Anda di halaman 1dari 6

Research: Science and Education

edited by
Diane M. Bunce
The Catholic University of America,
Washington, DC 20064

Logical Reasoning Ability and Student


Performance in General Chemistry
Lillian Bird
Department of Chemistry, University of Puerto Rico at Río Piedras, San Juan, PR 00931-3346
lbird@uprrp.edu

Since the 1970s, the identification of predictors of academic to the relation between operational level and student perfor-
performance in science courses has been an object of study mance in the ACS General Chemistry Examination and between
among science education researchers (1-20). During past years, operational level and student approach (algorithmic or con-
student performance in science courses has been analyzed on the ceptual) toward a partial exam question that may be answered
basis of high school experiences (15), precalculus grades (8), correctly using either strategy.
mathematics diagnostics (10), verbal and mathematics compo-
nents of the SAT (5, 15) and ACT (8), mental capacity measured Methodology
as M-Demand through the Figural Intersection Test (19), formal
The population studied comprised 466 students enrolled in
operational reasoning measured by using the Test of Logical
General Chemistry (CHEM 3001-3002) at UPR-RP who
Thinking (19) and the Group Assessment of Logical Thinking
took the GALT test and completed both semesters of the course
(GALT) test (5, 6, 10), and disembedding ability measured during academic year 2006-2007. Of these, 66.3% were female;
through the Group Embedded Figures Test (19), among many 48.9% were first-year students; and 26.6% took the course in the
others. Researchers have used these parameters, individually as Personalized System of Instruction (PSI) format, whereas the
well as combined, to try to find a true predictor of achievement in rest attended traditional lecture sessions. Most students (81.3%)
undergraduate science courses. belonged to the Faculty of Natural Sciences, 17.4% to the College
Advances in the field of cognitive psychology have empha- of Education, and the remaining 6 students were registered in
sized the importance of cognitive skills as key elements for the other colleges. More than 99% of the students were Hispanic,
acquisition of knowledge in introductory science courses (21). almost all Puerto Rican.
These advances have prompted researchers to give more serious The 12-item GALT test was translated into Spanish (with
thought to cognitive factors for predicting student achievement permission from its authors) and administered to students
at the undergraduate level. To better understand our own science during the first laboratory session of the Spring semester of
student population and contribute to this discussion, on March academic year 2006-2007. Students were allowed 45 min to
2007 a Spanish version of the GALT test was administered to complete the 12-item test. Ten of these items had two multiple-
students enrolled in the General Chemistry course at the choice questions for each item: the first to select the proper
University of Puerto Rico in Río Piedras (UPR-RP). This test answer to a particular situation and the other to select the
is a 12-item instrument developed by Roadrangka et al. (22) to rationale behind this answer. To determine internal reliability
measure logical reasoning skills in precollege and college-level and compare student performance in different logical reasoning
students. The test includes questions related to mass and volume skills, individual results for each test item were recorded. All the
conservation, proportional reasoning, correlational reasoning, data sets were saved in Excel (24) and exported to SPSS 15.0 (25)
control of experimental variables, probabilistic reasoning, and to perform statistical analyses.
combinatorial reasoning. The first of these skills (mass/volume All student data were obtained with approval by the
conservation) is typically mastered at the concrete operational Institutional Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects
level, whereas all others correspond to the formal domain. in Research. Student responses to a partial exam question were
Results of this 12-item test are used to determine the operational analyzed by the author by examining the test responses of
level of the responder. GALT results of 0-4 are characteristic of students enrolled in several sections of the course.
concrete thinkers, 5-7 of individuals in a transitional stage, and
8-12 of formal thinkers (16, 22). Studies conducted at institu- Results and Discussion
tions where the English version of this test was administered
show that, in terms of operational level, most introductory Internal Reliability of the GALT Test
college students seem to be at the transitional or formal opera- Internal reliability coefficients convey the degree of coher-
tional stages (4, 5, 16, 23). ence among items in an instrument intended to measure a given
An analysis of our students' ability to operate at a given level construct. For psychometric instruments, internal reliability is
and its relation to academic performance in the General Chem- most commonly determined by calculating Cronbach's R
istry course will be presented in this paper. We also discuss (26, 27). The magnitude of Cronbach's R, however, is not only
performance in the GALT test and in individual reasoning skills dependent on the degree of correlation among items, but also
by gender and by operational level, as well as findings with regards varies with test length, with longer tests yielding higher values

