Anda di halaman 1dari 9

Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts © 2010 American Psychological Association

2011, Vol. 5, No. 2, 126 –134 1931-3896/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021126

The Automatic Aesthetic Evaluation of Different Art


and Architectural Styles
Stefano Mastandrea, Gabriella Bartoli, and Giuseppe Carrus
University of Roma Tre

The subject under discussion concerns the existence of an automatic aesthetic evaluation. When we
encounter an object like an artwork or an architectural structure that activates an aesthetic response,
does the associated evaluation appear in our mind as an automatic process? From the broad field of
aesthetic appraisal, we will be considering a specific aspect that refers only to the positive and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

negative affects related to an individual’s preference between two art styles (figurative vs. abstract)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and two architectural styles (classic vs. contemporary). The hypothesis is that there is a preexistent
preference within the visual arts and architecture that can clearly be identified using implicit
measurements. Results from two experiments that were conducted with the use of the Implicit
Association Test (IAT) showed that participants’ reaction times were faster in associating positive
words to figurative art and classical architecture (the so-called compatible task) than to abstract art
and contemporary architecture (the so-called incompatible task). The results are in line with the
hypothesis that aesthetic preferences can also be experienced automatically. Prototypicality (i.e., the
degree to which an object is representative of a general class of object), familiarity and the relative
simplicity of figurative art and classical architecture (compared to abstract art and modern archi-
tecture) can explain the shorter reaction time and as a consequence, an implicit aesthetic preference
for these kinds of stimuli.

Keywords: automatic processes, aesthetic evaluation, Implicit Association Test

Many psychological investigations in the field of experimental frin & Schneider, 1977; Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Matthews,
aesthetics employ different kinds of explicit measurement meth- 1988). Implicit cognition is considered important because, differ-
ods, such as questionnaires, focus groups, interviews, and so forth. ently from explicit cognition, it can cast light on processes that
As a result, research has focused on explicit cognition processes cannot be observed in a controlled introspective process in which
involved in the evaluation of different kinds of art stimuli that people do not want to communicate overtly and that can be
occur when the individual makes use of controlled, intentional, expressed in a different way from explicit cognition.
deliberate, and conscious thought. Despite the great number of studies on automatic processes,
Nevertheless, there may be other processes that elude these research in the field of psychology and the arts is practically
explicit measurements, which we might define or label as “auto- nonexistent. Therefore, we argue that it would be important to
matic” or “implicit” cognition. In these cases, although the indi- investigate whether automatic aesthetic processes also play a role
vidual is the agent of thoughts and actions, their thoughts and in the process of art appreciation.
actions are not the product of conscious will (Nosek & Hansen, Indeed, in an information-processing model of aesthetic expe-
2008). According to Bargh (1989), automatic processes can be rience, Leder, Belke, Oeberst, and Augustin (2004) described a
characterized as being unintentional, unaware, uncontrollable, but series of processing stages that an individual goes through during
still efficient. In the last 30 years, there have been an increasing an aesthetic experience. One of these stages is described as “im-
number of research projects dedicated to automatic processes in plicit memory integration,” and refers to implicit processes like
different psychological domains (e.g., cognitive, social, clinical, familiarity and typicality that are used to judge art stimuli. Ac-
neuroscience, marketing; cfr. Brunel, Tietje, & Greenwald, 2004; cording to Locher, Krupinski, Mello-Thoms, and Nodine (2007), a
Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powel, & Kardes, 1986; Öhman, 1987; Shif- brief 100-ms glimpse of a digitized artwork presented on a com-
puter screen is enough for the viewer to detect a great amount of
information about it; not only structural characteristics, such as
This article was published Online First October 11, 2010. shape and color, but also more conceptual aspects, such as the
Stefano Mastandrea, Gabriella Bartoli, and Giuseppe Carrus, Depart- elements of the scene depicted. In another study, Smith and Smith
ment of Cultural and Educational Studies, University of Roma Tre. (2001) reported that visitors to the Metropolitan Museum of New
This research was partially supported by grant funding from the Depart-
York spent an average of about 30 seconds looking at individual
ment of Cultural and Educational Studies.
artworks. These findings could suggest that in some initial stages
We would like to thank Paul Locher and two anonymous reviewers for
their valuable comments on an earlier version of this article. of aesthetic exploration, individuals start building the basis that
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Stefano orientates their later aesthetic experience.
Mastandrea, University of Roma Tre, Via Milazzo, 11 B, 00185 Rome, According to these different two-stage models (Leder et al.,
Italy. E-mail: s.mastandrea@uniroma3.it 2004; Locher et al., 2007), the exploration of a work of art begins
126
AUTOMATIC AESTHETIC EVALUATION 127

with a rapid global view to acquire an initial overall impression of Several research projects that have employed explicit measures
the structural arrangement and the semantic meaning of the com- have clearly shown a general preference for figurative pictures
position. This tachistoscopic view, that in Neisser’s (1967) term compared to abstract ones (Boselie & Cesaro, 1994; Heinrichs &
can be called preattentive, precedes the second stage of the focal Cupchik, 1985; Kettlewell, Lipscomb, Evans, & Rosston, 1990).
attention of the structural organization of the visual display, which People who have a preference for abstract art tend also to have
leads to a deeper aesthetic appreciation (pleasingness, interesting- specific sociodemographic characteristics—a higher socioeco-
ness, etc.). nomic status, a higher level of education (often related to higher
The way we define the process as automatic or implicit is training in visual arts), and belong, for the most part, to the
different from the tachistoscopic or preattentive perception used in professional classes (Feist & Brady, 2004). With regard to person-
Locher et al.’s (2007) model. We refer to a process as being ality traits, a preference for abstract art is related to higher scores
automatic when a person does not have sufficient time to control on the “mental openness” scale, based on the Big Five Question-
the task that s/he has to perform. In the affective priming para- naire, and on Zuckerman’s “Sensation Seeking” scale (Crozier &
digm, for instance, if the prime is congruent with the target (both Chapman, 1984; Feist & Brady, 2004; Francès, 1979; Mastandrea,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

positive or both negative), the response to the target will be faster Bartoli, & Bove, 2009).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

than the response to incongruent pairs of stimuli (Fazio et al., We are aware that contemporary art may also arouse aesthetic
1986). The time latency can occur within a range about between negative emotions; the aesthetic category of beauty is no longer
600 to 1000 ms; this is more than a tachistoscopic view and more suitable for contemporary art or it plays only a secondary role.
than the average duration of one eye fixation. Nevertheless, the Artworks by Francis Bacon, Andres Serrano, and so forth can be
process can be defined as automatic or implicit because the re- provocative and distressing but also interesting and can be very
sponse is “unintentional,” “unaware,” and “uncontrollable” much appreciated. In our present research, we have taken into
(Bargh, 1989). consideration (from the broad field of aesthetic emotions) only
A widely used technique in social cognition research for the specific aspects that refer to diffuse positive and negative emotions
investigation of this kind of automatic affective processes is the related to the preference of two different art styles (figurative and
Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, abstract). Concerning contemporary art, we refer here only to
1998), which will be used in the present study and described in abstract artworks by well known artists, such as Kandinsky, Klee,
more detail in the method section. Albers, and so forth.
The only work we know of that focuses directly on the implicit With regard to architecture, Kameron’s reviews of a great num-
evaluation of art stimuli is a study by Cutting (2003) on French ber of studies on environmental perception would indicate that
Impressionism and the “mere exposure effect.” In a classical study, architects perceive building design in a very different way from
Zajonc (1968) found that repeated exposure to a stimulus increases that of the general public (Kameron, 1973). Concerning architec-
the affective preference for it. In everyday life, we unconsciously tural styles, several research studies have shown that classical
acquire information and attitudes by the fact that we repeatedly buildings are preferred to contemporary fashionable ones (Stamps
encounter objects, pictures, music, and events. The “mere expo- & Nasar, 1997). Moreover, buildings with a moderate level of
sure effect” is related directly to implicit learning because we complexity are preferred to overly simple or overly complex ones
remain unaware of the learning process, but the effect of repeated (Wohlwill, 1974). Regarding prototypicality (i.e., the degree to
exposure orientates our preferences in general, including aesthetic which an object is representative of a general class of object),
preferences. Based on these assumptions, Cutting (2003) selected laypeople tend to prefer architecture that is representative of the
66 images from the Caillebotte collection (an Impressionist painter prototypes (Tinio & Leder, 2009; Whitfield, 1983).
and collector of Impressionist paintings) and matched each image This research consists of two experiments. The first experi-
with the same style and the subject to another image by the same ment concerns the automatic aesthetic evaluation of two art
artist. He then counted every occurrence of the 132 images in styles (figurative vs. abstract art), while the second experiment
about 1000 books at Cornell’s University library. The frequency of is concerned with the automatic aesthetic evaluation of two
occurrence of the 132 pictures ranged from 2 to 278. These results architectural styles (classical vs. contemporary). The hypothesis
show that participants preferred the most frequently occurring is that there exists a preference toward figurative art and clas-
picture of each pair on 59% of all trials. To explain these findings, sical architecture that will also be reflected in the case of
the author argued for the existence of an implicit learning process, automatic evaluations.
as the preferred images occurred more frequently in the books at
Cornell University. This experiment also represents a mere expo-
sure effect regarding aesthetic preference. Experiment 1: Figurative Versus Abstract Art
The question addressed in this paper concerns the existence of
an automatic aesthetic evaluation; namely, is it possible to adopt
Method
implicit measures to study aesthetic preferences? When we en-
counter an object like an artwork or an architectural structure that Participants. The experiment involved a sample that was
activates an aesthetic response, the associated evaluation, in terms composed of 20 students (16 females and 4 males), aged between
of pleasant/unpleasant or positive/negative, may appear to be an 19 and 29 years old (M ⫽ 23.6; SD ⫽ 2.6), from the Faculty of
automatic and uncontrolled process. From the vast field of the Education within the University of Roma Tre. All participants had
aesthetic experience, we have focused on a specific aspect that normal or corrected to normal vision. None of them had received
refers to the preference for different styles in visual art and specific training in art or architecture. They all volunteered to
architecture. participate in the experiment.
128 MASTANDREA, BARTOLI, AND CARRUS

Materials. A set of 20 art paintings (10 figurative and 10 words with the left key, while categorizing abstract art with neg-
abstract) were selected. The images were high quality digital ative words with the right key. In block 7, the categorization task
reproductions of the paintings (see Appendix for list of artworks was inverted: participants categorized abstract art with positive
used). In a preexperiment picture assessment study (conducted words with the left key and with the right key they categorized
with 35 participants, other than those of the implicit task experi- figurative art with negative words.
ment), these 10 figurative and 10 abstract pictures were presented As we said before, results from explicit research showed a
to on a computer screen, without a time limit. Participants were general preference for figurative art rather than abstract art. Ac-
asked to express their preference, using a 7-point semantic differ- cordingly, we planned for two possible associations: the first
ential scale on five bipolar categories (figurative/abstract, simple/ between figurative art (e.g., artworks by Raffaello, Velasquez,
complex, typical/non typical, easy/difficult, familiar/unfamiliar). etc.) and positive words (e.g., beautiful, pleasant, etc.), the second
The results of the picture assessment study are given in Table 1. between abstract art (e.g., artworks by Kandinsky, Pollock, etc.)
Participants expressed significantly divergent evaluations on all and negative words (e.g., ugly, unpleasant, etc.). Therefore, in the
five of the bipolar items, indicating that the style of the pictures compatible task, participants had to press the left key whenever a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

employed significantly influenced subjects’ appraisal. figurative art picture (e.g., Velazquez) or a positive word appeared
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Implicit measure and procedure. To assess the automatic on the screen, and the right key whenever an abstract art picture
aesthetic evaluation of pictures, the Implicit Association Test (e.g., Pollock) or a negative word appeared on the screen; in the
(IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998) was used. The IAT is a paradigm incompatible task the contrary applied; that is, they had to press the
employed to measure the strength of the associative links between left key for Velasquez and negative words and the right key for
concepts and their affective attributes, through reaction times. This Pollock and positive words.
method does not require a conscious awareness of the association It is important to note that the terms “compatible” and “incom-
and has been used in various psychological research areas, partic- patible” are not used in absolute terms but are relative to the main
ularly in the field of social cognition. hypotheses put forward in our study. The compatible task reflects
The method consists of classifying stimuli (pictures and words the idea of an easier association between figurative art and positive
presented one at a time on the computer monitor-screenshot) into words and abstract art and negative words, on the one hand;
four categories, using only two response options, by pressing two whereas, the incompatible task should reflect the idea of a more
keys (“e” and “i”) on the computer keyboard. The two categories difficult association (figurative art/negative words and abstract
in the present experiment are 10 figurative art pictures and 10 art/positive words), on the other hand.
abstract art pictures. The two attributes are aesthetic positive Pictures from the two art categories (figurative and abstract) and
words and aesthetic negative words. Ten words with a positive word attributes (positive and negative) were presented to the
aesthetic meaning (attractive, beautiful, delicious, elegant, fasci- participants, on a computer monitor, one after the other, in a
nating, pleasant, magnificent, marvelous, splendid, wonderful) and random order. Each session always started with a picture. Partic-
10 with a negative aesthetic meaning (disgusting, dreadful, horri- ipants were asked to respond as quickly and accurately as they
ble, repellent, repulsive, revolting, rude, terrible, ugly, unpleasant) could. In case of (categorization) errors, a red X appeared in the
were also chosen. center of the screen, and participants had to answer again with
The logic of IAT is that this sorting task should be easier when the correct key; the reaction times of errors were not counted, but
the two concepts that share a response are strongly associated as the correction was important in order that the categorization task
compared to when they are weakly associated. was made clear (see Table 2).
The experimental task consisted of seven blocks of trials pre- To monitor for possible learning effects, half of the participants
sented in succession, as illustrated in Table 2. From these seven performed the session with the compatible block first (figurative/
blocks, five blocks were utilized for familiarization with the dif- positive and abstract/negative) followed by the incompatible block
ferent categorization tasks of pictures and words. The other two (figurative/negative and abstract/positive). For the remaining par-
blocks are defined as critical and are composed of the so-called ticipants, the presentation was reversed: first the incompatible
compatible task (block 4) and incompatible task (block 7). In the block (figurative/negative and abstract/positive) was presented,
block 4, participants had to categorize figurative art with positive followed by the compatible task (figurative/positive and abstract/

Table 1
Mean Scores, Standard Deviations, and T-Test Results of the Evaluation of the 20 Art Pictures
on the 5-Item Semantic Differential

Figurative art Abstract art

M SD M SD t-value

Figurative/Abstract 1.7 0.7 6.3 0.8 ⫺25.39


Simple/Complex 3.6 1.4 5.2 0.9 ⫺4.05
Typical/Non Typical 2.3 1.0 3.6 1.5 ⫺5.53
Easy/Difficult 3.4 1.2 4.7 0.7 ⫺4.29
Familiar/Unfamiliar 2.9 1.1 4.8 1.4 ⫺7.27

Note. All t-values are significant at p ⬍ .001; df ⫽ 34.


AUTOMATIC AESTHETIC EVALUATION 129

Table 2
Sequence of Blocks for Art Implicit Association Test (IAT). The Test Blocks Are the Fourth (Compatible Task) and the Seventh
(Incompatible Task)

Items assigned to left-key Items assigned to right-key


Block N. of trials response response Practice/Test

1 20 Figurative art Abstract art Practice


2 20 Positive words Negative words Practice
3 20 Figurative art ⫹ Positive words Abstract art ⫹ Negative words Practice
4 40 Figurative art ⫹ Positive words Abstract art ⫹ Negative words Test/Compatible Task
5 40 Abstract art Figurative art Practice inverted
6 20 Abstract art ⫹ Positive words Figurative art ⫹ Negative words Practice
7 40 Abstract art ⫹ Positive words Figurative art ⫹ Negative words Test/Incompatible Task
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

negative). A total of 40 trials (10 figurative pictures ⫹ 10 positive incompatible task. These mean scores were then compared. It is
words ⫹ 10 abstract pictures ⫹ 10 negative words) for each important to remember that the test is based on the assumption that
critical block were conducted. The independent variables consisted the task is faster when the concept and attribute are strongly
of pictures and words presented on a screen; dependent variables associated. In the compatible task (figurative/positive and abstract/
were the reaction times for each single stimulus, pictures (figura- negative) the mean reaction time was 684 ms (SD ⫽ 82), while in
tive and abstract) and words (positive and negative). the incompatible task (figurative/negative and abstract/positive),
The software Inquisit 2 (2006) was used to carry out the exper- the mean reaction time was 840 ms (SD ⫽ 170). The standard
iment and record reaction times. deviation in the incompatible task is greater than in the compatible
Explicit measures. In addition to the implicit task, we were task (170 vs. 82) because reaction times are slower and the range
also interested in measuring explicit evaluations, to ascertain of the responses is wider. The overall IAT effect (the difference
whether the implicit and explicit evaluations were correlated. In between the incompatible and compatible task) was 156 ms. A
particular, participants were asked to state a) how much they liked t test showed that the difference between these two tasks was
the figurative and abstract pictures presented during the IAT task, significant, t(19) ⫽ ⫺4.04; p ⫽ .001, d ⫽ ⫺.986. Moreover, more
and b) how much they liked figurative and abstract art in general errors were made in the incompatible task (N ⫽ 36) than in the
(without presenting any pictures). We asked these two different compatible task (N ⫽ 11), showing that the incompatible task was
questions because participants may have liked the specific pictures more difficult.
presented but not the art styles, or they might have liked a partic- Explicit evaluations results. With regard to the explicit mea-
ular style but wouldn’t have appreciated the specific pictures surements, our findings showed that the average score in the
presented. To answer these two questions, a 7-point Likert scale evaluation of the likeableness of the figurative pictures was 4.4
was used (1 ⫽ do not like at all; 7 ⫽ like very much). In the first (SD ⫽ 1.3), while the average score in the evaluation of the
explicit task, the 20 pictures (both figurative and abstract) previ- likeableness of the abstract pictures was 4.0 (SD ⫽ 1.2); the
ously evaluated in IAT session were presented one by one in a difference between these two groups of art pictures was not sta-
random order on the computer monitor without time limit. Partic- tistically significant. In the second explicit measurement, the eval-
ipants were asked to evaluate the pictures from 1 to 7, using the uations of the likeableness of the art styles in general were as-
computer number pad. In rating the second question (“How much sessed. The mean score for figurative art was 5.3 (SD ⫽ 1.3) and
do you like figurative and abstract art in general”) participants for abstract art was 4.6 (SD ⫽ 1.5); again, the difference between
used the same method. these two art styles was not statistically significant.
Correlation between implicit and explicit measures. Bi-
Results variate correlations between implicit and explicit measures on the
evaluation of figurative and abstract art were computed, taking into
The principal result of the experiment is the difference in the consideration the IAT effect for each participant (the difference
reaction times between the two critical IAT tasks, the compatible between incompatible and compatible task) and the difference
task (figurative art pictures/positive words and abstract art pic- between the average evaluation of figurative and abstract pictures
tures/negative words) and the incompatible task (figurative art in the explicit measurements. No significant correlation between
pictures/ negative words and abstract art pictures/positive words). implicit and explicit measures was detected.
Reaction times less than 300 ms and greater than 10,000 ms and
errors were excluded from the analysis (following Nosek, Green-
Discussion
wald, & Banaji, 2007).
To assess the internal consistency of all the stimuli used, Cron- The IAT effect revealed the existence of significant differences
bach’s alpha was calculated on the total of all reaction times, with between the evaluations of the two art styles. Participants were
good results (␣ ⫽ .96). faster in making automatic associations between figurative art and
First, for every participant, two means scores were calculated: positive words than between abstract art and positive words.
the first score coming from the latency times of the 40 stimuli This higher speed of the automatic association in the former
(pictures and words) of the compatible task and the second score case indicate an implicit preference for figurative art, replicating
from the latency times of the 40 stimuli (pictures and words) of the the findings obtained in literature regarding art explicit cognition
130 MASTANDREA, BARTOLI, AND CARRUS

measures (Boselie & Cesaro, 1994; Feist & Brady, 2004; Hei- roborate the importance of automatic aesthetic evaluation in dif-
nrichs & Cupchik, 1985). These findings seem to confirm that, as ferent domains, such as in visual art and architecture.
a first stage process, aesthetic preference for figurative art can also The question that we addressed utilizing implicit evaluations
be expressed automatically. was whether classical architecture is preferred to contemporary
In contrast, the results for explicit measurements did not show architecture. Our hypothesis was that a preference for classical
significant differences in participants’ preferences either for spe- architecture emerges using automatic measurements.
cific figurative versus abstract pictures or for figurative versus
abstract art styles in general. Participants liked both art styles Method
equally, contradicting the results for the implicit measures. The
difference between implicit and explicit tasks might partly be Participants. The experiment involved a sample that was
explained because of the order of presentation of the IAT and composed of 40 students (27 females and 13 males), aged 19 –28
self-report measurements, as suggested by Nosek et al. (2007). years (M ⫽ 24.6; SD ⫽ 2.2), from the Faculty of Education within
Presenting the explicit measurements first can make information the University of Roma Tre. All participants had normal or cor-
rected normal vision. None of them had received specific training
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

about the topic under investigation more accessible to the subjects.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

but, likewise, performing the implicit task first can provide sub- in art or architecture. They all volunteered to participate in the
jects with relevant information about the topic under investigation, experiment.
and this might influence the performance on the following explicit Materials. A set of 10 architectural pictures, five classical
measurements. This latter effect might have been the case in the palaces of the renaissance period and five contemporary buildings,
present experiment. Another aspect worthy of consideration relates were selected (see the Appendix for list of architectural buildings
to participants’ compliance with the experimental assignment. used). The images were high quality digital reproductions of
Participants might have thought that if an experimenter shows photographs. In the field of architecture, evaluation is influenced
them some works of art, these must be necessarily of a certain both by the contextual environment for the building and the formal
artistic value or interest; therefore, evaluations of both kinds of characteristics of the building itself (Stamps & Nasar, 1997). The
stimuli might have resulted in similar scores because of a so-called present research is a laboratory study, and therefore, the ecological
“ceiling” effect. This can be another motive to use implicit mea- information coming from a real context has been significantly
sures, avoiding the possible influence of conformity and compli- reduced. All the pictures that we used are reproductions of the
ance effects. Moreover, we found no correlation between implicit façades of the buildings considered, adopting a procedure common
and explicit answers. In any case, the main goal of the study was to the majority of research in this field.
to observe implicit appraisal of art stimuli and our findings con- A preexperiment picture assessment study, following the same
firm that also an aesthetic evaluation can be activated automati- procedure (with the same participants from the previous experi-
cally. ment), was conducted (see results in Table 3). Participants ex-
pressed significantly divergent evaluations on all five of the bipo-
Experiment 2: Classical Versus Contemporary lar items, indicating that the style of the architectures significantly
influenced subjects’ appraisal.
Architecture
Measures and procedure. As in Experiment 1, participants
The aim of the second experiment was also to use implicit completed an IAT task that implicitly measured the evaluation of
measurements in the assessment of preferences for different archi- two architectural styles. The two concept categories were classical
tectural styles. It is arguable that a pattern similar to preferences and contemporary architecture, and the two attributes were the
for figurative versus abstract art styles might also characterize same positive and negative words used in the previous art study.
preferences for different architectural styles. Indeed, a study by The experimental procedure was the same as described for Exper-
Gifford, Hine, Muller-Clemm, Reynolds, and Shaw (2000) showed iment 1. This time, in the compatible task, subjects had to cate-
that people prefer classical to contemporary architecture. Confir- gorize with the left key classical architectural buildings (e.g., a
mation of these findings with implicit measurements would cor- Renaissance building façade) and positive words (e.g., fascinating)

Table 3
Mean Scores, Standard Deviations, and T-Test Results of the Evaluation of the 10 Architecture
Pictures on the 5-Item Semantic Differential

Classical Contemporary
architecture architecture

M SD M SD t-value

Classical/Contemporary 2.2 1.2 6.8 0.3 ⫺19,73


Simple/Complex 3.8 1.0 6.0 0.6 ⫺9,82
Easy/Difficult 3.6 1.2 5.2 0.9 ⫺6,96
Familiar/Unfamiliar 2.7 1.1 4.9 1.6 ⫺6,98
Typical/Non Typical 2.3 0.8 3.4 1.6 ⫺4,24

Note. All t-values are significant at p ⬍ .001; df ⫽ 32.


AUTOMATIC AESTHETIC EVALUATION 131

and with the right key contemporary architectural buildings (e.g., Discussion
a Calatrava building) and negative words (e.g., disgusting).
Conversely, in the incompatible task, the left key was used for In line with our expectations, in the IAT task, classic architec-
classical architecture and negative words, while the right key was tural stimuli were processed more rapidly and were more easily
used for contemporary architecture and positive words. associated with positive words, compared to contemporary archi-
tectural stimuli. An important consideration is that all the partic-
As in Experiment 1, after completing the IAT, explicit evalua-
ipants live in the city of Rome and, therefore, are more familiar
tions were also collected. Participants were asked to evaluate, in
with ancient and classical architecture than to contemporary archi-
the first question, the likeableness of classical and contemporary
tecture. It could be argued that they would appreciate images of
architecture pictures presented in the IAT, and, in the second
classical architecture more than contemporary ones.
question, the likeableness of classical and contemporary architec- No significant differences were found in the explicit task for the
ture in general (without presenting any pictures), using the same 7 evaluation of the two different styles, either with the pictures
points Likert scale described in Experiment 1. presented or with the general class of stimuli (classic vs. contem-
porary). Moreover, in the art experiment, we did not find any
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

significant difference between the two classes of stimuli (figura-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Results
tive vs. abstract), whereas in the architecture experiment, we found
As in Experiment 1, reaction times under 300 ms and above a significant correlation between the IAT effect (the latency time
10,000 ms and errors were excluded from the analysis. Evaluations in the incompatible task minus the latency time in the compatible
task) and the explicit task (the difference between the evaluation of
for one participant were discarded due to technical problems;
classical and modern architecture), but no correlation was found in
therefore, the final analyses were conducted with 39 partici-
the art experiment. Participants who were fast or slow in associ-
pants. Total errors were 3.5%. Cronbach’s alpha was again
ating positive words to classical or contemporary styles in the IAT
good (␣ ⫽ .94).
task had the same orientation in the explicit evaluation task. It
In the compatible task (classical architecture/positive words and means that people with faster associative responses for positive
contemporary architecture/negative words), the average reaction words and classic stimuli on IAT gave higher scores in the clas-
time was 623 ms (SD ⫽ 108). In the incompatible task (classical sical architecture explicit evaluation, and people with faster asso-
architecture/negative words and contemporary architecture/ ciative responses for positive words and contemporary stimuli on
positive words), the average reaction time was 688 ms (SD ⫽ 189). IAT gave higher scores in the contemporary architecture explicit
As in the previous experiment on art, the standard deviation in the evaluation. This correlation was not obtained in the art experiment.
incompatible task is greater than in the compatible task (189 vs. A possible explanation may be attributed to the fact that architec-
108) because reaction times are slower and the range of the tural stimuli are much more similar to one another within the same
responses is wider. The overall IAT effect (the difference between category (classic or contemporary) compared to the wide variety of
compatible and incompatible task) was 65 ms. A t test showed that paintings (figurative and abstract). Building constraints mean that
the difference between the two tasks was significant, t(38) ⫽ architects cannot design buildings with just any kind of shape. In
⫺2.48; p ⫽ .01, d ⫽ ⫺.442. More errors were made in the particular, Renaissance architectural palaces share very similar
incompatible task (n ⫽ 47) than in compatible task (n ⫽ 38), structural characteristics, whereas figurative paintings can be very
showing, as in the previous experiment, that the incompatible task different from one each other because painters have much more
was more difficult. design freedom because they do not have to respond to functional
Regarding the explicit measures, again no significant differ- aims (e.g., from the Venice square by Canaletto to the natural
ences emerged between the average scores in the evaluation of the countryside view by Daubigny). It is probably for this reason that
classical (M ⫽ 4.5; SD ⫽ 1.3) and contemporary architecture participants maintain (at an individual level) the same evaluative
trend, both in the implicit and explicit tasks in the architecture
pictures (M ⫽ 4.8; SD ⫽ 1.3). Likewise, the mean scores evalu-
experiment rather than in the art experiment. The different variet-
ations of classic (M ⫽ 5.1; SD ⫽ 1.4) and contemporary (M ⫽ 5.1;
ies of the compositional features in the two art categories (figura-
SD ⫽ 1.3) architectural styles in general were the same. These
tive and abstract) can lead to a differentiated assessment in the two
findings may be surprising because previous research indicates
tasks, implicit and explicit.
that the general public prefers classical architecture on average Nevertheless, this experiment confirmed the findings of previ-
(Gifford et al., 2000). Moreover, in the explicit art appraisal ous experiments on art pictures; indeed, we can say that in the case
experiment, there were no significant differences between figura- of architectural stimuli, aesthetic evaluations can also be activated
tive and abstract art. The reason can be attributed to the fact that automatically.
IAT task was performed first, before the explicit evaluation. This
provided participants with relevant information about the topic
under investigation that influenced the following explicit evalua- General Discussion
tion of the architectural pictures. As we have said for the art The two experiments presented here were conducted with the
experiment before, implicit measurements are useful to avoid the aim of exploring the implicit evaluation of different artistic styles
possible influence of conformity and compliance effects. in visual art and architecture, given the lack of empirical data in the
In contrast to the results for Experiment 1, a significant corre- field of automatic aesthetic processes. The central core of the
lation was detected between implicit and explicit measures (r ⫽ studies was the two IAT experiments. The main finding was that
.633; p ⬍ .000). reaction times were faster in the compatible task than in the
132 MASTANDREA, BARTOLI, AND CARRUS

incompatible tasks in both experiments, even though the IAT Familiarity refers to how familiar or novel an object is.
effect was stronger in the case of the art stimuli than in the case of Another characteristic of the aesthetic evaluation is related to
architectural stimuli. These results provide support for the hypoth- the mere exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968). Several studies have
esis that the aesthetic evaluation of stimuli like art and architecture found that repeated exposure to a stimulus increases the affec-
can also be activated automatically. tive preference for it (Cutting, 2003; Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc,
But how can we explain why people are faster at making 1980; Zajonc, 1968). Familiar stimuli are processed faster and
automatic associations between positive words and figurative art preferred to novel stimuli. The association between familiarity
or classical architecture, on the one hand, compared to abstract art and positive affect may be grounded in a biological predispo-
or contemporary architecture, on the other hand? sition to exercise caution in encounters with potentially harmful
The assumption of the IAT measurement is that it is easier (and, novel objects (Zajonc, 1998). In our study, figurative art and
therefore, faster) to categorize stimuli belonging to two associated classical architecture appear to be more familiar and, thus, are
categories than when the categories are not associated; for exam- preferred to the less familiar abstract art and contemporary
ple, if figurative and abstract pictures had an equally strong asso- architecture. According to this explanation, it is plausible that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ciation with positive words, no difference would be found in participants in our studies encounter examples of figurative art
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

latency times. But suppose instead that the two image categories and classical architecture (all participants live in Rome) more
had a different appraisal association, figurative art being more often than abstract art and contemporary architecture; daily
strongly associated with positive words than abstract art; then, exposure and greater familiarity with these classes of stimuli
according to our hypothesis, it should be easier to perform the task would produce a preference for these art styles. We know that
that requires pushing the same key for figurative pictures and natural and built environments from different geographical lo-
positive words (compatible task) than for figurative pictures and cations (Australia and Italy) were preferred according to the
negative words (incompatible task). Moreover, participants were subjects’ nationality, with the home environment being the
asked to respond in a very short time (about 1000 ms) and, most appreciated (Purcell, Lamb, Mainardi Peron, & Falchero,
therefore, were unable to control their responses, which is why the 1994). It would be worth replicating the present research with
process is called automatic (Fiske & Taylor, 2008). people living in an area with a high concentration of contem-
We know from our preexperiment picture assessment study that porary architecture (e.g., New York, Chicago, Berlin, Copen-
figurative art and classical architecture are appraised as more hagen, etc.). We might find that exposure to modern and con-
typical, familiar, easier to understand, and less complex than temporary architecture would influence peoples’ evaluation
abstract art and contemporary architecture. All of these attributes either at an implicit and explicit level.
become important in the processing task because of the well- Complexity refers to the number and to the perceptual orga-
established link between figurative pictures and classical architec- nization of the elements in a composition. In general, a mod-
ture with a positive affect in laypersons’ mental schemata, leading erate level of complexity seems to be the most preferred in
to an implicit association between the two concepts. artworks, in architectures and also in environmental scenes.
To summarize, it is important to outline how, according to the (Berlyne, 1971; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Purcell, Peron, &
findings of our studies, the aesthetic response to different kinds of Berto, 2001; Ulrich, 1983). In general, people without art
stimuli, such as visual art and architecture, can also be expressed training prefer simple and symmetric visual elements, whereas
automatically. To discuss these results, we will refer to three people with art training prefer complex and asymmetric visual
different models of aesthetic appreciation: prototypicality, famil- elements (Locher & Nodine, 1989; McWhinnie, 1968; Silvia,
iarity, and complexity. 2005). Therefore, the appreciation of artistic complexity
Prototypicality is explained as the degree to which an object is (mainly abstract art) by our non art-trained participants takes a
representative of a general class of objects. Several studies have longer time to evaluate. In our case, one might argue that
shown that prototypical forms are preferred over nonprototypical figurative art and classical architecture (compared to abstract
ones (Winkielman, Halberstadt, Fazendeiro, & Catty, 2006). Pref- art and modern architecture) are more symmetrical and less
erence for prototypical examples has been found in several areas: complex and are, therefore, preferred.
design objects, facial attractiveness, architectural façades, and in These three plausible explanations of our findings (protypi-
the arts. A number of studies, for example, reveal a positive link cality, familiarity, and complexity) can be incorporated into a
between prototypicality and aesthetic appraisals in colored forms more general model of aesthetic preference, called Processing
(Martindale & Moore, 1988), in furniture (Whitfield & Slatter, Fluency (Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004). According to
1979), and in paintings (Hekkert & vanWieringen, 1996). More- this model, there is a strong relationship between the quantity
over, studies from the field of cognitive psychology have shown and the type of characteristics observed in the objects (such as
that prototypical stimuli are processed faster and more easily than typicality, familiarity, and complexity) and the individual who
nonprototypical stimuli (Posner & Keele, 1968). As suggested by is processing the objects: the more the object processing is
Winkielman, Halberstadt, Fazendeiro, and Catty (2006), prototyp- fluent (perception of the identity and the meaning), the more
ical items are evaluated more positively because they are easier to positive the aesthetic answer will be.
process. Prototypical stimuli also have shown stronger EMG re- In conclusion, we can envisage possible future developments of
sponses from the zygomaticus region, indicating a more positive the present research. An initial development could be that of
affect (Winkielman & Cacioppo, 2001). In our study, it can be collecting data from expert participants (e.g., artists and archi-
argued that figurative art and classical architecture are more typ- tects). By comparing the results across experts and laypeople, one
ical than abstract art and contemporary architecture, and therefore, could check whether a particular training in art or architecture can
the former are preferred over the latter. also affect the automatic aesthetic evaluations. One would expect
AUTOMATIC AESTHETIC EVALUATION 133

that experts, with their deeper knowledge on art and architectural aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psy-
styles, would not show a marked preference for a specific style, or chology, 95, 489 –508.
on the contrary, they might express a preference for contemporary Locher, P., Krupinski, E. A., Mello-Thoms, C., & Nodine, C. F. (2007).
art and architecture. Visual interest in pictorial art during an aesthetic experience. Spatial
Another potential extension of the present studies could be to Vision, 21(1–2), 55–77.
identify other art category stimuli (e.g., design objects) with the Locher, P., & Nodine, C. F. (1989). The perceptual value of symmetry.
aim of confirming and broadening these results into other aesthetic Computers and Math Applications, 17, 475– 484.
Martindale, C., & Moore, K. (1988). Priming, prototypicality, and prefer-
domains.
ence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 14, 661– 670.
References Mastandrea, S., Bartoli, G., & Bove, G. (2009). Preferences for ancient and
modern art museums: Visitor experiences and personality characteris-
Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic tics. Psychology of Aesthetic, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(3), 164 –173.
influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. McWhinnie, H. J. (1968). A review of research on aesthetic measure. Acta
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 3–51). New York, NY: Guilford Psychologica, 28, 363–375.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Press. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York, NY: Appleton-


Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York, NY: Century-Crofts.
Appleton-Century-Crofts. Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). The Implicit
Boselie, F., & Cesaro, A. (1994). Disjunctive ambiguity as a determinant Association Test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In
of the aesthetic attractivity of visual patterns. Empirical Studies of the J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Automatic processes in social thinking and behavior
Arts, 12(1), 85–94. (pp. 265–292). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Brunel, F. F., Tietje, B. C., & Greenwald, A. G. (2004). Is the Implicit Nosek, B. A., & Hansen, J. J. (2008). The associations in our heads belong
Association Test a valid and valuable measure of implicit consumer to us: Searching for attitudes and knowledge in implicit evaluation.
social cognition? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 385– 404. Cognition and Emotion, 22(4), 553–594.
Crozier, W. R., & Chapman, A. J. (1984). Cognitive processes in the Öhman, A. (1987). The psychophysiology of emotion: An evolutionary-
perception of art. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland.
cognitive perspective. Advances in psychophysiology, 2, 79 –127.
Cutting, J. E. (2003). Gustave Caillebotte, French Impressionism, and mere
Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas.
exposure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 319 –343.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353–363.
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powel, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986).
Purcell, A. T., Lamb, R. J., Mainardi Peron, E., & Falchero, S. (1994).
On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and
Preference or preferences for landscape? Journal of Environmental
Social Psychology, 50, 229 –238.
Feist, G. J., & Brady, T. R. (2004). Openness to experience, non- Psychology, 14(3), 195–209.
conformity, and the preference for abstract art. Empirical Studies of the Purcell, T., Peron, E., & Berto, R. (2001). Why do preferences differ
Arts, 22(1), 77– 89. between scene types? Environment and Behavior, 33(1), 93–106.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2008). Social cognition: From brains to Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and
culture. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill. aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience?
Francès, R. (1979). Psychologie de l’art et de l’esthétique. Paris, France: Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364 –382.
PUF. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human
Gifford, R., Hine, D. W., Muller-Clemm, W., Reynolds, D. J., & Shaw, information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and
K. T. (2000). Decoding modern architecture: A lens model approach for a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.
understanding the aesthetic differences of architects and laypersons. Silvia, P. J. (2005). Emotional response to art: From collation and arousal
Environment and Behavior, 32(2), 163–187. to cognition and emotion. Review of General Psychology, 9, 342–357.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring Smith, J., & Smith, L. (2001). Spending time on art. Empirical Studies of
individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. the Arts, 19, 229 –236.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1464 –1480. Stamps, A. E., & Nasar, J. L. (1997). Design review and public prefer-
Heinrichs, R. W., & Cupchik, G. C. (1985). Individual differences as ences: Effects of geographical location, public consensus, sensation
predictors of preference in visual art. Journal of Personality, 53, 502– seeking and architectural styles. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
515. 17(1), 11–32.
Hekkert, P., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1996). Beauty in the eye of expert Tinio, P. P. L., & Leder, H. (2009). Natural scenes are indeed preferred, but
and nonexpert beholders: A study in the appraisal of art. American
image quality might have the last word. Psychology of Aesthetics,
Journal of Psychology, 109, 389 – 407.
Creativity, and the Arts, 3(1), 52–56.
Inquisit 2.0.60616. (2006). [Computer software]. Seattle, WA: Millisecond
Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to natural environ-
Software.
ment. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Behaviour and the natural
Kameron, J. (1973). Experimental studies of environment perception. In
environment (pp. 85–125). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
W. H. Ittelson (Ed.), Environment and cognition (pp. 157–167). New
York, NY: Seminar Press. Whitfield, T. W. A. (1983). Predicting preference for familiar, everyday
Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psycholog- objects: An experimental confrontation between two theories of aes-
ical perspective. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. thetic behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 3, 221–237.
Kettlewell, N., Lipscomb, S., Evans, L., & Rosston, K. (1990). The effect Whitfield, T. W. A., & Slatter, P. E. (1979). The effects of categorisation
of subject matter and degree of realism on aesthetic preferences for and prototypicality on aesthetic choice in a furniture selection task.
paintings. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 8, 85–93. British Journal of Psychology, 70, 65–75.
Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination of Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Matthews, A. (1988).
stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557–558. Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Chichester, England:
Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, D. (2004). A model of Wiley.
134 MASTANDREA, BARTOLI, AND CARRUS

Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the Wohlwill, J. F. (1974). Human adaptation to levels of environmental
face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation leads to stimulation. Human Ecology, 2, 127–147.
positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 989 – Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of
1000. Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 1–27.
Winkielman, P., Halberstadt, J., Fazendeiro, T., & Catty, S. (2006). Pro- Zajonc, R. B. (1998). Emotions. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey
totypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychological (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 591– 632). Boston, MA:
Science, 17(9), 799 – 806. McGraw-Hill.

Appendix

Lists of Artworks and Architectures Used in the Two Experiments


This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

List of Figurative Paintings selected: 9. Jackson Pollock, Autumn Rhythm (Number 30), 1950

1. Bouguereau, Return From the Harvest, 1878 10. Emilio Vedova, Immagine nel Tempo 58-4V, 1958

List of Classical Buildings selected:


2. Canaletto, Il Campo di Rialto (Venezia), c. 1758 – 63

3. Caravaggio, Basket of Fruit, 1597 1. Biagio Rossetti, Palazzo dei Diamanti, ca. 1493, Fer-
rara, Italy
4. Constable, The Cornfield, 1826
2. Antonio da Sangallo the Young, Palazzo Farnese, ca.
5. Daubigny, Aldeia de Optevoz, 1857 1546, Rome, Italy

6. David, Conte Potocki, 1780 – 81 3. Giovanni Antonio De Rossi, Palazzo Altieri, 1650,
Rome, Italy
7. Raffaello, Bindo Altoviti, c. 1515
4. Michelozzo, Palazzo Medici Riccardi, 1460, Florence,
8. Guido Reni, Lot and His Daughters Leaving Sodom, Italy
1615-16
5. Formentone, Palladio and Sansovino, Palazzo Della
9. Velázquez. Queen Isabel, Standing, 1631/32 Loggia, ca. 1574, Brescia, Italy

10. Vermeer, Street in Delft, c. 1657–1658 List of Contemporary Buildings selected:

1. Frank O. Gehry, Guggenheim Museum, 1997, Bilbao,


List of Abstract Paintings selected:
Spain

1. Josef Albers, Study of Homage to the Square, 1954 2. Santiago Calatrava, Turning Torso, 2005, Malmö, Swe-
den
2. Robert Delaunay, Homage to Bleriot, 1914
3. Norman Foster, London City Hall, 2003, London,
3. Kandinsky, Composition VIII, 1923 United Kingdom

4. Paul Klee, Glass Façade, 1940 4. Zaha Hadid, Phaeno Science Center, 2005, Wolfsburg,
Germany
5. Lissisky, Proun 19D, c. 1922
5. Ben van Berkel, Mercedes-Benz Museum, 2006, Stutt-
6. Kasimir Malevich, Suprematist Painting, 1915 gart, Germany
7. Alberto Burri, Sacco su Tela, 1953
Received June 1, 2010
8. Piet Mondrian, Composition With Black, Red, Gray, Revision received July 27, 2010
Yellow, and Blue, 1920 Accepted July 28, 2010 䡲

Anda mungkin juga menyukai