Anda di halaman 1dari 18

Personal Relationships, 14 (2007), 149–166. Printed in the United States of America.

Copyright Ó 2007 IARR. 1350-4126=07

Speed-dating as an invaluable tool for studying


romantic attraction: A methodological primer

ELI J. FINKEL, a PAUL W. EASTWICK, a AND JACOB MATTHEWS b


a
Northwestern University and bUniversity of Chicago

Abstract
Research on initial romantic attraction flourished in the 1960s and 1970s but has now been partially eclipsed by
research on close relationships. The authors argue that speed-dating procedures, in which participants attend an
event where they experience brief ‘‘dates’’ with a series of potential romantic partners, permit researchers to ‘‘retro-
fit’’ the advances of close relationships research to the study of initial romantic attraction. Speed-dating procedures
also allow for strong tests of many fundamental attraction-related hypotheses and, via longitudinal follow-ups, could
unify the fields of initial romantic attraction and close relationships. This article will help investigators conduct
speed-dating studies by addressing the methodological and logistical issues they will face and by illustrating proce-
dures with a description of the Northwestern Speed-Dating Study.

Since its invention by Rabbi Yaacov Deyo in the


Eli J. Finkel, Department of Psychology, Northwestern
University; Paul W. Eastwick, Department of Psychology, late 1990s to help Jewish singles in Los Angeles
Northwestern University; Jacob Matthews, Department of meet one another, speed-dating has rapidly
Computer Science, University of Chicago. become an international phenomenon serving
Finkel’s and Eastwick’s contributions to the North-
western Speed-Dating Study (NSDS) and to this article diverse populations. In speed-dating, individuals
cannot be separated; they should both be viewed as first looking to meet potential romantic partners
authors. The NSDS was facilitated by grants from the attend an event where they go on a series of brief
University Research Grants Committee at Northwestern
University and from the Dispute Resolution Research Cen- ‘‘dates’’ with other attendees. These dates last
ter at the Kellogg School of Management, as well as a uniform number of minutes within each event,
a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellow- although their durations vary from one event to
ship awarded to PWE. We gratefully acknowledge Candida
Abrahamson, Dan Ariely, and Leonard Lee for their another (typically from 3 to 8 minutes). After the
insightful comments on earlier drafts of this paper and event, participants have the opportunity to say
Robert Lount for helping to suggest speed-dating as an ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ to indicate whether they would
effective strategy for investigating initial romantic attrac-
tion. We also thank the following individuals for their like to see each of their dates again. If two
assistance with conducting the NSDS: Layla Bermeo, speed-daters say ‘‘yes’’ to one another, they
Debra Blade, Christine Brooks, Bonnie Buik, Madelaine are given the ability to contact each other for
Eulich, Megan Graney, Jeff Jablons, Kristin Jones, Julie
Keller, Jennifer Leyton, Kaidi Liu, Mallory Martino, a future, presumably more traditional, date.
Ashley Mason, Jesse Matthews, Abby Mitchell, Jennifer Speed-dating has rapidly become big busi-
Rosner, Seema Saigal, Sarah Scarbeck, David Sternberg, ness, with millions of individuals shelling out
Laura Thompson, Ashley Treadway, Stephanie Yang, and
the Northwestern Class Alliance. tens of millions of dollars to attend the events. It
Correspondence (regarding the article or the NSDS in has also been featured in popular television pro-
general) should be addressed to Eli J. Finkel or Paul W. grams (e.g., Sex and the City), movies (e.g., The
Eastwick, Northwestern University, Department of
Psychology, Evanston, IL 60208-2710, e-mail: finkel@ 40-Year-Old Virgin), and virtually all main-
northwestern.edu (EJF); p-eastwick@northwestern.edu stream news outlets. Although these media
(PWE) or (concerning computer programming or database portrayals vary, they converge in suggesting
management) to Jacob Matthews, University of Chicago,
Department of Computer Science, Chicago, IL 60637, that speed-dating serves as a new option for
e-mail: jacobm@cs.uchicago.edu. individuals eager to meet potential romantic

149
150 E. J. Finkel, P. W. Eastwick, and J. Matthews

partners. For those who have never witnessed The Scientific Potential of
speed-dating first hand, it is perhaps useful to Speed-Dating Research
conceptualize a speed-dating event not as anal-
The 1960s and 1970s witnessed the heyday of
ogous to a series of first dates (which typically
research on initial romantic attraction. Schol-
last, we hope, longer than 4 minutes) but instead
ars unearthed diverse factors (e.g., physical
as analogous to those social settings, such as
attractiveness, similarity) that predict the
parties or bars, where individuals are exposed
to a large number of new potential romantic experience of romantic attraction. A laudable
partners. Of course, compared to those more handful of these early studies assumed the
traditional settings, speed-dating has the advan- ambitious task of setting men and women on
tage of removing significant barriers to initiat- actual dates (e.g., Byrne, Ervin, & Lamberth,
ing a conversation with a desirable stranger. For 1970; Walster, Aronson, Abrahams, & Rottman,
example, one can safely assume that speed- 1966; for a rare post-1980 example, see
daters are (at least somewhat) romantically Sprecher & Duck, 1994). Most studies, how-
available and eager to meet potential romantic ever, demonstrated principles of attraction in
partners, and one is virtually guaranteed a laboratory settings among participants who
few minutes of one-to-one time with all the never met the target of their attraction (e.g.,
preferred-sex speed-daters present. information about the target was presented to
Recently, at least two independent teams of them only on paper, in a photograph, or both)
social scientists have recognized the immense or, at the very least, who did not actually have
power of speed-dating procedures to address the opportunity to form a romantic relationship
diverse scientific inquiries and have run their with the target following the study.
own speed-dating studies. One study was con- In the early 1980s, research interests shifted
ducted with a graduate student sample by and scholars began directing attention and
Sheena Iyengar and her colleagues at Colum- resources to the study of ongoing romantic
bia University and another was conducted with relationships. In their comprehensive over-
an undergraduate student sample by ourselves view of the field of interpersonal relationships,
at Northwestern University. Berscheid and Regan (2005, p. 88) observe
The two principal goals of this report are (a) that ‘‘Although initially most attraction re-
to argue that speed-dating provides a particu- search was conducted with strangers in the
larly promising method for studying the dynam- laboratory., by the early 1980s attraction
ics of initial romantic attraction and early researchers had grown frustrated with the lim-
relationship development and (b) to serve as itations of such research. As a consequence,
a conceptual and methodological primer for they became more willing to confront the
investigators interested in conducting their many conceptual, methodological, and ethical
own speed-dating studies. The latter goal occu- difficulties associated with research on ongo-
pies the majority of this report, but we begin by ing relationships.’’ This increased emphasis on
addressing the former.1 close relationships led to a literature character-
ized by many advantages over the literature on
initial romantic attraction, including (a) a focus
1. Issues related to data storage, management, and analy-
sis are beyond the scope of this report. Investigators on relationships that have a future beyond the
interested in learning about data analytic issues relevant conclusion of the laboratory visit, (b) the
to analyzing speed-dating data are encouraged to begin emergence of insights into the dyadic nature
by perusing books by Kenny (1994), Raudenbush and
Bryk (2002), and Singer and Willett (2003). Unfortu- of romantic relationship processes, and (c) an
nately, fewer resources targeted toward social scientists enhanced understanding of how romantic
are available for learning about issues of data storage dynamics play out over time.
and data management. Investigators can learn about
such issues as the distinction between flat files and Unfortunately, these methodological and
relational databases (and the advantages of storing conceptual advances from the close relation-
and managing data in the latter) in books by Stanczyk, ships literature have not yet been completely
Champion, and Leyton (2001) and Kline, Kline & Hunt
(2004). Additional information is available from the ‘‘retrofitted’’ to the study of initial romantic
authors upon request. attraction. Garnering increased insights into
Speed-dating primer 151

the mechanisms of initial romantic attraction up assessments, to examine how these attrac-
and early relationship development is important tion dynamics play out over time (see Eastwick
both because of the direct impact that attraction & Finkel, in press). The speed-dating paradigm
processes have on individuals’ lives and thus harnesses the strengths of close relation-
because elevated insight into them will likely ships research and applies it to the domain of
enhance our understanding of ongoing relation- initial romantic attraction.
ship dynamics (e.g., distinguishing flourishing An additional virtue is that diverse features
relationships from those that end in divorce). of the speed-dating event are amenable to a
Berscheid and Regan (2005, p. 159) suggest large array of experimental manipulations.
that ‘‘to understand why others currently are As examples, investigators could (a) compare
in the relationships they are—and to understand 1-minute dates to 5-minute dates, (b) offer dif-
why we ourselves developed the relationships ferent instructional sets across the events or to
we did—it is usually necessary to retrace the certain individuals within an event (e.g.,
history of the relationship back to its very encouraging some participants to disclose per-
beginning and to identify the causal conditions sonal information and others not to do so; see
that were in force at that time.’’ We enthusias- Aron, Melinat, Aron, Vallone, & Bator, 1997;
tically agree that understanding ongoing rela- Sedikides, Campbell, Reeder, & Elliot, 1999),
tionship dynamics requires examining them or (c) include confederates of the experimenter
from their very beginning (and even before at the events to enact one interpersonal style on
partners meet one another), but we suggest that certain dates and another interpersonal style on
relationship scientists are severely handicap- others (assuming that the investigators can
ping themselves by settling exclusively for identify an ethical way to include confederate-
‘‘retracing history.’’ Retrospective reports on based manipulations). In addition to these ex-
this topic can be prone toward methodological perimental manipulations, investigators could
problems, including systematic memory and also employ audio or video procedures (or both)
selection biases (for a discussion of the pros to record each date for subsequent rating by
and cons of retrospective self-reports in rela- trained coders.
tionships research, see Metts, Sprecher, & Despite allowing for tight experimental
Cupach, 1991). control, speed-dating procedures are also
We suggest that great explanatory power strong in ecological validity. Most importantly,
will be gained when relationship scientists individuals’ behavior and decisions in the
intensively study initial romantic attraction speed-dating process strongly impact their real
and early relationship development from before romantic prospects. Being impressive on each
the two partners meet. Speed-dating provides speed-date (as one would strive to be in real-
a promising methodological paradigm for world settings when meeting potential roman-
studying initial romantic attraction and early tic partners) and taking one’s ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’
relationship development because it enables decisions seriously can powerfully impact
investigators to assess a large battery of back- one’s romantic life following the event. Also,
ground information about individuals before as in real-world dating situations—but unlike
they meet one another, to introduce them to most extant research on initial romantic attrac-
one another in a controlled laboratory setting tion—each participant is simultaneously eval-
(the speed-dating event), and to follow them uating potential partners and being evaluated
after the laboratory session to examine relation- by them. In addition, individuals really do meet
ship dynamics over the ensuing days, weeks, in virtually identical speed-dating events in the
and beyond. The manifold virtues of speed- real world (i.e., outside of the laboratory)—and
dating research allow investigators of initial individuals, even when they are not attending
romantic attraction dynamics to focus on rela- speed-dating events, frequently face circum-
tionships that have the potential to become stances (e.g., attending a party, going to a bar,
meaningful to participants in the future, to study attending a retreat for church or another so-
those aspects of attraction that are inherently cial group, entering a new college dormitory)
dyadic, and, by including longitudinal follow- that allow them to evaluate the romantic
152 E. J. Finkel, P. W. Eastwick, and J. Matthews

desirability of a series of potential partners The second type of analysis involves pre-
simultaneously. dicting postevent relationship dynamics from
Large-scale speed-dating studies consist- information gathered at the speed-dating
ing of enough participants to create hundreds event. For example, investigators could exam-
of romantic couples and including longitudi- ine the relative strength of initial perceptions
nal follow-up assessments could in principle of physical attractiveness versus initial percep-
produce a sufficient number of romantic rela- tions of personality in predicting satisfaction
tionships to integrate research investigating with a follow-up date. Other investigators
initial romantic attraction and ongoing close could explore whether individuals who are
relationships, two fields that barely speak to experimentally urged to disclose deeply per-
one another at present. For example, such sonal information on their speed-dates experi-
a study could reveal whether the best predic- ence elevated mood and well-being in the days
tors of strong initial romantic attraction are following the event.
correlated positively, negatively, or not at all The third type of analysis involves using
with the best predictors of healthy long-term variables assessed exclusively during the
relationships. It is possible that one reason for speed-dating process. For example, investiga-
the high divorce rate in Western cultures is tors have examined (a) whether reciprocal
that individuals make marriage decisions on romantic attraction emerges during the speed-
the basis of variables that are at best irrelevant date (Eastwick, Finkel, Mochon, & Ariely,
to long-term relationship well-being. A large- 2007) and (b) whether having more speed-dates
scale speed-dating study with a longitudinal at a given event differentially impacts the rates
follow-up portion could provide the data for at which men and women say ‘‘yes’’ to one
a compelling test of this speculative but another (Fisman, Iyengar, Kamenica, & Simon-
important hypothesis. son, 2006). Other investigators could explore,
for example, whether enjoying one date dimini-
shes the likelihood of saying ‘‘yes’’ to the sub-
Types of hypotheses that can be tested in
sequent date.
speed-dating research
This third type of analysis also allows
Speed-dating studies provide the opportunity investigators to capitalize on one of the most
to answer countless research questions. For powerful features of speed-dating research: It
illustrative purposes, we highlight three gen- is beautifully suited to take advantage of the
eral types of analysis that emerge in a speed- immense power of Kenny’s (1994; Kenny &
dating study. The first type of analysis La Voie, 1984) social relations model (SRM).
involves predicting speed-dating dynamics The SRM provides a data analytic procedure
on the basis of information collected prior to that allows investigators to examine the degree
the event. For example, investigators have to which features of a given social interaction
examined the role that variables such as phys- are due to (a) one partner, (b) the other partner,
ical attractiveness, height, age, body shape, or (c) the unique dynamics between the two
and race play in the number of ‘‘yesses’’ indi- partners. For example, suppose that Jennifer
viduals receive (Kurzban & Weeden, 2005). reports strong sexual desire toward David im-
Other investigators might be interested in mediately following their 4-minute speed-
examining how personality characteristics (or date. There are three independent reasons
other person-level variables such as having why she might experience this desire toward
divorced parents) relate to the dynamics of him. First, Jennifer might report strong levels
initial romantic attraction. They might hypoth- of attraction toward all the men she meets at
esize, for example, that having an agreeable the event (i.e., she finds everybody sexy,
disposition predicts receiving more yesses for which is an example of an actor effect). Sec-
women than it does for men or that the simi- ond, all the women at the event might find
larity-attraction link (cf. Byrne, 1971) is more David to be attractive (i.e., he is generally per-
robust for attitudinal than for personality or ceived as sexy, which is an example of a part-
demographic similarity. ner effect). And third, there is something
Speed-dating primer 153

unique about the dynamics between Jennifer ticipant sample. Several issues factor into this
and David that makes her desire him (i.e., decision, including the researchers’ theoretical
she has unique ‘‘chemistry’’ with him, which interests, convenience, and financial consider-
is an example of a relationship effect).2 If par- ations. For many research questions, under-
ticipants report on each of their speed-dates, graduate samples may be optimal because
then investigators can use the SRM to investi- they are frequently convenient to access, inex-
gate, among other things, the degree to which pensive to recruit, and, most importantly,
each of these three independent explanations interested in meeting new romantic partners.
accurately accounts for attraction phenomena. Nonetheless, investigators interested in exam-
In addition to all three types of analysis ining attraction dynamics following divorce,
available from speed-dating research, in- among singles in their 30s, or for individuals
vestigators could also examine the interplay looking for immediate marriage partners will
between them. For example, perhaps individ- frequently find undergraduate samples lacking.
uals with high preevent attachment avoidance Considering that professional speed-dating
scores tend not to enjoy follow-up dates with events are typically attended by people from
partners who were consensually rated as anx- the nonacademic community, recruiting from
ious. We encourage investigators to consider this population may be less difficult than is typ-
a three-part structural plan, including data col- ical in psychological research. Scholars could
lection prior to, during, and following the contact professional speed-dating companies
event. Although this three-part plan is far more with the hope of initiating a collaborative rela-
labor intensive and costly than the one-part tionship or they could use more traditional
plan involving only the speed-dating event, methods of participant recruitment (e.g., news-
we believe that its benefits in terms of richness paper or radio advertisements, flyers), perhaps
and texture are often worth the costs. trying to tantalize potential participants by
emphasizing that the research-based speed-
dating events will be free of charge.
Issues to Consider Prior to Conducting
Whether investigators employ undergradu-
a Speed-Dating Study
ate or other samples, they must decide on the
We now turn our attention from (a) the poten- age ranges of the participants at each session.
tial of speed-dating procedures to advance the Even investigators employing an undergradu-
science of initial romantic attraction and early ate sample will want to consider whether, say,
relationship development to (b) a nuts-and-bolts senior women (;22 years old) will be inter-
‘‘manual’’ on running speed-dating studies. Re- ested in dating freshman males (;19 years
searchers planning to conduct a speed-dating old). In the Northwestern Speed-Dating Study
study confront an array of decisions unique (NSDS), we resolved this concern by holding
from those typically encountered by attraction separate events for freshmen and sophomores,
or close relationships researchers. This section on the one hand, and for juniors and seniors, on
of the Primer discusses eight issues worth con- the other. Another important consideration is
sidering prior to conducting a speed-dating whether to include only heterosexual events or
study. also gay male and lesbian events.

Participant sample Speed-dating event details


After committing to speed-dating as an opti- Another series of decisions to be made early
mal method for studying a particular research on involves the structure of the speed-dating
question, investigators must determine the par- events themselves. Investigators must decide,
for example, how many participants will
attend each session, how long each date will
2. One important consideration for SRM analyses is that last, and whether participants will complete
investigators must include more than a single item to
assess each construct of interest to separate the relation- questionnaires after each date. Given that, as
ship effect from error. with all other experiences, there is a limit on
154 E. J. Finkel, P. W. Eastwick, and J. Matthews

how long individuals can speed-date before Investigators must also decide whether to
their enjoyment and concentration wanes, take photos of the speed-dating participants.
these decisions are interdependent; the reso- Although taking photos (and subsequently
lution of one influences the others. Although having them rated for attractiveness by objec-
no research has examined what total duration tive observers) can potentially cause some dis-
for a speed-dating event is optimal, we comfort for participants, it enables researchers
decided early on to hold our speed-dating to predict the dynamics of initial romantic
sessions to approximately 2 hours. Our infor- attraction beyond the effects of physical attrac-
mal perception is that NSDS participants tiveness. Such data are likely to be important
generally were able to stay focused, inter- in persuading skeptics that a given social pro-
ested, and attentive for the whole time and cess promotes initial romantic attraction inde-
that going much longer would have begun to pendent of physical attractiveness.
cause strain. This 2-hour block allowed us to
include, in addition to other procedural
Keeping event sex ratios near 1.0
logistics, approximately 12 dates, each last-
ing 4 minutes and followed by a 2-minute Sex ratios refer to the proportion of men to
questionnaire. Shortly after participating in women in a particular environment, and it
the events, 73.0% of participants reported is important for investigators to keep these
that the number of dates was ‘‘just right’’ ratios in mind as they prepare to conduct speed-
(with 6.7% saying it was ‘‘too few’’ and dating studies. Imagine that you are a woman
20.2% saying that it was ‘‘too many’’) and attending a speed-dating event consisting of
60.1% reported that having 4 minutes of 10 women and 6 men. You would find yourself
time for each date was ‘‘just right’’ (with waiting much of the time for an available man.
39.9% saying it was ‘‘too little’’ and 0.0% In addition, the gender imbalance would likely
saying that it was ‘‘too much’’).3 influence your experience in diverse ways
Investigators might presume a priori that (e.g., elevated feelings of intrasexual com-
including a larger number of dates (e.g., 20) petition, a perception that you cannot be par-
would generally result in a better speed-dating ticularly picky)—and men would likely be
experience and in a greater number of matches influenced in complementary ways. Although
than would including a smaller number of fascinating questions emerge in situations
dates (e.g., 10). After all, the larger number with imbalanced sex ratios, scholars generally
of dates should provide participants with the would not want such imbalances to emerge
opportunity to meet more people with whom accidentally.
they could be compatible. Recent evidence How can investigators maximize the likeli-
suggests, however, that this logic may be hood that sex ratios will approximate 1.0 in
incorrect: Participants tend to be happier with each heterosexual session? One important
their speed-dating experiences and to match strategy for avoiding biased sex ratios is lim-
with a larger number of people when they iting the number of available events that are
attend events with a smaller rather than a larger initially opened for registration. The most
number of speed-daters (Iyengar, Simonson, likely cause of imbalanced sex ratios is that
Fisman, & Mogilner, 2005). one sex will sign up substantially more quickly
or in larger numbers than will the other. In the
NSDS, for example, virtually all the female
slots filled in less than 48 hours, whereas the
3. Scholars and lay theorists might assume that individu- male slots took a week to fill. Although we did
als cannot learn anything substantive about another per- not predict this imbalance, we were thankful
son in only 4 minutes, but this assumption conflicts
strongly with (a) the robust literature on the importance that we had been circumspect in the number
and accuracy of ‘‘thin slices’’ of behaviors (e.g., of events we initially opened (seven) and that
Ambady & Rosenthal, 1993) and (b) the early findings we gave participants ample time to register. In
from the NSDS, which suggest that individuals are able
to make extraordinarily sophisticated social judgments addition to posting a relatively small number
on their speed-dates (e.g., Eastwick et al., 2007). of sessions at first, investigators can attend
Speed-dating primer 155

closely to the signup rates and systematically vated undergraduates to assist with activities
initiate proactive measures to recruit partici- such as generating publicity ideas, posting
pants of the sex that is signing up more slowly. flyers, and helping to conduct the speed-dating
events; (b) it meant that there were groups of
undergraduates on campus (active members of
Publicity and recruitment
the Northwestern Class Alliance) who were
Recruiting similar numbers of men and knowledgeable about the speed-dating events
women for each heterosexual session is just and eager to answer questions that other stu-
one of several issues to be considered when dents might have about it; and (c) it gave us
developing a publicity campaign to recruit par- access to the listservs to send e-mails about the
ticipants. The publicity campaign for the speed-dating event to all students in a given
NSDS consisted of two waves. The first took class.
place approximately 3 weeks before the initial
event and consisted of a series of ‘‘teaser
Participant payment
flyers’’ posted around campus. We used six
teaser flyers, all of which were eye-catching An additional feature of speed-dating studies
and designed to inspire students to think of is that participation is frequently a substantial
speed-dating as an appealing alternative to reward in its own right, rendering participant
the typical dating scene; some included quotes payment considerations relatively compli-
from fictitious Northwestern University cated. In most relationships studies that do
undergraduate students. Examples were as fol- not take advantage of ‘‘participant pools’’
lows: (a) ‘‘‘New people I’ve met since Fall (in which undergraduate students participate
Quarter? Does my TA count?’ — Rachel, in exchange for course credit), participants
Class of ’06,’’ and (b) ‘‘‘4 minutes? That’d typically must be paid enough to ensure ade-
be an improvement’ — Abby, Class of ’08.’’ quate participation. In contrast, speed-dating
The second wave took place approximately 10 studies leave open the options of (a) paying
days before the first event and included a single individuals to participate, (b) having the
flyer printed in color and posted around cam- events be free to individuals in exchange for
pus. As with the teaser flyers, this flyer was completing questionnaires, or (c) charging
designed to generate excitement while simul- individuals to participate. Although it may
taneously emphasizing that the speed-dating seem strange to consider charging partici-
events were part of a research study approved pants to participate in a research study (option
by Northwestern University’s institutional re- c), there are at least two arguments supporting
view board. All flyers are available from the the legitimacy of such an approach. First,
first author upon request. individuals who participate in real-world
Although we felt that this advertising cam- speed-dating events (as opposed to events
paign would convey the message that partici- run by relationship scientists) typically pay
pating in the speed-dating study was likely to a nontrivial sum to participate (a typical price
be a fun and interesting social event, we were in Chicago, IL, in 2007 was US $35), so it is
concerned that, on its own, it might not be not unreasonable to charge a smaller amount
sufficient to generate the ‘‘buzz’’ necessary and also ask that participants complete ques-
to recruit large numbers of participants. To tionnaires. Second, evidence across diverse
complement the advertising campaign, then, domains suggests that individuals place grea-
we formed a collaborative relationship with ter value on things for which they have paid
the Northwestern Class Alliance, an organiza- than on things they have received for free
tion consisting of four subgroups representing, (e.g., Thaler, 1980); participants may well
respectively, the freshman, sophomore, junior, take the speed-dating study more seriously
and senior classes. Bringing the Northwestern if they have paid to participate. Furthermore,
Class Alliance on as a cosponsor of the speed- these funds can be used to make the event
dating events benefited the study in three more enticing, perhaps by enabling the re-
important ways: (a) it added a group of moti- searchers to rent out a nicer location.
156 E. J. Finkel, P. W. Eastwick, and J. Matthews

The approach we took in the NSDS was to this will likely influence their behavior and
make the event free for participants in ex- their experience of the event and the dates
change for completing a series of interaction (not to mention the confidentiality concerns
records during the event (option b). We advo- that could emerge). Although some investiga-
cate this strategy, particularly for events with tors might decide they are explicitly interested
an undergraduate population, because it does in how dating behavior is altered by the pres-
not exclude participants who are particularly ence of spectators, most will want to eliminate
short of cash and allows the experimenter to this potentially confounding variable.
make salient that this typically expensive
event is being offered free of charge in
Institutional review board considerations
exchange for completing the questionnaires
diligently. Of course, payment decisions also Although ethical concerns are presumably more
need to be made for preevent questionnaires, or less constant across locations, there is an
follow-up questionnaires, and any other as- alarming amount of variability in what different
pects of the study that investigators might institutional review boards (IRBs) (i.e., institu-
include, especially if these components of the tionally sanctioned bodies tasked with oversee-
procedure are particularly time consuming for ing research ethics) determine to be ethical
participants. Our perception is that paying par- versus unethical research. Our opinion is that
ticipants for the parts of the procedure aside any reasonable IRB will allow investigators to
from the speed-dating event itself is frequently conduct speed-dating research as long as the
a good idea. investigators strive toward maximum scientific
and scholarly benefits while simultaneously
minimizing risks to participants.
Event location
The single most important step in garnering
We highlight here three important concerns in IRB approval is to initiate dialogue early in the
identifying the optimal location for the speed- process. As soon as we knew that we wanted to
dating events. First, an obvious concern is that conduct a speed-dating study with Northwest-
the location should be convenient for the par- ern University undergraduates and what our
ticipants. With an undergraduate sample, for general procedures would be, we scheduled
example, it will typically be best to host the a face-to-face meeting with the full-time IRB
event on campus or immediately off campus. employee who handles proposals from the psy-
Second, the location should be as elegant or chology department. We were forthright in this
fun as possible. Although we do not believe meeting about what we believed were the three
that hosting the events in an elegant or fun risks associated with participating in the stud-
location is necessary to conduct a high-quality ies. The first risk was a strong likelihood of
speed-dating study, we do believe that such experiencing social rejection. Most participants
a location promotes the quality of the experi- would say ‘‘yes’’ to at least one speed-dater
ence and leads to an aura of professionalism who would say ‘‘no’’ to them, and some partic-
that is likely to increase the probability that ipants would say ‘‘yes’’ to many who would say
participants will take the study seriously. As ‘‘no’’ to them. On rare occasions, participants
described below, we held the NSDS events in could even be rejected by everybody at the ses-
an elegant art gallery situated in Northwest- sion. The second risk was a strong likelihood of
ern’s centrally located student union. Third, embarrassment and social awkwardness. Speed
the location should be off the beaten path of dates could be uncomfortable, particularly for
potentially intrusive passersby. Although there dyads in which neither partner is blessed with
is no need to hold the events in a windowless talents for striking up interesting conversation
dungeon, they should not be held in a fishbowl. with strangers. Worse yet, there was a nonzero
Passersby and, in our experience, local media possibility that participants would find them-
figures, will likely be curious about the speed- selves on a 4-minute speed-date with a person
dating events. If speed-daters feel that they are they had met previously and did not like.
being watched by outsiders during the event, Finally, the third risk was a very small likelihood
Speed-dating primer 157

that an individual whom participants met at a more efficient and potentially superior prod-
a speed-dating event would harm them at a uct. We invested substantial effort to make our
later point in time. An example of this low- Web site (www.speeddating.northwestern.edu)
probability but very serious risk was that a look professional, inserting our logo and
female participant could subsequently go out header atop each page and formatting the
with a man she met at the speed-dating event pages to optimize aesthetic appeal. We used
and be sexually assaulted by him on the date. the Internet for, among other things, (a) partic-
In addition to having open dialogue with ipant signup, (b) administration of the preevent
the IRB about the risks associated with partici- questionnaire, (c) signing participants in at the
pating in the speed-dating study, investigators speed-dating event, (d) having participants
also need to balance between (a) employing an report their ‘‘yesses’’ and ‘‘nos,’’ (e) adminis-
effective advertising campaign designed to tration of the follow-up questionnaires, (f)
generate excitement about the events and (b) implementation of the messaging system that
IRB concerns about the protection of human allowed individuals to e-mail their matches
subjects. One of the more difficult conversa- through our Web site, and (g) sending re-
tions we had with the IRB had to do with our minder e-mails for the speed-dating events
desire to advertise in a way that was exciting and for all follow-up questionnaires. In addi-
and that eschewed excessive legalese. The IRB tion to making the study seem more profes-
was initially concerned, for example, that our sional, other benefits emerging from using
primary flyer came close to promising undue the Internet to conduct speed-dating research
benefits when it said, ‘‘Bring a friend. Make include (a) automated procedures that save
several.’’ Ultimately, the IRB concluded that time and minimize the likelihood of human
individuals signing up for a speed-dating event error, (b) the ability to set up a messaging
could not reasonably infer that we were prom- system (discussed below) for participant com-
ising them new friends, and it relented in its munication, (c) access to precise timing infor-
initial request to take this section off of the mation about when participants completed
flyer. We raise these details because advertis- Internet-based components of the study, and
ing for a speed-dating study will require an (d) reducing the need for manual data entry
awareness of different aspects of participants’ from the research team.
rights than those that might emerge in more On the other hand, setting up a Web site
traditional relationships research. Again, the sophisticated enough to run a speed-dating
best policy is for investigators to initiate dia- study requires computer programming skills
logue on these issues with the IRB as soon as that are not available in many psychological
possible, preferably at least 3 months before laboratories. We believe that investigators
they plan to conduct their first event. could conduct high-quality speed-dating stud-
Another issue to address with the IRB is ies without creating a study Web site, but it
using an online consent form. If participants would require crossing a few hurdles. One
sign up for the study online (which is recom- important hurdle would be developing a system
mended; see the section Using the Internet), it for the matching process. Investigators could
is significantly more convenient to have them do this by, for example, having participants
also complete the consent process online, turn in their ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ decisions at the
especially if they will also be completing a pre- end of the event to a member of the research
event questionnaire online. If participants will team, who would do the matching by hand and
complete these other procedures in person, e-mail matches to the participants. Preevent
there is no need to have them complete the and follow-up questionnaires would also
consent form online. require alternative solutions (e.g., mailing the
questionnaires to participants or asking them
to come to the lab for separate questionnaire
Using the Internet
sessions), but these certainly would not be
Speed-dating research can be conducted with- prohibitively difficult. In short, there are sev-
out using the Internet, but using it can result in eral advantages to conducting speed-dating
158 E. J. Finkel, P. W. Eastwick, and J. Matthews

research by relying extensively on the Internet, tion e-mail address; this e-mail included a
but investigators can conduct high-quality stud- clickable link that sent them to a page where
ies using more traditional research procedures. they logged in with their e-mail address and
password. (We incorporated this part of the
procedure to minimize the likelihood that,
The NSDS—Procedures and Materials
e.g., somebody might register a friend as
As we began NSDS preparation, we were only a practical joke.) After logging in, participants
able to rely on our own idiosyncratic speed- selected among the available speed-dating
dating experiences4 and unpublished manu- events and were sent to the 30-minute preevent
scripts from other speed-dating researchers. questionnaire.
In addition, empirical papers presenting results We held participants’ selected time slot for
from speed-dating studies are likely to be 3 hours to provide some flexibility for them to
somewhat meager on procedural detail due to complete this questionnaire (for which they
the space constraints placed on authors by edi- were paid US $5 at the event). They were
tors and publishers. Toward the goal of pro- allowed to stop in the middle of the question-
viding an in-depth, illustrative overview of the naire and return later in the 3-hour block to
methodological issues and procedures relevant complete it. If they failed to complete the pre-
to conducting speed-dating studies, the final event questionnaire within those 3 hours, they
section of this Primer explains in detail the were required to select an event again (if there
NSDS procedure and materials. were still open time slots) and could then con-
tinue completing the questionnaire from where
they had previously left off. This 3-hour time
Procedure
limit disallowed participants from occupying
Part I: Preevent procedures. After seeing a slot in an event without being serious about
our publicity flyers or e-mails, or after hearing the research component of the study. The time
about the study via word of mouth, interested limit proved useful, as 10% of the participants
individuals visited our Web site and viewed who began the preevent questionnaire never
a page that provided a basic outline of all study completed it (i.e., they in essence dropped
procedures. If they remained interested in par- out of the study). If those time slots had
ticipating after reading this additional infor- remained reserved, we could have faced
mation, they read and (electronically) signed a problematic number of no shows at the
the online consent form; the computer system events.
did not allow them to continue unless they In total, we conducted seven speed-dating
signed this form. Next, they provided their events involving 163 (81 women) North-
name and e-mail address, indicated the type western University undergraduate students
of event they wished to attend (e.g., men seek- between Wednesday, April 27, and Wednes-
ing women, men seeking men), and created day, May 4, 2005. Participants were 19.6 years
a password so they could log into the speed- old on average (SD ¼ 1.0 years); 36.2% were
dating Web site in the future. Moments after freshmen, 38.7% sophomores, 21.5% juniors,
this registration procedure was complete, par- and 3.7% seniors. (We recruited seniors less
ticipants received an e-mail at their registra- aggressively because they were scheduled to
graduate approximately 6 weeks after the
events.)
4. The first and second authors of this report participated
in a Chicago-based speed-dating event in February Part II: Event procedures. We hosted the
2004 to learn how speed-dating events ‘‘are supposed NSDS speed-dating events in the Dittmar Art
to go.’’ Although we do not believe it is necessary for
scholars (especially those who are not single) to partic- Gallery in Northwestern University’s Norris
ipate in a speed-dating event to develop a high-quality University Center, which is the university’s
speed-dating study, we recommend that investigators student union. We standardized the lighting
invest some time familiarizing themselves with profes-
sional speed-dating practices before conducting their setup and music selection across events. In
own events. advance of each session, members of the
Speed-dating primer 159

research team configured the room so there just-completed date. The reason why partici-
was a series of approximately 12 dyadic seat- pants rotated to the new location before rather
ing areas, each consisting of two chairs and than after completing the interaction record is
a small tablecloth-covered table adorned with that it would have been awkward for them to
a candle. The large entryway to the art gallery complete an interaction record about the per-
served as a location for refreshments, which son sitting across from them. It was less awk-
consisted of bottled root beer and sparkling ward to complete it while sitting across from
grape juice. somebody they had not yet dated.
When participants arrived for the event, The dyadic seating stations for the NSDS
they checked in immediately outside the art were set in a loosely circular configuration. In
gallery, where a researcher (PWE) assigned four of the seven speed-dating events, women
participant IDs (a number for women or a letter remained seated throughout the session, while
for men). Participants were given name tags the men rotated from station to station. In the
and were instructed to write their first names other three events, men remained seated while
and participant IDs on them. They were also women rotated.
given a clipboard containing a packet of inter-
action records (one-page questionnaires that Part III: Postevent procedures. When par-
participants completed following each date) ticipants returned home from the event, an
and a ‘‘scorecard’’ on which they could write e-mail containing a link to our Web site
notes to themselves about their dates. awaited them. After logging in, they arrived
Immediately after checking in for the event, at the page where they clicked ‘‘yes’’ or
participants entered the art gallery and posed for ‘‘no’’ next to the photo of each speed-dater5
their photograph. The research assistant taking they had met that evening and completed the
the photograph explained that it would be posted brief prematch questionnaire (discussed
on our speed-dating Web site to help speed- below). After completing the prematch ques-
daters remember one another when recording tionnaire, participants answered one more
their ‘‘yesses’’ and ‘‘nos’’ and when completing question before logging off: They indicated
the follow-up questionnaires. The research assis- whether or not they would allow ‘‘missed
tant used a digital camera for the photos, which matches’’ (speed-daters who had said ‘‘no’’
allowed her to show the picture to the speed- to the participant but to whom the participant
dater. We took as many photos as the partici- had said ‘‘yes’’) to learn that the participant
pants desired until they were happy with one. had said ‘‘yes’’ to them. If the participant said
The experimenter for the speed-dating ses- ‘‘yes’’ to the missed matches option, the missed
sions (EJF) filled a dual role as both experi- match would be given the option of changing
menter and emcee. As in most relationships his or her previous ‘‘no’’ response to a ‘‘yes’’
studies, the experimenter guided participants (after learning about his or her matches 24
through the session and answered any ques- hours later). If the missed match indeed chose
tions they raised. At the appropriate time in to change from ‘‘no’’ to ‘‘yes,’’ the dyad would
each event, he explained the speed-dating pro- become a match. In the NSDS, 100% of partic-
cedures, how to use the interaction records and ipants completed the matching process and
the scorecard, and the matching procedures. 54% selected the missed matches option. The
(The experimenter script is available upon 163 participants generated a total of 206 pairs of
request from the first author.) In addition to
these typical responsibilities, the experimenter
was also responsible for helping to make the 5. Other speed-dating researchers (e.g., Fisman et al.,
session an entertaining social event; he strived 2006) instead had participants indicate their ‘‘yes’’ or
‘‘no’’ decisions on the questionnaire administered
to remain lively and energetic throughout. immediately after each date. A fascinating program of
After each 4-minute date, the experimenter research might examine how ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ decisions
blew his whistle, which served as the cue for change as a function of whether participants are (a)
physically with the potential match, (b) at home but
participants to rotate to the next position and presented with a picture of the potential match, or (c)
complete the interaction record regarding the at home with only their memory of the potential match.
160 E. J. Finkel, P. W. Eastwick, and J. Matthews