_ _ _
r 2010 American Chemical Society and Division of Chemical Education, inc. pubs.acs.org/jchemeduc Vol. 87 No. 5 May 2010 Journal of Chemical Education 541
10.1021/ed8001754 Published on Web 03/12/2010
Research: Science and Education

than shorter ones. Thus, for short tests, the Spearman-Brown Table 1. Student Performance by Logical Reasoning Mode
prophecy coefficient is frequently calculated, which allows the Logical Reasoning Mode Mean Score Standard Deviation
researcher to predict the internal reliability that would result
from an increase in test length (26). In practical terms, reliability Mass/volume conservation 1.53 0.61
standards for psychometric instruments were set by Nunnally in Proportional reasoning 0.88 0.79
his seminal work on Psychometric Theory (26). According to Experimental variable control 1.36 0.71
Nunnally, a reliability value of 0.70 is sufficient for preliminary
Probabilistic reasoning 0.97 0.90
work on predictor tests, whereas for basic research on psycho-
metric instruments, reliability values need not exceed 0.80 (26). Correlational reasoning 0.71 0.68
For the Spanish version of the GALT test administered to Combinatorial reasoning 1.58 0.53
students at UPR-RP, Cronbach's R yielded a value of 0.69, and
the Spearman-Brown prophecy value calculated for a 21-item instrument) by McKinnon and Renner (1), who describe the
test was 0.79. These values are in the same range as those obtained distribution of their college students' levels of operation as 50%
by Bunce and Hutchinson (5), which were 0.62 and 0.74, concrete, 25% postconcrete, and 25% formal.
respectively, using the English version of the test with college-
level students. They are lower, however, than the R value of 0.85 Gender and Logical Reasoning Ability
reported by Roadrangka et al. (28), as well as by Bitner (29), all of A t-test for independent samples was performed to compare
whom worked with precollege students. GALT test scores among students of different genders. A
To compare the internal reliability of the test administered significant effect for gender was observed, t (464) = 5.09, p <
at UPR-RP to that obtained by Niaz and Robinson (4), the 0.001 (two-tailed), with male students obtaining higher scores.
Guttman split-half reliability coefficient was also determined. Similar findings regarding gender differences in test scores, not
This reliability coefficient indicates the degree of correlation necessarily using GALT, have also been reported by McKinnon
between the two halves of a test and is an alternate measure of and Renner (1), and Shibley et al. (14). A significant effect for
internal consistency (26). It should be noted that, for the GALT gender was observed with respect to operational level, χ2 (2, N =
test, the particular split-half item division is important because 466) = 20.16, p < 0.001, with male students being more likely to
the first two items address mass/volume conservation, the next be at a formal operations stage.
two deal with proportions, items 5 and 6 are based on control of
experimental variables, and so on, making the first half of the test GALT Scores and Other Parameters
different from the second half, yet the odd half equivalent to the No significant difference (p > 0.05) was found for the
even half. Taking this into account, a Guttman reliability GALT scores among students in the traditional lecture versus
coefficient of 0.74 was obtained by comparing odd-numbered PSI sections of the course. First-year students, however, did
items with their even-numbered counterparts. This value is better than upperclassmen, t (464) = 6.15, p < 0.001 (two-tailed).
higher than that obtained by Niaz et al. (4) for the same test, This seemingly paradoxical finding as, according to Piaget's levels
which was 0.63. of development, logical reasoning ability should increase with age
(12), may be explained by the fact that only those students with
Student Distribution by Operational Level both high College Board scores and high school grade point
On the basis of GALT test results, the operational level of averages were allowed to take the general chemistry course during
students enrolled in General Chemistry at UPR-RP during their first year, thus raising the stakes for this cohort. A
academic year 2006-2007 was determined. Students with scores significant difference in GALT test scores, t (458) = 6.71, p <
in the range of 0-4 were considered to be in the concrete 0.001, was also found between students belonging to the Faculty
operational level, 5-7 in the transitional stage, and 8-12 in the of Natural Sciences and those enrolled in the Faculty of Educa-
formal operational level (16, 22). On the basis of these ranges, tion. This finding may be explained on the basis of differences in
19% of the students were operating at a concrete level, 40% at a admission criteria for both faculties, the Faculty of Natural
transitional stage, and 41% had reached the formal operational Sciences having more rigorous entrance requirements.
level. Because the GALT test was administered at the beginning
of the second semester, these percentages do not include students Student Performance by Logical Reasoning Mode
who failed or withdrew from the course during the first semester Student scores in each of the six logical reasoning modes
of the course. It would be reasonable to speculate that this were analyzed to determine whether students performed at the
excluded cohort could further increase the percent of concrete same level in each of these modes or whether differential
and transitional operators, at the expense of that of formal performance would be observed. Results in Table 1 indicate
thinkers. In any case, the fact that at least 59% of all students that, of a maximum score of 2.00, students performed signifi-
enrolled in this course fall below the formal operational level is cantly better in items related to combinatorial reasoning, mass/
quite troubling, because mastery of most topics covered in this volume conservation, and control of experimental variables than
and subsequent chemistry courses requires formal reasoning in those related to probability, proportional reasoning, and
skills. correlation.
The above findings are comparable to those obtained by These results may be compared to those obtained by Bitner
McConnell et al. (16) for students in introductory geoscience (29) using the same test. Bitner found that students in grades
courses, which were found to be 24% concrete, 33% transitional, 9-12 performed significantly better in items related to mass/
and 43% abstract thinkers on the basis of the same test and score volume conservation (M = 1.42, SD = 0.68), control of experi-
ranges. They differ, however, from those obtained (using another mental variables (M = 0.91, SD = 0.76), and combinatorial

_ _ _
542 Journal of Chemical Education Vol. 87 No. 5 May 2010 pubs.acs.org/jchemeduc r 2010 American Chemical Society and Division of Chemical Education, inc.
Research: Science and Education

Table 2. Student Performance in Varied Logical Reasoning Modes by Gender


Mean Score (and SD) by Gender

Logical Reasoning Mode Male Female t-Test Values Significance Level (Two-Tailed)

Mass/volume conservation 1.63 (0.58) 1.47 (0.62) 2.68 p < 0.01


Proportional reasoning 1.15 (0.76) 0.74 (0.76) 5.51 p < 0.001
Experimental variable control 1.33 (0.71) 1.37 (0.70) -0.69 p > 0.05
Probabilistic reasoning 1.26 (0.85) 0.83 (0.88) 5.10 p < 0.001
Correlational reasoning 0.82 (0.72) 0.66 (0.66) 2.37 p < 0.05
Combinatorial reasoning 1.66 (0.51) 1.54 (0.54) 2.39 p < 0.05

Table 3. Student Performance in Varied Logical Reasoning Modes by Operational Level


Mean Score (and SD) by Operational Level

Logical Reasoning Mode Concrete Transitional Formal F Values Significance Level

Mass/volume conservation 0.97 (0.56) 1.49 (0.61) 1.82 (0.40) 79.93 p < 0.001
Proportional reasoning 0.16 (0.37) 0.61 (0.62) 1.46 (0.66) 171.76 p < 0.001
Experimental variable control 0.78 (0.69) 1.26 (0.68) 1.72 (0.50) 74.39 p < 0.001
Probabilistic reasoning 0.20 (0.51) 0.60 (0.78) 1.68 (0.58) 203.09 p < 0.001
Correlational reasoning 0.20 (0.41) 0.55 (0.59) 1.10 (0.66) 80.83 p < 0.001
Combinatorial reasoning 1.14 (0.51) 1.53 (0.53) 1.84 (0.37) 70.51 p < 0.001

reasoning (M = 0.75, SD = 0.75); than in those related to


proportional reasoning (M = 0.63, SD = 0.73), probabilistic
reasoning (M = 0.53, SD = 0.86), and correlational reasoning
(M = 0.19, SD = 0.42). Although not in the same order as our
findings, her results corroborate the students' difficulty with
respect to correlational reasoning and, to a lesser extent, with
probabilistic and proportional reasoning.

Student Performance in Logical Reasoning Modes by


Gender
Results of the t-tests calculated to compare mean scores in
each of the modes by gender are summarized in Table 2. Worth
mentioning are the findings that male students performed
markedly better in proportional and probabilistic reasoning than
female students (Mmale = 1.15:Mfemale = 0.74, and Mmale = 1.26:
Mfemale = 0.83, respectively); and that both male and female Figure 1. Final grade distribution in General Chemistry II by operational
students performed poorly when asked to identify a possible level (n = 400).
correlation between two variables (Mmale = 0.82:Mfemale = 0.66).
No statistically significant gender difference (p > 0.05) was found is the hardest to attain, even for students operating at the formal
with respect to experimental variable control. level.

Student Performance in Logical Reasoning Modes by Operational Level and Performance in the General Chemi-
Operational Level stry Course
Results of the ANOVA test used to compare means for each Final grades in General Chemistry I and II for students of
logical reasoning mode among students of different operational different operational level were analyzed. Results indicate that
level are summarized in Table 3. A posthoc comparison test using the final grades differed significantly by operational level, both
the Bonferroni correction confirmed that differences in mean for the first semester of the course, χ2(8, N = 466) = 52.89, p <
score by operational level were significant at the p < .05 level. 0.001, as well as for the second semester, χ2(8, N = 466) = 52.48,
On the basis of differences in relative mean score, these p < 0.001. Figure 1 shows the grade distribution by operational
results suggest that proportional, probabilistic, and correlational level for students who passed General Chemistry II. A similar
reasoning skills are the domain of the formal operational stage; graph was obtained for the first semester of the course. Inspection
whereas skills in mass/volume conservation, control of experi- reveals that, in terms of final grade, the mode for students
mental variables, and combinatorial reasoning may be mastered operating at a formal level is a grade of A, for those at a
by students operating at the transitional or concrete levels. As transitional level the mode is a grade of B, and for students at
mentioned above, it is evident that correlational reasoning ability a concrete level the mode is a grade of C.

_ _ _
r 2010 American Chemical Society and Division of Chemical Education, inc. pubs.acs.org/jchemeduc Vol. 87 No. 5 May 2010 Journal of Chemical Education 543
Research: Science and Education

Table 4. Correlation Coefficients for Each Logical Reasoning Mode as a Predictor of Student Performance in General Chemistry
General Chemistry I (N = 466, df = 464) General Chemistry II (N = 466, df = 464)

Logical Reasoning Mode R Values Significance Level (Two-Tailed) R Values Significance Level (Two-Tailed)

Mass/volume conservation 0.125 p < 0.005 0.130 p < 0.005


Proportional reasoning 0.211 p < 0.001 0.172 p < 0.001
Experimental variable control 0.213 p < 0.001 0.199 p < 0.001
Probabilistic reasoning 0.301 p < 0.001 0.288 p < 0.001
Correlational reasoning 0.164 p < 0.001 0.168 p < 0.001
Combinatorial reasoning 0.169 p < 0.001 0.160 p < 0.001

GALT Results and Student Performance in the General


Chemistry Course
A moderate correlation, a Pearson's R of 0.338 (df = 464;
p < 0.001, two-tailed) for General Chemistry I and of 0.318 (df =
464; p < 0.001, two-tailed) for General Chemistry II, was found
between GALT test results and the students' final grades in both
semesters of the course.

Logical Reasoning Modes and Student Performance in the


General Chemistry Course Figure 2. Example partial exam question that students could answer
correctly by using either algorithmic problem solving or conceptual
As mentioned above, most students in the sample show less reasoning.
proficiency in correlational, proportional, and probabilistic
reasoning than in other logical reasoning modes. This, however, Table 5. Distribution of Students' Operational Level and Reasoning
does not necessarily imply that these three modes are the best Approach toward a Partial Exam Question
predictors of performance in the course or that other modes are Operational Level
less important. A multiple regression was performed to find out Total
to what extent each of the six logical reasoning modes is able to Approach Concrete Transitional Formal (N = 106)
predict student achievement in the course. The results obtained
Algorithmic 10 13 10 33
indicate that probabilistic reasoning is the best independent
Algorithmic-Conceptual 11 21 13 45
predictor (of the six reasoning modes) of student performance in
both semesters of the course. Table 4 summarizes the correlation Conceptual 2 8 18 28
data for all modes.
Further studies are being conducted to compare these results students showed proficiency in the use of algorithms and low-
with those for other introductory science courses taken by the order cognitive skills, a significantly lower number could answer
same student population. However, in view of the fact that conceptual questions. More recently, using Nurrenbern and
Bitner's (29) as well as this author's findings suggest that some Pickering's questions (30), Cracolice et al. (36) correlated reason-
logical reasoning skills are acquired after others have been ing ability (students who were in the top one-third of reasoners vs
mastered, this finding should not be used to discard any mode those in the bottom one-third of reasoners on the basis of the
on the basis of its significance for success in the course. Classroom Test of Scientific Reasoning) with success in algo-
rithmic versus conceptual questions, and concluded (36)
Operational Level and Problem-Solving Ability [A] significant fraction of our students have no choice other
Algorithmic versus conceptual problem-solving ability of than to be algorithmic problem solvers because their reason-
general chemistry students has been the subject of many studies ing skills are not sufficiently developed to allow them to
in past years (30-35). Nurrenbern and Pickering (30) compared successfully solve conceptual problems.
student responses to algorithmic and conceptual questions, and To determine whether a relationship exists between opera-
found that many students who correctly answered algorithmic tional level and problem-solving ability, the students' approach to a
problems did not understand the chemical concepts behind these partial exam question that could be answered correctly either
problems. Lythcott (31) suggested that differences in cognitive by algorithmic problem solving or by conceptual reasoning was
development, among other factors, might explain these findings. analyzed by inspecting their responses to the problem in Figure 2.
On the basis of the work of Tobias (32) and using questions As is evident, all data required to calculate the pressure
similar to those of Nurrenbern and Pickering (30), Nakhleh (33, exerted by each gas is provided. However, given the fact that the
34) categorized students as high (H) or low (L) algorithmic (A) temperature, volume, and mass are exactly the same for the four
and conceptual (C) problem solvers, and encountered very few gases, no calculations are necessary. The gas in the flask
LA-HC students (only 5%). Through interviews she also containing more moles (i.e., that of lower molar mass) will exert
discovered that most students who had correctly responded to more pressure.
the conceptual questions had solved them by applying algorith- Of a total of 106 students whose responses were analyzed
mic strategies (34). Zoller et al. (35) found that, whereas most and who answered the question correctly, 31% calculated the

_ _ _
544 Journal of Chemical Education Vol. 87 No. 5 May 2010 pubs.acs.org/jchemeduc r 2010 American Chemical Society and Division of Chemical Education, inc.
Research: Science and Education

Table 6. Student Performance on the ACS General Chemistry Examination by Operational Level
Mean Score (and SD) by Operational Level

Test Concrete Transitional Formal F Value Significance Level

ACS General Chemistry Examination 27.60 (6.35) 31.35 (7.44) 34.50 (8.93) 17.99 p < 0.01

pressure for each gas and justified their answer in terms of their 2. Logical reasoning ability (as measured using the GALT test) is a
results (algorithmic approach); 42% did all the calculations, valid predictor of student performance in both semesters of the
analyzed the results and proceeded to give the correct answer course.
in terms of logical reasoning (more moles of gas, thus higher 3. Of the six logical reasoning modes, most students show marked
pressure), which represents the mixed algorithmic-conceptual deficiencies in proportional, probabilistic, and correlational
approach; and 26% gave the correct answer exclusively in terms reasoning.
of logical reasoning (more moles of gas, thus higher pressure) 4. Among the six logical reasoning modes, probabilistic reasoning
without doing any calculations (conceptual approach). It should is the single best predictor of student performance in general
be noted that the course instructors did not expect students to chemistry.
use the algorithmic approach to answer this question to the 5. Student approach toward a chemistry exam question that may
extent that very little space was provided for students to write be answered correctly using either an algorithmic or a con-
their answer. In the case of algorithmic-conceptual answers, the ceptual pathway varies significantly with the student's opera-
lack of space made it easier to determine which had come first, tional level, with formal thinkers having a stronger tendency to
the algorithmic calculations or the logical reasoning component apply the conceptual approach.
of the answer. 6. Students at a formal operational stage perform significantly
The results obtained, which are summarized in Table 5, better in the ACS General Chemistry Examination than
indicate that student approach toward the question differed students operating at lower levels.
significantly by operational level, with students at a formal
stage being more likely to choose a conceptual approach than
All of these findings imply that logical reasoning skills are
students at the transitional or concrete operational levels. Values
essential for student mastery of many of the concepts and more
obtained for the χ2 test were as follows: χ2 (4, N = 106) =
complex problem solving strategies required to succeed in general
11.90, p < 0.05.
chemistry. The fact that most students taking this course have
It should be noted that 33 out of 78 students (42%) who
not reached the formal stage means that, although they may
initially answered the question in an algorithmic fashion never
master the algorithmic component and some basic concepts of
provided the expected conceptual answer. All these findings
the course, many will not be able to interpret their results on the
support the conclusion of Cracolice et al. (36) with regard to the
poor development of reasoning skills as a determining factor for basis of chemical behavior, particularly at the molecular level.
What should we do with these students? There's much work
the students' inability to solve conceptual problems in chemistry,
to be done. But before placing stronger emphasis on aspects of
and are in agreement with Zoller et al.'s conclusion that “success
chemistry that we know most students cannot handle, perhaps
on algorithmic questions on exams does not imply success on
we should begin by facilitating student development of logical
conceptual questions” (35).
reasoning skills through cognitive enrichment experiences prior
to their enrollment in the course.
Operational Level and Performance in the ACS General
Chemistry Examination
Acknowledgment
To determine a potential relation between a student's logical
reasoning ability and performance in a national chemistry exam, The author expresses her gratitude to Vantipa Roadrangka
the mean scores for students of different operational level in the and Michael J. Padilla for granting her permission to translate
ACS General Chemistry Examination were compared. The and administer the GALT test in Spanish at UPR-RP; NIH-
results of the ANOVA test are summarized in Table 6. A PreMARC Grant 5T34GM07821 for partial funding of this
posthoc multiple comparisons test using the Bonferroni adjust- project; Statistics Professor Pedro Rodríguez-Esquerdo for his
ment confirmed that differences in mean score were significant helpful recommendations; all UPR-RP professors and students
at the p < 0.01 level. who collaborated in this study; and to the reviewers for their
These results indicate that there is a statistically significant useful comments.
difference in mean score among students of different operational
level, and once again suggest that the attainment of logical Literature Cited
reasoning skills is an essential element for mastery of general
chemistry concepts and problem-solving skills. 1. McKinnon, J. W.; Renner, J. Am. J. Phys. 1971, 39, 1047–1052.
2. Herron, J. D. J. Chem. Educ. 1975, 52, 146–150.
Conclusions 3. Williams, H.; Turner, C. W.; Debreuil, L.; Fast, J.; Berestiansky, J.
J. Chem. Educ. 1979, 56, 599–600.
On the basis of the aforementioned results, we may con- 4. Niaz, M.; Robinson, W. R. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1992, 29, 211–
clude that, for the student population assessed: 226.
1. Most students enrolled in general chemistry (59%) have not 5. Bunce, D. M.; Hutchinson, K. D. J. Chem. Educ. 1993, 70, 183–
reached the formal operational stage. 187.

_ _ _
r 2010 American Chemical Society and Division of Chemical Education, inc. pubs.acs.org/jchemeduc Vol. 87 No. 5 May 2010 Journal of Chemical Education 545
Research: Science and Education

6. Baird, W. E.; Shaw, E. L., Jr.; McLarty, P. Sch. Sci. Math. 1996, 96 Science Teachers Association, Gabel D. L. , Ed.; MacMillan Library
(2), 85–93. Reference: New York, 1994; Chapter 4, 131-176.
7. Norman, O. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1997, 34, 1067–1081. 22. Roadrangka, V.; Yeany, R. H.; Padilla, M. J. Group Assessment
8. Ryder, R. M.; Pang, Y. The Proceedings of ISECON 2000, of Logical Thinking Test; University of Georgia: Athens, GA,
Philadelphia, PA, November 9-12, 2000, v. 17, abstract 1982.
917. 23. Bunce, D. M.; VanderPlas, J. R. Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. 2006, 7,
9. Anderson, J. R.; Reder, L. M.; Simon, H. A. Applications and 160–169.
Misapplications of Cognitive Psychology to Mathematics Edu- 24. Microsoft Office Excel for Windows XP, version 2003; Microsoft
cation. Texas Educational Review 2000, Summer; http://act-r. Corporation, 2003.
psy.cmu.edu/papers/misapplied.html (accessed Feb 2010). 25. SPSS Base, version 15.0 for Windows XP; SPSS, Inc.: Chicago,
10. Nicoll, G.; Francisco, J. S. J. Chem. Educ. 2001, 78, 99–102. IL, 2006.
11. Samarapungavan, A.; Robinson, W. R. J. Chem. Educ. 2001, 78, 26. Nunnally, J. C. Psychometric Theory, 2nd edition; McGraw-Hill:
1107. New York, 1978; Chapter 7.
12. Bunce, D. M. J. Chem. Educ. 2001, 78, 1107. 27. Cronbach, L. J. Psychometrika 1951, 16, 297–334.
13. Legg, M. J.; Legg, J. C.; Greenbowe, T. J. J. Chem. Educ. 2001, 78, 28. Roadrangka, V. The Construction and Validation of the Group
1117–1121. Assessment of Logical Thinking (GALT) Test. Ph.D. Dissertation,
14. Shibley, I. A., Jr.; Milakofsky, L.; Bender, D. S.; Patterson, H. O. University of Georgia, Athens, GA, 1986; Dissertation Abstracts
J. Chem. Educ. 2003, 80, 569–573. International 46 (9), 2650.
15. Tai, R. H.; Sadler, P. M.; Loehr, J. F. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2005, 42, 29. Bitner, B. L. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1991, 28, 265–274.
987–1012. 30. Nurrenbern, S. C.; Pickering, M. J. Chem. Educ. 1987, 64, 508–
16. McConnell, D. A.; Steer, D. N.; Owens, K. D.; Knight, C. C. 510.
J. Geosci. Educ. 2005, 53, 462–470. 31. Lythcott, J. J. Chem. Educ. 1990, 67, 248–252.
17. Tsaparlis, G. Res. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2005, 23, 125–148. 32. Tobias, S. They're Not Dumb, They're Different: Stalking the Second
18. Yaman, S. J. Turk. Sci. Educ. 2005, 2, 31–33. Tier; Research Corporation: Tucson, AZ, 1990.
19. Lewis, S. E.; Lewis, J. E. Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. 2007, 8, 33. Nakhleh, M. B. J. Chem. Educ. 1993, 70, 52–55.
32–51. 34. Nakhleh, M. B.; Mitchell, R. C. J. Chem. Educ. 1993, 70, 190–192.
20. Hurst, M. O.; Howard, R. Chem. Educ. 2008, 13, 42–45. 35. Zoller, U.; Lubezky, A.; Nakhleh, M. B.; Tessier, B.; Dori, Y. J.
21. Lawson, A. E. Research on the Acquisition of Science Knowledge: J. Chem. Educ. 1995, 72, 987–989.
Epistemological Foundations of Cognition. In Handbook of Re- 36. Cracolice, M. S.; Deming, J. C.; Ehlert, B. J. Chem. Educ. 2008, 85,
search on Science Teaching and Learning: A Project of the National 873–878.

_ _ _
546 Journal of Chemical Education Vol. 87 No. 5 May 2010 pubs.acs.org/jchemeduc r 2010 American Chemical Society and Division of Chemical Education, inc.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai