Anda di halaman 1dari 9

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Postpsychotic Posttraumatic Stress Disorder


Associations With Fear of Recurrence and Intolerance of Uncertainty
Ross G. White, PhD, DClinPsy, and Andrew I. Gumley, PhD

no significant differences between subjects with and without PP-


Abstract: Experiencing psychosis can be sufficiently distressing to precip-
PTSD on the severity of psychotic symptoms. Chisholm et al. (2006)
itate symptoms of postpsychotic posttraumatic stress disorder (PP-PTSD).
found that the content of persecutory delusions (e.g., the power of
The current research sought to investigate potential associations that PP-
the persecutor) correlated with the level of PP-PTSD. This raises the
PTSD had with the Fear of Recurrence Scale and the Intolerance of
possibility that the nature of positive symptoms may be an important
Uncertainty Scale. Twenty-seven individuals diagnosed with DSM-IV
factor mediating the link with levels of PP-PTSD. Three studies
Schizophrenia and adjudged to be distressed by their experience of psychosis
investigating links between PP-PTSD and negative symptoms
were recruited by referral to the study. The Clinician Administered PTSD
(McGorry et al., 1991; Meyer et al., 1999; Harrison & Fowler, 2004)
Scale was used to assess participants for PP-PTSD. Clinical rating scales
have failed to produce a consistent pattern of findings.
(PANSS, HADS, and IES-R) and measures assessing appraisals of paranoia
Research has investigated potential links between PP-PTSD
and hallucinatory voices (BAPS and IVI) were also employed. The preva-
and traumatic hospital admissions and psychiatric treatment. It has
lence rate of PP-PTSD in the sample was 37%. PP-PTSD caseness was
been suggested that compulsory admission procedures, enforced
associated with being fearful about psychosis recurring, being intolerant of
sedation, restraint, and seclusion serve to heighten individuals’ sense
uncertainty, and making negative appraisals of paranoia. Logistical regres-
of fear, victimization, and helplessness (Brody, 1995; Rooney et al.,
sion analyses indicated that fear of recurrence was a significant predictor of
1996; Beveridge, 1998). Contrary to what has been widely hypoth-
PP-PTSD caseness. The implications of these results for understanding how
esized, there does not appear to be a clear and consistent link
fear and worry processes might influence emotional adaptation following
between psychiatric admission and PP-PTSD. McGorry et al.
psychosis are discussed.
(1991), Priebe et al. (1998), Morrison et al. (1999), and Shaw et al.
Key Words: Psychosis, trauma, fear, PTSD, distress. (2002) failed to find significant associations between the level of
PP-PTSD and number of psychiatric admissions. Morrison et al.
(J Nerv Ment Dis 2009;197: 841– 849)
(1999) also failed to find significant associations between the level
of PP-PTSD and the duration of admissions or the time since last
admission. Contrary to what had been hypothesized, patients with a

A person’s experience of psychosis can involve the perception of


threatened death or serious injury and can induce feelings of
intense fear, helplessness or horror (Herring, 1995; Jordan, 1995).
history of compulsory admission had significantly lower levels of
PTSD symptoms (Morrison et al., 1999). Only 6% of the variance in
PP-PTSD scores was accounted for by hospital experience com-
Williams-Keeler et al. (1994) claimed that the experience of psy- pared with 52% of the variance accounted for by symptoms of
chosis can be sufficiently traumatic to precipitate a reaction similar psychosis. Similarly, Meyer et al. (1999) found that 24% of the
to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Consequently, the term traumatic events reported by participants in their study were related
“postpsychotic PTSD” (PP-PTSD) has been coined to account for to hospitalization, compared with 69% to psychosis.
distress resulting from traumatic events associated with the experi-
ence of psychosis (Shaw et al., 1997; 2002). FEAR OF RECURRENCE
Of the 10 studies that have investigated PP-PTSD (McGorry Research indicating that PP-PTSD is associated with apprais-
et al., 1991; Priebe et al., 1998; Meyer et al., 1999; Morrison et al., als that individuals make about symptoms and psychiatric admis-
1999; Shaw et al., 1997 and 2002; Kennedy et al., 2002; Jackson et sions (Chisholm et al., 2006; Shaw et al., 2002; Jackson et al., 2004)
al., 2004; Harrison and Fowler, 2004; Chisholm et al., 2006), the is consistent with Ehlers and Clark’s (2000) claims that idiosyncratic
mean prevalence rate for PP-PTSD was 41.9% (n ⫽ 425). There negative appraisals of a traumatic event and/or its sequelae are a key
appeared to be clear evidence for an association between PP-PTSD feature of PTSD. They propose that negative appraisals prompt
and levels of depression (Morrison et al., 1999; McGorry et al., dysfunctional cognitive and behavioral responses aimed at reducing
1991; Kennedy et al., 2002; Harrison and Fowler, 2004). This distress, which inadvertently prevent cognitive change and therefore
association between PP-PTSD and depression is consistent with maintain the disorder. Ehlers and Clark (2000) claimed that idio-
research indicating that PTSD is often comorbid with a secondary syncratic negative appraisals also have the common effect of creat-
diagnosis of depression (Kessler et al., 1995; Perkonigg et al., 2000). ing a sense of current threat. This sense of current threat is accom-
Some studies investigating PP-PTSD have demonstrated links be- panied by intrusions, arousal, and strong emotions e.g., anxiety,
tween positive symptoms and PP-PTSD (Meyer et al., 1999; Harri- anger, shame, or sadness. Gumley and MacBeth (2006) highlighted
son and Fowler, 2004). However, Shaw et al. (1997 and 2002) found that threat/danger has been an overlooked life event dimension in
relation to the appraisals made about psychosis. They proposed that
a trauma-based perspective may be helpful in understanding the
Department of Psychological Medicine, Gartnavel Royal Hospital, The University
of Glasgow, Glasgow, Lanarkshire, United Kingdom.
transition into relapse in psychosis. Individuals who have previously
Send reprint requests to Ross G. White, PhD, DClinPsy, Clinical Research experienced psychosis may perceive changes in thought content
Fellow, Section of Psychological Medicine, 1st Floor, Administrative (e.g. having a suspicious thought) or the nature of their thinking (e.g.
Building, Gartnavel Royal Hospital, 1055 Great Western Road, Glasgow thoughts becoming faster), as evidence of catastrophic loss of
G13 1BP, Scotland. E-mail: Ross.White@clinmed.gla.ac.uk.
Copyright © 2009 by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins
control. Normal cognitive processes therefore become contaminated
ISSN: 0022-3018/09/19711-0841 via associative memories of previous episodes of psychosis (Gumley
DOI: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181bea625 and MacBeth, 2006). The resulting sense of reliving traumatic event

The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 197, Number 11, November 2009 www.jonmd.com | 841
White and Gumley The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 197, Number 11, November 2009

leads to the activation of negative threat beliefs, emotional distress, going distress associated with memories of their experience of
and affect dysregulation. Gumley and MacBeth (2006) proposed that psychosis. Participants were required to meet DSM-IV criteria
the individuals’ attempts to regulate this pattern of cognitive per- (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) for Schizophrenia. Diag-
ceptual experiences and associated distress may lead to the acceler- noses were ascertained by case-note review and discussion with the
ation of relapse via maladaptive coping. consultant psychiatrist responsible for their care. Participants had to
Based on this model, Gumley and Schwannauer (2006) de- possess sufficient literacy skills to be able to read and answer the
veloped the Fear of Recurrence Scale to assess psychosis-related measures used in the study. Individuals would have been excluded
threat experienced by individuals with remitted psychosis. In sub- if they were acutely unwell (defined by a score of 5 or more on any
sequent research, (A.I. Gumley et al., unpublished data, 2009a) 1 item of the PANSS Positive Component). Participants would also
investigated two approaches to the detection of relapse in a random- have been excluded by the presence of a learning disability, a
ized controlled trial comparing “symptom monitoring” versus “fear diagnosis of PTSD precipitated by factors independent of their
of recurrence monitoring.” No differences were found between psychosis, a primary diagnosis associated with psycho-active sub-
symptom based monitoring (sensitivity ⫽ 79%, specificity ⫽ 35%) stance use, the presence of an organic disorder, or language diffi-
and monitoring fear of recurrence (sensitivity ⫽ 72%, specificity ⫽ culties that precluded assessment (e.g. non-English speakers). Due
46%). The authors concluded that cognitive factors (including to the exploratory nature of the research, it was not possible to
heightened awareness, fear of hospitalization, and intrusive thoughts conduct an a priori power calculation.
and memories of relapse) are likely to be involved in affect dys- Twenty-seven participants aged between 24 and 59 years
regulation and the re-emergence of psychosis. To date, no research (mean age: 38.93 years, SD ⫽ 10.33) were recruited to the research
has been conducted to determine if individuals with PP-PTSD have from 4 different community mental health teams in NHS Greater
elevated levels of fear of recurrence. Glasgow and Clyde. Five individuals identified as being potentially
appropriate for the study were considered too unwell by their
INTOLERANCE OF UNCERTAINTY key-worker and referrals were not completed. The Male:Female
In addition, there has been an absence of research seeking to ratio of the sample was 20:7. All participants were white. The mean
explore how worry processes might be involved in the phenome- score on the PANSS Positive Component was 12.3 (SD ⫽ 3.1),
nology of PP-PTSD. Worry has been defined as a repetitive thought PANSS Negative Component 11.7 (SD ⫽ 4.8), HADS Depression
activity, which is usually negative and frequently related to feared 7.4 (SD ⫽ 5.0), and HADS Anxiety mean score 10.6 (SD ⫽ 5.1).
future outcomes or events (Borkovec, 1994). Physical, social, and/or The mean number of admissions for the sample was 3.9 (SD ⫽ 4.4).
psychological threat has been highlighted as a central aspect of The mean number of months since last psychiatric admission was
worry (Wells, 1994). Research has indicated that the experience of 72.3 months (SD ⫽ 56.3).
psychosis can in turn involve physical, social and psychological
threat (Freeman and Garety, 2000).
Drawing on Wells’s (1994 and 1995) meta-cognitive model
Measures
of GAD, Freeman and Garety (1999) found that persecutory delu- The Clinician-Administered Posttraumatic Stress
sion becomes most upsetting when the individual has worries about Disorder Scale
not being able to control his/her thoughts about the belief. Parallels The Clinician-Administered Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
were drawn between mechanisms suggested to maintain worry, and Scale for use with patients with Schizophrenia (CAPS-S) (J. S.
processes potentially maintaining delusional distress (e.g. thought Gearon et al., unpublished data, 2001)is a structured interview that
suppression, hypervigilance, etc). measures the 17 symptoms of PTSD noted in DSM-IV (American
Intolerance of uncertainty has been suggested to play a key Psychiatric Association, 1994). The frequency and intensity of each
role in the acquisition and maintenance of worry (Dugas et al., symptom is rated. The test is a modified version of the CAPS (Blake
1998). Intolerance of uncertainty is a tendency to find uncertain et al., 1990). The language has been changed to an 8th grade reading
situations excessively stressful and upsetting, to believe that unex- level, additional behavioral definitions and anchors have been in-
pected events are negative and should be avoided, and to think that serted, and examples relevant to the life experiences of this popu-
being uncertain about the future is unfair (Dugas et al., 2005). lation have been provided. The CAPS-S has demonstrated sound
Individuals who are intolerant of uncertainty are at an increased levels of reliability and validity (Gearon et al., 2003; 2004).
likelihood of interpreting ambiguous information as threatening
(Heydayati et al., 2003). The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS)
The current study sought to investigate if PP-PTSD is asso- The PANSS is a 30-item observer rated scale used to assess
ciated with fear of recurrence, negative idiosyncratic appraisals of the presence and severity of positive (e.g., delusions, hallucinatory
psychotic experiences, and intolerance of uncertainty. It was hypoth- behavior) and negative (e.g. blunted affect, emotional withdrawal)
esized that, relative to clinical controls, patients with PP-PTSD symptoms (Kay et al., 1987). Derived scores include “positive,”
would: “negative,” and “global psychopathology” scale scores. Psychomet-
1. Be more fearful of their psychosis recurring. ric studies have reported good inter-rater reliability (e.g. correlation
2. Make more negative idiosyncratic appraisals of psychotic expe- coefficients around 0.80) and satisfactory internal consistency, con-
riences, e.g. hallucinatory voices and paranoia. struct validity, and concurrent validity in relation to other measures
3. Have greater intolerance of uncertainty. of psychopathology (Kay et al., 1988; 1989).

MATERIALS AND METHODS The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale


The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) (Zig-
Participants mond and Snaith, 1983) is a widely used self-report instrument
A cross-sectional between groups design was employed. Par- designed as a brief assessment tool of the distinct dimensions of
ticipants were consecutively recruited on a referral basis. Commu- anxiety and depression in nonpsychiatric populations (Hermann,
nity Mental Health Team (CMHT) staff were encouraged to make 1997). It is a 14-item questionnaire that consists of 2 subscales of 7
referrals if individuals they were working with were experiencing on items designed to measure levels of both anxiety and depression.

842 | www.jonmd.com © 2009 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 197, Number 11, November 2009 Fear and Uncertainty After Psychosis

Bjelland et al. (2002) noted that the psychometric properties of the be presenting with PP-PTSD. With the individual’s consent, a
HADS are such that it can be used with confidence clinically. referral form was completed. The researcher then arranged to meet
with the individual. The patients’ hospital notes were reviewed to
The Impact of Event Scale-Revised ensure that they met inclusion/exclusion criteria. After providing
The Impact of Event Scale-Revised (IES-R) (Weiss and written consent to take part, participants were first interviewed about
Marmar, 1997) is a 22-item scale, with items loading onto 3 factors: their illness history. This included being asked: “Is there an episode
intrusions, avoidance, and hyper-arousal, and which provides a of being unwell that particularly stands out in your memory?” As a
measure of symptomatology relating to a specific traumatic stressor. prompt, patients were asked: “Have you experienced any intrusions
The instrument has been shown to have strong internal consistency about times when you were unwell with psychosis?” Holmes et al.
and test–retest reliability (Weiss and Marmar, 1997). (2005) definition of intrusions was then read to each participant: “By
intrusions we mean memories that suddenly pop into your mind as
The Fear of Recurrence Scale if from nowhere. They may take you by surprise, and feel like the
The Fear of Recurrence Scale (FoRSe) (Gumley and Schwan- events are happening again NOW in the present. We do not mean
nauer, 2006) consists of 29 items generated from participants’ times when you deliberately choose to think about these things.
idiosyncratic early signs of psychosis. Analysis of the factor struc- Intrusions may take the form of visual pictures, sounds, smells, as
ture of the FoRSe revealed 3 different factors: Intrusiveness: e.g. “I well as verbal thoughts.”
have experienced thoughts intruding into my mind”; awareness: e.g. Using the participants’ self-generated worst moment of
“The world has seemed more vivid and colorful”; fear of recurrence: their illness as the precipitating event, the CAPS-S (Gearon et al.,
e.g. “The thought of becoming unwell has frightened me.” Gumley 2001) was completed with each participant. The recommended
and Schwannauer (2006) have demonstrated that the FoRSe has cut-off score of 45 or higher (Weathers et al., 1999) was used to
good internal consistency, test–retest reliability and strong positive classify PP-PTSD caseness. The PANSS, HADS, IES-R, FoRSe,
correlations with the Early Signs Scale (Birchwood et al., 1989). IUS, IVI, and BAPS were then completed with the patients. The
order of administration of assessments was rotated to control for
The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale order effects.
The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (IUS) (Freeston et al., The research project was granted ethical approval by NHS
1994) consists of 27 items assessing: uncertainty, emotional and Greater Glasgow and Clyde Research Ethics Committee in April
behavioral reactions to uncertain situations, implications of being 2006 and was registered under Greater Glasgow and Clyde Research
uncertain, and attempts to control the future. The IUS has been and Development Directorate in May 2006. Data were collected
found to demonstrate sound validity (Freeston et al., 1994) and between November 22, 2006 and April 16, 2007.
reliability (Dugas et al., 1997).
Analyses
The Interpretation of Voices Inventory The independent variable in the between-group analyses was
The Interpretation of Voices Inventory (IVI) (Morrison et al., PP-PTSD status (PP-PTSD vs. Clinical Controls). The dependent
2002) is a 26-item questionnaire measuring beliefs that people hold variables were fear of recurrence (FoRSe), intolerance of uncertainty
about hearing voices. There are 3 subscales measuring: metaphysical (IUS), and appraisals (BAPS, IVI). Due to the small sample size,
beliefs, positive beliefs, and beliefs about loss of control. Questions nonparametric Mann Whitney tests were used to test the between-
are worded hypothetically (“If I were to hear sounds or voices that group hypotheses. In accordance with Field (2005), effect sizes for
other people could not hear, I would probably think that . . .”). the between group comparisons were calculated using the following
Participants respond to each item by indicating on a Likert scale how equation:
much they agree (1 ⫽ not at all, 2 ⫽ somewhat, 3 ⫽ moderately so,
4 ⫽ very much). The inventory was found to demonstrate sound
reliability and validity (Morrison et al., 2002). Z
e.s. ⫽
The Beliefs About Paranoia Scale 冑n
The Beliefs About Paranoia Scale (BAPS-Short Form) (Mor-
where, e.s. ⫽ effect size, Z ⫽ Z-score, n ⫽ number of participants.
rison et al., 2005; 关A. I. Gumley et al., unpublished data, 2009b兴) is
a self-report measure to assess meta-cognitive beliefs about paranoia Cohen (1988; pp 477– 478) provides an explanation of how to
in nonpatients. It has 4 empirically distinct subscales: negative interpret effect sizes and suggests that effect sizes of the magnitude
beliefs about paranoia, beliefs about paranoia as a survival strategy, of 0.2 be regarded as weak, 0.4 be considered medium, and 0.6 be
general positive beliefs, and normalizing beliefs. The scales were considered to be strong.
shown to have acceptable internal consistency and were associated Nonparametric Spearmen ␳ correlation analyses were per-
with measures of paranoia, delusional ideation, and anxiety (Mor- formed to determine the association between the CAPS-S scores and
rison et al., 2005). the dependent variables. Logistic regression analyses were used to
determine if dependent variables could be used to predict PP-PTSD
Procedures caseness. Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) analysis was
The research aims were presented to local community mental used to determine specificity and sensitivity of measures for pre-
health teams. A vignette was used to help mental health practitioners dicting PP-PTSD caseness.
(i.e. psychiatrists, clinical psychologists and community psychiatric
nurses) understand how PP-PTSD presents clinically. Using Tarri-
er’s (2005) classification system of trauma in psychosis, only trau- RESULTS
matic events that were either “dependent” or “illness-related” were Some 37% (N ⫽ 10) of those referred for PP-PTSD assess-
considered as possible precipitating traumas for PP-PTSD. On the ment met criteria for PP-PTSD on the CAPS-S. Sembi et al. (1998)
basis of this information, mental health practitioners were asked to used an IES (Horowitz et al., 1979) total score of greater than 30 to
discuss the prospect of a referral with patients they believed might classify PTSD caseness. The IES-R differs from the IES in having

© 2009 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins www.jonmd.com | 843


White and Gumley The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 197, Number 11, November 2009

7 extra items that constitute a Hyper-arousal subscale. In the current there was a highly significant overlap between the two methods of
study, IES scores were calculated by summing the IES-R Avoidance PP-PTSD classification.
and IES-R Intrusiveness subscores. Using the Sembi et al. (1998) Table 1 summarizes between-group comparisons on demo-
criteria, 33% (N ⫽ 9) of those referred for PP-PTSD assessment met graphic information and psychiatric ratings. Relative to clinical
criteria for PP-PTSD. controls, participants who met caseness for PP-PTSD had signifi-
Cramer’s V analysis of sameness was used to statistically test cantly higher scores on the PANSS Negative Component and
the degree of overlap between the 2 forms of classification. The PANSS total score (all p ⬍ 0.01). Similarly, participants who met
result of the analysis (Cramer’s V ⫽ 0.759, p ⬍ 0.001) indicated that caseness PP-PTSD scored significantly higher on HADS depression,

TABLE 1. Demographic Information and Psychiatric Ratings for Participants


Group Comparison
Non-PTSD (N ⴝ
Variable 17) PTSD (N ⴝ 10) ␹2 df p
Gender
Male N (%) 12 (70.6%) 8 (80.0%) 0.29 1 0.59
Female N (%) 5 (29.4%) 2 (20.0%)
Mann Whitney Z Effect Sizes

Age
Mean (SD) 38.5 (10.7) 39.6 (10.3) 79.50 ⫺0.28 ⫺0.1
Median (IQR) 38.0 (29.5–47.0) 37.0 (32.0–46.3)
No. months since last discharge
Mean (SD) 83.7 (57.6) 55.1 (52.4) 48.50 ⫺1.47 ⫺0.3
Median (IQR) 67.0 (48.0–104.0) 36.0 (13.3–93.5)
No. psychiatric admissions
Mean (SD) 3.0 (4.5) 5.2 (3.9) 48.50 ⫺1.86 ⫺0.4
Median (IQR) 1.0 (0.0–5.5) 5.5 (1.5–10.0)
PANSS positive component
Mean (SD) 11.4 (2.5) 13.8 (3.7) 53.50 ⫺1.60 ⫺0.3
Median (IQR) 11.0 (10.0–13.5) 14.0 (10.0–18.0)
PANSS negative component
Mean (SD) 10.0 (3.7) 14.7 (5.3) 32.00 ⫺2.69* ⫺0.5
Median (IQR) 9.0 (8.0–11.0) 13.0 (10.5–18.5)
PANSS total score
Mean (SD) 48.0 (6.3) 62.3 (13.4) 30.00 ⫺2.77* ⫺0.5
Median (IQR) 46.0 (43.5–53.0) 63.0 (50.5–70.8)
HADS depression
Mean (SD) 5.8 (4.7) 10.1 (4.7) 43.00 ⫺2.12* ⫺0.4
Median (IQR) 4.0 (2.0–10.5) 9.5 (6.5–12.0)
HADS anxiety
Mean (SD) 8.9 (4.8) 13.5 (4.4) 39.00 ⫺2.33* ⫺0.5
Median (IQR) 9.0 (4.0–12.0) 13.0 (9.8–18.0)
HADS total
Mean (SD) 14.7 (8.7) 23.6 (7.4) 34.00 ⫺2.57* ⫺0.5
Median (IQR) 15.0 (7.0–20.0) 23.0 (17.3–26.5)
IES-R: avoidance
Mean (SD) 9.8 (7.3) 19.4 (7.2) 31.50 ⫺2.69** ⫺0.5
Median (IQR) 10.0 (3.5–17.0) 19.0 (13.5–25.5)
IES-R: intrusiveness
Mean (SD) 4.2 (5.6) 19.0 (7.53) 9.00 ⫺3.83*** ⫺0.7
Median (IQR) 2.0 (0.0–7.0) 18.5 (14.0–24.3)
IES-R: hyperarousal
Mean (SD) 3.2 (3.1) 13.6 (5.3) 5.50 ⫺4.01*** ⫺0.8
Median (IQR) 3.0 (.5–6.0) 12.5 (10.0–17.0)
IES-R: total
Mean (SD) 17.2 (11.6) 52.0 (17.5) 6.50 ⫺3.95** ⫺0.8
Median (IQR) 17.0 (5.0–24.5) 52.5 (40.0–61.0)
*p ⬍ 0.05; **p ⬍ 0.01; *** p ⬍ 0.001.

844 | www.jonmd.com © 2009 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 197, Number 11, November 2009 Fear and Uncertainty After Psychosis

HADS anxiety (both p ⬍ 0.05), and HADS total score (p ⫽ 0.01). positive correlation between the CAPS-S total score and the PANSS
There were significant differences on the IES-R avoidance (p ⬍ Positive Component.
0.01), IES-R intrusiveness (p ⬍ 0.001), IES-R hyper-arousal The subscales of the CAPS-S had a variety of other signifi-
subscales, and the IES-R total scores (all p ⬍ 0.001). There were cant correlations with the dependent variables (Table 3). Of partic-
no significant differences regarding gender, age, scores on the ular note were the significant correlations that CAPS-S avoidance
PANSS Positive Component, number of psychiatric admissions had with the IUS, each of the PANSS indices, each of the HADS
and the number of months that had passed since the last psychi- indices, each of the IES-R indices, and each of the FoRSe indices
atric discharge. (apart from FoRSe Awareness). CAPS-S re-experiencing had sig-
Table 2 provides information on the differences between nificant positive correlations with the IUS, HADS anxiety, and the
those who met PP-PTSD caseness and clinical controls on the HADS total score, and each of the IES-R indices. CAPS-S re-
FoRSe, IVI, BAPS, and the IUS. Participants who met criteria for experiencing also had significant correlations with: the PANSS
PP-PTSD scored significantly higher on the FoRSe fear of relapse Negative component, PANSS Total score, and each of the Fear of
subscale, FoRSe intrusiveness subscales and FoRSe total score (all Recurrence scores (apart from FoRSe awareness). CAPS-S hyper-
p ⬍ 0.01). Participants with PP-PTSD had significantly higher arousal had significant positive correlations with the IUS, the
scores on the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale and the BAPS PANSS total score, PANSS Positive Component, IES-R intrusive-
Negative Beliefs subscale (both p ⬍ 0.05). There was no significant ness, IES-R hyper-arousal, and all of the HADS and IES-R indices.
difference between the groups in scores on the FoRSe awareness
subscale, IVI subscale/total scores, BAPS survival strategy subscale, Post Hoc Analysis
BAPS normalizing beliefs subscale, or the BAPS total score. Logistic regression analyses were used to create a model
The correlational analyses listed in Table 3 largely mirror the aimed at predicting PP-PTSD caseness. Indices representing each of
pattern of associations observed in the between-group comparisons. the 3 cognitive variables significantly linked to PP-PTSD in the
However, although there was no significant between-group differ- univariate analyses were included in the logistic regression analyses
ence on the PANSS Positive Component, there was a significant (i.e. the Beliefs about Paranoia Scale: negative beliefs, the FoRSe,

TABLE 2. Between-Group Comparisons for the FoRSe, IVI, BAPS, and the IUS
Variable Descriptive Statistic Non-PTSD (N ⴝ 17) PTSD (N ⴝ 10) Mann Whitney Z Effect Size
Fear of recurrence: fear of relapse Mean (SD) 12.59 (4.29) 20.70 (6.04) 24.50 ⫺3.05** ⫺0.6
Median (IQR) 12.00 (9.50–16.00) 21.50 (15.25–25.75)
Fear of recurrence: awareness Mean (SD) 21.18 (7.23) 25.90 (6.81) 55.50 ⫺1.49 ⫺0.3
Median (IQR) 20.00 (16.50–29.00) 26.00 (920.00–31.25)
Fear of recurrence: intrusiveness Mean (SD) 12.94 (5.41) 20.70 (5.77) 23.00 ⫺3.13** ⫺0.6
Median (IQR) 10.00 (9.00–16.50) 21.00 (16.00–25.50)
Fear of recurrence: total Mean (SD) 46.71 (9.01) 67.30 (15.08) 20.00 ⫺3.27** ⫺0.6
Median (IQR) 47.00 (40.00–55.00) 71.00 (55.25–75.40)
Interpretation of voices inventory: Mean (SD) 21.24 (6.32) 27.00 (10.14) 56.50 ⫺1.44 ⫺0.3
metaphysical
Median (IQR) 19.00 (15.50–25.00) 26.00 (18.25–35.00)
Interpretation of voices inventory: Mean (SD) 11.76 (4.21) 11.10 (3.41) 79.00 ⫺0.31 ⫺0.1
positive
Median (IQR) 11.00 (8.00–15.50) 10.50 (8.00–14.00)
Interpretation of voices inventory: Mean (SD) 10.00 (2.94) 12.80 (4.92) 55.50 ⫺1.49 ⫺0.3
loss of control
Median (IQR) 9.00 (7.50–12.50) 12.00 (9.25–17.00)
Interpretation of voices inventory: Mean (SD) 43.00 (11.40) 50.90 (15.55) 58.00 ⫺1.36 ⫺0.3
total
Median (IQR) 42.00 (32.00–51.50) 50.50 (38.00–62.00)
Beliefs about paranoia: survival Mean (SD) 9.29 (4.21) 9.70 (3.68) 75.50 ⫺0.48 ⫺0.1
strategy
Median (IQR) 8.00 (6.00–10.00) 8.50 (6.75–12.00)
Beliefs about paranoia: negative Mean (SD) 14.53 (4.39) 17.90 (5.26) 46.00 ⫺1.97* ⫺0.4
beliefs
Median (IQR) 16.00 (10.00–18.00) 20.50 (14.50–22.00)
Beliefs about paranoia: normalizing Mean (SD) 11.88 (3.31) 13.60 (3.44) 54.00 ⫺1.57 ⫺0.3
beliefs
Median (IQR) 11.00 (10.00–13.00) 13.00 (11.00–16.50)
Beliefs about paranoia: total Mean (SD) 35.71 (8.32) 40.20 (8.44) 59.50 ⫺1.28 ⫺0.3
Median (IQR) 34.00 (31.00–40.00) 39.50 (33.50–48.75)
Intolerance of uncertainty Mean (SD) 64.94 (21.52) 89.50 (28.75) 40.00 ⫺2.26** ⫺0.4
Median (IQR) 60.00 (50.00–78.00) 85.50 (64.75–120.00)
*p ⬍ 0.05; **p ⬍ 0.01.

© 2009 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins www.jonmd.com | 845


White and Gumley The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 197, Number 11, November 2009

TABLE 3. Correlations Between the CAPS-S and the PANSS, HADS, FoRSE, IES-R, IVI, BAPS,
and IUS
CAPS-S CAPS-S CAPS-S CAPS-S
Total Re-experiencing Avoidance Hyper-arousal
PANSS
PANSS positive component ␳ 0.42* 0.27 0.42* 0.41*
PANSS negative component ␳ 0.54** 0.42* 0.58** 0.26
PANSS total score ␳ 0.66** 0.50** 0.67** 0.48*
HADS
HADS depression ␳ 0.65** 0.39 0.62** 0.56**
HADS anxiety ␳ 0.61** 0.46* 0.52** 0.65**
HADS total ␳ 0.68** 0.43* 0.62** 0.64**
FoRSe
Fear of recurrence: fear of relapse ␳ 0.61** 0.49* 0.74*** 0.38
Fear of recurrence: awareness ␳ ⫺0.02 0.07 ⫺0.09 0.08
Fear of recurrence: intrusiveness ␳ 0.60** 0.54** 0.62** 0.34
Fear of recurrence: total ␳ 0.48* 0.42* 0.53** 0.33
IVI
Interpretation of voices inventory: metaphysical ␳ 0.24 0.32 0.26 0.08
Interpretation of voices inventory: positive ␳ ⫺0.16 0.08 ⫺0.20 0.00
Interpretation of voices inventory: loss of control ␳ 0.19 0.19 0.24 0.02
Interpretation of voices inventory: total ␳ 0.17 0.28 0.20 0.09
BAPS
Beliefs about paranoia: survival strategy ␳ 0.09 0.15 0.00 0.35
Beliefs about paranoia: negative beliefs ␳ 0.36 0.45* 0.20 0.20
Beliefs about paranoia: normalizing beliefs ␳ 0.20 0.16 0.16 0.37
Beliefs about paranoia: total ␳ 0.26 0.32 0.05 0.37
IUS
Intolerance of uncertainty ␳ 0.46* 0.44* 0.40* 0.30
*p ⬍ 0.05; **p ⬍ 0.01; ***p ⬍ 0.001.

ROC Analysis
TABLE 4. Results of the Logistic Regression Analyses
Predicting PP-PTSD Caseness ROC analysis was used to measure the predictive utility of the
FoRSe Total score to distinguish between participants meeting
Predictor ␤ SE ␤ Wald/␹2 Odds Ratio/e␤ p PP-PTSD caseness and those who did not. The rate of true-positive
Beliefs about Paranoia 0.223 0.154 2.096 1.250 0.199 predictions at different risk levels (the sensitivity) was plotted
scale: negative against the rate of false-positive predictions (1–specificity) to con-
beliefs struct a ROC curve. An area of 1.0 under the ROC curve represents
FoRSe: total score 0.193 0.086 5.463 1.213 0.019* a perfect model, and an area of 0.5, which is below the diagonal line,
Intolerance of ⫺0.014 0.029 0.229 0.986 0.632 represents a prediction made by chance (Fig. 1). The area under the
uncertainty scale curve (0.882 ⫾ 0.079, 95% IC: 0.73–1.04) was highly significant
(p ⫽ 0.001).
*p ⬍ 0.05.
The results of the ROC analysis indicated that the optimal
cut-off on the FoRSe for identifying PP-PTSD was a score ⬎56. The
sensitivity for this score was 80%, whereas the specificity was
and the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale). Because 2 of the 3 FoRSe 82.4%.
subscales were significantly associated with PP-PTSD caseness, the
FoRSe total score was entered into the logistic regression along with
the BAPS negative beliefs subscale and the Intolerance of Uncer- DISCUSSION
tainty Scale. To verify that this yielded the best model, 3 separate The current study aimed to investigate whether PP-PTSD was
logistic regression analyses were run using a different FoRSe index associated with individuals being more fearful of psychosis recur-
on each occasion i.e. FoRSe Total score, FoRSe intrusiveness score, ring. The research also sought to determine how a particular aspect
and then foRSe fear of relapse score. These analyses confirmed that of worry (intolerance of uncertainty) might be linked to PP-PTSD.
entering the FoRSe total score into the model produced the optimal The authors believe that this work has important implications for
␤ and Wald/␹2 values. Only the results for this particular model are understanding how the experience of psychosis can continue to
reported (Table 4). The backward stepwise selection method was impact on individual’s quality of life even when the symptoms of
used that involves the least significant variables being eliminated psychosis have remitted.
one by one on the basis of maximum likelihood. The analyses The PP-PTSD group recruited to the study had a mean time of
indicated that only the FoRSe total score contributed significantly to 55 months since last psychiatric discharge. This suggests that dis-
a model predicting PP-PTSD caseness. tress associated with psychosis is potentially enduring and sustained.

846 | www.jonmd.com © 2009 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 197, Number 11, November 2009 Fear and Uncertainty After Psychosis

during the episode and the perceived presence of crisis support after
the episode. However, the current study suggests that the FoRSe
represents a quick and easy to administer clinical tool for identifying
those distressed by the experience of psychosis. Unlike the
IES-R, the FoRSe was developed specifically for use in patients
diagnosed with psychosis and does require the individual to
identify a specific stressful life event for the measure to be
completed.
The current research is the first to investigate how worry
processes might be involved in Postpsychotic PTSD. Intolerance of
uncertainty has been shown to have strong links with Generalized
Anxiety Disorder (GAD), Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD)
and Panic Disorder (Dugas et al., 1998; Dugas et al., 2001; Tolin et
al., 2003). In the current research, individuals with PP-PTSD were
shown to be significantly more intolerant of uncertainty than clinical
controls. Individuals who were more intolerant of uncertainty were
also more fearful about psychosis recurring. This raises the possi-
bility that worry processes may also be helpful for understanding
emotional distress that can follow a remission from psychosis.
Although the reliving of previous traumatic events may create a
sense of current threat, it is possible that being intolerant of the
uncertainty that accompanies distinguishing “normal” cognitive pro-
cesses from psychotic phenomena may also contribute to this sense
of threat.
The significant differences between individuals meeting PP-
PTSD caseness and clinical controls on the Fear of Recurrence Scale
and the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale support Hypotheses 1 and
FIGURE 1. The ROC Curve for the Fear of Recurrence Scale 2. Associations between psychosis-related trauma, fear of recurrence
and PP-PTSD caseness. and intolerance of uncertainty require further investigation. Longi-
tudinal research is required to determine how the association be-
PP-PTSD caseness was significantly associated with elevated levels tween these variables might change over time.
of negative symptoms, depression and anxiety. However, there were Ehlers and Clark (2000) proposed that PTSD would be
no significant differences in positive symptoms between participants associated with negative appraisals of trauma-related phenomenon.
with PP-PTSD and controls. This is consistent with previous re- Consistent with this prediction, participants with PP-PTSD, made
search (Shaw et al., 1997; 2002) and supports the notion that more negative appraisals about the paranoid thoughts than did
PP-PTSD is not an artefact of differences between the groups in the clinical controls. This, however, was not the case for hallucinatory
levels of positive symptoms. voices. Consequently, Hypothesis 3 is only partially supported. The
Results on the subscales of the Fear of Recurrence Scale absence of a more clear-cut association between negative idiosyn-
indicated that, relative to clinical controls, the PP-PTSD group had cratic appraisals of symptoms of psychosis and PP-PTSD may be
significantly greater fear of relapse and more intrusive thoughts due to the fact that the IVI and BAPS were administered to
about their illness recurring. However, there were no significant participants irrespective of whether they had actually experienced
differences between the groups on the Awareness subscale of the hallucinatory voices or elevated levels of paranoia. These measures
FoRSe. It seems therefore that it is not simply about being aware of were therefore assessing hypothetical appraisals. In hindsight, using
changes in thought processes, but rather the appraisals that accom- measures aimed at assessing more general beliefs about the experi-
pany changes in thought processes that may be particularly impor- ence of psychosis might have been more appropriate, e.g. the
tant in PP-PTSD. There was a particularly strong association be- Personal Beliefs about Illness Questionnaire (Birchwood et al.,
tween the CAPS-S avoidance subscale and the FoRSe fear of relapse 1993).
subscale. This might suggest that fears about relapse may lead Alternative interpretations cannot be ruled out as to why the
individuals to avoid contact with stimuli they associate with trau- PP-PTSD groups were more fearful of recurrence, more intolerant of
matic aspects of the experience of psychosis. Equally, however, uncertainty, and more negative in their appraisals of paranoia. It
avoidance of trauma-related stimuli may potentially maintain fearful could be argued that the links between PP-PTSD status and these
responses by preventing processing and elaboration of intrusive variables were contaminated by significant differences between the
memories (Ehlers and Clark, 2000). groups in negative, depressive and anxiety symptoms. Future re-
By demonstrating for the first time that individuals with search may benefit from matching patients with and without PP-
PP-PTSD are more fearful of psychosis recurring than clinical PTSD on measures of these symptoms. However, it is important to
controls, the current study builds on previous research demonstrat- consider the possibility that increased levels of negative symptoms,
ing the utility of trauma theory for understanding emotional distress depression and anxiety observed in the PP-PTSD group in the
following psychosis (Shaw et al., 1997; 2002). Post hoc analyses current research may actually be a consequence of the same pathol-
revealed that a cut-off score of over 56 on the Fear of Recurrence ogy that drives the PP-PTSD (Stampfer, 1990). Previous researchers
Scale (FoRSe) demonstrated good sensitivity and specificity for have noted that the avoidance and detachment characteristic of
identifying PP-PTSD. Chisholm et al., (2006) previously identified PTSD may be confused with negative symptoms (Shaner and Eth,
a number of candidate psychological variables for identifying PP- 1989; Lundy, 1992). Meyer et al. (1999) claim that associations
PTSD including perceived helplessness, uncontrollability at the time between symptoms of PTSD and anxiety/depression are to be
of the index psychotic episode, content of persecutory delusions expected due to symptom overlap.

© 2009 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins www.jonmd.com | 847


White and Gumley The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 197, Number 11, November 2009

The current study has a number of strengths. Two measures tion of an early signs monitoring system using patients and families as
were used to assess PP-PTSD caseness: one self-report measure observers. Psychol Med. 19:649 – 656.
(IES-R) and an interview-based measure (CAPS-S). The substantial Birchwood M, Mason R, MacMillan F, Healy J (1993) Depression, demoralisation
and control over illness: a comparison of depressed and non-depressed patients
overlap between the measures provided good support for the reliable with a chronic psychosis. Psychol Med. 23:387–395.
identification of individuals meeting PP-PTSD. The fact that none of Blake D, Weathers F, Nagy L, Kaloupek D, Klauminzer G, Chamey D, Keane T
the participants had a previous diagnosis of PTSD (independent of (1990). Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS). Form 1: Current and
the experience of psychosis) maximized the likelihood that distress lifetime Diagnosis Version. West Haven, Conn, National Centre for Post-
experienced by the PP-PTSD group was due to trauma associated Traumatic Stress Disorder.
with psychosis that they described. Although the sample size was Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D (2002) The validity of the Hospital
comparatively small, the relatively large effect sizes obtained in the Anxiety and Depression Scale: An updated literature review. J Psychosom Res.
52:69 –77.
research suggest that the study was sufficiently powered. Recruit-
Borkovec TD (1994) The nature, functions and origins of worry. In GCL Davey,
ment to the study was inherently difficult because we sought to F Tallis, (Eds) Worrying: Perspectives on Theory, Assessment and Treatment
recruit individuals who have been traumatized by their experience of (pp 5-33). London: Wiley.
psychosis and these individuals may have been reluctant to meet Brody EB (1995) Editorial: The humanity of psychotic persons and their rights.
with mental health professionals to discuss their experiences. J Nerv Ment Dis. 183:193–194.
There were several limitations associated with the current Chisholm B, Freeman D, Cooke A (2006) Identifying potential predictors of
study. The cross-sectional nature of the study limits understanding traumatic reactions to psychotic episodes. Br J Clin Psychol. 45:545–559.
of potential longitudinal links between PP-PTSD and the dependent Cohen J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed).
variables. Future research should aim to prospectively follow par- Hillsdale (NJ): Erlbaum.
ticipants to help gain an understanding of how PP-PTSD impacts on Dugas MJ, Freeston MH, Ladouceur R (1997) Intolerance of uncertainty and
prognosis in psychosis. The absence of a diagnostic interview to problem orientation in worry. Cognit Ther Res. 21:593– 606.
confirm case-file diagnoses of schizophrenia is a weakness. The case Dugas MJ, Gosselin P, Ladouceur R (2001) Intolerance of uncertainty and worry:
Investigating specificity in a nonclinical sample. Cognit Ther Res. 25:551–558.
file review combined with discussions with the consultant psychia-
trist responsible for the participants’ care offered only a degree of Dugas MJ, Gagnon F, Ladouceur R, Freeston MH (1998) Generalized anxiety
disorder: A preliminary test of a conceptual model. Behav Res Ther. 36:215–226.
certainty regarding diagnosis. The use of a recognized screening tool
Dugas MJ, Hedayati M Buhr K, Phillips NA (2005) Intolerance of uncertainty and
for traumatic life events (e.g. the Stressful Life Experiences Screen; information processing: Evidence of biased recall and interpretations. Cognit
Stamm et al., 1996) may have provided a more standardized assess- Ther Res. 29:57–70.
ment of traumatic events independent of psychosis. Ehlers A, Clark DM (2000) A cognitive model of post-traumatic stress disorder.
Behav Res Ther. 38:319 –345.
CONCLUSIONS Field A (2005) Discovering Statistics Using SPSS (2nd ed). London: Sage.
Improving our understanding about the psychological pro- Freeman D, Garety PA, (1999) Worry, worry processes and dimensions of
delusions: an exploratory investigation of a role for anxiety processis in the
cesses potentially underpinning postpsychotic PTSD is desirable maintenance of delusional distress. Behav & Cognit Psych. 27:47– 62.
because it will facilitate the development of psychological interven-
Freeman D, Garety PA, (2000) Comments on the content of persecutory delu-
tions aimed at enhancing emotional recovery following psychosis. sions: does the definition need clarification? British Journal of Clinincal
Gumley and Schwannauer (2006) have highlighted the importance Psychology. 39:407– 414.
of narrative in working therapeutically with traumatic reactions to Freeston MH, Rheaume J, Letarte L, Dugas MJ, Ladouceur R (1994) Why do
psychotic experiences. Particular emphasis is placed on fragmenta- people worry? Personality and Individual Differences. 17:791– 802.
tion and disconnections in the individual’s discourse. The therapist Gearon JS, Kaltman SI, Brown C, Bellack AS (2003) Traumatic Life events and
helps the individual to develop a new narrative that elaborates on PTSD among women with substance use disorders and schizophrenia. Psychi-
atr Serv. 54:523–528.
and contextualizes their experiences. Particular attention is paid to
types of appraisal indicating loss, entrapment, and shameful humil- Gearon JS, Bellack AS, Tenhula WN (2004) Preliminary reliability and validity of
the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for Schizophrenia. J Consult Clin
iation. The current research has highlighted that postpsychotic PTSD Psychol. 72:121–125.
is associated with: making negative appraisals about paranoid Gumley A, MacBeth A (2006) Relationship between trauma and psychosis: From
thoughts, being intolerant of uncertainty, and being fearful that theory to therapy. In W Larkin, P Morrison (Eds), Trauma and Psychosis: New
changes in thinking might be indicative of psychosis recurring. Directions for Theory and Therapy. London and New York: Routledge.
These different factors are linked in the sense that each requires the Gumley AI, Schwannauer M (2006) Staying Well After Psychosis: A Cognitive
individual to think about thinking i.e. they are all meta-cognitive in Interpersonal Treatment Manual for Relapse Prevention. Chichester (WS):
nature. We suggest that problematic meta-cognitive beliefs may John Wiley and Sons.
represent a source of intrapsychic threat that serves to maintain Gearon JS, Kaltman SI, Brown C, Bellack AS (2003) Traumatic Life events and
PTSD among women with substance use disorders and schizophrenia. Psychi-
postpsychotic PTSD. Interventions aimed at addressing problematic atr Serv. 54:523–528.
meta-cognitive beliefs may be helpful in reducing the extent to Harrison CL, Fowler D (2004) Negative symptoms, trauma and autobiographical
which individuals feel threatened by their thinking. We propose that memory: An investigation of individuals recovering from psychosis. J Nerv
interventions of this type will be effective in treating postpsychotic Ment Dis. 192:745–753.
PTSD. These claims are, however, speculative and further research Hermann C (1997) International experiences with the Hospital Anxiety and
investigating psycho-therapeutic treatments for postpsychotic PTSD Depression Scale: a review of validation data and clinical results. Journal of
is required. Psychosomatic Research. 48:579 –584.
Heydayati M, Dugas MJ, Buhr K, Francis K (2003). The relationship between
intolerance of uncertainty and the interpretation of ambiguous and unambigu-
REFERENCES ous information. Poster presented at: The Annual Convention of the Associa-
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of tion for Advancement of Behaviour Therapy; Boston (MA).
Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) (4th ed). Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Holmes EA, Grey N, Young KAD (2005) Intrusive images and “hotspots” of
Association. trauma memories in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: an exploratory investi-
Beveridge A (1998) Psychology of compulsory detention. Psychiatr Bull. 22:115– gation of emotions and cognitive themes. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry.
117. 36:3–17.
Birchwood M, Smith J, Macmillan F, Hogg B, Prasad R, Harvey C, Bering S Horowitz M, Wilner M, Alvarez W (1979) Impact of Event Scale: A measure of
(1989) Predicting relapse in schizophrenia: The development and implementa- subjective stress. Psychosom Med. 41:209 –218.

848 | www.jonmd.com © 2009 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 197, Number 11, November 2009 Fear and Uncertainty After Psychosis

Jackson C, Knott C, Skeate A, Birchwood M (2004) The trauma of first episode 13:132–137.
psychosis: The role of cognitive mediation. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 38:327–333. Sembi S, Tarrier N, O’Neill P, Burns A, Faragher B (1998) Does post-traumatic
Jordan JC (1995) First person account: Schizophrenia-adrift in an anchorless stress disorder occur after stroke: A preliminary study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry.
reality. Schizophr Bull. 21:501–503. 13:315–322.
Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA (1987) The positive and negative syndrome scale Shaner A, Eth S (1989) Can schizophrenia cause posttraumatic stress disorder.
(PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 13:261–276. American Journal of Psychotherapy. 4:588 –597.
Kay SR, Opler LA, Lindenmayer JP (1988) Reliability and validity of the Positive Shaw K, McFarlane A, Bookless C (1997) The phenomenology of traumatic
and Negative Syndrome Scale for schizophrenics. Psychiatry Res. 23:99 –110. reactions to psychotic illness. J Nerv Ment Dis. 185:434 – 441.
Kay SR, Opler LA, Lindenmayer JP (1989) The positive and negative syndrome Shaw K, McFarlane A, Bookless C, Air T (2002) The aetiology of postpsychotic
scale (PANSS): Rationale and standardization. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 8:59 – 67. posttraumatic stress disorder following a psychotic episode. J Trauma Stress.
Kennedy BL, Dhaliwal N, Pedley L, Sahner C, Greenberg R, Manshadi MS 15:39 – 47.
(2002) Post-traumatic stress disorder in subjects with schizophrenia and bipolar Stamm BH, Rudolph JM, Dewane S, Gaines N, Gorton K, Paul G, McNeil F,
disorder. J Ky Med Assoc. 100:395–399. Bowen G, Ercolano M (1996) . Psychometric review of stressful life experi-
Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB (1995) Posttraumatic ences screening. In BH Stamm (Ed), Measurement of Stress, Trauma and
stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry. Adaptation. Lutherville (MD): Sidran Press.
52:1048 –1060. Stampfer HG (1990) ‘Negative symptoms’: A cumulative trauma stress disorder?
Lundy MS (1992) Psychosis-induced posttraumatic stress disorder. American Australian and New Zealand Journla of Psychiatry. 24:516 –528.
Journal of Psychotherapy. XLVI:485– 491. Tarrier N (2005) Comorbidity and associated clinical problems in schizophrenia:
McGorry P, Chanen A, McCarthy E, van Riel R, McKenzie D, Singh B (1991) Their nature and implications for comprehensive cognitive behavioural treat-
ment. Behav Change. 22:125–142.
Post-traumatic stress disorder following recent onset psychosis: An unrecog-
nized post psychotic syndrome. J Nerv Ment Dis. 179:253–258. Tollin DF, Abramowitz JS, Brigidi BD, Foa EB (2003) Intolerance of uncertainty
in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 17:233–242.
Meyer H, Taiminen T, Vuori T, Aeijaelae A, Helenius H (1999) Posttraumatic
stress disorder symptoms related to psychosis and acute involuntary hospital- Weathers FW, Ruscio AM, Keane TM (1999) Psychometric properties of nine
isation in schizophrenic and delusional patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 187:343–352. scoring rules for the Clinician Administered Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
Scale. Psychological Assessment. 11:124 –133.
Morrison AP, Bowe S, Larkin W, Nothard S (1999) The psychological impact of
psychiatric admission: Some preliminary findings. J Nerv Ment Dis. 187:250 – Wells A (1994) Attention and the control of worry. In G Davey, F Tallis (Eds),
253. Worrying: Perspectives on Theory, Assessment and Treatment (pp 91–114).
Chichester (WS): Wiley.
Morrison AP, Wells A, Nothard S (2002) Cognitive and emotional factors as
predictors of predisposition to hallucinations. Br J Clin Psychol. 41:259 –270. Wells A, Matthews G (1994) Attention and emotion. London: LEA
Morrison AP, Gumley AI, Schwannauer M, Campbell M, Gleeson A, Griffin E, Wells A (1995) Meta-cognition and worry: A cognitive model of generalised
Gillan K (2005) The Beliefs about Paranoia Scale: Preliminary Validation of a anxiety disorder. Behav Cognit Psychother. 23:301–320.
Metacognitive Approach to Conceptualizing Paranoia. Behav Cognit Psycho- Weiss D, Marmar C (1997) The impact of event scale -revised. In J Wilson, T
ther. 33:153–164. Keane (Eds), Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD. New York: Guild-
Perkonigg A, Kessler RC, Storz S, Wittchen HU (2000) Traumatic events and ford.
post-traumatic stress disorder in the community: Prevalence, risk factors and White & Gumley. PTSD in Psychosis: Assessing prevalence rates and correlates
comorbidity. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 101:46 –59. of Post-psychotic PTSD and PTSD comorbid to psychosis. In press.
Priebe S, Broker M, Gunkel S (1998) Involuntary admission and post-traumatic Williams-Keeler L, Milliken H, Jones B (1994) Psychosis as precipitating trauma
stress disorder symptoms in schizophrenia patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry. for PTSD: A treatment strategy. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 64:493–
39:220 –224. 498.
Rooney S, Murphy KC, Mulvaney F, O’Callaghan E (1996) A comparison of Zigmond AS, Snaith RP (1983) The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta
voluntary and involuntary patients admitted to hospital. Ir J Psychol Med. Psychiatr Scand. 67:361–370.

© 2009 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins www.jonmd.com | 849

Anda mungkin juga menyukai