscript. Portions of this article Anderson, M. C., & Shivde, G. S. (in preparation a). Inhibition
evolved from a planned paper to be entitled How to in episodic memory: Evidence for a retrieval-specic mech-
not nd inhibition in memory without really trying by anism.
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 443

Anderson, M. C., & Shivde, G. S. (in preparation b). Strength is Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1403
not enough: Evidence against a blocking theory of retrieval- 1414.
induced forgetting. Conway, A. R., & Engle, R. W. (1994). Working memory and
Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of retrieval: a resource-dependent inhibition model. Journal of
inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 354373.
model case. Psychological Review, 102, 68100. Conway, M. A., Harries, K., Noyes, J., Racsmany, M., &
Arbuckle, T. Y. (1966). Intratrial interference in immediate Frankish, C. R. (2000). The disruption and dissolution of
memory. Dissertation Abstracts, 26, 5554. directed forgetting: Inhibitory control of memory. Journal
Balota, D. A. (1994). Visual word recognition: The journey of Memory and Language, 43, 409430.
from features to meaning. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. H., & Barnhardt, T. M. (1990).
Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 303358). San Diego, Inhibitory semantic priming of lexical decisions due to
CA, US: Academic Press. failure to retrieve weakly activated codes. Journal of
Barnes, J. M., & Underwood, B. J. (1959). Fate of rst-list Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
associations in transfer theory. Journal of Experimental tion, 16, 328340.
Psychology, 58, 97105. Deprince, A. P., & Freyd, J. J. (2001). Memory and dissociative
B
auml, K. (1996). Revisiting an old issue: Retroactive interfer- tendencies: The roles of attentional context and word
ence as a function of the degree of original and interpolated meaning in a directed forgetting task. Journal of Trauma
learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 380384. and Dissociation, 2(2), 6782.
B
auml, K. (1997). The list-strength eect: Strength-dependent de Zubicaray, G. I., Andrew, C., Zelaya, F. O., Williams, S. C.
competition or suppression. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, R., & Dumanoir, C. (2000). Motor response suppression
4, 260264. and the prepotent tendency to respond: A parametric fMRI
B
auml, K. (1998). Strong items get suppressed, weak items do study. Neuropsychologia, 38, 12801291.
not: The role of item strength in output interference. Dodson, C. S., Johnson, M. K., & Schooler, J. W. (1997). The
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 459463. verbal overshadowing eect: Why descriptions impair face
B
auml, K. (2002). Semantic recall can cause episodic forgetting. recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25, 129139.
Psychological Science, 13, 356360. Dong, T. (1972). Cued partial recall of categorized words. Journal
B
auml, K., & Hartinger, A. (2002). On the role of item similarity of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 654661.
in retrieval-induced forgetting. Memory, 10, 215224. Dopkins, S., & Ngo, C. T. (2002). Inhibition of verbal memory
Barnhardt, T. M., Glisky, E. L., Polster, M. R., & Elam, L. retrieval as a consequence of prior retrieval. Journal of
(1996). Inhibition of associates and activation of syn- Memory and Language., 46, 606621.
onyms in the rare-word paradigm: Further evidence for Dunn, E. W., & Spellman, B. A. (2003). Forgetting by
a center-surround mechanism. Memory & Cognition, 24, remembering: Stereotype inhibition through rehearsal of
6069. alternative aspects of identity. Journal of Experimental
Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A., & Anderson, M. C. (1998). Varieties Social Psychology, 39, 420433.
of goal-directed forgetting. In J. M. Golding, & C. M. Finger, K., & Pezdek, K. (1999). The eect of cognitive
MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary interview on face identication accuracy: Release from
approaches (pp. 103137). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. verbal overshadowing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84,
Bjork, R. A. (1975). Retrieval as a memory modier. In R. 340348.
Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Fisher, R. P., & Craik, F. I. M. (1977). Interaction between
Loyola symposium (pp. 123144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. encoding and retrieval operations in cued recall. Journal of
Bjork, R. A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3,
mechanism in human memory. In H. L. Roediger III, & 701711.
F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and conscious- Freud, S. (1966). In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of
ness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 309330). the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (pp. 117
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 128). Hogarth.
Blaxton, T. A., & Neely, J. H. (1983). Inhibition from Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., & Stein, E. A. (1999). Right
semantically related primes: Evidence of a category-specic hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: An event-
retrieval inhibition. Memory & Cognition, 11, 500510. related functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National
Butler, K. M., Williams, C. C., Zacks, R. T., & Maki, R. H. Academy of Sciences USA, 96, 83018306.
(2001). A limit on retrieval-induced forgetting. Memory & Gardiner, J. M., Craik, F. I., & Bleasdale, F. A. (1973).
Cogntion, 27, 13141319. Retrieval diculty and subsequent recall. Memory &
Carrier, M., & Pashler, H. (1992). The inuence of retrieval on Cognition, 1, 213216.
retention. Memory & Cognition, 20, 633642. Geiselman, R. E., Bjork, R. A., & Fishman, D. L. (1983).
Casey, B. J., Trainor, R. J., Orendi, J. L., Schubert, A. B., Disrupted retrieval in directed forgetting: A link. Journal of
Nystrom, L. E., Giedd, J. N., Castellanos, F. X., Haxby, J. Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 5872.
V., Noll, D. C., Cohen, J. D., Forman, S. D., Dahl, R. E., & Gernsbacher, M. A., & Faust, M. E. (1991). The mechanism of
Rapoport, J. L. (1997). A developmental functional MRI suppression: A component of general comprehension skill.
study of prefrontal activation during performance of a Go- Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
No-Go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 835847. Cognition, 17, 245262.
Ciranni, M. A., & Shimamura, A. P. (1999). Retrieval-induced Golding, J. M., & MacCleod, C. M. (Eds.). (1998). Intentional for-
forgetting in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental getting: Interdisciplinary approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
444 M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445

Hayes-Roth, B. (1977). Evolution of cognitive structures and McDaniel, M. A., Friedman, A., & Bourne, L. E. (1978).
processes. Psychological Review, 84, 260278. Remembering the levels of information in words. Memory &
Hicks, J. L., & Starns, J. (in press). Retrieval-induced forgetting Cognition, 6, 156164.
occurs in tests of item recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & McGeoch, J. A. (1942). The psychology of human learning: An
Review. introduction. New York: Longmans.
Hicks, J. L., & Marsh, R. L. (1998). A decrement-to-familiarity McGeoch, J. A., & Irion, A. L. (1952). The psychology of human
interpretation of the revelation eect from forced-choice learning (2nd ed.). New York: Longmans, Green, & Co.
tests of recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). The inuence of
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1105 retrieval processes in verbal overshadowing. Memory &
1120. Cognition, 29, 176186.
Horton, D. L., & Kjeldergaard, P. M. (1961). An experimental Melcher, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (1996). The misremembrance
analysis of associative factors in mediated generalizations. of wines past: Verbal and perceptual expertise dierentially
Psychological Monographs, 75, 26. mediate verbal overshadowing of taste memory. Journal of
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to Memory and Language, 35, 231245.
behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century. Melton, A. W., & Irwin, J. M. (1940). The inuence of degree of
Hunt, R. R., & Smith, R. E. (1998, November). Retrieval may interpolated learning on retroactive inhibition and the overt
or may not cause forgetting. Paper presented at the annual transfer of specic responses. American Journal of Psychol-
meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Dallas, TX. ogy, 3, 173203.
Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship Mensink, G. J. M., & Raajimakers, J. G. W. (1988). A model of
between autobiographical memory and perceptual learn- interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 95, 434
ing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 455.
306340. Miyamoto, A., & Anderson, M. C. (in preparation). Suppress-
Jenkins, J. J. (1963). Mediated associations: Paradigms and ing awareness: Evidence for a link between the regulation
situations. In C. N. Cofer, & B. S. Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal of consciousness and inhibitory phenomena in memory.
behavior and learning: Problems and processes (pp. 210257). Psychological Science.
New York: McGraw-Hill. Moulin, C. J. A., Perfect, T. J., Conway, M. A., North, A. S.,
Johnson, S., & Anderson, M. C. (in press). The role of Jones, R. W., & James, N. (2002). Retrieval-induced
inhibitory control in forgetting semantic knowledge. Psy- forgetting in Alzheimers disease. Neuropsychologia, 40,
chological Science. 862867.
Kimball, D. R., & Bjork, R. A. (2002). Inuences of intentional Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. P., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of
and unintentional forgetting on false memories. Journal of processing versus t ransfer appropriate processing. Journal
Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 116130. of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519533.
Kjeldergaard, P. M. (1968). Transfer and mediation in verbal Muller, G. E., & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentalle Beitrage
learning. In T. R. Dixon, & D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal zur Lehre com Gedachtnis. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie, 1,
behavior and general behavior theory (pp. 10711075). 1300.
Englewood Clis, NJ: Prentice Hall. Myers, L. B., Brewin, C. R., & Power, M. J. (1998). Repressive
Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., Johnson, M. K., & Galluccio, coping and the directed forgetting of emotional material.
L. (1999). Facilitation and impairment of event memory Journal of Abnormal Psychoogy, 107, 141148.
produced by photograph review. Memory & Cognition, 27, Nader, A., Coles, M. E., Brigidi, B., & Foa, E. B. (2001). The
478493. eect of practice on recall of emotional information in
Levy, B., Reinholz, J., & Anderson, M. C. (in preparation). individuals with generalized social phobia. Journal of
Direct cuing of inhibitory processes in the Think/no-Think Abnormal Psychology, 110, 7682.
procedure. Nickerson, R. S. (1984). Retrieval inhibition from part-set
Levy, B. J., & Anderson, M. C. (2002). Inhibitory processes and cuing: A persisting enigma in memory research. Memory &
the control of memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Science, Cognition, 12, 531552.
6, 299305. Norman, K. A., & OReilly, R. C. (in press). Modeling
Lustig, C., Hasher, L., & Toney, S. T. (2001). Inhibitory control hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition
over the present and the past. European Journal of Cognitive memory: A complementary learning systems approach.
Psychology, 13, 107122. Psychological Review.
MacLeod, M. (2002). Retrieval-induced forgetting in eyewit- Osgood, C. E. (1946). Meaningful similarity and interference in
ness memory: Forgetting as a consequence of remembering. learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 277301.
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 135149. Osgood, C. E. (1948). An investigation into the causes of
MacLeod, M. D., & Macrae, C. N. (2001). Gone but not retroactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
forgotten: The transient nature of retrieval-induced forget- ogy, 38, 132154.
ting. Psychological Science, 12, 148152. Osgood, C. E. (1949). The similarity paradox in human
Macrae, C. N., & MacLeod, M. D. (1999). On recollections learning: A resolution. Psychological Review, 56, 132143.
lost: When practice makes imperfect. Journal of Personality Perfect, T. J., Moulin, C. J. A., Conway, M. A., & Perry, E.
and Social Psychology, 77, 463473. (2002). Assessing the inhibitory account of retrieval-induced
Macrae, C. N., & Roseveare, T. A. (2002). I was always on my forgetting with implicit-memory tests. Journal of Experi-
mind: The self and temporary forgetting. Psychonomic mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28,
Bulletin & Review, 9, 611614. 11111119.
M.C. Anderson / Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 415445 445

Postman, L. (1971). Transfer, interference, and forgetting. In J. and motivation (Vol. 37, pp. 293334). San Diego: Academic
W. Kling, & L. A. Riggs (Eds.), Woodworth and Schlos- Press.
bergs: Experimental psychology (3rd ed., pp. 10191132). Shaw, J. S., Bjork, R. A., & Handal, A. (1995). Retrieval-
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. induced forgetting in an eyewitness-memory paradigm.
Postman, L., Stark, K., & Fraser, J. (1968). Temporal changes Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 249253.
in interference. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Shivde, G., & Anderson, M. C. (2001). The role of inhibition in
Behavior, 7, 672694. meaning selection: Insights from retrieval-induced forget-
Raaijmakers, J. W., & Shirin, R. M. (1981). Search of ting. In D. S. Gorfein (Ed.), On the consequences of meaning
associative memory. Psychological Review, 88, 93134. selection: Perspectives on resolving lexical ambiguity (pp.
Rajaram, S., Srinivas, K., & Travers, S. (2001). The eects of 175190). Washington, DC: American Psychological Asso-
attention on perceptual implicit memory. Memory & Cog- ciation.
nition, 29, 920930. Shimamura, A. P. (1995). Memory and frontal lobe function. In
Radvansky, G. A. (1999). Memory retrieval and suppression: M. S. Gazaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 803
The inhibition of situation models. Journal of Experimental 813). MIT Press.
Psychology: General, 128, 563579. Simpson, G. B., & Kang, H. (1994). Inhibitory processes in
Radvansky, G. A., & Zacks, R. T. (1991). Mental models and the recognition of homograph meanings. In D. Dagen-
the fan eect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, bach, & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention,
Memory, and Cognition, 17, 940953. memory, and language (pp. 359381). San Diego, CA:
Roediger III, H. L. (1973). Inhibition in recall from cueing with Academic Press.
recall targets. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Smith, A. D. (1971). Output interference and organized recall
Behavior, 12, 644657. from long-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Roediger III, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit Verbal Behavior, 10, 400408.
memory in normal human subjects. In F. Boller, & J. Smith, A. D. (1973). Input order and output interference in
Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 8, organized recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100,
pp. 63131). 147150.
Roediger III, H. L., & Neely, J. H. (1982). Retrieval blocks in Smith, A. D., DAgostino, P. R., & Reid, L. S. (1970). Output
episodic and semantic memory. Canadian Journal of Psy- interference in long-term memory. Canadian Journal of
chology, 36, 213242. Psychology, 24, 8589.
Roediger, H. L., & Schmidt, S. R. (1980). Output interference Smith, E. E., Adams, N. E., & Schorr, D. (1978). Fact retrieval
in the recall of categorized and paired associate lists. Journal and the paradox of interference. Cognitive Psychology, 10,
of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 438464.
6, 91105. Smith, R. E., & Hunt, R. R. (2000). The inuence of distinctive
Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. A., & Rieger, G. processing on retrieval-induced forgetting. Memory & Cog-
H. (1992). Direct comparison of word stems and word nition, 28, 503508.
fragments in implicit and explicit retention tests. Journal of Smith, S. M., & Tindell, D. R. (1997). Memory blocks in
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni- word fragment completion caused by involuntary retrieval
tion, 18, 12511269. of orthographically related primes. Journal of Experimen-
Roskies, A. L., Fiez, J. A., Balota, D. A., Raichle, M. E., & tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23,
Petersen, S. E. (2001). Task-dependent modulation of 355370.
regions in the left inferior frontal cortex during semantic Thompson-Schill, S. (1997). Role of the left inferior prefrontal
processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 829843. cortex in the retrieval of semantic knowledge: A reevalua-
Rundus, D. (1973). Negative eects of using list items as tion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA,
retrieval cues. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal 94, 1479214797.
Behavior, 12, 4350. Tsukimoto, T., & Kawaguchi, J. (2001). Retrieval-induced
Sakagami, M., & Niki, H. (1994). Spatial selectivity of go/no-go forgetting: Is the baseline in the retrieval-practice paradigm
neurons in the monkey prefrontal cortex. Experimental true? Poster session presented at the International Confer-
Brain Research, 100, 165169. ence on Memory, Valencia, Spain.
Samuel, A. (1996). Does lexical information inuence the Tulving, E., & Arbuckle, T. Y. (1963). Sources of intratrial
perceptual restoration of phonemes. Journal of Experimen- interferences in paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal
tal Psychology: General, 125, 2851. Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 321334.
Saunders, J., & MacLeod, M. D. (2002). New evidence on the Tulving, E., & Arbuckle, T. Y. (1966). Input and output
suggestibility of memory: The role of retrieval-induced interference in short-term associative memory. Journal of
forgetting in misinformation eects. Journal of Experimental Experimental Psychology, 72, 145150.
Psychology: Applied, 8, 127142. Wheeler, M. A. (1995). Improvement in recall over time
Schooler, J. W., & Engstler-Schooler, T. Y. (1990). Verbal without repeated testing: Spontaneous recovery revisited.
overshadowing of visual memories: Some things are better Journal of Experimental Psyvchology: Learning, Memory,
left unsaid. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 3671. and Cognition, 21, 173184.
Schooler, J. W., Fiore, S. M., & Brandimonte, M. A. (1997). At Williams, C. C., & Zacks, R. T. (2001). Is retrieval-induced
a loss from words: Verbal overshadowing of perceptual forgetting an inhibitory process. American Journal of
memories. In D. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning Psychology, 114, 329354.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai