Anda di halaman 1dari 55

Running head: EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 1

Influence of White Noise, tDCS, tRNS, Attentiveness and Novelty Seeking on Response

Inhibition and Working Memory

Maria Teresa Vaos Solano

Lund University

Thesis for the Master of Science in Psychology

August 2016

Supervisor

Prof. Sverker Sikstrm


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 2

Abstract

The aim of the present study was to investigate if cognitive performance significantly and

differentially improves after the application of non-invasive brain stimulation compared to sham,

taking into consideration individual differences in attentiveness and novelty seeking. Thirty-two

healthy adolescents undertook four experimental conditions (eight participants per condition) in

which they performed a response inhibition task (Go/No-Go) followed by a visuospatial working

memory (WM) task (Corsi span). The tasks were conducted before and after the delivery of

anodal tDCS, tRNS, auditory white noise (WN) or sham over the left dorsolateral prefrontal

cortex. There was a significant improvement in WM following auditory WN. The results do not

show differential effects of tDCS, tRNS, and sham on response inhibition nor spatial WM. The

study does not support previous findings on individual differences in attentiveness influencing

results. Interestingly, a moderating and significant effect of novelty seeking was found on the

tDCS condition in the spatial WM task. Methodological limitations that may have contributed to

a lack of significant results in both tasks following tDCS and tRNS are discussed. Auditory WN

may have important implications for both healthy and at-risk populations to enhance cognition,

particularly at a crucial age for future development. Novelty seeking assessment may provide

new insights into the underlying mechanisms and differential effects of non-invasive brain

stimulation.

Keywords: cognitive enhancement, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS),

transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS), auditory white noise (WN), response inhibition,

working memory (WM), attentiveness, novelty seeking, adolescents


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 3

Influence of White Noise, tDCS, tRNS, Attentiveness and Novelty Seeking on Response

Inhibition and Working Memory

Psychologists have long used cognitive training and behavioral interventions for cognitive

enhancement. However, contemporary methods are placing more emphasis on direct

interventions to modify brain function, the source from which cognitive capabilities emerge

(Cohen, 2015).

Overall, current studies from different cognitive domains demonstrate the possibility of

improving cognitive functions with non-invasive transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) like

transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS)

as well as auditory white noise (WN). However, study results are inconsistent, and not much is

known about the mechanisms underlying these effects as well as the interaction of interventions

with individual differences and diverse cognitive processes.

Past studies have reported improvement in cognition with auditory WN depending on the low

or high level of attentiveness self-reported by the subjects (Sderlund, Marklund & Lacerda,

2009; Sderlund, Sikstrm, Lotuses & Sonuga-Barke, 2010; Sderlund, Sikstrm & Smart,

2007). Also, research has shown anodal stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPF)

via tDCS enhance working memory (WM), and executive functions (EFs) in a healthy

population (Fregni et al., 2005; Fregni et al., 2006; Ohn et al., 2013; Sparing, Dafotakis, Meister,

Thirugnanasambandam & Fink, 2008). Just as in patients (Boggio et al., 2006; Boggio et al.,

2007; Nitsche, Boggio, Fregni & Pascual-Leone, 2009). However, results are conflicting and

seem to depend on numerous variables. For reviews see Brunoni et al. (2012) and Kadosh

(2013).
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 4

Besides to individual differences, the results also vary according to the type of task, the

assessment, and sample used in the study. For instance, a meta-analysis by Brunoni and

Vanderhasselt (2014), showed anodal tDCS delivered to DLPFC improved reaction time (RT)

but not accuracy in an N-back task, measuring WM. Another meta-analysis found a small benefit

of tDCS on cognition (Hill, Fitzgerald & Hoy, 2016). However, the meta-analysis performed by

Horvath, Forte and Carter (2015), found no benefits of tDCS on WM in a healthy sample.

Moreover, a meta-analysis by Dedoncker, Brunoni, Baeken and Vanderhasselt (2016), revealed

that tDCS had differential effects in healthy participants vs. patients. Healthy participants

obtained higher accuracy on cognitive tasks only with higher current densities with the effect

more pronounced in women. In contrast, neuropsychiatric samples scored greater in accuracy

when anodal tDCS was applied online vs. offline protocol tasks. Finally, tRNS has revealed

similar effects to tDCS on increasing excitability and learning facilitation (Terney, Chaieb,

Moliadze, Antal, & Paulus, 2008) but not in a WM task with a healthy population (Mulquiney,

Hoy, Daskalakis & Fitzgerald., 2011).

In addition to the mixed results mentioned above, shared and distinct neural underlying

mechanisms have been suggested for auditory WN, tDCS, and tRNS. Auditory WN has been

proposed to act through the neuromodulation of the dopamine system explained by the stochastic

resonance (SR) phenomenon (Rausch, Bauch & Bunzeck, 2014; Sikstrm & Sderlund, 2007).

TDCS and tRNS have been suggested to act via a change in voltage-gated sodium (Na) channels

(Chaieb, Antal & Paulus, 2015,) causing a depolarization in the neural membrane potential

(Bikson, Radman & Datta, 2006). Moreover, the same mechanism as in auditory WN, SR, has

been signified by Fertonani, Pirulli, and Miniussi (2011), to be working in tRNS.


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 5

Although many studies have investigated the effect of non-invasive brain stimulation on

cognition, past research has largely overlooked the influence that individual differences,

characteristics of the sample and cognitive tasks have in the effect of the different interventions.

Moreover, no study to date has jointly tested the influence of attentiveness, novelty seeking,

auditory WN, tDCS, tRNS on response inhibition and spatial WM on a healthy adolescent

population, with a sham and control condition.

Before describing the study in detail, a theoretical background will be introduced. The first

part provides a description of the different non-invasive brain stimulation used in the present

study, auditory WN, tDCS, and tRNS, to provide the reader with information about its principles,

evidence of impact on cognitive abilities and mechanisms. The second part substantiates the

rationale for the choice of attentiveness and novelty seeking as individual characteristics that can

influence the results. Finally, the aim and hypotheses of the study will be stated.

White Noise

Principles. Auditory WN is described as a sound that contains every frequency within the

range of human hearing (generally from 20 Hz to 20 kHz) in equal amounts. The effect of noise

has a long tradition in the study of behavior, usually portraying noise as detrimental for efficient

task performance (Broadbent, 1958). This adverse effect is supposed to be due to competition for

attentional resources. However, in non-linear and neural systems, adding an optimal level of WN

has been shown to facilitate information processing through SR (Moss, Ward & Sannita, 2004).

Previous research shows improvement in cognitive performance through stimulation with

auditory WN under particular circumstances and has been suggested to occur through SR

(Sderlund & Sikstrm, 2008). SR is a phenomenon occurring in nonlinear systems whereby

adding random noise results in better detection of a weak signal or the content of the
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 6

information. The proper amount of noise, or threshold, will improve information processing.

However, an increase of noise intensity will result in an impairment to process information

(Moss et al., 2004). McDonnell and Abbott (2009), suggest understanding SR regarding

randomness that makes a nonlinearity less detrimental to a signal (p.1).

Evidence of impact on cognitive abilities. Auditory WN has been shown to affect behavior

in healthy populations. For example, memory retrieval improved in arithmetic tasks with

auditory WN, seen on faster reaction times (RTs) (Usher & Feingold, 2000). Also, auditory WN

reduced crying time and improved vital signs in newborns undergoing painful procedures

(Karako & Trker, 2014). Further experiments showed that WN, but not control sounds,

improved recognition memory in healthy humans, selectively enhancing stimulus-driven phasic

activity in dopaminergic pathways and auditory cortex (Rausch et al., 2014).

The improvement in cognitive and behavioral tasks in subjects with ADHD by auditory WN

has also been reported. For instance, in a case study of a child diagnosed with ADHD, auditory

WN led to improvements on his writing and math assignments (Cook et al., 2015). Also, a study

by Cook, Johnson and Bradley-Johnson (2015) reported three students with ADHD that

displayed lower levels of off-task behavior when listening to WN while doing school tasks.

However, auditory WN did not have any systematic effect on assignment completion or

accuracy. Although auditory WN can benefit both a healthy and clinical population, individual

differences on attentiveness seem to affect results. Thus, auditory WN has shown beneficial

effects in subjects with low attentiveness but has been proven detrimental to persons with high

attentiveness. For example, research by Sderlund et al. (2007) and Sderlund et al. (2010),

reports children with ADHD (i.e. low attention group) stimulated with auditory WN, eliminated

the difference in performance with the control group (children with no attention problems or high
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 7

attention group) in episodic memory. Further experiments also found differential effects in the

type of tasks performed. Helps, Bamford, Sonuga-Barke and Sderlund (2014), assessed a non-

clinical sample with two non-executive functions (EFs) tasks (episodic verbal recall and delayed

verbal recognition) and two EFs tasks (visuospatial WM and Go/No-Go). The teacher rated the

participants as super, regular and sub-attentive children. They found the addition of auditory WN

particularly enhanced performance in tasks requiring EF in sub-attentive children. The results

suggest a qualitative difference between non-EF tasks and EF tasks on the assessment of

attention processes, as well as a discrimination of the effect of the noise depending on the level

of attention of subjects. Moreover, in a study combining tDCS and auditory WN, Sikstrm et al.

(2016), found differential effects in the low and high attentiveness groups. The researchers report

that stimulation with tDCS and auditory WN interacted with performance in the low but not in

the high attentiveness group in a Go/No-Go task.

In conclusion, auditory WN appears to affect positively healthy population with low levels

of attention and subjects with ADHD but seems to be detrimental for subjects with high

attentiveness. Moreover, auditory WN mainly appears to improve the execution of tasks

involving EFs.

Mechanism. The underlying neural mechanisms of SR remain unclear. However, with the

moderate brain arousal (MBA) model, Sikstrm and Sderlund (2007) attempt to explain how

random noise causes different effects on subjects with high and low attention levels. The MBA

model suggests that the amount of noise required for an optimal cognitive performance will

depend on dopamine levels. According to the MBA model, auditory WN facilitates information

processing by compensating low tonic dopamine levels in inattentive subjects or children with

ADHD (Sikstrm & Sderlund, 2007; Sderlund et al., 2010). Neurocomputational modeling of
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 8

the MBA model shows how a neural system with low dopamine levels needs a higher standard of

noise for optimal performance. Hence, the model predicts that auditory WN will be beneficial for

inattentive and ADHD children, with low levels of dopamine, but will provide more stimulation

than necessary for those with average or high attentiveness (Moss et al., 2004). Indeed, in a

recent study using behavioral and fMRI measures, auditory WN slightly improved recognition

memory and enhanced stimulus-driven phasic activity on dopaminergic networks. The results

suggest that the mechanism by which auditory WN causes benefits in cognitive tasks is via the

neuromodulation of the dopamine system (Rausch et al., 2014). Despite this, other studies have

failed to show evidence of a direct connection between the dopamine system and the effects of

WN on cognition (Baijot et al., 2016; Plsson, Sderlund, Klamer & Bergquis, 2011).

General Introduction for tES (Transcranial Electrical Stimulation)

Employing current as a therapeutic technique dates back to antiquity. The use of electric sea

creatures is reported as early as 43 AD to treat a headache, gout, hemorrhoids, and neuralgia. The

invention in 1744 of an electric portable container, made possible the storage and control of the

electrical discharges, making electrotherapy a conventional treatment to heal neuralgia,

contractions, and paralysis. In the middle of the 20th century, interest in electrical brain

stimulation was extended to use in research and psychological disorders (for a review see

Kadosh & Elliott, 2013). However, electric stimulation rapidly fell into oblivion, likely due to an

increase in popularity of pharmacological treatments. It will not be until the publication of the

study by Nitsche and Paulus (2000) that interest in electrical stimulation of the brain will resume

(Coffman, Clark & Parasuraman, 2014). A further reason why this technique, although readily

available for decades, did not gain more attention in human research, is that different parameters

like the position of the electrodes, current intensities, and stimulation duration are critical to
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 9

change cerebral excitability. Therefore, much research had to be conducted before discovering

patterns working consistently (Nitsche & Paulus, 2000).

Nowadays, tES is delivered through two or more electrodes placed on the scalp, above the area

of interest, with a low intensity between 1 to 2mA. After approximately 5 minutes of this weak

current passing through the scalp, it alters neural activity under electrodes and connected

subcortical structures (Nitsche et al., 2003; Weber, Messing, Rao, Detre, & Thompson-Schill,

2014).

Several factors, like electrode size and positioning, in addition to the intensity and duration of

the stimulation, will influence the impact of tES. By varying these parameters, different effects

are induced. Electrode positioning is critical. Even though electrical fields are comparatively

nonfocal, the placement of electrodes is associated with how much current is delivered to the

brain and what targets (Guleyupoglu, Schestatsky, Edwards, Fregni & Bikson, 2013). In tRNS,

positive and negative current fields are oscillatory rather than direct, which ensures the

application is polarity independent (Chaieb et al., 2009). The size of electrodes is also relevant,

bigger ones reducing current density, therefore diffusing the current in a larger area. Eventually,

selecting a montage will depend on the specific results the researcher wants to achieve.

The sensations caused by both real and sham tES rapidly disappear thanks to habituation

(Gandiga, Hummel, & Cohen, 2006). In a sham condition, the same level of current than in a real

condition is delivered but for a shorter time, usually 20 or 30 seconds. Such a brief induction is

not enough to produce neuronal excitability (Fritsch et al., 2010) but it causes the same tingling

in the scalp as an experimental condition.

Studies show considerable variability regarding specific characteristics of the stimulation

protocol (e.g. number of sessions, electrode placement, current intensity, and features) as well as
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 10

the type of cognitive assessment performed. This variation impacts the results and makes it

harder the interpretation of research (Saturnino, Antunes & Thielscher, 2015).

A systematic review by Elmasry, Loo and Martin (2015), reported that tES + cognitive training

enhanced performance on most of the tasks showing improvements in WM, cognitive control,

approximate number sense and mathematical skills.

A limited but growing body of literature indicates that tES is a safe, noninvasive method of

brain stimulation (Fregni, Boggio, Nitsche & Pascual-Leone, 2005). For instance, in a study with

131 healthy subjects undertaking 183 actives and 94 sham tDCS sessions, no serious adverse

effects were found (Kessler, Turkeltaub, Benson & Hamilton, 2012). For more information about

tES see review by Cohen (2015).

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation

Principles. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) consists of the application of a

weak electric current over the scalp through the use of conductive electrodes. In tDCS, one

electrode is used as the anode (positive) and the other one as a cathode (negative). Anodal

stimulation of the motor cortex enhances excitability, while cathodal stimulation diminishes it.

The reason for this is most probably that anodal stimulation leads to neuronal depolarization and

increasing neuronal excitability while cathodal stimulation has the opposite effect, decreasing

cortical excitability through hyperpolarization (Nitsche et al., 2003).

TDCS has already been shown to modulate cortical excitability significantly and enhance

performance in numerous cognitive tasks (Coffman et al., 2014). Although this technique has

been readily available for decades, it did not gain much attention in human research till recently.

This circumstance is due to the different parameters used being critical to change cerebral

excitability
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 11

Evidence of impact on cognitive abilities. Although tDCS has been shown to improve

cognitive performance in both a healthy and clinical population, results are inconsistent.

Contradictory findings might be explained by the different parameters, like timing and placement

of the electrodes used in the studies (Martin, Liu, Alonzo, Green & Loo, 2014), together with the

diverse tasks employed for measurement.

Much research focusing on the enhancement of cognitive performance has targeted attention

processes. Many frontal areas have been shown to work in attentional processes. The right

inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) seems to be especially critical for response inhibition. For example,

Ditye, Jacobson, Walsh and Lavidor (2012) reported that 1,5mA of anodal tDCS during 15

minutes on the rIFG combined with cognitive training improved the ability to inhibit responses

on the stop signal task (SST), a measure of response inhibition. Also, Jacobson, Javitt, and

Lavidor (2011) found 10 minutes of anodal 1mA tDCS applied at the rIFG before subjects

performed the SST, enhanced response inhibition. The study also showed that stimulation of

rIFG, but not stimulation of the right anterior gyrus, improved response inhibition. An interesting

study by Clark et al. (2012) used brain imaging together with tDCS to improve learning of tasks.

In this case, the task was the identification of concealing objects in naturalistic surroundings.

Subjects received 30 min of 2.0 mA tDCS on-line over the rIFG and right parietal cortex. TDCS

significantly improved learning and performance in comparison with a sham condition,

suggesting better selective attention and response inhibition.

Response inhibition performance also improved with anodal stimulation of the left DLPFC in

nonclinical populations. A study by Fecteau et al. (2007) reported a decrease in risk-taking, a

behavior associated with an increased inhibitory control. Similarly, Boggio et al. (2007) reported

an enhanced inhibitory response in a Go/No-Go task compared to sham. Both studies utilized an
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 12

intensity of 2mA. Another study measuring the time the gaze remained fixated on a threat in

highly trait-anxious individuals, reported that participants receiving anodal tDCS on the left

DLPFC showed a significant decrease, suggesting an improvement of attentional control

(Heeren, Baeken, Vanderhasselt, Philippot & de Raedt, 2015).

Working memory performance has also been shown to improve with tDCS. For example,

Gladwin, den Uyl, Fregni and Wiers (2012) found the application of 10 min of 1mA anodal tDCS

on the left DLPFC, improved performance in a Sternberg task measuring WM and selective

attention. Another study by Mulquiney et al., (2011) also found improvement on a 2-back WM

task. However, the improvement was just shown in RTs and not in accuracy. No effect was found

on the Sternberg task.

Moreover, Javadi and Cheng (2013) and Javadi and Walsh (2012), found significant results

with the application of 1.5mA and 1mA, respectively, anodal tDCS delivered for 20 min on the

left DLPFC in a long-term verbal memory task.

For evidence of the implication of other frontal areas in attentional processes see reviews by

Juan and Muggleton (2012) and Clark, Squire, Merrikhi and Noudoost (2015). For evidence of

the clinical efficacy of tDCS in psychiatric disorders, see review by Kekic, Boysen, Campbell

and Schmidt (2016).

Mechanism. The precise mechanisms by which tDCS influences behavior are not known,

however, previous research reports that the electrical disturbance of cortical neurons environment

caused by the delivery of tDCS, can cause the separation of electric charge within dendrites and

cell body, producing a change in membrane potential (Bikson et al., 2006).

Additionally, the after-effects of tDCS seem to depend on NMDA receptors suggesting an

interaction of tDCS with glutamatergic systems, essential for learning processes (Liebetanz,
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 13

Nitsche, Tergau & Paulus, 2002; Bliss & Collingridge, 1993. The ability to modulate occipital

alpha rhythm, which is related to cognition and WM, has also been shown with the application of

tDCS at parietooccipital locations (Zaehle, Rach & Herrmann, 2010) and the left dorsolateral

prefrontal cortex (Stagg et al., 2013).

Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation

Principles. A more recent type of tES is tRNS. Used for the first time by Terney et al.,

(2008), tRNS implies the application of alternate currents with random frequencies on the scalp.

This technique usually entails the generation of samples at a rate between 1 and 640Hz. These

samples have random current amplitudes, commonly distributed normally with a mean of 0mA.

The random electric fluctuation of the samples between positive and negative amplitudes

generate the noise in the stimulated cortical regions (Terney et al., (2008).

TRNS has been shown to boost cortical excitability (Chaieb et al., 2009; Snowball et al.,

2013; Terney et al., 2008). However, the high-frequency spectrum of tRNS (101-640Hz) but not

the low-frequency (0.1-100Hz), seems to be responsible for inducing excitability in M1 (Terney

et al., 2008). The high-frequency spectrum is also identified for the effects in the treatment of

tinnitus (Joos, De Ridder & Vanneste, 2015) and improving the perception of facial identity

(Romanska, Rezlescu, Susilo, Duchaine & Banissy, 2015).

High-frequency tRNS (101-640Hz) has benefits over tDCS because it has a higher cutaneous

perception threshold (Ambrus, Paulus, y Antal, 2010), making it easier to keep experimental

blinds. Moreover, tRNS guarantee an application independent of polarity because of its

oscillatory electric wave (Chaieb et al., 2009).

Evidence of impact on cognitive abilities. The stimulation of the primary motor cortex (M1)

during 10 minutes with tRNS resulted in a cortical excitability increase of 20-50% using the
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 14

paired-pulse paradigm (Terney et al. 2008). Moreover, Terney et al. observed an enhancement in

the acquisition and consolidation of implicit motor learning skills with a serial reaction time task

(SRTT). The tRNS lasting only five minutes can also significantly enhance cortical excitability

(Chaieb, Paulus & Antal, 2011).

Additionally, tRNS produced improvement in other domains, including perceptual learning

(Fertonani et al., 2011), number discrimination (Cappelletti et al., 2013) and mathematical

learning (Snowball et al., 2013). However, unlike tDCS (Fregni et al., 2005), tRNS did not

improve WM in a healthy population (Mulquiney et al., 2011).

The effect produced by tRNS has been shown to facilitate cortical excitability in the operating

area of both electrodes. However, in contrast to the study by Terney et al. (2008), Ho, Taylor and

Loo (2013; 2015) found that tRNS with 1mA DC offset (mean amplitude displacement from

zero) is more likely to increase cortical excitability than tRNS with zero DC offset.

The effects of tRNS, like increasing excitability and learning facilitation (Terney et al.,

2008) resemble those reported by previous studies after anodal tDCS (Nitsche & Paulus, 2000).

However, in comparison with anodal tDCS, high-frequency tRNS (100 to 640Hz) seems to

obtain a higher impact (Fertonani, Ferrari & Miniussi, 2015; Saiote, Polana, Rosenberger,

Paulus & Antal, 2013).

Mechanism. The underlying neural mechanisms of tRNS are not well-known. Fertonani et al.

(2011) suggested SR, the same mechanism that Sderlund and Sikstrm (2008) prompted to

work in auditory WN. The effect of tRNS has been shown to differ depending on the frequency

of the current delivered. For instance, tRNS, at higher frequencies ranges (100Hz-640Hz) but not

lower frequencies ranges (0.1-100Hz), has been associated with plasticity processes (Grenier et

al., 2001) and learning (Ponomarenko et al., 2008). Besides, Terney et al., (2008) postulated that
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 15

the underlying mechanisms producing excitatory after-effects are modulated through the

depolarization of voltage- gated sodium (Na) channels. Indeed, the application of tRNS has been

shown to change voltage-gated Na channels (Chaieb et al., 2015).

An essential difference exists between tDCS and tRNS. TDCS modifies the transmembrane

neuronal potential directly, opening the Na channels just once (Bindman et al., 1964). In contrast,

tRNS does not possess a component of direct current and can induce multiple ionic influxes,

modulating the firing rate of neurons and achieving higher effects (Terney et al., 2008).

Moreover, the study by Chaieb et al., (2015) showed that tRNS after-effects do not depend on

NMDA receptors as tDCS does but on the Na channels.

In sum, tRNS seems to provide with a new qualitative way to alter brain plasticity. Similar to

tDCS, it allows a painless and imperceptible excitability alteration with a noninvasive and

reversible treatment. The main advantage over tDCS is that tRNS is insensitive to polarization.

Individual Differences

The assessment of attentiveness and novelty seeking will provide valuable information on

individual differences and its interaction with response to treatment.

Attentiveness. Attentiveness is a quantitative measure used to gauge the individual level of

attention. Subjects with low attentiveness usually have learning difficulties and attention

problems and might be diagnosed with ADHD (Thomas, Sanders, Doust, Beller, & Glasziou,

2015).

Attentional processes are a necessary foundation for most higher learning and EFs (Posner &

Boies, 1971). Attention deficits are found in clinical populations across the lifespan, being an

essential feature of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and traumatic brain injury
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 16

(TBI) as well as of various forms of dementia like Alzheimer dementia (Rizzo et al., 2000).

ADHD is a developmental disorder with a prevalence of 7, 2% in children and adolescents

(Rae, Sanders, Doust, Beller & Glasziou. 2015). The symptoms include inappropriate levels of

hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention (APA, 2013). Adolescents experiment the same

symptoms as children. However, the consequences can be more severe since they have greater

accountability and are less monitored by adults (Sibley, Kuriyan, Evans, Waxmonsky & Smith,

2014). ADHD is associated with significant deficiencies in areas including social and peer

relationships, academic attainment or emotional and cognitive performance (Tarver, Daley &

Sayal, 2014).

The symptoms of ADHD have been suggested to arise from a deficit in EFs such as response

inhibition and WM (Barkley, 1997; Castellanos & Tannock, 2002; Knudsen 2007; Pennington &

Ozonoff, 1996). A meta-analysis by Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone and Pennington (2005)

indicates a link between ADHD and deficits on measures of response inhibition, vigilance,

spatial WM, and planning. The same shortcomings are shared for other neuropsychiatric

disorders that differ in the neural networks affected, but that might share the same underlying

mechanisms, like a deficit in cortical inhibitory neurotransmission (Demirtas-Tatlidede,

Vahabzadeh-Hagh & Pascual-Leone, 2013) and deficits in the dopaminergic system (Biederman

& Faraone, 2005).

Previous research has shown that attentiveness moderates the effect that auditory WN

(Sderlund et al., 2010; Sderlund et al., 2007) and tDCS (Sikstrm et al., 2016) exert on

cognitive processes.

Novelty seeking. Novelty seeking, a component of Cloningers Psychobiological Model of

Personality (Cloninger, Svrakic & Przybeck, 1993), has been related, like attentiveness, to the
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 17

dopaminergic system. This model is based on findings from neuroanatomy, developmental and

clinical psychology and psychiatry. Differences in the primary brain systems for procedural

learning (automatic regulation of emotional responses) versus propositional learning (individual

differences in values, goals, and self-conscious emotions) result in four temperaments, Novelty

Seeking, Harm Avoidance, Reward Dependence and Persistence, and three character constructs,

Self-directedness, Cooperativeness, and Self-transcendence. The dimensions of human

temperament are the heritable, developmentally stable, non-influenced by sociocultural learning

and emotionally based components of personality, and correlate to specific biological

characteristics of the brain. For a review see Cloninger (2004).

Most relevant for the present study is the novelty seeking element since research has found

that high levels of novelty seeking are linked with a deficit of dopaminergic transmission (Gerra

et al., 2000) and hence attention processes (Badgaiyan, Sinha, Sajjad & Wack, 2015).

Individuals with high scores on novelty seeking are characterized as impulsive, fickle, excitable,

quick-tempered, extravagant and with great exploratory behavior, while those with low scores on

novelty seeking tend to be reflective, rigid, loyal, stoic, slow-tempered and frugal (Cloninger et

al., 1993).

Purpose and Hypotheses

The aim of this study is to examine the impact that auditory WN, tDCS, and tRNS have on

response inhibition and WM in comparison with a sham condition, in an adolescent population.

Also, the current study examines whether the effect of the different stimulation conditions on

cognitive performance is dependent on individuals' self-report attentiveness and novelty seeking

personality scores. To the extent of my knowledge, this is the only study to date investigating

jointly auditory WN, tDCS, tRNS, and sham. Additionally, no study till now included an
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 18

adolescent sample. Furthermore, the assessment of individual differences in novelty seeking as a

possible moderator of the effects of the different stimulation conditions has never been done

before.

To this end, participants will be screened for attentiveness and assigned to a "low" or "high"

attentiveness group. Subjects will be randomly allocated to the different stimulation conditions,

matched on attentiveness, in a Latin square counterbalanced design. All participants will perform

two tests, a Go/No-Go task and a Corsi span task, measuring response inhibition and visuospatial

WM respectively, before and after the stimulation condition. In the tDCS and tRNS conditions,

1.5mA will be delivered, placing the anode over the left DLPFC and the cathode on the

contralateral arm. In the auditory WN condition, 77dB will be broadcasted through stereo

headphones. Finally, the sham condition will be achieved with the tDCS sham set-up that will be

explained later in full detail. An ANCOVA will be performed to compare scores across

stimulation conditions controlling for pre-intervention scores in response inhibition (accuracy

and RT) and WM span. Additionally, a series of two-way ANOVA's will be performed to

examine interaction effects of stimulation conditions and individual differences (self-report

scores on attentiveness and novelty seeking).

Based on previous research and the MBA model, it was hypothesized that attentiveness (low

vs. high) would moderate the effect that auditory WN and tDCS have on response inhibition and

spatial WM scores. Because of the absence of previous studies investigating jointly differential

effects of auditory WN, tDCS, tRNS, and sham, as well as interaction effects of novelty seeking

with stimulation conditions on an adolescent population, no further hypotheses are made.

Method

Study Recruitment and Screening


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 19

Ethical approval was obtained from the authors supervisor professor at Lund University and

Psychology Ethics Committee. Written consent was obtained from the schools head teacher on

behalf of the students, and from the students themselves. Before the beginning of the study,

parents were sent information letters about the study and were given the option to opt their

children out. Three parents opt their children out of the study. All students were recruited from an

international school in Malmo, Sweden. All students, from school years MYP4 and MYP5 (76

students aged 15 to 17 years) were offered an informative session, without stating the

hypotheses, and were given the option to voluntarily and confidentially participate. Fifty-eight

students volunteer and were screened for level of attention using the Adult Attention-

Decit/Hyperactivity Disorder ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS v1.1) Symptom Checklist

(Kessler et al. 2005). After screening, 32 participants, 16 with the highest and 16 with the lowest

scores in attention levels were selected. Participants were randomly assigned to four groups of

equal size (8 subjects for each condition) matched on attentiveness. Conditions were

counterbalanced with a Latin square to control for order effects. A scheduled date and time was

mailed to the teachers and the participants for them to go to a class on school grounds for around

40 minutes. The study was conducted during two consecutive weeks, Monday to Friday, from

8:30 to 14:30h.

Participants

In total, 32 healthy adolescents volunteers participated, 21 male and 11 females (mean age =

15.44 years, SD = .56 years). All participants, except one, were right-handed. Subjects had

international backgrounds with eight different nationalities. However, all of them had been in an

English speaking school for at least five years, and their English language skills were excellent.

Procedure and Material


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 20

Participants were individually greeted by the experimenter and invited to go through a safety

questionnaire and consent form. They were further asked to clarify practical and ethical concerns

(Bostrom & Sandberg, 2009). No participant showed any safety issue. Subjects were invited to

sit in a comfortable chair in front of a laptop. The room was well aired and naturally lighted. The

experimenter explained the procedure and that further instruction would be appearing on the

computer monitor. The experimenter remained in the room to assist if necessary but sitting away

from the participant to avoid distraction.

Two laboratory tests, one response inhibition (Go/No-Go task) and one visuospatial WM task

(Corsi span) were employed. These tasks were presented in a fixed order and lasted

approximately 12 minutes. Afterward, subjects were set with headphones or the tES device,

depending on condition. In the sham condition, tDCS, set-up to sham, was used as it provides

with a better sham experience. After starting the stimulation (tES, sham, or auditory WN), a

questionnaire was given to the participants, the short version of the Temperament and Character

Inventory-Revised (TCI-R 48), which takes 7 to 10 minutes to complete. This time has been

shown to be enough for tES to alters neural activity under electrodes and connected subcortical

structures (Nitsche et al., 2008; Weber et al., 2014). When finished filling up the questionnaire,

subjects proceeded to perform the two cognitive tasks again. Participants completed the tasks

with the same experimenter present.

TES. Electrical stimulation was delivered by a battery-driven constant-current stimulator (v2

32Bit ARM Microprocessor, foc.us) through two (3.5 x 3.5; 12.25 cm2) saline-soaked sponges

with conductive electrodes. Anodal and cathodal electrodes were respectively placed

approximately at F3 (1020 EEG International System) and contralateral arm locations. The

placement was chosen following research proving these sites to be ideal for cognitive and
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 21

behavioral studies (Chaieb et al., 2009; Nitsche et al., 2003). Active stimulation was applied for

20 minutes with a 20-second ramp- up and ramp-down period. For sham stimulation, the current

was switched on for 30 seconds with a ramp time of 10 seconds and then switched off. For the

sham condition tDCS was used because subjects can feel better the sensation of the current than

with the tRNS, which has a higher cutaneous perception threshold (Ambrus et al., 2010), making

them think they are in an active condition. For tDCS, a current of 1.5mA and 20V was delivered.

For tRNS, a 1.5mA current with a DC offset set at zero producing a current that fluctuated

between 1.5 and 1.5 mA and with a high-frequency range (101-640 Hz) was delivered (Woods,

2016).

Auditory WN. The auditory WN was delivered binaurally, during all the experimental

condition, using headphones and an MP3 player. The output signal was set to 77dB based on

earlier studies (Baijot et al., 2016).

Cognitive tasks. Subjects participated in a Go/No-Go task followed by a visuospatial WM

spam task before and during the stimulation session. The Psychology Experiment Building

Language (PEBL), an open source, free application software, was used to program and deliver

the cognitive tasks. (Mueller & Piper, 2014).

Go/No-Go task. The Go/No-Go task is a computerized test measuring response time and

response inhibition included in the PEBL. During this task, participants were required to watch a

sequential presentation of letters and respond to a target letter by pressing the right shift button

on a laptop keyboard. The presentation began with instructions on a 17" computer monitor where

participants clicked enter to continue. Next, a screen with a 2 2 array of four stars (one in each

square of the matrix) was shown. A single letter (P or R) was then presented in one of the squares

for 500 ms with an interstimulus interval of 1,500 ms. In the first condition, (P-Go), subjects had
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 22

to press the right shift button when the target letter "P" appeared, and withhold their response to

the non-target letter "R". In the second condition, (R-Go), participants had to press the right shift

in response to the target letter "R", and withhold their response to the non-target letter "P". The

ratio of targets to non-targets was 80:20 in both conditions. A total of 320 trials, 160 in each

condition, were presented with practice trials before the task. Behavioral performance was

assessed by calculating accuracy, the number of errors in omission and commission both in the

Go and the No-Go conditions. The duration of the entire Go/No-Go task was approximately 10

minutes, including instructions and practice trials.

Corsi span task. The Corsi span task is a visuospatial WM task widely used for the

assessment of visuospatial short-term memory, both in clinical practice and in experimental

research settings. The Corsi test also relies heavily on memory for temporal information, because

the block sequences must be recalled in the proper sequential order (Kessels, van Zandvoort,

Postma, Kappelle & de Haan, 2000). The computerized Corsi span task used in the present study

was programmed using the PEBL. The trial started with the instructions for the task shown on a

17" screen. After the participant had pressed enter, an array of 9 blue squares (each 3 3 cm in

size) showed on a black background during 1000 ms. Then, the squares highlighted by flashing

in yellow for 1000 ms, before reverting to blue in a sequence of blocks, which the participant has

to repeat subsequently in the correct sequential order clicking them with the mouse. Two trials

were given per block sequence of the same length. If at least one of these was repeated correctly,

the next two trials of a sequence of an increased length were administered. Therefore, by

increasing the length of the sequences, the capacity of the visuospatial short-term memory was

measured. Series started at length 2 and gradually increased up to a maximum of 9. The trial

concluded when the subject did not reproduce the two sequences of equal length. The Total Score
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 23

(the product of the Block Span and the number of correct trials) was computed for each

participant. The duration of the task was for approximately 3 minutes.

Questionnaires. In order to assess individual differences in attentiveness and novelty

seeking, two self-report questionnaires were used.

ASRS-v1.1. The Adult Attention-Decit/Hyperactivity Disorder ADHD Self-Report Scale

(ASRS) v1.1 Symptom Checklist, is an 18-item scale developed in conjunction with the World

Health Organization (WHO), and the Workgroup on Adult ADHD as a valid self-assessment of

ADHD symptoms (Kessler et al. 2005). The questions in the ASRS v1.1 are consistent with the

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (DSM-IV) criteria for the assessment of

ADHD symptoms in adults. This screening instrument consists of part A, with six questions that

have been found to be the most predictive of the symptoms. Part B comprises the remaining

twelve questions and provides with additional cues and information about the frequency of the

symptoms (Kessler et al., 2005). A study by Adler et al. (2012), contrasted the (ADHD) Self-

Report Scale (ASRS) v1.1 Symptom Checklist with the clinician-administered ADHD Rating

Scale (ADHD-RS) in adolescents with ADHD with a mean age of 15 years. The results of the

study suggest that the ASRS v1.1 is an internally consistent and valid self-report scale for the

assessment of adolescents.

TCI-R 48. Cloninger (1999) developed the Temperament and Character Inventory-Revised to

measure seven personality dimensions. The shortened version TCI-R 48, used in this study, is a

48-item self-administered questionnaire designed to measure four temperaments, Novelty

Seeking, Harm Avoidance, Reward Dependence, and Persistence, and three characters, Self-

directedness, Cooperativeness, and Self-transcendence. The response option format of the TCI-R

48 ranged from 1 = definitely false to 5 = definitely true.


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 24

Analytical Strategy

In the dependent variables, measures of RTs and accuracy for response inhibition (Go/No-Go

task) and span spam for WM (Corsi span task) were obtained.

The distribution of the scores on response inhibition and WM of the four conditions were

examined with descriptive statistics (median, mean, standard deviation) and boxplots. If the

normality and equal variance assumptions were satisfied, the inferential statistical procedures

performed was the analysis of covariance ANCOVA for equal sample sizes, and Tukey's HSD, to

see which groups are significantly different. The pre-test (performance of the task without

stimulation), was the variable acting as covariate so as to adjust for pre-test differences. All

statistical tests were two-sided. All outliers (3 SD from the group mean score for each condition)

and any missing data was replaced with the group mean for that condition. A p-value of 0.05

was considered statistically significant. The one-way analysis of variance, ANCOVA, was

chosen over a mixed ANOVA because the study is looking for differences in the mean between

conditions and not for interaction effects between conditions and pre-post-tests. Moreover, a

mixed ANOVA analysis could be misleading as suggested by Dimitrov and Rumrill (2003). To

determine the reliability of group mean differences, as there are several groups or conditions,

performing multiple t-tests would not be convenient because the probability of a Type I error

would increase with each comparison between groups. An analysis of covariance, ANCOVA,

control for that error in the comparison of the four groups. If a statistically significant difference

was found, a Post Hoc test, Tukey, was used to establish between which groups were the

differences. The standard deviation and sample means of these groups will be reported. If

significant differences were found, the degree to which the IV and DV's were related was

assessed reporting the effect size or eta squared.


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 25

Additionally, with the aim of investigating interactions between individual differences

(attentiveness and novelty seeking) and stimulation conditions, a two-way between-subjects 42

ANOVA was performed, one for attentiveness and one for novelty seeking, for each dependent

variable. For this end, a high-low group for attentiveness and novelty seeking was established

depending on the scores, and the difference between pre and post-test scores was computed for

all dependent variables.

Results

Analysis of Covariance for Differential Effects

Three ANCOVAs (one for each dependent variable) were run to determine the effect of sham,

auditory WN, tRNS and tDCS on post-intervention scores in visuospatial WM, response

inhibition accuracy and response inhibition RT after controlling for pre-intervention scores.

Visuospatial WM. There was a linear relationship between pre- and post-intervention spatial

WM scores for each intervention type, as assessed by visual inspection of a scatterplot. There

was homogeneity of regression slopes as the interaction term was not statistically significant, F

(3, 24) = 1.805, p = .173. Standardized residuals for the interventions were normally distributed,

as assessed by Shapiro-Wilk's test (p > .05). There was homoscedasticity, as evaluated by the

standardized residuals plotted against the predicted values. There was homogeneity of variances,

as determined by Levene's test of homogeneity of variance (p = .598). There were no outliers in

the data, as assessed by no cases with standardized residuals greater than 3 standard deviations.

Adjusted means are presented unless otherwise stated. Post-intervention spatial WM scores were

higher in the auditory WN group (M = 85.50, SE = 7.44), tRNS group (M = 63.32, SE = 7.55)

and tDCS group (M = 76.01, SE = 7.32) compared to the sham group (M = 47.40, SE = 7.31).
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 26

After adjustment for pre-intervention spatial WM scores, there was a statistically significant

difference in post-intervention spatial WM scores between the conditions, F (3, 27) = 49.777, p

= .007, partial 2 = .356.

Post hoc analysis was performed with a Bonferroni adjustment. Post-intervention spatial WM

scores were statistically significantly greater in the auditory WN group (M = 85.50, SE = 7.44)

compared to the sham group (M = 47.40, SE = 7.31), a mean difference of 38.10, 95% CI [8.44,

67.76], p = .007. Post-intervention spatial WM scores were marginally statistically significantly

greater in the tDCS group than in the sham group, a mean difference of 28.61, 95% CI [-.83,

58.06], p = .061. No statistically significant differences were found between the tRNS and sham

group, a mean difference of 15.91, 95% CI [-14.10, 45.94], p = .857 (see Table 1).

Table1

Adjusted and Unadjusted Stimulation Condition Means and Variability for Posttest Spatial
Working Memory Scores with Pre-Test Spatial Working Memory Scores as a Covariate

Unadjusted Adjusted
N M SD M SE
Sham 8 46.75 22.72 47.40 7.31
tRNS 8 70.88 20.08 63.32 7.65
tDCS 8 74.75 24.77 76.01 7.32
WN 8 79.88 31.75 85.50 7.44
Note: N = number of participants, M = Mean, SD = Standard Deviation, SE = Standard Error, tRNS = Transcranial
Random Noise Stimulation, tDCS = Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation, WN = Auditory White Noise.

Response inhibition accuracy. There was a linear relationship between pre- and post-

intervention spatial WM scores for each intervention type, as assessed by visual inspection of a

scatterplot. There was homogeneity of regression slopes as the interaction term was not

statistically significant, F (3, 24) = .269, p = .847. Standardized residuals for the interventions
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 27

were normally distributed, as assessed by Shapiro-Wilk's test (p > .05). There was

homoscedasticity, as assessed by visual inspection of the standardized residuals plotted against

the predicted values. There was homogeneity of variances, as assessed by Levene's test of

homogeneity of variance (p = .868). There were no outliers in the data, as assessed by no cases

with standardized residuals greater than 3 standard deviations except in one instance, ID = 5

with a standardized residual of 3.35. It is decided to keep the outlier for further analysis because

the results of the analysis were not substantially affected.

Adjusted means are presented, unless otherwise stated. Post-intervention response inhibition

accuracy (number of omissions plus commissions errors) scores were similar in all conditions:

auditory WN group (M = 23.91, SE = 2.83), tRNS group (M = 26.95, SE = 2.90), tDCS group (M

= 24.06, SE = 2.85) and sham group (M = 24.55, SE = 2.85). After adjustment for pre-

intervention intervention response inhibition accuracy scores, there was not a statistically

significant difference in post-intervention response inhibition accuracy scores between the

conditions, F (3, 27) = 2.36, p = .871, partial 2 = .026 (See Table 2).

Table2

Adjusted and Unadjusted Stimulation Condition Means and Variability for Posttest Response
Inhibition Accuracy Scores with Pre-Test Posttest Response Inhibition Accuracy Scores as a
Covariate

Unadjusted Adjusted
N M SD M SE
Sham 8 26.63 15.15 24.65 2.85
tRNS 8 22.88 9.62 26.95 2.90
tDCS 8 26.25 11.23 24.06 2.85
WN 8 23.75 13.60 23.91 2.83
Note: N = number of participants, M = Mean, SD = Standard Deviation, SE = Standard Error, tRNS = Transcranial
Random Noise Stimulation, tDCS = Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation, WN = Auditory White Noise.
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 28

Response inhibition RT. There was a linear relationship between pre- and post-intervention

spatial WM scores for each intervention type, as assessed by visual inspection of a scatterplot.

There was homogeneity of regression slopes as the interaction term was not statistically

significant, F (3, 24) = .158, p = .924. Standardized residuals for the tDCS and tRNS conditions

were normally distributed, as assessed by Shapiro-Wilk's test (p > .05). However, standard

residuals for auditory WN and sham conditions were not normally distributed, Shapiro-Wilk's

test (p < .05). Because the one-way ANCOVA is relatively robust to deviations from normality

and the sample sizes are equal, it is decided to run the test as non-normality does not affect Type

I error rate substantially. There was homoscedasticity, as assessed by visual inspection of the

standardized residuals plotted against the predicted values. There was homogeneity of variances,

as assessed by Levene's test of homogeneity of variance (p = .624). There were no outliers in the

data, as assessed by no cases with standardized residuals greater than 3 standard deviations

except in one instance, ID = 5 with a standardized residual of 3.07. It is decided to keep the

outlier for further analysis because the results of the analysis were not substantially affected.

Adjusted means are presented unless otherwise stated. Post-intervention response inhibition RTs

were similar in all conditions: auditory WN group (M = 501.65, SE = 17.74), tRNS group (M =

470.71, SE = 17.73), tDCS group (M = 480.17, SE = 17.82) and sham group (M = 494.75, SE =

17.70). After adjustment for pre-intervention response inhibition RTs, there was not a statistically

significant difference in post-intervention response inhibition RTs between the conditions, F (3,

27) = .623, p = .606, partial 2 = .065 (see Table 3).


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 29

Table3

Adjusted and Unadjusted Stimulation Condition Means and Variability for Posttest Response
Inhibition RT Scores with Pre-Test Posttest Response Inhibition RT Scores as a Covariate

Unadjusted Adjusted
N M SD M SE
Sham 8 494.46 55.23 494.75 17.70
tRNS 8 473.04 42.34 470.71 17.73
tDCS 8 475.53 38.71 480.17 17.82
WN 8 504.25 71.45 501.65 17.74
Note: N = number of participants, M = Mean, SD = Standard Deviation, SE = Standard Error, tRNS = Transcranial
Random Noise Stimulation, tDCS = Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation, WN = Auditory White Noise.

Two-Way ANOVAs for Interaction Effects

To further investigate if the effect of the distinct stimulation depends on individual

differences, subjects were classified in a low or high attentiveness group and a low or high

novelty seeking group. A two-way between-subjects 42 ANOVA was performed, one for

attentiveness and one for novelty seeking, for each dependent variable (visuospatial WM,

response inhibition accuracy and response inhibition RT). For the dependent variables, the

difference between scores in the pre-test and post-test was computed. Subjects were assigned to

the low and high attentiveness group depending on the scores obtained in the ASRS-v1.1. The

subjects with scores from 1 to 17 were included in the high attention group (N = 17), and

subjects with scores from 18 to 34, in the low attention group (N = 15). Similarly, subjects were

assigned to the low or high novelty seeking group depending on the scores obtained in the TCI-R

48. The subjects with scores from 15 to 27 were included in the low novelty seeking group (N =

19), and subjects with scores from 28 to 41 (N = 13), in the high novelty seeking group. The two

independent variables were stimulation condition (auditory WN, tDCS, tRNS and Sham) and
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 30

attentiveness (low and high attentiveness) for the first ANOVA and low and high novelty seeking

for the second ANOVA. Residual analysis was performed to test the assumptions of the two-way

ANOVA. Inspection of a boxplot assessed outliers, normality, was assessed using Shapiro-Wilk's

normality test for each cell of the design and homogeneity of variances by Levene's test.

Attentiveness interaction with stimulation condition on spatial WM. There were no

outliers; residuals were normally distributed (p > .05), and there was homogeneity of variances

(p =. 278).

The interaction effect between stimulation condition and low-high attentiveness group for spatial

WM score was not statistically significant F (2, 24) = .855, p = .478, partial 2 = .097.

Therefore, an analysis of the main effects for stimulation condition and low-high attentiveness

group was performed, which indicated that the main effect of stimulation condition was

statistically significant, F (3, 24) = 4.988, p = .008, partial 2 = .384. There was no statistically

significant main effect of low-high attentiveness group on spatial WM score, F (1, 24) = .000, p

= .984, partial 2 = .000. All pairwise comparisons were run where reported 95% confidence

intervals and p-values are Bonferroni-adjusted. The marginal means of spatial WM scores for

auditory WN, tDCS, tRNS and sham low and high attentiveness group were 29.267 (SE = 8.024),

18.375 (SE = 7.769), 1.875 (SE = 7.769) and -10.500 (SE = 7.769), respectively.

Auditory WN stimulation was associated with a mean spatial WM score 39.767, 95% CI [7.655,

71.878] higher than the sham condition, a statistically significant difference, p = .010. The effect

of the stimulation conditions on spatial WM was not different for subjects classified as low or

high attentiveness.

Novelty seeking's interaction with stimulation condition on spatial WM. There were no

outliers as assessed by studentized residual value not greater than 2.5. Data was normally
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 31

distributed, as evaluated by Shapiro-Wilk's test (p > .05). There was homogeneity of variances,

as assessed by Levene's test for equality of variances (p =. 653).

There was a statistically significant interaction between stimulation condition and low-high level

of novelty seeking on spatial WM scores, F (3, 24) = 3.086, p = .046, partial 2 = .278.

Therefore, an analysis of simple main effects for stimulation condition was performed with

statistical significance receiving a Bonferroni adjustment and being accepted at the p < .025

level. There was a statistically significant difference in mean spatial WM scores between the low

and high novelty seeking group in the tDCS condition. In the tDCS condition, mean spatial WM

scores for the high novelty seeking group was 37.00 (SD = 24.99) and -.25 (SD = 6.46) for the

low novelty seeking group, a statistically significant mean difference of 37.25, 95% CI [8.70,

75.80], F (1, 24) = 7.252, p = .013, partial 2 = .232.

All pairwise comparisons were run for each simple main effect with reported 95% confidence

intervals and p-values Bonferroni-adjusted within each simple main effect. Spatial WM scores

for auditory WN, tRNS, tDCS and sham conditions in the high novelty seeking group were 27.33

(SD = 17.01), 2.50 (SD = 12.02), 37.00 (SD = 24.99) and -20.75 (SD = 28.76), respectively. The

high novelty seeking group, in the sham stimulation condition, had a statistically significantly

lower mean spatial WM score than the high novelty seeking group on the auditory WN, -48.08,

95% CI [-91.03, -5.12], p = .022, and the high novelty seeking group in the tDCS condition,

-57.75, 95% CI [-97.51, -17.98], p = .002. Mean spatial WM scores for auditory WN, tRNS,

tDCS and sham conditions for the low novelty seeking group were 31.20 (SD = 8.74), 1.66 (SD =

7.98), -.25 (SD = 9.78), and -.25 (SD = 9.78), respectively. No statistically significant differences

on mean spatial WM scores were found between stimulation conditions in the low novelty

seeking group.
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 32

The effect of tDCS on spatial WM was different for subjects classified as low or high

attentiveness. Subjects in the high novelty seeking group had statistically significantly higher

scores on spatial WM, compared to the subjects in the low novelty seeking group, with the

delivery of tDCS (see Figure 1).

80
Mean differences between pre and post test

60 Low NS High NS

40
on spatial WM scores

20

0
Sham tRNS tDCS WN
-20

-40

-60

Figure 1. Mean differences scores on spatial working memory between pre and posttest for
all stimulation conditions in both the Low and High novelty seeking (NS) groups. Error bar
denotes one standard deviation around the mean.

Attentiveness interaction with stimulation condition on response inhibition accuracy.

There were no outliers; residuals were normally distributed (p > .05), and there was homogeneity

of variances (p = .843).

There was not statistically significant interaction between stimulation condition and attentiveness
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 33

for response inhibition accuracy scores, F (3, 24) = .836, p = .487, partial 2 = .095. Therefore,

an analysis of the main effect for attentiveness was performed. There was not statistically

significant main effect of attentiveness on response inhibition accuracy scores, F (1, 24) = 0.902,

p = .352, partial 2 = .036.

Novelty seeking's interaction with stimulation condition on response inhibition accuracy.

There were no outliers; residuals were normally distributed (p > .05), and there was homogeneity

of variances (p =.182).

There was not statistically significant interaction between stimulation condition and attentiveness

for response inhibition accuracy scores, F (3, 24) = .503, p = .684, partial 2 = .059. Therefore,

an analysis of the main effect for novelty seeking was performed. There was not statistically

significant main effect of novelty seeking on response inhibition accuracy scores, F (1, 24) =

0.632, p = .435, partial 2 = .026.

Attentiveness's interaction with stimulation condition on response inhibition RT. There

were no outliers; residuals were normally distributed (p > .05), and there was not homogeneity of

variances (p =.043). However, it is decided to proceed with the analysis because group sample

sizes are equal, there is normality, and the ratio of the largest group variance to the smallest

group variance is less than 3. Under these circumstances, to run the two-way ANOVA it is

somehow robust to the heterogeneity of variance (Jaccard, 1998).

There was not statistically significant interaction between stimulation condition and attentiveness

for response inhibition RT scores, F (3, 24) = 2.32, p = .101, partial 2 = .225. Therefore, an

analysis of the main effect for attentiveness was performed. There was not statistically

significant main effect of attentiveness on response inhibition RT scores, F (1, 24) = 1.46, p

= .239, partial 2 = .057


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 34

Novelty seeking's interaction with stimulation condition on response inhibition RT.

There were no outliers; residuals were normally distributed (p > .05), and there was homogeneity

of variances (p = .184).

There was not statistically significant interaction between stimulation condition and novelty

seeking for response inhibition RT scores, F (3, 24) = 1.39, p = .267, partial 2 = .149.

Therefore, an analysis of the main effect for novelty seeking was performed. There was not

statistically significant main effect of novelty seeking on response inhibition RT scores, F (1, 24)

= .505, p = .484, partial 2 = .021.

Discussion

This study investigated the effect of auditory WN, tRNS and tDCS (over the left DLPFC) on

response inhibition and spatial WM together with the interaction of the stimulation conditions

with individual differences in attentiveness and novelty seeking. The performance of response

inhibition and WM tasks after auditory WN, tDCS, and tRNS was compared to performance

after the sham condition, with the scores on the cognitive tasks before the application of

stimulation conditions, acting as a control variable. Individual self-report scores on attentiveness

and novelty seeking were investigated as moderator variables of any significant effect. Accuracy

and RT were assessed for response inhibition with a Go/No-Go task. Spatial WM was assessed

with a Corsi visuospatial span task.

Differential effects of stimulation conditions

The application of tRNS and tDCS resulted in no significant change on any of the outcome

measures compared to sham when controlling for pre-intervention performance. However, it is

noteworthy that the study found marginally statistically significant difference in spatial WM
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 35

between tDCS and the sham condition, with higher scores after intervention with tDCS than

sham on spatial WM span. The results do not agree with previous studies reporting enhancement

of response inhibition and WM after application of tDCS on the left DLPFC (Boggio et al.,

2007). However, differences in the intensity, 1.5mA in the present study vs. 2mA in the study by

Boggio et al., and differences in the tasks employed to measure WM, a visuospatial WM task in

this study vs. a Sternberg task in the study by Gladwin et al. (2012), might explain discrepancies.

The results cannot be due to a lack of sensitivity of the outcomes measures as the Go/No-Go task

has been used extensively in previous studies and a visual inspection of the raw data revealed

neither ceiling nor flooring effects. On the other hand, this study is in agreement with prior

research that found the application on the left DLPFC of tDCS and tRNS (Mulquiney et al.,

2011) did not affect performance on response inhibition and WM tasks.

The auditory WN stimulation condition did yield a significant increase in the span of spatial

WM while it did not impact task accuracy or RT on the response inhibition Go/No-Go task,

compared with the sham condition. These results cannot be explained by the effects of pre-

existing group differences in response inhibition and WM because the use of ANCOVA corrected

for these effects. Results reveal a difference in the processes underlying response inhibition and

spatial WM. The investigation of the neural correlations underlying these processes can reveal

differential mechanisms involved in the various forms of brain stimulation. Moreover, the fact

that only the auditory WN stimulation condition yield a significant increase in spatial WM scores

over tDCS and tRNS suggests that auditory WN underlying mechanisms are not the same as

those that are at stake in tDCS and tRNS. An alternative explanation is that the placement of the

electrodes over the left DLPFC was not ideal to target response inhibition processes. Indeed, in

previous research reporting effects of tDCS on a Go/No-Go task measuring response inhibition,
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 36

the anodal electrode was placed above the rIFG, (Sikstrm et al., 2016). However, the effect was

only showed on RT and not accuracy.

Interaction Effects

The additional analyses of the interaction between the different stimulation conditions and the

low-high attentiveness groups on the performance on spatial WM and response inhibition

revealed no significant results. These findings do not concur with the hypothesis of the present

study nor with previous studies by Helps et al. (2014) and Sikstrm et al. (2016) that found

attentiveness moderates the effect of auditory WN and tDCS on a Go/No-Go task, measuring

response inhibition. However, unlike the present study, they did not use a sham condition to

avoid placebo effects, did not control for pre-intervention scores and conditions were not

counterbalanced. Nevertheless, the results do agree with Sikstrm et al. (2016) not finding any

interaction effects of attentiveness with auditory WN and tDCS on a spatial WM task. Although

the results of the current study do not agree with recent research, differences in the assessment of

attentiveness, self-report in the present study vs. parents and teachers report in the study by

Helps et al. (2014), might account for the conflicting results.

A relevant finding of the present study is that individual differences in novelty seeking

moderates the effect of tDCS on a spatial WM task. To date, this is the only known study that has

investigated the moderating effect that individual differences in novelty seeking have on different

stimulation conditions on measures of response inhibition and spatial WM. A significant increase

in spatial WM scores was found in the high vs. low novelty seeking group with the delivery of

anodal tDCS over the left DLPFC compared to the sham condition. Also relevant is the finding

that auditory WN stimulation, unlike tDCS, affects both, the low and high novelty seeking

groups, significantly improving their scores on spatial WM compared to the sham condition.
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 37

Although a significant correlation between novelty seeking and attentiveness has been

established in previous studies as well as the present one, r =.483, p= .005, the results indicate a

substantial discrimination between attentiveness and novelty seeking that could help to elucidate

the underlying working mechanisms of auditory WN and tDCS.

The involvement of the dopaminergic system on attentiveness (Biederman & Faraone, 2005),

novelty seeking (Bodi et al. 2009) and WM (Takeuchi et al., 2015) has been long established.

Even more, the MBA model (Sikstrm & Sderlund, 2007) explains how individual differences

in the dopaminergic system can lead to differential effects of auditory WN. However, the results

of the current study suggest that novelty seeking measures might be more sensible than

attentiveness to detect such differential effects.

Another intriguing finding of this study is the distinction between response inhibition and WM.

With auditory WN and tDCS exclusively affecting spatial WM and not response inhibition.

A possible alternative explanation to the MBA model (Sikstrm & Sderlund, 2007) could be

accounted for by the multiple-component theory of WM (Baddeley & Hitch 1974), the state-

based models of WM (Cowan, 2001) and the load theory of cognition (Lavie, 1995).

The multiple-component model of WM (Baddeley & Hitch 1974) describes two distinct

processes, WM storage, and control. Auditory WN might work allowing to expand the storage

capacity of WM. On the other hand, tDCS might affect the control process.

The WM storage is better explained by state-based models (Cowan, 2001; DEsposito & Postle,

2015; Fuster, 2014; Jonides, Lacey & Nee, 2005). These models are grounded in the idea that

mental representations are brought into WM by attentional selection. According to these models

(attention models), both attended and unattended sounds cause the occurrence of contents that
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 38

occupy resources in WM. The maintenance and processing of these sound-related contents in

WM would engage attentional resources that would no longer be available for other tasks. The

findings of the current study suggest that the application of auditory WN might help to disregard

distracting sounds that otherwise would occupy resources in WM, regardless of individual

differences. This effect can be explained by the Load Theory of Attention and Cognitive

Control (Lavie, 1995; Lavie, Hirst, de Fockert & Viding, 2004). The theory states that the level

of information irrelevant to the main task that is processed will depend on the amount of

perceptual load that the primary task involves. When a task with a high perceptual load

consumes all available capacity, the perception of stimuli irrelevant to the task is reduced or

eliminated. Conversely, when a task has a low perceptual load, available capacity allows the

perception of irrelevant stimuli (Parks, Hilimire & Corballis, 2011).

Cross-modal interactions between the visual and auditory perceptual systems are well-

documented (Spence, 2010). This study substantiates these findings as auditory WN influenced a

visuospatial WM task and might have contributed to eliminating distraction and expanding the

storage capacity of WM.

On the other hand, tDCS might affect the control process of WM. A critical mechanism of

WM function is the synchronization of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) with activity in other brain

regions. Top-down control signals from PFC have been suggested to enhance task-relevant

information and suppress task-irrelevant information (DEsposito & Postle, 2015). Therefore, the

stimulation on the left DLPFC with tDCS might differentially help to synchronize the activity of

PFC with other areas in individuals with higher scores in novelty seeking.

Some potential limitations of the present study require consideration. The current study

describes a relatively small sample size, meaning that the study may have lacked the power
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 39

needed to detect reliably changes in the response inhibition measures employed. Additionally,

despite the exact counterbalancing of the sessions achieved following randomization and the

matching of groups on attentiveness, other variables may have interfered with the equivalence of

the groups. Although no differences were found in age, gender, education level and novelty

seeking between groups.

In addition to addressing the limitations above through employing larger sample sizes, with

respect to tDCS and tRNS, in future studies, it may also be advantageous to alter the placement

of the electrodes, current density, and range as well as the time of off-line and online application

when employing this form of stimulation. In this study, the same intensity,1.5mA was delivered

for tDCS and tRNS, but past studies have shown increased brain excitability with tRNS only

with higher current density in comparison with tDCS (Moliadze, Antal & Paulus, 2010). It is

therefore suggested to increase current intensity when using tRNS. Additionally, tRNS with DC

offset of 1mA has been shown to be more efficient than tRNS with zero DC offset (Ho, Taylor &

Loo, 2013; 2015). However, the statistical proprieties of tRNS with zero DC offset (used in the

current study) are more similar to the random frequencies of auditory WN, thus more appropriate

to test if auditory WN and tRNS share the same working mechanism, SR (Fertonani et al., 2011).

Future studies on differential effects of non-invasive brain stimulation might find helpful to

assess novelty seeking in addition to attentiveness. The results of the present study suggest the

assessment of this component may provide valuable information on individual differences and its

interaction with response to treatment. For instance, to explore which of the two novelty

seeking's sub-dimensions -- exploratory excitability (drive for novel experience) and

impulsiveness (careless decision-making) -- interacts with tDCS to affect results could enable

further specification of personality and character differences interacting with stimulation.


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 40

Conclusion

The current study does not support the MBA model suggestion that both auditory WN and

current stimulation interact with individual differences in attentiveness on cognitive

performance. The results prompt to tRNS, tDCS and auditory WN targeting different

mechanisms and processes. A possible explanation of the results is that auditory WN allows

more capacity in the WM storage and that tDCS helps to activate or synchronized top-down

control signals that could be deficient in individuals with a high score in novelty seeking. The

lack of effect of tRNS, in contrast to auditory WN and tDCS, further indicates a different

working mechanism. Novelty seeking interaction with tDCS further opens new possibilities for

future studies to investigate differential effects and underlying mechanisms of the diverse

non/invasive brain stimulations.


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 41

References

Adler, L. A., Shaw, D. M., Spencer, T. J., Newcorn, J. H., Hammerness, P., Sitt, D. J., Faraone, S.

V. (2012). Preliminary examination of the reliability and concurrent validity of the

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder self-report scale v1.1 symptom checklist to rate

symptoms of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder in adolescents. Journal of Child &

Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 22(3), 238-244 7p. doi:10.1089/cap.2011.0062

Ambrus, G. G., Paulus, W., & Antal, A. (2010). Cutaneous perception thresholds of electrical

stimulation methods: Comparison of tDCS and tRNS. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121(11),

1908-1914. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.020

Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of

learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 4789). New York:

Academic Press.

Badgaiyan, R. D., Sinha, S., Sajjad, M., & Wack, D. S. (2015). Attenuated tonic and enhanced

phasic release of dopamine in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Plos One, 10(9),

e0137326-e0137326. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137326

Baijot, S., Slama, H., Sderlund, G., Dan, B., Deltenre, P., Colin, C., & Deconinck, N. (2016).

Neuropsychological and neurophysiological benefits from white noise in children with and

without ADHD. Behavioral & Brain Functions, 12, 1-13. doi:10.1186/s12993-016-0095-y

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:

Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94.

doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

Biederman, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2005). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet,

366(9481), 237-248. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66915-2


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 42

Bikson, M., Radman, T., & Datta, A. (2006). Rational modulation of neuronal processing with

applied electric fields. Conference Proceedings: Annual International Conference of the

IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.IEEE Engineering in Medicine and

Biology Society.Annual Conference, 1, 1616-1619.

Bliss, T. V., & Collingridge, G. L. (1993). A synaptic model of memory: Long-term potentiation

in the hippocampus. Nature, 361(6407), 31-39.

Bodi, N., Keri, S., Nagy, H., Moustafa, A., Myers, C., Daw, N., & ... Gluck, M. (2009). Reward-

learning and the novelty-seeking personality: a between- and within-subjects study of the

effects of dopamine agonists on young Parkinson's patients. Brain, 1322385-2395.

Boggio, P. S., Bermpohl, F., Vergara, A. O., Muniz, A. L. C. R., Nahas, F. H., Leme, P. B.,

Fregni, F. (2007). Research report: Go-no-go task performance improvement after anodal

transcranial DC stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in major depression.

Journal of Affective Disorders, 101, 91-98. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.10.026

Boggio, P. S., Ferrucci, R., Rigonatti, S. P., Covre, P., Nitsche, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Fregni,

F. (2006). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory in patients

with Parkinson's disease. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 249, 31-38. doi:

10.1016/j.jns.2006.05.062

Bostrom, N., & Sandberg, A. (2009). Cognitive enhancement: Methods, ethics, regulatory

challenges. Science & Engineering Ethics, 15(3), 311-341. doi:10.1007/s11948-009-9142-5

Broadbent, D. E. (1958). The effects of noise on behaviour. (pp. 81-107). Elmsford, NY, US:

Pergamon Press. doi:10.1037/10037-005

Brunoni, A. R., Nitsche, M. A., Bolognini, N., Bikson, M., Wagner, T., Merabet, L., Fregni, F.

(2012). Clinical research with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): Challenges and
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 43

future directions. Brain Stimulation, 5(3), 175-195.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.brs.2011.03.002

Brunoni, A. R., & Vanderhasselt, M. (2014). Working memory improvement with non-invasive

brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: A systematic review and meta-

analysis. Brain and Cognition, 86, 1-9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.008

Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(8), 617-628.

doi:10.1038/nrn896

Chaieb, L., Antal, A., & Paulus, W. (2015). Transcranial random noise stimulation-induced

plasticity is NMDA-receptor independent but sodium-channel blocker and benzodiazepines

sensitive. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 1.

Chaieb, L., Kovacs, G., Cziraki, C., Greenlee, M., Paulus, W., & Antal, A. (2009). Short-duration

transcranial random noise stimulation induces blood oxygenation level dependent response

attenuation in the human motor cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 198(4), 439.

doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1938-7

Chaieb, L., Paulus, W., & Antal, A. (2011). Evaluating after effects of short-duration transcranial

random noise stimulation on cortical excitability. Neural Plasticity, 2011, 1-5.

doi:10.1155/2011/105927

Clark, K., Squire, R. F., Merrikhi, Y., & Noudoost, B. (2015). Visual attention: Linking prefrontal

sources to neuronal and behavioral correlates. Progress in Neurobiology, 132, 59-80.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.06.006

Clark, V. P., Coffman, B. A., Mayer, A. R., Weisend, M. P., Lane, T. D. R., Calhoun, V. D., . . .

Wassermann, E. M. (2012). TDCS guided using fMRI significantly accelerates learning to


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 44

identify concealed objects. Neuroimage, 59(1), 117-128.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.036

Cloninger, C. R. (1999). The temperament and character inventoryRevised. St Louis, MO:

Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University.

Cloninger, C. R. (2004). Feeling good: The science of wellbeing. New York: Oxford University

Press.

Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of

temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975989.

Coffman, B. A., Clark, V. P., & Parasuraman, R. (2014). Review: Battery powered thought:

Enhancement of attention, learning, and memory in healthy adults using transcranial direct

current stimulation. Neuroimage, 85(-), 895-908. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.083

Cohen Kadosh, R. (2015). Modulating and enhancing cognition using brain stimulation: Science

and fiction. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(2), 141.

Cook, A., Johnson, C., & Bradley-Johnson, S. (2015). White noise to decrease problem

behaviors in the classroom for a child with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 37(1), 38-50 13p. doi:10.1080/07317107.2015.1000234

Cowan, N. (2001). Metatheory of storage capacity limits. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24(1),

154.

Dedoncker, J., Brunoni, A. R., Baeken, C., & Vanderhasselt, M. (2016). A systematic review and

meta-analysis of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the

dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in healthy and neuropsychiatric samples: Influence of

stimulation parameters. Brain Stimulation, 9(4), 501-517.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.006
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 45

Demirtas-Tatlidede, A., Vahabzadeh-Hagh, A. M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2013). Can noninvasive

brain stimulation enhance cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders? Neuropharmacology, 64,

566-578. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.06.020

D'Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R. (2015). The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual

Review of Psychology, 66, 115-142. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (2013). Arlington, Va.: American

Psychiatric Association, cop. 2013; 5. ed.

Dimitrov, D. M., & Rumrill, J.,Phillip D. (2003). Pretest-posttest designs and measurement of

change. Work, 20(2), 159.

Ditye, T., Jacobson, L., Walsh, V., & Lavidor, M. (2012). Modulating behavioral inhibition by

tDCS combined with cognitive training. Experimental Brain Research, 219(3), 363-368.

doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3098-4

Elmasry, J., Loo, C., & Martin, D. (2015). A systematic review of transcranial electrical

stimulation combined with cognitive training. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience,

33(3), 263-278. doi:10.3233/RNN-140473

Fecteau, S., Knoch, D., Fregni, F., Sultani, N., Boggio, P., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2007).

Diminishing risk-taking behavior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: A direct

current stimulation study. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(46), 12500-12505.

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3283-07.2007

Fertonani, A., Ferrari, C., & Miniussi, C. (2015). What do you feel if I apply transcranial electric

stimulation? safety, sensations and secondary induced effects. Clinical Neurophysiology,

126(11), 2181-2188. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.clinph.2015.03.015


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 46

Fertonani, A., Pirulli, C., & Miniussi, C. (2011). Random noise stimulation improves

neuroplasticity in perceptual learning. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of

the Society for Neuroscience, 31(43), 15416-15423. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2002-

11.2011

Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Nitsche, M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Transcranial direct current

stimulation. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 186, 446-

447.

Fregni, F., Marcondes, R., Boggio, P. S., Marcolin, M. A., Rigonatti, S. P., Sanchez, T. G.,

Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). Transient tinnitus suppression induced by repetitive transcranial

magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation. European Journal of

Neurology, 13(9), 996-1001. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2006. 01414.x

Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Nitsche, M., Bermpohl, F., Antal, A., Feredoes, E., Pascual-Leone, A.

(2005). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working

memory. Experimental Brain Research, 166(1), 23-30.

Fritsch, B., Reis, J., Martinowich, K., Schambra, H. M., Ji, Y., Cohen, L. G., & Lu, B. (2010).

Direct Current Stimulation Promotes BDNF-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity: Potential

Implications for Motor Learning. Neuron, 66(2), 198-204. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.035

Fuster, J. M. (2014). The Prefrontal Cortex Makes the Brain a Preadaptive System. Proceedings

of The IEEE, 102(4), 417. doi:10.1109/JPROC.2014.2306250

Gandiga, P. C., Hummel, F. C., & Cohen, L. G. (2006). Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): A

tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clinical

Neurophysiology, 117845-850. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 47

Gerra, G., Zaimovic, A., Timpano, M., Zambelli, U., Delsignore, R., & Brambilla, F. (2000).

Neuroendocrine correlates of temperamental traits in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology,

25(5), 479-496. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00004-4

Gladwin, T. E., den Uyl, T. E., Fregni, F. F., & Wiers, R. W. (2012). Enhancement of selective

attention by tDCS: Interaction with interference in a Sternberg task. Neuroscience Letters,

512(1), 33-37. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.01.056

Guleyupoglu, B., Schestatsky, P., Edwards, D., Fregni, F., & Bikson, M. (2013). Clinical

neuroscience: Classification of methods in transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) and

evolving strategy from historical approaches to contemporary innovations. Journal of

Neuroscience Methods, 219, 297-311. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.07.016

Heeren, A., Baeken, C., Vanderhasselt, M., Philippot, P., & de Raedt, R. (2015). Impact of

Anodal and Cathodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation over the Left Dorsolateral

Prefrontal Cortex during Attention Bias Modification: An Eye-Tracking Study. Plos ONE,

10(4), 1. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0124182

Helps, S. K., Bamford, S., Sonuga-Barke, E., & Sderlund, G. B. W. (2014). Different effects of

adding white noise on cognitive performance of sub-, normal and super-attentive school

children. Plos One, 9(11), 1-10. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112768

Hill, A. T., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Hoy, K. E. (2016). Effects of anodal transcranial direct current

stimulation on working memory: A systematic review and meta-analysis of findings from

healthy and neuropsychiatric populations. Brain Stimulation, 9, 197-208. doi:

10.1016/j.brs.2015.10.006

Ho, K. -., Taylor, J., & Loo, C. (2013). P 209. transcranial random noise stimulation: A new

approach to stimulating the brain doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.04.286


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 48

Ho, K., Taylor, J. L., & Loo, C. K. (2015). Comparison of the effects of transcranial random

noise stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation on motor cortical excitability.

Journal of ECT, 31(1), 67-72 6p. doi:10.1097/YCT.0000000000000155

Horvath, J. C., Forte, J. D., & Carter, O. (2015). Quantitative review finds no evidence of

cognitive effects in healthy populations from single-session transcranial direct current

stimulation (tDCS). Brain Stimulation, 8(3), 535-550. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.01.400

Jaccard, J. (1998). Interaction effects in factorial analysis of variance. [elektronisk resurs]

Thousand Oaks, Calif.] ; London : SAGE, cop. 1998.

Jacobson, L., Javitt, D. C., & Lavidor, M. (2011). Activation of inhibition: Diminishing

impulsive behavior by direct current stimulation over the inferior frontal gyrus. Journal of

Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3380-3387.

Javadi, A. H., & Cheng, P. (2013). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) enhances

reconsolidation of long-term memory. Brain Stimulation, 6(4), 668-674.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.brs.2012.10.007

Javadi, A. H., & Walsh, V. (2012). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the left

dorsolateral prefrontal cortex modulates declarative memory. Brain Stimulation, 5(3), 231-

241. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.brs.2011.06.007

Jonides, J., Lacey, S. C., & Nee, D. E. (2005). Processes of Working Memory in Mind and Brain.

Current Directions in Psychological Science, (1). 2.

Joos, K., De Ridder, D., & Vanneste, S. (2015). The differential effect of low- versus high-

frequency random noise stimulation in the treatment of tinnitus. Experimental Brain

Research, 233(5), 1433-1440. doi:10.1007/s00221-015-4217-9


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 49

Juan, C., & Muggleton, N. G. (2012). Brain stimulation and inhibitory control. Brain

Stimulation, 5(2), 63-69. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.012

Kadosh, R. (2013). Using transcranial electrical stimulation to enhance cognitive functions in the

typical and atypical brain. Translational Neuroscience, 4(1), 20.

Kadosh, R. C., & Elliott, P. (2013). Neuroscience: Brain stimulation has a long history. Nature,

500(7464), 529. doi:10.1038/500529d

Karako, A., & Trker, F. (2014). Original article: Effects of white noise and holding on pain

perception in newborns. Pain Management Nursing, 15, 864-870. doi:

10.1016/j.pmn.2014.01.002

Kekic, M., Boysen, E., Campbell, I. C., & Schmidt, U. (2016). A systematic review of the

clinical efficacy of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in psychiatric disorders.

Journal of Psychiatric Research, 74, 70-86.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.12.018

Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S., Hiripi, E., . . . Walters, E. E. (2005).

The world health organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): A short screening

scale for use in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 35(2), 245-256 12p.

Kessler, S. K., Turkeltaub, P. E., Benson, J. G., & Hamilton, R. H. (2012). Differences in the

experience of active and sham transcranial direct current stimulation. Brain Stimulation,

5(2), 155-162. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.brs.2011.02.007

Knudsen, E. I. (2007). Fundamental components of attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience,

30, 57-78.

Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of

Experimental Psychology.Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451-468.


EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 50

Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention

and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 339-354.

doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339

Liebetanz, D., Nitsche, M. A., Tergau, F., & Paulus, W. (2002). Pharmacological approach to the

mechanisms of transcranial DC-stimulation-induced after-effects of human motor cortex

excitability. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 125, 2238-2247.

Martin, D. M., Liu, R., Alonzo, A., Green, M., & Loo, C. K. (2014). Use of transcranial direct

current stimulation (tDCS) to enhance cognitive training: Effect of timing of stimulation.

Experimental Brain Research, 232(10), 3345-3351. doi:10.1007/s00221-014-4022-x

McDonnell, M. D., & Abbott, D. (2009). What is stochastic resonance? definitions,

misconceptions, debates, and its relevance to biology. PLoS Computational Biology, 5(5), 1-

9. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000348

Moliadze, V., Antal, A., & Paulus, W. (2010). Electrode-distance dependent after-effects of

transcranial direct and random noise stimulation with extracephalic reference electrodes.

Clinical Neurophysiology, 121(12), 2165-2171.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.033

Moss, F., Ward, L. M., & Sannita, W. G. (2004). Stochastic resonance and sensory information

processing: A tutorial and review of application. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(2), 267-281.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.clinph.2003.09.014

Mueller, S. T., & Piper, B. J. (2014). Basic neuroscience: The psychology experiment building

language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 222, 250-259.

doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 51

Mulquiney, P. G., Hoy, K. E., Daskalakis, Z. J., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2011). Improving working

memory: Exploring the effect of transcranial random noise stimulation and transcranial

direct current stimulation on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Clinical Neurophysiology,

122(12), 2384-2389. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.clinph.2011.05.009

Nitsche, M. A., & Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by

weak transcranial direct current stimulation. The Journal of Physiology, 527 Pt 3, 633-639.

Nitsche, M. A., Boggio, P. S., Fregni, F., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2009). Treatment of depression

with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): A review. Experimental Neurology,

219(1), 14-19. doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.expneurol.2009.03.038

Nitsche, M. A., Nitsche, M. S., Klein, C. C., Tergau, F., Rothwell, J. C., & Paulus, W. (2003).

Level of action of cathodal DC polarisation induced inhibition of the human motor cortex.

Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 600-604. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00412-1

Ohn, S. H., Park, C., Yoo, W., Ko, M., Choi, K. P., Kim, G., Kadosh, R. C. (2013). Time-

dependent effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on the enhancement of working

memory. England: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f2adfd

Plsson, E., Sderlund, G., Klamer, D., & Bergquist, F. (2011). Noise benefit in prepulse

inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex. Psychopharmacology, 214(3), 675-685.

doi:10.1007/s00213-010-2074-6

Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental

psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,

37(1), 51-87.

Posner, M. I., & Boies, S. J. (1971). Components of attention. Psychological Review, 78(5), 391-

408. doi:10.1037/h0031333
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 52

Rausch, V. H., Bauch, E. M., & Bunzeck, N. (2014). White noise improves learning by

modulating activity in dopaminergic midbrain regions and right superior temporal sulcus.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(7), 1469-1480. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00537

Rizzo, A. A., Buckwalter, J. G., Bowerly, T., Van, D. Z., Humphrey, L., Neumann, U., . . .

Sisemore, D. (2000). The virtual classroom: A virtual reality environment for the assessment

and rehabilitation of attention deficits. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 3(3), 483-499.

doi:10.1089/10949310050078940

Romanska, A., Rezlescu, C., Susilo, T., Duchaine, B., & Banissy, M. J. (2015). High-frequency

transcranial random noise stimulation enhances perception of facial identity. Cerebral

Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 25(11), 4334-4340. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv016

Saiote, C., Polana, R., Rosenberger, K., Paulus, W., & Antal, A. (2013). High-frequency TRNS

reduces BOLD activity during visuomotor learning. Plos One, 8(3), 1.

Saturnino, G. B., Antunes, A., & Thielscher, A. (2015). On the importance of electrode

parameters for shaping electric field patterns generated by tDCS. Neuroimage, 120, 25-35.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.067

Sibley, M. H., Kuriyan, A. B., Evans, S. W., Waxmonsky, J. G., & Smith, B. H. (2014).

Pharmacological and psychosocial treatments for adolescents with ADHD: An updated

systematic review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(3), 218-232.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.02.001

Sikstrm, S., Jrgensen, A., Haghighi, M., Mnsson, D., Smidelik, D., & Habekost, T. (2016).

Self-rated attentiveness interacts with transcranial direct current stimulation and noise

stimulation in reaction time in a Go/No-go task. Neural Plasticity, 1-5.

doi:10.1155/2016/5302538
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 53

Sikstrm, S., & Soderlund, G. (2007). Stimulus-dependent dopamine release in Attention-

Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Review, 114(4), 1047-1075.

Sderlund, G. B. W., Sikstrm, S., Loftesnes, J. M., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2010). The effects of

background white noise on memory performance in inattentive school children. Behavioral

& Brain Functions, 6, 55-64. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-6-55

Sderlund, G., Marklund, E., & Lacerda, F., (2009). Auditory white noise enhances cognitive

performance under certain conditions: Examples from visuo-spatial working memory and

dichotic listening tasks. Stockholm: Department of Linguistics, Stockholm Universitet.

Sderlund, G., Sikstrm, S., (2008). Positive effects of noise on cognitive performance:

Explaining the moderate brain arousal model. Noise as a Public Health Problem , 378.

Sderlund, G., Sikstrm, S., & Smart, A. (2007). Listen to the noise: Noise is beneficial for

cognitive performance in ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 48(8), 840-847

8p.

Sparing, R., Dafotakis, M., Meister, I. G., Thirugnanasambandam, N., & Fink, G. R. (2008).

Enhancing language performance with non-invasive brain stimulationA transcranial direct

current stimulation study in healthy humans. Neuropsychologia, 46, 261-268. doi:

10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.009

Spence, C. (2010). Crossmodal spatial attention. Annals of The New York Academy of Sciences,

1191182-200. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010. 05440.x

Stagg, C. J., Lin, R. L., Mezue, M., Segerdahl, A., Kong, Y., Xie, J., & Tracey, I. (2013).

Widespread modulation of cerebral perfusion induced during and after transcranial direct

current stimulation applied to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The Journal of
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 54

Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(28), 11425-11431.

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3887-12.2013

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Nouchi, R., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Kotozaki, Y., . . . Kawashima,

R. (2015). Working memory training impacts the mean diffusivity in the dopaminergic

system

Tarver, J., Daley, D., & Sayal, K. (2014). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): An

updated review of the essential facts. Child: Care, Health & Development, 40(6), 762-774

13p. doi:10.1111/cch.12139

Terney, D., Chaieb, L., Moliadze, V., Antal, A., & Paulus, W. (2008). Increasing human brain

excitability by transcranial high-frequency random noise stimulation. The Journal of

Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 28(52), 14147-14155.

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4248-08.2008

Thomas, R., Sanders, S., Doust, J., Beller, E., & Glasziou, P. (2015). Prevalence of attention-

deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 135(4),

e994-e1001 1p. doi:10.1542/peds.2014-3482

Usher, M., & Feingold, M. (2000). Stochastic resonance in the speed of memory retrieval.

Biological Cybernetics, 83(6), L11-L16.

Weber, M. J., Messing, S. B., Rao, H., Detre, J. A., & Thompson-Schill, S. (2014). Prefrontal

transcranial direct current stimulation alters activation and connectivity in cortical and

subcortical reward systems: A tDCS-fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 35(8), 3673-3686.

doi:10.1002/hbm.22429
EFFECTS OF WN, TES AND SHAM ON COGNITION 55

Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of

the executive function theory of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic

review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336-1346. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006

Woods, A. J., Antal, A., Bikson, M., Boggio, P. S., Brunoni, A. R., Celnik, P., . . . Nitsche, M. A.

(2016). A technical guide to tDCS, and related non-invasive brain stimulation tools. Clinical

Neurophysiology, 127(2), 1031-1048. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.11.012

Zaehle, T., Rach, S., & Herrmannz, C. S. (2010). Transcranial alternating current stimulation

enhances individual alpha activity in human EEG. Plos One, 5(11), 1-7. doi:

10.1371/journal.pone.0013766

Anda mungkin juga menyukai