Anda di halaman 1dari 10

JFS S: Sensory and Food Quality

Effect of Reference Foods in Repeated


Acceptability Tests: Testing Familiar
and Novel Foods Using 2 Acceptability Scales
A. NASSER EL DINE AND A. OLABI

ABSTRACT: Hedonic tests are routinely used to assess the acceptance of food products. However, these single tests
may not be the best approach for predicting long-term use. The objectives of this study were, first, to check whether a
difference from reference score is more sensitive to changes in hedonic scores, second, to assess whether the labeled
affective scale (LAM) is more sensitive to differences than the 9-point scale, and third, to assess the effect of repeated
exposure on the hedonic scores of neophilic and neophobic panelists for familiar and novel foods. Two groups of 41
panelists were tested with either the 9-point hedonic scale or LAM scale. Panelists received a food neophobia ques-
tionnaire and were subsequently classified to neophobic, neophilic, or neutral. Ten foods, including 5 novel and 5
familiar, were used. In each session, 5 to 6 foods were served twice/week for 4 wk. Serving frequency ranged between
1 and 8 times (1, 2, 4, 6, 8). Data analyses were performed 3 times, using either absolute acceptability scores or rela-
tive scores, that is, the difference between absolute scores and scores for either the reference (cracker, RELFAM) or a
novel food (pickled-ginger, RELNOV) served in every session. The 3 analyses (absolute, RELFAM, and RELNOV) gen-
erated similar results with respect to the number of significant differences between foods. There was no major drift
in acceptability scores with sessions. A significant food effect was obtained (P < 0.05) and a significant food × neo-
phobia (P < 0.05) was noted for 2 novel foods, pickled ginger, and lychee, whereby neophobic panelists were less
accepting of them. Both scales were equally sensitive with some advantages for LAM over the 9-point hedonic scale.
Keywords: acceptability, LAM scale, neophobia, reference foods, repeated exposure

Introduction The increase in acceptance with repeated exposure has been

S: Sensory & Food


F ood items are typically assessed for their acceptability in
single-taste sessions. Despite the short time needed for 1-time
described as a “mere exposure” effect (Pliner 1982; Crandall 1984;
Porcherot and Issanchou 1998) and has been shown to be ben-

Quality
hedonic assessments and the consequent cost savings, several eficial for increasing the acceptability of healthier foods. Tuorila-
problems have been noticed. These drawbacks range from the Olikainen (1987) reported a reduction in liking for whole milk and
serving size (Zandstra and others 1999), to the artificial testing an increase in liking for the skim milk version. Similarly, Mattes
environment (Meiselman and others 2000a) and the frequency of (1993) obtained a significant decrease in preference for 4 preferred
exposure to food items (De Graaf and others 2005). In these labo- high fat level foods after a long-term reduced fat diet.
ratory tests, the measurement is restricted to momentary impres- The development of food boredom or a monotony effect is
sions and the development of the appreciation of products over demonstrated by a decrease in hedonic ratings with repeated expo-
time is not considered. Thus, they could not be always considered sure (Stubenitsky and others 1999; Zandstra and others 2000b). The
as good predictors of consumers’ behavior toward foods in real- monotony effect has been shown to be dependent on several fac-
life situations. Accordingly, the acceptability of foods may change tors, including the type of food (Schutz and Pilgrim 1958), its initial
after repeated consumption (Kamen and Peryam 1961; Tuorila- pleasantness (Schutz and Pilgrim 1958; Siegel and Pilgrim 1958),
Ollikainen and others 1986; Vickers and Holton 1998; Hetherington typical frequency of intake (Hetherington and others 2000, 2002),
and others 2000; Zandstra and others 2000a, 2000b; Hetherington and the availability of choice (Kamen and Peryam 1961; Zandstra
and others 2002; Chung and Vickers 2007), though there are stud- and others 2000b; Raynor and others 2006), which generally in-
ies that have revealed consistent hedonic scores over a short time creases acceptability. Regarding the type of food, staple foods have
period of few days (Goldman 1994). Therefore, acceptance could been shown to be less susceptible to a monotony effect (Schutz and
be reinforced (Pliner 1982) or could decline with repeated exposure Pilgrim 1958).
(Hetherington and others 2000, 2002). Furthermore, initial hedo- In addition, the change in acceptability with repeated exposure
nic ratings were maintained for some foods while that of others could depend on the novelty of the food and the food neophobia
decreased in previous studies (Schutz and Pilgrim 1958; Siegel and level of panelists. The Food Neophobia Scale (FNS) was developed
Pilgrim 1958). by Pliner and Hobden (1992) and has been the tool of choice for
measuring food neophobia levels. Several studies have shown that
“mere exposure” to the taste of unfamiliar foods increased liking for
MS 20080536 Submitted 7/17/2008, Accepted 11/5/2008. Authors are with these foods (Birch and Marlin 1982; Pliner 1982). In Pliner’s (1982)
Nutrition and Food Science Dept., American Univ. of Beirut, Riad El Solh investigation, acceptability of novel tropical fruit juices increased
1107 2020, Beirut, Lebanon. Direct inquiries to author Olabi (E-mail: after 20 trials and the exposure effect was explained as the dissipa-
ammar.olabi@aub.edu.lb).
tion of food neophobia.


R

C 2009 Institute of Food Technologists Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009—JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE S97
doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3841.2008.01034.x
Further reproduction without permission is prohibited
Use of reference foods in acceptability . . .

A variety of sensory tools have been used to assess the accept- differences, and third, that neophobic panelists would be less ac-
ability of foods, including the 9-point hedonic scale (Peryam and cepting of novel foods. The objectives of this study were, first, to
Girardot 1952; Peryam and Pilgrim 1957), the labeled affective mag- check whether a difference from reference score is more sensitive
nitude scale (Schutz and Cardello 2001; Cardello and Schutz 2004), to changes in hedonic scores, second, to assess whether the labeled
the FACT scale (Schutz 1964), and others. However, despite the affective scale (LAM) is more sensitive to differences than the 9-
simplicity and ease of use of the 9-point hedonic scale, several point scale, and third, to assess the effect of repeated exposure on
problems were reported, including the lack of equivalence between the hedonic scores of neophilic and neophobic panelists for famil-
intervals (Peryam and Pilgrim 1957; Schutz and Cardello 2001), ex- iar and novel foods.
cessive use of the neutral category as a “safe place” (Jones and oth-
ers 1955), avoidance of end anchors, and lack of a true zero and Materials and Methods
of independent judgments (Stevens and Galanter 1957). Accord-
ingly, studies have compared the sensitivity and reliability of this Preliminary tests
scale compared with the magnitude estimation scale and obtained Preliminary tests were carried out with the aim of selecting ade-
better sensitivity with the magnitude estimation (Mc Daniel and quate foods for the experimental sessions. A total of 30 to 35 pan-
Sawyer 1981) or similar levels of sensitivity (Moskowitz and Sidel elists participated in each of the 7 preliminary tests. The panelists
1971; Lavenka and Kamen 1994; Barylko-Pikielna and others 2004) were mostly graduate students and staff from the Nutrition and
and similar levels of sensitivity, reliability, and precision (Warren Food Science Dept. at the American Univ. of Beirut. During each
and others 1982; Pearce and others 1986). The labeled affective session, panelists rated their acceptability for 5 food samples from
magnitude scale was developed to evaluate liking/disliking (Schutz different food categories using a 9-point hedonic scale. They also
and Cardello 2001) based on the study of Green and others (1993, rated the level of familiarity of each food on a 9-point categorical
1996) who devised the “labeled magnitude” scale (LMS), modeled scale with “very unfamiliar” and “very familiar” as its outermost
after the “category-ratio” scale (Borg 1982). Jeon and others (2004) categories. The goal of these preliminary tests was to select a ref-
compared the variation among the mean errors for these 2 scales erence food, in addition to 5 familiar and 5 novel foods. Standard
and demonstrated an equal level of discrimination ability between deviations as well as means of acceptability ratings and familiar-
the two, while studies by Green and others (2006) and Schutz and ity scores were calculated for the preliminary sessions. Food items
Cardello (2001) showed that the 9-point hedonic scale was a more with mean familiarity ratings higher than 6 were considered famil-
conservative sensory tool in determining differences among sam- iar products, and lower than 5 were considered novel products. In
ples and the LAM scale was more sensitive. addition, the selection of familiar and novel food items was lim-
Reference samples are regularly and successfully used in qual- ited to foods requiring little or no preparation. The familiar foods
ity control studies or in descriptive analysis as benchmarks for were typical of the Lebanese diet and were selected for the experi-
comparison and to stabilize panelists’ performance and reliability mental sessions based on high familiarity ratings, while the oppo-
(Nielsen and others 2005), in addition to its use in certain threshold site held true for novel foods, and varying acceptability levels to
methods such as the method of constant stimuli. However, the use span the acceptability spectrum. Foods from the main food cat-
of reference foods in hedonic tests is still rare despite the existence egories (main dish, side dish, dessert, soup, and beverage) were
S: Sensory & Food

of few studies. These included the use of the self-adjusting scale assessed in the preliminary tests to imitate all the items and the
(Gay and Mead 1992; Mead and Gay 1995) and comparisons be- variety of a meal. One to 2 foods were selected from each food
Quality

tween the 9-point hedonic scale with reference compared with the category for the experimental sessions based on the criteria re-
typical hedonic scale (Bergara-Almeida and da Silva 2002). A desert lated to novelty, familiarity, and acceptability as described previ-
chocolate bar formulated for warm-weather military rations was ously. Food items were purchased locally from different grocery
used by Lawless (1994) in the Barter scale to predict the total ap- stores.
peal of foods or meal combinations. In addition, Olabi (2001) used
reference foods in a study about the optimization of a space diet Main experiment
and found that the use of reference foods could be helpful when Stimuli. In every session, several foods were assessed, including
a significant session effect is expected and with incomplete design a reference food, which was included for the purpose of providing a
studies by increasing the precision of statistical tests. reference point for calculating relative scores, which could be more
In the development of innovative, high quality, and appealing sensitive than absolute scores, especially in the event of a session
foods, sensory evaluation is key. Foods that do not attain high con- effect. Several criteria were followed in the selection of the reference
sumer acceptance are destined to fail. It is important to provide the food. It had to have a neutral taste quality to minimize carryover
industry with methods and tools that will reduce risks and uncer- effects and was chosen to have little or no preparation to prevent
tainty in decision making and would be of great economical value. the changes in quality as well as in the sensory properties during
Previous studies discussed previously have revealed changes in ac- preparation. Thus, commercial foods that are staples fitted these
ceptability in repeated acceptability tests. Moreover, these changes requirements. In addition, the product had to have relatively high
were known to be affected by many factors, including context or acceptability and familiarity ratings with a low standard deviation

R
session effects. The main goal of this study was to assess the stabil- for acceptability. The salty cracker (ETI , Bursa, Turkey) fulfilled
ity and differentiating power of acceptance testing with and with- these requirements and was selected as the reference food in the
out the use of a hedonic reference. Of further interest was the use of present study.

R
the LAM scale, which is expected to stabilize consumers’ frame of The selected familiar foods were Falafel (Barbar , Beirut,
reference in making acceptability judgments (Schutz and Cardello Lebanon), canned Baba ghanouj (mashed roasted eggplants mixed

R
2001). Therefore, the hypotheses were first, that the use of a hedo- with tahini, lemon juice, and oil-Al Wadi Alakhdar , Chtoura,

R
nic reference would improve the sensitivity of assessing acceptabil- Lebanon), asparagus soup (Maggi , Bursa, Turkey), and apricot
ity changes through the use of relative scores, which in turn will nectar (mashed dried apricot, Damascus, Syria, dissolved in water
“capture” any session-related changes, instead of absolute scores, in the proportion of 1 kg:1 L in the laboratory) were obtained from
second, that the LAM would be more sensitive to acceptability local grocery stores.

S98 JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE—Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009


Use of reference foods in acceptability . . .

As for the novel foods, different ethnic processed foods such as elists filled in the beginning of every session a questionnaire, which
Chinese, Thai, and French were purchased locally. These included included questions about mood, which was rated on a 7-point cat-
R
lychee (Monoprix , France), which was used as a dessert; pick- egory smiley scale (Chen and others 1996; Morien and others 2008),
led ginger (Bangkok, Thailand); the known Chinese item was used with “very pleasant” and “very unpleasant” as end anchors, and

R
as a side dish. Potato soup (Le Potage , Sevres, France) and chili hunger that was rated on a 15-cm line scale. The panelists were

R
(Hormel , San Antonio, Tex., U.S.A.) represented the soup and classified based on their neophobia scores to 1 of 3 categories: neo-
main dish categories, respectively. Additionally, the beverage used phobic, neophilic, or neutral. The categories were based on a ± 1.0
was freshly squeezed sugar cane juice. All the familiar and novel standard deviation (positive for neophobic, negative for neophilic)
food items are listed in Table 1. difference from the mean of a large sample obtained in the Pliner
and Hobden’s (1992) study. A neutral category included those who
Participants had a score of < ±1 standard deviation from the mean. Pliner and
Eighty-two panelists participated in the study. The panelists Hobden (1992) obtained a mean of 34.51 and classified panelists
were 23 male and 59 female (mean age = 22, age range = 18 to to neophobic and neophilic based on a 1 standard deviation differ-
54, SD = 5.93), mainly graduate, undergraduate students, and staff ence (11.86) from the mean.
at the American Univ. of Beirut. They were recruited through an Furthermore, panelists were asked to taste the food samples in
advertisement in the university electronic bulletin and posters at the order provided and to rate their degree of liking for each food,
campus buildings. They were randomly and evenly assigned to 1 of the level of familiarity, and their attitude toward each food item.
the 2 groups (41 panelists/group), whereby each group of panelists The assessments of acceptability were performed using either a
conducted the taste sessions using either the 9-point hedonic or the 9-point hedonic scale, or on a 10-cm vertical LAM scale anchored
labeled affective magnitude (LAM, reference Schutz and Cardello by “greatest imaginable dislike” at 0 and “greatest imaginable like”
2001) scale. To be eligible for participation, panelists had not to at 10. The scale presented to the panelists had no numbers, only
have a dislike for a large number of foods and not to have dietary re- descriptors. Panelists were asked to indicate their acceptability rat-
strictions or food allergies. Table 2 summarizes the main character- ing on the LAM scale by marking a slash (/) on any location on the
istics of the panelists. Panelists were rewarded with money coupons scale. All respondents (both acceptability scales) also chose their fa-
for their participation. miliarity ratings on a 9-point category scale labeled with numbers
for each category and with only “not at all familiar” and “extremely
Procedure familiar” as end anchors, as well as their attitude toward the food
On each test day, panelists arrived at the laboratory at least 2 h af- products as measured by the FACT scale consisting of 9 categories
ter their last meal (either mid-morning or mid-afternoon). Upon ar- assigned to statements listed from the most positive attitude such
rival, respondents were seated in individual booths with white fluo- as category “1” or “I would eat this every opportunity I have” to the
rescent lighting located in the sensory evaluation laboratory of the most negative food attitude such as category “9” or “I would eat this
Dept. of Nutrition and Food Science. The environment was calm only if I were forced to” (Schutz 1964).
and undisruptive. Then panelists were supplied with the food tray, Twenty to 30 g of each sample were presented in individual
a pen, and a questionnaire. 50-mL portion plastic cups (produced locally) coded with 3-digit

S: Sensory & Food


In the 1st session, panelists filled a Food Neophobia Question- random numbers. Stimuli were prepared 1 d before use and were
naire (FNS, Pliner and Hobden 1992) including 10 questions on a refrigerated until half an hour before tasting, with the exception

Quality
7-point scale assessing the food neophobia level with levels ranging of the soups and falafel, which were prepared on the morning of
from 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly). In addition, the pan- the session day. The soups and chili were heated in a microwave
(Daewoo, Seoul, South Korea, KOR-6C2B- 1200 Watts) for 30 and 60
s, respectively. Food stimuli were presented on a tray with a nap-
Table 1 --- Food items for taste sessions.
kin, plastic forks and spoons, and spring water for rinsing between
Food Food Times Session samples. It is noteworthy to mention that the order of the sam-
category item/type served served ples was counterbalanced in each session based on the design for
Main dish Falafela (F) 4 2, 4, 6, 8 5 or 6 samples as described by Macfie and Bratchell (1989), which
Chilib (N) 4 1, 3, 5, 7 balances the carryover effects. The counterbalancing was adopted
Appetizer Baba ghanoujc (F) 6 1, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7
for several reasons, first to prevent any carryover and order effects,
Pickled gingerd (N) 8 1 to 8
Soup Asparagus soupe (F) 2 2, 5 second because this study was conducted in a laboratory environ-
Potato soupf (N) 1 8 ment, with small portions served for every food and accordingly did
Beverage Apricot nectar (F) 1 7 not mimic a home or restaurant environment, and third because
Sugar cane juice (N) 2 1, 4 with the presence of the aforementioned conditions, it is not pos-
Dessert Lycheeg (N) 6 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, 8
Reference food Salty crackersh (F) 8 1 to 8 sible to call the group of foods served in every session a meal but
rather a meal simulation. There are pros and cons for having a “real
F = familiar; N = novel. Brands: a Barbar, Beirut, Lebanon; b Hormel, San
Anotonio, Tex., U.S.A.; c Al Wadi Alakhdar, Chtoura, Lebanon; d Bangkok, life” compared with a laboratory type of scenario and obviously the
Thailand; e Maggi, Nestle, Bursa, Turkey. f Le Potage, Sevres, France.g Monoprix, choice in this study was for the latter, which provides better control
France. h ETI, Bursa, Turkey.
at the expense of having a more representative type of experience.
Table 2 --- Characteristics of panelists. Additionally, new 3-digit random numbers were used to code the
food items between sessions even for the repeated samples thus,
Gender Neophobia level
reducing number bias.
Female Male Neophobic Neophilic Intermediate
9-point 26 15 3 9 29 Experimental design
hedonic scale Eighty-two panelists were evenly and randomly divided be-
LAM scale 33 8 4 9 28 tween 2 groups, one using the 9-point hedonic scale and the
Total 59 23 7 18 57
other using the LAM scale. Ten foods, including 5 novel and

Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009—JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE S99


Use of reference foods in acceptability . . .

5 familiar, were served to all the panelists. The selection of foods ing frequency (TSERV: 1, 2, 4, 6, and 8 times), neophobia category
as familiar or novel was based on the familiarity scores of the (neophobic, neophilic, and neutral), food (1 to 10; nested within
preliminary sessions. The serving frequency ranged between 1 and novelty and TSERV), and panelist (nested within scale and neo-
8 times (1, 2, 4, 6, 8), whereby for each serving frequency there is phobia category). In addition, mood and hunger were added as
both a familiar and a novel food. The serving frequency was var- covariates to the model. An analysis of variance was performed
ied for the foods to assess the effect of different serving frequencies using PROC GLM of the SAS statistical software (SAS version 8.02,
on the changes in acceptability scores and to increase the number 1999-2001, SAS Inst. Inc., Cary, N.C., U.S.A.). In the statistical
of tested foods. Five to 6 foods were served to the panelists twice a model, the response variable was the acceptability of the foods.
week at lunchtime and included the typical items of a meal (main Panelist, food, and session were random effects, while scale type,
dish, appetizer, and so on) for a period of 4 wk. The foods were ran- food novelty, serving frequency, neophobia category, mood, and
domized over the sessions in such a way that the reference food hunger were fixed effects in the model. Each main effect was tested
(cracker) and 1 novel food (pickled ginger) were repeated every ses- as well as all 2-way interactions and selected 3-way interactions.
sion. It is important to note that pickled ginger was not meant to be In addition, the analysis was repeated twice using relative scores,
another reference food especially that it did not have a low standard which represented the difference between the absolute scores and
deviation for acceptability in the preliminary tests, had low aver- the reference scores from either the regular reference (cracker) or
age acceptability and low familiarity ratings, all opposite qualities the novel food (pickled ginger) that was served in every session.
to what is needed in a reference food. However, it was important The relative values were calculated as a difference score for each
for the sake of balancing the experimental design to include a novel individual panelist in each individual session. Mean separation was
food that was served in every session, similar to the reference food performed using Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD). Sig-
(cracker). Hence it was considered useful to do the analyses using nificance was preestablished at α < 0.05.
the relative scores with both the cracker and pickled ginger to com- Separate analyses of variance were conducted with the absolute
pare and contrast the results of these 2 analyses with each other and scores for the 9-point hedonic scale or the LAM original data to ob-
with that of the absolute scores. The detailed experimental design tain the results that are summarized in Table 5, in addition to calcu-
of the study is in Figure 1 and summarized in Table 3. lating the normality of data and the frequency of neutral responses.
Naturally, the analysis of variance model did not include any scale
Statistical analyses effect or any of its interactions. Mean separation was performed us-
Two types of analyses of variance were conducted. The 1st one ing Tukey’s HSD. Significance was preestablished at α < 0.05.
included the data for the 2 scales and is summarized in Table 4, 5, Moreover, the normality of the data per food and per scale was
and 6. For this analysis, the effect of the different predictor vari- checked using the Anderson–Darling (AD) values. AD values reflect
ables on the acceptability of the foods was assessed. The main the goodness of fit to a standard normal distribution (lower values
factors were: scale type (9-point compared with LAM), food nov- indicate a better fit), the LAM scale was closer to normal distribu-
elty (familiar compared with novel), session (1 through 8), serv- tion than the 9-point hedonic scale.
S: Sensory & Food

Subjects Figure 1 --- Outline of experimental design.


Quality

9-point LAM Scale Type


(41) (41) (9-point hedonic or LAM)

F(5) N(5) F(5) N(5) Novelty (number of foods)


(N: Novel; F: Familiar)

F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 Food
(Familiar: 1-5; Novel: 6-10)

1 2 4 6 8 1 2 4 6 8 1 2 4 6 8 1 2 4 6 8 Frequency of Serving (TSERV)


(1, 2, 4, 6, 8)

Table 3 --- Experimental design of the study.


Session/
times served 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
8 Crackers Crackers Crackers Crackers Crackers Crackers Crackers Crackers
8 Pickled Pickled Pickled Pickled Pickled Pickled Pickled Pickled
ginger ginger ginger ginger ginger ginger ginger ginger
6 Baba ghanouj Baba ghanouj Baba ghanouj Baba ghanouj Baba ghanouj Baba ghanouj
6 Lychee Lychee Lychee Lychee Lychee Lychee
4 Chili Falefel Chili Falafel Chili Falafel Chili Falafel
2 Sugar Asparagus Sugar Asparagus
cane juice soup cane juice soup
1 Apricot nectar Potato soup

S100 JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE—Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009


Use of reference foods in acceptability . . .

Results and Discussion of variance included in Table 5) are summarized in the 2nd column
of Table 4. The familiar reference food used (cracker) had the high-
Comparison of different models: absolute est acceptability score among the foods (8.1). On the other hand,
compared with relative scores the novel food (pickled ginger) that was served in every session re-
The strongest effect was the differences among the foods. The ceived the lowest acceptability rating (3.0). Differences (P < 0.05)
acceptability scores (absolute scores) for foods (from the analysis between the top 3 foods were found. These foods were cracker,
falafel, and lychee, with ratings of 8.1, 7.6, and 7.0, respectively,
Table 4 --- Acceptability means for 10 food items using and differences were also found between the bottom three, namely,
absolute scores and relative to both familiar (RELFAM) Baba ghanouj, sugar cane juice, and pickled ginger, with 5.3, 3.7,
and novel food (RELNOV). and 3.0, respectively. The other foods (apricot nectar, chili, aspara-
Food name Absolute values RELFAM RELNOV gus and potato soups) had scores ranging between 5.8 and 7.0 and
Pickled ginger (N) 3.0 a
5.2 a
−0.0a in general did not show differences. The average acceptability of fa-
Sugar cane juice (N) 3.7b 4.4b −0.8b miliar foods tended to be higher than that of novel foods, though
Baba ghanouj (F) 5.3c 2.9c −2.3c this was not shown to be significant in the analysis of variance
Apricot nectar (F) 5.8cd 2.3cd −2.9cd (Table 5).
Chili (N) 6.8de 1.4de −3.8de
Analysis of variance results for absolute scores. The analysis
Asparagus soup (F) 6.9ef 1.2def −4.0def
Potato soup (N) 7.0def 1.2def −4.0def of variance results for the absolute scores are summarized in the
Lychee (N) 7.0e 1.1ef −4.0ef 3rd column of Table 5. Differences were obtained for food (nested
Falefel (F) 7.6f 0.6f −4.6f within TSERV and novel; Table 4, P < 0.001). The panelist × TSERV
Crackers (F) 8.1g 0.0g −5.2g and panelist × novel interactions (P < 0.001) are an indication that
Means in a column followed by the same letter are not significantly different the serving frequency and novelty had a different effect on the gen-
(P > 0.05).
RELFAM: Relative to familiar reference food. eral acceptability of foods for different panelists. This outcome is
RELNOV: Relative to novel food item repeated in every session. to be expected because panelists always differ and hence the need

Table 5 --- Significance of predictor variables (F-values and P-values) in 3 different models (absolute scores, RELFAM,
and RELNOV) for main effects and their interactions.
Absolute scores RELFAMb RELNOVc FACTd
Variablesa df Error term F -values
Panelist (scale-neophobia) 76 Error 1 1.00 1.80∗ ∗ ∗ 4.73∗ ∗ ∗ 4.14∗ ∗ ∗
Session 7 Error 2 0.11 0.32 0.44 0.83
Scale 1 Error 3 0.05 1.00 0.26 1.39
Times served (TSERV) 4 Error 4 0.07 0.07 0.07 0.11
Novel (scale-neophobia) 1 Error 5 0.48 0.44 0.47 0.02
Neophobia level 2 Error 6 0.86 1.28 1.19 0.90
Hunger 1 MSE2 1.58 0.00 2.11 0.32

S: Sensory & Food


Mood 1 MSE 1.87 0.36 0.58 0.30
Food (TSERV-novel) 4 Error 7 64.27∗ ∗ ∗ 62.07∗ ∗ ∗ 59.28∗ ∗ ∗ 1.84

Quality
Panelist × TSERV 304 MSE 5.61∗ ∗ ∗ 5.32∗ ∗ ∗ 4.97∗ ∗ ∗ 3.55∗ ∗ ∗
Panelist × novel 76 MSE 13.07∗ ∗ ∗ 12.33∗ ∗ ∗ 11.50∗ ∗ ∗ 7.53∗ ∗ ∗
Scale × food 4 MSE 4.76∗ ∗ ∗ 4.49∗ ∗ 4.19∗ ∗ 146.15∗ ∗ ∗
Food × neophobia 8 MSE 2.52∗ ∗ 2.39∗ 2.12∗ 1.37
Hunger × food 4 MSE 6.06∗ ∗ ∗ 5.62∗ ∗ ∗ 4.80∗ ∗ ∗ 1.46
Mood × food 4 MSE 3.05∗ 2.85∗ 3.04∗ 0.80
Mood × neophobia 2 MSE 1.77 3.76∗ 0.04 0.47
Session × neophobia 14 Error 8 2.31 3.47 1.82 5.13
Novel × neophobia 2 Error 9 0.80 0.80 0.87 1.37
Scale × novel 1 Error 10 0.09 0.09 0.07 0.58
Scale × neophobia 2 Error 11 0.85 0.18 2.69 1.80
Session × novel 7 Error 12 1.93 2.19 2.58 0.86
TSERV × neophobia 8 Error 13 0.71 0.71 0.74 0.46
Scale × session 7 Error 14 1.04 1.90 0.95 0.55
Scale × TSERV 4 Error 15 0.29 0.28 0.30 0.23
Hunger × panelist 76 MSE 0.66 1.16 1.18 0.88
Hunger × scale 1 MSE 0.42 0.70 1.09 0.07
Mood × scale 1 MSE 1.52 0.53 1.80 3.13
Hunger × session 7 MSE 0.45 0.40 0.52 0.40
Mood × session 7 MSE 0.19 0.95 0.22 0.85
Hunger × TSERV 4 MSE 0.12 0.13 0.48 0.41
Mood × TSERV 4 MSE 0.20 0.24 0.10 0.59
Hunger × novel 1 MSE 1.79 1.58 0.34 0.43
Mood × novel 1 MSE 0.02 0.00 0.25 2.46
Hunger × neophobia 2 MSE 0.56 0.03 0.08 0.06
Hunger × mood 1 MSE 1.40 0.01 0.68 0.34
Scale × session × neophobia 14 MSE 0.61 0.72 1.43 0.69
Scale × session × novel 7 MSE 0.20 0.19 0.18 1.00
Session × novel × neophobia 14 MSE 0.34 0.34 0.31 0.34
Scale × TSERV × neophobia 8 Error 16 2.31∗ 5.32∗ ∗ ∗ 4.97∗ ∗ ∗ 1.37
Scale × novel × neophobia 2 Error 17 1.31 1.32 0.37 0.34
a
Response variable is acceptability.
b
RELFAM: Relative scores to cracker (familiar reference food), c RELNOV: Relative scores to the pickled ginger (novel food item repeated in every session), d FACT:
Food action rating scale, ∗ P < 0.05, ∗ ∗ P < 0.01,∗ ∗ ∗ P < 0.001.

Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009—JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE S101


Use of reference foods in acceptability . . .

to have a panelist effect. In addition, scale × food, hunger × food ceptions were panelist effect (P > 0.05) with the absolute ac-
(P < 0.001), and mood × food (P < 0.05) interactions reveal the ceptability scores and mood × neophobia level, which was sig-
different effects of scale, mood, and hunger on the acceptability nificant (P < 0.05) for RELFAM but not for the absolute values
of different foods, again showing the benefit of including mood or RELNOV. Session effect was also not significant (P > 0.05) in
and hunger as covariates in the model. Unfortunately, mood and all models, confirming the absence of a trend in the acceptabil-
hunger were continuous variables and accordingly the means of ity scores with time. The same level of significance (P-value) was
the different levels of these interactions could not be compared. found with the different models for most main factors and the 2-
The food × neophobia level interaction (P < 0.01) demonstrated or 3-way interactions; thus differences were noted for food (nested
that the acceptability of certain foods was different for different for time served and novelty), panelist × TSERV (nested for scale
neophobia groups. This is further discussed in the “Effect of food type and neophobia level), panelist × novel (nested for scale type
neophobia” section below. As for the scale × TSERV × neo in- and neophobia level), hunger × food and scale × neo × TSERV
teraction, naturally there were significant differences (P < 0.001) (P < 0.001), and mood × food interaction (P < 0.05). However,
between the levels of the different foods, as demonstrated by different levels of significance (P-value) were noted for food ×
differences for varying levels of TSERV (for example, the accept- neophobia, scale × food, and mood × neophobia. Scale × food
ability of TSERV = 4 for the 9-point scale and neophilic panelists was more significant with the absolute values model (P < 0.001)
was significantly different (P < 0.001) from the acceptability of than the RELFAM and RELNOV models (P < 0.01). The same ap-
TSERV = 8 for the LAM scale and neophobic panelists). However, plied to food × neophobia, though with different significance levels
when comparisons were made between the 3 neophobia groups for (0.001 compared with 0.05, respectively). As for mood × neopho-
the same scale and the same TSERV, differences (P < 0.05) were ob- bia interaction, it was significant (P < 0.05) only with the RELFAM
tained for some or all of the neophobia groups for the 9-point scale model. Given the results above, it could be noted that in general,
and TSERV = 6, and for the LAM scale and TSERV = 8, respectively. the relative sores models had a similar sensitivity to differences in
The least squares means were neophilic = 6.9a , intermediate = the investigated variables and interactions as the typical absolute
6.1ab , and neophobic = 5.2c ; neophilic = 6.6a , intermediate = 5.4b , scores model. All of the above-mentioned interactions have been
and neophobic = 4.3c for the 9-point scale and TSERV = 6 and for described in the previous section.
the LAM scale and TSERV = 8, respectively. Variables analyzed using the FACT scores are summarized in
Relative compared with absolute scores. The results of the the 6th column of Table 5. Similar results to the absolute scores
analyses of variance for the relative scores (cracker: RELFAM and were obtained for panelist × novel and panelist × TSERV and
pickled ginger: RELNOV) are summarized in Table 5. In general, scale × food. On the other hand, other variables were shown to
the relative scores models had similar sensitivity to the effects be significant with the absolute scores but not with the FACT.
and interactions of the variables as the absolute scores. The ex- This applied to food × neo, hunger × food, mood × food, and
scale × TSERV × neo.
The acceptability scores (absolute scores and relative scores) for
Table 6 --- Least squares mean absolute acceptability
foods are summarized in Table 4. The 10 different foods could be
scores for food items using the labeled affective mag-
nitude scale (LAM) and 9-point hedonic scale. arbitrarily classified to 3 groups: highly liked (cracker, falafel, ly-
S: Sensory & Food

chee), highly disliked (pickled ginger, Baba ghanouj, sugar cane


9-point Food
juice) and foods with intermediate level of acceptance (apricot nec-
Quality

Food name (type) LAM hedonic scale name (type)


tar, chili, and potato and asparagus soups). Comparable differences
Crackers (F) 8.2a 8.0a Crackers (F) were found with the different models. In addition, the differences
b ab
Falafel (F) 7.3 7.8 Falafel (F)
Lychee (N) 7.1b
7.6 abc
Potato soup (N) between the foods were more frequent for the absolute scores com-
Asparagus soup (F) 7.0b 6.9bc Lychee (N) pared to the RELFAM and RELNOV model where less differences
Potato soup (N) 6.8bc 7.2bc Chili (N) were noted in the highly liked and moderately liked foods (inter-
Chili (N) 6.5bc 7.0bc Asparagus soup (F) mediate). This outcome could be easily explained for the RELNOV
cd cd
Apricot nectar (F) 5.5 6.1 Apricot nectar (F)
model but is surprising for the RELFAM one. In terms of the REL-
Baba ghanouj (F) 5.3d 5.2d Baba ghannouj (F)
Sugar cane juice (N) 3.6e
3.8 e
Sugar cane juice (N) NOV model, the difference with the absolutes score model was due
Pickled ginger (N) 2.6e 3.3e Pickled ginger (N) to the fact that pickled ginger, which was used to calculate the
Means in a column followed by the same letter are not significantly different (P >
RELNOV scores had a bimodal type of distribution for its abso-
0.05). lute acceptability scores and a high standard deviation (SD = 2.7).
F = familiar food; N = novel food.
This in turn translated to higher variability when computing the

Table 7 --- Mean acceptability ratings and standard deviations for foods through taste sessions.
Session
Food 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 SD
a
Cracker 8 (1.1) 8 (1.2) 8.1 (1.2) 8.2 (1.2) 8.2 (1.3) 8.1 (1.3) 8.2 (1.3) 8.3 (1.2) 0.1
Pickled ginger 3 (2.9) 3 (2.8) 3.2 (3.2) 3.1 (3.2) 3.3 (3.1) 3.2 (3.2) 3.2 (3.1) 3.2 (3.2) 0.1
Baba ghanouj 5.7 (2.5) 5.3 (2.5) 5.2 (2.7) 5.2 (2.6) 5.3 (2.5) 5.4 (2.6) 0.2
Lychee 7 (2.2) 6.9 (2.3) 7.2 (2.1) 7.1 (2.3) 7.2 (2.3) 7.3 (2.2) 0.1
Falafel 7.8 (1.4) 7.8 (1.5) 7.8 (1.5) 7.9 (1.7) 0.1
Chili 7.1 (1.8) 6.9 (2.0) 7.1 (2.0) 7.2 (2.0) 0.1
Asparagus soup 6.7 (1.9) 6.8 (2.1) 0.1
Sugar cane juice 3.8 (2.2) 3.7 (2.5) 0.1
Apricot nectar 5.9 (2.3)
Potato soup 6.9 (1.9)
SD = standard deviation of mean ratings for each food in all sessions.
a
Standard deviation of mean ratings for individual food in single session.

S102 JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE—Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009


Use of reference foods in acceptability . . .

RELNOV scores and resulted in inflating the standard deviations noted for the other foods. Cracker was different from falafel and ly-
for all the foods with the RELNOV model, thereby resulting in less chee with the LAM, while no differences were obtained with the 9-
differences between foods. This solidified the original hypothesis point hedonic scale when similar comparisons were made between
of the authors that a hedonic reference should have low variability, the top 3 foods, namely, cracker, falafel, and potato soup. This di-
high familiarity, and high acceptability. vergence implies that the LAM scale could be more sensitive to dif-
After the identification of significant predictors (Table 5), in- ferences in highly liked items, an outcome that was obtained by
cluding 2- and 3-way interactions (P < 0.05), the number of sig- Schutz and Cardello (2001). They found that both scales had equiv-
nificant differences was counted for each significant variable or alent levels of sensitivity except for high level of effect and related
interaction for each model separately to assess the sensitivity of this outcome to the presence of the end-point “greatest imaginable
each model in detecting the maximum possible number of differ- liking/disliking” in the LAM scale that enables more extreme rat-
ences. Of course, this comparison remains one of qualitative na- ings than “like/dislike extremely” in the 9-point hedonic scale.
ture given that these variables are already significant and some- However, it is important to note that in the Schutz and Cardello’s
times have the same level of significance (P-value). This type of (2001) study the same panelists used both scales in separate ses-
comparison has been used in previous studies (Stoer and Lawless sions, while in our case, there were 2 separate groups, each group
1993; Schutz and Cardello 2001). The predictor variables that were evaluating foods using a specific scale (either the 9-point hedonic
compared in this study were food, scale × food, food × neo, and scale or the LAM scale). Using the same panelists for both scales
scale × TSERV × neo. The number of significant differences was might have improved the sensitivity of “between scales” compar-
relatively similar for food (34, 32, and 32), scale × food (116, 116, isons in a similar manner to the difference between a paired t-
and 105), and food × neo (228, 221, and 241) with the absolute, test compared with a 2-sample t-test due to less variability for the
RELFAM, and RELNOV models, respectively. The exception was same panelists than for different panelists. However, this was not
scale × TSERV × neo (109, 105, and 191) where more differences the case for this study because having the same panelists assess
were obtained with RELNOV compared to the 2 other models. Con- both scales would have introduced the training effect factor and
sequently, the 3 models had similar levels of sensitivity because no made an already complex design more intricate. In addition, the
major differences were observed by using either the absolute rat- order for moderately liked foods was different between the 9-point
ings or the relative ratings. However, it should be noted that these hedonic and LAM scale groups. For the 9-point scale the ranking
numbers were very similar in absolute and RELFAM, while RELNOV for acceptability scores in decreasing order was: lychee, chili and
had slightly different numbers. asparagus soup, and apricot nectar, while for LAM it was aspara-
gus soup, potato soup, chili, and apricot nectar. It is important to
mention that no significant differences existed between all of these
Comparison of acceptability scales foods for both scales. Mean acceptability scores suggested that al-
(9-point and LAM) though the 2 panelist groups used 2 different scales to evaluate ac-
Sensitivity to significant differences among food items. ceptability, the results showed similar tendencies for acceptance for
There was a scale × food interaction (P < 0.05) for all mod- both groups. The rank of the different foods in terms of their ac-
els as noted previously. The least squares mean absolute accept- ceptability was the same for both scales except for the moderately

S: Sensory & Food


ability scores for the 9-point and LAM scales are summarized in liked foods mentioned previously. In general, it could be concluded
Table 5. The results showed that foods were different (P < 0.05) to that the LAM scale discriminated better between samples in near

Quality
the same extent (that is, having the same superscripts) with both optimal liking because it was more sensitive to differences among
scales for the lower acceptability foods (pickled ginger, sugar cane well-liked foods and had relatively similar results when compared
juice, Baba ghanouj, and apricot nectar). However, a difference was to the 9-point hedonic scale for the other foods.

8 Figure 2 --- Mean absolute


acceptability scores (both
scales) of pickled ginger (),
lychee (), and chili ( ) for
7 low (solid symbols) and high
neophobia (open symbols)
groups in repeat sessions.
6

Pickled ginger
5
Pickled ginger

Lychee (Phil)
4
Lychee (Phob)

Chili
3
Chili

1
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009—JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE S103


Use of reference foods in acceptability . . .

Sensitivity to significant differences among variables. The Effect of repeated exposure


number of significant differences for comparisons between the dif- Acceptability ratings were drifting but at a very small extent from
ferent foods was assessed. A total of 45 can be made for food. Both 1 session to another (Table 5: session effect was not significant,
scales had a high number of significant differences for food, with P > 0.05). The average acceptability ratings for foods across ses-
a slightly higher number for LAM (32 compared with 27). A simi- sions and their standard deviations are summarized in Table 6.
lar trend was observed for the number of significant differences of Food type does not seem to have a specific effect on the changes in
food × neophobia (176 compared with 171), possibly indicating a acceptability ratings through time and the ratings were stable over
slightly higher level of sensitivity for the LAM scale. This hypothesis the different sessions. Moreover, the reference food and the novel
has received a considerable support in previous studies performed food served at every session were highly stable from sessions 1 to
by Schutz and Cardello (2001) where the LAM scale was more 8. The stability of the mean acceptability scores for reference foods
sensitive to differences; hence more significant differences were was considered a favorable result in Olabi’s (2001) study. In addi-
observed when panelists gave their ratings on the LAM scale as tion, TSERV that also gives an indication of the repeated exposure
compared to the 9-point hedonic scale. A similar outcome was effect was not significant (Table 5) nor were most of its interactions
found by Green and others (2006) where more differences among with other variables, with the exception of panelist × TSERV
peanut samples were detected by using the LAM scale to compare and scale × TSERV × NEO, which have been described
between the fruit flavored and nonfruit flavored peanuts. The la- previously.
beled magnitude scale for perceived satiety (SLIM), developed by Sustained acceptability ratings were also found in a study assess-
Cardello and others (2005), was also shown to have high sensitiv- ing the influence on acceptance of a bitter compound after 10 re-
ity to differences between pairs of foods and specifically at the ex- peated experiences (Mattes 1994), unlike another similar study by
tremes of hunger or fullness. Stein and others (2003), which was conducted in a setting chosen by
Normality of obtained data. The LAM scale was closer to nor- participants instead of the typical laboratory setting (Mattes 1994).
mal distribution than the 9-point hedonic scale. The AD values Other studies conducted on either staple or novel foods showed sig-
were lower in the data generated from the LAM scale for familiar nificant increase and/or decrease in acceptability through succes-
foods (asparagus soup: 1.2 compared with 2.8; apricot nectar: 0.9 sive sessions (Pliner 1982; Crandall 1984; Porcherot and Issanchou
compared with 1.2; falafel: 5.6 compared with 9.1; Baba ghanouj: 1998; Hetherington and others 2000, 2002). Food novelty, particu-
3.1 compared with 6.5 for LAM and 9-point hedonic, respectively) larly for staple foods, and high initial acceptability resulted in an
and novel foods (potato soup: 1.1 compared with 2.6; sugar cane increase and sustained acceptability, respectively, while foods with
juice: 1.1 compared with 1.6; chili: 5.8 compared with 8.9; lychee: low initial hedonic ratings showed a significant decrease in final he-
9.0 compared with 9.4). This confirmed the findings of Schutz and donic ratings (Siegel and Pilgrim 1958).
Cardello (2001) on the LAM scale, who compared this scale to the The results obtained in this study could be related to the rela-
9-point hedonic scale and magnitude estimation scale. Interest- tive short duration of the study (8 sessions in 4 wk) and the rela-
ingly enough, the trend of lower AD-values with the LAM scale was tively low number of repeated exposures, which could have been
reversed with the novel food that was repeated in every session insufficient to have a major effect on food acceptability and the
(pickled ginger) (24.2 compared with 29.2, respectively). This was consequent phenomena like the development of monotony and/or
S: Sensory & Food

not the case with the reference food (cracker), with 8.66 compared mere exposure effect. A comparison between this study and previ-
with 24.9 for LAM and 9-point hedonic scale, respectively. Both
Quality

ous studies for the number of exposures revealed either similar or


the reference food and pickled ginger had extreme acceptability higher levels of exposures for those studies. The studies with similar
levels. Therefore, it could be expected that one or both foods would levels included 5 exposures for bread (Zandstra and others 2000a),
not have normal distributions. Accordingly, this outcome needs to 7 exposures for a standard meal (Meiselman and others 2000b),
be further explored in future studies by assessing the normality of 10 exposures for meat sauces (Zandstra and others 2000b), and 13
highly liked/disliked foods using the 9-point and the LAM scales. exposures for doughnuts (Crandall 1984). As for those with higher
Neutral scores tendency in both scales. Percent of neutral levels, they included 18, 20, 20, 22, and 35 exposures for fla-
responses for each scale was calculated across food names and vored crackers (Porcherot and Issanchou 1998), iced tea (Vickers
panelists. A “5” point and a “5 cm” were considered the neutral re- and Holton 1998), a novel tropical fruit juice (Pliner 1982), 2 alter-
sponses for 9-point hedonic scale and LAM scale, respectively. Ac- nate daily menus (Siegel and Pilgrim 1958), and 41 food items, re-
cordingly, 6.0% neutral responses for the 9-point hedonic scale and spectively. Most of the above-cited studies have revealed changes
2.7% for the LAM scale were obtained. Thus, the 9-point hedonic in acceptability with repeated exposure and included home/field
scale was encouraging to a small extent judges to choose a neu- consumption rather than laboratory testing performed in the
tral response more than LAM scale, as a safe place to use whenever present study. Finally, the nature of food stimuli was different
no other option fits their opinion. This was supported previously because most of the above-mentioned studies evaluated 1 to 2
by Jones and others (1955) and was considered one of the weak- products per session (mainly snacks), with the exception of the
nesses of the 9-point hedonic scale because it decreases its effi- Siegel and Pilgrim (1958), Schutz and Pilgrim (1958), and Meisel-
ciency. This outcome and even the percentages obtained were in man and others (2000a, 2000b) studies, while a combination of 5
line with Schutz and Cardello’s (2001) study where the LAM scale or 6 foods imitating a complete meal were provided to the par-
was also found to have fewer neutral responses. The authors ob- ticipants in this study. In addition, increasing the sample size
tained 2.9% and 6.4% neutral responses for the LAM and 9-point might contribute to a drift in acceptability with time, although
scale when the neutral response on the LAM scale was defined as this could be more challenging when several foods are assessed
“50 mm.” A smaller percentage of neutral responses (5.2%) com- per session. On another note, the foods selected for this study
pared to the 9-point scale was also produced by the data generated had a wide acceptability range. This range factor might have pre-
from the LAM scale in the above study when a less conservative cri- vented the detection of drift over time, which could have been
terion with a range of 45 to 55 mm was used as indication of neutral more likely had foods with a narrow acceptability range been
responses. selected.

S104 JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE—Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009


Use of reference foods in acceptability . . .

Effect of food neophobia Conclusions


Acceptability of familiar and novel foods by panelists of dif-
ferent food neophobia groups. The average neophobia score on
the FNS for the 82 panelists was 30.44 and the standard devia-
M odels including absolute scores and relative-to-reference
scores were found to have comparable levels of sensitivity
to differences among single variables and 2- or 3-way interactions.
tion was 10.41. The scores ranged between 13 and 68. Cronbach’s Moreover, foods were similarly classified according to their accept-
alpha of the scale was 0.823 demonstrating a high level of in- ability into highly liked, highly disliked, and intermediate in the
ternal consistency and a similar level (0.847) to a previous study 3 models. The results showed that, in general, foods were signif-
where the FNS was used (Tuorila and others 2001). The low num- icantly different to the same extent with both scales with a dif-
ber of panelists in the high neophobia group for this study was ference noted in the top 3 most liked foods (crackers, falafel, and
consistent with the previous experience of the corresponding au- lychee or potato soup), whereby, cracker was significantly different
thor, and confirmed previous findings of a lower attendance for from falafel and lychee with LAM scale but not with the 9-point he-
high neophobia panelists in central laboratory tests compared to donic scale. This implied that both scales had equivalent levels of
an Internet survey (Henriques and others 2008). A previous study sensitivity except for high level of effect with the LAM scale pro-
by Olabi and others (personal communication) had revealed that viding better discrimination among highly liked foods. Compari-
around 20% of Lebanese college students were in the high neo- son of frequency of significant differences in the possible compar-
phobia group instead of the roughly 10% obtained in this study. As isons among foods and other variables demonstrated that the LAM
mentioned in the “Analysis of variance results for absolute scores” scale had slightly higher level of sensitivity in detecting differences
section, food × neophobia level was significant (P < 0.05). Dif- among attributes. Furthermore, data generated by the LAM scale
ferences (P < 0.05) among neophobia groups were found only had a distribution that is more normal than the traditional 9-point
for 2 unfamiliar foods and in the predicted directions, whereby hedonic scale and the LAM scale tended to minimize the use of
neophobics were less accepting of them than neophilics (Tuo- neutral responses. Repeated exposure was found to have no ma-
rila and others 1994; Raudenbush and Frank 1999; Tuorila and jor effect on the acceptability of foods during the 4-wk period. Only
others 2001; Henriques and others 2008). Lychee had a neopho- a slight increase and decrease in acceptability of both novel and fa-
bic mean = 6.3b , neophilic mean = 7.8b , and intermediate = miliar foods occurred throughout the study. A slight difference in
7.1ab and pickled ginger had a neophobic mean = 1.8c , neophilic acceptability ratings of novel foods between neophilics and neo-
= 4.2a , and intermediate = 3.0b . It is important to note that the phobics was observed. Neophobics gave lower ratings to pickled
TSERV was 6 for lychee and 8 for pickled ginger, which should ginger and lychee (P < 0.05). Furthermore, there was no significant
partially explain the significant differences obtained with the sig- change in the acceptability ratings of both neophobia groups over
nificant scale × TSERV × neo interactions between different time.
neophobia groups for the 9-point scale and TSERV = 6, and for the
LAM scale and TSERV = 8, respectively, as discussed in the “Analysis Acknowledgments
of variance results for absolute scores” section. The authors thank Omar Kebbe Baghdadi, Dr. Harry Lawless,
Session effect for food neophobia groups. There was no inter- Hamza Daroub, Dr. John Walker, and Dr. Ziad Mahfoud for tech-
action of session × neophobia level or novel × neophobia × session nical assistance and all the panelists who participated on the panel

S: Sensory & Food


(P > 0.05, Table 5). Ratings over the sessions were close between for their dedication.
the neophilic and neophobic panelists for most foods, with the ex-

Quality
ception of pickled ginger, and to a certain extent chili and lychee References
(Figure 2), 3 novel foods, which is a phenomenon noted previously. Barylko-Pikielna N, Matuszewska I, Jeruszka M, Kozlowska K, Brzozowska A,
Roszkowski W. 2004. Discriminability and appropriateness of category scaling ver-
However, it is important to mention that there was no novel × neo- sus ranking methods to study sensory preferences in elderly. Food Qual Pref 15:167–
phobia interaction effect. In addition, although repeated expo- 75.
Bergara-Almeida S, da Silva M. 2002. Hedonic scale with reference: performance in
sure tends to have a positive impact on neophobia, neophobics obtaining predictive models. Food Qual Pref 13:57–64.
in the present intervention were found to have similar acceptabil- Birch LL, Marlin DW. 1982. I don’t like it; I never tried it: effects of exposure on two-
year-old children’s food preferences. Appetite 3:353–60.
ity ratings for foods during the 4-wk study with a slight increase Borg, GAV. 1982. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sport Exer
and/or decrease through time. This outcome is not in agreement 14:377–81.
Cardello AV, Schutz HG. 2004. Numerical scale-point locations for constructing the
with other studies showing a positive effect of varied and multi- LAM (labeled affective magnitude) scale. J Sens Stud 19:341–6.
ple food exposures on neophobia in human infants (Gerrish and Cardello AV, Schutz HG, Lesher LL, Merill E. 2005. Development and testing of a la-
beled magnitude scale of perceived satiety. Appetite 44:1–13.
Mennella 2001), children (Loewen and Pliner 1999; Wardle and Chen AW, Resurreccion AVA, Paguio LP. 1996. Age appropriate hedonic scales to mea-
others 2003), and adults (Pliner and others 1993). On the other sure food preferences of young children. J Sens Stud 11:141–63.
Chung S, Vickers Z. 2007. Influence of sweetness on the sensory-specific satiety and
hand, the absence of a major effect of multiple food exposure long-term acceptability of tea. Food Qual Pref 18:256–64.
on food neophobia levels in a weight reduction program (WRP) Crandall CS. 1984. The liking of foods as a result of exposure: eating doughnuts in
of adolescents, which was maintained for a period of 9 mo (Ri- Alaska. J Soc Psychol 125(2):187–94.
De Graaf C, Kramer FM, Meiselman HL, Lesher LL, Baker-Fulco C, Hirsch ES, Warber
gal and others 2006), was in line with the findings of the present J. 2005. Food acceptability in field studies with US army men and women: relation-
experiment. ship with food intake ad food choice after repeated exposures. Appetite 44:23–31.
Gay C, Mead R. 1992. A statistical appraisal of the problem of sensory measurement. J
Given the results obtained in this study, it is recommended to Sens Stud 7:205–28.
have a higher number of exposures and a longer duration for sim- Gerrish CJ, Mennella JA. 2001. Flavor variety enhance food acceptance in formula-fed
infants. Am J Clin Nutr 73:1080–5.
ilar studies. Both of these changes could in turn induce significant Goldman A. 1994. Predicting product performance in the marketplace by immediate-
changes in acceptability with repeated exposure, an element that and extended-use sensory testing. Food Technol 48(10):103–6.
Green BG, Shaffer GS, Gilmore MM. 1993. Derivation and evaluation of a seman-
was relatively missing in this study. Another possible change would tic scale of oral sensation magnitude with apparent ratio properties. Chem Senses
be having the same panelists assess the foods with both scales to 18:683–702.
Green BG, Dalton P, Cowart B, Shaffer G, Rankin K, Higgins J. 1996. Evaluating the “la-
enable a more sensitive “between scales” comparison. In addition, beled magnitude scale” for measuring sensations of taste and smell. Chem Senses
repeating this study in either a better-controlled environment, such 21:323–35.
Green JL, Bratka KJ, Drake MA, Sanders TH. 2006. Effectiveness of category and line
as the army or boarding schools, or in a less controlled environment scales to characterize consumer perception of fruity fermented flavor in peanuts. J
such as an in-home use test would be advantageous. Sens Stud 21:146–54.

Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009—JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE S105


Use of reference foods in acceptability . . .

Henriques AS, King SC, Meiselman, HL. 2008. Consumer segmentation based on food Raudenbush B, Frank RA. 1999. Assessing food neophobia: the role of stimulus famil-
neophobia and its application to product development. Food Qual Pref 20:83–91. iarity. Appetite 32:261–71.
Hetherington MM, Bell A, Rolls BJ. 2000. Effects of repeat consumption on pleasant- Raynor HA, Niemeier HM, Wing RR. 2006. Effect of limiting snack food variety on
ness, preference and intake. Br Food J 102(7):507–21. long-term sensory-specific satiety and monotony during obesity treatment. Eating
Hetherington MM, Pirie LM, Nabb S. 2002. Stimulus satiation: effects of repeated ex- Behav 7:1–14.
posure to foods on pleasantness and intake. Appetite 38:19–28. Rigal N, Frelut M, Monneuse M, Hladik C, Simmen B, Pasquet P. 2006. Food neopho-
Jeon SY, O’Mahony M, Kim KO. 2004. A comparison of category and line scales under bia in the context of a varied diet induced by a weight reduction program in mas-
various experimental protocols. J Sens Stud 19:49–66. sively obese adolescents. Appetite 46:207–14.
Jones LV, Peryam DR, Thurstone LL. 1955. Development of a scale for measuring sol- Schutz HG, Cardello AV. 2001. A labeled affective magnitude scale for assessing food
diers’ food preferences. Food Res 20:512–20. liking/disliking. J Sens Stud 16:117–59.
Kamen JM, Peryam DR. 1961. Acceptability of repetitive diets. Food Technol 26:173–7. Schutz HG, Pilgrim FJ. 1958. A field study of food monotony. Psychol Reports 4:559–
Lavenka N, Kamen J. 1994. Magnitude estimation of food acceptance. J Food Sci 65.
59(6):1322–4. Schutz HG. 1964. A food action rating scale for measuring food acceptance. Paper
Lawless HT. 1994. Contextual and measurement aspects of acceptability. Final Report presented at the 24th Annual Meeting of the Institute of Food Technologists, May
nr TCN 94178, U.S. Army Research Office. 24-28, 1964, Washington, D.C.
Loewen R, Pliner P. 1999. Effects of prior exposure to palatable and unpalatable Siegel PS, Pilgrim FJ. 1958. The effect of monotony on the acceptance of food. Am J
novel foods on children’s willingness to try novel foods in stimulating and non- Psychol 71:756–9.
stimulating situations. Appetite 32:351–66. Stein LJ, Nagai H, Nakagawa M, Beauchamp GK. 2003. Effects of repeated exposure
Macfie HJ, Bratchell N. 1989. Designs to balance the effect of order of presentation and health-related information on hedonic evaluation and acceptance of a bitter
and first order carry over effects in hall tests. J Sens Stud 4:129–48. beverage. Appetite 40:119–29.
Mattes RD. 1993. Fat preference and adherence to a reduced-fat diet. Am J Clin Nutr Stevens SS, Galanter EH. 1957. Ratio scales and category scales for a dozen of percep-
57:373–81. tual continua. J Exp Psychol 54(6):377–411.
Mattes RD. 1994. Influences on acceptance of bitter foods and beverages. Physiol Be- Stoer NL, Lawless HT. 1993. Comparison of single product scaling and relative-
hav 56:1229–36. to-reference scaling in sensory evaluation of dairy products. J Sens Stud 8:257–
Mc Daniel MR, Sawyer FM. 1981. Preference testing of whiskey sour formulation: 70.
magnitude estimation versus the 9-point hedonic scale. J Food Sci 46:182–5. Stubenitsky K, Aaron JI, Catt SL, Mela DJ. 1999. Effect of information and extended
Mead R, Gay C. 1995. Sequential design of sensory trials. Food Qual Pref 6:271–80. use on the acceptance of reduced-fat products. Food Qual Pref 10:367–76.
Meiselman HL, Johnson JL, Reeve W, Crouch JE. 2000a. Demonstrations of the influ- Tuorila H, Meiselman HL, Bell R, Cardello AV, Johnson W. 1994. Role of sensory and
ence of the eating environment on food acceptance. Appetite 35:231–37. cognitive information in the enhancement of certainty and liking for novel and fa-
Meiselman HL, de Graaf C, Lesher L. 2000b. The effects of variety and monotony on miliar foods. Appetite 23:231–46.
food acceptance and intake at a midday meal. Physiol Behav 70:119–25. Tuorila H, Lahteenmaki L, Pohjalainen L, Lotti I. 2001. Food neophobia among the
Morien A, Garrison D, Keeney Smith N. 2008. Range of motion improves after massage Finns and related responses to familiar and unfamiliar foods. Food Qual Pref 12:29–
in children with burns: a pilot study. J Bodywork Movement Therap 12:67–71. 37.
Moskowitz HR, Sidel JL. 1971. Magnitude and hedonic scales of food acceptability. J Tuorila-Olikainen H. 1987. Selection of milk with varying fat contents and related to
Food Sci 36:677–80. overall liking, attitudes, norms and intentions. Appetite 8:1–14.
Nielsen D, Hyldig G, Sorensen R. 2005. An effective way to minimize drifting and mon- Tuorila-Ollikainen H, Lahteenmaki L, Salovarra H. 1986. Attitudes, norms, intentions
itor the performance of a sensory panel during long-term projects—a case study and hedonic responses in the selection of low salt bread in a longitudinal choice
from a project on Herring quality. J Sens Stud 20:35–47. experiment. Appetite 7:127–39.
Olabi A. 2001. The optimization of a bioregenerative life support space diet [PhD the- Vickers Z, Holton E. 1998. A comparison of taste test ratings, repeated consumption,
sis]. Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell Univ. and post-consumption ratings of different strengths of iced tea. J Sens Stud 13:199–
Peryam DR, Girardot NF. 1952. Advanced taste-test method. Food Engr 24:58–61. 212.
Peryam DR, Pilgrim FJ. 1957. Hedonic scale method of measuring food preferences. Wardle J, Herrera ML, Cooke L, Gibson EL. 2003. Modifying children’s food prefer-
Food Technol 11:9–14. ences: the effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable.
Pearce JH, Korth B, Warren CB. 1986. Evaluation of three scaling methods for hedo- Eur J Clin Nutr 57:341–8.
nics. J Sens Stud 1(1):27–46. Warren C, Pearce J, Korth B. 1982. Magnitude estimation and category scaling. ASTM
Pliner P. 1982. The effect of mere exposure on liking for edible substances. Appetite Standard News 10:15–6.
3:283–90. Zandstra EH, de Graaf C, van Trijp HCM, van Staveren WA. 1999. Laboratory hedonic
Pliner P, Hobden K. 1992. Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neo- ratings as predictors of consumption. Food Qual Pref 10:411–8.
phobia in humans. Appetite 19:105–20. Zandstra EH, de Graaf C. Mela DJ, van Staveren WA. 2000a. Short- and long-term ef-
S: Sensory & Food

Pliner P, Pelchat M, Grabski M. 1993. Reduction of neophobia in humans by exposure fects of changes in pleasantness on food intake. Appetite 34:253–60.
to novel foods. Appetite 20:111–23. Zandstra EH, de Graaf C, van Trijp HCM. 2000b. Effects of variety and repeated in-
Porcherot C, Issanchou S. 1998. Dynamics of liking for flavored crackers: test of pre- home consumption on product acceptance. Appetite 35:113–9.
Quality

dictive value of a boredom test. Food Qual Pref 9(1/2):21–9.

S106 JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE—Vol. 74, Nr. 2, 2009

Anda mungkin juga menyukai