Anda di halaman 1dari 19

Understanding Our Own Biology: The Relevance of

Auto-Biological Attributions for Mental Health


Katherine E. MacDuffie and Timothy J. Strauman, Department of Psychology and
Neuroscience, Duke University

As knowledge of the neurobiological basis of psy- disorders as a way to revolutionize how mental disor-
chopathology has advanced, public perceptions have ders are conceptualized, diagnosed, and treated (Wein-
shifted toward conceptualizing mental disorders as dis- berger & Goldberg, 2014). This shift in research
orders of biology. However, little is known about how priorities has been reflected in public opinion, with a
patients respond to biological information about their
majority of U.S. adults now believing that psychiatric
disorders like depression (67%) and schizophrenia
own disorders. We refer to such information as auto-
(86%) are caused by biological factors (Pescosolido
biologicaldescribing our own biological systems as a
et al., 2010). Moreover, consumer-focused messages,
component of our identity. Drawing on research from
such as depression is a chemical imbalance, originat-
attribution theory, we explore the potential for auto-
ing from pharmaceutical companies are now common-
biological information to shape how patients view place and have shaped public attitudes toward mental
themselves in relation to their disorders. We propose illness as well as the self-perceptions of individuals
an attributional framework for presenting auto-biologi- affected by it.
cal information in a way that encourages agency, rather Although originally expected to reduce social stigma
than destiny. We argue that this framework has the and blame, many biological messages about psychiatric
potential to change expectations and improve outcomes disorders have had the opposite effect. For example,
in the treatment of psychiatric disorders. when individuals endorse a brain cause for a psychiatric
Key words: attributions, beliefs, biology, depression, disorder like schizophrenia, they rate an affected indi-
intervention, psychopathology. [Clin Psychol Sci Prac vidual as more likely to be dangerous and less likely to
24: 5068, 2017] achieve symptom remission (Kvaale, Gottdiener, &
Haslam, 2013). Similar biases have been observed when
In the 25 years since the United States Congress a genetic cause for a mental disorder has been offered
declared the 1990s the Decade of the Brain, mental (Angermeyer, Holzinger, Carta, & Schomerus, 2011;
health researchers have increasingly accepted the notion Haslam & Kvaale, 2015; Kvaale et al., 2013). Of
that psychiatric disorders are fundamentally diseases of course, inadvertently worsening the stigma associated
the brain. Funding sources such as the National Insti- with these disorders is directly counter to the inten-
tute of Mental Health have incentivized the search for tions of the funding agencies and advocacy groups that
brain-based indicators and predictors of psychiatric have promoted biological messages. Those agencies and
groups operated under the assumption that providing
Address correspondence to Katherine E. MacDuffie, Duke information about the biological causes of mental illness
University, 417 Chapel Drive, Box 90086, Durham, NC would reduce the blame placed on those affected (Jones
27708. E-mail: kate.macduffie@duke.edu. & Mendell, 1999). In one sense, such messages have
had the intended effect: It does appear that public
doi:10.1111/cpsp.12188

2017 American Psychological Association. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc., on behalf of the American Psychological Association.
50 All rights reserved. For permissions, please email: permissions@wiley.com.
perceptions of blame for mental illness are reduced by example, that something as ethereal as mood can be
biological messages (Crisafulli, Von Holle, & Bulik, tied to something as mechanistic as the firing of neu-
2008; Lincoln, Arens, Berger, & Rief, 2008). How- rons? A conventional medical diagnosis like diabetes is
ever, other aspects of mental illness stigmasuch as linked to auto-biological beliefs, but in a less ambigu-
perceptions of dangerousness, desire for social distance, ous way. We have a clearly biological definition of the
and pessimism about changeare increased when disorder (dysfunctional insulin metabolism), and we can
members of the public endorse biological causes for be taught how our own behavior and treatment deci-
mental illness (Angermeyer et al., 2011; Haslam & sions interact with this biological signature. An individ-
Kvaale, 2015; Phelan, Yang, & Cruz-Rojas, 2006). ual diagnosed with depression, on the other hand, is
The majority of the research on the impact of bio- faced with a much more complex problem of attempt-
logical messages on public perceptions of mental disor- ing to understand how dysfunctional biology con-
ders has focused on lay beliefs about mental illnessor tributes to the subjective experience of emotional pain
folk psychiatry (Haslam, 2005). Studies have been (Kendler, 2005). This is a difficult problem for the sci-
conducted using national survey samples, undergraduate entist, clinician, and patient alike, and it deserves more
students, or online convenience samples, and have attention from the perspective of the individual who
assessed attitudes toward a fictional character with a must make treatment decisions based on his or her
particular disorder. The attitudes of those who are understanding of the disorder. From this auto-biologi-
actually suffering from mental illness have received far cal perspective, we examine the impact of the recent
less research attention (see Lebowitz, 2014, for a shift toward biological conceptualizations of mental dis-
review). This is a notable omission considering the orders on those who are affected by it most: individuals
proportion of the population affected, with one source with a psychiatric diagnosis.
estimating that 46% of U.S. adults will experience at
least one psychiatric disorder during their lifetime BIOLOGICAL BELIEFS AND SCIENTIFIC REALITIES
(Kessler, Berglund, et al., 2005). Consequently, when We begin our discussion with a brief review of the
we measure public beliefs about mental illness, what experimental consequences of introducing biological
we get is (almost) equally likely to reflect beliefs about information about mental disorders. A critical look at
ourselves as beliefs about others. the existing studies reveals that many of the biological
The term auto-biological, as we use it here, refers to messages that have been investigated are at best reduc-
ones knowledge, attitudes, or beliefs about ones own tionistic and at worst frankly inaccurate. When exam-
biological systems, whether measured at the level of ined alongside the scientific knowledge they attempt to
the gene, the neuron, the brain, or the entire organism. convey, the biological messages provided in many of
We hold that such information has the potential to these studies appear overly simplistic (and inadvertently
shape our personal identity by increasing our under- misleading).
standing of our own nature and history. After all, the
scientific reality that we are biological beings is separate Depression Is a Chemical Imbalance
from the degree to which we think of ourselves as The chemical imbalance explanation for depression
such. Individual differences in how people think about resulted from the discovery that drugs affecting levels
themselves in relation to their cells, genes, or organs of monoamines such as serotonin and norepinephrine
their auto-biologymay powerfully affect other aspects were effective in treating depression (Lacasse & Leo,
of self-directed thought and behavior, potentially even 2005). Direct-to-consumer advertising by companies
cycling back to influence the biology itself. marketing antidepressants that can correct the alleged
In this review, we consider the assertion that chemical imbalance has helped spread the message,
auto-biological beliefs may be particularly salient for along with articles in the popular press that have
individuals facing a disease state that is characterized by adopted the chemical imbalance terminology (Leo &
disordered subjective experience rather than simply dis- Lacasse, 2008). The result is that the chemical imbal-
ordered biology. How is one to understand, for ance explanation for depression is widely endorsed and

AUTO-BIOLOGICAL ATTRIBUTIONS  MACDUFFIE & STRAUMAN 51


yet scientifically distorted. For example, in a survey of total symptom remission (and note that efficacy rates
undergraduate students, 24% erroneously believed that for most psychotherapies are comparable; Amick et al.,
doctors can directly measure amounts of different brain 2015; DeRubeis et al., 2005; Elkin et al., 1989; Hollon
chemicals to diagnose depression, and 35% believed et al., 1992).
that doctors treat depression by adding or subtracting The current state of knowledge about the etiology
brain chemicals to find the right balance (France, Lysa- of depression suggests a model of causality that is far
ker, & Robinson, 2007). more complex, involving a dynamic interaction
The impact of the chemical imbalance message has between biological and psychological processes. The
been investigated experimentally. Undergraduates who behavioral link between stressful life events and depres-
reported previous or current depressive episodes were sion (Hammen, Marks, Mayol, & DeMayo, 1985) can
given a fictional Rapid Depression Test, which pur- now be explored mechanistically, and neuroscience
ported to measure neurotransmitter levels via cheek research has revealed the similarities between the
swab (Kemp, Lickel, & Deacon, 2014). Half of the stressed brain and the depressed brain. Inflammatory
participants were given test results that indicated their pathways that are triggered by chronic stress are also
depression was caused by a serotonin imbalance, and overactive in depression (Miller, Maletic, & Raison,
they were shown a bar graph depicting lower serotonin 2009), and chronic stress can lead to reduced neuroge-
levels compared to other neurotransmitters. Control nesis and cellular atrophy in the hippocampuschanges
participants were given test results depicting all neuro- that have been associated with depression in human
transmitters in the normative range. Participants in postmortem as well as animal studies (Saveanu &
the chemical imbalance group reported increased prog- Nemeroff, 2012). Antidepressant drugs may reverse
nostic pessimism and lower perceived ability to regulate some of these stress-induced changes (Pittenger &
negative mood states compared to the control group. Duman, 2008); for example, the downstream targets of
They also rated pharmacotherapy as more credible than antidepressant drugs can increase the activity of tran-
psychotherapy, confirming the finding that medication scription factors to induce neuroplastic changes at the
is considered the best option for treating a chemical level of the synapse. Thus, it appears that stress may
disorder (Deacon & Baird, 2009). lead to depression through mechanisms of altered
This specific investigation is a clear example of the molecular neuroplasticity, and antidepressant medica-
harm that can be caused by presenting incorrect, tions may work to reverse those changes. From the
reductionistic biological messages about mental disor- perspective of auto-biology, we imagine this conclu-
ders. Despite the ubiquity of the chemical imbalance sion may in fact hold more comfort for individuals
explanation in popular culture, we know from decades than the previously held notion that such drugs tem-
of scientific research that depression is not simply a porarily adjust relative levels of neurotransmitters.
chemical imbalance. Research on the mechanisms of
action of antidepressant medications has revealed that Mental Illness Is Genetic
these drugs affect brain plasticity and neural network The Human Genome Project completed the first full
dynamics and that medication efficacy (or lack thereof) sequence of the human genome in the early 2000s,
cannot be explained by simple local changes in brain coinciding with substantial growth of the field of
chemistry (Krishnan & Nestler, 2010). Recent work behavioral genetics and an expanded search for the
suggests that even the initially proposed mechanism of genes that underlie medical illnesses as well as psychi-
action of antidepressant medications was substantially atric disorders. Unfortunately, one-to-one mapping of
incorrect: These drugs do not work via acute increases genes and mental disorders has proved elusive and may
in monoamine transmission but rather operate over a be overly simplistic (Kendler, 2006). While the com-
longer timescale, affecting gene transcription factors in plexity of genetic contributions to psychopathology has
specific regions of the brain but not others (Pittenger been accepted by the scientific community, it has not
& Duman, 2008). And they do not work for everyone: been well communicated to the general public, with
In most clinical trials, around 50% of patients achieve the result that the public conception of psychiatric

52 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V24 N1, MARCH 2017


genetics is often frankly inaccurate. For example, 24% the serotonin transporter gene was shown to confer
of individuals surveyed in one study believed that the significantly greater vulnerability for depression only in
brain was the main location of genes in the body combination with exposure to stressful life events
(Lanie et al., 2004). (Caspi et al., 2003).
Genetic information is particularly challenging to A statistical geneenvironment interaction says noth-
communicate, as it is both scientifically complex and ing about the mechanism by which psychological pro-
ethically sensitive. Genetic explanations of behavior cesses or environmental factors affect the biological
have been said to appeal to the natural tendency to systems of some individuals but not others. One possi-
essentialize, or to categorize people and other living ble pathway by which such an interaction could occur
things based on what are perceived to be fundamental, is through epigenetics, which refers to modifications that
underlying natural characteristics (Haslam & Ernst, can alter gene function without changing the underly-
2002; Rothbart & Taylor, 1992). When a genetic ing DNA sequence (Dudley, Li, Kobor, Kippin, &
explanation for a particular behavior or outcome is Bredy, 2011; Tsankova, Renthal, Kumar, & Nestler,
evoked, it sets in motion a pattern of thinking called 2007). Modification of histonesthe structural proteins
genetic essentialism (Dar-Nimrod & Heine, 2011) that support strands of DNAcan affect whether or
through which a number of characteristics are automat- not a gene is ultimately transcribed into its end product
ically assigned: (a) immutability, that genetically caused (Sun, Kennedy, & Nestler, 2013). Similarly, the addi-
outcomes are inescapable and predetermined; (b) etio- tion of a methyl group to a cytosine residue in the
logical specificity, that genetic causes supersede all other DNA sequence can either enhance or silence gene
causes for behavior (e.g., environmental or psychologi- expression (Tsankova et al., 2007). Interestingly, these
cal causal factors); and (c) discreteness, that individuals in epigenetic tags (e.g., histone modification, methyla-
a genetic group are homogeneous and uniquely defined tion) will differ for DNA in different cells in different
by a shared characteristic. regions and can be induced or reversed in response to
The rigid categorization that results from genetic environmental events. For example, rat pups that
essentialism serves to exacerbate stigma against diag- receive frequent licking and grooming from their
nosed or marginalized groups (Dar-Nimrod & Heine, mothers in the first week of life show changes in the
2011) and has been implicated in the frequently DNA methylation of genes that regulate the stress
observed association between genetic explanations of response, making them less reactive to stress in adult-
mental illness and stigma (Haslam, 2011). For example, hood (Weaver et al., 2004). Thus, through epigenetics,
undergraduate participants tend to rate vignette charac- a stable, fixed DNA sequence can become a flexible,
ters as more dangerous and less likely to recover when adaptive system that is responsive to the environment
given a biogenetic explanation of their mental disor- (Sweatt, 2009).
ders, compared to environmental or psychosocial How exactly all these pieces fit togetherstress-
explanations (Bennett, Thirlaway, & Murray, 2008; induced changes in neuroplasticity, individual differ-
Boysen & Gabreski, 2012; Lincoln et al., 2008; Walker ences in stress sensitivity, and experience-dependent
& Read, 2002). epigenetic modificationis not yet clear. Nonetheless,
Inarguably, there is a genetic component to disor- it is evident from this cursory review that no single
ders like schizophrenia and depression. However, the conclusion can be drawn about what is the biological
statement mental illness is genetic obscures the criti- nature of a disorder like depression. Clearly, biological
cal observation that research in psychiatric genetics has processes are involved in the onset and maintenance of
revealed no simple genedisorder associations for major the disorder, but no single biological explanation can
mental illnesses. Instead, a complex interplay of devel- entirely account for what it means to be clinically
opmental, environmental, genetic, and epigenetic fac- depressed. Instead, from the convergence of genetics,
tors confers relative risk or resilience for disease. In one neuroscience, and psychology emerges a more com-
well-publicized example of a geneenvironment inter- plete picture of the biology of depression, one that
action, the short allele for the promoter region of stands in stark contrast to the most widespread beliefs

AUTO-BIOLOGICAL ATTRIBUTIONS  MACDUFFIE & STRAUMAN 53


about the nature of the disorder. This discrepancy is existing research has been largely atheoretical, leaving
unfortunate because a more dynamic view of the biol- the reader with the conclusion that biological messages
ogy of depression is arguably more optimisticwe are often harmful, but with little sense of why this
know that depression is responsive to various treat- might occur or what could be done to modify the
ments, subject to environmental influences, and not effects. At this point, the question of whether or not
genetically inevitable. biological information should be communicated to
At least one research group recently attempted to patients is immaterial. Biological conceptualizations are
measure the influence of a more nuanced biological already part of our national dialogue about mental
message on attitudes about depression. Lebowitz and health and have influenced beliefs in a majority of the
colleagues (2013) recruited a sample of individuals via U.S. population (Pescosolido et al., 2010; and note that
Amazons Mechanical Turk who scored in the clini- these figures do not reflect any further changes occur-
cally significant symptom range on the Beck Depres- ring in the past six years). Thus, given that dissemina-
sion Inventory (BDI 16). Half of the participants tion has already occurred, there is an urgent need to
watched a short video explaining environmental effects clarify what meaning an individual might subscribe to
on gene expression (i.e., epigenetics) and emphasized auto-biological informationespecially within the con-
that brain chemistry is malleable, whereas the other text of a psychiatric diagnosisand what psychological
half learned that depression is a biological illness that mechanisms might be harnessed to improve the way
is genetically inherited and associated with structural biological information is delivered to patients to miti-
brain differences. Encouragingly, participants in the gate any potential negative impacts.
malleable condition reported a greater sense of agency The focus of this review will now shift toward con-
with regard to dealing with their symptoms than those sidering depression in particular, although we are con-
in the biological illness condition. In a subsequent fident that much of the following discussion could be
replication sample, similar effects were shown both applied trans-diagnostically. Depression is the most
immediately and at six-week follow-up (Lebowitz & common mood disorder, affecting 16.6% of U.S. adults
Ahn, 2015). In contrast to the studies previously at some point in their lifetime, and is the leading cause
reviewed (which compared the effects of biological of disability for individuals aged 1645 (Kessler, Chiu,
versus nonbiological messages), this study was the first et al., 2005; World Health Organization, 2008).
to manipulate the content of the biological message, in Beyond its prevalence in the population, depression is
an attempt to move from simplistic biological descrip- a suitable target disorder for the current discussion for
tors (i.e., genetic disease or chemical imbalance) to two reasons. First, in contrast to disorders that are char-
a more nuanced explanation of the dynamic interplay acterized by psychotic symptoms (e.g., schizophrenia),
between environmental influences and psychopathol- public opinion of the etiology of depression is less
ogy. It may provide a useful starting point for interven- exclusively biological and tends instead to incorporate a
tions that aim to introduce more realistic, complex, number of different causal factors including stress, rela-
and arguably more adaptive messages about the biology tional difficulties, and personality variables (Schomerus
of mental illness. et al., 2012). Second, patient beliefs and expectations
play a powerful role in the treatment of depression,
ATTRIBUTION AS A CANDIDATE PSYCHOLOGICAL MECHANISM with placebo effects in therapy and drug trials account-
Having reviewed the existing literature on biological ing for as much as 30% of treatment response (Ruther-
beliefs about mental illness, we now move to a discus- ford, Wager, & Roose, 2010). Given that expectations
sion of the psychological processes that could mediate for improvement appear to be an active ingredient in
the impact of beliefs about the biology of mental illness the treatment of depression, meta-cognitive beliefs
on individuals with a disorder. As stated, research on about the disorder and its biology may be particularly
the consequences of such beliefs for affected individuals influential.
is limited, and results from existing studies are mostly Indeed, beliefs about the etiology of depression may
discouraging (Lebowitz, 2014). Furthermore, the affect an individuals tendency to both seek and engage

