Anda di halaman 1dari 14

Article

A Central Amygdala-Substantia Innominata Neural


Circuitry Encodes Aversive Reinforcement Signals
Graphical Abstract Authors
Yuting Cui, Guanghui Lv, Sen Jin, ...,
Fuqiang Xu, Tonghui Xu, Haohong Li

Correspondence
xutonghui@hust.edu.cn (T.X.),
hxli@hust.edu.cn (H.L.)

In Brief
Cui et al. show that central amygdala
PKC-d+ neurons can modulate negative
reinforcement learning by transmitting
aversive signals to the substantia
innominata.

Highlights
d CeL PKC-d+ neurons encode aversive signals in negative
reinforcement learning

d The CeLPKC-d+-SI neural circuit modulates negative


reinforcement learning

d SI glutamatergic neurons are responsible for receiving


aversive signals from the CeL

Cui et al., 2017, Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782


November 14, 2017 ª 2017 The Author(s).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.062
Cell Reports

Article

A Central Amygdala-Substantia Innominata Neural


Circuitry Encodes Aversive Reinforcement Signals
Yuting Cui,1,2 Guanghui Lv,1,2 Sen Jin,3 Jie Peng,1,2 Jing Yuan,1,2 Xiaobin He,3 Hui Gong,1,2 Fuqiang Xu,1,2,3
Tonghui Xu,1,2,* and Haohong Li1,2,4,*
1Britton Chance Center for Biomedical Photonics, Wuhan National Laboratory for Optoelectronics, Huazhong University of Science and
Technology, Wuhan, Hubei 430074, China
2MoE Key Laboratory for Biomedical Photonics, Collaborative Innovation Center for Biomedical Engineering, School of Engineering Sciences,

Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, Hubei 430074, China


3Center for Brain Science, Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance in Biological Systems and State Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance

and Atomic and Molecular Physics, Wuhan Institute of Physics and Mathematics, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence
Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences Wuhan, China
4Lead Contact

*Correspondence: xutonghui@hust.edu.cn (T.X.), hxli@hust.edu.cn (H.L.)


https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.062

SUMMARY aversive reinforcers remains a major endeavor of behavioral


neuroscience. An impressive body of literature suggests that
Aversive stimuli can impact motivation and support the amygdala complex is critical for the integration and control
associative learning as reinforcers. However, the of classical fear conditioning in which animals learn to associate
neural circuitry underlying the processing of aversive aversive events with stimulus cues (Ehrlich et al., 2009; Hauben-
reinforcers has not been elucidated. Here, we report sak et al., 2010; Herry et al., 2008). Thus, one candidate structure
that a subpopulation of central amygdala (CeA) for negatively reinforced associative learning is the central amyg-
dala (CeA), which contains both medial (CeM) and lateral (CeL)
GABAergic neurons expressing protein kinase
divisions and serves as a major output of the amygdala complex
C-delta (PKC-d+) displays robust responses to aver-
(Davis et al., 2003; Ehrlich et al., 2009; Paré et al., 2004). There
sive stimuli during negative reinforcement learning. are two major subpopulations of GABAergic neurons found in
Importantly, projections from PKC-d+ neurons of the CeL, one expressing the neuropeptide somatostatin (SOM)
the CeA to the substantia innominata (SI) could and the other expressing protein kinase C-delta (PKC-d) (Hau-
bi-directionally modulate negative reinforcement bensak et al., 2010; Li et al., 2013). These two subpopulations
learning. Moreover, consistent with the idea that have distinct functions in associative fear learning (Ciocchi
SI-projecting PKC-d+ neurons of the CeA encode et al., 2010; Haubensak et al., 2010; Li et al., 2013). SOM neurons
aversive information, optogenetic activation of this represent CeLon neurons (excited in response to CS) and PKC-d
pathway produces conditioned place aversion, a neurons represent CeLoff neurons (inhibited in response to CS)
behavior prevented by simultaneous ablating of SI during fear expression (Ciocchi et al., 2010; Fadok et al., 2017;
Haubensak et al., 2010; Li et al., 2013; Yu et al., 2016). Previous
glutamatergic neurons. Taken together, our data
research has revealed that the firing rates of CeA neurons
define a cell-type-specific neural circuitry modu-
increase selectively at the time of reward omission in well-trained
lating associative learning by encoding aversive rats during a forced-choice task (Calu et al., 2010). Amygdala
reinforcement signals. responses could be evoked by aversive unconditioned foot-
shock stimuli (McHugh et al., 2014). In addition, the parabrachial
INTRODUCTION nucleus, as an upstream brain region, selectively transmits the
US aversive signal to the CeL during aversive learning (Han
Associative learning is typified by associating a conditioned et al., 2015). However, it remains to be determined which cell
stimulus (CS) with a reinforcer (unconditioned stimulus [US]), types in the CeL encode aversive signals.
such as appetitive or aversive reinforced stimuli, to guide Previous research has shown that CeA neurons project prom-
learning (Fanselow and Poulos, 2005; LeDoux, 2000). In complex inently to and form symmetric synapses mainly in the subnuclei
environments, associating danger with conditioned or predictive ventrolateral substantia innominata (SI) of the basal forebrain
danger cues is important in animals for the rapid avoidance of (BF) (Jolkkonen et al., 2002). BF cholinergic neurons show short
threats. Negatively reinforced associative learning, which is a latency activation after the delivery of punishment in an auditory
form of operant conditioning that allows organisms to orient detection task and convey precisely time-locked signals to large
toward specific goals in the environment and flexibly control areas of the brain (Hangya et al., 2015). Two other populations in
actions, has been documented behaviorally (Avarguès-Weber the BF, GABAergic and glutamatergic neurons, have been impli-
et al., 2010; Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2009; O’Doherty et al., cated in encoding motivational salience in go/no-go associative
2004; Resnik et al., 2012). However, determining the precise learning (Avila and Lin, 2014; Hangya et al., 2015; Lin and Nico-
neuronal circuits and mechanisms underlying the processing of lelis, 2008). Interestingly, in a similar behavioral task, in vivo

1770 Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017 ª 2017 The Author(s).
This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
(legend on next page)

Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017 1771


microendoscopic calcium imaging showed that these three cell During training, the performance (Figure 1C) and correct rejec-
types could be activated by overt punishment stimuli (Harrison tion rate (Figures S1A and S1B) gradually increased within the
et al., 2016). These results suggest that neuronal activity in the initial 5–6 sessions, after which performance reached a stable
BF is highly heterogeneous and that the definition of cell types level, but there was a ceiling effect for the hit rate (Figures S1A
in the BF are relatively imprecise. A way to solve this imprecision and S1B) across learning phases. The well-trained mice could
is to account for the input and output of BF neurons with specific discriminate between go and no-go cues (Figure S1C).
function. It has been proposed that the CeA, via its influence in We used fiber photometry, which is a method for collecting
the BF, might function to enhance the gain in appetitive or aver- population intracellular [Ca2+] fluorescent signals from a geneti-
sive stimuli involved in associative learning (Gozzi et al., 2010; cally encoded Ca2+ indicator through a single chronic fiber optic
Pape, 2010; Peck and Salzman, 2014). However, the BF-projec- implant (Akerboom et al., 2012; Chen et al., 2013), to monitor
ting specific cell types in the amygdala and the precise neural CeLPKC-d+ and CeLSOM+ neuronal activities during a go/no-go
circuit involved in associative learning are unknown. task. A Cre-dependent adeno-associated virus (AAV) encoding
Here, we investigated the role of CeLPKC-d+ and CeLSOM+ neu- Ca2+ indicator GCaMP5g (AAV2-EF1a-DIO-GCaMP5g) or
rons in a go/no-go associative learning task. Fiber photometry re- AAV2-EF1a-Flex-GFP (control) was injected into the CeL of
vealed that the CeLPKC-d+ neurons, but not the CeLSOM+ neurons, transgenic PKC-d-Cre mice, and an optical fiber was then
represented an aversive signal during negative reinforcement implanted above the CeL for simultaneous delivery of a
learning of the go/no-go task. Circuitry mapping and functional 473-nm excitation laser and collection of GCaMP5g fluores-
manipulation suggested that the SI-projecting CeLPKC-d+ neurons cence emissions (Figure 1D). GCaMP5g expression was largely
could bi-directionally modulate negative reinforcement learning. limited to the CeL, with minor expression in the CeM (Figure 1D).
Further analysis found that these neurons recruited SI glutama- Immunofluorescence staining confirmed the high specificity of
tergic neurons (Vglut2) to drive conditioned place aversion GCaMP5g expression in the CeLPKC-d+ neurons (mean ± SEM,
(CPA). Together, our data uncovered a previously unknown func- 88.8% ± 2.1%; n = 3 mice; Figure 1E). In early associative
tion of the CeLPKC-d+-SIVgluT2+ neural circuitry to modulate nega- learning, the CeLPKC-d+ neurons showed time-locked increases
tive reinforcement learning by encoding aversive signals. during the air puff stimulus (average peak DF/F ± SEM,
11.67% ± 2.14% ) in negative reinforcement learning and peak
RESULTS DF/F without statistical significance was observed on reward
port entries (water) in appetitive learning (Figures 1F and 1G).
Role of CeLPKC-d+ Neurons in the Negative Similarly, during late learning (Figure 1H) and expression of
Reinforcement Learning learning (Figure 1I), the punishment event was still reliably asso-
As reported in previous work, PKC-d and SOM account for ciated with a significant increase in GCaMP5g signals in the
90% of all CeL neurons and are largely non-overlapping (Hau- CeLPKC-d+ neurons (late phase: average peak DF/F ± SEM,
bensak et al., 2010; Li et al., 2013). To identify the neuronal activ- 9.06% ± 1.84%; expression phase: average peak DF/F ± SEM,
ity patterns of these two subpopulations in associative learning, 7.98% ± 1.14%). We observed slight, non-significant suppres-
we trained head-fixed mice to perform a go/no-go task, which is sion in the activity of CeLPKC-d+ neurons in response to water
a well-established behavioral paradigm used to study associa- (Figures 1H and 1I). US-evoked responses were not observed
tive learning by associating CS with reinforcer reward or punish- in the PKC-d-GFP control mice during the training phase (Fig-
ment (US), respectively (Figures 1A and 1B). The go cue requires ure 1J). We next tracked the activity of CeLSOM+ neurons in
mice to lick within a response time window to acquire a reward response to punishment or reward in the go/no-go task
(appetitive learning), whereas the no-go cue requires mice to (Figures S2A, S2D, and S2E). AAV2-EF1a-DIO-GCaMP5g was
withhold their responses to avoid punishment (negative rein- injected into the CeL of Som-IRES-Cre mice, with an optic fiber
forcement learning) (Dolzani et al., 2013; Gomez et al., 2007). implanted above the CeL (Figures S2B and S2C). We found that

Figure 1. Aversive Stimulus Increases Ca2+ Signals of CeLPKC-d+ Neurons in Negative Reinforcement Learning
(A) Behavioral setup of the go/no-go task and definition of four behavioral outcomes.
(B) Paradigm of the go/no-go task. A tone (2 clicks/s) was used as the go cue. A light or tone (50 clicks/s) was used as the no-go cue to counterbalance the
modality-specific activity of CeLPKC-d+ neurons.
(C) Performances in the go/no-go task in 7 PKC-d-Cre mice (4 PKC-d-CreGCaMP5g mice and 3 PKC-d-CreGFP mice; light was used as the no-go cue) and 3 PKC-d-
CreGCaMP5g mice (tone was used as the no-go cue). Data are presented as means ± SEM.
(D) Schematic of GCaMP5g viral targeting to CeLPKC-d+ neurons (left). Confocal image of GCaMP5g expression in the CeL and location of optical fiber (right).
Scale bar, 100 mm.
(E) Top: representative images of GCaMP5g (green) and cell-type-specific markers (red: PKC-d+ neurons) expressed in the CeL. Scale bars, 100 mm. Bottom:
enlargements of boxed areas. Scale bar, 10 mm.
(F) Heatmap of Ca2+ signals from mouse aligned to the initiation of water and air puff during the early phase of learning. Color scales on the right indicate DF/F.
(G I) Mean Ca2+ transients from seven PKC-d-CreGCaMP5g mice aligned to events starting with water and air puff during early learning (G), late learning (H), and
expression of learning (I) (early phase of learning, 1–3 sessions; late phase of learning, 6 8 sessions; expression of learning, 9 11 sessions). **p = 0.002 (G);
**p = 0.002 (H); **p = 0.003 (I).
(J) Mean Ca2+ transients from three PKC-d-CreGFP mice aligned to events starting with air puff and water across whole phases.
A Mann-Whitney U test was used to identify differences between baseline (average of 0.5 s before cues) and mean peak values (average duration of water or air
puff delivered). Thick lines indicate mean, and shaded areas indicate SEM.
See also Figure S1.

1772 Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017


Figure 2. Inhibition of CeLPKC-d+ Neurons Impairs Negative Reinforcement Learning
(A) Bilateral stereotaxic delivery of virus and implantation of optical fibers into the CeL of PKC-d-Cre mice.
(B) Confocal image of a coronal brain section taken from a PKC-d-Cre mouse bilaterally injected with AAV-EF1a-Flex-ArchT-GFP in the CeL. Scale bar, 500 mm.
(C) Experimental timeline.
(D) Paradigm of go/no-go task.
(E–H) Compared with the control group (n = 8), ArchT-expressing mice (n = 8) showed significantly lower performance (E), discriminability (F), and correct-
rejection rates (H), but not hit rates (G). Two-way analysis of variance with mixed design; F(1, 14) = 12.48, **p = 0.003 (E); F(1, 14) = 6.41, *p = 0.024 (F); F(1, 14) =
9.28, **p = 0.009, n.s., not significant (H). Data represent means ± SEM.
(I) Distribution of the response delay in go trials.
(J) Median response delay for each group.
(K and L) Performance (K) and correct-rejection (L) rate of mice in the last session of learning without laser (training) and with laser (expression). n.s., not significant
(Mann-Whitney U test). Boxplots show the mean (solid square), median, quartiles (boxes), and range (whiskers).

no matter in the early learning, the late learning, or learning sensitive inhibitory proton pump (AAV9-EF1a-Flex-ArchT-GFP),
expression, there were no significant changes in GCaMP5g fluo- or control virus (AAV2-EF1a-DIO-EYFP) into the CeL of PKC-d-
rescence in SOM+ neurons following punishment, reward port Cre mice and implanted optic fibers bilaterally into the CeL (Fig-
entries (Figures S2F–S2I). In addition, across the whole training ures 2A and 2B). A laser was delivered to activate ArchT in vivo,
phases, SOM+ neurons had no response to an aversive condi- thereby inhibiting CeLPKC-d+ neurons in a cell-type-specific
tioned stimulus (Figures S2J). The above data illuminate the manner. Mice were trained to perform the go/no-go behavioral
dynamics of CeLPKC-d+ and CeLSOM+ neurons during associative task (Figures 2C and 2D). We found that the ArchT mice with
learning, demonstrating that CeLPKC-d+ neurons, but not SOM+ photoinhibition of PKC-d+ neurons exhibited significantly lower
neurons, encode aversive signals during negative reinforcement performance across the entire course of training (Figure 2E).
learning. Detailed analysis revealed that PKC-d+ArchT mice showed lower
To examine whether PKC-d+ neurons in the CeL are involved in discriminability (Figure 2F) (used to evaluate an animal’s ability
negatively reinforced associative learning, we bilaterally injected to distinguish between two stimuli) and a poorer correct-rejection
AAV encoding Cre-inducible archaerhodopsin (ArchT), a light- rate (Figure 2H) in the no-go trials. The hit rates in the go trials

Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017 1773


were similar between the two groups (Figure 2G). In addition, Considering CeLPKC-d+ neurons displayed robust responses to
photoinhibition of PKC-d+ neurons did not influence the speed aversive stimulus (Figure 1), we next sought to examine whether
of decision-making in learning, as assessed by reaction delay the activation of the CeLPKC-d+-SI circuit was sufficient to facili-
(Figures 2I and 2J). Furthermore, laser illumination did not impair tate negative associative learning as an aversive reinforcer.
the behavioral performance or correct rejection rate during the Accordingly, AAV encoding Cre-inducible channelrhodopsin-2
expression of associative learning (Figures 2K and 2L), suggest- (ChR2), direct light-activated cation-selective ion channel
ing that CeL PKC-d+ neurons could modulate the acquisition, but (AAV5-EF1a-DIO-hChR2-EYFP), or control virus (AAV2-EF1a-
not the expression, of negative reinforcement learning. DIO-EYFP) was injected into the CeL of PKC-d-Cre mice, with
an optical fiber implanted above the SI (Figures 4A and 4B).
Circuitry Mechanism of CeLPKC-d+ Neurons Encoding Mice were then trained to perform the go/no-go task (Figure 4C).
Aversive Signals In the task, mice were divided into three groups that received two
To elucidate the circuitry mechanism through which PKC-d+ different aversive reinforced stimuli or no stimulus (Figure 4C).
neurons convey aversive signals, we mapped the whole-brain Optogenetic activation of CeLPKC-d+-SI ChR2-expressing termi-
targets of CeLPKC-d+ neurons in combination with fluorescence nals accelerated the learning rate in the group in which the air
micro-optical sectioning tomography (fMOST) (Li et al., 2010; puff was replaced with a 473-nm laser (2 s, 30 Hz, 10 ms)
Xu et al., 2013). AAV2-EF1a-DIO-EYFP was injected into the compared with the other two groups (Figure 4D). The effects
CeL of PKC-d-Cre mice. 21 days later, the brain was embedded, on behavior were caused mainly by an increase in the correct
continuously sliced, and imaged by the fMOST system. Massive rejection rate of negative reinforcement learning (Figure 4G),
axonal fibers were observed in the ventral bed nucleus of the without significant changes in discriminability and the hit rate
stria terminalis (BNSTv), BF, CeM, retrorubral field (RRF), and of appetitive learning (Figures 4E and 4F). The ChR2-expressing
lateral parabrachial nucleus (LPB) (Figure S3). Considering the mice also performed better than the control mice trained with an
functions of the BF underlying associative learning (Avila and air puff as the aversive reinforced stimulus, which was related to
Lin, 2014; Hangya et al., 2015; Harrison et al., 2016; Lau and the higher correct rejection rate in the initial two sessions (Fig-
Salzman, 2008), we next focused on the study of the CeL-BF cir- ure 4G). These results indicate that the effects caused by activa-
cuit. Confocal imaging revealed that prominent terminals of the tion of the CeLPKC-d+-SI circuit were similar to the behavioral
CeLPKC-d+ neurons (minor terminals of the CeMPKC-d+ neurons) effects induced by air puff as an aversive reinforced stimulus,
projected to the BF, particularly in the SI (Figures 3A and 3B). which coincided with the activity pattern of CeLPKC-d+ neurons
To further determine whether the CeL neurons formed monosyn- (Figure 1). The above data raised the question of whether
aptic connections with neurons in the SI, we performed Cre- silencing the circuit would impair negative reinforcement
dependent rabies-virus-based transsynaptic retrograde tracing learning. We inhibited the activity of the CeLPKC-d+-SI circuit by
(Miyamichi et al., 2011; Wall et al., 2010; Wickersham et al., injecting AAV9-EF1a-Flex-ArchT-GFP or control virus (AAV2-
2007). It is known that cholinergic (ChAT+) neurons comprise EF1a-DIO-EYFP) into the CeL of PKC-d-Cre mice, with an optical
5% of total neurons in the BF, whereas GABAergic (GABA+) fiber implanted above the SI (Figures 4H and 4I). Mice were
neurons account for 35%, and others, including glutamatergic trained to perform the go/no-go task in accordance with previ-
neurons, account for 60% (Henny and Jones, 2008; Mayo ous experimental procedures (Figures 2C and 2D). Indeed,
et al., 1984). We co-injected helper viruses encoding Cre-depen- specific inhibition of the CeLPKC-d+-SI projections impaired
dent avian sarcoma leucosis virus glycoprotein EnvA receptor acquisition of negative reinforcement learning similar to the
(TVA) (AAV5-CAG-FLEX TVA66T-mCherry) and rabies virus effects of inhibition of PKC-d+ neurons, as indicated by the lower
envelope glycoprotein (RG) (AAV5-CAG-FLEX-RG) into the SI discriminability and lower correct rejection rate in the no-go tri-
of VgluT2-Cre, GAD2-Cre, or ChAT-Cre mice. 3 weeks later, als, but maintained the hit rate in the go trials (Figures 4J–4M).
the EnvA-coated rabies-YFP virus (DRG-YFP) was then injected The reaction delay was not altered (Figures 4N and 4O). Further-
into the same place (Figure 3C). Starter cells expressing both more, inhibiting the CeLPKC-d+-SI projections did not interfere
mCherry and YFP were observed in the SI (Figure 3D). Retro- with the expression of negative reinforcement learning (Figures
gradely labeled (YFP+) cells were observed in the CeL (Fig- 4P and 4Q). To further inhibit activity of the CeLPKC-d+-SI circuit
ure 3D), which confirmed that CeL neurons could form monosyn- in behavioral outcomes, we applied continuous laser illumination
aptic connections with the three cell types in the SI. The CeL-SI (532 nm) during the behavioral outcomes (Figure S4A). ArchT-
connection was further characterized by immunostaining of the expressing mice indeed showed poorer performance and
PKC-d protein in the CeL, with retrograde tracer cholera-toxin reduced discriminability and correct-rejection rates, but not hit
B (CTB) injected into the SI (Figure 3E). Results showed that rates, compared with mice in the control group (Figures
84.55% ± 2.35% of CTB-labeled neurons in the CeL overlapped S4B–S4E), which were similar to the effects of inhibiting
with PKC-d+ neurons (30.28% ± 5.9% of PKC-d+ neurons in the CeLPKC-d+-SI circuit across the entire period from the start of
CeL projected to the SI; Figure 3G). Furthermore, we tested the trials to the end of the response window in all trials (Figure 4).
whether neurons expressing SOM projected to the SI by inject- Together, these results demonstrate that the CeLPKC-d+-SI circuit
ing CTB-647 into the SI of Som-IRES-Cre; Ai3 mice and found could bi-directionally modulate negative reinforcement learning
that a few SOM+ neurons in the CeL projected to the SI in the go/no-go task.
(5.28% ± 0.56% of CTB-labeled neurons expressing SOM+ neu- As the CeLPKC-d+-SI neural circuitry transmits an aversive
rons; Figures 3F and 3G). These results demonstrate the contri- signal, we sought to test whether activation of the
bution of CeLPKC-d+ neurons to the SI-projecting cell population. circuitry could drive aversive-related behavior. We injected

1774 Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017


Figure 3. CeLPKC-d+ Neurons Project to the SI
(A) Virus expression in CeL of PKC-d-Cre mice. Scale bars represent 500 mm (left) and 100 mm (right). Approximate distance from the bregma is shown at the
bottom.
(B) Representative histological images of the SI innervated by axonal terminals from CeLPKC-d+ neurons. Scale bars represent 500 mm (left) and 200 mm (right).
Similar results were confirmed in two other mice. Approximate distance from the bregma is shown at the bottom. CPu, caudate putamen (striatum); LGP, lateral
globus pallidus; LH, lateral hypothalamic area.
(C) Virus injection procedures for rabies retrograde tracing. Helper viruses: AAV5-CAG-FLEX TVA66T-mCherry, AAV5-CAG-FLEX-RG (1:1). Rabies virus: EnvA-
RV-YFP.
(D) Typical monosynaptic rabies tracing coronal sections of the anatomical localization of the BF and CeL. Scale bars represent 200 mm (BF), 20 mm (starter
neuron), and 50 mm (CeL). The results were repeated in at least three mice per group.
(E) Top left: coronal brain section showing the SI injected with CTB-488 (green), scale bar, 500 mm. Top right: projecting neurons labeled with CTB (green). PKC-d+
neurons stained with Alexa Fluor 594 (red). Scale bar, 50 mm. Bottom: higher-magnification of images of the noted region in the CeL. Scale bar, 10 mm.
(F) Confocal images of a coronal brain section from a Som-IRES-Cre; Ai3 mouse with CTB-647 injected into the SI. Scale bars represent 100 mm (top) and 10 mm
(bottom, higher-magnification images).
(G) Statistical graphs showing co-labeling of CTB-labeled neurons and PKC-d+ or SOM+ neurons in the CeL. n = 3 mice/group. Boxplots show mean (solid
square), median, quartiles (boxes), and range (whiskers).
See also Figure S3.

AAV5-EF1a-DIO-hChR2-EYFP or control virus (AAV2-EF1a- (Figure 5A). On the conditioning day, laser stimulation (30 Hz,
DIO-EYFP) into the CeL of PKC-d-Cre mice and tested the con- 10 ms) was randomly paired to one of the two chambers and
sequences of optogenetically activating SI fibers from CeLPKC-d+ delivered whenever the mice entered the paired chamber.
neurons in a conditioned place preference/avoidance (CPP/A) Mice were tested twice more on day 3 (Figure 5A). On post-
assay. Using a revised 3-day protocol, ChR2- and EYFP-ex- test 1 without the laser, the ChR2CeL-SI terminals photostimu-
pressing (control) mice were tested in a two-chamber apparatus lated on day 2 mice displayed obvious CPA, whereas control

Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017 1775


Figure 4. CeLPKC-d+-SI Projections Modulate Negative Reinforcement Learning
(A) Schematic of the experimental approach.
(B) Top: confocal image of a coronal brain section taken from a PKC-d-Cre mouse with AAV5-EF1a-DIO-hChR2-EYFP injected into the CeL. Scale bar, 200 mm.
Bottom: ChR2-expresssion terminals were observed in the SI. Scale bar, 200 mm.
(C) Experimental design of the go/no-go task.
(D–G) Performance (D), discriminability (E), hit rate (F), and correct-rejection rate (G) of the three groups. Black line represents comparison of the ChR2-expressing
group with laser as punishment (n = 6) and the control group with laser as punishment (n = 7); green line represents comparison of the ChR2-expressing group with
laser as punishment and the control group with air puff as punishment (n = 6). n.s., not significant (two-way ANOVA with mixed design). (D) Black line:
(legend continued on next page)

1776 Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017


Figure 5. Activation of CeLPKC-d+-SI Projec-
tions Drives CPA
(A) Procedure of CPP/A performance.
(B) Traces on day 1 (pre-test) and day 3 (post-test 1
and post-test 2).
(C) Post-test 1/pre-test and post-test 2/pre-test
ratios of time spent in conditioned chamber.
CeLPKC-d+-SI-GFP (black), n = 7; CeLPKC-d+-SI-
ChR2 (blue), n = 7. Post-test 1, *p = 0.035; post-
test 2, **p = 0.002 (Mann-Whitney U test).
(D) Time spent in conditioned chamber on day 3
and day 1. Post-test 1: *p = 0.018; post-test 2: **p =
0.002 (Mann-Whitney U test). Boxplots show mean
(solid square), median, quartiles (boxes), and range
(whiskers) .

mice spent a similar amount of time in the two chambers (Figures S6B). Compared with the control condition (no laser), photoacti-
5B–5D). Moreover, on post-test 2, real-time stimulation of vation of the CeLPKC-d+ neuronal fibers induced c-Fos expres-
CeLPKC-d+ neuronal terminals whenever mice entered the condi- sion, a surrogate marker for neuronal activation, in SI VgluT2+
tioned chamber on day 2 caused a further increase in CPA (Fig- (Figures S6C and S6D) and ChAT+ neurons (Figures S6G and
ures 5B–5D). These data show that activation of the CeLPKC-d+-SI S6H), but not in GABA+ neurons (Figures S6E and S6F). These
circuit drives CPA by transmitting an aversive signal. results indicate that VgluT2+ and ChAT+ neurons are more likely
to receive an aversive signal, although the possible involvement
SI VgluT2+ Neurons Are Responsible for Receiving an of GABA+ neurons and c-Fos expressed in a cell-type-specific
Aversive Signal from CeLPKC-d+ Neurons way could not be ruled out. In order to validate it from the
Although enhanced activity of the CeLPKC-d+-SI circuit drove behavior, we optogenetically activated the three types of neu-
CPA, the subpopulation of SI neurons receiving aversive signals rons in a real-time place preference/aversion task (RTPP/A) (Fig-
from the CeLPKC-d+ neurons was unknown. To address this, we ure 6A). Only activation of the VgluT2+ neurons induced CPA
first recorded activity of the SI neurons when CeLPKC-d+-SI pro- (Figures 6B–6D). Activation of the GABA+ neurons induced a
jection terminals were activated (Figure S5A). A total of 53 single preference in the activation-paired chamber (Figures 6E and
units were recorded using a moveable, 32-channel optetrode 6F), but no obvious place preference or aversion was observed
microwire bundle in three mice in which electrode placements after activating ChAT+ neurons (Figures 6G and 6H). These
were confirmed in the SI (Figure S5B). We observed that the firing results suggest that VgluT2+ neurons in the SI are most likely
rates of 13.21% neurons (7/53 neurons; Figures S5C and S5D) to receive aaversive signals from the CeL. This speculation
were increased when the activity of the CeLPKC-d+-SI projection was corroborated after AAV2/9-FLEX-taCasp3-TEVp was
terminals was enhanced. The excited neurons exhibited a long infused into the SI of VgluT2-Cre mice selectively to kill VgluT2+
latency time following laser stimulation, indicating that informa- neurons and AAV9-CAG-ChR2-GFP was infused into the CeL of
tion was transmitted across SI multi-synaptic pathways (Ciocchi the same mice to activate the CeL-SI circuit (Figure 6I). Consis-
et al., 2015). However, excited neural types in the SI were tent with the effects of taCasp3 on inducing cell apoptosis (Yang
unknown. Neuronal heterogeneity in the BF is an important chal- et al., 2013), a mass of ablated neurons was observed by TUNEL
lenge in the study of such neurons (Harrison et al., 2016). To visu- apoptosis detection (Figure 6J). SI-VgluT2-taCasp3 mice had
alize the three spatially intermingled major cell types in the SI, we significantly reduced CPA induced by activation of the CeL-SI
injected AAV9-CAG-ChR2-GFP into the CeL of ChAT-YFP mice circuit (Figures 6K and 6L), suggesting that SIVgluT2+ neurons,
or VgluT2-Cre, GAD2-Cre, and ChAT-Cre mice in which as a relay station receiving aversive signals from CeLPKC-d+ neu-
VgluT2+, GABA+, and ChAT+ neurons were respectively labeled rons, are indispensable for aversive-related behavior induced by
by injecting AAV2-EF1a-Flex-GFP into the SI (Figures S6A and activation of CeLPKC-d+ neurons.

F(1, 11) = 17.26, **p = 0.002; green line: F(1, 10) = 8.23, *p = 0.017. (G) Black line: F(1, 11) = 18.11, **p = 0.001; green line: F(1, 10) = 9.76, *p = 0.011. Data represent
means ± SEM.
(H) Schematic of targeted genetic inactivation of the CeLPKC-d+-SI circuit.
(I) Representative images of the AAV injection site in the CeL (top) and ArchT-expressing terminals in the SI (bottom). Scale bar, 500 mm.
(J–M) Performance (J), discriminability (K), hit rate (L), and correct rejection rate (M) of ArchT (n = 8) and control mice (n = 8). n.s., not significant (two-way ANOVA
with mixed design). (J) F(1, 14) = 37.90, ***p < 0.001; (K) F(1, 14) = 16.81, **p = 0.001; (M) F(1, 14) = 42.35, ***p < 0.001. Data represent means ± SEM.
(N) Distribution of the response delay in the go trials.
(O) Median response delay of mice.
(P and Q) Performance (P) and correct-rejection rate (Q) of mice in the last session of learning without laser (training) and with laser (expression). n.s., not
significant (Mann-Whitney U test). Boxplots show mean (solid square), median, quartiles (boxes), and range (whiskers).

Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017 1777


Figure 6. SIVgluT2+ Neurons Are Indispensable for CPA Produced by CeLPKC-d+ Neurons
(A) Procedure for RTPP performance.
(B) Examples of pre-test and real-time behavior traces accompanying photostimulation of SI VgluT2+ neurons.
(C–H) Aversion or preference for place was shown when SI VgluT2+ (C and D, VgluT2-GFP, n = 4; VgluT2-ChR2, n = 4), GABA+ (E and F, GABA-GFP, n = 5; GABA-
ChR2, n = 4), and ChAT+ neurons (G and H, ChAT-GFP, n = 4; ChAT-ChR2, n = 5) were activated. n.s., not significant (Mann-Whitney U test). VgluT2+: RT/Pre,
*p = 0.021; avoidance score, *p = 0.021. GABA+: RT/Pre, *p = 0.027; avoidance score, *p = 0.014.
(I) Diagram of viruses injected into the SI and CeL. Optical fiber was implanted above the SI for photostimulation of CeLPKC-d+ outputs.

(legend continued on next page)

1778 Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017


DISCUSSION have recently been shown to fire in different patterns in response
to conditioned stimuli with different behavioral responses (Fadok
Combining the head-fixed go/no-go behavioral paradigm, fiber et al., 2017). Thus, to gain better understanding of the heteroge-
photometry, optogenetic manipulation, and in vivo electrophys- neity of responses in CeL PKC-d+ and SOM+ neurons, it would
iology and anatomy, we characterized a CeL-SI neural circuit be interesting to know the temporal profile of firing rate changes
that encodes aversive signals and serves as a negative reinforcer by multi-channel recording or imaging individual cells in different
to bi-directionally modulate associative learning. Importantly, we tasks in future research.
found that CeLPKC-d+ neurons drive CPA by transmitting an aver- Knowledge about the functions of the CeL-SI neural circuit
sive signal to SI glutamatergic neurons. remains limited. Previous studies have suggested that CeA
Prior studies have demonstrated amygdala responses evoked efferents and BF neurons mainly overlap and form symmetric
by aversive stimuli, but the specific neural type has remained synapses with neurons in the ventrolateral SI (Jolkkonen et al.,
unclear (Calu et al., 2010; McHugh et al., 2014). Neurons ex- 2002). Our anatomy results revealed the contribution of PKC-
pressing calcitonin gene-related peptide receptor (CGRPR) in d+ neurons to the SI-projecting cell population (Figure 3) and
the CeL, which can receive footshock-driven US aversive signals thus contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of the
from parabrachial nucleus CGRP neurons, have been found to CeL-BF circuit. The CeLPKC-d+ neurons mediated negative rein-
partially overlap with PKC-d+ neurons (Han et al., 2015). Consid- forcement learning by relaying an aversive signal to the SI. This
ering its expression in multiple neural populations, the CGRPR as result extends and complements the extensive roles of CeL
a marker to study US aversive signal transmission from the CeL neurons in encoding incentive motivation, conditioned reinforce-
to other downstream nodes might not be particularly accurate ment, and fear conditioning (Ciocchi et al., 2010; Haubensak
(Han et al., 2015). Using fiber photometry, we identified a sub- et al., 2010; Li et al., 2013; Tye and Janak, 2007; Yu et al.,
population of CeL neurons expressing PKC-d that showed a 2016). We observed that CeL PKC-d+ neurons also projected
robust response to the air puff as an aversive stimulus during to other brain regions, such as the BNSTv, which is a major target
negative reinforcement learning (Figure 1). The rapid increase of projections from the BLA and CeA and plays an important role
in GCaMP5g signal activity following punishment indicated in mediating negative emotion, such as anxiety (Duvarci et al.,
some homogeneity among the response patterns of the 2009). Inhibition of CeLPKC-d+-BNST circuit activity had no signif-
PKC-d+ neurons encoding an aversive signal. At the same icant effect on performance in the go/no-go task (data not
time, the response to aversive stimuli in the CeLPKC-d+ neurons shown). Considering that CeL PKC-d+ neurons can mediate
weakly decreased as training proceeded, leading to the specu- appetitive behavior such as feeding (Cai et al., 2014), our data
lation that CeLPKC-d+ neurons might be related to teaching raise several questions: What is the underlying neural circuit for
signals (Johansen and Fields, 2004; McNally et al., 2011). A prev- reward-related signals in the amygdala? What are the differ-
alent model for the functional organization of amygdala circuits ences between circuits transmitting appetitive signals and those
posits CeL PKC-d+ neurons as CeLoff neurons, a population transmitting aversive signals? We will expand our current
that shows inhibitory CS responses following fear conditioning research in future studies to map whole-brain monosynaptic
(Haubensak et al., 2010). On the contrary, we did not observe in- inputs to the amygdala and BF and manipulate those inputs
hibition of CeL PKC- d+ neural activity in response to CS during separately to test their effects on associative learning.
the whole training phases. Weak increase of CeL PKC-d+ neural Multi-channel results showed that certain neurons in the SI
activity in response to CS was observed during the early that were activated by enhancing CeL-SI circuit activity exhibited
learning, which might be caused by the possible that mice long latency or even double activation (Figure S5), which
mistake that the CS is aversive. It is noteworthy that we found suggests that functional connection from the CeLPKC-d+ neurons
that a substantial number of PKC-d+ neurons were aversive to the SI might be executed by multi-synaptic pathways of the SI,
‘‘US-on’’ neurons and modulate negative reinforcement learning thus highlighting the complexity of SI local circuits. Immuno-
by conveying an aversive US, implying that CeLPKC-d+ neurons staining revealed that photoactivation of the CeL-SI circuit
might encode different signals in different behavioral tasks. induced c-Fos expression in SI VgluT2+ neurons. Rabies-virus-
A weak (nonsignificant) increase in GCaMP5g fluorescence in mediated monosynaptic retrograde tracing suggested that CeL
the early stage and suppression in the later and expression neurons could target three cell types in SI (Figure 2D). As
stages were also observed when mice licked to acquire a CeLPKC-d+ outputs are GABAergic (Haubensak et al., 2010), the
reward, suggesting that CeL PKC-d+ neurons might participate most parsimonious explanation consistent with these results in-
in valence-specific behaviors. Although CeL SOM+ neurons volves a local SI disinhibitory circuitry mechanism that could
were activated by threat-predicting sensory cues after fear con- convert an inhibitory input from PKC-d+ neurons to activate SI
ditioning (Yu et al., 2016), we did not observe significant re- VgluT2+ neurons. Besides, by selectively ablating VgluT2+ neu-
sponses to the aversive CS (Figure S2J). This discrepancy could rons in the SI, we revealed that these neurons are essential for
be due to different behavioral tasks. In fact, CeA SOM+ neurons CeLPKC-d+-SI neural circuitry in driving CPA. Taken together,

(J) Images show apoptosis neurons by TUNEL apoptosis detection after AAV-flex-taCasp3-TEVp or AAV2-EF1a-Flex-GFP was injected into the SI of VgluT2-Cre
mice. Scale bar, 20 mm.
(K and L) Preference (K) and time spent (L) in the conditioned chamber after ablating SI VgluT2+ neurons compared with control (VgluT2-GFP, n = 7; VgluT2-
taCasp3, n = 6). RT/Pre, VgluT2-GFP: RT/Pre, *p = 0.022; avoidance score, *p = 0.032 (Mann-Whitney U test). Boxplots show mean (solid square), median,
quartiles (boxes), and range (whiskers).

Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017 1779


these data suggest that SI VgluT2+ neurons are responsible for Immunohistochemistry and Cell Quantification
receiving an aversive signal from CeL PKC-d+ neurons. We Immunohistochemical analysis was performed as described previously (Li et al.,
2011). Primary antibodies were anti-PKC-d (mouse, BD Biosciences, 610397,
could not exclude the possibility that the SI VgluT2+ neurons
1:700) and anti-c-Fos (rabbit, Synaptic Systems, 226003, 1:10,000). Detailed
can also be activated by behavior (CPA) elicited by CeL PKC- experimental methods can be found in Supplemental Experimental Procedures.
d+ neuron activation or that other SI neural subtypes also can
receive aversive signals from CeL PKC-d+ neurons but are Fiber Photometry
unable to drive a behavioral response. Previous studies have Following AAV2-EF1a-DIO-GCaMP5g or AAV2-EF1a-Flex-GFP virus injec-
mainly focused on the functions of the cholinergic system tion, an optical fiber (230 mm outer diameter [O.D.], 0.37 numerical aperture
involved in cortical plasticity, spatial learning, working memory, [NA]; Shanghai Fiblaser Technology, Shanghai, China) was placed in a ceramic
ferrule and inserted toward the CeL. The ceramic ferrule was supported with
and other mnemonic tasks (Chudasama et al., 2004; Do et al.,
dental acrylic. Mice were allowed to recover for at least 1 week. For recording
2016; Kilgard, 2003). Studies on the functions of BF non-cholin-
fluorescence signals, we used the fscope fiber photometry system
ergic neurons forming widespread inputs and projections are (BiolinkOptics, China). An optical fiber (230 mm O.D., NA 0.37; Thorlabs) guided
limited (Do et al., 2016). Our optogenetic manipulation revealed the 473-nm laser between the recording system and the implanted optical
that activating SI glutamatergic neurons elicited obvious CPA, fiber. The laser power was adjusted at 20–30 mW. GCaMP5g fluorescence
whereas activating SI GABA+ neurons elicited CPP, suggesting was further filtered through a low-pass filter (30 Hz cutoff). Signals were
that both cell types might perform contrary functions in some collected at a sampling frequency of 100 Hz. The relative change in fluores-
cence, DF/F = (F F0)/F0, was calculated by taking F0 to be the baseline of
contexts. In summary, our study of the neural circuitry involved
the fluorescence signal averaged over 4 s before CS (light or tone) in each ses-
in transmitting aversive signals furthers our current understand- sion. For analysis of the statistical significance of event-related fluorescence
ing of the functions of the amygdala in encoding aversive asso- changes, we defined the mean of the signals during the 0.5 s period before
ciative learning. CS onset as the baseline fluorescence signal and the US (air puff and water)
evoked responses as the mean of the fluorescence signals for the duration
of the US. Considering the peak US responses with delays longer than the
EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES
duration of US, we searched the peak US responses in each session within
a 1-s time window after US onset. All data were recorded by f-scopeACQ
Animals
software and analyzed using MATLAB.
Male and female mice 4–12 weeks of age were used for the experiments. All
mice used for go/no-go behavioral training were bred onto the C57BL/6J
genetic background. The Som-IRES-Cre, VgluT2-Cre, and GAD2-Cre mice CPA
were obtained from the Josh Huang Lab (Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, CPA was performed in a two-compartment conditioning apparatus (25 3 25 3
Cold Spring Harbor, NY, USA). Rosa26-loxP-STOP-loxP-H2B-EYFP reporter 50 cm/chamber). A video-tracking system (ANY-Maze software, Stoelting,
line (Ai3) and ChAT-IRES-Cre mice (Jackson Laboratories stock number Wood Dale, IL, USA) recorded all mice movements. For detailed behavioral
006410) were purchased from The Jackson Laboratory. PKC-d-Cre mice procedures, see Supplemental Experimental Procedures.
were provided from the Bo Li Lab (Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory). Som-
IRES-Cre mice were crossed with Ai3 mice to visualize SOM+ neurons. All Statistics
mice were group-housed under a 12-hr light/dark cycle (7:00 a.m. to 7:00 Sample Size
p.m. light) and bred in the animal facility at the Wuhan National Laboratory We did not perform a calculation on sample size. Sample size was based on
for Optoelectronics (Wuhan, China). All procedures involving animals were past experience using similar techniques and animal models.
approved by the Hubei Provincial Animal Care and Use Committee and Analysis
complied with the experimental guidelines of the Animal Experimentation Data are presented as means ± SEM or as box-and-whisker plots. Data collec-
Ethics Committee of Huazhong University of Science and Technology, China. tion and analysis were not performed blindly. We did not randomize subjects.
They were assigned to control or experimental groups based on whether they
Virus and Retrograde Tracer received EYFP, GFP, ChR2, or ArchT virus injection. p values were calculated
The retrograde tracer Alexa Fluor 488/647-conjugated CTB was purchased by Mann-Whitney U test. For comparisons across two learning curves, data
from Invitrogen. AAV viruses, including AAV9-EF1a-Flex-ArchT-GFP, were analyzed using two-way analysis of variance with mixed design. All
AAV9-CAG-ChR2-GFP, AAV2-EF1a-DIO-EYFP, AAV2-EF1a-Flex-GFP, and data were analyzed using SPSS software (version 22, IBM, New York, NY,
USA). Statistical significance was set at *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001.
AAV5-EF1a-DIO-hChR2-EYFP, were produced at the Gene Therapy Center
Vector Core Facility, University of North Carolina (Chapel Hill, NC, USA).
AAV2-FLEX-tasCasp3-TEVp was purchased from Shanghai Taiting Biolog- SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION
ical (Shanghai, China). AAV2-EF1a-DIO-GCaMP5g was purchased from
Obio Technology (Shanghai, China). The monosynaptic retrograde tracing Supplemental Information includes Supplemental Experimental Procedures
system, including AAV5-CAG-FLEX TVA66T-mCherry, AAV5-CAG-FLEX- and six figures and can be found with this article online at https://doi.org/10.
RG, and rabies virus EnvA-RV-YFP, was provided by the Wuhan Institute 1016/j.celrep.2017.10.062.
of Physics and Mathematics (WIPM), Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS).
For detailed viral injection procedures, see Supplemental Experimental AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Procedures.
Y.C., G.L., and S.J. performed most of the experiments. Y.C., G.L., and H.L.
The Go/No-Go Behavioral Task analyzed the data. J.Y., J.P., X.H., and H.G. performed partial imaging exper-
The go/no-go task included a shaping phase and a training phase. Mice were iments. H.L., F.X., and T.X supervised the project. Y.C. and H.L. designed the
trained for two sessions per day. Each session consisted of 200 trials. Air puff, study and wrote the manuscript.
water delivery, cue, and laser illumination were triggered through serial ports,
and licking events were recorded by a recording system (LabState ver1.0 for ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
operant behavior, AniLab Software & Instruments, China). For details on calcu-
lation of performance, the optical stimulation protocol, and additional informa- We thank B. Li (Cold Spring Harbor Lab) for the PKC-d-Cre mice, Y.Y. Li (Huaz-
tion, see Supplemental Experimental Procedures. hong University of Science & Technology) for help with the MATLAB coding,

1780 Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017


and L.W. Mu and Q.L. Hu (Huazhong University of Science & Technology) for €thi, A. (2009).
Ehrlich, I., Humeau, Y., Grenier, F., Ciocchi, S., Herry, C., and Lu
optogenetic setups. We thank Kristen Delevich (UC Berkeley) and Xiaohong Xu Amygdala inhibitory circuits and the control of fear memory. Neuron 62,
(Institute of Neuroscience, CAS) for critical reading and editing of the manu- 757–771.
script, Hailan Hu (Zhejiang University) for critical reading of the manuscript, Fadok, J.P., Krabbe, S., Markovic, M., Courtin, J., Xu, C., Massi, L., Botta, P.,
and members of the Li laboratory for helpful discussions. This study was sup- Bylund, K., Mu €ller, C., Kovacevic, A., et al. (2017). A competitive inhibitory cir-
ported by The National Key Research and Development Program of China— cuit for selection of active and passive fear responses. Nature 542, 96–100.
Stem Cell and Translational Research (2016YFA0102500), the National Natural
Fanselow, M.S., and Poulos, A.M. (2005). The neuroscience of mammalian
Science Foundation of China (grants 91432107 and 31671105), the Science
associative learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 207–234.
Fund for Creative Research Group of China (grant 61421064), and the Director
Fund of the Wuhan National Laboratory for Optoelectronics. Gomez, P., Ratcliff, R., and Perea, M. (2007). A model of the go/no-go task.
J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 389–413.
Received: July 12, 2017 Gozzi, A., Jain, A., Giovannelli, A., Bertollini, C., Crestan, V., Schwarz, A.J.,
Revised: September 26, 2017 Tsetsenis, T., Ragozzino, D., Gross, C.T., and Bifone, A. (2010). A neural switch
Accepted: October 16, 2017 for active and passive fear. Neuron 67, 656–666.
Published: November 14, 2017
Han, S., Soleiman, M.T., Soden, M.E., Zweifel, L.S., and Palmiter, R.D. (2015).
Elucidating an Affective Pain Circuit that Creates a Threat Memory. Cell 162,
REFERENCES 363–374.
Hangya, B., Ranade, S.P., Lorenc, M., and Kepecs, A. (2015). Central cholin-
Akerboom, J., Chen, T.W., Wardill, T.J., Tian, L., Marvin, J.S., Mutlu, S., Cal- ergic neurons are rapidly recruited by reinforcement feedback. Cell 162, 1155–
derón, N.C., Esposti, F., Borghuis, B.G., Sun, X.R., et al. (2012). Optimization 1168.
of a GCaMP calcium indicator for neural activity imaging. J. Neurosci. 32, Harrison, T.C., Pinto, L., Brock, J.R., and Dan, Y. (2016). Calcium imaging of
13819–13840. basal forebrain activity during innate and learned behaviors. Front. Neural Cir-
Avarguès-Weber, A., de Brito Sanchez, M.G., Giurfa, M., and Dyer, A.G. cuits 10, 36.
(2010). Aversive reinforcement improves visual discrimination learning in Haubensak, W., Kunwar, P.S., Cai, H., Ciocchi, S., Wall, N.R., Ponnusamy, R.,
free-flying honeybees. PLoS ONE 5, e15370. Biag, J., Dong, H.W., Deisseroth, K., Callaway, E.M., et al. (2010). Genetic
Avila, I., and Lin, S.C. (2014). Motivational salience signal in the basal forebrain dissection of an amygdala microcircuit that gates conditioned fear. Nature
is coupled with faster and more precise decision speed. PLoS Biol. 12, 468, 270–276.
e1001811. Henny, P., and Jones, B.E. (2008). Projections from basal forebrain to prefron-
Cai, H., Haubensak, W., Anthony, T.E., and Anderson, D.J. (2014). Central tal cortex comprise cholinergic, GABAergic and glutamatergic inputs to pyra-
amygdala PKC-d(+) neurons mediate the influence of multiple anorexigenic midal cells or interneurons. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 654–670.
signals. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1240–1248. Herry, C., Ciocchi, S., Senn, V., Demmou, L., Mu €ller, C., and Lu
€thi, A. (2008).
Calu, D.J., Roesch, M.R., Haney, R.Z., Holland, P.C., and Schoenbaum, G. Switching on and off fear by distinct neuronal circuits. Nature 454, 600–606.
(2010). Neural correlates of variations in event processing during learning in Johansen, J.P., and Fields, H.L. (2004). Glutamatergic activation of anterior
central nucleus of amygdala. Neuron 68, 991–1001. cingulate cortex produces an aversive teaching signal. Nat. Neurosci. 7,
Chen, T.W., Wardill, T.J., Sun, Y., Pulver, S.R., Renninger, S.L., Baohan, A., 398–403.
Schreiter, E.R., Kerr, R.A., Orger, M.B., Jayaraman, V., et al. (2013). Ultrasen- Jolkkonen, E., Miettinen, R., Pikkarainen, M., and Pitkänen, A. (2002). Projec-
sitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499, 295–300. tions from the amygdaloid complex to the magnocellular cholinergic basal
Chudasama, Y., Dalley, J.W., Nathwani, F., Bouger, P., and Robbins, T.W. forebrain in rat. Neuroscience 111, 133–149.
(2004). Cholinergic modulation of visual attention and working memory: disso- Kilgard, M. (2003). Cholinergic modulation of skill learning and plasticity.
ciable effects of basal forebrain 192-IgG-saporin lesions and intraprefrontal in- Neuron 38, 678–680.
fusions of scopolamine. Learn. Mem. 11, 78–86. Lau, B., and Salzman, C.D. (2008). Noncholinergic neurons in the basal fore-
Ciocchi, S., Herry, C., Grenier, F., Wolff, S.B., Letzkus, J.J., Vlachos, I., Ehrlich, brain: often neglected but motivationally salient. Neuron 59, 6–8.
I., Sprengel, R., Deisseroth, K., Stadler, M.B., et al. (2010). Encoding of condi- LeDoux, J.E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23,
tioned fear in central amygdala inhibitory circuits. Nature 468, 277–282. 155–184.
Ciocchi, S., Passecker, J., Malagon-Vina, H., Mikus, N., and Klausberger, T. Li, A., Gong, H., Zhang, B., Wang, Q., Yan, C., Wu, J., Liu, Q., Zeng, S., and
(2015). Brain computation. Selective information routing by ventral hippocam- Luo, Q. (2010). Micro-optical sectioning tomography to obtain a high-resolu-
pal CA1 projection neurons. Science 348, 560–563. tion atlas of the mouse brain. Science 330, 1404–1408.
Cybulska-Klosowicz, A., Zakrzewska, R., and Kossut, M. (2009). Brain activa- Li, B., Piriz, J., Mirrione, M., Chung, C., Proulx, C.D., Schulz, D., Henn, F., and
tion patterns during classical conditioning with appetitive or aversive UCS. Malinow, R. (2011). Synaptic potentiation onto habenula neurons in the learned
Behav. Brain Res. 204, 102–111. helplessness model of depression. Nature 470, 535–539.
Davis, M., Walker, D.L., and Myers, K.M. (2003). Role of the amygdala in fear Li, H., Penzo, M.A., Taniguchi, H., Kopec, C.D., Huang, Z.J., and Li, B. (2013).
extinction measured with potentiated startle. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 985, Experience-dependent modification of a central amygdala fear circuit. Nat.
218–232. Neurosci. 16, 332–339.
Do, J.P., Xu, M., Lee, S.H., Chang, W.C., Zhang, S., Chung, S., Yung, T.J., Fan, Lin, S.C., and Nicolelis, M.A.L. (2008). Neuronal ensemble bursting in the basal
J.L., Miyamichi, K., Luo, L., et al. (2016). Cell type-specific long-range connec- forebrain encodes salience irrespective of valence. Neuron 59, 138–149.
tions of basal forebrain circuit. eLife 5, e13214.
Mayo, W., Dubois, B., Ploska, A., Javoy-Agid, F., Agid, Y., Le Moal, M., and
Dolzani, S.D., Nakamura, S., and Cooper, D.C. (2013). A novel variable delay Simon, H. (1984). Cortical cholinergic projections from the basal forebrain of
Go/No-Go task to study attention, motivation and working memory in the the rat, with special reference to the prefrontal cortex innervation. Neurosci.
head-fixed rodent. F1000Res 2, 125. Lett. 47, 149–154.
Duvarci, S., Bauer, E.P., and Paré, D. (2009). The bed nucleus of the stria ter- ~o, L., Lima, J., Lowry, J.P., and
McHugh, S.B., Barkus, C., Huber, A., Capita
minalis mediates inter-individual variations in anxiety and fear. J. Neurosci. 29, Bannerman, D.M. (2014). Aversive prediction error signals in the amygdala.
10357–10361. J. Neurosci. 34, 9024–9033.

Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017 1781


McNally, G.P., Johansen, J.P., and Blair, H.T. (2011). Placing prediction into Wall, N.R., Wickersham, I.R., Cetin, A., De La Parra, M., and Callaway, E.M.
the fear circuit. Trends Neurosci. 34, 283–292. (2010). Monosynaptic circuit tracing in vivo through Cre-dependent targeting
Miyamichi, K., Amat, F., Moussavi, F., Wang, C., Wickersham, I., Wall, N.R., and complementation of modified rabies virus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
Taniguchi, H., Tasic, B., Huang, Z.J., He, Z., et al. (2011). Cortical representa- 107, 21848–21853.
tions of olfactory input by trans-synaptic tracing. Nature 472, 191–196.
Wickersham, I.R., Finke, S., Conzelmann, K.K., and Callaway, E.M. (2007).
O’Doherty, J., Dayan, P., Schultz, J., Deichmann, R., Friston, K., and Dolan, Retrograde neuronal tracing with a deletion-mutant rabies virus. Nat. Methods
R.J. (2004). Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental 4, 47–49.
conditioning. Science 304, 452–454.
Xu, D., Jiang, T., Li, A., Hu, B., Feng, Z., Gong, H., Zeng, S., and Luo, Q. (2013).
Pape, H.C. (2010). Petrified or aroused with fear: the central amygdala takes
Fast optical sectioning obtained by structured illumination microscopy using a
the lead. Neuron 67, 527–529.
digital mirror device. J. Biomed. Opt. 18, 060503.
Paré, D., Quirk, G.J., and Ledoux, J.E. (2004). New vistas on amygdala net-
works in conditioned fear. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 1–9. Yang, C.F., Chiang, M.C., Gray, D.C., Prabhakaran, M., Alvarado, M., Juntti,
Peck, C.J., and Salzman, C.D. (2014). The amygdala and basal forebrain as a S.A., Unger, E.K., Wells, J.A., and Shah, N.M. (2013). Sexually dimorphic neu-
pathway for motivationally guided attention. J. Neurosci. 34, 13757–13767. rons in the ventromedial hypothalamus govern mating in both sexes and
aggression in males. Cell 153, 896–909.
Resnik, J., Sobel, N., and Paz, R. (2012). Auditory aversive learning increases
discrimination thresholds. J. Mol. Neurosci. 48, S96–S96. Yu, K., Garcia da Silva, P., Albeanu, D.F., and Li, B. (2016). Central amygdala
Tye, K.M., and Janak, P.H. (2007). Amygdala neurons differentially encode somatostatin neurons gate passive and active defensive behaviors.
motivation and reinforcement. J. Neurosci. 27, 3937–3945. J. Neurosci. 36, 6488–6496.

1782 Cell Reports 21, 1770–1782, November 14, 2017

Anda mungkin juga menyukai