Anda di halaman 1dari 9

Research

Live music promotes positive behaviours in


people with Alzheimer’s disease
Elissa Cox,1 Madeleine Nowak,2 and Petra Buettner 3

Key words: Introduction: Behavioural symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease present barriers to


Alzheimer’s disease, providing a holistic standard of care. Limited attention has been given to the potential
live music, effects of live music, rather than background music or music therapy, on mood,
positive behaviour, function, and participation in and performance of daily activities. This exploratory
residential care. study investigated whether live music could facilitate the expression of positive
behaviour in people with Alzheimer’s disease.
Method: A live musical violin intervention, provided on a one-to-one basis in a
residential care facility, was employed to assess positive behaviour in people with
moderate-to-severe Alzheimer’s disease using a one-group repeated measures design.
Seven participants were video-recorded before, during, and after the intervention,
which was provided individually in three separate sessions. The investigator and
a blinded assessor used an investigator-modified version of the Cohen-Mansfield
Agitation Inventory to assess 16 positive behaviours. Quotations from study
participants are presented. Data were analysed using the Friedman test.
Results: The total number of positive behaviours increased significantly from a
median number of 4.3 (range 1.3–13) before the intervention to 30 (range 13–47.7)
during, and 24.3 (range 4–27.7) after the intervention (p = 0.001).
Conclusion: Live individualized music may provide occupational therapists with
a useful strategy to enrich the lives of people with Alzheimer’s disease.
1 Occupational Therapist, The Townsville Hospital

and Health Service, Kirwan, Queensland,


Australia; previously James Cook University,
School of Public Health and Tropical Medicine,
Introduction
Townsville, Queensland, Australia. The word ‘dementia’ is used to describe a number of disorders that cause chem-
2 Senior Research Fellow, James Cook
ical and structural changes in brain tissue leading to progressive deficits in
University, School of Public Health and
cognition, communication, and function. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a common
Tropical Medicine; School of Medicine and
Dentistry, Townsville, Queensland, Australia. form of dementia (Department of Health [DH] 2009). The number of people
3 Director and Statistician, Tropical Health with dementia in the United Kingdom (UK) is expected to double to 1.4 million
Solutions, Townsville, Queensland, Australia; over the next 30 years with associated healthcare costs expected to reach
previously Associate Professor, James Cook £50 billion per year (DH 2009). The challenge of meeting the increasing care
University, School of Public Health and Tropical needs for people with dementia is recognized internationally. Current dementia
Medicine, Townsville, Queensland, Australia.
care strategies require giving greater attention to maintaining and improving
Corresponding author: Elissa Cox, wellbeing and quality of life (DH 2009). Behavioural symptoms present an
Occupational Therapist, The Townsville additional barrier when providing a standard of care that seeks to sustain
Hospital and Health Service, Kirwan, dignity, independence, and quality of life. Enhancement of quality of life for
Queensland 4817, Australia. people with AD involves the presentation of meaningful activities as well as
Email: elissa.cox@bigpond.com
treatment of behavioural symptoms (Volicier 2007).
Reference: Cox E, Nowak M, Buettner P (2014)
Live music promotes positive behaviours in
people with Alzheimer’s disease. British Journal Literature review
of Occupational Therapy, 77(11), 556–564.
The therapeutic value of music has been recognized since antiquity (Else 2003).
DOI: 10.4276/030802214X14151078348477 Both music and music therapy have been frequently explored as therapeu-
tic modalities for agitated behaviour in people with dementia (Cox 2010,
© The College of Occupational Therapists Ltd.
Kneafsey 1997). Theoretical frameworks such as the Progressively Lowered
Submitted: 7 August 2013.
Stress Threshold (PLST) (Hall and Buckwalter 1987) and the Individualized
Accepted: 20 January 2014.
Music Intervention for Agitation (IMIA) (Gerdner 2000) have been used to

556 British Journal of Occupational Therapy November 2014 77(11)


Elissa Cox, Madeleine Nowak, and Petra Buettner

describe the influence of music on behaviour; however, there The Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (CMAI), a retro-
remains no widely accepted framework for choosing and spective carer scale that records the frequency of agitated
implementing such interventions among people with dementia behaviours, was modified to an observer format (mCMAI:
(Cox 2010). The literature on music and agitated behaviour 46 items) that can also measure positive behaviours (Cox
has been reviewed (Clarke et al 1998, Lou 2001, Sung and et al 2011). This was particularly relevant as there is no other
Chang 2005, Vink et al 2004) with music considered to have instrument available to measure positive behaviours.
practical benefits for managing the behavioural symptoms
associated with dementia. From an occupational therapy per- Study protocol
spective, Cox (2010) recently reviewed the literature and A quasi-experimental, one-group repeated measure design was
found evidence suggesting that people with severe AD still used for both financial and time-frame reasons. The study
remember songs from their childhood (Suzuki et al 2004). was designed to provide baseline information for a larger
Previous studies have primarily used recorded background randomized controlled trial (RCT).
music or music therapy for agitated behaviour, one excep- Participants were randomly allocated a time and day
tion being a recent study examining the effects of listening for each intervention and received a total of three musical
to live music on agitation in people with AD (Cox et al 2011). interventions on three separate occasions (Session One;
Compared to studies in the context of music therapy, the Session Two; Session Three). Each session consisted of three
authors found no studies exploring the effects of listening phases: pre-intervention (15-minute observation period);
to live music on mood, function, performance, and partici- intervention (18-minute musical intervention); and post-
pation for people with dementia. In this study, we differen- intervention (15-minute post-intervention observation
tiated between music therapy and live music in the following period). The musician sat approximately two metres from
way: music therapy involves music making conducted by the participant or followed them around at approximately
the therapist with the patient whereas, for the purposes of the same distance if they were wandering. She did not
this intervention, ‘live music’ is used to describe music being initiate conversation other than a standard greeting and
played by a musician to the study participant following farewell, and responded briefly and politely to participant-
the study protocol. initiated conversations.
Recently there has been some acknowledgement in the liter- Participants were discreetly video-recorded (from a dis-
ature of the potential for improvements in positive behaviours tance of more than ten metres) by a research assistant through-
(Mossello et al 2011, van Dijk et al 2012). Positive behaviours out the entire observation period. Details of the assess-
are those that are advantageous to supporting an individual’s ment of video recordings have been reported previously
function. As these benefits are of particular relevance and (Cox et al 2011). Briefly, the video footage provided a ‘top-
interest to occupational therapists, this study was designed to-toe’ view and was assessed for a list of behaviours by the
to examine the positive effect of live music on affective, func- researcher and a blinded assessor (see Table 1). The footage
tional, and leisure behaviours of people with AD. was separated into pre-intervention, intervention, and post-
intervention segments; coded; de-identified; and randomly
transferred to a video cassette prior to assessment.
Method Musical intervention
The methods used in this study have been described pre- The musical intervention consisted of a live violin recital
viously (Cox et al 2011). Briefly, the participants (n = 7; (duration 18 minutes) presented individually for each par-
4 female, 3 male) were all residents in the dementia units ticipant. The intervention was performed in the dementia-
of a private residential care facility and met the study inclu- specific unit using an informal, participatory style. The
sion criteria. These included participants having: a confirmed researcher, who performed the musical repertoire on a
‘probable’ diagnosis of AD with a score of less than 19/30 violin, remained in close proximity to the participant.
on the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), indicating Short song melodies familiar to this age group were
moderate-to-severe dementia; no change in medication dur- played, with each excerpt played twice, as the time taken
ing the previous 3 months; no auditory or visual deficits that to recognize melodies is longer for people with AD. Further
could impact on functional hearing or vision; displayed details of the music presentation are reported elsewhere
regular agitated behaviour on at least three occasions per week (Cox et al 2011).
in the previous 4 weeks; not commenced any new therapy
programmes or interventions over the previous 4 weeks (all Assessing behaviour
information was obtained from chart review). The study The type and frequency of behaviours, and the number of
was conducted in 2004–05 in Townsville (pop. 150,000), times they occurred during the observation periods, were
North Queensland, Australia. Ethical approval was obtained recorded. Behaviours were coded numerically based on
from the Townsville Health Service District and the residen- their frequency in each phase, using a ten-point scale
tial care facility where the study took place. Written informed developed for a previous study (Cox et al 2011). Observa-
consent for each participant was obtained from the person tions and quotations were documented from the video
with Enduring Power of Attorney. recording.

British Journal of Occupational Therapy November 2014 77(11) 557


Live music promotes positive behaviours in people with Alzheimer’s disease

Table 1. Median occurrences of positive behaviours before, during and after a musical intervention
Results are based on seven participants (triplicate observation) resident in a nursing home in Townsville, North Queensland, in 2004–05.
Pre-intervention During intervention Post-intervention p-value**
Affective behaviour
Smiling 0 [0, 0.33]; 0–0.33* 1.33 [1, 2.33]; 0.67–3.33 1 [0.67, 1]; 0.33–2 p = 0.002
Laughing 0 [0, 0.33]; 0–0.33 0.33 [0, 1.67]; 0–1.67 0.33 [0, 1]; 0–1.33 p = 0.029
Relaxed facial expression 0 [0, 0.33]; 0–0.67 5.33 [3, 7.67]; 2.67–8 5.33 [1.33, 8]; 0–8 p = 0.013

Functional behaviour
Initiating conversation 0.67 [0.33, 1]; 0–1 1.67 [0.67, 2]; 0.33–2.67 1.33 [0.67, 2]; 0.33–2 p = 0.032
Attention focused towards the direction of music 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 5.33 [3.67, 7.67]; 2.33–8 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 p = 0.001
Reminiscent conversation 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 0 [0, 0.33]; 0–1 p = 0.050
Sitting still 2.67 [0.33, 6.67]; 0–6.67 5.67 [5.33, 8]; 2–8 6 [3.33, 8]; 0.67–8 p = 0.446
Sleeping, dozing, closing eyes 0 [0, 0]; 0–0.33 0 [0, 0.33]; 0–0.67 0 [0, 0.67]; 0–2 p = 0.584
Alert to surroundings 2.67 [0.33, 5.33]; 0–5.67 4.33 [2.67, 6]; 1–8 8 [4, 8]; 0.67–8 p = 0.021

Participatory behaviour
Clapping 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 0 [0, 0.33]; 0–0.67 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 p = 0.050
Singing 0 [0, 0]; 0–0.33 0 [0, 2.67]; 0–2.67 0 [0, 0.33]; 0–0.33 p = 0.444
Tapping feet to music 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 2.67 [1, 3.33]; 0–3.67 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 p = 0.002
Dancing 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 0 [0, 0]; 0–0.33 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 p = 0.368
Whistling or humming 0 [0, 0]; 0–0.67 0 [0, 1.67]; 0–1.67 0 [0, 0.33]; 0–1 p = 0.060
Nodding 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 0.67 [0.33, 1]; 0–1.33 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 p = 0.002
Swaying to music 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 0.33 [0, 0.67]; 0–1 0 [0, 0]; 0–0 p = 0.018
*Median [Inter-quartile range]; range. **p-values are results of non-parametric Friedman tests; significant changes are in bold.

Statistical methods Affective behaviour


Pre-intervention, intervention and post-intervention periods Affective behaviours provided an observable insight into the
for each session were separately grouped and analysed for likely affect (emotional state) and mood of the participant,
each positive behaviour, and for all positive behaviours, to and included smiling, laughing, and relaxed facial expressions.
identify overall trends. Basic descriptive statistics were used Smiling: occurred either as a social response or sponta-
to analyse demographic data. As the data were not normally neously. Smiling was rarely displayed in the pre-intervention
distributed, non-parametric statistical methods were used. phase, but developed during the intervention phase for all
A paired Friedman test was used to determine the change in participants and persisted throughout the post-intervention
behaviour between the pre-intervention and post-intervention phase (p = 0.002).
phases. Data were analysed using SPSS Version 20 for Windows Laughing: occurred appropriately in response to songs,
(IBM SPSS, Chicago, Il, USA). An a priori significance level when interacting with other residents, and in response to
of 0.05 was adopted. musician error while playing the violin (such as squeaking).
Laughing was rare during the pre-intervention phase, occur-
ring on only two occasions among two participants with mod-
erate AD. When the intervention commenced, this behaviour
Results increased significantly (p = 0.029).
The participants’ age range was 70 to 85 years (median 77.0);
their MMSE range was 0 to 17.5 (median 0). The median Functional behaviour
time since admission to the facility was 13.5 months (range Functional behaviours are those that support engagement
3.5–55 months). Three participants were taking neuroleptic and participation within the physical and social environment.
medication for behavioural symptoms associated with AD. All Such behaviours included initiating conversation, alertness
but one participant were taking prescribed medications. The to surroundings, attention focused on direction of music,
three participants with the highest MMSE scores were exposed reminiscent conversation, sitting still, and sleeping /dozing
to structured musical performances on a regular basis. All par- or closing eyes.
ticipants recruited to the study continued to completion.
Positive behaviours were consistently displayed during Initiating conversation
and /or after the intervention by all participants, with these Participant-initiated conversation was appropriate and mean-
changes reaching significance for 9 of the 16 behaviours assessed ingful and included: compliments; comments about the music
(56%) (see Table 1). Three sub-types of positive behaviour or violin; personal and /or contextual links to the music;
were observed: affective behaviour; functional behaviour; and general social conversation; and philosophical or reflective
participating behaviour. conversation. Increases in conversation often persisted into

558 British Journal of Occupational Therapy November 2014 77(11)


Elissa Cox, Madeleine Nowak, and Petra Buettner

the post-intervention period (p = 0.032). Participants with Social conversation: An increase in socially appropriate
higher MMSE scores (> 8) and intact verbal communica- conversation was observed following the musical interven-
tion skills initiated conversation more regularly and suc- tion. Participants interacted with other residents, initiating
cessfully than those with aphasia or expressive communi- simple conversation such as ‘hello’ more readily than in
cation deficits, while those with severe AD and significant the pre-intervention period.
receptive and expressive verbal communication difficulties Philosophical/reflective conversation: After laughing at an
demonstrated increased non-verbal communication techniques accidental violin ‘squeak’, one participant made the follow-
such as social gesturing (reaching toward the violinist), ing comment: ‘You gotta have your laughs sometimes … all
smiling, and ‘conversational’ vocalizations. you can do is do your best’. At times, participants appeared
Compliments: The most common theme during initiation deep in thought.
of conversation was to compliment the musician on the per-
formance. The length of the comment varied with the extent Attention focused towards the direction of music
of expressive aphasia. Participants with significant expressive This behaviour was characterized by attending to the music.
communication deficits used short, simple phrases, such as Participants remained interested throughout the interven-
‘good’, ‘beautiful’, ‘wonderful’, and ‘that’s nice’. In contrast, tion period. Those with severe AD were more likely to be
participants with higher MMSE scores used longer, more briefly distracted (less than 10 seconds). However, one par-
descriptive phrases, such as ‘that is a beautiful one’, ‘that’s ticipant with severe AD and constant wandering behaviour
a good old song’, and ‘I haven’t heard that one in ages’. remained seated and concentrated on the music for at least
Comments about the music or instrument: Four participants 12 consecutive minutes. Staff reported this was extremely
demonstrated insight, reflection, and interest in performance unusual.
aspects of the music by commenting on the style of music,
the instrument, and the skill of the performer. Comments Reminiscent conversation
were appropriate, indicating reflection about the music. For This behaviour was characterized by participants talking
example: ‘What’s the name of that one?’, ‘That’s very nice about people and experiences from their past. It typically
playing I’ll tell you’, ‘That [playing the violin] would be hard, developed in the post-intervention period, with comments
wouldn’t it?’, ‘It’s [the song] got a good swing to it!’, ‘She’s being appropriate, self-reflective, and meaningful. The
playing well for her age, she doesn’t make any mistakes’, theme of the music, the title, and the lyrics of some songs
and ‘Do you still get lessons?’ appeared to foster reminiscence on childhood, family, and
Personal and/or contextual links: Six participants made musical talents. This behaviour was displayed by partici-
personal and contextual comments that suggested insight pants with intact expressive verbal communication skills
into the content of the song, such as the theme (for instance, (n = 2); however, one participant with severe AD also
love), the lyrics, or its personal significance to them. This attempted to engage in reminiscent conversation.
was highlighted by the response of two participants to the For example, after listening to the song I Do Like To Be
song Singing In The Rain. On completion of the song, one Beside The Seaside, one participant reflected, ‘My dad took
said ‘Wouldn’t it be nice if it rained?’, while the other me swimming all the time’. Remarkably, one participant
turned and pointed to the window and stated, ‘I can’t sing with severe expressive and receptive aphasia spontaneously
in the rain, there’s none out there!’. The second statement reflected on childhood pets, stating ‘We had prize goats’,
was remarkable, as this person presented with significant which was later confirmed by the Power of Attorney.
expressive aphasia and was typically unable to form coherent Another participant became aware of a family photo, and
sentences in daily conversation. reflecting on the photo said, ‘I reckon it’s a beaut photo …
Comments made by participants in the post-intervention Dad’s gone, I ‘spose they can’t live forever’.
period suggested they may still have been reflecting on the Two participants reflected on their own musical skills.
musical intervention. For example, one participant commented After singing along to Jingle Bells, one said, ‘I can’t sing very
to another resident 12 minutes post intervention: ‘Are you well, I wish I could sing’, while the other stated ‘My voice
a good singer? It’d be lovely to be a good singer … I try hard is very deep’ after singing along to several songs.
but I couldn’t even play me old mouth organ.’ Another par-
ticipant with severe AD stated: ‘We’ve lost her’, referring to Alertness to surroundings
the violinist, 4.5 minutes after the musical intervention. This was characterized by an apparent awareness of events
Comments initiated by another five participants also in the participant’s environment. Alertness was displayed by
reflected the therapeutic value of the musician’s presence. turning towards the source of noises, observing other residents,
‘It was lovely to listen to you’, ‘You’re a lovely person’, ‘You’re or becoming aware of the research assistant, although she
still hanging around, aren’t you?’, ‘That was lovely, I wish attempted to remain as discreet as possible. All participants
you didn’t have to stop’, and ‘Will she come back?’ These displayed increased alertness during the intervention (p =
comments were made although the musician made no 0.001). In the post-intervention phase, participants appeared
attempt to initiate conversation with the participant during to become more observant. For example, one participant made
the performance, but merely acknowledged any participant the following comment about another resident, ‘She won’t
comment with a short polite response. talk much’, while another, upon overhearing an adjacent staff

British Journal of Occupational Therapy November 2014 77(11) 559


Live music promotes positive behaviours in people with Alzheimer’s disease

conversation about a lost item, pointed it out to the staff, from activities and had very limited verbal and non-verbal
saying ‘It’s under the table’. communication). This resident was reported to have previously
derived a great deal of pleasure from dancing.
Participatory behaviour
These behaviours reflected active participation in the musical Overall positive behaviours
intervention and were identified as tapping feet to the music, Overall, the number of positive behaviours increased signifi-
singing, whistling or humming, swaying in time to the music, cantly from a median number of 4.3 (interquartile range
nodding, dancing, and clapping. [IQR] = 3.7–11.7; range 1.3–13) before the intervention to
a median of 30 (IQR = 28.7–34; range 13–47.7) during and
Tapping feet to the music 24.3 (IQR = 13–26.7; range 4–27.7) after the intervention
Six participants tapped their feet to the beat of the music (p = 0.001; see Fig. 1).
during the musical intervention (p = 0.002). This behaviour
was not normally seen despite recorded background music Fig. 1. Box-and-whisker plots (median and interquartile range)
often being played in the facility. Participants stopped tap- of total number of positive behaviours before, during, and
ping their feet at the end of each song, which enabled differ- after the musical intervention.
entiation between ‘tapping feet to music’ and ‘repetitious
mannerisms’. In addition, participants tapped their hands, Total number of positive behaviours

fingers, or legs during the songs. One participant, who had 50 p = 0.001
played the piano as a child, made finger motions as though
playing the piano in time with the melody of When We Are 40
Together. Songs with a strong, discernible rhythm, such as
Jingle Bells, Row Your Boat, The Can Can, and Waltzing Matilda, 30
resulted in tapping.
20
Singing
Singing was rare during the pre-intervention period, but 10
was displayed by five participants during the intervention
or post-intervention period. Participants sang songs they 0
Before During After
knew during the intervention, and were subsequently noted
to spontaneously sing songs (including those which were
Results are based on seven participants.
not in the repertoire) in the post-intervention period. The
speed with which participants recognized songs increased
as the intervention progressed. During the first 5 minutes
of the intervention, participants with moderate dementia
Discussion
typically required at least two verses of the song before appear- This study’s major findings were that a one-to-one live musical
ing to recognize it. However, after 5 minutes, participants intervention significantly increased positive behaviours for
typically required only a few bars to recognize songs. One the study participants. These behavioural changes often
participant with significant expressive and receptive aphasia persisted into the immediate post-intervention period. An
sang the lyrics of ten songs fluently and correctly. In daily extensive review of the literature failed to reveal any com-
tasks, this person was unable to successfully verbally com- parable studies demonstrating a similar effect of live musical
municate due to extensive aphasia. interventions among people with AD or other dementias
(Cox 2010).
Whistling or humming Participants exhibited improvements in participatory,
Whistling or humming was very rarely displayed before the functional, and affective behaviours during the study period.
intervention. Once the intervention had commenced, however, These behaviours may be of particular interest to occupa-
whistling or humming became more frequent, with partici- tional therapists, as improvements in these areas have impli-
pants accurately whistling or humming the melody of songs cations for supporting meaningful occupational performance
on 14 occasions. One participant continued whistling melodies and wellbeing.
from the repertoire after the completion of the intervention. There are a number of benefits to live music, particularly
when compared to background music. Firstly, it is a ‘multi-
Dancing sensory’ experience, with the visual presence providing appeal,
Dancing, which consisted of slowly stepping from side to side focus, and movement in addition to the auditory stimulation
in time to the music, was displayed once by one participant. and entertainment usually provided by background music. The
Staff reported never having seen this behaviour before in this novelty of the violin as an instrument, as well as the presence
resident, who had such severe AD it prevented her from engag- of the violinist, may also have contributed to its effective-
ing in most purposeful or meaningful activities (for example, ness. Multi-sensory interventions are particularly important
sitting for meals, sleeping, and she gained no apparent pleasure for people with dementia as often it is unknown which area(s)

560 British Journal of Occupational Therapy November 2014 77(11)


Elissa Cox, Madeleine Nowak, and Petra Buettner

of the brain have been individually affected by the disease for the whole 18 minutes and expressed a desire to hear more
process. Bowman (2004) suggests that music acts as a music: for example, ‘You’re not leaving now are you?’ Further
‘sensorimotor coupling’ between music and the body. He investigation is needed to determine the optimum length of
describes how music seems to engage with the listener’s body such interventions, although it is likely that individually-tailored
in a way that mirrors musical properties (for instance, tap- repertoire programmes will have the greatest benefit.
ping feet in time to the rhythm and tempo of the music).
The songs in the musical repertoire were structured to Benefits for participants
encourage reminiscence of previous life events (childhood, Sustained short-term benefits were observed for participants
parties, birthdays, marriage, World War II) and elicit emotions with both moderate and severe AD, with many of the
(such as romance, loneliness, tenderness, or joy). The songs behavioural responses emerging early in the intervention
chosen appeared to facilitate emotional expression, as and lasting for at least 15 minutes after completion of the
suggested by Sloboda and O’Neill (2001). The musician’s intervention. This supports results from earlier studies on
manipulation of the technical elements of the music (speed, musical interventions (Brotons and Pickett-Cooper 1996,
rhythm, volume, pitch, and melodic range) may also have Cox et al 2011, Gerdner 2000).
contributed to the musical expression of emotion (Gundlach Cohen-Mansfield et al (2010a) identified the benefits
1935 cited in Gabrielsson 2001, Juslin 2001). of matching stimuli to a person’s past preferences, while
A live, one-to-one musical intervention is a unique Hays (2005) suggested music plays a valued role for many
method of facilitating, nurturing, and guiding inter-personal people in late adulthood. This intervention provided the
interaction, and may present fewer social demands on the opportunity for participants to be involved in a musical
participants than a group programme. Live music has unique activity that may otherwise have been difficult due to the
properties for the listener, it differs from recorded music deficits associated with AD. Participants were thus able to
by being ‘live, improvisational, spontaneous, participatory engage in an activity with intrinsic personal meaning.
and social’ (Sloboda and O’Neill 2001, p423). This one-to- Psychosocial function is an important aspect of ageing
one approach also differs from a ‘concert’ setting, which is (Papalia et al 2001), encompassing feelings of belonging,
more formal, less personal, and presents more complex social connection, and the ability for reflection upon life’s
social and environmental challenges that may exceed the experiences in order to obtain a sense of value and acceptance
person’s capacity. (Erikson 1963, cited in Finnema et al 2000). Psychosocial
It is acknowledged that live music is a social activity in wellbeing is difficult to address for people with AD as it is
which the interaction between performer and listener essentially ‘intangible’. However, it remains an important
becomes a shared experience (Sloboda and O’Neill 2001). aspect of care, as people with AD ‘still feel a full range of
The ‘listening’ role is socially appropriate within the context emotions but lose the ability to express them’ (Brotons and
of a musical performance and the shared focus of the inter- Pickett-Cooper 1996). Spirituality has also been recognized
vention may contribute to a sense of purpose and connec- as an important coping mechanism for people with AD
tion that would otherwise be difficult for this population to (Beuscher and Grando 2009).
achieve. Small (1999, p13) has suggested that making music, While self-reflection is an important component of effec-
or what he called ‘musicking’, ‘brings into existence among tive psychosocial function in late adulthood, cognitive dete-
those present a set of relationships, and it is in those rela- rioration associated with AD limits effective processes which
tionships that the meaning of the act of musicking lies. It lies are typical of this life stage. In this study, the theme or style
not only in the relationships between the humanly organised of the music appeared integral to promoting reminiscence.
sounds that are conventionally thought of as being the stuff Several sources of emotion in music have been described by
of music, but also in the relationships that are established Bunt and Pavlicevic (2001), and include associative connec-
for the duration of the performance between the participants tions (associations with past memories, events, and places)
and the performance space.’ and iconic connections (sequences within the music which
Comments made by participants, such as ‘You’re a lovely can convey emotion or sentiment). Self-reflection was dis-
person’, ‘Will she come back?’, or ‘You’re still hanging around played by participants who talked about their role in their
aren’t you?’, suggest the musician played an important role family or about their individual skills (such as musical
in the participants’ experience of the intervention. Thus, the ability). For example, after listening to the song I Do Like
presence of the violinist in a familiar environment may to Be Beside the Sea Side, one participant fondly reminisced
itself provide therapeutic value, as the individualized, non- about swimming with her father – an associative connection.
demanding contact enabled experiences of meaningful, This style of intervention may provide an alternative to current
appropriate social connection and interaction. These feel- therapies (reminiscence or simulated presence) that have
ings in turn may have reduced distressing emotions, such as greater cognitive demands. Gerdner (2000, p51) suggested
isolation and loneliness, which can lead to the development a similar role for music and proposed that, ‘A stimulus
of agitated behaviour. associated with the recollection of memories may be more
The length of the intervention (18 minutes) appeared pleasing to the individual than stimuli in their present envi-
appropriate. It is possible the intervention could have been ronment. Music serves as a powerful catalyst for reminiscence,
extended as six of the seven participants remained interested precipitating either pleasant or unpleasant memories.’

British Journal of Occupational Therapy November 2014 77(11) 561


Live music promotes positive behaviours in people with Alzheimer’s disease

Thaut (1990, cited in Gerdner 2000) suggested that, ‘The and agitated behaviours that led to an overall reduction in
experience associated with music is not embodied in its anxiety. Models such as the Progressively Lowered Stress
content, but is reflective of emotions, memories, and private Threshold Model (Hall and Buckwalter 1987) and the Mid-
images that have a learned association with musical stimuli. Range Theory of Individualized Music Intervention for
For music to elicit positive memories, an assumption of this Agitation (Gerdner 1997), as discussed by Cox et al (2011),
theory is that music must be individualized.’ Throughout the lend support to this notion, and further exploration would
study, participants demonstrated individual preferences for assist in developing a theoretical framework for live musical
particular songs, and appeared to respond more intensely to interventions.
these songs. Gerdner (2000) reported that individualized
music is the most effective in reducing agitated behaviour Directions for future research
for people with AD. This may also be the case when choosing The results of this study demonstrate the need for further
music to support positive behaviours. investigation into the potential performance and partici-
Participants exhibited an apparent improvement in affect pation enhancement associated with increasing positive
and mood in response to the music, and demonstrated an behaviours by live musical intervention. The use of a live
increase in behaviours such as smiling, laughing, and relaxed musical intervention before ‘problematic’ tasks, such as
facial expressions. Similar behaviours have been noted in bathing or mealtimes, in order to support cooperation could
studies of background music and music therapy (Beck et al be explored, as well as potential sustained behavioural
2002, Brotons and Marti 2003, Brotons and Pickett-Cooper improvements over time. The possible benefits of care-
1996), as have improvements in language and social skills giver engagement extending beyond the period of the inter-
(Brotons and Marti 2003). Improved socialization may also vention have been observed in music therapy (Clair 2002)
reduce the isolation caused by the loss of effective commu- and a similar study would be useful for live music. In
nication skills (Parent 1978). In the absence of access to addition, the individual benefits of this intervention in
music therapy, this intervention, which does not require targeting psychosocial outcomes (such as self-reflection)
specialized training, may be a suitable alternative. could be expanded and implemented. This methodology
Cohen-Mansfield et al (2010b) identified one-to-one could also be used to support expressions of spirituality
socialization as a well-accepted and beneficial stimulus for in this population.
people with dementia, regardless of their cognitive level. Ways to tailor the intervention to be individually-specific
They recommended people in residential care have access and specifically targeted to various stages of the progres-
to this type of interaction, while acknowledging the chal- sion of AD should be investigated. The type and features
lenges of delivering such contact. The structure of this of instruments (for example, the pitch and volume) may
musical intervention provides a ‘non-threatening’ oppor- also influence responses to the music. The effectiveness of
tunity for participant-led, active engagement. No demands matching the type of instrument to individual musical
are placed on the participant; they can determine their preference or background could be investigated. Exploring
degree of interaction depending on their functional skills, the effectiveness of different types of musicians (for example,
interest, or comfort level. Participants successfully initiated music students, volunteers, or family members) could also
both verbal and non-verbal contact by discussing the music, be conducted.
complimenting the violinist, clapping, smiling, whistling, A theoretical framework explaining the relationship
humming, or dancing. between live music and behaviour would be helpful for occu-
There was also an apparent short-term improvement in pational therapists who wish to incorporate such interven-
attention to task, concentration, and alertness to surround- tions into their programmes. Occupational therapists have
ings throughout the musical intervention and beyond. Gregory the skills to identify the holistic functional benefits that could
(2002) has suggested active music listening can maintain flow from such an intervention and should be encouraged
purposeful selective attention for older adults with cognitive to engage in such research. They are in an ideal position to
processing disorders such as AD, which may explain the evaluate the potential for live music to enhance occupational
observed improvement in orientation to both current time performance, participation, and wellbeing for people with
(for instance, morning and afternoon) and lifespan awareness AD and other dementias.
(for instance, ‘Dad’s gone, I ‘spose they can’t live forever’).
Breur et al (2007) have also reported improvements in Limitations
cognitive function the morning following an accredited music As this exploratory study was conducted on seven partici-
therapy session. Of particular interest to occupational ther- pants, there are limitations to the extent of the conclusions,
apists are the possible sustained cognitive benefits following especially in identifying any increases in less frequently
musical interventions and their potential translation into displayed positive behaviours. The potential for inves-
enhanced participation and engagement. tigator rater-bias is acknowledged. However, we attempted
The expression of positive behaviours may have also been to minimize such bias by a number of techniques. As we
made possible by the concurrent reduction in agitated behav- were unable to identify a suitable standardized measurement
iours previously reported in this group (Cox et al 2011). It instrument, the mCMAI was developed specifically for this
may be this alteration in the relationship between positive study; this instrument has not been validated.

562 British Journal of Occupational Therapy November 2014 77(11)


Elissa Cox, Madeleine Nowak, and Petra Buettner

Conclusion Beuscher L, Grando VT (2009) Using spirituality to cope with early stage
Alzheimer’s disease. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 31(5), 583–598.
Live music performed on a one-to-one basis for people with Bowman W (2004) Cognition and the body: perspectives from music education.
AD resulted in positive behaviours. These preliminary results In: L Bresler, ed. Knowing bodies, moving minds: towards embodied
suggest live music presented in this manner may be partic- teaching and learning. Dordrecht: Kluwer. 29–50.
ularly effective in facilitating behaviours such as temporal Breur RA, Spitznagel E, Cloninger CR (2007) The temporal limits of cognitive
awareness, initiation of socialization, and improvement in change from music therapy in elderly persons with dementia or dementia-
affect, which are otherwise difficult to address in this popu- like cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Music
lation. This exploratory study suggests a potential avenue Therapy, 44(4), 308–328.
for further investigation by occupational therapists in order Brotons M, Pickett-Cooper PK (1996) The effects of music therapy intervention
to determine the full extent of positive behavioural responses, on agitation behaviors of Alzheimer’s disease patients. Journal of Music
the length of time that benefits are sustained, the presentation Therapy, 33(1), 2–18.
and appropriate musical selection for specific individuals, and Brotons M, Marti P (2003) Music therapy with Alzheimer’s patients and
any enhancement in occupational participation, performance, their family caregivers: a pilot project. Journal of Music Therapy, 40(2),
and wellbeing. To our knowledge, there has only been one 138–150.
other live music study of this nature reported for people with Bunt L, Pavlicevic M (2001) Music and emotion: perspectives from music therapy.
AD (Cox et al 2011), although music therapists do employ a In: P Juslin, J Sloboda, eds. Music and emotion: theory and research.
variety of specific musical activities and occupational thera- Oxford: Oxford University Press. 181–201.
pists are likely to incorporate music in their practice with this Clair AA (2002) The effects of music therapy on engagement in family caregiver
population. Live, individualized music may provide a useful and care receiver couples with dementia. American Journal of Alzheimer’s
strategy to enrich the lives of people with dementia. Disease and Other Dementias, 17(5), 286–290.
Clarke ME, Lipe AW, Bilbrey M (1998) Use of music to decrease aggressive
behaviors in people with dementia. Journal of Gerontological Nursing,
Key findings 24(7), 10–17.
■ A one-to-one live musical intervention may be a suitable strategy Cohen-Mansfield J, Marx MS, Thein K, Dakheel-Ali M (2010a) The impact
to facilitate the expression of positive behaviours among people of past and present preferences on stimulus engagement in nursing
with Alzheimer’s disease. home residents with dementia. Aging Mental Health, 14(1), 67–73.
■ Live music may improve affect, and support alert and attentive inter- Cohen-Mansfield J, Marx MS, Dakheel-Ali M, Regier NG, Thein K (2010b) Can
action for people with Alzheimer’s disease. persons with dementia be engaged with stimuli? American Journal of
Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(4), 351–362.
What the study has added Cox E (2010) Reviewing the role of music in the management of agitation
This is the first study in the occupational literature to investigate the potential in people with Alzheimer’s disease: can it be added to the occupational
for occupational therapists to use listening to live music to enhance well- therapy toolkit? Journal of Rural and Tropical Public Health, 9, 82–94.
being and participation among people with Alzheimer’s disease. Cox E, Nowak M, Buettner P (2011) Managing agitated behaviour in
people with Alzheimer’s disease: the role of live music. British Journal
of Occupational Therapy, 74(11), 517–524.
Acknowledgements Department of Health (2009) Living well with dementia: A national dementia
The authors wish to thank the participants, their families, and the staff of the strategy. London: DH. Available at: https://www.gov.uk/government/
residential care facility in which the study was undertaken. We would also uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/168220/dh_094051.pdf
like to thank Annette McLeod Boyle for acting as the blinded observer and Accessed 19.07.13.
reviewing video material, and Moira Holden who undertook the role of research Else L (2003) The power of music: show me emotion. New Scientist, 2423,
assistant for this study. The authors would also like to thank Professor Jiska 38–41.
Cohen-Mansfield for permission to adapt the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Erikson E (1963) Childhood and Society. London: Imago Publishing Co. Ltd.
Inventory for this study. Finnema E, Droes R, Ribbe M, Tilburg W (2000) A review of psychosocial
models in psychogeriatrics: implications for care and research. Alzheimer
Conflict of interest: None declared. Disease and Associated Disorders, 14(2), 68–80.
Funding: This research received no specific grant support from any funding Gabrielsson A, Lindstrom E (2001) The influence of musical structure on
agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. emotional expression. In: P Juslin, J Sloboda, eds. Music and emotion:
Research ethics: Ethical approval was obtained from The Townsville Health theory and research. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 223–248.
Service District and the residential care facility in which the study was carried Gerdner L (1997) An individualised music intervention for agitation. Journal
out (October 2004). of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association, 3(6), 177–184.
Gerdner LA (2000) Effects of individualized versus classical “relaxation”
References music on the frequency of agitation in elderly persons with Alzheimer’s
Beck CK, Vogelpohl TS, Rasin JH, Uriri JT, O’Sullivan P, Walls R, Phillips R, disease and related disorders. International Journal of Psychogeriatrics,
Baldwin B (2002) Effects of behavioral interventions on disruptive behavior 12(1), 49–65.
and affect in demented nursing home residents. Nursing Research, 51(4), Gregory D (2002) Music listening for maintaining attention of older adults
219–228. with cognitive impairments. Journal of Music Therapy, 39(4), 244–264.

British Journal of Occupational Therapy November 2014 77(11) 563


Live music promotes positive behaviours in people with Alzheimer’s disease

Gundlach RH (1935) Factors determining the characterization of musical phrases. Sloboda JA, O’Neill SA (2001) Emotions in everyday listening to music. In:
American Journal of Psychology, 47(4), 624–644. P Juslin, J Sloboda, eds. Music and Emotion: theory and research. Oxford:
Hall GR, Buckwalter KC (1987) Progressively lowered stress threshold: a Oxford University Press. 415–429.
conceptual model for care of adults with Alzheimer’s disease. Archives Small C (1999) Musicking: the meanings of performing and listening. A
of Psychiatric Nursing, 1(6), 399–406. lecture. Music Education Research, 1(1), 9–22.
Hays T (2005) Well-being in later life through music. Australasian Journal Sung HC, Chang AM (2005) Use of preferred music to decrease agitated
on Ageing, 24(1), 28–32. behaviours in older people with dementia: a review of the literature.
Juslin P (2001) Communicating emotion in music performance: a review and Journal of Clinical Nursing, 14(9), 1133–1140.
a theoretical framework. In: P Juslin, J Sloboda, eds. Music and emotion: Suzuki M, Kanamori M, Watanabe M, Nagasawa S, Kojima E, Ooshiro H, Nakahara D
theory and research. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 309–340. (2004) Behavioral and endocrinological evaluation of music therapy for
Kneafsey R (1997) The therapeutic use of music in a care of the elderly setting: elderly patients with dementia. Nursing and Health Sciences, 6(1), 11–18.
a literature review. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 6(5), 341–346. Thaut M (1990) Neurophysiological processes in music perception and their
Lou MF (2001) The use of music to decrease agitated behaviour of the relevance in music therapy. In: RF Unkafer, ed. Music therapy in the treatment
demented elderly: the state of science. Scandinavian Journal of Caring of adults with mental disorders. New York: Schimer Books. 3–32.
Science, 15(2), 165–173. van Dijk AM, van Weert JC, Dröes RM (2012) Does theatre improve the
Mossello E, Ridolfi A, Mello AM, Lorenzini G, Mugnai F, Piccini C, … quality of life of people with dementia? International Psychogeriatrics,
Marchionni N (2011) Animal-assisted activity and emotional status of 24(3), 367–381.
patients with Alzheimer’s disease in day care. International Psychogeriatrics, Vink AC, Bruinsma MS, Scholten R (2004) Music therapy in the care of people
23(6), 899–905. with dementia (Protocol for a Cochrane Review). Chichester, UK: John
Papalia DE, Olds SW, Feldman RD (2001) Human development. 8th ed. Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/
Boston: McGraw Hill. 10.1002/14651858.CD003477/abstract Accessed 26.08.14.
Parent LH (1978) Effects of a low-stimulus environment on behavior. American Volicer L (2007) Goals of care in advanced dementia: quality of life, dignity
Journal of Occupationaerapy, 32(1), 19–25. and comfort. The Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging, 11(6), 481.

564 British Journal of Occupational Therapy November 2014 77(11)

Anda mungkin juga menyukai