54 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V24 N1, MARCH 2017


in treatment. Endorsing a biological cause for depression Internal Versus External
has been associated with greater intent to accept a diag- Early attribution research focused on the basic distinc-
nosis among individuals who were experiencing depres- tion between attributing ones own behavior to inter-
sive symptoms (Van Voorhees et al., 2005). Patients nal or external causes (Heider, 1958). Experimental
with a diagnosis are more likely to choose a treatment paradigms using pill placebos provided an opportunity
that matches their etiological beliefs (Schweizer et al., to study the effects of attributing ones behavior (e.g.,
2010), and assigned treatments that are congruent with arousal level) to an internal cause (anxiety) or an exter-
a patients etiology beliefs are perceived as more likely nal cause (a pill). Several studies found benefits of
to be helpful (Iselin & Addis, 2003). And while two attributing arousal to external sources: For example,
studies have shown that preexisting etiological beliefs do Nisbett and Schachter (1966) found that participants
not influence treatment outcomes (Dunlop et al., 2012; who attributed a physiological fear reaction to at pla-
Leykin, DeRubeis, Shelton, & Amsterdam, 2007), one cebo, rather than the upcoming threat of shock, were
study suggested that patients who endorsed biological able to tolerate higher levels of shock and reported less
beliefs at baseline were more likely to respond to pain than participants who attributed their reaction to
antidepressant medication than those who did not an internal fear state. In a study with chronic insomni-
(Sullivan et al., 2003). Etiology beliefs also appear to be acs, Storms and Nisbett (1970) found that participants
shaped by treatment experiences: After successfully who expected to be aroused by a placebo taken a bed-
completing a treatment, patients are more likely to time had an easier time falling asleep compared to par-
endorse beliefs that are in-line with the treatment ticipants who expected to be relaxed by the pill (and
modality they received (Leykin et al., 2007). Thus, thus attributed their continued wakefulness to their
while it appears that beliefs about depression etiology own internal anxiety).
may be influential, we still have very little understand- These early misattribution studies concluded that
ing of the mechanism by which such beliefs might affect attributing a physiological state (arousal) to an external
an individuals engagement or success in treatment. source (drug) frees the individual from making internal
Our search for candidate psychological mechanisms attributions that could be distressing, such as I am afraid
drew us to the classic literature on attribution. Attribu- of this shock or My thoughts make it impossible to fall
tion is the process by which we infer cause-and-effect asleep. However, if generalization of a behavior to a
relationships in an attempt to explain behavior. broader context is desired, then it is better to attribute
Whether applied to our own behavior or the behavior that behavior to an internal as opposed to an external
of others, attributions are considered an aspect of factor. For example, Davison and Valins (1969) led par-
nave psychology (Heider, 1958) because they exist ticipants to believe that they had taken a vitamin that
naturally in the minds of laypeople and are distinct conferred skin insensitivity prior to receiving electric
from philosophical or academic explanations of behav- shocks; half of the participants were then debriefed and
ior. The attribution literature provides an appropriate told the truth about the vitamin, leading them to think
theoretical background for the present discussion that any increase in their shock tolerance was attributable
because it directly addresses the question: What is the to internal factors. These internal attributions allowed
impact of beliefs about causality on behavior? In pur- the debriefed group to tolerate higher levels of shock in
suit of this question, we drew relevant examples from a delayed posttest compared to the group who attributed
the attribution literature of attempts to alter individuals their earlier tolerance to the vitamin. This study makes
attributions about their own behavior along three pri- the simple yet crucial point that attributing a positive
mary dimensions: internal/external, stable/unstable, and outcome to an external source creates a reliance on that
controllable/uncontrollable. While examples in this source (in the same way that a patient on medication
section are drawn from disparate psychological litera- may become reliant on that medication for continued
tures, we illustrate how the same principles can be symptom management). Internal attributions, in con-
meaningfully applied to understanding how individuals trast, can persist beyond any single environmental con-
account for the causes of their own mental disorders. text and have more potential for generalizability.

AUTO-BIOLOGICAL ATTRIBUTIONS  MACDUFFIE & STRAUMAN 55


Extending the clinical implications of these early of junior high school. The incremental group reported
attribution studies to the diagnosis and treatment of increased motivation in math class and showed a rever-
depression, the extent to which an individual attributes sal of the normative decline in math grades that tends
his or her depression to internal versus external factors to occur during that developmental period. The same
may matter a great deal. For example, attributing effect appears to hold for students in higher education:
depression to a genetic risk could be viewed as external Wilson and Linville found that college freshmen who
(it was handed down to me by my parents), whereas were told that grades tend to improve over the four
attributing it to a personality factor could be viewed as years in college improved their academic performance
internal (there is something wrong with me). and had lower dropout rates compared to a group that
Patients may take comfort in an external attribution, in was not given any information about GPA trends
this case, as it could remove a sense of responsibility or (Wilson & Linville, 1982, 1985).
self-blame for the depression etiology. In turn, the The positive academic outcomes associated with
internal versus external distinction could be relevant to these brief attributional interventions speak to the
a patients expectations for improvement both during power of the stable/unstable dimension for affecting
and after a course of treatment. Such expectations beliefs as well as future behavior. The two types of
would likely differ depending on whether symptom implicit theories discussed in Dwecks workincre-
improvement is believed to be caused by factors within mental versus entitycan be readily applied to biologi-
an individualparticularly changes in depressogenic cal beliefs about depression. An entity perspective
processes or characteristicsor by something external, might emphasize the immutability of genetic risk, the
such as medication. structural changes that are thought to occur in the
depressed brain, and the chemical imbalance that
Stable Versus Unstable underlies depressed mood. An incremental perspective,
A second attributional dimension, stable/unstable, has in contrast, might emphasize geneenvironment inter-
been investigated most thoroughly in the educational actions, functional brain changes that occur with
and social psychology literatures. In the domain of aca- depression, and the potential for psychological as well
demic achievement, a students belief in the stability of as pharmacological treatments to reverse those changes.
his or her performance across time has clear implica- These are initial hypotheses at the moment, as to our
tions for sustaining motivation following success or fail- knowledge implicit beliefs about the biology of depres-
ure. For example, Dweck (1975) concluded that sion have yet to be measured systematically. Neverthe-
exposing students to failure and attributing it to lack of less, the recent research investigating the effects of a
effortan unstable factor that can change across time malleable message about the biology of depression
improved their performance compared to a group of (Lebowitz & Ahn, 2015; Lebowitz et al., 2013) sug-
students who were exposed to only success feedback. gests that this perspective has promise, and provides
Blackwell, Trzesniewski, and Dweck (2007) later inves- important evidence that shifting from an entity to an
tigated beliefs about the stability of intelligence as an incremental perspective on the biology of depression
individual difference: Entity theorists believe that intel- could have an impact on a patients expectations for
ligence is an unchangeable, fixed quantity that individ- improvement and intention to seek treatment.
uals possess to various degrees, whereas incremental
theorists believe that intelligence is malleable and cap- Controllable Versus Uncontrollable
able of being developed over time. In a large-scale, The final attributional dimensioncontrollable/uncon-
school-based intervention, the authors gave one group trollablehas been explored in the clinical as well as
of students an incremental message (that learning the social psychological literatures. Evidence from both
changes the brain by forming new connections and that fields suggests that attributing our behavior to factors
students are in charge of this process; Blackwell et al., within versus outside our control has dramatically dif-
2007, p. 254) and followed them, along with a control ferent implications for future behavior. The learned
group, through the acadmically crucial transition years helplessness model of depression (Abramson, Seligman,

56 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V24 N1, MARCH 2017


& Teasdale, 1978), based on the original learned help- (see Abramson et al., 1978, for a related discussion of
lessness work with animals, asserted that helpless- personal helplessness).
nessthe belief that reinforcement is independent of According to Bandura (1977), the most persuasive
behavioris a core factor in the etiology and mainte- source of efficacy information is performance accom-
nance of depression. In an early experimental investiga- plishmentsmultiple success experiences accumulated
tion, Klein and Seligman (1976) induced learned over time. This point will likely resonate with behav-
helplessness in a group of healthy college students by ioral therapists, who tend to build in success experi-
subjecting them to inescapable aversive noise. Half of ences early in treatment to boost patient confidence
these participants (along with a group of depressed par- and encourage engagement (Maddux, 2009). Verbally
ticipants who were presumed to have already learned persuading someone of their own efficacy, in contrast,
helplessness) then underwent a therapy experience is easier to implement but much less effective (Bandura,
completing a set of solvable discrimination problems 1977). Building self-efficacy one way or another is par-
that was expected to correct the belief that responses ticularly important in the treatment of depression: Low
are not tied to outcomes. They found that completing levels of self-efficacy have been shown to be related to
solvable therapy problems reduced the escape deficits anxiety and depressive symptoms (Muris, 2002), and
associated with helplessness in depressed and nonde- depressed patients who report greater efficacy in their
pressed subjects alike. Thus, learned helplessness was ability to control negative cognitions at the end of
reversed through a therapeutic experience in which treatment are less likely to relapse (Kavanagh & Wil-
responses were reliably tied to reinforcement, thus con- son, 1989).
ferring controllability in a situation previously deemed From the reattribution studies reviewed here, it can
uncontrollable. Subsequent work has confirmed that be seen that modifying dysfunctional attributions has
learned helplessness (or hopelessness) appears to play a the potential to bring about lasting change. We pro-
causal role in the etiology of depression and suicidality pose that this same psychological mechanism could be
(Nock & Kazdin, 2002; Waszczuk, Coulson, Gregory, used to shape auto-biological attributions in such a way
& Eley, 2016), but is amenable to intervention (Hand- as to encourage a more adaptive stance toward the
ley et al., 2013). diagnosis and treatment of mental disorders. Indeed,
In contrast to the other two attributional dimen- the studies reviewed in this section have attempted to
sions, for which information-only interventions are correct maladaptive beliefs (e.g., intelligence is fixed;
effective in shifting peoples beliefs (e.g., from stable to Blackwell et al., 2007) and demonstrated that a change
unstable), the controllable/uncontrollable dimension in beliefs led to a change in behavior. The challenge
may require direct experience of controllability to going forward is to use these same basic strategies and
change beliefs and behavior. Evidence in support of techniques to correct the reductionist perspective that
this assertion can be found in the literature on self-effi- dominates public beliefs about depression and introduce
cacy. In outlining his theory, Bandura (1977) made a a more accurate and hopeful picture of the disorder
distinction between two types of expectancies: An out- (Kendler, 2005). For patients suffering from depression,
come expectancy describes the belief that an outcome encouraging a perspective that validates the subjective
is controllable, whereas an efficacy expectancy is the as well as the biological nature of depression is essen-
belief that one is capable of controlling the outcome. tial. Equally important are the beliefs and behaviors that
Banduras theory, in effect, suggests that two conditions may be impacted by changing a patients perspective:
are required for the controllability dimension to influ- treatment seeking, motivation, and expectations for
ence behavior: (a) The process must be controllable, recovery. The remainder of this article directly
and (b) the person must believe in his or her ability to addresses the challenge of designing and disseminating
control it. If condition B is not met, condition A is no more accurate biological messages and suggests an attri-
longer useful and could even result in additional shame butional framework for considering the auto-biology of
about ones personal inability to affect the outcome depression.

AUTO-BIOLOGICAL ATTRIBUTIONS  MACDUFFIE & STRAUMAN 57


TOWARD A FRAMEWORK FOR INTERVENTION just the way it is (Laegsgaard, Stamp, Hall, & Mors,
Imagine a conversation between a therapist and patient 2010, p. 475). Taken together, applying a retrospective
in which the patient seeks to understand the biological biological frame to depressionone that emphasizes
aspects of his or her depression. A variety of approaches the biological/genetic aspects of etiology and the
could be taken by the therapista focus could be uncontrollable nature of the onsetcould be helpful
placed on etiology (the biological origins of the disor- for reducing blame by shifting the attribution from an
der), on the patients current experience (the biological internal cause to an external one.
factors that influence mood), or on the potential for However, while retrospective messages may reduce
treatment (the biological basis of symptom reduction). blame, they may be less adaptive for coping with cur-
To better define these approaches, we suggest that to rent symptoms or motivating engagement in treatment.
focus on etiology is to apply a retrospective frame, The results of the existing experimental work on bio-
whereas to focus on symptom reduction is to apply a logical explanations clearly implicate the genetic essen-
prospective frame. A review of the biological messages tialism perspective, and particularly that biological
provided in existing experimental studies reveals that information framed retrospectively leads to assumptions
most of those messages would fit into the retrospective of immutability or, to use the attribution theory term,
category, by focusing on the presumed neurochemical stability. The work of Blackwell and colleagues (2007)
or genetic causes of a disorder (that, in any individual has shown the considerable psychological costs of such
case, cannot be determined after the fact). We identi- stable, or entity, theories for multiple behavioral out-
fied only two recent experiments (Lebowitz & Ahn, comes. On this point, the extant work on biological
2015; Lebowitz et al., 2013) that included examples of messages, with its essentialist implications, reveals its
prospective framinga mention of the role of epige- most fundamental weakness: By portraying biology as a
netics and neuroplasticity in depression. fixed entity, which confers immutable illness status on
Framing auto-biological information in retrospective some but not others, these messages actually violate a
versus prospective ways may have strikingly different key principle of systems biology, that is, the dynamic
consequences for beliefs about a disorder. Evidence for and nondeterministic nature of biological systems over
the affective consequences of a retrospective frame time.
comes from the stigma literature (Weiner, Perry, & Biology at the level of the organism is characterized
Magnusson, 1988). Stigma related to physical ailments by both stability and mutability. That is, biological sys-
(like cancer) is considered onset-uncontrollable and is tems are dynamic, responsive to the environment, and
associated with altruistic emotional reactionsincreased in many cases highly plastic over time (Bateson et al.,
pity, liking, and help-giving. Stigma related to mental 2004; Turrigiano & Nelson, 2004; West-Eberhard,
behavioral problems that are often, justifiably or not, 1989). There are few, if any, identified biological sys-
considered onset-controllable (like addiction) is associ- temsfrom the level of molecules all the way up to
ated with lack of pity, dislike, and anger. Introducing ecosystemsthat do not have some mechanism for
biological information about depression in a retrospec- adaptation to changes in the environment. Therefore,
tive frame may help shift the disorder in the eyes of paradoxically, the inclusion of a systems-based, dynamic
the patient and/or the perceiver from the onset-con- understanding of biology in framing mental illness may
trollable category to the onset-uncontrollable category, have the salutary effect of shifting beliefs away from
thus reducing blame. Indeed, the only aspect of stigma stability and immutability, and toward the unstable and
that is reliably reduced by biological information is changeable end of the attributional spectrum. A
blame (Haslam & Kvaale, 2015; Kvaale et al., 2013). prospective biological frame, which emphasizes the
This finding has been confirmed in qualitative inter- possibility of change, has potential as an interventional
views, where patients report experiencing a sense of tool to encourage adaptive coping and treatment seek-
relief once they are able to attribute the cause of their ing in those affected by mental illness. Such a frame
disorder to biology rather than their own failings: If it may be especially useful when applied to depression, a
is a genetic diseasewellthen it is not your fault, its disorder in which hopelessness is a symptom and for

58 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V24 N1, MARCH 2017


which treatment expectations can significantly enhance convinced of his or her capacity to exert control (Ban-
symptom reduction (Rutherford et al., 2010). dura, 1977). This insight requires an interventional
Framing the biology of depression retrospectively strategy that is both persuasive and convincingone
versus prospectively leads to distinct predicted patterns that provides accurate and comprehensible information
of attributions. The attributions that are predicted to along with compelling evidence of its credibility.
follow from each type of frame are depicted in
Figure 1. According to this model, a retrospective Changing the Message
frame leads to external attributions of depression, in Thus far, we have identified the gap between what we
which the disorder is due to my biology instead of know about the biology of mental illness and we have
me. A retrospective frame also leads to attributions what is communicated to those who are affected, and
of stability and uncontrollability, both of which lessen reviewed the existing literature, which suggests that the
personal responsibility but increase pessimism (and in biological explanations provided to date have been
the case of depression, hopelessness) regarding progno- mostly harmful. We explored attribution theory as a
sis. In contrast, a prospective frame encourages internal putative psychological mechanism for these effects and
attributions through a rejection of static brainmind proposed a framework for presenting messages about
dualism (i.e., my biology is me; Kendler, 2005). The biology that more accurately communicate scientific
affective consequences of an internal biological attribu- reality and have the potential to shape healthier auto-
tion are unclear; personal responsibility may increase, biological attributions. But what might such a message
which can be accompanied by guilt (but note that actually look like? How can science communicators
guilt, unlike shame, tends to motivate behavior, and so and clinicians distill the essential biological truths into a
may actually be helpful in the context of treatment message that conveys complexity, while at the same
seeking; Weiner, 1994). time being simple enough for a nonscientist to under-
A prospective frame also leads to the attribution of stand and benefit from?
instability, which is critical for increasing effort and Although the scientific literature on neuroplasticity
motivation in response to the possibility of change and epigenetics is rapidly growing, the technical nature
(Dweck, 1975), and controllability, or the assertion that of these literatures can be intimidating to nonexperts.
an individual can exercise at least some degree of con- Luckily, a number of institutes and organizations exist
trol over auto-biological systems. This framing should with the explicit goal of relaying scientific information
reduce feelings of hopelessness and encourage the active to general audiences. These groups have created com-
engagement of strategies to reduce discomfort (Abram- munication resources such as videos and interactive
son et al., 1978). Importantly, however, according to web content that are broadly understandable and yet
self-efficacy theory, merely knowing that a process is maintain their scientific integrity (a selection of such
controllable is not sufficient; an individual must also be resources can be found in Appendix S1). These

Figure 1. Predictive model depicting the attributional consequences of framing the biology of depression with a retrospective or prospective frame.

AUTO-BIOLOGICAL ATTRIBUTIONS  MACDUFFIE & STRAUMAN 59


resources are ready and available for dissemination, but the best metaphor for explaining the concept of child
can also serve as a model for investigators who want to mental health (Erard, Kendall-Taylor, Davey, et al.,
develop their own materials for communicating their 2010). Here, the term levelness is used as it is applied to
science to a broader audience. A motivated consumer furniture, like a table. A table that is not level is not
in search of readily digestible scientific content need optimally functional, in the same way that a child with
look no further than the work of these groups to find poor mental health cannot function optimally. A childs
comprehensible, accurate descriptions of epigenetics brain developing on a level floor, with supportive
and neuroplasticity. relationships, good nutrition, and health care, will
The overarching message that should be communi- function better than a brain that develops on a sloped
cated in a prospective biological frame is that experi- or slanted floor, characterized by abuse or violence,
ence can change the brain. Indeed, one could go even unreliable relationships, and poor resources. The
further and argue that brains are designed to be strengths of this metaphor are that it can be used to
responsive to experience (Belsky & Pluess, 2009; Kolb advocate for multiple pathways of intervention (fix the
& Whishaw, 1998). In clinical practice, this message floor, fix the table, or both), as well as the importance
can be individually tailored to the relevant experiences of intervening early (little wobbles can become big-
or concerns of an individual. For example, a patient ger wobbles later if not fixed during childhood).
who is hesitant to practice emotion regulation strategies Three principles of effective messaging can be
outside of a session could be provided with readily gleaned from the Frameworks approach. First, an effec-
understandable information about long-term potentia- tive message need not contain biological language to
tion and the importance of repetition to achieve lasting explain a biological concept. Frameworks projects use
neurobiological change (e.g., Bliss & Collingridge, nonbiological metaphors to ensure comprehension from
1993; Carr, Jadhav, & Frank, 2011). the broadest possible audience (Erard, Kendall-Taylor,
A clinician or communicator need not start from Davey, et al., 2010). Second, the approach to back-
scratch in designing an auto-biological message. An ground research and the criteria for selecting an effec-
excellent example of messaging research can be found tive metaphor will depend on whether the goal is to
in the work of the Frameworks Institute, a nonprofit introduce a new concept (e.g., epigenetics; Erard, Ken-
organization that focuses on reframing social and scien- dall-Taylor, Simon, et al., 2010) or rework an
tific scholarship for public consumption. While the entrenched, counterproductive set of ideas (e.g., depres-
Frameworks Institute has not (yet) focused their efforts sion is a chemical imbalance). Third, an effective meta-
directly on framing biological information about mental phor must be generative, meaning that it can be built
disorders for patients, projects they have conducted on upon and expanded by its users without losing its con-
early childhood development and child mental health ceptual utility (Erard, Kendall-Taylor, Davey, et al.,
are a rich source of examples and ideas for translating 2010). We strongly advocate for the use of a Frame-
complex science into simple, comprehensible messages works-type approach for designing effective metaphors
for public audiences (Davey, 2010). to communicate complex biological concepts in a way
The first step in the Frameworks Institutes strategy that can be readily understood and used by patients.
is to collect information about the currently dominant The institutes website (listed in Appendix S1) is an
thought patterns on a topic (e.g., child mental health) excellent source of research reports, metaphor examples,
by surveying the general public and comparing the and training modules for prospective messengers.
results to data collected from scientific experts on the
topic (Kendall-Taylor, 2012). Having identified the Message Dissemination
gaps in knowledge between the two groups, they Once an effective biological message has been
then design a simplifying model or metaphor that designed, the next challenge lies in its dissemination.
can be used to convey a concept to the public in a Messages about mental health tend to receive the
simple, easily comprehensible format. For example, the broadest dissemination through public service
simplifying model Levelness emerged from testing as announcements by national organizations (e.g.,

60 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V24 N1, MARCH 2017


National Institute of Mental Health [NIMH], National structure for delivering information about the biology
Alliance on Mental Illness [NAMI]). As evidence accu- of a disorder to patients in the early stages of treatment.
mulates for the importance of thoughtful communica- Through biologically informed psychoeducation, a
tion of biological information about mental disorders, dynamic, systems-oriented biological theory of dys-
it is our sincere hope that such organizations will refine function could be integrated into treatment from the
the messages that they publicly disseminate. Such beginning and could play an integral role in identifying
efforts should be based on empirical testing of prospec- symptoms, setting improvement goals, and establishing
tive messages (similar to the Frameworks approach) a treatment rationale.
with both patient and nonpatient samples. An example of an approach that already integrates
For researchers seeking to communicate their findings biological information is the psychoeducational sessions
to the general public, we recommend consulting the that are incorporated into cognitive behavioral therapy
educational resources provided in Appendix S1 for (CBT) for children with obsessive-compulsive disorder.
examples. In addition, care should be taken in communi- Using kid-friendly metaphors, symptoms are described
cating with the media and general audiences to avoid as brain hiccups and the brain as a worry com-
reductionist explanations of behavior (Miller, 2010). puter that sometimes sends the wrong signals at the
Investigations of media portrayals of neuroscientific wrong times (March & Mulle, 1998). For parents, a
research find that reductionistic, essentialist messages discussion of the treatment rationale is accompanied by
predominate, along with enthusiastic endorsements of positron emission tomography (PET) images showing a
the potential of novel therapeutic techniques that, in reduction in abnormal neural activation following a
reality, require years of further testing before they can be course of CBT. Other psychoeducational approaches
incorporated into the treatment of psychiatric disorders may not reference biology directly but incorporate
(Racine, Waldman, Rosenberg, & Illes, 2010). Portray- some of the illness attributions discussed here. For
ing such techniques as biological silver bullets dimin- example, in interpersonal therapy for depression (IPT;
ishes the role of existing psychological treatments and Weissman & Markowitz, 1994), patients are given a
reinforces the notion of mental illness as a brain disease diagnosis and assigned a sick role early in treatment,
that can only be repaired through biological interven- which functions to reduce self-blame, externalize the
tion. Researchers can help reverse this trend by carefully disorder, and reinforce both its episodic nature and the
attending to media portrayals of their work and guarding likelihood that treatment will be beneficial. Consistent
against exaggerated claims that could alter public expec- with the present model, patients in IPT also take
tations for the future of biological interventions. responsibility to overcome the sick role throughout the
For clinicians, we recommend incorporating more course of treatment, in effect shifting themselves from a
accurate biological messages directly into the existing retrospective frame to a prospective one.
structure of clinical interactions. Indeed, providing The environment of trust fostered in an effective
patients with information about their symptoms and therapy relationship may be an ideal setting for explor-
linking that information to practical management skills ing the meaning of auto-biological information for
is not a new approach; rather, these two components identity. In the same way that clinicians across
are the basis of psychoeducationa catch-all term that approaches (e.g., acceptance and commitment therapy,
describes didactic exchanges between therapist and Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 2012; dialectical behavior
patient (Goldman, 1988). Psychoeducation can be therapy, Linehan, 1993) seek to find a balance between
delivered as a stand-alone intervention or in conjunc- encouraging acceptance and promoting change, a clini-
tion with a course of individual or group psychother- cian can help a patient find a balance between accept-
apy, and recent approaches have extended beyond the ing auto-biological truths (e.g., depression runs in my
traditional informational model to include more active family) and considering the potential for auto-biological
skill instruction and problem solving (McFarlane, change (e.g., therapy can change the brain). Metaphor
Dixon, Lukens, & Lucksted, 2003). As such, the exist- is already considered a useful tool in psychotherapy
ing psychoeducational framework provides an ideal (Martin, Cummings, & Hallberg, 1992), and effective

AUTO-BIOLOGICAL ATTRIBUTIONS  MACDUFFIE & STRAUMAN 61


biological metaphors could be employed early in treat- disseminate better biological messages. However, for
ment and tailored to individual patients to maximize many patients, simply providing a specific message may
their utility. In attributional terms, a skilled clinician not be enough. As we have learned from self-efficacy
armed with an effective biological metaphor could theory, information-only interventions are less effective
employ both retrospective and prospective framing than those that provide information along with demon-
based on the current needs of a patient, with the goal stration or practical skill instruction (Bandura, 1977;
of shifting the patients attributions away from guilt or Schindler et al., 2015). To achieve true attributional
self-blame and toward agency and motivation for change, patients may need to learn through active
change. experience that biological processes are unstable and
Some therapists already integrate biological informa- controllable. How might we provide such a demon-
tion into their practice and do so according to their stration? Demonstrating neuroplasticity or changes in
own clinical experience and intuition rather than via a gene expression within the current mental health treat-
structured, systematized approach. This type of com- ment or research context is technically unfeasible, but
munication may have great value, but we suggest it be other proxy measures can be used to help people link
approached with caution. Indeed, clinicians in a survey behavior and biology. Biofeedback, whether it be
study reported less empathy toward fictional patients physiological via electrocardiogram (Lehrer et al.,
whose disorders were attributed to biology compared 2003) or neural via EEG (Sterman, 1996) or real-time
to psychosocial factors (Lebowitz & Ahn, 2014). This fMRI (deCharms, 2008), offers a more direct way to
worrying replication of the stigma findings in a sample show an individual how a thought or behavioral strat-
of clinicians suggests that biological essentialism may egy can impact a biological system. Real-time fMRI,
bias interactions even within the context of mental while still expensive and not widely available, has the
health treatment. Keeping this in mind, therapists advantage of anatomical localization compared to other
should approach discussions of biology with the same biofeedback approaches and has been successfully
empathy and compassion that they bring to other clini- applied to reduce discomfort in chronic pain patients
cally sensitive issues and carefully assess each patients (deCharms et al., 2005). Another technique with a
auto-biological beliefs for signs of self-blame or guilt controversial but growing evidence base is neurocogni-
that could undermine treatment progress. tive training, which purports to enhance general intelli-
The conclusion we hope the reader will draw from gence or working memory through daily practice of a
this article is that biological information has the poten- cognitive task (e.g., Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, &
tial to powerfully impact an individual and thus should Perrig, 2008; though see Melby-Lervag & Hulme,
be communicated with careful consideration and fore- 2013, for a critical review of such studies). Self-directed
thought. Systematic research is needed to guide the brain training programs have grown in popularity,
information that clinicians and other providers give and customers pay to access cognitive tasks designed to
patients about the biology of depression, and the faster train specific domains through repeated exposure. How
the science changes, the more urgent this need any purported neural benefits are demonstrated to con-
becomes. Different types of messagesretrospective or sumers, however, is unclear (above and beyond the
prospectiveserve different purposes and ultimately observable improvement in behavioral performance).
can be shaped to the needs of the patient. The idiosyn- A promising technique that could be harnessed as a
cratic nature of mental illness beliefs does not preclude intervention tool for attributional change is the use of
research on the topic or discourage the development of mobile devices and smartphones to deliver attribution-
a set of guidelines for psychoeducation and clinical relevant information in real time (Boschen & Casey,
communication more broadly. 2008; Luxton, McCann, Bush, Mishkind, & Reger,
2011). One could imagine an application that integrates
Techniques for Reattribution instruction and strategy use with some type of feedback
We encourage clinicians and communicators to draw from physiological data collected by a sensor. For
from current examples (Appendix S1) to design and example, an existing app called Stress Check uses the

62 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V24 N1, MARCH 2017


iPhone camera to detect heart ratethe developers of value. From an individual patients perspective, how-
the application have marketed it as a tool to help peo- ever, such reductionist explanations can be harmful
ple monitor and learn to control their reactions to increasing stigma against those with a diagnosis and
everyday stressors (Dickinson, 2011). A potential app leaving them with little sense of agency or hope for
that could be relevant for depression would be one that improvement.
tracked mood and provided feedback about the efficacy We are not proposing that biological explanations
of cognitive regulation strategies using heart rate or should be dismissed entirelynor would such a dis-
some other proxy physiological marker that is linked to missal be desirable or even possible, given the degree
emotional distress. of dissemination of biological information that has
A number of questions deserve to be considered already occurred. Rather, we propose that efforts be
when designing and testing such interventions. For made at the level of national agencies and organizations
example, what level of biological detail is most useful? (i.e., NIMH, NAMI) to design and disseminate simple
Does demonstrating a direct link to the brain (e.g., and yet accurate biological messages about disorders
real-time fMRI neurofeedback) confer more credibility such as depression, which could also counteract reduc-
than can be achieved with peripheral physiology alone tionist messages from the pharmaceutical industry.
(e.g., heart-rate biofeedback)? How should metaphor Simultaneously, we encourage clinicians and researchers
and demonstration be used together to communicate to present auto-biological information to patients and
auto-biological information, and what are the qualities participants in a way that empowers recovery and
of such a metaphor? Considering the unintentional encourages optimism. Drawing on research from attri-
harm wrought by the chemical imbalance explana- bution theory, we explored the potential for auto-
tion, which concepts should a new biological metaphor biological information to shape how patients view
emphasize and which should it avoid (e.g., dynamic themselves in relation to their disorders. In doing so,
rather than static, growth-oriented rather than disease- we built on previous work (Lebowitz, 2014) and
oriented)? While preliminary answers to these questions proposed a theory-based framework for presenting bio-
can be gleaned from existing work on message framing logical information in a way that encourages agency,
and biofeedback interventions, they deserve to be rather than destiny. By focusing on the active future
explored in the context of theoretically driven inter- (prospective) rather than the causal past (retrospective),
ventions that attempt to link the biology of depression we suggest that a systems-based, dynamic understanding
to the subjective experience and beliefs of individuals of the biology of mental illness can shift beliefs away
with the disorder. from stability, entity, and immutability, and toward the
unstable, incremental, and changeable end of the
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION attributional spectrum.
In this review, we have sought to articulate a challenge The initial model and predictions presented here
that is as yet unmet by current mental health research. await testing but hopefully have established a starting
What emerges from a review of the neuroscience liter- point for interventions that aim to change beliefs about
ature is a growing set of biological mechanisms that are the biology of psychological disorders. Whether deliv-
dynamically involved in initiating and maintaining the ered as laboratory-based microinterventions (Strauman
symptoms of mental disorders such as depression. No et al., 2012) or integrated into a broader psychoeduca-
single biological cause, and no one level of analysis, tional approach with patients, auto-biological messages
can adequately define such inherently subjective, brain- have the potential to powerfully shape the attitudes and
based disorders (Kendler, 2005). The complexity of behaviors of individuals struggling with mental illness.
defining the biological nature of disorders like depres- Such an individual is unlikely to seek or engage in treat-
sion poses substantial difficulty for researchers and the ment if he or she believes that the treatment is incapable
general public alike. Perhaps unsurprisingly, reduction- of correcting underlying biology. Therefore, dissemina-
ist explanations such as depression is a chemical imbal- tion of evidence-based psychological treatments should
ance have persisted despite their limited scientific be accompanied by a parallel effort to educate the public

AUTO-BIOLOGICAL ATTRIBUTIONS  MACDUFFIE & STRAUMAN 63


that experienceincluding psychotherapycan change Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S.
the brain. By portraying biological systems as responsive (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict
to experience rather than fixed, we can achieve scientific achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal
accuracy as well as convey optimism. We argue that a study and an intervention. Child Development, 78(1), 246
263. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x
realistic understanding of the role biology plays in mood
Bliss, T. V. P., & Collingridge, G. L. (1993). A synaptic model
and behavior can be harnessed to correct dysfunctional
of memory: Long-term potentiation in the hippocampus.
attributions and improve mental health outcomes for the
Nature, 361(6407), 3139. doi:10.1038/361031a0
patients we serve. Boschen, M. J., & Casey, L. M. (2008). The use of mobile
telephones as adjuncts to cognitive behavioral
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS psychotherapy. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,
This work was supported by a National Science Foundation 39, 546552. doi:10.1037/0735-7028.39.5.546
Graduate Research Fellowship awarded to K.E.M. The Boysen, G. A., & Gabreski, J. D. (2012). The effect of
authors wish to thank Mark Leary, Ahmad Hariri, and Alison combined etiological information on attitudes about
Adcock for their insightful comments on an earlier version of mental disorders associated with violent and nonviolent
the manuscript. behaviors. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 31, 852
877. doi:10.1521/jscp.2012.31.8.852
REFERENCES Carr, M. F., Jadhav, S. P., & Frank, L. M. (2011).
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Hippocampal replay in the awake state: A potential
Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval. National
reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87(1), 49. Neuroscience, 14, 147153. doi:10.1038/nn.2732
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49 Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I.
Amick, H. R., Gartlehner, G., Gaynes, B. N., Forneris, C., W., Harrington, H., . . . Poulton, R. (2003). Influence of
Asher, G. N., Morgan, L. C., . . . Bann, C. (2015). life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism
Comparative benefits and harms of second generation in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301(5631), 386389. doi:10.
antidepressants and cognitive behavioral therapies in initial 1126/science.1083968
treatment of major depressive disorder: Systematic review deCharms, C. R. (2008). Applications of real-time fMRI.
and meta-analysis. BMJ, 351, h6019. doi:10.1136/bmj Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 720729. doi:10.1038/
.h6019 nrn2414
Angermeyer, M. C., Holzinger, A., Carta, M. G., & deCharms, R. C., Maeda, F., Glover, G. H., Ludlow, D.,
Schomerus, G. (2011). Biogenetic explanations and public Pauly, J. M., Soneji, D., . . . Mackey, S. C. (2005).
acceptance of mental illness: Systematic review of Control over brain activation and pain learned by using
population studies. British Journal of Psychiatry, 199, 367 real-time functional MRI. Proceedings of the National
372. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.110.085563 Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(51),
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory 1862618631. doi:10.1073/pnas.0505210102
of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191215. Crisafulli, M. A., Von Holle, A., & Bulik, C. M. (2008).
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191 Attitudes towards anorexia nervosa: The impact of
Bateson, P., Barker, D., Clutton-Brock, T., Deb, D., framing on blame and stigma. International Journal of Eating
DUdine, B., Foley, R. A. . . . Sultan, S. E. (2004). Disorders, 41, 333339. doi:10.1002/eat.20507
Developmental plasticity and human health. Nature, 430 Dar-Nimrod, I., & Heine, S. J. (2011). Genetic essentialism:
(6998), 419421. doi:10.1038/nature02725 On the deceptive determinism of DNA. Psychological
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). The nature (and nurture?) of Bulletin, 137, 800818. doi:10.1037/a0021860
plasticity in early human development. Perspectives on Davey, L. (2010). How to talk about childrens mental health.
Psychological Science, 4, 345351. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924. Retrieved from http://frameworksinstitute.org/assets/files/
2009.01136.x CMH_MM.pdf
Bennett, L., Thirlaway, K., & Murray, A. J. (2008). The Davison, G. C., & Valins, S. (1969). Maintenance of self-
stigmatising implications of presenting schizophrenia as a attributed and drug-attributed behavior change. Journal of
genetic disease. Journal of Genetic Counseling, 17, 550559. Personality and Social Psychology, 11(1), 25. doi:10.1037/
doi:10.1007/s10897-008-9178-8 h0027055

64 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V24 N1, MARCH 2017


Deacon, B. J., & Baird, G. L. (2009). The chemical Goldman, C. R. (1988). Toward a definition of
imbalance explanation of depression: Reducing blame at psychoeducation. Psychiatric Services, 39, 666668. doi:10.
what cost? Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 415 1176/ps.39.6.666
435. doi:10.1521/jscp.2009.28.4.415 Hammen, C., Marks, T., Mayol, A., & DeMayo, R. (1985).
DeRubeis, R. J., Hollon, S. D., Amsterdam, J. D., Shelton, R. Depressive self-schemas, life stress, and vulnerability to
C., Young, P. R., Salomon, R. M., . . . Gallop, R. (2005). depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 308319.
Cognitive therapy vs. medications in the treatment of doi:10.1037/0021-843X.94.3.308
moderate to severe depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, Handley, T. E., Kay-Lambkin, F. J., Baker, A. L., Lewin, T.
62, 409416. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.4.409 J., Kelly, B. J., Inder, K. J., . . . Kavanagh, D. J. (2013).
Dickinson, B. (2011). Azumio app turns iIPhone into a stress Incidental treatment effects of CBT on suicidal ideation
gauge. Retrieved from http://www.cnet.com/news/ and hopelessness. Journal of Affective Disorders, 151, 275
azumio-app-turns-iphone-into-a-stress-gauge/ 283. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.005
Dudley, K. J., Li, X., Kobor, M. S., Kippin, T. E., & Bredy, Haslam, N. (2005). Dimensions of folk psychiatry. Review of
T. W. (2011). Epigenetic mechanisms mediating General Psychology, 9(1), 3547. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.
vulnerability and resilience to psychiatric disorders. 1.35
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 15441551. Haslam, N. (2011). Genetic essentialism, neuroessentialism,
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.016 and stigma: Commentary on Dar-Nimrod and Heine.
Dunlop, B. W., Kelley, M. E., Mletzko, T. C., Velasquez, Psychological Bulletin, 137, 819824. doi:10.1037/a0022386.
C. M., Craighead, W. E., & Mayberg, H. S. (2012). Haslam, N., & Ernst, D. (2002). Essentialist beliefs about
Depression beliefs, treatment preference, and outcomes in mental disorders. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
a randomized trial for major depressive disorder. Journal of 21, 628644. doi:10.1521/jscp.21.6.628.22793
Psychiatric Research, 46, 375381. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires Haslam, N., & Kvaale, E. P. (2015). Biogenetic explanations
.2011.11.003 of mental disorder: The mixed-blessings model. Current
Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 399404. doi:10
attributions in the alleviation of learned helplessness. .1177/0963721415588082
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(4), 674685. Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2012).
doi:10.1037/h0077149 Acceptance and commitment therapy: The process and practice of
Elkin, I., Shea, M. T., Watkins, J. T., Imber, S. D., Sotsky, mindful change (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
S. M., Collins, J. F., . . . Parloff, M. B. (1989). National Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New
Institute of Mental Health treatment of depression York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. doi:10.1037/10628-000
collaborative research program: General effectiveness of Hollon, S. D., DeRubeis, R. J., Evans, M. D., Wiemer, M. J.,
treatments. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(11), 971982. Garvey, M. J., Grove, W. M., & Tuason, V. B. (1992).
doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110013002 Cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy for depression:
Erard, M., Kendall-Taylor, N., Davey, L., & Simon, A. (2010). Singly and in combination. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49
The power of levelness: Making child mental health visible and (10), 774781. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820100018004
concrete through a simplifying model. Retrieved from http://fra Iselin, M.-G. V., & Addis, M. E. (2003). Effects of etiology
meworksinstitute.org/assets/files/The_Power_of_Levelness_ on perceived helpfulness of treatments for depression.
Child_Mental_Health_Simplifying_Models_Report.pdf Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 205222. doi:10.1023/
Erard, M., Kendall-Taylor, N., Simon, A., & Davey, L. A:1023513310243
(2010). More to genes than that: Designing metaphors to explain Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J.
epigenetics. Retrieved from http://www.frameworksinstitute. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on
org/assets/files/ECD/Epigenetics_Simplifying_Models%20 working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Report.pdf Sciences, 105, 68296833. doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105
France, C. M., Lysaker, P. H., & Robinson, R. P. (2007). Jones, E. G., & Mendell, L. M. (1999). Assessing the decade
The chemical imbalance explanation for depression: of the brain. Science, 284(5415), 739. doi:10.1126/science
Origins, lay endorsement, and clinical implications. .284.5415.739
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 38, 411420. Kavanagh, D. J., & Wilson, P. H. (1989). Prediction of
doi:10.1037/0735-7028.38.4.411 outcome with group cognitive therapy for depression.

AUTO-BIOLOGICAL ATTRIBUTIONS  MACDUFFIE & STRAUMAN 65


Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 333343. doi:10.1016/ Laegsgaard, M. M., Stamp, A. S., Hall, E., & Mors, O.
0005-7967(89)90003-X (2010). The perceived and predicted implications of
Kemp, J. J., Lickel, J. J., & Deacon, B. J. (2014). Effects of a psychiatric genetic knowledge among persons with
chemical imbalance causal explanation on individuals multiple cases of depression in the family. Acta Psychiatrica
perceptions of their depressive symptoms. Behaviour Scandinavica, 122, 470480. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447
Research and Therapy, 56, 4752. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2014 .2010.01555.x
.02.009 Lanie, A. D., Jayaratne, T. E., Sheldon, J. P., Kardia, S. L.,
Kendall-Taylor, N. (2012). Conflicting models of mind: Anderson, E. S., Feldbaum, M., & Petty, E. M. (2004).
Mapping the gaps between expert and public understandings Exploring the public understanding of basic genetic
of child mental health. Science Communication, 34, 695726. concepts. Journal of Genetic Counseling, 13, 305320.
doi:10.1177/1075547011429200 doi:10.1023/B:JOGC.0000035524.66944.6d
Kendler, K. S. (2005). Toward a philosophical structure for Lebowitz, M. S. (2014). Biological conceptualizations of
psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 433440. mental disorders among affected individuals: A review of
doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.3.433 correlates and consequences. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Kendler, K. (2006). Reflections on the relationship between Practice, 21, 6783. doi:10.1111/cpsp.12056
psychiatric genetics and psychiatric nosology. American Lebowitz, M. S., & Ahn, W. (2014). Effects of biological
Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 11381146. doi:10.1176/ajp explanations for mental disorders on clinicians empathy.
.2006.163.7.1138 Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(50),
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., 1778617790.
Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime Lebowitz, M. S., & Ahn, W.-K. (2015). Emphasizing
prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV malleability in the biology of depression: Durable effects
disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey on perceived agency and prognostic pessimism. Behaviour
Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593602. Research and Therapy, 71, 125130. doi:10.1016/j.brat
doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593 .2015.06.005
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., & Walters, E. E. Lebowitz, M. S., Ahn, W.-K., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S.
(2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12- (2013). Fixable or fate? Perceptions of the biology of
month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81,
Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 617 518527. doi:10.1037/a0031730
627. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617 Lehrer, P. M., Vaschillo, E., Vaschillo, B., Lu, S.-E.,
Klein, D. C., & Seligman, M. E. (1976). Reversal of Eckberg, D. L., Edelberg, R. . . . Hamer, R. M. (2003).
performance deficits and perceptual deficits in learned Heart rate variability biofeedback increases baroreflex gain
helplessness and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, and peak expiratory flow. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 796
85, 1126. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.85.1.11 805. doi:10.1097/01.PSY.0000089200.81962.19
Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1998). Brain plasticity and Leo, J., & Lacasse, J. R. (2008). The media and the chemical
behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 4364. doi:10 imbalance theory of depression. Society, 45(1), 3545.
.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.43 doi:10.1007/s12115-007-9047-3
Krishnan, V., & Nestler, E. J. (2010). Linking molecules to Leykin, Y., DeRubeis, R. J., Shelton, R. C., & Amsterdam,
mood: New insight into the biology of depression. J. D. (2007). Changes in patients beliefs about the causes
American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 13051320. doi:10 of their depression following successful treatment.
.1176/appi.ajp.2009.10030434 Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 437449. doi:10.1007/
Kvaale, E. P., Gottdiener, W. H., & Haslam, N. (2013). s10608-007-9130-5
Biogenetic explanations and stigma: A meta-analytic Lincoln, T. M., Arens, E., Berger, C., & Rief, W. (2008). Can
review of associations among laypeople. Social Science & antistigma campaigns be improved? A test of the impact of
Medicine, 96, 95103. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.07 biogenetic vs. psychosocial causal explanations on implicit
.017 and explicit attitudes to schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
Lacasse, J. R., & Leo, J. (2005). Serotonin and depression: A Bulletin, 34, 984994. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm131
disconnect between the advertisements and the scientific Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of
literature. PLoS Medicine, 2(12), e392. doi:10.1371/ borderline personality disorder. New York, NY: Guilford
journal.pmed.0020392 Press.

66 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V24 N1, MARCH 2017


Luxton, D. D., McCann, R. A., Bush, N. E., Mishkind, M. Phelan, J. C., Yang, L. H., & Cruz-Rojas, R. (2006). Effects
C., & Reger, G. M. (2011). mHealth for mental health: of attributing serious mental illnesses to genetic causes on
Integrating smartphone technology in behavioral orientations to treatment. Psychiatric Services, 57, 382387.
healthcare. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 42, doi:10.1176/appi.ps.57.3.382
505512. doi:/10.1037/a0024485 Pittenger, C., & Duman, R. S. (2008). Stress, depression, and
Maddux, J. E. (2009). Self-efficacy: The power of believing neuroplasticity: A convergence of mechanisms.
you can. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder (Eds.), Oxford Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 88109. doi:10.1038/sj.npp
handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 335343). New .1301574
York, NY: Oxford University Press. Racine, E., Waldman, S., Rosenberg, J., & Illes, J. (2010).
March, J. S., & Mulle, K. (1998). OCD in children and Contemporary neuroscience in the media. Social Science &
adolescents: A cognitive-behavioral treatment manual. New Medicine, 71, 725733. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.05.017
York, NY: Guilford Press. Rothbart, M., & Taylor, M. (1992). Category labels and
Martin, J., Cummings, A. L., & Hallberg, E. T. (1992). social reality: Do we view social categories as natural
Therapists intentional use of metaphor: Memorability, kinds? In G. R. Semin, K. Fiedler, G. R. Semin, & K.
clinical impact, and possible epistemic/motivational Fiedler (Eds.), Language, interaction and social cognition (pp.
functions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 1136). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
143145. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.60.1.143 Rutherford, B. R., Wager, T. D., & Roose, S. P. (2010).
McFarlane, W. R., Dixon, L., Lukens, E., & Lucksted, A. Expectancy and the treatment of depression: A review of
(2003). Family psychoeducation and schizophrenia: A review experimental methodology and effects on patient
of the literature. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 29, outcome. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 6(1), 110. doi:10
223245. doi:10.1111/j.1752-0606.2003.tb01202.x .2174/157340010790596571
Melby-Lervag, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory Saveanu, R. V., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2012). Etiology of
training effective? A meta-analytic review Developmental depression: Genetic and environmental factors. Psychiatric
Psychology, 49, 270291. doi:10.1037/a0028228 Clinics of North America, 35(1), 5171. doi:10.1016/j.psc
Miller, G. A. (2010). Mistreating psychology in the decades .2011.12.001
of the brain. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 716 Schindler, H. S., Kholoptseva, J., Oh, S. S., Yoshikawa, H.,
743. doi:10.1177/1745691610388774 Duncan, G. J., Magnuson, K. A., & Shonkoff, J. P.
Miller, A. H., Maletic, V., & Raison, C. L. (2009). (2015). Maximizing the potential of early childhood
Inflammation and its discontents: The role of cytokines in education to prevent externalizing behavior problems: A
the pathophysiology of major depression. Biological meta-analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 53, 243263.
Psychiatry, 65, 732741. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.029 doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2015.04.001
Muris, P. (2002). Relationships between self-efficacy and Schomerus, G., Schwahn, C., Holzinger, A., Corrigan, P. W.,
symptoms of anxiety disorders and depression in a normal Grabe, H. J., Carta, M. G., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2012).
adolescent sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, Evolution of public attitudes about mental illness: A systematic
337348. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00027-7 review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125,
Nisbett, R. E., & Schachter, S. (1966). Cognitive manipulation 440452. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01826.x
of pain. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 227236. Schweizer, S., Peeters, F., Huibers, M., Roelofs, J., van Os,
doi:10.1016/0022-1031(66)90081-3 J., & Arntz, A. (2010). Does illness attribution affect
Nock, M. K., & Kazdin, A. E. (2002). Examination of treatment assignment in depression? Clinical Psychology &
affective, cognitive, and behavioral factors and suicide- Psychotherapy, 17, 418426. doi:10.1002/cpp.662
related outcomes in children and young adolescents. Sterman, M. B. (1996). Physiological origins and functional
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 48 correlates of EEG rhythmic activities: Implications for
58. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3101_07 self-regulation. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 21(1), 333.
Pescosolido, B. A., Martin, J. K., Long, J. S., Medina, T. R., doi:10.1007/BF02214147
Phelan, J. C., & Link, B. G. (2010). A disease like any Storms, M. D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1970). Insomnia and the
other? A decade of change in public reactions to attribution process. Journal of Personality and Social
schizophrenia, depression, and alcohol dependence. Psychology, 16, 319328. doi:10.1037/h0029835
American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 13211330. doi:10 Strauman, T. J., Goetz, E. L., Detloff, A. M., MacDuffie, K.
.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09121743 E., Zaunmuller, L., & Lutz, W. (2012). Self-regulation

AUTO-BIOLOGICAL ATTRIBUTIONS  MACDUFFIE & STRAUMAN 67


and mechanisms of action in psychotherapy: A theory- Weinberger, D. R., & Goldberg, T. E. (2014). RDoCs redux.
based translational perspective. Journal of Personality, 81, World Psychiatry, 13(1), 3638. doi:10.1002/wps.20096
542553. doi:10.1111/jopy.12012 Weiner, B. (1994). Integrating social and personal theories of
Sullivan, M. D., Katon, W. J., Russo, J. E., Frank, E., achievement striving. Review of Educational Research, 64,
Barrett, J. E., Oxman, T. E., & Williams, J. W. (2003). 557573. doi:10.3102/00346543064004557
Patient beliefs predict response to paroxetine among Weiner, B., Perry, R. P., & Magnusson, J. (1988). An
primary care patients with dysthymia and minor attributional analysis of reactions to stigmas. Journal of
depression. Journal of the American Board of Family Practice, Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 738748. doi:10.1037/
16(1), 2231. doi:10.3122/jabfm.16.1.22 0022-3514.55.5.738
Sun, H., Kennedy, P. J., & Nestler, E. J. (2013). Epigenetics Weissman, M. M., & Markowitz, J. C. (1994). Interpersonal
of the depressed brain: Role of histone acetylation and psychotherapy: Current status. Archives of General
methylation. Neuropsychopharmacology, 38, 124137. Psychiatry, 51, 599606. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1994
doi:10.1038/npp.2012.73 .03950080011002
Sweatt, J. D. (2009). Experience-dependent epigenetic West-Eberhard, M. J. (1989). Phenotypic plasticity and the
modifications in the central nervous system. Biological Psychiatry, origins of diversity. Annual Review of Ecology and
65(3), 191197. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.002 Systematics, 20, 249278. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.20
Tsankova, N., Renthal, W., Kumar, A., & Nestler, E. J. .110189.001341
(2007). Epigenetic regulation in psychiatric disorders. Wilson, T. D., & Linville, P. W. (1982). Improving the
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 355367. doi:10.1038/ academic performance of college freshmen: Attribution
nrn2132 therapy revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Turrigiano, G. G., & Nelson, S. B. (2004). Homeostatic 42, 367376. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.42.2.367
plasticity in the developing nervous system. Nature Reviews Wilson, T. D., & Linville, P. W. (1985). Improving the
Neuroscience, 5, 97107. doi:10.1038/nrn1327 performance of college freshmen with attributional
Van Voorhees, B. W., Fogel, J., Houston, T. K., Cooper, L. techniques. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49,
A., Wang, N.-Y., & Ford, D. E. (2005). Beliefs and 287293. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.1.287
attitudes associated with the intention to not accept the World Health Organization. (2008). The global burden of
diagnosis of depression among young adults. Annals of disease: 2004 update. Retrieved from http://www.who.
Family Medicine, 3(1), 3846. doi:10.1370/afm.273 int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/GBD_report_2004
Walker, I., & Read, J. (2002). The differential effectiveness update_full.pdf?ua=1
of psychosocial and biogenetic causal explanations in
reducing negative attitudes toward mental illness. Received November 14, 2015; revised July 21, 2016;
Psychiatry, 65, 313325. doi:10.1521/psyc.65.4.313.20238 accepted July 21, 2016.
Waszczuk, M. A., Coulson, A. E., Gregory, A. M., & Eley,
T. C. (2016). A longitudinal twin and sibling study of the SUPPORTING INFORMATION
hopelessness theory of depression in adolescence and Additional Supporting Information may be found
young adulthood. Psychological Medicine, 46, 19351949.
online in the supporting information tab for this article:
doi:10.1017/S0033291716000489
Weaver, I. C., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., DAlessio,
Appendix S1. Resources for communicating neu-
A. C., Sharma, S., Meaney, M. J., Seckl, J. R., Meaney,
roscience information to general audiences.
M. J. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal
behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 847854. doi:10.1038/
nn1276

68 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V24 N1, MARCH 2017

Anda mungkin juga menyukai