Anda di halaman 1dari 14

Journal of Personality Assessment

ISSN: 0022-3891 (Print) 1532-7752 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjpa20

The Mentalization Scale (MentS): A Self-Report


Measure for the Assessment of Mentalizing
Capacity

Aleksandar Dimitrijević, Nataša Hanak, Ana Altaras Dimitrijević & Zorana


Jolić Marjanović

To cite this article: Aleksandar Dimitrijević, Nataša Hanak, Ana Altaras Dimitrijević & Zorana
Jolić Marjanović (2018) The Mentalization Scale (MentS): A Self-Report Measure for the
Assessment of Mentalizing Capacity, Journal of Personality Assessment, 100:3, 268-280, DOI:
10.1080/00223891.2017.1310730

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2017.1310730

Published online: 24 Apr 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 460

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 2 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hjpa20
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT
2018, VOL. 100, NO. 3, 268–280
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2017.1310730

The Mentalization Scale (MentS): A Self-Report Measure for the Assessment


of Mentalizing Capacity
Aleksandar Dimitrijevic,1,2 Natasa Hanak,3 Ana Altaras Dimitrijevic,1 and Zorana Jolic Marjanovic1
1
Department of Psychology, University of Belgrade, Serbia; 2International Psychoanalytic University, Berlin, Germany; 3Faculty of Special Education
and Rehabilitation, Unviersity of Belgrade, Serbia

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


The psychometric properties of a new 28-item self-report measure of mentalization, the Mentalization Received 28 October 2015
Scale (MentS), were examined in 2 studies: with a sample of employed adults and university students Revised 6 February 2017
(N1 D 288 C 278) and with a sample of persons with borderline personality disorder (BPD) and matched
controls (N2 D 62 C 62). Besides the MentS, both studies employed measures of attachment and the Big
Five; Study 1 also included assessments of empathy and emotional intelligence. MentS whole-scale
internal consistency was good in the community and acceptable in the clinical sample (a D .84 and .75,
respectively). A principal components analysis of Study 1 data yielded 3 interpretable factors, or subscales:
Self-Related Mentalization (MentS-S), Other-Related Mentalization (MentS-O), and Motivation to Mentalize
(MentS-M). These showed acceptable reliabilities (a D .74–.79), except for MentS-M in the clinical sample
(a D .60). MentS scores further exhibited a coherent pattern of correlations with cognate constructs and
the Big Five, relating positively to empathy, trait and ability emotional intelligence, openness, extraversion,
and conscientiousness, and negatively to attachment avoidance and anxiety, and neuroticism. Persons
with BPD scored significantly lower on MentS total and MentS-S. The proposed scale is thus deemed
suitable for quick, yet meaningful, assessments of mentalization in both individual differences research
and clinical contexts.

Mentalization is defined as “the mental process by which an efforts to identify mental states underlying particular
individual implicitly and explicitly interprets the actions of behavior, recognition of developmental aspects of mental
himself and others as meaningful on the basis of intentional states, and awareness of mental states in relation to the
mental states such as personal desires, needs, feelings, beliefs, interviewer (Holmes, 2006).
and reasons” (Bateman & Fonagy, 2004, p. 21). It is conceptu- 4. Signs of distorted mentalization are also established and
ally closely aligned or even overlapping with such constructs as include a lack of interest in mental states, lack of imagi-
empathy, social cognition, emotional intelligence (EI), and the- nation about the mental world of others, an emphasis on
ory of mind (ToM; Allen, 2006). However, unlike empathy and external factors and behavioral descriptions, and loss of
social cognition, mentalization also involves self-reflection; it awareness of the relationship between internal and exter-
goes beyond perceiving and understanding emotions (the realm nal reality (Fonagy & Target, 2008; Luyten, Fonagy,
of EI) or attributing intentions, thoughts, and beliefs (the realm Lowyck, & Vermote, 2011).
of ToM), calling on various inner states and processes to inter- 5. Mentalizing problems, just like insecure attachment,
pret manifest behavior. might stem from a lack of parental sensitivity and failure
Moreover, the concept of mentalization has spurred a dis- to foster the exploration of mental states in children
tinctive and coherent line of research and clinical applications, (Bateman & Fonagy, 2006).
the main points of which can be summarized as follows: 6. Deficient mentalization seems to play an important role
1. The capacity to mentalize progresses along a distinctive in various mental disorders (for a detailed review, see
developmental trajectory (Fonagy & Allison, 2012). Bateman & Fonagy, 2011), and is particularly recognized
2. Mentalization is closely related to the quality of attach- as a core feature of borderline personality disorder
ment (Fonagy & Target, 2008), with securely attached (BPD), with BPD-diagnosed persons scoring signifi-
persons displaying a superior capacity to mentalize com- cantly lower than controls on an established measure of
pared to those with insecure, particularly disorganized mentalization.
patterns (Fonagy, Target, Steele, & Steele, 1998). 7. Vice versa, mentalization is thought to promote mental
3. Indicators of a well-developed mentalization capacity health and to be related with such qualities as resilience
include awareness of the nature of mental states, explicit (Stein, 2006).

CONTACT Aleksandar Dimitrijevic a.dimitrijevic@ipu-berlin.de International Psychoanalytic University, Stromstraße 3, 10555 Berlin, Germany.
Natasa Hanak is now at AWO Socio-Educational Institution for Children and Adolescents “Clearinghaus,” Dortmund, Germany.

© 2018 Taylor & Francis


THE MENTALIZATION SCALE (MENTS) 269

8. Finally, according to a specific analysis of systematic clin- Finally, a third path to mentalization assessment would
ical interviews (Abu-Akel & Bo, 2013), there appear to be be to employ self-report measures based on an analysis of
significant gender differences in mentalization in favor of the key indicators of the construct. Mentalization assess-
women, as has repeatedly been evidenced for the related ment would thereby espouse the same idea that has been
construct of empathy. fruitfully applied in the neighboring domain of EI research:
Performance measures demonstrate what one is capable of,
and self-reports tell us how much of that capability is expe-
Mentalization assessment
rienced in daily life (Mikolajczak, Nelis, Hansenne, &
Although the construct of mentalization has solid research Quoidbach, 2008). In other words, self-report measures of
and clinical underpinnings, its assessment is still fraught mentalization would make a valid complement to techni-
with considerable challenges. For one, a review of instru- ques like the RFS; at the same time, they would be much
ments used in mentalization assessment (Luyten & Fonagy, easier to apply with large samples, thus allowing mentaliza-
2014; Luyten et al., 2011) reveals a habit of borrowing tion research in the vein of differential psychology.
measures actually designed to assess other constructs, such Several commendable attempts have already been made to
as the Empathy Quotient (EQ; Baron-Cohen & Wheel- create paper-and-pencil measures of mentalization based on
wright, 2004), originally intended to assess empathic capac- self-report, yet at each instance certain reliability, validity, or
ity, or the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence applicability issues seem to have remained unresolved. To start
Test Version 2.0 (MSCEITv2.0), which is in fact a direct with, the Mentalization Questionnaire (MZQ; Hausberg et al.,
operationalization of the four-branch ability model of EI 2012) is an instrument comprising four subscales with a total
(Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002). Although there is an of 15 items: Refusing Self-Reflection, Emotional Awareness,
obvious kinship between mentalization and these other con- Psychic Equivalence Mode, and Regulation of Affect. It was val-
structs, their differences—briefly outlined in the opening idated in German on a large clinical sample and demonstrated
paragraph—should not be prematurely dismissed when it good total score reliability. Its subscales, however, are very
comes to assessment. It seems a more reasonable approach short, with reliabilities below .70. The instrument has not been
to operationalize mentalization adhering to its original tested on a community sample, and in the 4 years since its pub-
meaning and then validate it by locating its position within lication, it has been employed in only three independent empir-
the network of related constructs, than simply declare these ical studies.
aspects of mentalization and, by virtue of common mea- Next to appear was the Reflective Function Questionnaire
surement, blur any conceptual boundaries. for Youths (RFQY; Ha, Sharp, Ensink, Fonagy, & Cirino,
Efforts to develop specific measures of mentalization, on the 2013). As its name reveals, the RFQY has been validated in a
other hand, have resulted in time- and expertise-demanding sample of 12- to 17-year-olds, and is not intended for use with
interview-based techniques, such as the Reflective Function adults. It is only very recently that this gap has been filled by
Scale (RFS)—an elaborate coding system allowing one to derive the Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (RFQ; Badoud et al.,
a mentalization score from the Adult Attachment Interview 2015; Fonagy et al., 2016). Through an impressive series of vali-
(AAI; Fonagy et al., 1998), as well as from the Parent Develop- dation studies, its authors were able to show that the RFQ
ment Interview, the Mentalization Stories Test for Adolescents, correlates positively with related constructs (i.e., empathy,
or the Thematic Apperception Test (Luyten et al., 2011). mindfulness, and perspective-taking) and negatively with
Although recognized as the gold standard in mentalization indexes of psychopathology, while also discriminating between
assessment, the RFS is unsuitable for use in large-scale quanti- persons with personality disorders and controls, and predicting
tative research, and has actually only been validated in several attachment. Still, it should be noted that this instrument com-
clinical studies (e.g., Bouchard et al., 2008; Fischer-Kern et al., prises two fairly short subscales (six items each), namely
2010; M€ uller, Kaufhold, Overbeck, & Grabhorn, 2006). We Certainty versus Uncertainty in the mental states of self and
therefore lack any information on how RFS-assessed mentaliza- others. Not only does this adversely affect internal consistencies
tion is related to massive differential-psychological constructs (which only approach the .70 threshold), but it also calls into
(e.g., intelligence or the Big Five) or those conceptually most question the content validity of the instrument, which seems to
akin to it (e.g., empathy, EI). Practically the same goes for the be tapping only some aspects of mentalization. Moreover, even
Reflective Functioning Rating Scale (RFRS; Hill, Levy, Meehan, this huge endeavor remains entirely within the borders of
& Reynoso, 2007; Meehan, Levy, Reynoso, Hill, & Clarkin, mentalization research as we know it, associating reflective
2009): This 50-item instrument requires a competent observer functioning with clinical phenomena, empathy, and attach-
to judge various aspects of mentalization in another person, ment, but failing to establish links with “mainstream” personal-
and has thus been applied mostly in clinical contexts (e.g., eval- ity constructs (e.g., the Big Five).
uating the effects of psychotherapy), but barely beyond that.
The list thus boils down to one alternative interview-based
The current studies: Testing a new self-report measure
assessment procedure—the Brief Reflective Function Interview
of mentalization
(BRFI; Rudden, Milrod, & Target 2005). Requiring about
25 min of interview time, this procedure might actually be con- In light of the foregoing, the purpose of this study was to
sidered for use with larger samples, but has hitherto only been construct an easy-to-administer, reliable, self-report mea-
validated with a small group (N D 27) of undergraduate partici- sure of mentalizaton, which would be comprehensively val-
pants (Rutimann & Meehan, 2012). idated on both a community and a clinical sample. Aware
270  HANAK, ALTARAS DIMITRIJEVIC,
DIMITRIJEVIC,  JOLIC
 MARJANOVIC


of the difficulties imminent in such an endeavor, we Study 1: Internal consistency and validity of the MentS
decided to proceed along two main guidelines. First, with in a community sample
regard to the development of the instrument, we made it
Hypotheses
imperative to stick to the original conception of mentaliza-
tion, without unduly narrowing or diluting its boundaries. Several hypotheses were formulated as guidelines in evaluat-
Second, in addressing validity questions, we saw it as our ing the construct validity of the MentS. For one, we rea-
aim to merge the traditional mentalization-research per- soned that a valid measure of mentalization should be
spective with the broader viewpoint of differential psychol- positively related to measures of cognate constructs, such as
ogy. We thus sought to expand the list of probable empathy and trait EI, assessed via self-report (H1a), as well
correlates of mentalization beyond “the usual suspects” as ability EI, assessed via objective performance (H1b). Fur-
(i.e., attachment and BPD) to include other constructs such ther, building on mentalization theory and previous
as EI and the Big Five. research, we expected MentS scores to be substantially
The instrument we propose here was originally developed in related to the quality of attachment—specifically, that they
Serbian and later translated into other languages, including correlate negatively with indicators of attachment anxiety
English. This article details its validation with Serbian-speaking and avoidance (H2a), and that they differentiate signifi-
participants, with a Bulgarian and German validation under cantly between persons with secure and insecure attachment
way. patterns (H2b). With regard to personality, we postulated
that MentS scores should be associated with higher emo-
tional stability and Extraversion (H3a), two traits that have
Item generation and pilot studies
consistently been found to promote quality of life and inter-
Several sources were used in generating the initial item personal relationships (e.g., Austin & Deary, 2002); addi-
pool: the Mentalization subscale featured in the Revised tionally, we assumed that MentS scores would also correlate
Questionnaire for Attachment Assessment (QAA–R; positively with openness, as the latter generally predisposes
Hanak, 2004, 2010), the Manual for Reflective Functioning one to embrace and analyze experiences, including their
(Fonagy et al., 1998), and the Handbook of Mentalization- mental aspects (H3b). Finally, in line with prior findings,
Based Treatment (Allen & Fonagy, 2006). The resulting list women were expected to outscore men on MentS-measured
of 55 items was reviewed for face validity by two indepen- mentalization (H4).
dent experts in attachment and mentalization theory, and
administered to students of psychology and education
Method
(NP1 D 102) in the form of a 5-point Likert-type question-
naire, the anchor points being 1 (strongly disagree) and 5 Participants and procedure
(strongly agree). This sample provided feedback on the Participants in Study 1 were 288 adults (44.4% female; age
clarity of item content and instructions, as well as on the range D 21–61, M D 40.4, SD D 8.21) employed at various
format of the instrument. Items that were deemed odd or positions (e.g., production line workers, administrative staff,
ambiguous were considered for rephrasing or exclusion, managers) within a large dairy production factory, as well as
whereas those exhibiting low item–total correlations 278 college freshmen (72.3% female; age range D 18–33, M D
(<.30) or yielding an increase in alpha if deleted were 19.29, SD D 1.16) engaged in various areas of study (compara-
immediately left out. These revisions resulted in two tive literature, mechanical engineering, psychology, and special
shorter versions of the instrument (containing 37 and 30 education) at the University of Belgrade. In the first group,
items, respectively), which were piloted with two further 68.8% of participants had a high school degree (11–12 years of
samples of university students (NP2 D 87, NP3 D 91). To education), 26.2% held a university diploma (16–18 years of
increase alpha well beyond the .70 threshold, items education), and 3.5% obtained postgraduate academic titles
compromising internal consistency (i.e., showing only small (> 18 years of education); the remaining 1.5% did not provide
correlations with the total and yielding a higher alpha if information on their education.
removed) were again dropped, and the questionnaire was Because the management of the dairy company had
eventually reduced to its current 28-item form (see kindly agreed to let all employees of their Belgrade branch
Appendix). participate in the study, the employed adults were recruited
at their workplace and tested within working hours in suit-
able rooms on factory premises. The students were recruited
Main studies
and tested during regular lecture hours, courtesy of our col-
Described as the Mentalization Scale (MentS), this version leagues who were willing to spare some time from their les-
of the instrument was thoroughly examined for psychomet- sons for research purposes. All participants were briefed
ric properties in two studies: Study 1 was carried out with a about the character of the study and the confidentiality of
sample of employed adults and university students, and data, whereupon they signed the informed consent forms.
aimed at establishing the instrument’s reliability, factor They were not compensated financially, but were offered
structure, and convergent–discriminant validity; Study 2 and later provided individual feedback on their test results
involved a sample of persons diagnosed with BPD and (on request). The described research procedure had initially
matched controls, allowing us to also gauge the clinical util- been approved by the relevant committee of the Serbian
ity of the instrument. Psychological Association.
THE MENTALIZATION SCALE (MENTS) 271

Table 1. Study 1 descriptive statistics, reliabilities, and gender differences.

Sample descriptive statistics Descriptive statistics by gender ANOVA by gender

Males Females

N M SD Range Skew Kurtosis a M SD M SD F d

Mentalization
MentS-Self 540 27.73 5.61 10–40 ¡.16 ¡.27 .77 28.53 5.72 27.18 5.48 7.64 .24
MentS-Others 540 38.64 4.83 18–50 ¡.42 .54 .77 37.42 4.99 39.48 4.54 24.85 ¡.43
MentS-Motivation 540 37.94 5.51 14–50 ¡.26 .24 .76 35.62 5.44 39.54 4.96 75.17 ¡.75
MentS total 540 104.31 11.96 68–140 .03 ¡.00 .84 101.57 12.32 106.20 11.34 20.27 ¡.42
Emotional intelligence
MSCEIT EP 274 .45 .11 .11–.64 ¡.68 ¡.06 .92 .44 .11 .46 .11 1.01 ¡.18
MSCEIT UsE 274 .43 .09 .14–.60 ¡.82 .66 .78 .42 .09 .44 .09 1.77 ¡.22
MSCEIT UE 275 .43 .08 .18–.62 ¡.46 .29 .67 .43 .08 .43 .07 .62 .00
MSCEIT ME 273 .30 .07 .14–.48 .21 ¡.50 .77 .30 .07 .30 .07 .04 .00
MSCEIT total 272 .40 .06 .22–.53 ¡.50 ¡.22 .90 .40 .06 .41 .06 .57 ¡.16
TEIQue WB 282 5.47 .71 2.37–6.90 ¡.51 .82 .78 5.49 .72 5.44 .70 .27 .07
TEIQue SC 282 4.81 .80 2.57–6.90 .15 .03 .83 4.96 .85 4.62 .68 13.35 .44
TEIQue E 282 5.15 .68 3.38–6.77 .15 ¡.61 .80 5.12 .71 5.19 .65 .69 ¡.10
TEIQue S 282 4.82 .74 2.77–6.58 .16 ¡.46 .81 4.91 .77 4.72 .70 4.63 .26
TEIQue total 282 5.08 .61 2.94–6.60 ¡.01 ¡.07 .96 5.13 .66 5.01 .54 2.82 .20
Big Five
Neuroticism 286 29.62 7.37 12–57 .22 .51 .80 27.94 6.97 31.72 7.36 19.77 ¡.53
Extraversion 286 41.76 5.85 24–56 ¡.09 ¡.06 .69 41.70 6.24 41.84 5.33 .04 ¡.02
Openness 286 38.21 5.36 22–53 ¡.18 ¡.02 .57 37.87 5.31 38.63 5.43 1.42 ¡.14
Agreeableness 286 43.11 5.31 26–58 ¡.16 .53 .62 42.66 5.21 43.68 5.40 2.60 ¡.19
Conscientiousness 286 49.60 5.47 34–60 .13 ¡.55 .78 49.29 5.65 50.00 5.23 1.19 ¡.13
Empathy
Empathy Quotient 564 18.15 4.62 2–31 ¡.06 .02 .56 17.65 4.86 18.51 4.40 4.72 ¡.18
Attachment
ECR–R Avoidance 542 59.95 13.65 21–122 .55 1.28 .84 60.34 13.08 59.68 14.05 .30 .05
ECR–R Anxiety 542 53.76 16.80 18–108 .45 .088 .89 51.33 15.26 55.44 17.62 8.34,a ¡.25

Note. ANOVA D analysis of variance; MSCEIT D Meyer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test; MSCEIT EP D MSCEIT Emotion Perception; MSCEIT UsE D MSCEIT
Using Emotions; MSCEIT UE D MSCEIT Understanding Emotions; MSCEIT ME D MSCEIT Managing Emotions; TEIQue D Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire; TEI-
Que WB D TEIQue Well-Being; TEIQue SC D TEIQue Self-Control: TEIQue E D TEIQue Emotionality; TEIQue S D TEIQue Sociability; ECR–R D Modified Experiences in
Close Relationships–Revised.
a
Welch statistic.

p < .05.p < .01.

Measures shown to reliably assess the two attachment dimensions


The main measure used in this study and completed by all par- (Hanak & Dimitrijevic, 2013).
ticipants was the aforementioned 28-item version of the MentS.
Responses on the MentS are given on a 5-point Likert scale,
ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). Revised Questionnaire for Attachment Assessment. The
Other instruments administered to both subsamples were the QAA–R (Hanak 2004, 2010) is a self-report measure based
Modified Experiences in Close Relationships–Revised (ECR–R) on a theoretical analysis of the construct of attachment and
and the EQ. Measures employed only in the working adults sam- of other instruments designed to assess attachment quality,
ple were the QAA–R (another measure of attachment quality), a such as the AAI (Main & Goldwyn, 1998) and the Adult
questionnaire assessing trait EI, a test of ability EI, and an inven- Attachment Projective (C. George & West, 2001). The
tory of the five basic factors of personality. This, along with QAA–R features seven 11-item subscales, with internal con-
minor frequency of missing data, accounts for the present varia- sistencies in the .69 to .87 range. Using a combination of
tions in the number of participants per analysis. The respective subscale scores and employing K-means cluster analysis
measures are specified and described next, and their internal with initial cluster centers provided by the author, respond-
consistencies established in this study are reported in Table 1. ents were classified into four groups, corresponding to the
four attachment patterns: secure, dismissing, preoccupied,
The Modified Experiences in Close Relationships–Revised. The and fearful. The distribution observed in this sample was as
ECR–R (Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000) is a 36-item scale follows: 62 (23.6%) secure, 47 (17.8%) fearful, 63 (24.0%)
that assesses attachment avoidance (Av, odd items) and attach- dismissive, and 91 (34.6%) preoccupied. Parenthetically, this
ment anxiety (Ax, even items) in close relationships in general. distribution of attachment patterns diverges from what
The label “modified” refers to the fact that items have been would be expected based on available meta-analytic studies
reworded to probe beyond romantic relationships (e.g., “part- (Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, 2009), especially
ner” is substituted by “close person”). Responses are given on a in terms of the lower incidence of secure and higher inci-
7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 dence of unresolved attachment. These results are, however,
(strongly agree). The Serbian adaptation of the modified in accordance with those obtained in other studies in for-
ECR–R, which we employed in this study, has previously been mer Yugoslavia (Hanak, 2010).
272  HANAK, ALTARAS DIMITRIJEVIC,
DIMITRIJEVIC,  JOLIC
 MARJANOVIC


Empathy Quotient. The EQ (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, Results


2004) is a self-report measure of empathy based on the
Internal consistency and descriptive statistics
unifactorial model proposing a unity of cognitive and
The applied measures generally exhibit acceptable to high inter-
affective aspects of empathy. Responses are given on a 4-
nal consistencies (see Table 1), comparable to those previously
point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly
established for the same instruments. Cronbach’s alpha is fairly
agree). Nonempathic responses are scored 0, and empathic
high for the proposed MentS, as well (a D .84).
responses receive 1 or 2 points, depending on the degree
As can be further seen from Table 1, the distributions of
of empathy expressed. In this study, a shorter 22-item ver-
most scores, including those on the MentS, fit normality
sion of the Serbian translation of the instrument
assumptions. Kurtosis values range from –.66 to 1.28 and can
(Dimitrijevic, Hanak, Vukosavljevic Gvozden, & Opacic,
be considered acceptable (D. George & Mallery, 2010). Like-
2012) was employed, the theoretical score range thus being
wise, relatively low skew values (< 1) and adequate dispersions
0 to 44.
of scores (range) imply that present distributional asymmetries
are only slight.
Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire. The Trait Emo-
tional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue; Petrides, 2009) Differences regarding gender and education
measures EI defined as a constellation of emotional self-per- Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) revealed a number of signifi-
ceptions or emotional self-efficacy (Petrides, Pita, & Kokki- cant gender differences: Females scored higher on the MentS,
naki, 2007). It consists of 153 items accompanied by 7- EQ, ECR–R Anxiety, and Neuroticism, whereas males did so
point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 on TEIQue self-control and sociability (Table 1). According to
(strongly agree). Items are organized into 15 subscales and the cutoffs of the d statistic suggested by Cohen (1988), most
eventually load on four factors: well-being, self-control, effect sizes were small to medium.
emotionality, and sociability. A previous examination of the In the subsample of employed adults, we additionally tested
Serbian version of the instrument (Jolic Marjanovic & Alta- for differences in MentS scores between groups of differing lev-
ras Dimitrijevic, 2014) revealed very good reliability and els of education and for correlations with age. Participants
factorial validity. holding a university degree were found to score significantly
higher than those with 12 or fewer years of education on both
MentS total and the three subscales, F(1, 255) D 9.48–31.30,
Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test Version
p < .001, d D ¡.41– ¡.79. The same trend was observed for
2.0. The MSCEIT v2.0 (Mayer et al., 2002) test of ability EI
empathy, trait, and ability EI. The MentS score was not signifi-
contains 141 items organized into four pairs of tasks, so as
cantly related to participants’ age (r D ¡.08).
to operationalize the four branches of the Mayer–Salovey EI
model: Perceiving Emotions, Using Emotions, Understand-
ing Emotions, and Managing Emotions. The first two form MentS structure
the experiential area, and the latter two the strategic area of The MentS structure was examined by performing a principal
EI. Besides branch and area scores, the MSCEIT also yields component analysis (PCA) with an oblimin rotation. Sample
an overall ability EI score. Test items employ either a multi- adequacy and appropriateness of the PCA were confirmed by
ple-choice format, where test takers are required to choose the participants to variable ratio (540:28), the value of the Kai-
the best possible solution to the presented problem, or the ser–Meyer–Olkin statistic (KMO D .843), and results of the
rate-the-extent form, where the task is to judge the appro- Bartlett’s test of sphericity, x2(378) D 3778.43, p < .001. The
priateness of each of several proposed options. In this study, PCA initially yielded seven components with eigenvalues over
the Serbian translation of the instrument was used, which 1, altogether explaining 55.08% of the variance. The final num-
was approved by the publisher (Multi-Health Systems) and ber of extracted components was determined by means of par-
found to exhibit good reliability and convergent–discrimi- allel analysis, which was performed using an SPSS syntax
nant validity (Altaras Dimitrijevic & Jolic Marjanovic, created by O’Connor (2000). To identify the number of compo-
2010). Responses were scored by the publisher, using the nents with eigenvalues larger than those that might occur by
general consensus scoring method. chance, 1,000 random data sets were created, each with 512
cases and 28 variables. In 95% of randomly created data sets
only the first three eigenvalues (1.51, 1.44, and 1.38) were nota-
NEO Five-Factor Inventory. The NEO Five-Factor Inventory bly lower than the corresponding ones in the original data set
(NEO-FFI; McCrae & Costa, 2004) is a short, 60-item version (5.69, 2.98, and 1.83). Thus, the parallel analysis suggested a
of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R), an three-component solution as the most fitting, with 37.51% of
inventory widely used to assess the five basic factors of the variability explained. Individual item loadings on the
personality: Neuroticism (N), Extraversion (E), Openness (O), retained components are listed in Tables 2 and 3.
Agreeableness (A), and Conscientiousness (C). Although the The first component, explaining 20.34% of the variance, had
NEO-FFI scales (particularly O and A) exhibit somewhat lower 10 significant loadings (> .30; Hair, Anderson, Tatham, &
internal consistencies than those featured in the NEO PI–R, the Black, 1998) allowing it to be identified as Other-Related Men-
chosen inventory generally has good psychometric properties, talization (henceforth MentS-O). The second component,
which also pertains to its Serbian translation (Knezevic, explaining 10.64% of the variance, was determined by 8 items
Dzamonja-Ignjatovic, & Đuric-Jocic, 2004). concerning Self-Related Mentalization (henceforth MentS-S).
THE MENTALIZATION SCALE (MENTS) 273

Table 2. Pattern matrix for the MentS in general sample. Table 3. Pattern matrix for the MentS in clinical sample.

Component Component

Item numbers MentS-O MentS-S MentS-M Item numbers MentS-S MentS-O MentS-M

12 .66 ¡.11 .05 22 .81 ¡.08 .10


10 .65 .03 ¡.11 18 .76 .03 .30
3 .62 .05 ¡.02 11 .71 .08 .05
23 .60 ¡.01 .11 21 .66 ¡.28 ¡.20
5 .60 .02 .20 26 .58 ¡.35 ¡.30
20 .53 .16 .03 25 .51 .00 ¡.14
25 .47 .32 ¡.13 8 .50 ¡.39 ¡.28
6 .42 ¡.02 .37 19 .43 .14 .05
2 .37 .15 .07 27 .41 .16 ¡.19
28 .30 ¡.05 .18 5 .38 .37 ¡.16
21 .15 .75 ¡.25 9 .32 .10 .04
22 .20 .69 ¡.10 24 ¡.05 .70 ¡.11
11 .09 .68 ¡.07 12 ¡.03 .69 ¡.03
8 .06 .67 ¡.16 10 .15 .67 .08
18 ¡.11 .59 .16 16 .12 .63 .09
14 ¡.03 .48 .09 4 ¡.17 .63 ¡.04
26 .09 .43 .35 3 .28 .48 ¡.20
19 ¡.02 .42 .28 1 .05 .43 ¡.25
9 ¡.24 .33 .68 23 .04 .40 ¡.31
4 .02 ¡.09 .67 13 ¡.06 .26 ¡.65
27 ¡.19 .37 .66 7 .03 ¡.16 ¡.62
24 .10 ¡.10 .63 28 ¡.06 .12 ¡.59
16 .15 ¡.10 .50 17 .37 .20 .52
13 .27 .04 .45 2 .18 .24 ¡.47
17 .24 ¡.03 .36 14 .36 ¡.30 ¡.46
1 .17 ¡.05 .35 20 .07 .18 ¡.43
7 .20 ¡.11 .35 15 .01 .04 ¡.42
15 .31 ¡.03 .34 6 .19 .24 ¡.30

Note. Extraction method: principal component analysis; Rotation method: Oblimin Note. Extraction method: principal component analysis; Rotation method: Oblimin
with Kaiser normalization. Loadings set in boldface type indicate the highest with Kaiser normalization. Loadings set in boldface type indicate the highest
loading across components for that item. MentS-S D MentS-Self; MentS-O D loading across components for that item. MentS-S D MentS-Self; MentS-O D
MentS-Others; MentS-M D MentS-Motivation. MentS-Others; MentS-M D MentS-Motivation.

The third, explaining 6.53% of the variance, was labeled Moti- groups. MentS means for the four attachment organizations are
vation to Mentalize (henceforth MentS-M), as the 10 items given in Figure 1.
loading onto this component refer to one’s need to understand
the psychic world of self and others. MentS and Empathy. Significant moderate-to-large positive
correlations with the EQ were established, the strongest associ-
MentS internal consistency and item-level statistics ations being those of empathy with the MentS-S and total score.
Reliability analyses evidenced satisfactory internal consistencies
for the three components identified through PCA: a was .77 for MentS and EI. Similarly, a set of positive and generally signifi-
MentS-O and MentS-M, and .76 for MentS-S. cant correlations was observed between MentS scores and both
Item–total correlations were in the .26 to .51 range, and ability and trait EI. In the case of ability EI, these associations
there was no relevant change (neither diminishment nor were weak to moderate, becoming stronger as one progresses
improvement) in overall reliability if any of the items were from the lower, experiential branches (i.e., perceiving and using
deleted. The same was true for item-level analyses performed at emotions) to the higher, strategic branches of EI (understand-
the level of subscales. ing and managing emotions). The strongest correlations were
thus observed between MentS scores and managing emotions.
Associations of MentS with other study variables Correlations with the trait EI factors and total score ranged
Table 4 presents the correlations of MentS scores with other from weak to strong, with the lowest values observed for the
study variables. MentS-M subscale, and the highest for MentS-S and the total
score.
MentS and Attachment. MentS scores had moderate negative
correlations with ECR–R Avoidance; correlations with ECR–R MentS and Big Five. MentS scores were found to correlate
Anxiety were generally small, except for the one involving the positively with all basic personality traits, except Neuroticism,
MentS-S subscale, which was a large negative one. which was established as a significant negative correlate of
Additionally, an ANOVA indicated significant differences in mentalization. The observed associations with the NEO-FFI
MentS scores between groups with different attachment pat- were generally weak to moderate, with a single nonsignificant
terns, F(3, 257) D 17.82–61.31, p < .001. Post-hoc tests (MentS-O £ A) and a single large correlation (MentS-S £ N).
revealed that the “secure” group scored higher on MentS (at MentS-S correlated most strongly with Neuroticism, MentS-O
both whole and subscale level) than any of the three insecure with Extraversion and Conscientiousness, and MentS-M with
274
DIMITRIJEVIC,

Table 4. Zero-order correlations between study variables (Study 1 and Study 2).
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22
 HANAK, ALTARAS DIMITRIJEVIC,

1. MentS-Self 1.00 .30 .35 .79 ¡.57 .29 .42 .50 .28 ¡.53 ¡.56
2. MentS-Others .30 1.00 .47 .73 ¡.15 .45 .65 .30 .34 ¡.27 ¡.02
 JOLIC

3. MentS-Motivation .21 .53 1.00 .75 ¡.17 .02 .41 .40 .25 ¡.25 ¡.06
4. MentS total .69 .79 .78 1.00 ¡.46 .35 .63 .55 .38 ¡.49 ¡.34

5. MSCEIT EP .22 .11 .07 .18 1.00
6. MSCEIT UsE .31 .10 .12 .24 .47 1.00


7. MSCEIT UE .37 .13 .13 .29 .27 .30 1.00


 MARJANOVIC

8. MSCEIT ME .42 .38 .34 .49 .35 .42 .33 1.00


     
9. MSCEIT total .44 .24 .21 .40 .78 .76 .63 .69 1.00
10. TEIQue WB .45 .33 .24 .45 .28 .28 .28 .50 .45 1.00
11. TEIQue SC .56 .30 .22 .48 .18 .17 .29 .40 .34 .60 1.00
12. TEIQue E .57 .53 .44 .67 .28 .31 .31 .52 .48 .63 .60 1.00
         
13. TEIQue S .50 .46 .30 .55 .21 .24 .28 .47 .41 .67 .50 .67 1.00
14. TEIQue total .62 .47 .35 .63 .28 .29 .33 .56 .49 .85 .81 .87 .82 1.00
15. Neuroticism ¡.53 ¡.23 ¡.16 ¡.42 ¡.21 ¡.19 ¡.31 ¡.41 ¡.37 ¡.67 ¡.71 ¡.55 ¡.60 ¡.76 1.00
16. Extraversion .20 .41 .32 .39 .12 .13 .11 .36 .24 .63 .39 .49 .61 .62 ¡.45 1.00
17. Openness .31 .34 .41 .46 .06 .13 .08 .33 .19 .25 .25 .36 .28 .35 ¡.20 .17 1.00
18. Agreeableness .17 .08 .14 .18 .12 .10 .07 .10 .14 .22 .29 .27 ¡.09 .23 ¡.23 .11 .15 1.00
19. Conscientiousness .35 .40 .35 .48 .22 .23 .17 .36 .35 .57 .55 .51 .44 .62 ¡.49 .46 .24 .26 1.00
  
20. EQ .43 .36 .35 .51 .21 .27 .27 .45 .40 .44 .48 .61 .50 .60 ¡.42 .29 .34 .23 .37 1.00
21. ECR–R Avoidance ¡.31 ¡.31 ¡.31 ¡.41 ¡.07 ¡.15 ¡.19 ¡.27 ¡.22 ¡.40 ¡.34 ¡.48 ¡.39 ¡.48 .32 ¡.42 ¡.27 ¡.11 ¡.25 ¡.35 1.00
22. ECR–R Anxiety ¡.54 ¡.22 .04 ¡.32 ¡.32 ¡.31 ¡.31 ¡.46 ¡.47 ¡.48 ¡.52 ¡.57 ¡.51 ¡.62 .54 ¡.26 ¡.31 ¡.17 ¡.32 ¡.28 .32 1.00

Note. Correlations below the diagonal are for the general population sample, and those above the diagonal are for the clinical sample. Correlations for N D 256–540 in general population sample and N D 38–62 in clinical sample.
MSCEIT D Meyer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test; MSCEIT EP D MSCEIT Emotion Perception; MSCEIT UsE D MSCEIT Using Emotions; MSCEIT UE D MSCEIT Understanding Emotions; MSCEIT ME D MSCEIT Managing
Emotions; TEIQue D Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire; TEIQue WB D TEIQue Well-Being; TEIQue SC D TEIQue Self-Control; TEIQue E D TEIQue Emotionality; TEIQue S D TEIQue Sociability; EQ D Empathy Quotient;
ECR–R D Modified Experiences in Close Relationships–Revised.

p < .05.p < .01.
THE MENTALIZATION SCALE (MENTS) 275

120.00 model of the self were also more likely to report lesser capacity of
100.00 self-reflection. Finally, congruent with the third hypothesis, higher
MentS scores were related with higher emotional stability (reversed
80.00
Neuroticism), and Extraversion (H3a), as well as with higher
60.00 Openness (H3b), but a significant positive correlation with Consci-
40.00 entiousness also emerged. Even with this last construct unexpect-
20.00
edly entering the picture, the described pattern of correlations
serves to support the validity of MentS scores: a constellation of
0.00
MentS-S MentS-O MentS-M MentS total N–, EC, OC, and CC should arguably be conducive of an adaptive
Secure 33.36 42.21 40.85 116.43 and fruitful exploration of the mental world of self and others. The
Fearful 22.51 36.77 34.74 94.02 coherence of this general picture is even reflected in specific details
Dismissing 30.03 37.55 34.53 102.11 regarding MentS subscales: Those who are higher on Extraversion
Preoccupied 26.79 38.49 36.44 101.73
and Conscientiousness (i.e., who are people-oriented and socially
Figure 1. MentS means for four attachment quality categories. Note. Analysis of active, but also behaving in a self-controlled manner) were particu-
variance results: MentS-S, F(3, 258) D 61.53, p < .001; MentS-O, F(3, 258) D 16.86, larly more likely to also report higher other-related mentalization,
p < .001; MentS-M, F(3, 258) D 25.58, p < .001; MentS total, F(3, 258) D 57.31, whereas those who are higher on Openness and lower on Neuroti-
p < .001. Post-hoc tests results for MentS-S: mean difference(secure-fearful) D 10.85,
p < .001; mean difference(secure-dismissing) D 3.34, p < .001; mean difference(secure- cism (i.e., embracing new experiences and emotionally stable) were
preoccupied) D 6.57, p < .001; mean difference(fearful-dismissing) D ¡7.05, p < .001; particularly more likely to declare themselves ready to engage in
mean difference(fearful-preoccupied) D ¡4.28, p < .001; mean difference(dismissing-preoc- mentalization.
cupied) D 3.22, p < .001. Post-hoc tests results for MentS-O: mean difference(secure-
fearful) D 5.45, p < .001; mean difference(secure-dismissing) D 4.48, p < .001; mean dif-
Of note, the observed correlations were generally of moder-
ference(secure-preoccupied) D 3.72, p < .001; mean difference(fearful-dismissing) D ¡.98, ate strength, implying that the assessment domain of the MentS
p > .05; mean difference(fearful-preoccupied) D ¡1.79, p > .05; mean difference(dismiss- is rather distinctive from that of other applied measures. Over-
ing-preoccupied) D .74, p > .05. Post-hoc tests results for MentS-M: mean difference(se-
cure-fearful) D 6.12, p < .001; mean difference(secure-dismissing) D 6.30, p < .001; mean
all, mentalization related most strongly to trait EI, followed by
difference(secure-preoccupied) D 4.41, p < .001; mean difference(fearful-dismissing) D .20, empathy, which is understandable considering the conceptual
p > .05; mean difference(fearful-preoccupied) D ¡1.69, p > .05; mean difference(dismiss- closeness of the respective constructs, boosted by similarities in
ing-preoccupied) D 1.19, p > .05. Post-hoc tests results for MentS total: mean differen-
ce(secure-fearful) D 22.40, p < .001; mean difference(secure-dismissing) D 14.11, p < .001; their measurement. Still, not even these associations were so
mean difference(secure-preoccupied) D 14.70, p < .001; mean difference(fearful-dismissing) strong as to suggest redundancy of the MentS.
D ¡8.29, p < .001; mean difference(fearful-preoccupied) D ¡7.70, p < .001; mean dif- If some portion of the established correlations is to be writ-
ference(dismissing-preoccupied) D .59, p > .05.
ten off as an artefact of methodology, this is principally not
true for the overall significant positive associations between
Openness. Agreeableness stood out as the weakest correlate of MentS and MSCEIT scores. These actually serve as solid evi-
all four MentS scores. dence that those who describe themselves as good at grasping
the mental aspects of human functioning are indeed better at
solving emotion understanding and management tasks, at least
Discussion
in a controlled test situation. The construct validity of the
Study 1 evaluated the reliability, structure, and validity of the MentS is thus supported not only in relation to a self-report,
MentS in a sample of 566 adults from the general population. but also to a performance measure of EI.
Despite being relatively short, the MentS showed good internal On top of that, several intelligible group differences in
consistency and a normal distribution of scores at the whole- MentS-measured mentalization were found. First, women con-
scale level. A PCA yielded three interpretable components, sug- vincingly outscored men on the total score (d D ¡.42), as well
gesting that the instrument is comprised of three subscales: as on two subscales, Other-Related Mentalization (d D ¡.43)
MentS-O (10 items), MentS-S (8 items), and MentS-M (10 and Motivation to Mentalize (d D ¡.75), which is consistent
items). Reliability analyses indicated satisfactory internal con- with H4 and the findings of previous studies (e.g., Abu-Akel &
sistencies for all subscales, whereas item-level statistics (item– Bo, 2013; Baron-Cohen, 2004). The unexpected finding of
total correlations, reliability if item deleted) confirmed the higher Self-Related Mentalization in men (d D .24) could likely
instrument’s sound overall composition; that is, optimal num- stem from a higher tendency in women to agree with the
ber and relevant content of items. reverse-scored items of the respective subscale, which tend to
A meaningful pattern of correlations emerged between MentS refer to personal weaknesses and incapacities (e.g., “I find it dif-
scores and measures of other constructs. In line with our first ficult to …,” “Often I cannot …”). Although this interpretation
hypothesis, mentalization assessed via the MentS was positively is supported by the finding of a more negative model of the self
associated with cognate qualities of self-reported empathy and in women (i.e., higher Anxiety on the ECR–R, d D ¡.25), it
emotional self-efficacy (i.e., trait EI, H1a), as well as with cognitive also suggests that a rephrasing of several items of the MentS-S
processing of emotional information (i.e., ability EI, particularly subscale might be considered. Second, significantly higher men-
understanding and managing emotions, H1b). Moreover, attach- talization was reported by participants of higher education
ment avoidance and anxiety were established as significant negative (d D ¡.41–.79), just as was the case with related qualities—
correlates of MentS-measured mentalization, as was purported in empathy and EI. Finally, those identified as securely attached
H2a. Most indicative of the scale’s validity in this context is the (via the QAA–R) had by far the highest mentalization scores,
strong negative association between attachment anxiety and self- with the fearful group scoring lowest, and the preoccupied and
related mentalization; that is, the fact that those with a negative dismissive in between. This rank-order conforms to our
276  HANAK, ALTARAS DIMITRIJEVIC,
DIMITRIJEVIC,  JOLIC
 MARJANOVIC


hypothesis (H2b) and mirrors the findings obtained with other The male–female ratio established for the clinical and repro-
(well-established) measures of mentalization, such as the RFS duced in the control group was 20 (32.3%) to 42 (67.7%). The
(Fonagy et al., 1998). distribution across age groups (again, in both the clinical and
In sum, the results of Study 1 yield a rather favorable picture the control sample) was as follows: 18 to 20, n D 11 (17.7%); 21
of the psychometric properties of the proposed MentS, estab- to 30, n D 18 (29.0%); 31 to 40, n D 21 (33.9%); 41 to 50, n D 7
lishing fair internal consistency of scores and validating them (11.3%); and 51 to 60, n D 5 (8.1%). As for education, 3 (4.8%)
against a background of relevant variables. participants in each group had 8 years of elementary education,
42 (67.7%) had graduated from high school, and 17 (27.4%)
held a university degree.
Study 2: Clinical utility of the MentS
Participation in the study was voluntary, and the procedure
Going a step further, our second study aimed at establishing the was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Men-
validity of the MentS in a clinical sample. Given the nature of tal Health in Belgrade. After being briefed about the nature of
the construct assessed and the history of its research, persons the study, the protection of anonymity and confidentiality of
diagnosed with BPD were identified as the most suitable target the data, participants signed informed consent forms.
group to test the instrument’s criterion validity. We also sought
to confirm our previous findings on the questionnaire’s conver- Measures
gent–discriminant validity, as well as to gain preliminary The number of measures was substantially reduced for
insights into its structural stability. Study 2 to accommodate for the fact that persons with BPD
experienced the assessment procedure as more tiring than
Hypotheses members of the general population (which became apparent
Based on clinical research on mentalization, we expected the in the early stages of data collection). The choice of meas-
clinical group to score significantly lower on MentS than the ures against which the MentS would be validated eventually
controls (H1). Furthermore, we expected to observe the same included the ECR–R (for details about this measure see
pattern of correlations with attachment quality and the basic Study 1) and the Big Five Inventory. Even with this reduc-
personality traits (H2) as in Study 1. tion, a substantial number of participants from the clinical
group did not complete the session, which is why n is nota-
bly lower for the latter instrument.
Method
Participants and procedure The Big Five Inventory. The Big Five Inventory (BFI; John,
Participants were 62 persons with clinician-assigned diagnoses of Naumann, & Soto, 2008) is a brief self-report measure of the Big
BPD, treated as both inpatients and outpatients in a major mental Five personality dimensions that contains 44 items, to be rated
health institution in Belgrade (clinical group). We also recruited a on a 5-point scale. Reported alphas for all subscales are high.
control group of 62 persons with no psychiatric diagnosis, using
the snowball sampling method, relying on an initially small num-
Results
ber of subjects to recommend further participants based on crite-
ria of age, gender, and educational level. These three variables MentS internal consistency
were actually used for matching the two groups, so that eventu- To calculate MentS subscale scores we used the PCA-based
ally each member of the clinical sample had a “mate” of the same syntax generated in Study 1. After that, we examined internal
age, gender, and educational level in the control group. consistency at both the whole-scale and subscale level, for the

Table 5. Study 2 descriptive statistics, reliabilities, and group differences.


Sample descriptive statistics ANOVA

N M (SD) Range a

Clinical Control Clinical Control Clinical Control Clinical Control F (1, 122/1, 74) d

Mentalization
MentS-Self 62 62 27.73 (7.64) 32.42 (5.41) 13–45 21–41 .79 .74 15.58 ¡.71
MentS-Others 62 62 39.53 (5.58) 40.48 (4.82) 15–49 31–50 .75 .77 1.03 ¡.18
MentS-Motivation 62 62 33.00 (5.20) 33.39 (4.27) 21–43 24–44 .60 .74 .20 ¡.08
MentS total 62 62 100.26 (14.02) 106.29 (11.37) 58–135 84–131 .75 .77 6.92 ¡.47
Big Five
Neuroticism 38 38 3.82 (.87) 2.78 (.66) 1.50–5.00 1.75–4.13 .83 .82 34.53 1.35
Extraversion 38 38 3.26 (.77) 3.80 (.71) 1.88–4.75 1.50–4.88 .80 .83 9.98 ¡.73
Openness 38 38 3.95 (.74) 3.86 (.61) 1.70–5.00 1.80–4.90 .82 .86 .37 .13
Agreeableness 38 38 3.58 (.62) 3.96 (.50) 2.56–4.78 2.89–5.00 .72 .72 8.44 ¡.67
Conscientiousness 38 38 3.00 (.79) 3.73 (.55) 1.44–4.67 2.56–4.67 .84 .78 21.44 ¡1.07
Attachment
ECR–R Avoidance 62 62 63.39 (14.20) 53.79 (13.80) 31–93 28–98 .74 .84 14.56 .68
ECR–R Anxiety 62 62 71.48 (23.58) 46.98 (15.75) 29–115 19–95 .91 .89 46.29 1.22

Note. ANOVA D analysis of variance; ECR–R D Modified Experiences in Close Relationships–Revised.



p < .05.p < .01.
THE MENTALIZATION SCALE (MENTS) 277

clinical and control group separately. Cronbach’s alphas were Tentative analysis of factor similarity
found to be overall lower than in Study 1 (Table 5), but still We dismissed the option of examining the MentS factor struc-
above the .70 threshold, with the only exception being MentS- ture in the clinical sample via confirmatory factor analysis
M in the clinical group. (CFA), as none of the criteria for performing this analysis (i.e., N
 200 for theoretical, and N  300 for population models,
according to Myers, Ahn, & Jin, 2011; KMO > .70, according to
Associations with attachment and the Big Five Hair, Black, Babin, & Anderson, 2010) were met. However, to
The pattern of correlations between MentS and ECR–R scores in obtain at least preliminary insights into the instrument’s struc-
the clinical sample was quite similar to the one in the community tural stability, we tentatively performed a PCA, including a par-
sample: Associations with Avoidance ranged from ¡.25 to ¡.53, allel analysis, and then used Pearson correlations and Tucker’s
whereas those with Anxiety were lower and below significance congruence coefficients to establish the similarity of factors
level in two instances; however, the negative correlation between obtained in two samples (cf. Barrett, 1986): the community sam-
MentS-S and Anxiety was again the largest one (Table 4). ple from Study 1 and the clinical sample from Study 2. The
As in Study 1, mentalization scores were positively related to results of these analyses are referred to as tentative, because the
all Big Five traits except Neuroticism, which had a moderate sample to item ratio (62:28) was suboptimal for a PCA as well;
negative association with MentS-total and a large negative asso- still, the KMO value (.568) was acceptable, as was data suitability
ciation with MentS-S. A peculiarity of the clinical sample, how- as indicated by Bartlett’s test, x2(378) D 774.18, p < .001.
ever, was that Agreeableness surfaced as a moderate-to-strong The parallel analysis, equivalent to the one performed in
and Openness as the strongest personality correlate of Study 1, showed that only the first three (out of initially 10) data-
mentalization. derived eigenvalues (5.45, 3.38, and 2.22) were higher or equal to
those extracted in 95% of the simulated data sets (2.76, 2.43, and
2.22). The retained three components accounted for 39.47% of
Differences between the clinical and control group
the variance (Table 3). The first component (19.47% of variance
The control group scored consistently higher on the MentS
explained) was equivalent to the MentS-S component estab-
than the clinical group, with differences being significant for
lished in the community sample, with only minor differences in
the total and the MentS-S subscale scores (Table 5). Parentheti-
content. The second (12.08% of variance explained) largely cor-
cally, the mean MentS total score for the clinical sample was
responded to the MentS-O subscale, but encompassed several
also lower than the corresponding M for the community sam-
items from the MentS-M as well. Finally, the third component
ple in Study 1 (cf. Table 1).
(7.92% of variance explained) fairly resembled the MentS-M
A discriminant analysis with MentS subscales as independ-
subscale from Study 1, but also included items from MentS-O.
ents and group membership (clinical or control) as the group-
Correlations between regression scores on the three compo-
ing variable yielded one significant function (Rc D .34, L D .88,
nents and scores calculated by applying the syntax from Study
x2 D 15.26, p D .002) defined predominantly by MentS-S
1 were .97 for the first component and MentS-S, .85 for the sec-
(Table 6), and allowing 67.7% of cases to be correctly classified
ond component and MentS-O, and .61 for the third component
(66.1% in the clinical group, 69.4% in the control group).
and MentS-M. As for Tucker’s Fs, the obtained values were .91
Another discriminant analysis employing MentS subscale
for MentS-S, .61 for MentS-O, and –.33 for MenS-M.
scores and ECR–R subscales (the BFI was excluded due to small
number of cases) also resulted in a significant function (Rc D
.53, L D .71, x2 D 40.22, p < .001), which improved classifica- Discussion
tion accuracy (73.4% overall, 67.7% clinical, 79.0% control),
The main purpose of Study 2 was to establish the utility of the
largely defined by ECR–R Anxiety. However, in this constella-
proposed scale in differentiating persons with BPD from a
tion the MentS-S (along with ECR–R Avoidance) also contrib-
group of matched controls. A further goal was to test its con-
uted substantially to the differentiation between the clinical
vergent–discriminant validity in a clinical sample, and to tenta-
and the control group (Table 6).
tively explore its structural stability.
Regarding the first issue, MentS-assessed mentalization was
indeed found to be lower in persons with BPD, as would be
Table 6. Structure matrices for canonical functions discriminating the clinical and
control group. expected from current knowledge on these phenomena
(Fonagy & Bateman, 2008) and as was ascertained in H1. The
MentS subscales MentS C ECR subscales
observed effect, which is on the boundary of large (d D ¡.47),
Function Function can actually be traced back to large differences in self-related
Variables 1 Variables 1 mentalization, which contribute to discriminating BPD-diag-
MentS-Self .97 ECR–R Anxiety .97 nosed participants from controls even with attachment quality
MentS-Others .25 MentS-Self ¡.56 in the picture.
MentS-Motivation .11 ECR–R Avoidance .55 Additionally, MentS scores exhibited a very similar pattern of
MentS-Others ¡.14
MentS-Motivation ¡.06 correlations with indexes of the Big Five and attachment quality
as in the community sample (supporting H2), the few disparities
Note. Pooled within-groups correlations between discriminating variables and (e.g., larger correlations with Agreeableness and Openness in
standardized canonical discriminant functions. Variables ordered by absolute size
of correlation within function. ECR–R D Modified Experiences in Close Relation- the clinical sample) probably stemming from the difference in
ships–Revised. measures of the basic five factors of personality (i.e., the BFI vs.
278  HANAK, ALTARAS DIMITRIJEVIC,
DIMITRIJEVIC,  JOLIC
 MARJANOVIC


the NEO-FFI). Requirements regarding internal consistency that the clinical sample was (a) relatively small, (b) limited to
were also largely met, although alphas were lower than in Study persons diagnosed with BPD, and (c) not administered the full
1 and below .70 in one instance, namely for MentS-M. battery of instruments employed in Study 1. Although one might
Not surprisingly, this same subscale turned out to be particu- argue that the size of this sample reflected the actual number of
larly problematic in our tentative analysis of factor similarity in patients diagnosed with BPD in a relevant institution during the
the two studies. The results of this exploration would actually course of a whole year, we still see it as a necessary next step to
lead one to conclude that only the MentS-S subscale is stable extend the evidence on the scale’s clinical utility by recruiting
across different samples. However, apart from the fact that this other clinical samples. Besides, the control group in our study
finding was obtained with a relatively small and specific clinical was not evaluated for BPD or other forms of psychopathology,
group, there is another reason to refrain from more general which should preferably be included in future research.
inferences on the structural (in)stability of the MentS. In particu- Moreover, due to constraints in human resources (i.e.,
lar, we are referring here to the more recently acquired data availability of only one reliable coder), we were hitherto
from the ongoing German validation of the MentS (Dimitrijevic unable to judge the validity of the MentS against the RFS
et al., 2017), where the original factors (from Study 1) were repli- as the benchmark in mentalization assessment, but intend
cated in both a community (N D 277, Tucker’s F D .87–.93) to tackle this limitation in our ongoing research with the
and a student sample (N D 959, Tucker’s F D .89–.92). German translation of the instrument. The importance of
In sum, although casting a slight shadow on the instrument’s this task notwithstanding, we approach it with the following
internal consistency and structural stability, Study 2 puts forth caveat: The RFS allows a performance assessment of men-
additional evidence on the validity of the MentS. Moreover, talizing capacity, whereas the MentS, like other self-report
through the prism of MentS subscale scores, new light is shed instruments, seeks to capture an individual’s cognitive-affec-
on the mentalizing experiences of persons with BPD, revealing tive representations of it. Bearing in mind this difference,
that difficulties in self-related mentalization might appear we think it at least equally relevant to further explore the
alongside a rather “normal” motivation to mentalize. All this convergent validity of the MentS with relation to the newly
suggests that the MentS could serve as a sound assessment tool published RFQ or the MZQ. We should like to hope that
in clinical-psychological research and potentially grow into a through these efforts, and given these findings, the MentS
useful resource for diagnostic screening. might likely be recognized as a useful new tool for the study
of individual differences in mentalization and their mental
health and personality correlates.
General discussion and conclusions
Although mentalization has become a salient construct in clini-
cal and developmental psychology, its assessment has remained Funding
quite demanding, impeding the pursuit of large-scale quantita-
tive research. Our goal was therefore to construct and validate Funding was provided by the Ministry of Education and Science, Republic
of Serbia, grant no. 179018.
a brief self-report measure of mentalization that would yield
quick assessments of the construct in adults from both the gen-
eral and clinical population.
Tested in a large community sample, the proposed 28-item References
MentS evidenced good whole-scale reliability, but appeared to Abu-Akel, A., & Bo, S. (2013). Superior mentalizing abilities of female
actually cover three distinct aspects of mentalization: MentS-S, patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 210, 794–799.
MentS-O, and MentS-M. Admittedly, this structure was only doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.09.013
partially recovered in a much smaller sample of persons with Allen, J. G. (2006). Mentalizing in practice. In J. G. Allen & P. Fonagy
(Eds.), Handbook of mentalization-based treatment (pp. 3–30). West
BPD, yet acceptable internal consistencies were established in Sussex, UK: Wiley.
both studies for all subscale scores but the MentS-M in the clin- Allen, J. G., & Fonagy, P. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of mentalization-based
ical sample. treatment. West Sussex, UK: Wiley.
Regarding construct validity, the MentS was found to Altaras Dimitrijevic, A., & Jolic Marjanovic, Z. (2010). The Mayer–Salo-
display a meaningful pattern of correlations with measures vey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test: Psychometric properties of
the Serbian version. Psihologija, 43, 411–426. doi:10.2298/PSI1004411A
of related constructs and the basic personality traits, corre- Austin, E. J., & Deary, I. J. (2002). Personality dispositions. In R. J. Stern-
lating positively (but not too strongly) with empathy, trait berg (Ed.), Why smart people can be so stupid (pp. 187–211). New
and ability EI, Openness, Extraversion, and Conscientious- Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
ness, and negatively with attachment avoidance and anxiety, Badoud, D., Luyten, P., Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Eliez, S., Fonagy, P., &
and Neuroticism. Finally, as first evidence of criterion valid- Debbane, M. (2015). The French version of the Reflective Functioning
Questionnaire: Validity data for adolescents and adults and its associa-
ity and clinical utility, the MentS significantly discriminated tion with non-suicidal self-injury. PLoS ONE, 10(12), e0145892.
between persons with BPD and controls, primarily due to doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0145892
considerably lower MentS-S scores in the clinical group. Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2009). The
first 10,000 Adult Attachment Interviews: Distributions of adult
attachment representations in clinical and non-clinical groups.
Limitations and future directions Attachment & Human Development, 11, 223–263. doi:10.1080/
14616730902814762
Precluding a decisive positive judgment on the psychometric Baron-Cohen, S. (2004). Essential difference: Male and female brains and
properties and clinical utility of the MentS is, however, the fact the truth about autism. New York, NY: Basic Books.
THE MENTALIZATION SCALE (MENTS) 279

Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The Empathy Quotient: An Hair, J. F., Jr., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2010). Multi-
investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning variate data analysis (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson
autism and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmen- Education.
tal Disorders, 34, 163–175. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00 Hanak, N. (2004). Construction of a new instrument for assessment of
Barrett, P. (1986). Factor comparison: An examination of three methods. adult and adolescent attachment—QAA. Psihologija, 37(1), 123–142.
Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 327–340. doi:10.2298/PSI0401123H
Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2004). Psychotherapy for borderline personality Hanak, N. (2010). Razlike između trudnica sigurnog obrasca i nesigurnih
disorder: Mentalization-based treatment. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univer- obrazaca afektivne vezanosti u pogledu tranzicije ka materinstvu [Dif-
sity Press. ferences between pregnant women with secure and fearful attachment
Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2006). Mentalizing and borderline personality patterns with respect to the transition to motherhood]. Psiholoska
disorder. In J. G. Allen & P. Fonagy (Eds.), Handbook of mentalization- Istraz ivanja, 13(1), 131–147.
based treatment (pp. 185–200). West Sussex, UK: Wiley. Hanak, N., & Dimitrijevic, A. (2013). A Serbian version of modified
Bateman, A. W., & Fonagy, P. (Eds.). (2011). Handbook of mentalizing in and Revised Experiences in Close Relationships scale (SM–ECR–
mental health practice. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric R). Journal of Personality Assessment, 95, 530–538. doi:10.1080/
Publishing. 00223891.2013.778271
Bouchard, M. A., Target, M., Lecours, S., Fonagy, P., Tremblay, L. M., Hausberg, M. C., Schulz, H., Piegler, T., Happach, C. G., Kl€ opper, M.,
Schachter, A., & Stein, H. (2008). Mentalization in adult attachment Br€utt, A. L., & Andreas, S. (2012). Is a self-rated instrument appro-
narratives: Reflective functioning, mental states, and affect elaboration priate to assess mentalization in patients with mental disorders?
compared. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 25(1), 47–66. doi:10.1037/0736- Development and first validation of the Mentalization Question-
9735.25.1.47 naire (MZQ). Psychotherapy Research, 22, 699–709. doi:10.1080/
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd 10503307.2012.709325
ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hill, L. L., Levy, K. N., Meehan, K. B., & Reynoso, J. S. (2007). Dimensions
Dimitrijevic, A., Hanak, N., Vukosavljevic Gvozden, T., & Opacic, G. and new measures relevant to psychoanalysis: Reliability of a multidi-
(2012). Psychometric properties of the Serbian version of the mensional measure for scoring reflective function. Journal of the Amer-
Empathy Quotient (S-EQ). Psihologija, 44, 257–276. doi:10.2298/ ican Psychoanalytic Association, 55(1), 309–313. doi:10.1177/
PSI1203257D 00030651070550010201
Dimitrijevic, A., Ristein, L., Licata, M., Hamburger, A., Altaras Dimitrijevic, Holmes, J. (2006). Mentalizing from a psychoanalytic perspective: What’s
A., & Jolic Marjanovic, Z. (2017). Validation of the German version of new? In J. G. Allen & P. Fonagy (Eds.), Handbook of mentalization-
the Mentalization Scale (D-MentS). Manuscript in preparation. based treatment (pp. 31–49). West Sussex, UK: Wiley.
Fischer-Kern, M., Buchheim, A., H€ orz, S., Schuster, P., Doering, S., John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the
Kapusta, N. D., & Fonagy, P. (2010). The relationship between person- integrative Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and con-
ality organization, reflective functioning, and psychiatric classification ceptual issues. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.),
in borderline personality disorder. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 27, 395– Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 114–158). New
409. doi:10.1037/a0020862 York, NY: Guilford Press.
Fonagy, P., & Allison, E. (2012). What is mentalization? The concept and Jolic Marjanovic, Z., & Altaras Dimitrijevic, A. (2014). Reliability, con-
its foundations in developmental research. In N. Midgley & I. Vrouva struct and criterion-related validity of the Serbian adaptation of the
(Eds.), Minding the child: Mentalization-based interventions with chil- Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue). Psihologija, 47,
dren, young people and their families (pp. 11–34). London, UK: 249–262. doi:10.2298/PSI1402249J
Routledge. Knezevic, G., Dzamonja-Ignjatovic, T., & Đuric-Jocic, D. (2004). Petofaktorski
Fonagy, P., & Bateman, A. (2008). Mentalization-based treatment of bor- model licnosti [Five-factor personality model]. Belgrade, Serbia: Centar za
derline personality disorder. In E. L. Jurist, A. Slade, & S. Bergner primenjenu psihologiju.
(Eds.), Mind to mind: Infant research, neuroscience, and psychoanalysis Luyten, P., & Fonagy, P. (2014). Assessing mentalising in attachment con-
(pp. 139–166). New York, NY: Other Press. texts. In P. Holmes & S. Farnfield (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of
Fonagy, P., Luyten, P., Moulton-Perkins, A., Lee, Y. W., Warren, F., Howard, attachment: Assessment (pp. 210–226). Hove, UK: Routledge.
S., … Lowyck, B. (2016). Development and validation of a self-report Luyten, P., Fonagy, P., Lowyck, B., & Vermote, R. (2011). Assessment of
measure of mentalizing: The Reflective Functioning Questionnaire. PLoS mentalization. In A. W. Bateman & P. Fonagy (Eds.), Handbook of
ONE, 11(7), e0158678. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0158678 mentalizing in mental health practice (pp. 43–65). Arlington, VA:
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2008). Attachment, trauma, and psychoanalysis: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Where psychoanalysis meets neuroscience. In E. L. Jurist, A. Slade, & S. Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1998). Adult attachment classification system.
Bergner (Eds.), Mind to mind: Infant research, neuroscience, and psy- Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley.
choanalysis (pp. 15–49). New York, NY: Other Press. Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer–Salovey–Caruso
Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1998). Reflective-function- Emotional Intelligence Test: User’s manual. Toronto, ON, Canada:
ing manual, version 5.0, for application to adult attachment interviews. MHS.
London, UK: University College London. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2004). A contemplated revision of the NEO
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response Five-Factor Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 587–
theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment. Journal of 596. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00118-1
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 350–365. doi:10.1037/0022- Meehan, K. B., Levy, K. N., Reynoso, J. S., Hill, L. L., & Clarkin, J. F. (2009).
3514.78.2.350 Measuring reflective function with a multidimensional rating scale:
George, C., & West, M. (2001). The development and preliminary valida- Comparison with scoring reflective function on the AAI. Journal of the
tion of a new measure of adult attachment: The Adult Attachment Pro- American Psychoanalytic Association, 57(1), 208–213. doi:10.1177/
jective. Attachment and Human Development, 3(1), 30–61. 00030651090570011008
doi:10.1080/14616730010024771 Mikolajczak, M., Nelis, D., Hansenne, M., & Quoidbach, J. (2008). If you
George, D., & Mallery, M. (2010). SPSS for Windows step by step: A simple can regulate sadness, you can probably regulate shame: Associations
guide and reference (17.0 update, (10th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson. between trait emotional intelligence, emotion regulation and coping
Ha, C., Sharp, C., Ensink, K., Fonagy, P., & Cirino, P. (2013). The efficiency across discrete emotions. Personality and Individual Differen-
measurement of reflective function in adolescents with and without ces, 44, 1356–1368. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.004
borderline traits. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 1215–1223. M€uller, C., Kaufhold, J., Overbeck, G., & Grabhorn, R. (2006). The impor-
doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.09.008 tance of reflective functioning to the diagnosis of psychic structure. Psy-
Hair, J. F., Jr., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multi- chology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 79, 485–494.
variate data analysis (5th ed.). New York, NY: Macmillan. doi:10.1348/147608305£68048
280  HANAK, ALTARAS DIMITRIJEVIC,
DIMITRIJEVIC,  JOLIC
 MARJANOVIC


Myers, N. D., Ahn, S., & Jin, Y. (2011). Sample size and power estimates Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The location of trait emo-
for a confirmatory factor analytic model in exercise and sport: A tional intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psychol-
Monte Carlo approach. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82, ogy, 98, 273–289. doi:10.1348/000712606£120618
412–423. Rudden, M. G., Milrod, B., & Target, M. (2005). The Brief Reflective Func-
O’Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the num- tioning Interview. New York, NY: Weill Cornell Medical College.
ber of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Rutimann, D. D., & Meehan, K. B. (2012). Validity of a brief interview for
Behavior Research Methods, Instrumentation, and Computers, 32, 396– assessing reflective function. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic
402. doi:10.3758/BF03200807 Association, 60, 577–589. doi:10.1177/0003065112445616
Petrides, K. V. (2009). Technical manual for the Trait Emotional Intelli- Stein, H. (2006). Does mentalizing promote resilience. In J. G. Allen & P.
gence Questionnaires (TEIQue). London, UK: London Psychometric Fonagy (Eds.), The handbook of mentalization-based treatment (pp.
Laboratory. 308–326). West Sussex, UK: Wiley.

Appendix: MentS Questionnaire

This questionnaire consists of 28 items. Please, read each of the items carefully and CIRCLE a number on the scale from 1 to 5,

1 2 3 4 5
Completely incorrect Mostly incorrect Both correct and incorrect Mostly correct Completely correct

1. I find it important to understand reasons for my behavior. 1 2 3 4 5


2. When I make conclusions about other people’s personality traits I carefully observe what they say and do. 1 2 3 4 5
3. I can recognize other people’s feelings. 1 2 3 4 5
4. I often think about other people and their behavior. 1 2 3 4 5
5. Usually I can recognize what makes people feel uneasy. 1 2 3 4 5
6. I can sympathize with other people’s feelings. 1 2 3 4 5
7. When someone annoys me I try to understand why I react in that way. 1 2 3 4 5
8. When I get upset I am not sure whether I am sad, afraid, or angry. 1 2 3 4 5
9. I do not like to waste time trying to understand in detail other people’s behavior. 1 2 3 4 5
10. I can make good predictions of other people’s behavior when I know their beliefs and feelings. 1 2 3 4 5
11. Often I cannot explain, even to myself, why I did something. 1 2 3 4 5
12. Sometimes I can understand someone’s feelings before s/he tells me anything. 1 2 3 4 5
13. I find it important to understand what happens in my relationships with people close to me. 1 2 3 4 5
14. I do not want to find out something about myself that I will not like. 1 2 3 4 5
15. To understand someone’s behavior, we need to know her/his thoughts, wishes, and feelings. 1 2 3 4 5
16. I often talk about emotions with people that I am close to. 1 2 3 4 5
17. I like reading books and newspaper articles about psychological subjects. 1 2 3 4 5
18. I find it difficult to admit to myself that I am sad, hurt, or afraid. 1 2 3 4 5
19. I do not like to think about my problems. 1 2 3 4 5
20. I can describe significant traits of people who are close to me with precision and in detail. 1 2 3 4 5
21. I am often confused about my exact feelings. 1 2 3 4 5
22. It is difficult for me to find adequate words to express my feelings. 1 2 3 4 5
23. People tell me that I understand them and give them sound advice. 1 2 3 4 5
24. I have always been interested in why people behave in certain ways. 1 2 3 4 5
25. I can easily describe what I feel. 1 2 3 4 5
26. While people talk about their feelings and needs my thoughts often drift away. 1 2 3 4 5
27. Since we all depend on life circumstances, it is meaningless to think of other people’s intentions or wishes. 1 2 3 4 5
28. One of the most important things that children should learn is to express their feelings and wishes. 1 2 3 4 5

depending on how much the item is CORRECT FOR YOU PERSONALLY.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai