Anda di halaman 1dari 54

 

 
 

 
 
 

 
Factors  Affecting  Reading  Ability  
 

  in  School  Age  Children    


 

 
Prepared  by  Strategic  Marketing  &  Research,  Inc.  
 
February  2013  
   

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


 
MARKETING & RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
 

Contents  
 
Purpose 2
Background 2
Barriers to reading development 4
Required competencies 5
Importance of motivation 8
Theories on motivation 9
Factors for change 15
Importance of knowledge 17
Effects of individual temperaments 19
Average and gifted students 20
Average students 21
Gifted students 22
Motivating behaviors and interventions 23
Collecting instincts 25
Rewards as motivation 28
Triarchic instruction 29
Motivating factors reported by students 30
Technology’s motivational role 31
E-books 34
Gaming 35
Online collaboration 36
Email as a motivational tool 38

References 41

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING 1  &   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Purpose  

This secondary research report is intended to serve as a benchmark against which existing
reading enhancement programs and software can be compared. It can also serve to inform the
development of new programs and software superior to those now in the marketplace.
 
 
Background

Reading failure is a serious national problem and cannot simply be attributed to poverty,
immigration, or the learning of English as a second language.

A Statement to the Committee on Labor and Human Resources revealed that 44% of the nation’s
fourth-grade children had little to no mastery of the knowledge and skills necessary to perform
reading activities at the fourth-grade level (Lyon, 1998). By 2009, the percentage reading below
grade level had gradually improved to 33% but has remained unchanged through 2012.

Two-thirds of eighth-graders do not read at grade level. Only one-third of all students entering
high school are proficient in reading. Within this group, only about 15 percent of African-
American students and 17 percent of Hispanic students are proficient.

Boys lag behind girls in reading proficiency in all 50 states – in some states by as many as 10
percentage points.

Between 1971 and 2004, the NAEP scores of 12th-graders showed no improvement. Further, the
2005 scores of 12th-graders were generally lower than their counterparts in 1992.

Fifty percent of reading difficulties are believed to be preventable if students are given effective
language development experiences in preschool and kindergarten and effective reading
instruction in the primary grades (Slavin et al., 1996).

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING 2  &   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
A study by Yankelovich found most children are reading but they are not reading enough.
Despite the importance placed on reading for fun, only about 3 in 10 children can be classified as
high frequency readers who read books for fun every day.

– Age 8 is the critical drop-off age for reading engagement. Older children are less likely to
see benefits to reading and are less engaged in reading for fun.

– Data also indicate girls are more likely than boys to have positive attitudes about reading
and to regularly engage in reading for fun.

– The benefits of reading are evidenced by the attitudes of high frequency readers.
Compared to others, they are more likely to have positive self-perceptions and to
associate strong reading skills with future success.

– More than 40% of children ages 5 to 8 say they are high frequency readers; by ages 9 to
11 that proportion drops to 29%.

– Almost half of the 15- to 17-year olds (46%) are low frequency readers, compared with
14% of 5- to 8-year-olds.

Reader groups are defined as: High = Reads every day; Medium = Reads 4-6 times a week to
once a week, Low = 2-3 times a month or less (Yankelovich).

For students to develop into effective readers, they must possess both the skill and the will to
read. As noted by Guthrie and Wigfield (2000), “motivation is what activates behavior.”
Children’s motivation is increasingly multidimensional in the later elementary grades.

   

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING 3  &   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Barriers  to  reading  development    

Developmental factors and students’ perceptions about their own abilities also play into their
level of engagement with learning. The older students get, the less likely they are to take risks
and engage themselves fully in activities at which they are not sure they will succeed. According
to Lumsden (1994), young children tend to maintain high expectations for success even in the
face of repeated failure, while older students do not. To older students, failure following high
effort appears to carry more negative implications—especially for their self-concept of ability—
than failure that results from minimal or no effort (Lumsden, 1994).

Students’ attitudes about their capabilities and their interpretation of success and failure further
affect their willingness to engage themselves in learning (Anderman & Midgley, 1998). For
example, students who understand poor performance as a lack of attainable skills, rather than as
some innate personal deficiency, are more likely to re-engage themselves in a task and try again.
Students whose self-concept is bound up in their history of failure, on the other hand, are less
likely to be motivated to learn.

Motivated readers hold positive beliefs about themselves as readers (Guthrie & Wigfield, 1997).
Conversely, struggling readers assume they are responsible for their reading difficulties.

MacIver and Reuman (1994) add that middle school and high school-age students’ level of
engagement in school is also highly influenced by peers. As students grow older, their
motivation to engage in learning may be influenced by their social group just as much as, if not
more than, it is by teachers, parents, and other adults.

Recurring failures to succeed and self-concept issues often complicate a student’s ability to learn
any of a variety of reading skills. Schunk and Zimmerman (1997) found that students who doubt
their ability to learn give up quickly when faced with new challenges.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING 4  &   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Berliner (1981) found that success rates had a substantial impact on student learning. His studies
produced strong, consistent evidence that tasks completed with high rates of success were clearly
linked to greater learning and improved student attitudes, while tasks where students were
moderately successful were less consistently related to learning, and hard tasks had a negative
impact on learning.
 
Required  competencies

The ultimate goal in teaching reading is to have students comprehend the ideas in a piece of text
as they read. Reading helps children expand their thinking skills, learn to concentrate, and
enlarge their vocabulary and effectively master their environment.

Reading is a complex act that requires many years of experience and use in order to do well.
Many “models” of reading have been offered, each attempting to describe the essential
components of skilled reading. Reading can be described at many levels, from the neurological
to the psychological to the sociological. It is useful to think of skilled English readers as
individuals who:
• Understand and use the “alphabetic principle,” that is, how the sounds of spoken speech
can be represented by letters in the alphabet and how letters are used to identify printed
words;
• Have and use background knowledge, knowledge of words, and comprehension strategies
to obtain meaning from print;
• Read fluently and effortlessly, except when they come across unfamiliar text, when they
consciously use the alphabetic principle and comprehension strategies to make sense of
what they are reading; and;
• Are motivated to do all of the above on a regular and sustained basis.

Although reading is often thought of as a matter relating to the first component listed above, that
is, using letters in words to read or pronounce words, skilled reading involves much more.
Successful readers attend to all the relevant dimensions of reading, not simply word recognition,
as important as it is. Successful reading and writing also involves comprehension, fluency,
mastery of essential strategies, and motivation.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING 5  &   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Learning to read English presents particular challenges that are not seen in other languages and
reading systems. English is based upon the alphabetic principle. Alphabetic systems, (as
opposed to syllabic systems such as Japanese), represent sounds that are meaningless in and of
themselves but which acquire meaning when the sound units are combined. Before they can
begin to read or spell, children must understand that written words are made up of sounds or
phonemes.

Research indicates that instruction in phonological awareness should begin in preschool and
kindergarten so that children can learn that written words represent spoken sounds. This process
is known as “decoding” and means breaking down a word into its separate sounds. To decode,
children need to be explicitly taught how to sound out words into phonemes. As children
become more adept at decoding and reading words, they are able to focus on building their
comprehension.

Fluency comes when a student recognizes more and more words on sight. The more words are
recognized by sight the fewer the reading errors made. If a student is struggling with decoding,
or if she does not have good phonological skills, fluency and comprehension will suffer.

The problems children experience learning to read are often not related to their ability to learn
but to their phonological awareness; their ability to “hear” the English language and their
exposure to the English alphabet. Rhyming words and repeating lines help children develop an
“ear” for language—by appreciating the sounds of words.

The more a child is exposed to reading, the more likely the child is to acquire the requisite skills
for reading. Children must learn that words on a page have meaning and that reading is done
from left to right and from top to bottom.

Children who have not been read to before they enter school may not have experience listening
to rhythms and sounds. They may also not have developed an interest in reading; that is, they

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING 6  &   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
may not be motivated to learn to read. Children who have been read to in childhood are more
motivated to learn to read, and they appreciate that reading is a gateway to new ideas.

The enjoyment of reading comes from comprehension, however, and not decoding words.
Skilled readers “interact” with a text, thinking about what will happen next, creating questions
about the main characters and so on. Kids who love to read have good comprehension skills.
 
Elements  of  Reading  Comprehension:      
• identifying and summarizing the main idea
• comparing and contrasting
• identifying supporting facts and details
• making inferences and drawing conclusions
• predicting outcomes
• recognizing fact and opinion
• recognizing realism versus fantasy
• identifying cause and effect
• recognizing sequence of events
• identifying story elements such as main characters, settings, plot, conflict, and
resolution
• identifying the author’s purpose and point of view
• interpreting literary devices such as imagery, symbolism ,and metaphors

A study of third- and fifth-graders and eighth- and 10-graders by Guthrie, Wigfield, Metsala and
Cox (2009), found connections between the amount of time spent reading and comprehension
and between the motivation to read and the time spent reading. After statistically controlling for
reading achievement, prior topic knowledge, self-efficacy, and reading motivation, the study of
third- and fifth-graders showed reading motivation significantly predicted the amount of reading.
The study of older students (after similar statistical controls were imposed) found that, as with
the younger group, motivation predicted both the amount of time spent reading, and the amount
of time spent reading significantly predicted text comprehension (Guthrie, Wigfield, Metsala &
Cox, 2009).

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING 7  &   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Importance  of  motivation    

Student motivation “refers to a student’s willingness, need, desire and compulsion to participate
in, and be successful in, the learning process” (Bomia et al., 1997).

Skinner and Belmont (1991) develop the definition further, noting that students who are
motivated to engage in school “select tasks at the border of their competencies, initiate action
when given the opportunity, and exert intense effort and concentration in the implementation of
learning tasks; they show generally positive emotions during ongoing action, including
enthusiasm, optimism, curiosity, and interest.”

Motivation is the key to developing successful readers. It also affects how students approach
school in general, how they relate to teachers, how much time and effort they devote to their
studies, how much support they seek when they are struggling, how they perform on tests, and
many other aspects of education. If students are not motivated, it is difficult, if not impossible,
to improve their academic achievement, no matter how good the teacher, curriculum or school is.

Means, Jonassen, and Dwyer (1997) cite studies that showing motivation accounts for 16% to
38% of the variations in overall student achievement.

Motivation often declines as students progress from elementary through high school. Upwards of
40% of high school students are disengaged from learning, are inattentive, exert little effort on
school work, and report being bored in school, according to a 2004 analysis by the National
Research Council.

Metsala, Wigfield, and McCann (1997) found that young children, who are positively motivated,
have a strong sense of their personal competency and efficacy. Less motivated or disengaged
students, on the other hand, “are passive, do not try hard, and give up easily in the face of
challenges” (Skinner & Belmont, 1991).

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING 8  &   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Higher motivation to learn is linked not only to better academic performance but also to greater
conceptual understanding, satisfaction with school, self-esteem, social adjustment, and school
completion rates.

The amount of motivation that a student has for reading determines whether the learning derived
will be meaningful, deep, and internalized, or if it will be trivial (Gambrell, Morrow & Pressley,
2007).
 
 
Theories  on  motivation  

Theories abound on motivation. It has been defined on one extreme as the product of
environmental conditioning—of deprivation and reinforcement schedules. (Skinner, 1953) On
the other extreme, individuals like Carl Rogers (1951) and Abraham Maslow (1954) adopt a
humanistic approach where motivation is almost entirely a byproduct of free will and an internal
drive for self-actualization. Bandura’s (1969) social learning theory argued that learning and
motivation were a result not just of the environment or the individual’s free will but a
combination of the two. Gagné (1985) advised that both internal and external conditions existed
that influenced learning and motivation.

Wigfield (1994) developed the expectancy-value model of achievement choice as a framework


for understanding young children’s and adolescent’s choices and performance in mathematics.
Bembenutty (2008) discusses Jacquelynne Eccles expectancy-value theory and describes how
this model demonstrates that people are most likely to do things they perceive that they are good
at doing and when they consider what they are doing high in value. Eccles and Wigfield (2002)
believe that the expectations for an individual’s performance and the amount of value that a
person places on the task is highly socialized according to cultural settings in which one grows
up.
Motivation is difficult to define and measure, but scholars generally recognize two major types:
intrinsic and extrinsic. Intrinsic motivation is the desire to do or achieve something because one
truly wants to and takes pleasure or sees value in doing so.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING 9  &   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Extrinsic motivation is the desire to do or achieve something not so much for the enjoyment of
the activity itself, but because it will produce a certain result. The difference between the two is
more like a spectrum than a divide; any action can be motivated by a combination of intrinsic
and extrinsic factors, and the same person may be motivated differently in different contexts.

According to Dev (1997), “A student who is intrinsically motivated . . . will not need any type of
reward or incentive to initiate or complete a task. This type of student is more likely to complete
the chosen task and be excited by the challenging nature of an activity.” There is compelling
evidence that students who are more intrinsically than extrinsically motivated fare better (Brooks
et al., 1998; Lumsden, 1994).

Pachtman and Wilson (2006) found that motivation can develop from intrinsic or extrinsic
stimuli. Intrinsic motivation is developed through the choice of literacy activities based on
individual interest and the child’s beliefs that he/she can successfully complete the reading task.
Lapp and Douglas (2009) expand on this notion, finding that peer influence and intrinsic
motivation are primary factors associated with encouraging teens to want to read.

When students read for aesthetic reasons (Rosenblatt, 2005), they are motivated because the
reading provokes feelings, ideas, and attitudes that are linked through private, past experiences.
Therefore, when students’ readings evoke connections to individual responses, they will be more
likely to want to continue to read.

Metsala, Wigfield, and McCann (1997) suggest that if children are intrinsically motivated to read
and self-satisfied, they will increase the frequency with which they read.

Pachtman and Wilson (2006) discovered from their research findings that supplying ample books
in the class library, allowing students to choose their own books, participating in book counts
and celebrations, being able to visit the class library every day, and recording books in book logs
were the top results from student questionnaires and surveys on student motivation.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING10  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Intrinsically motivated students:
• Earn higher grades and achievement test scores, on average, than extrinsically motivated
students (Dev, 1997; Skinner & Belmont, 1991)
• Are better personally adjusted to school (Skinner & Belmont, 1991)
• Employ “strategies that demand more effort and that enable them to process information
more deeply” (Lumsden, 1994, p. 2)
• Are more likely to feel confident about their ability to learn new material (Dev, 1997)
• Use “more logical information-gathering and decision-making strategies” than do
extrinsically motivated students (Lumsden, 1994, p. 2)
• Are more likely to engage in “tasks that are moderately challenging, whereas
extrinsically oriented students gravitate toward tasks that are low in degree of difficulty”
(Lumsden, 1994, p.2)
• Are more likely to persist with and complete assigned tasks (Dev, 1997)
• Retain information and concepts longer, and are less likely to need remedial courses and
review (Dev, 1997)
• Are more likely to be lifelong learners, continuing to educate themselves outside the
formal school setting long after external motivators such as grades and diplomas are
removed (Kohn, 1993)

Some research demonstrates that using extrinsic motivators to engage students in learning can
both lower achievement and negatively affect student motivation (Dev, 1997; Lumsden, 1994).
Students who are motivated to complete a task only to avoid consequences or to earn a certain
grade rarely exert more than the minimum effort necessary to meet their goal. And when students
are focused on comparing themselves with their classmates, rather than on mastering skills at
their own rate, they are more easily discouraged and their intrinsic motivation to learn may
actually decrease. Brooks et al. (1998) observe that while external rewards sustain productivity,
they “decrease interest in the task, thereby diminishing the likelihood that the task will be
continued in the future.”

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING11  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
It should be noted here that there are also researchers who object to describing student
motivation as either intrinsic or extrinsic. Sternberg and Lubart (as cited in Strong, Silver, &
Robinson, 1995) for example, argue that this division is too simple to reflect the many complex
and interrelated factors that influence students’ motivation to succeed in school. They point out
that most successful people are motivated by both internal and external factors.

As Bandura explains, “perceived efficacy is a judgment of capability; self-esteem is a judgment


of self-worth” (Bandura, 2006; Mohajer & Earnest, 2009). High self-efficacy has been shown to
predict better performance in academics and sports; increased happiness, job satisfaction, and
persistence; improved safe sex practices; and successful smoking cessation and prevention (de
Vries et al., 1988; Judge & Bono, 2001; Kalichman & Nachimson, 1999; Martin & Gill, 1991;
Multon et al., 1991; Natvig et al., 2003). A meta-analysis conducted by Stajkovic and Luthans
(1998) found that self-efficacy accounted for a 28% improvement in work-related performance.

Evidence suggests that high self-esteem is related to high social support and resilience (Dumont
& Provost, 1999; Hoffman et al., 1988), whereas low self-esteem is related to depression,
anxiety, and suicidal ideation (Newbegin & Owens, 1996; Overholser et al., 1995; Rosenberg et
al., 1995). Boden and colleagues (2008) found self-esteem to be an important “risk marker
variable, with low self-esteem being associated with a range of negative outcomes.”

Gambrell, Palmer, Codling, and Mazzoni (1996) found that the amount of motivation a student
has to read depends on the amount of value students place on the particular reading task. These
researchers discuss the expectancy component of Eccles’s theory of motivation because it
extends the belief that students need to find personal value in reading. Students who believe they
are capable and knowledgeable readers will have more success than students who do not have
such positive thoughts.

Hidi (2001) found interest was a clear indicator of the quality of learning derived from reading.
When students’ interests were activated, readers were encouraged to go beyond the surface
elements of the text and focus on more elaborate, higher-order thinking skills, to help them

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING12  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
uncover the underlying meaning of the main ideas. Flowerday, Schraw, and Stevens (2004)
emphasize that materials that students find interesting affect the application and transfer of
deeper text processing. What students find interesting they consider meaningful (Fink &
Samuels, 2008).

Components of reading motivation identified by Baker and Wigfield et al., (1999) include:
(a) self-efficacy, (b) interest, (c) preference for challenge, and (d) social interaction (Baker &
Wigfield, 1999; Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997).

Bandura et al., (1996) finds there are four dimensions that define reading motivation. These are
defined below. At least one of these must be present for a student to be motivated (Bandura,
1996; Dweck, 2010; Murray, 2011; Pintrich, 2003; Ryan & Deci, 2000; Seifert, 2004)

• Competence — the student believes he or she has the ability to complete the task.

• Control/autonomy — the student feels in control by seeing a direct link between his
or her actions and an outcome and retains autonomy by having some choice about
whether or how to undertake the task.

• Interest/value — the student has some interest in the task or sees the value of
completing it.

• Relatedness — completing the task brings the student social rewards, such as a sense
of belonging to a classroom or other desired social group or approval from a person
of social importance to the student.

Christensen, Horn, and Johnson (2010) believe efforts to motivate students often founder
because the incentives are defined from the educator's point of view and not the student's.
They hypothesize that there are two core goals most students try to achieve every day: To feel
successful and make progress, and to have fun with friends. They note that educators operate as
if the delivery of education – their product — is the objective. As a result, the activities in
schools generally are not integrated in ways to help students be successful every day. The key
events embedded within typical curricula that might help students feel successful—examinations

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING13  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
and grades—are not sufficiently related in time to the performance they measure to reinforce the
desired behaviors (Christensen, Horn, & Johnson, 2010).

The researchers posit that all students are equally motivated to feel successful. For some, school
is a viable means for achieving this goal. This group likely includes those whose parents provide
a clear link between academic achievement and career success; those whose intellectual
capacities were honed through repeated, sophisticated verbal interaction with adults before the
age of three; and those whose way of learning or interests match that of their particular teachers.

Students who do not obtain feelings of success from school are not unmotivated to feel
successful. They simply do not or cannot feel successful at school, which often makes them feel
like failures. Correctly integrating activities in ways that would help students feel successful
every day might include a “project-based” learning strategy, in which students are organized into
teams and then undertake meaningful projects that require them to master the reading, writing,
math, science, and social science skills that the school wants them to learn. This method
integrates the delivery of curriculum with experiences that enable students to feel successful and
have fun with their friends every day (Christensen, Horn, & Johnson, 2010).

Computer-based learning can also be an important mechanism for achieving student-centric


learning and is discussed later in this report. By its very nature, software allows achievement to
be integrated with the delivery of content in ways that help students feel successful while they
learn. Achievement comes from completing reviews or examinations that are built into the
software. These require students to demonstrate mastery before they can move to the next body
of material. Feedback can be delivered frequently and in bite-sized pieces, as necessary, to help
each student feel successful.

   

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING14  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Factors  for  change

An ample body of research suggests that reading motivation and consequently performance can
be changed for the better (Brooks, Freiburger & Grotheer, 1998; Dev, 1997; Skinner & Belmont,
1991).

Focusing on student interests in selecting reading materials may be more beneficial in promoting
reading success than a focus on reading level. It turns out that interest is far more significant than
readability. When students have strong interest in what they read, they can frequently transcend
their reading level (Worthy, 1996). Many educators and researchers consider interest to be an
essential factor in all learning (Hidi, 1990; Schiefele, 1991).

Hidi (2001) found that all types of interests – topic and situational – serve as powerful
determinants that contributed to students’ increased recognition, comprehension, and recall.
Interest was a clear indicator of the quality of learning derived.

Under the right circumstances, a challenging task can be motivating. It often requires students to
use prior knowledge and construct an understanding of a topic. This practice increases the
personal meaning that students attach to an activity, therefore, increasing the likelihood of
becoming engaged in an activity (Miller, 2003).

Vygotsky (1978) argues that one internalizes the higher cognitive abilities applicable to reading
through social interaction. He perceived learning as a profoundly social process, suggesting that
individuals master their surroundings when immersed in dialogue and engaged in the social
construction of meaning.

Brooks et al., (1988) outline the following steps toward changing students’ motivation:
1. Ensure course materials relate to students’ lives and highlight ways learning can be
applied in real-life situations (Lumsden, 1994; Skinner & Belmont, 1991).
Schoolwork should be meaningful to students outside the school building, as well as
within. Students are more engaged in activities when they can build on prior
knowledge and draw clear connections between what they are learning and the world

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING15  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
in which they live. They also need to feel that “school work is significant, valuable,
and worthy of their efforts” (Policy Studies Associates, 1995).

2. Allow students to have some degree of control over learning (Brooks, et al., 1998).
This can be done in any number of ways, from giving students choices between
different assignments, to minimizing adult supervision over group projects, to letting
students monitor and evaluate their own progress (Anderman & Midgley, 1998; Dev,
1997; Policy Studies Associates, 1995). “Even small opportunities for choice, such as
whether to work with a partner or independently” give students a greater sense of
autonomy  (Anderman & Midgley, 1998).

3. Assign challenging but achievable tasks for all students. Tasks that seem impossible
easily discourage learners as do those tasks that are rote and repetitive (Dev, 1997;
Policy Studies Associates, 1995). Remedial programs that limit students to repetitive
basic skills activities actually “prompt students’ lack of engagement in their school-
work and frequently result in limited achievement.”

4. Arouse students’ curiosity about the topic being studied. Strong, Silver, and Robinson
(1995) suggest using the “mystery” approach, in which students are presented with
fragmentary or contradictory information about a subject and are then asked to
examine available evidence to develop their own hypotheses.  Give students an
opportunity to direct inquiry and “discover for themselves.”

5. Design projects that allow students to share new knowledge with others. Strong,
Silver and Robinson (1995) observe that when students do assignments that only the
teacher will read, they are entering into a non-reciprocal relationship.  Projects are
more engaging when students share what they are learning in reciprocal relationships
where each student’s knowledge is needed by others in the group to complete an
assignment.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING16  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Importance  of  knowledge  to  reading  and  comprehension  

Awareness about the world is needed to give context and meaning to words. More serious than
skill deficiencies are knowledge deficiencies that arise for children who have limited access to
the informal informational lessons that can be transmitted through day-to-day interactions with
the world around them.

Indications are that limited content knowledge might ultimately account for what appear to be
comprehension difficulties (Vellutino et al., 1996) or higher-order thinking difficulties in older
children.

Real leverage may be found in the continual, systematic, everyday ways children are engaged in
learning new knowledge and information, starting in the early years. Frede (1998) reported
children who had a broad base of experience in domain-specific knowledge were likely to move
more rapidly in acquiring complex skills. Basic skills must be used to develop coherent
understandings of knowledge and concepts, which are the very basic foundations for later
learning.

According to a recent literature review, there is a scarcity of informational text in primary-grade


classrooms (and, to some extent, throughout elementary school) (Palincsar & Duke, 2004). Low
income first-grade students were given extensive exposure to informational genres in an
experiment. By the end of the year, the experimental group of children was better writers of
informational text than children in the control groups, had progressed more quickly in reading
level, and had shown less decline in attitudes toward recreational reading (Palincsar & Duke,
2004).

Flowerday, Schraw, and Stevens (2004) identify differences and similarities between the
importance of topic interest and situational interest. They find that these variables correlate with
one another and result in positive outcomes. The researchers define topic interest as something
that students have prior knowledge about, personal experiences with, and that evokes some sort

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING17  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
of emotion. Situational interest is defined as something that depends on the present context and
tends to be informational in content.

Situational interest often precedes and facilitates an individual’s development of personal


interest. When combined these are an excellent way to activate students’ attention, increase
effort, engagement, and maintain deeper mental processing levels.
 
The enjoyment of reading comes from comprehension, not decoding words. Skilled readers
‘interact’ with a text, thinking about what will happen next, creating questions about the main
characters and so on. Children who love to read have good comprehension skills.

Elements of Reading Comprehension


 
Identifying  and  summarizing  the  main  idea   Identifying  cause  &  effect  
 
Comparing  &  contrasting     Recognizing  sequence  of  events  

Identifying  supporting  facts  and  details   Identifying  story  elements  such  as  main  
 
  characters,  settings,  plot,  conflict,  and  solution  

Making  inferences  &  drawing  conclusions   Identifying  the  author’s  purpose  and  point  of  
 
  view  

Predicting  outcomes   Interpreting  literary  devices  such  as  imagery,  


 
  symbolism,  and  metaphors  

Recognizing  fact  &  opinion     Recognizing  realism  versus  fantasy  

Hidi (2001) found that all types of interests (topic and situational) served as powerful
determinants that contributed to students’ increased recognition, comprehension and recall.
Interest was a clear indicator for the quality of learning derived. Students’ interests were
activated, which influenced readers to go beyond the surface elements of the text and focus on
more elaborate, higher-order thinking skills, to help them uncover the underlying meaning of the
main ideas.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING18  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Flowerday, Schraw, and Stevens (2004) emphasize materials that interest students affect the
application and transfer of deeper text processing.
 
 
Effects  of  individual  temperament  on  reading  motivation  

Temperament is a broad, multidimensional construct that can be defined as an individual’s style


of response to stimuli in the environment (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Attributes such as intensity
and activity levels, persistence, affect, and behavioral inhibition are temperamentally based child
characteristics that may contribute to teacher-child relationship quality. Certain attributes ease
children’s adjustment into student roles (Entwisle & Alexander, 1998).

People can be categorized into behaviorally inhibited and uninhibited styles. These
characteristics are highly salient and fairly stable over time. People who are apprehensive toward
new people, events, and things are categorically different from others who approach novelty with
ease (Caspi & Silva, 1995; Kagan, Snidman, & Arcus, 1992). They have a biologically based
predisposition to be inhibited.

In the general population, approximately 15% to 20% of people are inhibited, about 30% to 35%
of people are uninhibited, and the remainder of the population falls somewhere in between
(Kagan, 1997; Kagan et al., 1992; Kagan, Snidman, & Arcus, 1998). These classifications
remain fairly stable for 60% of children through at least age 9 (Kagan et al., 1998).

Children’s school experiences vary in part because of their temperamentally based individual
differences. Children who are inhibited are highly reactive; that is, they show fear in new
situations (Kagan, 1994). Kagan and colleagues (1992) reported that children who are
uninhibited are low reactive and approach-oriented in new situations, and display fewer fears at 9
and 14 months than their high reactive counterparts. Therefore, high reactivity and fear together
predict social inhibition in school.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING19  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Research shows that bold or uninhibited children are more socially competent and therefore have
more interactions with teachers than their less-bold counterparts (Lerner et al., 1985; Patrick,
Yoon, & Murphy, 1995; Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2002; Rimm–Kaufman & Kagan, 2005;
Skarpness & Carson, 1986). In addition, they are viewed by their teachers as more verbal and
academically competent compared to their shy counterparts (Lerner et al.).

On the other hand, inhibited children appear to fit well into student roles because they are likely
to be more compliant and refrain from speaking out of turn (Keogh, 2003). However, because of
their quiet style, they may elicit less attention from their teachers than uninhibited children;
additionally, they may receive less conversation and verbal feedback from their teachers, both of
which contribute to children’s early learning in the preschool classroom.

Uninhibited children appear to be more affected than other children by the sensitivity of their
teachers, with teacher sensitivity predicting more self-reliance, more positive affect and fewer
negative and off-task behaviors among socially bold children, but not for their wary counterparts
(Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2002). These findings suggest that bold children receive more exposure
to the teacher than inhibited children.
 
 
Average  and  gifted  students  

There is notable variability among average students. Both inter-individual and intra-individual
differences are observed. Treating the class as a homogeneous group and providing the same
lesson at the same time to all the students favors only a small fraction of the students (Blancas et
al., 1985).

The goal is to enhance the fit between the students’ personal needs and ways of learning and the
pedagogical activities. If each student operates in his or her “zone of proximal development,” a
concept proposed by the Russian psychologist and educator Lev Vygotsky (Vygotsky, 1978;
Wozniak, 1980), then quality education will be achieved. The speed with which students

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING20  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
progress can vary between students (inter-individual differences) but can also vary across
learning domains for a particular student (intra-individual variability).
 

Average  students    

University of South Carolina researchers found that average students believe that their effort,
more than their innate abilities, helps them to get ahead (Holland, 2000). In one experiment,
teachers told the students what their reading levels were and shared specific strategies for
improving their understanding of text. Included among these strategies were frequent quizzes so
students could break difficult material into smaller chunks. Early in the semester, teachers’ first
tests were designed to help every student score well. After two tests, teachers sent home notes to
boost students’ self confidence by congratulating them for having A averages. The difficulty of
subsequent tests was gradually increased. Threats of bad grades were ineffective with average
students but success motivated them (Holland, 2000).

Stevens (2001) described interest as a motivational device to encourage reading growth among
students. If a gifted student has an interest in a particular topic, he/she tends to apply a
significant amount of effort to reading about it using superior reading skills. This application of
effort in turn resulted in improved reading comprehension overall.

While personal interest in the subject matter is important to motivate reading for all ability
levels, the average or below average students are generally not sufficiently motivated by personal
interests alone to apply the significant effort required to tackle material above or beyond their
level or ability which hinders further progress. In part this may be due to lack of the topical or
general knowledge of the world around them that forms the framework for comprehending
information related to those interests. Flowerday, Schraw, and Stevens (2004)

However, as the USC researchers discovered average and below average readers are motivated to
exert effort to read what they believe they can succeed in comprehending. That is, when they are
given strategies for improving their understanding of text and when material is initially at or

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING21  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
below their reading level to build confidence. The USC researchers found this method of
coupling technique with initial successes to be an effective way of increasing students’
motivation to read and their comprehension of progressively more difficult material.

Characteristics of Average Students


Generally  hard  workers   Positive  self-­‐concept  
Believe  their  academic  success  is  due  to  effort,  
Lack  independent  work  habits  
not  talent  
Blend  into  classrooms  by  fitting  in    
Cooperative,  well-­‐behaved  
or  staying  in  the  background  
Wide  variation  in  academic  performance;  
Like  to  please  
inconsistent  

What Motivates Average Students


Material  that  is  interesting  and  important  more   Discussions,  cooperative  learning,  research  
than  difficult   projects,  opportunities  to  move  beyond  standard  
“seat  work”
Teachers  who  care  about  them  and    Success,  not  the  threat  of  bad  grades.  They  need  
communicate  their  high  expectations   to  know  their  hard  work  matters.
Realistic  targets;  teachers  must  tell  them  in   The  chance  to  earn  effort  grades  as  well  as  
advance  of  an  assignment  what  standards  they   achievement  grades  
need  to  reach  and  how  they  can  earn  good  
grades
 
 
 
Gifted  students  

Contrary to popularly held conceptions, research suggests that gifted children are not gifted
across all domains. They tend to have heterogeneous ability profiles, where abilities are
expressed especially strongly in one domain (such as mathematics) and average in others. Thus
an individualized educational program that allows gifted students to remain in their regular class
for certain subjects and to be in enriched or advanced classes for other subjects may be the best
overall solution given these pupils' cognitive and social needs.    

   
GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC
MARKETING22  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Motivating  behaviors  and  interventions    

Of the several theoretical perspectives most often used in motivation research, achievement goal
theory is, as the name indicates, most directly applicable to goal-setting. Achievement goal
theory posits that students’ academic motivation can be understood as attempts to achieve goals
(Seifert, 2004, p. 142). To feel competent, students need to see their goals as realistic and
achievable, which may require altering the goals or altering students’ perceptions of their own
abilities. To feel in control, students must be able to see a clear path to achieving the goal,
through means they can control rather than through luck or chance. Control is also maximized
when students set goals themselves, or at least agree with and internalize goals set for them by
someone else.

Goal theorists generally break education goals into two groups: “mastery” (or “learning”) goals
and “performance” goals. Mastery goals involve increased understanding, skills, and content
knowledge. Performance goals, on the other hand, involve reaching a pre-defined performance
level or outperforming others. Researchers have consistently found that students who have a
mastery goal in mind exhibit deeper cognitive processes, strategize more effectively, and are
more adaptable to challenges.

Performance-oriented students show more adverse reactions to failure, see less of a link between
effort and outcome, and focus more on their performance relative to the performance of others
(Pintrich, 2003; Seifert, 2004). The same student can have different mindsets and goals in
different contexts.

To implement motivational strategies, Hunter states it is often necessary to begin with extrinsic
motivation and outline a path that lets students see that they can generate intrinsic motivation
themselves—that they can set their own best effort and operate at the top of their potential.

Initially, when the focus is on extrinsic motivation, the students are doing the work to please
their teachers, or because they are told the result is going to be good. But as they continue to

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING23  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
work, they can develop greater intrinsic motivation. Students can be motivated by showing them
methods for visualizing and making concrete their own progress. Charts or reading logs can help
students keep track of the number of books they have read, the new words they have learned, or
the amount of time they have spent (e.g., Braunger & Lewis, 1998). Students need to know
what to expect. If they know something about what is coming up, they can get excited about it.
Children like to know what their peers think. They also like knowing their opinion is valued.
 
Students’ learning can be optimized by developing a sense of autonomy through reading
instruction that cultivates their abilities to make their own choices and to take responsibility for
their own actions. This freedom leads to the development of intrinsic motivation (Fink &
Samuels, 2008). When this occurs, students develop an inherent satisfaction with learning
because they believe they are in control, rather than being controlled (Stroudt, 2010).

Wigfield and McCann (1997) find autonomy to be a crucial element in fostering children’s
confidence that they can read. Building upon students’ own interests and curiosity about different
topics and immersing them in a variety of books is a proven way to do so. Students who believe
that they have ownership over their leaning are motivated, self-determining, and self-regulated.

Research shows that letting students select their own books helps to increase interest value and
boosts motivation (e.g., Wigfield, 1994). A wide variety of texts should be provided that are
interesting and appropriate for students’ age range and personally relevant to individuals.
Students should be matched to “just right” texts on their reading level that are challenging
enough to maintain interest and foster a sense of achievement but not so difficult that they are
discouraged. The teacher pushes the student to grow, while still making sure that it is possible for
the student to succeed (e.g., Gambrell, Palmer, & Codling, 1993; Morrow, 1996).    

Flowerday, Schraw, and Stevens (2004) examined the effects of choice, topic interest, and
situational interest on reading engagement, attitude, and learning and discovered that when
students are able to select their own reading materials they tend to report that they enjoyed the
task much more. Therefore, their attitude improves and they find the task to be more valuable.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING24  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Rich (2009) finds that students will value reading if they are able to choose their own books.
Students who chose a novel for class became engaged with it because they were interacting with
it, not just reading to find answers.
 
The  collecting  instinct  

G. Stanley Hall, one of America’s first students of the “child mind,” proposed instruction should
be a kind of collecting game where students are encouraged to gather scraps of information on
literature, geology, and so on Folsom, (1915); Bernstein, (2011). Others, including educator
Elizabeth Howe, thought the method would work best if it took as its starting point the collecting
interests of each individual child Howe, (1906).

There may be a relationship between motivation, book selection and personal library-building or
book logging activity and children’s behavioral proclivity to build collections.

Following a study of 90 first through fifth-grade students, Baker and Gentry suggest that children
are naturally motivated to collect because they (1) enjoy the process of collecting as it allows
them to escape boredom and sometimes reality, (2) learn or satisfy curiosity about their
collecting domain, (3) satiate a passion for the objects which are desired, (4) want to differentiate
themselves from others, and (5) desire to associate with others, especially family and friends.
Children often have multiple motives for collecting (Baker & Gentry, 1996).

The motives with which the collecting process is approached and the tasks associated with it aid
children in their developmental process and in their search for identity. In an exploration of
collecting in consumer research, Belk et al. (1991) suggest that two basic motivations can be
used to explain collecting: legitimization and self-extension. Legitimization motives are
characterized by collectors behaving within boundaries that the social world will accept.
Behavior which is done for “rational” purposes (e.g., creating, investing, building history) is not
considered to be self-indulgent if one labels it as “collecting.” Self-extension motives are
characterized by a desire to enhance or improve the self.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING25  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
What you really collect is always yourself. Whether your collection serves as a public display or
as a private preserve, it is a form of expression where you materialize that abstract thing called
the self, where you can thus see and handle yourself, even talk to yourself, taking comfort in the
way objects stabilize you as a subject.

Fifth-grade participants in the Baker and Gentry study appreciated collecting because it made
them unique. None of the first-graders talked about this value of collecting.

The role of family appears to be central to the collecting process, especially in a modeling sense.
There was modeling evident; for example, a few of the children mentioned that their collections
were started because of their parents’ or grandparents’ influence or interest. The involvement of
adults in the collections also results in longer term expectations for collecting. The major role
played by family in the collecting by the child is in its facilitation, by easing the child’s entry
into the process (Baker & Gentry, 1996).

Most mentions were of gifts that started the collection or of continuing additions on birthdays
and other holidays. Friends were also important motivators for children’s collecting behaviors.
For example, modeling of friends’ collecting behaviors was evident. Many children, especially
the first-graders, compared their material possessions to those of others. They suggested that
collecting was a way of getting “more” of something than others have. The facilitating role
played by parents diminishes as the child gains access to resources (Baker & Gentry, 1996).

Formanek (1991) found that collectors were motivated by investment, obsession, preservation,
and legitimization of the personal and social self. One thing all collectors have in common is
their passion for the items that they collect. Ease of entry is an important component of the type
of collection children pursue, as evidenced by the prominence of items that can be picked up
without cost or received as a small gift. Gender norms seem to affect what is collected. The
influence of movies, cartoons, and advertising on collections is also apparent.    Children are able
to express the goals they have in their collecting and enjoy talking about it. However, they are
more likely to do things rather than to ponder why they do them.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING26  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 

Collecting is a kind of play that results in something to play with.  These intrinsic satisfactions
have much to do with the “playability” of free choice collectibles, with how readily they inspire
an absorbing make-believe. Single function items or objects overly tied to pre-existing
narratives, for instance, often do not allow for the free rein of imagination. Things amenable to
symbolic transformation by the child best inspire play.

Collecting, like all forms of playing, exercises critical imaginative and cognitive skills. The items
collected served as repositories of distributed intelligence - handling, arranging and classifying
concrete objects, animating and empathizing in make believe adventures. Children explore an
embodied feel for the logical, narrative and social relationships of abstract knowledge. For a
time, the objects that are collected store both the “what” and the “how” of inner life as the child
learns to think, to imagine and to reflect. Sometimes a collection is not just a collection, but a
spur to the imagination, to learning and creating (Bernstein, 2011).

First-graders displayed vivid imaginations about what collecting means. Older children had both
more traditional collections and more traditional perspectives of what collecting entails. Further,
they were able in many cases to take a more active role in the acquisition of their collections due
to their access to more resources.

Although children have different definitions for collecting, they seem to have some expectations
as to what collecting will provide to them. Collecting was seen by many respondents as
something to do. Many said they collected because collecting is “fun” and because they enjoy
doing it. Not only is collecting enjoyable for children, but it also helps them to relieve their
boredom or put order in their lives. Besides using collecting as an escape from boredom, children
also use collecting as something to do to escape from reality. Some children suggested they
collected just because they enjoyed looking at their things. First-graders were much more likely
to acquire collections that served a functional purpose. Many informants saw their collection as
a way of “doing” something for the future. Gender and age differences were noted in this

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING27  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
“collecting as investment” perspective. Males mentioned expectations concerning their collection
growing in value.

Several of the children, boys in particular, talked about how they used their collections to learn
about features or phenomena related to them. The study found clear evidence of curiosity, at
least as conceptualized by Loewenstein’s (1994) information gap model. The children talked
about how they learn about their collections, which included talking to friends or reading books;
however, they also often mentioned catalogs, magazines, and stores as important sources of
information. One motive for most children in collecting was to find something that they like.
The intense passion for collecting evident in adult collectors (Baker & Mittelstaedt, 1995; Smith
& Lee, 1994) was not as clearly evident among pre-adolescents. Similar to adults, the interviews
with children show evidence of high enduring involvement both in the frequent maintenance
activities observed and the description of their “learning” activities related to their collections.
 
 
Rewards  as  motivation    

On the whole, research shows that reward programs can have positive effects if they are
implemented thoughtfully, carefully, and within a set of guidelines, and if they address the four
dimensions of motivation: competence, autonomy, interest, relatedness. Rewarding students for
mastery of a discrete task, skill, or subject, such as reading a book or solving a problem, works
better than rewarding them for performance, such as reaching a certain benchmark on a test.
Rewards that are too large can be counterproductive because students may feel pressured into
taking part. At the same time, poorly designed reward programs can actually decrease motivation
if they are targeted at the wrong students, do not build on the four dimensions of motivation, or
are implemented ineffectively (Lepper, 1973).

Parents who are actively involved in their children’s education and provide a stimulating
learning environment at home help their children develop feelings of competence, control,
curiosity, and positive attitudes about academics, according to various studies. Reading to
children, talking with children about what they read, interacting with children about academics,

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING28  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
and celebrating moments of intellectual discovery are among the activities that promote
achievement and motivation. Parents’ beliefs and expectations also appear to strongly influence
children’s motivation.

Triarchic  instruction

A series of studies with elementary and middle-school students showed that triarchically based
instruction involving a combination of analytic, creative and practical activities led to better
quality learning than traditional memory-oriented lessons or lessons based on only analytic,
critical thinking. Multiple angles are offered on each topic, and these approaches reinforce
learning through the multiple exposures— analytic, creative and practical—to a topic.

The triarchic approach is implicitly child-centered because there is something for each child in
the program that all children receive. Individual differences are taken into account when the
program is constructed because the program responds to a diversity of pupils. This approach
does not advocate individualized academic standards nor individualized ways of reaching a
common goal.

Analytic instruction emphasizes comparisons, contrasts, evaluation, and judgment. Creative


instruction focuses on inventing, discovering. Traditional memory-based instruction focuses on
straightforward knowledge acquisition (when, where, who, how). Practical instruction concerns
real-world applications and implications. This last component directly concerns the development
of the knowledge needed to build society and more generally social intelligence and cultural
knowledge.

In general, students who received triarchic instruction both (a) outperformed students receiving
analytic-critical thinking or traditional, memory-based instruction, and (b) reported that they
liked their lessons more than the analytic and traditional instruction groups (Sternberg, Torff, &
Grigorenko, 1998). The group receiving triarchic instruction outperformed other groups

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING29  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
on analytic, creative and practical, performance measures but also on traditional memory-based
measures of learning.  
 
A study of 708 fifth-grade students showed that triarchic methods were more powerful than
traditional methods for reading instruction (Grigorenko, Jarvin, & Sternberg, 2002). The research
included a pre-test of reading skills (with no difference between the instructional groups),
instruction via triarchic or traditional methods. A post-test showed significantly better
performance for students in the triarchic instructional condition. Nearly eighty-one percent of
students (80.8%) reported that they liked the program; teachers also liked it, and found that it
addressed a wide range of students’ needs.

 
Motivating  factors  reported  by  students  
Edmunds and Bauserman (2006) conducted a series of interviews with school children. Six
factors, or categories, were identified that appeared to get children excited about reading
expository and narrative text and about reading in general.

Category 1: Reading narrative text


The most popular factors leading to enthusiasm for reading were personal interests, book
characteristics, and choice.

Characteristics of the books included exciting book covers, action packed plots, and humor. The
importance of choice was revealed throughout the interviews as children shared the books they
were reading or had recently read: 84% of the children discussed books they had selected
themselves.

Category 2: Reading expository text


Children’s responses focused on the influence of the knowledge gained by reading the book, on
the relationship between the book and their personal interests, and on choice.

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING30  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Category 3: Reading in general
This category included characteristics of books and the opportunity to gain knowledge. Children
enjoyed books that were funny or scary or had great illustrations. The children also placed a
great deal of importance on the information they could learn from reading books.

Category 4: Sources of book referrals


Access to books is critical to the amount of reading children do and to their reading achievement
(Gambrell & Marinak, 1997). Children overwhelmingly reported that they found out about their
books from the school library. Other libraries such as the classroom library and the local or
county library were also mentioned but not as frequently. When discussing the narrative text they
were reading, the children often identified their teacher as the person who introduced books to
them. In terms of influence on their reading choices, children most frequently responded that
their mother had the greatest influence, followed by other family members and peers.

Category 5: Sources of motivation:


Children’s interest in and excitement about reading was sparked by various individuals including
family members (especially mothers), teachers, and themselves.

Category 6: Actions of family members, teachers, and peers


After discovering who motivated the children to read, the actions of these individuals were
examined. Three themes emerged: buying or giving children books, reading to them, and
sharing books with them. The two most (and equally) frequent responses were: buying or giving
books to the children and reading to them.

 
Technology’s  motivational  role    

There is a need to offer students a variety of educational tools, such as interactive exercises and
games that may promote motivation to learn and thereby enhance learning (Gee, 2003; Mayer,
2011; Mintz & Nachmias, 1998).

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING31  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Students in online environments may choose their own learning path based on their preferences
and needs. Their choice is exercised, for example, by the type or amount of content they
consume, the time they dedicate to learning, and the effort they are willing to make (Sims &
Hedberg, 1995). Motivated students consume more content and use more tools, dedicate more
time to thinking, and make an effort to answer questions correctly (e.g., Cocea & Weibelzahl,
2007).

Online learning environments contain a variety of educational tools to enhance students’


knowledge and skills in a specific subject domain. These tools are often rich in media such as
simulations, games, and other interactive tools (Mayer, 2011). The most prevalent tool for
students are the drills in which they get immediate feedback regarding the accuracy of their
answers (Egenfeldt-Nielsen, 2005; Weiss & Muller, 2008). It helps students to assess their
knowledge and competence and to focus on their learning process (Chickering & Gamson, 1987;
Gibbs & Simpson, 2004).

In some studies, it was found that these tools have achieved only limited success in helping
students develop advanced knowledge and skills. The reasons mentioned in this regard are that
such tools have been poorly designed and are simplistic, boring, and repetitious, and they do not
allow users any possibilities for active exploration (Kirriemuir & McFarlane, 2004; Schank,
2005).

Games are another common tool in online environments. A growing volume of research
indicates that games have promising potential as learning tools. Alongside their ability to
improve students’ knowledge and skills, they generate motivation for learning by means of
components such as competition, fun, and creativity (Gee, 2003; Gredler, 2004; Mintz &
Nachmias, 1998). Some studies even point out the link between playing games and learning
outcomes (Klopfer, Osterweil, & Salen, 2009; Mor, Winters, Cerulli, & Björk, 2006; Sandford,
Ulicsak, Facer, & Rudd, 2006).

There is a problem in integrating games into formal education (Jantke, 2006; Weiss & Muller,
2008). Research has found that there is a lack of acceptance of games as an educational tool
among the majority of teachers as well as among many students. The reason for this is that

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING32  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
games tend to be perceived as a leisure time activity with no pedagogic value (de Freitas, 2006;
Egenfeldt-Nielsen, 2005; Schrader, Zheng, & Young, 2006).

Assessment of learners’ motivation in online environments has been a challenge for researchers
as well as developers due to the fact that it is a difficult factor to evaluate without physically
observing the students in the learning process. Extracting students’ actions from well prepared
log files can help to achieve this goal for a large population of students (Castro, Vellido, Nebot,
& Mugica, 2007; Merceron & Yacef, 2005; Pahl, 2004; Srivastava, Cooley, Deshpande, & Tan,
2000).

Motivation usually impacts on the behavior of students in the learning environment and
determines the amount of content and tools they consume, the way they consume them, and the
effort they are willing to make on a specific assignment (Cocea & Weibelzahl, 2007; Dweck,
1986; Hershkovitz & Nachmias, 2008). In addition, in some cases, motivation influences the
learning outcomes (Martinez, 2003).

Current research places special emphasis on the features of games, such as the challenge, control,
and competition that can generate internal motivation (Gee, 2003; Gredler, 2004; Mayer, 2011).

Tracing the students’ actions in the learning environment makes is possible to assess students’
actions. Beck (2004), in his study, built a model to predict students’ engagement by measuring
the difficulty of the questions, the student’s response time, and the accuracy of his answer. Cocea
and Weibelzahl (2007) tested the students’ engagement by variables such as the number of pages
read, the number of tests taken, and the time spent on the assignment. Hershkovitz and Nachmias
(2009) developed a measuring tool for motivation which focuses on: the extent of engagement
and its energization and source based on variables such as time on task percentage, average
session duration, and average pace of activity within sessions.

Greater motivation to learn was found in the drills and the self-test than in the learning game. An
unexpected finding in both units was that students played the learning games the least and

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING33  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
focused on drills and self-tests, although, chronologically, the games appeared before the self-
tests, and the self-tests were located on different web pages.

These findings reveal the need to explore in greater length under which conditions learning
activities, in general, and games, in particular, should be integrated into online learning
environments (Ben-Zadok, Leiba, & Nachmias, 2011)

 
 
E-­‐books  
E-books provide students with many options to improve their literacy development. De Jong,
(2004); De Jong and Bus, (2003); Doty, Popplewell, and Byers, (2001); Shamir and Korat,
(2007) discuss the various e-book components they determined to be of benefit. Features include
multimedia and interactive components and the promotion of reading independence and
enjoyment.

Multimedia additions such as dynamic, film-like visuals, story sound effects, printed, and
spoken text invite children to interact with textual displays on the computer. De Jong and Bus
believe these dynamic visuals help to evoke feelings, moods, and attitudes that help to improve
children’s ability to make inferences about story events.

Shamir and Korat (2007) report that highlighting written phrases as the text is read orally by an
actor has the potential to advance children’s literacy development through the exposure to the
connection between oral and written language. It improves word recognition skills and enriches
vocabulary. Closely following the written words allows students to focus on the entire word and
make connections between its features and its parts. Post assessment results by Shamir and
Korat (2007) show that the activation of hot spots within the text that offered certain types of
assistance affected children’s recall of the story better than when the complete story had just
been read aloud to them several times. These interactive features contribute to the advancement
of emergent phonological awareness and word recognition. Doty, Popplewell, and Byers (2001)
researched the differences between young readers’ reading comprehension when one group read

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING34  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
an interactive CD-ROM storybook and the other group read from a conventionally printed book.
The mean scores for comprehension questions were much higher for the group reading the
interactive CD-ROM storybook suggesting the interactive exchange of information between the
reader and the text lessens the decoding burden allowing the focus to be placed on meaning. De
Jong and Bus (2004) and Robinson (2003) have found that e-books are not only good for
teaching and learning important literacy skills but they are also a tool for promoting reading
independence and enjoyment.

E-book features also provide distractions that can hinder literacy development. De Jong and Bus
(2004) report that the multimedia and interactive elements within e-books have drawbacks that
detract from literacy development. Oral narrations of the text frequently do not relate to the
interactive animations. Sound effects create interruptions that interfere with the oral narration
and the visual effects encourage children to think of the stories more as games. All of these tend
to diminish the reader's comprehension.

 
Gaming  
 
In recent years, there has been rapidly increasing interest in educational games. Some of this
interest has been based on the hypothesis that games will lead to better affect than existing
learning environments.

There is evidence that educational games may not have entirely positive effects on learners’
affect and motivation. Bragg (2007) found that students exhibited negative attitudes toward the
use of games as the main instructional method for learning mathematics. Vogel (2006) argues
that games and simulations that fail to make seamless connections between the subject matter
and the game play will also inhibit learners’ engagement and motivation.

In the research reported here, we have found that a traditional intelligent tutoring system can
produce equally good—or better— effect as an award-winning educational game. The key
question, therefore, appears not to be which type of learning environment is better, but how we

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING35  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
can leverage the best practices developed by each of these design communities in order to
develop a new generation of engaging and educationally effective learning environments.

While factors such as fantasy may make games more fun (Cordova & Lepper, 2006), the
interactivity and challenge common to both games and intelligent tutors may play a larger role in
making games affectively positive learning environments.

It has been found that intelligent tutoring systems lead to significantly improved Affect and
motivation as compared to traditional, non-computerized learning contexts (Schofield, 1995),
though not necessarily to expert human tutors. Intelligent tutors generally lack game-like features
like competition and fantasy, but share in common with games features such as instant
feedback and measures of continual progress

Online  collaboration  
In a learner-centered online collaborative environment, students work together to construct
knowledge and negotiate meanings through group-based collaborative learning activities.

Online collaborative learning is supported by socio-cultural theory positing that an individual’s


cultural development appears twice and on two levels; first on the social, and later on the
psychological level, that is, first between people as an inter-psychological experience, and later
as an intra-psychological experience (Vygotsky, 1998; Bonk & King, 1998; Ge, Yamashiro, &
Lee, 2000).

Based on this theory, assumptions can be drawn about collaborative learning and knowledge
building, which require communication, collaboration and negotiation on the common ground of
shared ideas, values and beliefs (Johnson & Johnson, 1996).

Recent practices have contributed to the growing body of knowledge on collaborative learning;
for example, the use of online asynchronous environments for project and inquiry based learning
(Collis, 1998). Harasim (1990) also emphasizes that teamwork enhances connectivity and socio-
GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC
MARKETING36  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
emotional engagement in the learning process as well as creates an intellectual climate that
encourages participation. In other words, a well designed online environment is conducive to
both learners’ affective and cognitive development.

The literature also acknowledges that cognitive achievement and metacognitive strategies are not
sufficient to promote student achievement and that students must also be motivated to learn
intentionally and in a self-regulated manner (Pintrich, 1989).

Student motivation is underpinned by a number of theoretical models and theories. The most
commonly applied is the expectancy value model of motivation (Schunk, 1994). According to
Pintrich & De Groot (1990), there are three motivational components that may be linked to the
different models of self-regulated learning:
1. An expectancy component: Students’ beliefs about their ability to perform a task;

2. A value component: Students’ goals and belief about the importance of the task; and

3. An affective component: Students’ emotional reactions to the task.

A review of the literature on self and peer assessment indicates that in order to promote the
development of these skills, the environment should be designed to encourage participants to:
• Have a clear understanding of the objectives
(Orsmond, Merry, & Reiling, 1996; Stefani, 1994);
• Identify valid assessment criteria
(Falchikov, 1995; Ford, 1997; Klenowski, 1995; Sluijsmans, Dochy, & Moerkerke, 1999;
Sullivan & Hall, 1997; Topping, Smith, & Swanson, 2000);
• Accurately and objectively judge success or failure (Oldfield & MacAlpine, 1995;
Woolhouse, 1999); and
• Become self-regulated and self-motivated.

Self-assessment refers to people being involved in making judgments about their own learning
and progress, which contributes to the development of autonomous, responsible and reflective
individuals (Sambell, McDowell, & Brown, 1998; Schon, 1987). This is also supported by Boud

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING37  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
(1992), who has expressed the defining characteristics of self-assessment as: the involvement of
students in identifying standards and/or criteria to apply to their work and making judgments
about the extent to which they have met these criteria.

 
E-­‐mail  as  a  motivational  tool  
   
It appears that simple, cost-effective, and easy-to-design mass e-mail messages show potential
for addressing some of the motivational needs and retention concerns associated with students
using electronic learning devices. With their simplicity and ease of use, motivational e-mails
represent another tool that distance educators can employ to complement other motivational
efforts.

Numerous scholars highlight the general lack of research concerning the motivational needs of
online learners (Astleitner & Keller, 1995; Gabrielle, 2003; Huett et al., 2007; Means, Jonassen,
& Dwyer 1997; Shellnut, Knowlton, & Savage 1999; J. Visser & Keller, 1990). According to
Song and Keller (2001), instructional designers often ignore motivational design concerns in
web- or site-based, computer-assisted instruction (CAI) and incorrectly assume that the novelty
effect of the technology is enough to stimulate learner motivation.

A need exists for simpler approaches to motivating and retaining online learners that are
appropriate for the audience, the delivery system, and the course. Such methods should be cost-
effective, fit within the time restraints of the class, and be easily integrated into the instruction.
One such approach that shows promise is the creation of simple, systematically designed mass
e-mail messages based on established ARCS (Attention, Relevance, Confidence, and
Satisfaction) model principles (Gabrielle, 2003; Huett et al., 2008; Keller & Suzuki, 2004; L.
Visser et al., 2002; L. Visser, Plomp, & Kuiper, 1999).

Keller and Burkman (1993) acknowledge that motivation is often thought of as solely a product
of learner personality and perceptions—much of which is assumed beyond the control of the
instructional designer. However, they believe that providing for motivation is largely the

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING38  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
responsibility of the designer. Additionally, they feel that motivation is a systematic process that
must be considered during all stages of design.

Many learners initially find web-based environments like distance education and other CAI
programs novel or fun. This often translates into a temporary increase in learner motivation.
Unfortunately, if the CAI is poorly designed or lacks continuing motivational appeal beyond that
of an initial novelty level, learners will eventually lose interest, and motivation and confidence
will wane (Keller & Suzuki, 1988).

To stimulate and manage student motivation to learn, Keller (1987a, 1987b, 1987c) created the
ARCS model of motivation. ARCS is short for (A)ttention, (R)elevance, (C)onfidence, and
(S)atisfaction and serves as the overall framework for the motivational mass e-mail messages.
The ARCS model is an attempt to synthesize behavioral, cognitive,
and affective learning theories and demonstrates that learner motivation can be influenced
through external conditions.

The attention, relevance, confidence, and satisfaction categories serve as a framework for
developing instructional strategies for capturing and maintaining learner attention, establishing
relevance of the material being taught, improving and sustaining learner confidence, and
providing a sense of learner satisfaction through intrinsic and extrinsic rewards.

Keller’s ARCS model more neatly parallels Bean and Eaton’s (2000) retention model, which
emphasizes the key concepts of locus of control, self-efficacy, and approach-avoidance theory as
integral to the student’s commitment to persist. Additionally, established research into general
retention theory often makes a connection between learner persistence and core ARCS principles
such as motivation, relevance, confidence, and satisfaction (Andreu, 2002; Dille & Mezack,
1991; Tinto, 1975, 1993, 2007).

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING39  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Keller’s ARCS model enjoys support in the literature, and researchers have applied its guidelines
to different learning and design environments. For example, ARCS research can be found
concerning the traditional classroom (Bickford, 1989; Klein & Freitag, 1992; Means,
Jonassen, & Dwyer, 1997; Moller, 1993; Naime-Diefenbach, 1991; Small & Gluck, 1994; J.
Visser & Keller, 1990); computer-assisted instruction (Astleitner & Keller, 1995; Bohlin &
Milheim, 1994; ChanLin, 1994; Lee & Boling, 1996; Shellnut, Knowlton, & Savage, 1999;
Song, 1998; Song & Keller, 1999; Suzuki & Keller, 1996); blended learning environments
(Gabrielle, 2003); and online, distant, and web-based classrooms (Chyung, 2001; Huett, 2006;
Song, 2000; L. Visser, 1998).

There is some limited research directly applicable to ARCS-based motivational messages. For
instance, Keller and Suzuki (2004) cited a 1998 report by L. Visser outlining a 70% to 80%
improvement in retention rates of distance learners when motivational messages based on the
ARCS model were used. Additionally, with their ARCS-based motivational communications,
L. Visser, Plomp, and Kuiper (1999) and L. Visser et al. (2002) found that motivational
messages “considerably increased the completion rates of students.”

Of the research available, J. Visser (1990) showed an increase in learner motivation through the
use of motivational messages in a conventional classroom. L. Visser, Plomp, and Kuiper (1999)
and L. Visser et al. (2002) used the ARCS model as a guide for developing motivational
communications with international distance education students. They found positive outcomes
for learner motivation. They also found no statistically significant difference between the use of
mass messages versus personalized messages and recommended using mass messages to
increase “the chance of successful implementation” (1999, 410). Additionally, L. Visser (1998)
conducted a pilot study and a main study using the Motivational Messages Support System
(MMSS), upon which the e-mails in this study were partly based. In both studies, she found that
the messages increased the confidence levels of students. Also, she found no statistically
significant difference in the use of mass messages versus personalized messages in terms of
effectiveness.

 
GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC
MARKETING40  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
References  
Abraham,  C;  Graham,  J.  (2009).    Reading:  Breaking  through  the  barriers,  http://www.multilit.com/wp-­‐
content/uploads/Reading-­‐Breaking-­‐Through-­‐the-­‐Barriers-­‐Australia.pdf].  

Anderman,  L.H.,  &  Midgley,  C.  (1998).  Motivation  and  middle  school  students  [ERIC  digest].  Champaign,  IL:  ERIC  
Clearinghouse  on  Elementary  and  Early  Childhood  Education.  (ERIC  Document  Reproduction  Service  No.  ED  421  
281.  
 
Andreu,  M.  L.  (2002).  Developing  and  implementing  local-­‐level  retention  studies:  A  challenge  for  community  
college  institutional  researchers.  Community  College  Journal  of  Research  and  Practice  26  (4):  333–344.  
 
Astleitner,  H.,  and  J.  Keller  (1995).  A  model  for  motivationally  adaptive  computer-­‐assisted  instruction.  Journal  of  
Research  on  Computing  in  Education  27  (3):  270–280.  
 
Baker  and  Gentry  (1996).  Kids  as  Collectors-­‐  A  Phenomenological  Study.  U.  of  Nebraska.      
 
Baker,  Stacey  Menzel  and  Robert  A.  Mittelstaedt  (1995),  "The  Meaning  of  the  Search,  Evaluation,  and  Selection  of  
'Yesterday's  Cast-­‐offs':  A  Phenomenological  Study  into  the  Acquisition  of  the  Collection,"  in  Enhancing  Knowledge  
Development  in  Marketing,  Barbara  B.  Stern  and  George  M.  Zinkhan  (Eds.),  152.  
 
Bandura,  A.  (1969).  Principles  of  behavior  modification.  New  York:  Holt,  Rinehart,  &  Winston.  
 
Bandura,  A.  (1996).  Social  cognitive  theory  of  human  development.  In  T.  Husen  &  T.  N.  Postlethwaite  (Eds.),  
International  Encyclopedia  of  Education  (2nd  ed.,  pp.5513-­‐5518).  Oxford:  Pergamon  Press.  
 
Bandura,  A.  (2006).  Guide  for  Constructing  Self-­‐Efficacy  Scales.  In  F.  Pajares  &  T.  Urdan  (Eds.),  Adolescence  and  
Education:  Self-­‐Efficacy  Beliefs  of  Adolescents  (Vol.  V,  pp.  307-­‐337).  Greenwhich:  Information  Age  Publishing.  
 
Baudrillard,  J.  (1968).  Le  système  des  objets  in  Brown  2001  University  of  Chicago.  
Bean,  J.  P.,  and  S.  B.  Eaton  (2000).  A  psychological  model  of  college  student  retention.  In  Reworking  the  student  
departure  puzzle,  ed.  J.  M.  Braxton,  48–61.  Nashville,  TN:  Vanderbilt  University  Press.  
 
Beck,  J.  E.  (2004).  Using  response  times  to  model  student  disengagement.  In  Proceedings  of  the  Workshop  on  
Social  and  Emotional  Intelligence  in  Learning  Environments,  7th  International  Conference  on  In-­‐telligent  Tutoring  
Systems,  Maceio,  Brazil.  
 
Belk,  R.  (1991),  "Possessions  and  the  Sense  of  the  Past,"  in  Highways  and  Buyways:  Naturalistic  Research  from  the  
Consumer  Behavior  Odyssey,  ed.  Russell  W.  Belk,  Provo,  UT:  Association  for  Consumer  Research,  114-­‐130.  
 
Bembenutty,  H.  (2008)  Self-­‐Regulated  Learning:  New  Directions  for  Teaching  and  Learning:  Wiley.  
 
Ben-­‐Zadok,  G.    Leiba,  M.  Nachmias,  R  (2011)  Interdisciplinary  Journal  of  E-­‐Learning  and  Learning  Objects  Volume  7.  
 
Berliner,  D.  C.  (1981).  Academic  learning  time  and  reading  achievement.  In  J.  T.  Guthrie  (Ed.),  Comprehension  and  
teaching:  Research  reviews  (pp.  203–226).  Newark,  DE:International  Reading  Association.  [cited  in  Student    
Attitudes  toward  Reading,  Seitz(2010)  Journal  of  Inquiry  &  Action  in  Education,  3(2),  2010].  
 
Bernstein,  R  and  Bernstein,  L.  Childhood  collecting  a  neglected  connection  between  playing  and  learning.  
Psychology  Today,  July  2011.  

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING41  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
 
Bickford,  N.  (1989).  The  systematic  application  of  principles  of  motivation  to  the  design  of  printed  instructional  
materials.  Ph.D.  diss.,  Florida  State  University,  Tallahassee.  
 
Blancas,  A.,  Blancas,  R.,  Dejaune,  A.,  Mur,  D.,  Ramplon,  A.(1985).  Pourquoi  ?  Comment  ?  Démarrer  en  pédagogie  
Freinet.  Cannes,  France  :  CEL.  
 
Boden,  J.  M.,  Fergusson,  D.  M.,  &  Horwood,  L.  J.  (2008).  Does  adolescent  self-­‐esteem  predict  later  life  outcomes?  A  
test  of  the  causal  role  of  self-­‐esteem.  Development  and  Psychopathology,  20(01),  319-­‐339.  
 
Bohlin,  R.,  and  W.  Milheim  (1994).  Applications  of  an  adult  motivational  instructional  design  model.  Paper  
presented  at  the  Association  for  Educational  Communications  and  Technology,  February,  Nashville,  TN.  
ChanLin,  L.  1994.  A  case  for  assessing  motivation  from  learning  a  computer  assisted  instruction.  ERIC,  ED  376803.  
 
Bomia,  L.,  Beluzo,  L.,  Demeester,  D.,  Elander,  K.,  Johnson,  M.,  &  Sheldon,  B.  (1997).  The  impact  of  teaching  
strategies  on  intrinsic  motivation.  Champaign,  IL:  ERIC  Clearinghouse  on  Elementary  and  Early  Childhood  
Education.  (ERIC  Document  Reproduction  Service  No.  ED  418  925).  
 
Boud,  D.  (1992).  The  use  of  self  assessment  schedules  in  negotiated  learning.  Studies  in  Higher  Education,  17(2),  
185-­‐200.  
 
Bragg,  L  (2007).:  Students’  conflicting  attitudes  towards  games  as  a  vehicle  for  learning  mathematics:  A  
methodological  dilemma.  Mathematics  Education  Research  Journal  19(1),  29–44.  
 
Braunger,  J.,  &  Lewis,  J.P.  (1998).  Building  a  knowledge  base  in  reading.  Portland,  OR:  Northwest  Regional  
Educational  Laboratory’s  Curriculum  and  Instruction  Services.  
 
Brewster,C  .    Fager,  J  (2000):  Increasing  student  Engagement  Northwest  Regional  Educational  Laboratory.  
 
Brooks,  S.R.,  Freiburger,  S.M.,  &  Grotheer,  D.R.  (1998).  Improving  elementary  student  engagement  in  the  learning  
process  through  integrated  thematic  instruction.  Unpublished  master's  thesis,  Saint  Xavier  University,  Chicago,  IL.  
(ERIC  Document  Reproduction  Service  No.  ED  421  274).  
 
Caspi,  A.,  &  Silva,  P.  A.  (1995).  Temperamental  qualities  at  age  three  predict  personality  traits  in  young  adulthood:  
Longitudinal  evidence  from  a  birth  cohort.  Child  Development,  66.,  
 
Castro,  F.,  Vellido,  A.,  Nebot,  A.,  &  Mugica,  F.  (2007).  Applying  data  mining  techniques  to  e-­‐learning  problems.  In  L.  
C.  Jain,  T.  Raymond  &  D.  Tedman  (Eds.),  Evolution  of  teaching  and  learning  para-­‐digms  in  intelligent  environment,  
62,  183-­‐221.  Berlin:  Springer-­‐Verlag.  
 
Chickering,  A.  W.,  &  Gamson,  Z.  F.  (1987).  Seven  principles  to  good  practice  in  undergraduate  education.  Racine,  
WI:  The  Johnson  Foundation.  
 
Christensen,  C.  Horn,  M.    Johnson,  C.  (2010)  Disrupting  Class:  How  Disruptive  Innovation  Will  Change  the  Way  the  
nd
World  Learns    2  edition  McGraw-­‐Hill.  
 
Chyung,  S.  Y.  (2001).  Systematic  and  systemic  approaches  to  reducing  attrition  rates  in  online  higher  education.  
The  American  Journal  of  Distance  Education  15  (3):  36–49.  
 

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING42  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Cocea,  M.,  &  Weibelzahl,  S.  (2007).  Cross-­‐system  validation  of  engagement  prediction  from  log  files.  In  E.  Duval,  R.  
Klamma,  &  M.  Wolpers  (Eds.),  Creating  new  learning  experiences  on  a  global  scale:  Pro-­‐ceedings  of  the  Second  
European  Conference  on  Technology  Enhanced  Learning,  EC-­‐TEL  2007,  Crete,  Greece,  September  17-­‐20,  2007.  
 
Collis,  B.  (1998).  WWW-­‐based  environments  for  collaborative  group  work.  Education  and  Information  
Technologies,  3,  231-­‐245.  
 
Cordova,  D.I.,  Lepper,  M.R.  (1996).  Intrinsic  motivation  and  the  process  of  learning:  Beneficial  effects  of  
contextualization,  personalization,  and  choice.  Journal  of  Educational  Psychology  88,  715–730.    
 
Craig  and  Sarlo  (2012)  Improving  Adolescent  Literacy.  Addressing  Student  Engagement  and  Motivation  Issues  
2012  Eye  On  Education,  Inc.  Larchmont,  NY.  
 
Dev,  P.C.  (1997).  Intrinsic  motivation  and  academic  achievement:  What  does  their  relationship  imply  for  the  
classroom  teacher?  Remedial  and  Special  Education,  18(1),  12-­‐19.  
 
de  Freitas  S.  (2006).  Learning  in  immersive  worlds:  A  review  of  game  based  learning.  JISC  (Joint  informa-­‐tional  
Systems  Committee)  report.  Retrieved  6  September  2009  from  http://www.jisc.ac.uk/eli_outcomes.html  
 
De  Jong,  M.  T.,  &  Bus,  A.  G.  (2003).  How  well  suited  are  electronic  books  to  supporting  literacy?  Journal  of  Early  
Childhood  Literacy,  3,  147-­‐164.  
 
De  Jong,  M.  T.,  &  Bus,  A.  G.  (2004).  The  efficacy  of  electronic  books  in  fostering  kindergarten  children’s  emergent  
story  understanding.  Reading  Research  Quarterly,  39,  378-­‐393.  
 
de  Vries,  H.,  Dijkstra,  M.,  &  Kuhlman,  P.  (1988).  Self-­‐efficacy:  the  third  factor  besides  attitude  and  subjective  norm  
as  a  predictor  of  behavioural  intentions.  Health  Educ.  Res.,  3(3),  273-­‐282.  
 
Dille,  B.,  and  M.  Mezack.  (1991).  Identifying  predictors  of  high  risk  among  community  college  telecourse  students.  
The  American  Journal  of  Distance  Education  5  (1):  24–35.  
 
Doty,  D.  E.,  Popplewell,  S.  R.,  &  Byers,  G.  O.  (2001).  Interactive  CD-­‐ROM  storybooks  and  young  readers'  reading  
comprehension.  Journal  of  Research  on  Computing  in  Education,  33,  374-­‐384.  
 
Duke,  N.D.,  Pearson,  P.D.,  Strachan,  S.L.,  &  Billman,  A.K.  (2011).  Essential  elements  of  fostering  and  teaching  
reading  comprehension.  In  S.J.  Samuels,  &  A.E.  Farstrup,  What  research  has  to  say  about  reading  instruction,  4th  
edition  (pp.  51-­‐93).  Newark,  DE:  International  Reading  Association.    
 
Dumont,  M.,  &  Provost,  M.  A.  (1999).  Resilience  in  Adolescents:  Protective  Role  of  Social  Support,  Coping  
Strategies,  Self-­‐Esteem,  and  Social  Activities  on  Experience  of  Stress  and  Depression.  Journal  of  Youth  and  
Adolescence,  28(3),  343-­‐363.  
 
Dweck,  C.  S.  (2010).  Mindsets  and  equitable  education  (2011).  Montessori  elementary  philosophy  
reflects  current  motivation  theories.  Montessori  Life,  23(1),  22-­‐33.  
 
Dweck,  C.  S.  (1986).  Motivational  processes  affecting  learning.  American  Psychologist,  41,  1040-­‐1048.  
 
Eccles,  J.    and  Wigfield,  A.  (2002).  Motivational  Beliefs,  Values  and  Goals  Annual  Review  of  Psychology.  53:109–32  
Edmunds  and,  Bauserman  (2006).  Reading  motivation  reported  by  students.    The  Reading  Teacher  Vol.  59,  No.  5  
February  2006  
 

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING43  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Egenfeldt-­‐Nielsen,  S.  (2005).  Beyond  edutainment:  Exploring  the  educational  potential  of  computer  games.  PhD  
thesis,  University  of  Copenhagen.  
 
Entwisle,  D.  R.,  &  Alexander,  K.  L.  (1998).  Facilitating  the  transition  to  first  grade:  The  nature  of  transition  and  
research  on  factors  affecting  it.  The  Elementary  School  Journal,  98,  351–364.  
 
Falchikov,  N.  (1995).  Peer  feedback  marking:  Developing  peer  assessment.  Innovations  in  Education  and  
Training  International,  32,  175-­‐187.  
 
Fink,  R.,  &  Samuels,  J.  S.  (2008).  Inspiring  reading  success:  Interest  and  motivation  in  an  age  of  high-­‐stages  testing.  
Newark,  DE:  International  Reading  Association.  
 
Flowerday,  T.,  Schraw,  G.,  &  Stevens,  J.  (2004).  The  role  of  choice  and  interest  in  reader  engagement.  The  Journal  
of  Experimental  Education,  72(2),  93-­‐114.  
 
Folsom  (1915)  Pedagogical  Seminary  in  Bernstein  2011  Psychology  Today.  

Ford,  A.  (1997).  Peer  group  assessment:  Its  application  to  a  vocational  modular  degree  course.  Journal  of  
Further  and  Higher  Education,  21(3),  285-­‐298.  
 
Formanek,  Ruth  (1991).,  "Why  They  Collect:  Collectors  Reveal  Their  Motivations,"  Journal  of  Social  Behavior  and  
Personality,  6(6),  275-­‐286.  
 
Frede,  E.  (1998).  Preschool  program  quality  in  programs  for  children  in  poverty.  In  W.S.  Earnett  and  S.S.  Boocock  
(eds),  Early  care  and  education  for  children  in  poverty:  Promises,  programs,  and  long-­‐term  outcomes.  77-­‐98.  New  
York:State  University  of  New  York  Press.  
 
Gabrielle,  D.  (2003).  The  effects  of  technology-­‐mediated  instructional  strategies  on  motivation,  performance,  and  
self-­‐directed  learning.  Ph.D.  diss.,  Florida  State  University,  Tallahassee.  
 
Gagné,  R.  M.  (1985).  The  conditions  of  learning,  4th  ed.  New  York:  Holt,  Rinehart,  &  Winston,  Inc.  
 
Gambrell,  L.B.,  Palmer,  B.,  &  Codling,  R.M.  (1993).  Motivation  to  read.  Washington,  DC:  U.S.  Department  of  
Education,  Office  of  Educational  Research  and  Improvement.  Morrow,  L.M.  (1996).  Motivating  reading  and  writing  
in  diverse  classrooms:  Social  and  physical  contexts  in  a  literature-­‐based  program  (NCTE  Research  Report  No.  28).  
Urbana,  IL:  National  Council  of  Teachers  of  English.  
 
Gambrell,L.  &  Marinak,B.  (1997).  Intrinsic  Motivation  and  Rewards:  What  Sustains  Young  Children’s  Engagement  
with  Text?.  Literary  Research  and  Instruction  47:  9-­‐26,  2008.  College  Reading  Association.  
 
Gambrell,  L.  B.,  Palmer,  B.  M.,  Codling,  R.  M.,  &  Mazzoni,  S.  A.  (1996).  Assessing  motivation  to  read.  The  Reading  
Teacher,  47,  518-­‐533.  
 
Ge,  X.,  Yamashiro,  K.  A.  &  Lee,  J.  (2000).  Pre-­‐class  planning  to  scaffold  students  for  online  collaborative  learning.  
Educational  Technology  and  Society,  3(3),  159-­‐167.  http://www.ifets.info/journals/3_3/b02.html  
 
Gee,  J.  P.  (2003).  What  video  games  have  to  teach  us  about  learning  and  literacy.  New  York:  Palgrave.  
 
Gibbs,  G.,  &  Simpson,  C.  (2004)  Conditions  under  which  assessment  supports  students’  learning?  Learning  and  
Teaching  in  Higher  Education,  1,  3–31.  
 

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING44  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Gredler,  M.  E.  (2004).  Games  and  simulations  and  their  relationships  to  learning.  Educational  Technology  Research  
and  Development,  21,  571-­‐582.  
 
Grigorenko,  E.,  Jarvin,  L.,  &  Sternberg,  R.  J.  (2002).  School-­‐based  tests  of  the  triarchic  theory  of  intelligence:  Three  
settings,  three  samples,  three  syllabi.  Contemporary  Educational  Psychology,  27,  167-­‐208.  
 
Guthrie,  Wigfield,  Metsala  &  Cox  (2009).  Scientific  Studies  of  Reading  Vol.  3,  Issue  3,  1999.  
 
Guthrie,  J.  T.,  &  Wigfield,  A.  (2000).  Effects  of  integrated  instruction  on  motivation  and  strategy  use  in  reading.  
Journal  of  Educational  Psychology.  92,  331-­‐342.  [cited  in  Student  Attitudes  toward  Reading,  Seitz(2010)  Journal  of  
Inquiry  &  Action  in  Education,  3(2),  2010]  
 
Harasim,  L.  (Ed.)  (1990).  Online  Education:  an  environment  for  collaboration  and  intellectual  amplification.  New  
York:  Praeger  
 
Hershkovitz,  A.,  &  Nachmias,  R.  (2008).  Developing  a  log-­‐based  motivation  measuring  tool.  The  First  International  
Conference  on  Educational  Data  Mining  (EDM'08),  Montreal,  Canada.  Retrieved  from  
http://www.educationaldatamining.org/EDM2008/uploads/proc/26_Hershkovitz_8.pdf  
 
Hidi,  S.  (1990).  Interest  and  its  contribution  as  a  mental  resource  for  learning.  Review  of  
Educational  Research,  60,  549-­‐571.  
 
Hidi,  S.  (2001).  Interest,  reading,  and  learning:  Theoretical  and  practical  considerations.  Educational  Psychology  
Review,  13,  191-­‐209.  
 
Hoffman,  M.  A.,  Ushpiz,  V.,  &  Levy-­‐Shiff,  R.  (1988).  Social  support  and  self-­‐esteem  in  adolescence.  Journal  of  Youth  
and  Adolescence,  17(4),  307-­‐316.  
 
Holland.  H.  (2000).  You’ve  got  to  know  them  to  show  them.  Middle  Ground,.  Volume  3,  Number  5,  April  2000,  10-­‐
13,  National  Middle  School  Association  2003.  
 
Howe,  (1906)  The  Collecting  Instinct.  Elementary  School  Teacher.  
 
Huett,  J.  (2006).  The  effects  of  ARCS-­‐based  confidence  strategies  on  learner  confidence  and  performance  in  
distance  education.  Ph.D.  diss.,  University  of  North  Texas,  Denton.  
 
Huett,J.    Kalinowski,  K.  Moller,  L.  (2008)  Improving  the  Motivation  and  Retention  of  Online  Students  Through  the  
Use  of  ARCS-­‐Based  E-­‐Mails.  The  Amer.  Journal  of  Distance  Education,  22:  159–176,  2008  
 
Huett,  J.,  L.  Moller,  J.  Young,  M.  Bray,  and  K.  Huett  (2008).  Supporting  the  distant  student:  The  effect  of  ARCS-­‐
based  strategies  on  confidence  and  performance.  Quarterly  Review  of  Distance  Education  9  (2):  113–126.  
 
Hunter,  P.  (2005).Raising  Students  Who  Want  to  Read.    Scholastic  Professional  Papers.  
 
Jantke,  K.  P.  (2006).  Games  that  do  not  exist:  Communication  design  beyond  the  current  limits.  Proceed-­‐ings  of  the  
ACM  Conference  on  Design  of  Communication.  
 
Johnson,  D.  Johnson,  R.  (1996).  Cooperation  and  the  use  of  technology.  In  D.  H.  Jonassen  (Ed),  
Handbook  of  research  for  educational  telecommunications  and  technology  (pp.  1017-­‐1044).  New  
York:  Simon  &  Schuster.  
 

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING45  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Judge,  T.  A.,  &  Bono,  J.  E.  (2001).  Relationship  of  core  self-­‐evaluations  traits-­‐-­‐self-­‐esteem,  generalized  self-­‐efficacy,  
locus  of  control,  and  emotional  stability-­‐-­‐with  job  satisfaction  and  job  performance:  A  meta-­‐analysis.  Journal  of  
Applied  Psychology,  86(1),  80-­‐92.  
 
Kagan,  J.,  Snidman,  N.,  &  Arcus,  D.  (1992).  Initial  reactions  to  unfamiliarity.  Current  Directions  in  Psychological  
Science,  171-­‐174.  
 
Kagan,  J.  (1992).  Yesterday's  premises,  tomorrow's  promise.  Developmental  Psychology,  28,  
990-­‐997.  
 
Kagan,  J.  (1992).  Behavior,  biology,  and  the  meanings  of  temperamental  constructs.  Pediatrics,  Vol.  90,  No.3,  pp.  
510-­‐513.  
 
Kagan,  J.  (1994).  Galen’s  prophecy:  Temperament  in  human  nature.  New  York:  Basic  Books.  
 
Kagan,  J.  (1997).  Temperament  and  the  Reactions  to  Unfamiliarity.  Child  Development,  Volume  68,  Issue  1,    139–
143,  February  1997  
 
Kagan  J,  Snidman  N,  Arcus  D  (1998a):  Childhood  derivatives  of  high  and  low  reactivity  in  infancy.  Child  
Development  69:1483–1493.  
 
Kagan  J,  Reznick  JS,  Snidman  N  (1988b):  Biological  bases  of  childhood  shyness.  Science  240:167–171.  
 
Kalichman,  S.  C.,  &  Nachimson,  D.  (1999).  Self-­‐efficacy  and  disclosure  of  HIV-­‐positive  serostatus  to  sex  partners.  
Health  Psychology,  18(3),  281-­‐287.  
 
Keller,  C.  (1983):  Motivational  Design  of  Instruction.  In:  Reige-­‐luth,C.(ed.)  Instructional  Design  Theories  and  
Models:  An  Overview  of  their  current  status.  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates,  Mahwah.  
 
Keller,  J.  M.  (1987a).  Strategies  for  stimulating  the  motivation  to  learn.  Performance  and  Instruction  26  (8):  1–7.  
 
Keller,  J.  M.    (1987b).  The  systematic  process  of  motivational  design.  Performance  and  Instruction  26  (9):  1–8.  
 
Keller,  J.  M.    (1987c).  Development  and  use  of  the  ARCS  model  of  motivational  design.  Journal  of  Instructional  
Development  10  (3):  2–10.  
 
Keller,  J.  M.,  and  E.  Burkman  (1993).  Motivation  principles.  In  Instructional  message  design:  Principles  from  the  
nd
behavioral  and  cognitive  sciences,  2  ed.,  eds.  M.  Fleming  and  W.  H.  Levie,  3–53.  Englewood  Cliffs,  NJ:  Educational  
Technology.  
 
Keller,  J.  M.,  and  K.  Suzuki  (1988).  Use  of  the  ARCS  motivation  model  in  courseware  design.  In  Instructional  designs  
for  microcomputer  courseware,ed.  D.  H.  Jonassen,  401–434.  Hillsdale,  NJ:  Erlbaum.  
 
Keller,  J.  M.,  and  K.  Suzuki  (2004).  Learner  motivation  and  e-­‐learning  design:  A  multi-­‐nationally  validated  process.  
Journal  of  Educational  Media  29  (3):  229–239.  
 
Keogh,  B.  (2003).  Temperament  in  the  classroom:  Understanding  individual  differences.  Baltimore:  Brookes.  
 
Kirriemuir,  J.,  &  McFarlane,  A.  (2004).  Literature  review  in  games  and  learning.  Nesta  Futurelab  series,  Report  8,  
Bristol.  
 

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING46  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Klamma,  &  M.  Wolpers  (Eds.),  Creating  new  learning  experiences  on  a  global  scale:  Pro-­‐ceedings  of  the  Second  
European  Conference  on  Technology  Enhanced  Learning,  EC-­‐TEL  2007,  Crete,  Greece,  September  17-­‐20,  2007.  
 
Klein,  J.  D.,  and  E.  T.  Freitag  (1992).  Training  students  to  utilize  self-­‐motivational  strategies.  Educational  Technology  
32  (3):  44–48.  
 
Klenowski,  V.  (1995).  Students’  self-­‐evaluation  processes  in  student-­‐centered  teaching  and  learning  contexts  in  
Australia  and  England.  Assessment  in  Education,  2(2),  145-­‐163.  
 
Klopfer  E.,  Osterweil,  S.,  &  Salen  K.  (2009).  Moving  learning  games  forward.  The  education  arcade,  MIT.  Available  
at  http://education.mit.edu/papers/MovingLearningGamesForward_EdArcade.pdf    
 
Lapp,  D.,  &  Douglas  F.  (2009).  It's  all  about  the  book:  Motivating  teens  to  read.  Journal  of  Adolescent  &  Adult  
Literacy,  57,  556-­‐561.  
 
Lee,  S.,  and  E.  Boling  (1996).  Motivational  screen  design  guidelines  for  effective  computer-­‐mediated  instruction.  
Paper  presented  at  the  Association  for  Educational  Communications  and  Technology,  November,  Indianapolis,  IN.  
 
Lepper,  M.R.,  Greene,  D.,  &  Nisbett,  R.E.  (1973).  Understanding  children’s  intrinsic  interest  with  extrinsic  
rewards.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  28(1),  129–137.  
 
Lerner,  J.  V,  Lerner,  R.  M.,  &  Zabski,  S.  (1985).  Temperament  and  elementary  school  children’s  actual  and  rated  
academic  performance:  A  test  of  ‘goodnessof-­‐fit’  model.  Journal  of  Child  Psychology  and  Psychiatry,  26,  125–136.  
 
Loewenstein,  G.  (1994).  The  Psychology  of  curiosity:  A  review  and  reinterpretation.  Psychological  Bulletin,  
116(1):75-­‐98.  
 
Lubart,T.    Individual  Student  Differences  and  creativity  for  quality  education  2004,  Education  for  All  Global  
Monitoring  Report,  UNESCO  2005.  
 
Lumsden,  L.S.  (1994).  Student  motivation  to  learn  (ERIC  Digest  No.  92).  Eugene,  OR:  ERIC  Clearinghouse  on  
Educational  Management.  (ERIC  Document  Reproduction  Service  No.  ED  370  200).  
 
MacIver,  D.J.,  &  Reuman,  D.A.  (1994).  Giving  their  best:  Grading  and  recognition  practices  that  motivate  students  
to  work  hard.  American  Educator,  17(4),  24-­‐31.  
 
Martin,  J.  J.,  &  Gill,  D.  L.  (1991).  The  relationships  among  competitive  orientation,  sport-­‐confidence,  self-­‐efficacy,  
anxiety,  and  performance.  Journal  of  Sport  &  Exercise  Psychology,  23,  149-­‐159.  
 
Martinez,  M.  (2003,  July).  High  attrition  rates  in  e-­‐learning:  Challenges,  predictors,  and  solutions.  The  e-­‐Learning  
Developers  Journal.  Retrieved  on  25/02/2006  from  http://www.elearningguild.com  
 
Maslow,  A.  (1954).  Motivation  and  personality.  New  York:  Harper.  
 
Mayer,  R.  E.  (2011).  Towards  a  science  of  motivated  learning  in  technology-­‐supported  environments.  Edu-­‐cational  
Technology  Research  and  Development,  59(2),  301-­‐308.  
 
McLoughlin,  C.  Luca,  J  (2004)  An  Investingation  of  the  motivational  aspects  of  peer  and  self-­‐assessment  tasks  to  
enhance  teamwork  outcomes.  In  R.  Atkinson,  C.  McBeath,  D.  Jonas-­‐Dwyer  &  R.  Phillips  (Eds),  Beyond  the  comfort  
zone:  Proceedings  of  the  21st  ASCILITE  Conference  (pp.  629-­‐636.  Perth,  5-­‐8  December.  
http://www.ascilite.org.au/conferences/perth04/mcloughlin.html  

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING47  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
 
Means,  T.,  D.  Jonassen,  and  F.  Dwyer  (1997).  Enhancing  relevance:  Embedded  ARCS  strategies  vs.  purpose.  
Educational  Technology  Research  and  Development  45:5–17.  
 
Mercedes,  T.  Rodrigo,  Baker,  R.  D’Mello,  S  et  al.  (2007),  Comparing  Learners’  Affect  While  Using  an  Intelligent  
Tutoring  System.  Human-­‐Computer  Interaction  Institute,  Carnegie  Mellon  University.  
 
Merceron,  A.,  &  Yacef,  K.  (2005).  TADA-­‐Ed  for  educational  data  mining.  Interactive  Multimedia  Elec-­‐tronic  Journal  
of  Computer-­‐Enhanced  Learning,  7(1).  Available  at:  http://imej.wfu.edu/articles/2005/1/03/index.asp  
 
Metsala,  J.  L.,  Wigfield,  A.,  &  McCann,  A.  D.  (1997).  Children’s  motivations  for  reading.  The  
Reading  Teacher,  50,  360-­‐362  
Miller,  S.  D.  (2003).  How  high-­‐and  low-­‐challenge  tasks  affect  motivation  and  learning:  Implications  for  struggling  
learners.  Reading  &  Writing  Quarterly,  19,  39-­‐57.  
 
Mintz,  R.,  &  Nachmias,  R.  (1998).  Teaching  science  and  technology  in  the  information  age.  Computers  in  education,  
45-­‐46,  25-­‐31.  
 
Mohajer,  N.,  &  Earnest,  J.  (2009).  Youth  empowerment  for  the  most  vulnerable:  A  model  based  on  the  pedagogy  of  
Freire  and  experiences  in  the  field.  Health  Education,  109(5),  424-­‐438.  
 
Moller,  L.  (1993).  The  effects  of  confidence  building  strategies  on  learner  motivation  and  achievement.  Ph.D.  diss.,  
Purdue  University,  West  Lafayette,  IN.  
 
Mor,  Y.,  Winters,  N.,  Cerulli,  M.,  &  Björk,  S.  (2006).  Literature  review  on  the  use  of  games  in  mathematical  learning,  
part  I:  Design.  Report  of  the  Learning  Patterns  for  the  Design  and  Deployment  of  Mathematical  Games  project.  
Available  at:  http://lp.noe-­‐kaleidoscope.org/outcomes/litrev/  
 
Multon,  K.  D.,  Brown,  S.  D.,  &  Lent,  R.  W.  (1991).  Relation  of  self-­‐efficacy  beliefs  to  academic  outcomes:  A  meta-­‐
analytic  investigation.  Journal  of  Counseling  Psychology,  38(1),  30-­‐38.  
 
Naime-­‐Diefenbach,  B.  (1991).  Validation  of  attention  and  confidence  as  independent  components  of  the  ARCS  
motivational  model.  Ph.D.  diss.,  Florida  State  University,  Tallahassee.  
 
Natvig,  G.  K.,  Albrektsen,  G.,  &  Qvarnstrom,  U.  (2003).  Associations  between  psychosocial  factors  and  happiness  
among  school  adolescents.  International  Journal  of  Nursing  Practice,  9(3),  166-­‐175.  
 
Neuman,  S.  (Spring  2006).    How  We    Neglect  Knowledge  and  Why.  American  Educator.  
 
Newbegin,  I.,  &  Owens,  A.  (1996).  Self-­‐esteem  and  anxiety  in  secondary  school  achievement.  Journal  of  Social  
Behavior  &  Personality,  11(3),  521-­‐530.  
 
Oldfield,  K.  A.,  &  MacAlpine,  M.  K.  (1995).  Peer  and  self  assessment  at  tertiary  level-­‐an  experiential  report.  
Assessment  and  Evaluation  in  Higher  Education,  20(1),  125-­‐132.  
 
Orsmond,  P.,  Merry,  S.  &  Reiling,  K.  (1996).  The  importance  of  marking  criteria  in  the  use  of  peer  assessment.  
Assessment  and  Evaluation  in  Higher  Education,  21(3),  239-­‐250.  
 
Overholser,  J.  C.,  Adams,  D.  M.,  Lehnert,  K.  L.,  &  Brinkman,  D.  C.  (1995).  Self-­‐Esteem  Deficits  and  Suicidal    
Tendencies  among  Adolescents.  Journal  of  Amer  Academy  of  Child  &  Adolescent  Psychiatry,  34(7),  919-­‐928.  
 

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING48  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Pachtman,  A.  B,  &  Wilson,  K.  A.  (2006).  What  do  the  kids  think?.  The  Reading  Teacher,  59,  680-­‐684.  
 
Pahl,  C.  (2004).  Data  mining  technology  for  the  evaluation  of  learning  content  interaction.  International  Journal  of  
E-­‐Learning,  3,  47-­‐55.  
 
Palincsar,  A.  S  and  Duke,  N.  K.  (2004).  “The  role  of  text  and  text-­‐reader  interactions  in  young  children’s  reading  
development  and  achievement,”  The  Elementary  School  Journal,  105(2),  p.  183-­‐197.  
 
Patrick,  H.,  Yoon,  K.  S.,  &  Murphy,  A.  (1995,  March).  Personality  characteristics,  social  competence,  and  early  
school  adjustment:  A  contextual  and  developmental  perspective.  Paper  presented  at  the  meeting  for  the  Society  
for  Research  in  Child  Development,  Indianapolis,  IN.  
 
Pintrich,  P.  R.  (2003).  A  motivational  science  perspective  on  the  role  of  student  motivation  in  learning  and  
teaching  contexts.  Journal  of  Educational  Psychology,  95(4),  667-­‐686.  
 
Pintrich,  P.  R.  &  De  Groot,  E.  V.  (1990).  Motivational  and  self-­‐regulated  components  of  classroom  
academic  performance.  Journal  of  Educational  Psychology,  82(1),  33-­‐40.  
 
Policy  Studies  Associates  (1995).  Raising  the  educational  achievement  of  secondary  school  students:  An  idea  book.  
Volume  1,  summary  of  promising  practices.  Washington,  DC:  U.S.  Department  of  Education.  Retrieved  October  4,  
2000,  from  the  World  Wide  Web:  http://www.ed.gov/pubs/Raising/vol1/  
 
Pourchot  (Eds),  Adult  learning  and  development:  Perspectives  from  educational  psychology  (pp.  67-­‐88).  Mawah:  
NJ:  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates.  
 
Regalado,  M.  Goldenberg,  C.  and  Appel,  E.  (2001)  Building  Community  Systems  for  Young  Children:  Reading  and  
Early  Literacy,  in  N  Halfon,  E  Shulman  and  M  Hochstein,  eds.,  Building  Community  Systems  for  Young  Children,  
UCLA  Center  for  Healthier  Children,  Families  and  Communities.  
 
Rich,  M.  (2009).  A  New  assignment:  Pick  books  you  like.  New  York  Times  September  10,  2009  
 
Rimm–Kaufman,  S.  E.,  Early,  D.  M.,  Cox,  M.  J.,  Saluja,  G.,  Pianta,  R.  C.,  Bradley,  R.  H.,  et  al.  (2002).  Early  behavioral  
attributes  and  teachers’  sensitivity  as  predictors  of  competent  behavior  in  the  kindergarten  classroom.  Applied  
Developmental  Psychology,  23,  451–470.  
 
Rimm–Kaufman,  S.  E.,  &  Kagan,  J.  (2005).  Infant  predictors  of  kindergarten  behavior:  The  contribution  of  inhibited  
and  uninhibited  temperament  types.  Behavioral  Disorders,  30,  329–346.  
 
Robinson,  L.  (2003).  Technology  as  a  scaffold  for  emergent  literacy:  Interactive  storybooks  for  toddlers.  Young  
Children,  58,  42-­‐48.  
 
Rogers,  C.  (1951).  Client-­‐centered  therapy;  its  current  practice,  implications  and  theory.  Oxford:  Houghton  Mifflin.  
 
Rosenberg,  M.,  Schooler,  C.,  Schoenbach,  C.,  &  Rosenberg,  F.  (1995).  Global  Self-­‐Esteem  and  Specific  Self-­‐Esteem:  
Different  Concepts,  Different  Outcomes.  American  Sociological  Review,  60(1),  141-­‐156.  
 
Rosenblatt,  L.  (2005).  Literature  S.O.S.!.  Making  meaning  with  texts.  Portsmouth,  NH:  Heinemann.  
 
Rothbart,  M.  K.,  &  Bates,  J.  E.  (1998).  Temperament.  In  W.  Damon  (Series  Ed.)  &  N.  Eisenberg  (Vol.  Ed.),  Handbook  
of  child  psychology:  Vol.  3.  Social,  emotional  and  personality  development  (5th  ed.,  pp.  105–176).  New  York:  
Wiley.  

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING49  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
 
Ryan,  R.  M.,  &  Deci,  E.  L.  (2000).  Self-­‐determination  theory  and  the  facilitation  of  intrinsic  motivation,  social  
development,  and  well-­‐being.  American  Psychologist,  55(1),  68-­‐78.  
 
Sambell,  K.,  McDowell,  L.  &  Brown,  S.  (1998).  "But  is  it  fair?":  An  exploratory  study  of  student  
perceptions  of  the  consequential  validity  of  assessment.  Studies  in  Educational  Evaluation,  23,  349-­‐  
371.  
 
Sandford,  R.,  Ulicsak,  M.,  Facer,  K.,  &  Rudd,  T.  (2006).Teaching  with  games:  Guidance  for  education.  Futurelab:  
Innovation  in  education.  Available  at:  http://www.futurelab.org.uk/projects/teaching-­‐ithgames/research/guidance  
 
Schank,  R  (2005).  Lessons  in  learning,  e-­‐learning,  and  training.  New  York:  Wiley  and  Sons.  
 
Schiefele,  U.  (1991).  Interest,  learning,  and  motivation.  Educational  Psychologist,  26,  299–323.  
 
Schon,  D.  A.  (1987).  Educating  the  reflective  practitioner:  Towards  a  new  design  for  teaching  and  learning  in  the  
professions.  San  Francisco,  CA:  Jossey-­‐Bass.  
 
Schrader,  P.  G.,  Zheng,  D.,  &  Young,  M.  F.  (2006).  Teacher’s  perception  of  video  games:  MMOGs  and  the  future  of  
preservice  teacher  education.  Journal  of  Online  Education,  2(3).  
 
Schunk,  D.  H.  (1991).  Self-­‐efficacy  and  academic  motivation.  Educational  Psychologist,  26,  207-­‐231.  
 
Schunk,  D.  H.,  &  Zimmerman,  B.  J.  (1997).  Social  origins  of  self-­‐regulatory  competence.    Educational  Psychologist,  
32,  195-­‐208.  [cited  in  Student  Attitudes  toward  Reading,  Seitz(2010)  Journal  of  Inquiry  &  ction  in  Education,  3(2),  
2010  
 
Seifert,  T.  L.  (2004).  Understanding  student  motivation.    Educational  Research  (46)2,  137-­‐149.  
 
Shamir,  A.,  &  Korat,  O.  (2007).  Developing  an  educational  e-­‐book  for  fostering  kindergarten  children's  emergent  
literacy.  Computers  in  the  Schools,  24(1/2),  125-­‐143.  
 
Shellnut,  B.,  A.  Knowlton,  and  T.  Savage  (1999).  Applying  the  ARCS  model  to  the  design  and  development  of  
computer-­‐based  modules  for  manufacturing  engineering  courses.  Educational  Technology,  Research  and  
Development  47:100–110.  
 
Sims,  R.,  &  Hedberg,  J.  (1995).  Dimensions  of  learner  control:  A  reappraisal  for  interactive  multimedia  instruction.  
In  J.  L  Pearce  &  A.  Ellis  (Eds.),  First  International  Workshop  on  Intelligence  and  Multi-­‐modality  in  Multimedia  
Interfaces:  Research  and  Applications,  University  of  Edinburgh,  Scotland.  
 
Skarpness,  L.  R.,  &  Carson,  D.  K.  (1986).  Temperament,  communicative  competence  and  the  psychological  
adjustment  of  kindergarten  children.  Psychological  Reports,  59,  1299–1306.  
 
Skinner,  B.  F.  (1953).  Science  and  human  behavior.  New  York:  Free  Press.  
Skinner,  E.,  &  Belmont,  M.  (1991).  A  longitudinal  study  of  motivation  in  school:  Reciprocal  effects  of  teacher  
behavior  and  student  engagement.  Unpublished  manuscript.    University  of  Rochester,  Rochester,  NY.  Cited  in  
Brewster,C  &  Fager,  J:  Increasing  student  Engagement  Northwest  Regional  Educational  Laboratory,  2000.  
 
Slavin,  R.,  Madden,  N.,  Dolan,  L.,  &  Wasik,  B.  (1996).  Every  child,  every  school:  Success  for  All.  Corwin  Press.  
 
 

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING50  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Sluijsmans,  D.,  Dochy,  F.  &  Moerkerke,  G.  (1999).  Creating  a  learning  environment  by  using  self-­‐  peer  and  
co-­‐  assessment.  Learning  Environments  Research,  1,  293-­‐319.  
 
Small,  R.  V.,  and  Gluck,  M.  (1994).  The  relationship  of  motivational  conditions  to  effective  instructional  attributes:  
A  magnitude  scaling  approach.  Educational  Technology  34  (8):  33–40.  
 
Song,  S.  H.  (1998).  The  effects  of  motivationally  adaptive  computer-­‐assisted  instruction  developed  through  the  
ARCS  model.  Ph.D.  dissertation.,  Florida  State  University,  Tallahassee.  
 
Song,  S.  H.  (2000).  Research  issues  of  motivation  in  web-­‐based  instruction.  Quarterly  Review  of  Distance  Education  
1  (3):  225–229.  
 
Song,  S.  H.,  and  Keller,  J.  M.  (1999).  The  ARCS  model  for  developing  motivationally-­‐adaptive  computer-­‐assisted  
instruction.  Paper  presented  at  the  Association  for  Educational  Communications  and  Technology,  February,  
Houston,  TX.  
 
Song,  S.  H.,  and  Keller,  J.  M.  (2001).  Effectiveness  of  motivationally  adaptive  computer-­‐assisted  instruction  on  the  
dynamic  aspects  of  motivation.  Educational  Technology,  Research  and  Development  49:5–22.  
 
Srivastava,  J.,  Cooley,  R.,  Deshpande,  M.,  &  Tan,  P.T.  (2000).  Web  usage  mining:  Discovery  and  applications  of  
usage  patterns  from  web  data.  SIGKDD  Explorations,  1.  
 
Stajkovic,  A.  D.,  &  Luthans,  F.  (1998).  Self-­‐efficacy  and  work-­‐related  performance:  A  meta-­‐analysis.  Psychological  
Bulletin,  124(2),  240-­‐261.  
 
Stefani,  L.  A.  J.  (1994).  Peer,  self,  and  tutor  assessment:  Relative  reliabilities.  Studies  in  Higher  Education,  19(1),  69-­‐
75.  
 
Sternberg,  R.  J.,  Torff,  B.,  &  Grigorenko,  E.  (1998).  Teaching  triarchically  improves  school  achievement.  Journal  of  
Educational  Psychology,  90(3),  374-­‐384.  
 
Stevens,  K.  (2001).  The  effect  of  topic  interest  on  reading  comprehension  of  higher  ability  
students.  Journal  of  Educational  Research,  73,  365-­‐368.  
 
Strong,  R.,  Silver,  H.F.,  &  Robinson,  A.  (1995).  What  do  students  want?  Educational  Leadership,  53(1),  8-­‐12.  
 
Sullivan,  K.  &  Hall,  C.  (1997).  Introducing  students  to  self-­‐assessment.  Assessment  and  Evaluation  in  
Higher  Education,  22(3),  289-­‐305.  
 
Suzuki,  K.  &  Keller,  J.  M.  (1996).  Applications  of  the  ARCS  model  in  computer-­‐based  instruction  in  Japan.  Paper  
presented  at  the  annual  meeting  of  the  Japanese  Educational  Technology  Association,  August,  Kanazawa,  
Japan.  
 
Tinto,  V.  (1975).  Dropout  from  higher  education:  A  theoretical  synthesis  of  recent  research.  Review  of  Educational  
Research  45:89–125.  
 
Tinto,  V.    (1993).  Leaving  college:  Rethinking  the  causes  and  cures  of  student  attrition,  2nd  ed.  Chicago:  University  
of  Chicago  Press.  
 

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING51  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
Tinto,  V.    (2007).  Research  and  practice  of  student  retention:  What  next?  Journal  of  College  Student  Retention:  
Research,  Theory,  and  Practice  8  (1):  1–20.  
 
Topping,  K.  J.,  Smith,  E.  F.  &  Swanson,  I.  (2000).  Formative  peer  assessment  of  academic  writing  
between  postgraduate  students.  Assessment  and  Evaluation  in  Higher  Education,  25(2),  149-­‐166.  
 
U.S.  Dept  of  Education  (2005).  National  Center  for  Educational  Statistics:    National  Assessment  of  Educational  
Progress  .  
 
U.S.  Dept  of  Education  (2007).  National  Center  for  Educational  Statistics:    National  Assessment  of  Educational  
Progress.  
 
U.S.  Dept  of  Education  (2009.)  National  Center  for  Educational  Statistics:    National  Assessment  of  Educational  
Progress.  
 
Usher,  A.  Kober,  N.  Jennings,  J.(2012).  Student  Motivation—An  Overlooked  Piece  of  School  Reform.  Center  on  
Education  Policy,  George  Washington  University.  
 
VanLehn,  K.  (2006).  The  behavior  of  intelligent  tutoring  systems.  International  Journal  of  Artificial  Intelligence  in  
Education  16(3),  227–265.  
 
Vellutino,  F.,  Scanlon,  D.,  Sipay,  E.,  Small  S.,  Pratt,  A.,  Chen,  R.,  et  al.  (1996).  Journal  of  Educational  Psychology,  88,  
601-­‐638.  
 
Visser,  J.  (1990).  Enhancing  learner  motivation  in  an  instructor-­‐facilitated  learning  context.  Ph.D.  diss.,  Florida  State  
University,  Tallahassee.  
 
Visser,  J.,  and  Keller,  J.M.  (1990).  The  clinical  use  of  motivational  messages:  An  inquiry  into  the  validity  of  the  ARCS  
model  of  motivational  design.  Instructional  Science  19:467–500.  
 
Visser,  L.  (1998).  The  development  of  motivational  communication  in  distance  education  support.  Den  Haag,  The  
Netherlands:  CIP-­‐gegevens  Koninklijke  Bibliotheek.  
 
Visser,  L.,  Plomp,  T.,  and  Kuiper,  W.  (1999).  Development  research  applied  to  improve  motivation  in  distance  
education.  Paper  presented  at  the  Association  for  Educational  Communications  and  Technology,  February,  
Houston,  TX.  
 
Visser,  L.,  Plomp,  T.,  Arimault,  R.,  and  Kuiper,  W.  (2002).  Motivating  students  at  a  distance:  The  case  of  an  
international  audience.  Educational  Technology  Research  and  Development  50  (2):  94–110.  
 
Vogel,  J.J.,  Greenwood-­‐Ericksen,  A.,  Cannon-­‐Bowers,  J.,  Bowers,  C.A.  (2006):  Using  virtual  reality  with  and  without  
gaming  attributes  for  academic  achievement.  Journal  of  Research  on  Technology  in  Education  39(1),  105–118.    
 
Vygotsky,  L.  S.  (1978).  Mind  in  society.  (M.  Cole,  V.  John-­‐Steiner,  S.  Scribner,  &  E.  Souberman,  Eds).  Cambridge,  MA  
:  Harvard  University  Press.  
 
Vygotsky,  L.  (1998).  Mind  in  Society.  Cambridge:  Cambridge  University  Press.Bonk,  C.  J.  &  Kim,  K.  A.  (1998).  
Extending  socio-­‐cultural  theory  to  adult  learning.  In  M.  C.  Smith  &  T.  
 
Weiss,  S.,  &  Muller,  W.  (2008).  The  potential  of  interactive  digital  storytelling  for  the  creation  of  educational  
computer  games.  Proceedings  of  Edutainment  2008,  China,  pp.  475–486.  Springer.  

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING52  
&   RESEARCH, INC.
 
 
 
 
Wigfield,  A.  (1994).  Expectancy-­‐value  theory  of  achievement  motivation:  A  developmental  perspective.  
Educational  Psychology  Review,  6(1),  49-­‐78.  
 
Wigfield,  A.  (1994).  The  role  of  children's  achievement  values  in  the  self-­‐regulation  of  their  learning  outcomes.  In  
D.  H.  Schunk  &  B.  J.  Zimmerman  (Eds.),  Self-­‐regulation  of  learning  and  performance:  Issues  and  educational  
applications  (pp.  101-­‐124).  Mahwah,  NJ:  Erlbaum.  
 
Wigfield,  A.  (1997).  Children's  motivations  for  reading  and  engagement.  In  J.  T.  Guthrie  &  A.  Wigfield  (Eds.),  
Reading  engagement:  Motivating  readers  through  integrated  instruction  (pp.  14-­‐33).  Newark,  DE:  International  
Reading  Association.    Wigfield,  A.,  &  McCann,  A.  D.  (in  press).  Children's  motivations  for  reading.  The  Reading  
Teacher.  
 
Wigfield,  A.,  &  Guthrie,  J.  T.  (1997).  Relations  of  children's  motivation  for  reading  to  the  amount  and  breadth  of  
their  reading.  Journal  of  Educational  Psychology.  89,  420-­‐433.  
 
Woolhouse,  M.  (1999).  Peer  assessment:  The  participants'  perception  of  two  activities  on  a  further  education  
teacher  education  course.  Journal  of  Further  and  Higher  Education,  23(2),  211-­‐219.  
 
Worthy,  J.  (1996).  A  matter  of  interest:  Literature  that  hooks  reluctant  readers  and  keeps  them  reading.  The  
Reading  Teacher,  50(3),  204-­‐212.  
 
Wozniak,  R.  H.  (1980).  Theory,  practice,  and  the  “zone  of  proximal  development”  in  Soviet  psychoeducational  
research.  ContemporaryEducational  Psychology,  5,  175-­‐183.  
 
Yankelovich  (2006)  Cited  in  Kids  &  Family  Reading  Report  (2006),  Scholastic.  
 

 
 
 

GUIDING CHANGE • INSPIRING CONFIDENCE • STRATEGIC


MARKETING53  
&   RESEARCH, INC.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai