Anda di halaman 1dari 11

Research Article

Research on Social Work Practice


22(3) 282-292
An Empirical Review of Internet ª The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Addiction Outcome Studies in China DOI: 10.1177/1049731511430089
http://rsw.sagepub.com

Chennan Liu1, Minli Liao1, and Douglas C. Smith1

Abstract
Objectives: The authors systematically reviewed the outcomes and methodological quality of 24 Internet addiction (IA) treatment
outcome studies in China. Method: The authors used 15 attributes from the quality of evidence scores to evaluate 24 outcome
studies. These studies came from both English and Chinese academic databases from 2000 to 2010. Results: Among
the 15 attributes, only sequence generation and intention-to-treat were reported by more than 50% of the 24 studies. None
of the studies contained treatment adherence ratings or collateral reports. Cognitive behavior therapy combined with family
therapy or group therapy emerged as possibly efficacious treatments. Conclusions: More rigorously designed studies, accompa-
nied by transparent reporting of methods and findings are needed to identify promising IA treatments.

Keywords
Internet addiction treatment, methodological quality, review

Internet addiction (IA; also called pathological Internet use, (CBT) and motivational enhancement therapy, were included
excessive computer and video game playing, Internet overuse, in this review. Widyanto and Griffiths (2006) also explored IA
problematic Internet use, etc.) has become a widespread prob- research but did not contain detailed information about treat-
lem among youth ages 9–23. According to a 2009 national Har- ment outcome studies. These above two reviews suggest that
ris poll survey with a randomly selected sample of American cognitive behavioral therapy has been used to treat IA, but with
youth ages 8–18, about 8% of video game players exhibited so few studies, conclusions about its effectiveness are prema-
pathological patterns of play (Gentile, 2009), which is also con- ture. In addition, Young (2009) describes a cognitive behavioral
sidered a type of IA. In a 2005 survey in Germany, among 323 therapy model that includes behavior therapy and self-
children from 11 to 14 years of age, 9.3% of the children ful- monitoring (i.e., a daily Internet use log) to treat IA. However,
filled all criteria for excessive computer and video game play- this article is only description of potential IA treatments but not
ing (Gruesser, Thalemann, Albrecht, & Thalemann, 2005). In actual empirical study.
Australia, excessive computer playing corresponding to addic- Although few studies outcome studies exist in the United
tive behavior was found in 12.3% of school-age children, ado- States, many outcome studies have been conducted in China.
lescents, and emerging adults (Batthyany, Muller, Benker, & Therefore, this review article aims to systematically review
Wolfling, 2009). This public health problem might be even IA treatment outcome studies published in English and Chinese
worse in China. According to the China Youth Internet Asso- language journals that focused on IA treatment in China from
ciation’s 2009 Report on Youth IA Disorder, around 14.3% 2000 to 2010. Our primary objective is to evaluate the metho-
of youth ages 13–17 and around 15.6% of youth ages 18–23 dological quality of different treatment outcome studies. Our
in China with computer access had IAs (China Youth Internet secondary objective is to find out which treatments are empiri-
Association, 2010). cally supported. Based on evidence-based practice require-
Although there are many cross-sectional studies on the ments, this systematic review on IA outcome studies will
correlates of IA and some review articles, most articles only help social workers identify the most effective treatments for
discuss promising treatments in passing (Brezing, Derevensky, IA. Further, by focusing on methodological attributes of these
& Potenza, 2010; Chou, Condron, & Belland, 2005; Huang, Li,
& Tao, 2010; Young, 2009a, 2009b). Thus, there is no compre-
hensive review of outcomes of different IA treatments, and there 1
School of Social Work, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL,
is no review article that focuses on the methodological quality of USA
such studies. Chou, Condron, and Belland (2005) systematically
Corresponding Author:
reviewed research on IA definitions, assessments, risk factors, Chennan Liu, School of Social Work, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign,
addictive potentials before 2005. However, only two empirical Room 2014, 1010 W Nevada, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
treatment studies, which used cognitive–behavior therapy Email: liu197@illinois.edu

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


Liu et al. 283

studies, we will identify those practices with the largest effects Wang, & Ma, 1999) to evaluate the treatment outcomes of
sizes from the highest quality studies. IA besides the IAS and CIAS in hospitals and clinical settings.
Definitions of IA vary considerably among different
scholars. Some definitions, and the resulting measures that have
been developed like Young’ (2004) IA scales, flow from the Method
conceptualizations of substance use disorders and pathological Study Selection
gambling in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
An extensive literature search was conducted both in English
Disorders (DSM-IV; Fisher, 1994; Skoric, Teo, & Neo, 2009;
academic databases (social sciences citation index [SSCI],
Young, 2004). Similar to these problems, IA is conceptualized
Social Services Abstracts, Social Work Abstracts, PsycINFO
as a problem where frequent use leads to significant impairment
w/ PsycARTICLES) and Chinese academic databases (Wan-
in one’s life (i.e., urges to play, ignoring personal responsibilities,
Fang Database, China national knowledge infrastructure [CNKI]
and loved ones). According to Block (2008), IA appears to be a
Chinese academic journal web databases, WeiPu databases)
common disorder that merits inclusion in DSM-V and concep-
published from 2000 to May 2010. Keywords such as ‘‘Internet
tually the diagnosis is a compulsive-impulsive spectrum disorder
addiction, pathological Internet use, excessive Internet use, pro-
that involves excessive online and/or computer usage use, with-
blematic Internet use, video game addiction’’ were used in the
drawal, and tolerance. Although a thorough discussion of diag-
searching combined with keywords of ‘‘treatment’’ or ‘‘interven-
nostic issues surrounding IA is outside of the scope of this
tion’’ or ‘‘evaluation’’. Based on this search, we selected studies
article, when we refer to IAhere, we include both problematic
that (a) reported posttreatment outcomes, (b) included partici-
Internet and computer use (i.e., video game) that has been diag-
pants ages 9–23 that were treated for an IA, (c) were conducted
nosed with the best available, empirically validated measures.
in China, and (d) for which we could obtain the full text report.
Some of the most commonly used scales in China are
Review papers and case study reports are not included in this
Young’s (1995–2004) diagnostic questionnaires. Young
review. We chose a large age rage since we preferred to contain
developed four questionnaires, based on pathological gambling
as many studies as possible on IA treatment and usually studies
measurement, called the IA scales (IAS). They range from 7 to
which were done in clinical settings in China had a large age
20 items (IAS-7, IAS-8, IAS-10, and IAS-20). Example items
range. Using these criteria, we located 24 studies, which are
include ‘‘do you feel the need to use the Internet with increas-
evaluated in this article.
ing amounts of time in order to achieve desired satisfaction?’’
and ‘‘have you repeatedly made unsuccessful efforts to control,
cut back, or stop Internet use?’’ The Chinese Internet Addiction Selection of Methodological Criteria
scales (CIAS 1998 and CIAS 2005) are based on Young’s Methodological quality attributes of IA outcome studies in this
(1995–2004) scales (Yang, Zheng, & Ruan, 2004; Zhang, article were established based on a recent review of adolescent
2009). The CIAS 1998 include four factors: tolerance, compul- substance abuse trials (Becker & Curry, 2008). Becker and
sive behavior and time, withdrawal symptoms, and related mental Curry reported whether 14 methodological attributes were
health problems (Yang et al., 2004). The CIAS 2005 were used in present in adolescent substance abuse treatment trials. These
a large epidemiological study in China, called the Chinese Ado- attributes are consistent with the Consolidated Standards of
lescent IA Report 2005 (Zhang, 2009). China developed its own Reporting Trials (CONSORT statement; Moher, Schulz, &
diagnostic criteria for IA in late 2008 and currently uses them in Altman, 2001). The study attributes that were coded included
treatment settings (Liu, Wang, & Zhuang, 2008). The criteria are objective, sample size, power, outcome, random sequence
as follows: (a) more than 6 hr a day of Internet use; (b) this heavy generation (i.e., how randomization was achieved), allocation
Internet use persists for more than 3 months; (c) social, study, and concealment, active comparison, baseline data, manualized
communication skills dysfunctions; (d) dependence symptoms treatment (1 ¼ yes, 0 ¼ no), treatment adherence rating,
(e.g., a strong desire and impulse) are present; and (e) withdrawal collateral report, collection of an objective measure besides
(e.g., uncomfortable, easy angered, attention deficit, sleep self-report (e.g., collateral report; 1 ¼ yes, 0 ¼ no), intention-
disorder without Internet use). to-treat (ITT) analysis, and blind assessment. In our study,
In addition to these criteria developed in China, there is we also coded whether the follow-up data were collected at
evidence that IA is associated with attention deficits, bad least 30 days after the study treatments ended. Thus, we used
academic performance, depression, self-injury, hostility, and 15 attributes to evaluate the methodological strength of the
violent behaviors (Batthyany et al., 2009; Bernardi & Pallanti, 24 IA treatment outcome studies (see Table 1).
2009; Bioulac, Arfi, & Bouvard, 2008; Caplan, Williams, &
Yee, 2009; Chan & Rabinowitz, 2006; Frolich, Lehmkuhl, &
Dopfner, 2009; Gentile, 2009; Ha, Yu, Park, & Lim, 2009; Rating Process
Hwang, Cheong, & Feeley, 2009; Kim, Namkoong, Ku, & The first and second authors independently reviewed the 24 stud-
Kim, 2008; Ko, Yen, Chen, Yeh, & Yen, 2009; Lam, Peng, ies based on the 15 attributes. After that, k coefficients were used
Mai, & Jing, 2009; Skoric et al., 2009; Sun, Ma, Bao, Chen, to assess the reliability of each attribute. Discrepancies were
& Zhang, 2008). Therefore, researchers sometimes use the resolved through discussion with the third author. All three
Symptoms Checklist 90 (SCL-90; Derogatis, 1994; Wang, authors engaged in a discussion until consensus was reached.

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


284 Research on Social Work Practice 22(3)

Table 1. Attributes of Methodological Quality

Attributes Criteria for Rating

Objective 1 ¼ Specific objectives and hypotheses. Hypotheses are amenable to explicit


statistical evaluation
0 ¼ Objectives or hypotheses not explicitly established
Sample size 1 ¼ Process for determining the sample size discussed along with any interim
analyses and stopping rules
0 ¼ Determination of sample size not discussed
Power 1 ¼ Study is sufficiently powered to detect differences between treatment groups
(e.g., at least 27 subjects per condition with passive comparison, at least 71
subjects per condition with active comparison)
0 ¼ Study is not sufficiently powered
Outcome 1 ¼ Established primary and secondary outcome measures. Primary outcome is
specified as outcome of greatest importance
0 ¼ Primary or secondary outcome measures are not specified
Random sequence generation 1 ¼ Process for generating a random sequence described with sufficient detail to
confirm that each participant had an unpredictable, independent chance of
receiving each intervention
0 ¼ Process was not purely random, unspecified
Allocation concealed 1 ¼ The result of initial assessment was not affected by whether participants in
treatment group or not. (e.g., initial assessment was before the random
assignment or initial assessment was done by unknown investigator)
0 ¼ Process was not concealed, unspecified
Active comparison 1 ¼ At least one active comparison (e.g., alternate model, treatment as usual)
0 ¼ All comparison conditions were passive (e.g., waitlist, no-treatment control,
placebo, attention control)
Baseline data 1 ¼ Baseline demographic and clinical characteristic reported by condition
0 ¼ Baseline demographic or clinical characteristic not reported
Treatment manual used 1 ¼ At least one treatment condition was guided by a manual
0 ¼ None of the treatments were guided by a manual, unspecified
Treatment adherence ratings 1 ¼ Treatment adherence monitored with scales, checklists, or rating forms
completed by therapist, supervisor, independent observer, an/or patients
0 ¼ Treatment adherence was not monitored using rating forms, unspecified
Collateral report 1 ¼ At least one outcome is a collateral report (e.g., parent, caregiver, teacher)
0 ¼ No collateral report
Objective measure 1 ¼ At least one outcome is an objective measure (e.g., computer recorded time
online, blood samples)
0 ¼ No objective verification
Intention-to-treat 2 ¼ ITT analysis. All subjects analyzed in groups to which they were assigned
1 ¼ Available cased analysis. Only subjects who completed one or more research
assessments were analyzed
0 ¼ Treated case analysis. Only subjects who completed a portion of the treatment
were analyzed
Blind assessment 1 ¼ Follow-up assessments completed by treatment-blind evaluator
0 ¼ Follow-up assessment not completed by treatment-blind evaluator, unspecified
Follow-up length >30 days posttreatment 1 ¼ At least part of follow-up data were collected at least 30 days after the study
treatments ended
0 ¼ No follow-up data were collected at least 30 days after the study treatments
ended

In conjunction with Becker and Curry’s (2008) article, we also to some designs. The first type of design is a one group pretest
used a composite quality of evidence score (QES) to indicate the and posttest, called a ‘‘preexpeerimental design’’ study. The
number of methodological attributes each study met. We coded second design style is nonrandomized comparison groups
each attribute 0 (not met or unclear) or 1 (met), except for the design. The third one is the experimental design (Rubin &
ITT analysis item where items ranged from 0 to 2 (0 ¼ treated Babbie, 2011). For preexperimental design studies, some attri-
case analysis, 1 ¼ available cased analysis, and 2 ¼ full ITT butes (e.g., random sequence generation, allocation conceal-
analysis). ment, active comparison, and baseline data) did not apply
We report QES scores separately for three different study and were coded as 0 for such studies. After summing items, the
designs found in the literature, as some QES items do not apply range of the total QES is 0–16 for experimental design studies,

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


Liu et al. 285

Table 2. Design Characteristics of Internet Use Disorder Outcome Studies

Preexperimental Nonrandomized Comparison Experimental


k (n ¼ 8; %) Groups (n ¼ 4; %) Design (n ¼ 12; %)

Design Characteristics
Objective 1 12.5 0 8.33
Baseline data 0.75 – 0 41.67
Sample size 1 0 0 8.33
Power 0.63 50 25 16.67
Outcome 0.57 50 50 33.33
Active comparison group 0.52 – 25 58.33
Random sequence generation 0.89 – – 50
Allocation concealed 0.79 – 0 16.67
Treatment manual used 0.63 12.5 0 16.67
Treatment adherence ratings 1 0 0 0
Collateral report 1 0 0 0
Objective measure 0.63 12.5 25 16.67
ITT 0.92 62.5 50 66.67
Blind assessment 0.65 0 0 16.67
Follow-up length >30 days 0.88 25 0 33.33
Total QES score (M, SD) – 2.75 (1.58) 2.25 (1.5) 4.58 (2.23)
Note. ITT ¼ intention-to-treat analysis; QES ¼ quality of evidence scores; SD ¼ standard deviation.

0–15 for nonrandomized comparison group design studies also transformed negative effect size scores to positive scores for
while the total QES is from 0 to 12 for preexperimental design better interpretation.
studies.

Effect Size Calculation Process Results


We calculated the effect sizes of the studies mainly by using
Methodological Attributes Across Studies
Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988). The studies primarily used IAS or The k values for each of the 15 methodological attributes are
SCL-90 as the outcome measurement tools in the 24 studies. described in Table 2. Except for 2 of the 15 attributes, outcome
According to these two measurement tools, higher scores indi- (k ¼ .57) and active comparison (k ¼ .52), we reached moder-
cate more severe IA or health problems, respectively. For studies ate or high agreement (ranged from 0.63 to 1.00). The average
which only used SCL-90, we chose depression factor as the rep- k value across the studies is .79, indicating a substantial agree-
resentative outcome factor to calculate effect size, since depres- ment between the two raters, in accord with Landis and Koch’s
sion was the most common correlate of IA disorders in China. If (1977) benchmark. Discrepancies were resolved through com-
the studies used continuous outcome measures and there were munication with the third author, who had advanced clinical
control groups, we calculated the effect size by subtracting postt- and research experience in outcome study evaluation. The two
est control group outcomes from those of the experimental treat- raters ultimately reached unanimous agreement on each attri-
ment group, dividing by the standard deviation of the controls. bute in order to calculate the QES.
Similarly, for studies with continuous outcome measures and The reviewed studies are classified into three categories. We
preexperimental designs, we calculated the effect size by sub- examined how each type of study meets the 16 characteristics
tracting pretest values of the dependent measures from post separately because some criteria are not applicable to all studies.
treatment values, dividing by the standard deviation of the pret- Over 50% of the studies were methodologically strong in two
est data wave. Finally, for studies using dichotomous outcome characteristics, sequence generation, and ITT analysis. However,
indicators, we used risk ratios as estimates of the effect size. less than 50% of the studies were strong in the following 13 attri-
Therefore, the effect sizes are initially expressed as negative val- butes: objective, baseline data, sample size, power, outcome,
ues, since posttest scores and treatment group scores were active comparison, allocation concealment, manualized treat-
smaller than the pretest scores and control group scores, respec- ment, treatment adherence rating, objective measure, blind
tively. So, we transformed negative values to positive ones so assessment, and 30 days follow-up.
that higher positive values indicate larger effect sizes in favor the Most frequently reported attributes. Random sequence gener-
treatment group (i.e., comparison group design) or posttest wave ation is one of the most frequently mentioned attributes. It is only
(i.e., preexperimental designs). In order to prevent outliers from applicable to the experimental design studies, as opposed to pre-
exerting too much influence on analyses, we used winsorizing to experimental studies and nonrandomized comparison group
compare effectiveness of single treatments and multimodal treat- design studies, which do not need to carry out this process. Half
ments (Erceg-Hum & Mirosevich, 2008). In this analysis, we of the experimental design studies (n ¼ 6) mentioned that they

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


286 Research on Social Work Practice 22(3)

implemented the process for generating a random sequence. For group and control group or that they collected the baseline data
example, these studies indicated that they used either a random without knowing who was in which group. Similarly, two stud-
number table or statistical software to generate random numbers ies conducted the follow-up assessment by means of a
in order to ensure that each participant had an equal chance to get treatment-blind evaluator.
intervention. For the remaining half of the studies, we cannot tell Approximately a quarter of the studies reported the fol-
how the randomization was achieved because they did not lowing characteristics: objective, manualized treatment, and
describe the process explicitly. objective measure. Only one of the eight preexperimental
The next attribute reported frequently is ITT analysis. Five studies and one of the 12 experimental studies specify their
of the eight preexperimental studies applied true ITT analyses. study objectives and hypotheses. The lack of information
The remaining three either used treated case analysis (n ¼ 2) or about hypotheses indicates that they have not articulated
available case analysis (n ¼ 1). Half of the nonrandomized which outcomes are the most important for their studies.
comparison design studies used ITT analyses, and the other There were few preexperimental studies (n ¼ 1) and experi-
half used treated case analyses (n ¼ 2). For the experimental mental studies (n ¼ 2) that mentioned using standardized
design studies, 66.67% of the studies applied ITT analyses training manuals to guide their treatments, whereas most
(n ¼ 8), whereas two used available case analyses, and two of them simply used self-designed manuals or no manual
used treated case analyses. Since sample attrition is inevitable at all. In terms of the measurement of the outcome, less than
in intervention studies, dealing with missing data in the analy- a quarter of any of these studies used objective measures as
sis is a challenge in program evaluation. ITT analysis, in which opposed to self-report scales. The final block of infrequently
all participants allocated to the study are included in the anal- encountered attributes included power, outcome, active
ysis, regardless of whether they received the treatment or not, comparison, and follow-up assessment longer than 30 days.
has been established to produce less bias in estimating the true Half of the preexperimental studies (n ¼ 4) achieved ade-
program effect than that produced by other analysis methods, quate statistical power for their design with at least 30 par-
such as available case analysis and treated case analysis (Freed- ticipants for the pretests and posttests. However, studies
man, 2005). The application of this method in these studies is with comparison groups were less likely to achieve adequate
an indication of researchers’ efforts to address the estimation statistical power because at least 71 subjects per condition
bias of the treatment. were needed for active comparison, and 27 subjects per con-
Least frequently reported attributes. Two of the least fre- dition were required for passive comparison (Kazdin &
quently reported attributes were treatment adherence ratings Bass, 1989). Half of the preexperimental studies (n ¼ 4) and
and collateral reports. None of the studies reported these half of the nonrandomized studies (n ¼ 2) established pri-
two characteristics. These studies did not use scales, check- mary and secondary outcome measures, while only two
lists, or rating forms to monitor the process of treatment experimental studies specified which outcome was more
implementation, in the absence of which we can determine important. One quarter of the nonrandomized experimental
neither whether the participants complied with the treat- studies (n ¼ 1) used active comparison involving psy-
ments nor whether the therapists implemented the models chotherapy to treat IA for the control group, and 7 of the
as intended. Consequently, it is difficult to tell what treat- 12 experimental studies applied active comparison such as
ments were provided. Similarly, none of the studies used medication or exercise therapy. Finally, two preexperimen-
collateral reports, Therefore there is no control of potential tal studies conducted a follow-up assessment after 30 days
bias in self-reported outcomes. to track the lasting effects of the treatment, whereas 4 of the
The next least frequently reported attributes included base- 12 experimental studies did so.
line data, sample size, allocation concealment, and blind
assessment. Only less than 50% of the experimental design
studies reported these four attributes. Nonrandomized compar-
ison group design studies ignored these four attributes com-
Quality of Evidence and Effect Sizes
pletely. None of preexperimental design studies reported the Among the 24 studies, the most commonly used treatments
baseline data, sample size, and blind assessment. Five of the for IA are exercise programs (EP), CBT, electroacupuncture
12 experimental studies provided information concerning base- (EA), family therapy (FT), group-based treatment (GT), moti-
line demographics and clinical characteristics of participants in vational interviewing (MI), and psychotropic medications
both experimental groups and control groups. For example, (M). As indicated in Table 3, more than half of the studies
they provided t tests or chi-square tests for the demographic have multiple treatments. Except for studies by Du, Jiang,
characteristics and participants’ Internet use behavior at base- and Vance (2010) and Pan and Dai (2010), the rest of 22
line, through which we established whether there was preexist- studies all have statistically significant effects favoring the
ing selection bias between the two groups. Only one treatment. The overall mean effect size is 1.89, indicative
experimental study explicitly showed their power calculation of very large effects. Some of the studies which use CBT,
formula to show how the sample size was determined. Two M, and FT have extremely high effect size (> 5), such as
experimental studies specifically mentioned that they collected Shen (2008), Yang, Shao, and Zheng (2005), and Shao,
their baseline data before they randomized the intervention Yang, Luo, and Zheng (2004). However, most of those

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


Liu et al. 287

Table 3. Effect Sized and Methodological Quality Scores for Chinese Internet Addiction Studies

Outcomes

Author, Year, (Quality Score) n Treatment(s) d (IAS) d (SCL-90)

Continuous Outcome Measures-Comparison Group Designs


1. Du, Jiang, and Vance, 2010 (10) 56 CBT 0.13
2. Guo, Yu, Chao, and Huang, 2008 (1) 32 GT, CBT 2.08
3. Li, Chen, Zhang, and Jia, 2009 (3) 32 EP 3.26 4.44
4. Liao and Wang, 2009 (1) 50 EP 0.82
5. Lin, Zou, Xie, Lv, and Lin, 2006 (4) 30 M, CBT 0.68
6. Liu and Li, 2008 (5) 60 CBT, FT, GT 2.58
7. Shao, Yang, Luo, and Yang, 2005 (5) 66 M, CBT 2.88 0.54
8. Sun and Wang, 2008 (4) 220 CBT, GT 2.80
9. Wei, 2008a (4) 60 MT, EP, GT, FT, HE 3.83
10. Wu, Yan, and Han, 2007 (3) 27 EA 3.78
11. Wu and Yang, 2008 (3) 126 M, CBT 4.06
12. Zhang, 2009 (4) 163 EP 0.3386
13. Zhan and Li, 2009 (5) 60 M, CBT 1.35
14. Zhong, Tao, Zhu, Sha, and Yang, 2009 (7) 51 MT, CBT, GT 1.27
15. Zhu, Jin, Zhong, Chen, and Li, 2008 (4) 45 EA, CBT 1.86
Continuous outcome measures-preexperimental designs
16. Li, Li, Wang, and Zhang, 2008 (4) 48 M, CBT, FT, GT 0.67
17. Pan and Dai, 2010 (1) 11 EBT 0.29 1.14
18. Shao, Yang, Luo, and Zheng, 2004 (2) 20 CBT 14.5
19. Shek, Tang, and Lo, 2009 (4) 59 CBT, MI, FT, VS 1.45
20. Shen, 2008 (0) 27 CBT, M 10.7 18
21. Wei, 2008b (3) 273 EP, CBT, FT, GT, HE 1.71
22. Yang and Hao, 2005 (4) 52 CBT, FT, SBT 2.69
23. Yang, Shao, and Zheng, 2005 (1) 23 CBT, FT, M 13.43
Dichotomous outcome indicators
24. Liu, Yang, and Yao, 2007 (4) 40 M, CBT 0.875

Note. CBT ¼ cognitive behavior therapy; EA ¼ electroacupuncture; EBT ¼ electroencephalographic biofeedback treatment; EP ¼ exercise program; FT ¼ family
therapy; GT ¼ group-based treatment; HE ¼ health education; M ¼ medication; MI ¼ motivational interview; MT ¼ military training; SBT ¼ solution-focused brief
therapy; VS ¼ voluntary service.

studies have low quality scores (<5), which means that the methodological quality for these studies is around 4. Based
quality of the evidence is low. on Tables 3 and 4, when there are extremely large effect sizes
CBT treatments combined with medications were the most (>5) as study 18, 20, 23 the QES falls below 2.
frequently used therapies in the 24 studies. The QES scores for
these studies ranged from 0 to 5 (mean ¼ 3.4). The average Single Treatments and Multimodal Treatments
effect size of CBT treatments combined with medications is
3.93. There are three studies which only used EP as the treat- We compared effect sizes of studies which used single treat-
ment (shown in Table 4). The average effect size of the three ments with studies which used multiple treatments. The aver-
studies is 1.47 while the mean QES is 2.7. Two studies only age effect size for single treatments is 1.77, and the average
used CBT. For one such study with a high QES (i.e., 10) the effect size for multiple treatments is 2.3. However, the t test
effect size is .13 (not statistically significant). However, for the is not statistically significant (t ¼ .39). Using Chambless and
lower quality study (QES ¼ 2), the effect size is 14.5. Two Hollons’ (1998) criteria most of the treatments we reviewed
studies used EA. This was a unique treatment in China, which would be considered possibly efficacious, with no replication
uses electric stimulation to the body to reduce the desire to use studies for any treatment we reviewed (Chambless & Hollon,
the Internet. Since 2009, this method has been suspended in 1998).
China due to the unclear negative consequences (Wei sheng
bu jiao ting dian ji zhi liao wang yin [Chinese Ministry of
Health ban on electric stimulus therapy for Internet addition
Discussion and Applications to
disorder], 2009). One study used EBT which is a new biofeed- Social Work
back machine treatment. However, the findings showed that The primary objective of the study was to review the published
biofeedback machine therapy did not have statistically signifi- outcome studies on IA in the Chinese context. Following the
cant effects on IA. The combination of CBT, GT, FT, or others procedures of Becker and Curry (2008) we evaluated 24 IA
have average effect sizes ranging from 1.27 to 5.86, but the treatment studies. Our main finding was that only 2 of the 15

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


288 Research on Social Work Practice 22(3)

Table 4. Quality of Evidence Scores and Effect Size by Type of Treatment

Treatment Study Number Mean QES (Range) Mean Effect Size (Range)

EP 12, 3, 4 2.7 (1–4) 1.47 (0.34–3.26)


CBT 1, 18 6 (2–10) 7.3 (0.13–14.5)
EA 10 3 3.78
EBT 17 1 0.29
CBT, M 11, 7, 13, 5, 20, 24 3.4 (0–5) 3.93 (0.68–10.7)
CBT, GT 2, 8 2.5 (1–4) 2.44 (2.08–2.8)
CBT, EA 15 4 1.86
CBT, GT þ others 14 7 1.27
CBT, FT þ others 22, 23, 19 3 (1–4) 5.86 (1.45–13.43)
CBT, GT, FT þ others 6, 21, 16 4 (3–5) 1.65 (0.67–2.58)
MT, BT, GT, FT, HE 9 4 3.83
Note. CBT ¼ cognitive behavior therapy; EA ¼ electroacupuncture; EBT ¼ electroencephalographic biofeedback treatment; EP ¼ exercise program; FT ¼ family
therapy; GT ¼ group-based treatment; HE ¼ health education; M ¼ medication; MI ¼ motivational interview; MT ¼ military training; QES ¼ quality of evidence
scores; SBT ¼ solution-focused brief therapy; VS ¼ voluntary service.

attributes were reported by more than 50% of the three types of review article had a QES lower than 5. Because of this finding,
study designs. Although random sequence generation is a it is important that future trials use more rigorous designs and
necessity for experimental studies, only half of them mentioned adhere to established standards of clinical trial reporting.
how they generated a random sequence, and the other half did According to Chambless and Hollon (1998), at least two
not indicate how they randomized participants. In addition, the repeated rigorous treatment studies are needed to identify a
remaining methodological criteria were poorly met, indicating promising treatment. Using Chambless and Hollons’ (1998)
most research did not establish reporting guidelines for clinical criteria most of the treatments we reviewed would be desig-
trials (Moher et al., 2001). nated as possibly efficacious, since no replication studies
None of the studies contained treatment adherence ratings or existed for any treatment we reviewed and power was too low
collateral reports, which indicates that research paid a minimal in most cases to detect moderate effect sizes. Rigorous designs
amount of attention to these areas. Only the experimental stud- and reporting can help researchers and social workers to iden-
ies reported baseline data, sample size, allocation concealment, tify whether studies are rigorously replicated. The rigorous
and blind assessment. Few studies indicated which outcomes design and reporting will also prevent potential biased estima-
were primary. Similarly, few studies used manualized treat- tion of effect size for the IA treatment in the future. There is
ments. Replication by outside investigators, a criterion for evidence that inadequate methodological approaches and
establishing a treatment as empirically supported (Chambless reporting are associated with over-estimation of treatment
& Hollon, 1998), would be difficult in the absence of treatment effects and failure to consider the quality of methodology and
manuals. The lack of objective measures also threatened the reporting limits the ability to detect potentially inflated treat-
reliability of the outcomes. Finally, the small sample sizes of ment estimates, identify sources of bias and inform best prac-
these studies was concerning, as inadequate statistical power tices in the field (Juni, Altman, & Egger, 2001; Moyer &
limits our abilities to detect true differences if they were indeed Finney, 2005).
present. There were some limitations for this study. First, we
Our secondary objective was to determine which treatment reported low k values with respect to two attributes, outcome
was more effective. However, based on low methodological and active comparison, indicating a relatively low interrater
quality scores here and the limited number of studies, this was reliability for some characteristics. There were high discrepan-
a difficult task. Thus, we have only described trends in this cies initially for these two characteristics. For example, one
study, and have not completed formal moderation analyses. rater believed the outcome was presented only if the study dif-
In future studies, the QES criteria could be explored as modera- ferentiated which outcome was primary and which was second-
tors of treatment effects. However, due to the limited number of ary. However, another rater was more generous in coding the
studies, there was not enough statistical power in this study to outcome as ‘‘1’’ as long as the study presented two or more out-
enable such an analysis. Another approach is to find studies comes in which at least one pertained to IA behavior. Similarly,
with high methodological quality, and then to see whether such one rater believed the study applied active comparison only if
studies have high effect sizes. Comparatively, in Becker and the comparison group had received evidenced-based interven-
Curry’s (2008) study, the median QES was 7, reflecting the rel- tion to treat Internet addition, whereas the other rater applied a
atively higher maturity of the evidence-base for that popula- broader rule and coded a ‘‘1’’ if the comparison group received
tion. That was, in our study, only 1 study of the 24 studies some treatments, no matter whether they were evidenced-based
examined that has a QES larger than 7. And this study had a or not. Despite the low interrater reliability for these two attri-
low effect size of .13 without statistically significant differ- butes, we observed modest or high k values for the rest of the
ences between treatment conditions. Most of the studies in this attributes. In addition, for these two attributes and other

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


Liu et al. 289

attributes having k values lower than one, unanimous is discouraging that the QES scores for experimental studies in
agreement was achieved through dialogue with an experienced this review are not higher. Indeed, they were lower than those
colleague. of randomized trials in other reviews (Becker & Curry, 2008),
It was also possible that studies were more rigorous than we indicating that methodology quality is relatively weak in the
have estimated, as we could only score studies based on their area of IA intervention studies. The lack of scientifically
reporting of methodological attributes. Thus, it was possible established evidence hampers researchers’ abilities to inform
that we underestimated the QES scores for some studies. As practitioners of promising treatments for IA. Therefore, we
long as the authors did not explicitly mention the attributes in suggest researchers apply more rigorous experimental designs
the article, we treated them as not having those attributes in their research because only through randomized controlled
although they may have simply not reported doing so. The experiment design can a counterfactual model be set up to
research conventions in the context of Chinese academic determine true treatment effectiveness (Shadish, Cook, &
language might divert the publication format away from the Campbell, 2002).
American academic conventions even further. For example, The second implication of this study is that there exists a
many articles were between one to four pages in length, which need for a more uniform reporting style for outcome studies
is a highly condensed publication format. in China. The CONSORT statement (Moher et al., 2001) is
The limited number of studies and nonconsistent outcome lauded as the gold standard for transparency in clinical trial
measurement tools constrained our ability to fulfill the second reporting in the United States. Chinese scholars could develop
main objectives of this review. Thus, we were limited by our similar reporting criteria or follow CONSORT criteria to
small sample size of 24 studies. In addition, these 24 studies enhance the quality of study and reporting.
used different outcomes measurement tools to report how peo- Finally, one implication for social work practice pertains to
ple recover from IA. Some of the studies only reported changes social workers choosing the best evidence about IA treatments.
in symptoms, such as depression or anxiety, without reporting In short, this study highlights how, social workers should not
changes in IA criteria. It was problematic to compare effect choose treatments only based on simple counts of how many
sizes calculated based on different outcome measurement tools. studies have found positive effects for a specific treatment.
Finally, we acknowledge that it is yet unclear how vulnera- As we have seen in this study, not all clinical trials are equally
ble and impoverished youth are affected by IA. For example, rigorous. Thus, criteria for determining whether a particular
Eamon’s (2004) analysis of a nationally representative sample treatment is empirically supported should consider a more
found that poor youth had less computer access and were less sophisticated weighting scheme to account for studies with low
likely to use computers less for nonacademic purposes. Future methodological quality, which could inflate effect sizes. To be
clinical research should explicitly report participant demo- specific, social workers should continue to receive training in
graphic characteristics so social workers know if vulnerable being good consumers of research, with emphasis on identify-
youth are affected by IA and represented in these studies. ing attributes of studies that are associated with higher
Given that few of the methodological standards were met, methodological quality. Although the literature is limited with
researchers are encouraged to include as many essential ele- regards to the most promising IA treatments, studies reviewed
ments of design as possible and contribute to establishing here with a QES equal or larger than 4 (i.e., the median) have
reporting guidelines for intervention studies. As the classic relatively higher methodological quality.
experimental design is characterized by criteria such as In terms of IA treatment, the most popular treatment is CBT.
sequence generation, allocation concealment, and baseline Unfortunately, replication studies are needed to conclusively
data, these attributes should be reported when they have been determine whether CBT is a promising treatment, since one
implemented. Some criteria such as using objective measures, study had a low QES score, and a more rigorous trial found a
establishing primary outcome measures, having adequate weak, statistically insignificant effect (d ¼ .13). With the above
power, and using manualized treatment, are easier to meet than evidence, social worker can only treat CBT as a possibly effi-
other criteria; therefore, researchers should report them. For cacious treatment. CBT combined with medication seems like
those attributes, including treatment adherence rating, collat- a promising therapy with a mean effect size of 3.93 and mean
eral report, objective measure, and blind assessment, as they QES of 3.4. However, when we looked deeply, we found that
are embedded in more rigorous designs and are difficult to be five of the CBT plus medication studies used the SCL-90 as the
fulfilled, researchers might consider including them while outcome measurement, and did not report actual changes in IA
designing their studies. The bottom line is that when imple- symptoms. Although the studies were using medications that
menting a design, researchers should not neglect to report targeted depression, it would also be informative to know if
them. Otherwise, the audience will assume that the study has these medications work for IA symptoms. Future trials could
not been conducted rigorously. use 2  2 designs to partition the effects of medications and
The overall QES of the 24 studies is relatively low (Mean ¼ cognitive behavioral treatments. EPs had a mean effect size
3.58, SD ¼ 2.12, Median ¼ 4, Range ¼ 0–10), suggesting there of 1.47 and mean QES of 2.7. The effect size was high however
are many improvements needed for experimental studies on IA the quality of the studies was low. Taking into consideration
treatment. As experimental designs are weighted heavily in the of effect size inflation, EP should be viewed somewhat
determination of whether practices are empirically supported, it skeptically, and studied further in more rigorous trials.

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


290 Research on Social Work Practice 22(3)

Electroencephalographic biofeedback treatment did not have Chan, P. A., & Rabinowitz, T. (2006). A cross-sectional analysis of
statistically significant effects (d ¼ .29), so social workers video games and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms
should proceed with caution with this treatment until additional in adolescents. Annals of General Psychiatry, 5, 16.
studies support its efficacy. CBT, combined with group therapy China Youth Internet Association. (2010, January). 2009 nian qing
and military training had a QES of 7 and effect size of 1.27. shao nian wang yin diao cha bao gao [2009 report on youth inter-
This treatment fulfilled the criteria with a relatively higher net addiction disorder]. Retrieved from http://www.docin.com/p-
methodological quality and effect size. Therefore, this treat- 46439854.html
ment was the most promising treatment we reviewed in this Chou, C., Condron, L., & Belland, J. C. (2005). A review of the
study. CBTs combined with either group or FT had a mean research on internet addiction. Educational Psychology Review,
QES of 4 and mean effect size of 1.65. The average effect size 17, 363-388.
was high, but these studies were only average in terms of Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavior sciences
methodological quality. Relative to other treatments we (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
studied, these seem promising, with the general caveat that Derogatis, L. R. (1994). Symptom Checklist 90-R: Administration,
greater specification of these treatments and more rigorously scoring, and procedures manual (3rd ed.). Minneapolis, MN:
designed studies are needed. Based on this review, possibly National Computer Systems.
efficacious treatments for IA are cognitive behavior therapies Du, Y. S., Jiang, W. Q., & Vance, A. (2010). Longer term effect of
that are combined with group therapy, military training, or FT. randomized, controlled group cognitive behavioral therapy for
internet addiction in adolescent students in Shanghai. Australian
Acknowledgments and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 129-134.
The authors would like to thank Mary Keegan Eamon and Jun Sung Eamon, M. K. (2004). Digital divide in computer access and use
Hong for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article. between poor and non-poor youth. Journal of Sociology and Social
Welfare, 31, 91-112.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Erceg-Hum, D. M., & Mirosevich, V. M. (2008). Modern robust
statistical methods. American Psychologist, 63, 591-601.
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
Fisher, S. (1994). Identifying video game addiction in children and
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 19, 545-553.
Frolich, J., Lehmkuhl, G., & Dopfner, M. (2009). Computer games in
Funding
childhood and adolescence: Relations to addictive behavior,
The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, ADHD, and aggression. Zeitschrift Fur Kinder-Und Jugendpsy-
and/or publication of this article. chiatrie Und Psychotherapie, 37, 393-402.
Gentile, D. (2009). Pathological video-game use among youth ages 8
References to 18: A national study. Psychological Science, 20, 594-602.
Batthyany, D., Muller, K. W., Benker, F., & Wolfling, K. (2009). Gruesser, S. M., Thalemann, R., Albrecht, U., & Thalemann, C. N.
Computer game playing: Clinical characteristics of dependence (2005). Excessive computer usage in adolescents: A psychometric
and abuse among adolescents. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift, evaluation. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift, 117, 188-195.
121, 502-509. Guo, M., Yu, F., Chao, X. L., & Huang, G. M. (2008). Impact evalua-
Becker, S. J., & Curry, J. F. (2008). Outpatient interventions for ado- tion of group counseling on internet addiction adolescents. Chinese
lescent substance abuse: A quality of evidence review. Journal of Journal of School Health, 29, 17-19.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 531-543. Ha, J., Yu, J., Park, D. H., & Lim, W. (2009). Depression is a major
Bernardi, S., & Pallanti, S. (2009). Internet addiction: A descriptive cause of internet addiction in adolescents and young adults: 2 year
clinical study focusing on comorbidities and dissociative symp- experience in internet addiction clinic. International Journal of
toms. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50, 510-516. Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 13, 30-31.
Bioulac, S., Arfi, L., & Bouvard, M. P. (2008). Attention deficit/ Huang, X. Q., Li, M. C., & Tao, R. (2010). Treatment of internet
hyperactivity disorder and video games: A comparative study of addiction. Current Psychiatry Report, 12, 462-470.
hyperactive and control children. European Psychiatry, 23, Hwang, J. M., Cheong, P. H., & Feeley, T. H. (2009). Being young and
134-141. feeling blue in Taiwan: Examining adolescent depressive mood and
Brezing, C., Derevensky, J. L., & Potenza, M. N. (2010). online and offline activities. New Media & Society, 11, 1101-1121.
Non-substance-addictive behaviors in youth: Pathological Block, J. J. (2008). Issues for DSM-V: Internet addiction. The
gambling and problematic internet use. Child and Adolescent American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 306-307.
Psychiatric Clinical of North America, 19, 625-641. Juni, P., Altman, D. G., & Egger, M. (2001). Systematic reviews in
Caplan, S., Williams, D., & Yee, N. (2009). Problematic internet use health care: Assessing the quality of controlled clinical trials.
and psychosocial well-being among MMO players. Computers in British Medical Journal, 323, 42-46.
Human Behavior, 25, 1312-1319. Kim, E. J., Namkoong, K., Ku, T., & Kim, S. J. (2008). The relation-
Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining empirically sup- ship between online game addiction and aggression, self-control
ported therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, and narcissistic personality traits. European Psychiatry, 23,
66, 7-18. 212-218.

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


Liu et al. 291

Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. S., Yeh, Y. C., & Yen, C. F. (2009). Shek, D. T. L., Tang, V. M. Y., & Lo, C. Y. (2009). Evaluation of an
Predictive values of psychiatric symptoms for internet addiction internet addiction treatment program for Chinese adolescents in
in adolescents: A 2-year prospective study. Archives of Pediatrics Hong Kong. Adolescence, 44, 359-373.
& Adolescent Medicine, 163, 937-943. Shen, Z. M. (2008). Xi tai pu lan zhi liao zhong du wang luo yi lai
Lam, L. T., Peng, Z., Mai, J., & Jing, J. (2009). The association 27 li liao xiao fen xi [Treatment effect of anti-depression drug
between internet addiction and self-injurious behavior among on internet addiction for 27 clients]. Tianjin Pharmacy, 20, 61-
adolescents. Injury Prevention, 15, 403-408. 74.
Li, L., Chen, Y., Zhang, Y., & Jia, F. (2009). Jian shen yun dong chu Skoric, M. M., Teo, L. L. C., & Neo, R. L. (2009). Children and video
fang dui da xue sheng wang yin gan yu xiao guo de ying xiang [The games: Addiction, engagement, and scholastic achievement.
effects of fitness exercise prescriptions on intervening college stu- Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 12, 567-572.
dent’s internet addiction]. Journal of Physical Education, 16, 55–58. Sun, D. L., Ma, N., Bao, M., Chen, X. C., & Zhang, D. R. (2008).
Li, L., Li, G. Y., Wang, Y. Y., & Zhang, S. Y. (2008). The effects of Computer games: A double-edged sword? Cyberpsychology &
residential hospital treatment on 48 internet addiction patients. Behavior, 11, 545-548.
Journal of Psychiatry, 21, 356-360. Sun, J., & Wang, H. C. (2008). Evaluation of multiple therapy inter-
Liao, X. H., & Wang, Z. Z. (2009). You yang jian shen cao dui wang vention to internet addiction for college students. Chinese Journal
luo cheng yin da xue sheng gan yu xiao guo de yan jiu [Intervention of School Health, 29, 1137-1139.
of aerobic exercise for internet addiction college student]. Journal Wang, X. D., Wang, X. L., & Ma, H. (1999). Xin li wei sheng ping
of Hunan University of Technology, 23, 86-88. ding liang biao shou ce [Mental health assessment manual].
Lin, Z. X., Zou, X. B., Xie, B., Lv, D., & Lin, J. D. (2006). Qing shao Beijing: Chinese Mental Health Journal Institution.
nian wang luo cheng yin xin li yao wu lian he zhi liao [Psycholo- Wei, Q. X. (2008a). The role of psychological nursing intervention in
gical and medical treatments on youth internet addiction disorder]. the treatment of internet addiction. Journal of Heze Medical Col-
Nervous Diseases and Mental Health, 6, 127-129. lege, 10, 16-18.
Liu, Q. S., & Li, Z. Q. (2008). Zhu yuan wang luo cheng yin huan zhe Wei, Q. X. (2008b). Xing wei jiao zhi yu xin li gan yu dui wang luo
xin li gan yu de kang fu xiao guo ping jia [Rehabilitation effect of cheng yin huan zhe kang fu xiao guo de ying xiang [Affection of
the psychological intervention on internet addiction patients in behavior correction and mental intervention on the rehabilitation
hospital]. Chinese Journal of School Health, 29, 1117-1119. efficacy of internet addicts]. Journal of Military Surgeon in South-
Liu, X. Q., Wang, J. G., & Zhuang, H. H. (2008, November 11). The west China, 10, 16-18.
Chinese diagnostic criteria for internet addiction was approved by Wei sheng bu jiao ting dian ji zhi liao wang yin [Chinese Ministry of
clinic experts. Xin Hua News Agency. Retrieved from http:// Health ban on electric stimulus therapy for internet addition disor-
www.gov.cn/jrzg/2008-11/08/content_1143277.htm der]. (2009, July 13). Retrieved from http://news.163.com/09/
Liu, Y., Yang, G. D., & Yao, X. M. (2007). The effect of pharma- 0713/17/5E4D5T7P000120GU.html
cotherapy and psychotherapy with 40 IAD cases. Chinese Journal Wu, L. Z., Yan, J. J., & Han, J. S. (2007). 2/100 Hz jing pi xue zhu dian
of Drug Abuse Prevention and Treatment, 13, 88-91. chi ji dui 27 li qing shao nian wang luo cheng yin zheng de zhi liao
Moher, D., Schulz, K. F., & Altman, D. (2001). The CONSORT zuo yong [Treatment on 27 adolescents with Internet addiction by
statement: revised recommendations for improving the quality 2/100 HZ Han’s acupoint nerve stimulation]. Chinese Journal of
of reports of parallel-group randomized trials. Lancet, 357, 1191- Drug Dependence, 16, 32-35.
1194. Wu, S. L., & Yang, Y. X. (2008). The evaluation of cognitive behavior
Moyer, A., & Finney, J. W. (2005). Rating methodological quality: therapy on internet addiction. China Medical Herald, 5, 161-162.
Toward improved assessment and investigation. Accountability Yang, F. R., & Hao, W. (2005). 52 li wang luo cheng yin qing shao
in Research, 12, 229-313. nian xin li she hui zong he gan yu de liao xiao guan cha [The effect
Pan, S. J., & Dai, X. Y. (2010). Nao dian sheng wu fan kui zhi liao of integrated psychosocial intervention on 52 adolescents with
zhong xue sheng wang luo cheng yin de xiao guo guan cha [Effi- internet addiction disorder]. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychol-
ciency of electroencephalographic biofeedback treatment in mid- ogy, 13, 343-346.
dle school students with internet addiction disorder]. Journal of Yang, R., Shao, Z., & Zheng, Y. (2005). zhong xue sheng wang luo
Ningxia Medical University, 32, 71-73. cheng yin zheng de zong he gan yu [Comprehensive intervention
Rubin, A., & Babbie, E. R. (2011). Research methods for social work on internet addiction of middle school students]. Chinese Mental
(7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole. Health Journal, 19, 457-459.
Shao, Z., Yang, R., Luo, K. L., & Yang, K. (2005). Fu xi ting dui ban Yang, R., Zheng, Y., & Ruan, K. L. (2004). Wang luo cheng yin shi
yi yu de zhong xue sheng wang luo cheng yin zheng de lin chuang zheng yan jiu jin zhan [Research progress on internet addiction dis-
jiao zhi zuo yong [The effect of fluoxetine in treating middle school order]. Journal of Southwest China Normal University, 30, 2-5.
students with depressant internet addiction disorder]. Chinese Young, K. (2009a). Internet addiction: Diagnosis and treatment
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorder, 31, 422-425. consideration. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 39, 241-
Shao, Z., Yang, R., Luo, K. L., & Zheng, Y. (2004). Xin li zhi liao zhong 246.
xue sheng wang luo cheng yin zheng de lin chuang yan jiu [Clinical Young, K. (2009b). Understanding online gaming addiction and treat-
research on psychological treatment for adolescent internet addic- ment issues for adolescents. American Journal of Family Therapy,
tion]. Chinese Journal of Child Health Care, 12, 548-549. 37, 355-372.

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015


292 Research on Social Work Practice 22(3)

Young, K. S. (2004). Internet addiction: A new clinical phenomenon Zhong, X., Tao, R., Zhu, S., Sha, S., & Yang, F. C. (2009). Tuan ti xin
and its consequences. American Behavioral Scientist, 48, 402-415. li gan yu dui qing shao nian wang luo cheng yin de xiao guo yan jiu
Zhan, L. Y., & Li, R. Q. (2009). Nei guan ren zhi liao fa dui wang yin [Effect of group psychological intervention in adolescent on
huan zhe xin li kang fu de zuo yong [Effect of neikan cognitive internet addiction]. Journal of Capital Medical University, 30,
therapy on psychological status of inpatients with internet 494-499.
addiction]. Journal of Nursing Science, 24, 77-79. Zhu, T. M., Jin, R. J., Zhong, X. M., Chen, J., & Li, H. (2008). Dian
Zhang, Y. H. (2009). Yi ti yu huo dong wei zhu yao shou duan gan yu zhen jie he xin li gan yu dui wang yin huan zhe jiao lv zhuang tai ji
da xue sheng wang luo cheng yin de shi zheng yan jiu tan jiu xie qing NE bian hua de ying xiang [Effects of electroacupuncture
[Experimental study to interfere with college students’ internet combined with psychological interference on anxiety state and
addiction by using sports as main means]. Journal of Tonghua serum NE content in the patient of internet addiction disorder].
Teachers College, 30, 74-76. Chinese Acupuncture & Moxibustion, 28, 561-564.

Downloaded from rsw.sagepub.com at OAKLAND UNIV on April 6, 2015

Anda mungkin juga menyukai