Anda di halaman 1dari 13

International Journal of

Research in
;II
ELSEVIER Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101
Marketing

Source effects in purchase decisions: The impact of physical


attractiveness and accent of salesperson
Oscar W. DeShields J r . a.*, A l i K a r a b, E r d e n e r Kaynak c
a Department of Marketing, School of Business Administration and Economics, California State UnitJersity, Northridge, 18111
Nordhoff Street, Northridge, CA 91330-8245, USA
b College of BusinessAdministration, Pennsylvania State University at York, York, PA 17403, USA
c School of Business Administration, Pennsylvania State University at Harrisburg, Middletown, PA 17057, USA
Received 15 November 1994; accepted 15 September 1995

Abstract

A study was conducted to examine the impact of salesperson physical attractiveness and accent on receiver
purchase intentions. Twenty-four broadcast commercials were produced by manipulating attractiveness and accent
factors. Participating in the study were 963 subjects who evaluated the product/service being advertised. Consistent
with the argument that if receivers behave according to the physical attractiveness stereotype, they should perceive
attractive salespersons more favorably and respond to their sales pitches more readily than they do to unattractive
salespersons, purchase intentions were more favorably influenced by attractive salespersons' message. Also, study
findings indicate that salespersons with a standard accent or dialect were perceived more favorably and created
higher purchase intentions than foreign-accented salespersons. Implications for advertising and personal selling are
discussed.

Keywords: Salesperson accent; Physical attractiveness; Categorization theory; Personal selling; Communication
effectiveness

I. Introduction receiver perception of the sender's physical ap-


pearance. Also, several studies have reported that
Marketing and social psychology literature re-
sender accent is an additional factor or moderat-
peatedly note that receiver perception of the
ing variable influencing receiver message ap-
sender influences message effectiveness (Alpert
proval (Bradac and Wisegarver, 1984; Foon, 1986;
and Anderson, 1973; Reingen and Kernan, 1994).
Tsalikis et al., 1991).
Kelman (1961) argued that sender attractiveness
However, findings and conclusions of the dif-
is an important dimension influencing receiver
ferent researchers mentioned above are inconsis-
approval of a message. Empirical studies demon-
tent. Reingen and Kernan (1994) reported that
strate a 'what is beautiful is good' halo effect in
more favorable selling skills are attributed to
highly attractive salespersons; buyers treat osten-
* Tel.: (818) 885-2458; fax: (818) 885-4903. sibly attractive sellers more cordially, and attrac-

0167-8116/96/$15.00 © 1996 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved


SSDI 0167-81 16(95)00036-4
90 o.w. DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101

tive persons get more donations when soliciting provides some explanations to receiver evaluation
for charitable organizations. In addition, attrac- of the sender. Hence, the objective of this study is
tive people are better liked and are assumed to to investigate the relationship between
be more sociable, independent, and exciting spokesperson attractiveness and accent and con-
(Bringham, 1980; Patzer, 1983; Patzer, 1985), sumer purchase intentions, and therefore to un-
while less attractive people are assumed to be derstand the usefulness of decoding theory in
deviant and are often stigmatized (Jones et al., explaining and predicting consumer purchase in-
1978; Unger et al., 1982). Wilson and Sherrell tentions.
(1993), on the other hand, reported that respon-
dent attitude change is not related to the attrac-
tiveness of the speaker. 2. Spokesperson dimensions and consumer deci-
Tsalikis et al. (1991) reported that an Ameri- sion making
can salesperson with a standard American-En-
glish accent has an advantage over a Greek- Persuasion research has focused on identifying
American salesperson with a Greek-English ac- those attributes of a spokesperson affecting the
cent when addressing an English-speaking audi- impact of persuasive communication. Two of the
ence. They also showed that there was no signifi- factors cited in current literature are: (1)
cant interaction between respondent exposure to spokesperson attractiveness (Alpert and Anderson,
an accent and the respondent evaluation of the 1973; Baker and Churchill, 1977; Joseph, 1982;
accent. Tsalikis et al. (1992) found that a sales Swartz, 1984; Eagly et al., 1991), and (2)
pitch presented in standard accent evokes more spokesperson accent (Bradac and Wisegarver,
favorable judgements on all measured dimensions 1984; Foon, 1986; Tsalikis et al., 1991).
than the same sales pitch presented in foreign
accent. 2.1. Spokesperson attractiveness
Tajfel's (Tajfel, 1981) categorization theory can
be utilized to shed light on this topic. Tajfel's Abundant research evidence attests to the pos-
(1981) social categorization, social identity, and itive expected marketing consequences of em-
social comparison theory is a category-based ploying attractive versus unattractive spokesper-
model explaining how the receiver decodes or sons and models (Baker and Churchill, 1977;
evaluates the spokesperson. According to the the- Joseph, 1982; Courtney and Whipple, 1983; Reid
ory, the receiver first evaluates the spokesperson and Soley,'1983; Caballero and Pride, 1984; Kahle
along the dimensions of attractiveness and ac- and Homer, 1985; McGuire, 1985; Belch et al.,
cent. Next, the receiver identifies himself or her- 1987; Kamins, 1990). Spokesperson attractive-
self with the spokesperson along those catego- ness, a general concept, encompasses three fea-
rized dimensions. Finally, these categories form tures: similarity, familiarity, and liking (Swartz,
the basis for a comparison with the spokesperson. 1984). Persuasion through an identification pro-
However, criteria for comparison are based upon cess is based on the receiver finding the source
the standards of the dominant group, such as attractive in terms of intellectual skills, personal-
white Anglo-Saxon males in the United States. ity properties, lifestyle characteristics, etc. Attrac-
Also, a positive identification with the spokesper- tive people are better liked and are assumed to
son, based on the standards of the dominant be more sociable, independent, and exciting, while
group, results in the receiver being positively in- less attractive people are assumed to be deviant.
fluenced by the spokesperson. Hence, advertisers Joseph's (Joseph, 1982) review of the experi-
who use presenters with a positive receiver iden- mental findings from advertising and related dis-
tity, coupled with attractiveness and accent di- ciplines on the impact of physically attractive
mensions, can expect a positive influence on spokespersons and models on opinion change,
members of the target market. product evaluation, source perception, and recall
It is posited in this study that decoding theory shows that attractive - as compared to unattrac-
O. W. DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101 91

tive - spokespersons are consistently liked more, characteristics theory (SCT) to explain the effects
are viewed in more favorable terms, and have a of physical attractiveness on psychological well-
positive effect on the products with which they being and achievement. Berger et al. (1977) de-
are associated. Chaiken (1986) argued that a mes- fined a status characteristic as any characteristic
sage p r e s e n t e d by a physically attractive that has differentially evaluated states that are
spokesperson will be more persuasive than a simi- associated directly or indirectly with expectation
lar message presented by a less physically attrac- states (p. 35). A status characteristic is a distin-
tive spokesperson. guishing feature that can be used to classify the
Snyder and Rothbart (1971) reported that individual; for example, a positive or negative
source attractiveness is related positively to category such as attractive or unattractive. Conse-
agreement, although the impact seems less con- quently, SCT and Berger and colleagues' (1977)
sistent when the spokesperson is female. Further- definition and use of a status characteristic fits
more, they note that there are no differences the criteria of a 'category' that individuals use to
between attractive and unattractive spokesperson differentiate and evaluate others.
on perceived honesty, competence, dominance, Webster and Driskell's (1983) review indicates
personal effectiveness and personal success. that attractiveness meets the criteria for a dif-
Baker and Churchill (1977) noted that the impact fused status characteristic in the sense that a
of physical attractiveness may be restricted by the generalized state of attractiveness may be associ-
type of product or topic, the sex of the receiver, ated with the ability to perform certain tasks
and the sex of the spokesperson. Kamins (1990) better. According to this line of thinking, a group
used social adaptation theory to explain that the of people consisting of white attractive males in
influence of a physically attractive celebrity is the United States may be connected with greater
moderated by the relationship between the perceived ability and skills (Webster and Driskell,
celebrity's image and the product's image. If the 1983; Eagly and Wood, 1985). As a result, the
product can be viewed as contributing to or com- status characteristic of white attractive male be-
patible with the celebrity's image, then the comes a criterion to categorize individuals.
celebrity as spokesperson can significantly en-
hance the credibility of the advertisement for that 2.2. Spokesperson accent
particular product. Landy and Sigall (1974) con-
cluded that a model's physical attractiveness Tsalikis et al. (1991) noted that the role of
would not be important if the product being accent in spokesperson credibility has been over-
promoted already had a strong brand image or looked in the current marketing literature, al-
clearly observable benefits. However, if the prod- though this area has received much attention in
uct did not contain these advantages, various the social psychology and linguistics literature. A
irrelevant cues, including a model's physical at- number of researchers have posited that speech
tractiveness, would significantly affect customer style, including accent, appears to have a power-
response to the product and the advertisement. ful impact on the ratings of respondents over a
Joseph (1977) reported that when a female broad range of characteristics attributed to the
spokesperson is an expert, her physical attractive- presenter (Lambert, 1967; Giles and Sassoon,
ness has little effect on subject preferences; how- 1983; Bradac and Wisegarver, 1984).
ever, when she is not an expert, subjects agree Edwards (1982) reported that a variety of in-
more with a highly attractive spokesperson than vestigations in English-speaking countries (e.g.,
with a medium or low attractive spokesperson. In the United States, Canada, Britain, and Aus-
other words, when objective or task-related char- tralia) found that presenters with a standard ac-
acteristics are weak, subjects resort to irrelevant cent or dialect are perceived to exhibit more
cues, such as physical attractiveness to form opin- traits of (a) competence as indicated by intelli-
ions. gence, confidence, ambition, and industriousness,
Umberson and Hughes (1987) used status and (b) status/prestige as indicated by profes-
92 O. IV.. DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. o f Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101

sionalism. However, presenters with nonstandard based on the standards of the dominant group,
accents are perceived as having more traits of (a) results in the receiver being positively influenced
personal integrity as indicated by sincerity, relia- by the spokesperson. Hence, using Tajfel's decod-
bility, and generosity, and (b) social attractiveness ing theory, it is hypothesized that an American
as indicated by friendliness and warmth (Lam- audience's purchase intentions would be more
bert, 1967; Powesland and Giles, 1975). Berechree favorably influenced by an American-English-
and Ball (1979) noted in an Australian investiga- accented spokesperson than by a Spanish-En-
tion that greater competence and social attrac- glish- accented spokesperson. Also, theory and
tiveness ratings are displayed for the Cultivated literature state that an attractive spokesperson
Australian as opposed to the two other dialect will have a more positive impact on consumer
varieties - Broad and General. Also, Carranza purchase intentions. Consequently, Tajfel would
and Ryan (1975) reported comparable results in view attractiveness as one of the categories or
an investigation of responses to Spanish and En- dimensions that consumers could use to evaluate
glish presenters, where English is viewed more the spokesperson, and a physically attractive
favorably than Spanish on the dimensions of in- source represents a cue to process the message
tegrity, attractiveness, and status. Additional sup- systematically. Therefore, the following two hy-
port for these findings is noted in an investigation potheses are proposed:
of speech patterns in a Canadian setting by Ed-
wards and Jacobsen (1987), and in indigenous 141: A n attractive salesperson will have a greater
versus foreign-accented presenters (Ryan et al., positive impact on purchase intentions than an
1977; Callan et al., 1983). Garcia (1984) explained unattractive salesperson.
how individuals categorize persons by speech
characteristics (e.g., accent, speech style). Using H2: A n American-English-accented salesperson will
Tajfel's (Tajfel, 1972) theory of social categoriza- have a greater positive impact on purchase inten-
tion and concepts of intergroup relations and tions than a Spanish-accented salesperson.
psychological distinctiveness (Tajfel, 1974), Gar-
cia (1984) obtained evidence that the speech Using the same theoretical framework, it can
characteristics of a person play a fundamental be further hypothesized that a salesperson speak-
role in assessing, among other things, race, age, ing English with a Cuban accent will have a
social class, profession, and way of dressing. greater positive impact on purchase intentions of
consumers in the City of Miami, where this study
was conducted, than a salesperson speaking En-
3. Research hypotheses glish with a Nicaraguan accent, because of the
City's socio-cultural composition. Since a signifi-
To provide a better understanding of the influ- cant proportion of the residents have a Cuban
ence of spokesperson attractiveness and accent, background (dominant ethnic culture) and are
Tajfel's decoding theory is used in this study as a more exposed to English with a Cuban accent,
conceptual framework. According to the theory, English with other Spanish accents will be evalu-
the receiver first evaluates the spokesperson along ated as less effective. Hence, it is hypothesized
the dimensions of attractiveness and accent. Next, that
the receiver identifies himself or herself with the
spokesperson along those categorized dimen- H3: A Cuban-English-accented salesperson will
sions. Finally, these categories form the basis for have a greater positive impact on purchase inten-
a comparison with the spokesperson. The criteria tions than a Nicaraguan-English-accented salesper-
for the comparison are based upon the standards son.
of the dominant group, for example, white
Anglo-Saxon males in the United States. Also, a H4: A Spanish-accented salesperson will have a
positive identification with the spokesperson, greater positive impact on purchase intentions of
O. I4( DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101 93

bilingual respondents than on purchase intentions 4.2. Data collection


o f monolingual respondents.
The p r o d u c t / s e r v i c e selected as the focus of
the ad message was automobile insurance. This
4. T h e s t u d y service was chosen because it is a service category
that students are aware of and frequently pur-
A 2 × 2 × 2 between-subjects factorial design chase. At the taping session, a hypothetical sales
was used with salesperson physical attractiveness pitch was p r e p a r e d with an American-English
(attractive and unattractive), accent (American- accent, Cuban-English accent, and Nicaraguan-
English, Spanish-English), and the speaking capa- English accent. Two American males, two Cuban
bility of the audience (English-only and bilingual males, and two Nicaraguan males were used for
- English and Spanish) as the between-subject authenticity. Each person read aloud the message
factors. several times, and the most natural rendition was
F o u r salesmen (two attractive and two chosen by the same group used to evaluate the
unattractive) were used for the study. The sales- accents. Naturalness, considered in terms of mes-
men were chosen from a list of ten males who sage flow and realistic sales pitch, was deter-
were rated on a seven-point attractiveness-unat- mined by the evaluating group. Each of the six
tractiveness scale by a sample of college students. recordings lasted approximately 55 seconds. The
Based on the results of the survey, three males following sales pitch message was read by the
were rated as attractive and three were rated as salesmen:
unattractive. An additional measure of attractive- You know, I am glad I started working for this
ness was derived from 67 respondents (28 males particular car insurance company. The car insur-
and 39 females). Gender, ethnicity, and culture ance business today is so complicated and has so
were taken into account, and video tapes were many types of coverage available, that the client
used for respondents to make evaluations. A two- can easily become confused. However, at this
way A N O V A with a repeated measure of one company we can cut through the clutter and
factor (level of attractiveness) indicated there was provide people with personalized service, help
a statistically-significant difference (F-value 45.56, them identify their insurance needs, and provide
p < 0.0001) between the attractiveness levels of them with the best policy at a very competitive
the salesmen. price.
Our claims d e p a r t m e n t is very efficient, and all
4.1. Pretest o f accent claims are processed in a speedy and professional
manner.
A panel of six judges - three C u b a n - A m e r i - Furthermore, this company has been in business
can and three N i c a r a g u a n - A m e r i c a n students - since, 1949, and with the way that we do business,
were used to determine an accent recognition we will be here for many more years. ! am so glad
factor. This procedure determined whether the I work for this company.
panel could distinguish a Cuban-English accent Twenty-four commercials were produced by
from a Nicaraguan-English accent. Four of the six using two different attractive persons, two differ-
judges recognized the difference on every occa- ent unattractive persons, two different American
sion. As a result, two Cuban-English and two accents, two different Cuban-English accents, and
Nicaraguan-English accents were selected for the two different Nicaraguan-English accents (four
study. The above procedure was also used for levels of attractiveness x six levels of accents =
selecting the American-English accents. Two 24). To eliminate any variation in audience evalu-
speakers from each of the three accent categories ation created by changes in voice quality, the
were selected for the study (two American-En- same three accents - American-English, Cuban-
glish, two Cuban-English, and two Nicaraguan- English, and Nicaraguan-English - were pre-
English). sented by using voice-overs for the spokesperson.
94 O.W. DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101

This procedure was feasible because the commer- presented to a multi-ethnic audience typical for
cials showed the spokesperson sitting at a desk South Florida communities. While the results are
and thinking about the sales pitch he would make in concordance with the linguistic and social psy-
to potential customers. The spokesperson was not chology literature, and similar to those of previ-
moving his lips; rather, his thoughts were 'heard' ous research findings, it was not possible to ex-
by the audience. haust all aspects of cross-cultural social percep-
A total of 963 respondents viewed the com- tion. For instance, favorable acceptance of the
mercials in a university classroom setting. The mainstream English accent may have been a local
students were told they would view/hear a sales- effect mediated by social class, relative lack of
man presenting a message, and they would be discriminatory climate, and high cost or political
asked to give their impressions of the spokesper- obstacles to travel to the country of origin. De-
son. It was a completely randomized design, where spite these potential limitations, the results of the
each respondent viewed only one commercial. study already provide good evidence that accul-
turation must be taken into account by advertis-
4.3. Questionnaire ers when choosing spokespersons for addressing
Latino customers.
After seeing the video tapes, respondents were
asked to complete a questionnaire consisting of
four separate measurement scales: a 12-item se- 5. Analysis and results
mantic differential scale for salesman credibility
(Brennan and Brennan, 1981; Seggie et al., 1982; 5.1. Sample profile
Brennan and Brennan, 1983; Callan et al., 1983;
Kalmar et al., 1987; Ohanian, 1990); a three-item Table 1 illustrates the demographic and socio-
semantic differential scale measuring respondent economic characteristics of the respondents. As
purchase intentions; three scales measuring voice can be seen from Table 1, the composition of the
characteristics of the salesmen; and fourteen re- sample is appropriate for testing the study hy-
spondent classification questions. Attractiveness potheses. That is, the sample contained 46% fe-
was one of the underlying dimensions in the males and 54% males, which approximately rep-
12-item semantic differential scale. resents the population measures. Also, approxi-
Salesperson Attractiveness was operationalized mately half the respondents are bilingual, which
by using a four-item semantic differential scale was necessary for testing the study hypotheses.
(attractive, beautiful, elegant, and sexy). Salesper- Since the product/service used was applicable to
son Accent was operationalized by using three the sample, use of a student sample is assumed
categories (i.e., American-English accented, not to create any study limitations.
Cuban-English accented, and Nicaraguan-English Table 2 illustrates the Pearson correlation co-
accented). A panel of three Cubans and three efficients and related significance levels for the
Nicaraguans were used to determine the recog- variables (elegant, sexy, beautiful, and attractive)
nizability of the Cuban-English and Nicaraguan- used to operationalize the attractiveness con-
English accents. Finally, Salesperson Effectiveness struct. All correlations are significant at the p <
was operationalized by using a three-item seman- 0.0001 level, and correlation coefficients are con-
tic differential scale (consider buying the insur- sidered moderately high. This provides support
ance policy, would inquire about the policy, and for aggregating the four variables to operational-
would purchase the policy) (Ohanian, 1991). ize the attractiveness construct.
To provide further support for operationaliza-
4.4. Limitations of the study tion of the attractiveness construct, four variables
were factor analyzed. The result of factor analysis
This study focused on standard English and is shown in Table 3.
Hispanic-English accents of male spokespersons, Table 3 shows the factor loading of four attrac-
O.W. DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101 95

Table 1 Table 3
Sample profile Factor pattern of attractiveness variables
Characteristics Proportion Variables Factor 1
(n = 963) Attractive/Unattractive 0.8681
Gender Beautiful/Ugly 0.8525
Female 46.5 Sexy/Not sexy 0.8396
Male 53.5 Elegant/Plain 0.6849
Eigenvalue 2.6544
Explained variance 0.6636
Age
18 to 24 66.9
25 to 29 21.2
30 to 35 7.4
35 to 44 3.2 Table 4
Over 45 1.3 Factor pattern of purchase intention variables
Variables Factor 1
Native language
Consider buying a policy/Will not 0.95013
English 55.2
Consider a policy 0.94002
Spanish 35.8
Inquire about a policy/Will not inquire 0.90320
Others 9.0
About a policy
Purchase policy/Will not purchase policy
Ethnicity Eigenvalue 2.60210
Caucasian 26.4 Explained variance 0.86741
Hispanic 54.6
African-American 6.1
Others 12.9

four variables was used to measure attractiveness


Household income
of the spokesperson. After analyzing the distribu-
Less than $10,000 13.1
$10,000-$29,999 27.7 tion of the responses on this aggregate attractive-
$30,000-$49,999 26.5 ness measure (which was close to normal distribu-
$50,000-$74,999 16.6 tion), a median split technique was used to divide
$75,000 and above 5.3 spokespersons into high and low-attractive cate-
gories. Hence, if the average attractiveness value
were higher than 4, it was placed in the high-at-
tractive group, and if it was less than 4 it was
tiveness variables used in the study. All four placed in the low-attractive group.
variables were loaded on one factor with high To operationalize the purchase intention con-
coefficients, and this factor alone explained 66.4% struct, three variables were factor analyzed, and
of the total variance. Hence, the average of these the result of factor analysis is shown in Table 4.

Table 2
Pearson correlation coefficients for attractiveness variables ( N = 963)
Elegant/Plain Sexy/Not sexy Beautiful/Ugly
Sexy/Not sexy 0.45942
(0.0001)
Beautiful/Ugly 0.44479 0.59388
(0.0001) (0.0001)
Attractive/Unattractive 0.42427 0.65450 0.69923
(0.0001) (0.0001) (0.0001)
96 O. W. DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101

Similar to the attractiveness construct, all three tive spokesperson was 3.6, while it was only 2.4
variables were loaded on the same factor with for the unattractive spokespersons. There is no
high factor loadings (0.95, 0.94, 0.90), and total interaction effect between attractiveness and au-
variance explained by this factor alone is 86.8%. dience, hence it is concluded that for both En-
The factor scores (purchase intention) are used glish-only and Spanish-English-speaking respon-
as the dependent variable for the rest of the dents, purchase intentions are more favorably
analysis. influenced by an attractive salesperson's message.
Using three variables (salesperson accent, at- This relationship is illustrated in Fig. 1A more
tractiveness and speaking ability of the audience) clearly.
as classification variables, and purchase intention Hypothesis H2 assesses the impact of salesper-
as the dependent variable, Analysis of Variance son accent on the purchase intentions of con-
was used to test the relationships. Table 5 illus- sumers. The A N O V A results shown in Table 5
trates the results of A N O V A and the mean pur- indicate that salesperson accent was significant
chase intention values for different treatment lev- ( F = 27.76, p < .0001). Therefore, results indicate
els. that the accent of the salesperson does in fact
Hypothesis H1 assesses the impact of salesman influence respondent purchase intentions toward
attractiveness on the purchase intentions of con- the service used in this study. Fig. 1B shows the
sumers. The results of the A N O V A indicate that mean purchase intentions of the respondents for
attractiveness has a large and significant effect on different accents (Scheffe's post-hoc test indi-
purchase intentions ( F = 62.04, p < .0001). The cates significant differences among means at p <
mean purchase intention rating for highly attrac- .05). Fig. 1B illustrates that American-English-

Table 5
Analysis of variance for attractiveness, accent, and audience
Variables df F-value p-value
Accent 2 27.76 0,0001
Attractiveness 1 62.04 0.0001
Audience 1 15.17 0.0001
Accent *attractiveness 2 2.91 0.0549
Accent * audience 2 1.59 0.2051
Attractiveness *audience 1 0.28 0.5994
Accent * attractiveness *audience 2 1.17 0.3123

Treatments Mean purchase intentions


Attractiveness
1 Spokesperson's attractiveness 3.5809
perceived high 2.4181
2 Spokesperson's attractiveness
perceived low 3.1656
Accent 2.4128
1 Spokesperson has an American-English 2.2711
accent
2 Spokesperson has a Cuban-English 2.4504
accent 2.8665
3 Spokesperson'has a Nicaraguan-
English accent
Audience
1 Respondent speaks English only
2 Respondent is bilingual
O.W. DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101 97

accented salespersons create more favorable pur- Although Cuban-English-accented salesper-


chase intentions than Cuban or Nicaraguan-En- sons created more favorable purchase intentions
glish-accented salespersons. than Nicaraguan-English-accented salespersons,

3.7 A
3.6

3.5

3.4

3.3

3.2

3.1
E
3

2.9 \

(L 2.8

2.7

2.6

2.5

2.4
~2

2.3 I
2
Attractiveness
E3 High and Low

3.2 B
3.1

3
\ \
2.9
\ \
2.8 \
2.7
\
2.6 \

2.5 \\
\
2.4

2.3

2.2 [ I
2 3

Accent
t3 English, Cuban, Nicaraguan
Fig. 1. M e a n p u r c h a s e i n t e n t i o n l e v e l s for t r e a t m e n t s .
98 O.W. DeShields, Jr. et aL ~Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101

the difference was not statistically significant. there is an interaction effect between attractive-
Hence, we do not have a strong support for ness and accent levels. Fig. 1C illustrates the
Hypothesis H3. A N O V A results indicate that purchase intentions for different levels of attrac-

4.4 C

4.2

3.8

3.6

3.4
E
3.2

I1. 2.8

2.6

2.4

2.2

2 I I
1 2

Attractiveness
[] American English + Cuban English o Nicaraguan English

2.9 D
2.85 -

2.8

2.75

E 2.7

2.65

2.6
O..
2.55

2.5

2.45

2.4 I I
1 2
Audience
[] English & Bilingual
Fig. 1 (continued).
O. IV..DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101 99

tiveness and accent. Fig. 1C shows that an attrac- able purchase intentions than Cuban or
tive American-English accented salesperson cre- Nicaraguan-English-accented salespersons. Pur-
ates the most favorable purchase intentions than chase intentions toward the advertised product/
any other factor level combination. service reached the highest level when the
Respondent speaking ability (monolingual or spokesperson was both attractive and had a
bilingual) also has a statistically significant impact standard accent (i.e., American-English).
on purchase intentions. Fig. 1D illustrates the The empirical relationships found in this study
mean-purchase-intention values for the audience. are consistent with the decoding theory and have
Fig. 1D shows that bilingual respondents have significant managerial implications in advertising
higher purchase intention levels than the mono- and sales management. Decoding theory and this
lingual respondents. However, there are no statis- supporting study offer direction for marketers
tically significant interactions between audience designing communications strategies for the
and accent levels. Therefore, Hypothesis H4 is global marketplace. It is very clear that U.S.
not supported. markets and consumers have become more di-
In summary, the mean-purchase-intention rat- verse than ever. Therefore, in their communica-
ings for those viewing attractive spokespersons tion and personal selling efforts, marketers may
with an American-English accent are significantly use attractive spokespersons with standard accent
higher than the purchase intention means of those to improve the effectiveness of communication.
viewing attractive or unattractive spokespersons Similarly, with the formation of the European
with a Spanish-English accent for both monolin- Union (EU), which eliminates trade barriers
gual and bilingual audiences. among member countries new marketing oppor-
tunities are presented for multicountry firms.
Based on the results of this study, such firms
6. Conclusions
should consider that standard language pronunci-
Our study was designed to determine the im- ations for respective member countries will likely
pact of the spokesperson's attractiveness and ac- be rated superior to any accented version of the
cent on the receiver's purchase intentions. Using host-country language. Thus, a potentially more
Tajfel's (1981) categorization theory, which sug- effective international marketing communication
gests that an attractive spokesperson will be eval- program would use host-country personnel as op-
uated more favorably than a less attractive posed to multilingual (accented) salespersons.
spokesperson, study findings show that there is a Also, advertisers in the respective countries may
statistically significant relationship between want to consider using a host-country national.
spokesperson attractiveness and accent and the Furthermore, although Latin American coun-
purchase intentions. Respondents had higher tries are, for the most part, Spanish speaking,
purchase intentions toward the product/service Edwards (1982) reported that a standard
advertised when the spokespersons were per- country-specific accent or dialect is perceived to
ceived as highly attractive than when the exhibit more traits of' competence, status, and
spokespersons were perceived as unattractive. prestige than a general accent or dialect. As a
Further, the results of the study support result, the standard Spanish-language pronuncia-
Tajfel's theory, which states that salespersons tion for a respective Latin American country (i.e.,
perceived as a part of mainstream society (i.e., a Guatemalan-Spanish accent used in that host
Anglo) and possessing a standard accent will have country) may be rated as superior to a general-
a more positive impact on the purchase inten- accented version of the host country's language.
tions of consumers in that society than salesper-
6.1. Generalizability o f the research and f u t u r e
sons who are not perceived as a part of the
studies
mainstream (i.e., Hispanics) and who possess a
foreign accent. Findings reveal that American- This study has a relatively large sample size of
English-accented salespersons create more favor- 963, and the sample contains multi-ethnic groups.
100 O.W. DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101

Also, the use of two spokespersons for each ac- Baker, M.J. and G.A. Churchill, Jr., 1977. The impact of
cent, and the inclusion of both Cuban and physically attractive models on advertising evaluations.
Journal of Marketing Research 14, 538-555.
Nicaraguan-English accents provide a basis for Belch, G.E., M.A. Belch and A. Villareal, 1987. Effects of
generalizing the findings for Spanish-speaking au- advertising communications: review of research. In: Jadg-
diences and spokespersons. ish Sheth, ed., Research in marketing 9 (JA1 Press, Inc.,
The experimental design used for the study Greenwhich, CT).
insures a degree of internal validity. Also, a vari- Berechree, P. and P. Ball, 1979. A study of sex, accent-broad-
ness and Australian sociolinguistic identity. Paper pre-
ety of additional procedures tests the internal sented at the second Australian conference on Language
validity of the instrument. It should be noted, and Speech.
though, that generalizing the effects of Berger, J., M.H. Fisk, R.Z. Norman and M. Zelditch, Jr.,
spokesperson accents in the Latin American im- 1977. Status characteristics and social interaction: an ex-
migrant community may require additional stud- pectation states approach (Elsevier Scientific Publishing
Company, New York).
ies in California and Mexico. Bradac, J.J. and R. Wisegarver, 1984. Ascribed status, lexical
There is a need to extend this work to other diversity, and accent: determinants of perceived status,
media vehicles that differ both in editorial/com- solidarity, and control of speech style. Journal of Lan-
mercial content and in target audience in order guage and Social Psychology 3, 239-255.
to deepen our understanding of how appearance Brennan, E.M. and J.S. Brennan, 1981. Measurements of
accent and attitude toward Mexican-American speech.
and accent acquire meaning. Also, the attractive- Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 10(5), 487-501.
ness construct needs to be operationalized differ- Brennan, E.M. and J.S. Brennan, 1983. Accent scaling and
ently, perhaps with multidimensional construct language attitudes: reaction to Mexican-American English
that more extensively reflects the impact of the speech. Language and Speech 24, Part 3, 207-221.
variable. Bringham, J.C., 1980. Limiting conditions of the physical
attractiveness stereotype: attributions about divorce. Jour-
This study should also be replicated using fe- nal of Research in Personality 14, 365-375.
male spokespersons and featuring different prod- Caballero, M.J. and W.M. Pride, 1984. Selected effects of
ucts and services. Also, attractiveness measure of salesperson sex and attractiveness in direct mail advertise-
the spokesperson should incorporate other non- ments. Journal of Marketing 48, January, 94-100.
physical attractiveness factors (i.e., personality, Callan, V.J., C. Gallois and P.A. Forbes, 1983. Valuative
reactions to accented English. Journal of Cross-Cultural
self-consciousness). Furthermore, the selection of Psychology 14(4), 407-426.
attractive spokespersons needs to be studied Carranza, M. and E. Ryan, 1975. Evaluative reactions of
carefully. Current practice is to intuitively select bilingual Anglo and Mexican American adolescents to-
exemplars that maximize physical attractiveness ward speakers of English and Spanish. International Jour-
on a global level. nal of the Sociology of Language 6, 83-104.
Chaiken, S., 1986. Physical appearance and social influence.
Finally, additional research is needed to better In: C.P. Herman, M.P. Zanna and E.T. Higgins, eds.,
understand the importance of accent in different Physical appearance, stigma, and social behavior (Erlbaum
cultures and languages. For example, might the Associates, Hove, UK), 143-177
effects of accent be different if the spokesperson Courtney, A.E. and T.W. Whipple, 1983. Sex stereotyping
used a more narrow, country-specific language advertising (Lexington Books, Lexington, MA).
Eagly, A.H. and W. Wood, 1985. Gender and influenceability:
such as Japanese, with an American or French stereotypes versus behavior. In: V.E. O'Leary, R.K. Unger
accent, versus English, which is an international and B.S. Wallston, eds., Women gender and social psy-
language? Hence, there is also a need for testing chology (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillside, N J), 225-256
the hypotheses used in this study in different Eagly, A.H., R.D. Ashmore, M.G. Makhijani and L.C. Longo,
cultures and using different languages. 1991. What is beautiful is good, but ...: a meta-analytic
review of research on the physical attractiveness stereo-
type. Psychology Bulletin 110, 109-128.
References Edwards, J., 1982. Language attitudes and implications among
English speakers. Language and Speech, 20-33.
Alpert, M.1. and W.T. Anderson, Jr., 1973. Optimal het- Edwards, J. and M. Jacobsen, 1987. Standard and regional
erophily and communication effectiveness: some empirical standard speech: distinctions and similarities. Language in
findings. Journal of Communications 23, 328-343. Society 16, 369-380.
O.W. DeShields, Jr. et al. / Intern. J. of Research m Marketing 13 (1996) 89-101 101

Foon, A.E., 1986. A social structural approach to speech Powesland, P.F. and H. Giles, 1975. Persuasiveness and ac-
evaluation. The Journal of Social Psychology 126(41, 521- cent-message incompatibility. Human Relations 28, 85-93.
530. Reid, L.N. and L.C. Soley, 1983. Decorative models and the
Garcia, M.R.I., 1984. Speech attitudes to speakers of lan- readership of magazine ads. Journal of Advertising Re-
guage varieties in a bilingual situation. International Jour- search 23(2), 27-32.
nal of Society and Language 47, 73-90. Reingen, P.H. and J.B. Kernan, 1994. Social perception and
Giles, H. and C. Sassoon, 1983. The effect of speaker's accent, interpersonal influence: some consequences of the physi-
social class background and message style on British lis- cal attractiveness stereotype in a personal selling setting.
teners' social judgments. Language and Communication 3, Journal of Consumer Psychology 2(11, 25-38.
305-313. Ryan, E.B., M.A. Carranza and R.W. Mollie, 1977. Reactions
Jones, W.H., R. Hannson and A.L. Philips, 1978. Physical toward varying degrees of accentedness in the speech of
attractiveness and judgments of psychotherapy. Journal of Spanish-English bilinguals. Language and Speech 20, 267-
Social Psychology 105, 79-84. 273.
Joseph, W.B., 1977. Effect of communicator physical attrac- Seggie, I., C. Fulmizi and J. Stewart, 1982. Evaluations of
tiveness and expertise on opinion change and information personality traits and employment suitability based on
processing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State various australian accents. Australian Journal of Psychol-
University, Columbus. ogy 34(31, 345-357.
Joseph, W.B., 1982. The credibility of physically attractive Snyder, M. and M. Rothbart, 1971. Communication attractive-
communicators: a review. Journal of Advertising 11(31, ness and opinion change. Canadian Journal of the Behav-
1 5 - 24. ioral Sciences, 377-387.
Kahle, L.R. and P.M. Homer, 1985. Physical attractiveness of Swartz, T.A., 1984. Relationship between source expertise
the celebrity endorser: a social adaptation perspective. and source attractiveness in an advertising content. Jour-
Journal of Consumer Research 11,954-961. nal of Advertising 13(2), 49-55.
Kalmar, 1., Z. Yong and X. Hong, 1987. Language attitudes in Tajfel, H., 1972. Some developments in European social psy-
Guangzhou, China. Language and Society 16, 499-508. chology. European Journal of Social Psychology 2, 307-
Kamins, M.A., 1990. An investigation into the 'match up' 322.
hypothesis in celebrity advertising: when beauty may be Tajfel, H., 1974. Social identity and intergroup behavior.
only skin deep. Journal of Advertising 19, 4-13. Social Information 13, 65-93.
Kelman, H.C., 1961. Processes of opinion change. Public Tajfel, H,, 1981. Human groups and social categories (Cam-
Opinion Quarterly 25, 57-78. bridge University Press, Cambridge).
Lambert, W.E., 1967. A social psychology of bilingualism. Tsalikis, J., O.W. DeShields and M.S. LaTour, 1991. The role
Journal of Social Issues 23, 91-109. of accent on the credibility of the salesman. Journal of
Landy, D. and H. Sigall, 1974. Beauty is talent: task evalua- Personal Selling and Sales Management 11, 31-41.
tion as a function of the performer's physical attractive- Tsalikis, J., M. Ortiz-Buonafina and M.S. LaTour, 1992. The
ness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 29, role of accent on the credibility and effectiveness of the
299-304. international business person: the case of Guatemala. In-
McGuire, W.J., 1985. Attitudes and attitude change. In: G. ternational Marketing Review 94, 57-72.
Lindzey and E. Aronson, eds., The handbook of social Umberson, D. and M. Hughes, 1987. The impact of physical
psychology 2, 3rd ed. (Random House, New York). attractiveness on achievement and psychological well-
Ohanian, R., 1990. Construction and validation of a scale to being. Social Psychology Quarterly 50, 227-236.
measure celebrity endorsers' perceived expertise, trustwor- Unger, R.K., A. Hildebrand and T. Madare, 1982. Physical
thiness, and attractiveness. Journal of Advertising 19(3), attractiveness and assumptions about social deviance: some
39-52. sex-by-sex comparisons, Personality and Social Psychology
Ohanian, R., 1991. The impact of celebrity spokesperson' Bulletin 8, 293-301.
perceived image on consumers' intention to purchase. Webster, M., Jr. and J.E. Driskell, Jr., 1983. Beauty as status.
Journal of Advertising Research February/March, 46-56. American Sociological Review 89, 140-165.
Patzer, G.L., 1983. Source credibility as a function of commu- Wilson, E.J. and D.L, Sherrell, 1993. Source effects in com-
nicator physical attractiveness. Journal of Business Re- munication and persuasion research: a meta-analysis of
search 112, 229-241. effect size. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science
Patzer, G.L., 1985. The physical attractiveness phenomena 212, 101--112.
(Plenum, New York).

Anda mungkin juga menyukai