Anda di halaman 1dari 11

New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

New Ideas in Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/newideapsych

Trajectory-based methods in clinical psychology: A person centred


narrative approach
Kieron O'Connor a, *, Marie Robert b, Guilhe
me Pe
rodeau b, Monique Se
guin b
a
Research Centre, University Institute of Mental Health at Montreal, 7331 Hochelaga St., Montreal, Quebec H1N 3V2, Canada
b
Department of Psychoeducation and Psychology, Universit e du Qu
ebec en Outaouais, 283 Alexandre-Tach e Blvd, Gatineau, Quebec J8X 3X7, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Life trajectories in clinical psychology research are often not treated as interactive trajectories but rather
Received 22 August 2014 as static transversal variables. But developmental pathways are often cumulative and conditional and
Received in revised form currently require sophisticated group-based modeling to tease out individual differences in trajectories.
25 June 2015
Clinical psychologists often require personal information on transitions and turning points in life which
Accepted 26 June 2015
Available online 20 July 2015
require eliciting information through qualitative life history approaches. A method is proposed for
identifying life events within the person's narrative and describing trajectories as event spaces likely to
reect end-point psychopathology.
Keywords:
Clinical psychology
2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Trajectory-based methods
Group based models
Person centred constructions
Narratives
Event spaces

1. Introduction Several authors have indeed criticized the a-historic nature of


the mainstream clinical psychology approach and suggested a
In clinical psychology early development, life events and bio- historically structured sampling method for comparing histories
graphical factors are recognized inuences on the development of which converge at nal points but diverge at important previous
pathology, both theoretically and practically. However, there is the bifurcation points. But the reality is that historical facts are usually
question of translating such biographical context into a meaningful collected formulaically (e.g. Muller et al 2011; Smith 2009). A life
account of life trajectories. Most attempts have either targeted: (1) trajectory implies a continual interaction of person and environ-
transversal designs following selected factors for usually specied ment in accordance with the construction of meaning and reality.
periods of time; (2) prospective and longitudinal studies which Unfortunately there is no current consensus on method or mea-
have targeted risk and resilience through examining the impact of sures appropriate for a trajectory approach in clinical psychology.
multiple separate predictor variables. At the same time, human Very few studies have specied trajectory as a continuous devel-
events occur in sequences, are often cumulative and require con- opment of a life sequence which necessarily involves transitions
ditional calculation of cascade risk. Apart from the obvious and turning points not always accessible to quantitative measures.
problem of separating cause and effect and disentangling prox- The study of life trajectories within clinical psychology is still in
imal from distal inuences, many questionnaire studies rely on its infancy. Whilst there are numerous studies in the risk-resilience
retrospective recall of general events which may be subsequently mode, in the main, they are dealing with static stage dependent
tainted by current experience. Also life events are transactional in variables, which, whilst contributing to pathological development,
nature and events require methods that address conditional do not capture the synergistic reciprocity essential to trajectories
probabilities and pathways. point of view. Indeed such longitudinal ndings, even whilst sta-
tistically robust in terms of signicant regression coefcients, could
be misleading. For example, isolated ndings of child impulsivity or
* Corresponding author. maternal depression as determinants of behavioural conduct dis-
E-mail addresses: kieron.oconnor@umontreal.ca (K. O'Connor), marie.robert@ orders, do not allow for the interactive effect that environment may
uqo.ca (M. Robert), guilheme.perodeau@uqo.ca (G. Perodeau), monique.seguin@
guin).
uqo.ca (M. Se
contribute to child characteristics and the subsequent interaction

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.06.001
0732-118X/ 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22 13

that child coping behaviour may exert on maternal response. In this family dysfunction, behavioural inhibition, genetics, negative life
sense, a key problem in cross-sectional risk studies is determining events and protective factors (Malkoff-Schwarts et al., 2000).
causality. An example here is the well established nding of a link The problems with this transversal approach are two fold:
between maternal over-protection and child anxiety (Turgeon, rstly, such a static picture failures to capture the synergy inter-
O'Connor, Marchand, & Freeston, 2002). Several authors pose the acting amongst distinct factors; secondly, in developmental terms,
question: Is this cause or effect? Does an over-protective mother one psychopathology (e.g., depression) may be the risk factor for
produce a timid child, or does the timid child invoke over- subsequent development problems. So ndings of contributions to
protection? Even longitudinal studies, if they follow only one or development even if proven strong predictors in regression equa-
two static variables, may miss out on this trajectorial philosophy. tions cannot easily translate into causal hypotheses. These studies
One answer may be to follow the evolution of episodes of have also mostly been termed back end research examining risk
overprotection. factors subsequent to an established diagnosis, rather than front
In an attempt to look at the temporal process relationship be- end research following factors in the development of pathology.
tween parental drinking, family function and child adjustment, This latter approach clearly has more implications for prevention.
Keller, Cummings, Davies, and Mitchell (2008) found the impact The aim of most front end studies is to improve predictions of
depended on parental gender, child gender, type of alcohol abuse. later diagnostic status, severity and treatment outcome. Again,
Interestingly, in a footnote, the authors note the possibility of developmental studies bring into question premature diagnoses
reversing the temporal order of the model where the child's where often early psychopathology is uid. For example, early life
behaviour predicts drinking and parental psychological problems, events seem linked to both depression and anxiety. The existence of
which suggests complex pathways. Again the ambivalence of such distinct trajectories may suggest the need for developmentally
pathways maybe best captured qualitatively. tailored intervention strategies independent of diagnosis. Devel-
The key debates here are how to structure multiple causal in- opmental research brings into question categorical diagnoses of, for
uences and whether equi-nality or multi-nality models are the example, anxiety and depression. In developmental terms, anxiety
most appropriate to explain multiple causal inuences. Finally, and depression may represent a common dimension of distress,
capturing the crucial inuence of life context in the form of namely negative reactivity. For example core internalizing prob-
determining life events will most likely remain a puzzle with ap- lems can be operationalized as the underlying common factor in
proaches which tend not to model dynamic interplay. anxiety and depression with two main pathways: the rst through
Latent curve analysis and growth modelling does offer a means temperament mediated by child emotionality mid-childhood; the
of quantifying trajectories by mapping sequential processes, but second through early contextual risk factors, namely family
again the statistical assumptions may not always make it appro- adversity before ve years of age. Whereas trajectory research
priate for human behaviour. However sophisticated the models, supports a transdiagnostic approach, such models do not currently
such designs may always be insufcient since they adhere to explain how multinality and divergent trajectories occur (Nolen-
inappropriate assumptions about human behaviour and human life Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011). Phillips, Hammen, Brennan, Najman,
events. Statistical sophistication is not necessarily the only solution and Bor (2005), for example, used logistic regression to predict
and a paradigm shift which changes the way we view life sequences group status from different early adversity, in a prospective
in terms of personal transitions and turning points can lead to exploration of the specicity of early childhood adversities as
adoption of more manageable approaches. A more person-centred, predictors of anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents. But
contextual approach to life trajectory methods is proposed for child adversities such as poor parenting, marital discord, mental
clinical psychology practice and research, including life history and illness and abuse, were predictive of a broad range of disorders.
life course narrative approaches. A person centred approach could Phillips et al. (2005) did nd that anxious adolescents experienced
accept from the start the necessity of conditional probability (if x more adversities than depressed ones. However, specicity remains
then y), and multidimensionality (e.g. thoughts and emotions in the elusive and as the authors note, the explanation for the association
same event) in trajectory research, and could consider a pattern of remains unclear. Also, the pathways to depression remain incon-
complex overlapping event spaces of life history rather than tem- clusive. The causes may be more proximal, or anxiety may be a
poral succession as the key metric for trajectories. precursor to depression, or there may be multiple pathways to
The new idea proposed here is that trajectory research begins depression. Furthermore, variables may be important at one age
with narratives or qualitative interviews which capture transition but not at another. Also, different variables may have a different
points and complex events which are then considered probable variance rate.
event spaces leading to nal endpoints within a Bayesian model of
alternative possibilities. Firstly we illustrate the difculties of 2.1. Individual differences in person-centred life trajectory
quantitative approaches, supported by selective examples from
research in anxiety and depression, in addressing: the interaction Smith (2009) considers the fundamental task is to understand
between temperament, life events and contextual interactions with why different individuals progress along different life trajectories,
specicity of pathways; the inuence of cognitive factors and and what forces increase the likelihood of successful versus non-
coping; how individual differences in sequencing and mediation successful life pathways. So trajectories should focus on person-
determine the importance of predictors and the impact of stage of centred rather than variable-centred approaches. In the person-
development on predictors. centred approach, the aim is to classify individuals into distinct
groups and categories based on individual response patterns. One
2. Methods attempt to overcome these dilemmas of non-linearity, differential
progression and person-centred approaches, has been the adoption
Transversal studies seek to identify specic precursors, risks and of growth mixture models. This can be accomplished using latent
resilience factors which relate to later development of psychopa- trajectory classes, which allow different groups of individuals to
thology. Such studies have focused on child temperament, trauma or vary around different means.
family environment as separate mechanisms. Periods of observation Traditional growth modeling techniques (for example, hierar-
are generally over short periods (5e7 yrs) and examining selected chical linear modeling) assume that participants belong to a single
explanatory risk factors (Luby, Belden, & Spitznagel, 2006) such as: homogeneous population and do not capture inter-individual or
14 K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22

intra-individual change over time. Finite growth mixture models distinct changes and variability over time.
are powerful models for mapping outcomes in apparently homo- Haviland, Nagin, Rosenbaum, and Tremblay (2008) conclude:
geneous groups which, however, differ in underlying processes. Making causal inferences with non-experimental data is fraught
There has been a move away from the interpretation of trajectory with ambiguity in almost all problem contexts. In developmental
groups as distinct entities and instead towards a view of trajectory studies much of this ambiguity takes a particular form, namely, the
groups as providing approximations for a more complex reality prior trajectory of the behaviour under study predicts both the
through the identication of clusters of individuals following tra- likelihood of the turning-point event and the future trajectory of
jectories over time. the behaviour (Pg 453). The prior trajectory of behaviour maybe
In not insisting on treating a single population, Latent Growth best revealed by examining person centred interactions with
Mixture Models (LGMM) can identify heterogeneous patterns of events.
responding (or trajectory classes) that represent diverse pop- A key factor inuencing future development may be coping
ulations (Muthe n, 2004). The mean growth curves of these distinct ability.
populations can then be modeled independently, allowing for wide
exibility and at the same time precision in locating the various 2.2. Coping trajectories
person-centred ways that people exhibit change across time. This
exibility results from simultaneous modeling of latent continuous Once psychological problems have developed, they may be
variables (e.g. trends) and latent categorical variables (trajectory maintained or resisted by different coping factors. For health re-
class assignment). In addition, LGMM considers other covariates searchers, resilience is an interactive concept which refers to the
that can either inuence or predict the formal trajectory classes, capacity for successful adaptation, the ability to rebound after
allowing it to distinguish people in different trajectory classes. encountering difculties (Rutter, 2006). It includes a sense of self-
However these studies reveal the difculties in trying to capture esteem or self-condence, humour and a problem-solving atti-
variables in person centred trajectories. tude (Connor & Davidson, 2003). A number of personal maintaining
For example studies using Growth Mixture Models (GMM) and psychological characteristics have been found to play an important
Latent Curve Analysis (LCA) have shown indeed that trajectories role, in particular, self-efcacy beliefs, cognitive distortions,
differ between individuals, some showing fast, some slow rises dysfunctional attributions, immature defence mechanisms, and
times in anxiety symptoms (Crocetti, Klimstra, Keijsers, Hale, & dysfunctional coping strategies (Carr, 1999). Coping strategies are
Meeus, 2009). Duchesne, Larose, Vitaro, and Tremblay (2010) assumed to be consciously and deliberately used methods for
identied four trajectory groups in childhood anxiety: low, low regulating negative emotions or to manage situations in which
increasing, high declining and high anxiety. M'Bailara, Cosnefroy, there is a perceived discrepancy between stressful demands and
Vieta, Scott, and Henry (2013) identied four trajectories of available resources. A distinction is made between problem- and
symptom change in bipolar disorder based on cognitive, emotional emotional-focused coping strategies or between functional and
and psychomotor assessment: including: persistent inhibition (no dysfunctional strategies (Fields & Prinz, 1997). Active ways of
recovery), transient inhibition (slow recovery), transient activation coping such as problem-solving, cognitive distraction, self-calming,
(linear recovery) and over-activation (fast recovery). The problem is and asking for help from others, contribute to better adaptation and
the stability of the categories which were independent of bipolar decrease depressive and anxiety symptoms, in contrast to the more
status and the assumption of randomly distributed variables. passive ways of coping such as avoidance and social isolation
Colman, Ploubidis, Wadsworth, Jones, and Croudace (2007) using (Compas, Malcarne, & Fondacaro, 1988).
LCA reported distinct proles of depressive and anxious symp- It is important to factor in the development and contribution of
tomatology over the life course and associated with neuro- strategies coping with adversity which requires autoregressive
developmental markers. However, the authors note LCA assumes modelling of sequential coping. An important element is recovery
independence of variables. style and whether people use covering up or sealing over,
Optimal individual matching analysis is required to explore avoidant strategies or adopting active integration of strategies for
complex life trajectories. This analysis enables researchers to take problem-solving. These strategies can change over time but sealing
into account the order, chronology and duration of states during a over has been identied as associated with negative self-evaluation
life trajectory and multiple life dimensions are combined with one and more insecure identity. Interestingly those with distinct coping
another using a multi-channel approach of optimal matching styles may be drawn to different services. Sealing over may lead to
(Gauthier, Widmer, & Dame, 2010). This methodological approach seeking low key non-confrontational services whereas approaches
is designed to reveal the overall structure associating various life may more likely seek services which actively engage the person
trajectories (e.g. co-residence, occupational and intimacy trajec- (Tait, Birchwood, & Trower, 2004).
tories) without inferring a causal order. This order is difcult to Parental behaviour and child rearing practices contribute to the
decide a priori, as those dimensions belong to interacting elds, child's development of a perception and appraisal of the life events
that is, phenomena that are interconnected rather than occurring and ways of coping with stressful situations (Kortlander, Kendall, &
independently (Gagadinho, Ritschard, Studer, & Mller, 2008). Panichelli-Mindel, 1997). Overprotective, anxious, or aggressive,
This technique allows trajectories nonetheless to be classied disapproving parents provoke low coping in children. Clinically
into distinct categories preserving individual information. Ge, depressed and anxious children use less efcient coping strategies
Lorenz, Conger, Elder, and Simons (1994), looking at the timing of that eventually increase their problem (Rapee, Schniering, &
distinct trajectories using LGMM, located the time point when a rise Hudson, 2009). There is evidence that authoritarian parenting is
in depressive symptoms in girls takes place. deRoon-Cassini, Rusch, signicantly associated with obsessional symptoms (Timpano,
Mancini, and Bonanno (2010) explored the individual trajectories of Keough, Mahaffey, Schmidt, & Abramowitz, 2010).
clients who do and do not develop post-traumatic stress disorder. Individual differences in coping interact with differences in
Nagin and Odgers (2010) reported on the use of group-based temperament and reactivity to stress.
trajectories permitting the identication of both group and
distinct trajectories. The group-based methodology is responsive to 2.3. Reactivity to stress
calls for the development of person-based approaches to devel-
opment. But such models still require averaging and aggregating The effects of life events are cumulative and interactive. The risk
K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22 15

and protection factors are not so much mediated or moderated by 2.4. Longitudinal and developmental psychopathology approaches
other variables as conditional on temperament, group membership,
stage of development, or attributes such as gender. The issue here is A few studies testify to the complex longitudinal relations be-
that the temperamental factors rather than discrete life events, tween temperamental and contextual risk factors and their effect
traumas, experiences determine the development of a reactivity to on, for example, internalizing symptoms. How to measure or test
stress which then may condition response to subsequent life events such complex interplay is a challenge when investigating medi-
and coping with such events, which may in turn protect from other ating, reciprocal, and dynamic relations between several variables.
problems. A variable that serves as a stressor in one relation might turn into a
Compas et al. (2006) present a model for the development of mediator in another relation. Different mediators have a shorter or
depression involving a complex series of relationships between life longer variation and time course. An exploratory approach to the
events, negative and positive affectivity and stress-related vari- relations between the specic variables would e when put together
ables, to the extent that coping strategies may be effective or not in one model e examine all relations across development at the
depending on the level of affectivity. This dependence, in turn, same time. Rutter, Kim-Cohen, and Maughan (2006) have pointed
depends on whether the person is female and excessively reactive out that there is a need for identifying mediating processes, insofar
to stressful events and whether anxious or depressive symptoms as we know too little to develop an all-encompassing model of
develop rst. In other words, the same events may produce distinct continuities in childhood and onwards. But it is doubtful if medi-
reactions, even opposite reactions and eventual pathologies, ation models can reveal the extent of conditional interaction. Psy-
depending on temperament, sex or age. But whether reactions chopathologies have multiple causes which interact with each
develop may depend crucially on trajectory, in the sense that an other and change over time (Smith, 2009). Researchers have moved
interacting series of previous events determine whether a nal away from discrete factors to look at transitional processes like
event or diagnosis arrives. tolerance of adversity, and to examine dynamic aspects such as
According to the tripartite model, high negative affectivity (NA), non-linear trajectory curves.
dened as unpleasurable arousal with elevated subjective distress, Ge, Natsuaki, and Conger (2006) suggest longitudinal pathways
is a non-specic factor related to both anxiety and depression. and trajectories take a curvilinear pattern but this varies depending
However, low positive affectivity (PA), characterized by a lack of on gender. The authors urge also considering age as a unit of
pleasurable engagement with the environment, a lack of sociability analysis. They note the importance of using interlocking trajec-
and enthusiasm is specically associated with depression. In a test tories of life events and symptoms. Their research raises the
of a tripartite model in the reactivity to stress approach, Fox, question of how life events should best be categorized. Conditional
Halpern, Ryan, and Lowe (2010) speculated that high NA may models are designed to explain individual variability by relating
relate to anxiety and physiological reactivity, whilst low PA may trajectories to multiple exploratory variables and selected cate-
relate to depression. In a second example, Compas, Connor-Smith, gories. Duchesne et al. (2010) attempted to address some of these
and Jaser (2004) presented a framework for the development of problems using a multinomial logistic regression and noted the
depression involving a complex series of relationships between presence of multiple trajectories of anxiety and the heterogeneous
temperament and stress-related variables. According to Compas nature of development.
et al., individual differences in temperament may facilitate or Attempts have combined discrete models with continuous ones,
restrict both automatic and voluntary responses to stress. Children in mixed models which allows creating discrete classes or groups,
with high NA, for instance, may be limited in the ability to regulate even when the distribution is continuous. Colman et al. (2007)
their emotions in response to stress, potentially leading to attempted to clarify life-course variations in the experience of
increased risk for depression. Even when youth do engage in coping anxiety and depression. Using a typological model, they identied
strategies, their impact on the development of depressive symp- six distinct longitudinal courses from 13 to 53 yrs. But such latent
toms may be a function of temperamental variation. For example, class analysis still assumes group response patterns underpin a
cognitively reframing a situation to focus on the positive may typological summary. The statistical modelling risks losing indi-
protect children with high PA from experiencing depression; hav- vidual developmental variation. Individual contextual factors may
ing low PA may interfere with the effectiveness of this coping determine vulnerability.
strategy and prevent children from reaping its protective benets. Several researchers have noted that there is a need to promote
However, individual differences depended on medium or high studies on contextual and relational factors which are person
levels of affectivity. Temperamental characteristics may moderate centred and how they mediate the environment with regard to
and be moderated by stress-sensitivity. The stress-sensitivity can child development (Luthar & Brown, 2007; Zucker, 2006). To
be operationalized either as NA reactivity or over reaction to small inform on risk and resilient pathways, it is important to focus on
daily hassles or stresses both of which may indicate a liability to- what makes individual children vulnerable. Are there sensitive
wards depression or anxiety. periods for a contextual impact on child mental health, and what
Although there may be a role for general underlying factors like are the mediating relations between different predictors?
stress-sensitivity exposure to life events may in turn contribute to Cummings, Davies, and Campbell (2000) described interacting risk
stress sensitivity. Wickers et al. (2009), for example, measured factors across different environments that mutually affect child
childhood adversity on the basis of a child trauma questionnaire. A adjustment, and specically adolescent depression. Family envi-
multi-level analysis took into account the variability associated ronment, child characteristics and everyday psychological func-
with each level of nesting, according to life events. There was a tioning mutually interacted and covaried with each other.
signicant association between daily stress and both childhood Finally emphasising the difculty pinpointing general factors in
adversity and negative life events. The effects of adult life adverse an interactive reciprocal eld, Karevold, Rysamb, Ystrom, and
event exposure were explained by previous childhood adversity Mathiesen (2009) looked at the specic developmental period
and stress sensitivity. The ndings are consistent with other where children are most exposed to risk from early infancy to early
studies emphasizing the contribution of early environment and adolescence. In other words, the authors examined both predictors
adult stress sensitivity. Progressively larger negative reactions are and pathways (Kendler, Gardner, & Prescott, 2002, 2006; Jo reskog
induced by stressors of similar magnitude, indicating a & So rbom, 1993). Outcome analysis comprised a higher order fac-
sensitization. tor model, including four observed variables (child- and parent-
16 K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22

reported indices of anxiety and depression), two rst-order factors sequential representation that each individual was experiencing at
(anxiety and depression), and a second order factor (core inter- each age. Stulz, Thase, Klein, Manber, and Crits-Christoph (2010)
nalizing problems). The predictor and mediator part of the model looked at symptom trajectories in those treated by cognitive
included all ve predictor variables observed at four time points. behaviour therapy (CBT) and pharmacotherapy and identied three
The basic model allowed cross-time, within-variable paths, to subgroups with different rates of recovery, depending on combi-
represent the stability of the variables. But no general risk factors nation of treatments and baseline anxiety. Stulz et al. used a latent
predicted child anxiety, although internalizing problems featured growth model to look at differential effects of treatment for chronic
as a general latent factor mediating other variables. Bayer, Hiscock, depression. The authors identied three distinct patient subgroups
Ukoumunne, Wake, and Price (2008) reported early temperamental who showed substantial heterogeneity in their treatment. Further
and environmental factors both contributed to later anxiety, and baseline characteristics permitted correct allocation of 61% of the
Mian, Wainwright, Briggs-Gowan, and Carter (2011) also reported sample to trajectory subgroups.
that temperament moderated environmental risks (such as The overall conclusions of these studies reveal that these are
depressed or anxious parent) in the later development of anxiety. different classes of trajectories, that the classes develop at different
The picture becomes even more complicated if we wish to look at rates, interact differentially with life events and may have different
complex issues such as development of identity (Crocetti et al., baseline characteristics. Focussing on specic variables and their
2009). interaction is essential. But these variables differ at different stages
and clinical psychologists are interested in the processes of devel-
2.5. Reciprocal perceptions on development opment, their degree and rates rather than an absolute event. It
often seems the very factors determining trajectories are not easily
Additionally in trajectory research, there may be different ac- identiable as variables but rather as personal transitions, changes
counts of life experiences from different sources. There is the child's and reverses in life. Turning points involve abrupt and substantial
perspective, the parents or authorities, and their interaction. One change from one state to another (Black, Holditch-Davis, & Miles,
theory that attempts to predict and explain major personality or 2009).
mental-health-related consequences with perceived parental Rutter, 1996 critically reviews the concept of turning points
acceptance and rejection is parental acceptance-rejection theory (apparent changes in early adult life of psychological life trajec-
(PART) conceptualized by Rohner, Rohner, and Rool (1984) and tories) in relation to developmental psychopathology. Turning
Rohner (1999). This aspect of the PART is viewed from two per- points have been associated with a wide range of experiences
spectives: from the subjective perception of a child (such as often including those over which the individual has no control and
presented in research) or parents and as objectively observable those subject to individual choice and actions. Changes in life
behaviour. Accepting parents are described as those who show trajectory may also reect genetic programming or internal bio-
their love and affection towards children and induced the child to logical alterations rather than external happenings. The author
feel loved and accepted. Rejecting parents in the PART are those concludes that turning points represent a heterogeneous range of
who dislike, disapprove of, or resent their children, which man- lasting changes in psychological functioning, and their investiga-
ifested in two principal ways: in the form of hostility and aggres- tion may shed light on developmental processes. Since personal
sion and in the form of parental indifference and neglect. turning points do not represent a simple mechanism, there is no
Vuli
c-Prtori
c and Macuka (2006), for example, compared the particular process responsible for them. They are nevertheless
interaction of contextual (perception of family) and personal crucial for an understanding of developmental continuities and
(coping strategies) factors. Different conceptions of the associations discontinuities.
between the child's early temperament, the family environment One attempt to overcome this limitation on examining the
and subsequent internalizing problems have been reported. For processes of development in trajectory research involves identi-
instance, one view holds that parent and child factors are not fying personal experiences which may lead on to cognitive ap-
separable in parent reports such that parent reports would simply praisals and coping mechanisms. Careau, O'Connor, Turgeon, and
reect the parent-child relationship (Foreman & Henshaw, 2002). Freeston (2012), in a series of studies, have tried to model the
Difcult child temperament could inuence parents' response to relationship between child experiences and the development of
signals and vice versa. Another view recognizes the importance of beliefs and appraisals in adulthood, which may mediate obses-
both reciprocal and transactional relationships within families, sional symptomatology. In a more recent paper, Careau, O'Connor,
with the child's characteristics inuencing his or her own devel- Turgeon, and Freeston (2014) explored the Affect Infusion Model
opmental course as a function of the complex interplay between (AIM) as an integrative framework for understanding cognitive
the parents (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). reactivity on the basis of the preferential reliance on different levels
(or strategies) of processing and the conditions of their use. In order
2.6. Clinical psychological consideration to explore the dynamics of cognitive reactivity in obsessive
compulsive disorder (OCD), this study provided an analysis of the
The preceding review emphasized the importance of individual longitudinal, naturalistic, covariation between transient mood
differences, both in terms of multicausal and multinality path- states and appraisals in eight individuals with obsessional rumi-
ways. Such progressions inevitably involve different conditional nation. Participants completed diaries recording mood-states and
probabilistic accounts and identifying transition points and are appraisals over the course of a cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT)
multicontextual including not only immediate life and social con- for OCD. Based on the sequential analysis of conditional probability,
texts but cultural contexts. There has been a move away from contingency magnitude estimations of covariations revealed
standardization towards pluralisation of trajectories in mental important rates of cognitive reactivity in ve participants. Indi-
illness. Trajectories of individuals with severe psychiatric problems vidual case analyses and aggregated correlational analyses revealed
are diverse with different outcomes (Harding, 1988; Mller, Sapin, that the results of the covariation analyses may be closely related to
Gauthier, Orita, & Widmer, 2011). There are wide differences in the participants' neutralization proles. Careau et al. (2015) linked
pluralisation across countries, gender and class. Mapping rela- adoption of neutralizing strategies and information processing
tionship trajectories, for example, takes the form of a retrospective styles to the importance accorded to type of emotional input. So
survey in episode format which allows construction of a again reactions to one variable may chain on to behavioural
K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22 17

reactions which may feedback to emotional coping and stress (Stulz experience and adaptive context of the person. In a narrative, one is
et al., 2010). not concerned with veridical perception. Also narratives occupy
space in that the events make up a cohesive space rather than
2.7. Narrative additions to trajectory research constitute separate points spread over time.
There are many different types of narrative analysis, depending
We have noted the importance of the perception on stress, of on the aspect of the narrative to be captured (Riessman, 1993). From
personal evaluations of events and interactions, and the impor- a constructionist point of view the narrative serves several pur-
tance of perceived coping. Narrative approaches lead to a poses: it reveals how the person makes sense of events; and how he
constructionist approach rather than a realist approach which fo- or she has represented the event to themselves (through language
cuses on events and narratives that the person perceives as and metaphor); at the same time, it reveals the client's point of
important. Most importantly these narratives and events are view and is persuasive employing rhetoric and reason; and it
complex and dynamic. The person can recall with the aid of a life- contextualizes the problem in surrounding events (White &
history approach key events from childhood. A narrative approach Epstein, 1990).
ts well with a dynamic person-based unit of trajectory analysis, The main advantage of a narrative approach is that it does not
talking of events, transition phases, turning points and generally treat observations as stand-alone thoughts units, judged according
human rather than statistical signicance. Promising results have to their orientation bias or compared with veridical perception, nor
come from qualitative/narrative approaches. Such accounts have does it reduce them to products of hypothetical schemas imposing
specically targeted specic transitional steps in the attempts to an embedded core network of perceptions on reality. A narrative
understand the contextual aspects of psychological processes. approach grounds thoughts in the experience and adaptive context
de Guzman, Llantino, See, Villanueva, and Jung (2008), for of the person. The unit becomes the voice(s) of the person intro-
example, probed the life trajectories of aged working professors duced within a narrative, to account for the problem, discuss its
and charted points where the respondents felt successful, fullled development, elaborate on its implications or relate it to other as-
or anxious. Their self-esteem coincided with success of their stu- pects of life alongside other problems, non-problems and other
dents and contribution to academic environments. Thomas and events. Zimmerman and Dickerson (1994) note that narratives
Hall (2008) adopted a narrative approach to following the pace of evolve through time and are uid and capture recursive patterns.
a healing trajectory in survivors of child abuse. These authors The following abbreviated narrative describes a childhood
identied four types of redemption narratives associated with experience.
successful adaptation. Similarly, Se guin, Renaud, Lesage, Robert,
I was fooling around with my younger brother who I always
and Turecki (2011) used a life course method in suicide research
tried to protect. I think we were playing soldiers and he hit me
to quantify burden over time and identied two distinct life tra-
hard which was unexpected and I reacted badly and got very
jectories of early and late onset differentially exposed to adversity.
angry which surprised me. I guess it was the beginning of my
Robert, Se guin, and O'Connor (2013) examined pathways and
seeing that I couldn't always trust people!
transition points in post-adolescent adaptation and found that
emerging new experiences can redirect life trajectories.
Nelson (2010) adapted the life history calendar methodology Here the person describes a transition point with marked re-
(LHC) for qualitative assessment, using a semi-structured interview. actions and emotions which dene this experience which is likely
The LHC approach captures the processes of engagement and carried over to other similar situations. This event was distinct from
disengagement in activities at specic times. It looks at attitudes, other events where the person describes for example a sense of
aspirations, interpretations and explorations of life transitions and belongingness in a group and pleasant emotions.
major events that would not be captured by objective events. At the same time this repertoire of event spaces allows us to see
Constructive activity captures the role of human agency. The LHC how the person did not react but may have reacted given their
can be completed by interview or by the person. It is reliable and sensitivity to subtle changes in the episode context. Narrative lled
includes a matrix dealing with event timing and domains and event spaces may reveal a pattern of reactions and although event
listing key reference events (birth, marriage). Both calendar and spaces are mutually exclusive the spaces may overlap and contain
memories activate recall through multiple pathways: sequentially complex multimodal reactions such as perception, emotion,
within a life theme and across parallel themes involving simulta- behaviour, which leaves the space open to be dened by a complex
neous or sequential events. The LHC can be adapted to open ended theme rather than connecting isolated variables. So for example a
questioning and to elicit narratives. The qualitative approach co- series of experiences reporting difculties in relating and feelings
constructs time and event cues with the person, creates rapport and perceptions of isolation might be considered a theme of
and elicits in depth narratives. Because interviewers do not know alienation dened by person centred content and interactions. A
the difcult periods, allowing respondents to discuss their future episode may be likely to elicit a similar theme. So similar
sequence of events captures the narrative of emotionally sensitive themes carry over different events and represent one pattern of
events, physical stickers and markers on the calendars give a per- experience e possibly distinct from other patterns in the same
sonal visual sense to LHC. An extension of the narrative approach is repertoire. A select number of complex themes could typify this
the life course theory approach. The life course approach as well as person's reaction over a range of situations.
emphasizing trajectories on paths of change in the developmental In the case of a hoarder who hoards garbage and objects she
process (van Geert, 1994) also looks at the impact of transitions and nds in the street, she recalls a deprived childhood; her parents
the entry points of new states. separated when she was ve and she was passed around among
From a constructionist point of view, narratives reveal how a relatives. She felt abandoned and she lost contact with her siblings.
person makes sense of events and contextualizes events in terms of She was bullied at school because of her teeth and traumatized by a
the person's experience and permit various modalities to gain re. She was addicted to drugs, and in addition was diagnosed with
expression. Russell and van den Broek (1992) have noted that the anxiety sensitivity and borderline personality. She has trouble
structure of narratives require events, relations and contingencies. maintaining relationships, keeping a job. She avoids contact and
Narratives capture the transitions, reactions to events and the social activities but tends to lurch from one crisis to another. What
relational structure of events. Events are grounded in the is her trajectory? At the individual case level, what were her
18 K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22

turning points? She recalls her parents separating around her fth conditional portrait of personally signicant factors. Reactions in
birthday. She remembers distinctly the raised voices and feeling the event space are conditioned by the perception of the event but
she was to blame. She remembers being an outsider among rela- form a series of conditional probabilities of how the person may
tives, and she remembers the occasion when she had to ask for a react to similar events in the future.
present. She remembers several occasions when she felt excluded This event approach is distinct from a mixed method approach
from events or treated like a stranger. At the same time she where qualitative and quantitative are combined. A problem with
remembered specic incidents where she was good at coping and the mixed methods approach is that subjectivity and objectivity are
problem-solving and helping others out. not exclusive in the sense that quantied variables are often self-
The person repeats several complex narratives which she recalls report, but there is a tendency for numerical methods to take
from childhood. These form discrete event spaces which she per- precedence over narrative approaches. The idea of dividing
ceives as key events and transition points in her life trajectory. We research into separate categories and creating a mixed method, is
can see from the ow of events in Fig. 1 that whereas there are erroneous, not because methods should be kept separate but
several possibilities at the beginning, certain events and self- because they should not have been divided at the outset (Gorard,
perceptions seem enhanced over the years, with other positive or 2007).
resilient factors conditioning the progression and her reection on In the constructed event approach the only data are the con-
the problem. These are her perceptions and constructions of life structed events which may be complex or thematic. Some con-
events. A more objective account might seek to measure the nature structions repeat themselves whereas other possibilities may occur
and impact of specic events, and perhaps look at underlying once and are not afterwards reected in subsequent constructed
temperament. But the narrative life history approach provides events. For example, the person is able to relate a whole series of
event spaces and transition points revealed by the person to be events from different points in her life where she felt excluded.
important in their life history and forming a necessary complex and These events marked her because partly she was not expecting

right or

Fig. 1. Schema combining qualitative event spaces, critical incidents, transition points, expressed as conjunctions/dysjunctions leading onto to multiple nal pathways.
K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22 19

them but at the same time they conrmed her difculty with theme rather than another. The likelihood of a theme manifesting
people. The events were reported as separate transition points but itself in future situations could be updated on the basis of Bayesian
the events nally seemed to form a composite pattern revealing her type prior and posterior likelihood (see O'Connor, 2015). The
identity as a strong but excluded person needing to nd her own presence or absence of themes characterising one episode rather
well-protected space. Such an individual person centred trajectory than another could be dened by correspondence analysis
encompassing her emotions, perceptions and behaviour in the (Greenacre, 2007).
same event belies an analysis which treats interacting factors as The event space is complex and thematic and dened qualita-
isolated variables which does not capture their embeddings. tively as for example The child who felt abandoned in family
Consequently the current dominant discourse concerns are avoid- interaction but developed a forceful identity with good coping
ance of people, a love of objects, a perfectionism, a feeling of skills. This complex theme could form a latent category which
possession of her house, a sense of loss of people but a selective instead of isolated or general features may predict behaviours in
coping ability, which lead on to the various diagnosable problems future episodes. This experiential grouping could replace an ab-
of schizotypy, borderline personality, hoarding and some depres- stract reied grouping for group based trajectory methods. Other
sion. In her constructed trajectory of events, a psychotic, anxiety critical incidents can always be identied to support or nuance a
and other personality problems seem less likely. dominant theme surrounding an event until a complex narrative
From a variable centred point of view, there were a number of comprising, turning points, personal themes and constructed in-
cumulative events. But from an experiential qualitative point of cidents becomes the most likely event space leading to the nal
view, she remembers distinctly how her mother valued objects endpoint. This example is illustrated in Fig. 1. Elements of early
over her, how she was reprimanded severely for breaking an object, themes most likely reecting a current dominant theme are given
how she felt abandoned and decided that by increasing the number together with endpoints. The example is individual but content
of objects around her, she would feel security. She remembers the analysis could focus on the frequency of terms across discourses or
turning point at which she felt a profound feeling of security sur- the implied themes across discourses.
rounded by objects. She remembered feeling objects could not hurt Alternatively discourse analysis could reveal repeatable re-
her, they were solid, dependable. She remembers when at school lations between events, people and actions from similar trajec-
she knew she couldn't trust others. Her best friend betrayed her tories. So, for example, a discourse about the person feeling rejected
over some trivial event and she recalls this incident marked her in an adoption family relates her emotion of rejection over time but
view of humans. Some of these transition points came as revela- within a specic series of events: a party, an outing; a family get
tions in recounting her narrative, other points from gradual insight, together, where the same pattern of her being treated separately
and she was aware that the development of her resilience and risk and apart from others repeated itself. If we look at what seem to be
factors processes grew at different rates. Her sense of rejection and constructions of events which were problematic, we certainly nd
loss grew faster than her realization of her good coping skills. a pattern where she recalls emotions of being abandoned, isolated
Hence the coping tended to t around her identity as different and and events (physical rejection by others) and reactions (judging
rejected. The individual variation in experience becomes informa- herself abnormal) which we can nd in the current dominant
tion not easily quantiable but revealed in a qualitative narrative discourse (I feel detached and I avoid people and I love objects and
whose key events form independent thematic event spaces whose give them names and keep well inside my house). But when
likely contributions can then be calculated (see Table 1a). focussing again on related events, she remembers that a teacher
The personal event approach seems to overcome some of the once made her favourite, she shared experiences listening to music
limitations noted above amongst quantitative methods by being with others, the friend who betrayed her apologized and didn't
person-centred, respecting individual trajectories, and the timing want to lose her as a friend. Plus she recalled numerous instances of
of such trajectories, considering the complex interplay of processes her helping out people and being appreciated. In re-telling the
in anyone reaction. The question is to what extent can constructed constructed experience the person may be able to identify
events which are certainly a mixture of fact and ction predict neglected aspects of lived experience that are not available in the
future events? In fact, by their nature narratives lead to connect- dominant discourse but become available through the retelling of
edness of events over time. Stories involve the plotting of events events. This retelling could lead to a therapeutic repositioning
over time but yet the narratives themselves can be encapsulated in which might eventually lead to dislodging the dominant discourse
event spaces. Early events are constructed as an event space rather (see Table 1b).
than a series of discrete isolated time points interacting together.
Each narrative reporting a separate episode reveals a unique con- 3. Discussion
struction and positioning of events, but the pattern can be thematic
and represent systematic patterns for any individual. A select The literature reveals a bewildering array of variables for in-
number of complex themes could typify this person's reaction over clusion in trajectory models. Life events, stress, temperament are
a range of situations. The themes could be combined into a likeli- important but inherently these factors interact and produce
hood distribution where a future episode will likely induce one distinct cumulative effects at distinct stages and groups. Further,

Table 1a
Gives objective events and comparable subjective constructions of events which lead to the dominant discourse, but could lead to alternative constructions of events.

Age (yrs.) Objective events Constructed events

5 Parental separation Recalls incident of parents ghting I am to blame


5e10 Adopted by family passed from relative to relative I mage of being outside family events I am strange
Authorisation parenting Rigid life I must have rules
10e12 Bullied at school due to appearance Sense of betrayal over a friend revealing a secret I can't trust people
13 Trauma in re Sense of relief at surviving I am a survivor
14e16 Dependence on drugs Recourse to escape people I will live in my own world
18e20 Success at school, Professional qualications Sense of coping by self I have no need of others
21 Abandonment of people in favour of material success Sense of security and safety around objects I'm Ok with my belongings
20 K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22

Table 1b
The universe of events ts into a bounded event space where independent constructions coalesce into a thematic narrative.

Dominant construction of the world possible discourse Alternative

I see myself as a strong but abnormal person I see myself as strong and independent person
who is best not to trust others but stay close to although I need be careful with others, others
myself and belongings and organize myself with little can be helpful and I have projected from having
contact with others to upset me. friends and contacts as well as being true to myself.

In Table 1b past Independent constructions come together in the dominant discourse, but importantly they permit construction of a possible
alternative springing from the same past constructions which fuelled the dominant discourse. Where the dominant discourse is dysfunctional
an alternative needs to be rendered feasible and credible for the client.

the interaction is not passive but depends on the person's possible to reposition a dominant narrative through generating
perception and on the way he or she copes. It may depend on alternatives.
general sensitivities, learned experiences, subtle adversities, ex- Risk and resilience are not static and unchangeable but dynamic,
periences and self-beliefs. Is it not time to abandon the practice of adaptive and malleable. Different interventions may be successful
isolated variables as single effects and dene units rather as pro- at different ages and may involve cognitive and metacognitive
cesses, dening events, transition points, meaningful to the strategies for dealing with apparently unresolvable environmental
person? issues. Clinical thinking needs to go beyond simple facts and iso-
Most studies have adopted linear analysis of variance models, lated attributes towards thinking in terms of conditional and cu-
hierarchical modelling or latent curve analysis. While the models mulative probabilities and contextual factors. Achieving this may
underlying hierarchical modelling and latent curve analysis can not require advanced quantitative methods but rather more
reveal important communalities, they assume a population distri- discerning qualitative approaches detailing a pattern of event
bution of trajectories. Modelling individual differences requires spaces.
that assumptions be made about the distribution of trajectory pa- Narrative and other qualitative methods may be indispensable
rameters as a multivariate or multinomial distribution. to track psychological factors in the trajectory equation. What does
Other complications arise when we consider that the same the person rather than the experimenter see as a turning point or as
factor at distinct stages of development may have distinct effects a resilience factor? On the other hand charting the development of
and that the behaviour at one stage may itself be an outcome due to cognitive and psychological characteristics of a subtle concept such
prior experiences rather than a predictor itself. One interesting as identity requires mapping not only self-perceptions but peer
nding covering a number of pathologies is that up to a certain perceptions, parenting, and most of all, the person's role
point, precursors of anxiety, depression and psychosis seem to experience.
share a common thread of negative affect and life adversity. This An important mission in clinical psychology is to develop
raises the debate of multi-nality or common pathways. Are there comprehensive methods that explain how different individuals
many routes to the same problem? Or do all pathologies share a progress along different life trajectories. All this can be very
common pathway with bifurcation or transition points on the way, beguiling for the clinical psychologist faced with taking a patient's
whereby peculiar inuences create the climate for depression or history. Asking the client to tell me about yourself, in an open
psychosis? question, may seem satisfactorily non-judgemental, but is the
The question then is what methods are appropriate for a tra- person likely to represent key personal events in an interactive and
jectory analysis? One recourse has been to attempt further and systematic enough way to catch their cumulative impact? In
further sophistication of path analyses and LGMM to attempt to highlighting certain events, the person may already be positioning
map and weight multivariate contributing variables. However, themselves for the interview. There have been attempts to develop
there are problems with such approaches if the pathways include trajectory-based interviews (e.g. Se guin, DiManbro, & Chawky,
the use of nested designs, where variables are conceived a priori as 2010) but more specialized approaches are required where the
nested within each other and explorations ordered according to person recounts in detail life events and their reactions to them.
short-term, medium- or long-term contributions. However, most Although group-based latent models go some way to individu-
promising seem methods which adhere more strictly to the prin- alizing trajectories, they are still bound by statistical constraints,
ciple of trajectory, that is events which are sequential, dependent, where these assumptions can be met and the groups satisfactorily
non-linear, interactive and reciprocal. These statistical models dened according to meaningful clinical criteria, pathways and
eschew conventional variance assumptions and employ event- particularly treatment pathways can be mapped. But otherwise
based methods, likelihood or probit models to assess the co- recourse to qualitative and narrative approaches seems appropriate
occurrence of events. Narrative approaches may be useful here and constructed themes can form the basis for group based models.
since they t a stream of events through a time-based stream of We know retrospective recall is a biased affair subject to
consciousness, and permit at least a qualitative appreciation of the contextual mood and selective recall. The selective recall may
syntax of multi-level events and experiences. These methods dis- further be biased by complaint behaviour. However, reporting on
cussed here also highlight micro-management of life trajectories in concrete experiences and transitions and turning points is likely to
clinical practice. be more accurate than abstract recall. The life calendar method of
Already clinical psychology has introduced cognitive subtleties Nelson (2010) offers a key to anchoring recall in key landmark
into the reciprocal nature of trajectories. Other cognitive variables events, rather like squares on a game board. The person is likely to
might be meta-cognition, personality traits, beliefs, subtle action recall their detailed experiences around these key points. One could
tendencies. There is also evidence that we can change organization then calculate the most likely events that were concurrent with the
of our trajectories for therapeutic purposes. Palgi and Shmotkin turning point, including cognitive and emotional reactions to
(2010) in a study of holocaust survivors found adopting a time outside events, and identify the a posteriori likelihood of the early
trajectory, according to perceived nearness or farness of the event patterns reported as early transition points reecting current pat-
improves psychological health. As illustrated in Table 1b it may be terns (as illustrated in Fig. 1).
K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22 21

References Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (2002). Toward a comprehensive
developmental model for major depression in women. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 159, 1133e1145.
Bayer, J. K., Hiscock, H., Ukoumunne, O. C., Wake, M., & Price, A. (2008). Early
Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (2006). Toward a comprehensive
childhood aetiology of mental health problems: a longitudinal population-
developmental model for major depression in men. American Journal of Psy-
based study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(11), 1166e1174.
chiatry, 163, 115e124.
Black, B. P., Holditch-Davis, D., & Miles, M. S. (2009). Life course theory as a
Kortlander, E., Kendall, P. C., & Panichelli-Mindel, S. M. (1997). Maternal expecta-
framework to examine becoming a mother of a medically fragile preterm infant.
tions and attributions about coping in anxious children. Journal of Anxiety
Research in Nursing and Health, 32(1), 38e49.
Disorders, 11, 297e315.
Careau, Y., O'Connor, K., Turgeon, L., & Freeston, M. H. (2012). Childhood experiences
Luby, J. L., Belden, A. C., & Spitznagel, E. (2006). Risk factors for preschool depres-
and adult beliefs in OCD: evaluating a specic aetiological model. Journal of
sion: the mediating role of early stressful life events. Journal of Child Psychology
Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 26(3), 236e256.
and Psychiatry, 47, 1292e1298.
Careau, Y., O'Connor, K. P., Turgeon, L., & Freeston, M. H. (2014). Cognitive reactivity
Luthar, S. S., & Brown, P. J. (2007). Maximizing resilience through diverse levels of
in obsessional rumination: a naturalistic and longitudinal investigation. Journal
inquiry: prevailing paradigms, possibilities and priorities for the future.
of Experimental Psychopathology, 5(4), 425e456.
Development and Psychopathology, 19, 931e955.
Carr, A. (1999). The handbook of child and adolescent clinical psychology. London:
Malkoff-Schwarts, S., Frank, E., Anderson, B. P., Hlastala, S. A., Luther, J. F.,
Routledge.
Sherrill, J. T., et al. (2000). Social rhythm disruption and stressful life events in
Colman, I., Ploubidis, G. B., Wadsworth, M. E. J., Jones, P. B., & Croudace, T. J. (2007).
the onset of bipolar and unipolar episodes. Psychological Medicine, 30,
A longitudinal typology of symptoms of depression and anxiety over the life
1005e1016.
course. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 1265e1271.
M'Bailara, K., Cosnefroy, O., Vieta, E., Scott, J., & Henry, C. (2013). Group-based
Compas, B. E., Beckjord, E., Agocha, B., Sherman, M. L., Langrock, A., Grossman, C. I.,
trajectory modeling: a novel approach to examining symptom trajectories in
et al. (2006). Measurement of coping and stress responses in women with
acute bipolar episodes. Journal of Affective Disorders, 145, 36e41.
breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 15(12), 1038e1054.
Mian, N. D., Wainwright, L., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., & Carter, A. S. (2011). An ecological
Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J., & Jaser, S. S. (2004). Temperament, stress reactivity,
risk model for early childhood anxiety: the importance of early child symptoms
and coping: implications for depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal
and temperament. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 501e512.
of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 21e31.
Mller, N. S., Sapin, M., Gauthier, J.-A., Orita, A., & Widmer, E. D. (2011). Pluralized
Compas, B. E., Malcarne, V. L., & Fondacaro, K. M. (1988). Coping with stressful
life courses? An exploration of the life trajectories of individuals with psychi-
events in older children and young adolescents. Journal of Consulting and
atric disorders. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 58(3), 266e277.
Clinical Psychology, 56, 405e411.
Muthe n, B. (2004). Latent variable analysis: growth mixture modeling and related
Connor, K. M., & Davidson, J. R. (2003). Development of a new resilience scale: the
techniques for longitudinal data. In D. Kaplan (Ed.), Handbook of quantitative
Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC). Depression and Anxiety, 18(2),
methodology for the social sciences (pp. 345e368). Newbury Park, CA: Sage
76e82.
Publications.
Crocetti, E., Klimstra, T., Keijsers, L., Hale, W. W., III, & Meeus, W. (2009). Anxiety
Nagin, D. S., & Odgers, C. L. (2010). Group-based trajectory modeling in clinical
trajectories and identity development in adolescence: a ve-wave longitudinal
research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 109e138.
study. Journal of Youth Adolescence, 38, 839e849.
Nelson, I. A. (2010). From quantitative to qualitative: adapting the life history cal-
Cummings, E. M., Davies, P. T., & Campbell, S. B. (2000). Developmental psychopa-
endar method. Field Methods, 22(4), 413e428.
thology and family process. New York: Guilford Press.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2011). A heuristic for developing trans-
Duchesne, S., Larose, S., Vitaro, F., & Tremblay, R. E. (2010). Trajectories of anxiety in
diagnostic models of psychopathology. Explaining multinality and divergent
a population sample of children: clarifying the role of children's behavioural
trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 589e609.
characteristics and maternal parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 22,
O'Connor, K. P. (2015). A constructionist clinical psychology for cognitive behaviour
361e373.
therapy. London and New York: Routledge.
Fields, L., & Prinz, R. J. (1997). Coping and adjustment during childhood and
Palgi, Y., & Shmotkin, D. (2010). The predicament of time near the end of life: time
adolescence. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 937e976.
perspective trajectories of life satisfaction among the old-old. Aging & Mental
Foreman, D. M., & Henshaw, C. (2002). Objectivity and subjectivity in postnatally
Health, 14(5), 577.
depressed mothers' perceptions of their infants. Child Psychiatry Human
Phillips, N. K., Hammen, C. L., Brennan, P. A., Najman, J. M., & Bor, W. (2005). Early
Development, 32, 263e275.
adversity and the prospective prediction of depressive and anxiety disorders in
Fox, J. K., Halpern, L. F., Ryan, J. L., & Lowe, K. A. (2010). Stressful life events and the
adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(1), 13e24.
tripartite model: relations to anxiety and depression in adolescent females.
Rapee, M. M., Schniering, C. A., & Hudson, J. L. (2009). Anxiety disorders during
Journal of Adolescence, 33, 43e54.
childhood and adolescence: origins and treatment. Annual Review of Clinical
Gagadinho, A., Ritschard, G., Studer, M., & Mller, N. S. (2008). Mining sequence data
Psychology, 5, 335e365.
in R with the TraMineR package: A user's guide. Geneva: University of Geneva.
Riessman, C. K. (1993). Narrative analysis. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Gauthier, J.-A., Widmer, E., & Dame, C. (2010). Multichannel sequence analysis
Robert, M., Se guin, M., & O'Connor, K. (2013). Pathways to adulthood for foster care
applied to social science data. Sociological Methodology, 40(1), 1e28.
adolescents: integrating the insights of developmental psychology and social
van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development: Change between complexity
work. International Journal of Humanities and Social Science, 3(7), 132e140.
and chaos. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Rohner, R. P. (1999). Acceptance and rejection. In D. Levinson, J. Ponzetti, &
Ge, X., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Simons, R. L. (1994). Trajectories
P. Jorgensen (Eds.), Encyclopedia of human emotions (Vol. 1, pp. 6e14). New York:
of stressful life events and depressive symptoms during adolescence. Develop-
Macmillan Reference.
mental Psychology, 30(4), 467e483.
Rohner, E. C., Rohner, R. P., & Rool, S. (1984). Perceived parental acceptance-rejection
Ge, X., Natsuaki, M. N., & Conger, R. D. (2006). Trajectories of depressive symptoms
and children's reported behavioral disposition. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy-
and stressful life events among male and female adolescents in divorced and
chology, 11, 221e225.
nondivorced families. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 253e273.
deRoon-Cassini, T. A., Rusch, M. D., Mancini, A. D., & Bonanno, G. A. (2010). Psy-
Goodman, S. H., & Gotlib, I. (1999). Risk for psychopathology in the children of
chopathology and resilience following traumatic injury: a latent growth
depressed mothers: a developmental model for understanding mechanisms of
mixture model analysis. Rehabilitation Psychology, 55(1), 1e11.
transmission. Psychological Review, 106, 458e490.
Russel, R. L., & Van den Brock, P. (1992). Changing narrative schema in Psycho-
Gorard, S. (2007). Mixing methods is wrong: An everyday approach to educational
therapy. Psychotherapy, 29(3), 344e354.
justice. London: British Educational Research Association.
Rutter, M. (1996). Connections between child and adult psychopathology. European
Greenacre, M. (2007). Correspondence analysis in practice (2nd ed.). London:
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 5, 4e7.
Chapman & Hall/CRC.
Rutter, M. (2006). Implications of resilience concepts for scientic understanding.
de Guzman, A. B., Llantino, M. L. S., See, V. C. L., Villanueva, R. F. P., & Jung, Y. (2008).
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 1e12.
Horizons, not boundaries, are the language of retirement: sense of fullment,
Rutter, M., Kim-Cohen, J., & Maughan, B. (2006). Continuities and discontinuities in
fears, and life trajectories of faculty extendees in the Philippines. Educational
psychopathology between childhood and adult life. Journal of Child Psychology
Gerontology, 34(9), 749e762.
and Psychiatry, 47, 276e295.
Harding, C. M. (1988). Course types in schizophrenia: an analysis of European and
guin, M., DiManbro, M., & Chawky, N. (2010). Questionnaire sur les trajectoires de
Se
American studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 14, 633e643.
vie. Montre al: Que bec: Premiere Publication. January 2002.
Haviland, A., Nagin, D. S., Rosenbaum, P. R., & Tremblay, R. E. (2008). Combining
guin, M., Renaud, J., Lesage, A., Robert, M., & Turecki, G. (2011). Early adversity as
Se
group-based trajectory modeling and propensity score matching for causal
risk factor for suicide among youth and young adult suicide: a comparative
inferences in nonexperimental longitudinal data. Developmental Psychology, 44,
study of life trajectory. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(7), 863e870.
422e436.
rbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8 user's reference guide. Chicago: SSI. Smith, G. T. (2009). Why do different individuals progress along different life tra-
Joreskog, K. G., & So
jectories? Perspective in Psychological Sciences, 4(4), 415e421.
Karevold, E., Rysamb, Ystrom, E., & Mathiesen, K. S. (2009). Predictors and path-
Stulz, N., Thase, M. E., Klein, D. N., Manber, R., & Crits-Christoph, P. (2010). Differ-
ways from infancy to symptoms of anxiety and depression in early adolescence.
ential effects of treatments for chronic depression: a latent growth model
Developmental Psychology, 45, 1051e1060.
reanalysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78(3), 409e419.
Keller, P. S., Cummings, E. M., Davies, P. T., & Mitchell, P. M. (2008). Longitudinal
Tait, L., Birchwood, M., & Trower, P. (2004). Adapting to the challenge of psychosis:
relations between parental drinking problems, family functioning, and child
personal resilience and the use of sealing-over (avoidant) coping strategies.
adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 195e212.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 410e415.
22 K. O'Connor et al. / New Ideas in Psychology 39 (2015) 12e22

Thomas, S. P., & Hall, J. M. (2008). Life trajectories of female child abuse survivors White, M., & Epston, D. (1990). Narrative Means to Therapeutic Ends. New York:
thriving in adulthood. Qualitative Health Research, 18(2), 149e166. W.W.Norton.
Timpano, K. R., Keough, M. E., Mahaffey, B., Schmidt, N. B., & Abramowitz, J. (2010). Wickers, M., Schrijvers, D., Geschwind, N., Jocobs, N., Myin-Germeys, I., Thiery, E.,
Parenting and obsessive compulsive symptoms: implications of authoritarian et al. (2009). Mechanisms of gene-environment interactions in depression:
parenting. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 24(3), evidence that genes potentiate multiple sources of adversity. Psychological
151e164. Medicine, 39, 1077e1086.
Turgeon, L., O'Connor, K. P., Marchand, A., & Freeston, M. (2002). Recollections of Zimmerman, J. L., & Dickerson, V. C. (1994). Using a narrative metaphor: implica-
parent-child relationships in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and tions for theory and clinical practice. Family Process, 33, 233e245.
panic disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 1e7. Zucker, R. A. (2006). Alcohol use and the alcohol use disorders: a devel-
Vuli
c-Prtoric, A., & Macuka, I. (2006). Family and coping factors in the differentia- opmentalebiopsychosocial systems formulations covering the life course. In
tion of childhood anxiety and depression. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, D. Cicchetti, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (2nd ed., pp.
Research and Practice, 79, 199e214. 620e656). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai