Anda di halaman 1dari 8

Diener, E. (2009). The science of well-being: The collected works of Ed these raise.

e raise. Worldwide predictors of SWB such as social support


Diener (Social Indicators Research Series, Vol. 37). Dordrecht, The
Netherlands: Springer. and fulfillment of basic needs have been uncovered, and there
Diener, E. (2009). Culture and well-being: The collected works of Ed are large differences in SWB between societies. A number of
Diener (Social Indicators Research Series, Vol. 38). Dordrecht, The culture-specific predictors of SWB have also been found. Re-
Netherlands: Springer.
Diener, E. (2009). Assessing well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener search on social comparison suggests that a world standard for
(Social Indicators Research Series, Vol. 39). Dordrecht, The Netherlands, a desirable income has developed. New findings on adaptation
Springer. indicate that habituation to conditions is not always complete
Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking the myster-
ies of psychological wealth. Malden, MA: Wiley/Blackwell. and that circumstances in some cases can have a large and
Diener, E., Diener, M., & Diener, C. (1995). Factors predicting the subjec- lasting effect on SWB. An important finding is that high SWB
tive well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, benefits health, longevity, citizenship, and social relationships.
69, 851– 864. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.851
Diener, E., & Eid, M. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of multimethod measurement Because of the benefits of SWB as well as the strong effects
in psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. societal conditions can have on it, I proposed national accounts
Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Tov, W., & Arora, R. (2010). Income’s associ- of SWB, which are now being seriously considered by nations.
ation with judgments of life versus feelings. In E. Diener, J. Helliwell, &
D. Kahneman (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp. 3–15). Finally, I review advances in methodology that are needed to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/ move beyond conclusions based on simple cross-sectional cor-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

9780199732739.003.0001 relations based on global self-report scales. Each of the findings


Diener, E., Lucas, R., Schimmack, U., & Helliwell, J. (2009). Well-being for
public policy. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: raises new and important questions for future research.
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195334074.001.0001
Diener, E., Lucas, R., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). Beyond the hedonic Subjective well-being (SWB) represents people’s evaluations
treadmill: Revising the adaptation theory of well-being. American Psy- of their lives, both in terms of cognitions (e.g., “My life is
chologist, 61, 305–314. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.4.305 satisfying”) and feelings (e.g., “My experiences are pleasant
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond money: Toward an
economy of well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, and rewarding”). SWB represents people’s beliefs and feel-
1–31. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00501001.x ings about whether they are leading a desirable and rewarding
Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (Eds.). (2000). Culture and subjective well-being. life. Three decades ago (Diener, 1984) I reviewed the field of
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. E. (1999). Subjective SWB, and in 1999 I published an updated review that de-
well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276 – scribed the advances since the time of Wilson’s 1967 review
302. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276
Diener, E., Tay, L., & Myers, D. (2011). The religion paradox: If religion
(Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). In this article I describe
makes people happy, why are so many dropping out? Journal of Person- new discoveries that refine our understanding of SWB, and I
ality and Social Psychology, 101, 1278 –1290. doi:10.1037/a0024402 list important issues that these findings raise for future re-
Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. W., Oishi, S., &
Biswas-Diener, R. (2010). New well-being measures: Short scales to
search. My hope is that investigators will embrace the new
assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Social Indicators developments that are emerging by advancing research in
Research, 97, 143–156. doi:10.1007/s11205-009-9493-y these areas.
Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). (1999). Well-being: The
foundations of hedonic psychology. New York, NY: Sage.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent Universal Versus Culture-Specific
positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, Causes of Subjective Well-Being
131, 803– 855. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (2008). The Satisfaction With Life Scale and the Early studies of SWB were conducted in wealthier, Westernized
emerging construct of life satisfaction. Journal of Positive Psychology, 3, nations. Thus, there was little knowledge of whether SWB
137–152. doi:10.1080/17439760701756946
Tov, W., & Diener, E. (2007). Culture and subjective well-being. In S. findings generalized across cultures. With the advent of large
Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. international studies such as the World Values Survey and the
691–713). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Gallup World Poll, there are now large and representative sam-

Editor’s Note. Ed Diener received the Award for Distinguished Scientific


New Findings and Future Directions for Contributions. Award winners are invited to deliver an award address at the
APA’s annual convention. This article is based on the award address
Subjective Well-Being Research presented at the 120th annual meeting, held August 2–5, 2012, in Orlando,
Florida. Articles based on award addresses are reviewed, but they differ
Ed Diener from unsolicited articles in that they are expressions of the winners’ reflec-
University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign and The tions on their work and their views of the field.
Gallup Organization, Omaha, Nebraska
Author’s Note. My sincere gratitude is expressed to Maike Luhmann,
Shige Oishi, Richard Lucas, Ulrich Schimmack, and Michael Eid for their
DOI: 10.1037/a0029541 suggestions on this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed toEd Die-
ner, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana–Cham-
Recent findings on subjective well-being (SWB) are presented, paign, 603 East Daniel Street, Champaign, IL 61820. E-mail:
and I describe the important questions for future research that ediener@illinois.edu

590 November 2012 ● American Psychologist


ples from diverse nations. Certain predictors of SWB do gener- and thus less subject to shifting standards? Why are some
alize across cultures. For example, feelings of social support, cultures happier? What is the explanation for the culture–
trust, and mastery predict individual SWB in all areas of the person fit findings?
world. Personality, as well as the fulfillment of basic needs, also Standards Used for Life Evaluations
predicts SWB across the globe.
Social Comparison
Mean-Level Differences Between Societies
One important influence on how people evaluate the condi-
Many of the large differences in SWB between countries can tions of their lives, beyond circumstances and personality, is
be explained by variations in conditions such as adequate the judgment standards they use. People might use their
food, health, lack of corruption, and so forth. There is evi- desires as a standard for evaluating their lives. For instance,
dence, however, that some cultural regions tend to be happier people’s income aspirations can outstrip their rising incomes
than others, even when one controls for conditions such as so that even successful individuals can feel dissatisfied (e.g.,
income. For example, Latin American nations tend to report Graham & Pettinato, 2006). Individuals might also use social
higher SWB than the nations of East Asia when material comparison, judging their life circumstances on the basis of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

conditions are controlled. Some explanations for this include how other people are doing. Findings from my laboratory and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cultural standards for feeling and expressing positive emo- those of others suggests that social comparison effects on
tions (Eid & Diener, 2001), differences in social support SWB do not fully follow from the theory proposed by Fest-
(Oishi & Schimmack, 2010), and differences in positivity inger (1954), which stated that individuals compare them-
(Diener, Scollon, Oishi, Dzokoto, & Suh, 2000). Societal selves to those who are similar and proximal to them. On the
circumstances can be associated with very large differences basis of the original theory, people ought to be more satisfied
in SWB, which are due both to objective circumstances and with their incomes and lives if they are better off materially
to other cultural factors. than their neighbors and co-workers, and less satisfied if they
fall below this standard. Findings reveal, however, that ma-
Predictors of Well-Being Across Cultures terial standards now extend across the nations of the world.
There are some differences between cultures in the predictors of Festinger (1954) constructed the theory of social compar-
SWB, and these often fit the pattern of culture–person congru- ison before television became ubiquitous. Now 84% of
ence (for reviews, see Diener, Oishi, & Lucas, 2003; Tov & households worldwide own a television (based on the Gallup
Diener, 2007; Oishi, 2012). To the extent that people have the World Poll). Many television programs aired around the
characteristics that are valued in their culture, they tend to be world are produced in Western nations and portray prosper-
happier. Because some characteristics are valued in some cul- ous lifestyles in these countries. The Internet and print media
tures more than in others, there are differences in what predicts provide information about material comforts in the most
happiness in societies. For instance, Diener and Diener (1995) economically developed nations. Thus, people may know as
found that self-esteem was a stronger predictor of life satisfac- much about the material lives of individuals in wealthy
tion in individualistic cultures than in collectivistic cultures. Suh nations, as portrayed in the media, as they do about their
(2002) found that a consistent personality is valued more in the neighbors. It is perhaps not surprising, then, that people now
West than in South Korea, and hence consistency was more judge their own incomes in reference to a world standard.
predictive of SWB in the West. Fulmer et al. (2010) found that In support of a worldwide standard for income, nations
extraverts tend to be happier in extraverted cultures than in line up well in life satisfaction based on their average
introverted cultures. incomes. Figure 1 shows the strong relation between in-
Suh, Diener, and Updegraff (2008) found that a person’s come and life satisfaction among the nations of the world,
moods and emotions were more predictive of life satisfaction indicating that national wealth is a major predictor of the
in an individualistic culture, whereas one’s social life was average SWB in them. When people’s incomes were stan-
more predictive of life satisfaction in a collectivistic culture. dardized by their age and occupation within their nations,
Recently, Diener, Tay, and Myers (2011) found that in very these predictors reflecting people’s local standing on in-
religious nations and states of the United States, religious come added only a small increment in predicting their life
people have higher life satisfaction than nonreligious people, satisfaction beyond people’s absolute incomes (Diener,
whereas in the least religious nations and states this differ- Tay, & Oishi, 2012). Furthermore, the slopes of income
ence disappears. Thus, whether religion is associated with and life satisfaction within nations are not as steep as the
SWB depends on the society. slope of mean income and mean life satisfaction across
The findings that there are both universal and society- nations, thus contradicting the idea that local social com-
specific predictors of SWB raise intriguing questions for parisons are primary.
future research: What are the universal predictors of SWB Becchetti, Castriota, and Giachin (2011) found that life
and why do they have effects across all societies? Are the satisfaction depends on the distance between people’s
universals more inherently evaluable (Hsee & Zhang, 2010) income and that in the richest nations. People’s satisfac-

November 2012 ● American Psychologist 591


Although people on average often adapt to marriage so
Figure 1 that it no longer makes them happier or unhappier than
Income and Life Satisfaction in Nations
before, they do not fully adapt to some conditions such as
8
unemployment (Clark, Diener, Georgellis, & Lucas, 2008) or
Life Satisfaction (0 to 10 Ladder)

Correlation = .82 severe disabilities (Lucas, 2007). We also know that over a
7 period of years, a significant number of individuals do change
substantially in their levels of life satisfaction (Fujita &
Diener, 2005). The income and SWB relation shown in
6
Figure 1 also suggests incomplete adaptation to circum-
stances, because people in rich and poor nations who have
5 been in these conditions for many decades remain quite
dissimilar in their levels of SWB.
4
The adaptation pattern for an individual can be revealing.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

I present in Figure 2 the momentary mood data of “Harry,” a


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

participant in an experience-sampling study. “Harry” re-


3
2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 4.5 5.0
corded his moods at random moments twice a day for six
weeks during a period in which he was receiving chemother-
Household Dollar Income (Log 10 units) apy for cancer. He was informed during the study that his
Note. N ⫽ 677,145 respondents in 154 nations. Life satisfaction is based on cancer was in remission. The horizontal line in the figure
Cantril’s (1965) Self-Anchoring Ladder scale. represents a neutral mood. Above the line are positive moods,
ranging from mild happiness to elation, and below the line
are negative moods. As can be seen, “Harry” had a “high” in
tion can decline even as their incomes rise if they fall his moods following the good news of remission but then
behind income growth in the wealthiest nations. Thus, returned back toward his original mood level. The figure also
although people may to some extent compare themselves shows, however, that his average moods after remission were
to their neighbors and co-workers in judging their income, higher than his average moods before (a mean change of 0.77
a world standard based on wealthy nations is now the standard deviation units). Thus, “Harry’s” feelings reveal
primary basis of evaluation. A troubling implication of this some adaptation but not complete long-term habituation to
is that there is a treadmill effect in which people’s view of circumstances.
the good life continues to climb as incomes and spending
A number of intriguing research questions are raised by
in the wealthiest nations climb.
the findings that people adapt to events but not always com-
A few research questions raised by the social comparison
pletely: When and how do people adapt to circumstances?
findings are as follows: When does rising income lead to
rising SWB, and when does it not? How do we ensure that When do they not fully adapt, and why? Can we speed
rising income is rewarding regardless of the income levels of adaptation to unavoidable bad conditions and slow adaptation
the wealthiest? Do people now use worldwide standards for to good conditions? Are there different rates of adaptation
other areas of life such as education, human and democratic depending on factors such as personality and social support?
rights, and housing?

Adaptation Figure 2
“Harry’s” Moods Over Six Weeks
The idea of adaptation to conditions prevailed in the field of 6
Cancer Remission
SWB for many decades. Brickman, Coates, and Janoff-Bul-
Average Daily Moods

5
man (1978) proposed that people react strongly to good and 4
bad events, with higher and lower SWB, respectively, but 3
then quickly adapt back to their original baseline levels. This 2
1
meant that in the long run, even extreme conditions such as
0
paraplegia or extreme poverty would have negligible influ-
-1
ences on a person’s SWB. However, recent longitudinal -2
findings suggest that adaptation to conditions is more intri- 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40
cate (Diener, Lucas, & Scollon, 2006) and that circumstances Day Number
can have a large impact on SWB. It may take many years for Note. Possible moods vary from 6 (extremely positive) to ⫺6 (extremely
adaptation to occur, and sometimes adaptation is not com- negative).
plete.

592 November 2012 ● American Psychologist


Why do people often adapt more quickly for feelings than for & Diener, 1996), which is consistent with the idea that
life satisfaction (Luhmann, Hofman, Eid, & Lucas, 2012)? positive feelings are frequently beneficial.
Despite the benefits found for high SWB, conclusions
Outcomes of Subjective Well-Being about its desirability must be qualified in a number of ways
An intriguing recent finding is that SWB is not only a (Diener, Oishi, & Suh, 2012). For example, although high
desirable outcome but can also be an important predictor of cheerfulness upon entering college predicted income 19 years
future life outcomes. A review by Lyubomirsky, King, and later, this effect was strongest among those coming from
Diener (2005) concluded that high SWB is predictive of high-income families and did not occur for college students
future health and the quality of people’s social life. Diener from the poorest families (Diener et al., 2002). Furthermore,
and Chan (2011) found that the evidence that high SWB the most cheerful college students rated their academic abil-
benefits health and longevity is “clear and compelling” (see ities highly but obtained slightly lower grades while in col-
also Chida & Steptoe, 2008). lege (Nickerson, Diener, & Schwarz, 2011). These findings
High SWB seems to foster success in the workplace. raise the question of in what circumstances SWB is benefi-
Cheerfulness while in college was found to predict later cial.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

income (Diener, Nickerson, Lucas, & Sandvik, 2002). There Not only are there issues about when SWB is beneficial,
is evidence linking certain forms of SWB, for example, job there are also questions about whether there is an optimal
satisfaction, to organizational citizenship (e.g., Brief & Mo- level for effective functioning. Diener and Biswas-Diener
towidlo, 1986). In a large meta-analysis, Oishi (2012) found (2008) argued that people without negative emotions can be
that “happy” workers are more productive. In a large study of deviant and debilitated and that people with positive emo-
thousands of work units, Harter, Schmidt, Asplund, and Kil- tions that are too intense and continuous can also suffer. In
ham (2010) found that job satisfaction prospectively pre- support of this line of reasoning, Oishi, Diener, and Lucas
dicted the performance of work units. Oswald, Proto, and (2007) found that the highest levels of SWB were optimal for
Sgroi (2012) found in both a laboratory experimental study social functioning, as measured by the stability of romantic
and in a naturalistic study that positive moods benefited relationships, but for the most effective functioning in
productivity on a speeded task but did not affect the quality achievement domains, as measured by school grades and
of work. Edmans (2012) found that stock price returns were income, being happy, but not extremely happy, was most
predicted by earlier job satisfaction in companies. These advantageous. George and Zhou (2007) found that positive
findings are compelling because they rule out reverse causal- mood was predictive of employee creativity but that creativ-
ity from good performance to job satisfaction, and they take ity was highest when some negative affect was also present.
into account the numerous channels such as customer satis- Gruber, Mauss, and Tamir (2011) argued that there might
faction and employee turnover through which employee be a wrong time for happiness, a wrong amount of it for the
SWB might influence organizational performance. occasion, and wrong ways of pursuing it. For example, they
Recent research ties SWB to desirable social behavior and found that people who were about to confront someone
good citizenship behavior in the community. For example, preferred to be in an angry mood and performed better if they
Aknin, Sandstrom, Dunn, and Norton (2011) found that in were.
122 of 136 nations, life satisfaction was associated with Much more research is needed into why SWB predicts
donating more money to charity. Priller and Schupp (2011) desirable behavioral and health outcomes. Low life satisfac-
found that people who were happy during the past month, tion predicts a greater likelihood of suicide, a connection that
who felt high in positive feelings and low in negative feel- seems straightforward. However, findings by Luhmann, Lu-
ings, were more likely to donate blood and donate money to cas, Eid, and Diener (2012) suggest that life satisfaction
charity, even after controlling for income, education, and predicts future events for varying reasons. It predicts events
employment. Shin et al. (2011) found that positive affect in such as divorce because a conflictual marriage leads both to
children was associated with peer acceptance, teacher-rated low life satisfaction and to a greater probability of divorcing.
adjustment, and initiating positive interaction with peers. Other events might be predicted by low life satisfaction
Boehm and Lyubomirsky (2008) reviewed evidence that because it accompanies a desire to change one’s circum-
happy people tend to be more popular and likable. stances. Consistent with this interpretation, Luhmann et al.
The correlates of high SWB are broad, including biolog- found that starting a new job and relocating to a new geo-
ical factors such as fertility and survival of healthy offspring. graphical location both followed low life satisfaction. Finally,
These findings led Diener, Oishi, and Suh (2012) to argue long-term life satisfaction might predict even temporally
that humans have been evolutionarily selected for positive distant events because the antecedents of life satisfaction,
levels of happiness. Although researchers analyze the corre- such as higher income and a happy disposition, also might
lates of differences in happiness, it is noteworthy that most increase the likelihood of certain events. Luhmann et al.’s
people are above neutral in their moods and emotions (Diener findings on a higher likelihood of parenthood for those high

November 2012 ● American Psychologist 593


in life satisfaction a number of years before childbirth are tors. Social support (being able to count on others in emer-
consistent with this idea. gencies and feeling respected), safety (not having been
There are additional possible reasons that high SWB pre- assaulted or having things stolen), and water quality (reflect-
dicts future life events. High levels of positive feelings might ing environmental well-being as well as societal spending on
lead to certain outcomes for reasons such as greater creativ- basic needs for everyone) all substantially predict life satis-
ity, risk taking, and goal-approach behavior. Discovering the faction and enjoyment of life after controlling for household
reasons why SWB predicts future behavior requires more income. Thus, SWB measures have the ability to capture
in-depth research, and understanding these associations will forms of quality of life besides money. The SWB measures
have significant applied as well as theoretical importance. can reflect aspects of quality of life such as social capital that
Behavioral scientists and others are now working on in- are not contained in social indicators such as crime statistics.
terventions to enhance people’s SWB (e.g., Lyubomirsky & SWB measures also can help policymakers estimate weights
Della Porta, 2010; Seligman, Rashid, & Parks, 2006). If these to give to various aspects of quality of life.
interventions are replicable, we should know what the full The SWB measures reflect facets of society and daily
range of outcomes will be, beyond people simply feeling living that are relevant for policy. For instance, life satisfac-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

better. For example, if happy employees are more productive, tion is lower when commuting to work is long and difficult
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

this is important information for organizations and societies. (Stutzer & Frey, 2008), when the air is polluted (Luechinger,
However, if people in some societies are already happy 2007), and when there is airport noise (van Praag & Baarsma,
enough for effective functioning, and further gains will not 2005). Each of these findings can have policy implications.
benefit productivity, citizenship, or other outcomes, this too For example, the SWB findings point to certain policies (e.g.,
is important information for citizens and leaders alike. zoning that allows work areas, shopping areas, and housing
Thinking of SWB as an antecedent of events, not just as areas to be proximal to one another) rather than to other
following from them, raises a host of intriguing new ques- policies (e.g., building more freeways and subways connect-
tions: When and why does SWB predict outcomes? In what ing to the suburbs).
circumstances is high SWB most beneficial, and when is it In the health area, SWB data can help apportion research
detrimental? Are there optimal levels of SWB for particular dollars between diseases based in part on the amount of
situations? Are societies best served by having individuals misery or unhappiness they cause (Dolan & White, 2007).
who differ in levels of SWB? Do cultures differ in the degree There are policy questions in education, housing, and many
to which the experience and expression of SWB are benefi- other areas on which the SWB data can help throw additional
cial? light on the issues. The data on “happiness” do not provide
complete answers about what policies should be adopted, of
National Accounts of Subjective course, but they can provide additional insights that can be of
Well-Being help in policy deliberations.
It can be argued that the good society is one offering the most There has now been notable success in nations discussing
SWB to the greatest number of its citizens. Thus, assessing and adopting measures of SWB to use in policy deliberations.
the SWB of societies is a way of measuring the quality of life For example, in 2010 the prime minister of the United King-
in them. My colleagues and I have argued that the SWB dom announced that the nation would monitor well-being,
approach captures important information that is not contained and the measures that were adopted included life satisfaction
in economic indicators (Diener, Lucas, Schimmack, & Helli- and positive feelings. The Organization of Economic Coop-
well, 2009; Diener & Seligman, 2004). eration and Development (OECD) is a major international
On the basis of the idea that an important aspect of a good organization that helps coordinate statistics between more
society is that people in it like and enjoy their lives, I than 30 of the wealthiest nations of the world. The OECD has
proposed that societies establish national accounts of subjec- been issuing reports on the validity of the SWB measures and
tive and psychosocial well-being (Diener, 2000). Because how they might be used in the national accounting systems of
quality of life includes more than money, the SWB measures nations. There are parallel efforts in nations such as Chile,
can help highlight quality of life factors such as the environ- Japan, and Australia. In the United States, some of the
ment, social relationships, health, and so forth that are not national health-monitoring activities of the Centers for Dis-
fully captured by economic indices. Furthermore, the bene- ease Control and Prevention (CDC) include surveys of SWB.
ficial outcomes of high SWB add an additional reason to The Himalayan nation of Bhutan has famously declared that
adopt measures of SWB at the national level. it will use GHP, or the gross happiness product, to guide its
Measures of SWB can provide a greater voice to factors policies. Thus, the idea of national and international accounts
beyond economic growth that are important to well-being and of SWB is becoming a reality.
quality of life. For instance, in the Gallup World Poll, after The finding that societies differ substantially in SWB
controlling for income, average life satisfaction and the en- leads to important applied questions: What societal factors
joyment of life in societies are both predicted by other fac- most influence SWB? What do the SWB measures assess that

594 November 2012 ● American Psychologist


the economic indicators do not? What variables should we be are likely to earn more money than their sad, angry, or
assessing that are relevant to policy and that might influence depressed counterparts? One strategy to explore causality in
SWB? What are the dangers of the government manipulating this case is to compare lottery winners with those who buy
measures of SWB (Frey, 2011)? Are there optimal levels of lottery tickets but do not win, which would thus provide a
SWB for societies? type of natural experiment. Research examining lottery win-
ners shows that they are happier than comparable individuals
Methodological Advances not winning lotteries and thus suggests that higher incomes
Although the limitations of self-report measures and cross- can raise SWB (Apouey & Clark, 2011; Smith & Razzell,
sectional correlations at one point in time have been under- 1975; Winkelman, Oswald, & Powdthavee, 2012). In longi-
stood for many decades, the field of SWB is still based tudinal research, Diener, Tay, and Oishi (2012) found that
primarily on these methods. Recent research using longitu- rising incomes in societies were accompanied by rising life
dinal and experimental designs as well as employing multi- satisfaction but not necessarily more positive feelings or
method measurement reveals that prior conclusions must fewer negative feelings.
often be revised when more sophisticated methodologies are In Diener, Tay, and Inglehart’s (2012) review of life
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

used. Thus, we need more rigorous research methods in the satisfaction scales, virtually all of the studies reviewed were
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

field. based on self-report scales. Global self-reports have known


A problem with cross-sectional correlational designs is limitations and shortcomings, and adding other forms of
that they fail to capture causal direction. Thus married people measures of SWB can be helpful in complementing the
might be happier because the married state makes people self-report assessments with measures that have other
happy or because people who marry tend to be dispositionally strengths. For example, reports of the target person’s SWB by
happier than never-married or divorced individuals. Lucas, family and friends can be used (e.g., Zou, Schimmack, &
Clark, Georgellis, and Diener (2003) found that people who Gere, 2012), as can measurements of momentary or daily
will later marry were happier even many years before mar- moods averaged over time (see Oishi, 2012, for a review).
rying than were people who did not marry and people who Biological measures based on brain activation (e.g., Urry et
got married and then divorced. Indeed, people who divorced al., 2004) or immune strength (e.g., Miller, Chen, & Cole,
were less happy even many years before they married in the 2009) can also be employed to reflect levels of SWB, as can
first place (Lucas et al., 2003; Luhmann & Eid, 2009). These facial measures of emotion (e.g., Harker & Keltner, 2001).
findings point to the possibility that many correlations re- Methodological advances in the field point to key ques-
ported in the literature may be due to selection of individuals tions for future research: Do findings generalize across dif-
into the condition rather than to the condition itself—for ferent forms of measures of SWB, and do they replicate in
example, having children, being unemployed, or getting di- different cultures? Are the correlates of SWB due to the
vorced. Thus, longitudinal designs are valuable in SWB likelihood of happy people experiencing those conditions or
research, and cross-sectional findings must be interpreted due to the effects of the conditions themselves? When does
cautiously. SWB causally influence later behavior?
Experimental and quasi-experimental studies can also sub-
stantially enhance SWB research. For example, in the health Conclusions
domain, one can analyze the effects of SWB on health There are many exciting research directions on SWB in
parameters by experimentally raising or lowering the moods addition to those I have described here. For example, Ly-
of participants and then assessing physiological variables (see ubomirsky and Della Porta (2010) and Seligman et al. (2006)
Diener & Chan, 2011, for examples). The research of Suh and have worked on interventions to increase happiness. Soto and
his colleagues described earlier is an example of using both Luhmann (2012) found, as have many others (Steel, Schmidt,
experimental and naturalistic methods to confirm the causes & Shultz, 2008), that personality is a reliable predictor of
of SWB. Researchers can also examine events such as natural SWB. Importantly, they also found that personality can mod-
disasters and assess the effects on health and mortality in a erate the impact of life circumstances. Across three large and
quasi-experimental way. Health and well-being researchers representative samples, Soto and Luhmann found that the life
can follow people over time and can also employ experiments satisfaction of neurotics, compared with that of nonneurotics,
with animals where moods and emotions are likely altered as was more influenced by their incomes. These promising lines
well as conduct longitudinal studies with humans. Thus, in of research, as well as many others in the field, demonstrate
studies on health and SWB, simple cross-sectional correla- that the field is vibrant and making progress.
tions are no longer sufficient, as is true in most areas of SWB Although I have described research on SWB by combin-
research. ing findings on the various types of SWB, there are different
Many studies report a positive correlation between income processes involved in life satisfaction, positive feelings, and
and SWB, but are these associations due to the influence of negative feelings. These types of SWB can differ in terms of
money on SWB or due to the fact that high-SWB individuals their causes (e.g., Diener, Kahneman, Tov, & Arora, 2010;

November 2012 ● American Psychologist 595


Schimmack, Schupp, & Wagner, 2008), consequences (Wi- Diener, E., & Chan, M. Y. (2011). Happy people live longer: Subjective
well-being contributes to health and longevity. Applied Psychology:
est, Schüz, Webster, & Wurm, 2011), and stability over time Health and Well-Being, 3, 1– 43. doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01045.x
(Eid & Diener, 2004). Thus far there is only a rudimentary Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological
understanding of the differences, and examining the overlap Science, 7, 181–185. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00354.x
Diener, E., & Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction
and divergence of various forms of SWB is a critical task for and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
the future. 653– 663. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.4.653
The three decades I have spent conducting research and Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Tov, W., & Arora, R. (2010). Income’s associ-
ation with judgments of life versus feelings. In E. Diener, D. Kahneman,
scholarship on SWB have been rewarding, with many scien- & J. F. Helliwell (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp.
tists and practitioners entering the field and with the range of )3–15. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi:doi:10.1093/acprof:
oso/9780199732739.003.0001
questions being asked being greatly expanded. Governments Diener, E., Lucas, R., Schimmack, U., & Helliwell, J. (2009). Well-being for
have grown interested in using SWB measures to help guide public policy. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10. 1093/
policy. acprof:oso/9780195334074. 001. 0001
Diener, E., Lucas, R., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). Beyond the hedonic
The sophistication of the research methods used has been treadmill: Revising the adaptation theory of well-being. American Psy-
improving, and many more multimethod and longitudinal chologist, 61, 305–314. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.4.305
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Diener, E., Nickerson, C., Lucas, R. E., & Sandvik, E. (2002). Dispositional
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

studies are needed. My hope is that young investigators will


affect and job outcomes. Social Indicators Research, 59, 229 –259. doi:
analyze new questions arising in the field with sophisticated 10.1023/A:1019672513984
methods. Although the past was often dominated by simple Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and
correlational studies, the biggest promise for the future will subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. An-
nual Review of Psychology, 54, 403– 425. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54
often come from experimental, longitudinal, and multim- .101601.145056
ethod approaches. I am hopeful that in the future we will Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Suh, E. (2012). The happy species and the evolu-
know how much SWB is desirable for effective functioning tionary importance of happiness: But should we be even happier? Un-
published manuscript, University of Illinois.
in various circumstances and know how best to achieve Diener, E., Scollon, C. K. N., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Suh, E. M. (2000).
sustainable SWB. Positivity and the construction of life satisfaction judgments: Global
happiness is not the sum of its parts. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1,
159 –176. doi:10.1023/A:1010031813405
REFERENCES Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond money: Toward an
economy of well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5,
Aknin, L. B., Sandstrom, G. M., Dunn, E. W., & Norton, M. I. (2011). 1–31. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00501001.x
Investing in others: Prosocial spending for (pro) social change. In R. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective
Biswas-Diener (Ed.), Positive psychology in social change (pp. )219 – well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276 –
234. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-90-481- 302. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276
9938-9_13 Diener, E., Tay, L., & Inglehart, R. (2012). Theory and validity of life
Apouey, B., & Clark, A. E. (2011). Winning big but feeling no better? The satisfaction scales. Social Indicators Research. Advance online publica-
effects of lottery prizes on physical and mental health, Manuscript sub- tion. doi:10.1007/s11205-012-0076-y
mitted for publication. Diener, E., Tay, L., & Myers, D. (2011). The religion paradox: If religion
Becchetti, L., Castriota, S., & Giachin, E. (2011). Beyond the Joneses: makes people happy, why are so many dropping out? Journal of Person-
Inter-country income comparisons and happiness (Society for the Study ality and Social Psychology, 101, 1278 –1290. doi:10.1037/a0024402
of Economic Inequality Working Paper 189. )Retrieved from http:// Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2012). Easterlin was wrong –and right:
www.ecineq.org/milano/WP/ECINEQ2011-189.pdf Income change and happiness. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Boehm, J. K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Does happiness lead to career Dolan, P., & White, M. P. (2007). How can measures of subjective well-
success? Journal of Career Assessment, 16, 101–116. doi:10.1177/ being be used to inform public policy? Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 2, 71– 85. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00030.x
1069072707308140
Edmans, A. (2012). The link between job satisfaction and firm value, with
Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and
implications for corporate social responsibility. Manuscript submitted for
accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social
publication.
Psychology, 36, 917–927. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.8.917
Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2001). Norms for experiencing emotions in different
Brief, A. P., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1986). Prosocial organizational behaviors.
cultures: Inter- and intranational differences. Journal of Personality and
Academy of Management Review, 11, 710 –725. Social Psychology, 81, 869 – 885. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.869
Cantril, H. (1965). The pattern of human concerns. New Brunswick, NJ: Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2004). Global judgments of subjective well-being:
Rutgers University Press. Situational variability and long-term stability. Social Indicators Re-
Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2008). Positive psychological well-being and search, 65, 245–277. doi:10.1023/B:SOCI.0000003801.89195.bc
mortality: A quantitative review of perspective observational studies. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human
Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 741–756. doi:10.1097/PSY Relations, 7, 117–140. doi:10.1177/001872675400700202
.0b013e31818105ba Frey, B. (2011). Subjective well-being, politics and political economy.
Clark, A. E., Diener, E., Georgellis, Y., & Lucas, R. E. (2008). Lags and Swiss Journal of Economics and Statistics, 147, 397– 415.
leads in life satisfaction: A test of the baseline hypothesis. The Economic Fujita, F., & Diener, E. (2005). Life satisfaction set-point: Stability and
Journal, 118, F222–F243. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2008.02150.x change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 158 –164.
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542– doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.158
575. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542 Fulmer, C. A., Gelfand, M. J., Kruglanski, A., Kim-Prieto, C., Diener, E.,
Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness, and a Pierro, A., & Higgins, E. (2010). On “feeling right” in cultural contexts:
proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55, 34 – 43. doi:10 How person– culture match affects self-esteem and subjective well-being.
.1037/0003-066X.55.1.34 Psychological Science, 21, 1563–1569. doi:10.1177/0956797610384742
Diener, E., & iswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking the mysteries George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2007). Dual tuning in a supportive context: Joint
of psychological wealth. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. contributions of positive mood, negative mood, and supervisory behav-

596 November 2012 ● American Psychologist


iors to employee creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 605– Oishi, S., & Schimmack, U. (2010). Culture and well-being: A new inquiry
622. into the psychological wealth of nations. Perspectives on Psychological
Graham, C., & Pettinato, S. (2006). Frustrated achievers: Winners, losers, Science, 5, 463– 471. doi:10.1177/1745691610375561
and subjective well-being in Peru’s emerging economy. Annals of the Oswald, A. J., Proto, E., & Sgroi, D. (2012). Happiness and productivity.
American Academy of Political and Social Science, 606, 128 –153. doi: Manuscript submitted for publication.
10.1177/0002716206288192 Nickerson, C., Diener, E., & Schwarz, S. (2011). Positive affect and college
Gruber, J., Mauss, I. B., & Tamir, M. (2011). A dark side of happiness? success. Journal of Happiness Studies, 12, 717–746.
How, when, and why happiness is not always good. Perspectives on Priller, E., & Schupp, J. (2011). Social and economic characteristics of financial and
Psychological Science, 6, 222–233. doi:10.1177/1745691611406927 blood donors in Germany. DIW Economic Bulletin, 6, 23–30.
Harker, L., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of positive emotions in Schimmack, U., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2008). The influence of
women’s college yearbook pictures and their relationship to personality environment and personality on the affective and cognitive component of
and life outcomes across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social subjective well-being, Social Indicators Research, 89, 41– 60. doi:10
Psychology, 80, 112–124. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.112 .1007/s11205-007-9230-3
Seligman, M. E. P., Rashid, T., & Parks, A. C. (2005). Positive psychology
Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., Asplund, J., & Kilham, E. A. (2010). Causal
progress: Empirical validation of interventions. American Psychologist,
impact of employee work perceptions on the bottom line of organizations.
60, 410 – 421. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.5.410
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 378 –389. doi:10.1177/
Shin, N., Vaughn, B. E., Akers, V., Kim, M., Stevens, S., Krzysik, L., . . . Korth, B.
1745691610374589 (2011). Are happy children socially successful? Testing a central premise of
Hsee, C. K., & Zhang, J. (2010). General evaluability theory. Perspectives positive psychology in a sample of preschool children. Journal of Positive
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

on Psychological Science, 5, 343–355. doi:10.1177/1745691610374586 Psychology, 6, 355–367. doi:10.1080/17439760.2011.584549


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Lucas, R. E. (2007). Adaptation and the set-point model of subjective Smith, S., & Razzell, P. (1975). The pools’ winners. London, England:
well-being: Does happiness change after major life events? Current Calibon Books.
Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 75–79. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721 Soto, C. J., & Luhmann, M. (2012). Who can buy happiness? Personality
.2007.00479.x traits moderate the effects of stable income differences and income
Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Reexamining fluctuations on life satisfaction. Social Psychological and Personality
adaptation and the set point model of happiness: Reactions to change in Science. Advance online publication (Ed.), 10.1177/1948550612444139
marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527– Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship between
539. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.527 personality and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 134, .138 –
Luechinger, S. (2007). Valuing air quality using the life satisfaction ap- 161. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138
proach. Unpublished manuscript, University of Zurich, Swiss Federal Stutzer, A., & Frey, B. S. (2008). Stress that doesn’t pay: The commuting
Institute of Technology. paradox. Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 110, 339 –366. doi:10
Luhmann, M., & Eid, M. (2009). Does it really feel the same? Changes in .1111/j.1467-9442.2008.00542.x
life satisfaction following repeated life events. Journal of Personality and Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being.
Social Psychology, 97, 363–381. doi:10.1037/a0015809 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1378 –1391. doi:10
Luhmann, M., Hofman, W., Eid, M., & Lucas, R. E. (2012). Subjective .1037/0022-3514.83.6.1378
well-being and adaptation to life events: A meta-analysis. Journal of Suh, E. M., Diener, E., & Updegraff, J. A. (2008). From culture to priming
Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 592– 615. doi:10.1037/ conditions—Self-construal influences on life satisfaction judgments.
a0025948 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 3–15. doi:10.1177/
Luhmann, M., Lucas, R. E., Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2012). The prospective 0022022107311769
effect of life satisfaction on life events. Social Psychological and Per- Tov, W., & Diener, E. (2007). Culture and subjective well-being. In S.
sonality Science. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/ Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp.
1948550612440105 691–713). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., Jackson, D. C., Dalton, K. M.,
Lyubomirsky, S., & Della Porta, M. (2010). Boosting happiness, buttressing
Mueller, C. J.,. . . ]?⬎Davidson, R. J. (2004). Making a life worth living:
resilience: Results from cognitive and behavioral interventions. In J. W.
Neural correlates of well-being. Psychological Science, 15, 367–372.
Reich, A. J. Zautra, & J. Hall (Eds.), Handbook of adult resilience:
doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00686.x
Concepts, methods, and applications (pp. 459 – 464). New York, NY:
van Praag, B. M. S., & Baarsma, B. E. (2005). Using happiness surveys to
Guilford Press. value intangibles: The case of airport noise. The Economic Journal, 115,
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L. A., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent 224 –246. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2004.00967.x
positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, Wiest, M., Schüz, B., Webster, N., & Wurm, S. (2011). Subjective well-
131, 803– 855. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803 being and mortality revisited: Differential effects of cognitive and emo-
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Cole, S. W. (2009). Healh psychology: Devel- tional facets of well-being on mortality. Health Psychology, 30, 728 –735.
oping biologically plausible models linking the social world and physical doi:10.1037/a0023839
health. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 501–524. doi:10.1146/annurev Wilson, W. (1967). Correlates of avowed happiness. Psychological Bulletin,
.psych.60.110707.163551 67, 294 –306. doi:10.1037/h0024431
Oishi, S. (2012). The psychological wealth of nations: Do happy people Winkelman, R., Oswald, A. J., & Powdthavee, N. (2010). What happens to
make a happy society? Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell people after winning the lottery? Manuscript submitted for publication.
Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (2007). The optimum level of Zou, C., Schimmack, U., & Gere, J. (2012). Towards an integrated theory
well-being: Can people be too happy? Perspectives on Psychological of the nature and measurement of well-being: A multiple-indicator-
Science, 2, 346 –360. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00048.x multiple-rater model. Manuscript submitted for publication.

November 2012 ● American Psychologist 597

Anda mungkin juga menyukai