Anda di halaman 1dari 26

CHAPTER 2 5

Reasoning and Thinking in


Nonhuman Primates

Josep Call
Michael Tomasello

Fifty years ago, a chapter with the title adaptations may be considered cognitive in
“Reasoning and Thinking in Nonhuman Pri- the sense that they involve the individual or-
mates” would have been a very short chapter. ganism’s learning and reasoning and thinking
Behaviorists, of course, did not believe in rea- on the basis of its own individual experience
soning and thinking, and people who studied before deciding on the best way to act in
animals in their natural habitats (eventu- a given circumstance. There are specifiable
ally known as ethologists) were interested ecological circumstances in which evolution
in other things. In the 1 960s, the cognitive favors the greater flexibility afforded by cog-
revolution transformed the way psycholo- nitive adaptations, as opposed to, for exam-
gists studied human behavior and cognition, ple, hardwiring specific behavioral responses
but much of this research was about hu- to specific environmental stimuli (Boyd &
man symbolic, propositional representations Richerson, 1 985 ).
(“the language of thought”) and was not eas- In the case of nonhuman primates in par-
ily applied to research with nonhuman an- ticular, there were actually two pioneers in
imals. The cognitive revolution thus came cognitive research in the early part of the
to the study of animal behavior only very twentieth century. In Germany, Wolfgang
slowly. But during the past two decades, Köhler was a Gestalt psychologist inter-
it has arrived, and in the modern study of ested in intelligence as something that took
animal behavior, questions of cognition are organisms beyond punctate sensations and
among the most prominent. blind trial-and-error learning. He studied a
Scientists who study animals typically small group of chimpanzees in a variety
have a background in biology, so every- of problem-solving situations, looking for
thing flows from the theory of evolution. cases of perceptual restructuring and insight
These behavioral biologists and psychobiol- (Köhler, 1 925 ). In America, Robert Yerkes
ogists are interested in how animals adapt to studied a variety of behavioral phenomena
their environments – both physically and be- in a number of primate species. His work in-
haviorally. In this context, some behavioral cluded studies in which animals had to solve
607
608 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

complex cognitive problems. In the mid- of research. Here we explore two aspects:
dle part of the century, behaviorists studied (1 ) how individuals navigate in large-scale lo-
such things as the speed with which differ- comotor space while traveling and (2) how
ent species could be taught through reward- individuals search for objects more locally
based training to make discriminations, form in small-scale manipulatory space. In both
learning sets, and the like (Harlow, 1 95 9; cases, the key skills involved in thinking and
Rumbaugh, 1 970) – phenomena which, to- reasoning enable an individual to predict
day, could be given interesting cognitive in- things – namely, the best path for its own
terpretations. locomotion or the likely future position of
The most exciting work in the modern moving objects.
context comes under the two titles “com-
parative cognition” and “cognitive ethology,” Travel Strategies
however. The former often refers to exper-
detours
imental work in the laboratory, and the lat-
ter often refers to observational work in the The use of detours was one of the main
natural environment. Ideally, for any given issues investigated by Köhler (1 925 ) with
phenomenon, the two approaches provide chimpanzees. He found that they were ca-
complementary types of information. pable of taking alternative routes to a goal
Our aim in this chapter is to provide an when the direct route was blocked. Since
up-to-date overview of research on think- then, little additional research has been done
ing and reasoning in nonhuman primates except in other animals species such as
(henceforth, simply “primates”). Thinking chickens or dogs. Recently, however, several
and reasoning, in our view, are character- researchers used computerized systems to
ized by mental transformations or leaps, not present mazes. Here, the subject does not
just direct perception or memory of partic- move, but it moves a cursor through the
ular stimuli; going “beyond the information maze to get to a goal box. This is a good
given.” We therefore focus on primates solv- tool to investigate detours because mazes
ing novel problems – that is, those that re- often involve the use of detours in which
quire them to do more than simply learn subjects have to move away from the direct
and remember. In terms of content, we focus approach to the goal box and use an indi-
on topics that constitute aspects of human rect route to reach it. Iversen and Matsuzawa
cognition represented by other chapters in (2001 ) trained two chimpanzees to navigate
the current volume, focusing in each case on through mazes presented on a computer
both selected classic studies and the latest re- touch screen. Chimpanzees gradually mas-
search findings. Our main topics are spatial tered a series of mazes of increasing diffi-
(Tversky, Chap. 1 0), relational, analogical culty. One of the chimpanzees learned to use
(Holyoak, Chap. 6), inferential, quantita- detours when the route on a familiar maze
tive (Gallistel & Gelman, Chap. 23 ), causal was blocked and later was able to use detours
(Buehner & Cheng, Chap. 7), and social rea- on novel mazes. The authors indicated, how-
soning and thinking. Although the chapter is ever, that subjects did not fully develop a
mainly about primates, readers interested in generalized ability to solve mazes, and some
fuller accounts of animal cognition in general practice with the particular mazes seemed
are referred to books published in the past to be required to solve the problem.
few years (Pepperberg, 1 999; Roberts, 1 998;
Shettleworth, 1 998; Tomasello & Call, 1 997; shortcuts
Vauclair, 1 996). Fieldworkers often report that several
species of primates travel from certain lo-
cations to others in an efficient manner –
Spatial Reasoning that is, taking the shortest routes possi-
ble (Garber, 1 989; Sigg, 1 986). Menzel
The spatial behavior and cognition of pri- (1 973 ) tested the ability of four young
mates and other animals is a very large field captive chimpanzees to use least-distance
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 609

strategies, traveling the least while obtain- dergone different invisible displacements. In
ing the most food rewards in a large enclo- object permanence tasks, the experimenter
sure. He found that chimpanzees minimized places a piece of food under a small container
the distance traveled. Similarly, Boesch and that is displaced under several other contain-
Boesch (1 984) found that wild chimpanzees ers and the food is left under one of them. To
traveled efficiently when collecting stones solve this problem effectively, subjects have
needed to crack open nuts. They selected to search under all and only boxes under
stones that were closer to their current loca- which the food might have been deposited
tion. Recently, a number of researchers have given the trajectory of the box that initially
described the use of least-distance strategies contained the food. Several apes pass this
in vervet monkeys, common marmosets, and task but monkeys do not (Call, 2000; De
yellow-nosed monkeys (Cramer & Gallistel, Blois, Novak, & Bond, 1 998; De Blois &
1 997; MacDonald, Pang, & Gibeault, 1 994; Novak, 1 994; Dumas & Brunet, 1 994; Natale
MacDonald & Wilkie, 1 990). et al., 1 986), although there are individual
There also is a computerized version of exceptions (Schino, Spinozzi, & Berlinguer,
the shortcut task. Washburn (1 992; see also 1 990). Apes also have problems if the two
Washburn & Rumbaugh, 1 992) presented locations visited are not adjacent; that is, the
rhesus monkeys with a moving target on a experimenter visits the box on the right and
screen that they had to intercept with a cur- the left, leaving the center box untouched
sor that subjects controlled with a joystick. (Call, 2000; De Blois, Novak, & Bond, 1 998;
To do so appropriately, subjects not only had Natale et al., 1 986). This is interpreted as
to chase the target but on many occasions reconstructing the trajectory of the reward.
had to predict its location and use shortcuts Other types of displacements recently
to ambush it because the target speed was have been investigated with apes. In ro-
equal or superior to that of the cursor. In tational displacements, a reward is hidden
other words, they had to take shortcuts to in- under one of two cups and the platform is
tercept the moving target. This skill may be rotated circularly – for instance, 1 80 degrees.
more demanding than using shortcuts when In transpositions, the reward is placed under
traveling between various food sources be- one of various containers and their locations
cause subjects have to adapt to a moving tar- are swapped while the platform remains
get. This may be a useful skill in intercept- stationary. Results show that chimpanzees,
ing prey or competitors who hold valuable orangutans, and bonobos are capable of solv-
resources. These authors found that sub- ing these displacements (Beran & Minahan,
jects again were more effective at intercept- 2000; Call, 2003 ). Taken together, this
ing targets when they followed predictable means that subjects can track a variety of dis-
rather than unpredictable paths. Although placements based on the movement of the
subjects required some experience to learn object (object permanence), the containers
the paths of the targets, results from pre- (transpositions), or the substrate on which
sentation of novel target paths (e.g., the tar- the object and containers rest (rotations). All
get disappearing on the top and reappearing these result in changes in the location of the
on the bottom of the screen) suggested that object and subjects can infer its position.
monkeys had learned a general rule about In summary, primates are capable of trav-
the target’s behavior rather than a set of elling efficiently by using detours and short-
stimulus–response associations. In a similar cuts and they track the displacement of
vein, Iversen and Matsuzawa (2001 ) indi- hidden objects and infer their new locations
cated that when mazes had one short and a after various spatial transformations.
long route to get to the goal box, the chim-
panzee selected the short one.
Relational Reasoning
search for moving objects
Several studies investigated the ability of pri- In simple discrimination problems, subjects
mates to retrieve objects after they have un- learn to respond to a single stimulus or to a
61 0 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

stimulus category at some level of abstrac- Fujita, 1 982; but see Wright, Shyan, &
tion. Discrimination learning of relational Jitsumori, 1 990). Monkeys trained with
categories, on the other hand, involves con- shapes and capable of solving identity prob-
cepts that can be learned only by compar- lems with novel shapes, for instance, do
ing stimuli to one another and inducing a not transfer their identity concept to other
relation (e.g., “same as” “larger than”). The dimensions such as color (see Doumas &
three most studied instances of relational Hummel, Chap. 4, for a discussion of
concepts are the identity relation as manifest relational generalization). The rule that
in generalized match-to-sample problems, monkeys seem to learn is therefore better
the oddity relation as manifest in general- characterized as “pick the same shape” rather
ized oddity problems, and the sameness– than “pick the same.”
difference relation as manifest in general-
ized relation-matching problems. In all three Oddity
cases, the basic idea is that the subject is
Numerous studies demonstrate that many
given some problems in training that can be
primate species can acquire the concept
solved by attending to a relation and then is
of oddity, as evidenced by their ability to
given transfer tests that use completely dif-
solve novel problems after a period of train-
ferent objects that can be seen as instanti-
ing (King & Fobes, 1 982; Rumbaugh &
ations of that same relation. If learning is
McCormack; 1 967; Thomas & Boyd, 1 973 ).
relatively fast in the transfer phase, the infer-
Some primate species have also been able
ence is that the subject acquired a relational
to solve dimension-abstracted oddity prob-
concept in the training phase and is now ap-
lems in which the odd object must be distin-
plying it in the transfer phase. If the learning
guished from four other alternatives that are
is at the same basic rate in training and trans-
not identical to one another (as in traditional
fer (with some allowance for the formation
oddity problems) but only resemble one
of a learning set), the inference is that the
another with respect to some dimensions
subject has not learned a relational concept
(e.g., objects of different shapes that are all
but is treating each new problem as a sepa-
red, as opposed to the odd object, which
rate entity with its own particular stimulus
is blue). Macaques, squirrel monkeys, chim-
characteristics.
panzees, and gorillas were capable of solv-
ing this problem (Bernstein, 1 961 ; Thomas
Identity & Frost, 1 983 ). Human children have been
presented with oddity problems in a number
Several studies have shown that monkeys
of studies and generally perform very well in
and chimpanzees can solve identity prob-
the earliest trials of transfer (e.g., Lipsett &
lems based on generalized matching to sam-
Serunian, 1 963 ).
ple (D’Amato & Salmon, 1 984; D’Amato
et al., 1 986; Nissen, Blum, & Blum, 1 948;
Sameness–Difference
Oden, Thompson, & Premack, 1 988). In
the only study of which we are aware in In the previous two tasks, subjects have to
which human children were tested in this respond either to similarity or difference.
same type of procedure, they, like the chim- Some tasks have investigated whether sub-
panzees, generalized immediately to new jects can decide whether a pair of stim-
match-to-sample problems using only two uli are similar or different simultaneously.
sets of stimuli in training (Weinstein, 1 941 ). Several monkey species, chimpanzees, and
It should be noted, however, that this suc- orangutans were capable of judging whether
cessful performance with one stimulus di- two stimuli were “same” or “different”
mension (e.g., shape) does not generalize in (Wright, Santiago, & Sands; 1 984; Fujita,
most studies across other stimulus dimen- 1 983 ; King & Fobes; 1 975 ; Robinson, 1 95 5 ,
sions (D’Amato & Colombo, 1 985 ; Jackson 1 960; King, 1 973 ). These studies invari-
& Pegram, 1 970a, 1 970b; Kojima; 1 979; ably involved subjects making judgments
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 61 1

at the perceptual level, not the functional Gillan, Premack, and Woodruff (1 981 )
level. Recently, however, Bovet and Vauclair presented the language-trained chimpanzee
(2001 ) investigated the ability of baboons Sarah (Premack, 1 976) with pairs of objects
to make same–different judgments based that had various relations; Sarah’s job was
on the functional properties of the stimuli. to identify another pair that had an analo-
They presented baboons with pairs of stim- gous relation. In so-called figural problems,
uli corresponding to two different categories Sarah was presented with an odd shape with
(i.e., food vs. nonfood). The items belong- a dot on it and that same shape without the
ing to these two categories varied in their dot; she was then presented with another
perceptual features. Results indicated that shape with a dot and had to choose from
baboons were capable of judging as “same” a pair of alternatives that same shape with-
items belonging to the same category despite out the dot (i.e., the analogous relation of
their perceptual dissimilarities and as “differ- two shapes with and without a dot). In so-
ent” items belonging to different categories. called conceptual problems Sarah was pre-
sented with household items with which she
was familiar and asked to draw analogies, for
example, between a key and lock and a can
Analogical Reasoning opener and can. On figural items, Sarah per-
formed correctly about three-quarters of the
Premack (1 983 ) argued that identity, odd- time and on conceptual items she was cor-
ity, and sameness–difference tasks as tradi- rect at a slightly higher rate. Having ruled
tionally administered do not require the kind out various possible alternative explanations,
of relational concepts that investigators have the investigators concluded that Sarah was
claimed. Because the matching takes place able to understand the relation in the first
across trials in all of these tasks, he claimed pair of stimuli at a level of abstraction suf-
that “the animal simply reacts to whether it ficient to allow her to identify it in subse-
has experienced the item before. Old/new quent stimulus pairs, both perceptually and
or familiar/unfamiliar would be better tags conceptually.
for this case than same/different” (Ref. 86, Recently, Thompson, Oden, and Boysen
p. 3 5 4). Instead, he advocated use of a gener- (1 997) found that language-naı̈ve chim-
alized match-to-sample procedure in which panzees were also able to solve analogies if,
the matching to be accomplished involves prior to testing, they had been trained to
the relations between items. Premack (1 983 ) associate a token of one shape with pairs
presented chimpanzees with a sample pair of of similar items and a different token for a
stimuli that either matched (so-called AA pair of items that were not similar. Presented
pairs, such as two apples) or that did not with the same token, they selected the sim-
match (so-called CD pairs, such as a pear and ilar pair, and presented with the different
an orange). Their task was to pick which of token, they selected the pair with unequal
two alternatives matched the relation exem- items (this is comparable to Burdyn and
plified in the sample – either a pair of new Thomas, 1 984, in which squirrel monkeys
items that matched (a so-called BB pair, such used a figure to choose between the identical
as two bananas) or a pair of new items that or the different objects in the pair). On the
did not match (a so-called EF pair, such as testing phase, subjects were presented with
a plum and a grape). When the sample was a pair of identical or different objects as a
AA, the subject was to choose BB (rather sample and two choices (one that bore the
than EF) because the relation between items same the relation as the sample and another
in both cases is one of “sameness.” If the with a different relation). Chimpanzees per-
sample was CD, the subject should choose formed above chance, indicating that they
EF (rather than BB) because the relation could identify the relation between relations.
between items in each case was one of In contrast, rhesus monkeys presented with
“difference.” an analogous procedure were unable to solve
61 2 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

this problem even though they were able to their instrumental behavior. Interestingly,
solve other kinds of relations such as cor- children perform like chimpanzees and dis-
rectly identifying the perceptual analogies tinguish the relations, whereas monkeys do
(Washburn, Thompson, & Oden, cited in not (Thompson & Oden, 2000). Thompson
Thompson & Oden, 2000). and Oden (2000), however, have indicated
Several authors have indicated that only that these represent implicit rather than ex-
chimpanzees that had received some train- plicit judgments of the kind shown in gen-
ing that involved using tokens represent- eralized relation-matching tasks (see Litman
ing “same” and “different” were able to & Reber, Chap. 1 8 on implicit thinking).
solve analogies (Premack, 1 983 ; Thompson Second, and more importantly, Vonk
& Oden, 2000). They argued that learn- (2003 ) has recently shown that orangutans
ing a symbolic code such as language fun- and a gorilla can solve analogies without
damentally changed the nature of the cog- any token experience or extensive training
nitive representations used by chimpanzees (see also Smith et al., 1 975 ) using a delayed
by providing them with an abstract proposi- matching to sample (DMTS) task in which
tional code (rather than a concrete imaginal subjects had to match the relation repre-
code) in terms of which they might inter- sented by a pair of geometric figures to those
pret their experience. This idea, however, of one of the two alternatives provided – the
has been challenged from two main direc- same method used by other authors to test
tions. First, Oden, Thompson, and Premack analogical reasoning in chimpanzees. There
(1 990) used a different procedure and found is also a study with baboons that showed
that four infant chimpanzees (around one they can match a sample depicting a set
year of age with no language training) also of identical or different items to the corre-
engaged in the matching of relations. They sponding alternative (Fagot, Wasserman, &
simply presented the subject with a sample Young, 2001 ). Unlike previous studies with
stimulus that consisted of a pair of objects apes, however, baboons reached high perfor-
mounted on a small board; the pair could mance only when the sample and alterna-
either match (AA) or not match (CD). Sub- tives were formed by multiple items. When
jects could play with this sample as desired, the number of items was reduced, there was
and their play time was recorded. They were a clear decrement in accuracy, particularly
then presented with two test pairs of objects, for the “different” samples. This effect, as
also mounted on board, that they might play well as the extensive training involved (ba-
with; one was a matching pair (BB) and one boons received thousands of trials before
was a nonmatching pair (EF). Subjects’ ini- they mastered the initial task), opens the
tial play with the sample affected their han- door to other interpretations based on the
dling time with the new test pairs. If sub- perception of perceptual entropy.
jects had played with the sample pair that One area in analogical reasoning that
matched (AA) they were no longer inter- has received some recent research attention
ested in the matching relation and so played from a comparative perspective is that of
more with the nonmatching test pair (EF); spatial analogy. Using a task pioneered by
if they had played with the nonmatching DeLoache and colleagues (DeLoache 1 995 ),
sample (CD), they played more with the Kuhlmeier, Boysen, and Mukobi (1 999)
matching test pair (BB). The conclusion of tested the ability of chimpanzees to make
these investigators was that chimpanzees can spatial analogies. Subjects were presented
understand relations among relations, even with a very accurate three-dimensional scale
if they do not always show this competence model of a room and subjects witnessed
in tasks in which they must actively choose how the experimenter placed an object (e.g.,
stimuli. The modified conclusion of these soda can) in a particular location in the
authors was that, although chimpanzees un- scale model (e.g., inside a cupboard). Sub-
derstand second-order relations, language jects then moved to the real room and were
training helps them incorporate this into allowed to search the room. Chimpanzees
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 61 3

were capable of using the scale model to pairs (D’Amato et al., 1 985 ; D’Amato &
accurately predict the location of the ob- Colombo, 1 988; Gillan 1 981 ; Boysen et al.,
ject in the room and vice versa; they were 1 993 ). This includes cases in which sub-
able to point to a location in the scale model jects have been trained with more than three
that corresponded to a location in the actual stimuli. This is important because the most
room. Initially, female chimpanzees were interesting cases are those that involve in-
more proficient than males at this task. Male termediate stimuli – that is, stimuli that are
chimpanzees tended to search the room in not the first or the last of the sequence, be-
a predetermined pattern until they eventu- cause those are always or never reinforced,
ally found the object rather than going to the respectively. D’Amato and Colombo (1 988)
specific places indicated in the scale model. trained capuchin monkeys to touch five ar-
When reward delivery was made contingent bitrary items in a specified order (labeled
on visiting the specific location on their first A, B, C, D, and E). After they had mas-
try, however, males’ performance became tered this task they were presented with
comparable to that of female chimpanzees novel pairs. Of particular importance were
(Kuhlmeier & Boysen, 2001 ). the internal pairs B–C, C–D, and B–D. The
In summary, primates are capable of per- B–D comparison was especially important
ceiving various types of relations between because these two items were both internal
objects. Moreover, apes can solve analogies to the series and were nonadjacent to one
regarding the similarity or difference be- another in the previous training. Subjects
tween pairs of objects, and chimpanzees can ordered these three internal pairs correctly
also solve spatial analogies involving the use 81 % to 88% of the time, well above chance.
of scale models. When presented with triplets from which
they were to choose the highest item, they
ordered the internal triplet B-C-D correctly
94% of the time, also well above chance.
Inferential Reasoning
This finding essentially replicates, with even
stronger results, the findings of McGonigle
Transitivity
and Chalmers (1 977) with squirrel monkeys.
The use of transitive inference has re- These authors also found evidence for a sym-
ceived much research attention. Although bolic distance effect – the farther apart two
human studies on transitivity (see Halford, items, the more successful the subjects, pre-
Chap. 22) have often used stimuli that vary sumably because the items were easier to
systematically and naturally along a quanti- distinguish.
tative dimension such as height (e.g., Piaget, One open question is, What is the mecha-
1 95 2), most studies with primates have used nism responsible for this performance? Two
so-called associative transitivity. This con- mechanisms have been postulated – the as-
sists of presenting subjects with pairs of ar- sociative mechanisms based on responding
bitrary stimuli and differentially reinforcing to the differential reinforcement and asso-
one of the stimulus of the pair, thereby creat- ciative strength of the stimuli, and the re-
ing different values. For instance, the red cup lational or linear mechanism based on cre-
is always reinforced when presented with ating a mental order of the stimuli. Bond,
the blue cup, whereas the blue cup is always Kamil, and Balda (2003 ) argued that, under
reinforced when presented with the yellow an associative mechanism, errors increase at
cup, and so on. Once the initial pairs are the end of the sequence, whereas latencies
trained, subjects are presented with pairs of should be unaffected regardless of the po-
stimuli that have not been paired before, for sition of the items. In contrast, the rela-
instance, red versus yellow cup. tional mechanism predicts that accuracy will
There is ample evidence showing that remain unchanged, whereas the latency to
primates can make transitive inferences respond will be affected. First, subjects’ la-
when subjects are presented with novel tency to respond to the first item of a pair
61 4 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

increased as that item moved down the se- ing with something more than an associative
ries: They responded most quickly to pairs chain; they were operating with some men-
in which the first item was A, then for pairs tally represented sequence of items in which
in which the first item was B, then C, then the ordinal position of each item was essen-
D. The implication is that each time they are tial information.
presented with a pair, the subjects are men-
tally reconstructing the entire five-item se-
Ordinality
ries (D’Amato & Colombo, 1 988). Second,
animals responded most quickly to the sec- Many of the studies on transitivity have al-
ond item of a pair for pairs with adjacent ready indicated that monkeys learn some-
items (e.g., A–B, C–D, etc.), then for pairs thing about the linear representation in a se-
separated by one gap (e.g., A–C, C–E, etc.), ries – they learn the order in which the items
then for pairs separated by two gaps (i.e., should appear. Some studies have pushed
A–D, B–E), and they were slowest on the this argument a bit further and have substi-
second item when the gap was three (A–E). tuted boxes by Arabic numerals represent-
Again, the implication is that subjects are ing different quantities. Boysen et al. (1 993 )
going through the entire series mentally on found that after chimpanzees were trained
every trial. Swartz, Chen, and Terrace (1 991 ) with pairs of adjacent numerals in the same
essentially replicated these results – both in pairwise as used in the studies previously re-
terms of ordinal judgments and in terms of ported, they were then presented with the
reaction times – for rhesus macaques. novel pair 2–4. In this study, after appropri-
Although these results are quite convinc- ate training with the initial pairs, subjects
ing, D’Amato and Colombo (1 989) pointed all were able to successfully choose the 4
out that the results of this study are com- over the 2 in the novel pairwise test. The
patible with an associative chain interpre- investigators concluded that with appropri-
tation in which each item simply serves as ate training, chimpanzees can learn the se-
a discriminative stimulus evoking the next rial order of symbolic stimuli. Washburn
item, obviating the need for some represen- and Rumbaugh (1 991 ) taught two rhesus
tation of serial order. To investigate whether macaques to associate Arabic numerals with
capuchin monkeys were also associating a the reception of a corresponding number of
specific serial position with each item in the food pellets. Because monkeys try to max-
associative chain, D’Amato and Colombo imize their food intake, they learned to se-
(1 989) used a procedure that essentially lect the larger quantity represented by the
broke the chain. Using monkeys who had various numerals that were presented to
already learned the ABCDE sequence, on them. The authors reserved some of the pairs
some trials they introduced a “wild card” for their transitivity tests. One of the two
item at a particular point in the sequence subjects was above chance in choosing the
(e.g., ABCXE). This was a novel item that larger member of the novel pair in the very
had never been used as part of the training first set of trials. Similarly, presented with
and therefore had no associations with any five numerals simultaneously, result of both
other items. These investigators found that were above chance immediately in choosing
no matter the position in which the wild card the largest one. These investigators interpret
item appeared, subjects treated it in a man- their results as indicating that the monkeys
ner similar to the item it replaced at above- formed a representation of a “matrix of val-
chance levels, touching it at the appropriate ues” corresponding to the numerals.
place in the sequence approximately 60% of Additional evidence for ordinality is pro-
the time. They performed just as well with vided by two studies not based on a transi-
sequences containing two wild card items. tivity paradigm. First, Brannon and Terrace
Consequently, D’Amato and Colombo ar- (1 998) trained rhesus macaques to touch
gued that the monkeys in this study, and a series of stimuli depicting different nu-
presumably in previous studies, were operat- merosities in ascending order. Initially, the
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 61 5

monkeys were trained on sets of stimuli de- the fruit. Chimpanzees solved this prob-
picting numerosities ranging from 1 to 4. In lem quickly, without trial-and-error, show-
transfer tests, monkeys were able to solve ing that they were able to infer that if the ex-
problems involving numerosities ranging 5 perimenter was eating the banana, the box
to 9. The authors argued that their results where the banana was deposited would be
demonstrated that rhesus macaques can rep- empty.
resent numerosities ranging from 1 to 9 in an More recently, Call (2004) presented all
ordinal manner. In a follow-up study, Bran- four great apes with two cups (one baited)
non and Terrace (2000) trained monkeys to and gave visual or auditory information
touch a series of stimuli in descending or- about the contents of one or both cups. Vi-
der. They failed to transfer the descending sual information consisted of removing the
rule into new numerosities, however. Mon- top of the cup so that subjects could look
keys also failed to learn to select numerosi- inside it. Auditory information consisted of
ties in a monotonic series despite extensive shaking the cup so that it produced a rattling
training. This suggests that ordinality may be sound when the food was inside. Subjects
an especially salient dimension for monkeys. correctly selected the baited cup both when
The accuracy in responding and the latency they saw the food and when they heard it.
indicated distance effects similar to those More importantly, subjects also selected the
in other studies, including human studies correct cup when only the empty cup was ei-
(e.g., Moyer & Landaeur, 1 967). Second, ther shown or shaken. This means that sub-
Kawai and Matsuzawa (2000) showed that jects chose correctly without having seen or
the chimpanzee Ai was capable of select- heard the food. Control tests showed that,
ing Arabic numerals presented on the com- in general, subjects were not more attracted
puter screen in ascending order. She had 90% to noisy cups or avoided shaken noiseless
accuracy with four-numeral series and 65 % cups. Also, subjects were unable to learn to
accuracy with five-numeral series. The re- use other comparable auditory cues such as
sponse latency was longest for the first item tapping on the baited cup to find the food.
in the series compared with the remaining The author argued that apes made inferences
ones, which suggests the chimpanzee was about the food location, rather than just as-
planning the sequence before executing the sociating an auditory cue with the reward.
entire sequence. This suggests that subjects understood that
the food caused the noise, not simply that
the noise was associated with the food.
Conjunctive Negation
There are also two studies in which chim-
This refers to the ability to infer that if a panzees were able to solve inferential ex-
given object can be located in one of two clusion in a matching to sample paradigm.
containers and, upon searching the first con- Hashiya and Kojima (2001 ) presented a
tainer, is not found there, then it must be in chimpanzee with two pictures of people
the other one. Premack and Premack (1 994) she knew and the voice of one of them.
presented chimpanzees with two boxes and The chimpanzees successfully matched the
two types of fruit, such as a banana and an voice with the correct picture. Then Hashiya
apple. Chimpanzees were allowed to witness and Kojima (2001 ) presented her with two
the experimenter deposit each fruit in one pictures (one of someone she knew and
of the boxes so that both boxes were baited. the other of someone she did not know)
Later, subjects saw the experimenter eating and an unfamiliar voice. The chimpanzee
one of the fruits (e.g., banana) and the ques- correctly matched the unfamiliar voice to
tion was whether given the opportunity to the unfamiliar picture. Beran and Washburn
select either box, they would select the one (2002) presented chimpanzees with pictures
in which the experimenter had deposited and lexigrams as samples and alternatives,
the food he was not currently eating (i.e., respectively. Pictures and lexigrams could
apple), presumably because it still contained be either familiar or unfamiliar. Familiar
61 6 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

samples and alternatives consisted of pic- their choices. Beran and Rumbaugh (2001 )
tures and lexigrams, respectively, that sub- argued that chimpanzees’ performance
jects had learned to associate before the test. decreased proportionally to the magnitude
As expected, chimpanzees reliably selected of the numerals presented. The authors
the familiar appropriate lexigrams repre- argued that the chimpanzees seemed to rep-
senting the sample pictures, but, in addi- resent quantities in a continuous rather than
tion, chimpanzees also were able to select a discrete fashion. This characterization
unfamiliar lexigrams when presented with differs in some way from that of Rumbaugh
familiar lexigrams and an unfamiliar picture. et al. (1 989), who indicated that these
Their success in this task did not translate subjects were counting in a way similar to
into the acquisition of the unfamiliar lexi- human children in that they knew not only
gram as a representation for the unfamiliar the ordinality but also the cardinality of the
picture, however. Arabic numerals involved.
In summary, primates are capable of Boysen and Berntson (1 989) trained the
making inferences about pieces of missing chimpanzee Sheba to count objects using
information in transitivity and conjunctive Arabic numerals using a different method.
negation problems of various types. First, they administered a one-to-one corre-
spondence task in which she had to place one
and only one object in each of six compart-
Quantitative Reasoning ments of a divided tray. Then, she was re-
quired to pick a card with the same number
of dots as the number of food items (rang-
Primates can perform operations on quanti-
ing from one to three pieces) presented on a
ties by adapting to novel arrays when some
tray. The researchers then replaced the cards
quantities are added, subtracted, or simply
with dots with cards having Arabic numer-
change in appearance but remain constant.
als and continued the training until Sheba
They also have some skills in counting, on
was able to select the Arabic numeral corre-
which these more complex skills depend.
sponding to the number of dots on a card.
Finally, the authors trained the subject in
Counting
Arabic numeral comprehension so that, pre-
Rumbaugh and coalleagues (Beran & sented with an Arabic numeral, she had to
Rumbaugh, 2001 ; Rumbaugh et al., 1 989) select the card with the corresponding num-
presented two chimpanzees with a com- ber of dots. After she mastered these tasks,
puter task in which subjects had to collect Boysen and Berntson conducted two trans-
the number of dots from the bottom of fer tests. First, they presented her with one,
the screen specified by an Arabic numeral two, or three common household items and
presented on the top of the screen. Subjects asked her to pick the corresponding Ara-
indicated when they had finished their bic numeral, which she readily did. Second,
selection with the use of the cursor. The they introduced the Arabic numerals 4, 5 ,
chimpanzees performed above chance and 0 directly (without first using cards with
with the numerals up to six and seven, dots), and Sheba readily learned to associate
respectively. Rumbaugh et al. (1 989) also these with the correct number of objects as
indicated that the chimpanzee Lana could well.
solve this task even if the squares disap- During this training of Sheba, Boysen and
peared as she touched them (with a tone Bernston noticed that she often engaged in
sounding as they disappeared) – implying “indicating acts” as she counted. That is, she
that she could keep track mentally of how touched, displaced, or “pointed to” objects
many she had already touched. The authors serially in attempting to determine the ap-
ruled out some explanations such as subitiz- propriate Arabic numeral – much as human
ing or using the temporal pattern rather children touch or otherwise indicate objects
than the number of dots as the basis for as they count them. In a follow-up study,
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 61 7

therefore, Boysen et al. (1 995 ) looked to see understood as a mental operation, however,
whether the number of indicating acts Sheba because subjects were not required to
used as she engaged in these tasks correlated perform any mental operations. Directly
with the number of items in the array (by the perceiving the larger of the two overall
time of this study, Sheba knew the numer- quantities in either side would suffice to
als 0–7). They gave her some counting tasks, solve this task. In other words, this can be
using the numerals 0 to 7, and found that seen as a relative numerousness judgment
she correctly counted 5 4% of the time (with over a large area, without any operation
errors distributed equally across the 1 to 7 beyond perception being implicated.
range). They found further that whereas the In an attempt to solve this problem, Call
absolute number of Sheba’s indicating acts (2000) presented orangutans with two quan-
did not correspond to the number of items tities in two dishes and then added a third
in the array precisely, typically being about one into one of the dishes. In some trials,
twice as large, these did correlate signifi- this resulted in the smaller of the two ini-
cantly (r = .74). It is unclear whether this tial quantities having more and sometimes
correlation is attributable to counting or to did not change. Call (2000) also subtracted
the fact that determining the numerosity of quantities from the initial quantities and
the larger numerals requires more time, so showed how much he had subtracted. The
that a constant rate of indicating acts across important point is that subjects never saw
all numerals would lead to the correlation. the final quantities directly, but they had
In any case, the investigators concluded that to decide based on how much had been
Sheba was counting objects in much the added to or subtracted from the quanti-
same way as human children, and that her in- ties. Orangutans were capable of perform-
dicating acts were serving a mediating func- ing above chance in both addition and
tion in the process. subtraction.
Sulkowski and Hauser (2001 ) also investi-
gated subtraction in rhesus macaques. They
Summation and Subtraction
showed subjects two quantities (up to three
Rumbaugh and colleagues (Perusse & items each), hid each of them in two sep-
Rumbaugh, 1 990; Rumbaugh, Savage- arate adjacent locations and then removed
Rumbaugh, & Hegel, 1 987; Rumbaugh, either one or no items from each location.
Savage-Rumbaugh, & Pate, 1 988) pre- Rhesus macaques selected the location with
sented two language-trained chimpanzees more items even when subtractions occurred
(Sherman & Austin) with two unequal from both locations and when some noned-
sets of candies (M&M) but presented as ible items rather than food were subtracted.
spatially distinct subsets. For instance, a Beran (2001 ) found that two chim-
trial consisted of presenting four and three panzees were also able to add quantities
candies compared with five and one candies. presented sequentially up to nine pieces of
Chimpanzees were capable of comparing candy (M&M). Unlike the previous study,
these two sets and combining the spatially each piece of candy was added individually
distinct subsets (e.g., 4 + 3 vs. 5 + 1 ) to net to one of two cups rather than presenting the
the larger total array (up to a maximum of array in its totality. In different experiments,
seven candies) on more than 90% of the subjects witnessed the experimenter plac-
trials. Although the investigators did not ing different quantities into the cups in var-
claim that their subjects “added” numbers ious steps. All candies may be added at one
in anything resembling the human method, time in a given cup or the addition rounds
they argued that the skills required for this could be alternated between the two cups.
task go beyond simple subitizing, because In the final experiment, subjects witnessed
the items in each of the two quantities to be the experimenter removing one candy from
compared are separated into two spatially one of the cups before being allowed
distinct subsets. This is far from summation to choose. Both chimpanzees performed
61 8 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

above chance in the addition trials, whereas ward the formation of logical thinking in
only one chimpanzee was above chance children. Seven-year-old children and older
in the subtraction trials. These two stud- understand that if two quantities were the
ies suggest that orangutans and chimpanzees same prior to a perceptual transformation
can represent quantities and mentally op- (and nothing has been added or removed)
erate on those quantities to net the largest they must be the same after the transforma-
array. tion has taken place. This logical necessity is
Olthof, Iden, and Roberts (1 997) raised the cornerstone of the conservation experi-
the bar a bit further and replaced the ac- ments (see also Halford, Chap. 22).
tual quantities by Arabic numerals. First the Although some studies on conservation
monkeys were trained to identify the numer- in monkeys have been done (e.g.,Thomas
als corresponding to quantities that subjects & Peay, 1 976), the lack of information
knew. Then subjects were given a choice be- about how subjects judge the two quanti-
tween different combinations of numerals ties prior to a transformation prevents us
involving two numerals in each pair, one nu- from drawing any conclusions. Two stud-
meral and two numerals, or three numerals ies with chimpanzees collected this informa-
in each pair. For instance (1 + 1 + 3 ) against tion and therefore can be interpreted more
(2 + 2 + 2). Squirrel monkeys were capable accurately. First, Woodruff, Premack, and
of selecting the larger total quantity. Addi- Kennel (1 978) presented Sarah with liquid,
tional tests indicated that this effect could solid, and number conservation tasks. Before
not be explained by choosing the largest this test, Sarah had learned to use plastic
numeral available or avoiding the smallest tokens to indicate whether a pair of stim-
number available. uli were “same” or “different.” In the liquid
Similarly, Boysen and Berntson (1 989) conservation task, she was presented with a
also reported that Sheba was able to visit pair of equal or unequal liquid quantities
three locations in the room, look for hid- in identical containers and asked to judge
den items that might be there and report them with the tokens. One of the quan-
the total number of items (up to four) at a tities was then poured into another con-
different location by picking up a card de- tainer with a different shape, and she was
picting the arabic numeral corresponding to asked to make a judgment on the novel
the total number of items available in the stimuli. Results indicated that she correctly
room. Sheba was able to do this by using ei- judged the quantities in liquid and solid
ther actual objects or Arabic numerals with but not in number conservation tasks. Addi-
an overall accuracy of 75 % (chance = 25 %). tional tests also showed that Sarah was un-
Given that Sheba also can make transitive able to judge correctly when she was pre-
inferences with an ordered series of items vented from seeing the quantities presented
(Boysen et al., 1 993 ) and uses indicating acts in identical containers first. This result led
as she attempts to determine the numeri- Woodruff, Premack, & Kennel (1 978) to con-
cal value of sets of objects (see previous sec- clude that she based her judgments on log-
tion), the investigators hypothesize that she ical necessity rather than perceptual esti-
is actually counting, in a human-like way, mation. Similarly, Muncer (1 983 ) reported
in these foraging tasks, and that her number that a chimpanzee was capable of selecting
concept is very much like that of a young the larger of two quantities after applying a
human child. transformation that changed the appearance
of the liquid. As in the previous study, the
chimpanzee was unable to select the larger
Conservation
quantity if she was prevented from seeing
Piaget and Inhelder (1 941 ) considered the the pretransformation quantities displayed
ability to understand that physical quanti- in identical containers.
ties remain constant after changing their per- Call and Rochat (1 996) investigated the
ceptual appearance an important step to- ability of four orangutans to solve liquid
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 61 9

conservation using a modified version of studies were not comparable, it is diffi-


Muncer’s procedure. Subjects were pre- cult to know whether chimpanzees and
sented with a pair of identical containers orangutans truly differ in the mechanisms
with unequal amounts of juice. Once sub- they use to solve conservation problems or
jects had indicated their choice by pointing the differences were a result of the methods
(which invariably was to the larger quan- used in each set of studies. Recently, Suda
tity), the experimenter transferred the liquid and Call (in press) set out to resolve this
quantities into a pair of unequal containers. discrepancy by studying chimpanzees and
In different experiments, the authors var- orangutans with the same procedures. They
ied either the shape of the containers or the presented apes with various liquid conser-
number of containers available (while keep- vation problems in which the initial quanti-
ing the shape constant). Although some apes ties were transferred into containers of dif-
still selected the larger quantity after a shape ferent shapes or into multiple containers, di-
transformation, this performance deterio- viding the total quantity. Results supported
rated when the contrast between the shape the notion that most apes relied on percep-
of the containers was increased. In contrast, tual estimation rather than logical necessity
some of the six- to eight-year-old children with orangutans being slightly more profi-
they tested performed satisfactorily. Further- cient than chimpanzees.
more, none of the apes solved the task when In summary, primates can solve quanti-
the quantities were transferred into mul- tative problems that require combining or
tiple containers. Call and Rochat (1 996) dissociating quantities, and they can develop
concluded that orangutans depended upon the notion of ordinality. In contrast, there is
perceptual information rather than logical little evidence that primates use logical ne-
necessity, thereby demonstrating “pseudo- cessity when confronted with various Piage-
conservation.” In a follow-up study, Call and tian conservation problems.
Rochat (1 997) investigated the use of per-
ceptual strategies underlying the orangutans’
pseudoconservation. The authors examined Causal Reasoning
three possible perceptual strategies to iden-
tify the larger amount of liquid: visual es-
Causal reasoning is a complex topic, and
timation of the liquid in the container, the
much hinges on the chosen definition of
use of information about quantity based
causality. Some researchers interpret causal-
on pouring the liquid, and a tracking strat-
ity as the ability to form stimulus–stimulus
egy that consisted of following the liq-
or stimulus–response associations. In this
uid that subjects had initially chosen. Re-
broad sense there is no doubt that many an-
sults indicated that the visual estimation
imals are sensitive to causality. We concen-
strategy best accounted for the orangutan’s
trate more narrowly on the understanding of
pseudoconservation. Overall, these investi-
the underlying “structures” and “forces” that
gators interpreted their results as indicating
are responsible for certain effects. This has
that orangutans are very good at estimating
been most studied in the domain of tool use,
quantities and at tracking the quantity they
but it has also been investigated in a variety
prefer across various spatial displacements,
of other types of physical events in which
but they do not conserve quantities across
the subject does not manipulate but only
perceptual transformations in a humanlike
observes.
manner.
The studies cited with chimpanzees sug-
Tool Use
gest the use of logical reasoning, whereas
studies with orangutans suggest the use of Many introductory texts to psychology men-
perceptual estimation in the solution of liq- tion the experiments involving tool use
uid conservation problems. Because both by chimpanzees as groundbreaking stud-
the species and methods employed in the ies. Since then, it has been shown that
62 0 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

several primate species use tools in a va- Hauser and colleagues recently investi-
riety of ways and for a variety of pur- gated this problem in detail with cotton-top
poses (see Beck, 1 980; Tomasello & Call, tamarins (Hauser, Kralik & Botto-Mahan,
1 997, for reviews). We concentrate on three 1 999). Their studies questioned whether
tasks that have been used to investigate these monkeys can distinguish between rele-
causality. vant and irrelevant features of tools – in this
case, the cloth. They found that tamarins
were able to master this problem. In par-
support problem ticular, presented with two cloths and two
In this problem, a reward is placed on a rewards from which to choose, they pulled
cloth. The reward itself is outside the sub- the cloth on which the reward rested. In
ject’s reach, but one of the ends of the cloth another experiment, subjects selected the
is within reach. The solution to this problem cloth that was connected somehow to the
consists of pulling the cloth to bring the re- reward but avoided the cloth that was not
ward within reach. Piaget (1 95 2) studied this connected to the reward. Once subjects had
problem in human infants and indicated that mastered these two problems, the authors
by 1 2 months of age children not only readily presented monkeys with both relevant and
pull in the cloth but, more importantly, they irrelevant changes to the problem. Relevant
withhold pulling when the reward is not in changes included the position of the re-
contact with the cloth. This indicates that ward in relation to the reward or the con-
children at this age understand that spatial nectedness between two pieces of cloth; ir-
contact is necessary for the tool to act on relevant changes included variations in the
the reward. color, texture, or shape of the cloth. The
Spinozzi and Potı̀ (1 989) tested several in- tamarins ignored irrelevant changes to the
fant primates (one Japanese macaque, two tool such as color or shape. They failed to
capuchin monkeys, two longtail macaques, solve some problems involving changes in
and one gorilla) on this problem. In one con- the relevant features, although they mas-
dition, the reward was placed on the cloth, tered those problems with additional expe-
whereas in another condition the reward was rience. The authors interpreted this as an
placed off the cloth to the side. All primates ability to distinguish between relevant and
responded appropriately by pulling in the irrelevant features.
cloth when the reward was on the cloth and Hauser et al. (2002) investigated whether
withheld pulling when the reward was off experience with tool use played a role in de-
the cloth. In a second experiment, Spinozzi ciding what constituted the relevant func-
and Potı̀ (1 989) tested the generality of these tional features of a tool. They presented
findings by modifying the conditions of the monkeys with a number of cloth problems
off-cloth condition by placing the reward that varied along several parameters except
near the end of the cloth rather than to that the correct alternative was always in-
the side of it. The authors reasoned that if dicated by the same color. Subjects there-
subjects had simply learned to respond ap- fore could solve the various problems by
propriately to a specific configuration of the either attending to the relevant features of
cloth and the reward rather than a more the problem (e.g., connectedness) or the
general relation between them, they would color of the cloth. Once animals had mas-
respond inappropriately to this novel con- tered the series of preliminary tests, they
figuration. Results confirmed their previous were presented with novel problems but
findings: All subjects pulled in the on-cloth with the color contingency reversed so that
condition but not in the off-cloth condition. color always signaled the incorrect alterna-
Recently, Spinozzi and Potı̀ (1 993 ) admin- tive. Results indicated that tool-experienced
istered the same support problem to two monkeys relied less on irrelevant cues such
infant chimpanzees and only one of them as color than tool-naı̈ve individuals in solv-
succeeded. ing the cloth problem. Nevertheless, all
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 62 1

monkeys experienced a postreversal decre- they varied either relevant or irrelevant fea-
ment performance, albeit performance of tures of the task, as previously done with
tool-experienced monkeys suffered less. the cloth problem. Results mirrored those
of the cloth problem and indicated that in-
dividuals selected tools most often based on
stick and hook problem relevant, as opposed to irrelevant, functional
A more challenging task than the sup- features. Hauser, Pearson, and Seelig (2002)
port problem consists of using a tool to recently investigated the role of experience
bring in a reward that is not in direct con- in the ability to distinguish relevant from
tact with the tool. This situation entails irrelevant features. They found that infant
putting the tool into contact with the re- tamarins, without much experience with
ward and then sweeping the reward within tools, also selected tools based on relevant
reach. According to Natale (1 989), solving features, reproducing the results of the adult
this task demonstrates an ability to under- subjects.
stand complex causal relations such as that
the stick must be of the appropriate size
and material (e.g., long and rigid) and that tube and trap problem
only certain kinds of contact (e.g., with a In this problem, the reward is placed inside
certain force and directionality) would be the middle portion of a transparent tube,
successful. and subjects have to use a stick to push the
Natale (1 989) presented eight subjects reward out the end opposite to which the
from the same four species tested by reward was inserted. In a series of studies,
Spinozzi and Potı̀ (1 989) with an out-of- Visalberghi and colleagues explored the abil-
reach reward and a stick placed in differ- ity of capuchin monkeys and apes (mainly
ent positions relative to the object in dif- chimpanzees) to solve this problem and to
ferent experimental conditions. Three of adjust to novel variations of this problem.
the four capuchin monkeys and the go- Visalberghi and Trinca (1 989) found that
rilla were moderately successful in obtaining three of four capuchin monkeys succeeded
the reward in various tool-reward spatial ar- in the basic version of the problem, and then
rangements. These results have been con- the authors administered three variations
firmed by other studies (see Beck, 1 980; of the problem involving different types of
Tomasello & Call, 1 997 for a review). Al- tools that required different solutions. In the
though none of the macaques tested by bundle task, subjects were given a bundle
Natale (1 989) was able to obtain the re- of sticks taped together that, as a whole,
ward with the stick, other studies have was too wide to fit in the tube; the solu-
shown that macaques and other primates, tion consisted of breaking the sticks apart.
including baboons, orangutans, and chim- In the short-sticks task, subjects were given
panzees, are capable of solving the stick three short sticks that, together, added up
problem (see Tomasello & Call, 1 997 for a to the length required; the solution consist-
review). ing of putting them all in the same end of
A refinement of the stick problem con- the tube to displace the food out the other
sists of presenting a hook-shaped tool and side. Finally, in the H-tool task, subjects were
a straight tool as alternatives for retrieving given a stick with transverse pieces on ei-
the reward. Hauser (1 997) presented cotton- ther end that prevented its insertion into the
top tamarins with two hooked tools, only tube; the solution consisted of removing the
one of which had the reward inside the blocking piece from the tool. Although all
hook so that pulling it would bring the re- three subjects eventually solved these varia-
ward. Once tamarins consistently solved this tions of the task, they made a number of er-
problem – that is, they preferred the stick rors such as attempting to insert the whole
with the reward inside the hook – the au- bundle or inserting one short stick in one
thors presented novel problems in which end of the tube and another short stick in
62 2 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

the other end. Moreover, these errors did formance of the majority of subjects actu-
not decrease significantly over trials, suggest- ally deteriorated over trials, indicating they
ing that capuchins understood little about may not have come to understand the causal
the causal relations between the elements in relations involved, although their young age
the task. Visalberghi, Fragaszy, and Savage- may have explained their poor performance.
Rumbaugh (1 995 ) essentially replicated the Visalberghi Fragaszy, & Savage-Rumbaugh
results of the bundle and the H-tool tasks (1 995 ) presented the bundle and H-tool
with six other capuchins. tasks to subadult and adult apes (four bono-
One recent study, however, suggests that bos, five chimpanzees, and one orangutan).
some capuchins may understand more about Eight of the ten apes solved the basic tube
causal relations than previously thought. task on the first trial, and the other two
Anderson and Henneman (1 995 ) tested the were successful later. When given a bun-
ability of two adult capuchin monkeys to an- dle of sticks, all subjects immediately disas-
ticipate (and solve) a variety of problems as- sembled the bundle and, unlike capuchins,
sociated with using a stick to extract honey no ape attempted to insert the bundle as
from a box with multiple holes. In a se- a whole. Apes proved less successful in the
ries of experiments of increasing complex- H-tool task, however, making some of the
ity, subjects were required to select a stick same mistakes as the capuchins. Indeed, a
of the appropriate diameter to fit the holes, statistical comparison of the two species in
rake in a stick of the appropriate diameter this condition revealed no significant differ-
with the help of another tool, modify a stick ence. Although there was an overall group
that was too thick or too twisted to fit the tendency to decrease the number of errors
holes, or construct a rake that would per- across trials, some subjects increased their
mit them to obtain a suitable stick to ex- errors.
tract the honey. Results indicated that both To examine further the understanding of
capuchins (especially the male) readily se- causal relations in the tube task, Visalberghi
lected sticks of a diameter suitable to fit the and Limongelli (1 994) presented a new tube
holes. This even included cases in which the problem that punished subjects who did not
box and the sticks available were not within foresee the consequences of their behavior.
the same visual field. This result contrasts The authors presented four capuchin mon-
with Visalberghi’s (1 993 ) findings in which keys with a tube that had a trap in its bottom
capuchins failed to select appropriate tools center, and placed the food next to the trap.
to solve the tube task when the tools were If subjects pushed the food in the direction
left in a room adjacent to the tube with food of the trap, it would fall in it and they would
in it. Moreover, Anderson and Henneman lose it; to get the food out, they had to push
(1 995 ) noted that one capuchin modified the food away from the trap toward the other
tools in a very purposeful manner without end of the tube. Visalberghi and Limongelli
committing the sort of errors described by (1 994) found that only one subject solved
Visalberghi and Trinca (1 989). The same ca- the task, systematically pushing the reward
puchin also used a tool (itself not suitable for away from the trap. Although this subject
honey-dipping) to rake in appropriate sticks seemed to be planning her moves in advance,
for honey-dipping. Neither of the subjects, the authors noted that in half the trials, she
however, was able to construct a rake to ob- inserted the tool in the wrong side of the
tain honey-dipping sticks. tube and, upon seeing that the reward was
The tube task has also been administered moving into the trap, withdrew the tool,
to apes. First, Bard, Fragaszy, and Visalberghi reinserted it in the other end, and pushed
(1 995 ) administered this task to young chim- out the reward. Visalberghi and Limongelli
panzees (two to four years old) and found (1 994) probed further her understanding of
that in the two most difficult versions of the the relation between the trap and the reward
task (i.e., short-sticks and H-tool), the per- by inverting the trap 1 80 degrees so that the
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 62 3

trap was on top of the tube, where it was more clearly seen in the tube task and its
no longer effective. The subject, however, variations.
persisted in her strategy of pushing the food
away from the trap, suggesting that she had
Perceiving and Judging Physical Events
apparently simply learned to push the food
away from the trap side without understand- One area that has received considerable at-
ing the causal relations between the trap and tention is that of object knowledge in in-
the reward. fants. These studies present subjects with a
Limongelli, Boysen, and Visalberghi series of events – some that follow the laws
(1 995 ) presented the trap-tube task to five of physics such as solidity or gravity and
chimpanzees who behaved at chance levels others that violate those laws. Using look-
for the first seventy trials, although two of ing measures, numerous studies have found
them learned to avoid the trap during sev- that human infants respond selectively to
enty additional trials. The authors admin- the violation of physics laws (Baillargeon,
istered an additional test to assess whether 1 995 ; Spelke et al., 1 995 ). These authors
chimpanzees understood the relationship have argued that even at this young age, chil-
between the position of the reward with re- dren show object knowledge. Hauser and
spect to the trap or whether they were sim- colleagues have been instrumental in intro-
ply using the simple rule of pushing the re- ducing this area of research in nonhuman
ward out the side to which it was closest, primates. They have concentrated on two
thus avoiding the trap. Limongelli, Boysen, topics: gravity and solidity.
and Visalberghi (1 995 ) varied the location In the gravity area, Hood et al. (1 999) pre-
of the trap in the tube. In some cases, the sented cotton-top tamarins with three con-
trap was located very close to one end with tainers arranged in a straight line. One of the
the food just beyond it, so that subjects ac- containers was connected to an opaque tube
tually had to push the food out the end from through which the experimenter dropped
which it was farthest. In other cases, the food. Subjects consistently searched for the
opposite arrangement was used. Both sub- food in the container over which the food
jects solved these variations easily, with al- was dropped. They did this regardless of
most no errors, so the researchers concluded whether the tube was connected to the
that these two chimpanzees understood the container or not. This indicates that mon-
causal relations in this task better than the keys failed to understand that the reward’s
capuchin monkeys. It should be noted, how- straight-fall trajectory can be deviated by the
ever, that the variations used in this experi- tube. This bias persisted despite variations
ment could still be solved by the rule “push on the incentives offered to the subjects for
the food away from the trap,” which could successful performance. Children presented
have been learned during the previous tri- with the same task also show a gravity bias,
als. Unfortunately, the authors did not in- although older children can eventually over-
vert the trap as was previously done with come it (Hood, Care, & Prasada, 2000). In a
capuchins. follow-up experiment, Hauser et al. (2001 )
In summary, this section has shown reported that when the reward trajectory
that various primates have some knowledge was horizontal rather than vertical (as in the
about causal relations regarding what makes original test), tamarins performed better and
a tool effective. They know that objects have the biases observed previously disappeared.
to be in contact for a tool to be effective, rec- Also subjects with experience with the hor-
ognize the relevant and irrelevant functional izontal version of the task performed bet-
features of a tool, and can choose the ap- ter in the original task (i.e., free-fall reward)
propriate dimensions of an effective tool in than subjects without such experience, even
a particular task. Nevertheless, these studies though the gravity bias was still apparent.
have also shown clear limitations, perhaps Taken together, these results suggest that
62 4 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

tamarins have a pervasive gravity bias that ure was especially striking because subjects
impairs their search for hidden objects. were tested after they had solved the initial
Hauser (2001 ) investigated the same problem. Second, their performance in this
topic with a different paradigm. He pre- tapping test was comparable to performance
sented rhesus macaques with a table and two in learning novel stimuli with arbitrary rela-
boxes. The first box was placed on top of the tions – for instance, learning that a green cup
table and the second box was placed under has food and a yellow cup does not. Sub-
the table right under the first box. The exper- jects responded correctly to the auditory cue
imenter then raised a screen that occluded when it held a causal connection to the food
both boxes (and the table) and dropped a re- but failed to do so when the auditory cue
ward over the top box. Because of the screen, held a noncausal connection to the food.
the monkeys never saw where the reward In a second study, Call (unpublished data)
entered the box, they just saw it falling to- investigated the ability of apes to use the
ward the table and disappearing behind the shape of objects to locate food. In the initial
screen. Monkeys searched for the food in problem, he presented two rectangular trays
the bottom box, thus showing a gravity bias. on a platform and hid a piece of food un-
Control tests indicated that subjects did not der one of them. One of the trays therefore
have a preference for the bottom box, nor rested flat on the platform whereas the other
did they avoid the top box in the absence rested in an inclined orientation (because of
of the reward drop. Interestingly, Santos and the food placed under it). Subjects selected
Hauser (2002) found that rhesus monkeys preferentially the inclined tray but failed to
tested with the same paradigm but using a do so in a control test in which the inclined
violation of expectation measures solved this tray was substituted by a wooden wedge
problem. In other words, subjects looked that produced the same visual effect as the
longer in trials in which the reward appeared inclined tray. This result was important be-
on the bottom (apparently going through a cause it ruled out the possibility that subjects
solid partition) than in trials in which the simply preferred the perceptual appearance
reward stayed on top of the partition. of the inclined tray, perhaps because it had
Call (2004) recently investigated two been reinforced in the past. More impor-
other aspects of the object knowledge that tantly, subjects failed to select the wedge,
subjects may use to find food. The first is an arbitrary stimulus, after they were suc-
whether apes know that food inside a con- cessful in the inclined tray test. This result
tainer when shaken makes a noise. He found is analogous to that of the previous study
that apes are capable of using the noise in which subjects failed to respond above
made by shaking food to identify the correct chance to stimuli with noncausal connec-
container (see inferential reasoning section). tions to the food after they had succeeded
Although one may argue that this simply with very similar stimuli with causal connec-
involves detecting an association between tions. It therefore is found again that when
the food and the cue rather than an un- there are arbitrary (i.e., noncausal) relations
derstanding that the food causes the noise, between the food and the elements of the
there are several lines of evidence that sug- problem, subjects perform poorly compared
gest that this interpretation oversimplifies with when the connection is nonarbitrary
the phenomenon. First, subjects performed (i.e., causal). It is unlikely that these results
well from the beginning, with no evidence are based solely on learning to associate a
of gradual improvement over trials. If sub- cue with a response without any insight into
jects had learned to associate a noise with the structure of the problem. Instead, it is
food in the past, it is unclear why, in control conceivable that subjects understood that it
tests, they failed to associate a noise made by was the food that caused the noise or made
tapping the baited cup, which was compara- the tray incline, not simply that the food was
ble to that made by shaking the food inside associated with the presence of the noise or
the cup, with the presence of food. This fail- the shape.
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 62 5

In summary, primates have some knowl- The basic finding was that subordinates
edge about the physical properties of did go for food only they could see much
objects, and they can use this knowledge to more often than they went for food that both
predict the location in which rewards can be they and the dominant could see. In some
found. cases, the subordinate may have been mon-
itoring the behavior of the dominant, but in
other cases this possibility was ruled out by
giving subordinates a small headstart, forcing
Social Reasoning them to make their choice (to go to the food
that both competitors could see, or to go to
Primates’ social cognition represents a large the food that only they could see) before the
area of research in its own right. At the dominant was released into the area. More-
most basic level, it involves how individu- over, we ran two other control conditions. In
als understand and predict the behavior and, one, the dominant’s door was lowered before
perhaps, the perceptual activities of others. the two competitors were let into the room
Of course, it also may involve how indi- (and again the subordinate got a small head-
viduals understand the psychological states start), so that the subordinate could not see
and activities of others, which are less di- which piece the dominant was looking at un-
rectly observable. So the question is whether der the door (i.e., it is possible that in the first
primates can reason about the psycholog- studies the subordinate saw that the domi-
ical states and activities of others. Despite nant was looking at the out-in-the-open food
many richly interpreted anecdotes, until re- and so went for the other piece). The re-
cently there was very little evidence that sults were clear. Subordinates preferentially
primates reasoned about what others were targeted the hidden piece. In the other con-
seeing, intending, wanting, and thinking (see trol study, we followed the same basic pro-
Tomasello & Call, 1 997, for a review). Some cedure as before (one piece of food in the
recent studies have demonstrated that pri- open, one on the subordinate’s side of a bar-
mates can reason about some – although rier) but we used a transparent barrier that
clearly not all – of the psychological states did not prevent the dominant from seeing
about which humans reason. the food behind it. In this case, chimpanzees
Hare et al. (2000) placed a subordinate chose equally between the two pieces of
and a dominant chimpanzee into rooms on food, seeming to know that the transparent
opposite sides of a third room. Each had a barrier was not serving to block the dom-
guillotine door leading into the third room inant’s visual access (and so her “control”
which, when cracked at the bottom, allowed of the food). The findings of these studies
them to observe two pieces of food at vari- suggest that chimpanzees know what con-
ous locations within that room – and to see specifics can and cannot see and, further, that
the other individual looking under her door. they use this knowledge to make inferences
After the food had been placed, the doors about what their competitor is about to do
for both individuals were opened and they next.
were allowed to enter the third room. The In a follow-up study, Hare, Call, and
basic problem for the subordinate in this sit- Tomasello (2001 ) investigated whether
uation is that the dominant will take all of chimpanzees were also able to take into ac-
the food it can see. In some cases, however, count past information such as whether the
things were arranged so that the subordinate dominant had seen the baiting. For these
could see a piece of food that the dominant experiments, two barriers and one piece of
could not see – because it was on her side of food were used, and what the dominant
a small barrier. The question in these cases saw was manipulated. In experimental tri-
was whether the subordinate knew that the als, dominants had not seen the food hid-
dominant could not see a particular piece of den, or food they had seen hidden was
food, so it was safe for her to go for it. moved to a different location when they
62 6 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

were not watching, whereas in control trials, quite often have some components, or some
they saw the food being hidden or moved. aspects, of the human version, and in some
Subordinates, on the other hand, always cases they possess skills that humans do not
saw the entire baiting procedure and could have or do not have to the same degree (e.g.,
monitor the visual access of the dominant some of the memory skills demonstrated by
competitor. Subordinates preferentially re- food-caching birds; Shettleworth, 1 998). We
trieved and approached the food that domi- need to compare the skills in detail if we
nants had not seen hidden or moved, which want to provide an anatomy of their struc-
suggests that subordinates were sensitive to ture from an evolutionary point of view.
what dominants had or had not seen during Because this book is mainly about human
baiting a few moments before. In this case, reasoning and thinking, we should conclude
deciding which piece of food to approach with a word about what we believe makes
depended on the subordinate’s making infer- human cognition different from that of other
ences about what the dominant knew about primates. The answer, in a word, is culture
the situation. (Tomasello & Call, 1 997; Tomasello, 1 999).
These studies of what may be called so- The thought experiment we use to demon-
cial problem solving demonstrate that some strate the point is to imagine a human child
primates may make inferential leaps not raised on a desert island without any social
just about directly perceivable things, but contacts. Our contention is that in adult-
also about less observable things such as hood this adult’s cognitive skills would not
what others do and do not see, or even differ very much – perhaps a little, but not
have or have not seen in the immediate very much – from those of other great apes.
past. This person would certainly not invent by
him or herself a natural language, or algebra
or calculus, or science or government. The
human cognitive skills that make the most
Conclusions and Future Directions difference are those that enable individu-
als of the species Homo sapiens, in a sense,
There was a time when the dominant view to pool their cognitive resources – to create
in the Western intellectual tradition was that and participate in collective cultural activ-
human beings were rational and all other an- ities and products. When viewed from the
imals were simply preprogrammed brutes or perspective of the individual mind, the cog-
automata. That view is demonstrably false. nitive skills necessary for cultural creation
All the evidence reviewed in this chapter and learning may not differ so very much
suggests that nonhuman primates interact from those of other primate species.
with their worlds in many creative ways, re- In any case, much can be learned about
lying on a variety of cognitive processes to do human cognition by looking at how it is
so. They reason and make inferences about similar to and how it is different from that
space, causality, objects, quantities, and the of closely related species. We hope to have
psychological states of other individuals, and shown in this chapter that, in many fun-
in some cases they can engage in relational damental respects, human cognition is sim-
and analogical reasoning concerning partic- ply one form of primate cognition. The vast
ular objects or even categories of objects. gulf that seems to separate what humans and
The main pitfall to avoid in attempting to other primates can do cognitively – in the do-
integrate our knowledge about the cognitive main of mathematics, as just one instance –
skills of other animals with our knowledge in many, if not most, cases is the result of
about human cognition is oversimplification. fairly small differences of individual psy-
Asking dichotomously whether or not ani- chology that enable humans to accumulate
mals reason or think or have a theory of mind knowledge across generations and to use col-
generally is not very useful (Tomasello, Call, lective artifacts such as linguistic and math-
& Hare, 2003 a, 2003 b). Nonhuman animals ematical symbols.
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 62 7

Acknowledgment Bovet, D., & Vauclair, J. (2001 ). Judgment of con-


ceptual identity in monkeys. Psychonomic Bul-
The authors wish to thank Keith Holyoak letin and Review, 8, 470–475 .
for comments made on an earlier draft of Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1 985 ). Culture and the
this chapter. Evolutionary Process. Chicago: The University
of Chicago Press.
Boysen, S. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1 989). Nu-
merical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan
References troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology,
1 03 , 23 –3 1 .
Anderson, J. R. & Henneman, M. C. (1 995 ). So- Boysen, S. T., Berntson, G. G., Shreyer, T. A., &
lutions to a tool-use problem in a pair of Cebus Hannan, M. B. (1 995 ). Indicating acts during
apella. Mammalia, 5 8, 3 5 1 –3 61 . counting by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes).
Baillargeon, R. (1 995 ). Physical reasoning in in- Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1 09,
fancy. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive 47–5 1 .
Neurosciences (pp. 1 81 –204) Cambridge, MA: Boysen, S. T., Berntson, G. G., Shreyer, T. A.,
MIT Press. & Quigley, K. S. (1 993 ). Processing of ordi-
Bard, K. A., Fragaszy, D. M., & Visalberghi, nality and transitivity by chimpanzees (Pan
E. (1 995 ). Acquisition and comprehension of troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology,
a tool-using behavior by young chimpanzees 1 07, 208–21 5 .
(Pan troglodytes): Effects of age and modelling. Brannon, E. M., & Terrace, H. S. (1 998). Ordering
International Journal of Comparative Psychol- of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. Science,
ogy, 8, 47–68. 2 82 , 746–749.
Beck, B. B. (1 980). Animal Tool Behavior. New Brannon, E. M., & Terrace, H. S. (2000). Rep-
York: Garland Press. resentation of the numerosities 1 –9 by Rhesus
Beran, M. J. (2001 ). Summation and numerous- macaques (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Exper-
ness judgments of sequentially presented sets of imental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,
items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal 2 6, 3 1 –49.
of Comparative Psychology, 1 1 5 , 1 81 –1 91 . Burdyn, L. E., & Thomas, R. K. (1 984). Condi-
Beran, M. J., & Minahan, M. F. (2000). Moni- tional discrimination with conceptual simulta-
toring spatial transpositions by bonobos (Pan neous and successive cues in the squirrel mon-
paniscus) and chimpanzees (P. troglodytes). In- key (Saimiri sciureus). Journal of Comparative
ternational Journal of Comparative Psychology, Psychology, 98, 405 –41 3 .
1 3 , 1 –1 5 . Call, J. (2000). Estimating and operating on
Beran, M. J., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (2001 ). “Con- discrete quantities in orangutans (Pongo pyg-
structive” enumeration by chimpanzees (Pan maeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology,
troglodytes) on a computerized task. Animal 1 1 4, 1 3 6–1 47.
Cognition, 4, 81 –89. Call, J. (2001 ). Object permanence in orangu-
Beran, M. J., & Washburn, D. A. (2002) Chim- tans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan
panzee responding during matching to sample: troglodytes), and children (Homo sapiens). Jour-
Control by exclusion. Journal of the Experimen- nal of Comparative Psychology, 1 1 5 , 1 5 9–
tal Analysis of Behavior, 78, 497–5 08. 1 71 .
Bernstein, I. S. (1 961 ). The utilization of visual Call, J. (2003 ). Spatial rotations and transposi-
cues in dimension-abstracted oddity by pri- tions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and chim-
mates. Journal of Comparative and Physiological panzees (Pan troglodytes). Primates, 44, 3 47–
Psychology, 5 4, 243 –247. 3 5 7.
Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1 984). Mental map Call, J. (2004). Inferences about the location of
in wild chimpanzees: An analysis of hammer food in the great apes. Journal of Comparative
transports for nut cracking. Primates, 2 5 , 1 60– Psychology, 1 1 8, 23 2–241 .
1 70. Call, J., & Rochat, P. (1 996). Liquid conservation
Bond, A. B., Kamil, A. C., & Balda, R. P. (2003 ). in orangutans. Individual differences and per-
Social complexity and transitive inference in ceptual strategies Journal of Comparative Psy-
Corvids. Animal Behaviour, 65 , 479–487. chology, 1 00, 21 9–23 2.
62 8 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

Call, J., & Rochat, P. (1 997). Perceptual strate- Fagot, J., Wasserman, E. A. & Young, M. E.
gies in the estimation of physical quantities by (2001 ). Discriminating the relation between
orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Com- relations: The role of entropy in abstract con-
parative Psychology, 1 1 1 , 3 1 5 –3 29. ceptualization by baboons (Papio papio) and
Cramer, A. E., & Gallistel, C. R. (1 997). Vervet humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Experimen-
monkeys as travelling salesmen. Nature, 3 87, tal Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2 7,
464. 3 1 6–3 28.
D’Amato, M. R., & Colombo, M. (1 985 ). Au- Fujita, K. (1 982). An analysis of stimulus con-
ditory matching-to-sample in monkeys (Ce- trol in two-color matching-to-sample behav-
bus apella). Animal Learning and Behavior, 1 3 , iors of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata fus-
3 75 –3 82. cata). Japanese Psychological Research, 2 4, 1 24–
135.
D’Amato, M. R., & Colombo, M. (1 988). Rep-
resentation of serial order in monkeys (Cebus Fujita, K. (1 983 ). Formation of the sameness-
apella). Journal of Experimental Psychology: An- difference concept by Japanese monkeys from
imal Behavior Processes, 1 4, 1 3 1 –1 3 9. a small number of color stimuli. Journal of
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 289–
D’Amato, M. R., & Colombo, M. (1 989). Serial
3 00.
learning with wild card items by monkeys (Ce-
bus apella): Implications for knowledge of ordi- Garber, P. (1 989). Role of spatial memory in pri-
nal position. Journal of Comparative Psychology, mate foraging patterns: Saguinus mystax and
1 03 , 25 2–261 . Saguinus fuscicollis. American Journal of Prima-
tology, 1 9, 203 –21 6.
D’Amato, M. R., & Salmon, D. P. (1 984). Cog-
nitive processes in cebus monkeys. In H. L. Gillan, D. J. (1 981 ). Reasoning in the chimpanzee:
Roitblat, T. G. Bever, & H. S. Terrace (Eds.), II. Transitive inference. Journal of Experimental
Animal Cognition (pp. 1 49–1 68). Hillsdale, NJ: Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 1 5 0–
Erlbaum. 1 64.
D’Amato, M. R., Salmon, D. P., Loukas, E., & Gillan, D. J., Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1 981 ).
Tomie, A. (1 985 ). Symmetry and transitiv- Reasoning in the chimpanzee: I. Analogical rea-
ity of conditional relations in monkeys (Cebus soning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: An-
apella) and pigeons (Columba livia). Journal of imal Behavior Processes, 7, 1 –1 7.
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 44, 3 5 – Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M.,
47. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics
D’Amato, M. R., Salmon, D. P., Loukas, E., & do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 5 9, 771 –
Tomie, A. (1 986). Processing of identity and 785 .
conditional relations in monkeys (Cebus apella) Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M., (2001 ). Do
and pigeons (Columba livia). Animal Learning chimpanzees know what conspecifics know
and Behavior, 1 4, 3 65 –3 73 . and do not know? Animal Behaviour, 61 , 1 3 9–
De Blois, S. T., & Novak, M. A. (1 994). Object 151.
permanence in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mu- Harlow, H. F. (1 95 9). The development of learn-
latta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1 08, ing in the rhesus monkey. American Scientist,
3 1 8–3 27. 47, 45 9–479.
De Blois, S. T., Novak, M. A., & Bond, M. (1 998). Hashiya, K., & Kojima, S. (2001 ). Hearing and
Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pyg- auditory-visual intermodal recognition in the
maeus) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sci- chimpanzee. In T. Matsuzawa (Ed.). Primate
ureus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1 1 2 , Origins of Human Cognition and Behavior
1 3 7–1 5 2. (pp. 1 5 5 –1 89). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
DeLoache, J. S. (1 995 ). Early understanding and Hauser, M. D., Kralik, J., & Botto-Mahan, C.
use of symbols: The model model. Currents Di- (1 999). Problem solving and functional design
rections in Psychological Science, 4, 1 09–1 1 3 . features: Experiments on cotton-top tamarins,
Dumas, C. & Brunet, C. (1 994). Pernamence de Saguinus oedipus. Animal Behaviour, 5 7, 5 65 –
l’objet chez le singe capucin (Cebus apella): 5 82.
Etude des desplacements invisibles. Revue Hauser, M., Pearson, H., & Seelig, D. (2002).
Canadienne de psychologie experimentale, 48, Ontogeny of tool use in cotton-top tamarins,
3 41 –3 5 7. Saguinus oedipus: Innate recognition of
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 62 9

functionally relevant features. Animal Be- King, J. E. (1 973 ). Learning and generalization
haviour, 64, 299–3 1 1 . of a two-dimensional sameness-difference con-
Hauser, M. D. (1 997). Artifactual kinds and func- cept by chimpanzees and orangutans. Journal of
tional design features: What a primate under- Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 84,
stands without language. Cognition, 64, 285 – 1 40–1 48.
3 08. Köhler, W. (1 925 ). The Mentality of Apes. London:
Hauser, M. D. (2001 ). Searching for food in Routledge & Kegan Paul.
the wild: A nonhuman primate’s expectations Kojima, T. (1 979). Discriminative stimulus con-
about invisible displacement. Developmental text in matching-to-sample of Japanese mon-
Science, 4, 84–93 . keys. Japanese Psychological Research, 2 1 , 1 89–
Hauser, M. D., Santos, L. R., Spaepen, G. M., & 1 94.
Pearson, H. E. (2002). Problem solving, inhi- Kuhlmeier, V. A., Boysen, S. T., & Mukobi, K. L.
bition and domain-specific experience: Exper- (1 999). Scale-model comprehension by chim-
iments on cotton-top tamarins, Saguinus oedi- panzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Compara-
pus. Animal Behaviour, 64, 3 87–3 96. tive Psychology, 1 1 3 , 3 96–402.
Hauser, M. D., Williams, T., Kralik, J. D., & Kuhlmeier, V. A., & Boysen, S. T. (2001 ).
Moskovitz, D. (2001 ). What guides a search The effect of response contingencies on scale
for food that has disappeared? Experiments model task performance by chimpanzees (Pan
on cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology,
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1 1 5 , 1 40– 1 1 5 , 3 00–3 06.
151. Limongelli, L., Boysen, S. T., & Visalberghi, E.
Hood, B., Carey, S. & Prasada, S. (2000). Pre- (1 995 ). Comprehension of cause-effect rela-
dicting the outcomes of physical events: Two- tions in a tool-using task by chimpanzees (Pan
year-olds fail to reveal knowledge of solidity troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology,
and support. Child Development, 71 , 1 5 40– 1 09, 1 8–26.
1 5 5 4. Lipsett, L. P., & Serunian, S. A. (1 963 ). Oddity-
Hood, B. M., Hauser, M. D., Anderson, L., & problem learning in young children. Child De-
Santos, L. (1 999). Gravity biases in a non- velopment, 3 4, 201 –206.
human primate? Developmental Science, 2 , 3 5 – MacDonald, S. E., Pang, J. C., & Gibeault, S.
41 . (1 994). Marmoset (Callithrix jacchus jacchus)
Iversen, I. H., & Matsuzawa, T. (2001 ). Ac- spatial memory in a foraging task: Win-stay ver-
quisition of navigation by chimpanzees (Pan sus win-shift strategies. Journal of Comparative
troglodytes) in an automated fingermaze task. Psychology, 1 08, 3 28–3 3 4.
Animal Cognition, 4, 1 79–1 92. MacDonald, S. E., & Wilkie, D. M. (1 990).
Jackson, W. J., & Pegram, G. V. (1 970a). Ac- Yellow-nosed monkeys’ (Cercopithecus asca-
quisition, transfer and retention of matching nius whitesidei) spatial memory in a simulated
by Rhesus monkeys. Psychological Reports, 2 7, foraging environment. Journal of Comparative
83 9–846. Psychology, 1 04, 3 82–3 87.
Jackson, W. J., & Pegram, G. V. (1 970b). Com- McGonigle, B. O., & Chalmers, M. (1 977). Are
parison of intra- vs. extradimensional transfer monkeys logical?. Nature, 2 67, 694–696.
of matching by Rhesus monkeys. Psychonomic Menzel, E. W. Jr. (1 973 ). Chimpanzee spa-
Science, 1 9, 1 62–1 63 . tial memory organization. Science, 1 82 , 943 –
Kawai, N., & Matsuzawa, T. (2000). Numerical 945 .
memory span in a chimpanzee. Nature, 403 , Muncer, S. J. (1 983 ). “Conservations” with a
3 9–40. chimpanzee. Developmental Psychobiology, 1 6,
King, J. E., & Fobes, J. L. (1 975 ). Hypothesis anal- 1 –1 1 .
ysis of sameness-difference learning-set by ca- Natale, F. (1 989). Causality II: The stick problem.
puchin monkeys. Learning and Motivation, 6, In F. Antinucci (Ed.), Cognitive Structure and
1 01 –1 1 3 . Development in Nonhuman Primates (pp. 1 21 –
King, J. E., & Fobes, J. L. (1 982). Complex learn- 1 3 3 ). Hilldsale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ing by primates. In J. L. Fobes, & J. E. King Natale, F., Antinucci, F., Spinozzi, G., & Potı̀,
(Eds.), Primate Behavior. (pp. 3 27–3 60). New P. (1 986). Stage 6 object concept in nonhu-
York: Academic Press. man primate cognition: A comparison between
630 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and Japanese difference discrimination problem. Journal of
macaque (Macaca fuscata). Journal of Compar- Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 5 3 ,
ative Psychology, 1 00, 3 3 5 –3 3 9. 3 68–3 70.
Nissen, H. W., Blum, J. S., & Blum, R. A. (1 948). Rumbaugh, D. M., & McCormack. (1 967). The
Analysis of matching behavior in chimpanzee. learning skills of primates: A comparative study
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- of apes and monkeys. In D. Starck, R. Schhei-
chology, 41 , 62–74. der, & H. J. Ruhn (Eds.), Progress in Prima-
Oden, D. L., Thompson, R. K. R., & Premack, tology (pp. 289–3 06). Stuggart: Gustav Fischer
D. (1 988). Spontaneous transfer of matching Verlag.
by infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Jour- Rumbaugh, D. M. (1 970). Learning skills of an-
nal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior thropoids. In L. A. Rosenblum (Ed.), Primate
Processes, 1 4, 1 40–1 45 . Behavior: Developments in Field and Labora-
Oden, D. L., Thompson, R. K. R., & Premack, D. tory Research (pp. 1 –70). New York: Academic
(1 990). Infant chimpanzees spontaneously per- Press.
ceive both concrete and abstract same/different Rumbaugh, D. M., Hopkins, W. D., Washburn,
relations. Child Development, 61 , 621 – D. A., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1 989). Lana
63 1 . chimpanzee learns to count by “Numath”: A
Olthof, A., Iden, C. M., & Roberts, W. A. summary of videotaped experimental report.
(1 997). Judgments of ordinality and summa- Psychological Record, 3 9, 45 9–470.
tion of number symbols by squirrel monkeys Rumbaugh, D. M., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., &
(Saimiri sciureus). Journal of Experimental Psy- Hegel, M. T. (1 987). Summation in the chim-
chology Animal Behavior Processes, 2 3 , 3 25 – panzee (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Experimen-
3 3 9. tal Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1 3 ,
Pepperberg, I. M. (1 999). The Alex studies. 1 07–1 1 5 .
Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey Rumbaugh, D. M., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., &
parrots. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Pate, J. L. (1 988). Addendum to “Summation
Press. in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)”. Journal of
Perusse, R., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1 990). Summa- Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro-
tion in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Effects cesses, 1 4, 1 1 8–1 20.
of amounts, number of wells, and finer ratios. Santos, L. R., & Hauser, M. D. (2002). A non-
International Journal of Primatology, 1 1 , 425 – human primate’s understanding of solidity:
43 7. Dissociations between seeing and acting. De-
Piaget, J. (1 95 2). The Origins of Intelligence in Chil- velopmental Science, 5 , 1 –7.
dren. New York: Norton. Schino, G., Spinozzi, G., & Berlinguer, L. (1 990).
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1 941 ). Le développement Object concept and mental representation in
des quantités physiques chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel, Cebus apella and Macaca fascicularis. Primates,
Switzerland: Delachaux; and Paris: Niestlé. 3 1 , 5 3 7–5 44.
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1 994). Levels of Shettleworth, S. J. (1 998). Cognition, Evolution,
causal understanding in chimpanzees and chil- and Behavior. New York: Oxford University
dren. Cognition, 5 0, 3 47–3 62. Press.
Premack, D. (1 976). Intelligence in Ape and Man. Sigg, H. (1 986). Ranging patterns in hamadryas
Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum. baboons: Evidence for a mental map. In J. G.
Premack, D. (1 983 ). The codes of man and Else, & P. C. Lee (Eds.), Primate Ontogeny, Cog-
beasts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 1 25 – nition and Social Behaviour (pp. 87–91 ). Cam-
1 67. bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Roberts, W. A. (1 998). Principles of Animal Cog- Smith, H. J., King, J. E., Witt, E. D., & Rickel, J. E.
nition. Boston: McGraw-Hill. (1 975 ). Sameness–difference matching from
Robinson, J. S. (1 95 5 ). The sameness-difference sample by chimpanzees. Bulletin of the Psycho-
discrimination problem in chimpanzee. Jour- nomic Society, 6, 469–471 .
nal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A., & Woodward, A. L.
48, 1 95 –1 97. (1 995 ). Infants’ knowledge of object motion
Robinson, J. S. (1 960). The conceptual basis of the and human action. In D. Sperber, D. Premack
chimpanzee’s performance on the sameness- & A. J. Premack (Eds.). Causal Cognition.
reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates 631

A Multidisciplinary Debate (pp. 44–78). New question is which ones and to what extent.
York: Oxford University Press. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 1 5 3 –1 5 6.
Spinozzi, G. & Potı̀, P. (1 989). Causality I: The Tomasello, M., Call, J. & Hare, B. (2003 b). Chim-
support problem. In F. Antinucci (Ed.), Cog- panzees versus humans: It’s not that simple.
nitive Structure and Development in Nonhu- Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 23 9–240.
man Primates (pp. 1 1 3 –1 1 9). Hilldsale, NJ: Vauclair, J. (1 996). Animal Cognition. An In-
Erlbaum. troduction to Modern Comparative Cognition.
Spinozzi, G. & Potı̀, P. (1 993 ). Piagetian Stage 5 in Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
two infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The Visalberghi, E. (1 993 ). Tool use in a South
development of permanence of objects and the American monkey species: An overview of the
spatialization of causality. International Journal characteristics and limits of tool use in Cebus
of Primatology, 1 4, 905 –91 7. apella. In A. Berthelet, & J. Chavaillon (Eds.),
Suda, C. & Call, J. (in press). Piagetian liquid con- The Use of Tools by Human and Non-Human Pri-
servation in the great apes. Journal of Compar- mates (pp. 1 1 8–1 3 1 ). New York: Oxford Uni-
ative Psychology. versity Press.
Sulkowski, G. M & Hauser, M. D. (2001 ). Can Visalberghi, E., Fragaszy, D. M., & Savage-
Rhesus monkeys spontaneously subtract? Cog- Rumbaugh, E. S. (1 995 ). Performance in
nition, 79, 23 9–262. a tool-using task by common chimpanzees
Swartz, K., Chen, S., & Terrace, H. (1 991 ). Serial (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), an
learning by rhesus monkeys I: Acquisition and orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and capuchin
retention of multiple four item lists. Journal of monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Compara-
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro- tive Psychology, 1 09, 5 2–60.
cesses, 1 7, 3 96–41 0. Visalberghi, E., & Limongelli, L. (1 994). Lack
Thomas, R. K., & Boyd, M. G. (1 973 ). A com- of comprehension of cause-effect relations in
parison of Cebus albifrons and Saimiri sciureus tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
on oddity performance. Animal Learning and Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1 08, 1 5 –
Behavior, 1 , 1 5 1 –1 5 3 . 22.
Thomas, R. K., & Frost, T. (1 983 ). Oddity and Visalberghi, E., & Trinca, L. (1 989). Tool use
dimension-abstracted oddity (DAO) in squirrel in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between
monkeys. American Journal of Psychology, 96, performing and understanding. Primates, 3 0,
5 1 –64. 5 1 1 –5 21 .
Thomas, R. K., & Peay, L. (1 976). Length Vonk, J. (2003 ). Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)
judgments by squirrel monkeys: Evidence for and orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of
conservation? Developmental Psychology, 1 2 , first- and second-order relations. Animal Cog-
3 49–3 5 2. nition, 6, 77–86.
Thompson, R. K. R., & Oden, D. L. (2000). Cat- Washburn, D. A. (1 992). Human factors with
egorical perception and conceptual judgments nonhumans: Factors that affect computer-task
by nonhuman primates: The paleological mon- performance. International Journal of Compar-
key and the analogical ape. Cognitive Science, ative Psychology, 5 , 1 91 –204.
2 4, 3 63 –3 96. Washburn, D. A., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1 991 ).
Thompson, R. K. R., Oden, D. L., & Boysen, Ordinal judgements of numerical symbols by
S. T. (1 997). Language-naive chimpanzees (Pan macaques (Macaca mulatta). Psychological Sci-
troglodytes) judge relations between relations ence, 2 , 1 90–1 93 .
in a conceptual matching-to-sample task. Jour- Washburn, D. A., & Rumbaugh, D. M.
nal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior (1 992). Comparative assessment of psychomo-
Processes, 2 3 , 3 1 –43 . tor performance: Target prediction by humans
Tomasello, M. (1 999). The cultural origins of hu- and macaques (Macaca mulatta). Journal of
man cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- Experimental Psychology: General, 1 2 1 , 3 05 –
versity Press. 3 1 2.
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1 997). Primate Cogni- Weinstein, B. (1 941 ). Matching-from-sample by
tion. New York: Oxford University Press. Rhesus monkeys and by children. Journal of
Tomasello, M., Call, J. & Hare, B. (2003 a). Chim- Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 3 1 ,
panzees understand psychological states – the 1 95 –21 3 .
632 the cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning

Woodruff, G., Premack, D., & Kennel, K. probe delay effects. Journal of Experimental
(1 978). Conservation of liquid and solid quan- Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1 0, 5 1 3 –
tity by the chimpanzee. Science, 2 02 , 991 – 5 29.
994. Wright, A. A., Shyan, M., & Jitsumori, M.
Wright, A. A., Santiago, H. C., & Sands, S. F. (1 990). Auditory same/different concept learn-
(1 984). Monkey memory: Same/different con- ing by monkeys. Animal Learning and Behavior,
cept learning, serial probe acquisition, and 1 8, 287–294.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai