Anda di halaman 1dari 21

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

Learning, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/98/S3.O0


1998, Vol. 24, No. 1,27-47

The Role of Attention During Encoding in Implicit


and Explicit Memory
Neil W. Mulligan
Illinois State University

In 5 experiments, participants read study words under conditions of divided or full attention.
Dividing attention reduced performance on the general knowledge test, a conceptual implicit
test of memory. Likewise, dividing attention reduced conceptual priming on the word-
association task, as well as on a matched explicit test, associate-cued recall. In contrast, even
very strong division of attention did not reduce perceptual priming on word-fragment
completion, although it did reduce recall on the matched explicit test of word-fragment-cued
recall. Finally, dividing attention reduced recall on the perceptual explicit tests of graphemic-
cued recall and graphemic recognition. The results indicate that perceptual implicit tests rely
minimally on attention-demanding encoding proceses relative to other types of memory tests.
The obtained pattern of dissociations is not readily accommodated by the transfer-appropriate-
processing (TAP) account of implicit and explicit memory. Potential extensions of the TAP
view are discussed.

Attention during encoding plays a critical role in later tion priming. Research on implicit memory has garnered
memory performance. Dividing attention during encoding great notice because repetition priming often differs from
reduces performance on many measures of memory, includ- performance on explicit tests in important ways. First, there
ing free and cued recall and recognition (e.g., Baddeley, have been numerous reports of population dissociations
Lewis, Eldridge, & Thomson, 1984; Broadbent, 1958; between implicit and explicit test performance, in which
Cherry, 1953; Craik, Govoni, Naveh-Benjamin, & Ander- participants from populations impaired on explicit memory
son, 1996; Fisk & Schneider, 1984; Moray, 1959; Murdock, tests (e.g., people with amnesia, depression, or schizophre-
1965; Norman, 1969b). The prevalence of these findings nia and older adults) show normal or near-normal levels of
encouraged earlier researchers to posit that attention is repetition priming on implicit memory tests (e.g., Denny &
required for the formation of durable memory traces (e.g., Hunt, 1992; Elliott & Greene, 1992; Light, 1991; Schwartz,
Broadbent, 1958; Cherry, 1953; MacKay, 1973; Moray, Rosse, & Deutsch, 1993; Shimamura, 1986, 1993). Second,
1959; Norman, 1969a). A similar, central role for attention in a number of experimental manipulations, such as levels of
encoding has also been espoused in more recent work (e.g., processing, the read-generate manipulation, and study mo-
Bentin, 1994; Cowan, 1988, 1995; Craik, 1989; Craik et al., dality, also have produced dissociations between repetition
1996; Fisk & Schneider, 1984). priming and performance on explicit tests (see Roediger &
The memory experiments that gave rise to this traditional McDermott, 1993, for a review).
view generally used explicit (or direct) tests of memory. In Given the centrality of attention in theories of memory
such tests, participants are directed to think back and try to encoding, it is of great interest that dividing attention during
recall information about some specific (usually experimenter- encoding may also dissociate performance on implicit and
provided) event. Memory may be assessed with implicit (or explicit tests. Several studies have reported that dividing
indirect) tests of memory, as well (Roediger & McDermott, attention during encoding can reduce explicit test perfor-
1993; Schacter, 1987). In these tests, memory for prior mance while leaving repetition priming unaffected (Bentin,
events is inferred from the enhanced reprocessing of re- Kutas, & Hillyard, 1995; Gabrieli et al., 1996; Isingrini,
cently experienced stimuli, a phenomenon known as repeti- Vazou, & Leroy, 1995; Jacoby, Woloshyn, & Kelley, 1989;
Kellogg, Newcombe, Kammer, & Schmitt, 1996; Mulligan
Neil W. Mulligan, Department of Psychology, Illinois State & Hartman, 1996; Parkin, Reid, & Russo, 1990; Parkin &
University. Russo, 1990; Russo & Parkin, 1993; M. E. Smith &
This research was supported in part by a university research Oscar-Berman, 1990; Szymanski & MacLeod, 1996). Other
grant from Illinois State University. The assistance of Robert studies have demonstrated that retention of very poorly
Hitlan, Clayton Lovitt, Rick Sarles, and Molly McGuire is attended stimuli is more likely to be detected with implicit
gratefully acknowledged. Thanks to Theresa Blaxton for supplying than explicit memory tests (Bornstein, Leone, & Galley,
experimental materials and to Maria Stone, Marilyn Hartman, 1987; Eich, 1984; Hawley & Johnston, 1991; Jelicic, Bonke,
Kavitha Srinivas, Steve Lindsay, and Leah Light for providing very Wolters, & Phaf, 1992; MacKay, 1973; Mandler, Nakamura,
useful comments on a draft of this article. & Van Zandt, 1987; Merikle & Reingold, 1991; Seamon,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marsh, & Brody, 1984). These results are so compelling that
Neil W. Mulligan, who is now at the Department of Psychology,
Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas 75275. Electronic some researchers have suggested that implicit test perfor-
mail may be sent via Internet to mulligan@mail.smu.edu. mance largely reflects automatic encoding processes (Ben-

27
28 MULLIGAN

tin, 1994; Bentin et al., 1995; Besson, Fischler, Boaz, & their implicit-explicit status. This parsimonious approach
Raney, 1992; Graf & Mandler, 1984; Isingrini et al., 1995; has had a good deal of success in accounting for and
Jacoby, Toth, & Yonelinas, 1993; Jacoby et al., 1989; Jelicic predicting dissociations among memory tests (see Roediger
et al., 1992; Parkin et al., 1990; Parkin & Russo, 1990; & McDermott, 1993, for a review).
Shallice et al., 1994; Szymanski & MacLeod, 1996; see Finally, a strength of this theory is that theorists need not
Koriat & Feuerstein, 1976, for a related view). However, rely on intuition to categorize a test as perceptual or
recent research on attention and implicit memory (Gabrieli, conceptual. Rather, the TAP framework proposes a set of
Stone, Shackleton, et al, 1996; Light & Prull, 1995b; criterial variables for operationally defining memory tests as
Mulligan, 1997; Mulligan & Hartman, 1996; Wood, Stadler, primarily conceptual or perceptual. A central criterion is
& Cowan, in press) suggests that this view is insufficient. To how test performance is affected by the read-generate study
provide a framework for describing and summarizing the manipulation. In this manipulation, some study words are
relevant research, the transfer-appropriate-processing (TAP) presented intact to be read, whereas others study words are
account of implicit and explicit memory (e.g., Blaxton, not physically presented but must be produced by the
1989,1992; Craik, Moscovitch, & McDowd, 1994; McDer- participant from a conceptual cue (e.g., an antonym). In the
mott & Roediger, 1996; Roediger, 1990; Roediger, Weldon, read condition, the perceptual characteristics of the study
& Challis, 1989; Weldon, Roediger, Beitel, & Johnston, word are processed to a greater degree than in the generate
1995) is useful. I first describe this framework and then condition, whereas in the generate condition, the conceptual
review the relevant studies on attention and implicit memory. characteristics of the word are processed to a greater degree
The TAP framework is predicated on the notion that (e.g., Jacoby, 1983b). In the TAP framework, memory tests
performance on a memory test is enhanced to the extent that that exhibit the typical generation effect, in which the
encoding processes are reengaged at the time of retrieval generate condition leads to superior performance to that of
(Blaxton, 1989; Roediger, 1990; Roediger et al., 1989), an the read condition (e.g., Slamecka & Graf, 1978), are
assumption consistent with the encoding-specificity prin- assumed to reengage primarily conceptual processes and are
ciple (Tblving & Tomson, 1973). Consequently, the TAP classified as conceptual memory tests. Memory tests exhibit-
framework proposes that memory tests should be character- ing the reverse generation effect, in which the read condition
ized primarily by the types of mental processes that they leads to greater performance than the generate condition, are
engage rather than by test instructions (i.e., implicit vs. assumed to reengage primarily perceptual processes and are
explicit). Toward this end, the TAP framework differentiates classified as perceptual memory tests (e.g., Blaxton, 1989;
between two broad classes of cognitive processes: percep- Roediger, 1990; Roediger & McDermott, 1993; Roediger et
tual processes, defined as the analysis of perceptual or al., 1989). Other primary criterial variables are the study-
surface-level features (but may also include other representa- modality manipulation and the levels-of-processing manipu-
tions required for stimulus identification; e.g., Weldon, lation (e.g., Blaxton, 1989; Roediger, 1990; Roediger &
1991), and conceptual processes, defined as the analysis of McDermott, 1993).1
meaning or semantic information. With regard to the effects of divided attention, the TAP
The most widely used implicit memory tests involve the distinction between perceptual and conceptual processes has
identification or completion of degraded or ambiguous two implications. If, as is traditionally assumed, dividing
perceptual cues, such as word fragments or stems, briefly attention at study reduces the amount of semantic or
presented words, or fragmented pictures. The TAP frame- conceptual processing (e.g., Broadbent, 1971; Craik, 1983;
work posits that these tests are primarily dependent on Craik &Byrd, 1982; Craik etal., 1996; Eagle &Ortof, 1967;
perceptual processes during retrieval and, as a consequence, Koriat & Feuerstein, 1976; Norman, 1969a) but has little or
are sensitive to variation in perceptual processes at encoding no effect on perceptual identification processes, then the
but relatively insensitive to variation in conceptual pro- TAP framework predicts (a) that divided attention should
cesses. In contrast, commonly used explicit tests (e.g., free have little or no effect on perceptual tests and (b) that
recall and recognition) are assumed to primarily use concep- divided attention should reduce performance on conceptual
tual processses during retrieval (Craik et al., 1994; Roediger tests.
et al., 1989). The TAP framework accounts for dissociations With regard to perceptual implicit tasks, the prediction of
between implicit and explicit tests in terms of the differences the TAP framework has been largely borne out. Provided the
between underlying processes rather than in terms of task manipulation of attention does not disrupt stimulus identifi-
instructions. Specifically, because the most common implicit cation, dividing attention leaves priming unaffected on the
and explicit memory tests rely differentially on conceptual perceptual tests of word-fragment completion (Mulligan &
and perceptual processes, performance on these two types of
tests benefits from different types of encoding procedures 1
and is thus dissociable. Although perceptual implicit tests are typically unaffected by
the levels-of-processing manipulation (Roediger & McDermott,
However, note that not all implicit tests are perceptual and 1993), at least some of these tests are sensitive to this manipulation
not all explicit tests are conceptual. With the appropriate under some conditions (Challis & Brodbeck, 1992; Thapar &
combinations of memory cues and task instructions, concep- Greene, 1994; see Brown & Mitchell, 1994, for a review). The
tual implicit tests and perceptual explicit tests may be exact significance of these results for the TAP framework is a
constructed. The TAP framework proposes that dissociations matter of current debate (e.g., Challis & Brodbeck, 1992; Thapar &
occur between conceptual and perceptual tests, regardless of Greene, 1994).
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 29
Hartman, 1996; Parkin et al., 1990), picture-fragment comple- focus this inquiry on the role of attention in postperceptual
tion (Parkin & Russo, 1990; Russo & Parkin, 1993), lexical aspects of memory encoding. Consequently, the attentional
decision (Kellogg et al., 1996; M. E. Smith & Oscar- manipulations used in these and subsequent experiments do
Berman, 1990; Szymanski & MacLeod, 1996), and percep- not prevent the identification of the study stimuli. Partici-
tual identification (Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al., 1996; pants in divided-attention conditions were asked to identify
see Mulligan & Hartman, 1996, for a review). These results study words as well as carry out a secondary task. Further-
imply that perceptual priming has minimal reliance on more, given the findings of Mulligan (1997), strong division
attentional resources (beyond those necessary for stimulus of attention seems important if one is to maximize the
identification; see Mulligan & Hartman, 1996, and Stone, probability of observing divided-attention effects on concep-
Ladd, Vaidya, & Gabrieli, 1996, for discussion). The only tual priming. Consequently, procedures were used that were
exception is priming in word-stem completion, which ap- similar to those used by Mulligan and Hartman (1996,
pears to be reduced by divided attention (Gabrieh, Stone, Experiment 1) and Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al. (1996)
Shackleton, et al., 1996). However, the nature of this implicit in uncovering effects of divided attention on conceptual
test is in dispute; some researchers argue that this test is priming in the category-exemplar production test. Two
primarily perceptual, whereas others argue that it is primar- groups of participants were visually presented with a set of
ily conceptual (cf. Gabrieli et al., 1994; Keane, Gabrieli, words under either full- or divided-attention conditions. The
Fennema, Growdon, & Corkin, 1991; Roediger, Weldon, divided-attention task was a digit-monitoring task in which
Stadler, & Riegler, 1992). participants were asked to monitor an aurally presented
Most of the studies on implicit memory and divided series of digits for sequences of three odd numbers. This task
attention have used perceptual priming tasks. The few provides a rigorous division of attention, eliminating prim-
studies using conceptual implicit tests have produced mixed ing in the category-exemplar production task (Mulligan &
results. Consistent with the prediction of the TAP frame- Hartman, 1996). The results of Mulligan (1997), which used
work, Mulligan and Hartman (1996) recently reported that various memory loads to divide attention, are consistent
dividing attention during encoding reduced later conceptual with this characterization. The digit-monitoring task in
priming on the category-exemplar production task, as well Mulligan and Hartman reduced conceptual priming to the
as reducing recall on a comparable explicit test, category- same (approximately zero) level as the strongest divided-
cued recall (see Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al., 1996; attention condition in Mulligan (1997).
Light & Prull, 1995b, for similar findings). However, In Experiment 1, the memory test consisted of a set of
Isingrini et al. (1995), using a similar experimental design questions, presented as a general knowledge test. The
and the same memory tests, found no effect of divided answers to some of the questions were presented in the study
attention on category-exemplar production, although cat- list. Within the TAP framework, the general knowledge test
egory-cued recall was reduced. Because the division-of- is considered conceptual because it is affected by conceptual
attention task in Isingrini et al. appeared to be a weaker encoding manipulations, such as the generation manipula-
manipulation of attention than that used by Mulligan and tion (Blaxton, 1989,1992) and levels of processing (Hamann,
Hartman and Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al. (1996) and 1990), and is unaffected by perceptual encoding manipula-
because Isingrini et al. (1995) reported a nonsignificant tions, such as varying study modality (Challis et al., 1993).
reduction in priming for all of the divided-attention groups The TAP framework implies that this implicit test should be
(Isingrini et al. compared full vs. divided attention in each of affected by divided attention during encoding.
four age groups), Mulligan (1997) hypothesized that the
strength of the attention manipulation was important in
accounting for the divergent results. Using a memory-load Experiment 1
paradigm to divide attention over several levels, Mulligan
found that category-cued recall was reduced in all the
Method
divided-attention conditions, whereas in the category- Participants. Sixty-four undergraduates at Illinois State Univer-
exemplar production task, priming was reduced (in fact, sity participated in exchange for extra credit in psychology classes.
eliminated) only for the strongest division of attention but Design and materials. The experiment used a 2 X 2 design in
not for milder divisions. This appears to resolve the discrep- which study status (old vs. new) was manipulated within subjects
ancy between Mulligan and Hartman's results and those of and attention at encoding (full vs. divided) was manipulated
Isingrini et al. Conceptual priming in the category-exemplar between subjects.
production task is affected by divided attention, but the The study and test materials were extracted from the set
developed by Blaxton (1989). One hundred general knowledge
division of attention in Isingrini et al.'s study was too mild to questions and their single-word answers (e.g., "What is the fastest
reveal this. In summary, conceptual priming in category- animal on earth?" Cheetah) were chosen from this source. The 100
exemplar production appears to be dependent on attention. answers were randomly divided into two sets of 50. These critical
items were preceded by a primacy buffer and followed by a recency
Experiments 1 and 2 tested the generality of the TAP buffer of 4 unrelated words each, yielding two study lists of 58
prediction that conceptual priming should be diminished by words. The general knowledge test consisted of the 100 questions
divided attention by examining the effects of this manipula- corresponding to the critical items (50 corresponding to studied, or
tion on the conceptual implicit tests of general knowledge old, items and 50 to new items), plus 8 filler questions, also drawn
questions (Experiment 1) and word association (Experiment from Blaxton's materials. The resulting 108 questions were ran-
2). The implications of the TAP framework derived earlier domly ordered.
30 MULLIGAN

The task used in the divided-attention condition was a digit- Table 1


monitoring task (as used in Mulligan & Hart man, 1996; see Craik, Experiment 1: Proportions of Correct Responses in the
1982; Jacoby, 1991). Participants monitored a tape-recorded series General Knowledge Test as a Function of Study Status and
of random digits, attempting to detect target sequences of three odd Attention at Encoding
digits in a row. The digits were played at the rate of 1 digit per 1.5 s.
There were 32 target sequences randomly distributed through the Study status
Attention
series of 180 digits, subject to two constraints: (a) A minimum of 1 condition Old New Old - new
and a maximum of 5 numbers occurred between the end of one
target sequence and the beginning of the next, and (b) not more than Full attention .30 .15 .15
2 even digits occurred in sequence. The list of 180 digits was Divided attention .25 .17 .08
recorded six times from beginning to end without pause between
repetitions.
sented as a function of attention at encoding and study
Procedure. Participants were tested individually. The experi- status. The results are straightforward: Dividing attention
ment began with the study phase, in which each study word was
displayed in the center of a computer screen for 3 s. Participants
reduced the amount of priming in the general knowledge
were instructed to read each word out loud and to try to remember test. The proportions correct were submitted to a 2 X 2
the words for a later, unspecified memory test. This was the sole analysis of variance (ANOVA), using study status (old vs.
task for participants in the full-attention condition. Participants in new) as a within-subjects variable and attention at encoding
the divided-attention condition simultaneously performed the digit- (full vs. divided) as a between-subjects variable. The
monitoring task. These participants were told to monitor the digits significance level for this and subsequent analyses was .05.
for strings of three odd numbers in a row and to tap a pen on the The analysis produced two significant effects. First, the main
desk whenever they detected a target string. If a participant missed effect of study status was significant, F(l, 62) = 60.25,
a target sequence, the experimenter prompted the participant by MSE = 0.0068, indicating significant overall priming.
saying "Miss." The experimenter monitored and recorded errors in Second, and more important for present purposes, the Study
reading the words and detecting target sequences of digits. Task Status X Attention interaction was significant, F(l, 62) =
instructions emphasized that the digit-monitoring task and reading
and remembering the words were both equally important. Partici-
5.17, MSE = 0.0068, indicating greater priming in the full-
pants in the divided-attention condition were allowed approxi- than divided-attention condition. Follow-up t tests indicated
mately 30 s of practice on the digit-monitoring task before the start a significant effect of study status (i.e., priming) in both the
of the study list. full- and divided-attention conditions, f(31) — 6.89 and
A 3-min distractor task followed the study phase. The task was f(31) = 4.01, respectively.
used to disguise the relationship between the study session and the The results of the present experiment were consistent with
subsequent memory test by making the memory test appear to be Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al. (1996), Mulligan and
one in a series of unrelated tasks. The distractor task was a city Hartman (1996), and Mulligan (1997), indicating that a
names completion task. Participants were presented with a set of strong division of attention reduces conceptual priming in
index cards, on each of which was printed the first three letters of a
U.S. city (e.g., Bos— for Boston). The participants were asked to
both the category-exemplar production task and in the
complete each city word stem with the name of a U.S. city. general knowledge test. However, one potential problem
The general knowledge test followed the distractor task. Partici- with using the general knowledge test as a measure of
pants were presented with a test booklet consisting of the general conceptual priming is that it may be more susceptible to
knowledge questions. Participants were instructed to try but were explicit contamination than other conceptual implicit tests
not required to answer each question. Answers were to be written in (Vaidya, Gabrieli, Demb, Keane, & Wetzel, in press; Vaidya
the space provided next to each question. Participants were given et al., 1996). For example, amnesics are frequently impaired
15 s per question. Time was kept with an audiocassette tape that on this test, which may be an indication of explicit contami-
emitted a beep every 15 s. No mention was made of the relationship nation (Blaxton, 1992, Experiment 1; Vaidya et al., in press).
between this task and the studied words. In Experiment 2, the effects of divided attention were
assessed on the conceptual implicit memory test of word
association. This task is arguably a better measure of
Results and Discussion implicit memory and one in which amnesics perform
During the study task, participants in the divided-attention comparably to nonamnesics (Graf, Shimamura, & Squire,
condition correctly identified 97% of the study words and 1985; Schacter, 1985). In addition, Experiment 2 used a
detected 88% of the target digit strings. Thus, participants in posttest questionnaire to determine whether participants
the divided-attention condition were able to identify the vast were aware of the relationship between the study episode
majority of the study words while simultaneously identify- and the implicit test and the extent to which they had
ing almost all of the target sequences. However, the identifi- deliberately tried to retrieve study items (Bowers & Schac-
cation of study words was not quite perfect in the divided- ter, 1990; Light & Albertson, 1989; Mulligan & Hartman,
attention condition. Consequently, the test data in this and all 1996). As is shown, the results from the posttest question-
subsequent experiments were analyzed in two ways, condi- naire indicated that the word-association task was minimally
tionalized on correct identification at study and uncondition- contaminated by explicit influences.
alized. In all cases, the two analyses led to identical conclus- The word-association task has been used in prior research
ions. Only the unconditionalized results are reported below. to assess the effects of divided attention. Anooshian (1989)
The results of the general knowledge test are summarized concluded that dividing attention reduced conceptual prim-
in Table 1. Mean proportions of correct answers are pre- ing in a variant of the traditional word-association task. In
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 31

this study, participants were aurally presented with homo- attention as that used by Gabrieli et al. and by Mulligan and
phones embedded in phrases biasing their less common Hartman.
meaning. The phrases were presented either in focally A final consideration is that Koriat and Feuerstein (1976)
attended questions or on the unattended channel in a dichotic did not use parallel explicit and implicit memory tests.
listening task. Participants were later asked to free associate Consequently, it is not possible to determine if the dissociat-
to a set of old and new aurally presented homophones. The ing effects of divided attention are due to the manipulation of
interpretations of the homophones were inferred from the test instructions (implicit vs. explicit) or to other differences
free associations produced. Anooshian found that old homo- between the word-association task and the free-recall and
phones were more frequently interpreted in line with their recognition tests (e.g., type of memory cue or response
less common meaning than were new homophones and that demands). As demonstrated by Craik et al. (1996), different
this difference was larger for the focally attended than explicit tests are differentially reliant on (i.e., sensitive to)
unattended condition (i.e., greater conceptual priming in a attention at encoding. To be more confident that divided
full-attention condition). However, in this study the implicit attention dissociates implicit and explicit test performance,
word-association test followed an explicit test of homo- one should use a robust division of attention and demon-
phone recognition, which may have jeopardized the implicit strate its effects on the explicit test before one can confi-
status of the word-association task. dently interpret a null result on the parallel implicit test
An early study by Koriat and Feuerstein (1976), using the (Mulligan & Hartman, 1996).
traditional version of word association, suggests a different Further investigation of the word-association test is
conclusion. Koriat and Feuerstein presented participants important for two reasons. First, it is important to further test
with an auditory study list either under intentional learning the generality of the TAP prediction that conceptual priming
instructions or under incidental learning instructions coupled will be diminished by divided attention. Second, the Koriat
with a distractor task. This latter (divided attention) condi- and Feuerstein (1976) results have been interpreted as
tion reduced performance on the explicit memory tests of evidence that attention may not be required for intact
free recall and recognition but did not reduce priming on the conceptual priming (e.g., Jacoby, Toth, & Yonelinas, 1993;
conceptual implicit test of word association. Thus, unlike Jacoby, Woloshyn, & Kelley, 1989; Smith & Oscar-Berman,
Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al. (1996) and Mulligan and 1990). In Experiment 2, we addressed some of these issues
Hartman (1996), Koriat and Feuerstein found an apparent to determine if conceptual priming in the word-association
dissociation between conceptual priming and conceptual task is sensitive to a strong division of attention. We used
explicit memory. Specifically, Koriat and Feuerstein's re- procedures similar to those used by Mulligan and Hartman
sults imply that conceptual priming may not rely on (1996, Experiment 1) and Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al.
attentional encoding processes. (1996) in uncovering effects of divided attention on concep-
tual priming in the category-exemplar production test. These
There are several potential reasons for the divergent
same procedures were used in the present Experiment 1, in
results of Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al. (1996) and
which divided attention reduced priming in the general
Mulligan and Hartman (1996), on the one hand, and Koriat
knowledge test. Participants were visually presented with a
and Feuerstein (1976), on the other. The most obvious has to
list of words under the same full- or divided-attention
do with the differences between the word-association and
conditions as in Experiment 1. As noted earlier, the digit-
the category-exemplar production tests. The word-associa-
monitoring task provides a strong division of attention,
tion task typically makes use of highly associated items
eliminating priming in the category-exemplar production
(e.g., doctor-nurse), whereas the examples in category-
task (Mulligan & Hartman, 1996).
exemplar production are typically less strongly associated
with the category cues (in both Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, Retention of the studied words was tested with the
et al., 1996, and Mulligan & Hartman, 1996, the studied implicit test of word association and with its explicit
examples did not rank in the 10 most frequently produced counterpart, associate-cued recall. The cued-recall task was
instances, according to the norms of Battig & Montague, identical to the word-association task in all ways except
1969). It is possible that the use of very strong associates instructions. Thus, it was possible to determine if the
eliminates the need for elaborative processing to produce divided-attention task was strong enough to reduce perfor-
relatively accessible memory traces (Bentin et al., 1995; mance on a parallel explicit test.
Rabinowitz, Craik, & Ackerman, 1982).
A second difference among the studies is that they used Experiment 2
different tasks to divide attention. A digit-monitoring task
was used by Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al. (1996) and by Method
Mulligan and Hartman (1996), a memory-load task was used
by Mulligan (1997), and a digit symbol task was used by Participants. Sixty-four undergraduates at Illinois State Univer-
sity participated in exchange for extra credit in psychology classes.
Koriat and Feuerstein (1976). The results of Mulligan (1997)
Design and materials. The experiment used a 2 X 2 X 2 design
underscore the importance of the strength of the divided- in which study status (old vs. new) was manipulated within
attention manipulation. A memory test dependent on atten- subjects and attention at encoding (full vs. divided) and memory
tion during encoding may not appear so if attention has been test (word association vs. associate-cued recall) were manipulated
weakly divided. It is difficult to gauge whether Koriat and between subjects.
Feuerstein's manipulation was as strong a division of Fifty critical cue-response word pairs were chosen from the
32 MULLIGAN

Shapiro and Palermo (1968) word-association norms. Each re- Table!


sponse word was the primary response to its associated cue. The Experiment 2: Proportions of Responses in Word
normative associative probabilities of the word pairs varied from Association and Associate-Cued Recall as a Function of
.19 to .61, with a mean of .34. The study lists were composed of the Study Status and Attention at Encoding
response word from each pair. The response words were randomly
divided into two groups of 25 critical words each. Eight additional Study status
words were used as primacy and recency buffers, 4 preceding and 4 Memory test Old New Old - new
following the critical words, creating two study lists of 33 words
each. Word association
Both the word-association test and the cued-recall test consisted Full attention .35 .28 .07
of the 50 critical cue words, 25 corresponding to studied words (the Divided attention .28 .27 .01
old words) and 25 to nonstudied words (the new words). The Associate-cued recall
Full attention .31 .10 .21
critical cue words were intermixed with 44 filler cues (also chosen
Divided attention .17 .07 .10
from Shapiro & Palermo, 1968), creating a test list of 94 cue words.
The divided-attention task was identical to that used in Experiment 1.
Procedure. The study and distractor phases were identical to
Experiment 1. After the distractor task, participants completed
either the word-association test or the cued-recall test. For the subjects variable. For the word-association task, the analysis
word-association test, participants were told that this was a revealed two significant effects: (a) a main effect of study
normative word-association study and that we would use their status, F(l,30) = 10.81, MSE = 0.0027, and (b) an
responses to develop materials for future research. The participants interaction between study status and attention, F(l, 30) =
were asked to read each cue word and write down the first word that 5.25, MSE = 0.0027. Overall, significant priming was
came to mind, working rapidly through the list. Participants obtained, but crucially, priming was greater in the full- than
responded to each cue word in this task. No mention was made of divided-attention condition. Follow-up t tests indicated a
the relationship between this task and the studied words. After significant effect of study status (i.e., priming) in the
finishing the word-association task, participants were given an full-attention condition, r(15) = 4.23, but not in the divided-
awareness questionnaire. The experimenter asked a series of
increasingly specific questions, to assess whether participants attention condition (p > .5). Thus, priming was obtained in
noticed the relationship between the study task and the memory test the full-attention condition but not in the divided-attention
and the extent to which they had deliberately tried to retrieve study condition.
items. Analysis of the cued-recall data also yielded significant
For the cued-recall task, participants were informed that this was effects of study status, F(l, 30) = 56.63, MSE = 0.0067, and
a memory test for the words that were read aloud earlier in the the Study Status X Attention interaction, F(l, 30) = 6.92,
experiment. These participants were told to use the cue words to try MSE = 0.0067. These results indicated that recall of old
to remember related studied words but that many of the cues were words exceeded the guessing rate for new words (i.e.,
not related to studied items. Participants were asked to go through participants showed above-chance retention) but that di-
the cue words in order, writing down remembered words next to the vided attention reduced the difference between old and new
cue. As is standard in this task, participants were not required to
respond to each cue. Both the cued-recall and word-association items. Therefore, divided attention also affected the explicit
tasks were self-paced. version of the word-association task. In both the full-
attention condition and the divided-attention condition, t
tests indicated an effect of study status, t(l5) = 6.08 and
Results and Discussion 4.44, respectively. In contrast to the implicit version of this
During the study task, participants in the divided-attention test, there was evidence of retention in both the full- and
condition correctly identified 99.5% of the study words and divided-attention conditions in the cued-recall test.
detected 95% of the target digit strings. Mann-Whitney U The current results indicate that divided attention has a
tests indicated that the percentages of study words identified very similar effect on the implicit and explicit versions of the
and target strings detected did not significantly differ as a word-association task. In fact, the present results are pre-
function of subsequent test condition (ps > .5), as would be cisely in accord with Mulligan and Hartman's (1996,
expected. Participants in the divided-attention condition Experiment 1) finding that conceptual priming in the cat-
were able to identify virtually all study words while egory-exemplar production task was also reduced to (essen-
simultaneously identifying the vast majority of the target tially) zero in an identical divided-attention condition. The
sequences. current results contrast with the findings of Koriat and
Table 2 summarizes the results of the memory tests. Feuerstein (1976) and imply that given a strong manipula-
Proportions of critical items produced are presented as a tion of attention, conceptual priming in the word-association
function of test type, attention at encoding, and study status. task is greatly affected by divided attention.
It can be seen from the table that dividing attention produced Whenever parallel implicit and explicit tests are affected
strong effects on associatet-cued recall and conceptual prim- in similar ways, there is always the potential concern that the
ing in the word-association task. Statistical analyses confirm putative implicit test has been compromised by intentional
this summary. The data from the word-association and retrieval strategies. In the present case, it is possible that the
cued-recall tests were submitted to separate 2 x 2 ANOVAs, results are due to more effective use of intentional retrieval
using study status (old vs. new) as a within-subjects variable in the full-attention condition of the word-association task.
and attention at encoding (full vs. divided) as a between- One way to assess the extent to which intentional retrieval
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 33

influences the results of a nominally implicit memory test is tion at study reduces conceptual priming in the word-
with the use of posttest questionnaires (e.g., Bowers & association task, as well as the category-exemplar produc-
Schacter, 1990; Light &-Albertson, 1989; Mulligan & tion and general knowledge tasks. The present results are
Hartman, 1996). In the present study, none of the word- important in demonstrating that when the division of atten-
association participants reported using intentional retrieval tion is strong enough to reduce performance on the explicit
at test. In fact, only 4 of the 16 full-attention participants and test of associate-cued recall, the comparable implicit test of
2 of the 16 divided-attention participants reported that they word association is also affected. This is significant because
were aware that some of the words that they had produced it is not known whether Koriat and Feuerstein's (1976)
were from the study list (the proportion of aware participants divided-attention task, which had no measurable impact on
did not significantly differ across the full- and divided- word association, would have affected a comparable cued-
attention conditions: Fisher's exact test, p > .3, one-tailed). recall test.
Excluding the aware participants from the analysis does not The effects of divided attention on priming are generally
alter the results. The 12 unaware participants in the full- consistent with the predictions of the TAP framework.
attention condition produced 36% of the old items and 28% Recall that the TAP view predicts that for divisions of
of the new items, whereas the 14 unaware participants in the attention that do not disrupt identification, perceptual prim-
divided-attention condition produced 26% of the old and ing will be unaffected but conceptual priming will be
25% of the new items. The amount of priming was reduced. Several studies indicate that this prediction holds
significantly greater in the full- than divided-attention condi- for perceptual priming on tests such as word-fragment
tion, F(l, 24) = 7.78, MSE = 0.0045. In addition, signifi- completion, picture-fragment completion, lexical decision,
cant priming was obtained in the full-attention condition, and perceptual identification (Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et
t(\X) = 3.53, but not in the divided-attention condition al., 1996; Kellogg et al., 1996; Mulligan & Hartman, 1996;
(P > -5). Parkin et al., 1990; Parkin & Russo, 1990; Russo & Parkin,
Consistent with this assessment is the finding that divided 1993; M. E. Smith & Oscar-Berman, 1990; Szymanski &
attention reduces conceptual priming to approximately zero MacLeod, 1996). In contrast, conceptual priming on tasks
but leaves associate-cued recall substantially above zero. such as category-exemplar production, word association,
(Mulligan & Hartman, 1996, Experiment 1, reported the and the general knowledge test is reduced by divided
same result for category-exemplar production and category- attention. The results of Mulligan (1997) and (arguably) the
cued recall.) If performance on the word-association test results of Experiment 2 underscore the importance of the
were badly contaminated by intentional retrieval, then strength of the divided-attention manipulation, indicating
priming in the divided-attention condition would likely be that weaker manipulations may not reduce conceptual
significant. This finding lends further credence to the priming.
proposal that these implicit and explicit tests do not rely This last point raises the possibility that strong division of
(entirely, at least) on the same retrieval processes or memory attention may also affect perceptual priming tasks, even if
encodings. the attentional manipulation does not disrupt stimulus identi-
As an aside, we note that the word-association task seems fication. To develop this possibility, first consider that some
to give rise to lower levels of intentional retrieval and test divided-attention studies used dual-task manipulations in
awareness, as measured by the posttest questionnaire, than which the identification of study items either was substan-
category-exemplar production, which is a more widely used tially reduced or was not required (Bentin et al., 1995;
conceptual priming test. In Mulligan and Hartman (1996, Hawley & Johnston, 1991; Light & Prull, 1995a; Weldon &
Experiment 1), the proportions of test-aware participants Jackson-Barrett, 1993). Generally, these studies indicate that
were 93% and 57% in the full- and divided-attention manipulations that reduce the likelihood of stimulus identifi-
conditions, respectively (the corresponding proportions cation also reduce the amount of perceptual priming (Haw-
claiming intentional retrieval were 50% and 14%). For the ley & Johnston, 1991; but see Bentin et al., 1995, for an
young adults in Light and Albertson's (1989) study (most important exception). These studies were excluded from our
comparable with the full-attention condition here), 88% earlier discussion because they may represent a qualitatively
reported being test aware whereas 34% reported an inten- different type of manipulation than divided-attention tasks
tional retrieval strategy. These figures compare with 25% that disrupt only postperceptual encoding operations. For
and 12.5% of the word-association participants claiming test example, reductions in perceptual priming observed in these
awareness in the full- and divided-attention conditions, studies may result from disruptions in lexical access, as
respectively (as noted above, 0% claimed an intentional argued by Weldon and Jackson-Barrett (1993, p. 523). Such
retrieval strategy). To the extent that the posttest question- an account does not apply to the present results or to those
naire is a measure of the purity of an implicit test, these reviewed earlier, because overt identification of study words
results suggest that the word-association task may be a (presumably requiring lexical access) was required in both
superior measure of conceptual implicit memory, at least in the full- and divided-attention conditions.
this regard. The lexical-access account and other accounts that at-
In summary, the results of this experiment add to the tribute decreased perceptual priming to truncated perceptual
findings of Experiment 1 and the previously cited work analysis are in accord with the TAP framework. Specifically,
(Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al., 1996; Mulligan, 1997; the TAP view posits that any manipulation disrupting
Mulligan & Hartman, 1996) indicating that dividing atten- perceptual processes will reduce perceptual priming. The
34 MULLIGAN

alternative view is that divided-attention tasks that disrupt within subjects and memory test (word-fragment completion vs.
stimulus identification represent especially strong divisions word-fragment-cued recall) was manipulated between subjects.
of attention and that the resulting division of attention makes The critical study materials consisted of 96 relatively low-
a unique contribution to the reduction of perceptual priming frequency words (mean frequency of 13; KuCera & Francis, 1967),
beyond the simple disruption of perceptual processing from 5 to 10 letters in length. Two master study lists of 48 items
each were created by randomly dividing the study materials in half.
(Hawley & Johnston, 1991).2 If so, a strong manipulation of
Within each master file, the words were randomly divided into four
attention may reduce perceptual priming, even in tasks that groups (each consisting of 12 words), and each group was rotated
have heretofore been unaffected by divided attention and through each of the attentional load conditions. This produced four
even when the attentional manipulation does not prevent study lists from each master list, for a total of eight study lists. The
stimulus identification. This contrasts with the TAP frame- words in each study list were randomly ordered. Twelve additional
work, which views the strength of the attentional manipula- (noncritical) words were also used; 4 were presented before the list,
tion as irrelevant, provided perceptual identification pro- as practice items, and 4 were placed at the beginning and end of
cesses are not disrupted. each list as primacy and recency buffers. Each study list was
presented to equal numbers of participants.
Mulligan and Hartman (1996, Experiment 2), using study
Each study item was preceded by an attentional load. Nonzero
procedures identical to Experiments 1 and 2, found that
loads were constructed by randomly selecting items from the set of
dividing attention with the digit-monitoring task had no digits (1-9) and a set of letters (B, C, D, F, G, H, J, K, L) according
effect on perceptual priming in word-fragment completion to the following rules: (a) Digits and letters occupied alternating
but significantly reduced recall on the matched explicit test positions, with a digit in the first position, and (b) no repetition of
of word-fragment-cued recall. In Experiment 3, I examine digits or letters occurred within a load. These materials and rules
the generality of this finding by using a short-term memory- were used in an attempt to minimize chunking of the loads.
load paradigm to vary attention during encoding. In this The memory tests consisted of a total of 102 word fragments.
experiment, each study word was preceded by a short-term The fragments consisted of between 2 and 6 letters, with under-
attentional load consisting of zero (the full-attention condi- scores in the place of missing letters. The fragments were
tion), three, five, or seven digits and letters. Participants constructed to have only one legitimate completion. This was
were instructed to maintain the digits and letters in memory verified with the spellcheck and dictionary facilities of the WordPer-
until a RECALL signal was presented a few seconds later. fect program (Version 5.1), which indicated that all fragments were
consistent with only one English word completion. In addition, the
This technique has been used to control the division of
experimental participants never completed a fragment with any
attention over several levels (e.g., Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; word other than the intended one. Of the 102 fragments, 96
Engle, Conway, Tuholslri, & Shisler, 1995; Logan, 1979; corresponded to the critical items from the two master lists, 48 of
Mulligan, 1997). The particular attentional loads were which corresponded to old items and 48 to new items. Fragments
chosen on the basis of prior research. Using this memory- from an additional 6 words were used as filler items. The fragments
load paradigm, Mulligan (1997) found that an attentional were randomly ordered with the constraint that there be no more
load of five not only reduced but eliminated conceptual than 2 items in a row of any single type (old, new, orfiller).The old
priming in the category-exemplar production task. In addi- and new items were counterbalanced so that across participants,
tion, these results indicated that the attentional load of five each word appeared equally often in the old and new conditions.
produced comparable levels of priming and cued recall as Procedure. Participants were tested individually. The experi-
the digit-monitoring task used to divide attention in Mulli- ment consisted of a study task and a memory test. The study task
gan and Hartman (1996) and in the present Experiments 1 was based on the procedures of Mulligan (1997). Each study trial
and 2 (see Mulligan, 1997, Table 1). Considering an atten- began with a ready prompt that was displayed for 500 ms. Next, the
digit-letter string constituting the attentional load (or a dash in the
tional load of five as a strong division of attention, I included
zero-load condition) was presented for 3,250 ms. For the non-zero-
that level of load, as well as an even stronger division (i.e., load trials, participants were instructed to read the digit-letter
the seven load). string aloud and retain it in memory until the recall signal was
Implicit memory for the study words was assessed with given. For the zero-load trials, participants were simply instructed
to say "blank" in response to the dash. Next, the study word was
the perceptual implicit test of word-fragment completion. As
presented for 3 s. Participants were instructed to read the word
noted above, this perceptual test has not been affected by aloud and try to remember it for a later, unspecified memory test.
other (although possibly weaker) manipulations of attention. Finally, either the word RECALL (in the non-zero-load conditions)
In the event that the present manipulation did not affect this or the word BLANK (in the zero-load condition) appeared for 2,750
test, it would be important to determine if the manipulation ms. Participants were instructed either to recall the digits and letters
affected a comparable explicit test. Consequently, a separate (in the non-zero-load conditions) or to again say "blank" (in the
group of participants were given a word-fragment cued- zero-load condition).
recall test.
2
Consistent with this view is the finding that explicit test
Experiment 3 performance may exceed chance following study conditions that
Method eliminated perceptual priming (e.g., Hawley & Johnston, 1991;
Weldon & Jackson-Barrett, 1993). This result may be problematic
Participants. Fifty-six undergraduates at Illinois State Univer- for the lexical-access account and other accounts positing truncated
sity participated in exchange for extra credit in psychology classes. perceptual processing because above-chance explicit memory
Design and materials. The experiment used a 4 x 2 design in implies that stimulus identification or lexical access occurred for at
which attentional load (zero, three, five, or seven) was manipulated least some study items.
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 35
After the study task, participants were given either the word- 3.62, MSE = 0.0258, but not for word-fragment completion
fragment-completion or word-fragment cued-recall test. For both (p > .5). For the cued-recall test, pairwise comparisons of
tests, participants were given a sheet of paper with the 102 the load conditions indicated significantly greater perfor-
fragments, which were numbered and arranged in columns. In the mance in the zero- than the seven-load condition, r(23) =
word-fragment-completion task, participants were asked to try to 2.51; in the three- than five-load condition, *(23) = 2.30; and
complete each fragment with an appropriate English word. Ex-
in the three- than seven-load condition, f(23) = 2.65. No
amples of a fragment and an appropriate completion were pro-
vided. No mention was made of the relationship between this task other differences were significant (all otherps > .15). Perfor-
and the study task. In the word-fragment cued-recall test, partici- mance in each of the load conditions was significantly
pants were informed that their memory was being tested. They greater than for new fragments, all f(23)s > 3.37. For the
were also informed that some of the fragments corresponded to word-fragment condition, performance in each of the load
words from the study list and some did not. The participants were conditions was significantly greater than for new fragments;
asked to try to recall words from the study list that completed the that is, priming occurred in all load conditions, all rs(23) >
fragments and, if they could not do so, to complete the fragment 6.84.
with any appropriate word that came to mind. These were the same
guessing instructions used by Mulligan and Hartman (1996, Finding that dividing attention had a significant impact on
Experiment 2) in an attempt to equate response requirements across word-fragment cued recall but no measurable impact on
the implicit and explicit tests. This renders the two memory tests word-fragment completion is theoretically important. Be-
comparable in all ways except test instructions (i.e., implicit vs. fore continuing, however, it is important to evaluate the
explicit). power of the latter statistical test. Power calculations were
For both memory tests, the participants were given 12 s for each performed (following Kirk, 1995, pp. 259-264) using a
fragment. Time was kept with an audiotape that emitted a beep to-be-detected effect size estimated from the cued-recall
every 12 s. Participants were instructed to use the entire 12 s on the data. The estimated effect size of attentional load on the cued
current fragment, not to work backward, and to move on to the next recall is / = 0.29, somewhat larger than Cohen's (1988)
fragment only after hearing the beep. The participants were given a definition of a "medium" effect size of/ = 0.25. With alpha
cover sheet to keep their place. equal to .05 and a sample size of 32, the power of detecting
Thirty-two of the participants were given the word-fragment an effect of attentional load on word-fragment completion of
completion test, and the other 24 were given the word-fragment the same size as that obtained with cued recall was .84.
cued-recall test.
Because earlier research has demonstrated the importance
Results and Discussion of the strength of attentional manipulations in the study of
attention and implicit memory (Mulligan, 1997), one motiva-
During the study task, the mean proportion of attentional tion for this experiment was to provide a range of divided-
load items recalled was .99, .94, and .76 for the three-, five-, attention conditions, from mild to very strong, within one
and seven-load conditions, respectively. Because these data experiment. A potential concern was that whereas it is
deviated substantially from the normal distribution (espe- reasonable to claim that increasing loads represent increas-
cially in the three- and five-load conditions), a nonparamet- ing division of attention, the present method did not
ric test (Friedman's test of ranks) was used. This test guarantee that the participants would maintain increasing
revealed significant differences across loads. Pairwise sign amounts of information in working memory. Consequently,
tests indicated that all three load conditions significantly the possibility that nominally different load conditions do
differed from one another. In addition, Mann-Whitney U not actually tax working memory to different extents must
tests performed within load conditions found no significant be entertained. One way to evaluate this is to examine the
effects of subsequent test condition (allps > .1). The mean average number of items retained by participants in the
proportion of study words correctly identified was .99 and different load conditions by converting percentage of load
did not vary as a function of attentional load (p > .5) or items recalled (given above) into the raw number of load
subsequent test condition (p > .25). Thus, participants were items recalled. The average numbers of load items recalled
able to identify virtually all study words while simulta- significantly increased across load conditions: 2.97, 4.70,
neously maintaining the attentional loads. and 5.32, in the three-, five-, and seven-load conditions,
The results of the memory tests are presented in Figure 1. respectively; Friedman's test of ranks: x2(2, N = 32) =
Mean proportions of fragments completed are presented as a 88.32. Not surprisingly, sign tests indicated that significantly
function of test type and attentional load. As statistical more items were maintained in either the seven- or five-load
analyses confirm, attentional load had a significant impact condition than in the three-load condition (both Zs = 7.35).
on performance in the explicit test of word-fragment-cued More important for present purposes, significantly more
recall but not in the comparable implicit test of word- items were maintained in the seven- than five-load condition
fragment completion. (Z- 2.81). This is consistent with the assertion that the
To measure retention, the proportion of completed new seven-load condition represents a stronger division of atten-
fragments was subtracted from the proportion of completed tion than either the three- or five-load condition.
old fragments in each of the load conditions. The resulting Another way to address this issue would be to limit the
data from the word-fragment cued recall and word-fragment analysis of the test data to only those participants showing
completion were submitted to separate ANOVAs, using high levels of performance in the seven-load condition at
attentional load as a within-subjects variable. The effect of study. As a cutoff, I included only those participants that
attentional load was significant for cued recall, F(3, 69) = recalled an average of more than five items in the seven-load
36 MULLIGAN

Word-Fragment Completion

.60-

.50

o
o
I .40

.30

.20
1+1 1 1

New
Attentlonal Load

B
Word-Fragment Cued Recall

.20
New
Attentional Load

Figure 1. Results of Experiment 3: Mean proportions of completions (and ± 1 standard error bars)
in (A) the word-fragment-completion task and (B) the word-fragment cued-recall test as a function of
attentional load at encoding.

condition. This guaranteed that the resulting set of partici- items, in the three-, five-, and seven-load conditions, respec-
pants maintained more information in working memory in tively. For each participant, the average number of load
the seven- than the five-load condition. Fourteen of the 24 items recalled increased in each successive load condition.
(58%) cued-recall participants and 18 of the 32 (56%) An analysis of the test data for this subset of participants
word-fragment completion participants exceeded this cutoff. revealed the same pattern of results as that obtained with all
On average, these participants recalled 2.99, 4.84, and 6.13 participants. Attentional load had a significant impact on the
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 37

explicit memory test of word-fragment cued recall, F(3, 39) = The final two experiments examine these issues. Experi-
3.15, MSE = 0.0323, but not on the comparable implicit test of ment 4 uses the graphemic-cued-recall test, an explicit
word-fragment completion (p > .5). memory test that has been classified as perceptual (see
Thus, even when attention is very strongly divided during Blaxton, 1989; Roediger, 1990; Roediger et al., 1989).
encoding, as in the five- and seven-load conditions of the Experiment 5 uses the graphemic recognition test, a new
present experiment, later performance on the perceptual memory test that appears to be primarily perceptual in nature
priming task is unaffected. This result is consistent with the (Challis, Velichkovsky, & Craik, 1996). In these memory
results of Mulligan and Hartman (1996, Experiment 2), in tests, participants are presented with memory cues bearing
which attention was divided with the digit-monitoring task graphemic similarity to studied items (e.g., cheetohs as a cue
used in the present Experiments 1 and 2. Of course, the for the studied word cheetah). In the graphemic-cued-recall
criticism could always be raised that an even more rigorous test, participants are informed of this relationship and asked
division of attention could be implemented (say, a nine-load to use the cues to try to recall studied words. In the
condition), which would reduce later perceptual priming graphemic recognition test, participants are asked to recog-
without reducing study-word identification, a possibility nize words that have a similar appearance to studied words,
difficult to rule out. However, using the memory-load without having to recall the studied words.
paradigm, it is questionable whether larger load conditions The results of Experiment 3 and Mulligan and Hartman
would actually have the effect of more rigorously dividing (1996, Experiment 2) are relevant to Experiments 4 and 5. In
attention. Preliminary results from our laboratory indicate these studies, the effects of divided attention were assessed
that for loads exceeding seven, retention of load items on an explicit version of a perceptual implicit test. Specifi-
decreases markedly, producing functional loads that may not cally, word-fragment cues were coupled with explicit test
differ from the seven-load condition. Other methods of instructions. Dividing attention reduced recall on the result-
dividing attention may circumvent this problem. In any ing word-fragment cued-recall test. This may be taken as
event, these results are consistent with the view that evidence that perceptual explicit tests are negatively affected
perceptual priming has minimal requirements of attention- by divided attention. However, it should be pointed out that
demanding postperceptual encoding processes. In addition, word-fragment cued recall has not been categorized as a
these results provide additional evidence that divided atten- perceptual test according to the TAP criterial variables. Also,
tion dissociates perceptual priming and explicit memory as noted above, refinements to the TAP view suggest that
performance, even when the implicit and explicit tests are explicit retrieval instructions render memory tests sensitive
matched in terms of memory cues and response require- to prior conceptual processing (e.g., Craik et al., 1994;
ments (Mulligan & Hartraan, 1996). Roediger et al., 1992). Consistent with this view, Roediger et
al. (1992) found that word-fragment cued recall was sensi-
The TAP framework has thus far been successful at
tive to a levels-of-processing manipulation that left word-
predicting and accounting for the effects of attention on
fragment completion unaffected. A more complete evalua-
implicit memory. However, the TAP framework, as initially
tion of the TAP view would use an explicit memory test
proposed, was posited as a general theory of both implicit
categorized as perceptual according to the TAP criteria.
and explicit memory performance (e.g., Blaxton, 1989;
Graphemic-cued recall has been so categorized because it
Roediger, 1990; Roediger et al., 1989). This framework
exhibits the reverse-generation effect (i.e., the read condition
emphasizes the importance of the types of processes en-
leads to higher performance than the generate condition), the
gaged by a memory test rather than the nature of the test
purported signature of a perceptual memory test (Blaxton,
instructions (i.e., implicit vs. explicit). Consequently, disso-
1989; Roediger, 1990). Consequently, Experiment 4 investi-
ciations are expected when perceptual and conceptual tests
gates the effects of divided attention on this perceptual
are compared, but tests within one of the categories (e.g.,
explicit test. A strong interpretation of the TAP framework
perceptual tests) are generally expected to be similarly
implies that this test will be relatively insensitive to divided
affected by experimental manipulations.3 For example, the
attention.
TAP framework implies that perceptual explicit tests should
react more like perceptual implicit tests than conceptual However, it has recently been reported that the graphemic-
explicit tests. This has been found with respect to several cued-recall test is affected by the traditional levels-of-
experimental manipulations, including read-generate, study processing manipulation (Challis et al., 1996; Thapar &
modality, imagery versus no-imagery instructions, and typog-
raphy manipulations (e.g., Blaxton, 1989; see Roediger &
McDermott, 1993, for a review). A strong interpretation of 3
This does not mean that the TAP view never predicts dissocia-
the TAP view (which appears consistent with the theory's tions between perceptual tests. If two perceptual tests cue memory
initial form) implies that because perceptual implicit tests by means of different modes or modalities (e.g., picture-fragment
are unaffected by divided attention, perceptual explicit tests completion vs. word-fragment completion), the TAP view predicts
should be similarly unaffected. Later versions of the TAP that certain study manipulations (e.g., studying pictures vs. words)
framework present a somewhat more differentiated view, will dissociate the tests (see Roediger, 1990, for discussion). In the
present discussion, I restrict consideration to perceptual tests that
proposing that explicit retrieval instructions may make a cue memory in the same mode and modality (e.g., all test cues are
memory test more reliant on prior conceptual processing visually presented words or word fragments). Without further
(e.g., Craik et al., 1994; Roediger et al., 1992; we return to refinement, the default prediction of the TAP view is that such tests
this issue in the General Discussion). should be similarly affected by study manipulations.
38 MULLIGAN

Greene, 1994). This implies that despite being categorized that were not tested. Participants were informed that many but not
as perceptual on the basis of the read-generate manipula- all of the cues were related to study items. Beyond this, no specific
tion, this memory test may have a conceptual component. guessing instructions were provided, and participants were not
Challis et al. developed a variant of this test, the graphemic required to respond to each cue. Participants were given 10 s per
recognition test, that is not affected by levels of processing. cue. Time was kept with an audiotape that emitted a beep every 10
s. Participants were provided with a cover sheet to keep their place.
Although this test has not been categorized according to the
They were asked to use the entire 10 s on one cue and to move onto
other TAP criterial variables, it is reasonable to assume that the next cue at the beep. Twenty-four of the participants were given
it is a perceptual explicit test, minimally influenced by prior the graphemic-cued-recall test, and the other 20 were given the
conceptual processing (as indicated by its insensitivity to the semantic-cued-recall test.
levels-of-processing manipulation). On this basis, the graphe-
mic recognition test is provisionally categorized as a percep- Results and Discussion
tual explicit test. To the extent that this test does not rely on
conceptual retrieval processes, both versions of the TAP During the study task, participants in the divided-attention
framework lead to the expectation that the graphemic condition correctly identified 97% of the study words and
recognition test will respond to experimental manipulations detected 90% of the target digit strings. Mann-Whitney U
in ways similar to perceptual implicit tests. Specifically, like tests indicated that the percentages of study words identified
perceptual implicit tests, the graphemic recognition test and target strings detected did not significantly differ as a
should have minimal reliance on attention at encoding. The function of subsequent test condition (ps > .10), as would
graphemic recognition test is examined in Experiment 5. be expected. As in the earlier experiments, participants in the
In contrast to the TAP view, the bulk of the attention and divided-attention condition were able to identify the vast
memory literature suggests that the effects of divided majority of the study words while simultaneously identify-
attention on explicit tests are pervasive and are to be ing almost all of the target sequences.
expected on the graphemic-cued-recall and graphemic recog- Table 3 summarizes the results of the memory tests.
nition tests. We begin by contrasting these alternative Proportions of critical items produced are presented as a
implications in Experiment 4 using graphemic-cued recall. function of test type, attention at encoding, and study status.
For comparison, we include a matched conceptual explicit The table demonstrates that dividing attention during acqui-
test, semantic-cued recall (Blaxton, 1989), which is ex- sition has strong effects on both the semantic- and graphemic-
pected to be adversely affected by divided attention.4 In the cued-recall tests. The data from the memory tests were
semantic-cued-recall test, memory for the same study items submitted to separate 2 X 2 ANOVAs, using study status
is tested with semantic rather than graphemic cues. (old vs. new) as a within-subjects variable and attention at
encoding (full vs. divided) as a between-subjects variable.
Beginning with the semantic-cued-recall test, all three
Experiment 4 effects were significant: (a) a main effect of attention, F(l,
Method 18) = 5.92, MSE = 0.0017; (b) a main effect of study status,
F(l, 18) = 32.86, MSE = 0.0022; and (c) an interaction
Participants. Forty-four undergraduates at Illinois State Univer- between study status and attention, F(l, 18) = 4.49, MSE =
sity participated in exchange for extra credit in psychology classes. 0.0022. The results indicated that more critical items were
Design and materials. The experiment used a 2 X 2 X 2 design produced in the full- than divided-attention condition, that
in which study status (old vs. new) was manipulated within recall of old words exceeded the guessing rate for new words
subjects and attention at encoding (full vs. divided) and memory
(i.e., participants showed above-chance retention), and that
test (graphemic-cued recall vs. semantic-cued recall) were manipu-
lated between subjects. the difference between old and new items (i.e., the measure
The study materials and digit-monitoring task were the same as of retention) was greater in the full- than divided-attention
those used in Experiment 1. For each of the critical study words, a condition. Follow-up t tests indicated an effect of study
graphemic cue and a semantic cue were drawn from Blaxton's status both in the full-attention condition, f(9) = 4.85, and
(1989) materials. For example, the graphemic and semantic cues the divided-attention condition, t(9) = 3.07, indicating
for the study word cheetah were cheetahs and jaguar, respectively. above-chance retention in both the full- and divided-
The graphemic- and semantic-cued-recall tests consisted of the attention conditions. In summary, the results of the semantic-
appropriate set of cues arranged in a random order on a sheet of cued-recall test indicated that dividing attention produced
paper. Each cue was followed by an answer space. Of the 100 cues, the expected effect on this conceptual explicit memory test:
50 corresponded to old items, and 50 corresponded to new items. It reduced memory for old items.
Production of new items served as the estimate of guessing rate.
Across participants, cues were equally often in the old and new The results of the graphemic-cued-recall test are identical
conditions. to the results of the semantic-cued-recall test. The analysis
Procedure. The study and distractor phases were identical to
Experiments 1 and 2. After the distractor task, participants were
4
presented with either the graphemic- or semantic-cued-recall test. The semantic-cued-recall test is similar to the associate-cued-
Participants were asked to use the provided cues to recall words recall test of Experiment 2 in that both tests use semantic associates
from the study list. The relationship between the cues and the as cues. Because the semantic-cued-recall test used in Experiment
to-be-recalled words was explained, and three examples from the 4 was derived from Blaxton's (1989) materials, I refer to this test by
primacy and recency portions of the study list were provided. the same name used in Blaxton's article rather than calling it an
Recall that the primacy and recency buffers consisted of filler items associate-cued-recall test.
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 39

Table 3 665-666), the comparable means were .23 and .11, respec-
Experiment 4: Proportions of Responses in tively. These results are similar to the results in the present
Graphemic- and Semantic-Cued Recall as a Function full-attention condition, in which the respective means were
of Study Status and Attention at Encoding .26 and . 11 (see Table 3). Other relevant data come from the
no-context condition of Blaxton's (1992) Experiments 1 and
Study status
2. This condition was similar to the present experiment in
Memory test Old New Old — new that participants read study words in isolation (as opposed to
Graphemic-cued recall generating study words or reading them in a semantic
Full attention .31 .05 .26 context). In the no-context condition, the graphemic-cued-
Divided attention .17 .05 .12 recall test led to greater mean difference scores than the
Semantic-cued recall semantic-cued-recall test for control participants (the exact
Full attention .14 .03 .11 figures are not provided in the article, but estimates from
Divided attention .07 .02 .05 graphs of the data indicate the graphemic advantage was at
least .20). The same pattern of results was obtained for
temporal lobe epileptics (Blaxton, 1992). The only compa-
rable experiment producing conflicting results is the no-
yielded significant effects of attention, F(\y 22) - 6.18, MSE =
context condition in Blaxton's (1989) Experiment 1, in
0.0093; study status, F(l, 22) = 73.40, MSE = 0.0057; and
which performance on the semantic-cued-recall test was
the Study Status X Attention interaction, F(l, 22) = 9.51,
somewhat higher than on the graphemic-cued-recall test.
MSE = 0.0057. These results reveal the same pattern as
above. Specifically, more critical items were produced in the There are quite likely several factors that differ across
full- than divided-attention condition, recall of old words these experiments that play a role in accounting for the
exceeded the guessing rate for new words, and the difference precise relationship between graphemic- and semantic-cued
between old and new items was greater in the full- than recall (as well as accounting for the variability in overall
divided-attention condition. As with the semantic-cued- performance levels). For example, the graphemic and seman-
recall test, t tests indicate an effect of study status in both the tic cues in the Blaxton (1989) materials, from which the
full- and divided-attention conditions, fs(ll) = 6.26 and present materials were drawn, were not equated on such
7.52, respectively, indicating above-chance retention in both variables as cue-target associative strength or search set
attention conditions.5 To summarize, the perceptual explicit size, both factors that can affect recall (see Nelson, Schreiber,
test of graphemic-cued recall suffered the same deleterious & McEvoy, 1992, for a review). However, taken as a group,
effects of divided attention as semantic-cued recall, a these results indicate that for the Blaxton (1989) materials,
conceptual explicit test the graphemic cues may be more effective than the semantic
A final and somewhat surprising aspect of the results in cues when nonelaborative-nongenerative encoding strate-
Table 3, although of less importance for present purposes, is gies are used. The opposite appears to be true for elaborative
that performance tended to be higher on the graphemic- than or generative encoding strategies (see Blaxton, 1989,1992).
the semantic-cued-recall test. To investigate, old-new differ- To the extent that this summary accurately describes the
ence scores were submitted to a 2 X 2 ANOVA, using test relationship between the efficacy of the graphemic and
(graphemic- vs. semantic-cued recall) and attention at semantic cues, it is in accord with general TAP tenets.
encoding as between-subjects variables. This analysis re- Specifically, to the extent that conceptual or elaborative
vealed significant main effects of attention, F(l,40) = processes are invoked at encoding, tests using semantic cues
13.81, MSE = 0.0082 (indicating, as above, that divided (which presumably reengage conceptual processes) should
attention reduced retention), and of test, F(l, 40) = 14.69, have an advantage over tests using nonsemantic cues.
MSE = 0.0082. The latter effect indicates greater recall of Although this aspect of the results may be in accord with
studied items when graphemic cues were provided at test. In the TAP framework, this digression should not obscure the
addition, the graphemic test led to significantly greater main point of the present experiment: Perceptual as well as
performance on new items (i.e., a higher guessing rate), F(l, conceptual explicit tests have at least some reliance on
42) = 5.73,A/S£ = 0.0013. attention at encoding. Contrary to the TAP expectation,
Although surprising, there is precedence for the finding of division of attention has similar effects on perceptual and
greater recall with the graphemic than semantic cues with
Blaxton's (1989) materials. We restrict consideration only to
5
those experiments and conditions that are most comparable This pattern of t values occurred because the divided-attention
to the full-attention condition in the present experiment condition had lower variance than the full-attention condition,
(e.g., intentional learning instructions, visual study presenta- despite producing a smaller mean effect of study status. Although
tion, same type font for study words and test cues, and no the difference in variances across the full- and divided-attention
imagery, elaborative, or generative encoding instructions). conditions was not significant by Levene's test, it was nevertheless
In Blaxton's (1989) Experiment 2 (visual study condition), prudent to reexamine the data with a nonparametric test. The
old-new difference scores were submitted to the Mann-Whitney V
the mean old-new difference scores for the graphemic- and test, resulting in a significant difference between attention condi-
semantic-cued-recall tests were .41 and .33, respectively. In tions (U = 26.0). Thus, the nonparametric test produced the same
Blaxton's Experiment 3 (same type font at study and test, conclusion as the parametric tests reported in the text: Divided
averaged over elite-elite and italic-italic conditions, see pp. attentionreducesretentionin the graphemic-cued-recall test.
40 MULLIGAN

conceptual explicit tests, whereas it dissociates perceptual detected 86% of the target digit strings. As in the earlier
implicit and explicit tests. experiments, participants in the divided-attention condition
As noted earlier, a potential limitation of the present study were able to identify the vast majority of the study words
is that the graphemic-cued-recall test can be affected by the while simultaneously identifying almost all of the target
levels-of-processing manipulation (Challis et al., 1996; sequences.
Thapar & Greene, 1994). Consequently, a stronger test of the Table 4 summarizes the results of the memory test. Mean
hypothesis that divided attention affects perceptual explicit proportions of "old" responses are presented as a function
memory may be garnered by using an explicit test that is not of test-associate type, attention at encoding, and study
affected by levels of processing. Graphemic recognition is status. Focusing on the graphemic test items, Table 4
such a test (Challis et al., 1996), and it is used in Experiment 5. demonstrates that dividing attention during acquisition re-
duced graphemic recognition. The results for the semantic
test items indicate that participants were able to (and did)
Experiment 5 follow the instructions to endorse only items graphemically
Method similar to studied words. A preliminary analysis confirmed
that the graphemic and semantic test items gave rise to
Participants. Sixty-four undergraduates at Illinois State Univer- different patterns of results and should be analyzed sepa-
sity participated in exchange for extra credit in psychology classes. rately. In this analysis, proportions of circled test items were
Design and materials. The experiment used a 2 X 2 X 2 design submitted to a 2 X 2 X 2 ANOVA, using study status (old vs.
in which study status (old vs. new) and test-associate type new) and test-associate type (graphemic vs. semantic) as
(graphemic vs. semantic) were manipulated within subjects and within-subjects variables and attention at encoding (full vs.
attention at encoding (full vs. divided) was manipulated between
divided) as a between-subjects variable. The relevant signifi-
subjects.
The study materials and digit-monitoring task were the same as cant effects were (a) the Study Status X Test Associate
those used in Experiment 4. The graphemic-recognition test was interaction, F(l, 62) = 56.42, MSE = 0.0060, indicating
modeled after Challis et al. (1996). The test lists were created from that as expected, the difference between old and new words
the same set of graphemic and semantic cues used in Experiment 4. was greater for graphemic than semantic test items and (b)
Specifically, each test list consisted of all 100 graphemic associates the three-way interaction, F(l, 62) = 4.34, MSE = 0.0060,
(50 corresponding to the old words and 50 to the new words) and indicating that the enhanced old-new difference for graphe-
50 of the 100 semantic associates (25 corresponding to old and 25 mic items was affected by attention at encoding. Although
to new words). Four test lists were created, so that both the there were other significant effects in this preliminary
graphemic associates and the semantic associates were counterbal- analysis, these interactions indicated that the data would be
anced over study status (i.e., old vs. new). The test items were best analyzed separately for the graphemic and semantic test
randomly ordered on the test sheet. The participant's task was to items.
circle test items that looked like the words presented during the
study phase. For this test, the correct answers are the 50 graphemic Data from the graphemic and semantic test items were
associates of the old words. All other circled test items are false submitted to separate 2 X 2 ANOVAs, using study status as a
alarms. The graphemic associates of new words served as the within-subjects variable and attention at encoding as a
false-alarm rate for graphemic recognition. The semantic associates between-subjects variable. For the semantic test items, none
were included to determine if participants adhered to instructions of the effects were significant (all ps > .1), consistent with
and circled only graphemically similar and not semantkally similar
the claim that participants were able to follow the instruc-
test items (Challis et al., 1996). If participants followed instruc-
tions, then false-alarm rates for the semantic associates of old tions and did not differentially endorse test items that were
words would be no higher than the false-alarm rate for the semantic semantically similar to old items. The analysis of the
associates of new words (the result obtained by Challis et al., graphemic items revealed two significant effects, a main
1996). effect of study status, F(l, 62) = 131.83, MSE = 0.0065,
Procedure. The study and distractor phases were identical to and an interaction between study status and attention, F(l,
Experiment 4. Following the distractor task, participants were 62) = 7.62, MSE = 0.0065. The results indicated that
presented with the graphemic-recognition test. Following Challis et recognition of items graphemically similar to old words (i.e.,
al. (1996), participants were told that some of the words on the test
sheet looked like words that had been read in the earlier study
phase. Participants were told to focus on the appearance of the test Table 4
words and ignore their meaning, circling those test items that Experiment 5: Proportions of "Old" Responses in the
looked like words from the study phase. Two examples from the Graphemic-Recognition Test as a Function of Study Status,
primacy and recency portions of the study list were provided. As in Test-Associate Type, and Attention at Encoding
the earlier experiments, the primacy and recency buffers consisted
of filler items that were not tested. Participants were instructed to Study status
Test-associate
take their time and try to be as accurate as possible. The memory type Old New Old - new
test was unpaced.
Graphemic
Full attention .33 .13 .20
Divided attention .28 .16 ' .12
Results and Discussion Semantic
During the study task, participants in the divided-attention Full attention .12 .10 .02
Divided attention .15 .13 .02
condition correctly identified 99% of the study words and
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 41
hits) exceeded the guessing rate for items graphemically making (Kellogg et al., 1996; M. E. Smith & Oscar-Berman,
similar to new words (i.e., false alarms). This indicated that 1990; Szymanski & MacLeod, 1996). In contrast to percep-
participants showed above-chance retention. More impor- tual implicit tests, priming in conceptual implicit tests
tant, the difference between old and new items (i.e., the generally appears to be reliant on attention at encoding,
measure of retention) was greater in the full- than divided- especially when attention is strongly divided (Mulligan,
attention condition. Follow-up f tests indicated an effect of 1997). Division of attention can reduce conceptual priming
study status in both the full- and the divided-attention on tests such as category-exemplar production (Gabrieli,
conditions, f(31) = 11.24 and 5.64, respectively. Stone, Shackleton, et al., 1996; Mulligan, 1997; Mulligan &
In summary, the results of the graphemic recognition test Hartman, 1996), word association (Experiment 2), and
converge with the results of Experiment 4, indicating that general knowledge questions (Experiment I). 6
dividing attention reduces memory for old items in percep- Before considering the theoretical implications of these
tual explicit tests. Moreover, these results make it unlikely results, I discuss two related methodological considerations
that divided attention reduced graphemic-cued recall solely in examining the role of attention in implicit and explicit
because this test has some sensitivity to prior conceptual memory. First, the results underscore the importance of
processing (as indicated by its sensitivity to the levels-of- using matched implicit and explicit tests when intepreting
processing manipulation). The graphemic recognition test, dissociations (Neely, 1989; Schacter, Bowers, & Booker,
which uses the same overt memory cues as graphemic-cued 1989). Before one can confidently assert mat the attentional
recall but is unaffected by prior conceptual processing requirements of implicit memory differ from those of
(indicated by its lack of sensitivity to the levels-of- explicit memory, it is important that dissociations be based
processing manipulation), is nonetheless very affected by on comparable implicit and explicit tests, ideally tests that
divided attention. differ only in task instructions. Second, the use of strong
manipulations of attention, coupled with multiple levels of
attention as in the memory-load paradigm of Experiment 3
General Discussion (also see Mulligan, 1997), is quite useful because memory
tests differ in the extent to which they are sensitive to
This article reports five experiments on the role of
attentional manipulations during encoding (e.g., Craik et al.,
attention during encoding on later implicit and explicit
1996).
memory. Integrating the present findings with prior research
yields a generally coherent set of findings, summarized here The relevance of these considerations is made clear by
in terms of the TAP taxonomy. First, as described in the considering some of the details of the results of Experiment
introduction, attentional state at encoding has pervasive 3, in which the attentional load variable significantly re-
effects on traditional tests of memory, such as free recall, duced performance on word-fragment-cued recall but not on
recognition, and cued recall, all conceptual explicit tests in word-fragment completion. Consider the zero- (i.e., full
the TAP nomenclature (e.g., Baddeley et al., 1984; Craik et attention) and three-load conditions. It is known from prior
al., 1996; Fisk & Schneider, 1984; Moray, 1959; Murdock, research (Mulligan, 1997) that manipulation of attention
1965; Norman, 1969b). Although not in great need of over this range reduces performance on the explicit test of
replication, this finding was reproduced in Experiments 2 category-cued recall (it is reasonable to assume that other
and 4, in which dividing attention reduced performance on explicit tests, such as free recall, would be likewise af-
cued-recall tests. More important, Experiments 4 and 5 fected). If Experiment 3 had consisted of only the word-
showed that performance on the perceptual explicit tests of fragment-completion test and only the zero- and three-load
graphemic-cued recall and graphemic recognition are also conditions, an inappropriate conclusion may have been
reduced by divided attention. Coupled with the finding that derived; to wit, division of attention dissociates implicit and
word-fragment-cued recall (arguably a perceptual explicit explicit tests of memory. That this conclusion is too strong
test; see Blaxton et al., 1996) is also affected by divided can be seen by considering the results of the matched
attention (the present Experiment 3; Mulligan & Hartman, explicit test, word-fragment-cued recall. Over the same
1996), the pattern of results supports the more general memory loads, this explicit test is also unaffected by divided
conclusion that performance on explicit tests, whether attention. Over that range of the attentional manipulation,
perceptual or conceptual, is dependent on attention at there is no dissociation of explicit and implicit memory tests
encoding. that use word-fragment cues, even though other explicit
tests, such as category-cued recall, are affected. In fact, the
Turning to implicit tests of memory, it appears that attention manipulation does dissociate word-fragment
perceptual priming is relatively unaffected by divided atten- completion and word-fragment-cued recall, but this is not
tion, provided that stimulus identification is not disrupted apparent unless the larger attention loads (i.e., stronger
(see discussion of Experiment 2). In Experiment 3, such divisions of attention) are considered.
results were found for word-fragment completion, even
using a very strong division of attention (see Gabrieli, Stone,
Shackleton, et al., 1996; Mulligan & Hartman, 1996; Parkin 6
This summary may be complicated by two recent conference
et al., 1990, for similar findings). Similar results have been presentations indicating that the semantic verification task, a
reported for perceptual identification (Gabrieli, Stone, Shack- priming task requiring a conceptual decision, is unaffected by
leton, et al., 1996), picture-fragment completion (Parkin & divided attention and levels of processing (Gabrieli, Stone, Vaidya,
Russo, 1990; Russo & Parkin, 1993), and lexical decision et al., 1996; Light & Prull, 1995b).
42 MULLIGAN

This discussion is relevant to the conflicting results Hartman, 1996), general knowledge questions (Experiment
concerning the word-association test. As noted earlier, 1), and word association (Experiment 2). In two of these
Koriat and Feuerstein (1976) found no effect of divided (category-exemplar production and word association), prim-
attention on word association, whereas the present Experi- ing was reduced essentially to zero. The encoding of what
ment 2 did. However, the Koriat and Feuerstein study did might be called core semantic features would appear to be an
not include a matched explicit test, and the strength of the important determinant of performance in these tasks. It
attentional manipulation was unclear. In the present Experi- could be argued that truly core semantic features do not
ment 2, a comparable explicit test was used (the associate- mediate priming in the category-exemplar production task,
cued recall), and the attentional manipulation was strong in which the critical exemplars are typically not the most
enough to reduce performance on that explicit test. Under common category examples from the category norms (Mul-
these circumstances, priming in the word-association test ligan & Hartman, 1996; Rappold & Hashtroudi, 1991).
was reduced, essentially to zero. Although there may well be However, core semantic features are presumably crucial for
other important differences between the discrepant studies", priming in the general knowledge test, in which many of the
the results of Experiment 2 strongly imply that priming in questions are essentially definitions of the to-be-produced
the word-association task relies on attention, at least under item, and in the word-association task, in which the critical
the present circumstances. response term is the most common associate to the cue. In
Next, I consider some of the theoretical implications of these cases, prior processing of what must be considered
the present results. As noted in the introduction, some core semantic features is crucial. In all cases, this prior
theorists have suggested that recollective memory (pre- processing appears to be importantly dependent on attention.
sumed to underlie explicit test performance) is crucially Passive identification of the study words does not produce
dependent on attention at encoding, whereas nonrecollec- conceptual priming on these tasks either at all or at least at
tive, or fluency, aspects of memory do not rely (or have much lower levels than in full-attention conditions. This
minimal reliance) on attentional encoding (Bentin, 1994; implies either (a) that contrary to Bentin (1994; Bentin et al.,
Bentin et al., 1995; Besson et al., 1992; Isingrini et al., 1995; 1995) and Rabinowitz et al. (1982), the encoding of core
Jacoby, Toth, & Yonelinas, 1993; Jacoby, Woloshyn, & semantic features requires attention, or (b) that the memorial
Kelley, 1989; Jelicic et al., 1992; Parkin et al., 1990; Parkin vestiges of automatic semantic processing during word
& Russo, 1990; Szymanski & MacLeod, 1996; see Shallice identification influence only some long-term memory tests.
et al., 1994, for a similar assumption). The present results, In this last section, I discuss the ramifications of the
coupled with the results of Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al. present results for the TAP framework. The TAP framework
(1996) and Mulligan and Hartman (1996), are inconsistent has been successful in accounting for much of the data from
with this view. The present results indicate that if prior implicit and explicit research (see Roediger, 1990, and
conceptual-elaborative processing influences the expression Roediger & McDermott, 1993, for reviews). Despite this
of nonrecollective memory, as it does on conceptual implicit success, a growing body of research, to which this study
tests, then attention at encoding is crucial for that expres- adds, indicates that the TAP dichotomy between perceptual
sion. and conceptual processing is not rich enough to account for
A related issue is the attentional requirements for encod- the data from implicit and explicit memory research, even,
ing semantic information. What aspects of meaning, if any, unfortunately, in broad strokes. Because the TAP view posits
are encoded as a mere consequence of identifying a word common perceptual processes underlying perceptual im-
(i.e., automatically)? There is substantial evidence for auto- plicit and explicit tests and common conceptual-elaborative
matic semantic activation during the course of word identifi- processes underlying conceptual implicit and explicit tests,
cation (e.g., Balota, 1983, 1990; Carr, 1992; Durante & this view suggests that dissociations between memory tests
Hirshman, 1994; Hirshman & Durante, 1992; Neely, 1977, are more likely to fall along the perceptual-conceptual
1991), but the effects of such activation are typically dimension than the dimension of implicit-explicit test
short-lived (e.g., Neely, 1977). Are there automatic semantic instructions.
encodings that lead to longer term retention? Rabinowitz et A first objection (noted by Roediger, 1990) has been that
al. (1982) and Bentin (1994; Bentin et al., 1995) argued that population dissociations are far more likely to occur along
core (or context-invariant) semantic features of words are the latter dimension than the former (e.g., Cermak, Verfael-
encoded automatically and impact long-term memory tests. lie, & Chase, 1995; Gabrieli, Stone, Shackleton, et al., 1996;
In contrast, elaborative encoding processes and the integra- Light & Albertson, 1989; Schwartz et al., 1993; see Mosco-
tion of words with the contexts in which they occur (both of vitch, Goshen-Gottstein, & Vriezen, 1994, and Moscovitch,
which also influence long-term retention) are hypothesized Vriezen, & Goshen-Gottstein, 1993, for reviews; although
to require attention (Bentin, 1994; Bentin et al., 1995; see Blaxton, 1992, for application of the TAP framework to
Rabinowitz etal., 1982). population dissociations). For instance, amnesics, older
The results of this and previous studies (Gabrieli, Stone, adults, and schizophrenics all show normal levels of concep-
Shackleton, et al., 1996; Mulligan, 1997; Mulligan & tual priming on category-exemplar production but reduced
Hartman, 1996) are relevant to this viewpoint. To review, performance on the matched explicit tests of category-cued
divided attention reduced conceptual priming on three recall, when compared with appropriate controls (Keane,
implicit tests: category-exemplar production (Gabrieli, Stone, Gabrieli, Monti, Cantor, & Noland, 1993; Light & Albert-
Shackleton, et al., 1996; Mulligan, 1997; Mulligan & son, 1989; Schwartz et al., 1993). Likewise, amnesics show
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 43

normal conceptual priming on the word-association test but ments to the TAP framework in terms of how they might
reduced recall on the matched-associate-cued recall test account for the effects of divided attention on implicit and
(Cermak et al., 1995). Finally, amnesics frequently show explicit tests of memory, as well as the other dissociations
normal levels of priming on perceptual implicit tests but not listed above.
on the perceptual explicit test of graphemic-cued recall First, dissociations that appear problematic for the TAP
(Cermak etal., 1995). view may simply indicate that memory tests are not purely
The TAP framework has had greatest success in account- conceptual or purely perceptual in nature (Blaxton, 1989;
ing for experimental dissociations in research with healthy, Weldon & Coyote, 1996; Weldon et al., 1995). If so, then an
normal participants (see Roediger & McDermott, 1993, for important challenge to the TAP framework is to establish a
review). Even in this arena, recently obtained dissociations principled characterization of the mixture of perceptual and
have failed to honor the perceptual-conceptual distinction in conceptual processes involved in a given memory test (see
much the same way as population dissociations. Weldon and Roediger & McDermott, 1993, for discussion).
Coyote (1996) found a picture-superiority effect on concep- Weldon et al. (1995; see also Craik et al., 1994; Roediger
tual explicit tests (category-cued recall and associate-cued et al., 1992) proposed a refinement to the TAP framework
recall) but not on comparable implicit tests (category- along these lines by positing that conceptual processes play
exemplar production and word association, respectively). A a larger role in explicit than implicit tests of memory,
similar dissociation was reported by Mulligan (1996) in an independent of the nature of the memory cues (e.g., Graf &
investigation of the perceptual interference effect in memory. Mandler, 1984). If explicit test instructions encourage
Under certain circumstances, interfering with (but not conceptual retrieval processes, then this would account for
preventing) stimulus perception at study can increase perfor- dissociations between perceptual implicit tests and compa-
mance on later explicit memory tests. Mulligan (1996) rable explicit tests, such as the dissociation between word-
reported that perceptual interference enhanced recall on the
fragment completion and word-fragment-cued recall caused
category-cued-recall test but had no effect on conceptual
by dividing attention at study. Specifically, if divided
priming in the category-exemplar production test. The
attention reduces conceptual encoding processes, then word-
results of Weldon and Coyote (1996) and of Mulligan (1996)
fragment completion, which engages perceptual retrieval
are both examples of experimental dissociations between
conceptual implicit and explicit tests. McDermott and processes, would be unaffected by the variation in prior
Roediger (1996) recently reported that both exact and conceptual processing. However, coupling the word-
conceptual repetition of study items influenced free recall fragment cues with explicit retrieval instructions renders a
but not category-exemplar generation or category-cued test that engages both perceptual and conceptual retrieval
recall. Although not a dissociation of comparable implicit processes. Consequently, word-fragment-cued recall would
and explicit tests, the results are a dissociation of different be sensitive to the divided attention manipulation (see
types of conceptual memory tests. Similarly, Cabeza (1994) Mulligan & Hartman, 1996, for related discussion). This
reported a dissociation between two conceptual implicit version of the TAP framework may well account for other
tests. dissociations between perceptual implicit and explicit tests
(e.g., Roediger et al., 1992; Weldon et al., 1995).
To summarize, there exist (a) population variables that However, such a modification of the TAP framework
dissociate performance on conceptual implicit and explicit faces three problems. First, by itself, it does not account for
tests, (b) population variables that dissociate performance on the observation that some perceptual explicit tests, like
perceptual implicit and explicit tests, (c) experimental graphemic-cued recall and word-fragment-cued recall, are
manipulations that dissociate conceptual implicit and ex- sensitive to the levels-of-processing manipulation, whereas
plicit tests (i.e., the picture-superiority and perceptual- others, such as graphemic recognition, are not (Challis et al.,
interference effects), (d) other experimental manipulations 1996; Roediger et al., 1992; Thapar & Greene, 1994).
that dissociate conceptual explicit tests from one another Second, it does not seem to account for dissociations
(i.e., exact and conceptual repetition), and (e) still other between conceptual implicit and explicit tests. According to
experimental manipulations that dissociate conceptual im- the TAP framework, conceptual tests, either implicit or
plicit tests from one another (see Cabeza, 1994). To this set explicit, are assumed to be sensitive to the same underlying
of results, may now be added a sixth: The experimental conceptual processes. The proviso that explicit test instruc-
manipulation of divided attention dissociates performance tions engage a common set of conceptual processes more
on perceptual implicit and explicit tests. fully does suggest that conceptual explicit tests may render
As noted by several researchers (e.g., McDermott & more sensitive measurement of prior conceptual processes
Roediger, 1996; Mulligan, 1996; Weldon & Coyote, 1996), (see Mulligan, 1997, for discussion), but without further
dissociations within the class of conceptual tests or within specification, this view does not explain why a conceptual
the class of perceptual tests are problematic for the TAP implicit test may be completely insensitive to a manipulation
view and suggest that its continued viability requires further that has a marked effect on a comparable explicit test. A
refinement. These refinements may take any of several forms complementary approach, suggested by Cabeza (1994), is to
(or, perhaps, multiple refinements may be necessary to articulate subclasses of conceptual processes and determine
accommodate all the data). Here, we consider two refine- both how encoding manipulations influence the subclasses
44 MULLIGAN

and how the subclasses map onto the various implicit and tests, is required to more fully evaluate this extension of the
explicit tests (this is similar to the component-process model TAP framework.
of Moscovitch, 1992; see also Moscovitch et al., 1993;
Vriezen, Moscovitch, & Bellos, 1995).
7
A third problem with this modification of the TAP One way around this problem is to expand the set of criterial
framework is that it renders the TAP criterial variables variables. This has two consequences for the utility of the TAP
superfluous. Recall that in the TAP framework, memory framework. First, it increases the chances of conflicting outcomes
tests are classified as primarily perceptual or conceptual (i.e., some criterial variables indicate that the test is perceptual, and
according to how they are influenced by a set of criterial others indicate that it is conceptual, as in the case of graphemic-
cued recall), thus decreasing the number of memory tests for which
variables, such as the read-generate manipulation and unambiguous predictions may be made. Second, as the set of
manipulations of study modality. Part of the power of the criterial variables increases, the utility of classification decreases.
TAP framework is that it allows predictions to be made on Specifically, if we must evaluate a memory test on increasingly
the basis of the classification. For instance, a memory test larger sets of variables before classification, this necessarily
classified as perceptual is predicted to be unaffected by any reduces the number of variables about which the theory makes
of a large number of conceptual encoding manipulations. If predictions.
we add the proviso that explicit test instructions encourage
conceptual retrieval processes, then the TAP framework
does not generate unambiguous predictions for explicit tests References
designated as perceptual by the criterial variables.7
Anooshian, L. J. (1989). Effects of attentive encoding on analytic
Another way to extend the TAP framework is based on the and nonanalytic processing in implicit and explicit retrieval
overt episodic nature of explicit tests of memory. In tasks. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 5-8.
particular, it is commonly assumed that spatiotemporal Baddeley, A., Lewis, V., Eldridge, M., & Thomson, N. (1984).
context plays a more important role in explicit than in Attention and retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of
implicit tests (e.g., Bower, 1996; Humphreys, Bain, & Pike, Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 518-540.
1989; Humphreys, Wiles, & Dennis, 1994; Jacoby & Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H.
Hollingshead, 1990; Light & LaVoie, 1993; Schwartz et al., Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp.
1993). According to this view, conceptual implicit and 47-89). New York: Academic Press.
explicit tests are both sensitive to prior conceptual- Balota, D. A. (1983). Automatic semantic activation and episodic
memory encoding. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
elaborative processing, but in addition, conceptual explicit Behavior, 22, 88-104.
tests are also sensitive (or are at least more sensitive) to prior Balota, D. A. (1990). The role of meaning in word recognition, hi
encoding of study context (Mulligan, 1997). Likewise, this D. A. Balota, G. B. Flores d'Arcais, & K. Rayner (Eds.),
view states that perceptual implicit and explicit tests may be Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 9-32). Hillsdale, NJ:
sensitive to the same prior perceptual processing, but in Erlbaum.
addition, explicit tests are sensitive to prior encoding of Battig, W. F, & Montague, W. E. (1969). Category norms of verbal
contextual information. Such a refinement parsimoniously items in 56 categories: A replication and extension of the
supplements the conceptual-perceptual processing distinc- Connecticut category norms. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
tion and provides an account for the effects of dividing ogy Monographs, 80(3, Pt. 2).
Bentin, S. (1994). Semantic processing of ignored stimuli: The role
attention. There is good evidence that dividing attention
of attention in memory. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.),
disrupts conceptual and contextual encoding (e.g., Craik, Attention & performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious
1989; Rabinowitz et al., 1982; see Cowan, 1995, for a information processing (pp. 551-569). Cambridge, MA: MIT
review). To the extent that perceptual processes are unaf- Press.
fected by divided attention, this account predicts that Bentin, S., Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1995). Semantic process-
dividing attention will disrupt tests that rely on contextual ing and memory for attended and unattended words in dichotic
encoding (i.e., perceptual and conceptual explicit tests), as listening: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal
well as tests that are sensitive to conceptual processing (i.e., of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfor-
conceptual implicit or explicit tests). mance, 21, 54—67.
Besson, M., Fischler, I., Boaz, T., & Raney, G. (1992). Effects of
A final point concerning this extension of the TAP automatic associative activation on explicit and implicit memory
framework is that it suggests that implicit tests should be tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
relatively unaffected by manipulations of spatiotemporal and Cognition, 18, 89-105.
context. To my knowledge, there are only two published Blaxton, T. A. (1989). Investigating dissociations among memory
studies on this topic, and they produced conflicting results. measures: Support for a transfer-appropriate processing frame-
Jacoby (1983a) reported that a change in environmental work. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
context bad no effect on the implicit test of perceptual and Cognition, 15, 657-668.
Blaxton, T. A. (1992). Dissociations among memory measures in
identification. In contrast, S. M. Smith, Heath, and Vela memory-impaired subjects: Evidence for a processing account of
(1990) found that manipulating environmental context sub- memory. Memory & Cognition, 20, 549-562.
stantially reduced priming in the homophone-spelling task. Blaxton, T. A., Bookheimer, S. Y., Zeffiro, T. A., Figlozzi, C. M.,
Additional research on this topic, including studies of the Gaillard, W. D., & Theodore, W. H. (1996). Functional mapping
influence of environmental context on conceptual implicit of human memory using PET: Comparisons of conceptual and
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 45

perceptual tasks. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, N. D. (1996). The effects of divided attention on encoding and
50, 42-56. retrieval processes in human memory. Journal of Experimental
Bornstein, R. F , Leone, D. R., & Galley, D. J. (1987). The Psychology: General, 125, 159-180.
generalizability of subliminal mere exposure effects: Influences Craik, F. I. M., Moscovitch, M., & McDowd, J. M. (1994).
of stimuli perceived without awareness. Journal of Personality Contributions of surface and conceptual information to perfor-
and Social Psychology, 53, 1070-1079. mance on implicit and explicit tasks. Journal of Experimental
Bower, G. H. (1996). Reactivating a reactivation theory of implicit Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 864—875.
memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 5, 27-72. Denny, E. B., & Hunt, R. R. (1992). Affective valence and memory
Bowers, J. S., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Implicit memory and test in depression: Dissociation of recall and fragment completion.
awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 101, 575-580.
Memory, and Cognition, 16, 404—416. Durante, R., & Hirshman, E. (1994). Retrospective priming and
Broadbent, D. E. (1958), Perception and communication. London: masked semantic priming: The interfering effects of prime
Pergamon Press. activation. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 112-127.
Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and stress. San Diego, CA: Eagle, M., & Ortof, E. (1967). The effect of level of attention upon
Academic Press. "phonetic" recognition errors. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Brown, A. S., & Mitchell, D. B. (1994). A reevaluation of semantic Verbal Behavior, 6, 226-231.
versus nonsemantic processing in implicit memory. Memory & Eich, E. (1984). Memory for unattended events: Remembering
Cognition, 22, 533-541. with and without awareness. Memory & Cognition, 12, 105-111.
Cabeza, R. (1994). A dissociation between two implicit conceptual Elliott, C. L., & Greene, R. L. (1992). Clinical depression and
tests supports the distinction between types of conceptual implicit memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 572-
processing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 505-508. 574.
Carr, T. H. (1992). Automaticity and cognitive anatomy: Is word Engle, R. W., Conway, A. R. A., Tuholski, S. W., & Shisler, R. J.
recognition automatic? American Journal of Psychology, 105, (1995). A resource account of inhibition. Psychological Science,
201-238. 6, 122-125.
Cermak, L. S., Verfaellie, M., & Chase, K. A. (1995). Implicit and Fisk, A. D., & Schneider, W. (1984). Memory as a function of
explicit memory in amnesia: An analysis of data-driven and attention, level of processing, and automatization. Journal of
conceptually driven processes. Neuropsyckology, 9, 281-290. Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
Challis, B. H., & Brodbeck, D. R. (1992). Level of processing 10, 181-197.
affects priming in word fragment completion. Journal of Experi- Gabrieli, J. D. E., Keane, M. H., Stanger, B. Z., Kjelgaard, M. M.,
mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, Corkin, S., & Growdon, J. H. (1994). Dissociations among
595-607. structural-perceptual, lexical-semantic, and event-fact memory
Challis, B. H., Chiu, C. -Y, Ken:, S. A., Law, J., Schneider, L., systems in Alzheimer, amnesic, and normal subjects. Cortex, 30,
Yonelinas, A., & Tulving, E. (1993). Perceptual and conceptual 75-103.
cueing in implicit and explicit retrieval. Memory' A 127-151. Gabrieli, J. D. E., Stone, M. V, Shackleton, K., Thompson-Schill,
Challis, B. H., Velichkovsky, B. M., & Craik, F. I. M. (1996). S. L., Ladd, S. L., Vaidya, C. J., & Chan, A. (1996). Attention
Levels-of-processing effects on a variety of memory tasks: New and implicit memory for words. Manuscript submitted for
findings and theoretical implications. Consciousness and Cogni- publication.
tion, 5, 142-164. Gabrieli, J. D. E., Stone, M. V., Vaidya, C. J., Askari, N., Zabinski,
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of M. F, & Rabin, L. (1996, November). Neuropsychological and
speech with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical behavioral evidence for the role of attention in implicit memory.
Society ofAmerica, 26, 554-559. Paper presented at the 37th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral Society, Chicago.
sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Graf, P., & Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more
Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable. Journal of
selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 553-568.
human information-processing system. Psychological Bulletin, Graf, P., Shimamura, A., & Squire, L. (1985). Priming across
104, 163-191. modalities and priming across category levels: Extending the
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated frame- domain of preserved functions in amnesia. Journal of Experimen-
work. New York: Oxford University Press. tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 386-396.
Craik, F. I. M. (1982). Selective changes in encoding as a function Hamann, S. B. (1990). Level-of-processing effects in conceptually
of reduced processing capacity. In F. Klix, J. Hoffman, & E. van driven implicit tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
der Meer (Eds.), Cognitive research in psychology (pp. 152— Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 970-977.
161). Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag. Hawley, K. J., & Johnston, W. A. (1991). Long-term perceptual
Craik, F I. M. (1983). On the transfer of information from memory for briefly exposed words as a function of awareness
temporary to permanent memory. Philosophical Transactions of and attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
the Royal Society of London, Series B, 302, 341-359. Perception and Performance, 17, 807-815.
Craik, F I. M. (1989). On the making of episodes. In H. L. Hirshman, E., & Durante, R. (1992). Prime identification and
Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn-
consciousness: Essays in honour ofEndel Tulving (pp. 43-57). ing, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 255-265.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Humphreys, M. S., Bain, J. D., & Pike, R. (1989). Different ways to
Craik, F. I. M., & Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive deficits: cue a coherent memory system: A theory for episodic, semantic,
The role of attentional resources. In F. I. M. Craik & S. E. Trehub and procedural tasks. Psychological Review, 96, 208-233.
(Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 191-211). New York: Humphreys, M. S., Wiles, J., & Dennis, S. (1994). Toward a theory
Plenum Press. of human memory: Data structures and access processes.
Craik, F. I. M., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Anderson, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 655-692.
46 MULLIGAN

Isingrini, M., Vazou, E, & Leroy, P. (1995). Dissociation between memory and attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
implicit and explicit memory tests: Effects of age and divided Psychology, 25, 22-40.
attention on category exemplar generation and cued recall. Mandler, G., Nakamura, Y., & Van Zandt, B. J. S. (1987).
Memory & Cognition, 23, 462-467. Nonspecific effects of exposure on stimuli that cannot be
Jacoby, L. L. (1983a). Perceptual enhancement: Persistent effects recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
of an experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn- Memory, and Cognition, 13, 646-648.
ing, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 21-38. McDennott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (1996). Exact and conceptual
Jacoby, L. L. (1983b). Remembering the data: Analyzing interac- repetition dissociate conceptual memory tests: Problems for
tive processes in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal transfer appropriate processing theory. Canadian Journal of
Behavior, 22, 485-508. Experimental Psychology, 50, 57-71.
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). Aprocess dissociation framework: Separating Merikle, P., & Reingold, E. (1991). Comparing direct (explicit) and
automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory indirect (implicit) measures to study unconscious memory.
and Language, 30, 513-541. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Jacoby, L. L., & Hoi lings head, A. (1990). Toward a generate/ Cognition, 17, 224-233.
recognize model of performance on direct and indirect tests of Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues
memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 433-454. and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimen-
Jacoby, L. L., Toth, J. P., & Yonelinas, A. P. (1993). Separating tal Psychology, 11, 56-60.
conscious and unconscious influences of memory: Measuring Moscovitch, M. (1992). Memory and working-with-memory: A
recollection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, component process model based on modules and central sys-
1-16. tems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 257-267.
Jacoby, L. L., Woloshyn, V, & Kelley, C. (1989). Becoming Moscovitch, M., Goshen-Gottstein, Y, & Vriezen, E. (1994).
famous without being recognized: Unconscious influences of Memory without conscious recollection: A tutorial review from a
memory produced by divided attention. Journal of Experimental neuropsychological perspective. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch
Psychology: General, 118, 115-125. (Eds.), Attention and performance (pp. 619-660). Cambridge,
Jelicic, M., Bonke, B., Wolters, G., & Phaf, R. H. (1992). Implicit MA: MIT Press.
memory for words presented during anaesthesia. European Moscovitch, M., Vriezen, E., & Goshen-Gottstein, Y. (1993).
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 71-80. Implicit memory in patients with focal lesions and degenerative
Keane, M. M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Fennema, A. C , Growdon, J. H., brain disorders. In F. Boiler & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of
& Corkin, S. (1991). Evidence for a dissociation between neuropsychology (pp. 133-173). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
perceptual and conceptual priming in Alzheimer's disease. Mulligan, N. W. (1996). The effects of perceptual interference at
Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 326-342. encoding on implicit memory, explicit memory, and memory for
Keane, M. M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Monti, L. A., Cantor, J. M,, & source. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
Noland, J. S. (1993). Amnesic patients show normal priming and and Cognition, 22, 1067-1087.
a normal depth of processing effect in a conceptually driven
Mulligan, N. W. (1997). Attention and implicit memory: The
implicit memory test. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 19,
effects of varying attentional load on conceptual priming.
1079.
Memory & Cognition, 25, 11-17.
Kellogg, R. T., Newcombe, C , Kammer, C , & Schmitt, K. (1996).
Attention and direct and indirect memory tasks with short- and Mulligan, N. W., & Hartman, M. (1996). Divided attention and
long-term probes. American Journal of Psychology, 109, 205- indirect memory tests. Memory <fe Cognition, 24,453-465.
217. Murdock, B. B. (1965). Effects of subsidiary task on short-term
Kirk, R. E. (1995). Experimental design (3rd ed.). New York: memory. British Journal of Psychology, 56, 413-419.
Brooks/Cole. Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical
Koriat, A., & Feuerstein, N. (1976). The recovery of incidentally memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-
acquired information. Acta Psychologica, 40, 463-474. capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Gen-
Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of eral, 106, 226-254.
present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown Univer- Neely, J. H. (1989). Experimental dissociations and the episodic/
sity Press. semantic memory distinction. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik
Light, L. L. (1991). Memory and aging: Four hypotheses in search (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour
of data. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 333-376. ofEndel Tulving (pp. 229-270). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Light, L. L., & Albertson, S. A. (1989). Direct and indirect tests of Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word
memory for category exemplars in young and older adults. recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories.
Psychology and Aging, 4, 487-492. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in
Light, L. L., & LaVoie, D. (1993). Direct and indirect measures of reading—visual word recognition (pp. 264-336). Hillsdale, NJ:
memory in old age. In P. Graf & M. E. J. Masson (Eds.), Implicit Erlbaum.
memory: New directions in cognition, development, and neuro- Nelson, D. L., Schreiber, T. A., & McEvoy, C. L. (1992).
psychology (pp. 207-230). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Processing implicit and explicit representations. Psychological
Light, L. L., & Prull, M. W. (1995a). Aging, divided attention, and Review, 99, 322-348.
repetition priming. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 54, 87-101. Norman, D. A. (1969a). Memory and attention: An introduction to
Light, L. L., & Prull, M. W. (1995b, November). Repetition human information processing. New York: Wiley.
priming, divided attention and aging. Paper presented at the 36th Norman, D. A. (1969b). Memory while shadowing. Quarterly
Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Los Angeles. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21, 85-93.
Logan, G. D. (1979). On the use of a concurrent memory load to Parkin, A. J., Reid, T. K., & Russo, R. (1990). On the differential
measure attention and automaticity. Journal of Experimental nature of implicit and explicit memory. Memory <fe Cognition,
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 189-207. 18,507-514.
MacKay, D. G. (1973). Aspects of the theory of comprehension, Parkin, A. J., & Russo, R. (1990). Implicit and explicit memory and
ATTENTION AND MEMORY 47

the automatic/effortful distinction. European Journal of Cogni- In P. Graf & M. E. J. Masson (Eds.), Implicit memory: New
tive Psychology, 2, 71-80. directions in cognition, development and neuropsychology (pp.
Rabinowitz, J. C , Craik, E I. M., & Ackerman, B. P. (1982). A 265-285). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum.
processing resource account of age differences in recall. Cana- Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect:
dian Journal of Psychology, 36, 325-344. Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
Rappold, V. A., & Hashtroudi, S. (1991). Does organization ogy: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592-604.
improve priming? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn- Smith, M. E., & Oscar-Berman, M. (1990). Repetition priming of
ing, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 103-114. words and pseudowords in divided attention and in amnesia.
Roediger, H. L., HI. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043-1056. Cognition, 16, 1033-1042.
Roediger, H. L., Ill, & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory Smith, S. M., Heath, F. R., & Vela, E. (1990). Environmental
in normal human subjects. In E Boiler & J. Grafman (Eds.), context-dependent homophone spelling. American Journal of
Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 63-131). Amsterdam: Psychology, 103, 229-242.
Elsevier. Stone, M., Ladd, S. L., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1996).
Roediger, H. L., ITI, Weldon, M. S., & Challis, B. H. (1989). Word identification priming for ignored and attended words.
Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit memory: Manuscript submitted for publication.
A processing account. In H. L. Roediger III & E I. M. Craik Szymanski, K. E, & MacLeod, C. M. (1996). Manipulation of
(Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour attention at study affects an explicit but not an implicit test of
ofEndel Tulving (pp. 3-41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 5, 165-175.
Roediger, H. L., m , Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. A., & Riegler, G. H. Thapar, A., & Greene, R. L. (1994). Effects of level of processing
(1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word on implicit and explicit tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
fragment and word stem completion. Journal of Experimental ogy: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 671-679.
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1251-1269. Thriving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and
Russo, R., & Parkin, A. J. (1993). Age differences in implicit retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review,
memory: More apparent than real. Memory & Cognition, 21, 80, 352-373.
73-80. Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Demb, J. B., Keane, M. M., &
Schacter, D. L. (1985). Priming of old and new knowledge in Wetzel, L. C. (in press). Impaired priming on the general
amnesic patients and normal subjects. Annals of the New York knowledge task in amnesia. Neuropsychology.
Academy of Sciences: Vol. 444 (pp. 44—53). New York: New Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Keane, M. M., Monti, L. A.,
York Academy of Sciences. Gutierrez-Rivas, H., & Zarella, M. M. (1996). Evidence for
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current multiple mechanisms of conceptual priming on implicit memory
status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, tests. Manuscript submitted for publication.
and Cognition, 13, 501-518. Vriezen, E., Moscovitch, M., & Bellos, S. A. (1995). Priming
Schacter, D. L., Bowers, J., & Booker, J. (1989). Intention, effects in semantic classification tasks. Journal of Experimental
awareness, and implicit memory: The retrieval intentionality Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 933-946.
criterion. In S. Lewandowsky, J. C. Dunn, & K, Kirsner (Eds.), Weldon, M. S. (1991). Mechanisms underlying priming on percep-
Implicit memory: Theoretical issues (pp. 47-65). Hillsdale, NJ: tual tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Erlbaum, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 526-541.
Schwartz, B. L., Rosse, R. B., & Deutsch, S. I. (1993). Limits of the Weldon, M. S., & Coyote, K. C. (1996). Failure to find the picture
processing view in accounting for dissociations among memory superiority effect in implicit conceptual memory tests. Journal of
measures in a clinical population. Memory & Cognition, 21, Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
63-72. 22, 670-686.
Seamon, J., Marsh, R., & Brody, N. (1984). Critical importance of Weldon, M. S., & Jackson-Barrett, J. L. (1993). Why do pictures
exposure duration for attentive discrimination of stimuli that are produce priming on the word-fragment completion test? A study
not recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, of encoding and retrieval factors. Memory <fe Cognition, 21,
Memory, and Cognition, 10, 465-469. 519-528.
Shallice, T., Fletcher, P., Frith, C D., Grasby, P., Frackowiak, Weldon, M. S., Roediger, H. L., m , Beitel, D. A., & Johnston, T. R.
R. S. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1994, April). Brain regions associated (1995). Perceptual and conceptual processes in implicit and
with acquisition and retrieval of verbal episodic memory. explicit tests with picture fragment and word fragment cues.
Nature, 368, 633-635. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 268-285.
Shapiro, S. I., & Palermo, D. S. (1968). An atlas of normative free Wood, N. L., Stadler, M. A., & Cowan, N. (in press). Is there
association data. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 2, implicit memory without attention? A re-examination of task
219-250. demands in Eich's (1984) procedure. Memory & Cognition.
Shimamura, A. P. (1986). Priming effects in amnesia: Evidence for
a dissociable memory function. Quarterly Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology, 38A, 619-644. Received December 4,1996
Shimamura, A. P. (1993). Neuropsychological analyses of implicit Revision received March 19,1997
memory: History, methodology, and theoretical interpretations. Accepted March 28,1997 •

Anda mungkin juga menyukai