Anda di halaman 1dari 9

J Appl Physiol 111: 1505–1513, 2011.

First published April 28, 2011; doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00210.2011. Review

HIGHLIGHTED TOPIC Physiology and Pathophysiology of Physical Inactivity

Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span


Michelle W. Voss,1,2 Lindsay S. Nagamatsu,3,4,5,6 Teresa Liu-Ambrose,3,4,5 and Arthur F. Kramer1,2
1
The Beckman Institute and Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana; 2Department
of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois; 3The Brain Research Centre and the
4
Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia; 5The Centre for Hip Health
and Mobility, Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute, Vancouver, British Columbia; and 6Department of Psychology,
University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
Submitted 16 February 2011; accepted in final form 25 April 2011

Voss MW, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T, Kramer AF. Exercise, brain, and
cognition across the life span. J Appl Physiol 111: 1505–1513, 2011. First
published April 28, 2011; doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00210.2011.—This is a brief
review of current evidence for the relationships between physical activity and
exercise and the brain and cognition throughout the life span in non-pathological
populations. We focus on the effects of both aerobic and resistance training and
provide a brief overview of potential neurobiological mechanisms derived from
non-human animal models. Whereas research has focused primarily on the benefits
of aerobic exercise in youth and young adult populations, there is growing evidence
that both aerobic and resistance training are important for maintaining cognitive
and brain health in old age. Finally, in these contexts, we point out gaps in the
literature and future directions that will help advance the field of exercise neuro-
science, including more studies that explicitly examine the effect of exercise type
and intensity on cognition, the brain, and clinically significant outcomes. There is
also a need for human neuroimaging studies to adopt a more unified multi-modal
framework and for greater interaction between human and animal models of
exercise effects on brain and cognition across the life span.
physical activity; aerobic training; strength training; brain function; brain structure;
mental health

IT IS INCREASINGLY PREVALENT in the print media, television, and Indeed, there is an increasing amount of research, much of it
the internet to be bombarded with advertisements for products epidemiological, that argues for numerous long-term health
and programs to enhance mental and physical health in a benefits of regular physical activity and exercise. For example,
relatively painless fashion through miracle elixirs, computer- studies have reported an inverse relationship between physical
based training or gaming programs, or brief exercise programs. activity and the risk of type II diabetes (58), cardiovascular-
Although there is little convincing scientific evidence for many related disease and death (89), osteoporosis (52), colon and
such claims (46), there have been some promising develop- breast cancer (62), and mental disorders (29). Despite this
ments in the scientific literature with regard to physical activity increasing wealth of knowledge concerning the relationship
and exercise effects on cognitive and brain health. In fact, a between physical activity and health we have become an
number of our forefathers appear to have anticipated some of increasingly sedentary society. For example, it has been esti-
the potential benefits of an active lifestyle. For example, mated that less than 50% of children (6 –11 yr) and only 8% of
Thomas Jefferson argued, “a strong body makes the mind adolescents (12–19 yr) are active the recommended 60 min
strong.” Hugh Blair, a Scottish Theologian from the 18th most days of the week, whereas only less than 5% of adults
century suggested that, “Exercise is the chief source of im- (20 –59 yr) and elderly (60⫹ yr) are active the recommended
provement in our faculties.” One of the first noted scientist/ 30 min a day for this age group (99). Furthermore, it has been
physicians, Hippocrates, opinioned, “If we could give every suggested that our current sedentary nature represents a mal-
individual the right amount of nourishment and exercise, we adaptation of our evolutionary history in which high levels of
would have found the safest way to health.” physical activity were required for survival (6).
In the present brief review we focus on the relationship
between physical activity and cognitive and brain health. We
briefly review molecular and cellular exercise-related changes
Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: A. F. Kramer,
Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, Dept. of Psychol-
in our discussion of the animal literature. However, the major-
ogy, Univ. of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 405 N. Mathews Ave, Urbana, IL ity of our review will focus on aerobic and strength training
61801 (e-mail: akramer@cyrus.psych.illinois.edu). effects on human cognition and brain health across the life span
http://www.jap.org 8750-7587/11 Copyright © 2011 the American Physiological Society 1505
Review
1506 EXERCISE EFFECTS ON BRAIN AND COGNITION

in healthy populations. Finally, we conclude with prescriptions and colleagues (47) found that higher-fit children who outper-
for additional research on this important topic. formed lower-fit children on the Eriksen flanker task (a test of
conflict resolution) showed a smaller error-related negativity
EFFECTS OF AEROBIC TRAINING ON BRAIN STRUCTURE, (ERN) on error trials, an ERP component thought to index
BRAIN FUNCTION, AND COGNITION conflict monitoring and error evaluation; higher-fit children
also showed a larger positivity error (Pe) component, thought
As we progress from elementary to high school, our brains to index awareness of one’s mistakes. Overall, higher-fit chil-
rapidly develop structural and functional circuitry that support dren were more accurate in trials following errors of commis-
higher-level cognitive abilities, such as our ability to regulate sion, while showing a smaller ERN and larger Pe, suggesting
and inhibit behavior, multi-task, and resist distraction (13). higher-fit children’s brains monitor conflict more efficiently
Consequently, behaviors that affect brain function play a vital (see also 77). Neuroanatomically, childhood aerobic fitness is
role in facilitating optimal cognitive development during child- associated with more brain volume in structures important for
hood. Physical activity is no exception. Research indicates that demanding information processing and relational memory,
childhood physical inactivity, and subsequently reduced aero- such as the dorsal striatum (16) and the hippocampus (17),
bic fitness, is associated with poorer academic achievement respectively.
(15, 19) and lower performance on standard neuropsycholog- In sum, cross-sectional and longitudinal training studies
ical tests (9, 90). The majority of scientific literature supports support a positive association between aerobic fitness and
a general benefit of aerobic fitness on childhood cognitive enhanced performance in both the classroom and laboratory
performance. For example, a meta-analysis that aggregated (for reviews, see Refs. 90, 97). Furthermore, neuroimaging
results across 44 studies found an overall effect size of 0.32 for evidence supports a beneficial association between childhood
the association between childhood physical activity and fitness aerobic fitness and improved brain function and structure.
and cognition, with significant effects across a range of abili- Following the rapid cognitive development in childhood,
ties, such as perceptual skills (0.49), creativity and concentra- young adulthood (i.e., 18 –35 yr) is characterized by relative
tion (0.40), academic readiness (0.39) and achievement (0.30), stability and peak cognitive performance. This may be one
IQ (0.34), and math (0.20) and verbal (0.17) tests (90). reason that comparatively fewer studies examine cognitive
Still some studies have shown selective benefits of exercise enhancements associated with physical activity and aerobic
on childhood cognition (for review, see Ref. 97). For example, fitness training during this part of the life span. This may also
one study increased task difficulty and thus pushed the limits of be the reason that studies have generally found mixed support
prefrontal function beyond what other studies have done (77). for the association between aerobic fitness and cognition in
This is consistent with a training study that found 3 mo of young adulthood (1, 85, 88). Yet several neuroimaging studies
aerobic exercise training improved prefrontally mediated ex- have shown evidence for increased efficiency of brain function
ecutive function abilities in 7- to 11-year-old overweight chil- without differences in behavioral performance (51, 95, 96). For
dren (27). Other studies demonstrating a selective association example, similar to the findings of Hillman and colleagues (47)
have shown, in agreement with a large animal literature, that with children, one study showed that higher-fit young adults
aerobic fitness is preferentially associated with a type of also had smaller ERNs coupled with larger Pe amplitude,
memory supported by the hippocampus, relational memory, specific to trials with errors of commission (95). Despite these
rather than item memory (17, 18). Unlike item memory, neuroelectric differences, there were no differences in behav-
relational memory requires forming associations, such as re- ioral performance between lower- and higher-fit adults. Studies
membering not only the face of a person you met last week, but like these suggest that aerobic fitness effects on behavior may
also remembering his name, what you talked about, and where only emerge in this high-functioning group when the task is
you met him. Finally, overall there is stronger support for extremely difficult or that young adults have a greater range of
aerobic fitness benefits on accuracy rather than speed of pro- compensatory strategies compared with children and older
cessing (9, 17, 18, 47, 77), which is consistent with the idea adults to achieve enhanced performance. There are very few
that accuracy may be a better measure of cognitive develop- training studies with young adults; however, some studies do
ment than speed (26). Yet the majority of literature on physical support the effectiveness of aerobic training for improving
activity, aerobic fitness, and childhood cognition is cross- cognition at this age (74, 92), particularly for those with a
sectional in nature, therefore more training studies with tem- genetic predisposition for impaired cognition due to lower
porally extended follow-up testing are needed for stronger dopamine levels in the brain (93). Thus overall, whereas
support of these summarized findings. cross-sectional behavioral results are mixed with young adults,
Nevertheless, neuroimaging research provides additional ev- neuroimaging and training results seem to support a positive
idence for both general and selective effects of exercise on association between greater aerobic fitness and brain function.
childhood cognition. Functional brain imaging studies using Future research that examines the effect of task difficulty, uses
event-related potentials (ERPs) have shown that more aerobi- more diverse imaging techniques, and examines the long-term
cally fit children have larger P300s during information pro- effects of aerobic fitness during adulthood will help clarify
cessing (47, 48, 77), an ERP component whose amplitude is many unanswered questions for this age group.
associated with our ability to effectively focus attention and Older adulthood mirrors childhood in some interesting ways,
that is thought to emerge, in part, from the temporoparietal when brain structure and function again enter a period of high
cortex (76). ERP brain recordings also indicate that aerobically interindividual variability and lifestyle factors, such as physical
fit children better regulate and monitor their mistakes, or activity, increasingly impact mental health. Although epidemi-
processing errors, abilities made possible by the anterior cin- ological and prospective studies largely support the role of
gulate and prefrontal cortices (47, 77). For example, Hillman physical activity and aerobic fitness in healthy cognitive (80,

J Appl Physiol • VOL 111 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jap.org


Review
EXERCISE EFFECTS ON BRAIN AND COGNITION 1507
109) and brain (83) aging and preventing the onset of all types creases in the lateral prefrontal anterior cingulate and lateral
of dementia (60, 64), it is less clear from training studies temporal cortices and increased anterior white matter volume.
whether increased aerobic fitness per se is the key ingredient Furthermore, another study found that a 1-yr, three times
for improved brain and cognition from behavior changes as- weekly aerobic training program increased volume of the
sociated with this lifestyle factor (22, 35). Much evidence, anterior hippocampus, which houses the dentate gyrus subre-
however, does support the notion that aerobic exercise benefits gion linked to neurogenesis in animal studies (38, 74, 103);
cognitive performance (22, 32, 57), brain function (10, 23, whereas there were no volumetric benefits for the posterior
108), and brain structure (21, 68) in elderly adults. hippocampus, thalamus, or caudate nucleus (33). Also consis-
Specifically, aerobic training in late life preferentially ben- tent with the animal literature, increased anterior hippocampus
efits executive functions, including brain processes such as volume was associated with increased peripheral brain-derived
multi-tasking, planning, and inhibition, all largely supported by neurotrophic factor (BDNF) for only the aerobic group. The
the prefrontal cortex (22, 23, 57). Several fMRI studies have significance of BDNF will be discussed in more depth below.
examined the effects of aerobic training on brain function. Finally, while this study found memory improvements for the
Colcombe and colleagues (23) examined the effects of a 6-mo, aerobic and nonaerobic control group, increased anterior hip-
three times weekly aerobic training program for sedentary pocampal volume was associated with improved memory for
adults on task-related brain activation during the Eriksen only the aerobic group, trends not shown in the caudate or
flanker task. Aerobically trained older adults had greater in- thalamus. However, change in BDNF was not associated with
creases in brain activity in the frontal and parietal cortices from changes in memory. Thus one caveat of most human studies is
pre- to postintervention, brain areas involved in processes the inability to unequivocally determine the nature of the
important for task performance, such as conflict resolution and cellular and molecular changes that underlie the observed
selective attention. Aerobically trained adults also had greater changes in brain volume and cognitive function. Although one
reduction in anterior cingulate cortex activation, a brain area study indicated that cerebral blood volume, potentially an in
involved in conflict and error monitoring. This pattern of vivo marker of neurogenesis, is at least one factor that may be
activation changes suggests that aerobic training led to in- involved with increased hippocampal volume and correspond-
creased efficiency of conflict and error monitoring and en- ing increases in learning performance (74). Nevertheless, these
hanced regulatory response from the prefrontal cortex follow- studies provide continuing support that beginning an aerobic
ing signals of conflict from the anterior cingulate (8). In line exercise program in late life can still translate into meaningful
with this theoretical account, functional improvements were benefits for the brain and cognition.
coupled with improvement in conflict regulation performance.
This study demonstrated that participating in an aerobic train- EFFECTS OF RESISTANCE/STRENGTH TRAINING ON BRAIN
ing program improves the aging brain’s ability to effectively STRUCTURE, BRAIN FUNCTION, AND COGNITION
engage task-relevant resources, particularly under cognitively
challenging conditions. Thus aerobic training had a selective Although resistance training has a broad range of systemic
rather than general effect on task-related brain function. benefits (7, 61), very few studies to date have specifically
Another way to assess brain function is to examine not how focused on the role of resistance training in promoting cogni-
much the brain activates under controlled cognitive conditions, tive health across the life span. To our knowledge, no studies
but rather how different areas of the brain communicate under have specifically assessed the effect of resistance training on
little to no cognitive demand. The benefit of the latter is an cognitive and brain function in children. A lack of such studies
absence of group differences in behavioral performance or may be partly due to the common misperception that resistance
strategy that could potentially confound differences in task- training is unsafe for children. Likewise, there is a general void
related brain activation. Voss and colleagues (108) examined in the literature regarding the role of chronic resistance training
changes in functional communication, or connectivity, in sed- in promoting cognitive and brain function in young adults.
entary older adults following a 1-yr, three times weekly aerobic For older adults, evidence regarding whether resistance
training program. The study examined whether aerobic training training has cognitive benefit has been equivocal. However, we
would enhance communication in the Default Network (a note that the trials with negative results were limited by small
network whose dysfunction is proposed to be a biomarker for sample sizes (i.e., 13–23 participants per experimental group)
normal cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s Disease), as well as or short intervention periods (i.e., 8 –16 wk) (32, 55, 75, 100).
brain networks involved in higher-level executive functions, For example, Tsutsumi and colleagues (100) demonstrated no
spatial attention, motor control, and audition. Of all the net- cognitive benefit of resistance training in their 12-wk random-
works examined, the Default Network was the primary brain ized controlled trial that compared the effect of high-intensity/
network showing enhanced connectivity following aerobic low-volume resistance training and low-intensity/high-volume
training, in addition to increased connectivity between the left resistance training with no exercise controls on cognitive
and right prefrontal cortices in an Executive Control network. function in 42 community-dwelling older adults (i.e., 14 par-
Thus aerobic fitness training benefits not only task-related ticipants per group). Recently, Kimura and colleagues (55) also
magnitude of brain activation, but also the resting coherence of demonstrated no effect of resistance training on the executive
brain networks important for cognition and neurological dis- process of task switching despite including 119 participants in
ease status. their 12-wk training study. In addition to sufficient duration of
In addition to functional brain changes, studies also support resistance training, the intensity or load of training appears to
significant changes in regional brain volume following aerobic be a key requirement to produce cognitive benefits. For exam-
training. Following a 6-mo, three times weekly aerobic pro- ple, Lachman and colleagues (59) conducted a 6-mo random-
gram, Colcombe and colleagues (21) found gray matter in- ized controlled trial of home-based resistance training among

J Appl Physiol • VOL 111 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jap.org


Review
1508 EXERCISE EFFECTS ON BRAIN AND COGNITION

210 sedentary community-dwelling older adults and found no Thus, while there is less literature across the life span on the
significant between-group differences in memory. The home- effects of resistance training on brain and cognition, compared
based resistance training program was a 35-min videotaped with aerobic training, preliminary evidence highlights its im-
program of 10 exercises using elastic bands. Participants were portance for future study.
instructed to use bands of greater resistance when they could
complete greater than 10 repetitions of an exercise without POTENTIAL MECHANISMS FROM ANIMAL MODELS
significant fatigue. This is a lower intensity protocol compared
with three recent randomized controlled trials with positive Animal models of exercise effects on brain physiology and
findings that used loading protocols ranging from 50 to 80% of structure have indicated several key pathways through which
a single-repetition maximum lift (i.e., 1 RM) (14). It is impor- aerobic and resistance training may enhance brain function.
tant to highlight that while there were no significant between- These pathways include improvement in both the structural
group differences in cognitive performance, Lachman and integrity of the brain (i.e., growth of new neurons and blood
colleagues (59) found that the change in resistance used by vessels) and increased production of neurochemicals that pro-
those in the intervention group was positively associated with mote growth, differentiation, survival, and repair of brain cells.
change in memory performance, after controlling for baseline In addition, animal models have begun to shed light on how
age, education, sex, and disability level. Hence, the investiga- aging interacts with these molecular and cellular models of
tors suggested that resistance training can benefit memory exercise effects on brain and cognition.
among older adults, especially when using higher resistance Many studies now suggest that aerobic training results in
levels. neurogenesis, or the generation of new neurons, in the hip-
pocampus (102, 103), which has been subsequently linked to
Randomized controlled trials of resistance training that are 6
improved hippocampal function (25, 74). To our knowledge
mo or greater in duration and delivered high-loading protocols
hippocampal neurogenesis has not been studied in animal
collectively provide emerging evidence that resistance training
models in the context of other forms of exercise programs (e.g.,
has cognitive benefits. Cassilhas and colleagues (14) demon-
aerobic vs. exercise analogous to resistance training in ro-
strated that 6 mo of either three times weekly moderate or
dents), therefore future research is needed to understand the
high-intensity resistance training improved memory perfor-
specificity of neurogenesis following different forms of exer-
mance and verbal concept formation among 62 community-
cise. Furthermore, although the rate of hippocampal neurogen-
dwelling senior men ages 65 to 75 yr. Moderate intensity was esis declines with normal aging, animal studies generally
defined as 50% of 1 RM and high intensity was defined as 80% support some protection from such decline with aerobic exer-
of 1 RM. Using a protocol similar to Cassilhas (14), recent cise training (54, 104). Age does appear to attenuate the effect
work by Busse and colleagues (11) suggests that resistance of aerobic exercise on neurogenesis compared with young
training may also be beneficial for sedentary older adults at adult animals and for 15- and 19-mo-old animals, respectively
greater risk for Alzheimer’s disease—those with objective mild (54, 104). One study showed no training-induced neurogenesis
memory impairment. in old (22 mo) animals despite some improvement on a spatial
The work of Cassilhas and colleagues (14) also provides pattern separation task and positive evidence for young animals
valuable insight into the possible mechanisms underlying the (25). Other studies have also found that neurogenesis is not
benefit of resistance training on cognitive performance. They necessary for improved performance (e.g., 53) or that neuro-
found serum insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1) levels were genesis is associated with improvement in some tasks (e.g.,
higher in the resistance training groups than in the control spatial memory) and not others (e.g., motor performance,
group. IGF-1 promotes neuronal growth, survival, and differ- conditioning) (20). Therefore, while hippocampal neurogen-
entiation and improves cognitive performance (24), which will esis is a highly replicable effect following aerobic exercise,
be discussed further below. In older adults, resistance training there are still some questions about how it supports behavioral
also reduces serum homocysteine (107). Increased homocys- improvements following aerobic exercise compared with other
teine levels are associated with impaired cognitive perfor- exercise-related factors.
mance (84), Alzheimer’s Disease (87), and cerebral white For example, another consistent effect in animal models is
matter lesions (106), although the cognitive relevance of re- exercise-induced increases in angiogenesis, or the growth of
ductions in homocysteine in longitudinal study remain unclear new blood vessels (25, 74, 104), which has in turn been linked
(e.g., 3). to improved learning and memory (53, 74). Like neurogenesis,
Finally, Liu-Ambrose and colleagues (65) demonstrated that no studies to our knowledge have examined the specificity of
12 mo of either once weekly or twice weekly progressive angiogenesis following different types of exercise. Unlike
resistance training improved selective attention and conflict neurogenesis, aerobic training produces angiogenesis outside
resolution performance among 155 community-dwelling se- of just the hippocampus, including areas directly activated by
nior women aged 65 to 75 yr. Enhanced selective attention and locomotion such as the cerebellum (4, 50) and primary motor
conflict resolution was also associated with increased gait cortex (56, 94). Furthermore, at least two studies have shown
speed. Clinically, improved gait speed predicts a substantial aerobic training-related increases in hippocampal angiogenesis
reduction in both morbidity (82) and mortality (31, 45). These in young but not aged mice (22 and 19 mo, respectively) (25,
results illustrate the clinical significance of cognitive gains 104). Similar to effects described above, the study by Creer and
induced by resistance training. Davis and colleagues (28) also colleages (25) showed that despite no significant hippocampal
provided novel evidence that cognitive benefits associated with angiogenesis (or neurogenesis), aerobically trained old rodents
resistance training are sustained for 1 yr after the intervention still showed moderate improvements in performance. Thus
has ended. while angiogenesis is consistently found in response to aerobic

J Appl Physiol • VOL 111 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jap.org


Review
EXERCISE EFFECTS ON BRAIN AND COGNITION 1509
exercise in young animals, the role of training-induced angio- tein for aged rodents (24 mo), despite positive results for young
genesis in cognitive improvements across the life span is also rodents (2).
a topic in need of future study. A known neurotrophic factor important for both aerobic and
Animal models have also indicated several candidates for resistance training is IGF-1. IGF-1 is produced both in the
circulating neurochemicals that may mediate effects of exer- central nervous system and in the periphery in response to
cise on brain health. Two that have received the most empirical aerobic (12, 30, 98) and resistance (14) exercise. Furthermore,
support include BDNF and IGF-1. BDNF is endogenously studies have shown that blocking entry of peripheral IGF-1 into
produced throughout the brain, with particularly high concen- the brain during aerobic training also blocks exercise-induced
trations in the hippocampus (71), and in animals is known to hippocampal neurogenesis (98), angiogenesis (66), and exer-
increase in the brain during single acute bouts (79) and fol- cise-facilitated brain injury recovery (12). Studies have also
lowing chronic aerobic exercise training (72, 73). While BDNF supported a dependence between IGF-1 and exercise-induced
is also produced in the periphery, some studies have suggested increases in BDNF (12, 30). For example, one study demon-
that peripheral BDNF in humans at rest and during an acute strated that blocking IGF-1 receptors in the hippocampus
bout of aerobic exercise is predominantly brain-derived (esti- during exercise abolished exercise effects on increased hip-
mated from arterial-to-venous difference of radial artery and pocampal BDNF mRNA and protein expression, and similar
the internal jugular vein), although it may dip during recovery effects were found for hippocampal levels of markers of
(79). Also, at least one training study from the same group has synaptic plasticity that are presumed to be end-products of
shown that 3 mo of aerobic training increases human resting BDNF action (exercise-induced increases in synapsin I, p-
peripheral brain-derived BDNF but did not affect peripheral CAMKII, p-MAPKII) (30). In turn, researchers that have
estimates of brain-derived BDNF during aerobic exercise (86). found attenuated exercise-related enhancement of BDNF in
Another study showed that peripheral BDNF in blood serum aged rodents have speculated that this may be partly due to the
was selectively increased following 30 min of high intensity reduction in IGF-1 associated with aging (2, 67).
(⬃85% of heart rate max) compared with lower intensity Thus animal models have shown that BDNF and IGF-1 play
(⬃70% of heart rate max) aerobic exercise (39). Finally, it is important roles in mediating the effects of exercise on brain
interesting to note that there is evidence that resting peripheral health and performance. Other neurotrophic factors and neu-
serum BDNF is selectively upregulated in humans following ropeptides shown to change with exercise behavior include
chronic aerobic (111) but not resistance training (63). Overall, nerve growth factor (70), fibroblast growth factor type 2 (42),
these studies suggest that BDNF release in the human cortex vascular endothelial growth factor (36), and VGF growth
and hippocampus during exercise and at rest can be estimated factor (49) and galanin (101). Furthermore, aerobic exercise
in the periphery from blood plasma or serum and demonstrate also enhances several neurotransmitter systems in the brain,
that examining the effects of acute bouts of exercise on the including increasing circulating dopamine (78), serotonin (5),
brain may help build links to animal models and (although it is and acetylcholine (40). Relatively less research has been done
unknown whether the mechanisms are the same) may inform on the mechanisms by which exercise affects production of
how the brain adapts to cumulative changes in different types these neurochemicals with links to cognition and learning and
of exercise behavior. A noted limitation of these studies, memory. Therefore, the extent to which these neurochemicals
however, is small sample sizes of young adults; thus, it will be also contribute to benefits of exercise across the life span
important for these results to be replicated with larger samples deserves future study.
of a broader age range. In summary, animal models have revealed much about the
Understanding how exercise affects BDNF production is potential neurobiological mechanisms of exercise effects on
important because BDNF is considered a critical factor in brain and cognition across the life span. Aerobic exercise has
exercise-induced benefits on learning and memory. For exam- a concentrated benefit on the hippocampus, increasing the
ple, Farmer and colleagues (38) demonstrated that BDNF is growth of new neurons and new blood vessels, and increasing
associated with aerobic exercise-induced increases in long- synaptic plasticity, which helps facilitate the integration of
term potentiation (LTP), which facilitates synaptic plasticity hippocampal neurons into existing brain networks. Several
and is considered a cellular model of learning and memory. important neurotrophins are integral to the effects of exercise
Furthermore, blocking BDNF receptors during exercise abol- on brain and cognition. While aerobic exercise seems to
ishes downstream effects on metabolic factors (43) and cogni- selectively upregulate central BDNF, both aerobic and resis-
tive performance (43, 44, 105). Although there is overwhelm- tance exercise upregulate central and peripheral IGF-1. Both
ing evidence in support of BDNF as an important factor in are key players in exercise-induced increases in learning and
exercise-induced improvement in brain and cognition, more memory, and IGF-1 may be particularly important in mediating
research is needed that investigates how age interacts with the effects of BDNF and promoting exercise-induced neuro-
these results. For example, Garza and colleagues (41) showed genesis and angiogenesis. However, it should be noted that
that aged rodents (22 mo) had increased hippocampal BDNF animal models often use cognitive measures of learning and
mRNA following short (i.e., 2 days) and chronic (i.e., 20 days) memory that rarely conceptually overlap with the cognitive
bouts of running; however, there were substantial differences paradigms used in human research. Thus the extent to which
in the regional pattern of sensitivity for exercise-induced in- the reviewed potential mechanisms account for benefits of
creases in BDNF, which the authors suggested may reflect exercise on executive function in humans is a topic that
age-related shifts in hippocampal physiology that in turn im- deserves future consideration. In addition, future research is
pact how exercise affects hippocampal structure and function needed to clarify the role of these molecular and cellular
in old age. Another study found that chronic aerobic exercise pathways in models of aging and exercise. Research has
was not associated with increases in hippocampal BDNF pro- generally supported the idea that aerobic exercise attenuates

J Appl Physiol • VOL 111 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jap.org


Review
1510 EXERCISE EFFECTS ON BRAIN AND COGNITION

age-related declines in neurogenesis and learning and memory, reports of effects of chronic exercise on fMRI activation, this
but it is still unclear how much overlap there is between the is an important gap to fill. Other cross-sectional research has
exercise model for young and aged animals. used DTI to show preliminary evidence for an association
between aerobic fitness and white matter integrity in the
CONCLUDING COMMENTS, UNRESOLVED ISSUES, uncinate fasciculus (connecting ventral frontal and temporal
AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS lobes) and cingulum bundle (transversing the midline between
anterior and posterior areas) (68, 69). Finally, near-infrared
The reviewed literature provides an overview of the effects spectroscopy is another technology that could be applied dur-
of exercise on brain and cognition throughout the life span. ing exercise, making it another useful tool to compare effects
Whereas research has focused primarily on the benefits of of acute and chronic exercise on brain and cognition (81, 110).
aerobic exercise in youth and young adult populations, there is Overall, while these studies generally have relatively small
growing evidence that both aerobic and resistance training are sample sizes and have examined targeted age groups, they
important for maintaining cognitive and brain health in old age. represent a starting point for future studies to expand on in both
Our review also points out gaps in the literature and important sample size and diversity, and in longitudinal designs.
future directions for the field. Finally, it will be important for future research to try to
Clearly there is a need for more research that investigates the integrate animal and human work. For example, more animal
effect of exercise type on the brain and cognition, and in turn, studies that incorporate a life span perspective could help
clinically significant outcomes such as quality of life, memory characterize potential similarities and differences of neurobio-
complaints, mobility, falls risk, and mortality. While standard logical mechanisms for exercise effects on brain and cognition
neuropsychological tests and brain imaging data provide valu- at different points in the developmental timeline. On the other
able data for characterizing the specific brain systems affected hand, it will be important for more human studies to incorpo-
by exercise behavior, the translation of these results into rate measures of neurobiological markers such as peripheral
clinically relevant outcomes is also important for building a measures of BDNF and IGF-1 both during acute exercise and
more interactive relationship between basic research findings following chronic exercise exposure. More work of this nature
and clinical practice. Also important for translating research will help build an understanding of how acute effects are
into practice will be a greater understanding of how individual similar to or different from chronic benefits on brain and
differences mediate or moderate the effects of different exer- cognition across the life span, ultimately furthering our under-
cise types on brain and cognition. For instance, it is possible standing of the mechanisms for how different types of cumu-
that aerobic training will be more effective for some individ- lated exercise behaviors affect the brain and cognition. In this
uals whereas resistance training would benefit others more, and spirit, future studies with humans may also gain valuable
their combination may be best for yet another group. Some insight from animal models about optimal methods for com-
factors important to examine may include (but not be limited bining interventions based on different exercise types and/or
to) genetics, personality, and personal health history or disease exercise and cognitive training programs or diets. For example,
status. Greater understanding of the role of these factors in the a study by Fabel and colleagues (37) supports the idea that the
effects of exercise training on brain and cognitive health may functional significance of hippocampal neurogenesis resulting
also help clarify the relative importance of aerobic fitness gains from aerobic exercise is further promoted if followed by
compared with engaging in physical activity without focus on environmental enrichment or cognitive training. This suggests
fitness gains per se. These issues have important practical that while aerobic exercise would be a good starting point for
significance for affecting public health recommendations for intervention programs, beneficial effects for brain and cogni-
physical activity behavior to improve brain and cognition. tion may be further enhanced if followed by the addition of
Within the realm of brain imaging, when comparing the other activity types, such as resistance training, cognitive
effects of different exercise types, it will be important for training, or some combination thereof.
future research to incorporate more of a multi-modal frame- Thus, while all exercise might not be painless or provide the
work. Currently our knowledge of exercise effects in childhood “easy fix” to enhance brain and cognition across the life span,
and young adulthood is based primarily from neuroelectric there is ample evidence to support it as one of the most
methods, whereas training studies with older adults have used effective means available to improve mental and physical
primarily hemodynamic measures of brain function. Therefore, health, without the side effects of many pharmacological treat-
it will be important for future research to incorporate multiple ments. In this review we highlighted some of the best evidence
methods of measuring brain function across the life span. in support of exercise benefits for nonpathological populations,
Along these lines, additional methods that will be important for but also pointed out important areas for future research to
future research include measures of blood volume and blood advance the field. It is our hope that this will encourage greater
flow such as arterial spin labeling (ASL) and measures of white diversity of approaches and methodologies, applied to a larger
matter microstructure, including diffusion tensor imaging diversity of populations, in the field of exercise neuroscience.
(DTI). For example, a recent study demonstrated the feasibility Such advancements promise to improve translation of positive
of measuring resting and task-related cerebral blood flow with findings in the research laboratory to improved quality of life
ASL following an acute bout of exercise, which will be from childhood to late life.
important for studies examining effects of acute exercise on
fMRI activation (91). In addition, we know of only one study GRANTS
(10) that has used ASL to examine effects of chronic exercise Preparation for this manuscript was supported by funding from the National
exposure, which suggested increased hippocampal blood flow Institute on Aging at the National Institutes of Health (Grant Numbers
following an aerobic exercise program. Given the increasing 05-R370-AG025667, RO1-AG25032) for private investigator A. Kramer. T.

J Appl Physiol • VOL 111 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jap.org


Review
EXERCISE EFFECTS ON BRAIN AND COGNITION 1511
Liu-Ambrose is a Canadian Institute Health Research New Investigator, and demic achievement? Positive results from public school children in the
Michael Smith is a Foundation for Health Research Scholar. northeastern. US J Sch Health 79: 30 –37, 2009.
20. Clark PJ, Brzezinska WJ, Thomas MW, Ryzhenko NA, Toshkov SA,
Rhodes JS. Intact neurogenesis is required for benefits of exercise on
DISCLOSURES
spatial memory but not motor performance or contextual fear condition-
No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors. ing in C57BL/6J mice. Neuroscience 155: 1048 –1058, 2008.
21. Colcombe S, Erickson K, Scalf P, Kim J, Prakash R, Mcauley E,
Elavsky S, Marquez D, Hu L, Kramer A. Aerobic exercise training
REFERENCES
increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Med Sci 61:
1. Aberg MAI, Pedersen NL, Torén K, Svartengren M, Bäckstrand B, 1166, 2006.
Johnsson T, Cooper-Kuhn CM, Aberg ND, Nilsson M, Kuhn HG. 22. Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of
Cardiovascular fitness is associated with cognition in young adulthood. older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci 14: 125–130, 2003.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 20906 –20911, 2009. 23. Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen
2. Adlard PA, Perreau VM, Cotman CW. The exercise-induced expres- NJ, Webb A, Jerome GJ, Marquez DX, Elavsky S. Cardiovascular
sion of BDNF within the hippocampus varies across life-span. Neurobiol fitness, cortical plasticity, aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 3316 –
Aging 26: 511–520, 2005. 3321, 2004.
3. Aisen PS, Schneider LS, Sano M, Diaz-Arrastia R, van Dyck CH, 24. Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie LA. Exercise builds brain health:
Weiner MF, Bottiglieri T, Jin S, Stokes KT, Thomas RG, Thal LJ, key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends Neurosci
Study ADC. High-dose B vitamin supplementation and cognitive decline 30: 464 –472, 2007.
in Alzheimer disease: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 300: 1774 – 25. Creer DJ, Romberg C, Saksida LM, van Praag H, Bussey TJ.
1783, 2008. Running enhances spatial pattern separation in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci
4. Black JE, Isaacs KR, Anderson BJ, Alcantara AA, Greenough WT. USA 107: 2367–2372, 2010.
Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angio- 26. Davidson MC, Amso D, Anderson LC, Diamond A. Development of
genesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 87: cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence
5568 –5572, 1990. from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuro-
5. Blomstrand E, Perret D, Parry-Billings M, Newsholme EA. Effect of psychologia 44: 2037–2078, 2006.
sustained exercise on plasma amino acid concentrations on 5-hydroxy- 27. Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, McDowell JE, Austin BP, Miller PH,
tryptamine metabolism in six different brain regions in the rat. Acta Yanasak NE, Allison JD, Naglieri JA. Exercise improves executive
Physiol Scand 136: 473–481, 1989. function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight
6. Booth FW, Laye MJ. The future: genes, physical activity and health. children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol 30: 91–98, 2011.
Acta Physiol (Oxf) 199: 549 –556, 2010. 28. Davis JC, Marra CA, Beattie BL, Robertson MC, Najafzadeh M,
7. Borst SE. Interventions for sarcopenia and muscle weakness in older Graf P, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T. Sustained cognitive and
people. Age Ageing 33: 548 –555, 2004. economic benefits of resistance training among community-dwelling
8. Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. senior women: a 1-year follow-up study of the Brain Power study. Arch
Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108: 624 –652, Intern Med 170: 2036 –2038, 2010.
2001. 29. DHHS. Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee Report. Wash-
9. Buck SM, Hillman CH, Castelli DM. The relation of aerobic fitness to ington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services, edited by
stroop task performance in preadolescent children. Med Sci Sports Services DoHaH, 2008.
Exercise 40: 166 –172, 2008. 30. Ding Q, Vaynman S, Akhavan M, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F. Insulin-
10. Burdette JH, Laurienti PJ, Espeland MA, Morgan A, Telesford Q, like growth factor I interfaces with brain-derived neurotrophic factor-
Vechlekar CD, Hayasaka S, Jennings JM, Katula JA, Kraft RA, mediated synaptic plasticity to modulate aspects of exercise-induced
Rejeski WJ. Using network science to evaluate exercise-associated brain cognitive function Neuroscience 140: 823–833, 2006.
changes in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2: 23, 2010. 31. Dumurgier J, Elbaz A, Ducimetière P, Tavernier B, Alpérovitch A,
11. Busse AL, Filho WJ, Magaldi RM, Coelho VA, Melo AC, Betoni RA, Tzourio C. Slow walking speed and cardiovascular death in well func-
Santarem JM. Effects of resistance training exercise on cognitive tioning older adults: prospective cohort study. BMJ 339: b4460, 2009.
performance in elderly individuals with memory impairment: results of a
32. Dustman R, Ruhling R, Russell E, Shearer DE, Bonekat HW, Shi-
controlled trial. Einstein 6: 402–407, 2008.
geoka JW, Wood JS, Bradford DC. Aerobic exercise training and
12. Carro E, Trejo JL, Busiguina S, Torres-Aleman I. Circulating insulin-
improved neuropsychological function of older individuals. Neurobiol
like growth factor I mediates the protective effects of physical exercise
Aging 5: 35–42, 1984.
against brain insults of different etiology and anatomy. J Neurosci 21:
33. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Bsak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L,
5678 –5684, 2001.
Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM, Wojcicki TR, Mailey E, Vieira
13. Casey BJ, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S. Imaging the developing
brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends VJ, Martin SA, Pence BD, Woods JA, McAuley E, Kramer AF.
Cognitive Sci 9: 104 –110, 2005. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory.
14. Cassilhas RC, Viana VAR, Grassmann V, Santos RT, Santos RF, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108: 3017–3022, 2011.
Tufik S, Mello MT. The impact of resistance exercise on the cognitive 35. Etnier J, Nowell P, Landers D, Sibley B. A meta-regression to examine
function of the elderly. Med Sci Sports Exercise 39: 1401–1407, 2007. the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance.
15. Castelli DM, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Erwin HE. Physical fitness and Brain Res Rev 52: 119 –130, 2006.
academic achievement in third- and fifth-grade students. J Sport Exercise 36. Fabel K, Fabel K, Tam B, Kaufer D, Baiker A, Simmons N, Kuo CJ,
Psychol 29: 239 –252, 2007. Palmer TD. VEGF is necessary for exercise-induced adult hippocampal
16. Chaddock L, Erickson K, Prakash R, Vanpatter M, Voss M, Pontifex neurogenesis. Eur J Neurosci 18: 2803–2812, 2003.
M, Raine L, Hillman C, Kramer A. Basal ganglia volume is associated 37. Fabel K, Wolf SA, Ehninger D, Babu H, Leal-Galicia P, Kemper-
with aerobic fitness in preadolescent children. Dev Neurosci 32: 249 – mann G. Additive effects of physical exercise and environmental en-
256, 2010. richment on adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice. Front Neurosci 10:
17. Chaddock L, Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Kim JS, Voss MW, Vanpat- 3: 50, 2009.
ter M, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Konkel A, Hillman CH, Cohen NJ, 38. Farmer J, Zhao X, Van Praag H, Wodtke K, Gage FH, Christie BR.
Kramer AF. A neuroimaging investigation of the association between Effects of voluntary exercise on synaptic plasticity and gene expression
aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume, and memory performance in pre- in the dentate gyrus of adult male Sprague-Dawley rats in vivo. Neuro-
adolescent children. Brain Res 1358: 172–183, 2010. science 124: 71–79, 2004.
18. Chaddock L, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Cohen NJ. Aerobic fitness and 39. Ferris LT, Williams JS, Shen CL. The effect of acute exercise on serum
executive control of relational memory in preadolescent children. Med brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Med Sci
Sci Sports Exerc 43: 344 –329, 2010. Sports Exerc 39: 728 –734, 2007.
19. Chomitz VR, Slining MM, McGowan RJ, Mitchell SE, Dawson GF, 40. Fordyce DE, Farrar RP. Enhancement of spatial learning in F344 rats
Hacker KA. Is there a relationship between physical fitness and aca- by physical activity and related learning-associated alterations in hip-

J Appl Physiol • VOL 111 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jap.org


Review
1512 EXERCISE EFFECTS ON BRAIN AND COGNITION

pocampal and cortical cholinergic functioning. Behav Brain Res 46: 62. Lee IM. Physical activity and cancer prevention– data from epidemio-
123–133, 1991. logic studies. Med Sci Sports Exerc 35: 1823–1827, 2003.
41. Garza AA, Ha TG, Garcia C, Chen MJ, Russo-Neustadt AA. Exer- 63. Levinger I, Goodman C, Matthews V, Hare DL, Jerums G, Garnham
cise, antidepressant treatment, and BDNF mRNA expression in the aging A, Selig S. BDNF, metabolic risk factors, and resistance training in
brain. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 77: 209 –220, 2004. middle-aged individuals. Med Sci Sports Exerc 40: 535–541, 2008.
42. Gómez-Pinilla F, Dao L, So V. Physical exercise induces FGF-2 and its 64. Lindsay J, Laurin D, Verreault R, Hébert R, Helliwell B, Hill GB,
mRNA in the hippocampus. Brain Res 764: 1–8, 1997. McDowell I. Risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease: a prospective analysis
43. Gomez-Pinilla F, Vaynman S, Ying Z. Brain-derived neurotrophic from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Am J Epidemiol 156:
factor functions as a metabotrophin to mediate the effects of exercise on 445–453, 2002.
cognition. Eur J Neurosci 28: 2278 –2287, 2008. 65. Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC,
44. Griesbach GS, Hovda DA, Gomez-Pinilla F. Exercise-induced im- Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month
provement in cognitive performance after traumatic brain injury in rats is randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med 170: 170 –178, 2010.
dependent on BDNF activation. Brain Res 1288: 105–115, 2009. 66. Lopez-Lopez C, LeRoith D, Torres-Aleman I. Insulin-like growth
45. Hardy SE, Perera S, Roumani YF, Chandler JM, Studenski SA. factor I is required for vessel remodeling in the adult brain. Proc Natl
Improvement in usual gait speed predicts better survival in older adults. Acad Sci USA 101: 9833–9838, 2004.
J Am Geriatr Soc 55: 1727–1734, 2007. 67. Markowska AL, Mooney M, Sonntag WE. Insulin-like growth factor-1
46. Hertzog C, Kramer AF, Wilson RS, Lindenberger U. Enrichment ameliorates age-related behavioral deficits. Neuroscience 87: 559 –569,
effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of 1998.
older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychol Sci Public Interest 9: 68. Marks B, Katz L, Styner M, Smith J. Aerobic fitness and obesity:
1–65, 2009. relationship to cerebral white matter integrity in the brain of active and
47. Hillman CH, Buck SM, Themanson JR, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM. sedentary older adults. Br J Sports Med 2010 Jun 17 [Epub ahead of
Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: Event-related brain potential print].
and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent 69. Marks BL, Madden DJ, Bucur B, Provenzale JM, White LE, Cabeza
children. Dev Psychol 45: 114 –129, 2009. R, Huettel SA. Role of aerobic fitness and aging on cerebral white matter
48. Hillman CH, Castelli DM, Buck SM. Aerobic fitness and neurocogni- integrity. Ann NY Acad Sci 1097: 171–174, 2007.
tive function in healthy preadolescent children. Med Sci Sports Exerc 37: 70. Molteni R, Ying Z, Gómez-Pinilla F. Differential effects of acute and
1967–1974, 2005. chronic exercise on plasticity-related genes in the rat hippocampus
49. Hunsberger JG, Newton SS, Bennett AH, Duman CH, Russell DS, revealed by microarray. Eur J Neurosci 16: 1107–1116, 2002.
Salton SR, Duman RS. Antidepressant actions of the exercise-regulated 71. Murer MG, Yan Q, Raisman-Vozari R. Brain-derived neurotrophic
gene VGF. Nat Med 13: 1476 –1482, 2007. factor in the control human brain and in Alzheimer’s disease and
50. Isaacs KR, Anderson BJ, Alcantara AA, Black JE, Greenough WT. Parkinson’s disease. Prog Neurobiol 63: 71–124, 2001.
Exercise and the brain: angiogenesis in the adult rat cerebellum after 72. Neeper SA, Gómez-Pinilla F, Choi J, Cotman C. Exercise and brain
vigorous physical activity and motor skill learning. J Cereb Blood Flow neurotrophins. Nature 373: 109, 1995.
Metab 12: 110 –119, 1992. 73. Neeper SA, Gómez-Pinilla F, Choi J, Cotman CW. Physical activity
51. Kamijo K, O’Leary KC, Pontifex MB, Themanson JR, Hillman CH. increases mRNA for brain-derived neurotrophic factor and nerve growth
The relation of aerobic fitness to neuroelectric indices of cognitive and factor in rat brain. Brain Res 726: 49 –56, 1996.
motor task preparation. Psychophysiology 47: 814 –821, 2010. 74. Pereira AC, Huddleston DE, Brickman AM, Sosunov AA, Hen R,
52. Kelley GA. Exercise and regional bone mineral density in postmeno- McKhann GM, Sloan R, Gage FH, Brown TR, Small SA. An in vivo
pausal women: a meta-analytic review of randomized trials. Am J Phys correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus.
Med Rehabil 77: 76 –87, 1998. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 5638 –5643, 2007.
53. Kerr AL, Steuer EL, Pochtarev V, Swain RA. Angiogenesis but not 75. Perrig-Chiello P, Perrig WJ, Ehrsam R, Staehelin HB, Krings F. The
neurogenesis is critical for normal learning and memory acquisition. effects of resistance training on well-being and memory in elderly
Neuroscience 171: 214 –226, 2010. volunteers. Age Ageing 27: 469 –475, 1998.
54. Kim YP, Kim H, Shin MS, Chang HK, Jang MH, Shin MC, Lee SJ, 76. Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin
Lee HH, Yoon JH, Jeong IG, Kim CJ. Age-dependence of the effect of Neurophysiol 118: 2128 –2148, 2007.
treadmill exercise on cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus of rats. 77. Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Johnson CR, Chaddock L, Voss MW, Cohen
Neurosci Lett 355: 152–154, 2004. NJ, Kramer AF, Hillman CH. Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible
55. Kimura K, Obuchi S, Arai T, Nagasawa H, Shiba Y, Watanabe S, modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. J Cogn Neu-
Kojima M. The influence of short-term strength training on health- rosci 23: 1332–1345, 2011.
related quality of life and executive cognitive function. J Physiol An- 78. Poulton NP, Muir GD. Treadmill training ameliorates dopamine loss
thropol 29: 95–101, 2010. but not behavioral deficits in hemi-parkinsonian rats. Exp Neurol 193:
56. Kleim JA, Cooper NR, VandenBerg PM. Exercise induces angiogen- 181–197, 2005.
esis but does not alter movement representations within rat motor cortex. 79. Rasmussen P, Brassard P, Adser H, Pedersen MV, Leick L, Hart E,
Brain Res 934: 1–6, 2002. Secher NH, Pedersen BK, Pilegaard H. Evidence for a release of
57. Kramer AF, Hahn S, Cohen NJ, Banich MT, McAuley E, Harrison brain-derived neurotrophic factor from the brain during exercise. Exp
CR, Chason J, Vakil E, Bardell L, Boileau RA, Colcombe A. Ageing, Physiol 94: 1062–1069, 2009.
fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature 400: 418 –419, 1999. 80. Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth MEJ. Does active leisure protect
58. Laaksonen DE, Lindström J, Lakka TA, Eriksson JG, Niskanen L, cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Soc Sci Med 56:
Wikström K, Aunola S, Keinänen-Kiukaanniemi S, Laakso M, Valle 785–792, 2003.
TT, Ilanne-Parikka P, Louheranta A, Hämäläinen H, Rastas M, 81. Rooks CR, Thom NJ, McCully KK, Dishman RK. Effects of incre-
Salminen V, Cepaitis Z, Hakumäki M, Kaikkonen H, Härkönen P, mental exercise on cerebral oxygenation measured by near-infrared
Sundvall J, Tuomilehto J, Uusitupa M; and study Fdp. Physical spectroscopy: a systematic review. Prog Neurobiol 92: 134 –150, 2010.
activity in the prevention of type 2 diabetes: the Finnish diabetes 82. Rosano C, Newman AB, Katz R, Hirsch CH, Kuller LH. Association
prevention study. Diabetes 54: 158 –165, 2005. between lower digit symbol substitution test score and slower gait and
59. Lachman ME, Neupert SD, Bertrand R, Jette AM. The effects of greater risk of mortality and of developing incident disability in well-
strength training on memory in older adults. J Aging Phys Act 14: 59 –73, functioning older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 56: 1618 –1625, 2008.
2006. 83. Rovio S, Spulber G, Nieminen LJ, Niskanen E, Winblad B,
60. Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, Teri L, Crane P, Tuomilehto J, Nissinen A, Soininen H, Kivipelto M. The effect of
Kukull W. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia midlife physical activity on structural brain changes in the elderly.
among persons 65 years of age and older. Ann Intern Med 144: 73–81, Neurobiol Aging 31: 1927–1936, 2010.
2006. 84. Schafer JH, Glass TA, Bolla KI, Mintz M, Jedlicka AE, Schwartz BS.
61. Layne JE, Nelson ME. The effects of progressive resistance training on Homocysteine and cognitive function in a population-based study of
bone density: a review. Med Sci Sports Exerc 31: 25–30, 1999. older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 53: 381–388, 2005.

J Appl Physiol • VOL 111 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jap.org


Review
EXERCISE EFFECTS ON BRAIN AND COGNITION 1513
85. Scisco JL, Leynes PA, Kang J. Cardiovascular fitness and executive 99. Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Mâsse LC, Tilert T, McDowell
control during task-switching: an ERP study. Int J Psychophysiol 69: M. Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer.
52–60, 2008. Med Sci Sports Exerc 40: 181–188, 2008.
86. Seifert T, Brassard P, Wissenberg M, Rasmussen P, Nordby P, 100. Tsutsumi T, Don BM, Zaichkowsky LD, Delizonna LL. Physical
Stallknecht B, Adser H, Jakobsen AH, Pilegaard H, Nielsen HB, fitness and psychological benefits of strength training in community
Secher NH. Endurance training enhances BDNF release from the human dwelling older adults. Appl Human Sci 16: 257–266, 1997.
brain. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 298: R372–R377, 2010. 101. Van Hoomissen JD, Holmes PV, Zellner AS, Poudevigne A, Dishman
87. Seshadri S, Beiser A, Selhub J, Jacques PF, Rosenberg IH, RK. Effects of beta-adrenoreceptor blockade during chronic exercise on
D’Agostino RB, Wilson PWF, Wolf PA. Plasma homocysteine as a risk contextual fear conditioning and mRNA for galanin and brain-derived
factor for dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 346: 476 – neurotrophic factor. Behav Neurosci 118: 1378 –1390, 2004.
483, 2002. 102. Van Praag H, Christie BR, Sejnowski TJ, Gage FH. Running en-
88. Shay KA, Roth DL. Association between aerobic fitness and visuospa- hances neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proc
tial performance in healthy older adults. Psychol Aging 7: 15–24, 1992. Natl Acad Sci USA 96: 13427–13431, 1999.
89. Shiroma EJ, Lee IM. Physical activity and cardiovascular health: 103. Van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage F. Running increases cell
lessons learned from epidemiological studies across age, gender, and proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat
race/ethnicity. Circulation 122: 743–752, 2010. Neurosci 2: 266 –270, 1999.
90. Sibley B, Etnier J. The relationship between physical activity and 104. Van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C, Gage FH. Exercise enhances
cognition in children: A meta-analysis. Pediatr Exerc Sci 15: 243–256, learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J Neurosci 25:
2003. 8680 –8685, 2005.
91. Smith JC, Paulson ES, Cook DB, Verber MD, Tian Q. Detecting 105. Vaynman S, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F. Hippocampal BDNF mediates
changes in human cerebral blood flow after acute exercise using arterial the efficacy of exercise on synaptic plasticity and cognition. Eur J
spin labeling: implications for fMRI. J Neurosci Methods 191: 258 –262, Neurosci 20: 2580 –2590, 2004.
2010. 106. Vermeer SE, Van Dijk EJ, Koudstaal PJ, Oudkerk M, Hofman A,
92. Stroth S, Hille K, Spitzer M, Reinhardt R. Aerobic endurance exercise Clarke R, Breteler MMB. Homocysteine, silent brain infarcts, and
benefits memory and affect in young adults. Neuropsychol Rehabil 19: white matter lesions: The Rotterdam Scan Study. Ann Neurol 51: 285–
223–243, 2009. 289, 2002.
93. Stroth S, Reinhardt RK, Thöne J, Hille K, Schneider M, Härtel S, 107. Vincent KR, Braith RW, Bottiglieri T, Vincent HK, Lowenthal DT.
Weidemann W, Bös K, Spitzer M. Impact of aerobic exercise training Homocysteine and lipoprotein levels following resistance training in
on cognitive functions and affect associated to the COMT polymorphism older adults. Prev Cardiol 6: 197–203, 2003.
in young adults. Neurobiol Learning Memory 94: 364 –372, 2010. 108. Voss MW, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Basak C, Chaddock L, Kim JS,
94. Swain RA, Harris AB, Wiener EC, Dutka MV, Morris HD, Theien Alves H, Heo S, Szabo A, White SM, Wojcicki TR, Mailey EL, Gothe
BE, Konda S, Engberg K, Lauterbur PC, Greenough WT. Prolonged N, Olson EA, McAuley E, Kramer AF. Plasticity of brain networks in
exercise induces angiogenesis and increases cerebral blood volume in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Front
primary motor cortex of the rat. Neuroscience 117: 1037–1046, 2003. Aging Neurosci 2: 1–17, 2010.
95. Themanson JR, Hillman CH. Cardiorespiratory fitness and acute aer- 109. Yaffe K, Fiocco AJ, Lindquist K, Vittinghoff E, Simonsick EM,
obic exercise effects on neuroelectric and behavioral measures of action Newman AB, Satterfield S, Rosano C, Rubin SM, Ayonayon HN,
monitoring. Neuroscience 141: 757–767, 2006. Harris TB, Study HA. Predictors of maintaining cognitive function in
96. Themanson JR, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH. Fitness and action moni- older adults: the Health ABC study. Neurology 72: 2029 –2035, 2009.
toring: evidence for improved cognitive flexibility in young adults. 110. Yanagisawa H, Dan I, Tsuzuki D, Kato M, Okamoto M, Kyutoku Y,
Neuroscience 157: 319 –328, 2008. Soya H. Acute moderate exercise elicits increased dorsolateral prefrontal
97. Tomporowski PD, Davis CL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Exercise and activation and improves cognitive performance with Stroop test. Neuro-
children’s intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Ed Psy- Image 50: 1702–1710, 2010.
chol Rev 20: 111–131, 2008. 111. Zoladz JA, Pilc A, Majerczak J, Grandys M, Zapart-Bukowska J,
98. Trejo JL, Carro E, Torres-Aleman I. Circulating insulin-like growth Duda K. Endurance training increases plasma brain-derived neu-
factor I mediates exercise-induced increases in the number of new rotrophic factor concentration in young healthy men. J Physiol Pharma-
neurons in the adult hippocampus. J Neurosci 21: 1628 –1634, 2001. col 59, Suppl 7: 119 –132, 2008.

J Appl Physiol • VOL 111 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jap.org

Anda mungkin juga menyukai