Anda di halaman 1dari 9

This article was downloaded by: [University of Saskatchewan Library]

On: 12 January 2015, At: 23:41


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Family Science
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rfsc20

Bereavement in family context: Coping with the loss of


a loved one
a
Margaret S. Stroebe
a
Department of Clinical Psychology , Utrecht University , The Netherlands
Published online: 04 May 2011.

To cite this article: Margaret S. Stroebe (2010) Bereavement in family context: Coping with the loss of a loved one , Family
Science, 1:3-4, 144-151, DOI: 10.1080/19424620.2010.576081

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19424620.2010.576081

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Family Science
Vol. 1, Nos. 3–4, June–October 2010, 144–151

REVIEW ARTICLE
Bereavement in family context: Coping with the loss of a loved one1
Margaret S. Stroebe*
Department of Clinical Psychology, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
(Received 23 October 2010; final version received 24 March 2011)

The loss of a close family member through death is associated not only with detrimental mental and physical health effects,
but it also has interpersonal consequences. This research is first briefly reviewed and the conclusion drawn that, while most
people suffer intensely in the weeks and months following the loss of a loved relative, a majority are able in time to come
to terms with their bereavement. It becomes important to examine for whom intervention is appropriate and whether it
Downloaded by [University of Saskatchewan Library] at 23:41 12 January 2015

is effective. Furthermore, we need to understand processes of coping associated with (mal)adjustment. Major theoretical
perspectives in the bereavement field have focused on the necessity to do “grief work” in order to come to terms with
loss. There are shortcomings to this conceptualization. Importantly, the family context of grief and grieving is neglected. The
limitations in theoretical formulations led to the development of the Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement (DPM,
Stroebe & Schut, 1999). It is shown how an interpersonal perspective can be incorporated into the model and illustrations of
its application to the family context are given.
Keywords: grief; bereavement; family; health; coping; interpersonal relations

Introduction It is important at the outset to define a few terms that


There can hardly be a greater life transition than the expe- are frequently used in the context of the loss of a loved
rience of loss through death of a loved person. It is a life person. Bereavement, the objective situation of having lost
event that impacts on individuals and families in complex someone significant, is an event that typically occurs with
and varied ways, and it is associated with inter- as well as increasing frequency across the life span, with people hav-
intrapersonal consequences. Indicative of the strength of its ing to face the death of parents, siblings, partners, friends,
impact is the fact that the Social Readjustment Rating Scale or even their own children. The intense distress associated
(Holmes & Rahe, 1967) ranked “death of a spouse” as the with bereavement is termed grief , defined as a primarily
event requiring the most intense readjustment, with “death emotional (affective) reaction to the loss of a loved one
of a close family member” also ranking very highly, in fifth through death (Stroebe, Hansson, Stroebe, & Schut, 2001).
place. In this paper, research on the impact of bereavement Although it is sometimes used interchangeably with grief,
for individuals and families is reviewed, selecting key find- the term mourning is here defined as the social expressions
ings. This leads to discussion about the implications for or acts expressive of grief that are shaped by the practices
intervention. If bereaved people suffer deleterious conse- of a given society or cultural group (Stroebe et al., 2001).
quences, can they be helped by counselling or therapy?
Following this line of argument, analysis of how peo- Intra- and interpersonal health consequences of
ple cope is relevant for understanding both differences in bereavement
adjustment and for guiding intervention programs: What is The most extreme physical health consequence would be
effective coping? Theories of coping are examined and our the death of the bereaved person him or herself. Indeed,
own model, the Dual Process Model of Coping with Loss through the centuries the notion that one can actually die
(DPM; Stroebe & Schut, 1999, 2010) is described. This of a “broken heart” has remained part of popular culture.
model was designed to address a number of shortcomings For example, the following epitaph was written in the sev-
inherent in previous models. In the context of the family, enteenth century by Sir Henry Wotton (1568-1639) on the
an important shortcoming was the focus on intrapersonal death of the wife of a gentleman named Albert Morton.
rather than interpersonal processes of coping. It is shown This lady’s death - apparently from grief - occurred shortly
how this is dealt with in the DPM. after that of her husband:


Email: m.s.stroebe@uu.nl

ISSN 1942-4620 print/ISSN 1942-4639 online


© 2010 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/19424620.2010.576081
http://www.informaworld.com
Family Science 145

He first deceased; disorder, depression, chest pains, infections) than do the


She for a little tried non-bereaved (for a review, see Stroebe, Schut, & Stroebe,
To live without him; 2007). For example, the Tübingen Longitudinal Study of
Liked it not, and died.
young widowed compared with matched married persons,
20% of the widowed (compared with 3% of the married)
Is the “broken heart” a valid assumption; is there scien- suffered from severe physical symptoms at four to six
tific evidence to back it up? Analyses of cross-sectional, months post-loss. After two years, the rate among the wid-
national data sets on mortality by marital status provide owed declined to 12% (Stroebe & Stroebe, 1993). The
the first clues. To illustrate, patterns of mortality by mar- percentages for mental and physical health difficulties vary
ital status reported by Shurtleff (1955) were based on the considerably, due not only to the type of debility and its pre-
death rates of adults in the United States from 1949 to cise measurement, but also to sample characteristics (e.g.,
1951. Shurtleff (1955) recorded high excesses in mortality type and nature of bereavement). For example, rates for
among widowed persons compared to those of other mar- complicated grief vary from as few as 5% but up to 33%
ital statuses, relatively higher excesses at earlier ages, and of the bereaved (Middleton, Raphael, Martinek, & Misso,
Downloaded by [University of Saskatchewan Library] at 23:41 12 January 2015

relatively higher excesses for widowers than for widows. 1993; for a review see Forstmeier & Maercker, 2006).2
Such patterns have frequently been replicated. Nevertheless, the overall picture is clear: for each type
However, clear though these patterns seem to be, one of debility substantial minorities of bereaved individuals
cannot actually assume that such excesses for the widowed do suffer from severe and quite lasting consequences fol-
are caused by the psychological impact of loss. Selection lowing bereavement. A rough but possibly conservative
and statistical artefacts could account for at least part of estimate of around 20-30% prevalence emerges from the
the increase in mortality rates; and other explanations such different health vulnerability areas. These figures receive
as homogamy or joint unfavourable environment of the confirmation from a different line of research conducted by
couple could challenge the conclusion that the increase is Bonanno, Boerner, and Wortman (2008) who has investi-
due to a broken heart (Kraus & Lilienfeld, 1959). gated resilience in bereaved people. Again, a majority of
Fortunately, longitudinal investigations are available people are indeed resilient.
too, which have managed to control for such factors and Given the actual number of people who are bereaved
which have typically confirmed the age and gender patterns in any one year, we are talking about large numbers of
found by Shurtleff (1955). One classic study was con- people at risk. In the Netherlands, for example, approxi-
ducted by Parkes, Benjamin, and Fitzgerald (1969) among mately 130,000 persons have been estimated to die each
older widowers. There was a higher risk of these men year, leaving four persons on average bereaved. Thus, at
dying (compared with married counterparts) in the first any one time, in this quite small country (with a population
six months of bereavement. Recent well-controlled stud- just under 17 million), 500,000 persons will be coming to
ies have also confirmed the excesses for bereaved people. terms with the death of a significant person in their lives
Most findings indicate an early excess risk of mortality, (van den Bout, Boelen, & De Keijser, 1998).
although excesses have indeed been shown to persist for So far the mental and physical health impact of
longer than six months after bereavement. It is impor- bereavement in the first months and years following loss
tant to note that sex difference patterns vary across types (i.e., during the acute grieving period, which varies between
of loss. Whereas partner loss appears to affect men rela- individuals and cultures) has been outlined. But are there
tively more than women, death of a child seems to have an longer-term consequences, as suggested in the Li et al.
even greater excessive mortality risk for mothers than for (2003) study on parent’s excess mortality? Considerable
fathers. A national-level study in Denmark by Li, Precht, research effort has focused particularly on the long-term
Mortensen, and Olsen (2003) showed that excess risk of consequences of the loss of a parent in childhood. For
dying for bereaved mothers compared with mothers who example, Luecken (2008) reviewed this body of literature
had not lost a child extended across the 18 years of the and identified important patterns. Notably, she found that
study, with deaths attributed to both natural and unnatural psychological (e.g., depression) and physiological (e.g.,
causes, whereas for fathers, greater risk was noted early on cortisol reactivity) reactions in adulthood were linked to
in bereavement, particularly from unnatural causes. parental loss in childhood. However, two important risk
Although mortality is a drastic consequence of losing factors for such difficulties in adulthood were (1) neglectful
a loved one, it must be remembered that it affects just parenting and (2) poor coping of the surviving parent.
a very small proportion of bereaved people. However, if These post-loss factors seemed to be the critical features,
some actually die, and from a variety of causes, many rather than the event of loss itself. In this context, it is
more are likely to suffer from diverse mental and phys- important to know that psychotherapeutic intervention pro-
ical health consequences. There is considerable evidence grams, such as the Family Bereavement Program developed
that bereaved people suffer more from a variety of men- by Sandler and his colleagues (Sandler et al., 2010), have
tal and physical health problems (e.g., post traumatic stress been shown to be effective for children and adolescents.
146 M.S. Stroebe

The Family Bereavement Program is a theoretically derived unstable marriage), while others may be driven apart by
intervention program for children who have experienced the loss of their child, not understanding each other’s grief
parental death. It was designed to be implemented with or experiencing incompatibility in dealing with their loss
bereaved families, to improve multiple risk and protective (so that overall calculations of the number of disrupted
factors (e.g., effective discipline; adaptive control beliefs) marriages would remain somewhat unchanged).
which were hypothesized to lead to improvements in child The second interpersonal issue, loneliness following
functioning. The program is delivered in 12 group sessions partner loss, has implications both for theory and prac-
and 2 individual sessions, with 5 sessions including joint tice. Given that bereavement involves the loss of a loved
activities for the caregivers and children. person, by its very nature it is an interpersonal event, one
To summarize: Regarding the intrapersonal conse- that is spoken to by a major perspective in the bereavement
quences of bereavement, in general one can say that there field, namely attachment theory (Bowlby, 1980). For exam-
are a wide range of reactions and health consequences, ple, in one longitudinal study of the impact of the loss of a
from mild and short-lived to extreme and long-lasting. child on bereaved parents, security of attachment-predicted
However it must be remembered that, although most people adjustment of these parents (e.g., they had lower levels
Downloaded by [University of Saskatchewan Library] at 23:41 12 January 2015

suffer intensely, only a minority have severe mental and/or of grief symptoms) over and above the personality factor
physical health complications. Bereavement is, after all, a of neuroticism (Wijngaards et al., 2007). Parents who felt
normal, natural life event, one that nearly everyone has to secure in their relationships, not only to their partners but
face sooner or later. to other persons in general (e.g., they endorsed question-
Turning from intrapersonal to interpersonal conse- naire items indicating that they did not worry that their
quences, again, a wide range of bereavement effects could partner loved them; they were not uncomfortable being
be reviewed, ranging from older widowed persons percep- close to others), adjusted better to the loss of their child
tions of intergenerational support (e.g., Ha, Carr, Utz, & during the first two years of their bereavement. Along dif-
Nesse, 2006) to meaning-making in bereaved families (e.g., ferent lines, but still following the attachment perspective,
Winchester Nadeau, 2008), to bereavement experiences of Weiss (1975) made a major distinction between emotional
close family members after the death of an old person (e.g., and social loneliness. Emotional loneliness refers to a sense
Moss, Moss, & Hansson, 2001). Two other consequences of utter aloneness and isolation, whether or not others are
can usefully be highlighted in the context of the family: (1) accessible, as illustrated by an item on a scale measuring
the possibility of marital disruption following the death of a this construct: “I feel lonely even when I am with others.”
child and, taking a very different aspect, (2) the experience A remark by Francis Bacon seems to capture the essence
of emotional and social loneliness following the death of a of this type of loneliness: “A crowd is not company, and
spouse. Many claims have been made that there is high risk faces are a gallery of pictures.” By contrast, social loneli-
of parental separation after the death of a child. However, ness has been defined as the feeling that there is nobody
important though this issue is, the literature is unfortu- to count on for support, absence of an engaging social net-
nately still unclear. On the one hand, one recent study by work, lack of a sense of social embeddedness, as illustrated
Rogers, Floyd, Seltzer, Greenberg, and Hong (2008) found in the statement: “I don’t have a really good set of friends.”
that bereaved parents had a higher rate of separation or The occurrence and impact of these two types of loneliness
divorce than non-bereaved (religious participation, or hav- across the first two years of bereavement were investigated
ing other children were both associated with less marital in the Tübingen longitudinal study of bereavement men-
disruption). On the other hand, Eilegard and Kreicbergs tioned above. Loneliness was examined in relationship to
(2010) reported that dissolution of the partnership was not depression among young widows and widowers, as was the
more common among parents who have lost a child to impact of social support from others on loneliness (Stroebe,
cancer, than it was among non-bereaved parents. A fairly Stroebe, Abakoumkin, & Schut, 1996). Does support pro-
recent review concluded that: “...insufficient empirical evi- tect from the loneliness of bereavement? It was found
dence exists to assert that divorce rates are higher among that widowed persons were particularly emotionally lonely
bereaved parents when compared with non-bereaved par- compared with their married counterparts. Examining the
ents (p. 362)” (Murphy, Clark Johnson, & Lohan, 2003). patterns according to social support by marital status more
More research and careful review needs to be done, for closely, a remarkable difference in the occurrence of the
example, it would be useful to extend the Eilegard and two types of loneliness was discovered. Social support was
Kreicbergs (2010) study to loss of a child through causes helpful in reducing social loneliness (for bereaved and non-
other than cancer, since it is conceivable that marital dis- bereaved persons). However, importantly, it was found that
ruption is relatively higher following certain types of death social support from friends and family did not reduce emo-
(e.g., suicide) than others. A possibility that was raised tional loneliness. Another thing that analyses showed was
over a decade ago needs further consideration: Dijkstra that emotional loneliness in bereavement-predicted depres-
and Stroebe (1998) drew attention to the possibility that sion. These data underline the attachment theory principle
some couples draw together (perhaps mending a previously that persons to whom we are closely emotionally bonded
Family Science 147

are not easy to replace. They highlight the central role of Would one expect widowers (compared with widows) to
loneliness in the bereavement process and perhaps lead benefit more from problem-focused intervention, since
one to modesty about how much others can actually help men typically focus less on emotions than do women (van
a bereaved person to adjust to his or her bereavement. This Heck & de Ridder, 2001). Or would one expect that wid-
leads to the next topic, namely, the impact of professional owers would gain from “switching” to emotion-focused
intervention. coping, since the preferred coping style had not worked
so far? What emerged from this research was that, for
those who need help, in the long-term: Emotion-focused
The efficacy of intervention intervention was found to be more helpful for widowers,
For whom is intervention after loss appropriate? Is inter- while problem-focused intervention was more helpful for
vention effective? Review of the literature has shown that widows, thus supporting the second of the two hypotheses
intervention which is open to all bereaved persons (the considered above. This study also illustrates the importance
criterion for participation being simply that one has expe- of different ways of coping in coming to terms with loss,
rienced a loss through death) is generally not very effective suggesting the need for flexibility. Theoretical analysis of
Downloaded by [University of Saskatchewan Library] at 23:41 12 January 2015

(Schut & Stroebe, 2005; Schut & Stroebe, 2010; Schut, the coping process promotes understanding of its rather
Stroebe, van den Bout, & Terheggen, 2001 ). Interventions complex relationship to adaptation.
which are confined to high-risk groups - those who can be
regarded as more vulnerable to the health risks of bereave-
ment (e.g., high levels of distress, traumatic loss, concur- Coping in theoretical perspective
rent life events, loss of a child) - are more promising. In Major theoretical perspectives in the bereavement field,
the context of family transitions, it is important to mention including the psychoanalytic (Freud, 1917) and attachment
the Family Focused Grief Therapy program of Kissane and (Bowlby, 1980) perspectives, have focused on the neces-
his colleagues. These researchers have worked for years sity for bereaved persons to do their “grief work” in order
to develop this program for high-risk families, and have to come to terms with loss. It has also been a fundamental
had considerable success in reducing distress of bereaved- principle in the design of therapy and counselling pro-
treated families, compared with untreated controls (e.g., grams. In particular, it is fundamental to Worden’s (1991,
Kissane & Lichtenthal, 2008). Finally, interventions which 2009) “Task Model” as described in his classic “Grief
are designed for those who suffer from complicated grief Counselling and Grief Therapy” handbook. In short, it was
have mostly been found to be helpful. The general conclu- understood by both theorists and practitioners that one must
sion - which has been supported by additional reviews in go over the events before and leading up to the death, and
the meantime (e.g., Currier, Neimeyer, & Berman, 2008) review the relationship with the deceased person. Without
- is that it is not appropriate to offer intervention to peo- such confrontation with one’s loss, adjustment would not
ple simply based on the fact that they have suffered the occur. Bowlby’s (1980) well-known phases cover shock,
loss of a loved one. Intervention should rather be planned yearning/protest, despair, and restitution. Worden’s (1991)
for high-risk groups and those who have complicated grief four tasks1 cover accepting the reality of loss; experienc-
manifestations. ing the pain of grief; adjusting to life without the deceased;
The efficacy of intervention can be illustrated through and withdrawing emotional energy from the deceased and
a research project by Henk Schut (see Schut, Stroebe, de reinvesting it in another relationship.
Keijser, & van den Bout, 1997). He conducted a study While there is much to be said in favour of its impor-
of widowers and widows, mean age 54 years, who were tance, there are also shortcomings to this “grief work”
assessed to be moderately to highly distressed just over conceptualization. To summarize the main arguments of
a year after their bereavement. A number of these per- Stroebe and Schut (1999, 2010): the usefulness of grief
sons were offered intervention and compared with a control work has been examined empirically and found not to work.
group who did not receive intervention. The intervention People who “worked through” their grief were no better
group was randomly assigned to one of two conditions: adjusted over time than those who did not. Importantly
either they received client-centred therapy, focusing on too, the grief work model fails to take account of other
their emotions, or they received behaviour therapy, focus- sources of stress that need to be coped with during bereave-
ing on tackling the problems that occur as a result of ment (e.g., dealing with the finances, learning how to cook,
bereavement. The intervention took place in seven ses- sorting out legal affairs, and so on). Neither did it fit
sions, from 14 to 17 months post-loss. Their levels of other cultural prescriptions: grief work does not seem to
distress were measured before therapy, after it and at be evident in some cultures, yet adaptation takes place (cf.
follow-up. Wikan, 1988, 1990). To illustrate, Wikan (1988) reported
In general, the therapies were quite effective. However, vastly different patterns among Muslim communities in
there were notable gender differences. It is interesting Egypt and Bali. While in Egypt “... the bereaved are
to consider potential predictions about these differences: encouraged to dwell profusely on their subjective pain in
148 M.S. Stroebe

an atmosphere where ... others also immerse themselves in the deceased person, and grief work is incorporated here.
tragic tales and expressed sorrow”, in Bali “... laughter and Restoration-oriented coping has to do with secondary stres-
cheerfulness fill the air while the bereaved are enjoined to sors that come about as a result of loss, things which also
contain their sorrow and may be made to feel they com- cause anxiety and distress, for example, in partner loss,
mit an injustice to others should they fail to abide by this learning the skills that the deceased had taken care of, or the
demand”. (p. 445) need to develop a new identity without the deceased. The
In the present context, a major limitation of the model places the coping process within everyday life expe-
grief work model is that the process of working through rience (one is not coping all the time). Fundamental is the
grief has been viewed as largely intrapersonal in nature. idea of oscillation, an emotion regulatory process, which
Accordingly, examination of interpersonal influences has we needed to postulate, since one cannot deal with loss
been neglected. Yet people do not typically grieve in iso- and restoration stressors at the same time. This dynamic
lation, other persons around them, in particular, family process distinguishes this model perhaps most significantly
members, are also affected by the loss of a loved one, and from the other coping models, as does the addition of the
this is likely to have consequences for a person’s adjust- restoration dimension. The latter would extend the four
Downloaded by [University of Saskatchewan Library] at 23:41 12 January 2015

ment to bereavement. For example, it would seem likely more loss-oriented tasks of Worden (1991) to include par-
that one family member’s grief intensity and way of griev- allel, restoration-oriented ones that can be summarized as:
ing will impact on that of another and vice versa. Extending acceptance of the reality of the changed world; taking time
scientific investigation to include an interpersonal perspec- off from the pain of grief; mastering the changed (sub-
tive would enable one to examine such influences, and it jective) environment; and developing new roles, identities,
would facilitate assessment of their impact on adjustment and relationships. It is interesting to note that in the most
of family members. The question we had to solve was how recent edition of his book, Worden’s (2009) third and fourth
to improve on the previous models. The so-called Dual tasks have become more restoration-oriented, namely, to
Process Model was developed as an alternative framework adjust to a world without the deceased and to find an endur-
to try to provide some guidance regarding these concerns. ing connection with the deceased in the midst of embarking
on a new life.
Some evidence in support of the model is beginning
The dual process model of coping with bereavement to accumulate (see Stroebe & Schut, 2010). For exam-
(DPM) ple, Shear, Frank, Houck, and Reynolds (2005) conducted
The DPM is a taxonomy for describing how people cope a study of the efficacy of intervention for bereaved per-
with bereavement, one which we think addresses the lim- sons with complications in their grieving. They were
itations of previous models mentioned above. The main either assigned (randomly) to traditional interpersonal psy-
parameters are sketched in Figure 1. chotherapy (IPT), or to a treatment program that followed
As can be seen, two types of stressor were postulated, the principles of the DPM, called complicated grief treat-
namely, loss- and restoration-oriented ones. Loss-oriented ment. Treatment was administered in 16 sessions during
coping involves dealing with those stressors to do with an average interval of 19 weeks per participant. The DPM

Everyday life
experience
Loss- Restoration-
oriented oriented
Attending to life
Grief work changes
Intrusion of grief Doing new things
letting go-continuing- Distraction from grief
relocating bonds / Denial/avoidance
ties of grief
Denial/avoidance New roles/
of restoration identities/
changes relationships

oscillation

Figure 1. The dual process model of coping with bereavement.


Family Science 149

condition involved description of the DPM model itself many of whom will be family members. Grieving is often
and also asked about loss and life goals at the outset. In very much a family affair (Kissane & Lichtenthal, 2008;
the main part of the therapy, loss and restoration processes Moss et al., 2001; Sandler et al., 2010). Take the case of a
were addressed in tandem, focusing on issues to do with widow and her two children wherein the mother oscillates
the loss itself and restoration of a satisfying life. Finally, between loss- and restoration-orientation, while one of
progress was reviewed, plans were made for the future, and her children, her daughter, is more loss- the other, her
feelings about completing the therapy were discussed. For son, more restoration-oriented. These patterns influence
those in the DPM treatment condition there was greater and each other and ongoing ways of coping. Some of our
quicker improvement than in the IPT condition. own ongoing research is directed towards identifying
The application of the DPM framework to gender how bereaved parents’ perceptions of their own and their
differences has implications for a family perspective on partners’ coping affect grieving over time. Clearly, there
grief. Traditional Western gender differences can be rep- is scope for further extension of such lines of research,
resented in the DPM, which are not evident in the grief for example, to include other family members such as
work models described earlier. In line with the focus of bereaved siblings. Unfortunately, this was beyond the
Downloaded by [University of Saskatchewan Library] at 23:41 12 January 2015

women on their emotions and of men being more problem- scope of our own investigation.
oriented, women would typically be more loss-, men more
restoration-oriented. Summary and conclusions
In line with this, it is important to note interper-
sonal coping processes that the model has the potential Negative health consequences across a variety of outcomes
to incorporate, acknowledging that one person’s way of and for some different types of bereavement have been
grieving impacts on that of another. The gender differences documented, and interpersonal consequences have been
described above provide an illustration: if, say, a bereaved illustrated. It was noted that grief is a normal, natu-
father is more restoration-oriented, a mother more loss- ral process following bereavement. Most reactions are
oriented, attributions may be made in terms of differences not complicated and for most bereaved persons, family
in extremity of grief, for example, a mother might assume and friends, religious and community groups and vari-
“he is grieving less than I am” rather than what may actu- ous societal resources will provide the necessary support.
ally be the case, that “he is grieving differently”. Making Professional psychological intervention is generally neither
the former attribution could negatively impact on the cou- justified nor effective for uncomplicated forms of grief.
ple’s adjustment to bereavement. Evidence from recent Considerable scientific effort has been directed towards
research regarding the influence of such interpersonal cop- understanding (mal) adaptive forms of grieving, not least
ing processes from a DPM perspective has been found: because coping is a variable that can be influenced and
Wijngaards et al. (2008) used the DPM framework to exam- potentially changed. A number of shortcomings are asso-
ine the relationship between a bereaved parent’s own and ciated with the traditional Western notion that in order to
their partner’s way of coping in relationship to their adjust- cope well and get over one’s loss, one has to work through
ment to the death of their child. Interpersonal factors were one’s grief. The DPM was put forward as a framework
indeed found to play a part in coping and adjustment. For to overcome some of the shortcomings. In particular, this
example, one of the main findings was that, for fathers, hav- model incorporates analysis of the interpersonal dynam-
ing a wife who was high in restoration-oriented coping was ics that occur following the loss of a loved one, and the
related to positive adjustment. interactive dynamics of families that may impact on pat-
The following two statements by a bereaved couple terns of grieving and adjustment. There is no doubt that
in one longitudinal study of bereaved parents, four years future scientific investigation in the bereavement field can
after the loss of their newborn child, reflect the gender dif- benefit from further integration of perspectives from fam-
ferences in coping that we have just identified (Dijkstra, ily relations research, and hopefully, the latter field will find
2000). These were the mother’s words: “Generally I think dialogue with bereavement researchers fruitful too.
of our son the whole day. Then my husband comes home,
and asks how I feel. Well, at such moments I almost Notes
explode. ‘You know how I feel’ I snap at him then.” The 1. This article is based on a keynote address presented at
the European Society on Family Relations 5th Congress,
father had this to say: “If I have been working, I am usu-
“Family Transitions & Families in Transition” Milan, Italy,
ally in a good mood. I can really find some distraction in 29th September–2nd October, 2010.
my work. But the closer I get to home, the tenser I feel, 2. Complicated grief has been defined as “a deviation from
because I know she will be feeling down again.” the (cultural) norm (i.e., that could be expected to pertain,
To broaden from gender differences between partners according to the extremity of the particular bereavement
event) in either (a) the time course or intensity of specific or
who have lost a child to bereavement in families: Earlier
general symptoms of grief and/or (b) the level of impairment
it was mentioned that for every death, an average of four in social, occupational, or other important areas of function-
people have been estimated to be deeply grief-stricken, ing” (Stroebe, Hansson, Schut, & Stroebe, 2008, p. 7).
150 M.S. Stroebe

3. Recently, Worden (2009) has adapted his tasks, with more W. Stroebe, & R.O. Hansson (Eds.), Handbook of bereave-
attention to ongoing relationships and restoration tasks. ment: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 44–61). New
Given that the current focus is on origins and reasons for York: Cambridge University Press.
development of the DPM, the tasks that were available at that Moss, M., Moss, S., & Hansson, R. (2001). Bereavement and old
time are detailed here. age. In M. Stroebe, R.O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut
(Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences,
coping, and care (pp. 241–260). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
References Murphy, S., Clark Johnson, L., & Lohan, J. (2003). Challenging
Bonanno, G., Boerner, K., & Wortman, C. (2008). Trajectories the myths about parents’ adjustment after a sudden, vio-
of grieving. In M.S. Stroebe, R.O. Hansson, H. Schut, lent death of a child. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 35,
& W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research 359–364.
and practice: Advances in theory and intervention (pp. Parkes, C.M., Benjamin, B., & Fitzgerald, R. (1969). Broken
287–307). Washington, DC: American Psychological heart: A statistical study of increased morality among win-
Association. dowers. British Medical Journal, 1, 740–743.
Bout, J. van den, Boelen, P., & De Keijser, J. (1998). Rogers, C., Floyd, F., Seltzer, M., Greenberg, J., & Hong, J.
Behandelingsstrategieën bij gecompliceerde rouw en (2008). Long-term effects of the death of a child on par-
Downloaded by [University of Saskatchewan Library] at 23:41 12 January 2015

verliesverwerking. [Treatment strategies for complicated ents’ adjustment in midlife. Journal of Family Psychology,
grief and coping with loss]. Houten: Bohn Stafleu Van 22, 203–211.
Loghum. Sandler, I., Ma, Y., Tein, J.-Y., Ayers, T., Wolchik, S., Kennedy,
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss (Vol. 3). Loss: Sadness C., et al. (2010). Long-term effects of the family bereavement
and depression. London: Hogarth. program on multiple indicators of grief in parentally bereaved
Currier, J., Neimeyer, R., & Berman, J. (2008). The effectiveness children and adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
of psychotherapeutic interventions for the bereaved: A com- Psychology, 78, 131–143.
prehensive, quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 134, Schut, H., & Stroebe, M. (2005). Interventions to enhance adap-
648–661. tation to bereavement. Journal of Palliative Medicine, 8,
Dijkstra, I. (2000). Living with loss: Parents grieving for the S140–147.
death of their child. Ph.D. thesis, Utrecht University, The Schut, H., & Stroebe, M. (2010). Effects of support, counselling,
Netherlands. and therapy before and after the loss: Can we really help
Dijkstra, I., & Stroebe, M. (1998). The impact of a child’s death bereaved people? In E. Zech & M. Stroebe (Eds.), Special
on parents: A myth (not yet) disproved? Journal of Family Issue on bereavement. Psychologica Belgica, 50, 89–102.
Studies, 4, 159–185. Schut, H.A.W., Stroebe, M.S., de Keijser, J., & van den Bout, J.
Eilegard, A., & Kreicbergs, U. (2010). Risk of parental dissolu- (1997). Intervention for the bereaved: Gender differences in
tion of partnership following the loss of a child to cancer: A the efficacy of grief counselling. British Journal of Clinical
population-based long-term follow-up. Archives of Pediatrics Psychology, 36, 63–72.
and Adolescent Medicine, 164, 100–101. Schut, H., Stroebe, M., van den Bout, J., & Terheggen, M. (2001).
Forstmeier, S., & Maercker, A. (2006). Comparison of two diag- The efficacy of bereavement interventions: Determining
nostic systems for complicated grief. Journal of Affective who benefits. In M. Stroebe, R.O. Hansson, W. Stroebe,
Disorders, 99, 203–211. & H. Schut(Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research:
Freud, S. (1917). Mourning and melancholia. In J. Strachey (Ed.), Consequences, coping, and care (pp. 705–737). Washington,
Standard edition of the complete works of Sigmund Freud. DC: American Psychological Association Press.
London: Hogarth Press (1957). Shear, K., Frank, E., Houck, P., & Reynolds, C. (2005).
Ha, J.-H., Carr, D., Utz, R., & Nesse, R. (2006). Older adults’ Treatment of complicated grief: A randomized controlled
perceptions of intergenerational support after widowhood. trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293,
Journal of Family Issues, 27, 3–30. 2601–2608.
Holmes, T., & Rahe, R. (1967). The social readjustment rating Shurtleff, D. (1955). Mortality and marital status. Public Health
scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213–218. Reports, 70, 248–252.
Kissane, D., & Lichtenthal, W. (2008). Family-focused grief ther- Stroebe, M.S., Hansson, R.O., Schut, H., & Stroebe, W. (2008).
apy: From palliative care into bereavement. In M.S. Stroebe, Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances
R.O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook in theory and intervention. Washington, DC: American
of bereavement research and practice: Advances in theory Psychological Association.
and intervention (pp. 485–510). Washington, DC: American Stroebe, M.S., Hansson, R.O., Stroebe, W., & Schut, H. (Eds.).
Psychological Association. (2001). Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences,
Kraus, A., & Lilienfeld, A. (1959). Some epidemiological aspects coping, and care. Washington, DC: American Psychological
of the high mortality rate in the young widowed group. Association.
Journal of Chronic Diseases, 10, 207–217. Stroebe, M.S., & Schut, H. (1999). The Dual Process Model of
Li, J., Precht, D., Mortensen, P., & Olsen, J. (2003). Mortality coping with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death
of parents after death of a child in Denmark: A nationwide Studies, 23, 197–224.
follow-up study. Lancet, 361, 363–367. Stroebe, M., & Schut, H. (2010). The Dual Process Model:
Luecken, L. (2008). Long-term consequences of parental death in A decade on. Omega: Journal of Death & Dying, 61,
childhood: Psychological and physiological mechanisms. In 273–291.
M.S. Stroebe, R.O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Stroebe, M., Schut, H., & Stroebe, W. (2007). Health conse-
Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances quences of bereavement: A review. The Lancet, 370, 1960–
in theory and intervention (pp. 397–416). Washington, DC: 1973.
American Psychological Association. Stroebe, W., & Stroebe, M. (1993). Determinants of adjust-
Middleton, W., Raphael, B., Martinek, N., & Misso, V. ment to bereavement in younger widows and widowers.
(1993). Pathological grief reactions. In M. Stroebe, In M. Stroebe et al. (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement:
Family Science 151

Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 208–226). New York: Wijngaards, L., Stroebe, M.S., Stroebe, W., Schut, H., van den
Cambridge University Press. Bout, J., van der Heijden, P., et al. (2008). Parents grieving the
Stroebe, W., Stroebe, M., Abakoumkin, G., & Schut, H. (1996). loss of their child: Interdependence in coping. British Journal
Social and emotional loneliness: A comparison of attachment of Clinical Psychology, 47, 31–42.
and stress theory explanations. Journal of Personality and Wikan, U. (1988). Bereavement and loss in two Muslim com-
Social Psychology, 70, 1241–1249. munities: Egypt and Bali compared. Social Science and
van Heck, G., & de Ridder, D. (2001). Assessment of cop- Medicine, 27, 451–460.
ing with losDimensions and measurement. In M. Stroebe, Wikan, U. (1990). Managing turbulent hearts: A Balinese formula
R.O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook for living. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
of Bereavement Research: Consequences, coping and care Winchester Nadeau, J. (2008). Meaning-making in bereaved fam-
(pp. 449–469). Washington, DC: American Psychological ilies: Assessment, intervention, and future research. In M.S.
Association. Stroebe, R.O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.),
Weiss, R. (1975). Loneliness: The experience of emotional and Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances
social loneliness. MA: MIT Press. in theory and intervention (pp. 511–530). Washington, DC:
Wijngaards-de Meij, L., Stroebe, M., Schut, H., Stroebe, W., van American Psychological Association.
den Bout, J., van der Heijden, P., et al. (2007). Neuroticism Worden, J. (1982/1991/2002/2009). Grief counselling and grief
and attachment insecurity as predictors of bereavement out- therapy: A handbook for the mental health practitioner. New
Downloaded by [University of Saskatchewan Library] at 23:41 12 January 2015

come. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 498–505. York: Springer.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai