Anda di halaman 1dari 14

Canadian Journal of Occupational

Therapy http://cjo.sagepub.com/

A Systematic Review of Interventions to Improve Handwriting


Monica M. P. Hoy, Mary Y. Egan and Katya P. Feder
Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy 2011 78: 13
DOI: 10.2182/cjot.2011.78.1.3

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://cjo.sagepub.com/content/78/1/13

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Canadian Association of Occupational Therapists/Association Canadienne des Ergotherapeutes

Additional services and information for Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://cjo.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://cjo.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://cjo.sagepub.com/content/78/1/13.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Feb 1, 2011

What is This?

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


A systematic review of interventions to improve handwriting
Monica M. P. Hoy, Mary Y. Egan, Katya P. Feder

Abstract
Key words Background. Handwriting difficulties are among the most common reasons for re-
Handwriting
ferral of children to occupational therapy. Purpose. To determine the effectiveness
Pediatric occupational therapy
Schools, elementary
of handwriting interventions. Methods. A systematic review was carried out. Includ-
ed studies were randomized or nonrandomized controlled trials of interventions that
could be used by an occupational therapist to improve written output (printing or
Mots clés writing) among school-aged children identified as having difficulties with handwrit-
Écoles, élémentaires ing. Electronic searches of relevant databases were conducted up to January 2010.
Écriture
Findings. Eleven studies met the inclusion criteria. These studies tested (1) relaxation
Ergothérapie en pédiatrie
and practice with or without EMG, (2) sensory-based training without handwriting
practice, and (3) handwriting-based practice (including sensory-focused or cognitive
focused handwriting practice). Regardless of treatment type, interventions that did
not include handwriting practice and those that included less than 20 practice ses-
Monica M. P. Hoy, MScS, OT Reg. (Ont.) sions were ineffective. Implications. Effective occupational therapy for improving
is Occupational Therapist, Five Coun- handwriting must include adequate handwriting practice.
ties Children’s Centre, 9 Russell St. East,
Lindsay, ON K9V 1Z7.
Mary Y. Egan, PhD, OT Reg. (Ont.) is
Associate Professor, School of Rehabilita- Résumé
tion Sciences, University of Ottawa,
Description. Les troubles de l’écriture comptent parmi les raisons les plus courantes
451 Smyth Rd., Ottawa, ON K1H 8M5.
Telephone 613-562-5800, ext 8043. pour aiguiller un enfant vers des services d’ergothérapie. But. Déterminer l’efficacité
E-mail: megan@uottawa.ca des interventions visant à améliorer l’écriture. Méthodologie. Revue systématique
des essais cliniques randomisés et non randomisés portant sur des interventions
Katya P. Feder, PhD, OT Reg. (Ont.)
is Postdoctoral Fellow, University of pouvant être utilisées par un ergothérapeute en vue d’améliorer la productivité écrite
Montreal and Adjunct Professor, School (documents imprimés ou écrits à la main) chez des enfants d’âge scolaire ayant de
of Rehabilitation Sciences, University of la difficulté à écrire. Des recherches électroniques dans des bases de données perti-
Ottawa, 451 Smyth Rd., Ottawa, ON nentes ont été effectuées jusqu’en janvier 2010. Résultats. Onze études répondaient
K1H 8M5. aux critères d’inclusion. Ces études évaluaient : 1) des séances de relaxation et de
l’entraînement à l’écriture avec ou sans EMG, (2) un entraînement sensoriel sans
exercice d’écriture, et (3) de l’entraînement à l’écriture (y compris de l’entraînement
à l’écriture axé sur des aspects sensoriels ou cognitifs). Les interventions qui ne
faisaient pas appel à de l’entraînement à l’écriture et celles qui comprenaient moins
de 29 séances d’exercices ont été inefficaces, et cela, peu importe le type de traite-
ment utilisé. Conséquences. Pour être efficaces, les interventions ergothérapiques
visant à améliorer l’écriture doivent comprendre des exercices d’écriture.

T
he Canadian Association of Occupational Therapists (2007) defines occu-
Citation: Hoy, M. M. P., Egan, M. Y., &
pational therapy as “the art and science of enabling engagement in everyday
Feder, K. P. (2011). A systematic review of
interventions to improve handwriting. living, through occupation” (p. 20). In the lives of children, school provides
Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, a context for engagement in a variety of occupations. Independent functioning at
78, 13-25. doi: 10.2182/cjot.2011.78.1.3 school generally requires an effective method of producing written output (Case-
Smith, 2005).
Submitted: 17 March 2010; Many academic activities require children to put their thoughts to paper either
Final acceptance: 23 July 2010.
in the form of printing or cursive writing. As much as 30 to 60% of a child’s school
This study was funded by a grant from day is comprised of fine-motor activities, with handwriting tasks accounting for
the Canadian Occupational Therapy the majority of this time (McHale & Cermak, 1992 as cited in Peterson and Nel-
Foundation. son, 2003). Handwriting has been described as a “complex perceptual-motor skill

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


© CAOT PUBLICATIONS ACE Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy February 2011 78(1) 13
Hoy et al.

encompassing a blend of visual-motor coordination abilities, ness of a particular intervention, with regards to one or more
motor planning, cognitive, and perceptual skills, as well as tac- specific outcomes. Such a review provides a synthesis of the best
tile and kinaesthetic sensitivities” (Feder & Majnemer, 2007, p. evidence regarding treatment effectiveness (Elwood, 2007).
313). An estimated 10 to 30% of school-aged children experi- This systematic review included studies that were ran-
ence handwriting difficulties (Karlsdottir & Stefansson, 2002). domized or nonrandomized controlled trials of interventions
Children with handwriting difficulties often require more that could be used by an occupational therapist to improve
time to complete assignments, which affects their academic written output (printing or writing) among school-aged chil-
performance (Berninger, Mizokawa, & Bragg, 1991). Com- dren identified as having difficulties with handwriting. Stud-
position is affected by handwriting difficulties; children may ies were excluded from this review if they did not include a
run into increasing difficulties as they progress in school and, control group, addressed the effectiveness of interventions that
due to handwriting problems, shy away from expressing what would not be used by an occupational therapist (e.g., medica-
they know (Medwell & Wray, 2008). When students demon- tion), examined interventions designed to improve typewrit-
strate poor handwriting legibility, and classroom efforts to ten or computer-generated output, or included children who
improve legibility do not result in substantial change, teachers did not demonstrate problems with written output. As well,
frequently refer students to occupational therapy (Hammer- studies were excluded if they were published in a language
schmidt & Sudsawad, 2004). In fact, handwriting remediation other than French or English.
is one of the most common reasons for referral of school-aged Electronic searches were conducted up to July 2008 and
children to occupational therapy (Feder, Majnemer, & Synnes, then updated in January 2010 using the following well-rec-
2000). ognized sources: MEDLINE from 1950, CINAHL from 1982,
Occupational therapists offer a variety of services to Cochrane CENTRAL, Education Abstracts at Scholar’s Portal,
improve handwriting. Therapy may be provided to the child, Education: A SAGE Full-Text Collection, Educational Admin-
or consultation may be provided to the teacher (Cramm, Pol- istration Abstracts, EMBASE from 1980, ERIC (education
lock, Dennis, Subramaniam, & Carkner, 2009; Hutton, 2009). database), PsycINFO from 1806, and SPECTRE (Campbell
In addition, the approach taken may focus on remediat- Collaboration). Search terms were identified by a reference
ing potential causes of handwriting problems (e.g., impaired librarian with extensive experience in systematic reviews and
kinaesthesia), or concentrate on the activity of handwriting are available upon request from the authors.
itself (Sugden & Chambers, 1998). While different theoretical Search results were independently examined by two of
approaches to handwriting intervention are recognized, Feder the authors (MH and ME). Articles were retrieved if either
and her colleagues (Feder et al., 2000) found that therapists reviewer deemed that the title or abstract indicated that the
surveyed from across Canada reported using an “eclectic” ori- article might describe the results of a study that met the inclu-
entation that included sensorimotor (90%), perceptual-motor sion criteria. Retrieved articles were reviewed for inclusion by
(74%), motor-learning (68%), cognitive training (64%), bio- both of these authors and decisions regarding inclusion were
mechanical (64%), sensory integrative (50%), and neurode- made by consensus. The first author (MH) then extracted the
velopmental (42%) approaches. A survey of occupational following information: type of design, number of participants
therapists’ interventions in the United States demonstrated a in each group, age and gender of participants, details regard-
similar diversity of approaches (Woodward & Swinth, 2002). ing the intervention (procedure, frequency, duration), out-
In a review of the literature, Bonney (1992) suggested come measures and post-test results. This information was
that occupational therapists must be aware of “the body of verified by the second author (ME). Following this, extracted
knowledge on handwriting performance and intervention” data were entered into RevMan version 5 (Review Manager,
(p. 7) in order to provide occupational therapy appropriate 2008), which was then used to calculate standardized effect
for each child’s unique difficulties, and urged that systematic sizes of the mean difference between the post-test scores of the
investigation be undertaken to examine the area of hand- treatment and control groups (Cohen’s d) and 95% confidence
writing assessment and intervention (p. 14). Feder, Racine, intervals. Finally, to examine study quality, risk of bias was
and Majnemer (2008) carried out a narrative review of the evaluated by assessing the adequacy of sequence generation,
evidence and concluded that “many of the studies reviewed allocation concealment, whether the study was free of selective
reported that handwriting intervention resulted in significant reporting, and whether the assessor was masked (Higgins &
gains for most children, regardless of the treatment duration or Green, 2009).
approach used” (p. E69). The objective of the current study was
to systematically review the effectiveness of interventions that
occupational therapists may use for children with handwriting Results
difficulties to both update and better quantify the conclusions From the original search, 296 studies were identified. Of
from this narrative review. these studies, 34 were retrieved for review, and 25 were later
excluded. The updated search produced 24 articles, of which 2
were retrieved for review and included. Studies were most often
Methods excluded on the basis of methodology, specifically absence of a
In a systematic review of effectiveness, such as the one reported nontreatment control group (Table 1).
here, one methodically locates and examines, using explicitly Of the 11 included trials, 9 were randomized controlled
defined criteria, the results of research regarding the effective- trials (including 1 randomized cluster trial) and 2 were non-

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


14 Revue canadienne d’ergothérapie février 2011 78(1)
Table 1
Excluded Studies

Reason For Exclusion


Study (Author, Year) Data Inadequate No Non-treat- Single-subject Children without No handwriting Medication- Computer-
to allow use ment Control design handwriting dif- outcome based based
ficulties included intervention intervention

Baroody & Kaufman (1993) X


Burkhalter & Wright (2002) X
Christensen (2004) X X X
Chwirka, Gurney, & Burtner (2002) X X
Graham, Harris, & Fink (2000) X
Hall & Case-Smith (2007) X
Handley-More, Deitz,
Billingsley, & Coggins (2003) X X
Harris & Livesey (1992) X
Hartley & Salzwedel (1980) X
Hirsh & Niedermeyer (1973) X
Karlsdottir (1996)
Krzesni (1971) X
Lerer, Lerer, & Artner (1977) X
Maybee (1988) X
McFadden (1998) X
McGarrigle & Nelson (2006) X
Peterson & Nelson (2003) X
Ratzon, Efraim, & Bart (2007) X

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


Roberts & Samuels (1993) X
Schilling, Washington, Billingsley, & Deitz (2003) X
Shen, Kang, & Wu (2003) X
Ste-Marie, Clark, Findlay, & Latimer (2004) X
Tallal, Chase, Russell, & Schmitt (1986) X
Tam, Reid, Naumann, & O’Keefe (2002) X X
Tucha & Lange (2005) X

Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy February 2011 78(1) 15


Hoy et al.
Table 2
Included Studies—Description
Hoy et al.

Sample Doseage

N
of

Study Design Treatment

Age/
Grade
session

% Boys
sessions
sessions

Time per

Total # of
Frequency

Experimental Group Experimental condition—Writing club


53 81 Gr. 4 In groups of 6-14 children
Children gave secret password, shared riddles and jokes (5 min).
Control Group Warm-up activities to promote automatic letter writing and
Berninger et RCT 41 71 Gr. 4 automatic phoneme-grapheme correspondences in spelling 56 60 min 2/wk for

16 Revue canadienne d’ergothérapie


al. (2006) (cluster) (15 min). 7 mo
Teacher-directed composing activities (“Writers` work”) (25
min).
Word-play activities (10 min).
Control condition—regular classroom instruction only.

février 2011
Total % boys: 76 Experimental conditions
Exp. (Motor Imitation) Motor imitation: copying teacher’s movement.

78(1)
24 U Gr. 1
Exp. (Visual Cue) Visual cue: following arrows next to letter.
24 U Gr. 1
Exp. (Memory) Memory retrieval: looking at letter, covering then printing.
Berninger et RCT 24 U Gr. 1 24 20 min 2/wk
al. (1997)
Exp. (Visual Cue + Memory) Visual cue + memory retrieval.
24 U Gr. 1
Exp. (Copy) Copy: from model, no cuing.
24 U Gr. 1
Control (Phonological Awareness) Control condition

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


24 U Gr. 1 Phonological awareness training.
NB: All conditions were carried out by children working in groups of 3.

Study 1 Study 1:
Experimental Group Experimental condition—EMG and visual imagery for relaxation
16 100 Elem. and homework exercises
Control Group 5 two-minute EMG readings recorded during first 10 minutes
Carter & Russell RCT 16 100 Elem. of session, followed by completion of handwriting exercises 10 + HW 50 min 2/wk for
(1985) during remaining 50 minutes of session. At home, listened to 5 wks
prerecorded 10-minute audiotape using passive muscle relax-
ation and visual imagery and then completed hand-writing
Participants in Study 1 and Study 2 homework exercises.
were boys classified as having a Control condition - No intervention. Continued ....
learning disability and attending
special resource classes
Table 2 Included Studies—Description (continued)

Study 2 Study 2:
Experimental Group Experimental condition—EMG only
15 100 Elem. 6 weeks of EMG + handwriting exercises (No visualization or
Control Group homework).
15 100 Elem. Control condition
No intervention.

Experimental Group Experimental condition—School-based direct occupational


29 83 8.6 therapy
Handwriting practice or activities designed specifically to Mean Mean Approx
Case-Smith NRCT Control Group
improve handwriting were implemented in 77% of the 16.4 32.2 2/m
(2002)
sessions (eclectic approaches). Range Range
Control group— 10-25 25-49
No occupational therapy min

Experimental group—Occupational therapy Mean


Activities for improving balance, fine and gross motor skills,
Experimental Group
Densem RCT responsiveness to touch, integration of both sides of the OT
19 79 8.1
body, and motor movement planning. 17.4 60 min 4.5 mo
et al. (1989) Control group—Physical Education
Control Group
Fitness activities, teaching and practice of physical skills and PE
17 82 7.1
games. 16

Experimental group—Sensorimotor treatment
10 minute sessions each of visual perception, visual-motor
68% boys
Denton RCT integration, proprioception/kinesthesia, and in-hand
Mean age: 9.0 (1.1)
et al. (2006) manipulation activities. 20 40 4/wk
Experimental group—Therapeutic practice over 5 wks
Handwriting practice books prepared for each participant,

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


including worksheets to practice, dictated and copied
handwriting and writing from memory, and real-life writing
practices.
Control group
No intervention

Experimental Group Experimental group—Relaxation


10 50 Gr 4 Listened to relaxation tape then copied a 6-line paragraph 9 U 3/wk
Jackson & RCT Control Group Control group— for 3
Hughes 10 50 Gr 4 Listened to verbal instruction on handwriting then copied on a 6 wks
(1978) line paragraph

Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy February 2011 78(1) 17


Hoy et al.
Table 2 Included Studies—Description (continued)
Sample Doseage
Hoy et al.

Study Design Treatment

N
of

Age/
Grade
session

% Boys
sessions
sessions

Time per

Total # of
Frequency

Experimental Group Experimental Group


Experimental group
18 78 8.9
In groups, children instructed in guided self-correction of letter 48 30 min 2/wk for
Jongmans NRCT Control Group formation with a model. 6 mo
et al. (2003) (cluster)
6 100 9.7 Control group
No intervention.

18 Revue canadienne d’ergothérapie



Kinaesthetic training Experimental group—Kinaesthetic training
15 U Gr. 1 2 training tasks for position and movement of arms in space.
Handwriting Practice Experimental group—Handwriting practice
Sudsawad RCT 6 30 min 6 school days
15 U Gr. 1 Children copied letters, words, and sentences. Verbal and visual
et al. (2002) over 2 wks

février 2011
Control Group feedback provided.
15 U Gr. 1 Control Group

No intervention

78(1)
Task oriented Experimental group—Task oriented
13 100 Gr. 2-4 Handwriting practice and feedback 8+
Weintraub RCT Sensorimotor Mneumonics, self-evaluation, word games, functional activities
et al. (2009) 13 77 Gr. 2-4 (e.g., writing birthday party invitations). 10 min
Control Group Experimental group—Sensorimotor HW 60 min 1/wk
17 100 Gr. 2-4 Preparatory activities (posture, coordination, stability, fine motor). Each
Kinesthetic, tactile, and auditory feedback. Mneumonics, self- day
evaluation.
Control group
No intervention

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


Experimental Group (Cognitive) Experimental group—Cognitive intervention
24 U Gr. 2 Alphabet warm-up: naming and identifying letters.
Zwicker & RCT Experimental Group (Multisensory) modeling, discussion of letters, talking through forming the letter,
Hadwin 24 U Gr. 2 self-evaluation. 10 30 min 1/wk
(2009) Control Group Experimental group—Multisensory intervention
24 U Gr. 2 Letter groups named and formation demonstrated.
71% of total sample were boys. Practice with enhanced sensory feedback (e.g., writing on sand-
paper).
Children copied each letter on regular lined paper and a work-
sheet page.
Control group
No intervention

Note. Ages given in years unless otherwise stated; U = unknown; Gr. = grade; elem = elementary school; HW = homework
Hoy et al.

randomized controlled trials (including 1 nonrandomized significant improvement over the course of 24 sessions.
cluster trial). Within the included studies, a variety of inter- In a subsequent study, Berninger et al. (2006) combined
ventions to remediate handwriting difficulties were examined. these forms of practice with a “handwriting club” interven-
These interventions were categorized as (1) relaxation and tion. During 60-minute sessions, twice per week over seven
practice with or without electromyogram (EMG) biofeedback, months, members of the club carried out exercises designed
(2) sensory-based training without handwriting practice, and to engage them as writers (e.g., students adopted a nom de
(3) handwriting-based practice (Table 2). plume, and composed their own stories). There was a small
Results of these interventions compared with control intervention effect that approached significance on the WJ-R
conditions are presented in Table 3. The effects of practice 3 Writing Samples (effect size: 0.32; 95% CI: -0.09, 0.73). In
following relaxation exercises were examined in two studies. addition, students who had taken part in the club intervention
Carter and Russell (1985) reported a statistically significant were almost 3 times more likely to pass a statewide high stakes
difference between the change scores of handwriting qual- writing examination than students in the control group (odds
ity for children receiving EMG biofeedback for muscle relax- ratio: 2.75; 95% CI: 1.06, 7.14), an effect that was statistically
ation, visualization exercises for relaxation recorded on an significant.
audiotape, and handwriting homework practice. However, Weintraub et al. (2009) observed no change in speed, but
an effect size for the post-test differences between the inter- a significant improvement in legibility, following eight weekly
vention and control groups could not be calculated due to sessions of task-oriented handwriting practice supplemented
absence of post-test data. It should be noted that this study by 10 minutes of daily homework practice. In contrast, Jong-
included a substantial handwriting practice component (50 mans et al. (2003) obtained no significant improvement over
minutes, twice per week over five weeks plus nightly home- 48 sessions of self-guided correction, Sudsawad et al. (2002)
work). Jackson and Hughes (1978) also studied the effects of found no improvement in legibility or speed following six
relaxation training on handwriting quality and speed; results sessions of handwriting practice carried out over a two-week
were not significant for this intervention. Notably, in this period, and Zwicker and Hadwin (2009) saw no significant
study both experimental and control participants carried out improvement over 10 weekly sessions of cognitively focused
some handwriting practice. handwriting practice. Case-Smith (2002) found no significant
Sensory-based training without handwriting prac- improvement over an average of 16 sessions of handwriting
tice interventions were examined in three studies (Densem, practice (average of 2 session per month, focus of intervention
Nuthall, Bushnell, & Horn, 1989; Denton, Cope, & Moser, not specified).
2006; Sudsawad, Trombly, Henderson, & Tickle-Degnen, When differences in post-test data between interven-
2002). The effect sizes were small (Cohen, 1988) and not sig- tion and control groups were reviewed according to the dose
nificant for improvement in handwriting legibility, speed, of treatment received, a trend was noted in terms of the rela-
or proportion of errors. In fact, in some cases, children who tionship between number of sessions and intervention effec-
received this intervention did more poorly than children in the tiveness. All studies with fewer than two practice sessions per
control conditions. week and fewer than 20 practice sessions in total (homework
Interventions that included handwriting were examined included) demonstrated ineffective results.
in seven studies. These studies tested interventions with a sen- In terms of risk of bias, potential threats to validity that
sorimotor focus (Weintraub, Yinon, Hirsch, & Parush, 2009; arose often in both the randomized controlled trials and non-
Zwicker & Hadwin, 2009) or a cognitive focus (Berninger et randomized controlled trials included unclear sequence gen-
al., 2006; Berninger et al., 1997; Jongmans, Linthorst-Bakker, eration and allocation concealment. Incomplete data was a
Westenberg, Smits-Engelsman, 2003; Sudsawad et al., 2002; potential issue in only 2 of the 11 included studies, in all of the
Weintraub et al., 2009; Zwicker & Hadwin). In one study of 11 studies there was avoidance of selective reporting of data,
a direct school-based intervention, the focus was not iden- and in 8 of the 11 studies the authors clearly stated that the out-
tified (Case-Smith, 2002) but likely varied from therapist to come assessors were masked. The quality of the included stud-
therapist. ies was therefore generally good. The quality of studies did not
In the two studies that examined handwriting practice vary enough to warrant separate analysis by level of quality.
with a sensorimotor focus, students received enhanced kin- A post hoc analysis of effect sizes using change scores
aesthetic, tactile, or auditory feedback while practising letter with imputed standard deviations of change was carried out.
formation. Following this intervention, children in the treat- In only two cases did this change the significance of the results
ment group did no better than those in the control group in (Case-Smith, 2002; Jongmans et al., 2003). The interventions
terms of legibility and speed (Weintraub et al., 2009; Zwicker in both studies included substantial amounts of handwriting
& Hadwin, 2009). practice.
In the studies with handwriting practice using a cognitive
focus, students received instruction to help them think their
way through letter formation and self-correction. Berninger et Discussion
al. (1997) examined the impact of different types of group prac- Nine randomized, controlled trials and two nonrandomized
tice on the number of letters formed correctly in 60 seconds. controlled trials provided the evidence in this systematic
Motor imitation and practice writing letters from memory review. These results seem to clearly demonstrate that a prac-
with or without visual cues (e.g., direction arrows) produced tice component is necessary for handwriting improvement.

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy February 2011 78(1) 19
Table 3
Evidence Table for Studies of Handwriting Interventions for Children with Disabilities and Handwriting Difficulties
Hoy et al.

Risk of bias

Study Comparison Outcome Standardized effect size


Adequate Sequence
Generation
Allocation Concealment
Incomplete Outcome
Data a Concern
Free of Selective
Reporting
Outcome Assessors
Masked

20 Revue canadienne d’ergothérapie


Berninger et Handwriting and Composition WJ-R 3 Writing Samples 0.32 [-0.09, 0.73] U U N Y U
al. (2006) Club vs. no treatment Met the state standard for writing on the 2.75 [1.06, 7.14]*
state-wide test: (odds ratio)

février 2011
Berninger et Number of correct words/60 seconds Copy1 0.93 [0.34, 1.53]*
al. (1997) Motor imitation vs. control Number of correct letters/60 seconds Dictation2 0.83 [0.24, 1.43]*

78(1)
WJ Writing Fluency 0.21 [-0.36, 0.78]

Number of correct letters/60 seconds Copy1 0.55 [-0.03, 1.13]


Visual cues vs. control Number of correct letters/60 seconds Dictation2 0.39 [-0.18, 0.97]
WJ Writing Fluency 0.22 [-0.34, 0.79]

Number of correct letters/60 seconds Copy1 0.70 [0.12, 1.29]* U U N Y Y


Memory practice vs. control Number of correct letters/60 seconds Dictation2 1.51 [0.86, 2.16]*
WJ Writing Fluency 0.38 [-0.19, 0.95]

Visual cues and memory Number of correct letters/60 seconds Copy1 1.17 [0.55, 1.79]*
practice vs. control Number of correct letters/60 seconds Dictation2 1.69 [1.03, 2.36]*

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


WJ Writing Fluency 0.93 [0.33, 1.53]*

Copying practice without Number of correct letters/60 seconds Copy1 0.58 [0.00, 1.16]
cuing vs. control Number of correct letters/60 seconds 0.13 [-0.44, 0.70]
Dictation (words)2 0.23 [-0.34, 0.80]
WJ Writing Fluency

Carter & EMG and visual imagery for Handwriting legibility3 Not estimable (no standard
Russell (1985) relaxation and homework vs. deviation provided)
no treatment

EMG and visual imagery for Handwriting legibility3 Not estimable (no standard U Y Y Y Y
relaxation vs. no treatment deviation provided)
Continued ...
Table 3
Evidence Table for Studies of Handwriting Interventions for Children with Disabilities and Handwriting Difficulties (continued)

Case-Smith School-based occupational Total letter legibility4 -0.26 [-1.01, 0.49]


(2002) therapy (direct service) vs. no N N N Y N
treatment Speed4 -0.02 [-0.77, 0.73]

Densem et al. Sensory integration interven- Proportion of letter errors5 0.22 [-0.44, 0.88]
(1989) tion vs. no treatment
U U N Y Y
Physical education interven- Proportion of letter errors 5 0.00 [-0.65, 0.65]
tion vs. no treatment

Denton et al. Sensorimotor intervention vs. Handwriting quality- from memory6 -0.24 [-1.08, 0.60]
(2006) no treatment Handwriting quality- dictated6 0.07 [-0.77, 0.91]

Therapeutic practice v.s no Handwriting quality- copied6 -0.14 [-0.98, 0.70] U U N Y Y


treatment Handwriting quality- from memory6 0.42 [-0.42, 1.25]
Handwriting quality- dictated6 0.99 [0.11, 1.88]*
Handwriting quality- copied6 0.98 [0.10, 1.86]*

Jackson & Relaxation training vs. no Handwriting quality7 2.21 [1.01, 3.40]* U U N Y Y
Hughes (1978) treatment

Jongmans et Guided self-correction of letter Handwriting quality8: 0.74 [-0.21, 1.70] N N N Y Y


al. (2003) formation with a model vs. no Speed8 0.20 [-0.72, 1.13]
treatment

Sudsawad et Kinaesthetic training vs. no Total letter legibility4 -0.26 [-0.98, 0.46]
al. (2002) treatment Total word legibility4 -0.40 [-1.12, 0.33]
Speed4 0.38 [-0.35, 1.10] U U N Y Y
Handwriting practice vs. no Total letter legibility4 -0.71[-1.43, 0.04]
treatment Total word legibility4 -0.69 [-1.43, 0.05]

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


Speed4 0.02 [-0.69, 0.74]

Weintraub et Task oriented approach vs no Speed9 0.03 [-0.48, 0.54]
al. (2009) treatment Overall legibility9 1.14 [0.36, 1.93]* U U Y Y U

Multisensory approach vs no Speed9 -0.04 [-0.69, 0.62]
treatment Overall legibility9 0.48 [-0.18, 1.15]

Zwicker & Cognitive approach vs no Total Letter Legibility4 -0.07 [-0.64, 0.49] U U N Y Y
Hadwin (2009) treatment
Multisensory approach vs no Total Letter Legibility4 0.06 [-0.50, 0.63]
treatment

Note. Standardized effect size is the standard mean difference between post test scores unless otherwise stated. 1Number of letters correctly copied from a text in 90 seconds; 2Number of letters correctly written from dictation (26 letters of

Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy February 2011 78(1) 21


alphabet randomly presented); 3Wide Range Achievement Handwriting subtest; 4Evaluation Tool of Children’s Handwriting (ETCH); 5Proportion of errors in writing name and address and copying a writing sample, Test of Handwriting Skills
Hoy et al.

(THS); 7Assessed quality of a copied 6-line paragraph; 8 Concise Assessment Scale for Children’s Handwriting (BHK); 9 Hebrew Handwriting Evaluation (HHE). * Statistically significant (p< 0.05).
Hoy et al.

That is, activities that target writing-related performance com- ing (Berninger et al., 2006; Berninger et al., 1997). Interven-
ponents, such as in-hand manipulation or kinaesthetic aware- tions including assigned homework practice also demonstrated
ness, without providing opportunity for handwriting practice, effectiveness (Carter & Russell, 1985; Weintraub et al., 2009).
do not appear to be effective in improving written expression. Second, it is paramount that building motivation for writ-
Practice of handwriting is required to improve this skill. These ing practice be incorporated into the treatment plan and strat-
findings confirm the results of the previous narrative review egy. Ryan and Deci’s (2000a) Self-Determination Theory may
(Feder et al., 2008) by demonstrating that not all interventions provide a useful framework for occupational therapists work-
are equal. Furthermore, they extend these results by demon- ing with students who, for multiple reasons, have varying lev-
strating the need for a minimum amount and frequency of els of motivation for handwriting. This empirically supported
practice. theory underscores the importance of encouraging the child
An approach to handwriting remediation that focuses to move toward integrated motivation, that is, a point at which
on handwriting practice is consistent with the theory of motor he or she values the activity for what it can do for them. This
learning, a theory that is increasingly key to occupational ther- is carried out by ensuring that intervention strategies allow for
apy intervention. Based on work by Schmidt and Lee (2005), the development of competence through optimal challenges
Zwicker and Harris (2009) described motor learning as “a set while supporting both the child’s sense of relatedness through
of processes associated with practice or experience leading to development of good relationships with the teacher and other
relatively permanent changes in the capability for movement” students and his or her sense of autonomy through allowing
(p. 302). As noted above, the results of this review demonstrate choice in the activity (Ryan & Deci, 2000b). Berninger et al.’s
that handwriting intervention approaches that do not include (2006) writers’ club demonstrated effective use of such strate-
a handwriting practice component do not result in changes in gies through providing effective practice strategies, supporting
handwriting performance. Furthermore, the findings of our the development of relationships through shared activity, and
systematic review indicate that a minimum number of prac- allowing the children choice regarding their writing personas
tice-based intervention or intervention and homework ses- and writing topics.
sions are required for handwriting to improve. These findings In addition, it will be important for occupational thera-
are consistent with the current definition and understanding pists to consult with teachers, parents, and students to deter-
of motor-learning theory as put forward by Zwicker and Har- mine whether handwriting practice should be the key focus of
ris (2009). intervention or whether other methods of producing written
It is unclear from our review whether this handwrit- work (e.g., keyboarding, scribing, voice-activated technolo-
ing practice component requires a cognitive or sensorimotor gies) should be explored or expanded upon; the question of
component or some combination of the two. However, in the which written output strategy would be more advantageous to
one study with adequate practice time that examined interven- attaining the child’s academic goals at that point in time, as
tions that focused on either cognitive or sensorimotor-based well as in the future, would need to be discussed. Consider-
activities, significant results were achieved only in the cogni- ations may include the child’s interest in pursuing this prac-
tively focused intervention (Weintraub et al., 2009). Interest- tice and the opportunity cost of doing so. That is, how will the
ingly, Zwicker and Hadwin (2009) reported an improvement time spent in practicing this handwriting detract from other
in Grade 2 children using the cognitive focus whereas Grade valued occupations (such as school work, extracurricular
1 students demonstrated better results when the sensorimo- activities, and family time)? Additional important consider-
tor-focused intervention was applied. This might indicate a ations include the present and future availability of alternative
developmental or maturational effect that may need to be con- methods of written communication. Occupational therapists
sidered when selecting the type of handwriting practice used are often involved in recommending alternative methods for
in an intervention. the production of written work (Freeman, MacKinnon, Miller,
Only Berninger’s (Berninger et al., 2006; Berninger et 2004; Marr & Dimeo, 2006; Rigby & Schwellnus, 1999). The
al., 1997) studies demonstrated any improvement in speed of results of this study may be helpful to therapists as they weigh
handwriting. Both of these studies evaluated interventions with the potential impact of remediation.
a relatively high number of practice sessions. Both legibility Finally, given the importance of handwriting practice
and speed are important concerns when examining the func- underlined by this review, one might ask whether occupa-
tionality of children’s handwriting (Amundson, 1992; Feder & tional therapy time may be better spent advocating for more
Majnemer, 2007). It is reasonable to believe that improvements handwriting instruction and practice for all children. Indeed,
in letter quality may precede improvements in speed, the latter a number of writers have pointed out that curriculum changes
requiring additional practice time. have dramatically lessened the amount of writing instruction
The clinical implications of the association of a minimum and practice. In many cases students who have not sufficiently
number of handwriting practice sessions to effect change in mastered basic writing tasks are forced to move on too quickly
handwriting are multiple. First, to ensure effective interven- to more advanced written assignments, and this is likely the
tion, occupational therapists need to determine ways to ensure cause of most handwriting problems in this population (Asher,
adequate practice time despite funding limitations. Potential 2006; Donica, 2010; Karlsdottir & Stefansson, 2002). It may be
solutions include group rather than individual sessions. Two of better to advocate for more practice for all children and inten-
the three studies that examined the effects of an intervention sive practice for those who continue to have problems with
provided in groups demonstrate positive changes in handwrit- handwriting, with therapeutic intervention offered only for

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


22 Revue canadienne d’ergothérapie février 2011 78(1)
Hoy et al.

those who have difficulty even after intensive practice (Feder in conjunction with educators examining the effectiveness of
& Majnemer, 2007). providing more handwriting practice to all students as a pre-
Such an alternative approach to the problem of handwrit- ventative measure against the development of handwriting
ing is more closely aligned to a consultative model of occu- problems appear to be warranted.
pational therapy. Such a model may be most effective when
occupational therapists are integrated into the school rather
than coming in from an outside agency and providing teach- Summary
ers with recommendations that they perceive as unfeasible This systematic review offers convincing evidence that hand-
in their classrooms (Cahill, 2010; Hutton, 2009; Wehrmann, writing interventions that involve handwriting practice and
Chiu, Reid, & Sinclair, 2006). that include practice at a minimum twice per week for a total
This systematic review of handwriting interventions was of at least 20 sessions are effective for improving handwriting
carried out with a number of features to ensure validity. These outcomes in children with handwriting difficulties. Handwrit-
included: a priori selection criteria, a search designed by a ing interventions that do not provide handwriting practice
research librarian with systematic review expertise, indepen- have been shown to be ineffective.
dent selection of articles, verification of extraction, and quality
assessment of included studies. However, three main limita- Key messages
tions remain. • To be effective, interventions must allow for handwrit-
The first limitation was that few studies included in this ing practice at a minimum twice per week for a total of
review considered individualization of the intervention to at least 20 sessions.
respond to each child’s specific problem areas (e.g., visual per- • Improvements in handwriting speed appear to require
ception, motor planning, etc.). However, given the effective- even higher levels of practice.
ness of interventions involving higher levels of practice and the • Subsequent handwriting intervention research should
ineffectiveness of interventions that addressed performance examine the effectiveness of programs designed to
components relating to handwriting only, practice may be maximize practice time.
more critical than specific identification of underlying issues.
The second limitation was that the sample sizes of the
included studies tended to be small. Small samples lead to
greater variability in outcome scores, leading to fewer statisti- References
cally significant findings even in the presence of large effect Amundson, S. J. (1992). Handwriting: Evaluation and intervention
sizes. However, it should be noted that effect sizes were often in school settings. In J. Case-Smith & C. Pehoski (Eds.), Devel-
small to moderate in the included studies, indicating that opment of hand skills in the child (pp. 63-78). Rockville, MD:
increasing the size of samples may not have had an impact on American Occupational Therapy Association.
the clinical significance of the results. Asher, A. V. (2006). Handwriting instruction in elementary schools.
The third limitation was that post-test scores rather than American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 60, 461-471.
change scores were used to determine the effect sizes. This was Baroody, A. J., & Kaufman, L. (1993). The case of Lee: Assessing and
done because use of change scores requires that assumptions remedying a numeral-writing difficulty. Teaching Exceptional
be made regarding the standard deviation of the change scores, Children, 25(3), 14-16.
and effect sizes can vary greatly depending on the assumptions Berninger, V. W., Mizokawa, D. T., & Bragg, R. (1991). Theory-
made, leading to unstable estimates (Higgins & Green, 2009). based diagnosis and remediation of writing disabilities.
Use of post-intervention scores may lead to more conserva- Journal of Educational Psychology, 29, 57-79. doi:10.1016/0022-
tive estimates of effect, particularly when pretest score means 4405(91)90016-K
are different for the intervention and control groups. How- Berninger, V. W., Rutberg, J. E., Abbott, R. D., Garcia, N., Anderson-
ever, post hoc analyses done using change scores and imputed Youngstrom, M., Brooks, A., & Fulton, C. (2006). Tier 1 and tier
standard deviations revealed no findings that contradicted the 2 early intervention for handwriting and composing. Journal of
overall conclusion of the review. School Psychology, 44, 3-30. doi: 10.1016/ j.jsp.2005.12.003
Given our findings, we disagree that future research Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K. B., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Rogan, L.
should further compare different modalities included in W., Brooks, A., & Graham, S. (1997). Treatment of handwriting
practice-based intervention (Feder et al., 2008; Woodward & problems in beginning writers: Transfer from handwriting to
Swinth, 2002). Rather, since it appears that amount of practice composition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 652-666.
is the essential ingredient for effective outcomes, we believe Bonney, M. (1992). Understanding and assessing handwriting diffi-
future research should examine interventions with enhanced culty: Perspectives from the literature. Australian Occupational
practice time using delivery methods that maximize practice Therapy Journal, 39, 7-15.
time (such as group intervention, inclusion of homework) Burkhalter, B. B., & Wright, J. P. (1984). Handwriting performance
while promoting student motivation. As well, outcomes with and without transparent overlays. Journal of Experimental
should give consideration to both speed and legibility results. Education, 52, 132.
Exploration of the impact of intensive practice time on other Cahill, S. M. (2010). Contributions made by occupational therapists
activities, using both subjective and objective data, should also in RtI: A pilot study. Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, &
be considered. Finally, population-based studies carried out Early Intervention, 3, 3-10. doi: 10.1080/19411241003683771

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy February 2011 78(1) 23
Hoy et al.

Canadian Association of Occupational Therapists. (2007). Profile of lems in beginning writers. Journal of Educational Psychology,
occupational therapy practice in Canada. Ottawa, ON: CAOT 92, 620-633.
Publications ACE. Hall, L., & Case-Smith, J. (2007). The effect of sound-based interven-
Carter, J. L., & Russell, H. L. (1985). Use of EMG biofeedback proce- tion on children with sensory processing disorders and visual-
dures with learning disabled children in a clinical and an edu- motor delays. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 61,
cational setting. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 18, 213-216. 209-215. doi: 10.5014/ajot.61.2.209
doi: 10.1177/002221948501800406 Hammerschmidt, S. L., & Sudsawad, P. (2004). Teachers’ survey on
Case-Smith, J. (2002). Effectiveness of school-based occupational problems with handwriting: Referral, evaluation, and outcomes.
therapy intervention on handwriting. American Journal of American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 58, 185-192.
Occupational Therapy, 56, 17-25. Handley-More, D., Deitz, J., Billingsley, F. F., & Coggins, T. E. (2003).
Case-Smith, J. (2005). Occupational therapy for children (5th ed.). St. Facilitating written work using computer word processing and
Louis, MO: Elsevier Mosby. word prediction. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 57,
Christensen, C. A. (2004). Relationship between orthographic-motor 139-151.
integration and computer use for the production of creative Harris, S. J., & Livesey, D. J. (1992). Improving handwriting through
and well-structured written text. British Journal of Educational kinaesthetic sensitivity practice. Australian Occupational Ther-
Psychology, 74, 551-564. doi: 10.1348/ 0007099042376373 apy Journal, 39, 23-27.
Chwirka, B., Gurney, B., & Burtner, P. A. (2002). Keyboarding and Hartley, S. T., & Salzwedel, K. D. (1980). Behavioral writing for an
visual-motor skills in elementary students: A pilot study. autistic-like child. Academic Therapy, 16(1), 101-110. doi:
Occupational Therapy in Health Care, 16, 39-51. doi: 10.1080/ 10.1177/105345128001600111
J003v16n02_03 Higgins, J. P. T., & Green, S. (Eds.). (2009). Cochrane handbook for
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). systematic reviews of interventions, [Version 5.0.2, updated Sep-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. tember 2009]. Retrieved from http://www.cochrane-handbook.
Cramm, H., Pollock, N., Dennis, D., Subramaniam, K., & Carkner, M. org/
(2009). Occupational therapy with school-aged children. Occu- Hirsch, E., & Niedermeyer, F. C. (1973). The effects of tracing prompts
pational Therapy Now, 11(6), 24-26. and discrimination training on kindergarten handwriting per-
Densem, J. F., Nuthall, G. A., Bushnell, J., & Horn, J. (1989). Effective- formance. Journal of Educational Research, 67, 81-86.
ness of a sensory integrative therapy program for children with Hutton, E. (2009). Occupational therapy in mainstream primary
perceptual-motor deficits. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, schools: an evaluation of a pilot project. British Journal of Occu-
221-229. doi: 10.1177/002221948902200405 pational Therapy, 72, 308-313.
Denton, P. L., Cope, S., & Moser, C. (2006). The effects of sensorimo- Jackson, K., & Hughes, H. H. (1978). Effects of relaxation training on
tor-based intervention versus therapeutic practice on improv- cursive handwriting of fourth grade students. Perceptual and
ing handwriting performance in 6- to 11-year-old children. Motor Skills, 47, 707-712.
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 60, 16-27. Jongmans, M. J., Linthorst-Bakker, E., Westenberg, Y., & Smits-
Donica, D. (2010). A historical journey through the development of Engelsman, B. C. M. (2003). Use of a task-oriented self-instruc-
handwriting instruction (Part 2): The occupational therapists’ tion method to support children in primary school with poor
role. Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, & Early Interven- handwriting quality and speed. Human Movement Science, 22,
tion, 3, 32-53. doi: 10.1080/ 19411241003683995 549-566. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2003.09.009
Elwood, M. (2007). Critical appraisal of epidemiological studies and Karlsdottir, R. (1996). Development of cursive handwriting. Percep-
clinical trials (3rd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. tual & Motor Skills, 82, 659-673.
Erhardt, R. P., & Meade, V. (2005). Improving handwriting without Karlsdottir, R., & Stefansson, T. (2002). Problems in developing func-
teaching handwriting: The consultative clinical reasoning pro- tional handwriting. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 623-662.
cess. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 52, 199-210. doi: Kephart, N. C. (1971). The slow learner in the classroom (2nd ed.).
10.1111/j.1440-1630.2005.00505.x Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.
Feder, K. P., & Majnemer, A. (2007). Handwriting development, com- Krzesni, J. S. (1971). Effect of different writing tools and paper on
petency, and intervention. Developmental Medicine and Child performance of the third grader. Elementary English, 48(7),
Neurology, 49, 312-317. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00312.x 821-824.
Feder, K., Majnemer, A., & Synnes, A. (2000). Handwriting: Current Lerer, R. J., Lerer, M. P., & Artner, J. (1977). The effects of meth-
trends in occupational therapy practice. Canadian Journal of ylphenidate on the handwriting of children with minimal brain
Occupational Therapy, 67, 197-204. dysfunction. Journal of Pediatrics, 91, 127-132.
Feder, K. P., Racine, M. B., & Majnemer, A. (2008). A review of hand- Mabee, W. S. (1988). The effects of academic positive practice on
writing performance and interventions: Does remediation cursive letter writing. Education & Treatment of Children, 11,
work? Israeli Journal of Occupational Therapy, 17(3), 69-88. 143-148.
Freeman, A. R., MacKinnon, J. R., & Miller, L. T. (2004). Assistive Marr, D., & Dimeo, S. B. (2006). Outcomes associated with a summer
technology and handwriting problems: What do occupational handwriting course for elementary students. American Journal
therapists recommend? Canadian Journal of Occupational of Occupational Therapy, 60, 10-15. doi: 10.5014/ajot.60.1.10
Therapy, 71, 150-160. McFadden, T. U. (1998). The immediate effects of pictographic rep-
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Fink, B. (2000). Is handwriting causally resentation on children’s narratives. Child Language Teaching &
related to learning to write? Treatment of handwriting prob- Therapy, 14, 51-67. doi: 10.1177/026565909801400103

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


24 Revue canadienne d’ergothérapie février 2011 78(1)
Hoy et al.

McGarrigle, J., & Nelson, A. (2006). Evaluating a school skills pro- reading and writing disorders in dyslexic children. Interna-
gramme for Australian Indigenous children: A pilot study. tional Journal of Psychophysiology, 4, 41-52.
Occupational Therapy International, 13, 1-20. doi: 10.1002/ Tam, C., Reid, D., Naumann, S., & O’Keefe, B. (2002). Perceived ben-
oti.10 efits of word prediction intervention on written productivity
McHale, K., & Cermak, S. A. (1992). Fine motor activities in elemen- in children with spina bifida and hydrocephalus. Occupational
tary school: Preliminary findings and provisional implications Therapy International, 9, 237-255. doi: 10.1002/ oti.167
for children with fine motor problems. American Journal of Tucha, O., & Lange, K. W. (2005). The effect of conscious control
Occupational Therapy, 46, 898-903. on handwriting in children with attention deficit hyperactiv-
Medwell, J., & Wray, D. (2008). Handwriting - A forgotten language ity disorder. Journal of Attention Disorders, 9, 323-332. doi:
skill? Language and Education, 22, 34-47. doi: 10.2167/le722.0 10.1177/1087054705279994
Peterson, C. Q., & Nelson, D. L. (2003). Effect of an occupational Wehrmann, S., Chiu, T., Reid, D., & Sinclair, G. (2006). Evaluation
intervention on printing in children with economic disadvan- of occupational therapy school-based consultation service for
tages. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 57, 152-160. students with fine motor difficulties. Canadian Journal of Occu-
Ratzon, N. Z., Efraim, D., & Bart, O. (2007). A short-term grapho- pational Therapy, 73, 225-235. doi: 10.2182/ cjot.05.0016
motor program for improving writing readiness skills of first- Weintraub, N., Yinon, M., Hirsch, I. B., & Parush, S. (2009). Effec-
grade students. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 61, tiveness of sensorimotor and task-oriented handwriting inter-
399-405. doi: 10.5014/ajot.61.4.399 vention in elementary school-aged students with handwriting
Review Manager (RevMan, Version 5.0) [Computer software]. difficulties. OTJR: Occupation, Participation & Health, 29, 125-
Copenhagen, Denmark: The Nordic Cochrane Centre: The 134. doi: 10.3928/15394492-20090611-05
Cochrane Collaboration. Woodward, S., & Swinth, Y. (2002). Multisensory approaches to
Rigby, P., & Schwellnus, H. (1999). Occupational therapy decision handwriting remediation: Perceptions of school-based occupa-
making guidelines for problems in written productivity. Physi- tional therapists. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 56,
cal and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 19(1), 5-27. doi: 305-312. doi: 10.5014/ajot.56.3.305
10.1300/J006v19n01_02 Zwicker, J., & Harris, S. (2009). A reflection on motor learning theory
Roberts, G. I., & Samuels, M. T. (1993). Handwriting remediation: in pediatric occupational therapy practice. Canadian Journal of
A comparison of computer-based and traditional approaches. Occupational Therapy, 76, 29-37.
Journal of Educational Research, 87, 118. Zwicker, J. G., & Hadwin, A. F. (2009). Cognitive versus multisen-
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000a). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: sory approaches to handwriting intervention: A randomized
Classic definition and new directions. Contemporary Educa- controlled trial. OTJR: Occupation, Participation & Health, 29,
tional Psychology, 25, 54-67. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1020 40-48.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000b). Self-determination theory and
facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-
being. American Psychologist, 55, 68-78. doi: 10.1037//0003-
066X.55.1.68
Schilling, D. L., Washington, K., Billingsley, F. F., & Deitz, J. (2003).
Classroom seating for children with attention deficit hyperac-
tivity disorder: Therapy balls versus chairs. American Journal of
Occupational Therapy, 57, 534-541.
Schmidt, R. A., and Lee, T. D. (2005). Motor control and learn-
ing: A behavioral emphasis (4th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Shen, I. H., Kang, S. M., & Wu, C. Y. (2003). Comparing the effect of
different design of desks with regard to motor accuracy in writ-
ing performance of students with cerebral palsy. Applied Ergo-
nomics, 34, 141-147. doi: 10.1016/S0003-6870(03)00002-4
Ste-Marie, D. M., Clark, S. E., Findlay, L. C., & Latimer, A. E. (2004).
High levels of contextual interference enhance handwrit-
ing skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 115-126.
doi: 10.3200/JMBR.36.1.115-126
Sudsawad, P., Trombly, C. A., Henderson, A., & Tickle-Degnen, L.
(2002). Testing the effect of kinesthetic training on handwriting
performance in first-grade students. American Journal of Occu-
pational Therapy, 56, 26-33. doi: 10.5014/ajot.56.1.26
Sugden, D. A., & Chambers, M. E. (1998). Intervention approaches and
children with developmental coordination disorder. Pediatric
Rehabilitation, 2, 139-147. doi: 10.3109/17518429809060945
Tallal, P., Chase, C., Russell, G., & Schmitt, R. L. (1986). Evaluation
of the efficacy of piracetam in treating information processing,

Downloaded from cjo.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on November 16, 2014


Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy February 2011 78(1) 25

Anda mungkin juga menyukai