Anda di halaman 1dari 9

685183

research-article2017
CDQXXX10.1177/1525740116685183Communication Disorders QuarterlyHenbest and Apel

Special Topic
Communication Disorders Quarterly

Effective Word Reading Instruction:


2017, Vol. 39(1) 303­–311
© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2017
Reprints and permissions:
What Does the Evidence Tell Us? sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1525740116685183
cdq.sagepub.com

Victoria S. Henbest, MS1 and Kenn Apel, PhD1

Abstract
This article is a report of recent evidence on methods for teaching young and struggling readers to read words. Specifically,
evidence comparing synthetic and analytic phonics instruction was reviewed as well as the benefit of instruction in encoding
and morphological awareness. The evidence reviewed suggests that instruction in synthetic and analytic phonics are both
effective methods for teaching word reading to young and struggling readers and the inclusion of encoding in early reading
instruction may provide added benefit. Furthermore, several investigations have found instruction in morphological
awareness to be beneficial for improving young students’ word reading. More research is needed to investigate the
effectiveness of incorporating encoding and morphological awareness instruction into early reading programs.

Keywords
word reading, instruction, phonics, decoding, encoding, morphological awareness

Research over the past several decades has been instru- Phonics-Based Instruction
mental in informing educational practice on the develop-
ment of reading and the best ways to foster this ability in One of the greatest realizations from literacy research is the
young children (e.g., Bus & van IJzendoorn, 1999; well-established finding that phonological awareness, the
McGeown & Medford, 2014; Melby-Lervåg, Lyster, & ability to think about and manipulate the sounds of one’s
Hulme, 2012; National Reading Panel [NRP], 2000). language, is one of the best predictors of literacy outcomes.
Although early reading instructional practices can differ Specifically, young children with superior phonological
considerably in the schools based on varying philosophies awareness skills are more likely to become successful word
(e.g., Allington, 2013), the overwhelming research evi- readers than children with poor phonological awareness
dence points to the use of a systematic phonics approach to skills (e.g., Bus & van IJzendoorn, 1999; MacDonald &
help young and struggling readers to develop their early Cornwall, 1995; Vellutino & Scanlon, 1987). The connec-
reading abilities (e.g., Bond & Dykstra, 1967; Chall, 1979; tion between phonological awareness (e.g., blending or seg-
NRP, 2000; Torgerson, Brooks, & Hall, 2006). The purpose menting sounds) and the ability to read is that when learning
of this article is to review the current evidence on the effec- to read, children must consciously think about the sounds in
tiveness of two specific phonics approaches, synthetic and their language and how these sounds relate to letters (i.e.,
analytic phonics, which were developed to support the orthographic awareness). Thus, having the early ability to
word-level reading of young struggling readers (i.e., early manipulate sounds prepares children for success when
elementary grades), or those who are at risk for reading learning to think about sounds and letters together. More
failure. In addition, given the strong relation between read- specifically, when children verbally blend the sounds
ing and spelling (Ehri, 2000), the effect of including train- together to decode an unknown word or segment sounds to
ing in encoding (i.e., spelling) in phonics-based instruction encode a word, they are using their phonological aware-
also is explored. Finally, recent investigations have found ness. Those who are readily able to match letters to sounds
that morphological awareness, the ability to consciously (e.g., “T” says /t/) and apply this information when they
consider and manipulate morphemes, the smallest units of read new words are thought to have broken the alphabetic
meaning in language, is a powerful contributor to an indi-
vidual’s success with word reading (e.g., Apel & Henbest, 1
University of South Carolina, Columbia, USA
2016; Apel, Wilson-Fowler, Brimo, & Perrin, 2012;
Deacon & Kirby, 2004). Thus, the evidence on morpho- Corresponding Author:
Victoria S. Henbest, Department of Communication Sciences and
logical awareness training’s effect on improving word Disorders, University of South Carolina, 1224 Sumter St., Suite 300,
reading is discussed as a possible component of early read- Columbia, SC 29201, USA.
ing instruction. Email: vhenbest@email.sc.edu
304 Communication Disorders Quarterly 39(1)

code, or mastered the alphabetic principle, a skill that is is based on differing views regarding the size of the ortho-
known to be highly predictive of later reading achievement graphic unit that should be taught to young and struggling
(e.g., Lonigan, Burgess, & Anthony, 2000; National Early readers. Synthetic phonics focuses on the smallest units of
Literacy Panel [NELP], 2008). language, that is, the connection between individual letters
In addition to evidence supporting the importance of and sounds, whereas analytic phonics deals in larger ortho-
phonological awareness and alphabetic knowledge for graphic units (e.g., Ehri & McCormick, 1998). With syn-
beginning readers, there is substantial evidence indicating thetic phonics, children are taught to segment sounds
that children’s knowledge of how to apply these skills when represented by each letter of a word and then blend the
learning to read must be explicitly taught (e.g., NRP, 2000; sounds together (i.e., decoding). For example, in synthetic
Torgerson et al., 2006). Instruction in the alphabetic princi- phonics, a child is likely to be instructed to isolate the
ple and its application when reading unknown words is a sounds represented by each letter of a word (e.g., CAT to /k/
skills-based approach to reading instruction known as pho- /ae/ /t/) and then combine them together to produce the
nics. Phonics is grounded in the view that word reading word (e.g., /kaet/). In analytic phonics, the child is likely to
skills must be explicitly taught to beginning and/or strug- be taught to manipulate the onset and rime (e.g., b-ack) of a
gling readers and is characterized by systematic and direct word rather than the individual letters and sounds. For
instruction focused on the relation between subunits of example, a child receiving analytic phonics instruction
words (e.g., graphemes, onsets, and rimes) and their corre- would be taught initial sound–letter correspondences (e.g.,
sponding sounds. Importantly, an accumulation of research B says /b/) and a corresponding rime (e.g., ACK says /aek/)
over the past 50 years has been overwhelmingly in support and then taught other initial grapheme–phoneme correspon-
of phonics-based approaches compared with other instruc- dences that can be paired with the rime. In analytic phonics,
tional methods (e.g., Bond & Dykstra, 1967; Chall, 1979; little or no attention is given to blending the individual
NRP, 2000; Torgerson et al., 2006). Other methods that sounds in words.
have been compared with systematic phonics include Researchers in favor of synthetic phonics believe that
embedded or implicit phonics, in which letter–sound rela- early in the process of learning to read, children use graph-
tions are taught in the context of meaningful text with little eme–phoneme correspondences to decode words, and this
to no time devoted to explicitly teaching the alphabetic process is what facilitates storage of these words in memory
code, whole word, or “look and say” methods in which stu- and prepares individuals to take on new, unfamiliar words
dents are taught to memorize whole words, and whole-lan- (e.g., Ehri & McCormick, 1998; Ehri & Robbins, 1992). In
guage approaches, in which children are provided with a fact, research by Ehri and Robbins (1992) indicated that
literacy-rich environment and expected to learn to read children must have at least some knowledge of grapheme–
independently without explicit instruction in word reading phoneme correspondences before they are able to read by
(e.g., Cain, 2010; NRP, 2000). In 2000, the NRP completed analogy, the goal of analytic phonics. A synthetic phonics
a meta-analysis examining the effects of systematic pho- approach is further supported by research suggesting that
nics–based instruction compared with the abovementioned when left to their own devices, young readers often rely on
other approaches on the word reading skills of young ele- phonological decoding (i.e., blending of sounds) to read
mentary-age students with varying abilities (e.g., struggling unfamiliar words, regardless of which method of phonics
readers, typical readers, children at risk for developing instruction they have received (e.g., Juel & Minden-Cupp,
reading difficulty), and results were largely in favor of sys- 2000; Walton, Walton, & Felton, 2001). However, support-
tematic phonics for improving children’s reading of decod- ers of analytic phonics suggest that children make use of
able, regularly spelled words, irregularly spelled words, and larger subunits of words early on in their process of learning
reading comprehension. to read (e.g., Goswami & Bryant, 1990) and that this devel-
opment should be capitalized on in early reading instruc-
Two Types of Systematic Phonics– tion. This view is based on research that shows a connection
between children’s early understanding of rhymes and the
Based Instruction relation between the ability to manipulate rhymes and later
The hallmark of systematic phonics is that instruction on reading skills. Furthermore, it is based on research suggest-
how to recognize and manipulate orthographic units of ing that children’s ability to consciously think about rhymes
words is explicit, structured, and developmentally precedes their ability to manipulate individual sounds (e.g.,
sequenced (NRP, 2000). However, not all systematic pho- Bryant, MacLean, Bradley, & Crossland, 1990).
nics approaches are the same, and there are two overarching The meta-analysis by the NRP (2000) sought to disen-
types of phonics instruction that are the topic of much tangle the controversy over which type of phonics instruc-
debate and investigation in the field of literacy research. tion is best by analyzing whether some types of
These two approaches are often referred to as synthetic and phonics-based approaches are better than others. Their anal-
analytic phonics. The difference between the two approaches ysis of 38 different studies revealed that the effect size for
Henbest and Apel 305

the synthetic phonics approaches reviewed was moderate (d decoding, but that contained different initial letters than
= 0.45), small to moderate for the analytic programs (d = words targeted in the analytic phonics instruction; (c) non-
0.34), and small for other miscellaneous systematic phonics words; and (d) words that could be read only by decoding.
programs (d = 0.27). Although the synthetic phonics Results from Walton et al. (2001) indicated that overall,
approaches collectively resulted in the largest of the three children in the analytic phonics group read significantly
effect sizes, the analysis did not detect a significant differ- more words following treatment than children in the syn-
ence among the effect of synthetic phonics compared with thetic phonics group, but those children in both treatment
analytic phonics. The NRP concluded that, although sys- groups read more words than the children with low preread-
tematic phonics programs are significantly more effective ing skills who did not receive intervention. Also, further
than nonphonics-based programs, there is no evidence analyses revealed that on three of the four reading mea-
favoring one type of phonics instruction over the other. sures, children in the synthetic phonics and analytic phonics
groups read an equal number of words, but that the analytic
Current Studies Comparing the Effects phonics group read a larger number of words that could not
be read using decoding strategies, resulting in a medium
of Synthetic Versus Analytic Phonics
effect size (d = 0.58). In addition, students in the synthetic
Although several investigations on the effectiveness of sys- phonics group did not read significantly more of these
tematic phonics instruction have been conducted since words than the low reading students receiving no interven-
2000, the majority have been multiple baseline studies or tion. Walton et al. noted that their results may be an indica-
investigations involving a control group not receiving any tion that instruction in analytic phonics lead to increased
kind of systematic phonics, such as business as usual or skills with decoding, but that experience with synthetic
implicit phonics (e.g., Blachman et al., 2004; Denton, phonics did not transfer to reading words by analogy. To
Fletcher, Anthony, & Francis, 2006; Pullen et al., 2005; investigate the long-term effects of the two treatment condi-
Ryder, Tunmer, & Greaney, 2008; Vadasy, Sanders, & tions, Walton et al. conducted a follow-up investigation 4
Tudor, 2007). As expected, the results of these studies have months after the completion of the intervention and found
been largely in favor of systematic phonics for improving that students in both treatment conditions maintained their
the word reading of young struggling readers. However, word reading abilities, and students in the analytic phonics
some studies have directly investigated the effect of syn- group, in general, continued to read more words than stu-
thetic phonics compared with analytic phonics on the read- dents in the synthetic phonics group including words that
ing outcomes of young readers (e.g., Christensen & Bowey, could not be read using decoding strategies, although the
2005; Comaskey, Savage, & Abrami, 2009; Di Stasio, effect size was smaller than at immediate posttest (d =
Savage, & Abrami, 2012; Finnegan, 2012; Johnston, 0.28). In addition, at follow-up, the analytic phonics group
McGeown, & Watson, 2012; Johnston & Watson, 2004; read significantly more words than the synthetic phonics
Savage, Abrami, Hipps, & Deault, 2009; Walton et al., groups for words that could be read using decoding or anal-
2001). For example, immediately following the release of ogy strategies, yielding a moderate effect size (d = 0.45).
the NRP (2000) report, Walton et al. (2001) compared the In contrast to the abovementioned finding, other studies
two systematic phonics–based approaches and their effects have found improvements in word reading scores to be in
on the reading skills of first-grade students identified as favor of synthetic phonics instruction. For example,
having poor prereading skills. Seventy-four first-grade stu- Johnston and Watson (2004) compared the two interven-
dents were randomly assigned within their classrooms to tions’ effects on word reading scores for 5-year-old students
one of four groups: synthetic phonics, analytic phonics, no in their first year of school. Different from that of Walton
intervention with low reading skills, or no intervention with et al. (2001), this study was implemented during classroom
high reading skills. Students in the treatment groups instruction, rather than small groups. Students were assigned
received either synthetic or analytic phonics instruction in to either the analytic or synthetic phonics group and received
groups of two to four students, for two sessions per week their respective interventions for 20 min per day for 16
for a total of 11 weeks. Both treatment groups also included weeks. Results indicated that children in the synthetic pho-
instruction in rhyming, phoneme identity, and letter–sound nics group outperformed those in the analytic phonics group
correspondences. Students in the synthetic phonics group, on multiple measures of word and emergent reading with a
however, were taught to “sound out” the letters in words large effect size for word reading age (d = 0.85) and percent
and then blend them together to pronounce the word, of nonwords read correctly (d = 1.34). In addition, the large
whereas children in the analytic phonics group were taught effect ( η2p = 0.11) for word reading skills of those in the
to segment words using onset-rime boundaries. Posttest synthetic phonics program was maintained over time
measures required that the children read four different sets (Johnston et al., 2012).
of words: (a) words that could not be read successfully by Christensen and Bowey (2005) also addressed which
decoding; (b) words that could be read by analogy or type of phonics approach was more effective for improving
306 Communication Disorders Quarterly 39(1)

reading outcomes, but in 7-year-old children. They found group intervention, on the reading outcomes of 85 children
that at posttest, instruction in grapheme–phoneme corre- with reading disabilities ranging from age 6 to 13 years.
spondences (i.e., synthetic phonics) was slightly more Children were matched and randomly assigned to one of the
favorable than the instruction in orthographic-rime (i.e., following five groups: (a) synthetic phonics only, (b) ana-
analytic). That is, at posttest, on most reading measures, the lytic phonics only, (c) synthetic phonics followed by ana-
performance of students in each of the treatment groups was lytic phonics, (d) analytic phonics followed by synthetic
not significantly different, except that the synthetic phonics phonics, or (e) classroom survival skills/math instruction.
group outperformed the analytic phonics group on accuracy Instruction occurred for 1 hr daily for a total of 70 hr.
and speed of reading transfer words (i.e., unfamiliar words Results indicated that, regardless of the order of lessons,
not worked on during intervention) with large and medium instruction that included both analytic and synthetic pho-
effect sizes (η2 = 0.23 and .11, respectively). nics was superior (i.e., larger effect sizes) to the analytic or
In contrast to the findings from the abovementioned stud- synthetic phonics instruction alone in improving the word
ies, but in line with the results from the NRP (2000), some reading skills of the children. In line with the results from
researchers have found little to no difference among the two Lovett et al., a study of teacher observations by Juel and
treatment approaches (e.g., Comaskey et al., 2009; Savage Minden-Cupp (2000) suggested that students with poor
et al., 2009; Savage, Carless, & Stuart, 2003) on measures of early reading skills may benefit more from instruction in
word and pseudoword reading. In a follow-up study by Di both decoding and onset-rime analogies. Furthermore, early
Stasio et al. (2012) examining the performance of students in and intensive instruction in letter–phoneme relations and
the Comaskey et al. (2009) study a year after receiving com- the sounding out and blending of phonemes may best ben-
puter-based phonics interventions, there continued to be no efit young struggling readers. In contrast, Savage et al.
difference in word reading scores among the two approaches; (2003) found that there was no meaningful difference in
however, the students who received training in analytic pho- performance on measures of word and pseudoword reading
nics demonstrated a large effect for growth in reading com- between 6-year-old children receiving the two intervention
prehension skills ( η2p = 0.15) beyond those of the students types alone, or when combined.
who received the synthetic approach. Similarly, Kyle, To date, there is no conclusive evidence regarding which
Kujala, Richardson, Lyytinen, and Goswami (2013) found type of phonics instruction is most effective for improving
no discernable difference between the effects of computer- word reading skills, and whether there is a substantial advan-
based synthetics and analytics phonics programs on 6- and tage of combining the programs; future research is necessary
7-year-olds’ word reading performance. to unravel the effects of the two major phonics-based
Overall, results of the investigations comparing syn- approaches. However, what is known is that systematic pho-
thetic and analytic phonics do not provide a clear picture of nics instruction, whether it be at the phoneme–grapheme
which of the two approaches is superior for helping young level or onset-rime, must be explicitly taught to young chil-
readers learn to read unfamiliar words and indicate that they dren in the early stages of learning to read. Importantly, chil-
are, for the most part, comparable. In addition, it is difficult dren in phonics-based classrooms and/or who receive
to make direct comparisons across the studies due to several supplemental phonics-based interventions perform better on
factors such as age of participants, method of instruction measures of word reading than their peers who do not receive
(e.g., whole class vs. small group), differences in the mate- any type of systematic instruction. In addition to the contin-
rials/methods in which each phonics-based program was ued investigation of the active ingredients in phonics-based
taught, and differences in dosage of treatment. Furthermore, instruction, investigators and educators should direct their
results of two meta-analyses have concluded that the effects attention to other aspects of early reading instruction such as
of synthetic and analytic phonics are indistinguishable the incorporation of encoding in phonics-based programs.
(NRP, 2000; Torgerson et al., 2006).
Benefits of Including Encoding in
Studies Investigating the Effect of Phonics Instruction
Combined Synthetic and Analytic
Given that it is often included in phonics-based reading pro-
Phonics grams, it is important to examine the inclusion of encoding
In light of the NRP (2000) findings, some teams of research- practice as a component of early reading instruction. Explicit
ers have investigated whether there is any added value in instruction in encoding for beginning readers consists of
combining the two systematic phonics–based approaches teaching individuals to segment letters and the sounds they
(e.g., Lovett et al., 2000; Savage & Carless, 2005; Savage make and build words using letter manipulatives. The idea is
et al., 2003). The results of these investigations, are again, to allow children the opportunity to consciously work with
mixed. For example, Lovett et al. (2000) investigated the letters and their sounds to make new words (Weiser &
effect of four different treatment approaches and a control Mathes, 2011). Although instruction in encoding is often
Henbest and Apel 307

incorporated into phonics-based instruction, the effect of The year following the review, Weiser (2012) aimed to
this added component has received little direct investigation. determine whether there is a synergistic relation between
For example, Christensen and Bowey (2005), as well as instruction in decoding and encoding. That is, the interest
Johnston and Watson (2004), included encoding activities in was whether a combination of encoding and decoding
at least one of the phonics-based approaches investigated in instruction was more effective in improving the literacy
their studies (e.g., when provided with a picture, the child skills of young readers above the contribution of decoding
“built” the word using magnetic tiles); however, the effect of and encoding separately. In her study, she coded the amount
the inclusion of encoding was not directly compared with a of time teachers spent embedding encoding instruction with
phonics-based intervention, which did not include encoding decoding instruction in the classroom and in a supplemental
as an instructional component. The potential effectiveness of phonics-based intervention program for first-grade students
encoding practice in a phonics-based program is an impor- at risk for reading disabilities. Results indicated that the
tant consideration given the relation among reading and amount of time students spent receiving encoding instruc-
spelling and the potential differences in outcomes that may tion simultaneously with decoding instruction, in both the
be attributed to whether instruction in spelling is included in classroom and in a supplemental intervention, explained
phonics-based treatments (Weiser & Mathes, 2011). It about 76% of the variance in the students’ literacy
should be noted that in the study by Christensen and Bowey, outcomes.
encoding instruction was included only in the synthetic pho- Although the review by Weiser and Mathes (2011) and
nics program, not the analytic one, and results suggested that the experimental investigation of Weiser (2012) provided
students in the synthetic phonics group performed signifi- support for including encoding in early phonics-based
cantly better on a measure of reading accuracy and speed of instruction, considerably more research is needed to inves-
recognition of unfamiliar words. tigate the specific benefits of including encoding in early
The NRP (2000) did not investigate the encoding com- reading programs and interventions. Future investigations
ponent of phonics-based instruction. However, some infor- should compare synthetic phonics plus encoding versus
mation is currently available regarding the effect of synthetic phonics without encoding, and the same for ana-
including encoding in phonics-based instruction. Weiser lytic phonics. Furthermore, the combination of the two pho-
and Mathes (2011) conducted a best-evidence synthesis on nics approaches and the inclusion of encoding practice
the impact of direct and explicit encoding instruction in should be investigated. However, the evidence suggests that
early reading interventions for children at risk for reading incorporating encoding into early phonics-based instruction
failure. They identified and reviewed 11 experimental stud- is likely to be beneficial for most students.
ies that incorporated instruction in encoding, and results
indicated that the inclusion of encoding in phonics-based Morphological Awareness in Early
instruction, as compared with other early reading approaches
(e.g., whole language, implicit phonics) or no interventions
Reading Instruction
at all, was highly effective in improving word reading and Thus far, attention has been on instruction focused on the
spelling outcomes for young struggling readers (d = 0.81). relation between individual sounds and letters. However, a
The authors concluded that explicit practice in building review of the evidence on the effective ways to teach young
phoneme–grapheme correspondences likely benefited stu- children to read words would be incomplete without consid-
dents’ overall literacy outcomes (i.e., reading, phonological ering instruction in morphological awareness as a promising
awareness, reading comprehension, spelling) due to it endeavor for improving the word reading skills of young
allowing them to develop more detailed orthographic repre- readers. Morphological awareness is the ability to con-
sentations. Although this review provides some support for sciously identify, reflect on, and manipulate morphemes, the
the incorporation of encoding in phonics instruction, it did smallest units of meaning in a language. Given that written
not directly compare whether there was an increased benefit English is a morphophonemic language (i.e., word spellings
to adding encoding instruction to decoding instruction. To reflect both the word and sound structure of the language),
clarify, the studies reviewed by Weiser and Mathes did not the relation between children’s reading outcomes and their
contain comparison groups that included one group of stu- ability to consciously think about morphemes has been a
dents who received phonics-based instruction plus encod- topic of increased interest in recent years. Notably, research-
ing versus a phonics-based approach with no direct focus on ers have found that children’s morphological awareness
encoding. The finding that the systematic phonics–based skills are positively related to their reading outcomes (e.g.,
programs that included encoding were more effective than Apel & Henbest, 2016; Apel et al., 2012; Deacon & Kirby,
other types of reading instruction is not a surprise consider- 2004; Kirby et al., 2012; Nagy, Berninger, & Abbott, 2006;
ing the well-established finding that any type of systematic Wolter, Wood, & D’zatko, 2009).
phonics is better than nonexplicit phonics, for increasing Instruction in morphological awareness involves teach-
the word reading of young readers. ing students to think about base words, prefixes and
308 Communication Disorders Quarterly 39(1)

suffixes, their meanings, and how base words and affixes morphological awareness following the intervention, but no
are combined and spelled. For example, during this type of significant gains were found on word reading scores.
instruction, a child might be encouraged to identify the base Similarly, a study by Wolter and Dilworth (2014) found
word (underlined) in the word “unfriendly” and then iden- that, for second-grade students who were in a letter–sound-
tify the prefix and suffix (bolded). Several review papers focused intervention, adding morphological awareness to
have been published on the effect of morphological aware- the instruction did not result in any greater gains than the
ness instruction on students’ literacy outcomes. In the first letter–sound intervention alone.
of these reviews, Reed (2008) examined three studies that The results of the aforementioned systematic reviews
directly investigated the effect of morphological interven- and meta-analyses support the incorporation of instruction
tions on word reading scores and found medium effect sizes in morphological awareness in early word reading instruc-
(ds ranged from 0.60 to 0.79), on word reading accuracy tion. However, some recent studies have resulted in null
and speed. Two years following, Bowers, Kirby, and Deacon effects for morphological awareness interventions’ contri-
(2010) completed a systematic review on 22 intervention bution to word reading skills. The mixed findings from
studies that included morphological awareness instruction recent studies may be due to differing focuses in the mor-
as a component. Overall results indicated that morphologi- phological awareness interventions, the length and dosage
cal awareness interventions as compared with untreated of the interventions, or age of participants. Undoubtedly,
control groups resulted in moderate effect sizes (d = 0.41) further research is needed to parse out the active ingredients
for word reading outcomes (e.g., word recognition accuracy in morphological awareness interventions, and future inves-
and speed) and that morphological awareness instruction tigations should examine which portions of morphological
was particularly effective for struggling readers and those in awareness interventions are most effective for increasing
early elementary grades. Furthermore, embedding morpho- the word reading skills of early readers. Nonetheless,
logical awareness instruction with other reading instruction instruction in morphological awareness seems to be a prom-
methods (e.g., phonics) was more effective than teaching ising component for early word reading instruction.
morphological awareness in isolation.
Contrastively, in a review by Goodwin and Ahn (2010),
although significant effects were found for morphological
Conclusion
awareness instruction on several literacy measures (e.g., To summarize the findings presented in this report, there is
phonological awareness, reading comprehension), no sig- evidence to support that explicit instruction covering mul-
nificant effects were found for morphological instruction on tiple linguistic aspects of written language is beneficial for
students’ decoding skills. However, more recently, a meta- improving the word reading skills of young readers.
analysis conducted by Goodwin and Ahn (2013) resulted in Particularly, evidence for explicit instruction in letter–
different findings. In their analysis of 30 studies, morpho- sound relations focusing on both grapheme–phoneme cor-
logical interventions resulted in significantly greater gains respondences and/or onset-rime is well established, and
on several reading measures including decoding (d = 0.59). educators can be confident that they are implementing evi-
Consistent with the review by Bowers et al. (2010), effects dence-based practice when they are providing these types
were greater for younger students. of instruction to young readers. It is recommended that
Since the publication of Goodwin and Ahn’s (2013) future research continue to investigate the effects of com-
meta-analysis, investigations on morphological awareness bining both synthetic and analytic phonics instruction for
interventions have yielded mixed results. For example, improving the word reading skills of early elementary-age
Apel, Brimo, Diehm, and Apel (2013) conducted a feasibil- students.
ity study investigating the effect of a morphological aware- Although further research is needed to parse out the
ness intervention on the literacy abilities of kindergarten, exact effects of instruction in encoding and morphological
first- and second-grade students. The intervention was awareness, there is mounting of evidence to date that sug-
implemented in small groups, 4 days per week for 9 weeks gests that including these components in early reading pro-
in 25-min sessions. The lessons provided explicit instruc- grams is beneficial for improving word reading skills. To
tion in prefixes and suffixes and how to manipulate these in provide stronger support for these methods of instruction,
various ways. For example, one activity was a word sort in however, the added benefit of including encoding practice
which the children were required to sort words based on the and instruction in morphological awareness should con-
presence or absence of the target affix (e.g., plural “s”). tinue to be investigated. These future investigations should
Results of this investigation indicated that effect sizes were include examining which types of morphological awareness
medium to large for all three grades on measures of word instruction are most effective, when, and with which popu-
reading and pseudoword reading (ds ranged from 0.50 to lations. Until some of these questions are answered, it is
0.87). However, in contrast, another study by Apel and recommended that early elementary educators provide sys-
Diehm (2013) found significant gains on measures of tematic phonics–based instruction to their early readers and
Henbest and Apel 309

Table 1.  Examples of the Linguistic Units Addressed in Three Instructional Strategies (Synthetic and Analytic Phonics and
Morphological Awareness) and Expected Learning Outcomes.

Instructional Linguistic units


strategy addressed Example What the child learns
Synthetic Letter–sound “fat” [f] - [a] - [t] /f/ - /æ/ - /t/ “I can read the word fat because I can match letters
phonics to sounds, like f says /f/, a says /æ/, and t says /t/.
When I blend these sounds together, I make the
word fat.”
Analytic Onset–rime “lip” [l] - [ip] /l/- /ɪp/ “I can read the word lip because I know the letter l
phonics makes the sound /l/, and the last group of letters, ip
make the sounds /ɪp/. I know this because I know
other words that end in /ɪp/ like dip, sip, hip, and
how they are spelled. This can help me read lip.”
Morphological Morphemes “magician” [magic]-[ian] “When I see magician, I see the word magic (but I
awareness don’t hear it). Still, I believe magician is related to
magic; it shares meaning. I see the ian. I’ve seen ian
in other words and it means—a person who does,
such as musician, or comedian. So, magician must
mean, someone who does magic. I can read the
word magician.”

consider the incorporation of other linguistic-based strate- Apel, K., & Henbest, V. S. (2016). Affix meaning knowledge in
gies in their reading programs. See Table 1 for examples of first through third grade students. Language, Speech, and
instructional strategies. Instruction on how to read unknown Hearing Services in Schools, 47, 148–156.
words should undoubtedly be explicit and direct. Based on Apel, K., Wilson-Fowler, E. B., Brimo, D., & Perrin, N. A. (2012).
Metalinguistic contributions to reading and spelling in second
the results of the studies reviewed here, providing early
and third grade students. Reading and Writing, 25, 1283–1305.
word reading instruction in a manner that draws students’
Blachman, B. A., Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D.
attention to the phonological, orthographic, and morpho- J., Clonan, S. M., Shaywitz, B. A., & Shaywitz, S. E. (2004).
logical relations in written language is appropriate for facil- Effects of intensive reading remediation for second and
itating early reading skills, especially for our youngest third graders and a 1-year follow-up. Journal of Educational
readers. Psychology, 96, 444–461.
Bond, G. L., & Dykstra, R. (1967). The cooperative research pro-
Declaration of Conflicting Interests gram in first-grade reading instruction. Reading Research
Quarterly, 2, 5–142.
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects
respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this of morphological instruction on literacy skills: A systematic
article. review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 80,
144–179.
Funding Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990).
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to
authorship, and/or publication of this article. read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429–438.
Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological aware-
ness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental train-
References ing studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403–414.
Allington, R. L. (2013). What really matters when working with Cain, K. (2010). Reading development and difficulties. Chichester,
struggling readers. The Reading Teacher, 66, 520–530. UK: John Wiley.
doi:10.1002/TRTR.1154 Chall, J. (1979). The great debate: Ten years later, with a modest
Apel, K., Brimo, D., Diehm, E., & Apel, L. (2013). Morphological proposal for reading stages. In L. G. Resnick & P. A. Weaver
awareness intervention with kindergartners and first- and sec- (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 1, pp. 29–56).
ond-grade students from low socioeconomic status homes: A Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
feasibility study. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Christensen, C. A., & Bowey, J. A. (2005). The efficacy of
Schools, 44, 161–173. orthographic rime, grapheme–phoneme correspondence,
Apel, K., & Diehm, E. (2013). Morphological awareness inter- and implicit phonics approaches to teaching decoding skills.
vention with kindergarteners and first and second grade stu- Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 327–349.
dents from low SES homes: A small efficacy study. Journal Comaskey, E. M., Savage, R. S., & Abrami, P. (2009). A ran-
of Learning Disabilities, 47, 65–75. domised efficacy study of Web-based synthetic and analytic
310 Communication Disorders Quarterly 39(1)

programmes among disadvantaged urban Kindergarten chil- Lovett, M. W., Lacerenza, L., Borden, S. L., Frijters, J. C.,
dren. Journal of Research in Reading, 32, 92–108. Steinbach, K. A., & De Palma, M. (2000). Components of
Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: effective remediation for developmental reading disabilities:
Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and Combining phonological and strategy-based instruction to
phonological awareness in reading development. Applied improve outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92,
Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238. 263-283.
Denton, C. A., Fletcher, J. M., Anthony, J. L., & Francis, D. J. MacDonald, G. W., & Cornwall, A. (1995). The relationship
(2006). An evaluation of intensive intervention for students between phonological awareness and reading and spell-
with persistent reading difficulties. Journal of Learning ing achievement eleven years later. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 39, 447–466. Disabilities, 28, 523–527.
Di Stasio, M. R., Savage, R., & Abrami, P. C. (2012). A fol- McGeown, S. P., & Medford, E. (2014). Using method of instruc-
low-up study of the ABRACADABRA web-based literacy tion to predict the skills supporting initial reading develop-
intervention in Grade 1. Journal of Research in Reading, ment: Insight from a synthetic phonics approach. Reading and
35, 69–86. Writing, 27, 591–608.
Ehri, L. C. (2000). Learning to read and learning to spell: Two Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012).
sides of a coin. Topics in Language Disorders, 20, 19–36. Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-
Ehri, L. C., & McCormick, S. (1998). Phases of word learning: analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322–352.
Implications for instruction with delayed and disabled read- Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions
ers. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 14, 135–163. of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of
Ehri, L. C., & Robbins, C. (1992). Beginners need some decoding upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of
skill to read words by analogy. Reading Research Quarterly, Educational Psychology, 98, 134–147.
27, 13–26. National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy.
Finnegan, E. G. (2012). Two approaches to phonics instruc- Washington, DC: Author.
tion: Comparison of effects with children with significant National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An
cognitive disability. Education and Training in Autism and evidence-based assessment of the scientific research litera-
Developmental Disabilities, 47, 269–279. ture on reading and its implications for reading instruction
Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2010). A meta-analysis of morpholog- (NIH Pub. No. 00–4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Government
ical interventions: Effects on literacy achievement of children Printing Office.
with literacy difficulties. Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 183–208. Pullen, P. C., Lane, H. B., Lloyd, J. W., Lloyd, J. W., Nowak, R.,
Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2013). A meta-analysis of morpho- & Ryals, J. (2005). Effects of explicit instruction on decoding
logical interventions in English: Effects on literacy outcomes of struggling first grade students: A data-based case study.
for school-age children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, Education and Treatment of Children, 28, 63–75.
257–285. Reed, D. K. (2008). A synthesis of morphology interventions and
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learn- effects on reading outcomes for students in grades K–12.
ing to read. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 23, 36–49.
Johnston, R. S., McGeown, S., & Watson, J. E. (2012). Long-term Ryder, J. F., Tunmer, W. E., & Greaney, K. T. (2008). Explicit
effects of synthetic versus analytic phonics teaching on the instruction in phonemic awareness and phonemically based
reading and spelling ability of 10 year old boys and girls. decoding skills as an intervention strategy for struggling read-
Reading and Writing, 25, 1365–1384. ers in whole language classrooms. Reading and Writing, 21,
Johnston, R. S., & Watson, J. E. (2004). Accelerating the develop- 349–369.
ment of reading, spelling and phonemic awareness skills in Savage, R. S., Abrami, P., Hipps, G., & Deault, L. (2009). A ran-
initial readers. Reading and Writing, 17, 327–357. domized controlled trial study of the ABRACADABRA read-
Juel, C., & Minden-Cupp, C. (2000). Learning to read words: ing intervention program in grade 1. Journal of Educational
Linguistic units and instructional strategies. Reading Research Psychology, 101, 590–604.
Quarterly, 35, 458–492. Savage, R. S., & Carless, S. (2005). Learning support assistants
Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade- can deliver effective reading interventions for “at-risk” chil-
Woolley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphologi- dren. Educational Research, 47, 45–61.
cal awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing, 25, Savage, R. S., Carless, S., & Stuart, M. (2003). The effects of
389–410. rime- and phoneme-based teaching delivered by learning sup-
Kyle, F., Kujala, J., Richardson, U., Lyytinen, H., & Goswami, U. port assistants. Journal of Research in Reading, 26, 211–233.
(2013). Assessing the effectiveness of two theoretically moti- Torgerson, C., Brooks, G., & Hall, J. (2006). A systematic review
vated computer-assisted reading interventions in the United of the research literature on the use of phonics in the teach-
Kingdom: GG Rime and GG Phoneme. Reading Research ing of reading and spelling. Nottingham, UK: Department of
Quarterly, 48, 61–76. Education and Skills.
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Vadasy, P. F., Sanders, E. A., & Tudor, S. (2007). Effectiveness
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in of paraeducator-supplemented individual instruction beyond
preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudi- basic decoding skills. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40,
nal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613. 508–525.
Henbest and Apel 311

Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological cod- Weiser, B. L., & Mathes, P. (2011). Using encoding instruction to
ing, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence improve the reading and spelling performances of elementary
from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer students at risk for literacy difficulties: A best-evidence syn-
Quarterly, 33, 321–363. thesis. Review of Educational Research, 81, 170–200.
Walton, P. D., Walton, L. M., & Felton, K. (2001). Teaching rime Wolter, J. A., & Dilworth, V. (2014). The effects of a multilin-
analogy or letter recoding reading strategies to prereaders: guistic morphological awareness approach for improving
Effects on prereading skills and word reading. Journal of language and literacy. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47,
Educational Psychology, 93, 160–180. 76–85.
Weiser, B. L. (2012). Ameliorating reading disabilities early: Wolter, J. A., Wood, A., & D’zatko, K. W. (2009). The influence
Examining an effective encoding and decoding preven- of morphological awareness on the literacy development of
tion instruction model. Learning Disability Quarterly, 36, first-grade children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
161–177. in Schools, 40, 286–298.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai