Anda di halaman 1dari 13

Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences © 2014 American Psychological Association

2014, Vol. 8, No. 2, 96 –108 2330-2925/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/h0099130

Ethnicity-Related Variation in Sexual Promiscuity, Relationship


Status, and Testosterone Levels in Men

Dario Maestripieri, Amanda Klimczuk, Daniel Traficonte, and M. Claire Wilson


The University of Chicago

The purpose of this study was to investigate potential ethnicity-related variation in


men’s relationship status, sexual promiscuity, and testosterone levels. Data from two
ethnically diverse subject populations were used. The first dataset included 302 male
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

graduate students (age range: 23–36 years; M ⫽ 28); the second dataset consisted of 77
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

male undergraduate and graduate students (age range: 18 –38 years; M ⫽ 23). For both,
we collected information on ethnicity (European American, African American, His-
panic, or Asian American), relationship status (single, in a short-term or long-term
relationship, or married), and sexual promiscuity (number of lifetime sexual partners,
number of one-night stands, extrapair sexual activity), in addition to measuring salivary
testosterone concentrations. In both datasets testosterone levels were significantly
higher in single men than in men in relationships but this difference was reversed in
men of Asian American ethnicity. Asian American men had the lowest number of
sexual partners, one-night stands, and extrapair sexual activity across ethnic groups.
Moreover, among Asian Americans, men in relationships had a higher average number
of sexual partners than single men. Our results indicate that to understand the associ-
ation between relationship status and testosterone levels in men, ethnicity-related
variation in sexual activity in single men and men in relationships must be taken into
consideration.

Keywords: relationship status, testosterone, ethnicity, sexual behavior

In humans and many other animal species, A growing number of human studies have
testosterone plays a key role in male mating- investigated variation in testosterone levels in
related competition for status and resources and relation to relationship/marital status and mat-
in male courtship and sexual behavior (e.g., ing and parenting effort. Two of the most con-
Nelson, 2011). In particular, because testoster- sistent findings that have emerged from this
one is expected to be elevated when males en- body of research are that single men have higher
gage in competition with other males as well as testosterone levels than men in relationships,
during courtship and mating, but low when and that testosterone levels are particularly low
males are monogamously paired and engaged in in married men with young children (e.g., Booth
parental behavior (e.g., Wingfield, Hegner, & Dabbs, 1993; Burnham et al., 2003; Gray et
Dufty, & Ball, 1990), understanding interindi- al., 2004; van Anders, Hamilton, & Watson,
vidual and intraindividual variation in male tes- 2007; van Anders & Watson, 2006; see Gray &
tosterone levels can provide insight into male Campbell, 2009, for a review). These correla-
mating and parenting effort, which are key as- tions between testosterone levels and relation-
pects of male life history (e.g., Bribiescas, 2006). ship, marital, and parental status, however, are
open to different interpretations. First, it is pos-
sible that there is no direct causal relationship
between testosterone levels and relationship sta-
Dario Maestripieri, Amanda Klimczuk, Daniel Trafi- tus but that these variables are independently
conte, and M. Claire Wilson, Institute for Mind and Biol- correlated with a third variable (e.g., a person-
ogy, The University of Chicago. ality trait). Second, it is possible that a causal
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Dario Maestripieri, The University of Chicago,
relationship exists such that high testosterone
5730 S. Woodlawn Avenue, Chicago, IL 60637. E-mail: induces males to remain single and low testos-
dario@uchicago.edu terone induces males to form monogamous repro-
96
TESTOSTERONE AND RELATIONSHIP STATUS 97

ductive relationships and to invest in children. monogamous men who engage in frequent ex-
Third, it is possible that being single raises a trapair sexual fantasies and encounters than in
male’s level of testosterone while being in a those who do not (McIntyre et al., 2006).
monogamous relationship and investing in chil- In this study, we further investigated the as-
dren lowers testosterone. Clearly, understand- sociation between relationship status, sexual
ing if and how relationship status and testoster- promiscuity (defined as desire for or interaction
one are causally related is crucial for with multiple sexual partners), and male testos-
understanding human male social and reproduc- terone levels. To this end, we investigated not
tive strategies, and more generally the evolution only differences between single men and men in
of male life history traits. relationships but also variation within each
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

So far, most evidence seems to support the group. Our working hypothesis was that, just as
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

hypothesis that a causal association exists and the category of paired men is heterogeneous and
that relationship status affects testosterone lev- includes both men who desire or engage in
els rather than the other way around. Some of extramarital sexual activities and those who
this evidence comes from longitudinal studies don’t, the single men category is probably het-
reporting a decrease in men’s testosterone with erogeneous as well and includes both highly
the transition to fatherhood (e.g., Berg, & sexually active men (e.g., men who are single
Wynne-Edwards, 2001; Gettler, McDade, Fera- by choice in order to maximize their opportu-
nil, & Kuzawa, 2011; Hirschenhauser et al., nities to engage in promiscuous sex) and less
2002; Kuzawa, Gettler, Muller, McDade, & Fe- sexually active men (e.g., men who are single
ranil, 2009; Perini, Ditzen, Fischbacher, & by choice to avoid sexual relationships alto-
Ehlert, 2012; Storey, Walsh, Quinton, & gether and focus on other aspects of their lives
Wynne-Edwards, 2000); the mechanisms re- as well as men who are single by constraint,
sponsible for the reduction in testosterone could because they are unable to find a partner).
be reduced frequency of sexual intercourse One source of variation in attitudes about
(e.g., Gettler, McDade, Agustin, Feranil, & Ku- being single and sexually promiscuous or in a
zawa, 2013), exposure to frequent infant cries faithful/unfaithful monogamous relationship is
(Fleming, Corter, Stallings, & Steiner, 2002; cultural norms concerning men’s sexual re-
Muller, Marlowe, Bugumba, & Ellison, 2009; straint and relationship models in different eth-
van Anders, Tolman, & Volling, 2012), ex- nic groups or societies (Gray & Campbell,
tended bodily contact with infants through 2009). For example, studies comparing differ-
cosleeping (Gettler, McKenna, McDade, Agus- ent ethnic groups in North America have indi-
tin, & Kuzawa, 2012), or simply significant cated that Asian American males exhibit greater
involvement in caregiving (e.g., Jasienska, sexual conservatism and less sexual experience
Jasienski, & Ellison, 2012). than men in other ethnic groups (Meston, Trap-
One possible explanation for high testoster- nell, & Gorzalka, 1996; Okazaki, 2002).
one levels in single men is that they are, on In this study, we tested the hypothesis that
average, more involved in courtship of multiple ethnicity-related variation in sexual promiscuity
potential sexual partners than men in stable could potentially affect both the magnitude and
monogamous relationships; in turn, interaction the direction of the relationship between testos-
with multiple (potential) sexual partners or sim- terone and relationship status: specifically, we
ply the desire for a high variety of sexual part- predicted that Asian American males should be
ners is known to raise a man’s testosterone (e.g., less sexually promiscuous than males in other
van Anders, Hamilton, & Watson, 2007; van ethnic groups, and that the previously reported
Anders & Goldey, 2010; Roney, Mahler, & difference in testosterone between single and
Maestripieri, 2003; Roney, Lukaszewski, & paired men should not occur among Asian
Simmons, 2007). For example, polygynously Americans or be significantly smaller in this
married Kenyan Swahili men exhibit higher tes- group than in other ethnic groups. To test our
tosterone levels than monogamously married hypotheses, we first investigated the relation-
men (Gray, 2003), polyamorous North Ameri- ship between ethnicity, relationship status, and
can men have higher testosterone levels than testosterone in a historical database established
partnered and even single men (van Anders et in 2008. Then we investigated the potential role
al., 2007), and testosterone levels are higher in of sexual promiscuity in a new study conducted
98 MAESTRIPIERI, KLIMCZUK, TRAFICONTE, AND WILSON

in 2013, which involved a smaller sample size took a 1-hr computerized test and gave two
but more detailed measures of sexual promiscu- saliva samples, one before and one after the test.
ity. To our knowledge, this study is the first All saliva samples were collected between 1:30
investigation of the role of ethnicity in the as- p.m. and 5:00 p.m. (time of sample collection
sociation between men’s relationship status, did not significantly affect testosterone levels).
sexual behavior, and testosterone levels. The Participants were asked not to eat any food,
study of human mating strategies has increas- drink anything other than water, or smoke for at
ingly incorporated physiological, social, and least 1 hr prior to the study. Saliva samples were
cultural variables and examined not only be- assayed for testosterone concentrations (see
tween-sex but also within-sex variation. There- Maestripieri et al., 2010; Sapienza et al., 2009;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

fore, this study enhances our knowledge of the for details on sample collection and hormonal
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

social and cultural context for within-sex vari- assays). The means of the testosterone concen-
ation in male mating strategies as well as our trations of the two samples were used for data
knowledge of the relationship between mating analyses.
effort and testosterone in male life histories. Mean age (⫾ SE) of the 325 study partici-
pants was 28.75 (⫾ 0.12) years; age range was
Method 23–36 years. Ethnicity and relationship status
were distributed as follows: 160 European
2008 Dataset American (the terms European American and
Caucasian are used interchangeably in this arti-
Study participants were 325 male MBA stu- cle; single ⫽ 100; in relationship ⫽ 60); 25
dents in a private midwestern university. These African American (for brevity, referred to as
individuals were part of a larger dataset that African; single ⫽ 20; in relationship ⫽ 5); 30
included a total of 348 males and 153 females. Hispanic American (for brevity, referred to as
Data from females were not considered in this Hispanic; single ⫽ 12; in relationship ⫽ 18); 50
study. Data from 23 male subjects were not Far East Asian American (for brevity, referred
included because information about some of the to as FE Asian; single ⫽ 26; in relationship ⫽
variables of interest in this study (see below) 24); 60 Indian American (for brevity, referred to
was incomplete or missing. These individuals as Indian; single ⫽ 30; in relationship ⫽ 30).
were recruited in 2008 to participate in a study Each ethnic group included both people born in
of financial risk-taking and stress reactivity foreign countries and people born in the U.S.A.
(Maestripieri, Baran, Sapienza, & Zingales, (e.g., the FE Asian category included both men
2010; Sapienza, Zingales, & Maestripieri, born in China and Japan and Chinese American
2009). Participation in the study was a course and Japanese American men born in the U.S.A.).
requirement therefore it was mandatory for the Indians included both people born in India or
entire 2008 cohort of MBA students. Students, Bangladesh and people born in the U.S.A., with
however, were paid $20 or more for their par- family origins in India or Bangladesh. There
ticipation. The use of human subjects was ap- were no significant differences in age or rela-
proved by the IRB and all students were asked tionship status among the five ethnic groups.
for informed written consent for their participa-
tion in the study. 2013 Dataset
Study participants filled out a questionnaire
in which they provided information about their Study participants were 77 male young
age, ethnicity (participants were given a list of adults. Approximately 80% of the study partic-
ethnicities and asked to choose the one with ipants were undergraduate or graduate students
which they identified; individuals with multiple (but there were no MBA students among them)
ethnicities were asked to identify a primary at the same university where the 2008 study was
one), and relationship status (single or in a conducted, and most of the others were em-
relationship). The survey also included ques- ployed by this university under various capaci-
tions about sexual orientation and number of ties (e.g., research or administrative staff). Par-
lifetime sexual partners but answering these two ticipants were part of a larger study that also
questions was not mandatory and many partic- included 75 female participants but female data
ipants did not answer them. The participants were not considered in this study. Study partic-
TESTOSTERONE AND RELATIONSHIP STATUS 99

ipants were recruited in 2013 through fliers Data for items (c) and (d) were analyzed only
posted on campus, mailing lists, or a human for participants who were in a relationship at the
subject recruitment Web site (Sona System). All time the study was conducted.
study participants completed a written informed Mean age (⫾ SE) of the 77 participants was
consent form before participating in the study 23.17 (⫾ 0.50) years; age range was 18 –38 years.
and were paid $ 20 after completion of the Ethnicity and relationship status were distributed
procedures. The use of human subjects was as follows: 42 European American (i.e., Cauca-
approved by the IRB. sian; single ⫽ 8; in relationship ⫽ 34); eight
Study participants were asked to provide a African (single ⫽ 4; in relationship ⫽ 4); 14
saliva sample and to complete a series of ques- Hispanic (single ⫽ 6; in relationship ⫽ 8); 13
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tionnaires online or on paper. Saliva samples Asian (single ⫽ 6; in relationship ⫽ 7; this


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

were collected and stored in ice using the same category included eight East Asian and five
procedures used for the 2008 study (e.g., all Indian men; data analyses provided similar re-
samples were collected in the early afternoon sults whether the five Indian men were included
and time of sample collection did not affect in the Asian group or excluded; therefore they
testosterone levels). Samples were later shipped were included to increase sample size). As was
to the Hominoid Reproductive Ecology Labo- the case for the 2008 database, each ethnic
ratory at the University of New Mexico, where group included both people born in foreign
they were assayed for testosterone, using the countries and people born in the U.S.A. There
Salimetrics salivary testosterone ELISA (Cata- were no significant differences in age or rela-
log 1–2402, Salimetrics, LLC, State College, tionship status among the four ethnic groups.
PA). The sensitivity of this assay is approxi- Sixty-eight participants self-described their sex-
mately 1 pg/ml. The antibody has significant ual orientation as heterosexual or mainly het-
cross-reactivities with dihydrotestosterone erosexual, seven as homosexual or mainly ho-
(36%) and 19-nortestosterone (21%), neither of mosexual, and two as bisexual.
which is expected in appreciable quantities in
the saliva of healthy adults. All other cross- Results
reactivities are ⬍2%. Interassay coefficients of
variation (N ⫽ 6) were 5% for a high sample 2008 Dataset
and 14% for a low sample. Intraassay CV (mean
CV of duplicates) was 3%. Testosterone data A two-way ANOVA examining the effects of
were available for only 62 of the 77 study ethnic group and relationship status on testos-
participants. terone concentrations revealed both a main ef-
An initial demographic survey asked infor- fect of relationship status, F(1, 315) ⫽ 4.16,
mation about participants’ age, ethnicity (as- (p ⬍ .05), with single men having higher tes-
sessed with the same procedure used for the tosterone than men in relationships, and a sig-
2008 dataset), sexual orientation, and relation- nificant interaction between relationship status
ship status (single or in a relationship; for some and ethnicity, F(4, 315) ⫽ 2.40, (p ⫽ .05). As
data analyses, the relationship category was di- Figure 1a illustrates, among Caucasians, Afri-
vided into subgroups; see below). The other cans, and Hispanics, single men had signifi-
questionnaires asked questions about personal- cantly higher testosterone than men in relation-
ity characteristics and romantic and sexual ex- ships (all p ⬍ .05). Among Indians this
perience and preferences. For the purposes of difference was much smaller and nonsignifi-
this study, four questionnaire items were used: cant, and Far East Asians showed a significant
(a) the total number of partners with whom difference in the opposite direction: single men
study participants had engaged in vaginal, anal, had lower testosterone than men in relationships
or oral sex over the lifetime; (b) the total num- (p ⬍ .05). There was no significant main effect
ber of one-night stands the study participants of ethnicity on testosterone concentrations.
had had in their lifetime; (c) the total number of
partners with whom they had had extrapair sex 2013 Dataset
in their lifetime; and (d) the frequency of fan-
tasizing about having extrapair sex (on a scale Figure 1b depicts testosterone concentrations
from 1 ⫽ Never, to 8 ⫽ at least once a day). in Caucasian, African, Hispanic, and Asian men
100 MAESTRIPIERI, KLIMCZUK, TRAFICONTE, AND WILSON
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 1. (a). Mean (⫹ SE) testosterone levels in single men and men in relationships in five
different ethnic groups. Data are from the 2008 dataset. Sample sizes were as follows: 160
Caucasian (single ⫽ 100; in relationship ⫽ 60); 25 African (single ⫽ 20; in relationship ⫽
5); 30 Hispanic (single ⫽ 12; in relationship ⫽ 18); 50 Far East Asian (single ⫽ 26; in
relationship ⫽ 24); 60 Indian (single ⫽ 30; in relationship ⫽ 30). (b). Mean (⫹ SE)
testosterone levels in single men and men in relationships in four different ethnic groups (for
brevity, European American is labeled Caucasian, African American is African, Hispanic
American is Hispanic, and Asian American is Asian). Data are from the 2013 dataset. Sample
sizes were as follows: 34 Caucasian (single ⫽ 4; in a relationship ⫽ 30); eight African
(single ⫽ 2; in a relationship ⫽ 6); 10 Hispanic (single ⫽ 5; in a relationship ⫽ 5); 10 Asian
(single ⫽ 5; in a relationship ⫽ 5).
TESTOSTERONE AND RELATIONSHIP STATUS 101

who were single or in a relationship. A two-way Of the men who were in relationships, Asian
ANOVA revealed a significant interaction be- men reported that they had never had extrapair
tween relationship status and ethnicity, F(3, sex in their lifetime, and the others reported that
54) ⫽ 4.84, (p ⫽ .004). The pattern was similar they had had on average approximately one
to that observed in the 2008 dataset: among episode (0.81 ⫹ 0.33) of extrapair sex in their
Caucasians, Africans and Hispanics, single men lifetime. Asian men in relationships also fanta-
had higher testosterone than men in relation- sized about extrapair sex, on average, less fre-
ships. The opposite was true among Asians. The quently (3.33 ⫹ 1.2) than other men in relation-
differences were statistically significant for ships (4.47 ⫹ 0.29). Neither the difference in
Caucasians (t ⫽ ⫺3.2; df ⫽ 32; p ⬍ .01), extrapair sex nor the difference in extrapair sex
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hispanics (t ⫽ ⫺3.11; df ⫽ 8; p ⫽ .01), and fantasy, however, was statistically significant


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Asians (t ⫽ ⫺3.02; df ⫽ 8; p ⬍ .05) but not for (Extrapair sex: t ⫽ ⫺0.87; df ⫽ 52; NS; Ex-
Africans (t ⫽ ⫺0.7; df ⫽ 6; NS), probably trapair sex fantasy: t ⫽ ⫺1.25; df ⫽ 48; NS).
because of the small sample size (only two Testosterone was not significantly correlated
African men were single). with lifetime number of sexual partners (r ⫽
Figure 1b shows that Asian single men had, .07; n ⫽ 61; NS), lifetime number of one-night
on average, the lowest testosterone levels stands (r ⫽ .03; n ⫽ 61; NS), lifetime number
among all single men, and Asian men in rela- of extrapair sex events (r ⫽ .06; n ⫽ 45; NS), or
tionships had, on average, the highest testoster- frequency of extrapair sex fantasies (r ⫽ .02;
one levels of all men in relationships. Variation n ⫽ 41; NS).
in testosterone among the single men in the four In the next set of analyses, testosterone data
ethnic groups was statistically significant, F(2, for Caucasians, Africans, and Hispanics were
15) ⫽ 4.91, (p ⫽ .01). Post hoc tests indicated pooled together and men in relationships were
that Asian single men had significantly lower subdivided into three categories depending on
testosterone than both Caucasian (p ⬍ .05) and whether they were in short-term (less than 6
Hispanic single men (p ⬍ .05). Asian men had months) relationships, in long-term (more than
lower testosterone than African single men (n ⫽ 6 months) relationships, or married. Figure 3
2) as well but the difference was not significant, illustrates the testosterone data for men in these
probably due to the small sample size for this relationship categories along with data for sin-
comparison. Variation in testosterone levels gle men. A one-way ANOVA revealed a signif-
among the men in relationships in the four eth- icant difference in testosterone concentrations
nic groups was not statistically significant, F(3, among the four groups, F(3, 51) ⫽ 10.54, (p ⬍
45) ⫽ 1.55; NS. .0001). Bonferroni-Dunn post hoc tests indi-
Figure 2a depicts the number of lifetime sex- cated that single men had higher testosterone
ual partners for Asian men who were single and than men in the other three groups (all p ⬍ .05),
in relationships and for men who were single married men had lower concentrations than men
and in relationships in the other ethnic groups in the other three groups (all p ⬍ .05), and men
(Caucasian, African, and Hispanic) combined. in short-term relationships had higher concen-
Asian men, regardless of relationship status, had trations than men in long-term relationships
a significantly lower number of sexual partners (p ⬍ .05).
than the other men, F(1, 72) ⫽ 6.19, (p ⫽ .01). Figure 4a illustrates the lifetime number of
Among Asians, men in relationships had a sexual partners for Caucasian, African, and His-
higher average number of sexual partners than panic men who were single and in short-term
single men, whereas the opposite was true for relationships, long-term relationships, or mar-
men in the other ethnic groups. The statistical ried. Although men who were single and in
interaction between ethnic group and relation- short-term relationships had a higher average
ship status, however, was not significant. Figure number of sexual partners than men in long-
2b shows the total number of one-night stands term relationships or married, the overall differ-
in Asian men and men from the other ethnic ence among the four groups was not statistically
groups, separately for single men and men in significant, F(3, 63) ⫽ 1.85; NS. Figure 4b
relationships. Asian men had significantly fewer shows the lifetime number of one-night stands
one-night stands than other men, regardless of for Caucasian, African, and Hispanic men who
relationship status, F(1, 72) ⫽ 4.86, (p ⫽ .03). were single and men who were in short-term
102 MAESTRIPIERI, KLIMCZUK, TRAFICONTE, AND WILSON
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 2. (a). Mean (⫹ SE) number of lifetime sexual partners for Asian men (one
homosexual Indian man who had 40 different sexual partners was a population outlier and
excluded from data analyses) who were single and in relationships and for men who were
single and in relationships in the other ethnic groups (Caucasian, African, and Hispanic)
combined. (b) Mean (⫹ SE) number of lifetime one-night-stands for Asian men who were
single and in relationships and for men who were single and in relationships in the other ethnic
groups (Caucasian, African, and Hispanic) combined.

relationships, long-term relationships, or mar- among the groups was not statistically signifi-
ried. Again, single men and men in short-term cant, F(3, 63) ⫽ 1.46; NS. When, however, data
relationships had, on average, more one-night from the long-term relationship and married
stands than men who were in long-term rela- groups were pooled together, both scored sig-
tionships or married but the overall difference nificantly lower than single men and men in
TESTOSTERONE AND RELATIONSHIP STATUS 103
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 3. Mean (⫹ SE) testosterone levels in single men, men in short-term relationships,
men in long-term relationships, and married men. Data combined for Caucasian, Hispanic,
and African men.

short-term relationships on both the lifetime Specifically, they showed that single men have
number of sexual partners (long-term ⫽ 5.51 ⫹ higher testosterone levels than men in relation-
0.81; others ⫽ 9.81 ⫹ 1.81; t ⫽ 2.21; df ⫽ 62; ships only in so far as single men are more
p ⬍ .05) and the lifetime number of one-night sexually promiscuous than men in relationships.
stands (long-term ⫽ 1.51 ⫹ 0.49; others ⫽ If single men show low levels of sexual promis-
3.22 ⫹ 0.82; t ⫽ 2.01; df ⫽ 62; p ⬍ .05). cuity, the difference in testosterone can be re-
Data for Asian men were insufficient for sta- versed, with single men having lower testoster-
tistical comparisons of testosterone levels, num- one than men in relationships. Therefore,
ber of lifetime sexual partners, and number of testosterone levels are not affected by relation-
one-night stands across different relationship ship status in itself (e.g., through some psycho-
categories. Moreover, there were no married logical condition associated with being single or
Asian men in the subject population. Neverthe- having a partner) but by differences in sexual
less, for the purposes of qualitative compari- promiscuity that are generally correlated with
sons, means and standard errors for the three differences in relationship status. Therefore, a
variables for Asian men who were single, in man’s testosterone specifically tracks his mat-
short-term relationships and in long-term rela- ing effort rather than his social life or psycho-
tionships are presented in Table 1. Similar to logical state.
what was shown for the men in the other three In our study, we used ethnicity as a tool with
ethnic groups (but with some differences in the which to investigate potential variation in tes-
absolute values), Asian men in short-term rela- tosterone within and across relationship status
tionships scored higher than those in long-term categories. First, using a larger, historical data-
relationships in all three variables. Unlike the base consisting of male MBA students, we con-
other ethnic groups, however, Asian single men firmed the previously reported finding (see be-
scored relatively low in all three variables, and ginning of article for references) that among
especially in testosterone levels. Western college students of predominantly Eu-
ropean American (i.e., Caucasian) ethnicity,
Discussion single men have, on average, higher testoster-
one levels than men in relationships. Then, we
The results of our study shed new light on the showed that this testosterone difference was
association between relationship status, sexual driven by Caucasian, Hispanic, and African
promiscuity, and testosterone levels in men. men. Among men of Indian origin, single men
104 MAESTRIPIERI, KLIMCZUK, TRAFICONTE, AND WILSON
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 4. (a). Mean (⫹ SE) number of lifetime sexual partners among single men, men in
short-term relationships, men in long-term relationships, and married men. Data combined for
Caucasian, Hispanic, and African men. (b). Mean (⫹ SE) number of lifetime one-night-stands
among single men, men in short-term relationships, men in long-term relationships and
married men. Data combined for Caucasian, Hispanic, and African men.

and men in relationships had similar testoster- mainly students at the same academic institu-
one levels, although among men of East Asian tion. Here again, Asian men (which included
origin (e.g., Japan, China, and Taiwan) the dif- mostly East Asians) who were single had sig-
ference was significant but reversed: single men nificantly lower testosterone than men in rela-
had lower testosterone than men in relation- tionships. Asian single men had the lowest tes-
ships. The same pattern was replicated in an- tosterone levels among single men of all ethnic
other ethnically diverse, but smaller sample of groups, and ethnic variation in testosterone lev-
TESTOSTERONE AND RELATIONSHIP STATUS 105

Table 1
Testosterone and Sexual Experience in Asian Men in Relation to
Relationship Status
Single (n) Short-Term (n) Long-Term (n)
Testosterone 66.32 ⫹ 10.88 (5) 139.28 ⫹ 78.50 (2) 86.78 ⫹ 21.41 (3)
Number of sexual partners 1.33 ⫹ 0.33 (6) 4.00 ⫹ 3.00 (2) 0.33 ⫹ 0.33 (3)
Number of one-night stands 0.33 ⫹ 0.21 (6) 0.50 ⫹ 0.50 (2) 0 (3)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

els among men in relationships was not statis- events and extrapair sex fantasies were analyzed
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tically significant. only for men in relationships) and the lowest


In our study, Asian American men, regardless testosterone levels of all men in our subject
of their relationship status, reported a lower population, including Asian American men in
number of lifetime sexual partners and a lower relationships. It is very likely that the testoster-
number of lifetime one-night stands than Euro- one levels of single Asian American men are
pean American, African American, and His- linked to their sexuality in a meaningful way
panic American men. The Asian American men although our data do not demonstrate clear di-
in our study also reported that they had never rectional causality between testosterone and
had extrapair sex in their lifetime and a slightly sexual promiscuity. The small sample size for
lower frequency of extrapair sex fantasies when analyzing relationships between ethnicity, sex-
compared with the other men. These results are ual promiscuity and testosterone is one of the
consistent with those of previous studies show- limitations of this study. It is possible that a
ing that male Asian American college students stronger association between these variables
are more sexually conservative and less sexu- would have emerged if a larger dataset had been
ally experienced than men of other ethnic available (e.g., see Bogaert & Fisher, 1995).
groups (Meston et al., 1996; Okazaki, 2002). In Until some of our findings are replicated with a
these previous studies, differences in sexuality larger dataset, they should be interpreted with
between Asian American and non-Asian Amer- caution. The association between ethnicity, re-
ican students were attributed to cultural norms. lationship status, and testosterone, however, ap-
For example, Meston et al. (1996) reported that peared to be robust and was replicated in two
the more acculturated Asian-Canadians were to different datasets.
Canadian culture the less conservative their sex- It is highly unlikely that our reported ethnic
ual attitudes. Although it should not be assumed differences in the relationship between male
that there are no differences in sexual norms testosterone levels and relationship status reflect
among college students of European American, any ethnicity-related genetic differences in tes-
African American, and Hispanic American eth- tosterone or in sexuality (e.g., Rohrmann et al.,
nicity, previous studies that have specifically 2007; but see Ellis & Nyborg, 1992). Our ten-
compared these three ethnic groups with Asian tative explanation of our findings, instead, has
Americans have reported that differences in to do with the heterogeneity of the “singles”
sexual conservatism among European Ameri- category. As already mentioned in the begin-
can, African American, and Hispanic American ning of this article, in subject populations that
youths are relatively minor when compared mainly consist of college students, it is likely
with those between these three groups and that among single males there are both highly
Asian Americans (e.g., East, 1998; Feldman, sexually active individuals and individuals with
Turner, & Araujo, 1999). low levels of sexual activity. It is possible that
In our study none of our four measures of among male college students of Caucasian, His-
sexual promiscuity was significantly correlated panic, and African ethnicity, highly sexually
with testosterone levels across men of all eth- active individuals represent the majority of sin-
nicities but it is worth noting that Asian Amer- gle individuals and less sexually active singles
ican single men had the lowest scores on the are relatively rare. The high testosterone levels
number of lifetime sexual partners and the num- of many sexually active singles may be respon-
ber of one-night stands (data on extrapair sex sible for the higher testosterone levels of singles
106 MAESTRIPIERI, KLIMCZUK, TRAFICONTE, AND WILSON

compared with men in relationships. Con- ethnic groups tracks variation in sexual activity
versely, it is possible that less sexually active and sexual experience. It is likely that both
individuals may have been the majority of psychological and physiological aspects of sex-
Asian single men in our study. If less sexually ual activity (e.g., both thinking about sex and
active individuals have relatively low testoster- ejaculating) are directly involved in regulation
one, this may explain why single Asian men had of testosterone production. Other psychological
low testosterone and particularly lower testos- aspects of relationship status that are not strictly
terone than Asian men in relationships. Why related to sexuality, such as romantic commit-
there may be lower sexual activity among Asian ment to a partner, stress, or social support, are
single men than among other single men re- unlikely to account for the association between
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mains unclear: it may be the result of choice or relationship status and testosterone. Similarly,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

constraints, both of which may be due to cul- the possible cultural and experiential factors
tural or experiential factors. that affect ethnic variation in sexual conserva-
Consistent with the above explanation, in the tism or sexual activity are likely to be linked to
Caucasian, Hispanic, and African ethnic groups, variation in testosterone through their effects on
single men scored higher, on average, than men sexual activity, rather than directly or through
in relationships on measures of lifetime sexual other variables. Other relatively stable charac-
partners and lifetime number of one-night- teristics such as personality could also affect
stands. In these ethnic groups, married men and variation in testosterone through their effects on
unmarried men in long-term relationships sexual activity.
scored lower on these sexuality measures than As for the specific aspects of sexual behavior
single men and men in short-term relationships. that are most directly linked to testosterone, our
Variation in testosterone levels appeared to measures of lifetime sexual experience (number
track relationship length, as men in short-term
of partners and number of one-night-stands) as
relationships had the second-highest levels of
well as of extrapair sex were measures of sexual
testosterone after single men, followed by men
promiscuity rather than of overall frequency of
in long-term relationships, who in turn had
sexual activity. Thus, our data indicated that
higher testosterone than married men. Simi-
among Caucasian, Hispanic, and African men,
larly, among Asian men in relationships, un-
married men in long-term relationships scored single men, and to a lesser extent also men in
lower than men in short-term relationships in short-term relationships, were more sexually
measures of testosterone, number of lifetime promiscuous but did not necessarily engage in
sexual partners, and lifetime number of one- more frequent sexual activity than men in long-
night stands. Again, the small sample sizes for term relationships or married men. Differences
the analyses of testosterone levels and relation- in testosterone matched these differences in sex-
ship status in different ethnic groups are among ual promiscuity.
the metholodogical limitations of this study. Previous studies have shown that a man’s
Our findings, however, are consistent with those testosterone increases following exposure to a
of previous studies showing a negative effect of potential sexual partner and that this increase is
relationship length and marriage on male testos- proportional to the intensity of his courtship
terone levels (see beginning of the article for effort (Roney et al., 2003; 2007; van der Meij,
references). The lack of married men among Buunk, van de Sande, & Salvador, 2008). In
Asian men in relationships may have contrib- contrast, engaging in sexual interactions with
uted to the relatively high average testosterone the same partner in the context of a stable mo-
observed in this group of study participants and nogamous relationship does not seem to affect
to the magnitude of the difference in testoster- testosterone. Differences in testosterone be-
one between single men and men in relationship tween individuals or changes in a man’s testos-
in the 2013 dataset. terone levels over time provide a reliable indi-
Although the Asian and non-Asian ethnic cator of variation in mating effort, which in men
groups showed an opposite difference in testos- mainly consists of an effort to mate with an
terone between single men and men in relation- increasing number of partners. Measuring tes-
ships, several lines of evidence suggest that tosterone, therefore, is an indispensable tool for
variation in male testosterone levels across all understanding variation in male mating strate-
TESTOSTERONE AND RELATIONSHIP STATUS 107

gies as well as the life history trade-offs be- thers? Evidence from the Philippines. PLOS ONE,
tween mating and survival or parenting efforts. 7, e41559. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041559
Gray, P. B. (2003). Marriage, parenting, and testos-
terone variation among Kenyan Swahili men.
References American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 122,
279 –286. doi:10.1002/ajpa.10293
Berg, S. J., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2001). Gray, P. B., & Campbell, B. C. (2009). Human male
Changes in testosterone, cortisol, and estradiol lev- testosterone, pair-bonding, and fatherhood. In P. T.
els in men becoming fathers. Mayo Clinic Pro- Ellison & P. B. Gray (Eds.), Endocrinology of
ceedings, 76, 582–592. social relationships (pp. 270 –293). Cambridge,
Bogaert, A. F., & Fisher, W. A. (1995). Predictors of MA: Harvard University Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

university men’s number of sexual partners. Jour- Gray, P. B., Chapman, J. F., Burnham, T. C., McIn-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

nal of Sex Research, 32, 119 –130. doi:10.1080/ tyre, M. H., Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T. (2004).
00224499509551782 Human male pair bonding and testosterone. Hu-
Booth, A., & Dabbs, J. M., Jr. (1993). Testosterone man Nature, 15, 119 –131. doi:10.1007/s12110-
and men’s marriages. Social Forces, 72, 463– 477. 004-1016-6
Bribiescas, R. G. (2006). Men: Evolutionary and Hirschenhauser, K., Frigerio, D., Grammer, K., &
life-history. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Magnusson, M. S. (2002). Monthly patterns of
Press.
testosterone and behavior in prospective fathers.
Burnham, T. C., Chapman, J. F., Gray, P. B., McIn-
Hormones and Behavior, 42, 172–181. doi:
tyre, M. H., Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T. (2003).
10.1006/hbeh.2002.1815
Men in committed, romantic relationships have
lower testosterone. Hormones and Behavior, 44, Jasienska, G., Jasienski, M., & Ellison, P. T. (2012).
119 –122. doi:10.1016/S0018-506X(03)00125-9 Testosterone levels correlate with the number of
East, P. L. (1998). Racial and ethnic differences in children in human males, but the direction of the
girls’ sexual, marital, and birth expectations. Jour- relationship depends on paternal education. Evolu-
nal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 150 –162. tion and Human Behavior, 33, 665– 671. doi:
doi:10.2307/353448 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.05.001
Ellis, L., & Nyborg, H. (1992). Racial/ethnic varia- Kuzawa, C. W., Gettler, L. T., Muller, M. N., Mc-
tions in male testosterone levels: A probable con- Dade, T. W., & Feranil, A. B. (2009). Fatherhood,
tributor to group differences in health. Steroids, 57, pair-bonding and testosterone in the Philippines.
72–75. doi:10.1016/0039-128X(92)90032-5 Hormones and Behavior, 56, 429 – 435. doi:
Feldman, S. S., Turner, R. A., & Araujo, K. (1999). 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.07.010
Interpersonal context as an influence on sexual Maestripieri, D., Baran, N. M., Sapienza, P., & Zin-
timetables of youths: Gender and ethnic effects. gales, L. (2010). Between- and within-sex varia-
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 9, 25–52. tion in hormonal responses to psychological stress
doi:10.1207/s15327795jra0901_2 in a large sample of college students. Stress, 13,
Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Stallings, J., & Steiner, M. 413– 424. doi:10.3109/10253891003681137
(2002). Testosterone and prolactin are associated McIntyre, M., Gangestad, S. W., Gray, P. B., Chap-
with emotional responses to infant cries in new man, J. F., Burnham, T. C., O’Rourke, M. T., &
fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 42, 399 – 413. Thornhill, R. (2006). Romantic involvement often
doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1840 reduces men’s testosterone levels-but not always:
Gettler, L. T., McDade, T. W., Agustin, S. S., Feranil, The moderating role of extrapair sexual interest.
A. B., & Kuzawa, C. W. (2013). Do testosterone
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91,
declines during the transition to marriage and fa-
642– 651. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.642
therhood relate to men’s sexual behavior? Evi-
dence from the Philippines. Hormones and Behav- Meston, C. M., Trapnell, P. D., & Gorzalka, B. B.
ior, 64, 755–763. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.08 (1996). Ethnic and gender differences in sexuality:
.019 Variations in sexual behavior between Asian and
Gettler, L. T., McDade, T. W., Feranil, A. B., & non-Asian university students. Archives of Sexual
Kuzawa, C. W. (2011). Longitudinal evidence that Behavior, 25, 33–72. doi:10.1007/BF02437906
fatherhood decreases testosterone in human males. Muller, M. N., Marlowe, F. W., Bugumba, R., &
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Ellison, P. T. (2009). Testosterone and paternal
USA, 108, 16194 –16199. doi:10.1073/pnas care in East African foragers and pastoralists. Pro-
.1105403108 ceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 276,
Gettler, L. T., McKenna, J. J., McDade, T. W., Agus- 347–354. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1028
tin, S. S., & Kuzawa, C. W. (2012). Does cosleep- Nelson, R. J. (2011). An introduction to behavioral
ing contribute to lower testosterone levels in fa- endocrinology, 4th ed. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
108 MAESTRIPIERI, KLIMCZUK, TRAFICONTE, AND WILSON

Okazaki, S. (2002). Influences of culture on Asian van Anders, S. M., & Goldey, K. L. (2010). Testos-
Americans’ sexuality. Journal of Sex Research, terone and partnering are linked via relationship
39, 34 – 41. doi:10.1080/00224490209552117 status for women and “relationship orientation” for
Perini, T., Ditzen, B., Fischbacher, S., & Ehlert, U. men. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 820 – 826. doi:
(2012). Testosterone and relationship quality 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.08.005
across the transition to fatherhood. Biological Psy- van Anders, S. M., Hamilton, L. D., & Watson, N. V.
chology, 90, 186 –191. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho (2007). Multiple partners are associated with
.2012.03.004 higher testosterone in North American men and
Rohrmann, S., Nelson, W. G., Rifai, N., Brown, women. Hormones and Behavior, 51, 454 – 459.
T. R., Dobs, A., Kanarek, N., . . . Platz, E. A. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.002
(2007). Serum estrogen, but not testosterone levels van Anders, S. M., Tolman, R. M., & Volling, B. L.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

differ between black and white men in a nationally (2012). Baby cries and nurturance affect testoster-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

representative sample of Americans. Journal of one in men. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 31–36.
Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 92, doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.09.012
2519 –2525. doi:10.1210/jc.2007-0028 van Anders, S. M., & Watson, N. V. (2006). Rela-
Roney, J. R., Lukaszewski, A. W., & Simmons, Z. L. tionship status and testosterone in North American
(2007). Rapid endocrine responses of young men heterosexual and non-heterosexual men and wom-
to social interactions with young women. Hor- en: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data. Psycho-
mones and Behavior, 52, 326 –333. doi:10.1016/j neuroendocrinology, 31, 715–723. doi:10.1016/j
.yhbeh.2007.05.008 .psyneuen.2006.01.008
Roney, J. R., Mahler, S. V., & Maestripieri, D. van der Meij, L., Buunk, A. P., van de Sande, J. P.,
(2003). Behavioral and hormonal responses of & Salvador, A. (2008). The presence of a woman
men to brief interactions with women. Evolution increases testosterone in aggressive dominant men.
and Human Behavior, 24, 365–375. doi:10.1016/ Hormones and Behavior, 54, 640 – 644. doi:
S1090-5138(03)00053-9 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.07.001
Sapienza, P., Zingales, L., & Maestripieri, D. (2009). Wingfield, J. C., Hegner, R. E., Dufty, A. M., & Ball,
Gender differences in financial risk aversion and G. F. (1990). The “challenge hypothesis:” Theo-
career choices are affected by testosterone. Pro- retical implications for patterns of testosterone se-
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, cretion, mating systems, and breeding strategies.
USA, 106, 15268 –15273. doi:10.1073/pnas American Naturalist, 136, 829 – 846. doi:10.1086/
.0907352106 285134
Storey, A. E., Walsh, C. J., Quinton, R. L., & Wynne-
Edwards, K. E. (2000). Hormonal correlates of
paternal responsiveness in new and expectant fa- Received October 30, 2013
thers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 79 –95. Revision received December 18, 2013
doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00042-2 Accepted January 9, 2014 䡲

Anda mungkin juga menyukai