Anda di halaman 1dari 17

Hendershot et al.

Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17


http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

REVIEW Open Access

Relapse prevention for addictive behaviors


Christian S Hendershot1,2*, Katie Witkiewitz3, William H George4 and G Alan Marlatt4

Abstract
The Relapse Prevention (RP) model has been a mainstay of addictions theory and treatment since its introduction
three decades ago. This paper provides an overview and update of RP for addictive behaviors with a focus on
developments over the last decade (2000-2010). Major treatment outcome studies and meta-analyses are
summarized, as are selected empirical findings relevant to the tenets of the RP model. Notable advances in RP in
the last decade include the introduction of a reformulated cognitive-behavioral model of relapse, the application of
advanced statistical methods to model relapse in large randomized trials, and the development of mindfulness-
based relapse prevention. We also review the emergent literature on genetic correlates of relapse following
pharmacological and behavioral treatments. The continued influence of RP is evidenced by its integration in most
cognitive-behavioral substance use interventions. However, the tendency to subsume RP within other treatment
modalities has posed a barrier to systematic evaluation of the RP model. Overall, RP remains an influential
cognitive-behavioral framework that can inform both theoretical and clinical approaches to understanding and
facilitating behavior change.
Keywords: Alcohol, cognitive-behavioral skills training, continuing care, drug use, psychosocial intervention, sub-
stance use treatment

Introduction historical and theoretical foundations of the RP model


Relapse poses a fundamental barrier to the treatment of and a brief summary of clinical intervention strategies.
addictive behaviors by representing the modal outcome Next, we review the major theoretical, methodological
of behavior change efforts [1-3]. For instance, twelve- and applied developments related to RP in the last dec-
month relapse rates following alcohol or tobacco cessa- ade. Specific emphasis is placed on the reformulated
tion attempts generally range from 80-95% [1,4] and evi- cognitive-behavioral model of relapse [8] as a basis for
dence suggests comparable relapse trajectories across hypothesizing and studying dynamic aspects of the
various classes of substance use [1,5,6]. Preventing relapse process. In reviewing empirical findings we focus
relapse or minimizing its extent is therefore a prerequi- on major treatment outcome studies, meta-analyses, and
site for any attempt to facilitate successful, long-term selected results that coincide with underlying tenets of
changes in addictive behaviors. the RP model. We conclude by noting critiques of the
Relapse prevention (RP) is a tertiary intervention strat- RP model and summarizing current and future direc-
egy for reducing the likelihood and severity of relapse tions in studying and preventing relapse.
following the cessation or reduction of problematic This paper extends recent reviews of the RP literature
behaviors. Three decades since its introduction [7], the [1,8-10] in several ways. Most notably, we provide a
RP model remains an influential cognitive-behavioral recent update of the RP literature by focusing primarily
approach in the treatment and study of addictions. The on studies conducted within the last decade. We also
aim of this paper is to provide readers with an update provide updated reviews of research areas that have
on empirical and applied developments related to RP, seen notable growth in the last few years; in particular,
with a primary focus on events spanning the last decade the application of advanced statistical modeling techni-
(2000-2010). We begin with a concise overview of the ques to large treatment outcome datasets and the devel-
opment of mindfulness-based relapse prevention.
* Correspondence: christian_hendershot@camh.net Additionally, we review the nascent but rapidly growing
1
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, 33 Russell St., Toronto, ON, M5S
2S1, Canada literature on genetic predictors of relapse following sub-
Full list of author information is available at the end of the article stance use interventions. In focusing exclusively on
2011 Hendershot et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 2 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

addictive behaviors (for which the RP model was initially and a set of treatment strategies designed to limit
conceived) we forego a discussion of RP as it relates to relapse likelihood and severity. Because detailed
various other behavioral domains (e.g., sexual offending, accounts of the models historical background and theo-
depression, diet and exercise) and refer readers to other retical underpinnings have been published elsewhere (e.
sources for updates on the growing range of RP applica- g., [16,22,23]), we limit the current discussion to a con-
tions [8,11]. cise review of the models history, core concepts and
clinical applications.
Definitions of relapse and relapse prevention Based on the cognitive-behavioral model of relapse,
The terms relapse and relapse prevention have seen RP was initially conceived as an outgrowth and augmen-
evolving definitions, complicating efforts to review and tation of traditional behavioral approaches to studying
evaluate the relevant literature. Definitions of relapse are and treating addictions. The evolution of cognitive-beha-
varied, ranging from a dichotomous treatment outcome vioral theories of substance use brought notable changes
to an ongoing, transitional process [8,12,13]. Overall, a in the conceptualization of relapse, many of which
large volume of research has yielded no consensus departed from traditional (e.g., disease-based) models of
operational definition of the term [14,15]. For present addiction. For instance, whereas traditional models often
purposes we define relapse as a setback that occurs dur- attribute relapse to endogenous factors like cravings or
ing the behavior change process, such that progress withdrawalconstrued as symptoms of an underlying
toward the initiation or maintenance of a behavior disease statecognitive-behavioral theories emphasize
change goal (e.g., abstinence from drug use) is inter- contextual factors (e.g., environmental stimuli and cog-
rupted by a reversion to the target behavior. We also nitive processes) as proximal relapse antecedents. Cogni-
take the perspective that relapse is best conceptualized tive-behavioral theories also diverged from disease
as a dynamic, ongoing process rather than a discrete or models in rejecting the notion of relapse as a dichoto-
terminal event (e.g., [1,8,10]). mous outcome. Rather than being viewed as a state or
Definitions of RP have also evolved considerably, due endpoint signaling treatment failure, relapse is consid-
largely to the increasingly broad adoption of RP ered a fluctuating process that begins prior to and
approaches in various treatment contexts. Though the extends beyond the return to the target behavior [8,24].
phrase relapse prevention was initially coined to denote From this standpoint, an initial return to the target
a specific clinical intervention program [7,16], RP strate- behavior after a period of volitional abstinence (a lapse)
gies are now integral to most psychosocial treatments for is seen not as a dead end, but as a fork in the road.
substance use [17], including many of the most widely dis- While a lapse might prompt a full-blown relapse,
seminated interventions (e.g., [18-20]). The National Reg- another possible outcome is that the problem behavior
istry of Evidence-based Programs and Practices, is corrected and the desired behavior re-instantiatedan
maintained by the U.S. Substance Abuse and Mental event referred to as prolapse. A critical implication is
Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), includes list- that rather than signaling a failure in the behavior
ings for numerous empirically supported interventions change process, lapses can be considered temporary set-
with relapse prevention as a descriptor or primary treat- backs that present opportunities for new learning to
ment objective (http://www.nrepp.samhsa.gov). Thus, RP occur. In viewing relapse as a common (albeit undesir-
has in many ways evolved into an umbrella term encom- able) event, emphasizing contextual antecedents over
passing most skills-based treatments that emphasize cog- internal causes, and distinguishing relapse from treat-
nitive-behavioral skills building and coping responses. ment failure, the RP model introduced a comprehensive,
While attesting to the broad influence of the RP model, flexible and optimistic alternative to traditional
the diffuse application of RP approaches also tends to approaches.
complicate efforts to define RP-based treatments and eval- Marlatts original RP model is depicted in Figure 1. A
uate their overall efficacy (e.g., [21]). In the present review basic assumption is that relapse events are immediately
we emphasize Marlatts RP model [7,16] and its more preceded by a high-risk situation, broadly defined as any
recent iteration [8] when discussing the theoretical basis context that confers vulnerability for engaging in the
of RP. By necessity, our literature review also includes stu- target behavior. Examples of high-risk contexts include
dies that do not explicitly espouse the RP model, but that emotional or cognitive states (e.g., negative affect,
are relevant nonetheless to its predictions. diminished self-efficacy), environmental contingencies
(e.g., conditioned drug cues), or physiological states (e.
Marlatts relapse prevention model: Historical foundations g., acute withdrawal). Although some high-risk situa-
and overview tions appear nearly universal across addictive behaviors
The RP model developed by Marlatt [7,16] provides (e.g., negative affect; [25]), high-risk situations are likely
both a conceptual framework for understanding relapse to vary across behaviors, across individuals, and within
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 3 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

Figure 1 Original cognitive-behavioral model of relapse (Marlatt & Gordon, 1985)

the same individual over time [10]. Whether a high-risk expectancies, readiness to change, and concomitant fac-
situation culminates in a lapse depends largely on the tors that could complicate treatment (e.g., comorbid dis-
individuals capacity to enact an effective coping orders, neuropsychological deficits). Using high-risk
responsedefined as any cognitive or behavioral com- situations as a starting point, the clinician works back-
pensatory strategy that reduces the likelihood of lapsing. ward to identify immediate precipitants and distal life-
Central to the RP model is the role of cognitive factors style factors related to relapse, and forward to evaluate
in determining relapse liability. For example, successful coping responses [16,24]. Ideally, this approach helps
navigation of high-risk situations may increase self-effi- clients to recognize high-risk situations as discriminative
cacy (ones perceived capacity to cope with an impending stimuli signaling relapse risk, as well as to identify cog-
situation or task; [26]), in turn decreasing relapse prob- nitive and behavioral strategies to obviate these situa-
ability. Conversely, a return to the target behavior can tions or minimize their impact. Examples of specific
undermine self-efficacy, increasing the risk of future intervention strategies include enhancing self-efficacy (e.
lapses. Outcome expectancies (anticipated effects of sub- g., by setting achievable behavioral goals) and eliminat-
stance use; [27]) also figure prominently in the RP model. ing myths and placebo effects (e.g., by challenging mis-
Additionally, attitudes or beliefs about the causes and perceptions about the effects of substance use).
meaning of a lapse may influence whether a full relapse The clients appraisal of lapses also serves as a pivotal
ensues. Viewing a lapse as a personal failure may lead to intervention point in that these reactions can determine
feelings of guilt and abandonment of the behavior change whether a lapse escalates or desists. Establishing lapse
goal [24]. This reaction, termed the Abstinence Violation management plans can aid the client in self-correcting
Effect (AVE; [16]), is considered more likely when one soon after a slip, and cognitive restructuring can help
holds a dichotomous view of relapse and/or neglects to clients to re-frame the meaning of the event and mini-
consider situational explanations for lapsing. In sum, the mize the AVE [24]. A final emphasis in the RP approach
RP framework emphasizes high-risk contexts, coping is the global intervention of lifestyle balancing, designed
responses, self-efficacy, affect, expectancies and the AVE to target more pervasive factors that can function as
as primary relapse antecedents. relapse antecedents. For example, clients can be encour-
Implicit in the RP approach is that the initiation and aged to increase their engagement in rewarding or
maintenance of behavior change represent separate pro- stress-reducing activities into their daily routine. Success
cesses governed by unique contingencies [12,28]. Thus, in these areas may enhance self-efficacy, in turn redu-
specific cognitive and behavioral strategies are often cing relapse risk. Overall, the RP model is characterized
necessary to maintain initial treatment gains and mini- by a highly ideographic treatment approach, a contrast
mize relapse likelihood following initial behavior change. to the one size fits all approach typical of certain tradi-
RP strategies fall into two broad categories: specific tional treatments. Moreover, an emphasis on post-treat-
intervention techniques, often designed to help the ment maintenance renders RP a useful adjunct to
patient anticipate and cope with high-risk situations, various treatment modalities (e.g., cognitive-behavioral,
and global self-control approaches, intended to reduce twelve step programs, pharmacotherapy), irrespective of
relapse risk by promoting positive lifestyle change. An the strategies used to enact initial behavior change.
essential starting point in treatment is a thorough
assessment of the clients substance use patterns, high- Developments in Relapse Prevention: 2000-2010
risk situations and coping skills. Other important assess- The last decade has seen numerous developments in the
ment targets include the clients self-efficacy, outcome RP literature, including the publication of Relapse
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 4 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

Prevention, Second Edition [29] and its companion text, are examples of distal variables that could influence
Assessment of Addictive Behaviors, Second Edition [30]. relapse liability a priori. Tonic processes also include
The following sections provide an overview of major cognitive factors that show relative stability over time,
theoretical, empirical and applied advances related to RP such as drug-related outcome expectancies, global self-
over the last decade. efficacy, and personal beliefs about abstinence or
relapse. Whereas tonic processes may dictate initial sus-
The reformulated cognitive-behavioral model of relapse ceptibility to relapse, its occurrence is determined lar-
Efforts to develop, test and refine theoretical models are gely by phasic responsesproximal or transient factors
critical to enhancing the understanding and prevention that serve to actuate (or prevent) a lapse. Phasic
of relapse [1,2,14]. A major development in this respect responses include cognitive and affective processes that
was the reformulation of Marlatts cognitive-behavioral can fluctuate across time and contextssuch as urges/
relapse model to place greater emphasis on dynamic cravings, mood, or transient changes in outcome expec-
relapse processes [8]. Whereas most theories presume tancies, self-efficacy, or motivation. Additionally,
linear relationships among constructs, the reformulated momentary coping responses can serve as phasic events
model (Figure 2) views relapse as a complex, nonlinear that may determine whether a high-risk situation culmi-
process in which various factors act jointly and interac- nates in a lapse. Substance use and its immediate conse-
tively to affect relapse timing and severity. Similar to the quences (e.g., impaired decision-making, the AVE) are
original RP model, the dynamic model centers on the additional phasic processes that are set into motion
high-risk situation. Against this backdrop, both tonic once a lapse occurs. Thus, whereas tonic processes can
(stable) and phasic (transient) influences interact to determine who is vulnerable for relapse, phasic pro-
determine relapse likelihood. Tonic processes include cesses determine when relapse occurs [8,31].
distal risksstable background factors that determine an A key feature of the dynamic model is its emphasis on
individuals set point or initial threshold for relapse the complex interplay between tonic and phasic pro-
[8,31]. Personality, genetic or familial risk factors, drug cesses. As indicated in Figure 2, distal risks may influ-
sensitivity/metabolism and physical withdrawal profiles ence relapse either directly or indirectly (via phasic

Figure 2 Revised cognitive-behavioral model of relapse (Witkiewitz & Marlatt, 2004)


Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 5 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

processes). The model also predicts feedback loops Systematic reviews and large-scale treatment
among hypothesized constructs. For instance, the return outcome studies
to substance use can have reciprocal effects on the same The first comprehensive review of RP treatment out-
cognitive or affective factors (motivation, mood, self-effi- come studies was Carrolls [35] descriptive account of
cacy) that contributed to the lapse. Lapses may also 24 interventions focusing on substance use. This review
evoke physiological (e.g., alleviation of withdrawal) and/ found consistent support for the superiority of RP over
or cognitive (e.g., the AVE) responses that in turn deter- no treatment, inconsistent support for its superiority
mine whether use escalates or desists. The dynamic over discussion control conditions, and consistent sup-
model further emphasizes the importance of nonlinear port that RP was equally efficacious to other active
relationships and timing/sequencing of events. For treatments. Carroll concluded that RP, though not con-
instance, in a high-risk context, a slight and momentary sistently superior to other active treatments, showed
drop in self-efficacy could have a disproportionate particular promise in three areas: reducing relapse sever-
impact on other relapse antecedents (negative affect, ity, enhancing durability of treatment gains, and match-
expectancies) [8]. Furthermore, the strength of proximal ing treatment strategies to client characteristics. RP also
influences on relapse may vary based on distal risk fac- showed delayed emergence effects in some studies, sug-
tors, with these relationships becoming increasingly gesting that it may outperform other treatments during
nonlinear as distal risk increases [31]. For example, one the maintenance stage of behavior change [35].
could imagine a situation whereby a client who is rela- Subsequently, a meta-analysis evaluated 26 RP treat-
tively committed to abstinence from alcohol encounters ment outcome studies totaling 9,504 participants [36].
a neighbor who invites the client into his home for a The authors examined two primary outcomes (substance
drink. Feeling somewhat uncomfortable with the offer use and psychosocial functioning) and several treatment
the client might experience a slight decrease in self-effi- moderators. Effect sizes indicated that RP was generally
cacy, which cascades into positive outcome expectancies successful in reducing substance use (r = .14) and
about the potential effects of having a drink as well as improving psychosocial functioning (r = .48), consistent
feelings of shame or guilt about saying no to his neigh- with its purpose as both a specific and global interven-
bors offer. Importantly, this client might not have ever tion approach. Moderation analyses suggested that RP
considered such an invitation as a high-risk situation, was consistently efficacious across treatment modalities
yet various contextual factors may interact to predict a (individual vs. group) and settings (inpatient vs. outpati-
lapse. ent). RP was most effective for reducing alcohol and
The dynamic model of relapse assumes that relapse polysubstance use and less effective for tobacco and
can take the form of sudden and unexpected returns to cocaine usea contrast to Carrolls [35] finding of com-
the target behavior. This concurs not only with clinical parable efficacy across drug classes. In addition, RP was
observations, but also with contemporary learning mod- more effective when delivered in conjunction with phar-
els stipulating that recently modified behavior is inher- macotherapy, when compared to wait-list (vs. active)
ently unstable and easily swayed by context [32]. While comparison conditions, and when outcomes were
maintaining its footing in cognitive-behavioral theory, assessed soon after treatment. Though some findings
the revised model also draws from nonlinear dynamical were considered tentative due to sample sizes, the
systems theory (NDST) and catastrophe theory, both authors concluded that RP was broadly efficacious [36].
approaches for understanding the operation of complex McCrady [37] conducted a comprehensive review of
systems [10,33]. Detailed discussions of relapse in rela- 62 alcohol treatment outcome studies comprising 13
tion to NDST and catastrophe theory are available else- psychosocial approaches. Two approachesRP and brief
where [10,31,34]. interventionqualified as empirically validated treat-
ments based on established criteria. Interestingly, Miller
Empirical findings relevant to the RP model and Wilbournes [21] review of clinical trials, which
The empirical literature on relapse in addictions has evaluated the efficacy of 46 different alcohol treatments,
grown substantially over the past decade. Because the ranked relapse prevention as 35th out of 46 treatments
volume and scope of this work precludes an exhaustive based on methodological quality and treatment effect
review, the following section summarizes a select body sizes. However, many of the treatments ranked in the
of findings reflective of the literature and relevant to RP top 10 (including brief interventions, social skills train-
theory. The studies reviewed focus primarily on alcohol ing, community reinforcement, behavior contracting,
and tobacco cessation, however, it should be noted that behavioral marital therapy, and self-monitoring) incor-
RP principles have been applied to an increasing range porate RP components. These two reviews highlighted
of addictive behaviors [10,11]. the increasing difficulty of classifying interventions as
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 6 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

specifically constituting RP, given that many treatments there was general support for the efficacy of pharmaco-
for substance use disorders (e.g., cognitive behavioral logical treatments [40].
treatment (CBT)) are based on the cognitive behavioral Recently, Magill and Ray [41] conducted a meta-analy-
model of relapse developed for RP [16]. One of the key sis of 53 controlled trials of CBT for substance use dis-
distinctions between CBT and RP in the field is that the orders. As noted by the authors, the CBT studies
term CBT is more often used to describe stand-alone evaluated in their review were based primarily on the
primary treatments that are based on the cognitive- RP model [29]. Overall, the results were consistent with
behavioral model, whereas RP is more often used to the review conducted by Irvin and colleagues, in that
describe aftercare treatment. Given that CBT is often the authors concluded that 58% of individuals who
used as a stand-alone treatment it may include addi- received CBT had better outcomes than those in com-
tional components that are not always provided in RP. parison conditions. In contrast with the findings of Irvin
For example, the CBT intervention developed in Project and colleagues [36], Magill and Ray [41] found that
MATCH [18] (described below) equated to RP with CBT was most effective for individuals with marijuana
respect to the core sessions, but it also included elective use disorders.
sessions that are not typically a focus in RP (e.g., job-
seeking skills, family involvement). Recent findings in support of RP model
An increasing number of large-scale trials have components
allowed for statistically powerful evaluations of psycho- The following section reviews selected empirical findings
social interventions for alcohol use. Project MATCH that support or coincide with tenets of the RP model.
[18] evaluated the efficacy of three interventionsMoti- Sections are organized in accordance with major model
vational Enhancement Therapy (MET), Twelve-Step constructs. Because the scope of this literature precludes
Facilitation (TSF), and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy an exhaustive review, we highlight select findings that
(CBT)for treating alcohol dependence. The CBT inter- are relevant to the main tenets of the RP model, in par-
vention was a skills-based treatment containing elements ticular those that coincide with predictions of the refor-
of RP. Spanning nine data collection sites and following mulated model of relapse.
over 1700 participants for up to three years, Project
MATCH was the largest psychotherapy trial conducted Self-efficacy
to that point. Multiple matching hypotheses were pro- Self-efficacy (SE), the perceived ability to enact a given
posed in evaluating differential treatment efficacy as a behavior in a specified context [26], is a principal deter-
function of theoretically relevant client attributes. Pri- minant of health behavior according to social-cognitive
mary analyses supported only one of sixteen matching theories. In fact, some theories view SE as the final com-
hypotheses: outpatients lower in psychiatric severity mon pathway to relapse [42]. Although SE is proposed
fared better in TSF than in CBT during the year follow- as a fluctuating and dynamic construct [26], most stu-
ing treatment [18]. Although primary analyses provided dies rely on static measures of SE, preventing evaluation
relatively little support for tailoring alcohol treatments of within-person changes over time or contexts [43].
based on specific client attributes, matching effects have Shiffman, Gwaltney and colleagues have used ecological
been identified in subsequent analyses (described in momentary assessment (EMA; [44]) to examine tem-
more detail later). poral variations in SE in relation to smoking relapse.
Since 2005 an ongoing Cochrane review has evaluated Findings from these studies suggested that participants
RP for smoking cessation [38,39]. As of 2009, meta-ana- SE was lower on the day before a lapse, and that lower
lyses had found no support for the efficacy of skills- SE in the days following a lapse in turn predicted pro-
based RP approaches in preventing relapse to smoking gression to relapse [43,45]. One study [46] reported
[38]. However, a recent re-analysis of these trials yielded increases in daily SE during abstinent intervals, perhaps
different results [40]. The re-analysis stratified beha- indicating mounting confidence as treatment goals were
vioral interventions based on specific intervention con- maintained [45].
tent while also imposing stricter analytic criteria In the first study to examine relapse in relation to
regarding the length of follow-up assessments. In these phasic changes in SE [46], researchers reported results
analyses, CBT/RP-based self-help interventions showed that appear consistent with the dynamic model of
a significant overall effect in increasing long-term absti- relapse. During a smoking cessation attempt, partici-
nence (pooled OR: 1.52, 95% CI: 1.15 - 2.01, based on 3 pants reported on SE, negative affect and urges at ran-
studies) and group counseling showed significant short- dom intervals. Findings indicated nonlinear relationships
term efficacy (pooled OR: 2.55, 95% CI: 1.58 - 4.11, between SE and urges, such that momentary SE
based on 2 studies). There was limited evidence for the decreased linearly as urges increased but dropped
efficacy of other specific behavioral treatments, although abruptly as urges peaked. Moreover, this finding
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 7 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

appeared attributable to individual differences in base- the first study to examine how daily fluctuations in
line (tonic) levels of SE. When urge and negative affect expectancies predict relapse [45], researchers assessed
were low, individuals with low, intermediate or high positive outcome expectancies for smoking (POEs)
baseline SE were similar in their momentary SE ratings. among participants during a tobacco cessation attempt.
However, these groups momentary ratings diverged sig- Lower POEs on the quit day were associated with
nificantly at high levels of urges and negative affect, greater abstinence likelihood, and POEs decreased in the
such that those with low baseline SE had large drops in days following the quit day. Lapses were associated with
momentary SE in the face of increasingly challenging higher POEs on the preceding day, and in the days fol-
situations. These findings support that higher distal risk lowing a lapse those who avoided a full relapse showed
can result in bifurcations (divergent patterns) of beha- decreases in POEs whereas those who relapsed did not
vior as the level of proximal risk factors increase, consis- [45]. These results suggest that outcome expectancies
tent with predictions from nonlinear dynamic systems might play a role in predicting relapse as both a tonic
theory [31]. and phasic risk factor.
A recent meta-analysis evaluated the association of SE Expectancy research has recently started examining
with smoking relapse [47]. The review included 54 stu- the influences of implicit cognitive processes, generally
dies that assessed prospective associations of SE and defined as those operating automatically or outside con-
smoking during a quit attempt. A major finding con- scious awareness [54,55]. Recent reviews provide a con-
cerned differential effect sizes based on the timing of SE vincing rationale for the putative role of implicit
assessments: the negative association of SE with likeli- processes in addictive behaviors and relapse [54,56,57].
hood of future smoking represented a small effect (d = Implicit measures of alcohol-related cognitions can dis-
-.21) when SE was assessed prior to the quit attempt, criminate among light and heavy drinkers [58] and pre-
but a medium effect (d = -.47) when SE was assessed dict drinking above and beyond explicit measures [59].
after the quit day. The authors concluded that, given the One study found that smokers attentional bias to
centrality of SE to most cognitive-behavioral models of tobacco cues predicted early lapses during a quit
relapse, the association of SE with cessation was weaker attempt, but this relationship was not evident among
than would be expected (i.e., SE accounted for roughly people receiving nicotine replacement therapy, who
2% of the variance in treatment outcome following showed reduced attention to cues [60].
initial abstinence). The findings also suggested that SE Initial evidence suggests that implicit measures of
should ideally be measured after the cessation attempt, expectancies are correlated with relapse outcomes, as
and that controlling for concurrent smoking is critical demonstrated in one study of heroin users [61]. In
when examining SE in relation to prospective relapse another recent study, researchers trained participants in
[47]. Finally, in analyses from a cross-national study of attentional bias modification (ABM) during inpatient
the natural history of smoking cessation, researchers treatment for alcohol dependence and measured relapse
examined self-efficacy in relation to relapse rates across over the course of three months post-treatment [62].
an extended time period [48,49]. Results indicated that Relative to a control condition, ABM resulted in signifi-
self-efficacy increased with cumulative abstinence and cantly improved ability to disengage from alcohol-
correlated negatively with urges, consistent with RP the- related stimuli during attentional bias tasks. While inci-
ory. Also, higher self-efficacy consistently predicted dence of relapse did not differ between groups, the
lower relapse rates across time and partly mediated the ABM group showed a significantly longer time to first
association of perceived benefits of smoking with relapse heavy drinking day compared to the control group.
events [48,49]. Additionally, the intervention had no effect on subjec-
tive measures of craving, suggesting the possibility that
Outcome expectancies intervention effects may have been specific to implicit
Outcome expectancies (anticipated outcomes of a given cognitive processes [62]. Overall, research on implicit
behavior or situation) are central to the RP model and cognitions stands to enhance understanding of dynamic
have been studied extensively in the domain of alcohol relapse processes and could ultimately aid in predicting
use [27]. In theory, expectancies are shaped by various lapses during high-risk situations.
tonic risk factors (e.g., environment, culture, personality,
genetics) and mediate these antecedent influences on Withdrawal
drinking [27]. Research supports that expectancies could Withdrawal tendencies can develop early in the course
partly mediate influences such as personality factors of addiction [25] and symptom profiles can vary based
[50], genetic variations [51,52], and negative affect [53] on stable intra-individual factors [63], suggesting the
on drinking. Outcome expectancies could also be involvement of tonic processes. Despite serving as a
involved as a phasic response to situational factors. In chief diagnostic criterion, withdrawal often does not
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 8 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

predict relapse, perhaps partly explaining its de-empha- who reported higher negative affect or increased nega-
sis in contemporary motivational models of addiction tive affect over time had the highest probability of heavy
[64]. However, recent studies show that withdrawal pro- and frequent drinking following treatment, and had a
files are complex, multi-faceted and idiosyncratic, and near-zero probability of transitioning to moderate drink-
that in the context of fine-grained analyses withdrawal ing. Heavier and more frequent alcohol use predicted a
indeed can predict relapse [64,65]. Such findings have greater probability of high negative affect and increased
contributed to renewed interest in negative reinforce- negative affect over time.
ment models of drug use [63]. Knowledge about the role of NA in drinking behavior
Although withdrawal is usually viewed as a physiologi- has benefited from daily process studies in which parti-
cal process, recent theory emphasizes the importance of cipants provide regular reports of mood and drinking.
behavioral withdrawal processes [66]. Whereas physiolo- Such studies have shown that both positive and negative
gical withdrawal symptoms tend to abate in the days or moods show close temporal links to alcohol use [73].
weeks following drug cessation, the unavailability of a One study [74] found evidence suggesting a feedback
conditioned behavioral coping response (e.g., the ritual cycle of mood and drinking whereby elevated daily levels
of drug administration) may leave the former user ill- of NA predicted alcohol use, which in turn predicted
equipped to cope with ongoing stressors, thus exacer- spikes in NA. These findings were moderated by gender,
bating and/or prolonging symptoms [66]. Current theory social context, and time of week. Other studies have
and research indicate that physiological components of similarly found that relationships between daily events
drug withdrawal may be motivationally inert, with the and/or mood and drinking can vary based on intraindi-
core motivational constituent of withdrawal being nega- vidual or situational factors [73], suggesting dynamic
tive affect [25,66]. Thus, examining withdrawal in rela- interplay between these influences.
tion to relapse may only prove useful to the extent that
negative affect is assessed adequately [64]. Self-control and coping responses
Strengthening coping skills is a goal of virtually all cog-
Negative affect nitive-behavioral interventions for substance use [75].
A large literature attests to the role of negative affect Several studies have used EMA to examine coping
(NA) in the etiology and maintenance of addictive beha- responses in real time. One study [76] found that
viors. NA is consistently cited as a relapse trigger in ret- momentary coping differentiated smoking lapses from
rospective reports (e.g., [67,68]), although participants temptations, such that coping responses were reported
might sometimes misattribute lapses to negative mood in 91% of successful resists vs. 24% of lapses. Shiffman
states[15]. In one study, individuals who were unable to and colleagues [68] found that restorative coping follow-
sustain a smoking cessation attempt for more than 24 ing a smoking lapse decreased the likelihood of a second
hours (compared to those with a sustained quit attempt) lapse the same day. Exactly how coping responses
reported greater depressive symptoms and NA in reduce the likelihood of lapsing remains unclear. One
response to stress and displayed less perseverance dur- study found that momentary coping reduced urges
ing experimental stress inductions [69]. Supporting the among smokers, suggesting a possible mechanism [76].
dynamic influence of NA on relapse, Shiffman and Some studies find that the number of coping responses
Waters [70] found that smoking lapses were not asso- is more predictive of lapses than the specific type of
ciated with NA in the preceding days, but were asso- coping used [76,77]. However, despite findings that cop-
ciated with rising NA in the hours leading up to a lapse. ing can prevent lapses there is scant evidence to show
Evidence further suggests that negative affect can pro- that skills-based interventions in fact lead to improved
mote positive outcome expectancies [53] or undermine coping [75].
situational self-efficacy [71], outcomes which could in Some researchers propose that the self-control
turn promote a lapse. Moreover, Baker and colleagues required to maintain behavior change strains motiva-
propose that high levels of negative affect can interfere tional resources, and that this fatigue can undermine
with controlled cognitive processes, such that adaptive subsequent self-control efforts [78]. Consistent with this
coping and decision-making may be undermined as idea, EMA studies have shown that social drinkers
negative affect peaks [25]. Witkiewitz and Villarroel [72] report greater alcohol consumption and violations of
found that drinking rates following treatment were sig- self-imposed drinking limits on days when self-control
nificantly associated with current and prior changes in demands are high [79]. Limit violations were predictive
negative affect and changes in negative affect were sig- of responses consistent with the AVE the following day,
nificantly associated with current and prior changes in and greater distress about violations in turn predicted
drinking state (effect size range = 0.13 (small) to 0.33 greater drinking [80]. Findings also suggested that these
(medium)). Overall, the results showed that individuals relationships varied based on individual differences,
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 9 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

suggesting the interplay of static and dynamic factors in hypothesis, whereby females with low baseline motiva-
AVE responses. Evidence further suggests that practicing tion and males with lower levels of alcohol dependence
routine acts of self-control can reduce short-term inci- and low baseline motivation who received MET as an
dence of relapse. For instance, Muraven [81] conducted aftercare treatment had better outcomes than those who
a study in which participants were randomly assigned to were assigned to receive CBT as an aftercare treatment.
practice small acts self-control acts on a daily basis for
two weeks prior to a smoking cessation attempt. Com- Genetic influences on treatment response and relapse
pared to a control group, those who practiced self-con- The last decade has seen a marked increase in the num-
trol showed significantly longer time until relapse in the ber of human molecular genetic studies in medical and
following month. behavioral research, due largely to rapid technological
advances in genotyping platforms, decreasing cost of
Emerging topics in relapse and relapse molecular analyses, and the advent of genome-wide
prevention association studies (GWAS). Not surprisingly, molecular
Using nonlinear methods to model relapse genetic approaches have increasingly been incorporated
A key contribution of the reformulated relapse model is in treatment outcome studies, allowing novel opportu-
to highlight the need for non-traditional assessment and nities to study biological influences on relapse. Given
analytic approaches to better understand relapse. Most the rapid growth in this area, we allocate a portion of
studies of relapse rely on statistical methods that assume this review to discussing initial evidence for genetic
continuous linear relationships, but these methods may associations with relapse. Specifically, we focus on
be inadequate for studying a behavior characterized by recent, representative findings from studies evaluating
discontinuity and abrupt changes [33]. Consistent with candidate single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) as
the tenets of the reformulated RP model, several studies moderators of response to substance use interventions.
suggest advantages of nonlinear statistical approaches It is important to note that these studies were not
for studying relapse. designed to evaluate specific components of the RP
In one study, researchers used catastrophe models to model, nor do these studies explicitly espouse the RP
examine proximal and distal predictors of post-treat- model. Also, many studies have focused solely on phar-
ment drinking among individuals with alcohol use disor- macological interventions, and are therefore not directly
ders [31]. Catastrophe models accounted for more than related to the RP model. However, we review these find-
double the amount of variance in drinking than that ings in order to illustrate the scope of initial efforts to
predicted by linear models. Similar results have been include genetic predictors in treatment studies that
found using the much larger Project MATCH dataset examine relapse as a clinical outcome. These findings
[33]. Two additional recent analyses of the MATCH may be informative for researchers who wish to incor-
dataset showed that nonlinear approaches can detect porate genetic variables in future studies of relapse and
processes that may go unobserved in the context of lin- relapse prevention.
ear models. Witkiewitz and colleagues [34,82] used cata- Broadly speaking, there are at least three primary con-
strophe modeling and latent growth mixture modeling texts in which genetic variation could influence liability
to re-assess two of the matching hypotheses that were for relapse during or following treatment. First, in the
not supported in the original studythat individuals low context of pharmacotherapy interventions, relevant
in baseline self-efficacy would respond more favorably genetic variations can impact drug pharmacokinetics or
to cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) than motivational pharmacodynamics, thereby moderating treatment
enhancement therapy (MET) and that individuals low in response (pharmacogenetics). Second, the likelihood of
baseline motivation would respond more favorably to abstinence following a behavioral or pharmacological
MET than CBT [18]. In the first study [34], catastrophe intervention can be moderated by genetic influences on
models provided the best fit to the data, and latent metabolic processes, receptor activity/expression, and/or
growth analyses confirmed the predicted interaction: fre- incentive value specific to the addictive substance in
quent drinkers with low initial self-efficacy had better question. For instance, SNPs with functional implica-
outcomes in CBT than in MET, while those high in tions for relevant neurotransmitter or metabolic path-
self-efficacy fared better in MET. Similarly, a second ways can influence the reward value of marijuana (e.g.,
study [82] found that individuals in the outpatient arm FAAH; CNR1); nicotine (e.g., CYP2A6, CHRNB2,
of Project MATCH with low motivation to change at CHRNA4); and alcohol (ALDH2, ADH1B), while others
baseline who were assigned to MET had better out- show potential for influencing the incentive value of
comes than those assigned to CBT. The authors also multiple drugs (e.g., ANKK1; DRD4; OPRM1). Third,
found a treatment by gender by alcohol dependence variants implicated in broad traits relevant for addictive
severity interaction in support of the matching behaviorsfor instance, executive cognitive functioning
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 10 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

(e.g., COMT) or externalizing traits (e.g., GABRA2, were not evident in participants receiving NTX and
DRD4)could influence relapse proneness via general CBI). A smaller placebo controlled study has also found
neurobehavioral mechanisms, irrespective of drug class evidence for better responses to NTX among Asp40 car-
or treatment modality. As summarized below, prelimin- riers [94]. The Asp40 variant has further been linked to
ary empirical support exists for each of these intermediate phenotypes that could influence relapse
possibilities. proneness, including hedonic responses to alcohol [95],
Genetic influences on relapse have been studied most increased neural responses to alcohol primes [96],
extensively in the context of pharmacogenetics, with the greater craving in response to alcohol use [97] and
bulk of studies focusing on nicotine dependence (for increased dopamine release in the ventral striatum dur-
recent reviews see [83,84]). Several candidate poly- ing alcohol challenge [98]. One study found that the
morphisms have been examined in response to smoking Asp40 allele predicted cue-elicited craving among indivi-
cessation treatments, especially nicotine replacement duals low in baseline craving but not those high in
therapy (NRT) and bupropion [84]. The catechol-O- initial craving, suggesting that tonic craving could inter-
methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism, act with genotype to predict phasic responses to drug
established as predicting variability in prefrontal dopa- cues [97].
mine levels, has been evaluated in relation to smoking Findings concerning possible genetic moderators of
cessation in several studies. Independent trials of NRT response to acamprosate have been reported [99], but
have found cessation rates to differ based on COMT are preliminary. Additionally, other findings suggest the
genotype [85-87]. A polymorphism in the nicotinic acet- influence of a DRD4 variable number of tandem repeats
ylcholine 2 receptor gene (CHRNB2) has been asso- (VNTR) polymorphism on response to olanzapine, a
ciated with length of abstinence and withdrawal dopamine antagonist that has been studied as an experi-
symptoms during bupropion treatment [88] and with mental treatment for alcohol problems. Olanzapine was
relapse rates and ability to quit on the target day during found to reduce alcohol-related craving those with the
NRT [89]. One bupropion trial found that DRD2 varia- long-repeat VNTR (DRD4 L), but not individuals with
tions predicted withdrawal symptoms, medication the short-repeat version (DRD4 S; [100,101]). Further, a
response and time to relapse [90]. In a study of the mu- randomized trial of olanzapine led to significantly
opioid receptor (OPRM1) Asn40/Asp40 variant during improved drinking outcomes in DRD4 L but not DRD4
NRT, those with the Asp40 variant had higher rates of S individuals [100].
abstinence and reduced negative affect compared to There is also preliminary evidence for the possibility
Asn40 individuals [91]. Additionally, post-hoc analyses of genetic influences on response to psychosocial inter-
indicated that Asp40 carriers were more likely to regain ventions, including those incorporating RP strategies. In
abstinence following a lapse, suggesting a possible role a secondary analysis of the Project MATCH data,
of the genotype in predicting prolapse. researchers evaluated posttreatment drinking outcomes
The most promising pharmacogenetic evidence in in relation to a GABRA2 variant previously implicated
alcohol interventions concerns the OPRM1 A118G poly- in the risk for alcohol dependence [102]. Analyses
morphism as a moderator of clinical response to nal- included MATCH participants of European descent who
trexone (NTX). An initial retrospective analysis of NTX provided a genetic sample (n = 812). Those carrying the
trials found that OPRM1 influenced treatment response, high-risk GABRA2 allele showed a significantly
such that individuals with the Asp40 variant (G allele) increased likelihood of relapse following treatment,
receiving NTX had a longer time until the first heavy including a twofold increase in the likelihood of heavy
drinking day and were half as likely to relapse compared drinking. Furthermore, GABRA2 interacted with treat-
to those homozygous for the Asn40 variant (A allele) ment condition to influence drinking outcomes. Among
[92]. This finding was later extended in the COMBINE those with the high-risk genotype, drinking behavior did
study, such that G carriers showed a greater proportion not appear to be modified by treatment, with outcomes
of days abstinent and a lower proportion of heavy drink- being similar regardless of treatment condition. How-
ing days compared in response to NTX versus placebo, ever, treatment differences emerged in the low-risk gen-
whereas participants homozygous for the A allele did otype group, such that TSF produced the best
not show a significant medication response [93]. More- outcomes, followed by MET [102]. In another psychoso-
over, 87.1% of G allele carriers who received NTX were cial treatment study, researchers in Poland examined
classified as having a good clinical outcome at study genetic moderators of relapse following inpatient alcohol
endpoint, versus 54.5% of Asn40 homozygotes who treatment [103]. Results showed that polymorphisms in
received NTX. (Moderating effects of OPRM1 were spe- BDNF (Val66Met) and COMT (Val158Met) significantly
cific to participants receiving medication management predicted relapse probability. Overall, evidence for
without the cognitive-behavioral intervention [CBI] and genetic moderation effects in psychosocial trials are
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 11 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

consistent with the notion that variants with broad [115], whereby clients are taught to view urges as analo-
implications for neurotransmitter function, cognitive gous to an ocean wave that rises, crests, and diminishes.
function, and/or externalizing traits can potentially Rather than being overwhelmed by the wave, the goal is
influence relapse proneness. In the absence of a plausi- to surf its crest, attending to thoughts and sensations
ble biological mechanism for differential response to as the urge peaks and subsides.
specific psychosocial treatments (e.g., MET vs. CBT) as Results of a preliminary nonrandomized trial supported
a function of genotype, the most parsimonious interpre- the potential utility of MBRP for reducing substance use.
tation of these findings is that some variants will impose In this study incarcerated individuals were offered the
greater risk for relapse following any quit attempt, chance to participate in an intensive 10-day course in
regardless of treatment availability or modality. Vipassana meditation (VM). Those participating in VM
Findings from numerous non-treatment studies are were compared to a treatment as usual (TAU) group on
also relevant to the possibility of genetic influences on measures of post-incarceration substance use and psy-
relapse processes. For instance, genetic factors could chosocial functioning. Relative to the TAU group, the
influence relapse in part via drug-specific cognitive pro- VM group reported significantly lower levels of substance
cesses. Recent studies have reported genetic associations use and alcohol-related consequences and improved psy-
with alcohol-related cognitions, including alcohol expec- chosocial functioning at follow-up [116].
tancies, drinking refusal self-efficacy, drinking motives, More recently, a randomized controlled trial com-
and implicit measures of alcohol-related motivation pared an eight-week MBRP course to treatment as usual
[51,52,104-108]. Overall, the body of research on genetic (TAU), which consisted of 12-step-based process-
influences on relapse and related processes is nascent oriented discussion and psychoeducation groups [117].
and virtually all findings require replication. Consistent The majority of MBRP participants (86%) engaged in
with the broader literature, it can be anticipated that meditation practices immediately posttreatment and
most genetic associations with relapse outcomes will be 54% continued practice for at least 4 months posttreat-
small in magnitude and potentially difficult to replicate. ment (M = 4.74 days/week, up to 30 min/day). Com-
Nonetheless, initial studies have yielded intriguing pared to TAU, MBRP participants reported significantly
results. It is inevitable that the next decade will see reduced craving, and increased acceptance and mindful
exponential growth in this area, including greater use of awareness over the 4-month follow-up period, consistent
genome-wide analyses of treatment response [109] and with the core goals of MBRP. Over the course of treat-
efforts to evaluate the clinical utility and cost effective- ment, MBRP evinced fewer days of use compared to
ness of tailoring treatments based on pharmacogenetics. TAU (MBRP: M = .06 days, TAU: M = 2.57 days).
Finally, an intriguing direction is to evaluate whether These differences persisted at 2-month follow-up (2.08
providing clients with personalized genetic information days for MBRP vs. 5.43 days for TAU). Secondary ana-
can facilitate reductions in substance use or improve lyses [118] showed that compared to TAU, MBRP parti-
treatment adherence [110,111]. cipants evinced a decreased relation between depressive
symptoms and craving following treatment. This
Mindfulness-based relapse prevention attenuation was related to subsequent decreases in alco-
In terms of clinical applications of RP, the most notable hol and other drug use, suggesting MBRP led to
development in the last decade has been the emergence decreased craving in response to negative affect, thereby
and increasing application of Mindfulness-Based Relapse lessening the need to alleviate affective discomfort with
Prevention (MBRP) for addictive behaviors [112,113]. alcohol and other drug use. Furthermore, individuals
Given supportive data for the efficacy of mindfulness- with moderate depression in the MBRP group had a sig-
based interventions in other behavioral domains, espe- nificantly lower probability of substance use, fewer
cially in prevention of relapse of major depression [114], drinks per drinking day, and fewer drinks per day than
there is increasing interest in MBRP for addictive beha- individuals with moderate depression in TAU. A larger,
viors. The merger of mindfulness and cognitive-beha- randomized trial comparing MBRP to TAU and RP is
vioral approaches is appealing from both theoretical and currently underway at the University of Washington to
practical standpoints [115] and MBRP is a potentially evaluate whether the addition of mindfulness to the
effective and cost-efficient adjunct to CBT-based treat- standard RP treatment leads to better substance use
ments. In contrast to the cognitive restructuring strate- outcomes following treatment. As is the case in other
gies typical of traditional CBT, MBRP stresses clinical domains [114], interest in MBRP for substance
nonjudgmental attention to thoughts or urges. From use disorders is increasing rapidly. Results of additional
this standpoint, urges/cravings are labeled as transient randomized controlled trials will be important for
events that need not be acted upon reflexively. This informing its broader application for various addictive
approach is exemplified by the urge surfing technique behaviors.
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 12 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

Critiques of the RP Model Mechanisms of treatment effects


Following the initial introduction of the RP model in the Elucidating the active ingredients of CBT treatments
1980s, its widespread application largely outpaced efforts remains an important and challenging goal. Consistent
to systematically validate the model and test its underly- with the RP model, changes in coping skills, self-efficacy
ing assumptions. Given this limitation, the National Insti- and/or outcome expectancies are the primary putative
tutes on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) mechanisms by which CBT-based interventions work
sponsored the Relapse Replication and Extension Project [126]. However, few studies support these presumptions.
(RREP), a multi-site study aiming to test the reliability One study, in which substance-abusing individuals were
and validity of Marlatts original relapse taxonomy. randomly assigned to RP or twelve-step (TS) treatments,
Efforts to evaluate the validity [119] and predictive valid- found that RP participants showed increased self-effi-
ity [120] of the taxonomy failed to generate supportive cacy, which accounted for unique variance in outcomes
data. It was noted that in focusing on Marlatts relapse [69]. In a recent study, Witkiewitz and colleagues
taxonomy the RREP did not comprehensive evaluation of (under review) found that individuals in the combined
the full RP model [121]. Nevertheless, these studies were behavioral intervention of the COMBINE study who
useful in identifying limitations and qualifications of the received drink-refusal skills training as part of the beha-
RP taxonomy and generated valuable suggestions [121]. vioral intervention had significantly better outcomes
The recently introduced dynamic model of relapse [8] than those who did not receive the drink-refusal skills
takes many of the RREP criticisms into account. Addi- training, particularly African American clients [127].
tionally, the revised model has generated enthusiasm Further, there was strong support that increases in self-
among researchers and clinicians who have observed efficacy following drink-refusal skills training was the
these processes in their data and their clients [122,123]. primary mechanism of change. In another study examin-
Still, some have criticized the model for not emphasiz- ing the behavioral intervention arm of the COMBINE
ing interpersonal factors as proximal or phasic influ- study [128], individuals who received a skills training
ences [122,123]. Other critiques include that nonlinear module focused on coping with craving and urges had
dynamic systems approaches are not readily applicable significantly better drinking outcomes via decreases in
to clinical interventions [124], and that the theory and negative mood and craving that occurred after receiving
statistical methods underlying these approaches are eso- the module.
teric for many researchers and clinicians [14]. Rather Despite findings like these, many studies of treatment
than signaling weaknesses of the model, these issues mechanisms have failed to show that theoretical media-
could simply reflect methodological challenges that tors account for salutary effects of CBT-based interven-
researchers must overcome in order to better under- tions. Also, many studies that have examined potential
stand dynamic aspects of behavior [45]. Ecological mediators of outcomes have not provided a rigorous
momentary assessment [44], either via electronic device test [129] of mechanisms of change. These results sug-
or interactive voice response methodology, could pro- gest that researchers should strive to consider alternative
vide the data necessary to fully test the dynamic model mechanisms, improve assessment methods and/or revise
of relapse. Ideally, assessments of coping, interpersonal theories about how CBT-based interventions work
stress, self-efficacy, craving, mood, and other proximal [77,130].
factors could be collected multiple times per day over
the course of several months, and combined with a Continued empirical evaluation of the RP model
thorough pre-treatment assessment battery of distal risk As the foregoing review suggests, validation of the refor-
factors. Future research with a data set that includes mulated RP model will likely progress slowly at first
multiple measures of risk factors over multiple days because researchers are only beginning to evaluate
could also take advantage of innovative modeling tools dynamic relapse processes. Currently, the dynamic
that were designed for estimating nonlinear time-varying model can be viewed as a hypothetical, theory-driven
dynamics [125]. framework that awaits empirical evaluation. Testing the
models components will require that researchers avail
Directions for Future Research themselves of innovative assessment techniques (such as
Considering the numerous developments related to RP EMA) and pursue cross-disciplinary collaboration in
over the last decade, empirical and clinical extensions of order to integrate appropriate statistical methods. Irre-
the RP model will undoubtedly continue to evolve. In spective of study design, greater integration of distal and
addition to the recent advances outlined above, we high- proximal variables will aid in modeling the interplay of
light selected areas that are especially likely to see tonic and phasic influences on relapse outcomes. As was
growth over the next several years. the case for Marlatts original RP model, efforts are
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 13 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

needed to systematically evaluate specific theoretical undoubtedly see a significant escalation in the number
components of the reformulated model [1]. of studies using fMRI to predict response to psychoso-
cial and pharmacological treatments. In this context, a
Integrating implicit cognition and neurocognition in critical question will concern the predictive and clinical
relapse models utility of brain-based measures with respect to predict-
Historically, cognitive processes have been central to the ing treatment outcome.
RP model [8]. In the last several years increasing
emphasis has been placed on dual process models of Conclusions and Policy Implications
addiction, which hypothesize that distinct (but related) Relapse prevention is a cognitive-behavioral approach
cognitive networks, each reflective of specific neural designed to help individuals anticipate and cope with
pathways, act to influence substance use behavior. setbacks during the behavior change process. The broad
According to these models, the relative balance between aim of RP, to reduce the incidence and severity of
controlled (explicit) and automatic (implicit) cognitive relapse, subsumes two basic goals: to minimize the
networks is influential in guiding drug-related decision impact of high-risk situations by increasing awareness
making [54,55]. Dual process accounts of addictive and building coping skills, and to limit relapse prone-
behaviors [56,57] are likely to be useful for generating ness by promoting a healthy and balanced lifestyle. Over
hypotheses about dynamic relapse processes and the past decade RP principles have been incorporated
explaining variance in relapse, including episodes of sud- across an increasing array of behavior domains, with
den divergence from abstinence to relapse. Implicit cog- addictive behaviors continuing to represent the primary
nitive processes are also being examined as an application.
intervention target, with some potentially promising As outlined in this review, the last decade has seen
results [62]. notable developments in the RP literature, including sig-
Related work has also stressed the importance of base- nificant expansion of empirical work with relevance to
line levels of neurocognitive functioning (for example as the RP model. Overall, many basic tenets of the RP
measured by tasks assessing response inhibition and model have received support and findings regarding its
working memory; [56]) as predicting the likelihood of clinical effectiveness have generally been supportive. RP
drug use in response to environmental cues. The study modules are standard to virtually all psychosocial inter-
of implicit cognition and neurocognition in models of ventions for substance use [17] and an increasing num-
relapse would likely require integration of distal neuro- ber of self-help manuals are available to assist both
cognitive factors (e.g., baseline performance in cognitive therapists and clients. RP strategies can now be dissemi-
tasks) in the context of treatment outcomes studies or nated using simple but effective methods; for instance,
EMA paradigms. Additionally, lab-based studies will be mail-delivered RP booklets are shown to reduce smok-
needed to capture dynamic processes involving cogni- ing relapse [135,136]. As noted earlier, the broad influ-
tive/neurocognitive influences on lapse-related ence of RP is also evidenced by the current clinical
phenomena. vernacular, as relapse prevention has evolved into an
umbrella term synonymous with most cognitive-beha-
Evaluating neural markers of relapse liability vioral skills-based interventions addressing high-risk
The use of functional magnetic resonance imaging situations and coping responses. While attesting to the
(fMRI) techniques in addictions research has increased influence and durability of the RP model, the tendency
dramatically in the last decade [131] and many of these to subsume RP within various treatment modalities can
studies have been instrumental in providing initial evi- also complicate efforts to systematically evaluate inter-
dence on neural correlates of substance use and relapse. vention effects across studies (e.g., [21]).
In one study of treatment-seeking methamphetamine Although many developments over the last decade
users [132], researchers examined fMRI activation dur- encourage confidence in the RP model, additional
ing a decision-making task and obtained information on research is needed to test its predictions, limitations and
relapse over one year later. Based on activation patterns applicability. In particular, given recent theoretical revi-
in several cortical regions they were able to correctly sions to the RP model, as well as the tendency for dif-
identify 17 of 18 participants who relapsed and 20 of 22 fuse application of RP principles across different
who did not. Functional imaging is increasingly being treatment modalities, there is an ongoing need to evalu-
incorporated in treatment outcome studies (e.g., [133]) ate and characterize specific theoretical mechanisms of
and there are increasing efforts to use imaging treatment effects.
approaches to predict relapse [134]. While the overall In the current review we have noted several areas for
number of studies examining neural correlates of relapse future research, including examining dynamic models of
remains small at present, the coming years will treatment outcomes, extensions of RP to include
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 14 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

mindfulness and/or self-control training, research on the aftercare services. It is also important that policy makers
mechanisms of change following successful treatment and funding entities support initiatives to evaluate RP
outcomes, the role of genetic influences as potential and other established interventions in the context of
moderators of treatment outcomes, and neurocognitive continuing care models. In general, more research on
and neurobiological examinations of the relapse process the acquisition and long-term retention of specific RP
using tests of implicit cognition and advanced neuroi- skills is necessary to better understand which RP skills
maging techniques. In addition to these areas, which will be most useful in long-term and aftercare treat-
already have initial empirical data, we predict that we ments for addictions.
could learn significantly more about the relapse process
using experimental manipulation to test specific aspects
of the cognitive-behavioral model of relapse. For exam- Author details
1
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, 33 Russell St., Toronto, ON, M5S
ple, it has been shown that self-efficacy for abstinence 2S1, Canada. 2Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, 250 College
can be manipulated [137]. Thus, one could test whether St., Toronto, ON M5T 1R8, Canada. 3Department of Psychology, Washington
increasing self-efficacy in an experimental design is State University, 14204 NE Salmon Creek Ave, Vancouver, WA, 98686, USA.
4
Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Box 351525, Seattle,
related to better treatment outcomes. Similarly, self-reg- WA 98195, USA.
ulation ability, outcome expectancies, and the abstinence
violation effect could all be experimentally manipulated, Authors contributions
CH wrote the manuscript with contributions from KW. BG and GAM assisted
which could eventually lead to further refinements of in conceptualizing the paper and provided critical review. All authors read
RP strategies. and approved the manuscript.
Ultimately, individuals who are struggling with beha-
Competing interests
vior change often find that making the initial change is The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
not as difficult as maintaining behavior changes over
time. Many therapies (both behavioral and pharmacolo- Received: 19 May 2011 Accepted: 19 July 2011 Published: 19 July 2011
gical) have been developed to help individuals cease or
References
reduce addictive behaviors and it is critical to refine 1. Brandon TH, Vidrine JI, Litvin EB: Relapse and relapse prevention. Annu Rev
strategies for helping individuals maintain treatment Clin Psychol 2007, 3:257-284.
goals. As noted by McLellan [138] and others [124], it is 2. Orleans CT: Promoting the maintenance of health behavior change:
Recommendations for the next generation of research and practice.
imperative that policy makers support adoption of treat- Health Psychol 2000, 19:76-83.
ments that incorporate a continuing care approach, such 3. Polivy J, Herman CP: If at first you dont succeed: False hopes of self-
that addictions treatment is considered from a chronic change. Am Psychol 2002, 57:677-689.
4. Miller WR, Westerberg VS, Harris RJ, Tonigan JS: What predicts relapse?
(rather than acute) care perspective. Broad implementa- Prospective testing of antecedent models. Addiction 1996, 91(Suppl):
tion of a continuing care approach will require policy S155-172.
change at numerous levels, including the adoption of 5. Hunt WA, Barnett LW, Branch LG: Relapse rates in addiction programs. J
Clin Psychol 1971, 27:455-456.
long-term patient-based and provider-based strategies 6. Kirshenbaum AP, Olsen DM, Bickel WK: A quantitative review of the
and contingencies to optimize and sustain treatment ubiquitous relapse curve. J Subst Abuse Treat 2009, 36:8-17.
outcomes [139,140]. 7. Marlatt GA, Gordon JR: Determinants of relapse: Implications for the
maintenance of behavior change. In Behavioral Medicine: Changing health
In support of continuing care approaches the United lifestyles. Edited by: Davidson PO, Davidson SM. New York: Brunner/Mazel;
States Office of National Drug Control Policy recently 1980:410-452.
published the 2010 National Drug Control Strategy in 8. Witkiewitz K, Marlatt GA: Relapse prevention for alcohol and drug
problems: That was Zen, this is Tao. Am Psychol 2004, 59:224-235.
the United States [141], which includes strategies to 9. Hendershot CS, Marlatt GA, George WH: Relapse prevention and the
integrate treatment for substance use disorders into the maintenance of optimal health. In The Handbook of Health Behavior
mainstream health care system and to expand support Change. Volume 2007.. 3 edition. Edited by: Shumaker S, Ockene JK, Riekert
K. New York: Springer Publishing Co; .
for continuing care efforts. One critical goal will be to 10. Witkiewitz K, Marlatt GA: Therapists Guide to Evidence-Based Relapse
integrate empirically supported substance use interven- Prevention London: Academic Press; 2007.
tions in the context of continuing care models of treat- 11. Marlatt GA, Witkiewitz K: In Relapse Prevention for Alcohol and Drug
Problems.. 2 edition. Edited by: Marlatt G Alan, Donovan Dennis M. Relapse
ment delivery, which in many cases requires adapting prevention: Maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive behaviors;
existing treatments to facilitate sustained delivery [140]. 2005:1-44, 2005..
Given its focus on long-term maintenance of treatment 12. Brownell KD, Marlatt GA, Lichtenstein E, Wilson GT: Understanding and
preventing relapse. Am Psychol 1986, 41:765-782.
gains, RP is a behavioral intervention that is particularly 13. Miller WR: Section I. theoretical perspectives on relapse: What is a
well suited for implementation in continuing care con- relapse? Fifty ways to leave the wagon. Addiction 1996, 91:S15-S27.
texts. Many treatment centers already provide RP as a 14. Maisto SA, Connors GJ: Relapse in the addictive behaviors: Integration
and future directions. Clin Psychol Rev 2006, 26:229-231.
routine component of aftercare programs. However, it is 15. Piasecki TM: Relapse to smoking. Clin Psychol Rev 2006, 26:196-215.
imperative that insurance providers and funding entities 16. Marlatt GA, Gordon JR: Relapse prevention: Maintenance strategies in the
support these efforts by providing financial support for treatment of addictive behaviors New York: Guilford Press; 1985.
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 15 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

17. McGovern MP, Wrisley BR, Drake RE: Relapse of substance use disorder 44. Stone AA, Shiffman S: Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in
and its prevention among persons with co-occurring disorders. Psychiatr behavorial medicine. Ann Behav Med 1994, 16:199-202.
Serv 2005, 56:1270-1273. 45. Gwaltney CJ, Shiffman S, Balabanis MH, Paty JA: Dynamic Self-Efficacy and
18. Project MATCH Research Group: Matching alcoholism treatments to client Outcome Expectancies: Prediction of Smoking Lapse and Relapse. J
heterogeneity: Project MATCH Posttreatment drinking outcomes. J Stud Abnorm Psychol 2005, 114:661-675.
Alcohol 1997, 58:7-29. 46. Gwaltney CJ, Shiffman S, Sayette MA: Situational Correlates of Abstinence
19. COMBINE Study Research Group: Testing combined pharmacotherapies Self-Efficacy. J Abnorm Psychol 2005, 114:649-660.
and behavioral interventions in alcohol dependence: rationale and 47. Gwaltney CJ, Metrik J, Kahler CW, Shiffman S: Self-efficacy and smoking
methods. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2003, 27:1107-1122. cessation: a meta-analysis. Psychol Addict Behav 2009, 23:56-66.
20. Rawson RA, Shoptaw SJ, Obert JL, McCann MJ, Hasson AL, Marinelli- 48. Herd N, Borland R: The natural history of quitting smoking: findings from
Casey PJ, Brethen PR, Ling W: An intensive outpatient approach for the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Four Country Survey. Addiction
cocaine abuse treatment. The Matrix model. J Subst Abuse Treat 1995, 2009, 104:2075-2087.
12:117-127. 49. Herd N, Borland R, Hyland A: Predictors of smoking relapse by duration
21. Miller WR, Wilbourne PL: Mesa Grande: A methodological analysis of of abstinence: findings from the International Tobacco Control (ITC)
clinical trials of treatments for alcohol use disorders. Addiction 2002, Four Country Survey. Addiction 2009, 104:2088-2099.
97:265-277. 50. Katz EC, Fromme K, DAmico EJ: Effects of outcome expectancies and
22. Cummings C, Gordon JR, Marlatt GA: Relapse: Strategies of prevention personality on young adults illicit drug use, heavy drinking, and risky
and prediction. The addictive behaviors: Treatment of alcoholism, drug abuse, sexual behavior. Cognit Ther Res 2000, 24:1-22.
smoking, and obesity Oxford: Pergamon; 1980, 291-321. 51. Hendershot CS, Neighbors C, George WH, McCarthy DM, Wall TL, Liang T,
23. Dimeff LA, Marlatt GA: Preventing relapse and maintaining change in Larimer ME: ALDH2, ADH1B and alcohol expectancies: integrating genetic
addictive behaviors. Clin Psychol Sci Prac 1998, 5:513-525. and learning perspectives. Psychol Addict Behav 2009, 23:452-463.
24. Larimer ME, Palmer RS, Marlatt GA: Relapse prevention: An overview of 52. McCarthy DM, Wall TL, Brown SA, Carr LG: Integrating biological and
Marlatts cognitive-behavioral model. Alcohol Res Health 1999, 23:151-160. behavioral factors in alcohol use risk: The role of ALDH2 status and
25. Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC: Addiction alcohol expectancies in a sample of Asian Americans. Exp Clin
Motivation Reformulated: An Affective Processing Model of Negative Psychopharmacol 2000, 8:168-175.
Reinforcement. Psychol Rev 2004, 111:33-51. 53. Cohen LM, McCarthy DM, Brown SA, Myers MG: Negative affect combines
26. Bandura A: Self-efficacy: The exercise of control New York: Freeman; 1997. with smoking outcome expectancies to predict smoking behavior over
27. Goldman MS, Darkes J, Del Boca FK: In Expectancy mediation of time. Psychol Addict Behav 2002, 16:91-97.
biopsychosocial risk for alcohol use and alcoholism. Edited by: Kirsch, Irving. 54. Stacy AW, Wiers RW: Implicit cognition and addiction: a tool for
Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.; 1999; explaining paradoxical behavior. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2010, 6:551-575.
1999:233-262, How expectancies shape experience. 55. Wiers RW, Stacy AW: Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction
28. Rothman AJ: Toward a theory-based analysis of behavioral maintenance. Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Health Psychol 2000, 19:64-69. Publications; 2006, 2006..
29. Marlatt GA, Donovan DM: Relapse prevention: Maintenance strategies in the 56. Field M, Wiers RW, Christiansen P, Fillmore MT, Verster JC: Acute alcohol
treatment of addictive behaviors. 2 edition. New York: Guilford; 2005. effects on inhibitory control and implicit cognition: implications for loss
30. Donovan DM, Marlatt GA: Assessment of addictive behaviors. 2 edition. New of control over drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2010, 34:1346-1352.
York: Guilford; 2005. 57. Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RC,
31. Hufford MR, Witkiewitz K, Shields AL, Kodya S, Caruso JC: Relapse as a Sher KJ, Grenard J, Ames SL, Stacy AW: Automatic and controlled
nonlinear dynamic system: Application to patients with alcohol use processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: A
disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 2003, 112:219-227. review and a model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2007, 86:263-283.
32. Bouton ME: A learning theory perspective on lapse, relapse, and the 58. Kramer DA, Goldman MS: Using a modified Stroop task to implicitly
maintenance of behavior change. Health Psychol 2000, 19:57-63. discern the cognitive organization of alcohol expectancies. J Abnorm
33. Witkiewitz K, Marlatt GA: Modeling the complexity of post-treatment Psychol 2003, 112:171-175.
drinking: its a rocky road to relapse. Clin Psychol Rev 2007, 27:724-738. 59. McCarthy DM, Thompsen DM: Implicit and Explicit Measures of Alcohol
34. Witkiewitz K, van der Maas HLJ, Hufford MR, Marlatt GA: Nonnormality and and Smoking Cognitions. Psychol Addict Behav 2006, 20:436-444.
divergence in posttreatment alcohol use: Reexamining the Project 60. Waters AJ, Shiffman S, Sayette MA, Paty JA, Gwaltney CJ, Balabanis MH:
MATCH data another way.. J Abnorm Psychol 2007, 116:378-394. Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation. Health Psychol
35. Carroll KM: Relapse prevention as a psychosocial treatment: A review of 2003, 22:378-387.
controlled clinical trials. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 1996, 4:46-54. 61. Marissen MA, Franken IH, Waters AJ, Blanken P, van den Brink W,
36. Irvin JE, Bowers CA, Dunn ME, Wang MC: Efficacy of relapse prevention: A Hendriks VM: Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse following
meta-analytic review. J Consult Clin Psychol 1999, 67:563-570. treatment. Addiction 2006, 101:1306-1312.
37. McCrady BS: Alcohol use disorders and the Division 12 Task Force of the 62. Schoenmakers TM, de Bruin M, Lux IF, Goertz AG, Van Kerkhof DH,
American Psychological Association. Psychol Addict Behav 2000, Wiers RW: Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modification training
14:267-276. in abstinent alcoholic patients. Drug Alcohol Depend 2010, 109:30-36.
38. Hajek P, Stead LF, West R, Jarvis M, Lancaster T: Relapse prevention 63. Piasecki TM, Jorenby DE, Smith SS, Fiore MC, Baker TB: Smoking
interventions for smoking cessation. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2009, Withdrawal Dynamics: III. Correlates of Withdrawal Heterogeneity. Exp
CD003999. Clin Psychopharmacol 2003, 11:276-285.
39. Lancaster T, Hajek P, Stead LF, West R, Jarvis MJ: Prevention of relapse 64. Piasecki TM, Niaura R, Shadel WG, Abrams D, Goldstein M, Fiore MC,
after quitting smoking: a systematic review of trials. Arch Intern Med Baker TB: Smoking withdrawal dynamics in unaided quitters. J Abnorm
2006, 166:828-835. Psychol 2000, 109:74-86.
40. Agboola S, McNeill A, Coleman T, Leonardi Bee J: A systematic review of 65. Piasecki TM, Fiore MC, Baker TB: Profiles in discouragement: Two studies
the effectiveness of smoking relapse prevention interventions for of variability in the time course of smoking withdrawal symptoms. J
abstinent smokers. Addiction 2010, 105:1362-1380. Abnorm Psychol 1998, 107:238-251, 2000.
41. Magill M, Ray LA: Cognitive-behavioral treatment with adult alcohol and 66. Baker TB, Japuntich SJ, Hogle JM, McCarthy DE, Curtin JJ: Pharmacologic
illicit drug users: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Stud and Behavioral Withdrawal from Addictive Drugs. Curr Dir Psychol Sci
Alcohol Drugs 2009, 70:516-527. 2006, 15:232-236.
42. Niaura R: Cognitive social learning and related perspectives on drug 67. Marlatt GA: Craving for alcohol, loss of control, and relapse: A cognitive-
craving. Addiction 2000, 95(Suppl 2):S155-163. behavioral analysis. In Alcoholism: New directions in behavioral research and
43. Shiffman S, Balabanis MH, Paty JA, Engberg J, Gwaltney CJ, Liu KS, Gnys M, treatment. Edited by: Nathan PE, Marlatt GA, Loberg T. New York: Plenum;
Hickcox M, Paton SM: Dynamic effects of self-efficacy on smoking lapse 1978:271-314.
and relapse. Health Psychol 2000, 19:315-323.
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 16 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

68. Shiffman S, Hickcox M, Paty JA, Gnys M, Kassel JD, Richards TJ: Progression transporter and receptor polymorphisms on smoking cessation in a
from a smoking lapse to relapse: Prediction from abstinence violation bupropion clinical trial. Health Psychol 2003, 22:541-548.
effects, nicotine dependence, and lapse characteristics. J Consult Clin 91. Lerman C, Wileyto EP, Patterson F, Rukstalis M, Audrain-McGovern J,
Psychol 1996, 64:993-1002. Restine S, Shields PG, Kaufmann V, Redden D, Benowitz N, Berrettini WH:
69. Brown RA, Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Strong DR: Distress tolerance and The functional mu opioid receptor (OPRM1) Asn40Asp variant predicts
duration of past smoking cessation attempts. J Abnorm Psychol 2002, short-term response to nicotine replacement therapy in a clinical trial.
111:180-185. Pharmacogenomics J 2004, 4:184-192.
70. Shiffman S, Waters AJ: Negative Affect and Smoking Lapses: A 92. Oslin DW, Berrettini W, Kranzler HR, Pettinati H, Gelernter J, Volpicelli JR,
Prospective Analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol 2004, 72:192-201. OBrien CP: A Functional Polymorphism of the mu-Opioid receptor gene
71. Gwaltney CJ, Shiffman S, Normal GJ, Paty JA, Kassel JD, Gnys M, Hickcox M, is associated with naltrexone response in alcohol-dependent patients.
Waters A, Balabanis M: Does smoking abstinence self-efficacy vary across Neuropsychopharmacology 2003, 28:1546-1552.
situations? Identifying context-specificity within the Relapse Situation 93. Anton RF, Oroszi G, OMalley S, Couper D, Swift R, Pettinati H, Goldman D:
Efficacy Questionnaire. J Consult Clin Psychol 2001, 69:516-527. An evaluation of mu-opioid receptor (OPRM1) as a predictor of
72. Witkiewitz K, Villarroel NA: Dynamic association between negative affect naltrexone response in the treatment of alcohol dependence: Results
and alcohol lapses following alcohol treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol from the Combined Pharmacotherapies and Behavioral Interventions for
2009, 77:633-644. Alcohol Dependence (COMBINE) study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008,
73. Armeli S, Tennen H, Affleck G, Kranzler HR: Does affect mediate the 65:135-144.
association between daily events and alcohol use? J Stud Alcohol 2000, 94. Ray R, Jepson C, Wileyto EP, Dahl JP, Patterson F, Rukstalis M, Pinto A,
61:862-871. Berrettini W, Lerman C: Genetic variation in mu-opioid-receptor-
74. Hussong AM, Hicks RE, Levy SA, Curran PJ: Specifying the relations interacting proteins and smoking cessation in a nicotine replacement
between affect and heavy alcohol use among young adults. J Abnorm therapy trial. Nicotine Tob Res 2007, 9:1237-1241.
Psychol 2001, 110:449-461. 95. Ray LA, Hutchison KE: A polymorphism of the mu-opioid receptor gene
75. Morgenstern J, Longabaugh R: Cognitive-behavioral treatment for alcohol (OPRM1) and sensitivity to the effects of alcohol in humans. Alcohol Clin
dependence: A review of evidence for its hypothesized mechanisms of Exp Res 2004, 28:1789-1795.
action. Addiction 2000, 95:1475-1490. 96. Filbey FM, Ray L, Smolen A, Claus ED, Audette A, Hutchison KE: Differential
76. OConnell KA, Hosein VL, Schwartz JE, Leibowitz RQ: How Does Coping neural response to alcohol priming and alcohol taste cues is associated
Help People Resist Lapses During Smoking Cessation? Health Psychol with DRD4 VNTR and OPRM1 genotypes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2008,
2007, 26:77-84. 32:1113-1123.
77. Litt MD, Kadden RM, Stephens RS: Coping and Self-Efficacy in Marijuana 97. van den Wildenberg E, Wiers RW, Dessers J, Janssen RGJH, Lambrichs EH,
Treatment: Results From the Marijuana Treatment Project. J Consult Clin Smeets HJM, van Breukelen GJP: A functional polymorphism of the mu-
Psychol 2005, 73:1015-1025. opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) influences cue-induced craving for
78. Muraven M, Baumeister RF: Self-regulation and depletion of limited alcohol in male heavy drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2007, 31:1-10.
resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychol Bull 2000, 98. Ramchandani VA, Umhau J, Pavon FJ, Ruiz-Velasco V, Margas W, Sun H,
126:247-259. Damadzic R, Eskay R, Schoor M, Thorsell A, et al: A genetic determinant of
79. Muraven M, Collins RL, Shiffman S, Paty JA: Daily Fluctuations in Self- the striatal dopamine response to alcohol in men. Mol Psychiatry 2010.
Control Demands and Alcohol Intake. Psychol Addict Behav 2005, 99. Ooteman W, Naassila M, Koeter MW, Verheul R, Schippers GM, Houchi H,
19:140-147. Daoust M, van den Brink W: Predicting the effect of naltrexone and
80. Muraven M, Collins RL, Morsheimer ET, Shiffman S, Paty JA: The Morning acamprosate in alcohol-dependent patients using genetic indicators.
After: Limit Violations and the Self-Regulation of Alcohol Consumption. Addict Biol 2009, 14:328-337.
Psychol Addict Behav 2005, 19:253-262. 100. Hutchison KE, Ray L, Sandman E, Rutter MC, Peters A, Davidson D, Swift R:
81. Muraven M: Practicing self-control lowers the risk of smoking lapse. The effect of olanzapine on craving and alcohol consumption.
Psychol Addict Behav 2010, 24:446-452. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006, 31:1310-1317.
82. Witkiewitz K, Hartzler B, Donovan D: Matching motivation enhancement 101. Hutchison KE, Wooden A, Swift RM, Smolen A, McGeary J, Adler L, Paris L:
treatment to client motivation: re-examining the Project MATCH Olanzapine reduces craving for alcohol: A DRD4 VNTR polymorphism by
motivation matching hypothesis. Addiction 2010, 105:1403-1413. pharmacotherapy interaction. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003,
83. Ho MK, Goldman D, Heinz A, Kaprio J, Kreek MJ, Li MD, Munafo MR, 28:1882-1888.
Tyndale RF: Breaking barriers in the genomics and pharmacogenetics of 102. Bauer LO, Covault J, Harel O, Das S, Gelernter J, Anton R, Kranzler HR:
drug addiction. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2010, 88:779-791. Variation in GABRA2 predicts drinking behavior in project MATCH
84. Sturgess JE, George TP, Kennedy JL, Heinz A, Muller DJ: Pharmacogenetics subjects. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2007, 31:1780-1787.
of alcohol, nicotine and drug addiction treatments. Addict Biol 2011. 103. Wojnar M, Brower KJ, Strobbe S, Ilgen M, Matsumoto H, Nowosad I,
85. Colilla S, Lerman C, Shields PG, Jepson C, Rukstalis M, Berlin J, DeMichele A, Sliwerska E, Burmeister M: Association between Val66Met brain-derived
Bunin G, Strom BL, Rebbeck TR: Association of catechol-O- neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene polymorphism and post-treatment
methyltransferase with smoking cessation in two independent studies relapse in alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2009, 33:693-702.
of women. Pharmacogenet Genomics 2005, 15:393-398. 104. Hendershot CS, Lindgren KP, Liang T, Hutchison KE: COMT and ALDH2
86. Johnstone EC, Elliot KM, David SP, Murphy MF, Walton RT, Munafo MR: polymorphisms moderate associations of implicit drinking motives with
Association of COMT Val108/158Met genotype with smoking cessation alcohol use. Addict Biol 2011.
in a nicotine replacement therapy randomized trial. Cancer Epidemiol 105. Hendershot CS, Witkiewitz K, George WH, Wall TL, Otto JM, Liang T,
Biomarkers Prev 2007, 16:1065-1069. Larimer ME: Evaluating a cognitive model of ALDH2 and drinking
87. Munafo MR, Johnstone EC, Guo B, Murphy MF, Aveyard P: Association of behavior. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2011, 35:91-98.
COMT Val108/158Met genotype with smoking cessation. Pharmacogenet 106. Miranda R, Ray L, Justus A, Meyerson LA, Knopik VS, McGeary J, Monti PM:
Genomics 2008, 18:121-128. Initial evidence of an association between OPRM1 and adolescent
88. Conti DV, Lee W, Li D, Liu J, Van Den Berg D, Thomas PD, Bergen AW, alcohol misuse. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2010, 34:112-122.
Swan GE, Tyndale RF, Benowitz NL, Lerman C: Nicotinic acetylcholine 107. Wiers RW, Rinck M, Dictus M, van den Wildenberg E: Relatively strong
receptor beta2 subunit gene implicated in a systems-based candidate automatic appetitive action-tendencies in male carriers of the OPRM1 G-
gene study of smoking cessation. Hum Mol Genet 2008, 17:2834-2848. allele. Genes Brain Behav 2009, 8:101-106.
89. Perkins KA, Lerman C, Mercincavage M, Fonte CA, Briski JL: Nicotinic 108. Young RM, Lawford BR, Feeney GFX, Ritchie T, Noble EP: Alcohol-related
acetylcholine receptor beta2 subunit (CHRNB2) gene and short-term expectancies are associated with the D2 dopamine receptor and GABA
ability to quit smoking in response to nicotine patch. Cancer Epidemiol receptor beta3 subunit genes. Psychiatry Res 2004, 127:171-183.
Biomarkers Prev 2009, 18:2608-2612. 109. Uhl GR, Liu QR, Drgon T, Johnson C, Walther D, Rose JE, David SP, Niaura R,
90. Lerman C, Shields PG, Wileyto EP, Audrain J, Hawk LH Jr, Pinto A, Lerman C: Molecular genetics of successful smoking cessation:
Kucharski S, Krishnan S, Niaura R, Epstein LH: Effects of dopamine
Hendershot et al. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011, 6:17 Page 17 of 17
http://www.substanceabusepolicy.com/content/6/1/17

convergent genome-wide association study results. Arch Gen Psychiatry 131. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ: The addicted human brain viewed in the
2008, 65:683-693. light of imaging studies: brain circuits and treatment strategies.
110. Hendershot CS, Otto JM, Collins SE, Liang T, Wall TL: Evaluation of a brief Neuropharmacology 2004, 47(Suppl 1):3-13.
web-based genetic feedback intervention for reducing alcohol-related 132. Paulus MP, Tapert SF, Schuckit MA: Neural Activation Patterns of
health risks associated with ALDH2. Ann Behav Med 2010, 40:77-88. Methamphetamine-Dependent Subjects During Decision Making Predict
111. Marteau TM, Munafo MR, Aveyard P, Hill C, Whitwell S, Willis TA, Relapse. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005, 62:761-768.
Crockett RA, Hollands GJ, Johnstone EC, Wright AJ, et al: Trial Protocol: 133. Mann K, Kiefer F, Smolka M, Gann H, Wellek S, Heinz A: Searching for
Using genotype to tailor prescribing of nicotine replacement therapy: a responders to acamprosate and naltrexone in alcoholism treatment:
randomised controlled trial assessing impact of communication upon rationale and design of the PREDICT study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2009,
adherence. BMC Public Health 2010, 10:680. 33:674-683.
112. Witkiewitz K, Marlatt GA, Walker D: Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention 134. Durazzo TC, Gazdzinski S, Mon A, Meyerhoff DJ: Cortical perfusion in
for Alcohol and Substance Use Disorders. J Cogn Psychother 2005, alcohol-dependent individuals during short-term abstinence:
19:211-228. Relationships to resumption of hazardous drinking after treatment.
113. Zgierska A, Rabago D, Chawla N, Kushner K, Koehler R, Marlatt A: Alcohol 2010, 44:201-210.
Mindfulness meditation for substance use disorders: a systematic 135. Brandon TH, Collins BN, Juliano LM, Lazev AB: Preventing relapse among
review. Subst Abus 2009, 30:266-294. former smokers: A comparison of minimal interventions through
114. Lau MA, Segal ZV: Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy as a relapse telephone and mail. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000, 68:103-113.
prevention approach to depression. In Therapists Guide to Evidence-Based 136. Brandon TH, Meade CD, Herzog TA, Chirikos TN, Webb MS, Cantor AB:
Relapse Prevention. Edited by: Witkiewitz K, Marlatt GA. London: Academic Efficacy and cost-effectiveness of a minimal intervention to prevent
Press; 2007:. smoking relapse: Dismantling the effects of amount of content versus
115. Marlatt GA: Buddhist philosophy and the treatment of addictive contact. J Consult Clin Psychol 2004, 72:797-808.
behavior. Cogn Behav Pract 2002, 9:44-49. 137. Shadel WG, Cervone D: Evaluating social-cognitive mechanisms that
116. Bowen S, Witkiewitz K, Dillworth TM, Chawla N, Simpson TL, Ostafin BD, regulate self-efficacy in response to provocative smoking cues: An
Larimer ME, Blume AW, Parks GA, Marlatt GA: Mindfulness meditation and experimental investigation. Psychol Addict Behav 2006, 20:91-96.
substance use in an incarcerated population. Psychol Addict Behav 2006, 138. McLellan AT: Have we evaluated addiction treatment correctly?
20:343-347. Implications from a chronic care perspective. Addiction 2002, 97:249-252.
117. Bowen S, Chawla N, Collins SE, Witkiewitz K, Hsu S, Grow J, Clifasefi S, 139. Humphreys K, McLellan AT: A policy-oriented review of strategies for
Garner M, Douglass A, Larimer ME, Marlatt A: Mindfulness-based relapse improving the outcomes of services for substance use disorder
prevention for substance use disorders: a pilot efficacy trial. Subst Abus patients*. Addiction 2011.
2009, 30:295-305. 140. McKay JR, Carise D, Dennis ML, Dupont R, Humphreys K, Kemp J,
118. Witkiewitz K, Bowen S: Depression, craving, and substance use following Reynolds D, White W, Armstrong R, Chalk M, et al: Extending the benefits
a randomized trial of mindfulness-based relapse prevention. J Consult of addiction treatment: practical strategies for continuing care and
Clin Psychol 2010, 78:362-374. recovery. J Subst Abuse Treat 2009, 36:127-130.
119. Maisto SA, Connors GJ, Zywiak WH: Replication and extension of Marlatts 141. Office of National Drug Control Policy: National Drug Control Strategy for
taxonomy: Construct validation analyses on the Marlatt typology of 2010.[http://www.ondcp.gov].
relapse precipitants. Addiction 1996, 91:S89-S97.
120. Stout RL, Longabaugh R, Rubin A: Replication and extension of Marlatts doi:10.1186/1747-597X-6-17
taxonomy: Predictive validity of Marlatts relapse taxonomy versus a Cite this article as: Hendershot et al.: Relapse prevention for addictive
more general relapse code. Addiction 1996, 91:S99-S110. behaviors. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 2011 6:17.
121. Donovan DM: Commentary on replications of Marlatts taxonomy:
Marlatts classification of relapse precipitants: Is the emperor still
wearing clothes? Addiction 1996, 91:S131-S137.
122. Hunter-Reel D, McCrady B, Hildebrandt T: Emphasizing interpersonal
factors: an extension of the Witkiewitz and Marlatt relapse model.
Addiction 2009, 104:1281-1290.
123. Stanton M: Relapse Prevention Needs More Emphasis on Interpersonal
Factors. Am Psychol 2005, 60:340-341.
124. McKay JR, Franklin TR, Patapis N, Lynch KG: Conceptual, methodological,
and analytical issues in the study of relapse. Clin Psychol Rev 2006,
26:109-127.
125. Walls TA, Schafer JL: Models for Intensive Longitudinal Data New York:
Oxford University Press; 2006.
126. Longabaugh R, Donovan DM, Karno MP, McCrady BS, Morgenstern J,
Tonigan JS: Active ingredients: How and why evidence-based alcohol
behavioral treatment interventions work. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2005,
29:235-247.
127. Witkiewitz K, Villarroel NA, Hartzler B, Donovan DM: Drinking outcomes
following drink refusal skills training: Differential effects for African
American and non-Hispanic White clients. Psychol Addict Behav 2011, Submit your next manuscript to BioMed Central
25:162-167. and take full advantage of:
128. Witkiewitz K, Bowen S, Donovan DM: Moderating effects of a craving
intervention on the relation between negative mood and heavy
Convenient online submission
drinking following treatment for alcohol dependence. J Consult Clin
Psychol 2011, 79:54-63. Thorough peer review
129. Kazdin AE, Nock MK: Delineating mechanisms of change in child and No space constraints or color figure charges
adolescent therapy: methodological issues and research
recommendations. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2003, 44:1116-1129. Immediate publication on acceptance
130. Morgenstern J, Longabaugh R: Cognitive-behavioral treatment for alcohol Inclusion in PubMed, CAS, Scopus and Google Scholar
dependence: A review of evidence for its hypothesized mechanisms of
Research which is freely available for redistribution
action. Addiction 2000, 95:1475-1490.

Submit your manuscript at


www.biomedcentral.com/submit

Anda mungkin juga menyukai