Anda di halaman 1dari 18

Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

DOI 10.1007/s10464-014-9659-x

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Navigating Middle Grades: Role of Social Contexts in Middle


Grade School Climate
Ha Yeon Kim Kate Schwartz Elise Cappella

Edward Seidman

Published online: 16 May 2014


Society for Community Research and Action 2014

Abstract During early adolescence, most public school Introduction


students undergo school transitions, and many students
experience declines in academic performance and social- Early adolescence is a vulnerable period for both academic
emotional well-being. Theories and empirical research and social-emotional development (Eccles and Midgley
have highlighted the importance of supportive school 1989; Simmons and Blyth 1987). Many early adolescents
environments in promoting positive youth development experience declines in their academic performance (Barber
during this period of transition. Despite this, little is known and Olsen 2004), motivation (Maehr and Midgley 1996),
about the proximal social and developmental contexts of and engagement (Archambault et al. 2009). Social-emo-
the range of middle grade public schools US students tional struggles ensue during this period, including
attend. Using a cross-sectional dataset from the eighth decreases in students sense of belonging in school (Maehr
grade wave of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study and Midgley 1996; Wang and Eccles 2012) and self-esteem
Kindergarten Cohort 19981999, the current study exam- (Archambault et al. 2010), as well as increases in anxiety,
ines the middle grade school social context from the per- depression, and behavioral difficulties (Grills-Taquechel
spectives of administrators and teachers in public schools et al. 2010; Way et al. 2007). These struggles may be
with typical grade configurations (k8 schools, middle magnified when students are undergoing a transition to a
schools, and junior high schools) and how it relates to new school during the early adolescent period (Eccles
students perceptions of school climate. We find that 2004; Eccles and Midgley 1989; Seidman et al. 2004).
administrators and teachers in k8 schools perceive a more Ecological theories and empirical research have high-
positive school social context, controlling for school lighted the importance of supportive developmental social
structural and demographic characteristics. This school settings, such as family, classrooms, schools, and neigh-
social context, in turn, is associated with students per- borhood community contexts, during early adolescence
ceptions of their schools social and academic climate. (Bronfenbrenner and Morris 2006; Cappella et al. 2013;
Implications for educational policy and practice are Delany-Brumsey et al. 2014; Eccles and Midgley 1989;
discussed. McCoy et al. 2013; Smith et al. 2013). In particular,
researchers have focused on the central role of school
Keywords Middle grade schools  School climate  social contexts in promoting positive youth adjustment and
School transitions  Adolescent development  School development (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
reform  Educational policy 2009; National Research Council 2004; Simmons and
Blyth 1987; Trickett and Rowe 2012). Increasingly, evi-
dence suggests that the middle grade school climate plays a
significant role in young adolescents academic and social-
emotional adjustment (Brand et al. 2003; Jia et al. 2009;
H. Y. Kim (&)  K. Schwartz  E. Cappella  E. Seidman
Way et al. 2007). Despite growing interest in the social
Department of Applied Psychology, New York University, 246
Greene Street, New York, NY 10003, USA context of middle grade schools, and evidence for its
e-mail: hayeon@nyu.edu influence on student adjustment, current understanding of

123
Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845 29

middle grade school social context is limited in scope. and Wellborn 1994). Across these studies, school climate is
Most studies focus on students perception of school cli- often conceptualized and measured using different dimen-
mate (e.g., Brand et al. 2003; Way et al. 2007) or the sions, reflecting the difficulties of defining school climate
structural characteristics of middle grade schools, particu- (Anderson 1982). Interpersonal and academic dimensions
larly around grade span configuration (e.g., Benner and of student-perceived school climate seem to be the most
Graham 2009; Kieffer 2013). Far less consideration has common and consistent predictors of multiple domains of
been given to other perspectives or dimensions of the student outcomes (Brand et al. 2003; Jia et al. 2009; Way
school social setting, such as administrators perception of et al. 2007).
student conduct and teachers reports of stress and pro- The current literature on school climate is incomplete.
fessional climate. In addition, to our knowledge, no First, with a few exceptions (e.g., Brand et al. 2003), most
empirical literature has examined school social context studies using student perceptions involve a small number
across a national sample with grade spans typical of US of schools. Replication of these studies with a large
public middle grade schools (e.g., k8 schools, middle national sample of schools is necessary to generalize the
schools, junior high schools).1 findings across the diversity of schools in the United States.
In this article, we aim to develop a multi-reporter Second, these studies conduct analyses on the individual-
understanding of the middle grade social context across level, focusing on how individual student perceptions are
these typical school grade spans. In addition, we examine linked to student outcomes. Given that individual charac-
the potential role of middle grade social context as a link teristics influence perceptions of social phenomena
between school grade span and students perceptions of (Mitchell et al. 2010), school-level analyses of social
their schools. Our ultimate goal is to provide theoretically- context are needed to adequately assess school social
and empirically-based evidence for structural and contex- context. Third, most studies rely on students reports of
tual school reform and policy change toward best sup- school social climate, and do not include other participants
porting public middle grade school students academic and in these settings, such as teachers and administrators. This
social-emotional well-being. is despite findings suggesting that teachers may be better
raters of school climate than students when comparing
School Climate and Grade Configurations in the Middle social climate between (as opposed to within) schools
Grades (Bryk et al. 2010; Nathanson et al. 2013). Lastly, despite
calls to improve school climate (National School Climate
Middle grade school context has been of increasing interest Council 2007), we know relatively little about how the
among developmental and educational researchers. Envi- broader school context relates to students perceptions of
ronment-stage fit theory (Eccles and Midgley 1989) sug- school climate. Given evidence linking student perceptions
gests certain types of middle grade schools may not be to outcomes, a better understanding of how school level
supportive of early adolescents development. Testing such factors influence student perceptions could be used to aid
assumptions, researchers have directly and indirectly policy makers and educators in strategically improving
examined middle grade school social contexts, with the school climate in the middle grades.
majority of the literature examining students perceptions Another major focus of research on middle grade school
of the school social climate (e.g., Eccles et al. 1993; contexts has been on structural aspects of middle grade
Seidman et al. 1994). Researchers have also examined a schools, with a particular focus on grade span configura-
broader range of school climate dimensions, including tions. This line of research suggests that the transition to
interpersonal (e.g., teacher support, peer support), organi- middle grade schools, compared to continuing attendance
zational (e.g., safety, chaos), and instructional/academic in k8 schools, is negatively associated with student aca-
(e.g., student commitment to achievement, instructional demic and social-emotional outcomes in both correlational
innovation) aspects that influence students adjustment in (e.g., Byrnes and Ruby 2007; Rockoff and Lockwood
multiple domains (Brand et al. 2003; Jia et al. 2009; Ku- 2010; Weiss and Kipnes 2006) and causal examinations
perminc et al. 2001; Rutter and Maughan 2002; Skinner (e.g., Kieffer 2013; Schwerdt and West 2013). For exam-
ple, Schwerdt and West (2013) find that students moving
1 from elementary to middle grade schools suffer a sharp
These are the terms that are typically used in the middle grade
school literature. Generally, k8 schools include schools that serve drop in academic achievement, experience an increase in
pre-kindergarten or kindergarten to 8th grade; middle schools include school absences, and are less likely to still be enrolled in
schools that primarily serve 6th, 7th, and 8th graders; and junior high school by grade 10 as compared with students who do not
schools serve 7th, 8th, or 7th, 8th, and 9th graders. Some of these
transition. There is no clear consensus on whether there are
schools may serve higher grade levels as well (e.g., schools serving
7th12th grade). This study uses the term k8, middle, and junior high differences in student outcomes between transitioning to
schools liberally, including those schools serving higher grades. middle schools versus junior high schools. Some studies

123
30 Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

demonstrate that, for students who experience a school Key dimensions of school-wide social contexts that have
transition in the middle years, a transition in 6th grade (to not been fully integrated in the school climate literature
middle schools) may be more detrimental than a transition include school disorder and stress. Specifically, chaotic
in 7th grade (to junior high schools: e.g., Cook et al. 2008; school environments with frequent and uncontrollable
Rockoff and Lockwood 2010). Others find transitions in interruptions and disturbances due to high levels of vio-
6th or 7th grade equally detrimental to student achievement lence, teacher mobility, and racial tensions may be highly
(e.g., Schwartz et al. 2011; Seidman et al. 1994). Thus far, stressful for students (Bellmore et al. 2012; Birnbaum et al.
this research has been conducted with a relatively small 2003; Grannis 1992; Wang and Gordon 2012). In addition,
number of schools or schools in specific states or cities school culture in which noncompliance and disruptive
(e.g., Rockoff and Lockwood 2010; Schwerdt and West behavior is prevalent not only contributes to negative
2013), precluding generalizations across districts and school climate but also affects students academic
regions. achievement (Flannery et al. 2009; Mitchell et al. 2010;
Taken together, little, if any, research has directly Warren et al. 2003). Successful interventions focused on
compared the social contexts of schools with different positive management of school-wide student behaviors
grade configurations. Although studies seem to suggest k8 have a positive influence on overall school climate (e.g.,
schools may better support students needs than middle Bradshaw et al. 2009; Mitchell et al. 2010).
grade schools (for a review, see Seidman et al. 2004), it is Teacher perceptions and experiencessuch as staff
unclear whether there are systematic differences in social professional climate, agency, and teaching burdensare
context between middle grade schools with varying grade also important dimensions of school social context.
span configurations. Teachers engage in social and professional interactions
with other staff in their unique school settings. There is
growing recognition that a positive professional climate
Social Contexts of Middle Grade Schools among staff is critical to positive school-wide climate and
successful school reforms (Edgerson et al. 2006; Rhodes
The school setting, like other social settings, has a set of et al. 2009). Teachers feelings of satisfaction and control
psychological and institutional attributes that give it a have been positively associated with teaching and
distinctive interpersonal context (Kuperminc et al. 1997). instructional behaviors (Skaalvik and Skaalvik 2007;
Within a school building, each individualincluding stu- Tschannen-Moran and Hoy 2001). Additionally, some
dents, administrators, and teachersare participants in evidence suggests middle grade school teachers feel less
social interactions, both dyadic and school-wide, with efficacious than elementary or high school teachers (Eccles
students and adults in different roles with different inter- et al. 1993; Midgley et al. 1995). Moreover, few training
ests. Each individual contributes to the overall school programs or certification requirements are geared toward
social context as a member of the school community, and specializing in the learning style and needs of middle grade
also has a unique perspective on the school context. For children. Lastly, teachers perception of the barriers to or
example, teachers perceptions of teaching burdens are burdens of teaching may play an important role in teachers
significantly associated with the overall school climate and daily interactions with students and other members of the
student outcomes (Jones et al. 2011; Thijs et al. 2008). school community (Raver et al. 2009; Wasik et al. 2006).
Despite this, the school climate literature has focused In sum, school-level disorder and stress, as well as
predominantly on students perspectives, overlooking the teacher experiences, perceptions, and training contribute to
views and processes of other participants of school social the social context of middle grade schools and should be
settings, such as teachers and administrators. Educational considered in combination as potential influences on stu-
and organizational health research suggests social pro- dents middle grade school experiences. By comparing the
cesses that do not directly involve students, for example overall social context of middle grade schools and exam-
teacherteacher and teacheradministrator relationships, as ining its association with students perception of school
well as teacher and administrator perceptions and attitudes, climate, we may better determine which types of middle
contribute to overall school health or climate (Hoy and school grade configurations are most supportive of early
Hannum 1997; Marks and Printy 2003; Sweetland and Hoy adolescent students development.
2000). In addition, students perceptions of school climate
are influenced by individual experiences and relationships Current Study
(Baker 1999; Goldstein et al. 2008). Examining the rela-
tions between administrators and teachers perspectives Informed by the current literature, this study examines
and student perceptions will provide a more complete social contexts across middle grade schools with different
picture of middle grade school social contexts. grade span configurations and the associations between

123
Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845 31

school-level social context and students perceptions of The data used in the present analysis were provided by
school climate. By doing so, we aim to bridge the gap students, teachers, and school administrators. Students
between the school climate literature focused on students were evenly split by gender (51 % female) with an average
individual perspectives and the middle school transition eighth grade student age of 14 years. They are predomi-
literature testing impacts of varying grade span configura- nately white (62 %), followed by Hispanic (18 %), African
tions. Incorporating the perspectives and experiences of American (9 %), Asian (6 %), and other (5 %). Students
administrators and teachers, we focus on understanding represent an even distribution of the range of socioeco-
school-wide social contexts and processes as an overarch- nomic statuses (SES) with the exception of an underrep-
ing construct that collectively influences students percep- resentation of the lowest quintile (22 % are in the top
tions of their schools. Specifically, utilizing the eighth quintile followed by 20, 21, 20, and 15 %). Teacher par-
grade cross-sectional data from a large national dataset ticipants include 5,085 unique English (n = 2,778) and
the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten math or science teachers (n = 3,307). The average teacher
Class of 19981999 (ECLS-K)we (1) describe the factor completed a bachelors degree, had 14 years of teaching
structure of middle grade school social context as reported (SD = 10.40), and was 44 years old (SD = 11.76) at this
by administrators and teachers; (2) examine variation in wave of data collection. School administrator surveys were
social context among middle grade schools with different reported by the principal (N = 1,712). On average, the
grade span configurations (k8 schools; 68 middle principals reported 12 years teaching (SD = 6.28), eight
schools; 79 junior high schools), controlling for school years as a principal (SD = 6.54), and five years as a
demographic and structural characteristics; and (3) test principal at their current school (SD = 4.55). The majority
middle grade school social context as a potential link had a masters degree or higher.
between school grade span configuration and students
perceptions of school climate. Using a multiple indicator Measures
multiple causes (MIMIC) approach with multi-level
structural equation models (MSEM), this study aims to School Social Context: Administrator- and Teacher-
enhance understanding of school-level differences in social Reports
contexts between different types of middle grade schools
and how these differences may influence the experiences To examine the factor structures of middle school social
and development of students within those schools. By contexts, we identified 25 social context items from the
doing so, this study seeks to inform the development of administrator-report (8 items) and teacher-report (17 items)
more supportive, positive school environments for all questionnaires (Table 2). These items were chosen from
public school students. within the constraints of the ECLS-K data with a focus on
capturing school-wide stressors as well as teachers expe-
riences in, and perceptions about, the school context.
In order to measure school-wide stressors, administra-
Method tors were asked the degree to which teacher turnover; gang
activities; and racial tensions are problems in their schools
Sample (five-point scale: 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly
agree). They also reported on the frequency of student
The data used in this study are drawn from the Early conduct problems in school, including class cutting, theft,
Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class vandalism, and bullying on a (five-point Likert scale:
19981999 (ECLS-K). The ECLS-K followed a nationally 1 = happens daily and 5 = never happens).
representative sample of 21,260 kindergarteners from the Teachers experiences and perceptions were captured
19981999 school year through the 20062007 school using questions from the eighth grade teacher question-
year, at which time the majority of participants were naire. The first three questions asked teachers about their
enrolled in eighth grade. It is a multi-method and multi- own teaching experience (e.g., I really enjoy my present
source study that includes: interviews with parents, prin- teaching job.). The second set of questions included 14
cipal and teacher surveys, student records, direct student items asking teachers about their perceptions of the various
assessments, and student self-reports. For this study, the aspects of the school social context, including staff climate,
sample consists of the 5,754 students from 1,712 schools difficulties in teaching, and their own attitude toward
who participated in the 2007 spring (Wave 7) data col- teaching (e.g., Staff members in this school generally
lection; and were: (1) in eighth grade; (2) attending a k8 have school spirit; The level of misbehavior in this
school, middle school, or junior high school; and (3) school interferes with my teaching; I feel that its part of
attending a regular public school. my responsibility to keep students from dropping out of

123
32 Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

Table 1 Descriptives of school structural and demographic variables how often students feel like they fit in; feel close to
N % classmates; feel close to teachers; enjoy being at school;
and feel safe at school. Responses were on a four-point
Grade span scale from 1 (never) to 4 (always). Students perception of
k8 schools (PK/K 8/12) 194 11 peers academic values were measured using three items:
Middle schools (6 8/12) 1,087 63 how important is it to their close friends that they attend
Junior high schools (7 8/9/12) 431 25 classes regularly; get good grades; continue their education
School location urbanicity past high school. The responses ranged from 1 (not
Urban 562 33 important) to 3 (very important). A questionnaire on stu-
Suburban 710 42 dents perception of peer support among their classmates
Rural 408 24 consisted of five items that asked whether their classmates
% Hispanic students think it is important to be their friend; like them the way
Less than 1 % 91 5 they are; care about their feelings; like them as much as
1 % to less than 5 % 533 33 they like others; and really care about them (five-point
5 % to less than 10 % 242 15 Likert scale: 1 = never to 5 = always).
10 % to less than 25 % 310 19 Both exploratory and confirmatory factor models of
25 % or more 447 28 these 12 items revealed and confirmed the three construct
% African American students modelschool attachment, peer academic values, peer
Less than 1 % 80 5 supportas the best fitting model (for details on factor
1 % to less than 5 % 606 37 analysis procedure and results, see Appendix 1).
5 % to less than 10 % 244 15
10 % to less than 25 % 316 20 School Structure
25 % or more 375 23
Based on our literature review, we hypothesized that the
N Mean SD Min Max
grade span configurations of a school as well as key
School size 1,700 783.53 341.13 100 5,000 demographics would influence the school context. In order
% free lunch 1,712 34.56 26.05 0 95 to both test and control for this, we included the following
Facility quality 1,689 60.81 28.92 0 100 variables in our analyses.

Grade Span Schools included in the ECLS-K dataset


have various school grade span configurations. For mean-
school). For all items, teachers responded on a five-point ingful comparison, school grade span was coded in three
Likert scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to categories based on transition timing: k8, middle, and
5 = strongly agree (see Table 2 for all items). junior high schools. Specifically, schools that started in
For this study, we calculated and used the mean teacher either pre-kindergarten or kindergarten and extended
response for each school. We also ran intra-class correla- through 8th or 12th grade were coded as k8 schools
tions (ICCs) for all of the teacher items, in order to assess (11 %); schools beginning in 6th grade and extending
how well items distinguish between schools as opposed to through 8th or 12th grade were coded as middle schools
between teachers within the same school. ICCs were suf- (64 %); and lastly, schools beginning in 7th grade and
ficiently large to confidently support interpreting teacher extending through 8th, 9th, or 12th grade were coded as
responses as school level context variables for all but four junior high schools (25 %).
items where the ICC B 0.032 (see Table 2 notes for items).
Structure and Demographics Past work suggests that
Student Perceptions of School Climate school urbanicity, school size, student composition, and
facility quality may contribute to school social context
Thirteen items from the 8th grade student survey were used (Anderson 1982; Buckley et al. 2005; Hannaway and
to measure student perceptions of school climate, including Talbert 1993; Opdenakker and Damme 2007). We selected
school attachment, peer academic values, and peer support. one variablewhether the school is in an urban, suburban,
Specifically, the five items on school attachment included: or rural locationas an indicator of school location urba-
nicity. For student composition, four variables were
2 selected: number of students enrolled, % Hispanic students,
We initially included these items in our analyses, eventually
dropping the final model which had low ICCs and did not load onto % black students, and % students eligible for free lunch.
the final factor structure model. Facility quality was measured using a composite score of

123
Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845 33

nine items reported by the administrator on the adequacy of clear guidelines for model fit evaluation of multilevel
the following facilities: cafeteria, computer lab, library/ models (Marsh et al. 2012). Following suggestions of
media center, art room, gymnasium, music room, play- recent studies, we used a strict cut-off value for SRMR-
ground/school yard, classrooms, auditorium/multi-purpose B B 0.06 to evaluate school-level models (Hsu 2009), and
room. Responses were originally coded on a five-point liberal criteria for RMSEA (B0.08) and CFI (C0.80) given
scale. However, due to the skewed distribution of the data the large ECLS-K sample size (Marsh et al. 2004a, b).
with the majority of respondents answering always ade-
quate for each question, the responses were dichotomized Question 1: Factor Structure of School Social Contexts
at the item level (0 = do not have, never adequate, often
not adequate, and sometimes not adequate and 1 = always To examine the factor structure of school social contexts,
adequate) Descriptive statistics for school structure and the first stage of analysis was to determine the best-fitting
demographic variables are listed in Table 1. measurement model. A series of preliminary exploratory
and confirmatory factor analyses were run. First, we used
Student Covariates 40 % of the randomly-split school sample to conduct
exploratory factor analyses with administrator- and tea-
We hypothesized that a number of student level demo- cher-report items, using a geomin rotation solution. We
graphics would influence students perceptions of school extracted two factors for administrator-report items (school
social processes. Therefore, we included the following chaos and student conduct problems) and four factors for
covariates at the student level: gender; race/ethnicity; teacher-report items (teacher professional climate, agency,
socioeconomic status (SES); seventh grade self-perception teacher burden, commitment), considering the model fits
of math and reading ability; internalizing problems, and and related theories and prior studies on school social
locus of control. context. Second, we confirmed these within-reporter factor
structures using the remaining 60 % of the sample. In the
next step, we combined teacher-report and administer-
Results report items and performed a confirmatory factor analysis
at the school level using the whole school sample. The
Prior to data analysis, we assessed missing information. For findings confirmed the six social context factors with a
the majority of variables included in the analysis, no more reasonable fit (v2(237) = 1314.85, p \ .001; RMSEA =
than 3 % of schools had missing data. The exceptions were 0.03; CFI = 0.88; SRMR-B = 0.06). The six social con-
% Hispanic (5 %), % African-American (5 %), % free text factors were strongly correlated with each other in the
lunch status (9 %), and school urbanicity (10 %). Our expected directions. Interestingly, the teacher commitment
analysis, nevertheless, included data for all 1,712 schools. factor was not correlated with these factors: administrators
School-level weights are not available for the schools reports of school chaos and student conduct (see Fig. 1 for
included in Wave 7 (8th grade) of the ECLS-K. In addition, details).
schools in this 7th wave are not representative of the initial Although school social context comprises six different
national school sample. factors, we expected the various dimensions to be related
To address the research aims, we utilized a series of and collectively influence student perceptions of school
single- and multi-level exploratory and confirmatory factor social climate. Thus, we subjected the six factors to a
analyses as well as multi-level structural equation models second-order factor analysis to see if these factors reflected
(MSEM). All models were estimated using maximum an overarching construct. The residual variances between
likelihood estimation with robust standard errors and a school chaos and student conduct problems were allowed
mean-adjusted chi square statistic test (Asparouhov and to correlate because these measures were completed by the
Muthen 2006) on Mplus 7.0 software (Muthen and Muthen same source (administrator), and allowing the errors to
2012). In all SEM models, factor variance was fixed at 1 correlate significantly improved the model fit (Bentler
and all item factor loadings were freely estimated. As 2000). The initial second order model with six first-order
suggested by Kline (2011), we evaluated the fit of all multi- factors had a significantly better fit than the first-order
level confirmatory analysis and MSEM using multiple factor model, as indicated by the Satorra-Bentler scaled chi
indices of model fit, including both level-specific indices square difference test results (Dv2(8) = 255.82, p \ .001)
such as standardized root mean square residuals for within- but was not fully satisfactory with SRMR-B = 0.07. In
model (SRMR-W) and between-level (SRMR-B); and addition, we found only a modest factor loading of teacher
overall model fit indices such as the model chi square commitment (0.32) on the overarching secondary factor.
statistic, the root mean square error of approximation To improve the model fit, we omitted the teacher com-
(RMSEA), and comparative fit index (CFI). There are no mitment factor and its items from the analysis. The

123
34 Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

modified model fit the data well with 5 latent factors of Question 3: Predicting Student Perceptions of School
social context: school chaos, student conduct problems, Climate
staff professional climate, teacher agency, and teaching
burden (v2(164) = 950.10, p \ .001; RMSEA = 0.03; To account for the nested structure of the data (students
CFI = 0.90; SRMR-B = 0.06). Factor loadings of each nested in schools), we conducted two-level structural
item are presented in Table 2. School chaos, student con- equation modeling, which accounts for the dependency
duct problems, and teaching burden factors negatively among student reports within schools and allows school-
loaded on the overarching social context factor; and staff and student-level variables to be modeled distinctly. The
professional climate and teacher agency factors had posi- use of different reporters and methods to measure school
tive loadings on the overarching social context factor. social context and student perceptions of school social
climate constructs reduces potential bias due to shared
reporters (Bank et al. 1990).
Question 2: Difference in School Social Context Prior to testing the hypothesized model, we examined an
by Grade Spans unconditional model to decompose the amount of variance
that existed between student- and school-levels. In this
To determine the extent to which the social context factor model, student reports of climate were allowed to be cor-
varied among k8 schools, middle schools, and junior high related at both student (r = 0.330.76, p \ .001) and
schools we used a multiple indicators and multiple causes school levels (r = 0.200.23, p \ .001), with random
(MIMIC) model. MIMIC models allow for simultaneous intercepts to consider mean level differences between
factor analysis and regression of factor scores on covariates schools. Student reports of school climate in the uncondi-
in order to test for heterogeneity in factor means and mea- tional model had low intraclass correlations (school
surement invariance across groups (Joreskog and Goldber- attachment = 0.03; peer support = 0.03; and peer aca-
ger 1975; Muthen 1989). In order to compare three different demic values = 0.05), suggesting most of the variance in
grade span configurations, we created two binary variables student reports could be explained by individual differ-
that indicated school membership for k8 schools and ences within schools rather than between schools. Typi-
middle schools with junior high schools as the reference cally such low ICCs are considered indicators of
group. Then, we regressed the second-order school context independency of individual scores from the cluster, and
factor on these two binary variables. In addition, we inclu- used for a rationale for ignoring the nested structure.
ded a host of school level covariates to control for school However, given our research focus on between-school
demographic and structural characteristics such as urba- differences in student outcomes, it remains important to
nicity, school size, free lunch %, Hispanic and black student consider the nested structure of the data. In addition, given
%, and school facility quality. The results of this MIMIC the highly significant correlations and shared respondents,
model are presented in Fig. 2. The best-fitting MIMIC error variance of the student-report of school climate
model provided a good fit to the data, v2(202) = 1,884.02, variables was allowed to covary for further analyses.
p \ .001; CFI = 0.82; RMSEA = 0.03; SRMR-B = 0.06. The next step was to include student-level covariates in
Further modifications did not improve the overall fit. the student-level models to examine within-school associ-
When school covariates were entered into the model, the ations. Students individual characteristics significantly
direction of loadings of the social context factors reversed. explained the within-school individual variations in per-
School chaos, student conduct problems, and teaching ceptions of school climate33 % of school attachment
burdens had positive loadings on overall school context; (p \ .001, 95 % CI [0.31, 0.35]), 29 % of peer support
and staff professional climate and teacher satisfaction had (p \ .001, 95 % CI [0.13, 0.17]), and 15 % of peer aca-
negative loadings. Thus, a higher score of overall school demic values (p \ .001, 95 % CI [0.13, 0.17]).
social context indicates a more negative social context. Lastly, the hypothesized model was specified as shown in
The SEM results (see Fig. 2) suggest that school social Fig. 3. The bolded arrows indicate the hypothesized path
context varies by school grade span configurations. Spe- between school grade span configurations, school social con-
cifically, k8 schools have a more positive social context text, and student-report of school climate. School and student
compared to middle schools and junior high schools con- covariates were included to control for differences between
trolling for school demographic and structural character- schools and students. The hypothesized model provided a good
istics. There was no significant difference between middle fit to the data, v2(413) = 1,906.26, p \ .001, CFI = 0.92,
and junior high schools. RMSEA = 0.03, SRMR-W = 0.001, SRMR-B = 0.06. To

123
Table 2 Standardized factor loadings of school social context variables for the second-order confirmatory factor analysis model
Second Order Factor: School Social Context
Factor 1: Factor 2: Factor 3: Factor 4: Factor 5:
School chaos Student conduct problems Staff professional climate Teacher agency Teaching burdensa

Factor loading -0.46 [-0.49, -0.43] -0.41 [-0.44, -0.38] 0.68 [0.65, 0.71] 0.73 [0.70, 0.76] -0.92 [-0.95, -0.89]
[95 %CI]

Mean (SD) Factor loading Mean (SD) Factor loading Mean (SD) Factor loading Mean (SD) Factor loading Mean (SD) Factor loading
(SE) (SE) (SE) (SE) (SE)

Teacher turnover 4.28 (0.90) 0.47 (0.03)


Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

Gang activity 3.57 (1.09) 0.80 (0.02)


Racial tension 1.79 (0.97) 0.67 (0.02)
Class cutting 3.51 (1.10) 0.70 (0.02)
Physical conflict 3.36 (0.89) 0.76 (0.02)
Theft 3.65 (0.69) 0.67 (0.02)
Vandalism 3.81 (0.65) 0.64 (0.02)
Student bullying 2.85 (0.99) 0.55 (0.02)
Teachers in this school are continually learning and seeking new ideas 4.04 (0.53) 0.68 (0.03)
Staff members in this school generally have school spirit 3.80 (0.61) 0.70 (0.03)
I feel accepted and respected as a colleague by most staff members 4.36 (0.46) 0.61 (0.03)
I really enjoy my present teaching job 4.38 (0.54) 0.87 (0.02)
I am certain I am making a difference in the lives of the students I teach 4.36 (0.43) 0.61 (0.03)
If I could start over, I would choose teaching again as my career 4.12 (0.65) 0.61 (0.02)
The level of misbehavior in this school interferes with my teaching 2.67 (0.90) 0.70 (0.02)
Many of the students I teach are not capable of learning the material I am supposed to teach them 2.23 (0.71) 0.54 (0.02)
Parents are supportive of school staff 3.50 (0.66) -0.61 (0.02)
The attitudes and habits students bring to my class greatly reduce their chances for academic success 3.30 (0.71) 0.65 (0.02)
My success or failure in teaching is due primarily to factors beyond my control rather than my own effort or ability 2.54 (0.71) 0.51 (0.03)
I feel sometimes it is a waste of my time to try to do my best as teacher 1.72 (0.61) 0.49 (0.03)
All factor loadings were significant at p \ .001
Four items, including I feel that its part of my responsibility to keep students from dropping out of school, If some students in my class not doing well, I feel that I should change my
approach to the subject, By trying different method, I can significantly affect a students achievement, and I work to create lessons so my students will enjoy learning and become
independent thinkers were factored into teacher commitment factorthis factor and the items were dropped due to low factor loading (\0.40) on the second-order school social context
factor. These items had very low ICCs (B0.03), suggesting that these items are better measures of individual differences and not of school level differences
a
One item, Routine administrative duties and paperwork interfere with my job of teaching, was dropped from the final CFA due to the low factor loading (\0.40) on the teaching burdens
factor
35

123
36 Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

avoid possible model misidentification due to the low ICC, we Discussion


ran the model with twenty different starting values. Each
yielded the same converged model. A full model with the Early adolescence is a time of significant developmental
standardized estimates of school level path coefficients is change. Despite an increasing focus on supportive school
illustrated in Fig. 3 and the total indirect and total effects of the environments during these years as a means to foster
main variables, along with standardized coefficients of covar- positive development, little is known about the school
iates, are reported in Table 3. Coefficients of school- and stu- context of middle grade schools. Similarly, there is little
dent-level covariates are reported in Appendix 2. research on whether school social context varies by grade
There was no direct association between school grade span configuration or relates to student perceptions and
span and any of the student perceptions of school climate experiences. In this study, we developed a measurement
variables, with social context in the model. However, the model for school social context that incorporated teacher
quality of the school social context varied by school grade and administrator perspectives in a large national sample.
configuration and negative school social context was itself We found that administrators and teachers in k8 schools
directly associated with lower levels of students school reported a more positive school social context than their
attachment, peer support, and peer academic values. Spe- colleagues in middle and junior high schools, controlling
cifically, k8 schools had a more positive school climate for school demographic and structural characteristics. This
compared to junior high schools; and as well as to middle positive school social context, in turn, explained the asso-
schools (k8 vs. middle school: Wald v2(1) = 5.91, ciation between grade span configuration and students
p \ .05). There was no significant difference between perceptions of their schools social and academic climates.
middle school and junior high school. These findings are consistent with research literature and
We further investigated the significance of the simple policy recommendations suggesting k8 schools best serve
indirect effects of grade span on students perceptions of the academic and socio-emotional needs of middle grade
school climate using a 2-2-1 multi-level mediation students (Seidman et al. 2004). Furthermore, these findings
approach (Bauer et al. 2006). Testing indirect effects with suggest school social context is likely of critical impor-
clustered data using the MSEM method provides more tance to students perceptions of school climate, which in
efficient and unbiased estimates compared to tests using turn play a central role in academic and social-emotional
multi-level modeling (Preacher et al. 2011). Simple indirect development following the middle grade school transition
effects by social context of the effect of k8 schools on (Brand et al. 2003; Jia et al. 2009; Way et al. 2007).
student school attachment (unstandardized estimate = 0.02; Our measurement model of school social context identi-
standardized = 0.05, p \ .05, 95 % CI [0.003, 0.100]) and fied five interrelated factors. Two of these factorsadmin-
peer academic values (unstandardized estimate = 0.01; istrator reports of school chaos and conduct problems
standardized = 0.03, p = .05, 95 % CI [-0.001, 0.068]) indicate the levels of disorder, such as racial tension, turn-
were marginally significant after controlling for school- and over, and student behavior problems, present in the school.
child-level covariates. The indirect effect on peer support The remaining three factorsstaff professional climate,
was not significant (unstandardized estimate = 0.01; stan- teacher agency, and teacher burdenreflect a range of
dardized = 0.04, p = .08, 95 % CI [-0.008, 0.084]). teachers perceptions, from teaching challenges and teaching
However, peer support was highly correlated with school efficacy to social and professional interactions among school
attachment and peer academic values, and was linked to staff. Collectively, these five factors form a single over-
more complex indirect paths (e.g., k8 schools ? school arching factor that encompasses school social context, to
social context ? school attachment ? peer academic which increased chaos, conduct problems, and teacher bur-
values). The significance of these complex indirect effects den negatively contribute, while feelings of a supportive
through two mediators cannot be tested. However, if all professional climate and agency positively contribute.
paths are significant for a given a level, such complex Incorporating teacher and administrator perspectives in
indirect effects can be accepted to be significant (Kline the measurement of school social context provides a more
2011). Total indirect effects by school social context of the nuanced, multidimensional view than in prior work, which
effect of k8 schools on student-report school climate has frequently relied on student perceptions. While student
variables are presented in Table 3. The school-level pre- perceptions are significant predictors of student achieve-
dictors included in the final model explained 30 % of the ment (Brand et al. 2003; Jia et al. 2009; Way et al. 2007),
variance in school attachment, 15 % in peer supports, 10 % they show greater differences between individuals than
in peer academic values at the school level. The student- they do differences between schools (Bryk et al. 2010;
level model explained 33 % of the variance in school Nathanson et al. 2013). Such individual-level variation
attachment; 30 % in peer support; and 15 % in peer aca- provides little information on overall school contexts and
demic values. makes it difficult to identify efficient targets of educational

123
Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845 37

Fig. 1 School social context factor structure, standardized factor Agency), teacher burden (T Burden), and teacher commitment (T
loadings, and correlations between factors. School chaos and student Commit) factors were identified from teacher reports of school
conduct (Conduct) factors were driven from school administrators context. Solid lines indicate significant correlations or factor loadings;
reports; and professional climate (Prof. Climate), teacher agency (T and dotted lines indicate non-significant correlations. ***p \ .001

Fig. 2 MIMIC model testing school social context varying by grade Solid lines indicate significant correlations or factor loadings; and
configurations with standardized coefficients and factor loadings. dotted lines indicate non-significant correlations. ***p \ .001;
**
School structural and demographic characteristics were controlled for. p \ .01

interventions and policies. For example, students percep- if it were found that decreasing chaos in the school as a
tions of low peer academic values are likely to be more whole has an average effect of raising everyones peer
greatly influenced by their direct interactions with their academic values, this would present a more efficient, set-
specific friend groups than their experience in the school as ting-level, primary prevention solution. In this way, iden-
a whole. Targeting individual-level social experience tifying more global measures of school context, may allow
requires identifying the primary contributor of individual us to identify better targets of policies and strategies to
variance for each child and tailoring interventions improve middle grade school climate.
accordingly, which may be labor- and time-intensive as Using this measurement model, our findings suggest k8
well as difficult to implement on a large scale. In contrast, schools have a more positive school social context than

123
38 Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

Fig. 3 Multi-level structural equation model (MSEM) testing the role full results, including unstandardized and understandized coefficients
of school social context with standardized coefficients of main paths of covariates and factor loadings for school social context factors, see
of interests. Solid lines indicate significant correlations or factor Appendix 2. ***p \ .001; **p \ .01; *p B .05
loadings; and dotted lines indicate non-significant correlations. For

Table 3 Unstandardized and standardized total indirect effects and total effects of k8 schools and school social context predicting student-
report school attachment, peer support, and peer academic values
k8 schools predicting School social context predicting
Total indirect Total Total indirect Total
Student-report school climate Estimate Std. estimate Estimate Std. estimate Estimate Std. estimate Estimate Std. estimate

School attachment 0.01 0.11 0.01 0.11 0.00 -0.62 -0.04 -1.15
Peer support 0.01 0.11 0.01 0.11 0.00 -0.74 -0.03 -0.42
Peer academic values 0.01 0.12 0.01 0.12 0.00 -0.83 -0.04 -1.17

middle and junior high schools, controlling for other school pedagogy, instead utilizing distinct subject organi-
demographic and structural characteristics. We found no zation as defined by junior high school pedagogy (Wil-
significant difference in social context between middle and liamson and Johnston 1999).
junior high schools. These results parallel prior research While most of the studies on the transition between
showing declines in academic achievement and engage- elementary and middle grade schools examine only the
ment among youths who transition to middle grade schools relations between the act of transitioning or not and student
as compared with those who do not (Byrnes and Ruby outcomes, the theoretical explanation given generally
2007; Cook et al. 2008; Rockoff and Lockwood 2010; focuses on the middle and junior high school setting.
Seidman et al. 2004). This literature does not generally Ecological systems theorists explain the declines in student
distinguish between the middle and junior high school outcomes during the transition to middle grade schools as a
transitions, an approach supported by our findings. The result of students experiencing less supportive social con-
lack of a significant difference between middle and junior texts in middle and junior high schools (Eccles et al. 1993;
high schools, which are intended to be rooted in very dif- Seidman et al. 2004; Trickett and Rowe 2012). This study
ferent pedagogical approaches, is an interesting finding on extends and supports this literature in two ways. First, our
its own. One possible explanation is that the majority of findings confirm prior research showing more negative
middle schools have failed to implement true middle student perceptions of school climate in middle and junior

123
Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845 39

high schools compared to elementary schools (Seidman perceptions. This adds to our understanding of the processes
et al. 2004) through teacher- and administrator-reports of by which student perceptions of school climate might be
school-level social context. Second, this study provides decreasing across the transition to middle or junior high
empirical evidence in support of the theoretical explanation school.
that school social context may be a central mechanism The study is also limited in a few ways. First, the use of
through which to explain differences in student experiences secondary data, albeit from a rich national dataset, restricts
by grade span configuration (Eccles et al. 1993; Seidman the variables at our disposal. We were unable to examine all
et al. 2004). Indeed, once school social context was taken aspects of school social context identified in the literature,
into account, we found no independent association between such as principal leadership and principalteacher relation-
school grade span configuration and school attachment, ships (Rhodes et al. 2009). In addition, we were unable to
peer support, or peer academic values. capture classroom (Cappella et al. 2013), afterschool pro-
Overall, the findings from this study suggest a central role grams (Smith et al. 2013), and neighborhood setting social
for school social context in explaining the relations between contexts (Delany-Brumsey et al. 2014; Duke et al. 2011;
the middle grade schools grade span configuration and McCoy et al. 2013) that may be critical to youths school
student level experience and suggest support for existing experiences and adjustment. Additional work with different
theories surrounding the mechanisms at work across this dimensions of school social context, and examination of
transition (Eccles and Midgley 1989; Seidman et al. 2004). associations with other developmental settings, should be
Specifically, they indicate that it may not be the timing of the undertaken to affirm and extend the findings. Second, due to
middle grade transition or even the act of the transitioning the unique data structure of the ECLS-K, we were unable to
that leads to declines in performance and well-being, but consider teachers nested within schools. Replicating our
rather the environment into which youth transition. Struc- measurement model with multiple teachers and administra-
tural changes may influence the resulting social contextfor tors per school in a two-level model would help isolate
example, through the presence/absence of younger students, school social context from individual teachers or principals
many versus one or two teachers, or the same/different experiences. Third, due to the limited data points in the
constellations of classmates. However, structural change ECLS-K during the middle grade years, this study examined
alone may not be sufficient in reversing the declining pat- cross-sectional relations between school social contexts and
terns of student academic and social-emotional develop- students perceptions of school climate for the middle grade
ment. Recent findings that older k8 schools perform better schools (6th, 7th, and 8th grade). While our findings suggest
than newly-converted k8 schools (Byrnes and Ruby 2007) a model in which school social context mediates the relation
support this in suggesting schools may need additional between grade span configuration and student outcomes, the
resources and strategies in order to best support adolescents directionality of such a model cannot be evaluated using this
needs and that the restructuring of grade configuration alone cross-sectional and correlational study. Intervention
may be insufficient. While further research is warranted as to research that targets school social context and assesses the
the factors underlying the relations between school grade impact on student perceptions would lend support and clarity
span configuration and school social context, our findings to the directionality of this association. Lastly, consistent
suggest that targeting social context directly, with or without with past research indicating students vary more within
shifts in grade span, may be an efficient way to support school than between schools (Bryk et al. 2010; Nathanson
positive student development during these years. et al. 2013), and given our low average number of students
per school, we found low intraclass correlations (ICCs) for
student responses. This may relate to the wording of items,
Study Strengths, Limitations, and Future Directions which asked about individual experiences rather than school
perceptions. Further research should draw from data with
This study has methodological and conceptual strengths. more students per school in order to increase the between-
First, it used a large national dataset with a range of public level association of student responses with questionnaires
schools represented, enabling between-school analyses on designed to measure school-level processes.
social context and climate in the middle years. Second, it
included child, teacher, and administrator reports for a Conclusion and Implications
nuanced conceptualization of school social context and an
examination of the association between school-level social This study identified school social context as a potentially
context and individual student perceptions. Third, it includes critical avenue of intervention toward supporting students
an array of covariates at school and child levels to increase social and academic development in the middle grades.
our ability to draw valid conclusions from these findings. Our findings suggest that researchers and practitioners
Lastly, this study separated school context from students should target school social context during these years and

123
40 Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

point to avenues through which this might be accom- Acknowledgments This research was conducted with support
plished. One avenue is structural change. Consistent with from Spencer Foundation (#201300077, PI: Elise Cappella, Co-PI:
Edward Seidman), the NYU Institute of Human Development and
past work, this study identified k8 schools as more sup- Social Change, and the NYU Predoctoral Interdisciplinary Research
portive contexts for youth. A second avenue, not incom- Training fellowship. All procedures and restricted data use were
patible with the first, is implementation of policies directly approved by the IES Data Security Office (#12040005) and the
targeting key aspects of social context. This could take the New York University Committee on Activities Involving Human
Subjects.
form of increasing relational support among teachers,
administrators, and parents (Jia et al. 2009); implementing
interventions to reduce conduct problems (Bradshaw et al.
2009); reducing teacher burden through streamlined Appendix 1: Student Perceptions of Social
administrative demands; or otherwise improving profes- and Academic Climate Exploratory and Confirmatory
sional climate. Further research as to the characteristics of Factor Model Testing
k8 schools most critical to their positive social context
would greatly aid this intervention work through informing Procedures
which areas of social context to target and how to target
these areas. Whether targeting social context along with, or Student perceptions of school climate were examined using
in the absence of, structural change, our findings indicate 13 items from the Wave 7 ECLS-K student questionnaires.
that in order to improve student perceptions of social and First, we used student reports from 40 % of the randomly
academic climate and, through those, student outcomes, selected schools to conduct an exploratory factor analysis
educators may need to focus on improving teacher and using a geomin extraction approach with varimax rotation.
administrative social processes as well as supporting indi- We found a three-factor model to be the most reasonable
vidual students. solution considering the eigen value ([1), scree plot, and
School social context is critical to student learning and model fit (v2(42) = 0.422, p \ .001, RMSEA = 0.06:
well-being. It may also help to explain why many youth further details available from primary author). Second, we
experience declines in achievement and adjustment across confirmed the factor structure using the rest of the student
the transition to middle or junior high school. Targeting sample from the remaining 60 % of the schools. In these
school social context may be a particularly effective way of analyses, we fixed the factor loadings to 1 and allowed the
addressing these declines while, preventatively, benefiting factor loadings to be freely estimated. Using a combination
a large number of youth. of theory, face validity, and modification indices, we then

Fig. 4 Student perceptions of school social and academic climate. v2(59) = 293.87, p \ .001 RMSEA = 0.03; CFI = 0.97; TLI = 0.97;
SRMR = 0.03

123
Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845 41

modified the measurement model until the best fitting sample. Factor loadings and fit statistics for the final
measurement model was obtained. In this process, we measurement model are presented in Fig. 4, below.
allowed the residual variances of some of the items within
factors to correlate. These measures are completed by the
same source (student), and allowing the errors to correlate Appendix 2
significantly improved the model fit (Bentler 2000). Lastly,
we applied the same factor model to the whole student See Table 4.

Table 4 Multi-level structural equation model (MSEM) testing the role of school social context with unstandardized and standardized coef-
ficients, and factor loadings
Unstandardized estimates Standardized estimates
Coefficient 95 % CI Coefficient 95 % CI

Within level
School attachment on
Gender 0.28*** [0.23, 0.33] 0.16 [0.13,0.18]
White -0.08* [-0.16, 0.00] -0.04 [-0.09, 0.00]
Black -0.19*** [-0.29, -0.09] -0.06 [-0.09, -0.03]
Hispanic -0.02 [-0.13, 0.09] -0.01 [-0.04, 0.03]
Asian -0.03 [-0.15, 0.09] -0.01 [-0.03, 0.02]
Other -0.07 [-0.18, 0.04] -0.02 [-0.04, 0.01]
Age 0.01* [0.01, 0.01] 0.03 [0.00, 0.05]
SES 0.00 [-0.02, 0.02] 0.00 [-0.03, 0.03]
Reading interest/competence 0.02 [-0.01, 0.05] 0.01 [-0.02, 0.04]
Math interest/competence 0.09*** [0.06, 0.12] 0.09 [0.06, 0.12]
Internalizing problems -0.12*** [-0.17, -0.07] -0.07 [-0.10, -0.04]
Locus of control 0.04 [-0.01, 0.09] 0.03 [-0.01, 0.06]
Self-concept 0.63*** [0.58, 0.68] 0.49 [0.45, 0.52]
Peer academic values on
Gender 0.20*** [0.15, 0.24] 0.12 [0.09, 0.15]
White 0.01 [-0.08, 0.09] 0.00 [-0.05, 0.06]
Black 0.13* [0.02, 0.24] 0.04 [0.01, 0.08]
Hispanic -0.02 [-0.15, 0.11] -0.01 [-0.05, 0.04]
Asian 0.19** [0.07, 0.30] 0.05 [0.02, 0.08]
Other 0.01 [-0.12, 0.14] 0.00 [-0.03, 0.04]
Age 0.00 [0.00, 0.01] 0.02 [-0.01, 0.05]
SES 0.03*** [0.01, 0.05] 0.05 [0.02, 0.08]
Reading interest/competence 0.10*** [0.07, 0.14] 0.09 [0.06, 0.12]
Math interest/competence 0.08*** [0.06, 0.11] 0.09 [0.06, 0.12]
Internalizing problems 0.12*** [0.07, 0.17] 0.08 [0.05, 0.11]
Locus of control 0.07** [0.02, 0.12] 0.05 [0.02, 0.09]
Self-concept 0.30*** [0.25, 0.34] 0.25 [0.21, 0.29]
Peer support on
Gender 0.33*** [0.28, 0.38] 0.18 [0.15, 0.20]
White -0.10* [-0.19, -0.02] -0.05 [-0.10, -0.01]
Black -0.19*** [-0.31, -0.07] -0.06 [-0.09, -0.02]
Hispanic -0.03 [-0.15, 0.09] -0.01 [-0.04, 0.03]
Asian 0.00 [-0.13, 0.13] 0.00 [-0.03, 0.03]
Other -0.04 [-0.16, 0.08] -0.01 [-0.04, 0.02]
Age 0.01*** [0.00, 0.01] 0.04 [0.02, 0.06]
SES 0.01 [-0.01, 0.03] 0.01 [-0.02, 0.04]
Reading interest/competence 0.00 [-0.04, 0.03] 0.00 [-0.03, 0.03]

123
42 Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

Table 4 continued
Unstandardized estimates Standardized estimates
Coefficient 95 % CI Coefficient 95 % CI

Math interest/competence 0.07*** [0.04, 0.10] 0.07 [0.04, 0.09]


Internalizing problems -0.08** [-0.13, -0.02] -0.04 [-0.08, -0.01]
Locus of control 0.04 [-0.01, 0.09] 0.03 [-0.01, 0.06]
Self-concept 0.64*** [0.59, 0.69] 0.47 [0.44, 0.51]
School attachment with
Peer academic values 0.19*** [0.17, 0.21] 0.35 [0.32, 0.38]
Peer support 0.51*** [0.48, 0.53] 0.89 [0.88, 0.90]
Peer academic values with
Peer support 0.19*** [0.17, 0.21] 0.33 [0.30, 0.36]
Between level
School chaos by
Racial tension 0.49*** [0.43, 0.55] 0.66 [0.61, 0.71]
Gang activity 0.67*** [0.60, 0.73] 0.80 [0.76, 0.85]
Teacher turnover 0.40*** [0.34, 0.45] 0.48 [0.42, 0.54]
Student conduct by
Theft 0.41*** [0.37, 0.45] 0.67 [0.62, 0.71]
Class cutting 0.68*** [0.63, 0.73] 0.70 [0.66, 0.74]
Physical conflict 0.59*** [0.54, 0.63] 0.75 [0.71, 0.78]
Vandalism 0.36*** [0.31, 0.40] 0.63 [0.58, 0.68]
Student bullying 0.47*** [0.42, 0.52] 0.53 [0.49, 0.58]
Teaching burden by
Misbehavior interferes -0.20*** [-0.30, -0.11] -0.72 [-0.75, -0.68]
Students not capable -0.12*** [-0.18, -0.06] -0.54 [-0.59, -0.49]
Parents support 0.13*** [0.07, 0.19] 0.64 [0.59, 0.68]
Student attitudes -0.14*** [-0.21, -0.07] -0.62 [-0.67, -0.58]
Factors beyond control -0.11*** [-0.17, -0.06] -0.50 [-0.55, -0.44]
Waste of time -0.09*** [-0.14, -0.05] -0.48 [-0.54, -0.41]
Teacher agency by
I am making difference -0.20*** [-0.23, -0.16] -0.61 [-0.67, -0.55]
Choose teaching again -0.30*** [-0.35, -0.26] -0.62 [-0.67, -0.56]
Enjoy teaching -0.35*** [-0.40, -0.31] -0.87 [-0.92, -0.82]
Professional climate by
Teachers continue to learn -0.27*** [-0.31, -0.22] -0.67 [-0.73, -0.60]
School spirit -0.34*** [-0.37, -0.30] -0.72 [-0.79, -0.65]
Accept me -0.20*** [-0.24, -0.17] -0.59 [-0.66, -0.52]
School social context by
Teaching burden -2.56*** [-3.94, -1.17] -0.95 [-1.00, -0.90]
School chaos 0.55*** [0.45, 0.66] 0.54 [0.47, 0.62]
Student conduct 0.44*** [0.36, 0.51] 0.45 [0.38, 0.52]
Teacher agency 0.73*** [0.59, 0.87] 0.65 [0.58, 0.72]
Professional climate 0.72*** [0.58, 0.85] 0.64 [0.58, 0.71]
School social context on
k8 -0.36** [-0.62, -0.10] -0.10 [-0.17, -0.03]
Middle -0.07 [-0.22, 0.08] -0.03 [-0.09, 0.03]
Urban 0.13 [-0.04, 0.29] 0.05 [-0.01, 0.11]
Rural 0.37*** [0.17, 0.56] 0.11 [0.05, 0.17]
Enrollment 0.10** [0.02, 0.18] 0.09 [0.02, 0.15]

123
Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845 43

Table 4 continued
Unstandardized estimates Standardized estimates
Coefficient 95 % CI Coefficient 95 % CI

Free lunch % 0.02*** [0.01, 0.02] 0.40 [0.33, 0.47]


Hispanic % 0.05 [-0.02, 0.11] 0.05 [-0.02, 0.12]
Black % 0.16*** [0.10, 0.22] 0.18 [0.11, 0.24]
Facility quality 0.00 [0.00, 0.00] -0.05 [-0.10, 0.01]
School attachment on
School social context -0.04*** [-0.07, -0.02] -0.53 [-0.88, -0.17]
Peer academic values on
School social context -0.04** [-0.07, -0.01] -0.34 [-0.59, -0.08]
Peer support on
School social context -0.03* [-0.06, 0.00] -0.39 [-0.79, 0.01]
School attachment on
k8 0.01 [-0.07, 0.09] 0.04 [-0.23, 0.31]
Middle 0.00 [-0.05, 0.06] 0.02 [-0.24, 0.28]
Peer academic values on
k8 -0.05 [-0.14, 0.04] -0.12 [-0.34, 0.10]
Middle 0.02 [-0.03, 0.08] 0.09 [-0.12, 0.29]
Peer support on
k8 -0.05 [-0.14, 0.04] -0.18 [-0.52, 0.16]
Middle -0.01 [-0.07, 0.04] -0.07 [-0.37, 0.22]
Student conduct with
School chaos 0.53*** [0.44, 0.61] 0.53 [0.44, 0.61]
Peer academic values with
School attachment 0.01* [0.00, 0.02] 0.88 [0.19, 1.57]
Peer support with
School attachment 0.01 [0.00, 0.01] 0.81 [0.53, 1.09]
Peer academic values 0.01* [0.00, 0.02] 0.92 [0.08, 1.76]
* ** ***
p \ .05; p \ .01; p \ .001

References family interaction (pp. 247279). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence


Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Anderson, C. S. (1982). The search for school climate: A review of Barber, B. K., & Olsen, J. A. (2004). Assessing the transitions to
the research. Review of Educational Research, 52, 368420. middle and high school. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19,
doi:10.3102/00346543052003368. 330. doi:10.1177/0743558403258113.
Archambault, I., Eccles, J. S., & Vida, M. N. (2010). Ability self- Bauer, D. J., Preacher, K. J., & Gil, K. M. (2006). Conceptualizing and
concepts and subjective value in literacy: Joint trajectories from testing random indirect effects and moderated mediation in multi-
grades 1 through 12. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, level models: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological
804816. doi:10.1037/a0021075. Methods, 11, 142163. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.142.
Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. (2009). Bellmore, A., Nishina, A., You, J., & Ma, T.-L. (2012). School
Adolescent behavioral, affective, and cognitive engagement in context protective factors against peer ethnic discrimination
school: Relationship to dropout. Journal of School Health, 79, across the high school years. American Journal of Community
408415. doi:10.1111/j.1746-1561.2009.00428.x. Psychology, 49, 98111. doi:10.1007/s10464-011-9443-0.
Asparouhov, T., & Muthen, B. (2006). Robust Chi square difference Benner, A. D., & Graham, S. (2009). The transition to high school as a
testing with mean and variance adjusted test statistics. Retrieved developmental process among multiethnic urban youth. Child
October 23, 2013, from http://statmodel2.com/download/web Development, 80, 356376. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01265.x.
notes/webnote10.pdf. Bentler, P. M. (2000). Rites, wrongs, and gold in model testing.
Baker, J. A. (1999). Teacherstudent interaction in urban at-risk Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 7,
classrooms: Differential behavior, relationship quality, and 8291. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM0701_04.
student satisfaction with school. The Elementary School Journal, Birnbaum, A. S., Lytle, L. A., Hannan, P. J., Murray, D. M., Perry, C.
100, 5770. doi:10.1086/461943. L., & Forster, J. L. (2003). School functioning and violent
Bank, L., Dishion, T. J., Skinner, M., & Patterson, G. R. (1990). behavior among young adolescents: A contextual analysis.
Method variance in structural equation modeling: Living with Health Education Research, 18, 389403. doi:10.1093/her/
glop. In G. R. Patterson (Ed.), Depression and aggression in cyf036.

123
44 Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845

Bradshaw, C., Koth, C., Thornton, L., & Leaf, P. (2009). Altering Grills-Taquechel, A. E., Norton, P., & Ollendick, T. H. (2010). A
school climate through school-wide positive behavioral inter- longitudinal examination of factors predicting anxiety during the
ventions and supports: Findings from a group-randomized transition to middle school. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 23,
effectiveness trial. Prevention Science, 10, 100115. doi:10. 493513. doi:10.1080/10615800903494127.
1007/s11121-008-0114-9. Hannaway, J., & Talbert, J. E. (1993). Bringing context into effective
Brand, S., Felner, R., Shim, M., Seitsinger, A., & Dumas, T. (2003). schools research: Urban-suburban differences. Educational
Middle school improvement and reform: Development and Administration Quarterly, 29, 164186. doi:10.1177/
validation of a school-level assessment of climate, cultural 0013161X93029002004.
pluralism, and school safety. Journal of Educational Psychology, Hoy, W. K., & Hannum, J. W. (1997). Middle school climate: An
95, 570588. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.570. empirical assessment of organizational health and student
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of achievement. Educational Administration Quarterly, 33,
human development. In R. M. Lerner & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook 290311. doi:10.1177/0013161X97033003003.
of child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 793828). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Hsu, H.-Y. (2009). Testing the effectiveness of various commonly
Bryk, A., Sebring, P., Allensworth, E., Luppescu, S., & Easton, J. used fit indices for detecting misspecifications in multilevel
(2010). Organizing schools for improvement: Lessons from structure equation models (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved
Chicago. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. from http://hdl.handle.net/1969.1/ETD-TAMU-2009-12-7411.
Buckley, J., Schneider, M., & Shang, Y. (2005). Fix it and they might Jia, Y., Way, N., Ling, G., Yoshikawa, H., Chen, X., Hughes, D.,
stay: School facility quality and teacher retention in Washington, et al. (2009). The influence of student perceptions of school
DC. Teachers College Record, 107, 11071123. climate on socioemotional and academic adjustment: A com-
Byrnes, V., & Ruby, A. (2007). Comparing achievement between K-8 parison of Chinese and American adolescents. Child Develop-
and middle Schools: A large-scale empirical study. American ment, 80, 15141530. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01348.x.
Journal of Education, 114, 101135. Jones, S. M., Brown, J. L., & Aber, J. L. (2011). Two-year impacts of a
Cappella, E., Kim, H. Y., Neal, J. W., & Jackson, D. R. (2013). universal school-based social-emotional and literacy intervention:
Classroom peer relationships and behavioral engagement in An experiment in translational developmental research. Child
elementary school: The role of social network equity. American Development, 82, 533554. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01560.x.
Journal of Community Psychology, 52, 367379. doi:10.1007/ Joreskog, K. G., & Goldberger, A. S. (1975). Estimation of a model
s10464-013-9603-5. with multiple indicators and multiple causes of a single latent
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2009). School connect- variable. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 70,
edness: Strategies for increasing protective factors among youth. 631639. doi:10.1080/01621459.1975.10482485.
Atlanta, GA: US Department of Health and Human Services. Kieffer, M. J. (2013). Development of reading and mathematics skills
Cook, P. J., MacCoun, R., Muschkin, C., & Vigdor, J. (2008). The in early adolescence: Do K-8 public schools make a difference?
negative impacts of starting middle school in sixth grade. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 6, 361379.
Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 27, 104121. doi:10.1080/19345747.2013.822954.
doi:10.1002/pam.20309. Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation
Delany-Brumsey, A., Mays, V., & Cochran, S. (2014). Does modeling. New York: Guilford press.
neighborhood social capital buffer the effects of maternal Kuperminc, G., Leadbeater, B., & Blatt, S. (2001). School social
depression on adolescent behavior problems? Journal of Com- climate and individual differences in vulnerability to psychopa-
munity Psychology,. doi:10.1007/s10464-014-9640-8. thology among middle school students. Journal of School
Duke, N. N., Borowsky, I. W., & Pettingell, S. L. (2011). Adult Psychology, 39, 141159. doi:10.1016/S0022-4405(01)00059-0.
perceptions of neighborhood: Links to youth engagement. Youth Kuperminc, G. P., Leadbeater, B. J., Emmons, C., & Blatt, S. J.
& Society, 44, 408430. doi:10.1177/0044118X11402852. (1997). Perceived school climate and difficulties in the social
Eccles, J. S. (2004). Schools, academic motivation, and stage- adjustment of middle school students. Applied Developmental
environment fit. In R. M. Lerner & L. D. Steinberg (Eds.), Science, 1, 7688. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads0102_2.
Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., pp. 125153). Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming school cultures.
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage-environment fit: Devel- Marks, H. M., & Printy, S. M. (2003). Principal leadership and school
opmentally appropriate classrooms for young adolescents. performance: An integration of transformational and instruc-
Research on Motivation in Education, 3, 139186. tional leadership. Educational Administration Quarterly, 39,
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Midgley, C., Reuman, D., Iver, D. M., & 370397. doi:10.1177/0013161X03253412.
Feldlaufer, H. (1993). Negative effects of traditional middle Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. (2004a). In search of golden
schools on students motivation. The Elementary School Journal, rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting
93, 553574. doi:10.1086/461740. cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu
Edgerson, D. E., Kritsonis, W. A., & Herrington, D. (2006). Analysis and Bentlers (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling: A
of the influence of principalteacher relationships on student Multidisciplinary Journal, 11, 320341. doi:10.1207/
academic achievement: A national focus. National Journal for s15328007sem1103_2.
Publishing and Mentoring Doctoral Student Research, 1, 15. Marsh, H. W., Ludtke, O., Nagengast, B., Trautwein, U., Morin, A.
Flannery, K., Sugai, G., & Anderson, C. (2009). School-wide positive J. S., Abduljabbar, A. S., et al. (2012). Classroom climate and
behavior support in high school. Journal of Positive Behavior contextual effects: Conceptual and methodological issues in the
Interventions, 11, 177. evaluation of group-level effects. Educational Psychologist, 47,
Goldstein, S. E., Young, A., & Boyd, C. (2008). Relational aggression 106124. doi:10.1080/00461520.2012.670488.
at school: Associations with school safety and social climate. Marsh, H. W., Wen, Z., & Hau, K.-T. (2004b). Structural equation
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37(6), 641654. doi:10.1007/ models of latent interactions: Evaluation of alternative estima-
s10964-007-9192-4. tion strategies and indicator construction. Psychological Meth-
Grannis, J. C. (1992). Students stress, distress, and achievement in an ods, 9, 275300. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.9.3.275.
urban intermediate school. The Journal of Early Adolescence, McCoy, D. C., Roy, A. L., & Sirkman, G. M. (2013). Neighborhood
12, 427. doi:10.1177/0272431692012001001. crime and school climate as predictors of elementary school

123
Am J Community Psychol (2014) 54:2845 45

academic quality: A cross-lagged panel analysis. American Strengths-based research and policy (pp. 233250). Washington,
Journal of Community Psychology, 52, 128140. doi:10.1007/ DC: American Psychological Association.
s10464-013-9583-5. Seidman, E., Allen, L., Aber, J. L., Mitchell, C., & Feinman, J.
Midgley, C., Anderman, E., & Hicks, L. (1995). Differences between (1994). The impact of school transitions in early adolescence on
elementary and middle school teachers and students: A goal the self-system and perceived social context of poor urban youth.
theory approach. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 15, 90113. Child Development, 65, 507522. doi:10.2307/1131399.
doi:10.1177/0272431695015001006. Simmons, R. G., & Blyth, D. A. (1987). Moving into adolescence:
Mitchell, M. M., Bradshaw, C. P., & Leaf, P. J. (2010). Student and The impact of pubertal chance and school context. New York:
teacher perceptions of school climate: A multilevel exploration Aldine Transaction.
of patterns of discrepancy. Journal of School Health, 80, Skaalvik, E. M., & Skaalvik, S. (2007). Dimensions of teacher self-
271279. doi:10.1111/j.1746-1561.2010.00501.x. efficacy and relations with strain factors, perceived collective
Muthen, B. O. (1989). Latent variable modeling in heterogeneous teacher efficacy, and teacher burnout. Journal of Educational
populations. Psychometrika, 54, 557585. doi:10.1007/ Psychology, 99, 611625. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.611.
BF02296397. Skinner, E. A., & Wellborn, J. G. (1994). Coping during childhood
Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (19982012). Mplus users guide and adolescence: A motivational perspective. In D. L. Feather-
(7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen. man, R. M. Lerner, & M. Perlmutter (Eds.), Life-span develop-
Nathanson, L., McCormick, M., & Kemple, J. (2013). Strengthening ment and behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 91133). Hillsdale, NJ:
assessments of school climate: Lessons from the NYC school survey. Lawrence Erlbaum.
New York: The Research Alliance for New York City Schools. Smith, E., Osgood, D., Caldwell, L., Hynes, K., & Perkins, D. (2013).
Retrieved from http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED543180.pdf. Measuring collective efficacy among children in community-based
National Research Council. (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering high afterschool programs: Exploring pathways toward prevention and
school students motivation to learn. Washington, DC: National positive youth development. American Journal of Community
Academies Press. Retrieved from http://www.loc.gov/catdir/toc/ Psychology, 52, 2740. doi:10.1007/s10464-013-9574-6.
ecip047/2003017626.html. Sweetland, S. R., & Hoy, W. K. (2000). School characteristics and
National School Climate Council. (2007). The school climate educational outcomes: Toward an organizational model of student
challenge: Narrowing the gap between school climate research achievement in middle schools. Educational Administration
and school climate policy, practice guidelines and teacher Quarterly, 36, 703729. doi:10.1177/00131610021969173.
education policy. Retrieved from http://nscc.csee.net/. Thijs, J. T., Koomen, H. M. Y., & van der Leij, A. (2008). Teacher
Opdenakker, M., & Damme, J. V. (2007). Do school context, student child relationships and pedagogical practices: Considering the
composition and school leadership affect school practice and teachers perspective. School Psychology Review, 37, 244260.
outcomes in secondary education? British Educational Research Trickett, E. J., & Rowe, H. L. (2012). Emerging ecological
Journal, 33, 179206. doi:10.1080/01411920701208233. approaches to prevention, health promotion, and public health
Preacher, K. J., Zhang, Z., & Zyphur, M. J. (2011). Alternative in the school context: Next steps from a community psychology
methods for assessing mediation in multilevel data: The perspective. Journal of Educational and Psychological Consul-
advantages of multilevel SEM. Structural Equation Modeling: tation, 22, 125140. doi:10.1080/10474412.2011.649651.
A Multidisciplinary Journal, 18, 161182. doi:10.1080/ Tschannen-Moran, M., & Hoy, A. W. (2001). Teacher efficacy:
10705511.2011.557329. Capturing an elusive construct. Teaching and Teacher Educa-
Raver, C. C., Jones, S. M., Li-Grining, C., Zhai, F., Metzger, M. W., tion, 17, 783805. doi:10.1016/S0742-051X(01)00036-1.
& Solomon, B. (2009). Targeting childrens behavior problems Wang, M.-T., & Eccles, J. S. (2012). Adolescent behavioral, emotional,
in preschool classrooms: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. and cognitive engagement trajectories in school and their differ-
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 302316. ential relations to educational success. Journal of Research on
doi:10.1037/a0015302. Adolescence, 22, 3139. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2011.00753.x.
Rhodes, J. E., Camic, P. M., Milburn, M., & Lowe, S. R. (2009). Wang, M. C., & Gordon, E. W. (2012). Educational resilience in
Improving middle school climate through teacher-centered inner-city America: Challenges and prospects. Mahwah, NJ:
change. Journal of Community Psychology, 37, 711724. Routledge.
doi:10.1002/jcop.20326. Warren, J. S., Edmonson, H. M., Griggs, P., Lassen, S. R., McCart,
Rockoff, J. E., & Lockwood, B. B. (2010). Stuck in the middle: A., Turnbull, A., et al. (2003). Urban applications of school-wide
Impacts of grade configuration in public schools. Journal of positive behavior support critical issues and lessons learned.
Public Economics, 94, 10511061. doi:10.1016/j.jpubeco.2010. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 5, 8091. doi:10.
06.017. 1177/10983007030050020301.
Rutter, M., & Maughan, B. (2002). School effectiveness findings Wasik, B. A., Bond, M. A., & Hindman, A. (2006). The effects of a
19792002. Journal of School Psychology, 40, 451475. doi:10. language and literacy intervention on Head Start children and
1016/S0022-4405(02)00124-3. teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 6374. doi:10.
Schwartz, A. E., Stiefel, L., Rubenstein, R., & Zabel, J. (2011). The 1037/0022-0663.98.1.63.
path not taken: How does school organization affect eighth- Way, N., Reddy, R., & Rhodes, J. (2007). Students perceptions of
grade achievement? Educational Evaluation and Policy Ana- school climate during the middle school years: Associations with
lysis, 33, 293317. doi:10.3102/0162373711407062. trajectories of psychological and behavioral adjustment. Amer-
Schwerdt, G., & West, M. R. (2013). The impact of alternative grade ican Journal of Community Psychology, 40, 194213. doi:10.
configurations on student outcomes through middle and high 1007/s10464-007-9143-y.
school. Journal of Public Economics, 97, 308326. doi:10.1016/ Weiss, C. C., & Kipnes, L. (2006). Reexamining middle school effects:
j.jpubeco.2012.10.002. A comparison of middle grades students in middle schools and k-8
Seidman, E., Aber, J. L., & French, S. E. (2004). The organization of schools. American Journal of Education, 112, 239272.
schooling and adolescent development. In K. I. Maton, C. Williamson, R., & Johnston, J. H. (1999). Challenging orthodoxy: An
J. Schellenbach, B. J. Leadbeater, & A. L. Solarz (Eds.), emerging agenda for middle level reform. Middle School
Investing in children, youth, families, and communities: Journal, 30, 1017.

123

Anda mungkin juga menyukai