matches (mean per participant ¼ 2.53; range ¼ completed and a US$10 bonus if they com-
0–9), and 20.9% of these pairs resulted from the pleted at least nine of them. Participant reten-
missed matches option. In fact, six participants tion was good, with 90% completing at least
with at least one match would have had zero three of the follow-up questionnaires and 64%
matches if not for this option. completing at least nine of them.
Then, 24 hours after the speed-dating event,
participants received another e-mail from us
Materials
directing them back to our Web site. After
logging in, they learned who their matches The NSDS materials consisted of five sections.
were and completed the brief postmatch ques- The preevent questionnaire, interaction re-
tionnaire (discussed below). As soon as partic- cords, and follow-up questionnaires were major
ipants learned who their matches were (and sections, whereas the prematch and the post-
decided whether to change from ‘‘no’’ to match questionnaires were smaller sections.
‘‘yes’’ for any missed matches), they could We discuss the five questionnaires in the order
use our Web site’s messaging system to write in which participants completed them.
to one or more of their matches without having
to divulge personal contact information. They Preevent questionnaire. The preevent ques-
arrived at the messaging page by clicking a but- tionnaire consisted of a broad array of demo-
ton next to the photograph of the match they graphic and background measures and required
wanted to contact; the recipient viewed any approximately 30 minutes to complete. Demo-
received messages by returning to the Web site graphic variables included sex, race, height,
and was offered the option to send a reply. Par- weight, hometown, and religion. We included
ticipants were permitted to use the messaging brief measures of a broad array of mainstream
system for 1 month following the speed-dating personality variables (e.g., self-esteem; the ‘‘big
event, during which time 111 participants sent five’’ personality characteristics) and of inter-
a total of 383 messages. These messages were personal dispositions (e.g., attachment anxiety
later coded by the research team (after all iden- and avoidance, sociosexuality, loneliness). We
tifying information had been removed).6 also included measures to allow participants to
Twenty-four hours after participants re- rate the degree to which a series of 28 character-
ceived their match information (and 48 hours istics (e.g., physically attractive, ambitious/
after attending the speed-dating event), partic- driven, friendly/nice, spontaneous, outgoing)
ipants received an e-mail directing them to the described their actual self, their ideal self, and
first wave of the 10-wave follow-up portion of their ideal partner.
the study. They received similar e-mails every
72 hours until all 10 waves of the follow-up Interaction records. Upon their arrival at
portion of the study had been completed (30 the speed-dating event, participants received
days after the speed-dating event). They were a packet of identical interaction records, one
instructed to complete each questionnaire for each date. These interaction records con-
before going to bed that night, although we sisted of three parts. The first part asked par-
accepted late questionnaires. They were paid ticipants about their experience of the date
US$3 for each follow-up questionnaire they itself and of the relationship between them-
selves and the partner (e.g., ‘‘I was sexually
attracted to my interaction partner,’’ ‘‘I think
6. Participants were aware that we (the researchers) were that my interaction partner was sexually
able to see the messages. Being able to see the messages attracted to me,’’ ‘‘I thought this interaction
has enormous scientific benefits in terms of providing went smoothly’’). The second part asked par-
a rich source of process-oriented information about
how individuals initiate romantic relationships with ticipants about their perceptions of 12 of the
desired partners, but it could have negative methodo- partner’s characteristics (e.g., physically
logical consequences as well (e.g., increasing demand attractive, ambitious/driven, charismatic).
characteristics), which could possibly alter the content
of the e-mails or the likelihood of sending one in the These characteristics represented a subset of
first place. the 28 characteristics participants had reported
Speed-dating primer 161

on the preevent questionnaire vis-à-vis the participants’ romantic lives were shaping up
actual self, the ideal self, and the ideal partner. in the wake of the speed-dating event. Broadly
Finally, the third part asked participants about speaking, the 10 follow-up questionnaires
the degree to which the partner made them feel (completed every third day for a month) con-
like they possessed each of these same 12 sisted of two sections. The first section did not
characteristics. mention any of the participants’ speed-dating
The experimenter emphasized to the partic- matches, instead focusing on the participants
ipants that some of the items (e.g., the degree themselves and their life circumstances. This
to which the other person is ‘‘dependable/ section included items such as ‘‘In general, I
trustworthy’’) might be somewhat difficult am pretty happy these days,’’ ‘‘I have high
to answer after knowing the person for only self-esteem,’’ and ‘‘Compared to the average
4 minutes. He reassured them that they were Northwestern University student, my physical
expected just to provide their best guess. health is excellent.’’ It also asked participants
to indicate the degree to which the same series
Prematch questionnaire. When partici- of 28 characteristics initially encountered in
pants returned home after the speed-dating the preevent questionnaire described their act-
event, they logged into our Web site and ual self at each follow-up wave.
checked either ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ next to the The second section consisted of a series of
photograph of each date to indicate whether match-specific items. The fact that participants
they would be interested in seeing that person had between zero and nine matches presented
again. After completing these ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ a significant complication: If participants res-
decisions, they filled out the brief prematch ponded to the identical set of items about each
questionnaire, which consisted of an array of of their matches, the follow-up questionnaires
items (e.g., ‘‘How many matches do you esti- would become disproportionately onerous for
mate you will get?’’ ‘‘Overall, how satisfied participants who had a large number of mat-
were you with the people you met?’’ and ‘‘I ches, potentially resulting in systematic bias in
enjoyed my speed-dating experience’’). Partic- participant retention. Two additional compli-
ipants also indicated the degree to which each of cations also would have arisen if participants
the 12 characteristics assessed on the interaction always answered the identical set of items
records influenced their ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ deci- about each of their matches: (a) It would have
sions, and they were provided an open-ended resulted in repetitive responding for partici-
opportunity to give us feedback about their pants who had neither corresponded with nor
speed-dating experience. intended to correspond with a particular match
as their opinions or attitudes about the match
Postmatch questionnaire. The evening after would have been unlikely to change in the in-
the speed-dating event, participants returned to tervening 72 hours and (b) many questions
our Web site to find out with whom they had (e.g., ‘‘I feel uncertain about [first name]’s true
matched. Immediately after learning about their feelings for me’’) would be irrelevant or poten-
matches, they filled out the brief postmatch tially bizarre if the participant had no romantic
questionnaire asking four general questions interest in the match.
about their satisfaction with their matches and We dealt with these concerns by (a) creat-
their self-views (e.g., ‘‘I am happy about my ing three different sets of questions that could
matches’’) and several questions regarding their apply to each match and (b) incorporating pro-
reactions to each of their matches (e.g., ‘‘I am cedures designed to have participants answer
extremely excited that I matched with [first only those questions that were relevant to a
name],’’ ‘‘I am very likely to initiate contact particular match. Specifically, the measures
with [first name],’’ ‘‘I hope that [first name] regarding each match were customized on
initiates contact with me’’). each follow-up wave depending on the partic-
ipants’ answers to the following pivot ques-
Follow-up questionnaires. The follow-up tion: ‘‘What is the current status of your
questionnaires were designed to assess how relationship with [first name]?’’ Participants
162 E. J. Finkel, P. W. Eastwick, and J. Matthews

were given the following response options to friend without romantic potential, (f) acquain-
the question: (a) dating seriously, (b) dating tance without romantic potential, and (g) no re-
casually, (c) friend with romantic potential, lationship at all. Table 1 provides an overview
(d) acquaintance with romantic potential, (e) of how participants’ responses to this pivot

Table 1. The structure of the match-specific section of the follow-up questionnaires

Questions Pivot answer Sample questionnaire items


Set 1 1. Dating seriously d ‘‘[First name] could have a romantic
2. Dating casually relationship with just about
3. Friend with romantic anyone he/she wanted’’
potential d ‘‘I think that [first name] is romantically

4. Acquaintance with interested in me’’


romantic potential d ‘‘Have you hung out with [first name]

5. Friend without in person or corresponded with [first name]


romantic potential not in person (e-mail, IM, phone, etc.)?’’
6. Acquaintance without d Participants indicated the degree to which

romantic potential this match possessed each of the


7. No relationship at alla 12 characteristics included on the
interaction records.

Set 2 1. Dating seriously d ‘‘If [first name] were going through a


2. Dating casually difficult time, I would put away my own
3. Friend with romantic potential concerns to help him/her out’’
4. Acquaintance with d ‘‘[First name] cares about my needs’’

romantic potential d ‘‘I feel comfortable opening up to

5. Friend without [first name]’’


romantic potential d ‘‘[First name] helps me become who

6. Acquaintance without I ideally want to be—s/he elicits


romantic potential the best in me’’

Set 3 1. Dating seriously d ‘‘I would like to have a serious romantic


2. Dating casually relationship with [first name]’’
3. Friend with d ‘‘I would like to have a one-night stand

romantic potential with [first name]’’


4. Acquaintance with d ‘‘I feel uncertain about [first name]’s

romantic potential true feelings for me’’


d ‘‘Have you engaged in any romantic

physical contact (kissing or other sexual


activities) with [first name]?’’

Note: Participants answered a different array of questionnaire items about each match at a given follow-up wave
depending upon their responses to the pivot question: ‘‘What is the current status of your relationship with [first name]?’’
The ‘‘pivot answer’’ column indicates which answers caused the program to present participants with a given question
set. The three question sets are ordered hierarchically, such that all participants who answered Set 2 items about a given
match also answered Set 1 items, and all participants who answered Set 3 items also answered Set 1 and Set 2 items.
Questionnaire items phrased as declarative statements were assessed on scales ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7
(strongly agree).
a
To avoid redundancy, if participants indicated that they had ‘‘no relationship at all’’ with a particular match on two or
more consecutive follow-up questionnaires, they were asked no additional questions about him or her (not even those in
Set 1). This procedure allowed us to get basic information regarding all matches at least once while avoiding burdening
participants unnecessarily by having them repeatedly report on a match with whom they had no relationship.
Speed-dating primer 163

question influenced the questions they answered speed-dating events might be dissimilar to
regarding that match at that wave. those that emerge in relationships initiated
Before concluding this discussion of the through other means and (b) participants who
follow-up questionnaires, we mention a final volunteer for a speed-dating study might con-
aspect of the procedure that investigators con- stitute a highly unusual sample.
ducting speed-dating research might also want Although both of these concerns are valid,
to consider incorporating into their own we suggest that neither should dissuade inter-
studies: Participants in the NSDS reported on ested researchers from conducting their own
romantic dynamics regarding individuals speed-dating studies at this time. In response
whom they met via avenues other than speed- to the first (processes) concern, it is worth not-
dating. We referred to these non–speed-dating ing that, unlike most laboratory-based proce-
romantic interests as ‘‘write-ins’’ and asked par- dures, speed-dating is a mainstream activity
ticipants a series of questions about them. that millions of people actually engage in out-
Information gleaned from the write-ins is valu- side the lab. In many cities, a typical speed-
able because it provides a more comprehen- dating company will host events weekly—
sive picture of relationship dynamics than even more frequently in major metropolitan
would have emerged had we maintained an centers such as New York or Los Angeles.
exclusive focus on participants’ relationships More importantly (and as noted earlier),
with other speed-daters. Furthermore, this pro- speed-dating shares several core features in
cedure ensured that all participants, even those common with other forms of real-world dat-
who had unsuccessful speed-dating experien- ing: Speed-daters are both evaluating others
ces, were able to provide important data about and being evaluated themselves, and, as in
their romantic lives when they completed a fol- many social situations in which individuals
low-up questionnaire. In addition to being meet potential romantic partners (e.g., bars,
interesting in its own right, this information parties, singles cruises, church social events),
about other romantic interests can also provide they meet a variety of potential romantic part-
insight into the source of sudden shifts in ners simultaneously. Finally, although it is
romantic interest in a particular speed-dating unknown whether speed-dating findings
match. would generalize to people meeting at a bar,
for example, it is equally unknown whether
meeting someone at a bar is similar to meeting
Potential Limitations of
someone on a softball team or at a dinner
Speed-Dating Research
party. To be sure, these are all social gather-
Thus far, we have served as unabashed supporters ings, and whether or not they differ systemat-
of the scientific potential of speed-dating ically from one another in ways that alter
procedures. As with all other methodolog- romantic processes is an interesting research
ies, however, there are potential limitations question, not an inherent limitation with
of speed-dating research. These potential limi- research designs that focus on one particular
tations can be divided into three categories: setting.
external validity, efficacy, and stigma. The second concern is that speed-dating
participants could constitute a highly unrepre-
sentative sample. Even if the results of
External validity
a speed-dating study accurately describe how
When researchers attempt to study any real- these particular individuals initiate relation-
world phenomenon (including speed-dating) ships across social settings, speed-daters might
in a controlled setting, external validity can be unusual, and perhaps we would not want to
potentially be compromised. There are two se- assume that their behavior describes what
parate reasons why generalizability might be ‘‘normal people’’ do. Although we acknowl-
threatened specifically in a speed-dating edge that speed-dating participants might be
study: (a) attraction and relationship develop- unusual in important and systematic ways,
ment processes that emerge during and after we observe that plausible a priori hypotheses
164 E. J. Finkel, P. W. Eastwick, and J. Matthews

are often contradictory regarding the ways in criteria should be used to validate speed-
which speed-daters are likely to be unusual. dating’s efficacy. It is clear that individuals
For example, it is easy to imagine that speed- who married, had children, and lived happily
dating samples are comprised of highly extra- ever after with a partner they met through
verted individuals who greatly enjoy meeting speed-dating would count as a success, but what
and talking with new people. Yet, it is equally about individuals who spent 8 months or even
easy to imagine that speed-daters are people 2 weeks in a romantic relationship with a partner
who have difficulty initiating conversations in they met through speed-dating—would such
more traditional social situations and look to outcomes count as successes?
speed-dating as a way around this difficulty. Although the extant literature cannot provide
The sanguine truth is that both of these definitive answers for any of these questions,
generalizability concerns can ultimately be ad- data from the NSDS shed some preliminary light
dressed empirically. One way we are striving on the issue of speed-dating efficacy. On the
to address the first (processes) concern is by follow-up questionnaires, participants indicated
examining whether the NSDS revealed sys- whether they had corresponded or hung out with
tematic differences between follow-up reports each match during each 3-day interval (see
of fellow speed-daters and follow-up reports of Table 1). Across the 10 waves, speed-daters
write-ins, as such differences could suggest reported 579 instances of corresponding or
that there is something unique or unusual hanging out with a match, and the substantial
about meeting a potential romantic partner at majority of these reports (78%) were with peo-
a speed-dating event. In a preliminary exami- ple they had not known prior to the speed-dating
nation of this issue, we found no evidence for event. At first glance, 579 seems like a large
systematic differences between the two groups number of potential romantic interactions, sug-
(Eastwick & Finkel, 2007). Researchers could gesting that speed-dating does indeed promote
address the second concern (sample) by studying contact between speed-daters in the wake of the
whether people who are willing or eager to try event (although there is no guarantee that this
speed-dating differ in systematic ways from par- was romantic contact). Upon reflection, how-
ticipants who are not willing or not eager to do so. ever, it becomes apparent that efficacy questions
Ultimately, concerns about generalizability are unanswerable at this stage because it is not
are appropriate, especially insofar as they gen- obvious what the appropriate base rate compar-
erate additional research questions that can ison should be. How often are romantic relation-
help highlight how and why people might dif- ships spawned when two singles meet at church
fer in their approach to dating. There is no or in a college chemistry class? If 1 out of every
evidence at this time, however, suggesting that 100 speed-daters eventually forms a serious
these concerns seriously threaten the use of relationship with another speed-dater, would
speed-dating as a research tool for understand- that be impressive? The question of speed-dat-
ing romantic relationship initiation. ing efficacy is fascinating, but it will likely be
some time before researchers have a handle on
the base rate of romantic relationship initiation
Efficacy
in any context, speed-dating included.
The second possible limitation of speed-dating
research is whether the events actually create
Stigma
romantic experiences of scholarly value to re-
lationship scientists. We have expressed our Although many other types of relationships
hopes that speed-dating, through the creation research (e.g., clinical interventions for sexual
of actual romantic relationships, might ulti- dysfunction) could be stigmatizing, speed-dat-
mately help to connect the fields of initial ing differs from most of these in that it is un-
romantic attraction and close relationships re- usually public. If individuals participate in a
search, but we acknowledge that there exist speed-dating session with 10 men, 10 women,
heretofore almost no efficacy data to back up and 5 members of the research staff, then at
this optimism. In fact, it is not clear which least 24 other people will know that they
Speed-dating primer 165

participated. Granted, all of the other partici- self versus indiscriminately toward all the
pants will be in the same situation, but this speed-daters at the session (Eastwick et al.,
could be little comfort to individuals who might 2007). We are currently conducting follow-up
be embarrassed about their participation. experiments to discern exactly how participants
Given that a speed-dating study had never are able to make these fine-grained distinctions
(to our knowledge) been conducted with an un- after only 4 minutes. As a second example, the
dergraduate student sample prior to the NSDS, NSDS data revealed that participants’ self-
we experienced considerable concern that few reported romantic partner preferences at pre-
individuals would sign up to participate, leav- test did a poor job of predicting whom they
ing us with a small and quirky sample. One of actually liked at and after the speed-dating
our primary goals as we began planning for the event (Eastwick & Finkel, 2007; also see
NSDS was making it a cool event on campus Iyengar et al., 2005). We are currently conduct-
rather than having it seem like a nerdy alterna- ing follow-up experiments investigating what it
tive to a real social life. We strived to generate is about meeting and getting to know a potential
‘‘buzz’’ by means of an intensive publicity romantic partner that causes individuals not to
campaign and a collaborative relationship with compare this person with their preexisting ideal
a student organization. We have reason to partner template. These two examples represent
believe that our efforts were effective at elim- just the tip of the iceberg and are included to
inating much of the stigma potential. Not only provide illustrations of the sorts of research pro-
were we forced to turn away hundreds of inter- grams that can be enhanced by the high quality
ested participants after our events had filled, and plentiful quantity of data provided by
but several favorable write-ups appeared in speed-dating studies.
the Northwestern daily newspaper, with one In sum, speed-dating has rapidly become an
explicitly thanking the first author of this report international phenomenon, helping individuals
for doing something to help the local dating meet compatible romantic partners in diverse
scene. Our intuition is that students at many nations, from Japan (e.g., tokyospeeddating.com)
(if not most) institutions also believe that their to South Africa (e.g., xfactordates.com), and
school has ‘‘no dating scene’’; therefore, there among diverse populations, such as devout
is a good chance that a similarly conducted ad Muslims (MacFarquhar, 2006). We have ar-
campaign would be effective at reducing gued that speed-dating could well serve as
speed-dating’s stigma potential among many a significant methodological innovation for
undergraduate populations. the science of initial romantic attraction (see
Eastwick & Finkel, in press). Speed-dating
could help investigators unravel relationship
Preliminary NSDS Findings and
initiation processes (without depending upon
Concluding Remarks
retrospective reports) among a sample of indi-
The present report provides a conceptual and viduals who are motivated and eager to meet
methodological primer for investigators inter- potential romantic partners. It could also help
ested in conducting their own speed-dating them apply to initial attraction research some
studies. We hope that speed-dating research of the best elements of close relationships
catches on among scholars interested in study- research: the emphasis on (potentially) mean-
ing initial romantic attraction or early relation- ingful relationships and the use of dyadic and
ship development (or both), as we have found longitudinal data collection procedures. Fin-
it to be an extremely generative method for ally, speed-dating has the potential to enhance
such pursuits. Thus far, the NSDS has led us the landscape of research on initial romantic
to many interesting findings that have since attraction and link the fields of attraction and
spawned full-fledged programs of research. close relationships research into a single, com-
As one example, an SRM analysis (Kenny, prehensive field of inquiry. This merger could
1994) of the NSDS data revealed that partici- bring important age-old questions into focus
pants can distinguish between another’s roman- (i.e., whether the predictors of attraction are
tic desire that is directed uniquely toward the compatible with those that predict a successful
166 E. J. Finkel, P. W. Eastwick, and J. Matthews

marriage) and highlight critical theoretical and and revealed preferences. Paper presented at the
annual meeting of the Society of Personality and
practical directions for future research. Social Psychology (SPSP), New Orleans, LA.
Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception: A social
relations analysis. New York: Guilford.
References Kenny, D. A., & La Voie, L. (1984). The social relations
model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimen-
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a minute: Pre- tal social psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 142–182). Orlando,
dicting teacher evaluations from thin slices of nonver- FL: Academic.
bal behavior and physical attractiveness. Journal of Kline, K., Kline, D., & Hunt, B. (2004). SQL in a nutshell
Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 431–441. (2nd ed.), Sebastopol, CA, O’Reilly Media.
Aron, A., Melinat, E., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R., & Bator, R. Kurzban, R., & Weeden, J. (2005). HurryDate: Mate pref-
(1997). The experimental generation of interpersonal erences in action. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26,
closeness: A procedure and some preliminary find- 227–244.
ings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, MacFarquhar, N. (2006, September 19). It’s Muslim boy
363–377. meets girl, but don’t call it dating. New York Times ,
Berscheid, E., & Regan, P. (2005). The psychology of in- p. A1.
terpersonal relationships. New York: Prentice-Hall. Metts, S., Sprecher, S., & Cupach, W. R. (1991). Retrospec-
Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: tive self-reports. In B. M. Montgomery & S. Duck (Eds.)
Academic Press. Studying interpersonal interaction (pp. 162–178). New
Byrne, D., Ervin, C. E., & Lamberth, J. (1970). Continuity York: Guilford Press.
between the experimental study of attraction and real- Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical
life computer dating. Journal of Personality and Social linear models: Applications and data analysis methods
Psychology, 16, 157–165. (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2007). Sex differences in Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., Reeder, G. D., & Elliot, A.
mate preferences revisited: Do people really know J. (1999). The relationship closeness induction task.
what they desire in a romantic partner? Manuscript Representative Research in Social Psychology, 23, 1–4.
submitted for publication. Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal
Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (in press). Speed-dating: A data analysis. New York: Oxford University Press.
powerful and flexible paradigm for studying relation- Sprecher, S., & Duck, S. (1994). Sweet talk: The im-
ship initiation. In S. Sprecher, A. Wenzel, & J. Harvey portance of perceived communication for romantic
(Eds.), Handbook of relationship beginnings. New and friendship attraction experienced during a get-
York: Guilford Press. acquainted date. Personality and Social Psychology
Eastwick, P. W., Finkel, E. J., Mochon, D., & Ariely, D. Bulletin, 20, 391–400.
(2007). Selective versus unselective romantic desire: Not Stanczyk, S., Champion, B., & Leyton, R. (2001). Theory
all reciprocity is created equal. Psychological Science, and practice of relational databases (2nd ed.). New
18, 317–319. York: Taylor & Francis.
Fisman, R., Iyengar, S. S., Kamenica, E., & Simonson, I. Thaler, R. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer
(2006). Searching for a mate: Evidence from a speed- choice. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organiza-
dating experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, tion, 1, 39–60.
121, 673–697. Walster, E., Aronson, V., Abrahams, D., & Rottman, L.
Iyengar, S. S., Simonson, I., Fisman, R., & Mogilner, C. (1966). Importance of physical attractiveness in dating
(2005, January). I know what I want but can I find it?: behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Examining the dynamic relationship between stated ogy, 4, 508–516.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai