Anda di halaman 1dari 394

Exploring Implicit

Cognition:
Learning, Memory, and Social
Cognitive Processes
Zheng Jin
Zhengzhou Normal University, China & University of California, Davis, USA

A volume in the Advances in Psychology, Mental


Health, and Behavioral Studies (APMHBS) Book
Series
Managing Director: Lindsay Johnston
Acquisitions Editor: Kayla Wolfe
Production Editor: Christina Henning
Development Editor: Erin O’Dea
Typesetter: Cody Page
Cover Design: Jason Mull

Published in the United States of America by


Information Science Reference (an imprint of IGI Global)
701 E. Chocolate Avenue
Hershey PA, USA 17033
Tel: 717-533-8845
Fax: 717-533-8661
E-mail: cust@igi-global.com
Web site: http://www.igi-global.com

Copyright © 2015 by IGI Global. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored or distributed in
any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, without written permission from the publisher.
Product or company names used in this set are for identification purposes only. Inclusion of the names of the products or
companies does not indicate a claim of ownership by IGI Global of the trademark or registered trademark.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Exploring implicit cognition : learning, memory, and social cognitive processes / Zheng Jin, editor.
pages cm
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978-1-4666-6599-6 (hardcover) -- ISBN 978-1-4666-6600-9 (ebook) -- ISBN 978-1-4666-6602-3 (print & perpetual
access) 1. Cognition. 2. Implicit learning. 3. Implicit memory. 4. Social perception. I. Jin, Zheng, 1984-
BF311.E889 2015
154.2--dc23
2014032309

This book is published in the IGI Global book series Advances in Psychology, Mental Health, and Behavioral Studies
(APMHBS) (ISSN: pending; eISSN: pending)

British Cataloguing in Publication Data


A Cataloguing in Publication record for this book is available from the British Library.

All work contributed to this book is new, previously-unpublished material. The views expressed in this book are those of the
authors, but not necessarily of the publisher.

For electronic access to this publication, please contact: eresources@igi-global.com.


Advances in Psychology,
Mental Health, and Behavioral
Studies (APMHBS) Book Series

ISSN: pending
EISSN: pending
Mission
The complexity of the human mind has puzzled researchers and physicians for centuries. While widely
studied, the brain still remains largely misunderstood.
The Advances in Psychology, Mental Health, and Behavioral Studies (APMHBS) book series
presents comprehensive research publications focusing on topics relating to the human psyche, cogni-
tion, psychiatric care, mental and developmental disorders, as well as human behavior and interaction.
Featuring diverse and innovative research, publications within APMHBS are ideally designed for use
by mental health professionals, academicians, researchers, and upper-level students.

Coverage
• Brain Mechanisms IGI Global is currently accepting manuscripts
• Human Cognitive System for publication within this series. To submit a pro-
• Implicit Learning posal for a volume in this series, please contact our
• Implicit Measures Acquisition Editors at Acquisitions@igi-global.com
• Implicit Memory or visit: http://www.igi-global.com/publish/.
• Implicit-Explicit Relations

The Advances in Psychology, Mental Health, and Behavioral Studies (APMHBS) Book Series (ISSN pending) is published by IGI Global,
701 E. Chocolate Avenue, Hershey, PA 17033-1240, USA, www.igi-global.com. This series is composed of titles available for purchase in-
dividually; each title is edited to be contextually exclusive from any other title within the series. For pricing and ordering information please
visit http://www.igi-global.com/book-series/advances-psychology-mental-health-behavioral/110200. Postmaster: Send all address changes to
above address. Copyright © 2015 IGI Global. All rights, including translation in other languages reserved by the publisher. No part of this
series may be reproduced or used in any form or by any means – graphics, electronic, or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, taping,
or information and retrieval systems – without written permission from the publisher, except for non commercial, educational use, including
classroom teaching purposes. The views expressed in this series are those of the authors, but not necessarily of IGI Global.
Titles in this Series
For a list of additional titles in this series, please visit: www.igi-global.com

Exploring Implicit Cognition Learning, Memory, and Social Cognitive Processes


Zheng Jin (Zhengzhou Normal University, China & University of California, Davis, USA)
Information Science Reference • copyright 2015 • 337pp • H/C (ISBN: 9781466665996) • US $195.00 (our price)

701 E. Chocolate Ave., Hershey, PA 17033


Order online at www.igi-global.com or call 717-533-8845 x100
To place a standing order for titles released in this series, contact: cust@igi-global.com
Mon-Fri 8:00 am - 5:00 pm (est) or fax 24 hours a day 717-533-8661
Editorial Advisory Board
Maria Clara, Saarland University, Germany
Barbaranelli Claudio, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Hilde Haider, University of Cologne, Germany
Misyak Jennifer, University of Warwick, UK
Yang Lee, Haskins Laboratory, Yale University, USA
Bryan Roche, National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland
Melanie Takarangi, Flinders University, Australia
Martin Voracek, University of Vienna, Austria

List of Reviewers
R. Jun Chen, Zhengzhou Normal University, China
Caroline Crawford, University of Houston – Clear Lake, USA
Jan De Houwer, Ghent University, Belgium
Chad E. Forbes, University of Delaware, USA
G. Gao, Beijing University, China
Robert Gaschler, University of Koblenz – Landau, Germany
Neha John-Henderson, University of California, Berkeley, USA
Junehee Lee, Gyeongsang National University, South Korea
Wesley Moons, University of California, Davis, USA
Dirk Wentura, Saarland University, Germany
Yan Wu, The University of Southern Mississippi, USA
Table of Contents

Preface..................................................................................................................................................xiii

Acknowledgment................................................................................................................................. xix

Introduction.......................................................................................................................................... xx

Chapter 1
Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits: A Brief Review of Measurement and
Validity Issues.......................................................................................................................................... 1
Francesco Dentale, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Michele Vecchione, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Claudio Barbaranelli, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Chapter 2
Priming................................................................................................................................................... 16
Christina Bermeitinger, University of Hildesheim, Germany

Chapter 3
Individual Differences in Implicit Learning: Current Problems and Issues for Research..................... 61
Daisuke Nakamura, Aoyama Gakuin University, Japan

Chapter 4
Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy: Implicit Cognitive Vulnerability as an Impetus towards Higher
Order Thinking Skills............................................................................................................................ 86
Caroline M. Crawford, University of Houston – Clear Lake, USA
Marion S. Smith, Texas Southern University, USA

Chapter 5
Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research.............................................................. 104
Andrew J. Pantos, Metropolitan State University of Denver, USA

Chapter 6
Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership........................... 118
Gwendolyn A. Kelso, Boston University, USA
Leslie R. Brody, Boston University, USA
Chapter 7
Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity: An Overview................................................. 138
Sabine Glock, Bergische Universität Wuppertal, Germany
Ineke M. Pit ten-Cate, University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg

Chapter 8
Function over Form: A Behavioral Approach to Implicit Attitudes.................................................... 162
Anthony G. O’Reilly, National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland
Bryan Roche, National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland
Aoife Cartwright, National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland

Chapter 9
The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem from Social Threats.............................. 183
Jordan B. Leitner, University of Delaware, USA
Chad E. Forbes, University of Delaware, USA

Chapter 10
How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet............................... 205
Albena Antonova, Sofia University, Bulgaria

Chapter 11
Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions: Implicit Cognition in Action............. 216
Lucija Jančec, Society “Our Children” Opatija, Croatia
Sanja Tatalović Vorkapić, University of Rijeka, Croatia
Jurka Lepičnik Vodopivec, University of Primorska, Slovenia

Chapter 12
Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script in Persistent Virtual Worlds: Theoretical Foundations .
and Decision Processes for Constructing a Socio-Cultural Learning Framework............................... 243
Nikolaos Pellas, University of the Aegean, Greece

Afterword............................................................................................................................................ 281

Related References............................................................................................................................. 283

Compilation of References................................................................................................................ 307

About the Contributors..................................................................................................................... 364

Index.................................................................................................................................................... 369
Detailed Table of Contents

Preface..................................................................................................................................................xiii

Acknowledgment................................................................................................................................. xix

Introduction.......................................................................................................................................... xx

Chapter 1
Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits: A Brief Review of Measurement and
Validity Issues.......................................................................................................................................... 1
Francesco Dentale, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Michele Vecchione, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Claudio Barbaranelli, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

This chapter reviews the studies that applied the Implicit Association Test (IAT) to assess the Big Five
personality traits, focusing on issues related to measurement and validity. After a brief introduction on the
implicit self-concept of personality, the following five issues are critically reviewed: (1) the experimental
procedure of both classical and questionnaire-based Big Five IATs, (2) the factorial structure and reliability
of the implicit traits and the degree of dissociation between implicit and explicit measures, (3) the state
and trait components of implicit scores, (4) the predictive validity of the Big-Five IATs with respect
to relevant behavioural criteria, and (5) the robustness to faking of the Big Five IATs. Future research
directions for the implicit measures of the Big Five were discussed.

Chapter 2
Priming................................................................................................................................................... 16
Christina Bermeitinger, University of Hildesheim, Germany

This chapter is about the wide variety of priming encountered in cognitive and social psychology. In
cognitive psychology, the priming paradigm is mainly used to study memory phenomena or the pre-
activation of concepts and motor reactions by related stimuli. In social psychology, the term priming is
used for a broader range of phenomena in which an event triggers a subsequent behavior. In this chapter,
some definitions of priming and the origin of the term’s use in psychology are presented. Then, the chapter
examines different types and variants of priming as well as their features and principal elements, using
seven organizing principles. After this methodical and empirical classification, some important theories
of response priming, semantic priming, affective/evaluative priming, negative priming, and macro-level
priming are summarized. Last but not least, some general questions and problems in priming research
are outlined and a brief outlook is given.
Chapter 3
Individual Differences in Implicit Learning: Current Problems and Issues for Research..................... 61
Daisuke Nakamura, Aoyama Gakuin University, Japan

This chapter reviews research on whether individual differences in psychometric intelligence, working
memory, and other less investigated variables, such as emotion and personality, affect implicit learning,
with particular focus on Reber’s evolutionary theory and Kaufman’s dual-process theory for implicit
learning. The review shows that while the null effects of psychometric intelligence on implicit learning
seems robust as both theories claim, those of working memory were unclear due to methodological
insufficiency. For the effects of emotion and personality, further investigation is needed as studies in this
direction have just begun to proliferate. The chapter concludes that the research findings on the effects of
these individual difference variables on implicit learning are still inconclusive, except for psychometric
intelligence, and provides suggestions for future research.

Chapter 4
Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy: Implicit Cognitive Vulnerability as an Impetus towards Higher
Order Thinking Skills............................................................................................................................ 86
Caroline M. Crawford, University of Houston – Clear Lake, USA
Marion S. Smith, Texas Southern University, USA

Implicit cognition is an intriguing area of focus when one considers the impact of implicit memory
theories upon each learner’s cognitive vulnerability when framed through Bloom’s Taxonomy of the
Cognitive Domain. Specifically, consider the learner’s cognitive understanding and movement from the
lower order thinking skills, say from the Knowledge realm and Comprehension realm, towards the higher
order thinking skills, Synthesis realm and Evaluation realm, or one of the revised domains to reflect
Digital Age expectations. Although much is available on the different levels of cognitive achievement,
the “in between” leaps in a learner’s ability to work with the information in new and different manners
may suggest that the cognitive vulnerability may impact the learner’s implicit memory and the learner’s
movement between different taxonomic levels of informational understanding.

Chapter 5
Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research.............................................................. 104
Andrew J. Pantos, Metropolitan State University of Denver, USA

This chapter argues for incorporation of concepts and methods from the domain of Implicit Social
Cognition (ISC) into the field of language attitudes research. As support, this chapter reports on a
quantitative study that employed both an audio Implicit Association Test and traditional self-report
questionnaires to measure participants’ implicit and explicit attitudes toward foreign and U.S. accented
speech stimuli. The IAT revealed a pro-U.S. accent bias, while the explicit measure found a pro-foreign
accent bias. These results support the argument that the distinction between implicit and explicit attitudes
as separable attitude constructs resulting from distinct mental processes is an important one for language
attitudes research and that both attitude constructs should be evaluated when studying language attitudes.
Chapter 6
Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership........................... 118
Gwendolyn A. Kelso, Boston University, USA
Leslie R. Brody, Boston University, USA

Stereotype threat about leadership ability may trigger emotional and cognitive responses that reduce
women’s leadership aspirations. This chapter reviews literature and presents a study on the effects of
implicit (covert) and explicit (overt) leadership stereotype threat on women’s emotions, power-related
cognitions, and behaviors as moderated by exposure to powerful female or male role models. Emotional
responses were measured using self-report (direct) and narrative writing (indirect) tasks. Undergraduate
women (n = 126) in the Northeastern U.S. were randomly divided into three stereotype threat groups:
none, implicit, and explicit. Implicit stereotype threat resulted in higher indirectly expressed (but not
self-reported) anxiety, behaviors that benefited others more than the self, and when preceded by exposure
to powerful female role models, higher self-reported negative emotion but also higher indirect positive
affect. Explicit stereotype threat resulted in higher indirect optimism, and when preceded by exposure
to powerful female role models, lower self-reported sadness but also lower implicit power cognitions.

Chapter 7
Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity: An Overview................................................. 138
Sabine Glock, Bergische Universität Wuppertal, Germany
Ineke M. Pit ten-Cate, University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg

This chapter focuses on implicit attitudes toward smoking and provides the first systematic review of
research in this domain. Implicit attitudes are suggested to guide automatic behavior, thereby playing
a pivotal role for automatic processes inherent in addictive behaviors. This chapter further explores
the extent to which implicit attitudes are context-sensitive. More specifically, it reviews studies that
have focused on the differential effects of external cues such as warning labels and internal cues (e.g.,
deprivation). Results of 32 studies show that although smokers generally have more positive implicit
attitudes than non-smokers, the valence of implicit attitudes varies as a result of the applied method or
stimuli. Studies reveal that implicit attitudes toward smoking partly depend on external cues, especially
outcome expectancies. Similarly, internal cues affect implicit attitudes whereby the level of nicotine
deprivation seems vital. Implications for intervention and future research are indicated in the discussion.

Chapter 8
Function over Form: A Behavioral Approach to Implicit Attitudes.................................................... 162
Anthony G. O’Reilly, National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland
Bryan Roche, National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland
Aoife Cartwright, National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland

Research surrounding the construct of “implicit attitudes” and the various methodologies for measuring
that construct is currently founded on the social cognitive paradigm. However, no robust and agreed
upon theoretical framework has emerged from this paradigm, despite the widespread adoption of implicit
testing methodologies and their associated theoretical assumptions. The current chapter outlines a
functional approach to implicit testing, describing research stemming from Relational Frame Theory
that was developed in parallel with the emergence of the IAT, and arguing for the benefits of connecting
these two strands of research to improve the understanding of attitude behaviors and create better
understood implicit testing methodologies. The chapter concludes with descriptions of two examples of
such methodologies: the IRAP and the FAST.
Chapter 9
The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem from Social Threats.............................. 183
Jordan B. Leitner, University of Delaware, USA
Chad E. Forbes, University of Delaware, USA

Previous research has demonstrated that people have the goal of self-enhancing, or viewing themselves in
an overly positive light. However, only recent research has examined the degree to which the relationship
between self-enhancement goals and outcomes are a result of explicit deliberative mechanisms or implicit
automatic mechanisms. The current chapter reviews evidence on unconscious goal pursuit, autobiographical
memory, social neuroscience, and implicit self-esteem that suggests that implicit mechanisms play a
powerful role in producing self-enhancement outcomes. Furthermore, this chapter reviews evidence
that these implicit mechanisms are activated by social threats and thus contribute to successful coping.
Finally, the authors discuss the implications of implicit self-enhancement mechanisms for targets of
stigma, individuals who frequently encounter threats to well-being.

Chapter 10
How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet............................... 205
Albena Antonova, Sofia University, Bulgaria

The influence of Internet on knowledge acquisition increases as it became the primary source of reference,
especially for young generations. Therefore, it is important to understand how social factors and Internet
influences not only explicit but also implicit knowledge construction. Several theories explain the impact of
the social groups on the knowledge formation as Social Cognitive Theory of Bandura and Lay Epistemic
Theory of Kruglanski. Thus, the authors propose an analysis of the implicit knowledge construction
processes through the theoretical overview of Internet communication. More specifically, they discuss
how the mechanisms of Internet increase the social and group impact on the implicit knowledge processes.

Chapter 11
Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions: Implicit Cognition in Action............. 216
Lucija Jančec, Society “Our Children” Opatija, Croatia
Sanja Tatalović Vorkapić, University of Rijeka, Croatia
Jurka Lepičnik Vodopivec, University of Primorska, Slovenia

The hidden curriculum is described in terms of values, attitudes, norms, rules, and rituals that are taught
in school, but its main characteristic is its unplanned and officially unorganized form. It appears in every
day kindergarten and school routine where children and pupils learn it as the official curriculum. From
the psychological aspect, it comes close to implicit learning. In this chapter, the authors argue that the
influence of the hidden curriculum and the implicit learning deserve far more attention and scientific
research because of their significant contribution to the learning outcomes and for their constant presence
in the education process: in a learning organization that means all the time and for all the participants.
Chapter 12
Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script in Persistent Virtual Worlds: Theoretical Foundations .
and Decision Processes for Constructing a Socio-Cultural Learning Framework............................... 243
Nikolaos Pellas, University of the Aegean, Greece

The radical utilization of collaborative learning processes in Three-Dimensional (3D) multi-user virtual
worlds has been widely investigated. However, a study to delineate a comprehensive pedagogical model
aimed at facilitating the same productive-argumentative knowledge practices is still lacking. This chapter
presents the most reliable principles of contemporary socio-cognitive learning theories and teaching
models. These socio-cognitive constructs can reinforce the majority of collaborative social-cultural students’
pursuits and capabilities in the learning process in order to better handle their cohesion or coordination
with other peers in 3D multi-user virtual worlds. Consequently, with the adaptation of the Progressive
Inquiry (PI) teaching model, this chapter proposes a novel framework for the implementation of this
model in online collaborative processes that can be held in 3D multi-user virtual worlds. Educational
implications for a reputable instructional format with the conjunction of the PI model and further
suggestions are also discussed.

Afterword............................................................................................................................................ 281

Related References............................................................................................................................. 283

Compilation of References................................................................................................................ 307

About the Contributors..................................................................................................................... 364

Index.................................................................................................................................................... 369
xiii

Preface

Beginning in the 1950s, behaviorism gradually began to lose its dominant position in psychology as
interest in consciousness research was revitalized. Unsatisfied with speculating on the interactions be-
tween environmental stimuli and behavioral responses, psychologists attempted to identify and better
understand the internal mental structures mediating the stimulus-response process and its underlying
mechanisms. Scholars soon realized that several mental structures and mechanisms governing human
behavior are independent of consciousness and therefore cannot be studied through introspection and
self-control. Further research eventually revealed that implicit cognition is, like explicit cognition, a
common cognitive mechanism in humans. Furthermore, it was shown to be a fundamental and primary
regulator whose functions could be performed by explicit cognitive mechanisms.
As a basic form of cognition, implicit cognition has various manifestations, including perception of
subliminal stimuli, repetitive priming effects, unconscious learning, and rule generalization/abstraction.
Implicit cognition is a hierarchical structural system that can both process surface features (e.g., stimuli)
and extract the inherent laws of complex systems.

METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES WITH IMPLICIT MEASURES

Unconscious cognition has long been the subject of philosophical debate. Western psychoanalytic theory
suggests that humans have a conscious and unconscious mind. The unconscious mind predominates;
Freud likened it metaphorically to the part of the iceberg beneath the sea’s surface (Schneider, 1993).
Experimental study of unconscious cognition can be traced back to Ebbinghaus’s memory research. In
1885, Ebbinghaus classified memory into three categories, two associated with consciousness and one
with unconscious memory. Ebbinghaus stated that forgotten experiences hidden from consciousness
cannot be fully consciously recalled but can exert an important influence on the memory. Later, Thorn-
dike and McDougall et al. also described and classified unconscious memory, but for decades, most
human cognition research was limited to conscious cognition. In the 1970s, Warrington et al.’s study of
implicit memory in patients with amnesia and Reber et al.’s study on the implicit learning of artificial
grammar inaugurated the systematic study of unconscious cognitive processes. Currently, it is gener-
ally believed that the unconscious cognitive paradigms include implicit memory, implicit learning, and
automatic processing. Technological innovations have facilitated the development of scientific theories;
as in other areas of psychology, the progress of implicit cognition research depends on the sophistica-
tion of its research methods. In particular, implicit cognition research has benefited from the gradual
improvement of the indirect measurement methods used in implicit memory research. Implicit cognition
xiv

research methods based on the response time paradigm became popular among researchers due to their
innovativeness. These methods were then employed to develop a series of research methods, including
the Implicit Association Test (IAT) (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) and its variants and the
priming paradigm and its variants. These response time paradigm-based research methods have been
subjected to scrutiny and debate during their development; for example, the IAT has been challenged
by the Quad Model (Conrey, Sherman, Gawronski, Hugenberg, & Groom, 2005) and the replicability of
the priming paradigm has been brought into question (e.g. Doyen, Klein, Pichon, & Cleeremans, 2012;
LeBel & Lorne, in press; Shanks, et al., 2013). Therefore, the effects of these research methods on the
theoretical and practical applications of implicit cognition should be further explored.
Dentale, Vecchione, and Barbaranelli (Chapter 1) explore the results and validity of the IAT when
used to assess the Big Five personality traits. They show that the five factors of personality are adequately
assessed by the IAT and its variants, such as the questionnaire-based IAT. They also demonstrate that
implicit measures of traits primarily reveal the stable inter-individual differences that predict actual
behavior and should be distinguished conceptually from explicit measures of the Big Five. Moreover,
implicit measures were found to be less prone to error than self-report measures. Christina Bermeitinger
(Chapter 2) examines different types of priming and their features and principal elements using several
organizing principles, including macro-, midi-, and micro-perspectives; type of dependent variable;
and task. She summarizes several important theories of response priming, semantic priming, affective/
evaluative priming, negative priming, and macro-level priming and discusses several general problems
researchers encounter when applying priming methodologies. A brief overview is given.

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCE, PROCESSING SKILLS, AND IMPLICIT LEARNING

For decades, psychological research, dominated by cognitive psychology, has been expanding the scope
of consciousness research, resulting in the development of new fields such as implicit learning. Prior to
1990, studies on individual differences primarily improved researchers’ understanding of independent
implicit learning systems. The artificial grammar paradigm proposed that, in contrast to the large age
difference observed in explicit learning capability, no differences were observed in the implicit learn-
ing capabilities of adults and children. This finding was confirmed by several later pathological and
neuropsychological studies. These studies found that patients with Alzheimer’s disease and amnesia
have impaired explicit learning abilities. However, their implicit learning abilities are less affected
by their dysfunction or disorder, and they have the same capacity to learn complex rules as a healthy
person (c.f., Machado, et al., 2009). In addition to age and neuropathological factors, intelligence also
does not affect implicit learning (e.g., Kaufman, et al., 2010). These findings interested scholars, and a
number of studies were subsequently conducted on a variety of variables, such as neuro-injury, age, and
intelligence. Since the 1990s, in addition to continued exploration of the impact of factors such as age
and intelligence, researchers have pursued the comprehensive analysis and theoretical construction of
individual difference factors. According to Reber, implicit learning is a basic human learning system and
a primitive knowledge acquisition mechanism, an adaptation mechanism preserved through long-term
evolution and natural selection. Using the laws of evolution, he deduced that implicit learning is age-
independent, highly stable, IQ-independent, and has lower individual variability than explicit learning.
The hypothesis that implicit learning has lower individual variability has been confirmed by a number
of studies, but not all studies have supported this hypothesis. Spurred by the dispute that ensued, Reber
xv

suggested that the so-called “individual differences” resulted from other variables, “the differences that
did not cause differences,” an assertion that appears to be reasonable (Reber, 1989; Reber & Allen,
2000). Meta-analyses have demonstrated that the discovery of individual differences is closely related
to the methodology used. Multiple factors, such as conceptual approaches, experimental materials, and
experimental paradigms, have a direct impact on the experimental results. Daisuke Nakamura (Chapter
3) reviews studies examining whether individual differences in psychometric intelligence, working
memory, and other less investigated variables, such as emotion and personality, affect implicit learning.
Nakamura particularly focuses on Reber’s evolutionary theory and Kaufman’s dual-process theory of
implicit learning. This chapter concludes that the findings on the effects of these individual difference
variables on implicit learning are, with the exception of psychometric intelligence, which is still incon-
clusive, and provides suggestions for future research.
From a theoretical perspective, research on implicit learning has provided new insights into the nature
of human cognitive processes and has helped identify the psychological mechanisms underlying the pro-
cesses used to acquire abundant and complex knowledge. It is imperative to develop a theoretical frame-
work for understanding the thought processes associated with implicit cognitive vulnerability (Chapter
4). Therefore, the theoretical and modeling efforts in this section focus on theories of implicit memory,
Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain, and Anderson and Krathwohl’s Revised Taxonomy of
the Cognitive Domain. The objectives of Chapter 4 are to develop an implicit cognitive framework that
facilitates the advances in conception and understanding proposed by Bloom’s work and Anderson and
Krathwohl’s revision. Several aspects of implicit cognitive vulnerability suggest the dissonance, resis-
tance, fear, creativity, inspiration, and innovative effort associated with the increased exertion and shift
in processing experienced by learners as they engage in cognitively focused higher order thinking skills.

SOCIAL COGNITIVE PERSPECTIVES ON ATTITUDES

Cognitive psychologists believe that most of the stimuli discussed in the socio-cognitive literature have
social significance and thus lead to complex psychological reactions in participants. These stimuli can
be either consistent with or in contradiction to the participants’ inherent needs or implicit attitudes. Due
to differences in the social significance of the stimuli, the complexity of information processing varies
among participants, resulting in different response times. In rapid response tasks, participants’ responses
to the stimuli are not controlled consciously, and the observed social cognition can therefore be consid-
ered implicit. As mentioned earlier, IAT remains the most important measurement method. Although its
theories and methodology remain in question, this method has been widely used in studies on various
topics, including implicit attitude, ego, self-esteem, stereotypes, and so on. In addition, because of its
flexibility in adopting different target and attribute concepts, this method can also be adapted to measure
different aspects of implicit cognition. Chapter 5 presents the results of an audio IAT test, which support
the incorporation of concepts and methods from Implicit Social Cognition (ISC) into language attitudes
research. Measuring both implicit and explicit attitudes provides a more complete picture of reactions to
foreign accented speech. Therefore, the integration of concepts and methods from the ISC domain can
contribute significantly to language attitudes research.
Implicit attitudes have long been understood as unconscious traces accumulated from past experi-
ences and attitudes. Such traces and their impact are beyond an individual’s conscious awareness, but
they potentially affect one’s emotional orientation, awareness, and behavior towards a socio-object.
xvi

Therefore, exploration of implicit attitudes is undoubtedly crucial to understanding social behaviors


or tendencies. Chapter 6 presents an empirical study that represents an important attempt into gaining
a fuller understanding of the effects of stereotype threat on the implicit processes that may underlie
performance and motivation in stereotyped domains. Kelso and Brody’s chapter indicates that implicit
emotions often differ from self-reported emotions in meaningful ways that can inform the field’s under-
standing of stereotype threat.
Researchers have suggested that implicit attitudes guide automatic behavior, playing a pivotal role
in the automatic processes involved in addictive behaviors. Chapter 7 analyzes the impact of implicit
attitudes towards smoking, primarily by examining smoking behavior, to identify the factors that should
be considered when investigating implicit attitudes toward smoking. The basic mechanisms underlying
implicit attitudes are not well understood. Chapter 8 describes a new method whose functional approach
toward implicit attitude research has the potential to clarify several outstanding issues. Within a functional
framework, attitudes can be measured as histories of verbal behavior. Functionally oriented research into
implicit attitude mechanisms has produced alternative testing methodologies.

THE ROLE OF IMPLICIT MECHANISMS IN SELF-ENHANCEMENT

Since James’s (1890) proposal of the concept of ego and division of the ego into the “empirical ego” and
“pure ego,” the ego has been vigorously researched by psychologists. Similar to learning and attitude,
self-motive is divided into two types: implicit motive and explicit motive. Explicit self-motive is a cogni-
tive representation of personal values derived from conscious thinking and is relatively well controlled.
Explicit self-motive can be developed through speech-transmission-based socialization experiences and
is determined by conscious choices made after careful consideration. The concept of implicit self-motive
falls under the purview of implicit self-evaluation. Implicit self-motive is a result of self-communication
that occurs during unconscious and relatively uncontrolled excessive learning. In other words, implicit
self-evaluation is developed in the absence of introspection. Self-enhancement is a common motive.
Individuals tend to use more positive words to describe themselves and typically believe that they possess
more positive personality traits than others. Self-enhancement is developed through speech-transmission-
based socialization experiences and can be inferred by observing spontaneous or habitual responses.
Thus, individuals’ unconscious positive attitudes towards themselves or the display of such attitudes
during automatic processing are considered instances of implicit self-enhancement. Chapter 9 reviews
the evidence on unconscious goal pursuit, autobiographical memory, social neuroscience, and implicit
self-esteem that suggests that implicit mechanisms play a significant role in producing self-enhancement
outcomes. This chapter also reviews evidence that implicit mechanisms are activated by social threats
and thus contribute to successful coping. The implications of implicit self-enhancement mechanisms
for targets of stigma, who frequently encounter threats to their well-being, are discussed in this chapter.

TACIT KNOWLEDGE AND EDUCATIONAL TECHNOLOGY

As an important component of implicit cognitive processes, tacit knowledge is, like implicit memory and
implicit learning, implicit (Reber, 1989). From an information-processing perspective, implicit memory
does not require intentional recall, and implicit knowledge tasks can be automatically affected by an
xvii

individual’s past experience. Implicit learning is associated with unconscious learning processes, while
implicit knowledge is more closely associated with the products or outcomes of learning. As Polanyi
suggests, explicit knowledge can be fully expressed through human symbol systems. In contrast, implicit
knowledge refers to knowledge maintained in the mind but not readily articulated. In this Information
Age, computer network-centered information technology is changing the way people access knowledge.
Implicit knowledge acquisition requires a free and open environment; depending on the scenario, learners
engage in implicit knowledge construction through collaboration and communication during the problem-
solving process. Chapter 10 explores how social factors influence implicit knowledge construction on the
Internet. Albena Antonova analyzes implicit knowledge construction theories in the context of Internet
communication. More specifically, she analyzes how the Internet increases the impact of social factors
on implicit knowledge construction.
From a practical perspective, the theories and methods used to study implicit learning have been
applied in various fields, including advertising, social interaction, economics, and management. The
outcomes of implicit learning are more effective than those of explicit learning, raising the question of
whether implicit learning has positive implications for education. I believe that concepts such as Bloom’s
taxonomy (also mentioned in Chapter 4), for example, have had an extensive and profound effect on class-
room teaching practices in Mainland China. Bloom’s taxonomy divides classroom educational objectives
into six levels. In its implementation of the taxonomy, China’s Ministry of Education has modified the
definitions of the six levels. “Comprehension,” the second-most basic educational objective, is generally
considered accomplished when the learner can understand the meaning of the knowledge or material and
can convert, interpret, and infer from the acquired knowledge in his or her own language. This definition
clearly excludes the possibility of implicit learning. In such a scientism-oriented educational paradigm,
the concept of “comprehension” is absolutely rational, and logical thinking is assumed to prevail in the
mental processes. Therefore, general education institutions struggle to increase students’ true understand-
ing of the world. According to the theory of implicit learning, during the teaching process, especially
when teaching scientific principles, it is unnecessary to force students to intentionally learn the presented
material; instead, it is more important to ensure that they understand the meaning and significance of
natural laws by engaging in appropriate practice. This viewpoint is consistent those discussed in Chap-
ter 11. Jančec, Vorkapić, and Vodopivec argue that the influence of the hidden curriculum and implicit
learning deserve increased attention and scientific research because of their significant contribution to
learning outcomes and their constant presence in the education process: in a learning environment, they
are present all the time and for all the participants.
We believe that the theory of implicit cognition can provide a theoretical foundation for the devel-
opment of educational information technologies. Implicit knowledge acquisition requires an informal
learning environment. Because of its situationality, interactivity, and convenient management tools, an
Internet-based virtual learning environment supports the conversion of implicit knowledge into explicit
knowledge, and an individual’s construction of implicit knowledge. Chapter 12 presents a literature
review of the most reliable contemporary socio-cognitive learning theories and models (e.g., the Innova-
tive Behavioral Model) developed in the early 21st century. These socio-cognitive constructs reinforce
the majority of students’ collaborative social-cultural pursuits and capabilities in the learning process.
A novel framework for implementing collaborative processes through 3D multi-user virtual worlds is
proposed in this chapter.
xviii

CONCLUSION

This book provides a meaningful research tool by describing new ideas and the latest achievements in
the field of implicit cognition. It is a convenient reference for undergraduates, graduate students, and
educational researchers engaging in cognitive and social psychological research and also provides useful
information for those interested in implicit cognition.

Zheng Jin
Zhengzhou Normal University, China & University of California, Davis, USA

REFERENCES

Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., & Groom, C. J. (2005). Separating mul-
tiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 89(4), 469–487.
Doyen, S., Klein, O., Pichon, C. L., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Behavioral priming: It’s all in the mind,
but whose mind? PLoS ONE, 7, e29081.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit
cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry.
Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jiménez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit
learning as an ability. Cognition, 116(3), 321–340.
LeBel, E. P., & Campbell, L. (2013). Heightened sensitivity to temperature cues in individuals with high
anxious attachment real or elusive phenomenon? Psychological Science.
Machado, S., Cunha, M., Minc, D., Portella, C. E., Velasques, B., & Basile, L. F. et al. (2009). Alzheimer’s
disease and implicit memory. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 67(2A), 334–342.
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Gen-
eral, 118, 219–235.
Reber, A. S., & Allen, R. (2000). Individual differences in implicit learning: Implications for the evolu-
tion of consciousness. In R. G. Kunzendorf & B. Wallace (Eds.), Individual differences in conscious
experience (pp. 227–247). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Schneider, S. (2003). The mystical and the spiritual in the large group. In S. Schneider & H. Weinberg
(Eds.), The large group re-visited. London: Jessica Kingsley.
xix

Acknowledgment

My sincere gratitude goes to the chapter authors who contributed their time and expertise to this book.
Some of the authors also served as referees; I appreciate their double task.

I wish to acknowledge the valuable contributions of the reviewers regarding the improvement of quality,
coherence, and content presentation of chapters, especially, Prof. Martin Voracek, Prof. Dirk Wentura,
and Prof. Jan De Houwer. In addition, I would like to acknowledge Dr. Keith Payne’s advice regarding
the editing of this book. I also appreciate the support from China Scholarship Council (CSC).

Zheng Jin
Zhengzhou Normal University, China & University of California, Davis, USA
xx

Introduction

Implicit Cognition: A New Window for Understanding Human Behavior

The cognition activities of humans are an integration of conscious and unconscious processes. The
existence, universality and effectiveness of implicit cognition indicate that the cognitive process can be
classified into explicit cognition and implicit cognition systems. They comprise two types of information
processing systems with different natures, which are relatively independent of each other in terms of
structure and function. According to their roles in the cognitive process, explicit cognition is the basic
cognitive approach, while implicit cognition is a necessary and independent source of cognition. These
two cognition systems interact synergistically and can be transformed into each other under certain con-
ditions. They work together to help humans understand the world. Research into implicit cognition has
expanded our exploration of human cognition activities to a much larger scope of the cognition process,
which is beyond consciousness control, cannot be expressed in language, and exists in a hidden process.
This research area has long been neglected by psychology and epistemology. Such neglect is directly
related to the fact that implicit cognition itself is difficult to control consciously or express in language;
it is also partially attributed to the level of knowledge and research means available in the past. This
forgotten corner has recently begun to draw much attention because many difficulties and challenges
encountered in the developmental process of cognitive science are rooted in the implicit cognition do-
main. The collective efforts of philosophy, psychology and other disciplines are thus urgently needed.
Cognitive psychology is closely related to epistemology. Both of these disciplines concern the spiritual
world and the human cognitive realm, differing only in their research methods and levels of expression.
The exploration of the underlying mechanism of implicit cognition was inspired by a series of important
changes in cognitive theories in recent years. These types of changes have deepened our understanding
about the basic characteristics of the internal information of cognitive subjects and greatly expanded
our knowledge regarding the nature and concept of information processing. It offers new explanatory
approaches for many previous theories that resided on the experience description level as well as for
many confusing experimental phenomena.
Discussions about implicit cognition have increased markedly in the past two decades. A large number
of publications have also appeared, from the most famous methodology in the implicit social cognition
field, Implicit Association Test (IAT), to Psychology of Science: Implicit and Explicit Processes, edited
by Proctor and Capaldi, and then to the latter Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition, edited by Gawronski
and Payne. However, arguments about the theories, methods and techniques of implicit cognition have
never ceased. For example, oppugners of the social priming effect even subverted the “scientificity”


xxi

of psychology (e.g., Kahneman, 2012; Doyen, et al., 2012; Yong, 2012) and questioning the implicit
components has also become a research focus in recent years (e.g., Conrey, et al., 2005; Sherman, 2006).
This is acceptable if one simply considers this book to be a supplement to the research field of implicit
cognition. Our efforts are directed towards understanding those thinking processes that have been ex-
cluded by the conscious mind and by the research field for some time. After all, we have always believed
that at least a part of unconscious information processing is carried on silently in the human thinking
process without interference. More importantly, this book offers a new approach to the research on im-
plicit cognition: as mentioned above, the exploration of implicit cognition urgently needs the collective
efforts of psychology and other disciplines. One such effort is to comprehensively consider the common
problems involved in different disciplines, which is actually the basic method of cognitive science. In
fact, the development of cognitive science metalogic and the long-term accumulation of experimental
materials in the past provide a theoretical and experimental basis for such efforts.

Zheng Jin
Zhengzhou Normal University, China & University of California, Davis, USA

REFERENCES

Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., & Groom, C. J. (2005). Separating mul-
tiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 89(4), 469–487. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.469
Doyen, S., Klein, O., Pichon, C. L., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Behavioral priming: It’s all in the mind,
but whose mind? PLoS ONE, 7(1), e29081. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0029081
Kahneman, D. (2012). A proposal to deal with questions about priming effects. Nature.
Sherman, J. W. (2006). On building a better process model: It’s not only how many, but which ones and
by which means. Psychological Inquiry, 17(3), 173–184. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1703_3
Yong, E. (2012). Replication studies: Bad copy. Nature, 485(7398), 7398. doi:10.1038/485298a
1

Chapter 1
Applying the IAT to Assess
Big Five Personality Traits:
A Brief Review of Measurement
and Validity Issues

Francesco Dentale
Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Michele Vecchione
Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Claudio Barbaranelli
Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

ABSTRACT
This chapter reviews the studies that applied the Implicit Association Test (IAT) to assess the Big Five
personality traits, focusing on issues related to measurement and validity. After a brief introduction on
the implicit self-concept of personality, the following five issues are critically reviewed: (1) the experi-
mental procedure of both classical and questionnaire-based Big Five IATs, (2) the factorial structure
and reliability of the implicit traits and the degree of dissociation between implicit and explicit measures,
(3) the state and trait components of implicit scores, (4) the predictive validity of the Big-Five IATs with
respect to relevant behavioural criteria, and (5) the robustness to faking of the Big Five IATs. Future
research directions for the implicit measures of the Big Five were discussed.

INTRODUCTION Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism


(or Emotional Stability), and Openness (or Intel-
As widely demonstrated by many years of theoreti- lect). The five-factor structure has proved to be
cal and empirical research, human personality can stable across time (Costa & McCrae, 1988) and
be adequately described referring to five general consistent across different languages and raters
dimensions, namely Extraversion (or Energy), (McCrae & Allik, 2002). Furthermore, empirical

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch001

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

studies have demonstrated the predictive valid- to assess the implicit self-concept of personality
ity of the five factors in diverse areas of human while self-report scales refer to the explicit self-
functioning, as mental health (Widiger, Costa, concept of personality.
& McCrae, 2002), job performance (Barrick, In this regard, Back, Schmukle and Egloff
Mount, & Judge, 2001), and academic achieve- (2008) proposed a Behavioral Process Model of
ment (Poropat, 2009). Personality (BPMP), derived by the Reflective–
Classical measures of personality traits are Impulsive Model by Strack and Deutsch (2004).
list of adjectives, markers of the Big Five, and The BPMP assumes an implicit and an explicit way
phrase-based questionnaires aimed at measur- of social information processing, both converging
ing the five dimensions. However, self-report on behavioral schemata. According to this model,
measures suffer from well-known limitations, when a stimulus or a situation trigger the cognitive
including: a) their vulnerability to faking, namely system, associative (implicit) and propositional
individual’s deliberate alteration of responses (explicit) representations are activated, both of
aimed to present a favourable impression (Griffith which are able to regulate behavioral schemata.
& Peterson, 2008), and b) the introspective lim- Personality self-attributions may be connected to
its of respondents, that may lead to inaccurate the characteristics of the situation, to motivational
responses. In order to overcome these problems, tendencies of the individual, and the subsequent
several authors have recently developed a series impulsive behaviors, developing a pattern of
of experimental paradigms that permit to assess chronic associations that can be automatically
many psychological constructs without the use of and implicitly activated. Therefore, implicit
subjective introspection (see Gawronski & Payne, personality self-attributions can be conceived as
2010, for a review). a consequence of many precedent activations of
Among implicit measures, the most popular automatic behaviors, that ultimately assume the
experimental paradigm is the Implicit Associa- form of implicit chronic associations. In contrast,
tion Test (IAT, Greenwald, McGhee & Schwartz, explicit representations of the personality self-
1998). Although the IAT has been originally devel- concept refer to controlled behavior, and assume
oped to measure stereotypes and prejudices, it has the form of a propositional self-judgments. In the
been subsequently applied to other psychological section of this chapter that deals with the predictive
constructs (Greenwald, Nosek & Banaji, 2003; validity of implicit traits we will illustrate that,
Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann & Banaji, 2009; as a consequence of their sensitivity to chronic
Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2007), including automatic associations, Big Five IATs are expected
personality traits. to be better predictors of impulsive behavior
Depending on target-categories and attributes than self-report scales. Implicit Big Five, on the
that are employed, the IAT can be applied to assess contrary, should be worse predictors of reflective
the association between the self and a specific behavior as they are not appropriate to evaluate
personality trait. According to Asendorpf, Banse deliberate self-judgments.
and Mücke (2002), a part of the self-concept In this chapter, the main issues addressed by
regards some typical personality characteristics implicit Big Five literature are reported and criti-
(such as traits), traditionally measured through cally examined, focusing the attention on: (1) the
psychometric instruments, that are labeled as the experimental procedures developed to assess the
personality self-concept. Big five IATs, as well Big Five using the IAT (e.g., Back, Schmukle &
as other implicit measures of personality, allow Egloff, 2009; Schnabel, Asendorpf & Greenwald,

2

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

2008, (2) the factor structure and reliability of left of the screen or into two opposite target and
the implicit Big Five personality measures and attribute categories (e.g. others and introversion),
the degree of dissociation between implicit and located on the right of the screen. Subsequently,
explicit measures (Schmukle, Back & Egloff, they are asked to perform a second task in which
2008), (3) state and trait components of implicit the two targets are switched (e.g., others, and
scores (Schmukle & Egloff, 2005), (4) the predic- extraversion, and me and introversion). The two
tive validity of the Big-Five IATs with respect to categorization tasks have to be performed as
relevant behavioural criteria (Back et al., 2009; quickly and accurately as possible.
Perugini, 2005; Perugini, Richetin & Zogmaister, Most of earlier studies used 4 or 5 stimuli-
2010; Steffens & Schulze-König, 2006), and (5) words for each category, with words presented in
the robustness to faking of Big Five IATs (e.g. random order within each block of trials. Using
Steffens, 2004). the classical procedure (Greenwald et al., 1998),
each Big Five IAT consists of seven blocks of
trials: 1 (me vs. not-me), 2 (e.g. extraversion vs.
1. EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES introversion) and 5 (not-me vs. me) are single
OF BIG FIVE IATs categorization blocks of 20 trials whereas 3-4 and
6-7 are combined blocks (me or extraversion vs.
A way to assess the Big Five implicitly is to not-me or introversion) of 20 (3-6) and 40 (4-7)
develop five different IATs (Greenwald et al., trials (see Table 1 for more details).
1998; Schmukle & Egloff, 2005) in which the The IAT scores are based on the difference
target labels (e.g. me vs. not-me) and their relative between the mean response latencies in each task.
stimuli-words are the same for each IAT, whereas Assuming that reaction times are faster when
the attribute labels (e.g., extraversion vs. intro- the target concept and the attribute are strongly
version; agreeableness vs. disagreeableness; connected in memory than when they are not
conscientiousness vs. carelessness; anxiety vs. (Greenwald et al., 1998), the size of such differ-
calmness; openness vs. narrow-mindedness) and ence can be used to infer the extent to which the
their relative stimuli-words varied across traits. individual’s self-concept is associated to a given
In each IAT, respondents are asked to press personality characteristic, such as extraversion.
a left- or right-hand key to classify a series of In order to compute the individual’s score in
stimuli-words into a target and an attribute cat- the implicit trait, referred to as D score, an ap-
egory (e.g., me, and extraversion) located on the propriate algorithm is applied (Greenwald et al.,

Table 1. Task sequence and stimuli of an implicit association test to measure the implicit personality
self-concept of extraversion (modified from Schnabel, Asendorpf & Greenwald, 2008)

Block N of Trials Task Left Key Right Key


1 20 Target discrimination Me Others
2 20 Attribute discrimination Introversion Extraversion
3 20 First block of first combined task Me, Introversion Others, Extraversion
4 40 Second block of first combined task Me, Introversion Others, Extraversion
5 40 Reversed target discrimination Others Me
6 20 First block of second combined task Others, Introversion Me, Extraversion
7 40 Second block of second combined task Others, Introversion Me, Extraversion

3

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

2003). In this algorithm the difference between the sentences, concerning self-related events that are
mean latencies of the first (e.g., Me, Introversion necessarily true for the experimental subjects (e.g.
vs. Others, Extraversion) and second (e.g. Others, I’m participating in an experiment in psychology)
Introversion vs. Me, Extraversion) combined block while introversion self-related sentences (e.g. I
is calculated and divided for the pooled standard don’t talk a lot) have to be associated with other
deviation of all trials, using the following formula: sentences necessarily false for the experimental
subjects (e.g. I’m climbing a steep mountain). In a
D scores = (M1 – M2) / SD; second categorization task, self-related sentences
concerning extraversion have to be associated with
where M1 = mean latency of blocks 3-4, M2 = self-related sentences that are necessarily false
mean latency of blocks 6-7, SD = pooled standard while introversion self-related sentences have to
deviation of all trials. be associated with other sentences necessarily true.
As mentioned before, the classic IAT experi- Therefore, in Yovel and Friedman’s variant, the
mental paradigm uses single words or pictures as qIAT should measure the degree to which a set
stimuli, showing a certain degree of flexibility of extraversion items (derived from an ordinary
for different applications in many psychological self-report questionnaire) are associated with
research areas. However, this paradigm does not necessarily true versus false self-related sentences.
permit to develop implicit measures that make use
of sentences as stimuli, reducing in this manner
its applicability to several domains. 2. PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES
A variant of the classic IAT that permits to OF BIG FIVE IATs: CONSTRUCT
identify which of two contrasting autobiographical VALIDITY, INTERNAL
events is true for a subject has been recently de- CONSISTENCY, AND IMPLICIT-
veloped. This procedure, named Autobiographical EXPLICIT DISSOCIATION
IAT (aIAT, Sartori, Agosta, Zogmaister, Ferrara &
Castiello, 2008), showed adequate psychometric Validity of the five-factor model for the implicit
properties and, across different studies, exhibited self-concept of personality was investigated in
high levels of criterion validity. A similar vari- two studies (Schmukle et al., 2008) applying
ant, the Questionnaire based IAT (qIAT; Yovel & confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) on partial IAT
Friedman, 2013), has been developed to measure scores for all dimensions. In the first study, partial
indirectly a number of psychological constructs, scores were based on latency differences of single
using sentences derived from questionnaire items. couples of opposite adjectives (e.g., sociable – shy)
Self-report personality measures are typically on compatible (e.g. Me/Extraversion vs. Other/
based on the degree to which respondents consider Introversion) and no compatible (Me/Introversion
a series of short statements as accurate descrip- vs. Other/Extraversion) blocks. Log transformed
tions of their personalities. The qIAT has been scores of these latency differences (Greenwald et
designed to reproduce the classical procedure of al., 1998) were allowed to load only on the factor
self-report scales but using an indirect method of to which they belong, according to five-factor
measurement. Specifically, subjects are instructed model. In the second study, partial scores were
to categorize a number of self-related sentences in based on halves of the IAT that directly derive
two different target categories, such as extraversion from Greenwald’s improved scoring algorithm
and introversion. In a first categorization task, (Greenwald et al., 2003). In both studies, a CFA
extraversion self-related sentences (e.g. I am the showed that the five-factor model fitted the data
life of the party) have to be associated with other well, supporting the validity of the FFM model

4

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

at the implicit level. This model fitted consider- the classic IAT documented acceptable levels of
ably better than an alternative model positing a internal consistency for all traits. In the study by
single factor representing a generalized tendency Schmukle et al. (2008), for example, Cronbach’s
to associate the self with positive rather than with alpha reliability coefficients ranged from .74 to
negative attributes, irrespective of the semantic .88. In the study by Grumm and Von Collani
content of the attribute stimuli. This suggests that (2007), split-half reliability coefficients ranged
implicit traits cannot be reduced to individual dif- from .64 to .82. Appropriate levels of internal
ferences in affective valence. Moreover, a second- consistency (split-half = .87) were found also
order CFA model, positing five first-order factors for the questionnaire-based IAT, although data
and one second-order factor, fitted the data worse on the psychometric properties of this procedure
than a model with five correlated factors. This are still scarce.
result suggests that factors intercorrelations can- A further issue has to do with the extent to which
not be reduced to affective valence. This has been implicit and explicit measures of traits refer to the
confirmed in a study by Grumm and von Collani same constructs. Results of different studies on big
(2007) showing that a self-esteem IAT (consid- five IATs (Grumm & von Collani, 2007; Schnabel
ered as an implicit measure of affective valence) et al., 2008; Schmukle et al., 2008) showed: (1)
is only moderately correlated with Extraversion low to moderate implicit-explicit correlations with
IAT (r = .22) and Neuroticism IAT (r = -.38), and small to medium variations among the studies
that the Extraversion-Neuroticism correlation (r for both classical and questionnaire-based IATs,
= -.51) remained substantially unchanged after and (2) patterns of means and correlations among
controlling for it (r = -.47). implicit measures that were similar to those of
In order to disentangle meaning associative the explicit measures. Moreover a CFA including
effects from valence confound, Schnabel, Asen- two correlated factors (i.e. implicit and explicit
dorpf and Greenwald (2008) developed two Big factors) for each trait fitted the data better than a
Five IATs and two self-report trait rating scales one-factor model that does not distinguish between
opposing personality categories (i.e., Conscien- implicit and explicit traits (Schnabel et al., 2008).
tiousness vs. Extraversion; Conscientiousness vs. Taken together, these findings suggest that Big
Agreeableness) that, differently from the classic Five IATs and self-report scales refer to distinct
Big Five IATs (e.g., Extraversion vs. Introversion), but related constructs.
were substantially equal in valence. Two versions
for each IAT and self-report scale were developed:
in the first version, only positive personality 3. STATE AND TRAIT COMPONENTS
attributes were included (e.g. conscientious vs. OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT
sociable), while the second version includes only PERSONALITY TRAITS
stimuli with a negative valence (e.g. chaotic vs.
reserved). Results showed that both implicit/ Similarly to self-report questionnaires, the Big
positive-implicit/negative and explicit/positive- Five IATs are expected to measure personality
explicit/negative correlations were moderate to dispositions that are relatively consistent across
high, suggesting that Big Five IATs and self-report situations. However, it cannot be excluded that oc-
scales are able to measure not only valence but casion specific components are involved in either
also semantic associations related to personality explicit or implicit personality measures. In this
self-concept. regard, the latent state-trait (LST) represents a
Regarding the reliability of the implicit mea- statistical model that can be used to estimate the
sures of the Big Five, studies conducted with impact of situational factors on specific measures

5

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

of traits. Specifically, LST models permits to parse posed a framework of seven different patterns of
the variance of a measure into trait and occasion- prediction. In this section of the chapter we will
specific components (e.g. Steyer, Schmitt & Eid, briefly review these patterns.
1999).
Schmukle and Egloff (2005) have used the 1. The simple association pattern refers to a
LST models to estimate the occasion specificity, single implicit measure that predicts a single
the consistency, and the reliability of two IATs behaviour. This pattern has been widely used
assessing anxiety and extraversion, comparing in many studies conducted within the social
them with structurally similar self-report scales. cognition approach (e.g., Arcuri, Castelli,
To this aim, two IATs and two self-report scales Galdi, Zogmaister & Amadori, 2008).
were administered to 65 subjects in two occa- 2. The moderation pattern is aimed at identi-
sions separated by a temporal interval of one fying the conditions that enhance or reduce
week. A model that included measurement error, a simple association between an implicit
occasion-specific and trait components was ap- measure and a criterion, measuring or
plied to the two IATs. A similar model was applied manipulating potential moderating factors
to the self-report scales. The LST models fitted in order to test their effect (e.g., Perugini,
the data well for both implicit and explicit tests. O’Gorman & Prestwich, 2007).
Reliability coefficients were all adequate (.82 for 3. The addictive pattern is based on the hypoth-
the anxiety-IAT,.96 for the anxiety scale, .88 for esis that an implicit measure may explain a
the extraversion-IAT, and .95 for the extraversion unique portion of variance of the criterion
scale). Both implicit and explicit tests seemed to over and above the explicit measure. In other
capture consistent stable components that were words, this pattern allows one to assess the
substantially higher (ranging from .56 to .81) than incremental validity of the implicit measure
occasion specificity components (ranging from (e.g. Schnabel, Banse & Asendorpf, 2006).
.02 to .26). However, the size of occasion-specific According to Perugini et al. (2010), using
variance was higher for IATs (.26 for anxiety and an implicit measure is likely to have practi-
.15 for extraversion) than for self-report scales (.09 cal costs related to its implementation that
for anxiety and .02 for extraversion), suggesting should be counterbalanced by appropriate
that situational factors influence more implicit than benefits, such as, first of all, the unique
explicit measures. Moreover, the implicit-explicit contribution that it provide with respect to
correlations of occasion-specific factors were not a corresponding explicit measure.
significant neither for anxiety nor for extraversion, 4. The multiplicative pattern assumes that
indicating that IATs and self-report scales were implicit and explicit measures interact
differently influenced by contextual variables. in the prediction of a behavioral criteria.
Empirically, this pattern is aimed at test-
ing whether the interaction term between
4. THE PREDICTIVE VALIDITY implicit and explicit measures provides a
OF BIG FIVE IATs significant unique contribution over and
above main effects (e.g. Schröder-Abé,
In order to give a comprehensive overview of Rudolph, Wiesner, & Schütz, 2007). A
the predictive validity of the implicit measures, possible interpretation of significant inter-
Perugini and colleagues (Perugini, 2005; Perugini, action terms involves the examination of
Richetin & Zogmaister, 2010) have recently pro- the discordance or concordance between

6

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

associative and propositional representa- whereas the other predicts both of them
tions underlying implicit and explicit test (e.g., Richetin, Perugini, Adjali, & Hurling,
scores (Strack & Deutsch, 2004). In the case 2007).
of discordance, the impact that associative 7. The double additive pattern is a logical pos-
and propositional representations have on sibility, only weakly supported by empirical
behavior could hamper each other, induc- studies. It assumes that both implicit and
ing a less fluent action in the execution of explicit measures predict spontaneous and
the behavior. In contrast, when associative deliberate behaviors. Spontaneous behaviors
and reflexive representations are congruent, and processes, indeed, may contain a minor
their different impact on behavior become but significant proportion of deliberation;
synergistic, making both implicit and explicit on the contrary, deliberate behaviors and
measures more predictive of behavior. In this processes may imply a minor but significant
regard, a recent meta-analysis by Greenwald proportion of spontaneity. As a consequence,
and colleagues (2009) documented an high it is possible that implicit and explicit mea-
predictive power of both IAT and self-report sures give unique and significant contribu-
measures in studies where implicit and tions to both spontaneous and deliberate
explicit measures were highly correlated. actions.
It is important to note that, according to
Perugini et al. (2010), this pattern does not Most of empirical studies on the predictive
necessarily conduct to a significant interac- validity of the IAT assumed an additive pattern of
tion between implicit and explicit measures. prediction. A recent meta-analytic study (Green-
Indeed, the synergistic effect that is due a wald et al., 2009) reviewed the predictive power of
substantial congruence between associative many IAT measures on social behavior, judgment,
and propositional representations may also and decision making, using an additive pattern
emerge indirectly, through stronger main model. The meta-analysis summarized findings
effects on behavioral criteria. of 122 reports included in 184 research studies.
5. The double-dissociation pattern assumes The review included studies in several research
that implicit measures are able to predict areas such as consumer preferences, Black–White
spontaneous behaviors but not deliberate interracial behavior, personality differences, clini-
ones. On the contrary, as anticipated in this cal phenomena, alcohol and drug use, non-racial
chapter, explicit measures are able to predict intergroup behavior, gender and sexual orientation,
deliberate behaviors but not spontaneous close relationships, and political preferences. As
ones (e.g., Friese, Hofmann, & Wänke, 2008; argued by Greenwald et al. (2009), four important
Perugini, 2005). Studies that used this pattern conclusions can be drawn: 1) IAT measures were
of prediction are very informative and stimu- found to be useful predictors of social behavior
lated researchers to formulate appropriate and judgment across all research areas; 2) IAT
interpretations of results within both mono and self-report measures predict the criterion
and dual representation models of social variables independently from each other, suggest-
information processing (see Greenwald & ing that both types of measures may be useful in
Nosek, 2008). surveys and applied research; 3) for consumer and
6. The partial (or asymmetrical, weak) dis- political preferences, both IAT and self-report
sociation pattern assumes that one between scales effectively predicted behavior, but explicit
implicit and explicit measures predict measures had greater predictive validity; 4) for
only spontaneous or deliberate behaviors, more socially sensitive research areas, such as

7

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

interracial and other intergroup behaviors, the scales were controlled for. This confirms that the
IAT had significantly greater predictive validity prediction of actual behavior can be enhanced by
than explicit measures. including these measures. Moreover, the predic-
As regards implicit and explicit Big Five addi- tive power of implicit traits did not depend by the
tive pattern, initial results for predictive validity confounding effect of valence as measured by a
were reported by Schmukle et al. (2008). These self-esteem IAT (Back et al., 2009).
authors showed that both the Big Five IATs and As regards the predictive validity of the qIAT,
the NEO-FFI scales correlated with self-reported both implicit and explicit measures of extraver-
behaviors, but the explicit personality measures sion were hypothesized to predict the number of
exhibited higher correlations. Importantly, the Big errors in another IAT task linked to impulsivity
Five IATs remained correlated with self-reported and proneness to error in reaction-time paradigms
behaviors, even after the explicit traits were con- (e.g., Dickman, 1990; Eysenck & Eysenck,
trolled for, supporting the incremental validity 1977; Kirkcaldy, 1984). Results confirmed the
of the Big Five IATs. More recently, the BPMP hypothesis, showing that both the explicit (r =
proposed by Back and colleagues (2009) specified .30) and the implicit measures of extraversion
explicit and implicit aspects of the self-concept of (r = .33) significantly predicted the number of
personality as unique predictors of actual behavior. errors on the additional IAT task. In order to test
An extensive behavioral study (N =130) on many the incremental validity of the extraversion qIAT,
different relevant social situations was conducted, a hierarchical regression has been conducted, by
permitting to measure more than 50 indicators of including the explicit measure of extraversion at
actual behavior, each assigned to the relative Big the first step and adding the qIAT at the second
Five dimension. Three main findings emerged: step. Results, presented in Table 2 indicated that
First, the predictive validity of self-reported traits, the qIAT explained a unique and significant
as measured by a standard questionnaire, was portion of variance of the number of errors, sup-
largely confirmed. Second, also implicit traits porting its incremental validity over the explicit
were able to predict actual behavior, but this was scale. Therefore, the extraversion qIAT seems to
true only for neuroticism and extraversion (and capture unique information that was not provided
in part for agreeableness). Third, the predictive by a self-report scale of extraversion that contains
power of neuroticism and extraversion IATs re- the same items.
mained stable even if the corresponding explicit

Table 2. Hierarchical regression analysis predicting number of errors in the additional task (modified
from Yovel & Friedman, 2013)

Steps Predictors ΔR2 Β


1 .09**
Explicit .30**
2 .06*
Explicit .21*
Implicit .26*
Note: Explicit and implicit measures were based on the same extraversion items.
qIAT = questionnaire-based implicit association test.
* p < .05; ** p < .01 ; *** p < .001.
1
Importantly to interpret the results, responses to these items had to be given as quickly as possible but the speed was neither enforced
nor measured.

8

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

As regards the double dissociation pattern, both prevalence and severity of response distortion
Steffens and Schulze König (2006) tested the (e.g. Donovan, Dwight & Hurtz, 2003). In a meta-
hypothesis that the Big Five IATs predict spon- analysis, a series of studies that employed either a
taneous behaviors better than explicit measures between- or a within-subjects experimental design
of the same traits. To this aim, the Big Five IATs were analyzed (Viswesvaran & Ones, 1999). In
were administered to 89 subjects (69 females and the between-subjects studies, the effect size of the
20 males). A set of indicators of spontaneous differences (Cohen’s d) across instructional condi-
behaviors associated to each trait were measured, tions (i.e. fake good vs. respond honestly) ranged
such as: 1) observer ratings of impulsive reactions from .48 (Agreeableness) to .65 (Openness). In
(in the case of neuroticism, for example, reactions the within-subjects studies, Cohen’s d ranged from
to a stressful situation were observed); 2) self- .47 (Agreeableness) to .93 (Emotional stability).
ratings under time pressure (in the case of open- On the basis of these results, it seems particu-
ness, for example, participants were asked how larly important to supplement the use of self-report
often attend theater and museum exhibitions)1. questionnaires with appropriate implicit measures,
Results showed that observer ratings of impulsive such as the Big Five IATs, that are more diffi-
reactions correlated in the expected direction cult to fake. As expected, indeed, many studies
with the neuroticism, extraversion, agreeable- showed that the IAT is less prone to impression
ness, and conscientiousness IATs (Pearson’s rs management confounds than self-report mea-
ranged from .22 to .36). Correlations with explicit sures. Moreover, it has been shown that the IAT
measures, by contrast, were not significant. On predicts behavioural criteria even if they refer to
the contrary, self-ratings of behaviors associated social sensitive concerns and to situations at risk
with extraversion and openness correlated with of impression management biases (Greenwald et
the corresponding NEO-FFI scales (with r = .22 al., 2009).
and r = .29, respectively) but not with the IATs. The IAT, however, is not completely immune
As a potential explanation, authors suggested that to faking, especially when respondents are not new
participants might have ignored the instruction to to the technique and when they are instructed on
respond as quickly as possible. As a consequence, how to control IAT scores (e.g., Steffens, 2004).
their responses may have been guided primarily Interestingly, recent studies (Agosta, Ghirardi,
by controlled processes. On the whole, according Zogmaister, Castiello & Sartori, 2010; Cvencek,
to the authors, results of this study are in line with Greenwald, Brown, Gray & Snowden, 2011) intro-
the double dissociation pattern of prediction, as duced several indexes that have proved to be par-
automatic components of behavior seem to be bet- ticularly useful in detecting fakers and in adjusting
ter predicted by IATs than by explicit measures, faked IAT scores. Specifically, the Combined Task
while controlled components of behaviors seem Slowing (CTS) indicator developed by Cvencek
to be better predicted by explicit scales. et al. (2011) allowed to correctly classify 75% of
fakers, while the indexes developed by Agosta et
al. (2010) were able to distinguish fakers from
5. ROBUSTNESS OF non-fakers in the 83% of cases.
PERSONALITY IATs TO FAKING Other studies focused more specifically on the
EFFECT IN RESPECT TO SELF- fakability of IAT personality measures. Vianello,
REPORT MEASURES Robusto and Anselmi (2010), for instance, found
that participants’ willingness to manage their
Many studies have dealt with the susceptibility to impression increased explicit (but not implicit)
faking of Big Five self-report scales, investigating scores on the conscientiousness trait. Egloff and

9

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

Schmukle (2002) assigned participants to one has been demonstrated, implicit measures of traits
of two conditions: an hypothetical job applicant mostly capture stable inter-individual differences
scenario (where they were instructed to make a that are able to predict actual behaviour, although
good impression), and a control condition (where they should be distinguished conceptually from
they were not given any specific instruction). The explicit measures of the Big Five. Moreover, they
mean scores obtained in the two conditions were were found to be less prone to faking than self-
significantly different for an explicit measure of report measures, especially if participants have
anxiety (d = .63), but not for an IAT devised to not experienced the IAT.
provide an implicit measure of the same construct. Whereas above results provide compelling
More recently the extent to which response distor- evidence for the utility of the Big Five IATs
tion occurs when big five IATs are administered in (Schmukle, Back & Egloff, 2008), some issues
real applied settings was investigated (Vecchione, related to reliability and implicit-explicit dis-
Dentale, Alessandri and Barbaranelli, 2014). As sociation still need to be clarified. For example,
expected participants under evaluative testing reliability was found to be substantially different
conditions showed higher scores than control when measured in terms of internal consistency
subjects on the explicit measures of agreeable- or temporal stability (e.g., Schnabel et al., 2008).
ness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability As discussed earlier in the chapter, Cronbach’s
while no significant differences were found on alpha revealed adequate levels of reliability, rang-
the implicit measures. ing from .70 to .90 in earlier studies; test-retest
Steffens (2004) demonstrated that subjects with reliability coefficients, by contrast, were found to
a precedent experience with a conscientiousness be low, ranging from .25 to .69. As observed by
IAT to whom was asked to appear conscientious Teige-Mocigemba, Klauer and Sherman (2010)
(first condition) or not conscientious (second the relatively low test-retest reliability may depend
condition) were unable to fake the scores. By on many sources of influence that can potentially
contrast, they were perfectly able to fake the NEO- affect the IAT scores, and that appear to be dif-
FFI scale scores. Differently, when the subjects ficult to identify and to control.
were informed that the crucial variable on which Regarding the observed dissociation between
to intervene in order to manipulate the scores was implicit and explicit self-concept of personality,
the trial latency, they showed significant faking results of a confirmatory factor analysis seems
effects also for the IAT scores of extraversion, suggest that the Big Five IATs and self-report
although the size of the effects was higher for the measures refer to distinct constructs (Schnabel et
explicit measures. In sum, Big Five IATs appeared al., 2008). However, further research are needed to
to be less prone to faking than self-report scales, further investigate this issue, for example by using
although they were not immune to it, especially if multi-trait, multi-method assessment procedures
participants were given relevant information about (MTMM). A similar approach has been adopted
the functioning mechanism of the implicit test. by Nosek and Smyth (2007), who used a MTMM
approach across seven-attitude objects (flower-
insect, creation-evolution, democrat-republican,
CONCLUSION humanities-science, straight-gay, thin-fat and
white-black) to investigate the discriminant valid-
The studies reviewed in this chapter have shown ity of implicit measures with respect to self-report
that the five-factors of personality can be assessed scales. In particular, they searched for the best
by using the Implicit Association Test and its vari- fitting solution among three models: (1) a model
ants, such as the Questionnaire based IAT. As it with two method factors (implicit vs. explicit),

10

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

(2) a model with two method factors (implicit REFERENCES


vs. explicit) plus a single attitude factor for each
domain, and a (3) model with two method factors Agosta, S., Ghirardi, V., Zogmaister, C., Castiello,
(implicit vs. explicit) plus two correlated attitude U., & Sartori, G. (2011). Detecting fakers of the
factors for each domain, one for the implicit autobiographical IAT. Applied Cognitive Psychol-
measure, and the other for the explicit one. The ogy, 25(2), 299–306. doi:10.1002/acp.1691
last solution obtained the best fit. In this model, Arcuri, L., Castelli, L., Galdi, S., Zogmaister,
each IAT correlated with the explicit measure C., & Amadori, A. (2008). Predicting the vote:
of the attitude towards the same object (and not Implicit attitudes as predictors of the future be-
with the explicit measures toward other objects). havior of decided and undecided voters. Political
However, both IATs and the explicit measures Psychology, 29(3), 369–387. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
retained unique components that were not reduc- 9221.2008.00635.x
ible to shared method variance. In similar manner,
it may be useful to apply the same approach to Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2009).
IAT and self-report of Big Five measures across Predicting actual behavior from the explicit and
all personality traits. implicit self-concept of personality. Journal of Per-
Moreover, even though previous studies sonality and Social Psychology, 97(3), 533–548.
already documented the predictive validity and doi:10.1037/a0016229 PMID:19686006
the robustness to faking of the Big Five IATs,
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A.
further research appear to be necessary in order to
(2001). Personality and performance at the begin-
explore their practical utility in real-life contexts.
ning of the new millennium. What do we know
In this regard, it will be important to demonstrate
and where do we go next? International Journal
the predictive validity of the Big Five IATs with
of Selection and Assessment, 9(1&2), 9–30.
respect to important outcomes, such as aca-
doi:10.1111/1468-2389.00160
demic proficiency and job performance, and its
incremental validity over established self-report Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G., & Saklofske, H.
measures. At the same time, it may be relevant (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of Personality Theory
to demonstrate that IATs scores are resistant to and Testing. London: Sage.
response distortion in sensible contexts, such as
Burt, C. (1948). Factor analysis and canonical
personnel selection settings, as well as to accumu-
correlations. British Journal of Psychology. Sta-
late empirical findings on methods developed to
tistical Section, 1, 95–106.
detect fakers and to correct their scores (Agosta
et al., 2010; Cvencek et al., 2011). Future studies Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality
should also explore the utility of using alternative in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of self-
procedures for assessing the Big Five in an implicit reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Personal-
way, such as the qIAT. Although the flexibility ity Inventory. Journal of Personality and Social
offered by this procedure is particularly appealing Psychology, 54(5), 853–863. doi:10.1037/0022-
for the assessment of personality, further studies 3514.54.5.853 PMID:3379583
are needed to investigate in a systematic manner
the psychometric properties of the qIAT, as well
as its advantages and disadvantages with respect
to the IAT.

11

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

Cvencek, D., Greenwald, A. G., Brown, A. S., Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz,
Gray, N. S., & Snowden, R. J. (2010). Faking J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences
of the Implicit Association Test is statistically in implicit cognition: The implicit association
detectable and partly correctable. Basic and Ap- test. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
plied Social Psychology, 32(4), 302–314. doi:10 chology, 74(6), 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-
.1080/01973533.2010.519236 3514.74.6.1464 PMID:9654756
Do Yeong, K. (2003). Voluntary controllabil- Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Atti-
ity of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). tudinal dissociation: What does it mean? In R. E.
Social Psychology Quarterly, 66(1), 83–96. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes:
doi:10.2307/3090143 Insights from the new implicit measures (pp.
65–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Donovan, J. J., Dwight, S. A., & Hurtz, G. M.
(2003). An assessment of the prevalence, severity, Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R.
and verifiability of entry-level applicant faking (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit As-
using the randomized response technique. Hu- sociation Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm.
man Performance, 16(1), 81–106. doi:10.1207/ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
S15327043HUP1601_4 85(2), 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197
PMID:12916565
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (in press). The
MODE model: Attitude-behavior processes as a Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E.,
function of motivation and opportunity. In J. W. & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using
Sherman, B. Gawronski, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis
process theories of the social mind. New York: of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and
Guilford Press. Social Psychology, 97(1), 17–41. doi:10.1037/
a0015575 PMID:19586237
Fiedler, K., & Bluemke, M. (2005). Faking the
IAT: Aided and unaided response control on Griffith, R. L., & Peterson, M. H. (2008). The
the implicit association tests. Basic and Applied failure of social desirability measures to capture
Social Psychology, 27(4), 307–316. doi:10.1207/ applicant faking behavior. Industrial and Orga-
s15324834basp2704_3 nizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science
and Practice, 1(3), 308–311. doi:10.1111/j.1754-
Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Schmitt, M. (2008).
9434.2008.00053.x
When and why do implicit measures predict be-
havior? Empirical evidence for the moderating role Grumm, M., & von Collani, G. (2007). Measur-
of opportunity, motivation, and process reliance. ing Big-Five personality dimensions with the
European Review of Social Psychology, 19(1), implicit association test-Implicit personality
285–338. doi:10.1080/10463280802556958 traits or self-esteem? Personality and Individual
Differences, 43(8), 2205–2217. doi:10.1016/j.
Gawronski, B., & Payne, B. K. (2010). Handbook
paid.2007.06.032
of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory,
and applications. New York: Guilford Press.

12

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

McCrae, R. R., & Allik, J. (2002). The five- Sartori, G., Agosta, S., Zogmaister, C., Ferrara,
factor model of personality across cultures. New S., & Castiello, U. (2008). How to accurately
York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. assess autobiographical events. Psychological
doi:10.1007/978-1-4615-0763-5 Science, 19(8), 772–780. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2008.02156.x PMID:18816284
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R.
(2007). The Implicit Association Test at age 7: A Schmukle, S. C., Back, M. D., & Egloff, B. (2008).
methodological and conceptual review. In J. A. Validity of the five-factor model for the implicit
Bargh (Ed.), Automatic processes in social think- self-concept of personality. European Journal
ing and behavior (pp. 265–292). Psychology Press. of Psychological Assessment, 24(4), 263–272.
doi:10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.263
Nosek, B. A., & Smyth, F. L. (2007). A multitrait-
multimethod validation of the Implicit Association Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2005). A latent
Test: Implicit and explicit attitudes are related but state-trait analysis of implicit and explicit personal-
distinct constructs. Experimental Psychology, ity measures. European Journal of Psychological
54(1), 14–29. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14 Assessment, 21(2), 100–107. doi:10.1027/1015-
PMID:17341011 5759.21.2.100
Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Greenwald, A.
implicit and explicit attitudes. The British G. (2008). Implicit Association Tests: A land-
Journal of Social Psychology, 44(1), 29–45. mark for the assessment of implicit personality
doi:10.1348/014466604X23491 PMID:15901390 self-concept. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D.
H. Saklofske (Eds.), Handbook of Personality
Perugini, M., O’Gorman, R., & Prestwich, A.
Theory and Testing (pp. 508–528). London: Sage.
(2007). An ontological test of the IAT: Self-activa-
tion can increase predictive validity. Experimental Schnabel, K., Banse, R., & Asendorpf, J. B.
Psychology, 54(2), 134–147. doi:10.1027/1618- (2006). Assessment of implicit personality self-
3169.54.2.134 PMID:17472097 concept using the Implicit Association Test (IAT):
Concurrent assessment of anxiousness and angri-
Perugini, M., Richetin, J., & Zogmaister, C. (2010).
ness. The British Journal of Social Psychology,
Prediction of behavior. In B. Gawronski & B.K.
45(2), 373–396. doi:10.1348/014466605X49159
Payne (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social Cogni-
PMID:16762106
tion: Measurement, Theory, and Applications (pp.
256-278). New York: Guilford Press. Schröder-Abé, M., Rudolph, A., Wiesner, A., &
Schütz, A. (2007). Self-esteem discrepancies and
Poropat, A. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-
defensive reactions to social feedback. Interna-
factor model of personality and academic perfor-
tional Journal of Psychology, 42(3), 174–183.
mance. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 322–338.
doi:10.1080/00207590601068134
doi:10.1037/a0014996 PMID:19254083
Smith, D. B., & Robie, C. (2004). The implica-
Richetin, J., Perugini, M., Adjali, I., & Hurling,
tions of impression management for personality
R. (2007). The moderator role of intuitive versus
research in organizations. In B. Schneider & D.
deliberative decision making for the predictive
B. Smiths (Eds.), Personality and organizations
validity of implicit and explicit measures. Eu-
(pp. 111–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ropean Journal of Personality, 21(4), 529–546.
doi:10.1002/per.625

13

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

Smith, D. B., & Robie, C. (2004). The implica- Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., & Sherman,
tions of impression management for personality J. W. (2010). Practical guide to Implicit Associa-
research in organizations. In B. Schneider & D. tion Test and related tasks. In B. Gawronski &
B. Smiths (Eds.), Personality and organizations B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social
(pp. 111–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applica-
tions (pp. 117-139). New York: Guilford Press.
Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the Implicit Association
Test immune to faking? Experimental Psychology, Vecchione, M., Dentale, F., Alessandri, G., &
51(3), 165–179. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.51.3.165 Barbaranelli, C. (2014). Fakability of Implicit
PMID:15267125 and Explicit Measures of the Big Five: Research
findings from organizational settings. Interna-
Steffens, M. C., & Schulze König, S. (2006).
tional Journal of Selection and Assessment, 22(2),
Predicting spontaneous Big-Five behaviour with
211–218. doi:10.1111/ijsa.12070
Implicit Association Tests. European Journal
of Psychological Assessment, 22(1), 13–20. Vianello, M., Robusto, E., & Anselmi, P. (2010).
doi:10.1027/1015-5759.22.1.13 Implicit conscientiousness predicts academicper-
formance. Personality and Individual Differences,
Steiger, J. H. (1980a). Tests for comparing ele-
48(4), 452–457. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.019
ments of a correlation matrix. Psychological
Bulletin, 87(2), 245–251. doi:10.1037/0033- Visveswaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1999). Meta-
2909.87.2.245 analysis of fakability estimates: Implications
for personality measurement. Educational and
Steiger, J. H. (1980b). Testing pattern hypotheses
Psychological Measurement, 59(2), 197–210.
on correlation matrices: Alternative statistics
doi:10.1177/00131649921969802
and some empirical results. Multivariate Behav-
ioral Research, 15(3), 335–352. doi:10.1207/ Widiger, T. A., Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R.
s15327906mbr1503_7 R. (2002). A proposal for Axis II: Diagnos-
ing personality disorders using the five-factor
Steyer, R., Schmitt, M., & Eid, M. (1999). La-
model. In P. T. Costa & T. A. Widiger (Eds.),
tent state-trait theory and research in personality
Personality disorders and the five-factor model
and individual differences. European Journal
of personality (2nd ed., pp. 431–456). Washing-
of Personality, 13(5), 389–408. doi:10.1002/
ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
(SICI)1099-0984(199909/10)13:5<389::AID-
doi:10.1037/10423-025
PER361>3.0.CO;2-A
Yovel, I., & Friedman, A. (2013). Bridging the gap
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective
between explicit and implicit measurement of per-
and impulsive determinants of social behavior.
sonality: The questionnaire-based implicit associa-
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3),
tion test. Personality and Individual Differences,
220–247. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1
54(1), 76–80. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.08.015
PMID:15454347

14

Applying the IAT to Assess Big Five Personality Traits

KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS Questionnaire Based-IAT (qIAT): A phrase-


based time-reaction test developed to increase
Behavioral Process Model of Personality the similarity in content between the IAT and the
(BPMP): A model assuming that both implicit corresponding self-report measures.
and explicit personality associations may influ- Reflective–Impulsive Model (RIM): A model
ence behavioral schemata in the course of action. assuming that social behavior is controlled by two
Big Five Personality Factors: A series of di- interacting systems following different operating
mensions that are assumed to adequately describe principles: The reflective system and the impul-
human personality. sive system.
Faking: A response distortion deriving from Self-Concept of Personality: The implicit
participant’s intentional attempt to convey a favor- and explicit personality characteristics associated
able impression of oneself. to the self.
Implicit Association Test (IAT): A time-
reaction test developed to assess the implicit level
of several psychological constructs.

15
16

Chapter 2
Priming
Christina Bermeitinger
University of Hildesheim, Germany

ABSTRACT
This chapter is about the wide variety of priming encountered in cognitive and social psychology. In cogni-
tive psychology, the priming paradigm is mainly used to study memory phenomena or the pre-activation
of concepts and motor reactions by related stimuli. In social psychology, the term priming is used for
a broader range of phenomena in which an event triggers a subsequent behavior. In this chapter, some
definitions of priming and the origin of the term’s use in psychology are presented. Then, the chapter
examines different types and variants of priming as well as their features and principal elements, using
seven organizing principles. After this methodical and empirical classification, some important theories
of response priming, semantic priming, affective/evaluative priming, negative priming, and macro-level
priming are summarized. Last but not least, some general questions and problems in priming research
are outlined and a brief outlook is given.

INTRODUCTION the broad spectrum of phenomena and methods


that are subsumed under the header of “priming”.
What is a prime? This question was (a substantial) “Priming sessions” at scientific conferences can
part of an action letter for a priming paper writ- thus often be rather confusing – such sessions
ten by me and some co-authors a few years ago can contain presentations reporting the effects
in which we investigated priming at a micro level of a “half-empty” versus a “half-full” tumbler on
using EEG measures. Of course, we thought that participants’ mood or the time they require to solve
we already had sufficiently explained the core a crossword puzzle, as well as presentations report-
concepts of priming and what a prime is. The ing the effects of subliminal arrows on the speed
feedback showed that this was apparently not the (measured on a millisecond scale) of responses to
case. In this specific case, some of the confusion congruent trials (arrow pointing left/right and word
arouse because the action editor was a medical “left”/”right”) or incongruent trials (arrow point-
scientist. However, even in psychology it is not ing left/right and word “right”/“left”). The authors
clear to everybody what priming is and what a of both studies called their approach “priming” (I
prime is. This is not surprising, however, given was one of the presenters of the second example).

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch002

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Priming

In fact, most of my work has been related to the priming types. Last but not least, I will address
second example, and so when I listened to talks some general questions and problems in priming
of the first variant, I wondered—why are such research, and give a brief outlook.
experiments in “my” priming session? Likewise,
I can imagine that researchers interested in the
first priming variant were similarly confused by DEFINITIONS AND ORIGIN
the second type of priming experiment.
This example demonstrates that there is a broad In psychology, the term priming is generally
range of different interpretations of the term prim- used for pre-activations or facilitations. Priming
ing, and the question “Actually, what is a prime?” is defined, for example, as “the improvement of
seems perfectly justified. The term priming is used the processing of a stimulus as a function of a
in cognitive psychology, in emotion research, in previous presentation.” (Anderson, 2001, p. 471,
social and media psychology, and increasingly translation by CB) Stroebe, Jonas, and Hewstone
also in developmental, clinical, or motivational (2003, p. 138, translation by CB) defined priming
psychology. Thus, I now endeavor to form an in a result-oriented fashion as well: Priming is “the
integrative view of these diverse interpretations. finding that a pattern will be activated with higher
Here, one could firstly ask: What are the com- probability if it was presented recently or if it was
mon aspects of these different interpretations of used in the past.” In the same vein, Major (2008,
priming? To anticipate: In all cases of priming, p. ii) wrote: „Priming is the benefit that an event
there is “something” that has an influence on (the receives when its processing has been preceded
processing of) the “following”. In the “normal” by the processing of a related or identical event.”
case, priming is reflected in the fact that the prime A more extended definition, which differentiates
(whatever the prime may be) makes one of at least between the phenomenon of “priming” and the
two options (word choices, reactions, actions, etc.) method or technique of “priming” (i.e., the priming
a little bit more likely to occur than the other(s). paradigm) was given by Chartrand und Jefferis
Whether this influence lasts a few milliseconds, a (2004, p. 854):
couple of minutes, some hours or even days, weeks
or years, depends on different factors. An individual’s experiences in the environment
The following text is about a wide variety temporarily activate concepts that are mentally
of priming as used in different domains in psy- represented. The activation of these concepts,
chology. First, I will present some definitions which can include traits, schemata, attitudes, ste-
of priming and the origin of the term’s use in reotypes, goals, moods, emotions, and behaviors,
psychology. Then, I will examine different types heightens their accessibility. These concepts are
and variants of priming as well as their features said to be primed; that is, they become more likely
and principal elements using seven organizing to influence one’s subsequent thoughts, feelings,
principles. These seven organizing principles are judgments, and behaviors. Priming also refers to
neither exhaustive nor orthogonal to each other. an experimental technique that is used to simulate
However, they help clarify important differences the activation of concepts that usually occurs
between different priming types and illustrate through real-world experiences.
the wide variety of priming research. After this
methodical and empirical classification, I will The central point, therefore, is that a stimulus
summarize some important theories of different or event A has an effect on what follows, which

17

Priming

can either be something internal (an emotion, a or sentences, “expressive word units” have to
decision etc.) or concern something external (a be partially pre-activated, because otherwise we
further event B and its processing). Departing from would only be able to produce one word after the
one of the definitions cited above, the effect does other in an isolated manner (this idea can be dated
not have to be invariably positive. In fact, there is back at least to James, 1890, who assumed two
also negative or inverse priming (e.g., auch Krüger, ‘awakening’ processes when words are spoken).
Klapötke, Bode, & Mattler, 2013; sometimes also Lashley called this pre-activation “ ‘Priming‘ of
called contrast effect, anti-priming or reverse expressive units“ (p. 125). Thus, the term “prim-
priming, e.g., Fiedler, 2003; Glaser, 2003), where ing” was introduced and it was used thereafter for
the presentation of a stimulus leads to reduced the pre-activation of mental concepts. In these early
performance or to opposite effects and evalua- days (cf. Lashley), only internal stimuli or thoughts
tions of subsequent identical or similar stimuli were considered triggers of such a pre-activations.
(e.g., Negative Priming: Frings, Bermeitinger, However, the term priming was soon used in the
& Gibbons, 2011; Neill, 1997; Tipper, 2001; sense it is still predominantly used today, that is,
negative semantic priming effects: Bermeitinger, for pre-activations by external stimuli or events.
Frings, & Wentura, 2008; Bermeitinger, Wentura, In the following, I will discuss different vari-
Koppermann, Hauser, Grass, & Frings, 2012; ants of priming. In general, any event or stimulus
Dagenbach, Carr, & Barnhardt, 1990; Frings, A (which occurs prior to an action, event, or
Bermeitinger, & Wentura, 2008; Frings, Göbel, stimulus B) is able to prime subsequent actions
Mast, Sutter, Bermeitinger, & Wentura, 2011; or the processing of subsequent stimuli or events
Kahan, 2000; Wentura, & Frings, 2005; negative B. Thus, we call stimulus/event A the “prime”
affective/evaluative priming effects: Chan, Ybarra, (although note that in some cases the prime is
& Schwarz, 2006; Glaser, & Banaji, 1999; Klauer, also called “distractor”). Stimulus B – which
Teige-Mocigemba, & Spruyt, 2009; negative follows the prime – is called the “target”. Typi-
compatibility effects in response priming: Ber- cally, participants have to respond to the target
meitinger, 2013; Eimer, & Schlaghecken, 1998). in a well-defined task. In some cases, however,
The possibility of negative effects is included in a target in the actual sense is not present, for
the definition of Hsu and Schütt (Eds., 2012, book example if a specific behavior is primed (e.g.,
description): “Priming is the implicit memory consumer behavior primed by advertising, e.g.,
effect in which exposure to a stimulus influences Bermeitinger, Goelz, Johr, Neumann, Ecker, &
response to a later stimulus. It can occur following Doerr, 2009, or priming of prosocial behavior, see
perceptual, semantic or conceptual stimulus repeti- below). In this chapter, the focus is on priming in
tion.” However, this definition focuses too much a broad sense, that is priming will be discussed
on the priming paradigm of cognitive psychology, as both a phenomenon and a technique or method
and thus neglects some broader interpretations of (originally from cognitive psychology). That is, I
priming (see the above definition by Chartrand will try to treat priming with an inclusive attitude.
& Jefferis, 2004). Additionally, the „perceptual,
semantic, or conceptual“ range of possible fea-
tures that can be pre-activated by a prime seems TYPES AND VARIANTS OF PRIMING
incomplete (see below).
The phrase “priming” was first used in psy- First, I will introduce different priming variants
chology in the 1950s during debates on how and classify them according to different aspects.
fluent language or fluent reading is possible. Different types and variants of priming can be
Karl Lashley (1951) was of the opinion that just classified using the following seven organizing
before the internal or overt utterance of words principles (which are surely not exhaustive):

18

Priming

1. Macro, midi, micro perspective – including Macro, Midi, Micro Perspective


the question of how global the influence of
the prime is? Essentially, anything can be a prime, that is,
2. Prime type. anything can be a stimulus or feature influenc-
3. Target type (or: Is there a target at all?). ing what follows. To this effect, a person can be
4. Type of dependent variable and task—and a prime for another person, the person’s behavior
the related question what (behavioral) pa- can be a prime, what the person says can be a
rameters the prime affects? prime, the person’s clothing can be a prime etc.
5. Relation between prime and target/action Any event that we perceive, but also our own
(also: Variants of the priming paradigm). movements or thoughts are able to influence us
6. Implicit/explicit, unconscious/conscious, and the perception, processing, evaluation etc.
automatic/strategic. of following internal or external events (see also
7. Influences of the primes: facilitation or Bargh, 1997).1 Based on this very broad concep-
inhibition. tion, one could ask, for instance, how the way a
participant is treated (e.g., friendly vs. unfriendly)
In the following, I will introduce each organiz- makes a difference in terms of their ability to
ing principle with several examples. solve crossword puzzles, their general mood, their

Figure 1. Priming from a macro, midi, or micro perspective or at a macro, midi, or micro level. The
borders are fluid.

19

Priming

music preference, etc. In this macro sense, each given criterion (e.g., as positive/negative, living/
stimulus, each context, each action could be a non-living, word/non-word, left/right, etc.). Origi-
prime that has an effect on subsequent thoughts, nally, it was only used in cognitive psychology,
actions, and feelings. Such a macro conception is but is now used for various questions in social
often combined with the assumption that the prime (e.g., Degner, & Wentura, 2010), personality (e.g.,
does not only pre-activate semantic concepts, but Frings, & Neubauer, 2005; Wentura, Kulfanek,
rather that it activates longer-lasting motivational & Greve, 2005), developmental, emotional (e.g.,
processes (e.g., Sela, & Shiv, 2009). Bermeitinger, & Kappes, 2013; Kappes, Bermeit-
On a more specific level of conception (at the inger, & Greve, subm.) motivational (e.g., Leipold
midi level) of priming, the interest is no longer in et al., subm.), and clinical (e.g., Weisbrod et al.,
the general actions and feelings of a person. The 1999) psychology. At the micro level, priming
question at the midi level is whether the prime is (also) related to the pre-activation of specific
activates specific other concepts (still relatively concepts, reactions, goals, attitudes, or valences.
globally). For example, many memory and rec- However, the period of observation and the dura-
ognition experiments can be located at this midi tion of a prime’s influence are rather short (often
level: For instance, when participants are given just a few dozens of milliseconds). Additionally,
some words in the first experimental phase and are priming at the micro level rarely aims to persuade
asked to produce words in the next experimental or influence a person or change a behavior or an
phase. Without any instructions to that effect, par- attitude (as would be the case with priming in an
ticipants produce words in the second phase (e.g., advertising context). Rather, the priming paradigm
when participants should complete word stems, (i.e., micro-level priming) is used to investigate
e.g. HOU_ _) that are identical or semantically existing relations between different concepts,
related to words processed in the first phase, and between concepts and actions, between concepts
they do so with an increased probability relative to and attitudes, etc. The results of such priming
a control condition that does not involve the first experiments then inform models of category
phase (e.g. Warrington, & Weiskrantz, 1970, 1974; representation, the activation of specific attitudes
see also e.g. Bassili, Smith, & MacLeod, 1989). or reactions, or the recall of memory contents.
The principle of pre-activation of specific Depending on the types of prime and target used
concepts (or specific actions, etc.) is also relevant and prime-target relationship, different variants
in the even more specific conception of priming of the priming paradigm can be distinguished (see
at the micro-process level. At this micro level, “relation between prime and target/behavior”). In
researchers are interested in time scales of fractions the following, I will provide some examples of
of seconds up to a maximum of approximately two studies conducted at the macro, midi and/or micro
seconds. This level corresponds to the narrowest level of priming.
interpretation of priming, which is the dominant
understanding of the term in cognitive psychol- Macro and Midi Level
ogy. In the so-defined priming paradigm, most
often sequential priming is used; that is, a prime In a global sense, a lot of phenomena can be
(which is not part of the participant’s task and can understood as priming: the effects of subliminal
be ignored) and a target stimulus are presented in advertising (Bermeitinger et al., 2009; Bermei-
rapid succession. The prime is usually shown for tinger, & Unger, 2013), effects of aphrodisiacs
a maximum of a few hundred milliseconds only. (e.g., Bermeitinger, Feldkötter, Hildebrand,
Typically, participants are required to react to the Schmieder, & Sellner, 2012), anchoring effects
target, for example by classifying it according to a (e.g., Bermeitinger, & Unger, 2014; for review

20

Priming

see e.g., Furnham, & Boo, 2011), framing effects sequence. For example, there might be a sequence
(e.g., Levin, Schneider, & Gaeth, 1998), contrast of 12 stimuli (consisting of four different letters)
effects (e.g., Major, 2008), mood inductions (e.g., which is presented repeatedly. Participants usually
Bermeitinger, & Unger, 2014, Exp. 3; Gerrards- do not become consciously aware of the sequence.
Hesse, Spies, & Hesse, 1994), conditioning (e.g., However, reaction times show that participants
Skinner, 1953), mere exposure effects (e.g., Za- learn the sequence – they become perpetually faster
jonc, 1968), effects in task switching tasks (e.g., with increasing practice, but increase considerably
Waszak, Hommel, & Allport, 2003), embodiment when the standard sequence is violated (e.g., in
related variations of bodily positions or states and case of a transposition). In such a setting, a single
there influence on emotions, actions, concept ac- stimulus or the reaction to this stimulus can be
tivations or thinking patterns (e.g., Bermeitinger, described as a prime for the subsequent stimulus.
Koch, & Wilborn, 2011; Bermeitinger et al.,
2013; Niedenthal, 2007; see also Bermeitinger, Prime Type
& Kiefer, 2012), activations of stereotypes (e.g.,
Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996), or findings in In macro-level priming, a prime can be any
memory studies varying the context (e.g., the stimulus, presented by any method, including (1)
testing room) between learning and recognition explicit instructions or information given by the
phase (e.g., Godden, & Baddeley, 1975). A lot investigator or automatic computer presentation,
of priming experiments in social psychology are activating concepts or guiding the participant’s
conducted based on this broad interpretation of actions or thinking patterns; (2) verbal or non-
priming in a broad sense (e.g., Bargh, 2006). verbal interactions between of the investigator
or a confederate (e.g., contact); (3) presence/
Midi and Micro Level absence of persons, animals, or things; (4) dif-
ferent pictures or actually present objects; (5)
At the micro level, priming is a relatively clearly odors or food (ingredients); (6) sounds or music;
defined paradigm with the trial sequence: fixa- (7) video clips, news reports, computer games, or
tion – prime – (blank) – target – response (see other media pieces; (8) global variations of the
also Figure 2). However, priming at the micro context (e.g., room); (9) specific tasks given to the
level can also differ from the straightforward participants (i.e., task-set activation); (10) stimuli
sequential priming paradigm: It can be integrated used earlier in the experiment or in a previous
into another paradigm (e.g., into divided attention experiment (e.g., words, pictures, symbols etc.);
tasks, Hansen, & Shantz, 1995; into rapid serial (11) sometimes even administration of medication,
visual presentation streams in an attentional blink drugs, or hormones.
experiment, Frings, Bermeitinger, & Wentura, Micro-level priming, by contrast, usually in-
2011) or it can emerge as an influence in other volves visual stimuli as primes, although stimuli
paradigms (e.g., in task switching, Allport, & of other modalities are increasingly being used
Wylie, 1999). Implicit sequence learning experi- (e.g., auditory stimuli: Bermeitinger, Wentura
ments can be interpreted as priming experiments, et al., 2012; tactile stimuli: Frings, Amendt, &
too. In implicit sequence learning experiments Spence, 2011). Visual prime stimuli are typically
(e.g., Bermeitinger, Feldkötter et al., 2012), par- words but can also be sentences (e.g., Burke, &
ticipants usually perform a rather simple task, Yee, 1984), pictures (Dell’Acqua, & Grainger,
for example, to classify four different letters by 1999), photos of faces, face parts, or other body
pressing the corresponding key. Unbeknown to the parts (e.g., Rohr, Degner, & Wentura, 2012; Ruys,
participants, stimuli are arranged in a repeating & Stapel, 2008), phantasy characters (e.g., Frings,

21

Priming

Figure 2. Typical trial sequence of a micro-level priming trial. The example is from semantic priming.

Göbel et al., 2011), symbols and/or directional do not feature a target in the actual sense, that is,
stimuli (e.g., arrows; Vorberg, Mattler, Heinecke, there is usually no well-defined target to which
Schmidt, & Schwarzbach, 2003), colours (e.g., participants should react. By contrast, in macro-
Schmidt & Seydell, 2008), arbitrary words or level priming experiments, it is the participant’s
non-words (e.g., Abrams, & Greenwald, 2000; behavior (independent of any specified target)
Masson, & Isaak, 1999), letters (e.g., Jacobs, & that is of interest – although behavior can include
Grainger, 1999), digits (e.g., Campbell, & Reyn- responses to specific stimuli, such as responses
voet, 2009), or movements (e.g., Bermeitinger, to questionnaires items (e.g., to measure anxiety
2013). Auditory prime stimuli likewise are often levels, etc.).
words (e.g., Bermeitinger, Wentura et al., 2012), In micro-level priming, generally, all stimuli
but also include voices, sentences, sounds, music, used as primes can be used as targets, too. Cross-
chords (e.g., Bharucha, & Stoeckig, 1987), tones, modal combinations (primes and targets are pre-
or rhythms. Rhythms are used in tactile presenta- sented in different modalities) are possible (e.g.,
tions, too (e.g., Frings, Amendt et al., 2011). Holcomb & Anderson, 1993).

Target Type (Or: Is There Type of Dependent Variable and Task


a Target at All?)
Priming (in its broader sense) uses a variety of
For instance, in learning/memory experiments different dependent variables. In the following, just
with cued recall or with old/new decisions, the cues a few will be discussed to illustrate the range of
or recognition probes could be called targets. In possible dependent variables; they include evalu-
advertising experiments, the products concerned ations or classifications (of a situation, object,
or advertising slogans/logos can be seen as targets person, etc., potentially involving response time
to be evaluated, classified, or consumed. In gen- measures), self-evaluations, intentions, normative
eral, however, macro-level priming experiments knowledge and normative behavior, as well as ac-

22

Priming

tual behavior and performance. In addition, some & Baddeley, 1975). Even the choice of what task
studies measure mental processes (e.g., mental to do with a given stimulus can be the dependent
rotation) or (neuro-) physiological parameters. variable in a priming study (e.g., Reuss, Kiesel,
Mere exposure experiments look at valence Kunde, & Hommel, 2011).
judgments regarding presented materials (e.g., Various studies have looked at task persistence;
Zajonc, 1968). Experiments using mood manipula- Riskind and Gotay (1982), for example, tallied the
tions (sometimes induced by body movements or number of cards a person drew from a stack in
states) focus on mood changes (Gerrards-Hesse an impossible task before moving on to the next
et al., 1994), which are typically measured by task with a different stack. The authors initially
questionnaires. The anxiousness of participants manipulated participants’ posture and found higher
has been used as a dependent variable, for example persistence in participants with a straight posture
in priming studies using religious words as primes compared to a slouching posture. Toburen and
(e.g. Toburen, & Meier, 2010). Experiments using Meier (2010) reported that participants persisted
tasks as primes for accommodation vs. assimila- longer with a tiring task after viewing religious
tion (cf. Brandtstädter, 2009) have looked at the (vs. neutral) words.
handling of (fictitious) goal blocking (Leipold et Two articles on priming in its broader sense
al., subm.). Topolinski and Sparenberg (2010) have attracted particular attention; these studies
measured participants’ preference for familiar vs. used movement parameters as the dependent
novel stimuli and their general openness to novelty variable, and will be discussed in more detail in
after moving something (or watching something the following. Bargh, Chaiken, Raymond, and
being moved) in a clockwise or counter-clockwise Hymes (1996) measured participants’ walking
direction. These examples (e.g., open-mindedness, speed after they had been exposed to either words
accommodation vs. assimilation) demonstrate that prime an “elderly” stereotype or age-neutral
that priming experiments can have dependent words, and found that participants in the stereotype
variables that are obtained via questionnaire and group took longer to walk down the hall after the
that are—at least to a certain extent—regarded experiment. In their article “Smells like clean
as personality traits. Often, these studies aim spirit,” Holland, Hendricks, and Aarts (2005)
to induce inter-individual differences on these investigated the cleanliness of their participants.
personality measures, using appropriate primes. In a series of experiments, half the participants
Investigations of the effects of (subliminal) were tested when a lemon-scent—used in many
advertising usually focus on consumption behavior all-purpose cleaners—was present in the testing
(e.g., Bermeitinger et al., 2009) but sometimes also cubicle. The scent was subtle and was only con-
measure consumption intention (e.g., Karremans, sciously noticed by one out of 168 participants.2
Stroebe, & Claus, 2006). Normative behavior was In a first experiment, Holland and colleagues
the dependent variable in a study by Aarts and Di- used a lexical decision task requiring participants
jksterhuis (2003), who presented participants with to decide whether words were correctly spelled.
pictures of a library or a train station and found that There were two word categories: neutral control
participants spoke more softly after viewing the words (such as table, cycling, computer) and
library images. Media psychology has a particular cleaning-related words (such as cleaning, hygiene,
interest in the link between on-screen violence tidying). Participants in the scented cubicles
and aggressive behavior (e.g., Jo, & Berkowitz, responded faster to cleaning-related words than
1994), while learning (viz. performance in free participants in the non-scented cubicles, but there
recall, cued recall, or old/new recognition tasks) is was no group difference for the control words.
the focus of memory experiments (e.g., Godden, This seems to imply that words associated with

23

Priming

cleaning (and thus lemon-scent) facilitated the if not, whether they helped after the experimenter
activation of semantically related concepts. In drew their attention to the sticks and eventually
a second experiment, participants were asked to asked for help explicitly. The results were clear-
write about some activities they had planned for cut: Children who were primed with two dolls
the day. While only 11% of participants in non- facing each other were very likely to help: about
scented cubicles listed a cleaning-related activity, 80% of children helped the experimenter pick up
36% of participants in the scent condition indicated the sticks (about 60% helped spontaneously). In
an intention to clean something (the dependent the other three conditions, only around 40% of
variable in this case thus pertains to a planned children showed helping behavior (about 20%
behavior). This suggests that the priming-based helped spontaneously).
activation of a concept can affect the consideration The prosocial behavior of adults—or their
and planning of future behavior. Finally, in a third generosity—was investigated by Shariff and No-
experiment, the authors demonstrated a priming renzayan (2007). Their participants engaged in a
effect on actual cleaning behavior. After filling ‘dictator’ game; in this game, the participant plays
in an unrelated questionnaire in a cubicle with or the role of an anonymous giver who has a certain
without the scent present, participants moved to amount of money at their disposal, and needs to
another (unscented) room to participate in an al- decide how much money to keep, and how much
legedly unrelated experiment that involved savor- to give to another anonymous “player.” Before
ing a (particularly crumbly) biscuit. Participants the dictator game, participants were exposed to
were filmed, and the dependent variable was the religious words (e.g., God, spirit, divine, sacred,
number of times participants removed crumbs prophet), secular-moral words (e.g., civic, jury,
from their table to keep it clean. Participants in court, police, contract), or neutral words (e.g.,
the scent condition indeed cleaned their table spoon, table, evening) in a task requiring the ‘un-
more frequently, suggesting that a scent prime scrambling’ of scrambled sentences. Participants
can influence actual behavior, possibly via the of the neutral-prime condition gave the recipient
activation of a semantic concept (i.e., in this case, player significantly less money than participants
the concept of cleaning). of the two other conditions.
Even prosocial behavior has been utilized as a Priming on a macro-level can thus involve a
dependent variable in some recent priming studies. broad range of dependent variables and tasks.
Over and Carpenter (2009), for example, tested By contrast, priming on the micro-level primar-
18-month old infants who each viewed one of four ily involves the latencies and error rates of target
sets of images. Each image depicted an object (e.g., responses. Tasks used in these studies are typi-
a teapot) in the foreground and, smaller and in the cally classification tasks requiring, for example,
background, either (1) two manikins facing each the classification of target stimuli regarding
other, (2) two manikins facing away from each affective valence (affective priming; e.g., Ber-
other, (3) a single manikin, or (4) two stacks of meitinger, Kuhlmann, & Wentura, 2012; Bohn
toy-blocks. The experimenter commented on the & Bermeitinger, 2012; Fazio, Sanbonmatsu,
foreground object—for example naming its color Powell, & Kardes, 1986), threat (threat priming;
or explaining its function; the background object(s) e.g., Bermeitinger & Kappes, 2013; Kappes et al.,
were not mentioned. The experimenter then asked subm.), orientation or direction (response priming;
the child to play with them and—seemingly by e.g., Bermeitinger, 2013; Vorberg et al., 2003),
accident—dropped some sticks during play. It lexicality (semantic priming; e.g., Frings et al.,
was then tested whether the child spontaneously 2008; Bermeitinger, Frings et al., 2008; Neely,
helped pick up the sticks within 10 seconds, and 1991), or its identity (e.g., in negative priming

24

Priming

studies; cf. Frings, Bermeitinger, & Gibbons, Depending on the mind-set, incoming informa-
2011). Sometimes, categorization tasks (such as tion is processed in a particular way; in terms
animacy tasks; e.g., Küper, Groh-Bordin, Zimmer, of priming this implies that not all aspects of a
& Ecker, 2012) are used. Responses are usually representation that could in principle be activated
measured via simple key presses using pre-speci- by a prime are in fact activated (see Bermeitinger
fied keys on a keyboard, although other response & Kiefer, 2012, for a summary regarding the
types such as pointing movements (e.g., Schmidt, representation of concepts). The question whether
2002; Schmidt, & Seydell, 2008) are occasionally or not a prime automatically pre-activates aspects
used. Instead of classification, some tasks merely of a target may thus be too narrowly formulated.
involve naming or reading a target object or word Arguably, in most cases there will be qualified
(e.g., Wentura & Frings, 2005, Exp. 4). Typically, pre-activation—depending on the mind- or task-
the dependent variable for statistical analysis set, the prime will automatically activate some
in this case is a difference measure, that is, the but not other aspects of a representation (see
response time difference between an ‘unrelated’ Bargh, 1997, and also below). This notion meshes
(‘incongruent,’ ‘inconsistent,’ or ‘incompatible’) well with results of studies using lists of varying
condition and a ‘related’ (‘congruent,’ ‘consistent,’ composition—in this case a list can provide and
or ‘compatible’) condition. A positive priming ef- establish a task-set that in turn produces a top-
fect thus means that a person responded faster to down attitude towards the experiment (Klauer,
related than unrelated targets; a negative priming Roßnagel, & Musch, 1997; Major, 2008). Also,
effect means exactly the opposite. the kind of task and the specifics of the task can
In a hierarchical ‘priming-of-priming’ fashion, orient the task-set towards one or the other aspect
micro-level priming effects can be the targets of of a word (Klauer, & Musch, 2002; Spruyt, De
macro-level ‘mindset’ priming. Mind-sets or task- Houwer, Hermans, & Eelen, 2007; for goals as
sets are top-down attitudes that influence informa- task sets, see Bargh et al., 1996).
tion processing in terms of a global orientation. Further, order effects in priming experiments
This influence is task- or at least situation-specific, (in the sense of carry-over effects from one trial to
operates over a prolonged period of time, and is the next) can hence be interpreted as “priming of
independent of a specific stimulus or its constitu- priming” (with the superordinate goal to optimize
ent features (cf. Bermeitinger, & Kappes, 2013; the mind-set and to maximize cognitive control).
Bermeitinger & Unger, 2014; Bermeitinger, For example, in affective/evaluative priming stud-
Wentura, & Frings, 2008, 2011; Kiefer, 2007; ies, there is often a smaller priming effect on trial
Kiefer, & Martens, 2010; Spruyt, De Houwer, & n if trial n-1 featured an inconsistent rather than
Hermans, 2009). The insight and awareness that a consistent prime-target pair (e.g., Greenwald,
priming effects can depend on such mind-sets Draine, & Abrams, 1996; also see the „Gratton
has only developed over the last decade.3 Prior effect“ with non-valent material, e.g., Gratton,
to that, priming effects (or their absence) were at Coles, & Donchin, 1992; Goschke, & Dreisbach,
least implicitly taken to shed light on the (rigid) 2008). Such carry-over effects make sense if one
organization of memory or the invariable mecha- assumes that it is a general aim of cognition to
nisms behind the activation of representations and adjust the current mind-set to optimally map onto
motor responses. Nowadays, representation is present task requirements and thus maximise
assumed to be flexible and dynamic (for a similar cognitive control in the most efficient way.
claim in a different context, see Huber, 2008):

25

Priming

Relation between Prime and target are then either compatible/consistent or


and Target/Action incompatible/inconsistent. In affective/evaluative
priming, or when perceptual aspects are more
Macro-level priming is often subdivided into per- important, the relation is usually referred to as
ceptual priming, semantic priming, categorization/ congruency (and prime-target pairs as congruent/
construal priming, behavior priming, and goal/ incongruent). These terms are not used entirely
motivational priming. Semantic priming implies consistently, however, and sometimes congruency
that a prime leads to quicker responses to semanti- is used as an umbrella term.
cally related concepts, or to a higher rate or speed The different types of priming are defined by
of these concepts being associatively generated. different prime-target relations. On the micro-
Construal priming means that the perception of a level, there are two large classes of priming: re-
person (or a behavior or a situation, etc.) is shifted sponse priming and semantic priming. In response
towards the prime—for example, when a person is priming (e.g., Bermeitinger, 2013; Vorberg et
perceived as relatively more aggressive or friendly. al., 2003), there is a relation between the target
In behavior priming it is the participant themselves response and the response called for by the prime
that reacts more in line with the prime (e.g., more (i.e., the response that would be required if the
aggressively, more friendly); this could also be prime were a target stimulus). In the ‘related’
taken to include mood induction and emotional (i.e., compatible) case, prime and target responses
priming. Finally, goal and motivational priming would typically be the same—for example, in a
refers to a prime that increases one’s motivation task using two response keys, both prime and
to actively pursue a behavior that is associated target might call for a ‘key 1’ response. Affec-
with the prime (e.g., Bargh, 2006; Loersch, & tive or evaluative priming (e.g., Bermeitinger,
Payne, 2011). Generally, primes in both micro Kuhlmann et al., 2012; Fazio et al., 1986) is usu-
and macro-level priming can affect almost any ally a variant of response priming, where valent
behavior and almost any cognitive process (e.g., stimuli are used as primes and targets, and where
Fockenberg et al., 2008). target classification is based on (positive/nega-
In micro-level priming, one examines the tive) valence. The second large class of priming
processing of clearly defined target stimuli and paradigm is semantic priming (Bermeitinger,
aims to infer the underlying cognitive processes Frings et al., 2008; Bermeitinger, Wentura et
and structures. Figure 2 illustrates a typical trial al., 2012; Frings et al., 2008; McNamara, 2005;
sequence of a micro-level priming experiment, Neely, 1991), where even in the ‘related’ case the
with an associative relation between the prime and prime does not predict the target response. The
target in the ‘related’ condition (e.g., monkey— typical task in this paradigm is a lexical decision
banana) and a lack of prime-target association in task, requiring participants to decide for each
the ‘unrelated’ case (e.g., car—banana). ‘Related’ target word whether or not it is spelled correctly
means that prime and target are related in terms (or for each target letter-string whether or not it
of the dimension under investigation; this relation is a word). The orthography of targets can be
can be associative—as in the example given—but an additional factor in the experimental design
can also be semantic, conceptual, perceptual, or of a semantic priming task, although typically
affective in nature. If the relation involves a mo- only the correctly-spelled words are of interest
tor aspect (i.e., when a related prime-target pair (and the orthography factor is hence irrelevant
both require the same motor response) it is often for analysis). There are many possible types of
referred to as compatibility or consistency; prime prime-target relation in semantic priming, which

26

Priming

can be broadly differentiated into actual semantic Implicit/Explicit, Automatic/


and associative relations. Semantic priming in the Strategic, Unconscious (Sub-
narrower sense involves a semantic relation based Conscious)/Conscious
on shared attributes, such as the features shared
by members of a given category (e.g., horses and In general, priming is an implicit method because
cows are members of the category ‘animals’ and participants are not explicitly asked to provide
have in common that they are alive, have four information regarding the variable of interest—for
legs, a tail, etc.). In associative priming, there is example, the relation between prime and target or
(usually) no such feature overlap. The relation is their representation in memory (e.g., De Houwer,
due to the regular co-occurrence of prime and 2003; also see Bermeitinger & Kappes, 2013).
target and their resulting association (for further Implicit methods measure participant characteris-
discussion of the semantic vs. associative priming tics, attitudes, processing mechanisms, or memory
debate, see Hutchison, 2003, und Lucas, 2000). representations in an automatic fashion (e.g., De
A priming variant that takes on a special Houwer, & Moors, 2007). The core property of
position is the negative priming paradigm (e.g., implicit measures is that “the measurement out-
Frings, Bermeitinger, & Gibbons, 2011). As in come is causally produced by the to-be-measured
the classic priming paradigm it involves two attribute in the absence of certain goals, awareness,
displays presented in close succession. How- substantial cognitive resources, or substantial
ever, each display in a typical negative priming time” (De Houwer, Teige-Mocigemba, Spruyt,
trial (i.e., both the prime and the probe display) & Moors, 2009, p. 350). Implicit methods and
comprises (at least) two stimuli, one of which is measures thus bypass various problems associated
to be ignored (i.e., the distractor) and one is to with explicit self-report measures (cf. Dovidio,
be attended (i.e., the target). The crucial relation & Fazio, 1992; Greenwald, & Banaji, 1995): For
between the displays is then between the prime example, implicit measures can shed light onto
distractor and the probe target. In the so-called aspects that participants are unaware of (e.g.,
‘ignored-repetition’ (IR) condition, the probe concerning the structure of memory), and they
target corresponds (is identical) to the prime are less susceptible to biases, such as the bias
distractor. In the control (C) condition there is no resulting from participants’ attempts to present
repetition of distractor or target stimuli. Usually themselves differently from who they actually are
there is a third condition—the ‘attended-repetition’ (which is an issue in studies regarding stereotypes
(AR) condition—where the prime target is also and prejudice, for example).
the probe target. The negative priming effect is On the other hand, it cannot always be assumed
then calculated as the difference between the C that strategic effects play no role in priming. For
and IR conditions. This difference is typically example, early studies on semantic priming already
negative (hence the paradigm’s name), which showed that strategic influences increase with
at first may seem surprising given that stimulus longer stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs; i.e.,
repetition usually leads to response facilitation. the interval between prime-onset and target-onset),
In negative priming, however, it is the other way meaning that priming effects can then no longer
around: the repetition of an ignored stimulus as be attributed (only) to automatic pre-activation
a target stimulus on the subsequent display is as- (Neely, 1977). Such strategic effects occur, for
sociated with prolonged responses compared to instance, when participants start thinking about
a no-repetition control condition. what target might follow a particular prime. Even

27

Priming

though in many cases (and even without any accurate) responses to related targets compared
special precautionary measures) primes are not to unrelated targets, and a behavior occurs more
consciously processed (e.g.,. Holland et al., 2005, frequently (or more readily) when activated by a
where only one out of 168 participants noticed prime than in a control condition (e.g., responses
the lemon-scent prime), some provisions can be to advertisements or priming of prosocial behavior,
made to avoid strategic influences, considering see above). However, there are examples where
various parameters. To avoid strategic effects in the opposite is true and a prime leads to inverse,
micro-level priming studies, it is recommended to that is, negative priming effects. Various aspects
use short SOAs (i.e., up to about 300 ms) and to come into play, such as temporal parameters
keep the relative number of related trials low (e.g., (in particular, the prime-target SOA; e.g., Ber-
Neely, 1991). Strategic effects can also be reduced meitinger, 2013), task-specific requirements and
or even eliminated by presenting the primes in a design (e.g., co-occurrence of to-be-processed
way that precludes conscious processing (even and to-be-ignored stimuli in negative priming;
if it is attempted). This is usually achieved by e.g., Frings, Bermeitinger, & Gibbons, 2011),
presenting items below the conscious-perception and non-temporal aspects of prime presentation
threshold by reducing presentation times and (in particular, whether the prime is masked and
presenting a masking stimulus immediately after features of the masking stimulus; Krüger et al.,
(and sometimes even before) the prime stimulus. 2013, or whether the primes move, Bermeitinger,
Presenting the primes outside of the focus of at- 2013). In the following, I will briefly address a
tention or during an interval with reduced avail- number of central priming variants (especially of
ability of attentional resources can also lower the the priming paradigm) in which negative effects
conscious perceivability of the primes (Frings, arise under certain conditions.
Bermeitinger, & Wentura, 2011). Sub-threshold
prime presentation can be implemented both in Response Priming
micro-level priming studies (Bermeitinger, Frings
et al., 2008; Bermeitinger, Kuhlmann et al., 2012; In response priming (e.g., Schmidt, Haberkamp,
Bermeitinger, Wentura et al., 2012; Eimer & & Schmidt, 2011; Vorberg, Mattler, Heinecke,
Schlaghecken, 2002, 2004; Frings et al., 2008; Schmidt, & Schwarzbach, 2003, 2004) with static
Frings, Göbel et al., 2011) as well as macro-level stimuli (such as geometric shapes or colors, e.g.,
priming (although perhaps not in all cases, with Schmidt, 2002) there are usually positive priming
ethical issues requiring particular consideration; effects with quicker target responses in compat-
Bermeitinger et al., 2009; Bermeitinger, & Unger, ible compared to incompatible conditions. A few
2013; Karremans et al., 2006). For a discussion exceptions have shown negative priming effects
of the criteria for ‘sub-conscious’ perception, see in response priming paradigms with clearly vis-
Schmidt (2007) or Schmidt and Vorberg (2006). ible, unmasked primes (e.g., Versace, & Allain,
2001, with words as primes and targets and SOAs
Influence of the Prime: between 600 and 800 ms). Negative response prim-
Facilitation or Inhibition ing is more common when participants are unable
to consciously perceive the prime (or at least the
As discussed above, primes can have either a response-related feature of the prime; e.g., Fehrer,
facilitating or an inhibitory effect on the process- & Raab, 1962; Klotz & Neumann, 1999; Mattler,
ing of a subsequent stimulus or the execution 2006; Neumann & Klotz, 1994; Schmidt, 2000,
of a subsequent response. Facilitation is the 2002; Vorberg et al., 2003, 2004). In that case,
norm—primes lead to quicker (and often more the priming effect depends on the proportion of

28

Priming

compatible trials (e.g., Klapp, 2007), perceptual task was to indicate the direction of movement.
aspects of the masking stimulus, and the temporal The results depended on the SOA: With short 38
parameters of the prime, the mask, and the target, ms SOAs, participants responded faster to targets
as well as the response execution (e.g., Barbot & that moved in the direction opposite to the prime
Kouider, 2012; Lingnau & Vorberg, 2005; Praam- (negative priming). With longer 88-238 ms SOAs,
stra & Seiss, 2005; Schlaghecken & Eimer, 2002; however, there were positive priming effects with
for a review, see Sumner, 2007). With masked quicker responses in congruent trials (i.e., when
primes, for instance, the direction of the priming prime and target rotated in the same direction)
effect often depends on the SOA: Responses are compared to incongruent ones (i.e., when prime
often quicker in compatible than incompatible and target rotated in opposite directions).
trials with short SOAs under 200 ms (sometimes Following on from this work, Bermeitinger
under 160 ms), while the reverse effect (i.e., quicker (2013) used directional movements (i.e., to the
responses in incompatible trials) arises with longer left, to the right) as primes for static arrow targets.
SOAs (= negative priming effect, inverse prim- Again, the sign of the resulting priming effect de-
ing effect, or negative compatibility effect (e.g., pended on temporal parameters; however, in these
Eimer, & Schlaghecken, 2002). There is ongoing studies a clear and stable negative priming effect
debate whether this results pattern is due to mo- resulted with long SOAs, and positive priming with
tor or perceptual mechanisms (e.g., Jaśkowski & short SOAs. Specifically, Bermeitinger (2013)
Verleger, 2007; Klapp & Hinkley, 2002; Krüger, used lines of dots that move to the left or right and
Klapötke, & Mattler, 2011; Mattler, 2005, 2007; found positive priming effects with SOAs of 100
Schlaghecken, Klapp, & Maylor, 2009; Sumner, to 150 ms, and negative priming effects—quicker
2007; also see detailed discussion in “Theoretical responses in incongruent compared to congruent
Classification”); recent investigations suggest that trials—with SOAs longer than 250 ms (no priming
multiple mechanisms contribute to this negative effect resulted with a 200 ms SOA). This pattern
response priming to varying degrees (e.g., see emerged both with between-subject and within-
Krüger et al., 2013). subject (150 vs. 350 ms) SOA manipulation,
and both with blocked and trial-wise (random)
Response Priming with Moving Stimuli variation of SOAs. Using single moving dots as
In response priming with moving stimuli, priming primes rather than lines of dots (Bermeitinger,
effects are also strongly dependent on temporal subm.), positive priming was found with SOAs
parameters. Mattler und Fendrich (2007) used up to at least 360 ms, and negative priming with
a response priming paradigm to investigate the SOAs of at least 800 to 1200 ms.
impact of moving dots on the processing of a
subsequent display that also comprised moving Semantic Priming
dots.4 In particular, they were interested in the
effects of movements that are so quick that they Semantic priming effects can be demonstrated
are not consciously perceivable. Primes consisted with a range of specific relations between prime
of dots moving on a circular trajectory at a speed and target, although it is safe to assume that purely
that made it impossible for participants to deter- semantic priming, with no associative relation
mine the direction of movement. The subsequent between prime and target, will be smaller than
target display also contained a circular array of priming in cases where such an associative rela-
dots, which moved perceivably in a clockwise or tion (additionally) exists (e.g., Lucas, 2000). Some
anti-clockwise direction, and the participants’ studies have reported negative semantic priming,

29

Priming

in particular with masked prime presentation predictor of target-onset, which holds true with
(e.g., Carr, & Dagenbach, 1990; Dagenbach, et long SOAs, trial-wise and random SOA variation,
al., 1990; Dagenbach, Carr, & Wilhelmsen, 1989; and when participants are not yet prepared for the
Kahan, 2000). For negative priming to occur, it target onset (Exp. 4).
seems crucial that masking prevents (full-blown)
conscious prime perception and that the primes are Negative Priming
relatively strong despite the masking (e.g., high
prime strength can result from longer presentation As discussed earlier, in negative priming the (to-
times). These conditions can be met, for example, be-ignored stimulus within the) prime exerts an
by repeated masking conditions (Frings et al., inhibitory effect on the subsequent target stimu-
2008; Bermeitinger, Frings et al., 2008; Frings, lus—typically, participants respond more slowly
Göbel et al., 2011), by prime presentation dur- to the target in the probe display if it matches
ing the attentional blink in a rapid serial visual the distractor in the prime display, compared to
presentation (RSVP) sequence (Frings, Bermeit- a situation where there is no repetition of primes
inger, & Wentura, 2011) or auditory presentation in the target display. The standard result is thus
(Bermeitinger, Wentura et al., 2012; but also see a negative effect. A pre-activation hypothesis,
Stone, 2012, who reported negative priming with however, would predict the exact opposite pat-
no masking). It also seems important that there is tern: As the probe target was already presented
a categorical relation between prime and target in the prime display, it should have been partially
(e.g., with category labels as primes and category pre-activated and should thus be responded to
exemplars as targets; Frings et al., 2008; Frings, more quickly, compared to a target stimulus not
Göbel et al., 2011; also see Avons, Russo, Cinel, featured (as a distractor) in the prime display.
Verolini, Glynn, McDonald, & Cameron, 2009). Rather, negative priming is explained by inhibi-
tion mechanisms and memory effects. There is
Affective/Evaluative Priming evidence for a contribution of both inhibition
and memory, although the extent of contribution
The typical finding in affective/evaluative priming of one or the other is assumed to depend on the
studies is also a positive priming effect, which particular variant of negative priming. Recently,
is, however, only found reliably with SOAs up to a number of authors have argued to integrate ap-
300 ms. Klauer et al. (2009) reviewed the condi- proaches, focusing on commonalities rather than
tions under which negative priming effects (also differences (e.g., Frings, 2004; Frings, Bermeit-
referred to as ‘contrast effects’ in this context) tend inger, & Gibbons, 2011).
to occur (also see Glaser, 2003; Klauer, & Musch,
2003), namely with long SOAs (1000-1200 ms), Priming in the Broader Sense
with primes of extreme valence in a naming task
(though in some cases only in high-anxious par- Inverse priming effects are not only found in
ticipants), and in a few experiments with masked conventional priming paradigms—priming in its
primes or primes that frequently re-occur. In their broader sense can also show such effects, which
own series of experiments, the authors were then are then often called contrast effects (for a review
able to show that—as predicted by their psycho- of various contrast effects, see Glaser, 2003, or
physical evaluation window model—a negative Klauer et al., 2009). For example, Bermeitinger
priming effect becomes more likely to occur when et al. (2009) investigated the effects of subliminal
(1) participants are highly motivated to respond advertising depending on people’s need for the
accurately, and (2) prime-onset is not a good advertised product. They found that people who

30

Priming

were tired (and thus had a need to boost their I will review theoretical approaches that focus
concentration) consumed more dextrose tablets mostly on automatic, non-controlled processes.
of a subliminally presented brand than tablets of The various priming variants will be discussed
a control brand. By contrast, people who were not successively, first introducing generic theories
tired showed a tendency for the reverse consump- that can explain most cases of positive priming,
tion behavior. and then the theories used to address instances of
negative priming.

THEORETICAL CLASSIFICATION Response Priming

After the hitherto rather methodological and General Remarks


empirical review of various aspects of priming,
the following section will give a brief overview In response priming, it is generally assumed
of theoretical explanations of selected priming that both the prime and the target are classified
variants. according to the given response categories, and
Essentially, all priming variants can be ex- that the appropriate response is thus pre-activated.
plained with reference to strategic/controlled or The prime thereby exerts its influence on (at
automatic processes (for a more differentiated least) one processing level (perceptual, central/
view, using semantic priming as an example, see semantic, response-related; cf. Kiesel, Kunde, &
Bermeitinger, 2009; McNamara, 2005). Explana- Hoffmann, 2007). The processes on the motor,
tions that focus on strategic processing rest on the response-related level are of particular relevance
assumption (or observation) that participants build for response priming. Most theories of response
expectations during the experiment regarding the priming are based on Neumann’s (1990) direct
prime-target relation. Based on these expectations, parameter specification account, which was
participants can try to use the prime prospectively originally formulated for this purpose. The theory
to predict the target, or to retrospectively com- of direct parameter specification assumes that in
pare (“match”) target and prime to either check the initial experimental trials, participants acquire
the plausibility of their contingency hypotheses mapping rules regarding the stimuli and responses
(e.g., Neely, Keefe, & Ross, 1989) or to more or involved. After a number of trials, these stimulus-
less specifically counteract the prime’s influence response mappings are learned to a degree that they
(e.g., Degner, 2009; Klauer, & Teige-Mocigemba, can be retrieved automatically. After this learning
2007; Teige-Mocigemba, & Klauer, 2008, 2013). phase, responses can be elicited directly by sensory
Strategic effects can also have an impact with input—that is, in case of priming, by particular
masked primes, namely when participants attempt features of the presented stimuli—obviating the
to retrospectively decipher the masked prime using ‘detour’ of semantic processing. The stimulus
the target (e.g., Kahan, 2000). These strategic or features do not have to be processed consciously
controlled processes thus do not rely on automatic for this to occur. A stimulus and its particular
pre-activations triggered by the prime; they can features specify a response parameter that was
occur with any priming variant but can be reduced left unspecified until stimulus representation
by adequate provisions (see above; e.g., short (e.g., the specification “press the left key”), with
SOAs, a relatively low proportion of related trials, all other parameters necessary for the response
masking of primes without participants suspecting already specified before stimulus presentation.
masks to contain primes, etc.). In the following, Because of the experimental situation and possibly

31

Priming

the fast temporal sequence of prime and target feedback elements (e.g., Schmidt, 2002; Vath &
(source uncertainty etc., cf. Huber, 2008), prime Schmidt, 2007). The effects of masked primes are
stimuli also elicit a response based on the direct explained by the assumption that the feed-forward
parameter specification. process triggered by the prime can proceed un-
The action trigger account introduced by Kiesel hindered, and ahead of the slower feedback pro-
et al. (2007) can be considered a more elaborate cesses, from visual to motor areas, while recurrent
version of direct parameter specification. This processes necessary for conscious processing of
approach assumes that stimuli do not trigger re- the prime are suppressed by the mask.
sponses based on semantic analysis or acquired
stimulus-response mappings, but to the extent that Negative Priming Effects
they fit “existing action release conditions” (which
the authors then call action triggers). This means In a series of papers that attracted much atten-
that it is not concrete stimuli that are mapped onto tion, Eimer and Schlaghecken (1998, 2002;
the possible responses, but that various possible Schlaghecken & Eimer, 2002) presented negative
or expectable stimuli are classified regarding response priming findings (negative compat-
expected or observed task requirements and the ibility effects; NCEs) with masked primes. Both
corresponding motor response, and thus specified primes and targets were arrows pointing to the
as action triggers. When stimuli requiring process- right or left. They found positive priming effects
ing occur in the experiment, they are compared with short prime-target SOAs but negative prim-
to existing action triggers. If a stimulus matches ing with SOAs exceeding about 150 ms (e.g.,
the response-triggering conditions defined by Schlaghecken & Eimer, 2002). Schlaghecken,
the action trigger, the corresponding response is Eimer, and colleagues (Schlaghecken, Bowman,
automatically activated. The action trigger ac- & Eimer, 2006; Schlaghecken & Eimer, 2002,
count is expandable to account for the influence of 2006; Schlaghecken, Rowley, Sembi, Simmons, &
other processing levels (perceptual and semantic) Whitcomb, 2007; also see Klapp, 2005; Sumner,
but does not address them. The basic idea of the 2007) argued that these negative priming effects
approach is that cognitive pre-adjustments are with masked primes reflect (perhaps in addition to
made—in the sense of a task set or mind set—that other mechanisms, but still essentially) an inhibi-
allow quick responding in a relatively well-defined tion mechanism that operates on an early stage of
and predictable environment. motor control. The masked prime initially induces
The rapid chase theory by Schmidt and col- a response tendency that corresponds with the
leagues (e.g., Schmidt, Niehaus, & Nagel, 2007) reaction associated with the prime. If the target
is in essence also based on direct parameter speci- response can occur during this phase (e.g., because
fication. The basic idea behind rapid chase theory of a short SOA), a positive priming effect results.
is that there is a race of activations elicited by The mask, however, reduces or eliminates prime
prime and target, from visual to motor areas. Some visibility, such that the early motor activation is no
findings from studies using pointing-movements longer supported by sensory evidence. The initial
support the notion that the prime indeed elicits a response tendency is hence inhibited, resulting in
response that is, however, subsequently influenced negative priming effects.
by the target and diverted into the direction called Negative priming effects with moving but
for by the target if necessary. The activation of clearly visible primes (Bermeitinger, 2013, subm.)
motor responses by the prime and then the target suggest that this kind of self-inhibition can occur
is therefore described as a sequential and thus independent of masking. One could argue that
simple feed-forward process, without recurrent or self-inhibition is utilized whenever a (motor) ac-

32

Priming

tivation is classed as not (or no longer) founded. only made or noted during particular episodes (i.e.,
This seems to make sense because responses can be “evaluation windows”), even when stimuli outside
triggered quickly and involuntarily—in particular of the evaluation window also provide evidence for
by perceived movements (cf. Machado, Wyatt, one or the other response category. Events (prime
Devine, & Knight, 2007)—but can turn out to be presentation, target presentation, etc.) within the
baseless shortly after. continuous stream of information are used as
Following Eimer and Schlaghecken’s original markers for the segmentation and the classification
explanations, other authors argued that negative of evaluation windows. This approach proposes
priming effects only occur if primes and targets (or the following sequence of processes: 1) Over
other intervening stimuli) share certain (geometric, the course of the experiment, participants learn
spatial, and/or temporal) features (e.g., Jaśkowski, to segment inflowing information into distinct
Białuńska, Tomanek, & Verleger, 2008; Lleras & episodes, using the regularly occurring stimuli
Enns, 2004; Verleger, Jaśkowski, Aydemir, van as markers. 2) Stimuli appearing (immediately)
der Lubbe, & Groen, 2004). For example, masks before the target act as go signals, triggering an
used to mask arrow symbols (e.g., such as “>>” evaluation window to be opened. 3) Decisions
or “<<”) often contain lines, too, or even consist regarding the correspondence of a stimulus with
of the two target arrows superimposed. According one of the possible (response) categories are af-
to this line of thought, negative priming effects fected by changes in evidence for the respective
result from object updating (e.g., Lleras, & Enns, category; however, evidence changes supporting
2004) in a sense that more weight is given to new a response become more difficult to detect (ac-
information (viz. elements of the mask that are cording to the Weber-Fechner Law), the more
not contained in the prime and thus point in the evidence for a response has already accumulated.
opposite direction). Another similar explanation 4) Negative priming effects emerge when the
is that the mask may inhibit the ongoing response prime does not fall within the evaluation window
and concurrently activate the opposite response (the relative increase in evidence for one response
(i.e., mask-triggered inhibition). However, nega- option in the interval from target onset to response
tive priming effects have also been reported with execution in the related case will not exceed the
masks that do not share features with the prime increase in case of a neutral prime); positive prim-
(e.g., Klapp, 2005; Schlaghecken & Eimer, 2006; ing effects emerge when the prime falls within
Schlaghecken et al., 2009), meaning that an expla- the evaluation window (in this case, the relative
nation of negative priming based (only) on mask increase in evidence for one response option in
characteristics is untenable. The fact that negative the interval between prime onset and response is
priming with moving primes occur without any larger in the related compared to the neutral or
masking at all (Bermeitinger, 2013) buttresses the unrelated condition). Klauer and Dittrich (2010)
interpretation that negative priming effects cannot further assume that a variety of factors determine
result (entirely) from an overlap of (perceptual) whether or not a prime falls into the evaluation
features between the stimuli involved. window. For example, these factors might include
Negative priming without masks or other inter- the predictability of target onset from the prime.
vening stimulus between prime/distractor and tar- Target onset may be more difficult to predict from
get were reported by Klauer and Dittrich (2010) as certain types of prime (e.g., movements), which
well as Machado et al. (2007). Klauer and Dittrich is why negative priming might eventuate in these
developed a conceptual framework that assumes cases (cf. Bermeitinger, 2013).
that participants segment prime-target sequences In sum, one should still assume that several
into episodes, and that task-relevant evaluations are mechanisms concur to produce (negative) response

33

Priming

priming effects. These mechanisms may also and a particular pattern of specifically weighted
include more attention-related mechanisms—as feature links. Positive semantic priming effects
postulated in typical attentional paradigms (e.g., arise when target features are pre-activated by
cuing and inhibition of return, cf. Bermeitinger, the prime—in the related case, prime and target
2013). features overlap and hence the prime activation
can quickly be converted into the target activa-
Semantic Priming tion (in associative priming, it is assumed that
the conversion from prime to target activation is
General Remarks learned based on its frequency of occurrence and
is thus quicker as with non-associated concepts).
Traditionally, semantic priming effects have been A completely different (and less frequently
explained with reference to automatic spread of drawn upon) view regarding semantic priming is
activation from the prime to the target representa- offered by so-called compound cue models (e.g.,
tion (Anderson, 1976, 1983; Anderson, & Pirolli, Dosher, & Rosedale, 1989; Ratcliff, & McKoon,
1984; Collins, & Loftus, 1975; Loftus, 1975; 1988) that draw on episodic memory theories.
Neely, 1991), or at least spreading activation Compound cue models assume that semantic
was considered one of the candidate mechanisms priming does not result from the activation of
(e.g., Neely, & Keefe, 1989). Spreading activation individual concepts, but from the combined prime-
is assumed to take place within local memory target episode. The target and context elements
networks where 1) each concept is represented (including the prime) together form a retrieval
by a specific node (e.g., Quillian, 1968), and 2) cue. Semantic priming effects are thought to be
related concepts/nodes are either spatially proxi- due to the familiarity of the retrieval cue, which
mal or linked by strong connections. Spreading is higher in case of a related prime-target pair
activation models assume that retrieval of a con- compared to an unrelated one. In the unrelated
cept from memory equates to the activation of case, participants thus require additional time and
the internal representation (i.e., activation of the processing resources, which in turn lead to longer
corresponding node). When prime information response times and the priming effect.
activates the node corresponding to the prime Based on this research, one can assume that
concept, activation flows on to related nodes. If the semantic priming (and possibly automatic spread-
subsequent stimulus (i.e., the target) is related to ing activation) depends on a participant’s transient
the prime, the spreading activation will thus have task or mind sets, but also habitual mind sets (e.g.,
pre-activated its representation, and the target can Bermeitinger, Wentura et al., 2008, 2011). Prim-
thus be processed and responded to more quickly ing effects do not occur unconditionally and not
than non-related targets. in each case in which the prime-target relation
Today it is now longer assumed that a singular actually is relevant for memory representations,
(cerebral) node actually represents a concept. The and in which the other experimental conditions
basic idea of spreading activation models has thus are, in principle, conducive to priming effects. For
been transferred to more contemporary (neural) example, in a neural network model, a particular
network models, which presume distributed mind set may generally inhibit part of the activa-
representation. Such network or vector models tion pattern representing a certain concept, while a
(e.g., Masson, 1999) assume that each concept is different mind-set might activate it. If this part of
represented through a range of features (sometimes the activation pattern—which may, for example,
referred to as “mini-nodes”, cf. Neely, 1991, or code an object’s function, which is a particularly
“micro-features”, cf. Sharkey, & Sharkey, 1992) relevant dimension for the discrimination between

34

Priming

various man-made objects—is crucial for the oc- be completed rapidly, permitting speedy target
currence of priming effects (e.g., with man-mad responses. By contrast, in cases where a relation
objects), then priming effects may or may not exists but is not immediately obvious (e.g., with
result, depending on the mind-set. Mind-set in atypical category exemplars), more time and re-
this case can be considered a superordinate layer sources are required to identify the prime, leading
with inhibitory or excitatory connections to the to delayed target responses. However, this theory
conceptual representation layer (or other layers/ of retrospective prime elucidation is plausible only
systems that process input and prepare output; if the participant actually makes the assumption
see Wentura & Rothermund, 2003, for a similar that there is information (and roughly what kind
notion with regard to affective priming). of information) concealed in the mask. Such as-
sumptions arise mainly in experiments that use a
Negative Priming Effects threshold setting procedure prior to the priming
trials, which informs participants of the presence
Negative priming effects in semantic priming of stimuli in the masks. In the typical experiments
paradigms emerge reliably only if (1) primes are that have looked into masking techniques with
not consciously perceivable but are presented for relatively long prime presentation times, however,
a relatively long time (Bermeitinger, Frings et al., participants will be unaware of the presence of
2008; Bermeitinger, Wentura et al., 2012; Frings, meaningful masked primes—thus obviating the
Bermeitinger, & Wentura, 2011) and (2) there motivation to elucidate the primes.
is a categorical (and not just purely associative The second popular account of negative seman-
or repetitive) relation between prime and target tic priming assumes an inhibition mechanism. The
(Avons et al., 2009; Frings et al., 2008) and the center-surround inhibition theory of Dagenbach
target does not lie in the (possibly idiosyncratic) and colleagues (e.g., Carr & Dagenbach, 1990;
center of the category (i.e., is not prototypical; Dagenbach et al., 1990, 2007) proposes that rep-
Frings, Göbel et al., 2011). Comparable results resentations that are weakly activated by masked
with visual (Frings et al., 2008; Bermeitinger et primes are subsequently surrounded by a “ring of
al., 2008; Wentura, & Frings, 2005; with different inhibition.” Due to the activation difference be-
types of visual presentation: Frings, Bermeitinger, tween the weakly activated stimulus representation
& Wentura, 2011) and auditory presentation and the inhibited surround, access to the prime is
(Bermeitinger, Wentura et al., 2012) suggest facilitated. This relative activation boost caused by
that the phenomenon is not a sensory but rather the inhibition of the surround can also be referred
a central-semantic phenomenon that is unspecific to as contrast sharpening. It follows that if the
with regard to modality and presentation. subsequent target falls into the inhibited region,
In essence, there are two competing explana- more time will be required for target activation
tory accounts of negative semantic priming ef- and response, compared to a situation where there
fects.5 One (Kahan, 2000) assumes a retrospective is no (positive) pre-activation (i.e., with unrelated
(and rather controlled/strategic) process from the targets). By contrast, if the target falls into the
target to the prime and proposes that the target prime-activated center (Frings et al., 2008; Frings,
is used to clarify the identity of the prime. In Göbel et al., 2011), it is already pre-activated and
cases that allow for a quick ascertainment of the target responses will be fast. Compatible with
prime-target relation (e.g., when prime and target this, an EEG study by Bermeitinger, Frings et al.
are identical), and in cases that allow for a quick (2008) found a corresponding pattern in the N400
ascertainment of the absence of such a relation component, which is a correlate of the ease with
(i.e., in unrelated trials), prime identification can which a concept can be accessed. Typically, the

35

Priming

N400 is more negative for unrelated than related stimulus is classified based on its valence (positive/
targets. In case of a negative semantic priming negative). This leads to an early pre-activation of
effect this is reversed, and there is a more nega- the response corresponding to the valence (e.g.,
tive waveform for related compared to unrelated right or left key press). Given adequate time
targets. This finding also corroborates the notion constraints, this pre-activation lasts at least until
that the N400 reflects temporary (or temporarily the target is presented. A target stimulus of equal
reduced) access to a concept and not (merely) the valence will then be responded to faster because
integration of a word into an established semantic the response has already been pre-activated by
context (e.g., Kutas, Lindamood, & Hillyard, the prime. In case of diverging valences, the pre-
1984), or the a priori correspondence of prime activation (of the incorrect response) by the prime
and target. The center-surround inhibition theory is a disadvantage rather than an advantage because
thus continues to offer a viable explanation for the incorrect response and its pre-activation re-
negative semantic priming effects (as well as the quire inhibition before the target response can be
absence of negative effects with repetitive or as- executed (e.g., Klauer et al., 1997; Rothermund,
sociative prime-target relations). & Wentura, 1998).
Stroop explanation. Related to the response pre-
Affective/Evaluative Priming activation account, this approach seeks to explain
affective priming with reference to mechanisms
General Remarks that operate in Stroop and Stroop-like tasks (such
as the Eriksen flanker task; e.g., Eriksen, 1995).
The typical finding in affective/evaluative priming This is because priming effects can be described
is also a positive priming effect. This is usually as Stroop effects if one equates the irrelevant
explained by the congruency of motor responses to to-be-ignored dimension with the prime, and the
prime and target, by an affective matching process, relevant to-be-processed dimension with the target
and by some of the mechanisms also assumed (Musch, Elze, & Klauer, 1998; also see Wentura,
to account for semantic priming (in particular, 1999). The dimension/information that is assessed
spreading activation). Based on experiments with first or more quickly (i.e., verbal meaning in the
masked primes that produce priming effects (cf. Stroop task, the first word in the affective prim-
Bermeitinger, Kuhlmann et al., 2012), it is argued ing task) will thereby affect the processing of the
that affective priming and thus the activation of dimension that is assessed second or more slowly
prime valence (or, in applied research, the acti- (i.e., color in the Stroop task, the second word in
vation of attitudes and prejudices regarding the the affective priming task; this can be termed the
prime concept) proceeds automatically. In the distractor). This influence can be facilitative or, in
following, I will discuss some of the prominent particular in the incongruent case, disruptive be-
models (e.g., Klauer, & Musch, 2003; Wentura, cause of interference (e.g., Musch, 2000). In most
& Rothermund, 2003). cases it is assumed that the interference occurs at
Response pre-activation and response conflict. a relatively late, response-related stage; interfer-
Affective priming with a valence decision task is ence at earlier, central-semantic stages is rarely
basically the affective variant of response priming. assumed (cf. Wentura & Rothermund, 2003).
It is therefore assumed that a large portion of the Affective matching. According to this ap-
priming effect in the valence decision task is due proach, prime and target stimuli automatically
to the prime pre-activating the correct response activate the attitudes/appraisals associated with
in the congruent case, and the incorrect response them. These are then spontaneously tested for
in the incongruent case. In a first step, the prime consistency. On the phenomenal level, consistent

36

Priming

stimuli evoke a feeling of credibility and familiar- is not the only determining factor for affective
ity, whereas inconsistent stimuli convey a lack of priming effects, but that stimuli of the same va-
credibility. These spontaneous feelings facilitate a lence are typically also more strongly associated
particular response behavior. In less introspective (e.g., vacation, sun, love, warmth, beach, etc., or
terms, the facilitative mechanism with consistent murder, death, hatred, terror, anger, etc.). This
stimuli can be described as “resonance” in the would imply that the effect is not an affective but
relevant fields of representation (e.g., Wentura & a semantic/associative one (also see Rothermund,
Rothermund, 2003). A feeling of credibility leads & Wentura, 1998).
to a tendency to respond approvingly, and a lack The current and dominant view of affective
of credibility leads to a tendency to respond with priming can thus be described as multi-factorial,
rejection (e.g., Klauer, & Stern, 1992; Wentura, with several mechanisms contributing to affec-
2000). tive priming, most likely with different ratios for
Semantic network: Valence. Affective priming different variants of affective priming (e.g., see
effects in tasks that do not require valence decisions Fazio, 2001; Klauer, & Musch, 2003; Wentura,
(e.g., naming or pronunciation tasks; for a review & Rothermund, 2003). Recent EEG studies (e.g.,
of the evidence that affective priming effects also Eder, Leuthold, Rothermund, & Schweinberger,
occur in these tasks, see Glaser, 2003; Klauer & 2012) corroborated this view, by finding evalu-
Musch, 2003), however, require an explanation ative priming effects both in a response-related
that goes beyond response pre-activation pro- component (lateralized readiness potential, LRP)
cesses. Initial explanatory frameworks reverted to and a component related to semantic pre-activation
spreading activation models that were developed (N400, see above). In sum, the available evidence
in the semantic priming literature. This approach converges on the notion that both motor and ear-
assumed that all positive (negative) words/con- lier, more central processes contribute to affective
cepts were linked either directly or indirectly via priming; the strongest influence is thereby usually
a “positivity node” (“negativity node”). When a attributed to the motor processes (Klauer, Musch,
positive word is activated, for example, activity & Eder, 2005).
spreads through the network to all linked (posi-
tive) concepts, which are thus pre-activated. If Negative Priming Effects
one of the pre-activated stimuli is subsequently
presented, the response is quicker than in case of Recent more systematic findings of negative af-
a non-activated stimulus (e.g., Fazio et al., 1986). fective priming effects (e.g., Glaser, & Banaji,
Network, vector, and distributed models. 1999; Klauer et al., 2009) are mainly explained
Contemporary approaches assume that any con- by correctional mechanisms, activation and inhi-
cept can be represented by a vector or activation bition phenomena on various processing levels,
pattern in a network, and that part of this vector/ and/or temporal influences (although essentially,
pattern codes valence. The conversion of one variations of the explanations used for negative
vector/pattern into a different but similar vector/ response priming effects or contrast effects more
pattern takes less time than the conversion into a generally can also be applied; e.g., Huber, 2008).
dissimilar one. It follows that the conversion will Glaser (2003; also see Glaser, & Banaji,
be quicker for two concepts of equal valence due 1999) proposed a correctional mechanism that
to the vector/pattern overlap. leads to negative affective priming because of
Semantic network: Association: Other ap- over-compensation. Glaser observed negative
proaches are based on the assumption that valence priming effects (in naming tasks) mainly with

37

Priming

primes of extreme valence and participants with specific stimuli (the so-called object field). In the
a strong motivation for accurate responding. He congruent case, there is competition only in the
assumes that, in order to avoid prime-induced latter system but not the affective subsystem. At
incorrect responding, extreme primes (possibly first glance, the congruent constellation seems to
after an initial activation of their actual valence) be easier to resolve; however, reciprocal feedback
initiate an evaluation contrary to their meaning connections between the systems are assumed to
and thus lead to negative (or at least attenuated) exist. In the congruent case, these can uphold the
priming effects. Glaser further assumes that these interference (in the object field) for a prolonged
corrections occur automatically and do not require period of time because higher-level prime and
a conscious intention. target representations constantly feed back into the
Wentura and Rothermund (2003) also sug- object field without providing inhibitory informa-
gested that the occurrence of negative priming tion. In this case, competition in the object field
effects may depend on the participant’s strategy cannot be resolved through supporting (inhibitory)
towards the affective priming task. They proposed information, which can lead to longer response
that two strategies need to be differentiated. Strat- times for congruent compared to incongruent pairs
egy 1 entails that the response is made based on if strategy 2 (“pay attention to the source of the
any supporting evidence available, without con- evidence”) is used.
sidering the source of that evidence. This strategy Further explanatory alternatives for negative
seems particularly useful if responding needs to affective priming assume (1) that activation is
be very fast (e.g., under speed instructions), or in transformed to inhibition when a certain level
experiments where the prime predicts the target of activation is surpassed (activation dependent
valence and thus the correct response with a high inhibition model, Berner & Maier, 2004), (2)
probability. In these cases there should usually that center-surround inhibition (e.g., Dagenbach
be positive priming effects because the response & Carr, 1994, see above) operates in affective
depends partially on the evidence provided by the priming, in particular with primes of less extreme
prime—the response occurs as soon as sufficient valence (Hermans et al., 2003), (3) that repeated
evidence for one response alternative has accumu- occurrence of a previously activated valence leads
lated, irrespective of where the evidence originates attentional mechanisms to increase the salience of
from (i.e., from the prime or the target). In strategy the opposite valence (for a review, see Klauer et
2, by contrast, there is the attempt to distinguish al., 2009), or (4) that a contrast develops between
between the various sources of evidence in order a shared prime-target episode (which resides in a
to base responding only on the “correct” evidence perceptual snapshot) and information outside the
(i.e., the target). In this case, it is not just the episode (Fockenberg et al., 2008).
response-related evaluation stage—the stage at A relatively new approach to negative affective
which in the incongruent case the conflict needs priming comes from the evaluation window ac-
to be resolved—that is involved, but also early count of Klauer et al. (2009; originally called the
processing stages (Wentura and Rothermund’s psychophysical account but renamed in Klauer &
explanation is based on an expansion of the se- Dittrich, 2010; this account was already discussed
lective attention model of Houghton and Tipper, in the section on negative response priming). This
1994, to which they added an affective system). account places the origin of priming effects at
Incongruent prime-target pairs create not only the central processing stage, and integrates and
response interference but also competition in both expands various elements of the above-mentioned
the affective subsystem (positive vs. negative) and earlier theories and hypotheses regarding (nega-
the system responsible for the representation of tive) affective priming effects.

38

Priming

Negative Priming is stored with the target that is incompatible with


it. This response interferes with the correct to-be-
In negative priming, the standard result is a executed target response.
negative priming effect of the to-be-ignored prime As mentioned earlier, contemporary views
display distractor on the to-be-processed probe assume that multiple mechanisms contribute to
display target (which is related or identical to the negative priming. In a recent EEG study (Frings,
prime distractor). Competing theories assume Bermeitinger, & Gibbons, 2011), we demonstrated
either an inhibition mechanism (e.g., Houghton that motor pre-activations play a role in negative
& Tipper, 1994; Tipper, 1985) or refer to memory priming even when the response to the prime target
retrieval processes (e.g., Mayr & Buchner, 2006, is given after the response to the probe target. This
2007; Milliken, Joordens, Merikle, & Seiffert, corroborated the notion that interference processes
1998; Neill, 2007; Rothermund, Wentura, & De on a motor level are relevant for negative prim-
Houwer, 2005). Current inhibtion theories propose ing, which argues for the involvement of retrieval
that the internal representation of the distractor is proceses (i.e., memory models).
inhibited, while the representation of the target is
strengthened (Houghton & Tipper, 1994). Whether Priming in Its Broader Sense
something is classified as a distractor or target is
determined specifically for each display by the task General Remarks and Remarks
and the resulting top-down modulation. Another on Subliminal Priming
important assumption is that inhibition and activa-
tion processes continue after the presentation of a In macro-level priming (i.e., priming in its broader
display, causing the prime distractor activation to sense), pre-activations on various levels (e.g.,
go below baseline. Then, when the probe display perceptual, semantic, cognitive, affective, moti-
is presented and the participant needs to respond vational, motor, physical; cf. Clore & Colcombe,
to the previously inhibited stimulus—which is 2003; Niedenthal, 2007; Yi, 1990; also Bermei-
now the probe target (= IR condition)—activa- tinger & Unger, 2014; Bermeitinger et al., 2013;
tion needs to be increased first up to baseline and Bermeitinger, Koch et al., 2011) and the resulting
then up to the response threshold. By contrast, increase in accessibilty to prime-activated aspects
the activation increase in control trials starts at are also thought to account for the positive effects
baseline, and thus with a headstart relative to the of a prime on subsequent tasks and actions etc. The
IR condition. general assumption is that certain features of the
Current versions of memory retrieval theories prime are conferred upon the target; for example,
(the stimulus response theory, Rothermund et al., the prime might move the focus to a certain aspect
2005; the prime response retrieval theory, Mayr of the target (e.g., Mandel & Johnson, 2002); more
& Buchner, 2006) propose that negative priming generally speaking, selective attention processes
arises because the perception of a target activates might set in (e.g., Bargh, 2006), or own actions
memory representations that are associated with might be attributed to an internal source rather
the target. If a prime distractor repeats as the probe than the external prime stimulus e.g., Loersch &
target, the most recent memory of the target stems Payne, 2011). Analogous to micro-level priming
from the prime episode. This episode representa- theories, it is further assumed that the perception
tion will include the probe target (= prime distrac- of a stimulus or event can also cause direct and
tor), the prime target, as well as the prime target automatic activation of behavior (e.g., Holland et
response. This means that in IR trials, a response al., 2005). Wheeler and DeMarree (2009) review

39

Priming

the various direct and indirect influences of primes case, a current need—of a participant is pivotal in
on behavior, including the assumed mediating determining the effects of subliminal advertising.
processes. This view implies that different com- Recently, Verwijmeren, Karremans, Stroebe, and
binations of mechanisms can, in principle, lead Wigboldus (2011) showed that a chronic mind set
to the same behavioral outcome. (i.e., a „habit“) of a participant can also play an
Recent research on subliminal priming has important role: Habitual consumers of the studied
been able to show that the (primed/induced, pre- drink did not show any additional effect of sub-
existing, or even chronic) mind set of a participant liminal advertising for the already preferred brand.
is also, or perhaps particularly, crucial in macro- Veltkamp, Custers, and Aarts (2011; also see
level priming (for similar conclusions from other Veltkamp, Aarts, & Custers, 2009; Loersch, Durso,
fields of study, see Loersch & Payne, 2011; also & Petty, 2013) explored another aspect of general
Bermeitinger, Feldkötter et al., 2012; Bermeitinger importance for subliminal advertising. The authors
et al., 2013; Sela & Shiv, 2009). Since the alleged posed the question what it means, essentially, to
experiments of James Vicary in the 1950’s, sub- have a need, goal, or motivation. They argued
liminal advertising has been a topic of high public that in the given context, the core aspect is that
interest (for a review, see Bermeitinger & Unger, things that are related to the need/goal are positive
2013). Times in which subliminal advertising was as long as the need exists or the goal is current
believed to have a substantial influence on product (for a different perspective, see Brandtstädter,
reviews and consumer decisions—often based 2002, 2009). Subliminal primes thus only work
on less than scientific evidence—alternated with when they have positive valence—for example,
times in which such an influence was believed to based on a current motivation that can be due to
be disproven—partially based on well-controlled deprivation. The authors were able to show that
scientific studies. In a well-received article, Kar- raising the valence of a behavior (by subliminally
remans et al. (2006) revived the topic. The authors pairing words describing the behavior with posi-
subliminally presented the brand name of a drink tive words) can prime the behavior, even when
(or a control prime) and then measured to what there is no current deficiency (i.e., no need). In
extent participants preferred the primed drink sum, this implies that for subliminal advertising
over a control drink, as well as their intention to to indeed increase consumption, the advertised
consume the primed and two control drinks. It product must be associated with positive valence
was also recorded (Experiment 1) or manipulated in the participant’s current mind set—either based
(Experment 2) how thirsty participants were. In on a current need or a temporarily increased va-
both experiments, the intention to consume the lence of the product or an associated behavior (cf.
primed drink (rather than the control drink) de- Milyavsky, Hassin, & Schul, 2012).
pended on people’s thirst: Thirsty participants had
a higher intention than non-thirsty participants. Negative Priming Effects
In Bermeitinger et al. (2009) we were able to
conceptually replicate this finding and expand it In macro-level priming, negative priming effects
to an actual consumption situation: Participants are usually called contrast effects. On the one
consumed more dextrose pills of a subliminally hand, such contrast effects are often explained with
primed brand (compared to a control brand) the reference to comparison standards (the prime sets
higher their fatigue and the associated motivation an extreme standard against which the target is
to boost their concentration. These experiments then contrasted and assessed); on the other hand,
demonstrate that the current mind set—in this (automatic) correction mechanisms are assumed to

40

Priming

counteract the impact of a prime on a task response 2012). This is why interactional features, role in-
(in an advertising context, see Laran, Dalton, & formation and associated behaviors are important
Andrade, 2011; for a review, see Glaser, 2003). for concepts. Bargh thus assumes that it’s not a
singular (semantic) concept that is primed but
(because of interlinked and ultimately embodied
FINAL REMARKS representation) “conceptual structures, whether
they be called metaphors, roles, perspectives, or
Two Problems… mindsets“ (p. 158). Hence, the generation problem
is theoretically solved because all possible aspects
Whichever review on priming (in its broader sense) are activated by the prime. The selection of one of
one consults—and as illustrated in this chapter—it the possible reactions is then determined by aims,
is clear that a virtually endless variety of primes, motivations, and their attention-guiding function.
embedded in a wide range of tasks, can influence A motivational behavior (e.g., helping someone in
an almost equally vast number of different psycho- an emergency situation) should dominate a non-
logical variables. This raises the question of how motivational behavior (e.g., imitating a person
to predict the impact of a particular prime. In this in an emergency situation). A behavior is thus
context, Bargh (2006) formulated two problems: influenced by a prime only as long as there is no
the generation problem (also known as the many higher aim conflicting with the behavior. In the
effects of one prime problem, or the theoretical experimental context, a prime can essentially have
mystery of priming, cf. Loersch, & Payne, 2011) an impact on that dependent variable that is of
and the reduction problem. The question in case the experimenter’s interest. This view makes the
of the generation problem is how a specific prime absence of micro-level priming effects in some
can have so many qualitatively different effects; tasks (e.g., the absence of affective priming effects
the question in case of the reduction problem is in a naming task) plausible.
how a reaction is selected from the many possible Sela and Shiv’s (2009) activation-striving
pre-activated reactions. Theories that account model also explains when a prime influences one
for specific priming effects and the underlying or the other response. In contrast to Bargh (2006),
mechanisms are plentiful (see above or Wheeler, this model assumes that there is already a differ-
& De Marree, 2009), but integrative theories that ence at the time that the concept is activated. In
can explain what priming effect occurs under what this model, (positive) primes are initially assessed
conditions, are rare. Three such theories are briefly at the continuum from self-consistent (the prime
reviewed in the following section. represents a part of the active self-concept) to
Taking recourse to embodiment ideas (cf. self-discrepant and desirable (a positive prime
Bermeitinger, Koch et al., 2011; Bermeitinger associated with a goal a person wants to achieve).
& Kiefer, 2012), Bargh (2006) assumes that Whether a prime influences behavior via semantic
confrontation with an event, object, or word (in- activation or goal activation will depend on this
cluding primes) not only leads to the activation assessment of self-consistency. Self-consistent
of semantic information regarding the associated primes should lead to a strong initial activation
concept, but also to the concurrent activation of of associated constructs but no motivational ten-
other (emotional, behavioral, interactional, moti- dencies. For a brief time, these primes generate
vational, etc.) aspects of the concept. According a semantic priming effect, which is short-lived.
to this view, concepts are there to facilitate real- Self-discrepant primes, however, initially lead to
world actions (i.e., they are for doing, not knowing; weaker associative activations (and thus smaller
Lakoff & Johnson, 1980; Bermeitinger & Kiefer, semantic priming effects) but stimulate goal

41

Priming

and motivational processes, which should even is no such thing as fully unconditional activa-
increase in strength over time. This can also tion—and even if there appears to be one only
explain temporal differences in priming effects, needs to take one step back to understand how
although the explanatory power of this approach is the activation depends on more permanent (e.g.,
restricted by difficulties to determine the primes’ chronically activated) attitudes (which can also
self-consistency versus self-discrepancy. be change; e.g., Bermeitinger, Wentura et al.,
Loersch and Payne’s (2011) situated inference 2008, 2011). The preconscious effects assumed by
model addresses the generation problem by assum- Bargh (1997), whereby environmental stimuli can
ing a single process that produces all the different trigger something in a goal-independent fashion,
types of priming—as such the model certainly thus appear to depend on superordinate ‘chronic’
has a broader scope as the approach of Sela and goals. Most researchers assume that given the pre-
Shiv (2009). In Loersch and Payne’s model, two adjustment, the priming processes themselves can
aspects—situational variables on the one hand and run automatically (e.g., Glaser, 2003).
a process of misattribution on the other—come So what pervades the various priming variants
to the fore. The model is based on a three-stage and levels? An ‘it-depends,’ conditional activation
process: (1) A prime makes related information and processes, situational influences, the steering
highly accessible. (2) This activation is perceived influence of selective attention, and a convergence
as a natural response to situational stimuli, due towards the view that an integrative model or at
to misattribution. (3) The misattributed content least a co-existence of several theories is neces-
is used in preparing the response to the most sary to explain all effects (e.g., Loersch, & Payne,
salient question in the given situation (e.g., ‘who 2011, p. 248: “it seems likely that both models
is that?’ vs. ‘what do I want?’). However, the situ- [Bargh’s 2006 model and the authors’ model,
ation already directs attention to one or the other viz. an early vs. late selection model; remark of
‘primeable’ aspect, and can thus lead to an early CB] may describe priming effects under certain
selection of prime meaning and relevant prime boundary conditions“; Bargh, 2006, p. 164, cites
aspects. Given the fact that in macro-level priming, Cacioppo, Berntson, Sheridan, & McClintock,
a substantial period of time can lie between the 2000, p. 829: „a multilevel integrative analysis
prime and the dependent variable’s measurement, may be required“, and writes further (p. 163):
this early-selection seems less plausible than a late „We must seek to extend the findings […] into
selection as assumed by Bargh (2006). the complex and noisy real world in which they
all combine, somehow, to drive our actions“;
…and an ‘It Depends’ Wentura & Rothermund, 2003, offer a model of
the influence of affective stimuli and processes
It is clear on all levels of priming that the direction that can be applied mutatis mutandis to other
and strength of priming effects can depend on a priming phenomena and influences).
large variety of factors. Different priming effects
result depending on individual pre-adjustments Outlook
(task-set, mind-set, current state, chronically
activated goals, personality characteristics, etc.; To some, priming seems like a tantalizing, mys-
e.g., Bargh, 2006; Bermeitinger, Wentura et al., terious phenomenon that occasionally produces
2008, 2011; Glaser, 2003; Bermeitinger et al., amusing effects (some of which remain con-
2009; Bermeitinger et al., 2014) and/or situational troversial and difficult to replicate; cf. Abbott,
conditions (Bargh, 2006). It therefore seems there 2013) but that is now past this infancy phase; it

42

Priming

has come of age, requiring explanations of how ACKNOWLEDGMENT


certain influences set the course for certain effects
(Bargh, 2006). This holds particularly (but not My special thanks go to Ulli Ecker who is respon-
exclusively) for macro-level priming. For oth- sible for the precise English.
ers, priming (on the micro-level) is “one of the
most important ‘windows to the mind’” (Wentura
& Frings, 2005, p. 108; cf. also Schmidt et al., REFERENCES
2011). One can thus infer two directions for future
developments in (both macro and micro-level) Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2003). The silence
priming research. On the one hand, there will be of the library: Environment, situational norm, and
a search for integrative models that can explain social behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
which priming phenomena will emerge under Psychology, 84(1), 18–28. doi:10.1037/0022-
which conditions, and when various primes will 3514.84.1.18 PMID:12518968
have what effects (see above). This theorizing Abbott, A. (2013). Disputed results a fresh blow for
will continue to borrow models from other areas social psychology: Failure to replicate intelligence-
of cognitive psychology—for example, attention priming effects ignites row in research community.
and memory models—and it will require a more Nature, 497(7447), 16. doi:10.1038/497016a
integrative examination of cognitive phenomena, PMID:23636371
which thus far have been investigated in isola-
tion. Attentional mechanisms in particular seem Abrams, R. L., & Greenwald, A. G. (2000).
predestined to elucidate moderating variables and Parts outweigh the whole (word) in unconscious
influences on various levels, since all priming analysis of meaning. Psychological Science,
variants involve the enhancement or attenuation of 11(2), 118–124. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00226
selective accessibility to features associated with PMID:11273418
the prime. On the other hand, priming paradigms
Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (1999). Task-switching:
will be used increasingly to investigate other psy-
Positive and negative priming of task-set. In G.
chological questions; for example, using response
W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.),
priming as a tool to investigate consciousness or
Attention, space, and action: Studies in cognitive
the perception of motion and color (cf. Bermei-
neuroscience (pp. 273–296). New York: Oxford
tinger, 2013; Schmidt et al., 2011). However, in
University Press.
doing so, researchers should always keep in mind
the no-longer-ignorable evidence for situational, Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory, and
individual, and task-induced influences. thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Thus, even more than 60 years after the in-
Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation
troduction of the term ‘priming’ into the psycho-
theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and
logical literature, priming remains an attractive
Verbal Behavior, 22(3), 261–295. doi:10.1016/
field of future study. With the aid of temporally
S0022-5371(83)90201-3
microscopic influences and effects (on a micro-
second scale), it will help elucidate questions of Anderson, J. R. (2001). Kognitive Psychologie.
fundamental and far-reaching significance. Heidelberg, Germany: Spektrum.

43

Priming

Anderson, J. R., & Pirolli, P. L. (1984). Spread Bassili, J. N., Smith, M. C., & MacLeod, C.
of activation. Journal of Experimental Psychol- M. (1989). Auditory and visual word-stem
ogy. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(4), completion: Separating data-driven and con-
791–798. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.791 ceptually driven processes. Quarterly Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 41A(3), 439–453.
Avons, S. E., Russo, R., Cinel, C., Verolini,
doi:10.1080/14640748908402375
V., Glynn, K., McDonald, R., & Cameron, M.
(2009). Associative and repetition priming with Bermeitinger, C. (2009). Facts and artifacts about
the repeated masked prime technique: No prim- tureens and artichokes: Natural and artifactual
ing found. Memory & Cognition, 37(1), 100–114. categories investigated with semantic priming.
doi:10.3758/MC.37.1.100 PMID:19103980 Göttingen: Cuvillier.
Barbot, A., & Kouider, S. (2012). Longer is not Bermeitinger, C. (2013). Response priming with
better: Nonconscious overstimulation reverses apparent motion primes. Psychological Research,
priming influences under interocular suppres- 77(4), 371–387. doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0436-x
sion. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, PMID:22526718
74(1), 174–184. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0226-3
Bermeitinger, C. (submitted). Moving single dots
PMID:22033950
as primes for static arrow targets: Negative effects
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday at very long SOAs.
life. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in social cogni-
Bermeitinger, C., Feldkötter, A. L., Hildebrand, J.,
tion (pp. 1–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schmieder, L., & Sellner, L. (2012). Strawberry
Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming arouses forever? Effects of pre-information and
all these years? On the development, mechanisms, real consumption of a common aphrodisiac on
and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. arousal ratings. In M. Mokryš & A. Lieskovský
European Journal of Social Psychology, 36(2), (Eds.), Proceedings in advanced research in
147–168. doi:10.1002/ejsp.336 PMID:19844598 scientific areas (pp. 751–754). Žilina, Slovak
Republic: EDIS.
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes,
C. (1996). The automatic evaluation effect: Un- Bermeitinger, C., Frings, C., & Wentura, D.
conditional automatic attitude activation with (2008). Reversing the N400: Event-related po-
a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental tentials of a negative semantic priming effect.
Social Psychology, 32(1), 104–128. doi:10.1006/ Neuroreport, 19(15), 1479–1482. doi:10.1097/
jesp.1996.0005 WNR.0b013e32830f4b0b PMID:18797301
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Bermeitinger, C., Frings, C., & Wentura, D.
Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of (submitted). Which cognitive mechanisms cause
trait construct and stereotype activation on action. evaluative priming? New insights from event-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, related potentials.
71(2), 230–244. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230
PMID:8765481

44

Priming

Bermeitinger, C., Goelz, R., Johr, N., Neumann, Bermeitinger, C., & Unger, B. (2014). Influences
M., Ecker, U. K. H., & Doerr, R. (2009). The hidden on the marking of examinations. Psychology (Sa-
persuaders break into the tired brain. Journal of vannah, Ga.), 5, 91–98.
Experimental Social Psychology, 45(2), 320–326.
Bermeitinger, C., Wentura, D., & Frings, C.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.10.001
(2008). Nature and facts about natural and arti-
Bermeitinger, C., & Kappes, C. (2013). Threat factual categories: Sex differences in the semantic
priming: Age, focus, and timing. In K. A. Moore, priming paradigm. Brain and Language, 106(2),
K. Kaniasty, P. Buchwald, & A. Sesé (Eds.), 153–163. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2008.03.003
Stress and Anxiety: Application to health and PMID:18442848
well-being, work stressors, and anxiety assessment
Bermeitinger, C., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2011).
(pp. 219–226). Berlin, Germany: Logos.
How to switch on and switch off semantic priming
Bermeitinger, C., & Kiefer, M. (2012). Embodied effects for natural and artifactual categories: Acti-
concepts. In S. C. Koch, T. Fuchs, M. Summa, vation processes in category memory depend on fo-
& C. Müller (Eds.), Body memory, metaphor cusing specific feature dimensions. Psychonomic
and movement (pp. 121–140). Amsterdam: John Bulletin & Review, 18(3), 579–585. doi:10.3758/
Benjamins. doi:10.1075/aicr.84.11ber s13423-011-0067-z PMID:21380588
Bermeitinger, C., Koch, F., & Wilborn, D. L. Bermeitinger, C., Wentura, D., Koppermann, C.,
(2011). Embodied Emotions – Verkörperte Hauser, M., Grass, B., & Frings, C. (2012). The
Gefühle: Emotionen als Sprache des Körpers? direction of masked auditory category priming
L.O.G.O.S. INTERDISZIPLINÄR, 19, 244-258. correlates with participants’ prime discrimina-
tion ability. Advances in Cognitive Psychol-
Bermeitinger, C., Kuhlmann, M., & Wentura, D.
ogy, 8(3), 210–217. doi:10.5709/acp-0116-y
(2012). Reading a standing wave: Figure-ground-
PMID:22956986
alternation masking of primes in evaluative
priming. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), Berner, M. P., & Maier, M. A. (2004). The direction
1109–1121. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.03.015 of affective priming as a function of trait anxiety
PMID:22521264 when naming target words with regular and ir-
regular pronunciation. Experimental Psychology,
Bermeitinger, C., Machmer, A. M., Schramm, J.,
51(3), 180–190. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.51.3.180
Mertens, D., Wilborn, D. L., & Bonin, L. et al.
PMID:15267126
(2013). Facial feedback of surprise in implicit
sequence learning. International Journal of Psy- Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1987). Priming
chology & Psychological Therapy, 13, 145–162. of chords: Spreading activation or overlapping
frequency spectra? Perception & Psychophys-
Bermeitinger, C., & Unger, B. (2013). Subliminal
ics, 41(6), 519–524. doi:10.3758/BF03210486
persuasion: Psychological and juristic history and
PMID:3615148
current state of affairs. In D. Halkias (Ed.), Psy-
chology and the search for certainty in everyday Bohn, M., & Bermeitinger, C. (2012). Affektives
life (pp. 149–162). Athens, Greece: Atiner. Priming. In C. Bermeitinger (Ed.), Paradigmen
der Kognitiven Psychologie: Affektive Reize I (pp.
13–38). Berlin: Uni-Edition.

45

Priming

Brandtstädter, J. (2002). Searching for paths to Chartrand, T. L., & Jefferis, V. E. (2004).
successful development and aging: Integrating de- Priming. In M. Lewis-Beck, A. Bryman, & T.
velopmental and action-theoretical perspectives. Futing Liao (Eds.), The Sage encyclopedia of
In L. Pulkkinnen & A. Caspi (Eds.), Paths to suc- social science research methods (pp. 854–855).
cessful development (pp. 380–408). Cambridge, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/ doi:10.4135/9781412950589.n747
CBO9780511489761.016
Clore, G. L., & Colcombe, S. (2003). The parallel
Brandtstädter, J. (2009). Goal pursuit and goal worlds of affective concepts and feelings. In J.
adjustment: Self-regulation and intentional self- Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of
development in changing developmental contexts. evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and
Advances in Life Course Research, 14(1-2), 52–62. emotion (pp. 335–369). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
doi:10.1016/j.alcr.2009.03.002 Erlbaum.
Burke, D. M., & Yee, P. L. (1984). Semantic Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A
priming during sentence processing by young and spreading-activation theory of semantic pro-
older adults. Developmental Psychology, 20(5), cessing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407–428.
903–910. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.20.5.903 doi:10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Sheridan, J. Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. H., & Barnhardt, T. M.
F., & McClintock, M. K. (2000). Multilevel (1990). Inhibitory semantic priming of lexical
integrative analyses of human behavior: Social decisions due to failure to retrieve weakly acti-
neuroscience and the complementing nature of vated codes. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
social and biological approaches. Psychological ogy. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(2),
Bulletin, 126(6), 829–843. doi:10.1037/0033- 328–340. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.2.328
2909.126.6.829 PMID:11107878
Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. H., Menzer, D., Duquette,
Campbell, J. I., & Reynvoet, B. (2009). Context- P. J., Chalk, H. M., & Rupard, M. et al. (2007).
dependent semantic priming in number naming. Adventures in inhibition: Plausibly, but not certifi-
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, ably, inhibitory processes. In D. S. Gorfein & C.
Memory, and Cognition, 35(6), 1552–1556. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Inhibition in cognition (pp.
doi:10.1037/a0017012 PMID:19857024 45–63). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association. doi:10.1037/11587-003
Carr, T. H., & Dagenbach, D. (1990). Semantic
priming and repetition priming from masked Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. H., & Wilhelmsen,
words: Evidence for a center-surround attentional A. (1989). Task-induced strategies and near-
mechanism in perceptual recognition. Journal of threshold priming: Conscious influences on
Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and unconscious perception. Journal of Memory and
Cognition, 16(2), 341–350. doi:10.1037/0278- Language, 28(4), 412–443. doi:10.1016/0749-
7393.16.2.341 PMID:2137871 596X(89)90020-X
Chan, E., Ybarra, O., & Schwarz, N. (2006). Re- De Houwer, J. (2003). A structural analysis of
versing the affective congruency effect: The role indirect measures of attitudes. In J. Musch & K.
of target word frequency of occurrence. Journal of C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation:
Experimental Social Psychology, 42(3), 365–372. Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.04.008 219–244). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

46

Priming

De Houwer, J., & Moors, A. (2007). How to define Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of
and examine the implicitness of implicit measures. masked stimuli on motor activation: Behavioral and
In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experi-
measures of attitudes: Procedures and controver- mental Psychology. Human Perception and Per-
sies (pp. 179–194). New York: Guilford Press. formance, 24(6), 1737–1747. doi:10.1037/0096-
1523.24.6.1737 PMID:9861720
De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt,
A., & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Links
normative analysis and review. Psychological between conscious awareness and response
Bulletin, 135(3), 347–368. doi:10.1037/a0014211 inhibition: Evidence from masked priming.
PMID:19379018 Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 514–520.
doi:10.3758/BF03196307 PMID:12412891
Degner, J. (2009). On the (un)controllabil-
ity of affective priming: Strategic manipula- Eriksen, C. W. (1995). The flankers task
tion is feasible but can possibly be prevented. and response competition: A useful tool for
Cognition and Emotion, 23(2), 327–354. investigating a variety of cognitive prob-
doi:10.1080/02699930801993924 lems. Visual Cognition, 2(2-3), 101–118.
doi:10.1080/13506289508401726
Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic preju-
dice in childhood and early adolescence. Journal Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activa-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(3), tion of associated evaluations: An overview.
356–374. doi:10.1037/a0017993 PMID:20175618 Cognition and Emotion, 15(2), 115–142.
doi:10.1080/02699930125908
Dell’Acqua, R., & Grainger, J. (1999). Unconscious
semantic priming from pictures. Cognition, 73(1), Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C.,
B1–B15. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00049-9 & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activa-
PMID:10536225 tion of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 50(2), 229–238. doi:10.1037/0022-
Dosher, B. A., & Rosedale, G. (1989). Integrated
3514.50.2.229 PMID:3701576
retrieval cues as a mechanism for priming in
retrieval from memory. Journal of Experimen- Fehrer, E., & Raab, D. (1962). Reaction time to
tal Psychology. General, 118(2), 191–211. stimuli masked by metacontrast. Journal of Experi-
doi:10.1037/0096-3445.118.2.191 mental Psychology, 63(2), 143–147. doi:10.1037/
h0040795 PMID:13892020
Dovidio, J. F., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). New tech-
nologies for the direct and indirect assessment Fiedler, K. (2003). The hidden vicissitudes of
of attitudes. In J. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about the priming paradigm in evaluative judgment
questions: Meaning, memory, expression and research. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The
social interaction in surveys (pp. 204–237). New psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in
York: Sage. cognition and emotion (pp. 109–137). Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Eder, A. B., Leuthold, H., Rothermund, K., &
Schweinberger, S. R. (2012). Automatic response Fockenberg, D. A., Koole, S. L., & Semin, G.
activation in sequential affective priming: An ERP R. (2008). Priming in concert: Assimilation and
study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosci- contrast with multiple affective and gender primes.
ence, 7(4), 436–445. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr033 Social Cognition, 26(6), 647–669. doi:10.1521/
PMID:21642351 soco.2008.26.6.647

47

Priming

Fox, E. (1995). Negative priming from ignored dis- Frings, C., & Neubauer, A. (2005). Are masked-
tractors in visual selection: A review. Psychonomic stimuli-discrimination-tests in masked priming
Bulletin & Review, 2(2), 145–173. doi:10.3758/ studies measures of intelligence? An alternative
BF03210958 PMID:24203653 task for measuring inspection time. Personality
and Individual Differences, 39(7), 1181–1191.
Frings, C. (2004). Inhibition von Distraktorinfor-
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.024
mation. Kassel: University Press.
Furnham, A., & Boo, H. C. (2011). A litera-
Frings, C., Amendt, A., & Spence, C. (2011). When
ture review of the anchoring effect. Journal of
seeing doesn’t matter: Assessing the after-effects
Socio-Economics, 40(1), 35–42. doi:10.1016/j.
of tactile distractor processing in the blind and
socec.2010.10.008
the sighted. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Human Perception and Performance, 37(4), 1174– Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., & Hesse, F.
1181. doi:10.1037/a0022336 PMID:21553991 W. (1994). Experimental induction of emo-
tional states and their effectiveness: A review.
Frings, C., Bermeitinger, C., & Gibbons, H.
British Journal of Psychology, 85(1), 55–78.
(2011). Prime retrieval of motor responses in
doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02508.x
negative priming: Evidence from lateralized
readiness potentials. Brain Research, 1407, Glaser, J. (2003). Reverse priming: Implications
69–78. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.037 for the (un)conditionality of automatic evalu-
PMID:21764039 ation. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The
psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in
Frings, C., Bermeitinger, C., & Wentura, D.
cognition and emotion (pp. 87–108). Mahwah,
(2008). Center-surround or spreading inhibition:
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Which mechanism caused the negative effect from
repeated masked semantic primes? Experimental Glaser, J., & Banaji, M. R. (1999). When fair is
Psychology, 55(4), 235–243. doi:10.1027/1618- foul and foul is fair: Reverse priming in automatic
3169.55.4.234 PMID:18683620 evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 77(4), 669–687. doi:10.1037/0022-
Frings, C., Bermeitinger, C., & Wentura, D. (2011).
3514.77.4.669 PMID:10531668
Inhibition from blinked category labels: Combin-
ing the attentional blink and the semantic priming Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975).
paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(4), Context-dependent memory in two natural
514–521. doi:10.1080/20445911.2011.527328 environments: On land and underwater. Brit-
ish Journal of Psychology, 66(3), 325–331.
Frings, C., Göbel, A., Mast, F., Sutter, J., Ber-
doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01468.x
meitinger, C., & Wentura, D. (2011). Category
priming with aliens: Analysing the influence of Goschke, T., & Dreisbach, G. (2008). Conflict-
targets’ prototypicality on the centre surround triggered goal shielding: Response conflicts at-
inhibition mechanism. Memory (Hove, England), tenuate background monitoring for prospective
19(6), 585–596. doi:10.1080/09658211.2011.59 memory cues. Psychological Science, 19(1),
2498 PMID:21919586 25–32. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02042.x
PMID:18181788

48

Priming

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. Hsu, N., & Schütt, Z. (Eds.). (2012). Psychology
(1992). Optimizing the use of information: of priming. New York: Nova.
Strategic control of activation and responses.
Huber, D. E. (2008). Immediate priming and cogni-
Journal of Experimental Psychology. General,
tive aftereffects. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
4(4), 480–506. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.480
ogy. General, 137(2), 324–347. doi:10.1037/0096-
PMID:1431740
3445.137.2.324 PMID:18473662
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Im-
Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming
plicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and
due to association strength or featural overlap?
stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27.
A micro-analytic review. Psychonomic Bul-
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4 PMID:7878162
letin & Review, 10(4), 785–813. doi:10.3758/
Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. BF03196544 PMID:15000531
L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of uncon-
Jacobs, A. M., & Grainger, J. (1991). Automatic
scious semantic activation. Science, 273(5282),
letter priming in an alphabetic decision task.
1699–1702. doi:10.1126/science.273.5282.1699
Perception & Psychophysics, 49(1), 43–52.
PMID:8781230
doi:10.3758/BF03211615 PMID:2011452
Hansen, C. H., & Shantz, C. A. (1995). Emotion-
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology.
specific priming: Congruence effects on affect and
New York: Holt. doi:10.1037/11059-000
recognition across negative emotions. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(6), 548–557. Jaśkowski, P., Białuńska, A., Tomanek, M., &
doi:10.1177/0146167295216001 Verleger, R. (2008). Mask- and distractor-triggered
inhibitory processes in the priming of motor
Hermans, D., Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Eelen,
response: An EEG study. Psychophysiology, 45,
P. (2003). Affective priming with subliminally
70–85. PMID:17887961
presented pictures. Canadian Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology, 57(2), 97–114. doi:10.1037/ Jaśkowski, P., & Verleger, R. (2007). What
h0087416 PMID:12822839 determines the direction of subliminal prim-
ing. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1),
Holcomb, P. J., & Anderson, J. E. (1993).
181–192. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0024-1
Cross-modal semantic priming: A time-course
PMID:20517508
analysis using event-related brain potentials. Lan-
guage and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 379–411. Jo, E., & Berkowitz, L. (1994). A priming ef-
doi:10.1080/01690969308407583 fect analysis of media influences: An update. In
J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects:
Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., & Aarts, H.
Advances in theory and research. LEA’s com-
(2005). Smells like clean spirit. Psychological
munication series (pp. 43–60). Hillsdale, NJ:
Science, 16(9), 689–693. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
Lawrence Erlbaum.
9280.2005.01597.x PMID:16137254
Kahan, T. A. (2000). Negative priming from
Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of
masked words: Retrospective prime clarification
inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D.
or center-surround inhibition? Journal of Ex-
Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes
perimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and
in attention, memory, and language (pp. 53–112).
Cognition, 26(6), 1392–1410. doi:10.1037/0278-
Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
7393.26.6.1392 PMID:11185772

49

Priming

Kappes, C., Bermeitinger, C., & Greve, W. (sub- Klapp, S. T., & Hinkley, L. B. (2002). The
mitted). Fear-of-crime priming by means of a negative compatibility effect: Unconscious in-
threat decision task in younger and older adults. hibition influences reaction time and response
selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Karremans, J. C., Stroebe, W., & Claus, J. (2006).
General, 131(2), 255–269. doi:10.1037/0096-
Beyond Vicary’s fantasies: The impact of sub-
3445.131.2.255 PMID:12049243
liminal priming and brand choice. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 42(6), 792–798. Klauer, K. C., & Dittrich, K. (2010). From sun-
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.002 shine to double arrows: An evaluation window
account of negative compatibility effects. Journal
Kiefer, M. (2007). Top-down modulation of
of Experimental Psychology. General, 139(3),
unconscious ‘automatic’ processes: A gating
490–519. doi:10.1037/a0019746 PMID:20677896
framework. Advances in Cognitive Psychology,
3(1), 289–306. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0031-2 Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2002). Goal-dependent
PMID:20517515 and goal-independent effects of irrelevant evalua-
tions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional
28(6), 802–814. doi:10.1177/0146167202289009
sensitization of unconscious cognition: Task
sets modulate subsequent masked semantic Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective
priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. priming: Findings and theories. In J. Musch & K.
General, 139(3), 464–489. doi:10.1037/a0019561 C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation:
PMID:20677895 Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp.
7–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., & Hoffmann, J. (2007).
Mechanisms of subliminal response prim- Klauer, K. C., Musch, J., & Eder, A. B. (2005).
ing. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1), Priming of semantic classifications: Late and
307–315. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0032-1 response related, or earlier and more central?
PMID:20517516 Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 897–903.
doi:10.3758/BF03196783 PMID:16524008
Klapp, S. T. (2005). Two versions of the negative
compatibility effect: Comment on Lleras and Enns Klauer, K. C., Roßnagel, C., & Musch, J. (1997).
(2004). Journal of Experimental Psychology. List-context effects in evaluative priming. Journal
General, 134(3), 431–435. doi:10.1037/0096- of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory,
3445.134.3.431 PMID:16131274 and Cognition, 23(1), 246–255. doi:10.1037/0278-
7393.23.1.246 PMID:9028030
Klapp, S. T. (2007). Nonconscious control mim-
ics a purposeful strategy: Strength of Stroop-like Klauer, K. C., & Stern, E. (1992). How attitudes
interference is automatically modulated by propor- guide memory-based judgments: A two-process
tion of compatible trials. Journal of Experimental model. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
Psychology. Human Perception and Perfor- chology, 28(2), 186–206. doi:10.1016/0022-
mance, 33(6), 1366–1376. doi:10.1037/0096- 1031(92)90038-L
1523.33.6.1366 PMID:18085949

50

Priming

Klauer, K. C., & Teige-Mocigemba, S. (2007). Laran, J., Dalton, A. N., & Andrade, E. B.
Controllability and resource dependence in (2011). The curious case of behavioral back-
automatic evaluation. Journal of Experimental lash: Why brands produce priming effects and
Social Psychology, 43(4), 648–655. doi:10.1016/j. slogans produce reverse priming effects. The
jesp.2006.06.003 Journal of Consumer Research, 37(6), 999–1014.
doi:10.1086/656577
Klauer, K. C., Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Spruyt,
A. (2009). Contrast effects in spontaneous evalu- Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial or-
ations: A psychophysical account. Journal of Per- der in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral
sonality and Social Psychology, 96(2), 265–287. mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–136). New
doi:10.1037/a0013248 PMID:19159132 York: John Wiley.
Klotz, W., & Neumann, O. (1999). Motor ac- Leipold, B., Bermeitinger, C., Greve, W., Meyer,
tivation without conscious discrimination in B., Arnold, M., & Pielniok, M. (subm.). Short-term
metacontrast masking. Journal of Experimental induction of assimilation and accommodation.
Psychology. Human Perception and Performance,
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998).
25(4), 976–992. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.25.4.976
All frames ar not created equal: A typology and
Krüger, D., Klapötke, S., Bode, S., & Mattler, U. critical analysis of framing effects. Organiza-
(2013). Neural correlates of control operations tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
in inverse priming with relevant and irrelevant 76(2), 149–188. doi:10.1006/obhd.1998.2804
masks. NeuroImage, 64, 197–208. doi:10.1016/j. PMID:9831520
neuroimage.2012.09.018 PMID:22989624
Lingnau, A., & Vorberg, D. (2005). The time
Krüger, D., Klapötke, S., & Mattler, U. course of response inhibition in masked priming.
(2011). PRP-paradigm provides evidence for Perception & Psychophysics, 67(3), 545–557.
a perceptual origin of the negative compatibil- doi:10.3758/BF03193330 PMID:16119400
ity effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3),
Lleras, A., & Enns, J. T. (2004). Negative
866–881. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.014
compatibility or object updating? A caution-
PMID:20947385
ary tale of mask-dependent priming. Journal
Küper, K., Groh-Bordin, C., Zimmer, H. D., & of Experimental Psychology. General, 133(4),
Ecker, U. K. H. (2012). Electrophysiological cor- 475–493. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.475
relates of exemplar-specific processes in implicit PMID:15584802
and explicit memory. Cognitive, Affective & Be-
Loersch, C., Durso, G. R. O., & Petty, R. E.
havioral Neuroscience, 12(1), 52–64. doi:10.3758/
(2013). Vicissitudes of desire: A matching
s13415-011-0065-7 PMID:22038704
mechanism for subliminal persuasion. Social
Kutas, M., Lindamood, T., & Hillyard, S. A. Psychological and Personality, 4(5), 624–631.
(1984). Word expectancy and event-related brain doi:10.1177/1948550612471975
potentials during sentence processing. In S. Korn-
Loersch, C., & Payne, B. K. (2011). The situated
blum & J. Roquin (Eds.), Preparatory states and
inference model: An integrative account of the
processes (pp. 217–238). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
effects of primes on perception, behavior, and mo-
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we tivation. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 6(3), 234–252. doi:10.1177/1745691611406921

51

Priming

Loftus, E. F. (1975). Spreading activation within Mattler, U. (2005). Inhibition and decay of motor
semantic categories: Comments on Rosch’s and nonmotor priming. Perception & Psychophys-
‘cognitive representations of semantic cat- ics, 67(2), 285–300. doi:10.3758/BF03206492
egories’. Journal of Experimental Psychology. PMID:15971692
General, 104(3), 234–240. doi:10.1037/0096-
Mattler, U. (2006). On the locus of priming and
3445.104.3.234
inverse priming effects. Perception & Psychophys-
Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without as- ics, 68(6), 975–991. doi:10.3758/BF03193359
sociation: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic PMID:17153192
Bulletin & Review, 7(4), 618–630. doi:10.3758/
Mattler, U. (2007). Inverse target- and cue-priming
BF03212999 PMID:11206202
effects of masked stimuli. Journal of Experimental
Machado, L., Wyatt, N., Devine, A., & Knight, Psychology. Human Perception and Performance,
B. (2007). Action planning in the presence of 33(1), 83–102. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.83
distracting stimuli: An investigation into the time PMID:17311481
course of distractor effects. Journal of Experi-
Mattler, U., & Fendrich, R. (2007). Priming by
mental Psychology. Human Perception and Per-
motion too rapid to be consciously seen. Perception
formance, 33(5), 1045–1061. doi:10.1037/0096-
& Psychophysics, 69(8), 1389–1398. doi:10.3758/
1523.33.5.1045 PMID:17924806
BF03192954 PMID:18078229
Major, J. C. (2008). List composition effects for
May, C. P., Kane, M. J., & Hasher, L. (1995).
masked semantic primes: Evidence inconsistent
Determinants of negative priming. Psychologi-
with activation accounts. Theses and Dissertations
cal Bulletin, 118(1), 35–54. doi:10.1037/0033-
(Comprehensive). Paper 1059. Retrieved from
2909.118.1.35 PMID:7644605
http://scholars.wlu.ca/etd/1059
Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2006). Evidence for
Mandel, N., & Johnson, E. J. (2002). When web
episodic retrieval of inadequate prime responses
pages influence choice: Effects of visual primes
in auditory negative priming. Journal of Ex-
on experts and novices. The Journal of Consumer
perimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Research, 29(2), 235–245. doi:10.1086/341573
Performance, 32(4), 932–943. doi:10.1037/0096-
Masson, M. E. J. (1999). Semantic priming in a 1523.32.4.932 PMID:16846289
recurrent network: Comment on Dalrymple-Alford
Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2007). Negative priming
and Marmurek (1999). Journal of Experimental
as a memory phenomenon: A review of 20 years
Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
of negative priming research. Zeitschrift für Psy-
25(3), 776–794. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.3.776
chologie. The Journal of Psychology, 215, 35–51.
Masson, M. E. J., & Isaak, M. I. (1999). Masked
McNamara, T. P. (2005). Semantic prim-
priming of words and nonwords in a naming
ing: Perspectives from memory and word
task: Further evidence for a nonlexical basis for
recognition. New York: Psychology Press.
priming. Memory & Cognition, 27(3), 399–412.
doi:10.4324/9780203338001
doi:10.3758/BF03211536 PMID:10355231

52

Priming

Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P. M., Neely, J. H., Keefe, D. E., & Ross, K. L. (1989).
& Seiffert, A. E. (1998). Selective attention: Semantic priming in the lexical decision task:
A reevaluation of the implications of nega- Roles of prospective prime-generated expectancies
tive priming. Psychological Review, 105(2), and retrospective semantic matching. Journal of
203–229. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.203 Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and
PMID:9577237 Cognition, 15(6), 1003–1019. doi:10.1037/0278-
7393.15.6.1003 PMID:2530303
Milyavsky, M., Hassin, R. R., & Schul, Y.
(2012). Guess what? Implicit motivation boosts Neill, W. T. (1997). Episodic retrieval in nega-
the influence of subliminal information on tive priming and repetition priming. Journal of
choice. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and
1232–1241. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.06.001 Cognition, 23(6), 1291–1305. doi:10.1037/0278-
PMID:22776628 7393.23.6.1291
Musch, J. (2000). Affektives Priming: Kon- Neill, W. T. (2007). Mechanisms of transfer-
gruenzeffekte bei der evaluativen Bewertung. inappropriate processing. In D. S. Gorfein & C.
Inauguraldissertation zur Erlangung des Grades M. MacLeod (Eds.), Inhibition in cognition (pp.
eines Doktors der Philosophie an der Fakultät 63–78). Washington, DC: American Psychological
für Sozial- und Verhaltenswissenschaften der Association. doi:10.1037/11587-004
Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg.
Neumann, O. (1990). Direct parameter specifi-
Musch, J., Elze, A., & Klauer, K. C. (1998). Gibt cation and the concept of perception. Psycho-
es Wortlängeneffekte in der evaluativen Entsche- logical Research, 52(2-3), 207–215. doi:10.1007/
idungsaufgabe? Zeitschrift für Experimentelle BF00877529 PMID:2281129
Psychologie, 45, 109–119.
Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor re-
Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and re- sponses to nonreportable masked stimuli: Where
trieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibition- is the limit of direct parameter specification? In
less spreading activation and limited-capacity C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and
attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology. performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious in-
General, 106(3), 226–254. doi:10.1037/0096- formation processing (pp. 124–150). Cambridge,
3445.106.3.226 MA: MIT Press.
Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion.
visual word recognition: A selective review of Science, 216(5827), 1002–1005. doi:10.1126/
current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. science.1136930 PMID:17510358
W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading:
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Eighteen-
Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale,
month-old infants show increased helping fol-
NJ: Erlbaum.
lowing priming with affiliation. Psychological
Neely, J. H., & Keefe, D. E. (1989). Semantic Science, 20(10), 1189–1193. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
context effects on visual word processing: A 9280.2009.02419.x PMID:19674388
hybrid prospective-retrospective processing
theory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of
learning and motivation: Advances in research
and theory (pp. 207–248). New York: Academic
Press. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60538-1

53

Priming

Praamstra, P., & Seiss, E. (2005). The neurophysiol- Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & De Houwer,
ogy of response competition: Motor cortex activa- J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus-re-
tion and inhibition following subliminal response sponse associations as a source of negative
priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, priming. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
17(3), 483–493. doi:10.1162/0898929053279513 ogy. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3),
PMID:15814007 482–495. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.3.482
PMID:15910132
Quillian, M. R. (1968). Semantic Memory. In M.
Minsky (Ed.), Semantic information processing Ruys, K. I., & Stapel, D. A. (2008). Emotion elici-
(pp. 216–260). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. tor or emotion messenger? Subliminal priming
reveals two faces of facial expressions. Psychologi-
Ramachandran, V. S., & Anstis, S. M. (1983).
cal Science, 19(6), 593–600. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
Extrapolation of motion path in human vi-
9280.2008.02128.x PMID:18578850
sual perception. Vision Research, 23(1),
83–85. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(83)90044-5 Schlaghecken, F., Bowman, H., & Eimer, M.
PMID:6868383 (2006). Dissociating local and global levels
of perceptuo-motor control in masked prim-
Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1988). A retrieval
ing. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
theory of priming in memory. Psychological
Human Perception and Performance, 32(3),
Review, 95(3), 385–408. doi:10.1037/0033-
618–494. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.618
295X.95.3.385 PMID:3406246
PMID:16822128
Reuss, H., Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., & Hom-
Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2002). Motor acti-
mel, B. (2011). Unconscious activation of task
vation with and without inhibition: Evidence for a
sets. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3),
threshold mechanism in motor control. Perception
556–567. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.014
& Psychophysics, 64(1), 148–162. doi:10.3758/
PMID:21396830
BF03194564 PMID:11916298
Riskind, J. H., & Gotay, C. C. (1982). Physical
Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2006). Active
posture: Could it have regulatory or feedback
masks and active inhibition: A comment on Lleras
effects on motivation and emotion? Motiva-
and Enns (2004) and on Verleger, Jaśkowski, Ay-
tion and Emotion, 6(3), 273–298. doi:10.1007/
demir, van der Lubbe, and Groen (2004). Journal
BF00992249
of Experimental Psychology. General, 135(3),
Rohr, M., Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2012). 484–494. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.484
Masked emotional priming beyond global va- PMID:16846277
lence activations. Cognition and Emotion, 26(2),
Schlaghecken, F., Klapp, S. T., & Maylor, E. A.
224–244. doi:10.1080/02699931.2011.576852
(2009). Either or neither, but not both: Locating the
PMID:21970398
effects of masked primes. Proceedings. Biologi-
Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (1998). Ein fairer cal Sciences, 276(1656), 515–521. doi:10.1098/
Test für die Aktivationsausbreitungshypothese: rspb.2008.0933 PMID:18945665
Untersuchung affektiver Kongruenzeffekte in der
Stroop Aufgabe. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle
Psychologie, 45, 120–135. PMID:9677859

54

Priming

Schlaghecken, F., Rowley, L., Sembi, S., Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God
Simmons, R., & Whitcomb, D. (2007). The is watching you: Priming god concepts in-
negative compatibility effect: A case for self- creases prosocial behavior in an anonymous
inhibition. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, economic game. Psychological Science, 18(9),
3(1), 227–240. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0027-y 803–809. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01983.x
PMID:20517511 PMID:17760777
Schmidt, F., Haberkamp, A., & Schmidt, T. Sharkey, A. J. C., & Sharkey, N. E. (1992). Weak
(2011). Dos and don’ts in response priming contextual constraints in text and word prim-
research. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 7(- ing. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(4),
1), 120–131. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0092-2 543–572. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(92)90028-V
PMID:22253674
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behav-
Schmidt, T. (2002). The finger in flight: Real- ior. New York: Free Press.
time motor control by visually masked color
Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Hermans, D.
stimuli. Psychological Science, 13(2), 112–118.
(2009). Modulation of automatic semantic
doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00421 PMID:11933993
priming by feature-specific attention allocation.
Schmidt, T. (2007). Measuring unconscious Journal of Memory and Language, 61(1), 37–54.
cognition: Beyond the zero-awareness crite- doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.03.004
rion. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1),
Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., & Eelen,
275–287. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0030-3
P. (2007). Affective priming of non-affective
PMID:20517514
semantic categorization responses. Experimental
Schmidt, T., Niehaus, S., & Nagel, A. (2006). Psychology, 54(1), 44–53. doi:10.1027/1618-
Primes and targets in rapid chases: Tracing sequen- 3169.54.1.44 PMID:17341014
tial waves of motor activation. Behavioral Neuro-
Stone, A. (2012). Centre-surround inhibition is
science, 120(5), 1005–1016. doi:10.1037/0735-
a general aspect of famous person recognition:
7044.120.5.1005 PMID:17014252
Evidence from negative semantic priming from
Schmidt, T., & Seydell, A. (2008). Visual at- clearly visible primes. Memory & Cognition,
tention amplifies response priming of pointing 40(4), 652–662. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0176-y
movements to color targets. Perception & Psycho- PMID:22203608
physics, 70(3), 443–455. doi:10.3758/PP.70.3.443
Stroebe, W., Jonas, K., & Hewstone, M. R. C.
PMID:18459255
(2003). Sozialpsychologie: Eine Einführung.
Schmidt, T., & Vorberg, D. (2006). Criteria for Berlin: Springer.
unconscious cognition: Three types of dissocia-
Sumner, P. (2007). Negative and positive
tion. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(3), 489–504.
masked-priming: Implications for motor inhibi-
doi:10.3758/BF03193692 PMID:16900839
tion. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1),
Sela, A., & Shiv, B. (2009). Unraveling priming: 317–326. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0033-0
When does the same prime activate a goal versus PMID:20517517
a trait? The Journal of Consumer Research, 36(3),
418–433. doi:10.1086/598612

55

Priming

Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Klauer, K. C. (2008). Veltkamp, M., Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2009).
‚Automatic’ evaluation? Strategic effects on af- Unravelling the motivational yarn: A frame-
fective priming. Journal of Experimental Social work for understanding the instigation of im-
Psychology, 44(5), 1414–1417. doi:10.1016/j. plicitly motivated behaviour resulting from
jesp.2008.04.004 deprivation and positive affect. European
Review of Social Psychology, 20(1), 345–381.
Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Klauer, K. C. (2013).
doi:10.1080/10463280903388665
On the controllability of evaluative-priming ef-
fects: Some limits that are none. Cognition and Veltkamp, M., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2011).
Emotion, 27(4), 632–657. doi:10.1080/0269993 Motivating consumer behavior by subliminal
1.2012.732041 PMID:23075050 conditioning in the absence of basic needs: Strik-
ing even while the iron is cold. Journal of Con-
Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming ef-
sumer Psychology, 21(1), 49–56. doi:10.1016/j.
fect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The
jcps.2010.09.011
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.
A, Human Experimental Psychology, 37(4), Verleger, R., Jaśkowski, P., Aydemir, A., van der
571–590. doi:10.1080/14640748508400920 Lubbe, R. H. J., & Groen, M. (2004). Qualitative
PMID:4081101 differences between conscious and nonconscious
processing? On inverse priming induced by
Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect
masked arrows. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integra-
ogy. General, 133(4), 494–515. doi:10.1037/0096-
tion of conflicting views. Quarterly Journal of
3445.133.4.494 PMID:15584803
Experimental Psychology, 54A(2), 321–343.
doi:10.1080/713755969 PMID:11394050 Versace, R., & Allain, G. (2001). Negative prim-
ing in a gender decision task and in a semantic
Toburen, T., & Meier, B. P. (2010). Priming god-
categorization task. Acta Psychologica, 108(1),
related concepts increases anxiety and task persis-
73–90. doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00041-5
tence. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
PMID:11485194
29(2), 127–143. doi:10.1521/jscp.2010.29.2.127
Verwijmeren, T., Karremans, J. C., Stroebe, W.,
Topolinski, S., & Sparenberg, P. (2012). Turn-
& Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2011). The workings
ing the hands of time: Clockwise movements
and limits of subliminal advertising: The role of
increase preference for novelty. Social Psycho-
habits. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(2),
logical & Personality Science, 3(3), 308–314.
206–213. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2010.11.004
doi:10.1177/1948550611419266
Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T.,
Vath, N., & Schmidt, T. (2007). Tracing sequential
& Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses
waves of rapid visuomotor activation in lateralized
for visual perception and action priming. Proceed-
readiness potentials. Neuroscience, 145(1), 197–
ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
208. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.11.044
United States of America, 100(10), 6275–6280.
PMID:17258867
doi:10.1073/pnas.0931489100 PMID:12719543

56

Priming

Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2005). Repeated
T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2004). Invariant time masked category primes interfere with related
course of priming with and without awareness. In exemplars: New evidence for negative semantic
C. Kaernbach, E. Schröger, & H. Müller (Eds.), priming. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
Psychophysics beyond sensation: Laws and invari- ogy. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(1),
ants of human cognition (pp. 271–288). Mahwah, 108–120. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.1.108
NJ: Erlbaum. PMID:15641909
Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1970). Am- Wentura, D., Kulfanek, M., & Greve, W. (2005).
nesic syndrome: Consolidation or retrieval? Na- Masked affective priming by name letters:
ture, 228(5272), 628–630. doi:10.1038/228628a0 Evidence for a correspondence of explicit and
PMID:4990853 implicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 41(6), 654–663. doi:10.1016/j.
Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1974). The
jesp.2004.02.005
effect of prior learning on subsequent retention
in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 12(4), Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (2003). The
419–428. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(74)90072-4 ‘meddling-in’ of affective information: A gen-
PMID:4437740 eral model of automatic evaluation effects. In J.
Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology
Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003).
of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition
Task-switching and long-term priming: Role
and emotion (pp. 51–86). Mahwah, New Jersey:
of episodic stimulus: Task bindings in task-
Lawrence Erlbaum.
shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46(4),
361–413. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(02)00520-0 Wheeler, S. C., & DeMarree, K. G. (2009). Mul-
PMID:12809680 tiple mechanisms of prime-to-behavior effect. So-
cial and Personality Psychology Compass, 3/4(4),
Weisbrod, M., Trage, J., Hill, H., Sattler, H.,
566–581. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00187.x
Maier, S., & Kiefer, M. et al. (1999). Emotional
priming in depressed patients. German Journal Yi, Y. (1990). Cognitive and affective priming
of Psychiatry, 2, 19–47. effects of the context for print advertisements.
Journal of Advertising, 19(2), 40–48. doi:10.10
Wentura, D. (1999). Activation and inhibition of
80/00913367.1990.10673186
affective information: Evidence for negative prim-
ing in the evaluation task. Cognition and Emotion, Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere
13(1), 65–91. doi:10.1080/026999399379375 exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 9(2, Pt.2), 1–27. doi:10.1037/h0025848
Wentura, D. (2000). Dissociative affective and
PMID:5667435
associative priming effects in the lexical decision
task: Yes vs. no responses to word targets reveal
evaluative judgment tendencies. Journal of Ex-
perimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and ADDITIONAL READING
Cognition, 26(2), 456–469. doi:10.1037/0278-
7393.26.2.456 PMID:10764106 Bohn, M., & Bermeitinger, C. (2012). Affektives
Priming. In C. Bermeitinger (Ed.), Paradigmen
der Kognitiven Psychologie: Affektive Reize I (pp.
13–38). Berlin: Uni-Edition.

57

Priming

Bowers, J. S., & Marsolek, C. J. (2003) (Eds.). Laver, G. D., & Burke, D. M. (1993). Why do
Rethinking implicit memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford semantic priming effects increase in old age? A
University Press. meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 8(1), 34–43.
doi:10.1037/0882-7974.8.1.34 PMID:8461113
Bowers, J. S., & Marsolek, C. J. (Eds.). (2003).
Rethinking implicit memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without as-
University Press. sociation: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 7(4), 618–630. doi:10.3758/
De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt,
BF03212999 PMID:11206202
A., & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A
normative analysis and review. Psychological Maxfield, L. (1997). Attention and semantic prim-
Bulletin, 135(3), 347–368. doi:10.1037/a0014211 ing: A review of prime task effects. Consciousness
PMID:19379018 and Cognition, 6(2-3), 204–218. doi:10.1006/
ccog.1997.0311
Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activa-
tion of associated evaluations: An overview. May, C. P., Kane, M. J., & Hasher, L. (1995).
Cognition and Emotion, 15(2), 115–142. Determinants of negative priming. Psychologi-
doi:10.1080/02699930125908 cal Bulletin, 118(1), 35–54. doi:10.1037/0033-
2909.118.1.35 PMID:7644605
Fleischman, D. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998).
Repetition priming in normal aging and Al- Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2007). Negative priming
zheimer’s disease: A review of findings and as a memory phenomenon: A review of 20 years
theories. Psychology and Aging, 13(1), 88–119. of negative priming research. Zeitschrift für Psy-
doi:10.1037/0882-7974.13.1.88 PMID:9533193 chologie. The Journal of Psychology, 215, 35–51.
Fox, E. (1995). Negative Priming from ignored dis- McNamara, T. P. (2005). Semantic prim-
tractors in visual selection: A review. Psychonomic ing: Perspectives from memory and word
Bulletin & Review, 2(2), 145–173. doi:10.3758/ recognition. New York: Psychology Press.
BF03210958 PMID:24203653 doi:10.4324/9780203338001
Hsu, N., & Schütt, Z. (Eds., 2012). Psychology McNamara, T. P., & Holbrook, J. B. (2003). Se-
of priming. New York: Nova. mantic memory and priming. In A. F. Healy &
R. W. Proctor (Eds.), Experimental Psychology
Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming
(vol. 4, pp. 447-474). New York: Wiley.
due to association strength or featural overlap?
A micro-analytic review. Psychonomic Bul- Minzenberg, M. J., Ober, B. A., & Vinogradov,
letin & Review, 10(4), 785–813. doi:10.3758/ S. (2002). Semantic priming in schizophrenia:
BF03196544 PMID:15000531 A review and synthesis. Journal of the Inter-
national Neuropsychological Society, 8(05),
Klauer, K. C. (1998). Affective priming. In W.
699–720. doi:10.1017/S1355617702801357
Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Re-
PMID:12164679
view of Social Psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 67–103).
Chichester, UK: Wiley. Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in
visual word recognition: A selective review of
Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective
current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G.
priming: Findings and theories. In J. Musch & K.
W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading:
C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation:
Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale,
Affective processes in cognition and emotion.
NJ: Erlbaum.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

58

Priming

Neill, W. T. (2007). Mechanisms of transfer- KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS


inappropriate processing. In D. S. Gorfein & C.
M. MacLeod (Eds.), Inhibition in cognition (pp. Affective/Evaluative Priming: Variant of
63–78). Washington, DC: American Psychological priming; relation between prime and target is
Association. doi:10.1037/11587-004 defined by same (i.e., congruent) vs. different
(i.e., incongruent) affect.
Pomarol-Clotet, E., Oh, T. M. S. S., Laws, K. Lexical Decision Task: Participants have to
R., & McKenna, P. J. (2008). Semantic prim- decide as fast and as accurate as possible whether
ing in schizophrenia: Systematic review and a string was a real/correctly written word or not.
meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, Often used in semantic priming.
192(2), 92–97. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.106.032102 Negative Priming: In the typical case, two
PMID:18245021 displays with two stimuli each are presented. Par-
Schacter, D. L., & Buckner, R. L. (1998). ticipants have to identify one stimulus of each dis-
Priming and the brain. Neuron, 20(2), 185– play and to ignore the other stimulus. The crucial
195. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80448-1 conditions are: 1. The to-be-ignored stimulus of
PMID:9491981 display one is repeated as to-be-attended stimulus
in display two (i.e., ignored repetition condition);
Schmidt, F., Haberkamp, A., & Schmidt, T. 2. Both stimuli of display two were not presented
(2011). Dos and don’ts in response priming in display one (i.e., control condition). Typical
research. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 7(- finding: Faster reactions in the control trials than
1), 120–131. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0092-2 ignored repetition trials.
PMID:22253674 Negative Priming Effects: Participants re-
Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect acted faster in unrelated/incongruent/incompatible
inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integra- trials than in related/congruent/compatible trials.
tion of conflicting views. Quarterly Journal of Positive Priming Effects: Participants reacted
Experimental Psychology, 54A(2), 321–343. faster in related/congruent/compatible trials than
doi:10.1080/713755969 PMID:11394050 in unrelated/incongruent/incompatible trials.
Prime: In the priming paradigm, the prime
Van den Bussche, E., Van den Noortgate, W., precedes the target. Most often, participants do
& Reynvoet, B. (2009). Mechanisms of masked not have to react on the prime.
priming: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bul- Priming: Phenomenon that a first stimulus
letin, 135(3), 452–477. doi:10.1037/a0015329 influences the processing of a second stimulus.
PMID:19379025 Response Priming: Variant of priming; rela-
Wentura, D., & Degner, J. (2010). A practical tion between prime and target is defined by same
guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In (i.e., congruent, compatible) vs. different (i.e.,
B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of incongruent, incompatible) response tendencies.
implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, Semantic Priming: Variant of priming;
and applications. New York: Guilford. relation between prime and target is defined by
semantically or associatively related vs. unrelated
concepts.

59

Priming

Target: In the priming paradigm, the target ing, which was developed by Zajonc (1968).
follows the prime. The target has to be classified This paradigm looks at the evaluation or
or categorized regarding a pre-defined category, classification of targets that are ambiguous
named or identified. The target is either related or neutral regarding the to-be-evaluated
or unrelated to the prime. The target is most often dimension, dependent on the prior presenta-
presented visually or auditory. tion of a prime from one or the other end of
the dimension. The typical result in affec-
tive priming sensu Zajonc is that (masked)
ENDNOTES positive/negative primes lead to a positive/
negative evaluation of neutral stimuli (e.g.,
1
See also the statement of Fockenberg, Koole, Chinese characters presented to Western
and Semin (2008, S. 647): “Recent research participants).
indicates that virtually any psychological
5
A further approach, characterizing nega-
reaction can be primed.” tive semantic priming as a negative priming
2
This is a common finding in priming stud- phenomenon in its narrower sense, was ruled
ies: Participants are often unaware of the out experimentally because an approxima-
manipulation or even the prime stimuli, and tion of a negative priming procedure yielded
in most cases state that their behavior was positive priming effects (Frings et al., 2008).
unaffected by the primes. Likewise, this finding ruled out the respond-
3
Similar considerations were made somewhat ing optimally with unknown sources of evi-
earlier in the literature on affective priming, dence (ROUSE) model of Huber (2008). The
to account for task-specific priming effects ROUSE model assumes a process of habitu-
(cf. Bargh, Chaiken, Raymond, & Hymens, ation that operates on multiple processing
1996). However, such findings (also regard- levels. It uses this notion of habituation—
ing list effects) were primarily interpreted which is common in perceptual processing
as evidence against spreading activation models—to deal with the issue of unclear
accounts of affective priming, which viewed sources of evidence (source confusion, cf.
spreading activation as unconditional and Wentura & Rothermund, 2003). Source
context-independent (cf. Klauer & Musch, confusion can arise in priming when a par-
2003; Wentura & Rothermund, 2003). ticipant fails to determine what information
4
Some of the conditions of Mattler and comes from the prime and what information
Fendrich’s (2007) experiment featured comes from the target. The ROUSE model
elements of so-called ‘motion priming,’ explicitly predicts a negative priming effect
where the direction of a moving prime or in a repetition priming paradigm (i.e., when
an own movement influences the percep- the prime is repeated as the target) of the kind
tion of a subsequent ambiguously moving implemented by Frings et al. (2008)—who,
target (e.g., Ramachandra & Anstis, 1983). however, found a positive priming effect (as
Structurally, motion priming corresponds to mentioned earlier).
a priming variant known as affective prim-

60
61

Chapter 3
Individual Differences
in Implicit Learning:
Current Problems and Issues for Research

Daisuke Nakamura
Aoyama Gakuin University, Japan

ABSTRACT
This chapter reviews research on whether individual differences in psychometric intelligence, working
memory, and other less investigated variables, such as emotion and personality, affect implicit learning,
with particular focus on Reber’s evolutionary theory and Kaufman’s dual-process theory for implicit
learning. The review shows that while the null effects of psychometric intelligence on implicit learning
seems robust as both theories claim, those of working memory were unclear due to methodological in-
sufficiency. For the effects of emotion and personality, further investigation is needed as studies in this
direction have just begun to proliferate. The chapter concludes that the research findings on the effects of
these individual difference variables on implicit learning are still inconclusive, except for psychometric
intelligence, and provides suggestions for future research.

INTRODUCTION recent interest in the dual-process theory of cogni-


tion (e.g., Kaufman, 2011; Kaufman, DeYoung,
Individual differences (IDs) in implicit learning Gray, & Brown (2011);Kaufman, DeYoung,
have been a neglected area of research. Empiri- Reis, & Grey, 2011; Stanovich & Toplak, 2012)
cal studies were rather sporadic and focus was shed new light on IDs in other variables such as
on psychometric intelligence during the 1990s. Working Memory (WM), emotion and personal-
Findings have been evaluated according to Reber’s ity in the implicit learning literature, and begin
(1993; Reber & Allen, 2000) evolutionary theory to provide empirical findings on the effects of
of implicit learning where he argued that IDs in these variables on implicit learning. The aim of
psychometoric intelligence (and other variables this paper, then, is to review the recent findings
such as affect) should contribute only to variability of the effects of these ID variables on implicit
in explicit but not implicit learning. However, learning and to provide state of the art research

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch003

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

with special references to two prominent theories In the typical SRT task, the screen is divided
of IDs in implicit learning. In so doing, the paper, into quadrants and a stimulus appears in one
after providing brief characteristics of implicit of the quadrants. Participants are required to
learning, considers theoretical treatments of IDs press keys corresponding to each location. A se-
in implicit learning, especially focusing on Reber’s quence of a stimulus follows a fixed pattern and
(1993; Reber & Allen, 2000) evolutionary theory participants do not know of its existence. After
of implicit learning and Kaufman’s (2011) dual- repeated exposure to this fixed pattern, reaction
process theory of intelligence. Then, reviews of time (RT) on the fixed pattern decreases. Later
empirical research on the two most investigated a random sequence of a stimulus is inserted into
areas of research with relation to implicit learning, the key-pressing trial. Since participants’ RTs on
psychometric intelligence and WM, are provided this random pattern significantly increases, it is
in addition to other less investigated areas such argued that they have learned the fixed pattern of
as emotion and personality. Before concluding, the stimulus sequence.
some issues for further research are provided each One of the characteristics of implicit learning
for psychometric intelligence, WM, and emotion is the nature of acquired knowledge: tacit complex
and personality. knowledge, largely unavailable to consciousness at
the point of learning itself (not “after” the learn-
ing; see Reber, 1989). Although the amount of
BACKGROUND research on the implicit nature of implicit learning
has proliferated in the literature, this issue of the
Implicit learning, learning without awareness of implicitness of learned knowledge and/or learn-
learning processes and/or learning outcomes, has ing processes has not been settled. This is largely
been extensively studied in cognitive psychol- because of methodological problems on how to
ogy (see Perruchet, 2008; Pothos, 2007; Shanks, measure participants’ awareness of the learning
2005 for recent reviews). Two learning tasks have processes and/or the learning outcomes at the point
been employed in the implicit learning literature: of learning, or more generally, during learning
artificial grammar (AG) learning and serial reac- (Nakamura, 2013a, b; Shanks & St. John, 1994).
tion time (SRT) tasks (see Nissen & Bullemer, Reber (1993; Reber & Allen, 2000), from an
1987and Reber, 1967 for representative studies). evolutionary point of view (described below),
Since participants typically cannot verbalise the provided other characteristics of implicit learning:
contents of rules or regularities underlying stimuli lesser influences by IDs such as psychometric
but nevertheless show learning in both tasks, the intelligence on implicit learning, compared with
nature of their learning is argued to be implicit. explicit learning. Although this issue of IDs has
In the typical AG learning task, participants are been less investigated, compared with the vast
presented with a series of digits (e.g., XVXXV) amount of research on the first issue, investigation
without information on the existence of rules, or into the individual variability of implicit learn-
“grammar”, that underlie such digits. They are just ing is necessary for clarifying the (dissociable)
asked to memorise the digits. After training, they nature of implicit learning (from the explicit
are told of the existence of the rules and asked to learning) and thus the paper provides are view
judge the grammaticality of test digits as well as on the individual variability in implicit learning,
to indicate the content of such rules. Participants’ particularly focusing on the relation between in-
performance on the grammaticality judgment is telligence and implicit learning, and its relations
above chance (thus showing evidence of learning). to other cognitive-conative-affective abilities1

62

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

such as WM, motivation, and personality. The (McGeorge, Crawford, & Kelly, 1997; Meulemans,
next section provides a summary of theoretical Van der Linden, &Perruchet, 1998; Weinert,
underpinnings on these IDs in implicit learning. 2009;seeJanacsek, Fiser, & Nemeth, 2012 and
Thomas et al., 2004, for counterexamples). For
Reber’s Evolutionary Theory of example, Meulemans et al. (1998) employed
Implicit Learning and Kaufman’s the SRT task and compared 6- and 10-year-olds
Dual-Process Theory of Intelligence with adults. Two types of stimuli sequences were
created: repeating and random sequences. After
The two most prominent theories in relation to IDs a series of the repeating sequences, the random
in implicit learning are Reber’s evolutionary theory sequence appeared, and if participants learned the
of implicit learning and Kaufman’s dual-process pattern for the repeating sequences, RTs on the ran-
theory of intelligence.2 Reber (1993), based on evo- dom sequence would increase. Therefore, learning
lutionary biology, provided the following axiom was measured by differences in RTs between the
about consciousness: “Consciousness is a late repeating and the random sequences, or abrupt
arrival on the evolutionary scene. Sophisticated increases in RTs at the point where the repeating
unconscious perceptual and cognitive functions and the random sequences were interchanged.
preceded its emergence by a considerable margin” Both groups of children indeed showed a similar
(p. 86). As a consequence of evolutionary precur- reduction of RTs on the repeating sequences to that
sors of implicit learning abilities, Reber (1993, of adults. In contrast, no groups could distinguish
p. 88) provided the following five characteristics old sequences from new sequences, suggesting
of implicit learning: (1) robustness in the face of that acquired knowledge of the stimuli sequences
psychological disorders such as amnesia; (2) age was implicit.
independence; (3) low variability; (4) intelligence Finally, implicit learning can be found in other
quotient (IQ) independence; and (5) commonality species and thus shows cross-species commonality.
of process. Direct evidence has recently emerged. Christie and
First, the implicit learning system is robust in colleagues (Christie & Dalrymple, 2004; Christie
the face of neuropsychological disorders (Abrams &Hersch, 2004) constructed the rat version of the
& Reber, 1988; Knowlton & Squire, 1994, 1996). SRT tasks and comparative RT increases on the
For instance, Knowlton and Squire (1996) com- random sequence as with those of human subjects
pared amnesiacs whose declarative memory was were discovered in rats (Christie & Dalrymple,
severely impaired with normal participants on 2004) and mice (Christie & Hersch, 2004).
AG learning. They found that the amnesiacs as Low variability and IQ independence are of
well as the normal participants performed well particular importance to the relation between IDs
on grammatical judgment. However, amnesiacs’ in cognitive abilities and implicit learning. This is
recognition of the letter strings (bigrams [e.g., because the above three predictions are concerned
XV] and trigrams [e.g., XVX]) was significantly with “group differences in unconscious cognitive
worse (lower than that of the normal participants). processing [that is, implicit learning]” but low
Therefore, this discrepancy in amnesiacs’ implicit variability and IQ independence are concerned
and explicit learning systems is often interpreted with “the degree of interindividual variation that
as evidence for the robustness of the former learn- is expected when we directly compare implicit
ing system (see Nakamura, 2013a for a problem and explicit functions within the same population”
concerning this interpretation). (Reber& Allen, 2000, p. 236). Low variability in
Second, children sometimes perform the im- implicit learning is operationalised by differences
plicit learning tasks at the same level as adults do between standard deviation of implicit learning

63

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

and that of explicit learning. Therefore, one way “incidental” learning such as implicit learning
to investigate consequences of the evolutionary of complex stimulus displays and automatic or
early emergence of implicit learning abilities spontaneous behaviours and thus, “implicit”
is, they argued, direct comparisons of standard abilities are largely responsible. According to this
deviations (SDs) in implicit and explicit learning theory, all intelligent behaviours depend on these
tasks given that both tasks require the same level two types of cognitive processes and the degree
of (external) factors such as attention, motiva- of contributions by these two processes depends
tion, and depression. There have only been a few on the nature of the behaviours. Of relevance
studies that compared SDs in implicit and explicit to the present paper is that IDs in psychometric
learning because of methodological difficulties intelligence and WM are largely responsible for
discussed in the next section provided byReber, explicit learning while the effects of these explicit
Walkenfeld, and Hernstadt (1991). In their stud- abilities are smaller for implicit learning.
ies, a typical AG learning task was employed as Kaufman’s dual-process theory has important
an implicit learning task while explicit learning differences from Reber’s theory. In Kaufman’s
was operationalised as a series solution task where theory, different abilities are responsible for
participants chose one (e.g., D) out of two solutions controlled and spontaneous cognition, while in
(e.g., D or C) given a series (e.g., ABCBCDCDE_). Reber’s theory IDs in cognitive abilities are as-
Reber et al. (1991) found significantly larger SDs sumed to be “secondary” factors affecting vari-
in explicit (15.41) than in implicit learning (7.2) ability in implicit learning and thus it seems to be
tasks. Robinson (2005b) found similar results in the case that the existence of the effects of IDs on
a replication of this study. cognitive-conative-affective variables in implicit
With regard to IQ independence, Reber et al. learning are epiphenomenal for Reber (1993).
(1991) also provided the first study. They employed
a short form (Picture Arrangement, Vocabulary,
Block Design, and Arithmetic) of the Wechsler MAIN FOCUS OF THE CHAPTER
Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) as an
IQ test and found that IQ had significant positive Issues, Controversies, and Problems
correlations with the explicit series solution task
but not with the implicit AG learning task. This As we have seen above, there are two ways to
way of comparing implicit with explicit learning is investigate IDs in implicit learning: the direct
“inferential”, Reber and Allen (2000) stated, since comparison of SDs in implicit learning with those
the study implies IDs from differential contribu- in explicit learning, and the indirect, inferential ap-
tions by moderator variables (e.g., IQ) to implicit proach to consider correlations of IDs as moderator
and explicit learning performance. variables with implicit learning performance. As
Recently, Kaufman (2011), based on the Reber and Allen noted, a direct comparison is not
dual-process theory of human cognition (Evans easily feasible since two comparable implicit and
& Frankish, 2009), divided cognitive processes explicit learning tasks with the same difficulty
into controlled cognition and spontaneous cogni- and same levels of attention and motivation are
tion. Control cognition deals with “goal-directed” difficult to construct.3 And since it has been sug-
behaviours and thus the central executive WM, gested and found that there are IDs in attentional
psychometric intelligence, and other “explicit” functioning as well as conative-affective factors
cognitive abilities are responsible. On the other such as motivation and anxiety (Robinson, 2005a)
hand, spontaneous cognition is concerned with during any task performance, such a direct method

64

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

of comparison is almost impossible. Unlike Re- has provoked controversies over its definition and
ber’s evolutionary theory of implicit learning, many definitions have been provided (see Wasser-
Kaufman (2011) does not make the low variability man &Tulsky, 2005 for a historical review). For
assumption. example, Spearman (1927) discussed possibili-
On the other hand, the existence of such IDs as ties that the term “intelligence” means memory,
moderator variables themselves validates another imagination, perception, attention, etc., but none
indirect, inferential way to investigate differential of these cangive sufficient explanations for all
effects of these moderator variables on implicit intelligent behaviours. And this situation remains
and explicit learning performance. Research the same at the present moment (see a report by the
in this direction was sporadic in the 1990s and American Psychological Association Task Force
exclusively focused on the relation between psy- chaired by Ulric Neisser; Neisser et al., 1996).
chometric intelligence and implicit learning (e.g., Furthermore, various “standardized” intelligence
Reber et al., 1991) and has continued to appear in test batteries such as the Wechsler scales (Zhu
the literature (e.g., Danner, Hagemann, Schankin, & Weiss, 2005), the Stanford-Binet Intelligence
Hager, & Funke, 2011; Gebauer & Mackintosh, Scales (Roid & Pomplun, 2005), the Kaufman
2007; Kaufman et al., 2010; Robinson, 2005b). Assessment Battery for Children (Kaufman,
In addition to research on the relation between Kaufman, Kaufman-Singer, & Kaufman, 2005),
intelligence and implicit learning, recent studies etc. (see Flanagan & Harrison, 2005 for descrip-
have shed new light on other dimensions of IDs, tions of other tests) have also been developed,
some of which are what Reber and Allen (2000: sometimes in an a theoretical way, and this test
240) called “clearly far from complete”. These proliferation implies that no single battery of in-
include WM (e.g., Bo, Jennet, & Seidler, 2011), telligence measures the complex characteristics
thinking styles (e.g., Xie, Gao, & King, 2013), of intelligence.
cognitive styles (e.g., Pretz, Totz, & Kaufman, Intelligence that has been employed in relation
2010), emotion (e.g., Shang, Fu, Dienes, Shao, to implicit learning is psychometric intelligence,
& Fu, 2013; Whitmarsh, Udden, Barendregt, & “the human differences measured by mental tests”
Petersson, 2013), and personality (e.g., Kaufman (Deary, 2001a, p. 127). Although psychometric
et al., 2010; Woolhouse & Bayne, 2000). The most statistics, specifically (exploratory) factor analy-
investigated areas of IDs, intelligence and WM, sis, is often said to describe, but not to explain
are reviewed first and then other less investigated individual variability in intellectual patterns (e.g.,
areas are reviewed together below with issues for Deary, 2001b; Howe, 1996), the psychometric
further research. intelligence is targeted here since all studies in
implicit learning have focused on it in relation to
Intelligence and Implicit Learning IDs in implicit learning.

Intelligence has been a rigorously investigated Research Findings


area of research and different types of intelligence
such as successful intelligence, emotional intel- Similar null effects of intelligence on implicit
ligence, and multiple intelligences (to name but learning as in Reber et al. (1991) were found in
a few) have been proposed in the literature (see Feldman, Kerr, and Streissguth (1995) and Brooks
Mackintosh, 2011; Sternberg & Kaufman, 2011, and Kempe (2012). In Feldman et al.’s (1995)
for reviews). Intelligence by definition is required study, 14-year-olds performed the SRT task and
in many complex “intelligent” behaviours but their performance was measured in two ways:
defining the exact characteristics of the construct key-pressing (procedural learning, typical in the

65

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

SRT task)4 and generation (declarative learning) Some studies found significant correlations of
with other ID tasks such as forward and backward intelligence with implicit learning but the strength
digit span. In the generation task, they were asked of the correlations was larger in implicit than in
to predict the next position by pressing a cor- explicit learning as demonstrated by McGeorge et
responding key given a sequence. Correlational al. (1997). As in Reber et al. (1991), IQ measured
analyses of the scores with the full form of the by the WAIS-R full scale showed significant
WAIS-R measured at age 7 showed significant correlations with scores on the explicit series
positive correlations with declarative (generation) solution task but not with implicit AG learning.
but not procedural tasks. Likewise, Brooks and However, when two different types of subtests
Kempe (2012)5 found null correlations between were considered separately, verbal IQ showed
auditory sequence learning and intelligence mea- significant positive correlations only with explicit
sured by the Culture Fair Intelligence test (CFI). learning while performance IQ showed significant
They used, as an implicit learning task, Misyak positive correlations with both kinds of learning,
and Christiansen’s (2007) language-like, auditory but the magnitude was larger in explicit than in
sequence learning task where a grammatical non- implicit learning. Similar tendencies were found
word (e.g., hep) sequence was created according in Gebauer and Mackintosh (2007). With a large
to the following “grammar” rules: S (sentence) -> German population, Gebauer and Mackintosh
NP (noun phrase) + VP (verb phrase), NP -> d (2007) employed three different implicit learning
(determiner) + N (noun), NP -> D+A (adjective) tasks (AG learning, SRT, and problem solving).
+ N, and VP -> V (verb) + (NP). During the test, Explicit learning was measured by giving explicit
participants chose one grammatical sequence that instructions on these three implicit learning tasks
followed these rules out of two auditory sequences. and three different types of the latent general or
The results showed the null effects of CFI of G factor (Jensen, 1998; Spearman, 1927) were
correct performance on the auditory sequence created by 18 German intelligence tests: Gf (fluid
learning task. In Kaufman et al.’s (2010) studies, intelligence, or inductive and deductive reason-
Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices test, the ing for novelty), Gc (crystallised intelligence,
verbal reasoning section of the Differential Ap- or accumulated knowledge), and Gy (explicit
titude Test (DAT), and the Mental rotations test memory for storing and retrieving information).
were employed as psychometric intelligence test The results showed that a majority of intelligence
measures and only the scores on the DAT verbal factors positively correlated with explicit learn-
reasoning task showed significant correlations ing (Gf with explicit AG learning (r = .25), Gf
with the implicit SRT task. Moreover, intelligence with explicit problem-solving task (r = .27), Gf
had no significant relation with implicit SRT at with explicit SRT task (r = .20), Gc with explicit
the latent analysis level. Finally, Unsworth and problem-solving task (r = .21), Gy with explicit
Engle (2005) found the null effects of intelligence AG learning (r = .27) while only a few intel-
on the SRT task in terms of Raven’s Progressive ligence factors correlated with implicit learning
Matrix. In their study, they created incidental (Gf with implicit problem-solving task (r = .10),
and intentional learning groups based on the and Gy with implicit AG learning (r = .10) and
awareness questionnaire rather than differences in their magnitude was smaller.
instructional orientation. The results showed that Although the findings described above follow
those who developed awareness of the sequence the direction predicted by Reber’s (1993; Reber
showed positive correlations with intelligence but & Allen, 2000) evolutionary theory of implicit
those who were unaware of the regularities did learning, the two findings seemingly contradict
not show significant correlations. this. Danner et al. (2011) found positive correla-

66

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

tions between implicit learning and intelligence at task. As a consequence, they were distracted by
the level of latent variables. They employed two complexities in regularities displayed by the AG
versions of AG learning as well as the Berlin Intel- learning materials. Therefore, the findings of the
ligence Structure test and found significant positive null effects of intelligence on implicit learning
correlations between the latent variables “implicit are robust even though different psychometric
learning” and “intelligence”. In contrast, Robinson intelligence batteries are employed (see Table 1)
(2005b) found significant negative correlations and thus provide evidence for both Reber’s and
between intelligence and implicit AG learning in a Kaufman’s theories.
replication study of Reber et al. (1991). Japanese For future research, we could expand this area
learners of English engaged in similar implicit AG by including different types of intelligence such as
learning and series solution tasks and their intel- Sternberg’s successful intelligence and Gardner’s
ligence was tested on the Japanese version of the multiple intelligences, or emotional intelligence, to
WAIS-R, short form. Surprisingly, correlations name but a few (see Sternberg & Kaufman, 2011).
between explicit series solution and intelligence Does implicit learning still hold less variability as
were not significant. Table 1 provides a summary Reber claimed, or are there different intelligences
of the findings described above. responsible for implicit and explicit learning as
Kaufman (2011) implied?
Solutions and Future Directions
Working Memory and
Overall trends in the literature follow the direc- Implicit Learning
tions predicted by Reber’s (Reber, 1993; Reber
& Allen, 2000) evolutionary theory of implicit WM is assumed to be required to process new
learning. Kaufman (2011) stated that intelligence information while accessing and maintaining
tests were originally developed to predict benefits old information in long-term memory (LTM),
of “explicit” instruction in school. So it is natural and therefore differs from short-term memory
that there are significant and larger correlations (STM) which only transiently maintains informa-
between explicit learning and intelligence than tion. Complex, or higher-order cognition such as
those between implicit learning and intelligence. problem solving, language comprehension, and
However, if psychometric intelligence measures decision-making demands such concurrent pro-
explicit learning abilities, then how can we ex- cessing and storage. Although both intelligence
plain the discrepant findings, particularly those and WM are required in higher-order cognition
of Danner et al. (2011) and Robinson (2005b)? and thus show positive correlations, neither is an
First, in Daneman’s case, the authors argued that isomorphic construct (Conway, Getz, Macnamara,
their findings coincided with those of Reber et & Engle de Abreu, 2011; Conway, Macnamara, &
al. (1991) and Gebauer and Mackintosh (1997) Engle de Abreu, 2013; Nakamura, 2010).
in the sense that intelligence could explain only The most influential model of WM is Baddeley
10.24% of the variance in the implicit learning and Hitch’s multi-componential model (1974;
tasks and thus a majority of performance on Baddeley, 1986, 2000a, b; Baddeley & Logie,
implicit learning remains unexplained. Second, 1999; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993; Logie, 2003).
Robinson (2005b) argued that learners in his study The multi-componential model of WM originally
were more analytic in implicit AG learning than consisted of a domain-general “central executive”,
those in Reber et al. (1991) and thus those who and two domain-specific slave systems called “a
were higher on psychometric intelligence tried phonological loop” and “a visuo-spatial sketch
to analyse materials in the implicit AG learning pad”, the latter two of which deal with process-

67

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

Table 1. A summary of intelligence studies

Study Learning Tasks Intelligence Direct Comparison Significant


of SDs Correlations
Brooks & Kempe Auditory sequence CFI nr. ns.
(2012) learning
Danner et al. (2011) AG learning Berlin Intelligence nr. IQ with implicit learning
Structure test (r = .31 at the latent
variable level)
Feldman et al. (1995) SRT (procedural) WAIS-R full scale nr. IQ with declarative (r
Generation (but measured at = .284)
(declarative) different time course)
Gebauer & Mackintosh (1) AG learning 18 German intelligence nr. Gf with implicit
(2007) (2) SRT tests problem-solving task (r
(3) Problem solving = .10)
(4) Explicit AG learning Gy with implicit AG
(5) Explicit SRT learning (r = .10)
(6) Explicit problem- Gf with explicit AG
solving tasks learning (r = .25)
Gf with explicit
problem-solving task (r
= .27)
Gf with explicit SRT
task (r = .20)
Gc with explicit
problem-solving task (r
= .21)
Gy with explicit AG
learning (r = .27)
Kaufman et al. (2010) (1) SRT DAT verbal reasoning (r
(2) Explicit associative = .22)
learning
McGeorge et al. (1997) (1) AG learning WAIS-R full scale nr. IQ with explicit learning
(implicit) (r = .67)
(2) Series solution Verbal subtests with
(explicit) explicit learning
(r = .53)
Performance subtests
with explicit learning
(r = .65)
Performance subtests
with implicit learning
(r = .19)
Reber et al. (1991) (1) AG learning WAIS-R short form Explicit (SDs=.15.41) IQ with explicit learning
(implicit) > Implicit (7.2) (r = .69)
(2) Series solution
(explicit)
Robinson (2005b) (1) AG learning WAIS-R short form Explicit > Implicit IQ with implicit learning
(implicit) Japanese version (r = -.34)
(2) Series solution
(explicit)
Unsworth & Engle SRT (explicit & implicit) Raven’s Progressive nr. Intelligence with
(2005) Matrix awareness of sequences
(r = .48)
Note: nr. (not reported) included studies that employed ANOVAs but did not run correlational analyses for the correlation cell or t-tests
for the direct comparison cell.

68

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

ing and storage of verbal, or more precisely, activation states of knowledge held in LTM: (1)
phonological information and of visuo-spatial inactivated knowledge, (2) activated parts of
information, respectively. The central executive knowledge, which is a subset of (1). Although
deals with various attention control processing activated, it is still out of conscious awareness and
such as coordination of dual tasks, switching therefore some form of unconscious activation
retrieval plans, inhibition of irrelevant stimuli, such as semantic priming is possible in this state,
and maintenance and updating of stimuli, and is and (3) knowledge focused on focal attention that
similar to Norman and Shallice’s (1986) Super- the central executive controls, which is a subset of
visory Attentional Control in nature (Baddeley, (2). Activated parts of LTM are time-constrained
1996). The central executive itself does not have in the sense that their activation is temporary and
a storage subcomponent. The phonological loop is they begin to decay without rehearsal. In contrast,
related to language learning, especially vocabulary focused parts are capacity-constrained since only
acquisition (Gathercole & Baddely, 1993) and is an average of four chunks can be brought into
further divided into “a phonological store” sub- focus regulated by the central executive (Cowan,
component and “an inner speech” subcomponent, 2001). The central executive functions as an at-
which “offer, respectively, temporary memory for tention director in this model.
the acoustic and phonological properties of words,
letters, and numbers together with serial ordered, Research Findings
subvocal (mental) rehearsal of those items” (Logie,
2003, p. 41). The visuo-spatial sketch pad also If WM is concerned with active maintenance of a
comprises two subcomponents, “a visual cache” learning episode in relevant storage systems (e.g.,
and “an inner scribe”, which “support respectively the episodic buffer) while this episode is further
temporary memory for the visual appearance and processed by the central executive, which results
layout of a scene together with pathways or move- in conscious awareness in Baddeley’s model, or
ments through the scene” (Logie, ibid.). Baddeley activated parts of LTM in Cowan’s model, then
(2000a) further added a fourth component, “the WM should have an effect on explicit learning
episodic buffer”, which is a temporary storage but not necessarily on implicit learning. Based
system dealing with the integration of multimodal on Cowan’s model, Frensch and Miner (1994)
stimuli from other components such as episodic assumed that central executive control was re-
LTM (Tulving, 1983) as well as the phonological sponsible for explicit learning, which resulted
loop and the visuo-spatial sketch pad, and holds in elaborative memory coding in LTM (see also
resultant transient episodes. The central executive Unsworth & Engle, 2005, for similar arguments).
accesses and modifies episodic contents of the However, results obtained in the literature are
buffer by conscious awareness, allocating attention mixed (Janacsek & Nemeth, 2013).
to sources of information (phonological or visual Feldman et al. (1995) found such null effects
stimuli, or information stored in LTM). of WM on the implicit SRT task. In their study,
Alternately, other models shed light on inti- backward digit span, one of the typical measures
mate relationships between WM and LTM and of WM, showed positive correlations only with
assume that WM is the currently activated part declarative learning (generation of sequences)
of LTM. These models do not assume multiple and not with differences between RTs on the last
components for different modalities. The most block of repeated sequence trials and those on
influential model in this “modality-free” per- the random sequence trials (that is, learning),
spective is Cowan’s (1999) embedded-processes though RTs on the first repeated sequence block
model of WM. Cowan (1999) distinguished three showed positive correlations with backward digit

69

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

span. Likewise, Unsworth and Engle (2005) found order. The results showed that WM measured by
that those who were high on the operation span the computation span task had effects on faster
where participants had to verify a simple equation RTs in both populations. Bo et al. (2011) con-
and remember each single word at the same time structed two speed tests for WM in addition to
(e.g., “Is [9/3] -1 = 17, DOG”, Turner & Engle, typical backward digit span and reading span. In
1989) showed a faster learning rate and a greater the Visuo-spatial WM task, participants were in-
amount of learning than those who were low in structed to judge whether the test array containing
the intentional learning condition. However, this several coloured circles was the same as an array
was not the case in the incidental learning condi- presented just 1,000ms before or not. Alphabets
tion. Similar null effects of WM were found on were used instead of the coloured circles in the
the Auditory SRT task in Brooks and Kempe’s Verbal WM test. Although RTs correlated with
(2012) study and the SRT task in Kaufman et neither Visuo-spatial nor Verbal WM, regression
al.’s (2010) study. analyses showed that Visuo-spatial WM but not
However, opposite results were found by Verbal WM explained most of the variance (R2 =
Frensch and Miner (1994). In their first experi- .43) in the SRT task, suggesting that relevant WM
ment, participants were either provided with in- components for the SRT task were visuo-spatial
tentional or incidental instructions on regularities but not verbal in nature. Table 2 provides a sum-
underlying sequences in the SRT task. Response- mary of the studies reviewed above.
Stimulus Interval (RSI) was set either at 500ms
or 1500ms, the former of which maximised Solutions and Future Directions
the possibility of implicit learning. The results
showed that WM measured by the location span How can we explain these discrepant results de-
task showed significant negative correlations only scribed above? As Frensch and Miner (1994) and
with the most demanding condition (intentional Unsworth and Engle (2005) hypothesised, there is
condition with 500ms RSI), indicating that those some evidence for the positive effects of WM on
who were high in WM showed faster RTs than “explicit” learning (Bo, Borza, &Seidler, 2009;
those who were low in implicit SRT learning. In Bo &Seidler, 2009). Methodological insufficiency
their second experiment, a tone-counting dual might explain the mixed results.
task was introduced instead of differences in First, it was pointed out that span measures
instructional orientation and WM measured by such as the operation span task showed meth-
the digit and location spans correlated only with odological deficits when they were used alone.
the most demanding condition (dual task with For instance, Conway et al. (2005) showed that
500ms RSI) again. Schwartz, Howard, Howard, there were possibilities of misclassifications of
Hovaguimian, and Deutsch (2003) employed a participants. They reported that when participants
modified version of the SRT task in the normal were classified into high vs. low span groups,
and schizophrenic populations. WM was tested by the reading span classified four people into the
computation span where participants were asked low span group while the operation span classi-
to solve a simple arithmetic computation while fied the same people into the high span group. A
memorising the second number digit, and by the majority of the studies showed the null effects of
Letter-Number sequencing subtest of the WAIS- WM on implicit learning (Feldman et al., 1995;
3 where they were asked to remember a list of Kaufman et al., 2010; Unsworth& Engle, 2005)
letters and numbers and then recall the numbers and did indeed employ only a single span measure
in ascending order and the letters in descending (see Table 2). In relation to this, Bo et al. (2011)

70

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

Table 2. A summary of WM studies

Studies Learning Tasks WM Significant Correlations


Brooks & Kempe (2012) Auditory sequence learning Reading span ns.
Bo et al. (2011) SRT Visuo-spatial WM task ns.
Verbal WM task
Backward digit span
Reading span
Feldman et al. (1995) (1) SRT (procedural) Backward digit span WM with declarative learning
Experiment 1 (2) Generation (rs = .207-.236)
(declarative)
Frensch & Miner (1994) SRT (explicit & implicit) Digit span Location span with implicit
Experiment 1 Location span SRT with 500ms RSI (r =
-.057)
Frensch & Miner (1994) SRT (single vs. dual task) Digit span Both spans with SRT under
Experiment 2 Location span the dual-task condition with
500ms RSI
(r = -.61 for location span and
r = -.49 for digit span)
Kaufman et al. (2010) (1) SRT Operation span ns.
(2) Explicit associative learning
Schwartz et al. (2003) SRT Letter-Number sequencing (WAIS- Computation span with RTs in
3) Session 1 (r = -.42)
Computation span
Unsworth & Engle (2005) SRT Operation span nr.
(explicit & implicit)
Note: nr. (not reported) included studies that employed ANOVAs but did not run correlational analyses.

claimed that in order to prevent chunking and Finally, WM, or more specifically central
rehearsing strategies of sequences in WM, we executive, has multiple functions. Miyake and
need some time-constrained measurement of WM. colleagues (see Miyake & Friedman, 2012, for a
Second, Janacsek and Nemeth (2013) pointed latest review), in a series of factor-analytic studies,
out that the majority of the studies on the relations showed this. For example, Miyake et al. (2000)
between implicit SRT learning and WM employed first extracted three latent factors of executive
only an online learning session. If some longer functions: (1) shifting between tasks or mental
time intervals were inserted (24-hour delay), they sets (shifting), (2) updating and monitoring of
argued, then we might be able to see the relations working memory representation (updating), and
between “consolidation” of sequence knowledge (3) inhibition of dominant or prepotent response
and WM. (inhibition). Subsequently, they investigated which
Third, Janacsek and Nemeth (2013) again sug- of these latent variables predicted or explained
gested that WM is concerned more with general performance in additional executive functioning
skill development than skills specific to learned tasks such as a Wisconsin Card Sorting task. The
sequences. They cited Bo et al.’s findings (2011) results showed that different variables or combi-
where only RTs on the repeated sequence in the nations of them predicted outcomes of these ad-
first block, but not learning calculated by the dif- ditional executive functioning. For instance, the
ferences in RTs on the repeated and those on the Wisconsin Card Sorting task was related only to
random sequence, showed significant correlations shifting, while only updating played a significant
with WM. role in performance of the operation span task.

71

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

For these methodological reasons, the relation Xie et al. (2013) investigated the relations
between WM and implicit learning is less clear, between thinking styles and implicit learning,
let alone the smaller number of empirical stud- specifically based on Sternberg’s (1997) theory of
ies. In particular, future studies should specify thinking styles. Of particular relevance to this study
which functions of WM they aim to investigate were Type 1 thinking style (legislative, judicial,
and employ multiple measures of WM. hierarchical, global, and liberal styles of thinking)
and Type 2 thinking style (executive, monarchic,
Other Cognitive, Conative, local, and conservative styles of thinking). Implicit
and Affective Variables and explicit AG learning were distinguished in
and Implicit Learning terms of differences in instructional orientation.
Xie et al. (2013) found that the legislative com-
Although there have been studies on the effects ponent of Type 1 thinking style showed positive
of affection on implicit learning with clinical correlations with explicit AG learning while the
populations (e.g., Naismith, Hickie, Ward, Scott, conservative component of Type 2 thinking nega-
& Little, 2005; Naismith et al., 2010), the overall tively correlated with explicit AG learning. On the
number of studies investigating the effects on IDs other hand, implicit learning had no correlation
in conative-affective variables is quite small and with either thinking styles. Since the legislative
the area of research in this direction is “clearly far style refers to “preference for creating rules and
from complete”, as Reber and Allen (2000, p. 240) autonomy” (cited in Xie et al., 2013, p. 270, Ap-
concluded. We begin the review by considering pendix), the positive correlations with explicit
other cognitive variables such as processing speed, learning seem legitimate. In contrast, negative
thinking, and cognitive styles and then conative- correlations between the conservative component
affective variables such as affection, personality, and explicit learning are counter-intuitive since
and motivation. Since the literature in this direction conservation refers to “Liking for adhering to
is just beginning to proliferate, I provide reviews existing rules and minimizing changes” (ibid.).
in detail to promote future research. Perhaps, participants in the explicit learning
condition formed an initial (wrong) hypothesis
Research Findings on the underlying rules and did not change the
hypothesis according to subsequent stimuli. Note
The effects of processing speed on implicit SRT that participants in this condition were given only
task performance were investigated by Kaufman rule-search instruction not actual instruction on
et al. (2010). Kaufman et al. (2010) hypothesised, the rules themselves.
following Anderson (1992) and Jensen (1998), that Seemingly similar concepts to thinking styles,
since processing speed might “partially reflect the cognitive styles, developed by Pacini and Epstein
overall efficiency and speed of the nervous sys- (1999), were first investigated by Pretz et al.
tem” (p. 323) it could contribute to IDs in implicit (2010) in relation to implicit AG learning and
learning. In this study, three types of processing the SRT task. In Pacini and Epstein’s cognitive
speed, verbal, numerical, and figural processing style (1999), two constructs were distinguished:
speed, were considered with other cognitive- rational ability and favourability vs. experiential
affective variables in a latent variable analysis. ability and favourability. Pretz et al. (2010) also
Three speed measures constituted a latent variable included another dimension: negative-positive
Gs (speed) and Gs in turn positively correlated mood (affection). Following previous studies,
with the scores on SRT, confirming Kaufman et the authors assumed that positive mood would
al.’s (2010) hypothesis. promote intuitive processing, which would lead to

72

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

greater implicit learning by those who preferred an exposure effect (implicit emotional preference
experiential cognitive style. Those who preferred for grammatical stimuli in implicit learning; see
a rational cognitive style, in contrast, would show Nakamura, 2013a, for a review of this effect) on
greater implicit learning if they were in a nega- implicit AG learning. Participants’ emotional
tive mood since a negative mood would promote states were primed by emotional faces (positive,
analytic processing. In the experiment, mood was neutral, and negative) during the preferential rating
operationalised in such a way that participants saw and grammaticality judgment tests and measured
pleasurable photos in the positive mood condition by a questionnaire at the end of the experiment.
while participants in the negative mood condition The results showed that mindfulness negatively
saw non-pleasurable photos such as war scenes. correlated with both the preferential rating (those
The results showed that rational cognitive style who were high on mindfulness preferred less
positively correlated with both implicit learning grammatical stimuli than those who were low) and
tasks while experiential cognitive style had no grammaticality judgment (mindful people were
correlations with either implicit learning tasks. less correct than those who were less mindful).
Moreover, negative mood led to greater implicit Therefore, mindful states had negative effects on
AG learning. Since cognitive style was a sig- implicit learning performance.
nificant covariate in ANCOVAs where mood had Woolhouse and Bayne (2000) considered Intui-
marginal significant effects, the results indicated tive vs. Sensing personality distinction measured
that negative mood promoted analytic processing, by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) in
which resulted in greater learning. This contradic- the context of an implicit suitability-rating task.
tory finding was due, Pretz et al. (2010) argued, In this task, participants judged the suitability of
to the fact that the implicit processing involved each job applicant based on his/her personality
in implicit learning tasks was holistic in nature, profile during training. Then they were given
which was different from heuristic, inferential the results of the suitability rating by the expert.
implicit processing. At a certain point in the test, the pattern of the
As in Pretz et al. (2010), the effects of affection suitability rating of test personality profiles was
were investigated in Shang et al. (2013). Affection changed. If participants learned regularities in the
was induced by music in this study and subse- suitability rating from the expert during training,
quently confirmed by the rating scale (called the then this sudden change would cause a decrease
self-assessment Manikin). Music was also played in performance as in the SRT task. Addition-
at some points in the SRT learning with coloured ally, their strategy use (e.g., use of intuition, use
shapes so that target moods were maintained. Un- of explicit knowledge, etc.) was measured by a
like Pretzet al. (2010), negative mood deteriorated multiple-choice question. The results showed that
the magnitude of implicit learning measured by those who were classified as intuitive used more
the SRT task. Shang et al. (2013) interpreted this intuition in their learning strategies and showed
discrepancy as being partly due toparticipants a greater amount of implicit learning. In contrast,
in a positive mood payingattention to irrelevant those who had a sensing personality argued in
stimulus colour, which had some predictive value favour ofexplicit knowledge during learning and
for sequence regularities in the experiment as thus an intuitive personality had positive effects
positive mood broadened the scope of attention on implicit learning. Moreover, Kaufman et al.
(Fredrickson &Branigan, 2005). (2010) provided findings on three different con-
Whitmarsh et al. (2013) investigated the effects structs of personality, Intellect, Openness, and
of mindfulness, namely resistance to automatic, Intuition, as well. Intellect reflects “a combina-
habitual responses in the context of the mere tion of perceived cognitive ability and tendency

73

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

toward intellectual engagement” (p. 324) while based on characteristics of a character (e.g., force-
Openness reflects “artistic and completive quali- ful action for power motivation, and being close to
ties and engagement with sensory and perceptual others for affiliation motivation) and was induced
information” (ibid.). Based on the findings in Mc- by emotional face priming (surprise, joy, and
Crae (1994) where Openness and Intuition were anger). In the differential implicit learning task,
closely related, Kaufman et al. hypothesised that participants were presented acoloured sequence
these two constructs would correlate with implicit either with an emotional face, a neutral face, or
learning. On the other hand, the authors, based on no face under a dual-task condition (simultaneous
DeYoung and colleagues (DeYoung, Peterson, & tone counting). Although neither motivation had
Higgins, 2005), assumed that intelligence would correlations with any of the emotional sequences
show positive correlations with explicit associative in terms of both RTs and error rates, the results of
learning, intelligence, and WM. As in Woolhouse regression analyses showed that power motivation
and Bayne (2000), Intuition showed positive cor- deteriorated SRT learning of sequences with joy
relations with implicit learning at the latent level. faces but promoted learning of sequences with
Moreover, there were dissociations in such a way surprise faces of the same gender but impeded se-
that Intellect positively correlated with explicit quences with surprise faces of the opposite gender
associative learning such as paired associates on both RTs and error rates. The authors argued
learning as well as psychometric intelligence, that joy expressed others’ low dominance status,
WM and processing speed while Openness did which in turn functioned as a positive incentive for
so with implicit learning. those who had implicit power motivation. On the
The effects of motivation were measured by other hand, joy could indicate a high dominance
Eitam, Hassin, and Schul (2008). Participants’ and thus be aversive for those who had power
explicit motivation for implicit problem solving motivation. Table 3 provides a summary of the
and SRT tasks was measured by a questionnaire studies reviewed above.
while implicit motivation in the sense of implicit
goal pursuit was measured by word search. In the Solutions and Future
word search task, participants in the goal pursuit Research Directions
condition searched for achievement words such as
win while those in the control condition searched There are no competing results in the effects of
for neutral words such as carpet. The results IDs in conative-affective dimensions on implicit
showed that those who were in the goal pursuit learning and thus the paper provides some issues
condition outperformed those who were in the for future research in these areas. Focus is on the
control condition on both implicit learning tasks. interactive effects of cognitive-conative-affective
On the other hand, the implicit goal pursuit had abilities, partially inspired by Snow (1994) and
no effects on explicit motivation. While this study Robinson (2007).
suggested that explicit and implicit motivation may Some of the studies reviewed in this section
have no relation, the effects of explicit motivation include several ability measures. However, many
on implicit learning were not investigated. Future of these studies investigated independent contri-
studies should analyse this. butions of each variable to IDs in implicit (and
Finally, Schultheiss, Pang, Torges, Wirth, and sometimes explicit) learning. Although these
Treynor (2005) investigated the differential effects provide useful information on whether implicit
of implicit power and affiliation motivation on dif- and explicit learning are dissociable in terms of
ferential implicit learning. Participants’ motivation IDs and thus confirm/disconfirm two prominent
was measured by imaginary story writing, scored theories of IDs in implicit learning (Kaufman’s

74

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

Table 3. A summary of emotion and personality studies

Studies Learning Tasks ID Variables ID Tasks Results


Eitam et al. (2008) (1) Problem solving Motivation Word search Implicit goal pursuit promoted
(2) SRT (implicit goal both implicit learning tasks
pursuit)
Kaufman et al. (1) SRT Personality (1)Myers-Briggs Type (1) Intellect positively
(2010) (2) Explicit associative (Intuition, Indicator (MBTI) correlated
learning Openness, (2)NEO-PI-R with explicit learning.
Intellect) (3)Rational-Experiential (2) Openness and Intellect
Inventory (REI) positively
(4)UPPS Impulsivity Scale correlated with implicit
learning.
Processing Speed (1) English version of Processing speed had positive
(Verbal, Numerical, the Berlin Intelligence correlations with implicit
and Figural speed) Structure Test learning.
(2) Speed of Information
Processing subtest
(British Ability Scales)
(3) Digit-Symbol Coding
(WAIS-R)
Pretz et al. (2010) AG learning & SRT Cognitive style REI Rational style promoted
(rational vs. greater AG learning.
experiential)
Affection (1) Picture Negative mood promoted
(positive vs. neutral (2) Positive Affect Negative greater AG learning.
vs. negative mood) Affect Scale
(PANAS)
Schultheiss et al. SRT with emotional Motivation Picture story exercise Power affiliation motivation
(2005) faces (Power vs. promoted
Affiliation motives) implicit learning of sequences
with surprise faces
but impeded sequences with
joy faces.
Shang et al. (2013) SRT Affection (1) Music Negative affect impaired
(positive vs. (2)Self-Assessment Manikin learning
negative) (SAM)
Whitmarsh et al. AG learning Affection (1) Five Factor Mindfulness Mindfulness deteriorated
(2013) (grammaticality (mindfulness) Questionnaire performance
judgment (2) Priming by emotional on both grammaticality
and the mere exposure faces judgment
effect) and preferential rating.
Woolhouse & Bayne Judgment of job Personality MBTI form G Intuitive personality led to
(2000) applicants’ (sensing vs. more implicit learning.
suitability from their intuition)
personality
Xie et al. (2013) AG learning (implicit Thinking style Thinking Styles Inventory- Type 1 thinking style
& explicit) (type 1 vs. type 2) Revised 2 promoted
explicit AG learning.
Type 2 thinking style
deteriorated
explicit AG learning.

75

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

dual-process theory and Reber’s evolutionary with those of negative affect on a different measure
theory), only a few studies investigated interactive of cognitive control: delayed word recognition.
effects of target variables (mood and cognitive Participants’ positive and negative affects were in-
styles, Pretz et al., 2010). However, Snow (1994) duced by pleasurable (rewards) or aversive liquids
and Robinson (2007) argued that different learn- (punishments), and IDs in personality and moti-
ing conditions (explicit and implicit learning in vational orientations were measured as well. The
the present context) required different clusters of results showed that both rewards and punishments
abilities, indicating that interactional analyses of enhanced task performance. Furthermore, IDs in
several target variables are necessary. Indeed, there motivational orientations, especially punishment
has been vast research on the interactive effects sensitivity, explained much of the performance
of variables, especially the effects of affection on during the punishment block of word recognition
cognition and vice versa (Blanchette& Richards, where participants received a punishment when
2010; De Houwer & Hermans, 2010; Derakshan their performance was incorrect. This means that
& Eysenck, 2010; Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2010; those who had a stronger punishment avoidance
Yiend, 2010, for reviews). Only two of the studies orientation had fewer errors and faster responses
are reviewed here due to limited space. when they received punishments.
Yang, Yang, and Isen (2013) investigated What do these studies reviewed here indicate?
whether positive affect improved both controlled First, if we concentrate only on the effects of one
processing in WM and short-term storage in STM. or more variables but do not analyse their interac-
Positive affect was induced by giving unexpected tive effects, then the apparent effects of particular
gifts (candies) and those who had positive affect variables on explicit and implicit learning might
outperformed those who had neutral affect in the be caused by other moderator variables. As a
operation span task. However, such facilitating concrete example, apparent positive effects of
effects were rather weak on the word span task WM on explicit and implicit learning as in some
(STM), suggesting that positive affect better facili- studies (e.g., Bo et al., 2009; Frensch & Miner,
tated in controlled processing (executive control) 1994) might be caused by participants’ positive
than in storage systems, as the authors concluded affect. Second, if different interactive effects of
(see also Carpenter, Peters, Vastfjall, & Isen, cognitive-conative-affective variables are avail-
2013). In relation to this, Savine, Beck, Edwards, able for explicit and implicit learning, then this
Chiew, and Braver (2010) investigated the effects could provide further evidence for ability dissocia-
of emotion on cognitive control. In experiment 1, tions of explicit/implicit learning as predicted by
efficiency in cognitive control was measured by Kaufman’s dual-process theory of intelligence.
task switching where participants alternated their
responses on a gender discrimination of faces by
responding to syllable judgment according to a cue CONCLUSION
presented on a screen. Participants’ emotion was
induced by the existence of monetary incentives This paper reviewed ID research in the context of
for correct and faster performance and informed by implicit learning in relation to two theories of IDs
a cue during the experiment. Savine et al. (2010) in implicit learning. Although the overall number
found that the monetary incentives reduced switch of studies has been and is quite small, future studies
costs, suggesting that positive affect induced by should shed more light on the important relation
monetary motivation enhanced the functioning of between IDs in cognitive abilities and implicit
cognitive control. In experiment 2, Savine et al. learning. In the case of intelligence, the results
(2010) directly compared the effects of positive seem clear: IDs in psychometric intelligence

76

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

have no relation or weak relations with implicit Anderson, M. (1992). Intelligence and develop-
learning, unlike with explicit learning. This is ment: A cognitive theory. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
natural in the sense that psychometric intelligence
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford,
originally aimed to measure benefits from explicit
UK: Oxford University Press.
instructions in school (Kaufman, 2011). There-
fore, the findings are consistent with both Reber’s Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central exec-
evolutionary theory and Kaufman’s dual-process utive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy-
theory. On the other hand, the relations between chology, 49A(1), 5–28. doi:10.1080/713755608
WM and implicit learning are rather mixed and
Baddeley, A. D. (2000a). The episodic buffer: A
this comes from methodological insufficiency
new component of working memory? Trends in
such as the use of the single span task for WM
Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423. doi:10.1016/
measures. Future research should pay close atten-
S1364-6613(00)01538-2 PMID:11058819
tion to methodological rigour.
We also examined the beginning of research Baddeley, A. D. (2000b). Short-term and working
on the other conative-affective dimensions such memory. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.),
as affection, motivation, and personality. While The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 77–92).
the research is just beginning to proliferate in the Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
literature, partially reflecting deeper interest in the
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working
dual-process theory of human cognition (e.g., Ev-
memoy. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of
ans & Frankish, 2009; Kaufman, 2011; Stanovich
learning and motivation (pp. 47–89). Hove, UK:
& Toplak, 2012), more empirical studies must be
Academic Press.
conducted in order to confirm or disconfirm the
two theories of IDs in implicit learning. Also of Baddely, A. D., & Logie, R. H. (1999). The mul-
importance are analyses of interactive effects of tiple-component model. In A. Miyake & P. Shah
several ability variables so that we can eliminate (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms
apparent null or significant effects of some vari- of active maintenance and executive control (pp.
ables and discover hidden moderator variables. 28–61). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
The issue of differential contributions of IDs in Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139174909.005
cognitive-conative-affective abilities for implicit
Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2010). The influence
and explicit learning and thus of ability differen-
of affect on higher level cognition: A review of re-
tiation has not settled down and considerations
search on interpretation, judgment, decision mak-
of the issues for further research pointed out in
ing and reasoning. Cognition and Emotion, 24(4),
this paper would provide a much clearer picture
561–595. doi:10.1080/02699930903132496
of the nature of IDs in implicit learning.
Bley-Vroman, R. (1988). The fundamental charac-
ter of foreign language learning. In W. Rutherford
REFERENCES & M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.), Grammar and
second language teaching: A book of reading (pp.
Abrams, M., & Reber, A. S. (1988). Implicit 19–30). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
learning: Robustness in the face of psychiatric
disorders. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research,
17, 425–439. PMID:3193378

77

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

Bo, J., Borza, V., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Christie, M. A., & Hersch, S. M. (2004). Dem-
Age-related declines in visuospatial working onstration of nondeclarative sequence learning
memory correlate with deficits in explicit motor in mice: Development of an animal analog of the
sequence learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, human reaction time task. Learning & Memory
102(5), 2744–2754. doi:10.1152/jn.00393.2009 (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), 11(6), 720–723.
PMID:19726728 doi:10.1101/lm.85404 PMID:15537741
Bo, J., Jennett, S., & Seidler, R. D. (2011). Working Conway, A. R. A., Getz, S. J., Macnamara, B.,
memory capacity correlates with implicit serial & Engel de Abreu, P. M. J. (2011). Working
reaction time task performance. Experimental memory and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg &
Brain Research, 214(1), 73–81. doi:10.1007/ S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge hand-
s00221-011-2807-8 PMID:21809082 book of intelligence (pp. 394–418). Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
Bo, J., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Visuospatial work-
CBO9780511977244.021
ing memory capacity predicts the organization of
acquired explicit motor sequences. Journal of Neu- Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F.,
rophysiology, 101(6), 3116–3125. doi:10.1152/ Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W.
jn.00006.2009 PMID:19357338 (2005). Working memory span tasks: A method-
ological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic
Brooks, P. J., & Kempe, V. (2012). Individual
Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 769–786. doi:10.3758/
differences in adult foreign language learning:
BF03196772 PMID:16523997
The mediating effect of metalinguistic aware-
ness. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 281–296. Conway, A. R. A., Macnamara, B., & Engel de
doi:10.3758/s13421-012-0262-9 PMID:23055121 Abreu, P. M. J. (2013). Working memory and
intelligence. An overview. In T. P. Alloway &
Carpenter, S. M., Peters, E., Vastfjall, D., & Isen,
R. G. Alloway (Eds.), Working memory: The
A. M. (2013). Positive feelings facilitate working
connected intelligence (pp. 13–35). London:
memory and complex decision making among
Psychology Press.
older adults. Cognition and Emotion, 27(1),
184–192. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.698251 Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-process model of
PMID:22764739 working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.),
Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. (1959). Modern Lan-
maintenance and executive control (pp. 62–101).
guage Aptitude Test Form A. New York: The
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Psychological Cooperation.
doi:10.1017/CBO9781139174909.006
Christie, M. A., & Darlymple-Alford, J. C. (2004).
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in
A new rat model of the human serial reaction
short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental
time task: Contrasting effects of caudate and
storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
hippocampal lesions. The Journal of Neurosci-
24(1), 87–185. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003922
ence, 24(5), 1034–1039. doi:10.1523/JNEURO-
PMID:11515286
SCI.3340-03.2004 PMID:14762121

78

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

Danner, D., Hagemann, D., Schankin, A., Hager, Evans, J.B., & Frankish, K. (Eds.). (2009). In two
M., & Funke, J. (2011). Beyond IQ: A latent state- minds: Dual processes and beyond. Oxford, UK:
trait analysis of general intelligence, dynamic deci- Oxford University Press.
sion making, and implicit learning. Intelligence,
Feldman, J., Kerr, B., & Streissguth, A. P. (1995).
39(5), 323–334. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2011.06.004
Correlational analyses of procedural and declara-
De Houwer, J., & Hermans, D. (Eds.). (2010). Cog- tive learning performance. Intelligence, 20(1),
nition and emotion: Reviews of current research 87–114. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(95)90007-1
and theories. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Flaganan, D. P., & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.). (2005).
Deary, I. J. (2001a). Human intelligence dif- Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories,
ferences: A recent history. Trends in Cognitive tests, and issues. New York: Guilford Press.
Sciences, 5(3), 127–130. doi:10.1016/S1364-
Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive
6613(00)01621-1 PMID:11239813
emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-
Deary, I. J. (2001b). Human intelligence differenc- action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19(3),
es: Towards a combined experimental-differential 313–332. doi:10.1080/02699930441000238
approach. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(4), PMID:21852891
164–170. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01623-5
Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects
PMID:11287270
of presentation rate and individual differences
DeKeyser, R. M. (2000). The robustness of criti- in short-term memory capacity on an indirect
cal period effects in second language acquisition. measure of serial learning. Memory & Cogni-
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, tion, 22(1), 95–110. doi:10.3758/BF03202765
499–533. PMID:8035689
Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2010). Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The moti-
Introduction to the special issue: Emotion- vational dimensional model of affect: Implications
al states, attention, and working memory. for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive
Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 189–199. categorization. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2),
doi:10.1080/02699930903412120 322–337. doi:10.1080/02699930903378305
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993).
(2005). Sources of openness/intellect: Cognitive Working memory and language. Hove, UK: Psy-
and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth fac- chology Press.
tor of personality. Journal of Personality, 73(4),
Gebauer, G. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2007).
825–858. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00330.x
Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit
PMID:15958136
and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental
Eitam, B., Hassin, R. R., & Schul, Y. (2008). Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
Nonconscious goal pursuit in novel environments: 33(1), 34–54. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.34
The case of implicit learning. Psychological PMID:17201553
Science, 19(3), 261–267. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2008.02078.x PMID:18315799

79

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

Howe, M. J. A. (1996). Concepts of ability. In I. Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R.,
Dennis & P. Tapsfield (Eds.), Human abilities: Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2009). Associa-
Their nature and measurement (pp. 39–48). Mah- tive learning predicts intelligence above and
wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. beyond working memory and processing speed.
Intelligence, 37(4), 374–382. doi:10.1016/j.in-
Jackson, D. O. (2013). Implicit second language
tell.2009.03.004
learning and individual differences. In J. M. Berg-
sleithner, S. N. Forota, & J. K. Yoshioka (Eds.), Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R.,
Noticing and second language acquisition: Stud- Jimenez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010).
ies in honor of Richard Schmidt (pp. 271–287). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116(3),
Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii, National 321–340. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.011
Foreign Language Resource Center. PMID:20573341
Janacsek, K., Fiser, J., & Nemeth, D. (2012). The Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Reis, D. L.,
best time to acquire new skills: Age-related dif- & Gray, J. R. (2011). General intelligence pre-
ferences in implicit sequence learning across the dicts reasoning ability even for evolutionarily
human lifespan. Developmental Science, 15(4), familiar content. Intelligence, 39(5), 311–322.
496–505. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01150.x doi:10.1016/j.intell.2011.05.002
PMID:22709399
Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1994). In-
Janacsek, K., & Nemeth, D. (2013). Implicit formation acquired during artificial grammar
sequence learning and working memory: learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Correlated or complicated? Cortex, 49(8), Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(1), 79–91.
2001–2006. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2013.02.012 doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.1.79 PMID:8138790
PMID:23541152
Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1996). Artificial
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of grammar learning depends on implicit learning of
mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers. both abstract and exemplar-specific information.
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learn-
Kaufman, J. C., Kaufman, A. S., Kaufman-Singer,
ing, Memory, and Cognition, 22(1), 169–181.
J., & Kaufman, N. L. (2005). The Kaufman As-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.169 PMID:8648284
sessment Battery for Children – (2nd ed.) and
the Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Logie, R. H. (2003). Spatial and visual working
Test. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), memory. In The psychology of learning and mo-
Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tivation (pp. 37–78). Hove, UK: Academic Press.
tests, and issues (pp. 344-370). London: The
Mackintosh, N. J. (2011). IQ and human intelli-
Guilford Press.
gence (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Kaufman, S. B. (2011). Intelligence and the Press.
cognitive unconscious. In R. J. Sternberg & S.
McCrae, R. R. (1994). Openness to experi-
B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook
ence: Expanding the boundaries of factor 5.
of intelligence (pp. 442–467). Cambridge, UK:
European Journal of Personality, 8(4), 251–271.
Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
doi:10.1002/per.2410080404
CBO9780511977244.023

80

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

McGeorge, P., Crawford, J. R., & Kelly, S. W. Naismith, S. L., Lagopoulos, J., Ward, P. B., Davey,
(1997). The relationships between psychometric C. G., Little, C., & Hickie, I. B. (2010). Fronto-
intelligence and learning in an explicit and an striatal correlates of impaired implicit sequence
implicit task. Journal of Experimental Psychol- learning in major depression: An fMRI study.
ogy. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(1), Journal of Affective Disorders, 125(1-3), 256–261.
239–245. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.1.239 doi:10.1016/j.jad.2010.02.114 PMID:20219248
PMID:9028029
Nakamura, D. (2010). Are intelligence and work-
Meulemans, T., Van der Linden, M., & Perruchet, ing memory isomorphic? Aoyama Gakuin. English
P. (1998). Implicit sequence learning in children. Studies, 34, 25–64.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Nakamura, D. (2013a). Methodological consid-
69(3), 199–221. doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2442
erations in studying awareness during learning.
PMID:9654439
Part 1: Implicit learning. Polish Psychological
Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H. (2007). Bulletin, 44, 102–117.
Extending statistical learning farther and further:
Nakamura, D. (2013b). Methodological consid-
Long-distance dependencies, and individual dif-
erations in studying awareness during learning.
ferences in statistical learning and language. In
Part 2: Second language acquisition. Polish Psy-
McNamaraD. S.TraftonJ. G. (Eds.), Proceedings
chological Bulletin, 44, 337–353.
of the 29th Annual Conference of the Cognitive
Science Society (pp. 1307-1312). Austin, TX: Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J. Jr, Boykin,
Cognitive Science Society. A., Brody, N., & Ceci, S. J. et al. (1996). Intel-
ligence: Knowns and unknowns. The American
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The na-
Psychologist, X(2), 77–101. doi:10.1037/0003-
ture and organization of individual differences in
066X.51.2.77
executive functions: Four general conclusions.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional
21(1), 8–14. doi:10.1177/0963721411429458 requirement of learning: Evidence from perfor-
PMID:22773897 mance measure. Cognitive Psychology, 19(1),
1–32. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Wit-
zki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to
The unity and diversity of executive functions and action: Willed and automatic control of behavior.
their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.),
A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in
41(1), 49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 research and theory. New York: Plenum.
PMID:10945922
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of
Naismith, S. L., Hickie, I. B., Ward, P. B., Scott, rational and experiential information processing
E., & Little, C. (2005). Impaired implicit sequence styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bi-
learning in depression: A probe for frontostriatal as phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social
dysfunction? Psychological Medicine, 36(03), Psychology, 76(6), 972–987. doi:10.1037/0022-
313–323. doi:10.1017/S0033291705006835 3514.76.6.972 PMID:10402681
PMID:16359605

81

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

Perruchet, P. (2008). Implicit learning. In H. L. Robinson, P. (2005a). Cognitive complexity


Roediger (Ed.), Learning and memory: Cognitive and task sequencing: A review of studies in a
psychology of memory (vol. 2, pp. 597-621). Componential Framework for second language
Oxford, UK: Elsevier. task design. International Review of Applied
Linguistics in Language Teaching, 43(1), 1–32.
Pothos, E. M. (2007). Theories of artificial gram-
doi:10.1515/iral.2005.43.1.1
mar learning. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2),
227–244. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.227 Robinson, P. (2005b). Cognitive abilities, chunk-
PMID:17338598 strength and frequency effects in implicit artificial
grammar and incidental L2 learning: Replications
Pretz, J. E., Totz, K. S., & Kaufman, S. B. (2010).
of Reber, Walkenfeld, and Hernstadt (1991) and
The effects of mood, cognitive style, and cognitive
Knowlton and Squire (1996) and their relevance
ability on implicit learning. Learning and Indi-
for SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
vidual Differences, 20(3), 215–219. doi:10.1016/j.
15, 413–438. doi:10.1017/S0272263100012365
lindif.2009.12.003
Robinson, P. (2007). Aptitudes, abilities, contexts,
Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial
and practice. In R. M. DeKeyser (Ed.), Practice in
grammar. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
a second language: Perspectives from applied lin-
Behavior, 6(6), 855–863. doi:10.1016/S0022-
guistics and cognitive psychology (pp. 256–286).
5371(67)80149-X
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit
Robinson, P. (2010). Implicit artificial grammar
knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
learning and incidental natural second language
General, 118(3), 219–235. doi:10.1037/0096-
acquisition: How comparable are they? Lan-
3445.118.3.219
guage Learning, 60(supplement 2), 245–263.
Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00608.x
knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious.
Roid, G. L., & Pomplun, M. (2005). Interpreting
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales. In D. P.
Reber, A. S., & Allen, R. (2000). Individual dif- Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary
ferences in implicit learning. In R. G. Kunzendorf intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues
& B. Wallace (Eds.), Individual differences in (5th ed., pp. 325–343). New York: The Guilford
conscious experience (pp. 227–247). Philadelphia: Press.
John Benjamins. doi:10.1075/aicr.20.11reb
Savine, A. C., Beck, S. M., Edwards, B. G., Chiew,
Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., & Hernstadt, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Enhancement of
R. (1991). Implicit learning: Individual differ- cognitive control by approach and avoidance mo-
ences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychol- tivational states. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2),
ogy. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(5), 338–356. doi:10.1080/02699930903381564
888–896. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.17.5.888 PMID:20390042
PMID:1834770

82

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

Schultheiss, O. C., Pang, J. S., Torges, C. M., Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. New
Wirth, M. M., & Treynor, W. (2005). Perceived York: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
facial expressions of emotion as motivational CBO9780511584152
incentives: Evidence from a differential implicit
Sternberg, R. J., & Kaufman, S. B. (Eds.). (2011).
learning paradigm. Emotion (Washington, D.C.),
The Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp.
5(1), 41–54. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.41
442–467). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
PMID:15755218
Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511977244
Schwartz, B. L., Howard, D. V., Howard, J.
Thomas, K. M., Hunt, R. H., Vizueta, N., Sommer,
H., Hovaguimian, A., & Deutsch, S. I. (2003).
T., Durston, S., Yang, Y., & Worden, M. S. (2004).
Implicit learning of visuo-spatial sequences
Evidence of developmental differences in implicit
in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 17(3),
sequence learning.An fMRI study of children and
517–533. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.517
adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8),
PMID:12959517
1339–1351. doi:10.1162/0898929042304688
Shang, J., Fu, Q., Dienes, Z., Shao, C., & Fu, PMID:15509382
X. (2013). Negative affect reduces performance
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory.
in implicit sequence learning. PLoS ONE, 8(1),
New York: Oxford University Press.
e54693. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054693
PMID:23349953 Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working
memory capacity task dependent? Memory &
Shanks, D. R. (2005). Implicit learning. In K.
Cognition, 29, 484–492.
Lamberts & R. Goldstone (Eds.), Handbook
of cognition (pp. 202–220). London: Sage. Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Individual
doi:10.4135/9781848608177.n8 differences in working memory capacity and
learning: Evidence from the serial reaction time
Shanks, D. R., & St John, M. F. (1994). Charac-
task. Memory & Cognition, 33(2), 213–220.
teristics of dissociable human learning systems.
doi:10.3758/BF03195310 PMID:16028576
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 367–447.
doi:10.1017/S0140525X00035032 VanPatten, B. (1994). Evaluating the role of con-
sciousness in second language acquisition: Terms,
Snow, R. E. (1994). Abilities in academic tasks.
linguistic features and research methodology.
In R. J. Sternberg & R. W. Wager (Eds.), Mind in
AILA Review, 11, 27–36.
context (pp. 3–37). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press. Wasserman, J. D., & Tulsky, D. S. (2005). A history
of intelligence assessment. In D. P. Flanagan &
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. Lon-
P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual
don: Macmillan.
assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp. 3–22).
Stanovich, K. E., & Toplak, M. E. (2012). Defin- New York: Guilford Press.
ing features versus incidental correlates of Type
Weinert, S. (2009). Implicit and explicit models
1 and Type 2 processing. Mind & Society, 11(1),
of learning: Similarities and differences from a
3–13. doi:10.1007/s11299-011-0093-6
developmental perspective. Linguistics, 47(2),
241–271. doi:10.1515/LING.2009.010

83

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

Whitmarsh, S., Udden, J., Barendregt, H., & Peters- KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS
son, K. M. (2013). Mindfulness reduces habitual
responding based on implicit knowledge: Evidence Implicit Learning: Learning without aware-
from artificial grammar learning. Consciousness ness of learning processes and/or learning out-
and Cognition, 22(3), 833–845. doi:10.1016/j. comes.
concog.2013.05.007 PMID:23774456 Individual Differences: Variability of cog-
nitive, affective, and conative abilities such as
Woolhouse, L. S., & Bayne, R. (2000). Per- intelligence, anxiety, and motivation.
sonality and the use of intuition: Individual Psychometric Intelligence: Intelligence
differences in strategy and performance on measured by standard intelligence batteries such
an implicit learning task. European Journal as WAIS-R and inferred from statistical methods
of Personality, 14(2), 157–169. doi:10.1002/ such as factor analyses.
(SICI)1099-0984(200003/04)14:2<157::AID- Working Memory: Higher-order memory
PER366>3.0.CO;2-L systems or processes to deal with incoming stimuli
Xie, Q., Gao, X., & King, R. B. (2013). Thinking with simultaneous further processing of those
styles in implicit and explicit learning. Learn- stimuli or other ones.
ing and Individual Differences, 23, 267–271.
doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.014
ENDNOTES
Yang, H., Yang, S., & Isen, A. M. (2013). Positive
affect improves working memory: Implications 1
This cognitive-conative-affective distinction
for controlled cognitive processing. Cognition is amenable to Richard Snow (e.g., 1994).
and Emotion, 27(3), 474–482. doi:10.1080/026 2
DeKeyser (2000), based on the fundamen-
99931.2012.713325 PMID:22917664 tal difference hypothesis by Bley-Vroman
Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on at- (1988), argued that since adults had no
tention: A review of attentional processing of available implicit domain-specific abilities
emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, unlike children, only those who had high
24(1), 3–47. doi:10.1080/02699930903205698 efficiency in (explicit) learning abilities,
namely grammatical sensitivity measured
Zhu, J., & Weiss, L. (2005). The Wechsler Scales. by the Modern Language Aptitude test (Car-
In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Con- roll &Sapon, 1959) could attain native-like
temporary intellectual assessment: Theories, performance in second languages. However,
tests, and issues (pp. 297–324). New York: The natural language learning differs from im-
Guilford Press. plicit learning discussed in this paper: stimuli
Zimmerer, V. C., Cowell, P. E., & Varley, R. in the latter typically do not contain mean-
A. (2011). Individual behavior in learning of ing (see, Robinson, 2010; VanPatten, 1994
an artificial grammar. Memory & Cognition, for discussions). See Jackson (2013) for a
39(3), 491–501. doi:10.3758/s13421-010-0039-y review of IDs in relation to implicit second
PMID:21264600 language learning.

84

Individual Differences in Implicit Learning

3
Zimmerer, Cowell, and Varley (2011) ana- 4
I would like to thank Peter Robinson for
lysedIDs in implicit learning tasks them- informing this paper.
selves, not in relation to IDs in cognitive 5
Feldman et al. (1995) employed the proce-
abilities. However, the findings of such dural/declarative distinction not the implicit/
studies cannot fix the ID issue of implicit explicit distinction. Although both distinc-
learning unless the studies compare standard tions show some overlap in their character-
deviation of implicit and explicit learning istics, they do not refer to the same construct
task performance. (see Squire, 1992 for these distinctions).

85
86

Chapter 4
Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy:
Implicit Cognitive Vulnerability
as an Impetus towards Higher
Order Thinking Skills

Caroline M. Crawford
University of Houston – Clear Lake, USA

Marion S. Smith
Texas Southern University, USA

ABSTRACT
Implicit cognition is an intriguing area of focus when one considers the impact of implicit memory
theories upon each learner’s cognitive vulnerability when framed through Bloom’s Taxonomy of the
Cognitive Domain. Specifically, consider the learner’s cognitive understanding and movement from the
lower order thinking skills, say from the Knowledge realm and Comprehension realm, towards the higher
order thinking skills, Synthesis realm and Evaluation realm, or one of the revised domains to reflect
Digital Age expectations. Although much is available on the different levels of cognitive achievement,
the “in between” leaps in a learner’s ability to work with the information in new and different manners
may suggest that the cognitive vulnerability may impact the learner’s implicit memory and the learner’s
movement between different taxonomic levels of informational understanding.

INTRODUCTION Taxonomy of the Cognitive Realm (Bloom, 1956;


Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill & Krathwohl, 1956;
Implicit cognition is an intriguing realm to con- Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, 1964) as well as
sider; the impact of implicit cognition upon the Anderson and Krathwohl’s revised Taxonomy
ways that people understand knowledge is associ- for the Digital Age(2001), is more explicitly as-
ated with the ways through which people perceive, sociated with a person’s cognitive understanding
remember and work with learned information. of and ability to work with information from the
Engagement with knowledge at higher levels of lower order thinking skills towards the higher
understanding, such as reflected within Bloom’s order thinking skills. Of importance are memory
DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch004

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

theories, due to the ability of the learner to ac- may be a differentiation in thoughtful reflection
cess learned knowledge while framing that same and understanding and the physical and mental
knowledge in new and different ways; as such, abilities and processes of the learner:
the implicit memory of the learner is of vital
importance towards the success of the learner’s Why are people so strongly drawn to believe, in the
understanding of and ability to work with the face of their own daily experience, that the intel-
subject matter in new, creative and innovative ligent execution of an operation must embody two
ways. To more fully support the discussion, it is processes, one of doing and another of theorizing?
of vital importance to more directly discuss four Part of the answer is that they are wedded to the
different areas of emphasis within the discussion, dogma of the ghost in the machine. Since doing
namely: implicit memory theories; Bloom’s Tax- is often an overt muscular affair, it is written off
onomy of the Cognitive Domain (Bloom, 1956; as a merely physical process. On the assumption
Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill & Krathwohl, 1956; of the antithesis between ‘physical’ and ‘mental’,
Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, 1964); Anderson and it follows that muscular doing cannot itself be a
Krathwohl’s Revised Taxonomy of the Cognitive mental operation. To earn the title ‘skilful’, ‘cun-
Domain (2001); and, the concept of Vulnerability ning’, or ‘humorous’, it must therefore get it by
as pertains to the learner’s cognitive efforts. After transfer from another counterpart act occurring
more fully framing the background information, not ‘in the machine’ but ‘in the ghost’; for ‘skilful’,
the objectives of the chapter focus upon framing ‘cunning’ and ‘humorous’ are certainly mental
the implicit cognitive framework through which predicates. (p. 32)
these leaps in conception and understanding may
occur within Bloom’s work and Anderson and One may suggest that Ryle’s discussion re-
Krathwohl’s revised efforts. volved around the mental operations that naturally
are of an interworking nature with the physical
manifestations. More importantly, towards the
BACKGROUND topic framed within this manuscript, Ryle offers
the following:
The theoretical framework undergirding the
thought processes associated with the suggestion But it is admitted by those who believe in the
of implicit cognitive vulnerability is imperative. As legend of the ghost in the machine that no one yet
such, the theoretical and modeling efforts within knows much about the laws governing the sup-
this section revolve around implicit memory theo- posed workings of the mind, while the postulated
ries, Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain interactions between the workings of the mind and
(Bloom, 1956; Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill & the movements of the hand are acknowledged to
Krathwohl, 1956; Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, be completely mysterious. Enjoying neither the
1964), and Anderson and Krathwohl’s Revised supposed status of the mental, nor the supposed
Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain (2001). status of the physical, these interactions can-
not be expected to obey either the known laws
Implicit Memory Theories of physics, or the still to be discovered laws of
psychology. (p. 52)
To begin this discussion on implicit memory theo-
ries, one may reflect upon Ryle’s (1949) discus- Although much has been learned since 1949,
sion as pertains to the motives behind why there the basic emphasis offered by Ryle (1949) is that

87

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

there is not only a distinction in ways of “know- contained within them: mental represen-
ing” but also pulls from Bergson’s (1911) work tations of events are encoded as memory
that postulates, … memory – we shall try to prove traces, which are retained in memory stor-
it in the course of this work – is just the intersec- age and retrieved in the course of ongoing
tion of mind and matter” (p. xii). An intriguing experience, thought, and action.
conclusion drawn by Bergson (1911) frames not • Elaboration: Memory for an event is a
only his offered text, but also this current discus- function of the degree to which that event
sion, namely: is related to pre-existing knowledge at the
time of encoding.
But through this complexity, which is due to the • Organization: Memory is also a function
complexity of reality itself, we believe that the of the degree to which events are related to
reader will find his way if he keeps a fast hold on each other.
the two principles which we have used as a clue • Time-Dependency: Memory fades with
throughout our own researches. The first is that time, mostly by virtue of interference
in psychological analysis we must never forget among competing memory traces.
the utilitarian character of our mental functions, • Availability vs. Accessibility: Encoded
which are essentially turned towards action. The memories, available in memory storage,
second is that the habits formed in action find their may not be accessible when retrieval is
way up to the sphere of speculation, where they attempted.
create fictitious problems, and that metaphysics • Cue-Dependency: The probability of re-
must begin by dispersing this artificial obscurity. trieving an event is a function of the infor-
(pp. xvi-xvii) mational value of cues provided at the time
of retrieval.
Through this process of understanding, one • Encoding Specificity: Retrieval is most
may suggest that learned information delves into effective when cues present at the time of
the framework of memory which may then be retrieval match those processed at the time
rejuvenated in new and different ways so as to of encoding.
engage the recalled knowledge in news ways of • Schematic Processing: Events that are rel-
understanding and thought process. This sense evant to currently active beliefs, expecta-
of understanding may be supported through the tions, and attitudes are remembered better
theoretical appreciation related to implicit memory than those that are irrelevant; events that
and explicit memory. are incongruent with these mental sche-
The discussion that revolves around the concept mata are remembered better than those that
of implicit memory must initially be discussed are congruent.
as regards to explicit and implicit memory, so • Reconstruction: Memory reflects a mix of
as to more fully frame an understanding of the information contained in the memory trace
conscious recollection of information as well as and knowledge derived from other sources;
to try and explain how the human memory may in the final analysis, memories are beliefs,
operate. Kihlstrom (1996) offers basic principles and remembering an event is more like
of the conscious recollection so as to embrace a writing a story from fragmentary notes,
well-articulated recognition of the memory: than reading it from a book.
• Interpersonal: Remembering is an act of
• Stage Analysis: Memories are analogous interpersonal communication as well as
to books in a library, or the information of information retrieval, and so memories

88

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

are shaped by the social context in which common to all individuals. The theory captures
remembering occurs. (Kihlstrom, 1996, ‘‘behavioral patterns in human decision making
as quoted in Kihlstrom, Dorfman & Park, better than traditional economic theory’’ (Nils-
2007, para. 3) son, in his presentation speech for the 2002 Nobel
Prize) and provides a more behaviorally rich model
The question is posed, “How does conscious for the complexity of real-life decision problems.
recollectionactually impacts one’s understanding
of and memory associated with knowledge?” Prospect theory predicts that decision options (or
Conscious recollection may impact understand- prospects) are evaluated in terms of subjective
ing of and memory associated with knowledge, values and likelihoods. Then those two types of
as framed through the sense of vulnerability and information are integrated to make a choice. (p. 12)
cognitive efforts.
Intriguing is Kahneman’s work (Kahneman & Of further significance is the work of Tversky
Tversky, 1979; Kahnerman, 2003; Kahneman & and Kahneman (1992) wherein they extended the
Frederick, 2007), as relates to vulnerability and theory into what is termed Cumulative Prospect
cognition with specific interest focused upon Theory. As stated,
Prospect Theory and the concept of “availability
heuristic” which may offer a supportive explana- Two principles, diminishing sensitivity and loss
tion as to “why some issues are highly salient in the aversion, are invoked to explain the characteristic
public’s mind while others are neglected. People curvature of the value function and the weighted
tend to assess the relative importance of issues functions. A review of the experimental evidence
by the ease with which they are retrieved from and the results of a new experiment confirm a
memory ….” (Kahneman, 2011, p. 8). Kahneman distinctive fourfold pattern of risk attitudes: risk
furthers the discussion wherein the differentiation aversion for gains and risk seeking for losses of
associated with the ability of different persons high probability; risk seeking for gains and risk
to successfully express or repress emotional aversion for losses of low probability. (Tyversky
responses to different cognitive experiences, to & Kahneman, 1992, p. 297)
varying degrees (Kahneman & Frederick, 2007),
suggesting that learners are significantly different Such a sense of uncertainty when framed
in their ability to embrace levels of vulnerability through decision making efforts by an individual,
and discomfort when working with information in which emphasizes the cognitive choices made by
new and different ways such as the differentiation in learners which may be based within implicit and
cognitive domain levels. However, Kahneman and explicit memory. Framed through this discussion,
Tversky’s (1979) work revolving around Prospect the concept of implicit memory and an appro-
Theory suggests the sense of cognitive processing priate consideration towards vulnerability and
and outcome decisions framed through a sense of discomfort with the expression or repression of
vulnerability and uncertainty. As described by one’s emotional responses to different cognitive
Peters, Hess, Vastfjall & Auman (2007): experiences may directly impact one’s success as
regards implicit memory and the learner’s sense
The theory predicts that the processing of infor- of vulnerability and potential resistance and dis-
mation about probabilities and consequences sonance towards working with information in
does not follow normative economic theory but is new and different ways. This sense of uncertainty
driven by perceptual and attentional mechanisms within a learner’s cognitive efforts may suggest the

89

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

importance as related to long-term memory and 1868/1931, p. 40), suggesting that unconscious
short-term memory, wherein ‘episodic memory’ memory, perhaps referred to as implicit memory,
is integrally important to the discussion. is worthy of further consideration due to the lack
With the work of Schacter (1987), a distinction of awareness as concerns the depth and breadth
arose between two manners of something framed of actual knowledge extension and understand-
as ‘episodic memory’ which may be described as ing. Implicit memory is the primary focus of
long-term memory that is more knowledge-based this discussion, as the basic learned knowledge
as regards to events, experiences and context- is meant to be further engaged by the learner, so
based knowledge. Schacter (1987) suggested as to understand the knowledge at higher levels of
that episodic memory can be delineated into two thinking and engagement, such as the higher order
distinct types of memory; namely, explicit memory thinking skills embraced by Bloom’s Taxonomy
and implicit memory. Explicit memory refers to of the Cognitive Domain (Bloom, 1956; Bloom,
prior knowledge-based information, such as what Englehart, Furst, Hill & Krathwohl, 1956; Krath-
may be perceived within memorized information wohl, Bloom & Masia, 1964), as well as Anderson
that is not necessarily developed within a con- and Krathwohl’s (2001) revised Taxonomy for the
ceptual framework of understanding (Vygotsky, Cognitive Domain for the Digital Age.
1933/1966, 1934, 1934/1987, 1935, 1962, 1978a,
1978b, 1981), while implicit memory reflects fur- Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain
ther engagement with the learned knowledge, so as
to experience the knowledge in new and different The concept of understanding the arrangement
ways. Significant research has occurred throughout or classification efforts related to each learner’s
the 20th Century and into the 21st Century, with in- progressive cognitive understanding of subject
triguing theories on implicit memory consistently matter, as well as the ability of the learner to
presenting themselves as worthy efforts (Bowers implement knowledge in new and different ways,
& Marsolek, 2003; Butler, 1872, 1880, 1903; Co- was first developed by Benjamin Bloom, in his
hen & Squire, 1980; Hering, 1870/1880; Jacoby, Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain (Bloom,
1991; Kihlstrom, 1995; Mandler, 1980; Reber, 1956; Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill & Krathwohl,
1993; Roediger & McDermott, 1993; Schacter 1956; Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, 1964). How-
& Tulving, 1994; Squire & Zola-Morgan, 1991; ever, Anderson and Krathwohl (2001) recognized
Tulving & Schacter, 1990), with the essential that the Digital Age began to change the ways
element and focus of implicit learning being that that people learned; they introduced a Revised
there is a sense of unconscious learning that occurs. Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain to
More specifically, although the learner is working reflect learner efforts when working with subject
with the learned knowledge in new and different matter within the Digital Age facilitated by the
ways, the unconscious mental efforts associated use of the Internet and World Wide Web during
with analysing the knowledge in new and differ- the late 1990s.
ent ways, so as to think about the information Bloom’s original Taxonomy of the Cognitive
within new environments and situational problems, Domain could be considered as static cognitive
lends itself to an unconscious deliberation and efforts, wherein the designated levels of subject
understanding of the information. An intriguing matter-focused cognitive understanding were
statement by von Hartmann denotes that one’s offered as labels that clearly stated the level’s
unconscious memory and working understanding expectation as regards the actual cognitive under-
of the knowledge “can really outdo all the per- standing and specific ability associated with that
formances of conscious reason” (von Hartmann, level of cognitive understanding. The pyramid

90

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

structure within Bloom’s taxonomic representa- 1964) is one of the three identified domains:
tion was clearly understood due to the base of the Cognitive; Affective; and, Psychomotor (Bloom,
structure being the first level of learner cognitive 1956). The origin of this Taxonomy is intriguing, as
understanding and slowly moving up each level of offered by Krathwohl (2002a) when he offers that,
the pyramid towards the highest level of cognitive
understanding, evaluation, and ability to use the The Taxonomy of Educational Objectives is a
subject matter appropriately. framework for classifying statements of what we
With the introduction of the Digital Age, aca- expect or intend students to learn as a result of
demics began to recognize that people were learn- instruction. The framework was conceived as a
ing in new and different ways. As such, Anderson means of facilitating the exchange of test items
and Krathwohl (2001) attempted to appropriately among faculty at various universities in order to
reflect the differentiation in ways that learners create banks of items, each measuring the same
actively engaged and worked with subject matter educational objectives. (p. 212)
at different levels of understanding. Anderson and
Krathwohl maintained the pyramid structure as An intriguing quotation that offers a definition
originally implemented within Bloom’s Taxonomy of knowledge is offered in the Appendix which is
of the Cognitive Domain’s graphic representa- quoted as, “Knowledge, as defined here, involves
tion, with the base of the pyramid designating the recall of specifics and universals, the recall
the initial learner’s efforts associated with the of methods and processes, or the recall of a pat-
subject matter and slowly moving up the pyramid tern, structure, or setting” (Bloom, 1956, p. 201).
structure until the top, or highest level of work This definition of knowledge helps support the
with the subject matter, is reached. An interest- concept of implicit versus explicit memory within
ing shift in Anderson and Krathwohl’s domains the Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain, wherein
designated an active engagement with the subject the explicit memory efforts revolve around the
matter, as denoted through the “-ing” designation learner’s ability to recall specific information,
for each level within the revised Taxonomy of methods, patterns, structures, and other delinea-
the Cognitive Domain. Further, Anderson and tions of specific recall, while the implicit memory
Krathwohl revised the levels of thinking skills aspects focus more so upon more universal and
implemented, which further differentiated their abstract understandings and a more unconscious
revised cognitive-focused taxonomy from Bloom’s level of understanding and structural information,
original taxonomic framework. Further discussion such that learning occurs at the higher levels of
of both Taxonomies of the Cognitive Domain are the Cognitive Domain. As the Cognitive domain
appropriate, to more fully articulate the importance is the one specifically focused upon one’s knowl-
associated with each taxonomic effort, as well as edge, or mental efforts, this is the one domain
framing the undergirding importance of the sense that will be focused upon within this discussion
of vulnerability within the learner’ that revolves around a learner’s efforts. Within
the Cognitive Domain, there are six categories
Bloom’s Taxonomy of the wherein “Knowledge” is designated as the low-
Cognitive Domain est order of thinking and each category becomes
a higher level until the highest level is reached,
Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain designated as “Evaluation”: Knowledge; Com-
(Bloom, 1956; Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill & prehension; Application; Analysis; Synthesis;
Krathwohl, 1956; Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, and, Evaluation.With the dawning of the Digital

91

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

Age, the introduction of the Internet offered an the information, with the ability to move from a
opportunity to consider revisions to Bloom’s lower-level category to the next higher category?
original Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain This is a question worthy of consideration.
(Bloom, 1956; Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill &
Krathwohl, 1956; Krathwohl, Bloom &Masia, Anderson and Krathwohl’s Revised
1964), and consider enhancing and further delin- Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain
eating categories worthy of further consideration.
As stated by Krathwohl (2002a): Anderson and Krathwohl (2001) offered a revised
view of Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Do-
In the original Taxonomy, the Knowledge cat- main, to more fully embrace a matrix in which
egory embodied both noun and verb aspects. The the desired focus is upon not only the knowledge
noun or subject matter aspect was specified in but also the learner’s cognition, suggested as
Knowledge’s extensive subcategories. The verb being “Our revision of the original Taxonomy
aspect was included in the definition given to is a two-dimensional framework: Knowledge
Knowledge in that the student was expected to and Cognitive Processes” (Krathwohl, 2002a, p.
be able to recall or recognize knowledge. This 218), wherein “The Taxonomy of Educational
brought unidimensionality to the framework at Objectives is a scheme for classifying educational
the cost of a Knowledge category that was dual goals, objectives, and, most recently, standards.
in nature and thus different from the other Taxo- It provides an organizational structure that gives
nomic categories. This anomaly was eliminated a commonly understood meaning to objectives
in the revised Taxonomy by allowing these two classified in one of its categories, thereby en-
aspects, the noun and the verb, to form separate hancing communication” (Krathwohl, 2002a,
dimensions, the noun providing the basis for the p. 218). The Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy of the
Knowledge dimension and the verb forming the Cognitive Domain more fully embraces the levels
basis for the Cognitive Process dimension. (p. 213) of knowledge accumulation and understanding,
offered as The Cognitive Process Dimension:
What is important to acknowledge is that the Remember (lower level of cognitive processing);
subject matter is not a detriment; instead, the Understand, Apply; Analyze; Evaluate; and, Cre-
subject matter is not important when considering ate (higher level of cognitive processing). What is
the procedures inherent towards enhancing the intriguing is that Anderson and Krathwohl (2001)
learner’s understanding of the information within offer a matrix wherein four different levels of The
the Cognitive Domain. Knowledge Dimension understanding occurs,
The importance of Bloom’s Taxonomy of the specifically being: Factual Knowledge; Concep-
Cognitive Domain (1956) cannot be more fully em- tual Knowledge; Procedural Knowledge; and,
phasized, as Bloom’s efforts emphasize the major Metacognitive Knowledge (Krathwohl, 2002a, p.
points of a learner’s understanding of knowledge, 217). The Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy (Anderson
as regards the ability to work with knowledge at & Krathwohl, 2001) suggests a more dynamic
procedurally higher levels of understanding. What understanding of the learner’s cognitive processes
is important to question, at this point, is how the associated with understanding the information
learner successfully moves from category to cat- and how one might think about the information;
egory, within the cognitive domain? How does however, as with Bloom’s Taxonomy (Bloom,
the learner develop a working understanding of 1956) although to a lesser degree, there continues

92

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

to be an important question, specifically revolving Vulnerability is not good or bad, it is simply real.
around the question as to how the learner success- Some kinds of vulnerability feel awesome and
fully moves from category to category, within the some feel terrifying. All vulnerability is profoundly
cognitive domain? How does the learner develop experiential and memorable. The more we learn
a working understanding of the information, with how to nurture our vulnerability, the more intimacy
the ability to move from a lower-level category we can experience and the safer and happier we
to the next higher category? are likely to be in the world.

Vulnerability But live in a culture where vulnerability is ridi-


culed, abused, medicated and repressed. Because
Within the context of learning, the concept of of this, many reading this article do not even know
vulnerability can be a frightening consideration. about their vulnerability – consciously. Some
The question is “why”? Within the world of pro- literally have been abused out of a connection to
fessional acting, actors consistently delve into their vulnerability. In these people, vulnerability
the concept of vulnerability. As suggested by seems bad and the need to “escape it” drives
Bennett (2007): much of what they do. (Bennett, 2007, para. 7-8)

To have a successful career as an actor you must The discussion emphasizes the experiential and
have a process for relating to and accessing your memorable aspects associated with feeling one’s
core vulnerability -- the most sensitive, open, em- own sense of vulnerability while understanding
pathic, deeply feeling, “real, inner you” -- your that the cultural impact on one’s self can negatively
“inner child.” By learning to respect and nurture impact one’s ability to recognize, access and em-
your own vulnerability, you can realize profound brace the vulnerability one feels within life experi-
creativity and depth in your acting. Conversely, ences. If this is a correct recognition of the cultural
lack of access to one’s vulnerability is the cause impact upon aspects related to vulnerability, this
of much bad acting, and can result in actors is a significant commentary associated with the
shouting at one another with no real “heart” learner’s ability to embrace the concepts related
connection. (para. 1) to vulnerability within the cognitive processes
associated with learner at progressively higher
This same conception of vulnerability may be levels of informational understanding. Again to
inherent within the cognitive learning process, quote Bennett (2007), “Vulnerability underlies
wherein recognizing one’s own feelings of vul- every human conflict – and thus is at the core of
nerability and discomfort must be respected and all meaningful acting. Acting that is devoid of
nurtured, so as to delve more deeply into a recogni- genuine vulnerability almost always fails” (para.
tion of knowledge acquisition, understanding and 3). This suggestion that the inability to embrace
a shift in informational understanding that is new, vulnerability disengages one from the ability to
different, and may be initially uncomfortable for offer a meaningful acting experience may parallel
the learner as the cognitive processes rethink prior the learner’s need to more fully embrace aspects
understandings and one’s conceptual framework of of cognitive dissonance and vulnerability within
understanding shifts (Vygotsky, 1933/1966, 1934, the cognitive learning endeavour, so as to more
1934/1987, 1935, 1962, 1978a, 1978b, 1981). fully understand the shifting metacognitive un-
As we continue consideration towards Bennett’s derstandings that occur when a learner begins to
(2007) discussion revolving around actors and think about and apply information at a higher level
the necessary need to access one’s vulnerability: of categorical implementation. One may ponder

93

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

and speculate as to why the potential impact of Anderson and Krathwohl (2001) offered a revised
vulnerability upon the learner and cognitive learn- version of Bloom’s Taxonomy of Educational
ing process is not more fully explored. Objectives for the Digital Age, wherein the six
Of interest is the literature review search that primary categories in the cognitive domain were
this author delved into, shifting from an intrigu- delineated as: Remembering, Understanding,
ing and cursory review of available scholarly Applying, Analysing, Evaluating, and Creating.
resources into an expedition wherein the terms As framed within Bloom’s Taxonomy, Anderson
“vulnerability” and “learning” offered one or two and Krathwohl (2001) designated Remembering
existential or creative resources that were worthy of as being the “lowest” categorical level, slowly
review.Yet, the vast majority of scholarly resources moving up to the “highest” level of Creating.
focused upon cognitive vulnerability to depres- These delineations are intriguing; however, the
sion, emotional disorders and other comparatively implicit cognitive framework through which these
negative areas of research focus. It was difficult leaps in conception and understanding may occur
to locate the term “vulnerability” or “cognitive are lacking.
vulnerability”, “cognition” and “learning” with To fill this cognitive leap in a learner’s un-
positive endeavours such as successful cognitive derstanding, one must consider the needs of the
learner efforts towards working with information learner to frame the information already learned
in new, innovative ways so as to meet learning and integrated into one’s memory a bit differ-
objectives. ently. As such, a discussion as revolves around
the implicit cognitive vulnerability of the learner
Vulnerability within the as an impetus towards moving “up” the cognitive
Learning Process domains of information understanding must be
realized. Specifically focusing upon the learner’s
Engaging in the learning process can be unnerv- ability to engage with the subject matter in new
ing for the learner, especially as one considers the and different ways, with a sense of vulnerability
understandings regarding the unconscious influ- and engagement so as to understand and work with
ences revolving around the concept of implicit the information at a new, different and sometimes
cognition. Much interest has revolved around frighteningly uncomfortable manner of being.
how a learner actually learns information, inte- The concept of vulnerability is inherent
grates new information into one’s working and within the learner’s implicit cognitive explora-
long-term memory, as well as the sociological tions as regards moving up the cognitive domain
impact upon one’s cognitive processes. Bloom’s categories. Framing this discussion more easily,
Taxonomy of Educational Objectives, specifically a graphic representation of Bloom’s Taxonomy
focused upon the Cognitive Domain (Bloom, and Anderson and Krathwohl’s revisions for the
1956; Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill & Krathwohl, Digital Age are integrated:
1956; Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, 1964) wherein The conception for this understanding of vul-
six primary categories in the cognitive domain nerability as being an impetus within a learner’s
were delineated: Knowledge, Comprehension, understanding of the information and ability to
Application, Analysis, Synthesis and Evaluation. engage with the information in new and different
The “lowest” categorical level was designated as ways is integral towards recognizing the inherent
being Knowledge, slowly moving one’s cogni- strength of the learner’s cognitive ability as well
tive understanding and ability to work with the as intriguing aspects associated with a learner’s
knowledge up to the highest level, being Evalua- humanist connection to the process of learning
tion. Further, in the early part of the 21st Century, and understanding.

94

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

Figure 1. Bloom’s Taxonomy of Educational Objectives, specifically focused upon the Cognitive Domain,
with the concept of “vulnerability” delineated. (Original Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain
referenced in Bloom, 1956; Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill & Krathwohl, 1956; Krathwohl, Bloom &
Masia, 1964)

How Did This Implicit Cognitive connections and perceptions, I began to consider
Vulnerability Theory Begin? the discomfort and vulnerability associated with
the learner’s continuously developing understand-
This intriguing rethinking of delineated cognitive ing of knowledge and associated information, the
domains of learning and information understand- learner’s conceptual framework of understanding
ing arose in a strangely intriguing manner. The au- that supports and engages new information while
thor was learning about an academic’s focus upon also working towards placing the new information
human communicative styles and the “humanness” into prior frameworks of conceptual understand-
of our ability to commiserate and understand each ing that have already been imprinted; as well,
other, primarily through a conceptual focus upon rethinking and reframing a learner’s conceptual
expanding perception yet more specifically upon framework of understanding as new information
“the power of vulnerability” (Brown, 2010, 2011). presents itself through socialization (Vygotsky,
While listening to the initial discussions and “dis- 1933/1966, 1934, 1934/1987, 1935, 1962, 1978a,
comfort” associated with human communication, 1978b, 1981). The social discourse associated

95

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

Figure 2. Anderson and Krathwohl’s revisions to Bloom’s Taxonomy for the Digital Age, with the concept
of “vulnerability” delineated. (Original Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain referenced
inAnderson & Krathwohl, 2001)

with meaning-making through the chosen words the importance associated with creative types of
that people use to communicate with each other thinking and ability so as to develop new ideas
(Wittgenstein, 1960) further framed my thought and creative re-imaginings of previously routine,
process, until I began to seriously consider commonplace or conventional ideas.
Bloom’s Taxonomy of Educational Objectives as
specifically focused upon the Cognitive Domain Issues, Controversies, Problems
(Bloom, 1956, 1984; Bloom, Englhart, Furst, Hill
& Krathwohl, 1956; Bloom &Krathwohl, 1956; Issues with presenting this theoretical concept of
Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, 1964) as well as implicit cognitive vulnerability within the learning
Bloom’s Revised Taxonomy of Educational Ob- process, more specifically within the Original or
jectives of the Cognitive Domain for the Digital the Revised Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain
Age (Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001; Krathwohl, (Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001; Bloom, 1956;
2002a, 2002b) while also thinking back to Pink’s Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill & Krathwohl, 1956;
(2006) framing of the Conceptual Age wherein Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, 1964), revolves
working with information and ideas relates to around the lack of research associated with the

96

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

learner’s sense of cognitive vulnerability as the creative inspiration. To successfully accomplish


learner tries to move from a lower level of cogni- this modelling within the instructional environ-
tive understanding and working with the subject ment, the instructor or facilitator must not only have
matter, towards the next higher level of cognitive this also modelled successfully towards their own
understanding and actively engaging or working experiential understanding and implicit memory
with the subject matter so as to focus upon a higher engagement, but this sense of implicit cognitive
order thinking skill necessary to be a successful vulnerability must also be actively instructed and
learner. The learner may engage with a sense of engaged within different instructional and train-
vulnerability, perhaps one may refer to as implicit ing realms so as to support learners at all levels
cognitive vulnerability, wherein a deep-rooted of pedagogical and andragogical engagement.
sense of resistance, dissonance, fear and perhaps
concerns related to revealing their own creativity Solutions and Recommendations
and innovative inspiration within a more public
forum due to associated fears reflecting the po- Recommendations associated with this endeav-
tentially undesirable expectation to “think outside our are to begin the conversation as regards the
the box” and reflect their own cognitive thought potential towards significance associated with
processes that may be different than their peer the concept of implicit cognitive vulnerability. It
learners and may actually present the learner as is imperative that research occurs, to delve into
cognitively beyond the bounds of the instructor this topic so as to positively engage the learners
or facilitator’s understanding. It’s a frightening with the subject matter, more fully embrace the
endeavour, to work with information in new and ability to work with and understand subject matter
different ways, especially when the learner may knowledge, and develop a more positive sense of
not be positively supported by peers, instructional the concept of “vulnerability” within the cognitive
faculty and staff, as well as creative endeavours domain of understanding and learning process.
that may naturally offer a sense of cognitive re- A second recommendation would be to ac-
sistance and dissonance to the learner who may knowledge the importance of vulnerability within
strive towards overcoming the potential resistant the learning process, and to not only instruct but
feelings and delve into new ways of working with also model positive engagement with vulnerability
and thinking about the subject matter that may within the learning environment. Developing an
feel uncomfortable or strangely unnatural for a academic tolerance towards vulnerability may
short period of time. not only encourage the learner’s strengthening
The learner must naturally develop an under- understanding of the learning process, but may
standing as regards this sense of vulnerability at more fully engage the learner in delving into
imperative points throughout the learning process. emotional responses associated with vulnerabil-
But how is the learner to accomplish this impor- ity, such as dissonance, resistance, fear and also
tant task? An understanding by the instructor or creativity, inspiration and innovativeness. Shifting
facilitator is necessary and appropriate, but beyond the thought process away from a negative sense
one’s understanding, the instructor or facilitator of “vulnerability” towards a positive experiential
must endeavour to model the need to embrace attitude, may not only embrace positive academic
the sense of vulnerability as a naturally occurring change but also further engage the concept of
sense of resistance, dissonance and fear, so as to lifelong learning as a worthy endeavour and an
embrace vulnerability as a discomfort that merely exciting adventure.
strengthens one’s implicit cognitive processes and

97

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS CONCLUSION

Implicit cognition is an intriguing consideration Concepts related to implicit memory, implicit


within the realm of learning; specifically within cognition and implicit cognitive vulnerability as
the realm of implicit memory necessities within the pertains to the learning endeavour is a vital area
learner’s ability to work with information at differ- of intrigue and is worthy of significant future
ent levels of cognitive domains of understanding. study. The conceptions revolving around Bloom’s
As Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Do- Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain and Anderson
main (Bloom, 1956; Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill and Krathwohl’s (2001) Revised Bloom’s Tax-
& Krathwohl, 1956; Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, onomy of the Cognitive Domain (Bloom, 1956;
1964) and Anderson and Krathwohl’s (2001) Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill & Krathwohl, 1956;
Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy are vitally impor- Krathwohl, Bloom & Masia, 1964) embrace the
tant within the academic milieu, considerations concept of cognition within the learning realm;
regarding not only designing learning objectives however, what is lacking is an explanation as
is worthy of consideration but also the shifts in regards how the learner successfully moves from
cognitive efforts on the part of the learners is also one level of cognitive effort to a higher level of
important. Recognition that there are different cognitive effort. Anderson and Krathwohl (2001)
ways through which to work with subject matter attempt to provide a two-dimensional explana-
knowledge is a strength of Bloom’s Taxonomy tion that furthers a thoughtful understanding of
of the Cognitive Domain, as well as Anderson The Knowledge Dimension as regards Factual
and Krathwohl’s Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy; Knowledge, Conceptual Knowledge, Procedural
however, how the learner is able to successfully Knowledge, and Metacognitive Knowledge
transfer or pass into a higher order thinking process within the different Cognitive Process Dimen-
is immensely necessary due to the need to support sions (Krathwohl, 2002a, p. 217), but continues
and nurture the learner’s cognitive endeavours. to ignore the important concepts that frame the
The suggestion that a sense of learner experi- differentiation between the dimensional cogni-
ence as concerns implicit cognitive vulnerability tive steps towards higher order thinking skills.
may indeed be a cognitive reality as the learner This discussion is merely the beginning, wherein
shifts ways through which to work with different aspects related to implicit cognitive vulnerabil-
subject matter information, is worthy of future ity suggests the sense of dissonance, resistance,
research effort. fear, creativity, inspiration and innovative efforts
More directly related to the book’s theme, associated with the shifting efforts and thought
the exploration of implicit cognition from the processes as the learner delves into cognitively
academic focus is pedagogically and andragogi- focused higher order thinking skills.
cally inherent and necessary. Implicit cognition,
much as implicit memory, are integrally important
towards cognitive processes, implicit cognitive REFERENCES
vulnerability is a necessary topic of engagement
so as to more fully understand successfully over- Anderson, L.W., & Krathwohl. (Eds.). (2001). A
coming the resistance, dissonance, fear, creativity Taxonomy for Learning, Teaching, and Assessing:
and inspiration that comes from acknowledging A Revision of Bloom’s Taxonomy of Educational
and embracing implicit cognitive vulnerability. Objectives. New York: Longman.

98

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

Bennett, J. (2007). Great actors access vulnerabil- Butler, S. (1872). Erewhon. London: Penguin
ity. The Jason Bennett Actor’s Workshop. Retrieved Books. Retrieved from http://www.joh.cam.ac.uk/
from http://www.jbactors.com/actingreading/ erewhon-1872
actorsaccessvulnerability.html
Butler, S. (1880). Unconscious Memory. London:
Bergson, H. (1911). Matter and Memory. Lon- David Bogue. Retrieved from http://www.joh.cam.
don: Swan Sonnenschein. Retrieved from https:// ac.uk/unconscious-memory-1880
archive.org/details/matterandmemory00berguoft
Butler, S. (1903). The Ways of all Flesh. London:
Bloom, B. S. (Ed.). (1956). Taxonomy of educa- Grant Richards. Retrieved from http://www.joh.
tional objectives: The classification of educational cam.ac.uk/way-all-flesh-1903
goals: Handbook I, cognitive domain. New York:
Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved
Longmans, Green.
learning and retention of pattern analyzing skill
Bloom, B. S. (1984). Taxomony of educational in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and
objectives. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon. knowing that. Science, 210(4466), 207–210.
doi:10.1126/science.7414331 PMID:7414331
Bloom, B. S., Englhart, M. D., Furst, E. J., Hill, W.
H., & Krathwohl, D. R. (Eds.). (1956). Taxonomy Hering, E. (1870/1880). On memory. In S. Butler
of educational objectives, the classification of (Ed.), Unconscious memory (New Ed., pp. 63-86).
educational goals, Handbook I: cognitive domain. London: Fifield.
New York: Longmans.
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociaton
Bloom, B. S., & Krathwohl, D. R. (1956). Tax- framework: Separating automatic from inten-
onomy of Educational Objectives: The Classifi- tional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and
cation of Educational Goals, by a committee of Language, 30(5), 513–541. doi:10.1016/0749-
college and university examiners: Handbook I: 596X(91)90025-F
Cognitive Domain. New York: Longman, Green.
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on
Bowers, J. S., & Marsolek, C. J. (Eds.). (2003). judgment and choice: Mapping bounded ra-
Rethinking implicit memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford tionality. The American Psychologist, 58(9),
University Press. 697–720. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697
PMID:14584987
Brown, B. (2010, December). The power of
vulnerability. TEDxHouston. TED Conferences, Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow.
LCC. Retrieved from http://www.ted.com/talks/ New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
brene_brown_on_vulnerability.html
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2007, February).
Brown, B. (2011, January). Brene Brown: The Frames and brains: Elicitation and control of re-
Power of Vulnerability. Retrieved from: http:// sponse tendencies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
www.youtube.com/watch?v=iCvmsMzlF7o 11(2), 45–46. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.007
PMID:17188556
Butler, L. T., & Berry, D. C. (2001). Implicit
memory: Intention and awareness revisited. Trends Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect
in Cognitive Sciences, 5(5), 192–197. doi:10.1016/ theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econo-
S1364-6613(00)01636-3 PMID:11323263 metrica, 47(2), 263–291. doi:10.2307/1914185

99

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

Kihlstrom, J. F. (1995). Memory and conscious- Peters, E., Hess, T. M., Vastfjall, D., & Auman, C.
ness: An appreciation of Claparede and Recogni- (2007). Adult age differences in dual information
tion et Moiiete. Consciousness & Cognition. Inter- processes: Implications for the role of affective
national Journal (Toronto, Ont.), 4(4), 379–386. and deliberative processes in older adults’ decision
making. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Memory research: The
2(1), 1-23. Retrieved from http://130.154.3.8/
convergence of theory and practice. In D. Her-
content/dam/rand/www/external/labor/aging/rsi/
mann, C. McEvoy, C. Hertzog, P. Hertel, & M.
rsi_papers/2008/peters1.pdf
K. Johnson (Eds.), Basic and applied memory
research: Theory in context (Vol. 1, pp. 5–25). Pink, D. (2006). A Whole New Mind: Why Right-
Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum. Brainers Will Rule the Future. New York, NY:
Penguin Group.
Kihlstrom, J. F., Dorfman, J., & Park, L. (2007).
Implicit and Explicit Memory and Learning. In Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit
M. Velmans & S. Schneider (Eds.), The Black- knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious.
well Companion to Consciousness. Oxford, UK: Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9780470751466.ch42
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1993).
Krathwohl, D. R. (2002a). A revision of Bloom’s Implicit memory in normal human subjects. In
Taxonomy: An overview. Theory into Practice, F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of Neu-
41(4), 212–218. doi:10.1207/s15430421tip4104_2 ropsychology (pp. 63–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier
Science Publishers.
Krathwohl, D. R. (2002b, Autumn).The Role of
Metacognitive Knowledge in Learning, Teach- Ryle, G. (1949). The Concept of Mind. Chi-
ing, and Assessing. Theory into Practice, 41 (4), cago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Retrieved
220-227. from https://archive.org/details/conceptofmind-
032022mbp
Krathwohl, D. R., Bloom, B. S., & Masia, B. B.
(1964). Taxonomy of educational objectives: The Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (Eds.). (1994).
classification of educational goals: Handbook 11: Memory systems 1994. Cambridge, MA: The
The affective domain. New York: David McKay. MIT Press.
Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The
of previous occurrence. Psychological Review, medial temporal lobe memory system. Sci-
87(3), 252–271. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.252 ence, 253(5026), 1380–1386. doi:10.1126/sci-
ence.1896849 PMID:1896849
Nilsson, L. G. (2002). Presentation speech for the
2002 Bank of Sweden prize in economic sciences Tulving, E., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming
in memory of Alfred Nobel. Retrieved from http:// and human memory systems. Science, 247(4940),
www.nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/economic- 301–306. doi:10.1126/science.2296719
sciences/laureates/2002/presentation-speech.html PMID:2296719

100

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances ADDITIONAL READING


in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of
uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5(4), Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M., & Pelz, J. B.
297–323. doi:10.1007/BF00122574 (1995). Memory representations in natural tasks.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1), 66–80.
von Hartmann, E. (1931). Philosophy of the un- doi:10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.66 PMID:23961754
conscious: Speculative results according to the
inductive method of physical science. London: Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit
Routledge and Kegan Paul. (Original work pub- learning. East Sussex, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
lished 1868) Associates Ltd.

Vygotsky, L. (1934). Thinking and speaking. Biederman, I. (1972). Perceiving real-world


Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. scenes. Science, 177(4043), 77–80. doi:10.1126/
science.177.4043.77 PMID:5041781
Vygotsky, L. S. (1933/1966). Play and its role
in the mental development of the child. Social Brady, T. F., & Chun, M. M. (2007). Spatial
Psychology, 12(6), 62–76. constraints on learning in visual search: Model-
ing contextual cuing. Journal of Experimental
Vygotsky, L. S. (1935). Mental development of Psychology. Human Perception and Performance,
children during education. Moscow: Uchpedzig. 33(4), 798–815. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.798
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and Language. PMID:17683229
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. doi:10.1037/11193- Brockmole, J. R., Castelhano, M. S., & Henderson,
000 J. M. (2006). Contextual cueing in naturalistic
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978a). Mind in Society. Cam- scenes: Global and local contexts. Journal of
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 32(4), 699–706. doi:10.1037/0278-
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978b). Mind in society: The 7393.32.4.699 PMID:16822141
development of higher psychological processes
(M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Brockmole, J. R., & Henderson, J. M. (2006).
Souberman, Trans. & Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Recognition and attention guidance dur-
Harvard University. ing contextual cueing in real-world scenes:
Evidence from eye movements. Quarterly
Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The genesis of higher Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(7),
mental functions. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The 1177–1187. doi:10.1080/17470210600665996
concept of activity in Soviet Psychology. Armonk, PMID:16769618
NY: Sharpe.
Brockmole, J. R., & Henderson, J. M. (2006).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of Using real-world scenes as contextual cues
L. S. Vygotsky (R. W. Rieber & A. Carton, Eds.). for search. Visual Cognition, 13(1), 99–108.
New York: Plenum Press. (Original work pub- doi:10.1080/13506280500165188
lished 1934)
Wittgenstein, L. (1960). Tractatus logico-philo-
sophicus. London: Routledge.

101

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

Buckner, R. L., Petersen, S. E., Ojemann, J. G., Jiang, Y., & Leung, A. W. (2005). Implicit learn-
Miezin, F. M., Squire, L. R., & Raichle, M. E. ing of ignored visual context. Psychonomic Bul-
(1995). Functional anatomical studies of explicit letin & Review, 12(1), 100–106. doi:10.3758/
and implicit memory retrieval tasks. The Journal BF03196353 PMID:15948286
of Neuroscience, 15(1), 12–29. PMID:7823123
Kunar, M. A., Flusberg, S., Horowitz, T. S.,
Chun, M. M. (2000). Contextual cueing of visual & Wolfe, J. M. (2007). Does contextual cuing
attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(5), guide the deployment of attention? Journal of
170–178. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01476-5 Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
PMID:10782102 Performance, 33(4), 816–828. doi:10.1037/0096-
1523.33.4.816 PMID:17683230
Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual
cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual Olson, I. R., & Chun, M. M. (2001). Temporal
context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psy- contextual cuing of visual attention. Journal of
chology, 36(1), 28–71. http://web.mit.edu/yuhong/ Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and
www/CogPsy_98_ChunJiang.pdf. doi:10.1006/ Cognition, 27(5), 1299–1313. doi:10.1037/0278-
cogp.1998.0681 PMID:9679076 7393.27.5.1299 PMID:11550756
Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (2003). Implicit, long- Olson, I. R., & Chun, M. M. (2002). Percep-
term spatial contextual memory. Journal of Ex- tual constraints on implicit learning of spatial
perimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and context. Visual Cognition, 9(3), 273–302.
Cognition, 29(2), 224–234. doi:10.1037/0278- doi:10.1080/13506280042000162
7393.29.2.224 PMID:12696811
Peterson, M. S., & Kramer, A. F. (2001). At-
Chun, M. M., & Nakayama, K. (2000). On the tentional guidance of the eyes by contextual
functional role of implicit visual memory for information and abrupt onsets. Perception &
the adaptive deployment of attention across Psychophysics, 63(7), 1239–1249. doi:10.3758/
scenes. Visual Cognition, 7(1-3), 65–81. BF03194537 PMID:11766947
doi:10.1080/135062800394685
Roediger, H. L. (1990, September). Implicit
Heindel, W. C., Salmon, D. P., Shults, C. W., memory: Retention without remembering. The
Walicke, P. A., & Butters, N. (1989). Neuro- American Psychologist, 9(45), 1043–1056. http://
psychological evidence for multiple implicit academic.udayton.edu/psy49305/Articles/Roedi-
memory systems: A comparison of Alzheimer’s, ger%20%281990%29.pdf. doi:10.1037/0003-
Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s disease patients. 066X.45.9.1043 PMID:2221571
The Journal of Neuroscience, 9(2), 582–587.
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1993).
PMID:2521896
Implicit memory in normal human subjects.
Jiang, Y., & Chun, M. M. (2001). Selective atten- Handbook of neuropsychology, 8, 63-63.
tion modulates implicit learning. The Quarterly
Rugg, M. D., Mark, R. E., Walla, P., Schloers-
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section
cheidt, A. M., Birch, C. S., & Allan, K. (1998).
A, 54(4), 1105–1124. doi:10.1080/713756001
Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit
PMID:11765735
and explicit memory. Nature, 392(6676), 595–598.
doi:10.1038/33396 PMID:9560154

102

Rethinking Bloom’s Taxonomy

Schacter, D. L. (1992, Summer). Priming and Higher Order Thinking Skills: This is a con-
multiple memory systems: Perceptual mechanisms cept within the educational realm, based upon dif-
of implicit memory. Journal of Cognitive Neu- ferent instructional or learning taxonomies, such
roscience, 3(4), 244–256. http://www.mitpress- as Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain
journals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/jocn.1992.4.3.244. or Anderson and Krathwohl’s Revised Bloom’s
doi:10.1162/jocn.1992.4.3.244 PMID:23964881 Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain.
Implicit Memory: This is a memory type
Schacter, D. L., Bowers, J., & Booker, J. (1989).
wherein one may not actually recognize a con-
Intention, awareness, and implicit memory: The
scious understanding of prior knowledge nor
retrieval intentionality criterion.
experiential efforts associated with information,
Schacter, D. L., Chiu, C. Y. P., & Ochsner, K. subject matter or performance.
N. (1993). Implicit memory: A selective review. Resistance: This is the cognitive sense of
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16(1), 159–182. opposition to or slowed cognitive movement as-
doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.001111 sociated with the learner. Within this discussion,
PMID:8460889 it is focused upon the learner’s feelings of subject
matter discomfort and sense of opposition to or
Wolfe, J. M., Klempen, N., & Dahlen, K. (2000).
slowed cognitive movement associated with the
Postattentive vision. Journal of Experimental
learner’s attempts to think about and implement
Psychology. Human Perception and Performance,
the subject matter information in new and dif-
26(2), 693–716. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.693
ferent ways.
PMID:10811170
Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cogni-
tive Domain: Anderson and Krathwohl (2001)
redesigned Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive
KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS Domain in the early years of the Digital Age,
wherein there was a different way to think about
Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain: the understanding of knowledge and higher order
Dr. Benjamin Bloom and colleagues developed a thinking skills, as well as framing the design and
Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain wherein they development of instructional objectives for the
tried to lay out a framework of understanding as learners within a two-dimensional matrix-style
regards the ways that people understand knowledge framework.
and work with information. The primary focus of Vulnerability: This is the label that the au-
this effort was to more easily design and develop thor implements, to reflect the sense of cognitive
instructional objectives for the learner to meet dissonance, resistance, fear, as well as creativity,
within an instructional endeavour or environment. inspiration and innovative cognitive thought pro-
Dissonance: This is a cognitive sense of un- cesses that occur as the learner moves between
certainty and discomfort. Within this discussion, cognitive domains within Bloom’s Taxonomy
it is focused upon the learner’s feelings of subject of the Cognitive Domain and Anderson and
matter discomfort and uncertainty as the learner Krathwohl’s Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy of the
attempts to think about and implement the subject Cognitive Domain.
matter information in new and different ways.

103
104

Chapter 5
Implicit Social Cognition and
Language Attitudes Research
Andrew J. Pantos
Metropolitan State University of Denver, USA

ABSTRACT
This chapter argues for incorporation of concepts and methods from the domain of Implicit Social
Cognition (ISC) into the field of language attitudes research. As support, this chapter reports on a quan-
titative study that employed both an audio Implicit Association Test and traditional self-report question-
naires to measure participants’ implicit and explicit attitudes toward foreign and U.S. accented speech
stimuli. The IAT revealed a pro-U.S. accent bias, while the explicit measure found a pro-foreign accent
bias. These results support the argument that the distinction between implicit and explicit attitudes as
separable attitude constructs resulting from distinct mental processes is an important one for language
attitudes research and that both attitude constructs should be evaluated when studying language attitudes.

INTRODUCTION and comprehensive approach that augments


existing language attitudes research. Using this
This paper reports on the findings of a recent quantitative research study of reactions to foreign
quantitative experimental study that provides accented speech as a foundation, this paper argues
promising support for the integration of concepts for the general application of the ISC framework
and methods from social psychology’s domain to language attitudes research and at the same
of Implicit Social Cognition (ISC) into language time cautions against mischaracterizing implicit
attitudes research (e.g., Pantos & Perkins, 2013). attitudes.
The ISC’s characterization of attitudes as either
implicit (immediate) or explicit (thoughtful), its
compatible methods for measuring these two BACKGROUND
separate attitude constructs, and its corresponding
attitude processing models (e.g., the Associative- Over the past half century or more, traditional
Propositional Evaluation [APE] Model; [Gawron- language attitudes studies have produced a rich
ski & Bodenhausen, 2006]) present a consistent body of literature and a variety of findings,

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch005

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

establishing quantitatively—and not inconse- membership status (Bresnahan & Kim, 1993;
quentially—that reactions to foreign accented Bresnahan et al., 2002; Giles, Hewstone, Ryan &
speech are complex. Specifically, traditional Johnson, 1987; Reid & Giles, 2005; Ryan, 1983;
language attitudes research have confirmed not White & Li, 1991) and are therefore at the very
only the existence of a general negative affect core of intergroup behavior. The power of group
toward nonstandard accents (e.g., Gluszek & membership was also studied in narrower focus
Dovidio, 2010; Lambert, 1967; Rubin & Smith, in many traditional language attitudes studies that
1990), but also a variety of reactions to specific, considered reactions to specific, identified foreign
identified accents, attributing this variation to a nationalities, including Mexican (Ryan, Carranza
number of factors, including reaction type (e.g., & Moffie, 1977); Malaysian (Gill, 1994); Japanese
affective and evaluative reactions), speaker trait (Cargile & Giles, 1998); Chinese (Cargile, 1997);
(e.g., solidarity [kindness, friendliness, etc.] and Mexican, Lebanese and German (Frumkin, 2007);
status [competence, intelligence, etc.]) (Cargile & and Italian, Norwegian, and Eastern European
Giles, 1997; Ryan, 1982), the aggressiveness of (Mulac, Hanley & Prigge, 1974).
the message (Cargile & Giles, 1997), stereotypes
associated with the speaker’s nationality (Frum- Problems with the
kin, 2007; Kristiansen, 2001), and the degree of Traditional Approach
nonstandardness of the speaker’s accent (Brennan
& Brennan, 1981; Bresnahan, Ohashi, Nebashi, While this body of research provides a wealth of
Liu, & Shearman, 2002). Interestingly, the idea information about a general anti-foreign bias and
of a general negative affect was never reconciled reactions to specific accents, it fails to provide a
with the sometimes positive reactions revealed consistent theoretical framework that explains
in these studies of reactions to specific accents. the characterization and formation of these reac-
The general bias against nonstandard lan- tions. That is not to say that explanations have
guage varieties has led to the conclusion that not been posited. Several studies relied on the
nonstandard accents are generally dispreferred Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM; Petty &
(e.g., Gluszek & Dovidio, 2010; Lambert, 1967; Cacioppo, 1986) to conceptualize the process-
Rubin & Smith, 1990). This finding seems to ing of language attitudes (e.g., Frumkin, 2007;
explain the significant negative social, political, Giles, Williams, Mackie & Rosselli, 1995). The
judicial, and economic consequences potentially ELM, however, was never intended to describe
suffered by individuals who speak with a nonna- the cognitive processing of anything other than
tive accent (Kinzler, Shutts, DeJesus & Spelke, persuasive messages, a content limitation that
2009; Lippi-Green, 1997; Matsuda, 1991). For makes the model unsuitable as an explanation for
the underlying cause of these negative reactions, language attitude formation, in general.
researchers have relied historically on concepts In addition to the ELM, a number of research-
from Social Identity Theory (SIT; Tajfel & Turner, ers—largely working independently of one another
1986/2004) and its notions that social identity and independently of general attitudes research—
is derived from group membership, that social have posited a variety of original processing
interaction is essentially intergroup interaction, models. Notably, Cargile and colleagues (1994)
and that the negative reactions results from postulated a theoretical model of social processes
relative unfavorable comparison of outgroup involved in the formation of language attitudes that
members (Turner & Giles, 1981). These studies emphasizes the role of perceived cultural factors
demonstrated that language attributes signal group on listener attitudes and evaluations. Bradac and

105

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

colleagues (2001) present a similar theoretical cally has been indirect questioning based on the
model with the addition of an expanded nodal net- speaker evaluation paradigm (e.g., Cargile et al.,
work that includes a distinction between automatic 1994). Working within this paradigm, researchers
and controlled information processing, following avoid direct questions about language, preferring
Greenwald and Banaji (1995). Bradac, however, instead ‘indirect questions’ about the speaker (e.g.,
also proposes the use of ELM to explain how it- about the speaker’s competence, friendliness,
erations of the nodal network are spread. Neither etc.). It was believed that such questions were
of these models was ever generally endorsed by less sensitive to reflection and social desirability
other researchers or supported by quantitative biases than direct questions about the speaker’s
language attitudes studies. language. As such, researchers mistakenly be-
Perhaps symptomatic of the lack of a consistent lieved these indirect questions provided insight
theoretical basis for understanding language at- into the participant’s affective—as opposed to
titudes, terminology is inconsistently and impre- the participant’s thoughtful—attitudes towards
cisely used in the traditional language literature. the language. Attitudes research conducted within
These inconsistencies are not limited to differences the ISC framework, however, has established that
in usage by different researchers. Even within the eliciting responses to questions, regardless of
same study, it is not uncommon to find a variety whether the questions are characterized as direct
of loosely defined terms used interchangeably. In or indirect, necessarily evokes participant intro-
one study, for example, the terms emotionally and spection (Nosek et al., 2005; 2007). Introspection
affectively are used equivalently without explana- requires cognitive processing. Consequently, this
tion, as are the terms evaluatively, thoughtfully, ‘indirect questions’ method cannot access anything
and cognitively, to describe listener reactions other than a participant’s thoughtfully processed
to a speaker’s accent and message (Cargile & attitudes. In other words, the attitudes captured by
Giles, 1997). The use of cognitive for thoughtful these ‘indirect questions’ were not the affective
is particularly confusing, because it causes the reactions the researchers had hoped to capture.
reader to speculate as to whether the authors are
suggesting that the contrasting affective reactions Implicit Social Cognition as
involve no cognitive processing at all. Thus, while a Promising Solution
the traditional literature has established the exis-
tence of biased reactions and their social costs, a Outside the purview of traditional language
clear and consistent understanding of the nature attitude research, the concepts and methodolo-
of listener reactions to foreign accented speech gies suggested by the domain of Implicit Social
(i.e., emotional, affective, evaluative, cognitive, Cognition (ISC; Greenwald et al., 2002) provide
etc.) and an explanation of the manner in which a clear, coherent, and consistent understanding of
they are cognitively processed (i.e., automatically, attitudes and their formation. Applying this frame-
thoughtfully, simultaneously, concurrently, etc.) work to linguistic analysis would not only bring
were never established. these qualities to language attitudes research but
The lack of a clear conceptual framework in would also align language attitudes studies with
traditional language attitudes research has also broader attitudes research and eliminate the need
led to a number of methodological inconsisten- to theorize why reactions to language variation
cies. The preferred methodology for quantitative should be analyzed differently from other attitudes.
experimental language attitudes research histori- Appropriately, applying ISC to language attitudes

106

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

research would place it in context with research can yield different attitudes toward the same at-
conducted on social judgment and decision- titude object, and that these attitudes can be held
making (Bodenhausen & Todd, 2010), prejudice concurrently by the individual toward the same
and stereotyping (Amodio & Mendoza, 2010), and object. The ISC domain thus provides the field of
social justice (Payne & Cameron, 2010). language attitudes research with what it has been
ISC maintains that implicit attitudes and ex- lacking: a clear characterization of attitude con-
plicit attitudes are two separate attitude constructs structs, a consistent cognitive processing model,
associated with affective (impulsive/immediate) and an appropriate methodology that aligns with
and evaluative (reflective/thoughtful) reactions the theory.
formed by distinct cognitive processes (Strack &
Deutsch, 2004; Wittenbrink & Schwarz, 2007). Applying ISC to Language Attitudes
Implicit attitudes are unavoidable, immediate
reactions based on pre-existing associations and One example of the application of the ISC to lan-
stereotypes held by the individual, while explicit guage attitudes research involves an assessment
attitudes are thoughtful reactions formed through of reactions to foreign accented speech (Pantos
more extensive cognitive processing (Greenwald & Perkins, 2013). By using separate methods
& Banaji, 1995). Consistent with the foregoing, for measuring each of the attitude constructs in
the Associative Propositional Evaluation (APE) accordance with ISC theories, this study shows
Model (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006)—an that a listener’s reaction to language variation is
attitudes processing model—explains attitude comprised of both implicit and explicit attitudes,
formation as occurring through two separate, but which may diverge. Furthermore, this study found
potentially interactive, mental processes, referred the existence of a proportionally oppositional
to as associative and propositional. Correspond- relationship between the two attitude constructs,
ing to implicit attitudes, associative processing indicating an interaction between them. Both of
is performed immediately and involves affective these findings support the integration of the ISC
reactions based on the pre-existing salient as- theoretical framework and methodologies, as well
sociations the listener possesses. Because they as the APE Model, into the study of language
cannot be captured using methods that require attitudes.
introspection, implicit attitudes can be captured
only by using indirect measures (Nosek et al.,
2007) and implicit measurement tasks such as METHOD
the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald,
McGhee & Schwartz, 1998), which measures The study measured participants’ implicit (with an
relative reaction time in stimulus-to-attribute audio IAT) and explicit (with self reports) attitudes
sorting tasks. Propositional processing, which toward U.S. and foreign accented speech (Pantos
must be invoked, involves thoughtful reactions & Perkins, 2013). Although there are a number
and yields explicit attitudes. Because they involve of methodologies associated with ISC—includ-
evaluation and are thus subject to cognitive con- ing the Go/No-go Association Task (Nosek &
trol (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter & Cohen, Banaji, 2001), Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (De
2001), explicit attitudes can be measured with Houwer, 2003), and several iterations of the IAT
self reports. Importantly, the APE Model allows such as the Single-block IAT (Teige-Mocigemba,
that these different types of cognitive processes Lauer, & Rothermund, 2008) and the Recoding-

107

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

free IAT (Rothermund, Teige-Mocigemba, Gast, Participants


& Wentura, 2009)—the present study employed a
standard IAT for several reasons. First, a standard A total of 165 participants took part in this study.
IAT that measures relative attitudes towards two The number of participants was determined by
targets was appropriate for a comparison study of power calculations based on an alpha of .05, a
one accent against another. In addition, the IAT (1 - β) = .80, a standard deviation of 1, and an
has been repeatedly tested, and finally, the IAT error standard deviation of 1 for a linear regression
enjoys a high level of measurement reliability analysis. Participants were sought from a univer-
(Bodenhausen & Todd, 2010). sity’s graduate and undergraduate population.
The study, set within the context of a hypotheti- Participation was voluntary, and a fee of $10 per
cal medical malpractice trial, consisted of three participant was paid in compensation. Participants
tasks: 1) an audio IAT; 2) a self report; and 3) a ranged in age from 17-22 years of age, with an
confirmation task. The tasks asked participants to average age of 20. Of the participants, 114 were
compare the speech of two actors: a native U.S. women and 51 were men; 63 self identified their
English speaker from the mid-Atlantic region, race as ‘Caucasian/white/European’, 40 as ‘Asian/
and a Korean native, who moved to the U.S. at Chinese/Taiwanese’, 20 as ‘Hispanic/Mexican/
the age of 30 to attend graduate school and is Latino’, and 17 as ‘African/African American/
fluent in English. The entire study was computer Black’. The majority of participants self identi-
based and took place in one of the university’s fied their nationality as ‘US/American’ (131); 34
behavioral laboratories. Participants sat alone in participants designated their nationality as foreign
a sound-controlled room in front of the screen (16 as ‘Chinese’; 4 as ‘Korean’; 4 as ‘Mexican’;
of a Windows™-based laptop computer with a 2 and ‘El Salvadoran’, 2 as ‘English’, and 6 as
built-in standard keyboard, a track pad, external ‘other’). Although all participants were proficient
mouse, and headphones plugged into the com- enough in English to attend a US university, 35
puter’s headphone port. All tasks were created indicated their native language to be something
using Inquisit™ software (Draine, 1998). The other than English (14 as ‘Chinese/Mandarin/
total time required for a participant to complete Cantonese’, 8 as ‘Spanish’, 6 as ‘Korean’, and 7
all three tasks was approximately 25 minutes: as another language).
approximately 10 minutes for Task 1, ten minutes
for Task 2, and five minutes for Task 3. Tasks Task 1: Implicit Associations
1 and 2 measured the participants’ implicit and Test (IAT)
explicit responses, respectively, to the audio
stimuli presented. Task 3 checked the behavioral The IAT was incorporated to provide a means to
motivations found in Tasks 1 and 2. A distraction measure participants’ associative, or affective,
task was inserted between Tasks 1 and 2, which responses to the stimuli presented. The language
consisted of a Rational Evaluation Inventory list stimuli consisted of recorded audio excerpts, in-
of 40 questions asking participants to rate to what cluding lexical items and collocations, taken from
degree they emotionally or thoughtfully answer the audio recordings used in Task 2 (at 2:25; 2
questions (Pacini & Epstein, 1999). Participants options; assistance first; training and experience;
were asked to supply demographic information it is my opinion; I have frequently encountered;
after completing Task 3. Approval and authori- perform charting; probability). These excerpts
zation from the university’s Institutional Review were equalized for loudness using SoundForge™
Board was obtained before the commencement of and trimmed to assure no delay at the start of each
the study and any data was collected. clip. The stimuli were repeated three times. This

108

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

use of audio stimuli was deemed essential to the made, a red X appeared on the computer screen
linguistic nature of the study, and represents an for 400 milliseconds. Subsequent trials started
expansion of this methodology, which in published 400 milliseconds after the previous response or
literature to-date has relied on visual stimuli such the disappearance of the red X. The assignment
as printed words and images. of screen position to positive or negative valence
As attribute stimuli, positive and negative was counterbalanced for the participant pool, so
valence words were used (marvelous, superb, that half the participants saw positive words on the
pleasure, beautiful, joyful, glorious, lovely, won- left and negative on the right, and half saw them
derful; tragic, horrible, agony, painful, terrible, reversed. To aid in sorting, attribute stimuli and
awful, humiliate, nasty). Instructions informed labels appeared in white font and target concepts
participants at the outset that they would hear a in green font consistently in all testing blocks.
recorded spoken word or word cluster through their
earphones and that they would have to categorize Task 2: Self-Report Measure
the stimulus as belonging to the attribute shown on
the left or right of the computer screen by pressing For the explicit attitudes (self report) portion of
the appropriate computer key. Participants catego- the study, participants were presented with audio
rized the stimulus by pressing the ‘E’ (for left) testimony of two actors portraying doctors, one
or ‘I’ (for right) key on the computer keyboard. the treating physician and the other an expert wit-
The four categories used in this study were: US ness who disagrees with the course of treatment.
and Foreign (target concepts), and good and bad Participants were randomly assigned to one of
(attributes). Participants were instructed to work four test condition groups: Group 1 heard both
quickly without sacrificing accuracy. They were testimonies produced by the U.S. English native;
told that this portion of their participation would Group 2 heard both testimonies produced by the
take no more than about 10 minutes. Korean native; Group 3 heard the testimony of the
In accordance with the methodology estab- treating physician produced by the U.S. native and
lished in previous IAT research (e.g., Greenwald the contradictory testimony of the expert produced
et al., 1998), the IAT was presented in five stages. by the Korean native; and Group 4 heard the tes-
The first stage was a practice stage, in which timony of the treating physician produced by the
participants were presented with each of the eight Korean native and the contradictory testimony of
positive and eight negative attribute stimuli, for a the expert produced by the U.S. native. All nec-
total of 16 trials. The second stage was also a prac- essary controls were implemented to neutralize
tice stage, in which participants were presented potential confounds, including those for speaker,
with each of the eight audio stimuli spoken by the script, voice, and order effects.
US accented physician and the eight audio stimuli Presentation of test condition was counterbal-
spoken by the Korean-accented physician, for a anced across participants to allow for analysis of
total of 16 trials. In the third stage, a total of 32 both accent and presentation order, and to test
trials were created from the combination of the first for a bias for order irrespective of accent. In all
two test blocks. The fourth stage repeated stage versions, the treating physician testifies first, fol-
two, but with the screen position of the attribute lowed by the expert witness. The two testimonies
stimuli reversed. In the fifth stage, the 32 trials are separated by the survey questions asking
from stage three were repeated, but with the re- participants to rate the speaker on a scale from 1
versed screen positions for the attributes, as shown to 11, low to high, for the 14 speaker trait vari-
in stage four. The order of the trials within each ables (believability, credibility, trustworthiness,
block was randomized. If incorrect selections were knowledge, expertise, intelligence, competence,

109

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

likeability, friendliness, warmth, judgment, per- accented speaker (M = 5.40, SD = 1.90, t = -2.64,
suasiveness, presentation style, and clarity). The p = .01). Further, a within-treatment ANOVA was
presentation order of variables was randomized not significant (F = 2.49, p = .12), suggesting that
across participants. After hearing both witness the Korean expert witness was preferred regard-
testimonies, participants were asked to indicate less of whether he testified for the defendant or
their preference for each witness in two ways. the plaintiff. No results obtained on the fairness
First, participants were asked to make a binary or speaker trait measures.
choice (‘which doctor would you side with in this
dispute?’). Second, they were asked to indicate IAT Results
on a Likert scale (1-11) to what extent they sided
with one witness over the other. The raw IAT data was aggregated and transformed
using the D measure (Greenwald, Nosek & Banaji,
Task 3: Self-Report 2003). The D measure may be interpreted as
equivalent to an effect size measure, similar to
Task 3 consisted of a second written self report Cohen’s d measure; however, Cohen’s d uses a
that measures participants’ reactions to a written pooled within-treatment standard deviation, while
statement regarding the outcome of the case, with the IAT’s D-measure uses a standard deviation
the purpose of checking participants’ reactions calculated from only the scores in both measure-
in Task 2. Because the APE Model assumes that ment blocks. Previous IAT studies (Greenwald
the associative and propositional processes can et al., 2003) recommend that data be eliminated
inform each other constantly, it is helpful to test for ostensibly random responses (latencies of less
explicit results twice for consistency and robust- than 300 ms for more than ten per cent of trials
ness. In Task 3, the participants were asked to in combined task blocks) or lapses of concentra-
pretend they were an alternate juror in the trial tion (latencies of greater than 10,000 ms). Seven
who heard all the testimony but could not vote on participants’ data were eliminated because they
the verdict. They were then asked for their reac- contained extreme numbers of sorting errors or for
tions to fictional case outcomes. It was expected being extreme outliers. Following conversion to
that fairness ratings in this task would match the the D measure, participants’ IAT responses were
explicit attitudes toward accent found in Task 2. submitted to a two-tailed T-Test. Overall, results
indicated an implicit bias in favor of the US ac-
cented speaker over the foreign accented speaker
RESULTS (DAmerican = .32, t = 10.42, p < .001). Additionally,
no differences obtained between the treatment
Explicit Results and control conditions (MTreatmentD = .29, MControlD
= .33, F = .43, p = .51), or within the treatment
The relative preference measure was recoded such conditions (MKoreanFstD = .29, MAmericanFstD = .30, F
that responses closer to 1 indicated preference = .01, p = .91), such that all respondents revealed
for the Korean accent and responses closer to 11 the same a priori US accent bias.
indicated a preference for the American accent,
with the midpoint of the scale (6 on a 1 to 11 Correlations between IAT
scale) indicating no preference for either. This Results and Explicit Findings
recoded measure was submitted to a two-tailed
T-test. Across both treatment conditions, analysis In order to examine the relationship between the
revealed a significant bias in favor of the Korean IAT measure of implicit accent bias and witness

110

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

preference, correlational analysis was conducted. attitudes are cognitively controllable, while im-
Interestingly, a significant correlation between plicit attitudes are not (Botvinick, Braver, Barch,
the IAT and witness preference obtained, such Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Nosek et al., 2007). The
that a stronger pro-US implicit bias predicted difference in the implicit and explicit measures
an increased likelihood of preferring the Korean found in this study potentially explains the seeming
expert witness explicitly (r = .26, p = .03). This inconsistency revealed in traditional language at-
result did not obtain for the control condition titudes studies between the general negative affect
that incorporated expert witnesses who both had toward foreign accented speech (e.g., Gluszek &
Korean accents (r = .02, p = .89) or American Dovidio, 2010) and the varying reactions to identi-
accents (r = -.08, p = .62) suggesting that the fied foreign accents (e.g., Cargile & Giles, 1997).
relationship between implicit accent attitude and Specifically, this study suggests that individu-
witness preference is only significant when the als determine accent foreignness quickly. The aver-
participant heard two different accents during age response time in the IAT task was 1.23 seconds
the testimony. and the average length of an iteration of the stimuli
This study’s IAT found that the participants’ was 1.25 seconds. Thus, before the completion of
implicit attitudes favor the US accented speaker a single iteration of the average stimulus, partici-
over the Korean accented speaker, confirming the pants had determined that the accent was foreign.
ISC’s basic tenet of the distinct nature of implicit This was enough to trigger an immediate reaction
and explicit attitudes. Additionally, the IAT pre- and for the participants to form an implicit attitude
dicted expert witness choice, such that participants about the speaker. Implicit attitudes are immedi-
that revealed a stronger pro-US bias on the IAT ate, difficult to control, and thus correspond to
were more likely to choose the Korean accented participants’ affective responses (Wittenbrink &
speaker, regardless of whether the Korean accented Schwarz, 2007). This study thus supports earlier
speaker was the defendant treating physician or language attitudes research that recognizes the
the expert witness for the plaintiff. Further, this existence of a general negative affect towards for-
effect only obtained when participants heard expert eign accents and the research that emphasizes the
testimony from both the US- and Korean accented importance of accent foreignness—irrespective of
witnesses. Because the explicit results obtained specific nationality identification—in determining
only in the accent comparison conditions, these ingroup/outgroup status (Lindemann, 2003). This
findings show that the explicit preference for one study adds to that research by providing a coherent
speaker over the other is related to accent and not label for those reactions—implicit attitudes—and
to witness role or to the order of accent presen- by demonstrating an effective method for measur-
tation. Additionally, the predictive oppositional ing them—the IAT.
relationship between the pro-US bias on the IAT At the same time, these results and the char-
and the likelihood of favoring the Korean accent acterization of the reaction as an implicit attitude
in the explicit measures suggest two important within the ISC framework allow for an individual
conclusions with regard to language attitudes. to form an additional attitude based on further re-
flection—an explicit attitude. This explicit attitude
may vary from the implicit attitude, as the results
DISCUSSION of this study indicate. Once again, this result is
consistent with earlier language attitudes studies
This study’s results underscore the difference that, perhaps unintentionally, evoked explicit at-
between the disparate natures of the two attitude titudes using the speaker evaluation paradigm.
constructs and support the argument that explicit Those studies indicated that participants could

111

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

exhibit a variety of reactions to various identified the measurement of both implicit and explicit at-
foreign accents based on a number of factors, titudes to provide a more complete picture of the
including nationality stereotypes, message con- individual’s attitudes, judgment, social perception,
tent, and the aggressiveness of the message (e.g., and potential behavior than measuring only one
Cargile & Giles, 1997). ISC can thus explain both of these attitude constructs to the exclusion of the
the findings of a general negative affect towards other (Rohner & Björklund, 2006).
foreign accents and the findings of potentially The findings of a predictive oppositional
positive reactions to specific accents, an explana- relationship between the two attitude constructs
tion lacking in the traditional language attitudes indicate that listeners can hypercorrect their ex-
research. By incorporating concepts and methods plicit attitude reporting. Such hypercorrection in
from ISC, then, the present study adds to tradi- attitude reporting has been shown to occur when
tional language attitudes research by defining and participants are concerned about perceptions of
distinguishing these different reactions as implicit their self presentation if they suspect their implicit
and explicit attitudes and by underscoring the attitudes might reveal a socially unacceptable bias
need for separate methodologies to capture the (Baron & Banaji, 2006; de Jong, van den Hout,
different attitude constructs. Rietbroek & Huijding, 2003; Lane et al., 2007).
Although different constructs, both implicit In the present study, almost half of the participant
and explicit attitudes are nonetheless authentic pool was comprised of students from an introduc-
attitudes; both are reflective of an individual’s tory sociolinguistics class that was learning about
reactions to the stimulus; and both are cognitively the negative consequences of unfounded biases
formed by the individual—albeit through differ- against foreign accented speech. If that is, in fact,
ent cognitive processes. It is, therefore, not the the reason for the bias in favor of the native Korean
case that implicit attitudes are any more ‘real’ speaker, then this result might indicate a type of
than explicit attitudes (Lane et al., 2007). The hypercorrection in attitude reporting. It should be
fact that the formation of explicit attitudes may noted that the proportional aspect of the predictive
be influenced directly or indirectly by social or oppositional result eliminates the possibility that
other external factors does not diminish their au- the explicit bias in favor of the Korean speaker
thenticity, as long as they are reported honestly. is related to the identification of the accent or
It is also important to emphasize that the differ- the status of the speaker. Accent identification
ence between the implicit bias in favor of the US or status considerations alone cannot explain a
accented speaker and the explicit bias in favor of higher likelihood of explicitly favoring the Korean
the foreign accented speaker does not indicate accented speaker when implicit reactions favor
a change in attitude. It is not the case that the the US accented speaker. The proportionally op-
divergent attitudes mean that the pro-US implicit positional attitude relationship between the two
attitudes changed to the pro-foreign explicit at- attitude constructs suggests there is an interaction
titudes between tasks. Implicit attitudes remain between them.
separate from, and continue to co-exist alongside,
the individual’s explicit attitudes (Rohner & Limits to the Conclusions
Björklund, 2006) and both can affect judgment That Can Be Drawn
and behavior (Perkins & Forehand, 2010). For
language attitudes study, these results indicate One unsettled question in the ISC literature is
that the same individual can process different at- whether individuals are aware of their implicit at-
titudes toward the same set of language traits and titudes. ISC developed from two different research
hold them concurrently, and consequently support traditions, each with a different characterization

112

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

of the attitude constructs and a different view on in the self report tasks. This is consistent with the
the issue of awareness. The attention research hypercorrection motivation posited in the stud-
tradition (e.g., Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977) char- ies discussed earlier. In the context of language
acterizes attitude processing as either automatic attitudes research, or at least the study discussed
or controlled. Automatic processing demands in this paper, it is not necessary to answer the
little cognitive effort, is difficult to suppress, and question of awareness. The central finding of
has no capacity limitations. Controlled process- the distinction between attitude types (implicit
ing demands a great deal of cognitive effort, is and explicit) remains supported by the research
voluntary, and is limited in capacity. Awareness regardless of whether or not the participants were
and/or consciousness are not central to attention aware of their implicit attitudes.
research, and the concepts are mentioned only
rarely in the literature. This model maintains that
well-learned associations, including prevailing CONCLUSION
social values and stereotypes, are activated auto-
matically (e.g., Fazio, 1995), raising the question The integration of concepts and methods from
of whether measures of automatic attitudes, such the domain of ISC can contribute significantly to
as sequential priming and the bogus pipeline ap- language attitudes research. Despite ambiguities
proaches, reveal the participant’s attitudes or social regarding claims about listeners’ awareness of
values and cultural beliefs. Significant variance their implicit attitudes, which should be withheld
in the measurement of automatic attitudes among until the issue is resolved in the implicit memory
individuals within the same society, however, literature, the ISC framework provides clear and
supports the view that these measures do indeed comprehensive explanations for a number of
capture individual and not social attitudes. questions left unanswered by traditional language
In contrast, the implicit memory tradition attitudes studies. Observations of variation in reac-
(e.g., Banaji, 2001) defines implicit attitudes as tions to the same language traits can be explained
introspectively unidentified (or misidentified) in- by ISC’s distinction between implicit and explicit
fluences of past experience on later performance. attitudes. A clearer understanding of the merits
The IAT is most closely associated with the implicit of employing different methods for measuring
memory tradition. This tradition’s definition has distinct attitude constructs, in particular those
developed into a view that participants are unaware methods established by researchers working within
of their implicit attitudes. Critics argue that the the ISC domain, ensures that the attitudes captured
implicit memory tradition originally made no such are the intended ones. In addition, a theory of
claim, and that it was the past experience that was cognitive processing consistent with the concepts
forgotten or suppressed in memory, and not the and methods of ISC resolves the question of how
attitude itself. The study discussed in this paper attitudes are formed. Finally, the adoption of the
supports the view that participants may, indeed, ISC framework aligns language attitudes research
be aware of their implicit attitudes, at least when with broader attitudes research, eliminating the
they participate in a study that seeks to elicit those need to invent a unique theory for the nature and
attitudes. In fact, several participants mentioned processing of language attitudes and explain why
in debriefing that they realized during the IAT language attitudes should be treated differently
that they might be biased against the Korean ac- from other attitudes.
cented speaker, so they were careful to save face

113

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

REFERENCES Bresnahan, M. I., & Kim, M. S. (1993). The impact


of positive and negative messages on change in
Amodio, D. M., & Mendoza, S. A. (2010). Im- attitude toward international teaching assistants.
plicit intergroup bias: Cognitive, affective, and Folia Linguistica, 27(3-4), 347–363. doi:10.1515/
motivational underpinnings. In B. K. Payne & B. flin.1993.27.3-4.347
Gawronski (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social
Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applica- Bresnahan, M. J., Ohashi, R., Nebashi, R., Liu,
tions (pp. 353–374). New York: Guildford Press. W. Y., & Shearman, S. M. (2002). Attitudinal
and affective response toward accented English.
Banaji, M. R. (2001). Iimplicit attitudes can be Language & Communication, 22(2), 171–185.
measured. In H. L. Roediger, J. S. Nairne, I. doi:10.1016/S0271-5309(01)00025-8
Neath, & A. Surprenant (Eds.), The Nature of
Remembering: Essays in Remembering Robert Cargile, A. C. (1997). Attitudes toward Chinese-
G. Crowder (pp. 117–150). Washington, DC: accented speech: An investigation in two contexts.
American Psychological Association. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 16(4),
434–444. doi:10.1177/0261927X970164004
Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The de-
velopment of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race Cargile, A. C., & Giles, H. (1997). Understand-
evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. ing language attitudes: Exploring listener affect
Psychological Science, 17(1), 53–58. doi:10.1111/ and identity. Language & Communication, 17(3),
j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x PMID:16371144 195–217. doi:10.1016/S0271-5309(97)00016-5

Bodenhausen, G. V., & Todd, A. R. (2010). Auto- Cargile, A. C., & Giles, H. (1998). Language
matic aspects of judgment and decision making. In attitudes toward varieties of English: An
B. K. Payne & B. Gawronski (Eds.), Handbook of American-Japanese context. Journal of Applied
Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, Communication Research, 26(3), 338–356.
and Applications (pp. 278–294). New York: doi:10.1080/00909889809365511
Guildford Press. Cargile, A. C., Giles, H., Ryan, E. B., & Bradac,
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., J. J. (1994). Language attitudes as a social pro-
Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict cess: A conceptual model and new directions.
monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Language & Communication, 14(3), 211–236.
Review, 108(3), 624–652. doi:10.1037/0033- doi:10.1016/0271-5309(94)90001-9
295X.108.3.624 PMID:11488380 De Houwer, J. (2003). The extrinsic affec-
Bradac, J. J., Cargile, A. C., & Hallett, J. S. (2001). tive Simon task. Experimental Psychology,
Language attitudes: Retrospect, conspect and 50(2), 77–85. doi:10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.77
prospect. In W. P. Robinson & H. Giles (Eds.), PMID:12693192
The New Handbook of Language and Social de Jong, P. J., van den Hout, M. A., Rietbroek,
Psychology (pp. 137–155). Chichester, UK: John H., & Huijding, J. (2003). Dissociations between
Wiley & Sons, Ltd. implicit and explicit attitudes toward phobic
Brennan, E. M., & Brennan, J. S. (1981). Ac- stimuli. Cognition and Emotion, 17(4), 521–545.
cent scaling and language attitudes: Reactions to doi:10.1080/02699930302305
Mexican American English speech. Language Draine, S. C. (1998). Inquisit [Computer Soft-
and Speech, 24(3), 207–221. ware]. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software.

114

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L.
& Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in auto- A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D.
matic activation as an unobtrusive measure of S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes,
racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psycho-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), logical Review, 109(1), 3–25. doi:10.1037/0033-
1013–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013 295X.109.1.3 PMID:11863040
PMID:8531054
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz,
Frumkin, L. (2007). Influences of accent and J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences
ethnic background on perceptions of eyewitness in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association
testimony. Psychology, Crime & Law, 13(3), Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
317–331. doi:10.1080/10683160600822246 chology, 74(6), 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.74.6.1464 PMID:9654756
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006).
Associative and propositional processes in evalua- Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R.
tion: An integrative review of implicit and explicit (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit As-
attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), sociation Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm.
692–731. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
PMID:16910748 85(2), 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197
PMID:12916565
Giles, H., Hewstone, M., Ryan, E. B., & Johnson,
P. (1987). Research on language attitudes. In H. Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke,
U. Ammon, N. Dittmar, K. J. Mattheier (Eds.), E. S. (2009). Accent trumps race in guiding
Sociolinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 585-597). Berlin: children’s social preferences. Social Cognition,
Walter de Gruyter. 27(4), 623–634. doi:10.1521/soco.2009.27.4.623
PMID:21603154
Giles, H., Williams, A., Mackie, D. M., & Ros-
selli, F. (1995). Reactions to Anglo- and Hispanic- Kristiansen, G. (2001). Social and linguistic ste-
American-Accented speakers: Affect, identity, reotyping: A cognitive approach to accents. Es-
persuasion, and the English-only controversy. tudios Ingleses De La Universidad Complutense,
Language & Communication, 15(2), 107–120. 392(9), 129–146.
doi:10.1016/0271-5309(94)00019-9
Lambert, W. E. (1967). A social psychology
Gill, M. (1994). Accent and stereotypes: of bilingualism. The Journal of Social Issues,
Their effect on perceptions of teachers and 23(2), 91–109. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1967.
lecture comprehension. Journal of Applied tb00578.x
Communication Research, 22(4), 349–361.
Lane, K. A., Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Gre-
doi:10.1080/00909889409365409
enwald, A. G. (2007). Understanding and using
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Im- the Implicit Association Test: IV: What we know
plicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and (so far) about the method. In B. Wittenbrink &
stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–26. N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit Measures of Attitudes
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4 PMID:7878162 (pp. 59–102). New York: The Guilford Press.

115

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

Lindemann, S. (2003). Koreans, Chinese or Payne, B. K., & Cameron, C. D. (2010). Divided
Indians? Attitudes and ideologies about non- minds, divided morals: How implicit social cogni-
native English speakers in the United States. tion underpins and undermines our sense of social
Journal of Sociolinguistics, 7(3), 348–364. justice. In B. K. Payne & B. Gawronski (Eds.),
doi:10.1111/1467-9481.00228 Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measure-
ment, Theory, and Applications (pp. 445–462).
Lippi-Green, R. (1997). English with An Accent:
New York: Guildford Press.
Language, Ideology, and Discrimination in the
United States. London: Routledge. Perkins, A. W., & Forehand, M. R. (2010a).
Implicit social cognition and indirect measures
Matsuda, M. J. (1991). Voices of America: Ac-
in consumer behavior. In B. Gawronski & B. K.
cent, antidiscrimination law, and a jurisprudence
Payne (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social Cog-
for the last Reconstruction. The Yale Law Journal,
nition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications
100(5), 1329–1407. doi:10.2307/796694
(pp. 535–547). New York: Guilford.
Mulac, A., Hanley, T. D., & Prigge, D. Y. (1974).
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Commu-
Effects of phonological speech foreignness upon
nication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral
three dimensions of attitude of selected American
Routes to Attitude Change. New York: Springer.
listeners. The Quarterly Journal of Speech, 60(4),
doi:10.1007/978-1-4612-4964-1
411–420. doi:10.1080/00335637409383250
Reid, S. A., & Giles, H. (2005). Intergroup rela-
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R.
tions: Its linguistic and communicative parameters.
(2005). Understanding and using the Implicit Asso-
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 8(3),
ciation Test: II: Method variables and construct va-
211–214. doi:10.1177/1368430205053938
lidity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
31(2), 166–180. doi:10.1177/0146167204271418 Rohner, J. C., & Björklund, F. (2006). Do self-
PMID:15619590 presentation concerns moderate the relationship
between implicit and explicit homonegativity mea-
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M.
sures? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47(5),
R. (2007). The Implicit Association Test at age
379–385. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00522.x
7: A methodological and conceptual review. In
PMID:16987207
J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Social Psychology and the
Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Rothermund, K., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Gast,
Processes (pp. 265–292). New York: Psychology A., & Wentura, D. (2009). Minimizing the
Press. influence of recoding in the IAT: The Recod-
ing-Free Implicit Association Test (IAT-RF).
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
rational and experiential information procesing
62, 84–98. doi:10.1080/17470210701822975
styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-
PMID:18609400
bias problem. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76(6), 972–987. doi:10.1037/0022- Rubin, D. L., & Smith, K. A. (1990). Effects of
3514.76.6.972 PMID:10402681 accent, ethnicity, and lecture topic on undergradu-
ates’ perceptions of nonnative English-speaking
Pantos, A. J., & Perkins, A. (2013). Measur-
teaching assistants. International Journal
ing implicit and explicit attitudes toward
of Intercultural Relations, 14(3), 337–353.
foreign accented speech. Journal of Lan-
doi:10.1016/0147-1767(90)90019-S
guage and Social Psychology, 32(1), 1, 3–20.
doi:10.1177/0261927X12463005

116

Implicit Social Cognition and Language Attitudes Research

Ryan, E. B. (1982). Prolegomena for developing a Wittenbrink, B., & Schwarz, N. (2007). In B. Wit-
social psychological theory of language attitudes. tenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit Measures
In E. B. Ryan & H. Giles (Eds.), Attitudes Toward of Attitudes (pp. 1–3). New York: Guilford.
Language Variation: Social and Applied Contexts
(pp. 208–223). London: Arnold.
Ryan, E. B. (1983). Social Psychological Mecha- KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS
nisms Underlying Native Speaker Evaluations
of Non-Native Speech. Studies in Second Lan- Affective Reactions: Impulsive reactions
guage Acquisition, 5(2), 148–159. doi:10.1017/ based on emotional responses.
S0272263100004824 APE (Associative Propositional Evalua-
tion) Model: An attitudes processing model that
Ryan, E. B., Carranza, M. A., & Moffie, R. W. explains attitude formation as occurring through
(1977). Reactions toward varying degrees of two separate, but potentially interactive, mental
accentedness in the speech of Spanish-English processes, referred to as associative and propo-
bilinguals. Language and Speech, 20, 267–273. sitional.
PMID:613187 Associative Processing: Unavoidable process-
Shiffrin, R., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled ing performed immediately and based on salient
and automatic human information processing: associations the listener already possesses.
II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and Attitudes: Personally held, enduring inclina-
a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), tions towards, or reactions to, an attitude object.
127–190. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127 Evaluative Processing: Processing invoked
by the individual that involves thoughtful reac-
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (2004). The social tions and the evaluation of impulsive reactions
identity theory of intergroup behavior. In J. T. as either consistent or inconsistent with other
Jost & J. Sidanius (Eds.), Political Psychology: relevant propositions.
Key Readings (pp. 276–293). New York: Psychol- Explicit Attitudes: Thoughtful reactions
ogy Press. formed through more extensive cognitive process-
Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., & Rother- ing than implicit attitudes.
mund, K. (2008). Minimizing method-specific Foreign Accent: Speech that exhibits phono-
variance in the IAT: The Single Block IAT. logical and prosodic traits not typically associated
European Journal of Psychological Assessment, with native speakers of a language.
24(4), 237–245. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.237 IAT (Implicit Attitudes Test): A method for
accessing and measuring implicit attitudes based
Turner, J. C., & Giles, H. (1981). Introduction: on relative reaction time in stimulus-to-attribute
The social psychology of intergroup behaviour. sorting tasks.
In J. C. Turner & H. Giles (Eds.), Intergroup Be- Implicit Attitudes: Unavoidable, immediate
haviour. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers, Ltd. reactions based on pre-existing associations and
White, M. J., & Li, Y. (1991). Second- stereotypes held by an individual.
language fluency and person perception in ISC (Implicit Social Cognition): A theoretical
China and the United States. Journal of Lan- domain of social psychology that maintains that
guage and Social Psychology, 10(2), 99–113. implicit attitudes and explicit attitudes are two
doi:10.1177/0261927X91102002 distinct attitude constructs.
Nonnative Accent: A foreign accent.

117
118

Chapter 6
Implicit Processes and
Emotions in Stereotype Threat
about Women’s Leadership
Gwendolyn A. Kelso
Boston University, USA

Leslie R. Brody
Boston University, USA

ABSTRACT
Stereotype threat about leadership ability may trigger emotional and cognitive responses that reduce
women’s leadership aspirations. This chapter reviews literature and presents a study on the effects of
implicit (covert) and explicit (overt) leadership stereotype threat on women’s emotions, power-related
cognitions, and behaviors as moderated by exposure to powerful female or male role models. Emotional
responses were measured using self-report (direct) and narrative writing (indirect) tasks. Undergraduate
women (n = 126) in the Northeastern U.S. were randomly divided into three stereotype threat groups:
none, implicit, and explicit. Implicit stereotype threat resulted in higher indirectly expressed (but not
self-reported) anxiety, behaviors that benefited others more than the self, and when preceded by exposure
to powerful female role models, higher self-reported negative emotion but also higher indirect positive
affect. Explicit stereotype threat resulted in higher indirect optimism, and when preceded by exposure
to powerful female role models, lower self-reported sadness but also lower implicit power cognitions.

INTRODUCTION business, and higher education in the United


States (US). Traditional Western, white, middle
Implicit (indirect and presumably at least partly class gender role stereotypes of women as com-
unconscious) cognitions and emotions may be munal and passive may influence attitudes and
activated by stereotype threat about leadership in beliefs that women are not qualified to be leaders.
women and contribute to the leadership gender Women themselves may internalize these attitudes,
disparity in many domains, including government, leading to lowered aspirations to attain powerful

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch006

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

positions. Given these conditions, it is important aspirations develop in a sociocultural context in


to understand the implicit and explicit emotions which traditional female roles are antithetical to
and cognitions that stereotype threat may activate. the traits ascribed to effective leaders (Eagly &
Stereotype threat is a process that can be implic- Karau, 2002). In the context of the predominantly
itly or explicitly initiated by situational cues that white, middle class culture in the US, traditional
highlight negative stereotypes about members of female gender roles describe and prescribe per-
social groups in various domains based on salient sonality characteristics for women that include
aspects of identity, e.g. gender, race, age, or ethnic- gentleness, passivity, communalism, emotionality,
ity. Stereotype threat has been shown to negatively and the idea that women “rarely act as leaders”
impact performance, emotions, and cognitions (Broverman, Vogel, Broverman, Clarkson, &
in the stereotyped domain (Aronson, Quinn, & Rosenkrantz, 1972; Rudman & Glick, 2008). In
Spencer, 1998; Marx & Stapel, 2006; Schmader contrast, traditional male gender roles describe
& Johns, 2003; Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999). men as being assertive, dominant, independent
Implicit stereotype threat is activated with subtle and rational (Koenig, Eagly, Mitchell, & Ris-
environmental cues such as being told that a test tikari, 2011); thus men are expected to be able
is diagnostic of one’s abilities in a stereotyped to lead by virtue of the personality qualities and
domain (e.g., an African American being told a test traits ascribed to the definition of masculinity.
measures intellectual ability; Steele & Aronson, Men and women alike endorse these cultural and
1995) or being a numerical minority representing stereotypical gender roles (Jost, Banaji, & Nosek,
the group about which there is a stereotype, (e.g., 2004; Ridgeway, 2001). Leaders are perceived to
being the only female in a room of males taking be stereotypically masculine; that is, leaders are
a math test; Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000). Explicit seen as similar to men but not to women and as
stereotype threat involves exposure to overt and being more agentic (a characteristic ascribed more
direct statements about the stereotype (e.g., being to men) than communal (a characteristic ascribed
told that men are stronger negotiators than women; more to women; Koenig, Eagly, Mitchell, & Ris-
Kray, Thompson & Galinsky, 2001). tikari, 2011). Further, studies have largely found
In this chapter, we (1) review the literature that women, when depicted in male-dominated
about, and (2) present a study examining (a) the domains such as sports coaching, business, and
effects of explicit and implicit leadership stereo- politics, were evaluated more negatively than men
type threat on women’s emotions (assessed using (Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992; Phelan &
both indirect and direct measures), power-related Rudman, 2010; Okimoto & Brescoll, 2010). A
cognitions, and behaviors, and (b) the potential study using an observational method of measur-
buffering effects of exposure to same-sex role ing attitudes found that male and female German
models on women’s reactions to stereotype threat. undergraduate students displayed more negative
emotional responses to female leaders compared to
male leaders in a variety of contexts (Koch, 2005).
BACKGROUND Given the predominantly negative attitudes
toward women in power, women tend to be more
Traditional Gender Role Stereotypes conflicted than men about possessing it (Lips,
2000; Fong & Tiedens, 2002). Lips (2000) found
Women in the US are under-represented in leader- that young women rated themselves less likely
ship roles despite women’s increased workforce to hold certain powerful positions (e.g., politi-
participation in recent decades (Eagly & Carli, cal leader) and to be less positive about holding
2004; Eagly & Carli, 2007). Women’s career these positions than did young men. Women who

119

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

imagined themselves in high status positions re- and verbal ability; it was elicited by describing
ported simultaneously experiencing positive and a verbal Graduate Records Examination (GRE)
negative emotions when compared with women test as “diagnostic” prior to the start of the test.
in low status positions, demonstrating emotional African American students under stereotype threat
ambivalence about their high status (Fong & Tie- made more errors and attempted to answer fewer
dens, 2002). Internalization of traditional female problems than Caucasians and African American
gender roles may contribute to women finding students to whom the test had not been described
leadership roles less attractive (Lips, 2000), seeing as diagnostic of verbal ability (Steele & Aronson,
themselves as less of a good-fit with leadership 1995). Stereotype threat has since been shown to
roles (Bosak, & Sczesny, 2008), and being less undermine performance in task domains nega-
likely to attempt to achieve powerful positions tively stereotyped for a number of social groups,
(Van Vianen & Fischer, 2002). such as women and math performance (Inzlicht
Implicit and unconscious processes may play & Ben-Zeev, 2000; Spencer, Steele, & Quinn,
a role in maintaining incongruent associations 1999), Latino/as and intellectual task performance
between power and femininity (Haines & Kray, (Aronson, Quinn, & Spencer, 1998; Gonzales,
2005). Because gender role socialization begins Blanton, & Williams, 2002), and white men and
early in life, implicit gender stereotypes are de- athletic performance (Stone, Lynch, Sjomeling,
veloped over time so that the association of men & Darley, 1999). Lowered performance has been
and women with stereotypic traits, abilities, and found to relate to anxiety (Marx & Stapel, 2006),
roles becomes automatic, even when participants higher cognitive loading (Schmader & Johns,
overtly reject these traditional beliefs (Nosek, 2003; Croizet, Despres, Gauzins, Huguet, Leyens,
Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002; Rudman, Greenwald, & Meot, 2004), and physiological arousal (Hoyt &
& McGhee, 2001). Rudman and Kilianski (2000) Blascovich, 2010). The ways in which these and
demonstrated that the difficulty women had in other factors contribute to compromised perfor-
seeing themselves and other women as capable mance are not yet well understood. Few studies
of being leaders functioned outside of conscious have examined the effects of stereotype threat on
awareness. In this study with racially diverse emotions specific to the task and domain. Further,
undergraduate participants, women endorsed less no studies have investigated stereotype threat ef-
explicit prejudice toward female authority figures fects on implicit or indirect emotional, cognitive,
via self-report than did men. However, when at- or on gender-role related behavioral processes.
titudes were assessed with the Implicit Association
Test (IAT), comparing response times between Leadership Ability Stereotype Threat
positive and negative adjectives with images of
female authority, women’s attitudes did not differ Studies have shown that mentioning gender role-
from men’s. The difference in outcomes between related stereotypes about women’s leadership
implicit and explicit measures makes it especially and negotiation abilities can produce negative
important to measure the effects of stereotype effects on women’s expectations, aspirations,
threat indirectly as well as directly, in order to and self-views (Burnette, Pollack, & Hoyt, 2010;
capture automatic and habitual responses. Davies, Spencer, & Steele, 2005; Hoyt, Johnson,
Murphy, & Skinnell, 2010; Latu, Mast, Lammers,
Stereotype Threat & Bombari, 2013). In one study, after viewing
commercials portraying women behaving in
The first empirical test of stereotype threat involved gender-stereotypic ways to gender-stereotypic
a negative stereotype about African Americans products (e.g., a young woman jumping for joy on

120

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

her bed after putting on acne medicine) or neutral 2006). Kray, Thompson, and Galinsky (2001)
commercials that did not portray humans and ad- found that undergraduate women for whom gender
vertised non-gender-stereotypic products (e.g., cell was made salient prior to negotiations returned less
phones, insurance), women who had viewed the advantageous outcomes for themselves compared
gender-stereotypic commercials showed greater with men and women not under stereotype threat.
interest in a lower status, problem-solver position
than in a leadership role compared with women Stereotype Threat and Emotion
who had viewed neutral commercials (Davies et
al., 2005). In another study of university students, Stereotype threat in a number of domains, e.g.
compared with men and women not under stereo- gender and math performance, race and verbal
type threat, women reported lower expectations performance, and gender and leadership, has
of good outcomes when told their performance been associated with emotions such as anxiety
on a negotiation task would be diagnostic of their (Aronson et al., 1999; Oswald & Harvey, 2001;
negotiation ability (Kray, Thompson, & Galinsky, Steele, 1997), dejection and frustration (Keller
2001). Gustafsson & Björklund (2008) examined & Dauenheimer, 2003; Marx & Stapel, 2006),
the effects of stereotype threat about salary ne- and depressive affect (Hoyt & Blascovich, 2010).
gotiation ability on gendered self-stereotyping in However, some studies have found that stereotype
a sample of Swedish business school students. threat was not associated with anxiety (Marx &
Women who were told that their negotiation Stapel, 2006; Osborne, 2001; Spencer et al., 1999).
ability would be assessed by a negotiation task These studies have relied on self-reports of emo-
rated themselves as more feminine in their trait tion at different time points during the stereotype
characteristics compared to women who were threat manipulation and task. In contrast, another
told the negotiation was unrelated to ability. En- study measured anxiety using observational non-
trepreneurial intentions in predominantly white verbal ratings as well as self-reports in gay men
female undergraduate business majors were lower interacting with pre-school-aged children under
when stereotypes of successful entrepreneurs were stereotype threat about homosexuality. Nonverbal
implicitly presented as “aggressive, risk taking, ratings of anxiety (e.g., observer-coded behaviors
and autonomous” than in women who were not such as fidgeting, chewing on lip, biting nails,
exposed to the stereotype (Gupta, Turban, & nervous smiling, and averting eyes) were higher
Bhawe, 2008). for homosexual men who were implicitly cued
Being told that women are less likely to suc- by identifying their sexual orientation prior to
ceed in stereotypically male domains has been interacting with children than for homosexual
found to compromise women’s performance men who did not identify their sexual orientation
on leadership-related tasks such as managerial (Bosson, Haymovitz, & Pinel, 2004). Further,
decision-making (Bergeron, Block, & Echten- nonverbal anxiety mediated the effects of stereo-
kamp, 2006) and negotiation (Kray, Thompson, type threat on the quality of the men’s childcare
& Galinsky, 2001). Undergraduate women study performance. Although nonverbal anxiety rat-
participants who were told that they were replac- ings were found to be significantly higher in the
ing a previous manager described as a man with stereotype threat condition, self-report ratings of
stereotypically male characteristics made fewer anxiety were similar across conditions, showing a
and poorer decisions compared with women told difference based on measurement method. Indirect
the previous manager was a woman with feminine or implicit (as opposed to direct and explicit self-
characteristics (Bergeron, Block, & Echtenkamp, report) measures of emotion may be less subject

121

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

to impression management and may help in bet- vation and determination to prove the stereotype
ter understanding the emotions that mediate the wrong. This may be true especially for women with
effects of stereotype threat, some of which may high efficacy in the domain in question. Hoyt &
be unconscious. Blascovich (2007) found that women with high
levels of leadership self-efficacy responded to an
Moderators of Stereotype Threat explicitly activated stereotype by performing at a
higher level in a virtual reality conference meeting
Explicit Activation of Stereotypes than women with low self-efficacy. However, even
while demonstrating high levels of performance
The manner in which a negative stereotype is and self-evaluation, high self-efficacy female
presented, either implicitly (e.g., stating that per- leaders still exhibited physiological signs of threat
formance on the upcoming task will be diagnostic (i.e. heart rate increases) when exposed to explicit
for the stereotyped skill deficit) or explicitly (e.g., stereotype threat (Hoyt & Blascovich, 2010).
stating that men have certain personality traits Further support for the differential effects of
that make them better leaders), has been found to explicitly versus implicitly activated stereotypes
moderate its effects. Explicit stereotype threat has was found in a meta-analysis that showed that ex-
been found to boost performance in some domains, plicitly activated stereotypes about women’s math
and has sometimes been referred to as stereotype performance were associated with a reduction of
reactance (Kray, Thompson, & Galinsky, 2001; detrimental effects on women’s math performance
Hoyt & Blascovitch, 2007). Kray, Thompson, as compared to implicit, threat-activating cues.
and Galinsky (2001) demonstrated the stereotype Implicit threat showed the largest negative effects
reactance effect with women who were told that on women’s math performance (Nguyen & Ryan,
their performance on a negotiation task was linked 2008). Other work, however, has found that both
to masculine characteristics such as rationality and implicitly and explicitly activated stereotypes
assertiveness, and that there were usually gender lowered math performance in women (Smith &
differences in negotiation abilities. The women White, 2002). In brief, some, but not all, research
who were exposed to the explicit stereotype nego- suggests that explicit activation of threats may
tiated more effectively than women who had not trigger reactance and improved performance,
been exposed to stereotype threat. Hoyt, Johnson, perhaps mediated by emotions and cognitions
Murphy, and Skinnell (2010) conducted several such as optimism and positive emotions that pro-
studies with undergraduate women where “bla- mote engagement or hopeful expectations about
tant,” or explicit activation of the stereotype about leadership positions.
women and leadership led women to respond with
higher self-appraisals of their leadership activity Powerful Female Role Models
performance, self-esteem, and lower self-rated
anxiety than women for whom there was no ste- Exposure to powerful same-sex role models in
reotype activation. Explicitly-activated negative the gender-stereotyped domain has sometimes,
stereotypes may prevent detrimental effects on but not always, been found to buffer the effects of
performance by allowing the individual to utilize stereotype threat. Female role models have been
cognitive defense mechanisms or to deliberately protective for female students primarily in test
engage in behaviors that are in contrast to those as- performance and aspirations in the fields of math
sociated with the negative stereotype (Greenwald and science, but have been less protective in the
& Banaji, 1995; Kray, Thompson, & Galinsky, leadership domain where effective leadership traits
2001), perhaps by mobilizing participants’ moti- go hand-in-hand with stereotypical masculine per-

122

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

sonality traits. Presenting high-achieving female studies in which powerful female role models
role models portrayed as good at math buffered are presented with whom participants can iden-
stereotype threat on women’s math performance tify. These studies have shown that these models
so that their scores were no different than test- protect participants’ leadership aspirations and
takers not under stereotype threat (Marx & Roman, attitudes (Asgari, Dasgupta, & Stout, 2012). For
2002). In another study, reading about individual example, college females rated themselves higher
women who succeeded in several male-dominated as potential leaders after exposure to a high-level
fields buffered the effects of stereotype threat on role model with whom they identified compared
a math test (McIntyre, Paulson & Lord, 2003). with males exposed to male, female, or no role
Female students who were exposed to images of models and females exposed to male or no role
female scientists performed better on a science test models (Lockwood, 2006). Similarly, in the As-
than those who were exposed to images of male gari, Dasgupta, and Stout (2012) study, women
scientists (Good, Woodzicka & Wingfield, 2010). responded positively to role models who were
Similarly, undergraduate women who viewed “ordinary individuals similar to other women who
female science professors who were presented as attained success through hard work, discipline, and
role models reported having pro-science career persistence.” A recent study showed that women
aspirations and more positive attitudes (measured who were exposed to a powerful female role
both implicitly and explicitly) about science ca- model (e.g., Hilary Clinton) gave higher quality
reers for women compared to women who were and lengthier speeches to a mixed-gender group
not exposed to female science professors (Young, in a virtual environment than women exposed
Rudman, Buettner, & McLean, 2013). to a male or no role model (Latu, Mast, Lam-
In leadership domains, however, powerful or mers, & Bombari, 2013). These results point to
successful female role models tend to threaten the importance of identification with successful
women’s self-views. Women who were exposed counter-stereotypic women role models and to
to elite female leaders before performing a virtual complex implicit processes involved in perceptions
reality leadership task reported lower self-eval- of self and others in nontraditional roles that do
uations of their performance, higher feelings of not typically offer high social regard.
inferiority, and lower leadership role aspirations In sum, the literature suggests that exposure
compared to women exposed to elite male leaders to gender role stereotypes produces cognitive and
and lower-level female role models (Hoyt & Si- motivational consequences that have deleterious
mon, 2011). A study that involved the participants’ effects on self-view, performance, and aspirations
vision of themselves in a powerful role found in the leadership domain. The activation of these
that when a single female was portrayed in an gender-linked stereotypes is a form of stereotype
advertisement for a managerial position, women threat. Investigating factors that contribute to and
were less likely to see themselves as suitable for also help mitigate these deleterious effects may be
the position compared with women who were helpful for women’s self-views about leadership
shown an advertisement depicting both a male ability and aspirations. No systematic literature
and a female (Bosak & Sczesny, 2008). Women exists of how stereotype threat about leadership
who were primed with nontraditional roles (e.g., affects women’s emotions or power-related in-
female surgeon and male nurse) endorsed lower terpersonal behaviors measured either directly
self-association with leadership and showed less or indirectly.
interest in masculine jobs (Rudman & Phelan, The current study examined the effects of
2010). A few exceptions to the detrimental effects both implicitly and explicitly activated stereotype
of successful female leader role models include threat about leadership on power-related emotions,

123

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

cognitions, and gender role-related behavior in (as opposed to leadership roles) and who inter-
women. Our investigations were based on litera- acted with female (as opposed to male) partners
ture that has found specific types of behaviors incorporated more communal themes into their
and emotions to be associated with high and low memories. In the current study, we also measured
positions of power. High power is associated with the activation of implicit power/competition cog-
self-interested behavior, or acting in accordance nitions with a word-fragment completion task by
with one’s own goals and desires and against social calculating the ratio of words connoting power/
convention, in contrast with low power that in- competition to total words completed.
hibits self-interested action and is associated with
behaving in more situationally-contingent ways
(Galinsky, Gruenfeld, & Magee, 2003). Lottery METHOD
ticket allocation was used in the present study to
measure the extent to which participants allo- Participants
cated tickets to themselves (denoting high power
behaviors) versus others (denoting low power One hundred and twenty-six female college
behaviors). High power has also been found to be students from introductory psychology courses
associated with positive emotions such as pride, at a large private university in the Northeast par-
optimism, achievement and self-confidence that ticipated in the current study for course credit.
promote pursuit of goals and rewards. In contrast, Participants were 18.96 years old on average (SD
low power has been associated with negative = 2.43); their reported median parental income
emotions of guilt, sadness, tentativeness, fear, and was $75,000 to $100,000; 69.8% self-identified
anxiety that result from increased threat, social as Caucasian, 20.6% as Asian, 4.0% as Black, and
constraint, and vigilance (Keltner, Gruenfeld, & 5.6% as Hispanic. Of the entire sample, 12.7%
Anderson, 2003). were international students.
We measured explicit, or direct, emotions with
the Positive and Negative Affect Scales-Expanded Procedure
(PANAS-X; Watson & Clark, 1994), a self-report
measure, and we measured indirect, or implicit, Upon arrival to the laboratory, a female experi-
emotions by counting the frequency of emotion menter introduced participants to the study, stat-
words included in autobiographical narratives with ing that its purpose was to study leadership and
the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count - Second cognitive processes, and obtained informed con-
version program (LIWC; Pennebaker, Francis, & sent. Participants then completed a demographic
Booth, 2001). Autobiographical narratives reflect questionnaire and were randomly assigned to one
automatic and often unconscious cognitions, emo- of three experimental conditions: no stereotype
tions, and values associated with self-schemas, threat (44 participants), implicit stereotype threat
and have previously been used to measure implicit (36 participants), or explicit stereotype threat (46
emotions. For example, Nakash & Brody (2007) participants). Within each condition, half of the
used a similar narrative writing paradigm and participants completed the powerful female role
found that varying the level of participants’ power model matching task and the other half com-
as well as the gender of the person with whom pleted the powerful male role model matching
participants interacted in an experimental task task. Stereotype threat manipulations were then
significantly affected the themes and structure conducted, followed by the lottery allocation
of participants’ subsequent autobiographical task. When finished, participants completed the
memories. Participants who were assigned helping PANAS-X, the word completion task, and wrote

124

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

three autobiographical narratives: a memory of and an anonymous participant, and was told that
what happened in the experiment (neutral), of only half of participants in the study had the op-
being in a leadership position (agentic), and of portunity to make the allocation and that at the
being in a cooperative situation (communal). After end of data collection, one lottery winner would be
completion of the measures, participants were selected to receive a $25 gift certificate to a local
fully debriefed and thanked for their participation. restaurant. The experimenter left the room after
Exposure to role models was embedded in a reading the instructions and collected the tickets
matching task in which participants were asked when the participant indicated she was finished
to draw lines connecting names or occupations to and recorded the number of tickets that participants
images of powerful and recognizable individuals allocated to themselves. The Positive and Negative
(e.g., Hilary Clinton in the female role model Affect Scale – Expanded form (PANAS-X; Watson
condition and Bill Clinton in the male role model & Clark, 1994) is a 60-item self-report question-
condition) or to images of people in powerful naire that measures present-moment positive and
occupations (e.g., male or female police officer, negative emotions on a 5-point Likert-type scale.
man or woman dressed in business attire). The Individual emotions (displayed in Table 1) were
stereotype threat manipulation was embedded in averaged for total scores to create six subscales
the instructions to complete the lottery ticket al- for general positive affect, general negative affect,
location task. In all stereotype threat conditions, a fear, guilt, sadness, and self-assurance.
male experimenter read aloud the instructions for In the autobiographical writing task, partici-
the lottery ticket allocation with the description, pants were instructed to write three autobiographi-
“this is a task about leadership and the thinking cal narratives: (1) agentic: memory of being in a
processes involved in making decisions.” After position of power (e.g., being a camp counselor,
this introduction, participants in the no stereotype making a decision that influenced their friends,
threat condition proceeded to the task. Participants etc.), (2) communal: memory of participating in a
in the implicit stereotype threat condition were communal activity (e.g., working cooperatively on
told, “the activities you will participate in are a group project), and (3) neutral: their memory of
designed to see which individuals have leadership the experiment. The frequencies of specific emo-
abilities; your performance will be diagnostic tion words used in participants’ autobiographical
of your leadership abilities.” Participants in the writing were analyzed with the Linguistic Inquiry
explicit stereotype threat condition were told, and Word Count - Second version program (LIWC;
“men are more often in positions of leadership Pennebaker, Francis, & Booth, 2001). Word
than women, and this may be related to qualities frequencies (calculated as ratios based on total
associated with males, such as assertiveness and number of words per narrative) were tallied under
being able to take charge. Your performance here eight word categories: positive feeling, negative
will be diagnostic of your leadership abilities.” emotion, optimism, achievement, tentativeness,
The lottery ticket allocation task (adapted anger, sadness, and anxiety (see Table 1 for word
from Galinsky, Magee, Inesi, & Gruenfeld, 2006), examples). The word-fragment completion task
measured the extent to which participants acted in was used to assess the activation of the cognitive
accordance with low power, stereotypically female constructs of high power/competition. Twenty-
and socially-responsible behavior, rather than high four word fragments were created by the omission
power, stereotypically male and self-interested of one or more of the letters of a word; nine could
behavior, by allocating resources to others rather be completed as power- or competition-related
than to themselves. Each participant was given words. For example, _ o w e r could be completed
seven lottery tickets to allocate between herself as “power” or “lower,” c _ _ p _ _ _ t i v e could

125

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

Table 1. Subscales Created from the Positive and Negative Affects Scale - Expanded Version (PANAS-
X) and Examples of Words Included in Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) Word Categories

PANAS-X general affects:


Positive affect scale: active, alert, attentive, determined, enthusiastic, excited, inspired, interested, proud, strong
Negative affect scale: afraid, scared, nervous, jittery, irritable, hostile, guilty, ashamed, upset, distressed
PANAS-X (self-reported) affects:
Self-assurance scale: proud, strong, confident, bold, daring, fearless
Fear scale: afraid, scared, frightened, nervous, jittery, shaky
Guilt scale: guilty, ashamed, blameworthy, angry at self, disgusted with self, dissatisfied with self
Shyness scale: shy, bashful, sheepish, timid
Autobiographical narrative measure: LIWC word categories and examples of included words:
Achievement: earn, hero, win
Anger: hate, killed, annoyed
Anxiety: worried, fearful, nervous
Negative emotion: hurt, ugly, nasty
Optimism: hope, confident, positive
Positive feeling: love, nice, sweet
Tentativeness: maybe, perhaps, guess
Sadness: crying, grief, sad

be completed as “cooperative” or “competitive”. pleted. We also used eight two-way MANOVAs


These key word fragments were randomly inter- for repeated measures ((2) gender of role model
spersed among 15 neutral word fragments. The x (3) stereotype threat condition), with the three
proportion of the number of words connoting types of narratives (agentic, communal, neutral)
power and competition to total words completed as repeated measures. Outcome variables were
was created. This type of task has been previously the eight Pennebaker LIWC word categories
used to test of the activation of the constructs of including achievement, anger, anxiety, positive
power (Chen, Lee-Chai, & Bargh, 2001) and of feeling, negative emotion, optimism, sadness and
competition (Kay, Wheeler, Bargh, & Ross, 2004). tentativeness. Examples of words that are included
We analyzed the self-report emotion data, in each word category are shown in Table 1.
lottery ticket allocation behavior, and implicit
cognition data using a series of eight two-way
ANOVAs consisting of a 2 (gender of role model) RESULTS
x 3 (stereotype threat condition) design. Six of the
outcome variables (one in each ANOVA) were Stereotype Threat Main Effects
PANAS-X subscales: general positive affect,
general negative affect, sadness, self-assurance, Consistent with some previous research (Aronson
fear, and guilt. The remaining two outcomes were et al., 1999; Oswald & Harvey, 2001; Steele, 1997),
number of tickets allocated to self versus other in results revealed significantly higher implicit anxi-
the lottery ticket allocation and the proportion of ety (F(2, 120) = 5.20, p = .007, partial ŋ2 = .08)
high power word completions to total words com- and more lottery tickets allocated to others (F(2,

126

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

120) = 6.64, p = .002, ŋ2 = .10) under implicit ste- to show the impact of implicit stereotype threat on
reotype threat compared with the other stereotype heightening altruistic, communal behavior, which
conditions. Means and p-values for main effects tends to be stereotypically associated with women.
are displayed in Table 2. No differences in explicit Results also revealed that higher implicit op-
self-reported affect or in the word completion timism was expressed under explicit stereotype
task were found by stereotype condition. These threat (F(2, 120) = 3.86, p = .02, partial ŋ2 = .06).
findings, that implicit stereotype threat elicited Because optimism is a positive emotion theorized
implicit, but not self-reported, anxiety may help to be related to power (Anderson & Galinsky,
clarify the inconsistent findings about the as- 2006), this finding suggests a positive reactance-
sociation of anxiety with stereotype threat in the effect to explicit threat (Hoyt & Blascovitch, 2005;
literature; type of measurement may have critical Kray, Thompson, & Galinsky, 2001).
effects on results. Further, these results are the first

Table 2. Means, Standard Deviations, and ANOVA Results for Self-Reported Emotion (PANAS-X),
Word-fragment Completion, and Lottery Ticket Allocation Tasks by Stereotype Threat Condition and
Gender of Role Model

Mean (SD) ANOVA F


PANAS-X Subscales Female RM Male RM Total Threat RMb T x RMa
(T)a
Positive Affect Scale 0.03 1.19 2.43†
•      No threat 2.70 (.88) 2.55 (.78) 2.63 (.82)
•      Implicit threat
2.38 (.57) 2.96 (.60) 2.63 (.64)
•      Explicit threat
•      Total 2.65 (.82) 2.66 (.78) 2.65 (.79)
2.59 (.78) 2.71 (.73) 2.65 (.75)
Negative Affect Scale 0.59 4.48* 3.51*
•      No threat 1.50 (.42) 1.34 (.48) 1.42 (.45)
•      Implicit threat
1.72 (.70) 1.25 (.27) 1.48 (.57)
•      Explicit threat
•      Total 1.32 (.36) 1.41 (.50) 1.37 (.43)
1.50 (.51) 1.34 (.44) 1.42 (.48)
Fear Scale 0.89 4.22* 3.51*
•      No threat 1.48 (.57) 1.21 (.43) 1.34 (.52)
•      Implicit threat
1.76 (.84) 1.26 (.44) 1.51 (.70)
•      Explicit threat
•      Total 1.32 (.34) 1.46 (.65) 1.39 (.52)
1.50 (.61) 1.32 (.53) 1.41 (.58)
Guilt Scale 1.37 8.21** 1.58
•      No threat 1.31 (.51) 1.11 (.38) 1.21 (.46)
•      Implicit threat
1.65 (.86) 1.15 (.29) 1.40 (.70)
•      Explicit threat
•      Total 1.30 (.52) 1.20 (.37) 1.25 (.45)
1.41 (.65) 1.16 (.35) 1.28 (.53)
Sadness Scale 1.16 3.54† 3.18*

continued on following page

127

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

Table 2. Continued
Mean (SD) ANOVA F
PANAS-X Subscales Female RM Male RM Total Threat RMb T x RMa
(T)a
•      No threat 1.61 (0.61) 1.44 (.63) 1.52 (.62)
•      Implicit threat
2.01 (.77) 1.41 (.49) 1.71 (.70)
•      Explicit threat
•      Total 1.45 (.54) 1.57 (.77) 1.51 (.6)
1.67 (.67) 1.48 (.64) 1.57 (.66)
Self-Assurance Scale 0.34 0.23 0.85
•      No threat 1.58 (.83) 1.30 (.51) 1.44 (.70)
•      Implicit threat
1.63 (.67) 1.59 (.56) 1.61 (.61)
•      Explicit threat
•      Total 1.40 (.52) 1.46 (.53) 1.43 (.52)
1.53 (.68) 1.44 (.54) 1.48 (.61)
Shyness Scale 1.08 0.61 0.92
•      No threat 1.58 (.83) 1.30 (.51) 1.44 (.70)
•      Implicit threat
1.63 (.67) 1.59 (.56) 1.61 (.61)
•      Explicit threat
•      Total 1.40 (.52) 1.46 (.53) 1.43 (.52)
1.53 (.68) 1.44 (.54) 1.48 (.61)
Lottery Ticket Allocation to Self 6.64** 1.67 1.07
•      No threat 4.18 (1.10) 4.18 (1.56) 4.18 (1.33)
•      Implicit threat
3.05 (1.68) 3.11 (1.59) 3.08 (1.62)
•      Explicit threat
•      Total 3.70 (1.43) 4.57 (1.53) 4.13 (1.53)
3.67 (1.46) 4.00 (1.65) 3.84 (1.56)
Word-Fragment Task (High-Power Words) 2.38 0.58 3.16*
•      No threat .09 (.05) .07 (.05) .08 (.05)
•      Implicit threat
.06 (.05) .07 (.04) .06 (.05)
•      Explicit threat
•      Total .07 (.05) .10 (.04) .09 (.05)
.08 (.06) .08 (.05) .08 (.05)
PANAS-X = Positive and Negative Affect Scales - Expanded, RM = role model
a
df = 2, 120.b df = 1, 120.
†p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

Gender of Role Model Main Effects These results are in agreement with other studies
on leadership stereotype threat showing that female
Exposure to female role models across all condi- role models did not generally provide protections
tions (implicit, explicit, and no stereotype threat) against detrimental stereotype threat effects, and
was related to higher levels of explicit, self- further, that women report ambivalence about
reported guilt (F (1, 120) = 8.21, p = .005, ŋ2 = seeing women in positions of power. However,
.06), fear (F (1, 120) = 4.22, p = .04, ŋ2 = .03) and the significant interactions between gender of
general negative emotion (F (1, 120) = 4.48, p = role model and type of stereotype threat reported
.04, ŋ2 = .03) than exposure to male role models. below qualify these findings. No significant main

128

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

effects for gender of role model were found for helpful in eliciting positive emotions experienced
implicit emotions in narratives or for the cognitive outside of women’s awareness. Under explicit
and behavioral outcomes. stereotype threat, female role model exposure
related to lower self-reported sadness compared
Role Model Gender X Stereotype to no threat and implicit threat, suggesting that
Threat Interaction Effects when stereotypes about leadership are more ex-
plicit, exposure to female role models can reduce
Significant interactions between gender of role self-reported, consciously experienced negative
model X type of stereotype threat were found for emotions such as sadness.
self-reported negative affect (F(2, 120) = 3.51, A significant interaction effect of gender
p = .03, ŋ2 = .06), fear (F(2, 120) = 3.51, p = of role model X stereotype threat condition on
.03, ŋ2 = .06), sadness (F(2, 120) = 3.18, p = implicit cognition (F(2, 120) = 3.16, p = .046,
.045, ŋ2 = .05), and implicit positive feeling (F(2, ŋ2 = .05) showed that exposure to female (versus
120) = 3.24, p = .04, partial ŋ2 = .05). Table 2 male) role models under explicit stereotype threat
displays means and standard deviations. Under activated significantly fewer implicit power- and
implicit stereotype threat, exposure to female role competition-related cognitions. Thus, exposure
models brought about greater explicit emotional to explicit gender role-related stereotypes along
vulnerability than exposure to male role models, with powerful female leaders may reinforce gen-
including significantly higher self-reported nega- der stereotypes that women are not as powerful
tive affect, fear, and sadness. However, exposure as men, despite our other findings that the same
to female role models also brought about higher combination of explicit threat and exposure to
implicit positive feelings under implicit threat powerful female role models resulted in height-
when compared to no threat (see Table 3 for means ened positive and diminished negative emotions.
and standard deviations). Consistent with previous
literature, the explicit self-reported negative emo- Implicit Emotion Word Use
tions we found in response to powerful female role Differences by Narrative Type
models suggest that female leaders may generate
conscious discomfort and that implicit stereotype We found a statistically significant difference in
threat exacerbates the negative emotion. However, word language used by type of narrative. Main ef-
exposure to female role models may actually be fects of narrative type on emotions with significant

Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations by Narrative Type for Indirectly-measured Emotions with
Significant Effects

Mean (SD)
Female Role Model Male Role Model
Stereotype Threat (1) None (2) Implicit (3) Explicit (1) None (2) Implicit (3) Explicit
Anxiety
•      Narrative 1 .21 (.41) .69 (1.08) .26 (.56) .35 (.54) .25 (.44) .15 (.35)
•      Narrative 2
.17 (.45) .35 (.69) .15 (.42) .05 (.24) .27 (.45) .19 (.40)
•      Narrative 3
.47 (.90) .72 (.60) .55 (.73) .69 (.60) .73 (.60) .22 (.33)
Note. Types of memories: Narrative 1 = Agentic, Narrative 2 = Communal, Narrative 3 = Neutral

129

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

findings are displayed in Table 4. As suggested by DISCUSSION


previous literature on women, narratives depicting
power (compared to those depicting communion) The current study examined the effects of gender-
tended to be associated with more anxiety and role related stereotype threat about leadership
more tentativeness (Fong & Tiedens, 2002). ability on women in several domains: emotions,
Other findings generally indicate that compared measured implicitly and explicitly, implicit cog-
to narratives describing the experiment, narratives nition, and self- versus other-oriented behavior.
depicting power and communion were associated The majority of previous research has examined
with more positive affect, including optimism and the effects of implicit stereotype threat on ste-
achievement. Memories of the experiment were reotyped skill deficits, such as performance on
associated with more tentativeness across stereo- mathematical or verbal tasks (Spencer, Steele,
type threat conditions, which may indicate that & Quinn, 1999; Steele, & Aronson, 1995). Our
participants were intimidated about remembering findings make a new contribution to the literature
the details of the experiment correctly. in demonstrating that implicit and unconscious

Table 4. Means and Standard Deviations of Emotion Word Usage by Narrative Type and Significant
Main Effects of Narrative Type on Emotion Word Usage

(1) Agentic (2) Communal (3) Neutral


Mean (SD) ANOVA F Condition, p-level
Anxiety 0.32 (.05) 0.20 (.04) 0.56 (.06) 4.55*** (1.90, 227.74) 1 v. 2, p = .07
1 v 3, p = .004
2 v 3, p < .0005
Optimism 0.8 (.09) 0.64 (0.06) 0.57 (.06) 2.16 (1.92, 230.18) 1 v 2, p = .195
1 v 3, p = .032
2 v 3, p = .540
Achievement 1.81 (.15) 2.82 (.18) 1.13 (0.08) 34.66*** (1.77, 212.79) 1 v 2, p < .0005
1 v 3, p < .0005
2 v 3, p < .0005
Negative emotion 1.17 (0.11) 1.02 (0.11) 1.37 (0.11) 2.70† (2, 239.24) 1 v 2, p = .32
1 v 3, p = .197
2 v 3, p = .020
Positive feeling 0.52 (0.08) 0.63 (0.08) 0.30 (0.05) 1.97** (3.71, 236.33) 1 v 2, p = .239
1 v 3, p = .009
2 v 3, p = .001
Tentative 1.55 (0.14) 1.19 (0.11) 2.36 (0.14) 48.12*** (1.87, 224.80) 1 v 2, p = .025
1 v 3, p < .0005
2 v 3, p < .0005
Note. p < .10. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

130

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

emotions and cognitions are important responses One of the unique aspects of the current study
to gender role-related stereotype threat and other was the use of implicit measures of emotion. We
environmental threats that may have long term utilized autobiographical memories and compared
consequences for behavior. Results indicated the expression of various emotions in memories
that women exposed to implicit stereotype threat about power-related experiences (stereotypically
expressed more anxiety and behaved in ways male characteristic), communal experiences (ste-
typically associated with having lower power, i.e. reotypically female characteristic) and about the
making choices that benefited others more than experiment itself (neutral). As suggested by previ-
the self, when compared to women who were ous literature on women, power-related narratives
not exposed to stereotype threat or who were (compared to communal narratives) tended to be
exposed to explicit stereotype threat. Further, associated with more anxiety and more tentative-
unconscious cognitive processes assessed with ness (Fong & Tiedens, 2002). More importantly,
a word completion task indicated that exposure implicit emotions expressed in narratives showed
to female role models under explicit stereotype responses that were not captured by self-report
threat was associated with fewer words connoting and may provide important clues to women’s
power than exposure to male role models. Women unconscious feelings and motivation, such as the
who were exposed to female (versus male) role mixture of higher anxiety along with more posi-
models also reported higher levels of self-reported tive emotions with exposure to powerful female
negative emotion, including guilt, sadness and role models under implicitly activated threat,
fear in all stereotype threat conditions. Traditional and more optimism under explicitly activated
theories and attitudes about leadership, i.e. people stereotype threat. These findings were in contrast
are born leaders (versus people can be made into to our results using directly measured emotion
leaders), and traditional gender role socialization, that showed higher levels of negative emotion,
i.e. personality traits associated with good lead- i.e. guilt, fear, and sadness, associated with expo-
ers are stereotypically masculine, may continue sure to powerful female role models, particularly
to negatively influence women’s emotions and when under implicitly-activated stereotype threat.
cognitions around women’s leadership potential These self-reported emotions may be assessing
(Burnette, Pollack, & Hoyt, 2010). more conscious self-schemas or long-standing
On the other hand, when exposed to female role self-representations. Implicit or indirect measures
models, less self-reported sadness was expressed of emotions may differ from explicit measures of
in explicit stereotype threat and more unconscious emotion because they may tap into emotions that
positive feelings were expressed in implicit ste- are not in participants’ awareness and/or they are
reotype threat. Thus, even though women may be not distorted by self-presentation, impression man-
aware of negative feelings surrounding women in agement or social desirability concerns (Bosson,
power (and even unconsciously believe them to be Haymovitz, & Pinel, 2004).
less powerful than men, as evidenced by the results Some researchers argue that gender equality
of our word completion task), they appear to have has been achieved so that the new generation of
unconscious positive feelings in the presence of young women no longer faces the same obstacles
female leaders when stereotypes are operating. as previous generations, partly because of chang-
These implicit positive feelings may potentially ing social and family structures and ideologies
serve to mitigate the effects of stereotype threat, over the last several decades (Kindlon, 2006).
and reinforce the importance of having power- While it is true that women have made strides in
ful female role models for heightening women’s attaining leadership positions and that cultural
leadership aspirations. attitudes about traditional gender roles are shift-

131

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

ing in favor of affording equal opportunities for REFERENCES


both genders, the present study’s results present
a different perspective and suggest that implicitly Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power,
activated traditional beliefs, attitudes, and fears optimism, and risk-taking. European Journal of
may continue to contribute to the persistence of Social Psychology, 36(4), 511–536. doi:10.1002/
gender disparities in leadership positions in the ejsp.324
US. Enthusiastic embrace of the demise of tradi- Anderson, C., & Thompson, L. L. (2004). Affect
tional gender roles needs to be tempered, in light from the top down: How powerful individuals’
of the evidence that for many young people, these positive affect shapes negotiations. Organizational
stereotypes are alive and well (e.g., Gustafsson & Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95(2),
Björklund, 2008) and, as in the present study, can 125–139. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.05.002
be activated by situational cues. Acknowledging
or making these stereotypes explicit and providing Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K.,
female role models may help to overcome gender Steele, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When White
role stereotypes that compromise performance. men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient fac-
tors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 35(1), 29–46. doi:10.1006/
LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE jesp.1998.1371
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS Aronson, J., Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (1998).
Stereotype threat and the academic underperfor-
The current investigation is an important foray
mance of minorities and women. In J. K. Swim
into gaining a fuller understanding of the effects
& C. Stangor (Eds.), Prejudice: The target’s per-
of stereotype threat on implicit processes that
spective (pp. 83–103). San Diego, CA: Academic
may underlie performance and motivation in ste-
Press. doi:10.1016/B978-012679130-3/50039-9
reotyped domains. Results indicate that implicit
emotions often differ from self-reported emotions Asgari, S., Dasgupta, N., & Stout, J. G.
in meaningful ways that inform our understanding (2012). When do counterstereotypic ingroup
of stereotype threat. In future research, it would be members inspire versus deflate? The effect
useful to examine the relationship between cogni- of successful professional women on young
tive outcomes of stereotype threat and observed women’s leadership self-concept. Personal-
behaviors. It would also be interesting to provide ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(3),
live role models with whom participants can 370–383. doi:10.1177/0146167211431968
identify, because proximity to competent female PMID:22205625
models has been shown to benefit women’s per-
Bergeron, D. M., Block, C. J., & Echtenkamp,
formance when under stereotype threat (Marx &
B. A. (2006). Disabling the able: Stereotype
Roman, 2002). A physiological measure of anxiety
threat and women’s work performance. Human
and emotional arousal would be another important
Performance, 19(2), 133–158. doi:10.1207/
indirect and implicit measure of stereotype threat
s15327043hup1902_3
response. Finally, investigating racial/ethnic and
gender differences in stereotype threat effects, as Bosak, J., & Sczesny, S. (2008). Am I the right
well as testing these concepts in samples of women candidate? Self-ascribed fit of women and men
who differed in age and socioeconomic status from to a leadership position. Sex Roles, 58(9-10),
the current sample should be examined in future 682–688. doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9380-4
work to assess for generalizability.

132

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

Bosson, J. K., Haymovitz, E. L., & Pinel, E. C. Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. H. (2004). Women and
(2004). When saying and doing diverge: The ef- men as leaders. In J. Antonakis, A. T. Cianciolo,
fects of stereotype threat on self-reported versus & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The nature of leader-
non-verbal anxiety. Journal of Experimental ship (pp. 279-301). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Social Psychology, 40(2), 247–255. doi:10.1016/ Publications, Inc.
S0022-1031(03)00099-4
Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. H. (2007). Women and
Broverman, I. K., Vogel, S. R., Broverman, the labyrinth of leadership. Harvard Business Re-
D. M., Clarkson, F. E., & Rosenkrantz, P. S. view. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
(1972). Sex-role stereotypes: A current reap-
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role
praisal. The Journal of Social Issues, 28(2), 59–78.
congruity theory of prejudice toward fe-
doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1972.tb00018.x
male leaders. Psychological Review, 109(3),
Burnette, J. L., Pollack, J. M., & Hoyt, C. L. 573–598. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573
(2010). Individual differences in implicit theories PMID:12088246
of leadership ability and self‐efficacy: Predicting
Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B.
responses to stereotype threat. The Journal of
G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders:
Leadership Studies, 3(4), 46–56. doi:10.1002/
A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1),
jls.20138
3–22. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.3
Chen, S., Lee-Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A.
Fong, C. T., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2002). Dueling
(2001). Relationship orientation as a mod-
experiences and dual ambivalences: The emotional
erator of the effects of social power. Journal
and motivational ambivalence of women in high
of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(2),
status positions. Motivation and Emotion, 26(1),
173–187. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.173
105–121. doi:10.1023/A:1015198209285
PMID:11220439
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., &
Croizet, J., Després, G., Gauzins, M., Huguet,
Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power and perspectives
P., Leyens, J., & Méot, A. (2004). Stereotype
not taken. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1068–
threat undermines intellectual performance by
1074. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01824.x
triggering a disruptive mental load. Person-
PMID:17201789
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(6),
721–731. doi:10.1177/0146167204263961 Gonzales, P. M., Blanton, H., & Williams,
PMID:15155036 K. J. (2002). The Effects of stereotype threat
and double-minority status on the test per-
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., & Steele, C. M. (2005).
formance of Latino women. Personality and
Clearing the air: Identity safety moderates the ef-
Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(5), 659–670.
fects of stereotype threat on women’s leadership
doi:10.1177/0146167202288010
aspirations. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 88(2), 276–287. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.88.2.276 PMID:15841859

133

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

Good, J. J., Woodzicka, J. A., & Wingfield, L. C. Hoyt, C. L., Johnson, S. K., Murphy, S., & Skin-
(2010). The effects of gender stereotypic and coun- nell, K. (2010). The impact of blatant stereotype
ter-stereotypic textbook images on science perfor- activation and group sex-composition on female
mance. The Journal of Social Psychology, 150(2), leaders. The Leadership Quarterly, 21(5), 716–
132–147. doi:10.1080/00224540903366552 732. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.07.003
PMID:20397590
Hoyt, C. L., & Simon, S. (2011). Female leaders:
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Im- Injurious or inspiring role models for women?
plicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and Psychology of Women Quarterly, 35(1), 143–157.
stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27. doi:10.1177/0361684310385216
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4 PMID:7878162
Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2000). A threatening
Gupta, V. K., Turban, D. B., & Bhawe, N. M. (2008). intellectual environment: Why females are sus-
The effect of gender stereotype activation on en- ceptible to experiencing problem-solving deficits
trepreneurial intentions. The Journal of Applied in the presence of males. Psychological Science,
Psychology, 93(5), 1053–1061. doi:10.1037/0021- 11(5), 365–371. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00272
9010.93.5.1053 PMID:18808225 PMID:11228906
Gustafsson, U., & Björklund, F. (2008). Women Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004).
self-stereotype with feminine stereotypical traits A Decade of System Justification Theory: Ac-
under stereotype threat. Current Research in Social cumulated Evidence of Conscious and Uncon-
Psychology, 13, 219–231. scious Bolstering of the Status Quo. Political
Psychology, 25(6), 881–919. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
Haines, E. L., & Kray, L. J. (2005). Self-power
9221.2004.00402.x
associations: The possession of power impacts
women’s self-concepts. European Journal of Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S., Bargh, J. A., & Ross,
Social Psychology, 35(5), 643–662. doi:10.1002/ L. (2004). Material priming: The influence of
ejsp.252 mundane physical objects on situational construal
and competitive behavioral choice. Organizational
Hoyt, C. L. (2005). The role of leadership ef-
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95(1),
ficacy and stereotype activation in women’s
83–96. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.06.003
identification with leadership. Journal of Lead-
ership & Organizational Studies, 11(4), 2–14. Keller, J., & Dauenheimer, D. (2003). Stereotype
doi:10.1177/107179190501100401 threat in the classroom: Dejection mediates the
disrupting threat effect on women’s math perfor-
Hoyt, C. L., & Blascovich, J. (2007). Leader-
mance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
ship efficacy and women leaders’ responses
29(3), 371–381. doi:10.1177/0146167202250218
to stereotype activation. Group Processes
PMID:15273014
& Intergroup Relations, 10(4), 595–616.
doi:10.1177/1368430207084718 Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C.
(2003). Power, approach and inhibition. Psycho-
Hoyt, C. L., & Blascovich, J. (2010). The role of
logical Review, 110(2), 265–28. doi:10.1037/0033-
leadership self-efficacy and stereotype activation
295X.110.2.265 PMID:12747524
on cardiovascular, behavioral and self-report
responses in the leadership domain. The Leader- Kindlon, D. J. (2006). Alpha girls: Understanding
ship Quarterly, 21(1), 89–103. doi:10.1016/j. the new American girl and how she is changing
leaqua.2009.10.007 the world. New York, NY: Rodale Press.

134

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

Koch, S. C. (2005). Evaluative affect display McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R., & Lord, C. (2003).
toward male and female leaders of task-oriented Alleviating women’s mathematics stereotype
groups. Small Group Research, 36(6), 678–703. threat through salience of group achievements.
doi:10.1177/1046496405281768 Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(1),
83–90. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00513-9
Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., &
Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes mas- Nakash, O., & Brody, L. (2007). The effects of
culine? A meta-analysis of three research para- power imbalances and gender on autobiographi-
digms. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 616–642. cal memory. Self and Identity, 6(4), 304–321.
doi:10.1037/a0023557 PMID:21639606 doi:10.1080/15298860601109495
Kray, L. J., Thompson, L., & Galinsky, A. Nguyen, H.-H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2009).
(2001). Battle of the sexes: Gender stereotype Does stereotype threat affect test performance
confirmation and reactance in negotiations. of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, experimental evidence. The Journal of Applied
80(6), 942–958. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.942 Psychology, 93(6), 1314–1334. doi:10.1037/
PMID:11414376 a0012702 PMID:19025250
Latu, I. M., Mast, M., Lammers, J., & Bombari, Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A.
D. (2013). Successful female leaders empower G. (2002). Math = male, me = female, therefore
women’s behavior in leadership tasks. Journal of math ≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social
Experimental Social Psychology, 49(3), 444–448. Psychology, 83(1), 44–59. doi:10.1037/0022-
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.01.003 3514.83.1.44 PMID:12088131
Lips, H. M. (2000). College students’ visions of Okimoto, T. G., & Brescoll, V. L. (2010).
power and possibility as moderated by gender. The price of power: Power seeking and back-
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24(1), 39–43. lash against female politicians. Personal-
doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01020.x ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(7),
923–936. doi:10.1177/0146167210371949
Lockwood, P. (2006). ‘Someone like me can
PMID:20519573
be successful’: Do college students need same-
gender role models? Psychology of Women Osborne, J. W. (2001). Testing stereotype threat:
Quarterly, 30(1), 36–46. doi:10.1111/j.1471- Does anxiety explain race and sex differences
6402.2006.00260.x in achievement? Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 262(3), 291–310. doi:10.1006/
Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role
ceps.2000.1052 PMID:11414722
models: Protecting women’s math test performance.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(9), Oswald, D. L., & Harvey, R. D. (2001). Hostile
1183–1193. doi:10.1177/01461672022812004 environments, stereotype threat, and math per-
formance among undergraduate women. Cur-
Marx, D. M., & Stapel, D. A. (2006). It’s all in
rent Psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.), 19(4),
the timing: Measuring emotional reactions to
338–356. doi:10.1007/s12144-000-1025-5
stereotype threat before and after taking a test.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 36(5),
687–698. doi:10.1002/ejsp.310

135

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M.
J. (2001). Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math
(LIWC): LIWC2001. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
Erlbaum Associates. chology, 35(1), 4–28. doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.1373
Phelan, J. E., & Rudman, L. A. (2010). Preju- Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How
dice toward female leaders: Backlash effects and stereotypes shape intellectual identity and per-
women’s impression management dilemma. Social formance. The American Psychologist, 52(6),
and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(10), 613–629. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613
807–820. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00306.x PMID:9174398
Ridgeway, C. L. (2001). Gender, status, and Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype
leadership. The Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), threat and the intellectual test performance of Afri-
637–655. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00233 can Americans. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 69(5), 797–811. doi:10.1037/0022-
Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2008). The social
3514.69.5.797 PMID:7473032
psychology of gender: How power and intimacy
shape gender relations. New York, NY: Guilford Stone, J., Lynch, C. I., Sjomeling, M., & Darley, J.
Press. M. (1999). Stereotype threat effects on Black and
White athletic performance. Journal of Person-
Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., & McGhee,
ality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1213–1227.
D. E. (2001). Implicit self-concept and evalu-
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213
ative implicit gender stereotypes: Self and in-
group share desirable traits. Personality and van Vianen, A. M., & Fischer, A. H. (2002). Illumi-
Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(9), 1164–1178. nating the glass ceiling: The role of organizational
doi:10.1177/0146167201279009 culture preferences. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology, 75(3), 315–337.
Rudman, L. A., & Kilianski, S. E. (2000). Implicit
doi:10.1348/096317902320369730
and explicit attitudes toward female authority. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(11), Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-
1315–1328. doi:10.1177/0146167200263001 X: Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect
Schedule-Expanded Form. Unpublished manu-
Rudman, L. A., & Phelan, J. E. (2010). The effect of
script, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA.
priming gender roles on women’s implicit gender
beliefs and career aspirations. Social Psychology, Young, D. M., Rudman, L. A., Buettner, H. M., &
41(3), 192–202. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000027 McLean, M. C. (2013). The influence of female
role models on women’s implicit science cogni-
Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evi-
tions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(3),
dence that stereotype threat reduces working mem-
283–292. doi:10.1177/0361684313482109
ory capacity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85(3), 440–452. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.85.3.440 PMID:14498781
KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS
Smith, J. L., & White, P. H. (2002). An examina-
tion of implicitly activated, explicitly activated, Emotion: Affective states or experiences
and nullified stereotypes on mathematical perfor- that can influence thought and behavior; can
mance: It’s not just a woman’s issue. Sex Roles, have physiological, cognitive, and behavioral
47(3/4), 179–191. doi:10.1023/A:1021051223441 components.

136

Implicit Processes and Emotions in Stereotype Threat about Women’s Leadership

Explicit Emotion: An overt or conscious ex- social group based on salient aspects of identity,
pression of an emotion that can be self-reported. e.g. gender, race, age, or ethnicity that has been
Explicit Stereotype Threat: An overt expres- found to put individuals in the group at risk for
sion of a negative stereotype about a social group behaving in ways that confirm the stereotype.
based on salient aspects of identity, e.g. gender, Leadership: In a group, holding a position
race, age, or ethnicity (e.g., women fare less well that has the power to directly influence other
at math than men) that has been found to put group members’ behavior; usually associated
individuals in the group at risk for behaving in with personal qualities that include agency and
ways that confirm the stereotype. dominance.
Female Gender Role: Culturally assigned Stereotype: A rigidly held, oversimplified
norms for acceptable thoughts, emotions, attitudes, and overly generalized view of a person or group.
roles, and behaviors on the part of females; these Stereotype Threat: The state of being at risk
norms may differ from one culture to another. of behaving in ways that confirm a negative ste-
Implicit Emotion: A covert or unconscious reotype about a social group to which one belongs
expression of an emotion that is not self-reported (based on salient aspects of identity such as age,
but may be observable through indirect measure- ethnicity, race, and gender) that is triggered by
ment (e.g., behavioral or physiological measures situational cues or overt expression of stereotypes.
or emotion word usage in narratives).
Implicit Stereotype Threat: A covert or in-
direct expression of a negative stereotype about a

137
138

Chapter 7
Smoking, Implicit Attitudes,
and Context-Sensitivity:
An Overview

Sabine Glock
Bergische Universität Wuppertal, Germany

Ineke M. Pit ten-Cate


University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg

ABSTRACT
This chapter focuses on implicit attitudes toward smoking and provides the first systematic review of
research in this domain. Implicit attitudes are suggested to guide automatic behavior, thereby playing
a pivotal role for automatic processes inherent in addictive behaviors. This chapter further explores the
extent to which implicit attitudes are context-sensitive. More specifically, it reviews studies that have
focused on the differential effects of external cues such as warning labels and internal cues (e.g., depri-
vation). Results of 32 studies show that although smokers generally have more positive implicit attitudes
than non-smokers, the valence of implicit attitudes varies as a result of the applied method or stimuli.
Studies reveal that implicit attitudes toward smoking partly depend on external cues, especially outcome
expectancies. Similarly, internal cues affect implicit attitudes whereby the level of nicotine deprivation
seems vital. Implications for intervention and future research are indicated in the discussion.

INTRODUCTION Asian regions (OECD, 2012b). Smoking rates in


the Pacific regions and the USA are slightly lower
Smoking is one of the leading causes of prevent- than in Europe with less than 20% of the adult
able death (World Health Organization, 2011), population smoking daily (Centers for Disease
whereby tobacco is responsible for about 10% Control and Prevention, 2012; OECD, 2012b).
adult deaths worldwide (OECD, 2012a). Smoking Given the detrimental health effects of smok-
rates vary across countries, with at least 20% of the ing as well as the continuously high prevalence
adult population smoking daily in 20 of the 27 EU rates, several studies have aimed to determine
member states (OECD, 2012a) and over 40% in factors that influence substance use and addic-
DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch007

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

tive behaviors. Studies have indicated that the in which object-evaluation activation takes place
maintenance of smoking may partly be due to the determines the extent to which implicit attitudes
fact that smokers do not know how to quit (Wiers will control a person’s behavior, i.e. induces a
& de Jong, 2006). Other studies have linked the spontaneous reaction to the perceived situation and
maintenance of smoking with motivational factors offsets alternative behavior choices that require
related to positive outcome expectancies such as more deliberate effort (Schuette & Fazio, 1995).
stress reduction or the reduction of withdrawal In other words, the extent to which an attitude is
symptoms (Baker, Brandon, & Chassin, 2004), or strong enough to be automatically activated by an
suggested a causal effect of implicit attitudes (De object determines the extent to which the behavior
Houwer, Custers, & De Clercq, 2006). occurs spontaneously from the attitude or can
Over the last 15 years, implicit cognitions be influenced by more conscious and controlled
have received more and more attention in order to processing (Olson & Fazio, 2009). Applied to
gain a deeper understanding of substance use and smoking, one could argue that for smokers the
misuse (Wiers & Stacy, 2006). Implicit cognitions –positive– implicit attitudes toward smoking are
entail implicit attitudes, attentional bias, implicit so strong that they would automatically lead to
arousal, and memory associations (Rooke, Hine, smoking behavior and offset conscious efforts to
& Thorsteinsson, 2008). To this extent, Rooke and quit smoking (De Houwer et al., 2006).
colleagues (2008) reported in their meta-analysis This distinction between implicit and explicit
consistent findings of moderate associations attitudes does not only exist on a theoretical but also
between implicit cognitions and substance use. on a measurement level (Hofmann, Gschwendner,
Implicit attitudes refer to immediate automatic Nosek, & Schmitt, 2005). Implicit measures tap
affective reactions to stimuli (Fazio, 1990; Strack into automaticity (De Houwer, 2006; De Houwer
& Deutsch, 2004). They are defined as automatic & Moors, 2007). Automatic processes can be
evaluations of an attitude object (Gawronski & defined as unconscious, unintentional, unaware,
Bodenhausen, 2006) that are activated by the and efficient processes (Bargh, 1994) and implicit
mere presence of the object (Fazio, 2007; Olson measures should at least possess one of these prop-
& Fazio, 2009). Implicit attitudes have been found erties (Hofmann et al., 2005). As automaticity is a
to influence automatic behavior (Fazio, 2001) and key component of implicit attitudes (Petty, Fazio,
therefore may play a pivotal role for the automatic & Briñol, 2009), by inference implicit measures
components inherent in addictive behaviors (Tif- are assumed to assess implicit attitudes. Implicit
fany, 1990; Wiers & de Jong, 2006). Implicit attitudes are suggested to be the result of life-long
attitudes can be theoretically distinguished from learning (Briñol, Petty, & McCaslin, 2009; Rud-
explicit attitudes, which are defined as conscious man, 2004), and hence difficult to change (Briñol
evaluations resulting from propositional reason- et al., 2009). However, since implicit measures are
ing (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006; Strack as context-sensitive (De Houwer, 2006) as implicit
& Deutsch, 2004). The question then arises attitudes (Blair, 2002; De Houwer, 2006), an object
what kind of attitudes are related to behavior, may be associated with more than one attitude.
under what conditions, and by what processes. This context-sensitivity seems to play a pivotal
The MODE model (Fazio, 1990) identifies the role for addictive behaviors, as substance-related
associations between attitude objects and their cognitions are highly context-sensitive (Krank,
evaluations and focuses on the strength of such Wall, Stewart, Wiers, & Goldman, 2005; Reich,
associations to predict behavioral responses. From Goldman, & Noll, 2004; Sherman, Rose, Koch,
the MODE model it follows that the relative ease Presson, & Chassin, 2003; Wiers et al., 2003).

139

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

That is, substance-related cognitions might change METHODS OF THE REVIEW


by the cues given in a particular situation, which
increase or decrease the probability of addictive Types of Studies
behaviors to occur.
In this chapter, we systematically review the We only considered studies which investigated
research on implicit attitudes toward smoking, implicit attitudes toward smoking, whereby we
which has provided rather inconsistent results. specifically included studies that used implicit at-
Some studies revealed positive (e.g., De Houwer titude measures that enabled drawing conclusions
et al., 2006), others negative (e.g., Huijding, de about the valence of implicit attitudes. Therefore
Jong, Wiers, & Verkooijen, 2005), and a few studies using implicit attitudes for the aim of pre-
neutral implicit attitudes (e.g. Robinson, Meier, dicting behavior or other personal characteristics
Zetocha, & McCaul, 2005). Those inconsistent without information on the valence of implicit
results are assumed to be partly due to the mea- attitudes were excluded.
surement methods and stimulus materials applied
in the different studies (Wiers & de Jong, 2006). Types of Participants
However, the inconsistency could also result from
contextual factors prevalent in the test situation. Studies including smokers, ex-smokers, as well
Indeed, referring back to the MODE model (Fazio, as non-smokers were included. We reviewed only
1990), the situational context might determine the studies where the legal age for smoking was re-
activation of attitudes whereby the strength of spected. Cigarette purchase age in Belgium is 16
automatic activation results in either a spontane- years, in other countries such as the United States,
ous behavioral response or allows for a conscious United Kingdom, the Netherlands or Germany
deliberation of behavioral alternatives. Smokers the legal age is 18.
have both positive as well as negative outcome ex-
pectancies from smoking (Hendricks & Brandon, Types of Outcome Measures
2005; Mullenix, Kilbey, Fisicaro, Farnsworth, &
Torrento, 2003). These may follow from positive Implicit attitudes reported as response latencies
experiences, e.g. positive mood and stress reduc- or index scores.
tion, as well as negative experiences, e.g. health
consequences or negative reactions from others, Search Strategy
which might then be reflected in implicit attitudes
(Petty et al., 2009; Rudman, 2004). The specific We searched via EBSCO HOST in Health Sci-
cue in a particular situation might activate different ence and Psychology and Sociological Databases
implicit attitudes than any other cue in any other including MEDLINE, MEDLINE with full text,
situation, activating either positive or negative Academic Search Premier, European Views of
implicit attitudes. Those cues might not only be the Americas: 1493 to 1750, SocINDEX with
external, such as the environment in which the full text. Additionally, we screened the reference
study takes place, but could also be internal such lists of key publications and of the publications
as the motivation to smoke. Certain situations found via database search. We used the following
or stimuli will therefore induce automatic (non) search terms: Smoking AND implicit attitudes OR
smoking behaviors whilst other situations/stimuli smoking AND implicit evaluations OR smoking
would enable a controlled behavioral response af- AND implicit cognition OR smoking AND im-
ter mindful deliberation of behavioral alternatives. plicit associations OR smoking AND automatic

140

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

attitudes OR smoking AND automatic evaluations RESULTS


OR smoking AND automatic cognition OR smok-
ing AND automatic associations. The 20 publications included in this systematic
We had three stages in the review process (see review reported on 32 studies regarding the im-
Figure 1). First, we screened the titles and abstracts plicit attitudes toward smoking. Table 1 sum-
of the electronic findings. Articles were excluded marizes the studies and their features including
if implicit attitudes toward smoking were not part the country, in which the studies were conducted,
of the title or mentioned in the abstract. Second, the samples, the sample sizes, smoking status of
remaining articles were obtained in full text and the participants within each sample, the applied
then screened for content. Third, the reference implicit measurement methods, the materials, the
lists of the articles as well as of other significant context manipulation if applicable, the dependent
publications were screened, relevant articles iden- variables, and the results.
tified, titles and abstracts screened, and full texts Nineteen studies were conducted in the
assessed if the articles were eligible. Finally, 20 United States, three in the United Kingdom,
articles remained for qualitative synthesis. three in Germany, three in Belgium, and four in

Figure 1. Flow chart of the search history

141

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

Table 1. Summary of the included studies

Author Country Sample Sample Smoking Method Materials Context DV Attitudes


Size Status

Basset & US undergraduates 39 Smokers GNAT Smoking vs. none GNAT/ Positive
Dabbs, (N=11) (Go/NoGo neutral words RT score among
2005 Non-smokers Association smokers
(N=28) Task)

Chassin, US adults 449 Smokers > 1 IAT Smoking none IAT score Negative
Presson, cigarette per pictures vs. among
Sherman, day (CPD) geometric smokers
Seo, & shapes
Macy,
2010

Czyzewska US undergraduates 226 Not IAT Smoking vs. Anti-tobacco IAT score Negative; no
& differentiated non-smoking ads effect of anti-
Ginsburg, between (neutral) tobacco ads
2007 smokers and pictures
Non-smokers

De Belgium undergraduates 63 Smokers ≥ 1 IAT Smoking vs. none IAT score Relatively
Houwer, CPD (N=31) non-smoking neutral
Custers & Non-smokers pictures among
De Clercq, (N=32) (difference: smokers
2006 cigarette)

De Houwer Belgium undergraduates 63 Smokers ≥ 1 Approach-IAT Smoking vs. none IAT score Positive
et al., 2006 CPD (N=31) non-smoking among
Non-smokers pictures smokers
(N=32) (difference:
cigarette)

De Houwer Belgium undergraduates 51 Smokers ≥ 1 Personalized Smoking vs. none IAT score Positive
et al., 2006 CPD (N=26) IAT non-smoking among
Non-smokers (attributes: pictures smokers
(N=25) I like vs. I (difference:
dislike) cigarette)

Glock, Germany undergraduates 30 Daily Affective Smoking vs. Intervention: Response Positive
Unz, & smokers priming non-smoking warning latencies among
Kovacs, pictures labels smokers
2012 (difference: contradicting before,
cigarette) positive neutral after
outcome intervention
expectancies

Glock, Germany undergraduates 91 Daily Affective Smoking vs. Smell of Response Negative
Kovacs, & smokers priming non-smoking cigarettes vs. latencies among
Unz, 2013 (N=48) pictures no smell smokers; no
Non-smokers (difference: influence
(N=43) cigarette) of smell
condition

Huijding The undergraduates 64 Smokers ST-IAT Smoking None IAT score Positive
& de Jong, Netherlands (N=31) (Single pictures among
2006 Non-smokers Target-IAT) smokers
(N=33)

Huijding, The undergraduates 48 Smokers IAT Smoking None IAT score Negative
de Jong, Netherlands (N=24) words vs. among
Wiers, & non-smokers Exercise smokers
Verkooijen, (N=24) words
2005

Huijding et The undergraduates 48 Smokers IAT Smoking Smoking IAT score Negative
al., 2005 Netherlands words vs. setting vs. among
writing words non-smoking smokers; no
setting influence of
setting

continued on following page

142

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

Table 1. Continued

Author Country Sample Sample Smoking Method Materials Context DV Attitudes


Size Status

Huijding et The undergraduates 48 Smokers AST Smoking Smoking Response Neutral


al., 2005 Netherlands (Affective words vs. setting vs. latencies among
Simon Task) writing words non-smoking smokers, no
setting influence of
setting

Kahler, US adults 67 Smokers ≥ IAT-variant Smoking none IAT score More


Daughters, 10 CPD (attributes: pictures vs. negative
Leventhal, social vs. furniture consequences
Gwaltney, negative pictures associated
& Palfai, consequences) with smoking
2007 (not really
attitudes)

Mogg, UK undergraduates 43 Daily SRC Smoking vs. Correlational: Response Positive


Bradley, and staff from smokers (Stimulus non-smoking attention latencies among
Field, & De the university (N=20) response pictures to smoking smokers;
Houwer, non-smokers compatibility (difference: stimuli and higher
2003 (N=23) task) cigarette) urge to smoke attention
for smoking
stimuli =
higher urge
to smoke =
more positive
valence

Mogg, UK undergraduates 41 Smokers ≥ 1 SRC Smoking vs. nicotine Response Positive


Field, & and staff from CPD non-smoking dependence; latencies among
Bradley, the university pictures correlational: smokes;
2005 (difference: attention lower
cigarette) to smoking nicotine
stimuli, CPD, dependence
and time = Higher
since last attention
cigarette for smoking
stimuli =
higher urge
to smoke =
more positive
valence

Payne, US undergraduates 60 Smokers ≥ 1 AMP (Affect Smoking vs. None Pro- Neutral
McClernon, CPD (N=35) Misattribution non-smoking portion of among
& Dobbins, non-smokers Procedure) pictures pleasant smokers
2007 (N=25) (difference: responses
cigarette)

Perugini, UK undergraduates 48 Smokers IAT Smoking None IAT score Negative


2005 (N=25) words vs. among
non-smokers exercise smokers
(N=23) words

Robinson, US undergraduates 48 Smokers ≥ 1 IAT Smoking None IAT score Neutral


Meier, CPD (N=20) words vs. among
Zetocha, & non-smokers words from smokers
McCaul, (N=28) heterogeneous
2005 categories

Robinson et US undergraduates 52 Smokers > 1 IAT Smoking None IAT score Neutral
al., 2005 CPD (N=24) words vs. among
non-smokers words from smokers
(N=28) heterogeneous
categories

Robinson et US undergraduates 52 Smokers ≥ 1 IAT Smoking None IAT score Positive


al., 2005 CPD (N=24) words vs. among
non-smokers stealing words smoking
(N=28)

continued on following page

143

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

Table 1. Continued
Author Country Sample Sample Smoking Method Materials Context DV Attitudes
Size Status

Rudman, US undergraduates 82 Smokers IAT Smoking vs. None IAT D Negative


Phelen, & and young non-smoking score among
Heppen, adults from the pictures smokers
2007 community (difference:
cigarette)

Rydell, US adults 50 Smokers ≥ IAT Smoking Nicotine IAT score Positive


Sherman, 10 CPD pictures vs. deprivation among
Boucher, geometric (4 hrs) and deprived
& Macy, shapes anti-tobacco smokers;
2012 message non-deprived
quality smokers
negative after
perceiving
strong
messages

Sherman, US undergraduates 54 Smokers ≥ 1 Affective Smoking Smoking Priming Positive


Rose, CPD priming package Pictures: score toward
Koch, pictures and Costs vs. sensory
Presson, & smoking sensory aspects,
Chassin, sensory aspects of negative
2003 pictures vs. smoking toward cost
positive and aspects
negative among
pictures smokers

Sherman et US undergraduates 54 Smokers ≥ 1 IAT Smoking Smoking IAT score Neutral


al., 2003 CPD package Pictures: among
pictures and Costs vs. smokers
smoking sensory
sensory aspects of
pictures vs. smoking
positive and
negative
pictures

Sherman et US undergraduates 235 Smokers Affective Smoking Smoking Priming Positive


al., 2003 < 15 CPD priming sensory status (none, score among light,
N=93) pictures vs. light, or non-deprived
smokers positive and heavy) and smokers,
≥ 15 CPD negative nicotine negative
(N=63) pictures deprivation among heavy,
non-smokers (4h before or non-deprived
(N=79) immediately smokers,
before the positive
experiment among heavy,
smoked) deprived
smokers

Sherman et US undergraduates 235 Smokers IAT Smoking Smoking IAT score More
al., 2003 < 15 CPD sensory status (none, negative
N=93) pictures vs. light, or among light
smokers positive and heavy) and than heavy
≥ 15 CPD negative nicotine smokers, no
(N=63) pictures deprivation influence of
non-smokers (4hrs deprivation
(N=79) before or
immediately
before the
experiment
smoked)

continued on following page

144

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

Table 1. Continued

Author Country Sample Sample Smoking Method Materials Context DV Attitudes


Size Status

Süssenbach, Germany undergraduates 82 Smokers ST-IAT Smoking Graphic IAT score Negative
Niemeier, (N=63) words warning among
& Glock, non-smokers labels smokers, no
2013 (N=19) influence
of graphic
warning
labels

Swanson, US undergraduates 42 Smokers IAT Smoking None IAT-score Negative


Rudman, & (N=38) words vs. among
Greenwald, non-smokers exercise smokers
2001 (N=46) words

Swanson et US undergraduates 42 Smokers IAT Smoking None IAT score Negative


al., 2001 (N=38) words vs. among
non-smokers eating sweets smokers
(N=46) words

Swanson et US undergraduates 96 Smokers IAT Smoking None IAT score Positive


al., 2001 (N=37) words vs. among
non-smokers stealing words smokers
(N=59)

Swanson et US undergraduates 76 Smokers IAT Smoking vs. None IAT score Negative
al., 2001 (N=35) non-smoking among
non-smokers pictures smokers
(N=41) (difference:
cigarette)

Waters et US adults 57 Smokers ≥ IAT Smoking Smoking IAT score Negative


al., 2007 10 CPD pictures abstinence among
vs. neutral (12hrs – smokers,
pictures smoke 40 more negative
min before when not
vs. 12 hrs – abstinent
no smoking and smoking
vs. no allowed 40
abstinence min before
– 40 min
before vs. no
abstinence –
no smoking)

The Netherlands. None of the publications was order to assess implicit attitudes toward smoking.
published in the last century, with only four of Two studies used the single target variant of the
them including a participant group other than IAT, four studies applied an affective priming
undergraduate students. The sample sizes ranged measure (Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams,
from 30 to 449 with a total of 2736 participants 1995), two a variant of the Stimulus Response
and a subsample of 1815 smokers across the stud- Compatibility task (Mogg, Bradley, Field, &
ies. The criteria regarding the smoking status of De Houwer, 2003; SRC), one the Go/No-Go-
participants varied between at least one cigarette Association Task (Nosek & Banaji, 2001; GNAT),
per day (CPD) for daily smokers to more than one the Affect Misattribution Procedure (Payne,
10 CPD. One study did not differentiate between Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005;AMP), and one
smokers and non-smokers, neither in the sample the Affective Simon Task (De Houwer & Eelen,
description, nor in the reported results. A majority 1998;AST). Twelve studies used words as stimuli,
of 22 studies used the Implicit Association Test of which three studies contrasted smoking and
(Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998; IAT) in exercise words, two studies smoking and neutral

145

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

words, one study smoking words and words from of the two IATs into one score, indicating negative
heterogeneous categories, two studies smoking attitudes toward smoking among smokers (IAT
and stealing, two studies smoking and writing, score = -300) as well as non-smokers (IAT score
one study smoking and eating sweets, and one = -354), with no difference between the groups. In
study used only smoking words. The remaining a second experiment, smoking words were paired
20 studies used pictures as materials, of which with words reflecting the category stealing. Thirty-
nine studies contrasted smoking and non-smoking seven smokers and 59 non-smokers participated
pictures, whereby the display of the cigarette was in this study. Findings indicated positive implicit
the only difference between the pictures, two stud- attitudes toward smoking for both smokers (IAT
ies used smoking pictures and geometric shapes, score = 173) and non-smokers (IAT score = 137),
two studies smoking and neutral pictures, two with no significant difference between the groups.
studies smoking pictures displaying package and A third study contrasted smoking and non-smoking
sensory information of smoking compared with pictures among a sample of 35 smokers and 41
positive and negative pictures, two studies pictures non-smokers. This test revealed negative implicit
with sensory aspects compared with positive and attitudes toward smoking for smokers (IAT score
negative pictures, one study smoking pictures and = -69) and for non-smokers (IAT score = -245).
furniture pictures, and one study used smoking Although the authors did not report whether the
pictures only. Only 14 of the studies manipulated IAT scores differed significantly from zero, the
context in its broadest sense. With one exception difference between groups was significant (p =
(Payne, McClernon, & Dobbins, 2007), all studies .008), indicating non-smokers’ implicit attitudes
assessed reaction times as dependent variables. were more negative than those of smokers.
Below we will review the results of the indi- Contrasting smoking words and exercise
vidual studies. We will first review the studies in words, Perugini (2005), found a negative IAT score
which context variables were not manipulated, of -89 among smokers, but did not report whether
followed by the studies that did consider context this score differed from zero; however, this score
variables. Among these context studies, we first was less negative than that of non-smokers (p =
present studies giving anti-tobacco information .006). Similarly, Huijding and colleagues (2005)
in order to investigate how implicit attitudes reported negative IAT scores for both smokers
change as a function of information, followed by (IAT score = -177) and non-smokers (IAT score
studies manipulating the testing context. Lastly, = -329) after applying an IAT contrasting smoking
we report on studies that consider the effects of words with exercise words among a sample of 24
internal cues, such as motivation and deprivation, Dutch smokers and 24 non-smokers, with smokers
on implicit attitudes. Studies were conducted in being less negative than non-smokers (p < .01).
the US with undergraduate students as participants These studies show that the IAT results mainly
unless stated otherwise. depend on the used contrast category and that the
Swanson, Rudman, and Greenwald (2001) were results might partly reflect cultural knowledge
one of the first to assess implicit attitudes toward about smoking (Robinson et al., 2005). Consider-
smoking. In the first experiment, they adminis- ing those disadvantages, Robinson and colleagues
tered two different IATs contrasting smoking vs. (2005) contrasted smoking words with words
exercise and smoking vs. eating sweets words, matched for initials and length stemming from
respectively. A sample of 38 smokers and 46 heterogeneous categories, all considered to be
non-smokers was randomly assigned to either IAT neutral in valence. In their sample of 20 smokers
version. As the researchers did not find an effect of and 28 non-smokers, neutral implicit attitudes
the contrasting category, they collapsed the results were found among smokers (IAT score = 41).

146

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

This score did not differ from zero but was more .009), indicating a stronger association between
positive than scores of non-smokers (IAT score = smoking and approach than between smoking
-154; p < .001). Robinson and colleagues (2005) and avoid. On both the attitude and the approach
replicated this study in another sample of 24 smok- IAT, smokers were significantly more positive
ers and 28 non-smokers, yielding similar results. than non-smokers. In another study, reported in
Although in the 2nd experiment the IAT score for the same publication, De Houwer and colleagues
smokers was negative (IAT score = -41), implicit (2006) applied a personalized variant of the IAT,
attitudes were again considered neutral because thereby replacing positive and negative adjectives
they did not differ from zero. Like in their first through positive and negative nouns as well as
study, non-smokers’ implicit attitudes (IAT score replacing the Labels “Positive” and “Negative”
= -131) were more negative than those of smokers through “I like” and “I dislike”. Participants were
(p = .02). In order to provide evidence for the IAT 26 Belgian smokers and 25 non-smokers. They
assessing cultural knowledge and demonstrate that found a positive IAT score of 228, which differed
IAT results depend on the contrast category, Rob- significantly from zero (p = .002). Implicit atti-
inson and colleagues (2005) administered a third tudes from smokers differed from non-smokers, i.e.
IAT to the same participants as in the replication smokers’ implicit attitudes were positive, whereas
study, but applied different stimuli. The neutral non-smokers’ implicit attitudes were shown to be
category words were replaced through words negative. Rudman, Phelan, and Heppen (2007) also
related to the category “stealing”. This IAT score reported negative implicit attitudes toward smok-
revealed positive attitudes among both smokers ing in their sample of 82 smokers. The sample was
and non-smokers, with no differences between the a mixture of people from the community (N=37)
groups (IAT score = 181, for the whole sample). and students. The IAT stimuli were smoking and
Using a web based IAT with smoking pictures non-smoking pictures. The authors reported the
versus geometric shapes, Chassin, Presson, Sher- IAT D score (a proxy for effect size) of -0.36 with
man, Seo, and Macy (2010) found an IAT score an effect size of d = -0.53 for the difference be-
of -0.41 among 449 adult smokers. Although this tween smoking – positive and smoking – negative.
score indicated slightly negative attitudes towards Moving away from the classical IAT with
smoking, the authors did not report whether this positive and negative words, Kahler, Daughters,
score differed significantly from zero, and hence Leventhal, Gwaltney, and Palfai, (2007) adminis-
the interpretation of the results is limited. How- tered an IAT in which they replaced positive and
ever, De Houwer and colleagues (2006) reported negative categories through positive and negative
a negative IAT score of -62 in their sample of 31 social consequences of smoking to 67 smokers (≥
Belgian smokers, which did not significantly dif- 10 CPD; community sample) during the intake
fer from zero hence reflecting neutral attitudes. interview of a smoking cessation intervention.
More specifically, in this study, the attitude IAT Hence, this IAT did not reveal implicit attitudes in
with smoking and non-smoking pictures was ap- the common way. Nevertheless, the pairing with
plied. The only difference between the pictures positive and negative social consequences does
was the presence/absence of a cigarette and the provide information on the automatic evaluation
matching pictures were paired with positive and of smoking. The IAT D score for negative conse-
negative adjectives. The authors also applied an quences (0.30) indicated negative attitudes, with
approach IAT, using approach and avoid words participants being faster on those trials compared
instead of the positive and negative adjectives. In to trials pairing smoking with positive social
this IAT, they found a positive IAT score of 75, consequences (p < .001). This IAT was only as-
which was significantly different from zero (p = sessed as a baseline measure, and not repeated at

147

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

different steps of cessation and therefore it is not of anti-smoking TV advertisements on implicit


possible to assess if and to what extent attitudes attitudes. They compared implicit attitudes toward
changed over time. smoking in a sample of 226 students randomly
In order to facilitate implicit attitudes research, assigned to different viewing conditions. The two
Bassett and Dabbs (2005) tested a portable ver- experimental groups were either presented with an
sion of the GNAT among a sample of 11 smokers anti-tobacco advertisement or an anti-marijuana
and 28 non-smokers. Smoking words were paired advertisement. In their IAT, they contrasted smok-
with ‘good’ and ‘bad’ words. They found an IAT ing pictures with neutral pictures. They found
score of 18 and -54 for smokers and non-smokers IAT scores of -0.17 for the group who watched
respectively, which indicated relatively more the anti-tobacco advertisement and -0.14 for the
positive attitudes among smokers compared to group who watched the anti-marijuana advertise-
non-smokers. However, the authors did not report ment. The two scores did not significantly differ
whether the two scores differed significantly from and the authors reported the scores for the whole
zero, hence it is not possible to draw conclusions sample without differentiating for tobacco use
if scores should be interpreted as negative, posi- among participants.
tive, or neutral. Although anti-smoking TV advertisements
Avoiding the need of a contrast category and might reach many people, health-related warning
the relative character of the IAT, Huijding and de labels on cigarette packages confront each smoker
Jong (2006) applied a ST-IAT in a Dutch sample with anti-tobacco messages every time he or she
of 31 smokers and 33 non-smokers, using smoking takes a cigarette. Süssenbach, Niemeier, and Glock
pictures targeting the sensory aspect of smoking. (2013) presented 63 German smokers with graphic
Among smokers, they found a positive IAT score and text-only cigarette warning labels. Afterwards
of 22, which was significantly different from participants underwent a ST-IAT with smoking
zero (p < .05), and a negative IAT score of -37 words. Implicit attitudes toward smoking were
among non-smokers, which also differed from negative (IAT D score = -0.54), whereby the
zero (p < .01). presentation of warning labels did not change at-
Applying a new measure, Payne and colleagues titudes. The authors did not investigate whether
(2007) investigated implicit attitudes via the AMP the IAT score differed significantly from zero.
with smoking and non-smoking pictures. Unlike However, this score differed significantly from
other implicit measures, the AMP does not rely on the score for non-smokers (N = 19; IAT D score
response latencies but on pleasant and unpleasant = -1.03, p < .01).
ratings. In their study, 35 smokers (≥ 1 CPD) and In another study, and assuming that the smell of
25 non-smokers participated. The authors found nicotine might serve as an external cue triggering
negative attitudes among non-smokers (difference positive implicit attitudes, Glock, Kovacs, and Unz
score = -0.34) and neutral implicit attitudes among (2013) manipulated the smell in the lab as context
smokers (difference score = 0.07). variable. One lab smelled neutral and the other
Studies described so far assessed implicit at- lab smelled of burnt cigarettes. Participants were
titudes without considering contextual factors. 48 German daily smokers and 43 non-smokers
However, one could argue that, given that implicit and all completed an affective priming task with
cognitions are context sensitive, implicit attitudes smoking and non-smoking pictures. The authors
may vary under different circumstances. Consider- compared the responses to negative words follow-
ing the importance of external cues, Czyzewska ing smoking and non-smoking pictures (difference
and Ginsburg (2007) investigated the influence score = -18.71) and found implicit attitudes to

148

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

be negative for both smokers and non-smokers. least 1 CPD. They scored the IAT measure such
This score differed from zero (p < .05). Implicit that negative scores represent positive implicit
attitudes did not vary as a result of smoking status attitudes and vice versa. In the IAT scores, the
or the smell manipulation. authors found no difference between cost (IAT
In a similar vein, Huijding and colleagues score = 11) and sensory aspects of smoking (IAT
(2005) also varied the context, in which 48 Dutch score = -13). The same sample also completed
smokers were administered an IAT contrasting an affective priming task with the same stimuli.
smoking words with words related to the neutral The affective priming score (relative facilitation
category “writing”. In the non-smoking setting, of positive words opposed to negative words and
the situation was a typical lab testing situation and compared with a baseline measure of positive and
participants were told that it would not be allowed negative words, whereby negative scores reflect
to smoke in the room. In contrast, in the smoking positive attitudes) revealed differences in implicit
setting, participants were told that they would be attitudes as a function of the stimuli. Smokers were
allowed to smoke when they had finished the ex- implicitly positive toward sensory aspects (score
periment, and to demonstrate this the experimenter = -27) and negative toward cost aspects of smok-
lit up a cigarette. All participants were instructed ing (score = 43, p < .02). Thus, implicit attitudes
not to smoke for two hours before entering the varied as a function of external smoking cues.
lab. The authors found no effect of setting and a Considering the possibility that health-related
negative IAT score (mean across the two setting warning labels might lead to defensive responses
groups = -136), which differed significantly from among smokers (Ruiter & Kok, 2005), Glock,
zero (p < .01). The same participants also under- Unz, and Kovacs (2012) confronted smokers with
went an AST using the same stimuli. In this task, warning labels contradicting positive outcome
participants were instructed to say “positive” or expectancies. They investigated implicit attitudes
“negative” depending on whether the word was among a German sample of 30 smokers using an
presented in plural or singular. The AST revealed affecting priming task, with smoking and non-
neutral implicit attitudes (exact value not provided) smoking pictures, and compared the response
and again, the setting had no effect. The results latencies for positive and negative words following
of this study demonstrated that implicit attitudes smoking pictures, before and after confrontation
were not influenced by the smoking setting, i.e. with warning labels. At baseline, before confronta-
the information that it was either allowed or not tion, implicit attitudes were found to be positive
allowed to smoke in the room, did not change (difference score for smoking pictures = 39), and
their implicit attitudes. significantly different from zero (p < .05). After
Considering the fact that implicit attitudes confrontation with warning labels, this score was
might vary as a function of the activated aspects not only slightly less positive (20), but also did not
as external cues and that smoking might have significantly differ from zero anymore (p = .12).
positive as well as negative aspects (Hendricks The above studies show that specific external
& Brandon, 2005; Mullenix et al., 2003), Sher- cues, especially outcome expectancies, influence
man and colleagues (2003) compiled pictures implicit attitudes. However, implicit attitudes
displaying cigarette packaging, which reflected of smokers might not only be influenced by ex-
the costs of smoking in terms of health and price, ternal, but also by internal cues. In one of their
and pictures displaying the sensory aspects of studies, Sherman and colleagues (2003) focused
smoking, such as a cigarette being held in hand. on internal cues, using the sensory pictures as
They administered an IAT with these stimuli smoking stimuli and compared them with positive
among a sample of 54 smokers, all smoking at and negative pictures. The 235 participants were

149

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

divided by their smoking status: ninety-three light 12 hours before the experiment. Half of these
smokers (less than 15 CPD), 63 heavy smokers (at abstinent participants were allowed to smoke
least 15 CPD), and 79 non-smokers. The authors 40 minutes before the experiment started; the
manipulated smoking deprivation, whereby all other half was not allowed to smoke. The other
participants were asked to refrain from smoking half of the sample was not asked to abstain and
four hours before the experiment. Half of their half of them were allowed to smoke 40 minutes
smoking participants were then asked to complete before the experiment, whereas the remaining
the measures without smoking, whereas the other half was not allowed. Thus, four different smok-
half was allowed a cigarette just before commenc- ing groups were realized. The analysis revealed
ing the tasks. Participants completed the IAT as an IAT D score of -48 for all four conditions,
well as the affective priming task. Overall, smok- indicating negative implicit attitudes. This score
ers attitudes were negative in the IAT (IAT score differed significantly from zero (p<.0001). The
= 31). The IAT revealed significant differences manipulation of smoking abstinence also had an
depending on the smoking status of participants. effect. Smokers refraining from smoking for 12
Smokers (IAT = 32) were less negative than non- hours showed negative implicit attitudes, which
smokers (IAT = 104), and light smokers were more were independent of whether they were allowed
negative (IAT score = 50) than heavy smokers to smoke before the experiment (IAT D score =
(IAT score = 7, p < .05). Deprivation status did -48) or not (IAT D score = -41). Among the non-
not affect the IAT scores. More specifically, there abstinent smokers, smokers who were allowed
were no significant differences between deprived to smoke before the experiment (IAT D score =
light (IAT score = 45), deprived heavy (IAT score -64) were more negative than those not allowed
= -7), non-deprived light (IAT score = 54), and (IAT D score = -46), and the two other groups
non-deprived heavy smokers (IAT score = 22). (p < .05). In addition, the authors reported that
The affective priming scores, however, showed a IAT scores were associated with nicotine depen-
different pattern. Although no differences in affec- dence (Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence,
tive scores were found between smokers (score = Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker, & Fagerström,
17) and non-smokers (score = 34), priming scores 1991), whereby low dependence smokers had more
yielded differences between the four smoking negative implicit attitudes than high dependence
groups. Light smokers’ implicit attitudes were smokers.
more negative when deprived (score = 25) than Focusing on internal cues such as the urge to
when not deprived (score = 6), whereas heavy smoke, Mogg and colleagues (2003) investigated
smokers’ implicit attitudes were more negative implicit stimulus valence among 20 British daily
when not deprived (score = 54) than when deprived smokers and 23 non-smokers. The sample included
(score = -7, p < .05). Heavy smokers who were university students and staff. Those approach and
non-deprived were even more negative than non- avoidance tendencies reflect how smokers implic-
smokers. The authors did not investigate whether itly evaluate smoking and can thus be interpreted
all these scores differed from zero. as implicit attitudes. In this study, smoking and
Thus, implicit attitudes varied as a function of non-smoking pictures were implemented in an
nicotine deprivation as internal cues. In a similar SRC task. Results showed positive implicit at-
vein, Waters and colleagues (2007) applied the titudes among smokers (difference score = 153)
IAT with smoking and neutral pictures among a whereby the valence was significantly different
community sample of 57 smokers, all smoking from zero (p < .01). In a correlational design,
at least 10 CPD. Additionally, one half of the the authors also investigated the relationship
sample were asked to refrain from smoking for between implicit attitudes, attention for smoking

150

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

stimuli, and the urge to smoke. They found that Only one study investigated the combined/
higher attention for smoking stimuli was positively interactive effect of external and internal cues.
related with implicit attitudes (r = .49, p < .05). Rydell, Sherman, Boucher, and Macy (2012) com-
Although the authors did not report on the correla- bined external cues (Public Service Announce-
tion between urge to smoke and implicit attitudes, ment message) with internal cues (deprivation).
from the paper it can be inferred that there was They instructed 50 smokers, who smoked at least
a relationship between the two measures. These 10 CPD, to refrain from smoking for at least four
results were supported by findings of Payne and hours before the session started. The context was
colleagues (2007), who reported that although varied by allocating participants randomly to two
the implicit attitudes of their smokers group as a different groups. One group was allowed to smoke
whole were neutral, within this group differences a cigarette immediately before the session began
related to withdrawal symptoms were found. More (non-deprived), whereas the other group was not
specifically, smokers experiencing more with- allowed to smoke (deprived). The authors also
drawal symptoms had positive implicit attitudes manipulated context via the level of forcefulness
whilst smokers experiencing fewer withdrawal of a Public Service Announcement message,
symptoms had negative attitudes. thereby providing half of the smokers in each
Implementing nicotine dependence instead deprivation group with a message containing a
of urge to smoke as internal cue, Mogg, Field, & strong argument (e.g. Smoking causes death) and
Bradley (2005) applied the SRC with smoking the remaining half with a message containing a
and non-smoking pictures among a university weak argument (Smoking induces hair loss). The
sample (students and staff) of 41 British smokers IAT administered contrasted smoking pictures
(≥ 1 CPD). Additionally, they assessed nicotine with geometric shapes. Non-deprived smokers’
dependence using the modified version of the implicit attitudes (IAT D score = -55) were more
Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire (Prokhorov negative than those of deprived smokers (IAT D
et al., 2000). They found that smokers’ implicit score = -38, p = .05). The study also revealed an
attitudes toward smoking were positive (p < .01) effect of message forcefulness, but only when
and that smokers with low nicotine dependence smokers were non-deprived. More specifically,
were more positive than smokers with moder- non-deprived smokers’ implicit attitudes were
ate nicotine dependence (p < .05). The authors more negative after confrontation with a strong
did not report exact descriptive statistics for the than with a weak message (p = .02), whereas for
SRC measure. They found a positive relation- deprived smokers, no effect of message forceful-
ship between the implicit attitudes measure and ness could be found (exact values of this interaction
attention to smoking stimuli (r = .53, p < .01), were not reported). As a whole, and independent
in other words more positive attitudes were as- of the external and internal cues, implicit attitudes
sociated with an attentional bias to smoking cues, were negative among smokers.
and a negative relationship between the implicit
attitudes measure and CPD (r = -.33, p < .05),
i.e. more positive attitudes were associated with DISCUSSION
fewer cigarettes smoked per day. In line with this
negative relationship, longer time since smoking Among the reviewed studies, 13 studies adminis-
the last cigarette and lower levels of nicotine tered a variant of the IAT using various stimulus
dependence were associated with more positive materials and contrast categories. As various as
implicit attitudes (r = .31, p <.06 and r = -.39, p the materials were, the implicit attitudes were.
< .05, respectively). Four studies reported positive implicit attitudes,

151

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

six negative ones, and three neutral ones. The Commission, 2012; World Health Organization,
findings depended on the contrast category and 2013), and are more and more aware of the harmful
variant of the IAT used, whereby only those studies consequences of their smoking behavior (Glock &
that indicated to what extent IAT scores differed Kneer, 2009; Hahn & Renner, 1998; Wiers & de
from zero, allowed for conclusions about the Jong, 2006), about a fifth of all adult Europeans
nature of implicit attitudes toward smoking. The and Americans still smoke and continue to smoke.
contrast category “stealing” was used in two stud- One study considered that implicit attitudes
ies (Robinson et al., 2005; Swanson et al., 2001), toward smoking reflect global attitudes and that
revealing positive attitudes. The contrast category those global attitudes might be neutral rather
“exercise” was used in three studies (Huijding et than positive or negative (Sherman et al., 2003).
al., 2005; Perugini, 2005; Swanson et al., 2001), Due to positive and negative aspects of smoking
always yielding negative attitudes toward smok- (Hendricks & Brandon, 2005; Mullenix et al.,
ing, as did the contrast category “eating sweets”, 2003), smoking can be perceived both positive
which was used once (Swanson et al., 2001). From and negative (Sherman et al., 2003), depending
these studies one could conclude that smoking on which aspects are prevalent in the assessment
behavior is implicitly liked more than negative situation. Sherman and colleagues (2003) showed
contrast behaviors, but less than positive contrast that smoking is related to different aspects which
behaviors, by both smokers and non-smokers, with are implicitly associated with different automatic
varying differences between the two groups. With evaluations. Cost information was negatively
more neutral contrast categories, the variance in evaluated, while sensory information was posi-
implicit attitudes increased. Attitudes were posi- tively evaluated. However, the IAT seemed to be
tive, negative, as well as neutral. This diversity less sensitive in detecting those differences in
in results might be due to how implicit attitudes implicit attitudes than the affective priming task
toward smoking were measured in these studies. (Sherman et al., 2003). The IAT is suggested to
Although the results generally show that smok- have more controlled components than the affec-
ing is regarded worse than some behaviors (e.g. tive priming task (Fazio & Olson, 2003; Sherman
exercise) but better than others (e.g. stealing), et al., 2003), as it involves the pairing of smoking
they are limited in the extent to which they can with positive and negative words, whereas the
guide the development of effective intervention priming task only asks participants to indicate
strategies. In addition, most of the studies assessed the valence of the presented words independent
global attitudes (Huijding et al., 2005), which of the category in question. Furthermore, the IAT
might reflect societal norms (De Houwer et al., measures the association of the category label with
2006; Fazio & Olson, 2003; Olson & Fazio, 2004) the positive and negative categories rather than the
rather than individual attitudes. association of the specific exemplars (presented
Interestingly, in a personalized IAT with “I as members of the category) with the positive
like” and “I dislike” as categories, implicit attitudes and negative categories (De Houwer, 2001).
were found to be positive (De Houwer et al., 2006). Thus, it seems obvious that the affective priming
This study shows that when avoiding the influence task, which assesses the facilitation of positive or
of societal norms by personalization, smokers’ negative automatic evaluations as a function of
implicit attitudes towards smoking are positive. the presented exemplars, is more sensitive to the
Although replication of this result is necessary, different aspects of smoking than the IAT. The
this result fits the seemingly contradicting notion affective priming task was used in four of the
that although smokers generally support regula- reviewed studies, published in three publications
tions to ban smoking in public areas (European (Glock et al., 2013, 2012; Sherman et al., 2003)

152

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

and revealing mixed attitudes. Nonetheless, the Other studies, investigated the influence of anti-
IAT remains the most popular method for the as- tobacco messages such as advertisements, graphic
sessment of implicit attitudes. Two studies used warning labels, and labels turning positive out-
an ST-IAT, one showing negative attitudes (Süs- come expectancies into negative, revealed mixed
senbach et al., 2013) and one revealing positive results. Advertisements (Czyzewska & Ginsburg,
attitudes (Huijding & de Jong, 2006). Among the 2007) and graphic warning labels (Süssenbach et
reviewed studies, other assessment methods were al., 2013) did not influence implicit attitudes, while
sparse. Two studies used the SRC (Mogg et al., labels contradicting positive outcome expectancies
2003, 2005) and found positive attitudes among did (Glock et al., 2012). However, these studies
smokers. This method is similar to the approach- did not consider the interaction with other context
and-avoid-IAT used in the study by De Houwer relevant variables, such as situational context or
and colleagues (2006), who also reported posi- nicotine dependence, which might contribute on
tive implicit attitudes. The AMP was used once, how messages are processed.
showing neutral implicit attitudes (Payne et al., Nicotine dependence might indeed be an
2007), as did the AST (Huijding et al., 2005). The important context-relevant variable. Smoking is
one study using the GNAT found positive implicit highly addictive because of positive reinforcement
attitudes among a very small sample of smokers with each single puff (Bandura, 2004; Shadel,
(Bassett & Dabbs, 2005). Shiffman, Niaura, Nichter, & Abrams, 2000).
In this review, studies that manipulated the Moreover, nicotine dependence is related to the
context to investigate its effect on implicit attitudes automatic components inherent in smoking (Ber-
were also included. These studies considered the lin et al., 2003) – those components of behaviors
context-sensitivity of implicit attitudes (Blair, which are suggested to be mainly affected by
2002; De Houwer, 2006), which is particularly implicit attitudes (Fazio, 1990; Olson & Fazio,
relevant for addictive behaviors, as substance- 2009). Thus, the level of nicotine dependence
related cognitions have been shown to be highly might have an influence on implicit attitudes.
context sensitive (Krank et al., 2005; Reich et al., Three studies could show this relationship. Us-
2004; Sherman et al., 2003; Wiers et al., 2003). ing a correlational design, Mogg and colleagues
There were two studies which directly manipulated (2005) found a positive association between de-
the context in which the assessment took place pendence and attitudes, i.e. the higher the nicotine
(Glock et al., 2013; Huijding et al., 2005). Implicit dependence, the more positive implicit attitudes.
attitudes were assessed in a lab that smelled of Similarly, Waters and colleagues (2007) reported
burned cigarettes (Glock et al., 2013), and in a lab that implicit attitudes of high dependence smokers
in which smoking was allowed and the experi- were more positive than those of low dependence
menter lit up a cigarette (Huijding et al., 2005). smokers. Sherman and colleagues (2003) opera-
Although different methods were used (IAT, AST, tionalized nicotine dependence via the smoking
affective priming) and Sherman and colleagues level of their participants, dividing them in heavy
(2003) demonstrated the higher sensitivity of the and light smokers. It is plausible that the higher
affective priming task, this context manipulation the number of smoked CPD is, the higher the
had no effect on implicit attitudes in both studies. nicotine dependence is (Heatherton et al., 1991).
One may conclude that the situational context The IAT revealed differences between light and
alone is not the main factor determining implicit heavy smokers, implying that light smokers were
attitudes, and that the interaction with other fac- implicitly less positive toward smoking than heavy
tors should also be considered. smokers, however, the affective priming measure

153

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

did not show differences as a function of smoking CONCLUSION


level. The result of the IAT is in line with findings
that heavy smokers show less negative affect in This review revealed factors that should be consid-
regards to smoking than light smokers (Shiffman ered when investigating implicit attitudes toward
& Paty, 2006). Thus, nicotine dependence might be smoking. First, methodological issues such as the
a context factor that influences implicit attitudes; contrast categories used in implicit measures, as
nonetheless, it seems not to be sufficient to induce well as the different implicit measures turned out
differences in implicit attitudes. to be more or less sensitive for different aspects
Another factor, obviously related to nicotine of smoking, or individual attitudes versus societal
dependence, is deprivation. Higher levels of norms. Thus, in future research, different methods
deprivation lead to higher levels of craving, i.e. should be applied within one sample in order
the felt need for a cigarette (Shadel et al., 2000). to ensure that differences in implicit attitudes,
Craving in turn, results in a motivational state which cannot be detected by one method, might
which leads to action (Marlatt & Gordon, 1985), in be revealed by the other. Moreover, using different
this case, smoking a cigarette. Hence deprivation methods within one sample and applying the same
reinforces craving. Deprivation was experimen- stimulus materials within the different implicit at-
tally manipulated in three studies (Rydell et al., titudes measures, can shed light on the question of
2012; Sherman et al., 2003; Waters et al., 2007) whether the inconsistent results, identified in this
and two others used a correlational design (Mogg review, might be due to different methods or due
et al., 2003; Payne et al., 2007). Positive associa- to different stimuli materials. This review has also
tions were found, more specifically, the higher the shown that although the IAT is the most applied
urge to smoke, the more positive implicit attitudes method, future research should consider the fact
(Mogg et al., 2003; Payne et al., 2007). The over- that the classical IAT, with pleasant and unpleasant
all pattern of results showed that deprivation led categories, assessed cultural knowledge and social
to more positive attitudes than non-deprivation norms rather than individual attitudes (De Houwer
(Rydell et al., 2012; Waters et al., 2007) and that et al., 2006; Robinson et al., 2005). In this regard,
deprivation exhibited its influence in interaction this review has shown that methods such as the
with other factors. Interestingly, heavy smokers affective priming task are more sensitive than the
were more negative when not deprived, whereas IAT and that replacing the pleasant and unpleasant
light smokers were more negative when deprived categories in a personalized variant of the IAT
(Sherman et al., 2003). Thus, nicotine depriva- revealed more individual attitudes. Thus, future
tion, rather than dependence, was identified as research should apply other implicit measures or
an important factor influencing implicit attitudes the personalized variant of the IAT, particularly
and should not be neglected when investigating if conclusions about individual implicit attitudes
implicit attitudes toward smoking. Deprivation toward smoking should be made.
level also determined how anti-tobacco mes- Furthermore, although implicit attitudes
sages were processed (Rydell et al., 2012). Only research seems to lead to inconsistent results,
non-deprived smokers were influenced by strong they should be considered when investigating
anti-tobacco messages – they had more negative the effects of intervention campaigns (Wiers, de
attitudes after perception of the messages, while Jong, Havermans, & Jelicic, 2004). Few studies
deprived smokers did not show an influence of have examined the relationship between implicit
anti-tobacco messages on implicit attitudes (Rydell attitudes and maintenance or cessation of smok-
et al., 2012). This finding seems to be crucial for ing behavior, although such studies could inform
the effectiveness of anti-tobacco messages. intervention. For example, Chassin et al. (2010)

154

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

demonstrated that implicit attitudes were associ- related to smoking interventions administered di-
ated with giving up smoking, but only under certain rectly after smoking might turn into more positive
conditions. Implicit attitudes toward smoking have ones during deprivation, thereby neutralizing the
been rarely assessed as a result of interventions, effects of the interventions. This also implies that
but may be an indication of its effectiveness and implicit attitude changes due to smoking inter-
hence future research should introduce implicit ventions might only be short-term. Thus, future
attitudes measures when evaluating interventions. research should (a) investigate whether the impact
For instance, it has recently been decided that of smoking interventions on implicit attitudes
graphic warning labels will be mandatory in all changes, not only between different deprivation
countries of the EU (European Parliament, 2013), groups, but also within smokers experiencing
however, there is only one study investigating the different deprivation levels during the daily rou-
influence of graphic warning labels on implicit tine. This research design involves the need for
attitudes. Obviously, implicit attitudes are not the repeated implicit attitudes measures as a function
only determinant that should be looked at when of smoking intervention and deprivation level
investigating interventions, but they might be a and the question of long-term attitude changes.
causal (De Houwer et al., 2006), or at least mod- Research on implicit racial cognition has shown
erating factor for smoking behavior (Huijding et that changes in implicit cognition might last for
al., 2005). Interventions might exhibit differential 24 hours (Kawakami, Dovidio, Moll, Hermsen, &
influences on implicit and explicit levels (Wiers, Russin, 2000), whereby motivational states such as
Luitgaarden, Wildenberg, & Smulders, 2005), deprivation certainly did not play a role. Thus, we
hence to rely on explicitly assessed variables only do not know how long-lasting intervention effects
seems to be insufficient to evaluate interventions. are, and how they might change as a function of
Thus, future research conducted for intervention deprivation. This highlights the need for smok-
evaluation purposes should investigate the effect ing interventions that might remain unaffected
on both implicit and explicit levels. by nicotine dependence and deprivation level.
All reviewed studies that assessed internal cues Therefore, future research should also (b) search
such as nicotine-dependence, deprivation, or urge for smoking interventions not susceptible to depri-
to smoke, revealed that these internal cues are of vation levels of smokers. This is one of the great
high relevance when assessing implicit attitudes. challenges in the future. A factor which could be
This review identified nicotine dependency and used successfully in interventions is self-efficacy
especially nicotine deprivation as vital factors enhancing information, as self-efficacy plays an
influencing implicit attitudes, demonstrating their important role in behavioral change (Bandura,
context-sensitivity. This context-sensitivity might 2004; Peters, Ruiter, & Kok, 2013; Schwarzer,
prove critical when it comes to the effectiveness of 2001). Self-efficacy enhancing interventions have
interventions. Even though the context-sensitivity been shown to lead to more continuous abstinent
provides opportunities for influencing implicit smokers than other interventions (Dijkstra, De
attitudes, at the same time they may undermine Vries, & Roijackers, 1998), and smokers who
the effectiveness of interventions. Non-deprived prepare to quit benefit most from this information
smokers might be affected by interventions, (Dijkstra, De Vries, Roijackers, & van Breukelen,
whereas deprived smokers might be unaffected. 1998). However, whether this intervention strategy
Moreover, and probably most importantly, in- might be affected by deprivation level and whether
tervention effectiveness might change within this intervention is able to affect implicit attitudes,
individual smokers as a function of deprivation is not known so far. Probably, the effectiveness
level. Stated otherwise, negative implicit attitudes of this intervention strategy changes as implicit

155

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

attitudes change as a function deprivation level. Blair, I. (2002). The malleability of automatic
Most importantly, this review shows the evolution stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and Social
in understanding the role of implicit attitudes in Psychology Review, 6(3), 242–261. doi:10.1207/
smoking. From the different studies it has become S15327957PSPR0603_8
clear that the relationship between implicit atti-
Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & McCaslin, M. J. (2009).
tudes and smoking behavior is complex and that
Changing attitudes on implicit versus explicit
not the focus on the implicit attitudes per se but
measures: What is the difference. In R. E. Petty,
that especially the understanding of the effect of
R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights
implicit attitudes on behavior in relation to the
from the new implicit measures (pp. 285–326).
situational context and internal motivation may
New York, NY: Psychology Press.
lead to targeted and effective intervention.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
(2012). Morbidity and mortality weekly report.
REFERENCES Weekly, 61. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/
mmwr/pdf/wk/mm6144.pdf
Baker, T. B., Brandon, T. H., & Chassin, L. (2004).
Motivational influences on cigarette smoking. Chassin, L., Presson, C. C., Sherman, S. J., Seo,
Annual Review of Psychology, 55(1), 463–491. D.-C., & Macy, J. T. (2010). Implicit and explicit
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142054 attitudes predict smoking cessation: Moderating
PMID:14744223 effects of experienced failure to control smoking
and plans to quit. Psychology of Addictive Be-
Bandura, A. (2004). Health promotion by social haviors, 24(4), 670–679. doi:10.1037/a0021722
cognitive means. Health Education & Behavior, PMID:21198227
31(2), 143–164. doi:10.1177/1090198104263660
PMID:15090118 Czyzewska, M., & Ginsburg, H. J. (2007).
Explicit and implicit effects of anti-marijuana
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of auto- and anti-tobacco TV advertisements. Addictive
maticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and Behaviors, 32(1), 114–127. doi:10.1016/j.ad-
control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. dbeh.2006.03.025 PMID:16675148
Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp.
1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. De Houwer, J. (2001). A structural and process
analysis of the Implicit Association Test. Jour-
Bassett, J. F., & Dabbs, J. M. (2005). A portable nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37(6),
version of the go/no-go association task (GNAT). 443–451. doi:10.1006/jesp.2000.1464
Behavior Research Methods, 37(3), 506–512.
doi:10.3758/BF03192721 PMID:16405147 De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures
and why are we using them? In R. W. Wiers & A. W.
Berlin, I., Singleton, E. G., Pedarriosse, A.-M., Stacy (Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition
Lancrenon, S., Rames, A., Aubin, H.-J., & Niaura, and addiction (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks, CA:
R. (2003). The Modified Reasons for Smoking Sage Publisher. doi:10.4135/9781412976237.n2
Scale: Factorial structure, gender effects and re-
lationship with nicotine dependence and smoking De Houwer, J., Custers, R., & De Clercq, A. (2006).
cessation in French smokers. Addiction (Abing- Do smokers have a negative implicit attitude
don, England), 98(11), 1575–1583. doi:10.1046/ toward smoking? Cognition and Emotion, 20(8),
j.1360-0443.2003.00523.x PMID:14616184 1274–1284. doi:10.1080/02699930500484506

156

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1998). An affective Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object-evaluation
variant of the Simon paradigm. Cognition and Emo- associations of varying strength. Social Cognition,
tion, 12(1), 45–62. doi:10.1080/026999398379772 25(5), 664–703. doi:10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.603
PMID:19424447
De Houwer, J., & Moors, A. (2007). How to
define and examine the implicitness of implicit Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C.,
measures. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in auto-
Implicit measures of attitudes: Procedures and matic activation as an unobtrusive measure of
controversies (pp. 179–194). New York, NY: racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal
Guilford Press. of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6),
1013–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013
Dijkstra, A., De Vries, H., & Roijackers, J. (1998).
PMID:8531054
Long-term effectiveness of computer-generated
tailored feedback in smoking cessation. Health Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit
Education Research, 13(2), 207–214. doi:10.1093/ measures in social cognition research: Their
her/13.2.207 PMID:10181019 meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychol-
ogy, 54(1), 297–327. doi:10.1146/annurev.
Dijkstra, A., De Vries, H., Roijackers, J., & van
psych.54.101601.145225 PMID:12172003
Breukelen, G. (1998). Tailored interventions
to communicate stage-matched information to Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006).
smokers in different motivational stages. Journal Associative and propositional processes in evalua-
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(3), tion: An integrative review of implicit and explicit
549–557. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.66.3.549 attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5),
PMID:9642894 692–731. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692
PMID:16910748
European Parliament. (2013). Tobacco: Larger
warnings, flavours banned, e-cigarettes regu- Glock, S., & Kneer, J. (2009). Are deterrent
lated. Retrieved October 17, 2013, from http:// pictures effective? The impact of warning labels
www.europarl.europa.eu/news/en/news-room/ on cognitive dissonance in smokers. Applied
content/20131004IPR21539/html/Tobacco- Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 1, 356–373.
larger-warnings-flavours-banned-e-cigarettes- doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01019.x
regulated
Glock, S., Kovacs, C., & Unz, D. (2013). Im-
Fazio, R. H. (1990). In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Multiple plicit attitudes toward smoking: How the smell
processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The of cigarettes influences college-age smokers and
MODE model as an integrative framework (Vol. non-smokers. Journal of Health Psychology.
23, pp. 75–109). Advances in Experimental Social doi:10.1177/1359105313476974
Psychology. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Glock, S., Unz, D., & Kovacs, C. (2012). Beyond
Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activa- fear appeals: Contradicting positive smoking
tion of associated evaluations: An overview. outcome expectancies to influence smokers’
Cognition and Emotion, 15(2), 115–141. implicit attitudes, perception, and behavior. Ad-
doi:10.1080/02699930125908 dictive Behaviors, 37(4), 548–551. doi:10.1016/j.
addbeh.2011.11.032 PMID:22178602

157

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, Kahler, C. W., Daughters, S. B., Leventhal, A.
J. K. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences M., Gwaltney, C. J., & Palfai, T. P. (2007). Im-
in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association plicit associations between smoking and social
Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- consequences among smokers in cessation treat-
chology, 74(6), 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022- ment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(9),
3514.74.6.1464 PMID:9654756 2066–2077. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.004
PMID:17448442
Hahn, A., & Renner, B. (1998). Perception of
health risks: How smoker status affects defensive Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen,
optimism. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 11(2), S., & Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (to stereo-
93–112. doi:10.1080/10615809808248307 typing): Effects of training in the negation of
stereotypic associations on stereotype activation.
Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
R. C., & Fagerström, K. O. (1991). The Fager-
78(5), 871–888. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871
ström Test for Nicotine Dependence: A revision
PMID:10821195
of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire.
British Journal of Addiction, 86(9), 1119– Krank, M., Wall, A.-M., Stewart, S. H., Wiers,
1127. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x R. W., & Goldman, M. S. (2005). Context ef-
PMID:1932883 fects on alcohol cognitions. Alcoholism, Clinical
and Experimental Research, 29(2), 196–206.
Hendricks, P. S., & Brandon, T. H. (2005). Smoking
doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000153545.36787.C8
expectancy associates among college smokers. Ad-
PMID:15714043
dictive Behaviors, 30(2), 235–245. doi:10.1016/j.
addbeh.2004.05.006 PMID:15621395 Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (Eds.). (1985). Re-
lapse prevention. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Nosek, B.
A., & Schmitt, M. (2005). What moderates Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Field, M., & De Houwer,
implicit—explicit consistency? European Re- J. (2003). Eye movements to smoking-related pic-
view of Social Psychology, 16(1), 335–390. tures in smokers: Relationship between attentional
doi:10.1080/10463280500443228 biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimu-
lus valence. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 98(6),
Huijding, J., & de Jong, P. (2006). Automatic
825–836. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00392.x
associations with the sensory aspects of smok-
PMID:12780371
ing: Positive in habitual smokers but negative
in non-smokers. Addictive Behaviors, 31(1), Mogg, K., Field, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2005).
182–186. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.04.014 Attentional and approach biases for smoking
PMID:15919160 cues in smokers: An investigation of competing
theoretical views of addiction. Psychopharmacol-
Huijding, J., de Jong, P. J., Wiers, R. W., &
ogy, 180(2), 333–341. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-
Verkooijen, K. (2005). Implicit and explicit
2158-x PMID:15696322
attitudes toward smoking in a smoking and a
nonsmoking setting. Addictive Behaviors, 30(5),
949–961. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.09.014
PMID:15893091

158

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

Mullenix, J. W., Kilbey, M. M., Fisicaro, S. A., Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of
Farnsworth, L. M., & Torrento, R. (2003). The implicit and explicit attitudes. The British
representation of smoking expectancies by single- Journal of Social Psychology, 44(1), 29–45.
word adjectives and their relationsship to smoking doi:10.1348/014466604X23491 PMID:15901390
characteristics. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 5,
Peters, G.-J. Y., Ruiter, R. a. C., & Kok, G. (2013).
681–694. doi:10.1080/1462220031000158654
Threatening communication: A critical re-analysis
PMID:14577985
and a revised meta-analytic test of fear appeal
Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The Go/ theory. Health Psychology Review, 7, S8-S31. do
No-Go-Association Task. Social Cognition, 19(6), i:10.1080/17437199.2012.703527
625–664. doi:10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886
Petty, R. E., Fazio, R. H., & Briñol, P. (2009). The
OECD. (2012a). Health at a Glance: Europe 2012. new implicit measures: An overview. In R. E. Petty,
OECD Publishing. doi:10.1787/9789264183896- R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights
en from the new implicit measures (pp. 3–18). New
York, NY: Psychology Press.
OECD. (2012b). Health at a Glance: Asia/Pacific
2012. OECD Publishing. Prokhorov, A. V., De Moor, C., Pallonent,
U. E., Hudmon, K. S., Koehly, L., & Hu, S.
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing
(2000). Validation of the modified Fagerström
the influence of extra-personal associations on the
Tolerance Questionnaire with salivary cotinine
Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the IAT.
among adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 25(3),
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
429–433. doi:10.1016/S0306-4603(98)00132-4
86(5), 653–667. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.653
PMID:10890296
PMID:15161392
Reich, R. R., Goldman, M. S., & Noll, J. (2004).
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2009). Implicit and
Using the false memory paradigm to test two
explicit measures of attitudes: The perspective of
key elements of alcohol expectancy theory. Ex-
the MODE model. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, &
perimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology,
P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new
12(2), 102–110. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.12.2.102
implicit measures (pp. 19–63). New York, NY:
PMID:15122954
Psychology Press.
Robinson, M. D., Meier, B. P., Zetocha, K. J., &
Payne, B. K., Cheng, S. M., Govorun, O., &
McCaul, K. D. (2005). Smoking and the Implicit
Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes:
Association Test: When the contrast category
Affect misattribution as implicit measurement.
determines the theoretical conclusions. Basic
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
and Applied Social Psychology, 27(3), 201–212.
89(3), 277–293. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277
doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2703_2
PMID:16248714
Rooke, S. E., Hine, D. W., & Thorsteinsson, E.
Payne, B. K., McClernon, F. J., & Dobbins, I. G.
B. (2008). Implicit cognition and substance use:
(2007). Automatic affective responses to smoking
A meta-analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 33(10),
cues. Experimental and Clinical Psychophar-
1314–1328. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.06.009
macology, 15(4), 400–409. doi:10.1037/1064-
PMID:18640788
1297.15.4.400 PMID:17696687

159

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

Rudman, L. A. (2004). Sources of implicit at- Sherman, S. J., Rose, J. S., Koch, K., Presson, C.
titudes. Current Directions in Psychological C., & Chassin, L. (2003). Implicit and explicit
Science, 13(2), 79–82. doi:10.1111/j.0963- attitudes toward cigarette smoking: The effects
7214.2004.00279.x of context and motivation. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 22(1), 13–39. doi:10.1521/
Rudman, L. A., Phelan, J. E., & Heppen, J. B. (2007).
jscp.22.1.13.22766
Developmental sources of implicit attitudes. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(12), Shiffman, S., & Paty, J. (2006). Smoking pat-
1700–1713. doi:10.1177/0146167207307487 terns and dependence: Contrasting chippers
PMID:18000104 and heavy smokers. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
chology, 115(3), 509–523. doi:10.1037/0021-
Ruiter, R. A. C., & Kok, G. (2005). Saying is not (al-
843X.115.3.509 PMID:16866591
ways) doing: Cigarette warning labels are useless.
European Journal of Public Health, 15, 329–330. Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective
doi:10.1093/eurpub/cki095 PMID:15985460 and impulsive determinants of social behavior.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3),
Rydell, R. J., Sherman, S. J., Boucher, K. L., &
220–247. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1
Macy, J. T. (2012). The role of motivational and
PMID:15454347
persuasive message factors in changing implicit
attitudes toward smoking. Basic and Applied So- Süssenbach, P., Niemeier, S., & Glock, S. (2013).
cial Psychology, 34(1), 1–7. doi:10.1080/019735 Effects of and attention to graphic warning labels
33.2011.637847 PMID:23606782 on cigarette packages. Psychology & Health,
28(10), 1192–1206. doi:10.1080/08870446.201
Schuette, R. A., & Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitude
3.799161 PMID:23713958
accessibility and motivation as determinants of
biased processing: A test of the MODE model. Swanson, J. E., Rudman, L. A., & Greenwald, A.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(7), G. (2001). Using the Implicit Association Test
704–710. doi:10.1177/0146167295217005 to investigate attitude-behavior consistency for
stigmatised behavior. Cognition and Emotion,
Schwarzer, R. (2001). Social-cognitive factors in
15(2), 207–230. doi:10.1080/02699930125706
changing health-related behaviors. Current Di-
rections in Psychological Science, 10(2), 47–51. Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug
doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00112 urges and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and
nonautomatic processes. Psychological Review,
Shadel, W. G., Shiffman, S., Niaura, R., Nichter,
97(2), 147–168. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.97.2.147
M., & Abrams, D. B. (2000). Current models of
PMID:2186423
nicotine dependence: What is known and what
is needed to advance understanding of tobacco Waters, A. J., Carter, B. L., Robinson, J. D.,
etiology among youth. Drug and Alcohol De- Wetter, D. W., Lam, C. Y., & Cinciripini, P. M.
pendence, 59(Suppl 1), S9–S22. doi:10.1016/ (2007). Implicit attitudes to smoking are associated
S0376-8716(99)00162-3 PMID:10773435 with craving and dependence. Drug and Alcohol
Dependence, 91(2-3), 178–186. doi:10.1016/j.
drugalcdep.2007.05.024 PMID:17658701

160

Smoking, Implicit Attitudes, and Context-Sensitivity

Wiers, R. W., & de Jong, P. J. (2006). Implicit KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS
and explicit alcohol, smoking and drug-related
cognitions and emotions. In J. Z. Arlsdale (Ed.), Affect Misattribution Procedure: Priming-
Advances in social psychology research (Vol. based technique to assess affective responses to
005, pp. 1–35). New York, NY: Nova Science cues.
Publishers. Affective Priming: Latency-based technique
to assess memory activation.
Wiers, R. W., de Jong, P. J., Havermans, R., & Affective Priming Score: Relative facilitation
Jelicic, M. (2004). How to change implicit drug of positive words opposed to negative words and
use-related cognitions in prevention: A transdisci- compared with a baseline measure of positive and
plinary integration of findings from experimental negative words, whereby negative scores reflect
psychopathology, social cognition, memory, and positive attitudes.
experimental learning psychology. Substance Use Affective Simon Task: Latency-based tech-
& Misuse, 39(10-12), 1625–1684. doi:10.1081/ nique to assess influences of stimulus valence.
JA-200033206 PMID:15587947 Extrinsic Affective Simon Task: Latency-
Wiers, R. W., Luitgaarden, J., Wildenberg, E., & based technique to assess the association between
Smulders, F. T. Y. (2005). Challenging implicit and a singular target and its evaluations.
explicit alcohol-related cognitions in young heavy Go / No Go Association Task: Latency
drinkers. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 100(6), based-technique to assess the association strength
806–819. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01064.x between a singular target and its evaluations.
PMID:15918811 Implicit Association Test: Latency-based
technique to assess the association strength be-
Wiers, R. W., & Stacy, A. W. (2006). Implicit tween two targets and their evaluations.
cognition and addiction. Current Directions Implicit attitudes: Automatic evaluations of
in Psychological Science, 15(6), 292–296. an object.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00455.x Implicit attitudes measures: Methods to
Wiers, R. W., Wood, M. D., Darkes, J., Corbin, W. measure a construct without asking participants
R., Jones, B. T., & Sher, K. J. (2003). Changing directly.
expectancies: Cognitive mechanisms and context Outcome expectancy: Expected consequenc-
effects. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimen- es of a behavior.
tal Research, 27(2), 186–197. doi:10.1097/01. Public Service Announcement: Messages to
ALC.0000051023.28893.8A PMID:12605068 influence public attitudes and behavior, contrib-
uted by the media.
World Health Organization. (2011). WHO report Stimulus Response Compatibility Task:
on the global tobacco epidemic, 2011: Warning Latency-based technique to assess the tendency
about the dangers of tobacco. WHO. to avoid or approach a stimulus.
World Health Organization. (2013). WHO report
on the global tobacco epidemic, 2013: Enforcing
bans on tobacco advertising: Executive summary.
WHO.

161
162

Chapter 8
Function over Form:
A Behavioral Approach to Implicit Attitudes

Anthony G. O’Reilly
National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland

Bryan Roche
National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland

Aoife Cartwright
National University of Ireland – Maynooth, Ireland

ABSTRACT
Research surrounding the construct of “implicit attitudes” and the various methodologies for measuring
that construct is currently founded on the social cognitive paradigm. However, no robust and agreed
upon theoretical framework has emerged from this paradigm, despite the widespread adoption of im-
plicit testing methodologies and their associated theoretical assumptions. The current chapter outlines
a functional approach to implicit testing, describing research stemming from Relational Frame Theory
that was developed in parallel with the emergence of the IAT, and arguing for the benefits of connect-
ing these two strands of research to improve the understanding of attitude behaviors and create better
understood implicit testing methodologies. The chapter concludes with descriptions of two examples of
such methodologies: the IRAP and the FAST.

INTRODUCTION These mental representations are conceptualized


in terms of hypothetical constructs infered from
Form: The Implicit Attitude Construct observable behavior, which are in turn thought
to explain that behavior. In the field of Social
The central pillar of the cognitive paradigm is the Psychology, there is no more ubiquitous construct
position that mental representations mediate how than the Attitude.
information is perceived, processed, analyzed, Despite its central place in explaining human
stored in the brain, and that these representations social behavior (Allport, 1935), there is no uni-
precede behavior in the chain of cause and effect. versally agreed upon definition of what precisely

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch008

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Function over Form

is represented by an attitude. There is, however, of behavior. More specifically, the behavior was
broad agreement on the general form of the attitude considered to be mediated by implicit attitudes,
construct. An attitude is usually defined as being which in turn can be defined as “the introspec-
a combination of cognitive (i.e. propositional) and tively unidentified (or inaccurately identified)
affective evaluations of an object with a variable trace of past experience that mediates [favorable
strength (Olson & Kendrick, 2008). An attitude or unfavorable feeling, thought or action towards
is thought to be stored in the mind as a set of as- social objects]” (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995, p.8).
sociations between the attitude object and these The seminal IAT experiment used a “known
evaluations, with attitude strength being a function groups” paradigm, presenting participants with
of the relative accessibility of these associations. flower names (e.g. Tulip), “insect” names (e.g.
When activated, attitudes predispose the person Spider), pleasant words (e.g. Love) and unpleas-
towards a favorable or unfavorable behavioral ant words (e.g. Ugly) and required participants
response towards the attitude object (see Olson to categorize them by means of a key press.
& Zanna, 1993, for a review). Attitudes can be (Greenwald, McGhee & Schwartz, 1998). In the
formed in relation to any given object including first (consistent) condition, the same response
other individuals (e.g., McConnell, Rydell, Strain, key was assigned to both “flower” and “pleasant”
& Mackie, 2008), social groups (e.g., Dovidio, words, while the other response key was assigned
Kawakami, & Gaertner, 2002), or even abstract to “insect” and “unpleasant” words. In the second
concepts (e.g., nationality; Devos & Banaji, (inconsistent) condition, one response key was
2005). Stereotypes, prejudice, self-esteem, general assigned to “unpleasant” words and “flower”
positive or negative evaluations and biases all fall words, and the other to “pleasant” and “insect”
under the umbrella of “attitude”. As such, attitude words. The researchers found (as expected) that
research has historically been a key topic in social reaction times were shorter in the consistent con-
cognitive research, and it is likely to continue to dition than in the inconsistent condition. In line
be so for many years to come. However, attitude with the pre-experimental assumptions outlined
research underwent a minor revolution in the last above, Greenwald and colleagues stated that the
years of the 20th century, with the introduction of IAT effect (i.e., the difference in response times
the concept of implicit attitudes. between the two conditions) was indicative of
Greenwald and Banajii’s 1995 paper simul- the existence of an implicit attitude construct in
taneously introduced the concept of implicit which flowers were associated with positive evalu-
attitudes and a methodology designed to detect ations and insects with negative associations. The
this new hypothetical construct – The Implicit magnitude of the difference between the normed
Association Test (IAT). Drawing upon research reaction times in each condition is taken to be an
in implicit memory, the authors described how indicator of the associative strength between the
past experience can influence present attitudes category of interest and a positive/negative at-
and the responses mediated by those attitudes tribute (e.g., Hoffman, Gawronski, Geschwnder,
without conscious awareness. Central to the new Le, & Schmidt, 2005). The implicit associations
concept was the suggestion that some experiences so measured by the IAT are assumed to be rela-
lead to the formation of evaluative associations tively stable, trait-like cognitive associations that
(i.e., attitudes) that were not readily accessible by are existent objects in the individual (Nosek &
introspection and whose influence on behavior is Hanson, 2008).
outwith the control of the subject. The implicit Despite the widespread adoption of the IAT
attitude construct was said to explain why self- methodology and the tacit acceptance of its theo-
reported attitudes were not reliable predictors retical assumptions, Greenwald and his colleague

163

Function over Form

are “theory uncommitted” (Greenwalk, Nosek, from the bottom up, in well defined technical
Banaji & Klauer, 2005) with regard to the pre- terms, each and every apsect of an implicit test
cise structure of the mental associations the IAT performance, as well as identifying the sources
purports to measure, and with regard to precisely of historical and environmental control over test
what mental construct causes the IAT effect. The outcomes. This approach yields a far less specula-
main empircal evidence for Greenwald’s account tive account of the processes involved, which in
of implicit attitudes (which informed the creation turn leads to more robust theory.
of the IAT) comes from the IAT effect itself, which Jan DeHouwer (2011) has outlined the benefits
it turn relies on the associative assumption within of integrating functionally oriented research into
the implicit cognition theory in order to explain the the cognitive paradigm, leading to what he calls
effect. This position represents a form of circular a functional-cognitive framework. DeHouwer
reasoning, rendering the IAT and implicit social argued that the functional approach is useful to
cognition research more generally lacking in a the social cognitivist in that it provides useful and
unaminously agreed-upon testable model than actionable information about the environmental
can explain the IAT effect (Fiedler, Messner, & causes of behavior and the environmental variables
Blumke, 2006; Gavin, Roche & Ruiz, 2008; Roche, that can be experimentally manipulated to produce
O’Reilly, Gavin, Ruiz, & Aranciba, 2012). There or alter a behavioral effect (such as the IAT ef-
is widespread concern in the social cognitive field fect) without requiring any reference to mental
(e.g., Blanton & Jaccard 2006; De Houwer, 2009; constructs as causal events. This information
Fazio & Olson, 2003; Gawronsk, Lebel, Peters & allows the cognitively oriented psychologist to
Banse, 2009; Hughes et al, 2011; Rothermund & make more informed inferences about the mental
Wentura, 2004) regarding the “deplorable discon- constructs assumed to mediate such behavioral
nect between basic research on the mechanisms effects by eliminating a priori assumptions about
underlying implicit measures and the somewhat the processes underpinning those constructs and
wider reception of research using these methods” by providing clear information about the input to
(Gawronski, Lebel & Peters, 2007). those mental processes. The functional approach
It is precisely that “deplorable disconnect” that informs the cognitive approach as to the facts (be-
this chapter aims to address. Using behavioral haviors) that need to be accounted for with mental
manifestations such as response latencies of as- explanations, without reference to those mental
sumed cognitive structures, such as unconscious explanations themselves. This is a pragmatic ap-
bias, to infer the existence of those very constructs proach which puts aside the philosophical differ-
is the source of much of the conceptual ambuguity ences regarding the ontological status of mental
and disagreement surrounding the theory underly- representations in favor of developing a research
ing implicit attitude research (De Houwer, 2011). program with strong theoretical underpinnings
When the sources of an effect are hypothethical and on the process level, leaving the debate regarding
conceptual controlling the outcomes of tests for the neccesity of mental representations as causal
those processes becomes a haphazard endeavour, objects to a future in which the body of evidence
in which changes in experimental methodology is more complete.
are dictated by a disputed or ambiguous point of
theory. Functionally oriented approaches to the Function: The Stimulus Equivalence/
analysis of behavior aim their scrutiny directly Relational Frame Theory Account
at the underlying mechanisms of behavior. A
behavior-analytic account of implicit attitudes As it happens, there is already a wealth of function-
and implicit testing methodologies can explain ally-oriented behavior-analytic work examining

164

Function over Form

the same phenomena that inspired the Implicit believe that RFT can be a powerful explanatory
Social Cognition revolution and the development tool for researchers interested in implicit attitudes.
of the IAT. This work has been proceeding in Relational Frame Theory offers the attitude re-
parrallel with the development of the the IAT, searcher an account of language and cognition that
with the two threads only rarely making contact. is founded upon a single core process – relational
The following section will detail the history of framing. This process is precisely articulated in a
the behavior-analytic research that informs this bottom up account that specifies the interactions
functional account of implicit attitudes, before necessary between organism and environment (see
bringing the threads together by offering a func- below) for this behavioral process to emerge. The
tional account of the Implicit Association Test bottom-up, functional-analytic approach taken by
and of Implicit Attitudes. RFT builds incrementally from elementary core
Relational Frame Theory (RFT: Hayes, processes, but retains the explanatory power nec-
Barnes-Holmes & Roche, 2001) emerged from essary to tackle more complex forms of behavior
the behavior-analytic literature around the same with the same precise terminology. The relational
time that Implicit Social Cognition research began framing account of language has the potential to
to take off in the social-cognitive mainstream, shed light on the fundamental processes which
and while the methodologies and theoretical/ underlie attitude constructs, allowing for a more
philosophical underpinnings of the work are quite nuanced theoretical account of their formation
different, there are many interesting parallels in the and change, and to guide research into method-
subject matter of both fields. As RFT will likely be ologies which might more accurately and reliably
unfamiliar to many readers, the authors will now measure attitudes.
describe its basic premises and development with Much of the research that underpins current
regard to implicit attitude phenomena in detail. thinking on relational framing has grown from the
The RFT approach is a modern behavioral ac- discovery of the phenomenon of stimulus equiva-
count of human language and cognition that has lence over forty years ago. The way in which this
proved able to tackle questions long thought to simple phenomenon was studied and built upon
be out of reach of the behavior-analytic approach. exemplifies the functional approach.
RFT has made a multitude of advances in the Murray Sidman’s (1971) investigations into
behavioral understanding of such areas of cogni- this phenomenon began while conducting research
tion as analogical reasoning (Carpenter, Smeets & into the behavior of participants who experienced
Barnes-Holmes, 2003; Stewart & Barnes-Holmes difficulty reading, writing, and speaking. He used
2004; Stewart, Barnes-Holmes & Weil, 2009), a Matching-to-Sample (MTS) procedure, a con-
assessment and training of intellectual skills ditional discrimination procedure in which two
(e.g., Cassidy, Roche & Hayes, 2011; O’Toole & stimuli (let’s call them B1 and B2) are presented
Barnes-Holmes, 2009), in applied developmental as response options to be discriminated between
and clinical arenas (McHugh, Barnes-Holmes & and the correct response is determined by the
Barnes-Holmes, 2004; Rehfeldt, Dillen, Ziomek & presence of the conditional stimulus (let’s call it
Kowalchuck, 2007; Villatte, Monestes, McHugh, A1). In effect, an “if-then” relation is established
Freixa i Baque & Loas, 2010a, 2010b; Weil, Hayes for the child, such as “If A1 is present, select B1
& Capurro, 2011) and generative verbal behavior rather than B2”. Or, “If A2 is present, select B2,
in developmentally delayed children (e.g., Heagle rather than B1”. In Sidman’s study each trial
& Rehfeldt, 2006; Moran, Stewart, McElwee & involved the presentation of a sample stimulus,
Ming, 2010; Murphy & Barnes-Holmes 2009). In either a picture of the object to be named (e.g.,
addition to its success in these areas, the authors a picture of a cat), a word (e.g., “cat”), or an

165

Function over Form

auditory stimulus (e.g., the word “cat” spoken a strong link between stimulus equivalence and
aloud to the participant). In matching tests, the language. With the emergence of this new phe-
participants were required to choose the correct nomenon, behavior analysts began developing a
comparison stimulus (a picture or a word) from new model of verbal behavior involving derived
an array of eight choices (pictures or words). In or emergent stimulus relations (For a full review
oral naming tests, the participant was required to of the evidence linking the stimulus equivalence
name the sample stimulus aloud. phenomenon to language the reader is referred
Using the matching to sample procedure, the to Dymond and Roche, 2013). Expanding their
researchers taught the first participant to match explanation beyond simple stimulus equivalence
the spoken word samples to the correct written into more varied forms of stimulus relations,
word. Without any further direct training, the par- behavior analysts developed Relational Frame
ticipant was then able to match written words to Theory, which in turn informs the functional
pictures (and vice versa) and to name the written account of implicit testing and implicit attitudes
words. This emergent behavior caused significant described later in this chapter.
excitement for Sidman and his collaborators (Sid- The Relational Frame Theory account of lan-
man, 1982). guage and cognition draws upon and elaborates
The vital finding was that teaching two sets of the stimulus equivalence phenomenon. As well as
conditional discriminations caused novel behav- being able to discriminate (i.e., detect and respond
iors to emerge without direct training. This early to) specific stimuli, organisms are also capable
glimpse of the phenomenon caused Sidman to of responding to relations between stimuli such
focus his research on defining stimulus equiva- as similarity, difference, distance, greater than,
lence and establishing the necessary and sufficient and so on. These forms of responding are known
historical and current conditions required to pro- collectively as relational responding or relational
duce and test it in the laboratory. framing. Nonverbal organisms are capable of
Stimulus equivalence was defined procedurally learning to respond to such formal relations as
(Sidman, 1982) as responding that displays the size and distance, via traditional learning pro-
properties of reflexivity, transitivity, and sym- cesses such as operant conditioning (see Reese,
metry. When a verbally able human participant is 1968). Verbal organisms, however, display the
trained in (at least) two conditional discriminations unique ability to respond to arbitrary stimulus
(e.g., given A1 pick B1 and not B2, given A2 pick relations such as oppositeness, value and time,
B2 and not B1, given B1 pick C1 and not C2, and that are not tied to the formal properties of the
given B2 pick C2 and not C1) the participant will stimuli involved (e.g., a small coin can be worth
behave in ways that have not been reinforced by more than a large coin). This form of responding
the experimenter. Specifically, when a participant is called arbitrarily applied relational responding
is presented with A1, she will pick A1, match- (AARR; Hayes et al., 2001).
ing each stimulus with itself (reflexivity). When As seen in stimulus equivalence, verbal organ-
presented with B1, she will pick A1, reversing the isms can derive equivalences between stimuli that
direction of the trained relation (symmetry). When have never been explicitly matched. The different
presented with C1, she will select A1, deriving forms of relational responding (difference, op-
the untrained identity relation between the stimuli position, greater than, less than, etc) can also be
that were never paired (transitivity). derived without explicit training (e.g., Dymond
The generativity and stimulus substitutability & Barnes, 1995; Lipkens, Hayes, & Hayes, 1993;
characteristics of Stimulus Equivalence suggested Roche & Barnes, 1996). This leads us to derived

166

Function over Form

relational responding (DRR), perhaps the core predictable where control over trained stimulus
behavior underpinning language and cognition. relations is possible. Specifically, AARR emerges
The following paragraphs describe the specific from the process of operant conditioning involved
properties of DRR. in such tasks as simply learning to name objects.
Mutual entailment means that if A is related For example, a child is trained to name an object
to B, then B is related to A in a complimentary out loud when presented with the object, and to
fashion. For example, if A is opposite to B, then B orient towards the object when the object’s name
is opposite to A. If A is more than B, then B is less is spoken. After a number of object-name and
than A. Combinatorial entailment occurs when name-object relations are trained, the generalized
three or more stimuli are related. If A is opposite operant response class of “naming” is established
to B, and B is opposite to C, then the relation that in the presence of appropriate contextual cues such
is derived between A and C is one of equivalence, as the word “is”, as employed in the utterances
because both are opposite to B. Combinatorial “This is your shoe” or “What is that?” The history
entailment refers to the reciprocal relationships of responding establishes the specific contextual
that exist between two stimuli as mediated by cues for “naming”, a form of relational responding
other intermediary stimuli (Blackledge, 2003). (equivalence). Likewise, other forms of relational
The RFT account of attitudes begins to come responding, both basic and derived, are established
into focus with the introduction of one final feature through multiple exemplar training under the
-Transformation of function. Expressed simply, control of environmental cues (usually words).
transformation of function refers to the well docu- In effect, no further process at the psychological
mented fact that when two stimuli are related, the level needs to be appealed to in order to understand
psychological functions of each stimulus changes how humans come to derive relations.
the functions of the other, according to how the The ability of an individual to derive stimulus
stimuli are related (see Dymond & Rehfeldt, 2000, relations and transform stimulus functions accord-
for a review). For instance, if an individual shows a ingly need not be explained by such phenomena
preference for a particular soft-drink labelled using as “propositions” or insight”. Rather, the process
a specific term, and that term in turn participates of arbitrarily applicable relational responding
in a derived (i.e., untrained) relation with other explains those phenomena. This is a radical de-
sitmuli, a similar preference will also be shown parture in conceptual terms from the mainstream
for novel drinks labelled using those other sti- view that behavior is ultimately controlled from
mili (Barnes-Holmes, Keane, Barnes-Holmes & the inside out. However, this perspective brings
Smeets, 2000; Smeets & Barnes-Holmes, 2003). with it a remarkable and parsimonious explanatory
In effect, the psychological response functions power that requires no mental representational
established for the original label by virtue of its constructs or the attendant obligation to study these
being directly related to a preferred drink (i.e., sali- rather than the original behavioral phenomenon
vation, appetitive thoughts, increased behavioral of interest. Researchers can focus on researching
probability of drinking the liquid) transform the the specific histories that lead to the emergence of
response functions of all indirectly related stimuli the verbal behaviors associated with the attitude
(e.g., other related labels and drinks). construct. The following section details the RFT
A critical feature of the RFT approach to lan- model of attitudes, and the methodologies that
guage is that the derivation of stimulus relations emerged from that model parallel to the IAT and
and the transformation of stimulus functions is other social-cognitive implicit tests.
entirely controlled by historical events and is

167

Function over Form

Measuring Attitudes as word “woman” may serve as a contextual cue


Histories of Verbal Behavior for relating the word “complain” to the words
“nag” or “bitch”. In a structurally similar sen-
The phenomena of transfer of function and re- tence “the man complained and complained”,
lational responding combine to inform the RFT the word “man” serves as a contextual cue which
model of attitudes. From the behavior-analytic occasions relating “complain” with “assertive”
perspective, an attitude may now be conceived of or “forceful”. In Experiment 1 of their study,
as a history of derived and explicitly reinforced participants were exposed to training to form six
stimulus relations according to which the functions four-member equivalence classes using nonsense
of events are transformed (e.g., Grey & Barnes, syllables as stimuli. The members of each class
1996). As such, an attitude can be thought of as a were contingent on a contextual cue – either a
verbal event (or series thereof) which emerges from male or female name. That is, when a male name
our interactions with others and with our environ- was present, responding to A1-B1-C1 and D1 as
ment across our personal learning history. Thus, equivalent was reinforced, and when a female
an attitude is established and maintained through name was on screen, responding to A1-B1-C3
a history of both explicitly reinforced relations and D3 was reinforced. The experimenters then
and untrained derived relations between verbal tested for derived equivalence relations using novel
stimuli. The remote social contingencies which male and female names as contextual cues. Par-
support these networks of relational responses ticipants related the novel male and female names
can be represented by the verbal practices of the according to the trained derived relations despite
wider community (i.e., the culture with which having no experimental history of responding
the participant interacts). For instance, rules, to those names. In effect this demonstrated that
norms, mores, and taboos all constitute forms of control over the trained equivalence classes had
verbal contingency that specify relations between generalized through classes of by pre-existing
stimuli (e.g., “sex” and “dirty”) in often complex contextual cues (i.e., gender classifications). The
and subtle (i.e., indirect) ways. A participant’s researchers had in effect demonstrated a means
past participation in a verbal environment (i.e., a by which socially established classes based on
culture) provides many hundreds of training ex- gender could be assessed subtly and indirectly
emplars that establish complex derived relational without engendering social desirability.
networks through which functions of stimuli may Experiment 2 brought the process under further
be transformed, and this can explain the emer- control by generating pre-experimental equiva-
gence of apparently untrained or indirectly trained lence classes of nonsense syllables, which were
responses and attitudes. This occurs because of then used as the contextual cues in place of male
the way in which the various terms were framed and female names. Thus, the researchers modeled
relationally in language, whether explicitly, or in the process shown in experiment 1 with entirely
turn by further derived relations (e.g., innuendo, laboratory-controlled histories (whereas Male
jokes; see Roche, Barnes-Holmes & Barnes- and Female names would have had extensive pre-
Holmes, 2002). experimental histories which the experimenters
In an early example of a behavior-analytic could not know the details of). These experiments
study of attitudes, Kohlenberg, Hayes, and Hayes showed that contextual control could transfer
(1991) investigated a model of social stereotyping without direct training from one stimulus to other
based on verbal control over equivalence classes. stimuli with which it shares equivalence class
They suggested, for example, that in the sentence membership. This process can be studied in a
“the woman complained and complained”, the highly controlled manner, thus shedding light on

168

Function over Form

the types of histories which could produce social rorist attacks in the USA. They suggested that
stereotyping behavior without any need for an on hearing of the terrorist (A) attack a white
appeal to an explanatory mental construct. American male may instantly experience feelings
In another early study, Grey and Barnes of rage (B). The media may claim that Terrorists
(1996) suggested that a negative attitude towards (A) are responsible for these acts, and pictures
normal heterosexual interactions can be seen as of these Terrorists may be shown on the televi-
responding in accordance with an equivalence sion (C). In effect, an A-B and A-C relation has
relation between normal opposite-sex adults been established by direct media reports, but a
and descriptive terms such as ‘disgusting’. Their derived relations account explains how images of
empirical study demonstrated a transformation the terrorists themselves may now come to elicit
of a trained attitudinal or evaluative response feelings of hate or rage. This occurs by virtue of
from one member of an equivalence class to the a transfer of response function across the stimuli
other members of the equivalence class. These in the newly created relation (i.e., B to C). As
researchers provided participants with the neces- the most salient features of the terrorists are their
sary conditional discrimination training to form race, religion, and country of origin these feelings
the following three derived equivalence relations; of hate and rage towards the terrorists begin to
A1-B1-C1, A2-B2-C2, and A3-B3-C3, using non- transfer to other persons sharing the same skin
sense syllables as stimuli. Participants then viewed colour, religion, and country of origin because
the video contents of VHS videocassettes that of a formal similarity between them and the ter-
were clearly labeled with the A1 and A2 stimuli. rorists (i.e., simple stimulus generalization). That
One of the cassettes contained sexual/romantic is, innocent Muslims of a Middle Eastern descent
scenes, while the other contained religiously are now responded to as equivalent to the A, B
themed scenes. Subsequently, participants were and C stimuli and the feelings of hate and rage
asked to categorize four novel videocassettes, each felt by the hypothetical American may now have
labeled as B1, C1, B2 or C2. They were given no transferred beyond the terrorists themselves to all
information about these cassettes, but they catego- middle Eastern people. This process was modeled
rized them according to the derived equivalence in the laboratory by Dixon et al. (2003).
classes. That is, participants classified the B1 and The above studies showed the potential of an
C1 cassettes in the same way as the A1 cassette, RFT paradigm to detail the processes involved
and the B2 and C2 cassette in the same way as in the establishment of behaviors that we might
the A2 cassette. In effect, the study demonstrated refer to as attitudes. RFT and stimulus equiva-
the sexual and religious evaluative functions of lence researchers were able to leverage derived
the A-labeled cassettes and the derived relations relational responding processes to experimentally
in which the A stimuli participated, transformed study attitudes and to use these processes as part
the functions of the C-labeled cassettes, such that of a theoretical account, but one challenge still
these were responded to as sexual or religious as remained- to develop a method which would
appropriate. This study demonstrated a process allow a researcher to detect those histories.
by which evaluative responses (i.e. attitudes) can This breakthrough occurred when researchers
be indirectly trained and measured. (Watt, Keenan, Barnes, & Cairns, 1991) began
Dixon, Dymond, Rehfeldt, Roche, & Zlomke to investigate how a particular history of verbal
(2003) applied the transfer-of-function model to behavior might interfere with the formation of
the understanding of attitude changes to Muslim new relational responses – and as such, reveal
men in the aftermath of the September 11th ter- itself to the experimenter.

169

Function over Form

The Methodological Breakthrough: randomly presented. Interspersed with these were


The Watt et al. Paradigm ten presentations each of six other stimulus com-
binations. Each of the three Protestant symbols
In their seminal study, Watt et al. (1991) used a served as sample stimuli and two of the Catholic
simple stimulus equivalence paradigm in which names served as comparison stimuli. An additional
participants were trained to relate stimuli with Protestant name was included as a comparison
strong socially established functions in ways that stimulus for each of these three combinations of
were inconsistent with their social history. Specifi- sample and comparison stimuli. The results of the
cally, they took advantage of the fact that people Watt et al. study showed that during equivalence
in Northern Ireland often respond to names as testing, all of the English participants correctly
indicative of religious background, and utilized matched the Catholic names with the Protestant
stimuli representative of Catholic and Protestant symbols, but 12 of the 19 Northern Irish partici-
names and symbols. pants chose a novel Protestant name in the pres-
A three-phase experimental procedure was ence of the Protestant symbols, thereby failing to
employed. Participants were first exposed to a derive the equivalence relations that the procedure
matching to sample procedure comprising the usually occasions. These findings strongly sug-
presentation of either a nonsense syllable or a gested that the social contingencies operating in
first and last name at the top of the screen (the Northern Ireland interfered with the establish-
“sample” stimulus). Three “comparison” stimuli ment of equivalence relations in the laboratory.
were displayed separately below. Participants The equivalence test required Northern Irish
were instructed to select a comparison stimulus by participants to juxtapose names and symbols in
pressing a corresponding key. Training comprised a manner that was counter-cultural for this group
of one of three Catholic names being randomly of participants. As such, the Watt et al. procedure
chosen to serve as the sample stimulus. Beneath had hit upon a basic methodology for inferring
this, three nonsense syllables served as compari- the social histories of participants, without using
son stimuli and were arranged in a random order a direct questioning approach. It was, in effect, an
across the screen. Participants were required to early behavior-analytic implicit test.
select the correct comparison in the presence of the Subsequent studies supported the suggestion
sample stimulus (A-B Relations). The second stage made by Watt et al. that social history interferes
trained B-C relations. Here, the sample stimuli with the formation of equivalence classes and
were selected from the list of nonsense syllables, that this phenomenon could be used to indirectly
and the comparison stimuli were selected from the assess participants’ personal and social histories.
list of Protestant symbols. Feedback was provided For instance, in a study on gender identity, Moxon
on all trials during Stage 1. During stage 2, cor- and Keenan (1993) found that participants had
rective feedback was presented following 50% of more difficulty forming equivalence classes when
responses. The stimulus combinations described in the classes included female names and stereotypic
Stage 1 were all presented in random order during male occupations. Leslie, Tierney, Robinson,
this condition. Each stimulus combination was Keenan, Watt, and Barnes (1993) also employed
presented twice and participants were required the Watt et al. procedure in a study designed to
to meet a 100% criterion. differentiate between anxious and non-anxious
Stage 3 of the Watt et al. procedure involved participants. They found that anxious participants
Equivalence Testing. For this stage, no corrective had more difficulty in matching pleasant-state
feedback was provided. Ten presentations of each adjectives to threatening situation descriptors
of the stimulus combinations from Stage 1 were than did control participants. In another study,

170

Function over Form

Merwin and Wilson (2005) required participants to created and incongruous functional stimulus
form equivalence classes between self-referential classes (A1-C2 and A2-C1). In a second experi-
terms and negative evaluations, and in a second ment, Roche et al. firstly established the sexual and
procedure, between those terms and positive evalu- nonsexual functions for the A1/C2 and A2 / C2
ations. Participants who reported high distress and pair respectively, and then presented participants
low esteem made significantly more errors when with the matching-to-sample procedure. In this
required to match “self” terms with positive items. case, participants matched stimuli based on their
Plaud (1995) showed that participants required conditioned sexual/non-sexual functions (i.e., A1
significantly more training to derive equivalence with C2 and A2 with C1) rather than forming the
classes made up of aversive stimuli (snake-related equivalence classes required by the matching to
words) than classes made up of innocuous stimuli sample procedure. This demonstrated that once
(flower related words). Importantly, the increase an equivalence class is formed, it is difficult to
in the amount of training required to establish the disrupt with succeeding functional relations, and
equivalence classes correlated with self-reported that the reverse is also true; it is difficult for par-
fear of snakes. ticipants to form equivalence relations when they
These studies supported the assertion that pre- are incongruous with existing functional relations.
experimental functions of stimuli could interfere This issue was further examined by Tyndall,
with the formation of equivalence classes in the Roche and James (2004). They established two
laboratory. However, in order to gain complete functional classes of stimuli; Six S+ stimuli
experimental control over this effect it was also (responding to the stimulus was reinforced)
necessary for researchers to create their own and Six S- stimuli (responding away from the
stimuli and to establish psychological functions stimuli was reinforced). In matching to sample
for those stimuli using respondent and operant training, participants were trained to form two
learning methods. Only in this way could the three-member equivalence classes using four dif-
process by which individual words (i.e., stimuli ferent combinations of S+/S- stimuli. Participants
with conditioned evaluative functions) interfere required more training to establish two distinct
with class formation (derived or trained) be fully equivalence classes from amongst 6 S+ stimuli
understood in functional, rather than theoretical, (i.e., stimuli sharing the same response function)
terms than from amongst 6 S- stimuli (i.e., without a
The first study to address this issue was re- shared response function). Further, participants
ported by Roche, Barnes, and Smeets (1997). The found it easier to form equivalence classes when
experimenters trained participants on a matching to they were required to separate S+ and S- stimuli
sample procedure that formed two three-member than when they were required to form classes
equivalence classes using nonsense syllables as which mixed S+ and S- stimuli.
stimuli (i.e. A1-B1-C1 and A2-B2-C2, where While in the 2004 paper, the stimulus func-
the A-C linkages are derived). The authors then tions were emotionally neutral, Tyndall, Roche
paired two of the stimuli (A1 and C2) with sexu- & James 2009 later studied the process using
ally explicit film clips and two other stimuli (A2 emotive stimuli. They created these stimuli by
and C1) with nonsexual film clips, establishing pairing six stimuli with aversive images and six
conflicting sexual response functions for stimuli further stimuli with neutral images, as an analog
that had been trained in the laboratory as equiva- of everyday evaluative experiences often studied
lent. When re-exposed to the equivalence testing, in attitude research. They then tested for the
participants reproduced the original equivalence formation of those functional classes. In equiva-
relations (A1-C1 and A2-C2) and not the newly lence training and testing, the authors found that

171

Function over Form

participants required significantly more training producing verbal responses that have been me-
to establish two three-member equivalence classes diated by further private verbal responses (such
from amongst the six aversive stimuli than from as “I had better not say that” is) what behavior
amongst the six neutral stimuli. In effect, Tyndall analysts refer to as thinking (or more technically
et al. (2004, 2009) more clearly demonstrated that behavior-behavior relations). It does not in itself
learning tasks involving the formation of new constitute a special process different to other forms
stimulus relations can be used as indicators of of relational responding or simple speaking. In
the pre-existing relations between those stimuli. effect, we take the word “implicit” to refer to the
The studies above were conducted at the same observation that the contingencies controlling
time that the IAT emerged as the preeminent the relevant responding are not discriminable by
Implicit Test. However, this research thread was the participant to a sufficient degree to allow a
developed independently from the social cognitive conscious (i.e., verbally-mediated) alteration of
paradigm, with little or no contact between the the response so that it is relationally consistent
two. In recent years, however, RFT researchers with anything other than the stimulus presented
have begun to apply the lessons gleaned from this during the implicit testing task. Put simply, the
research to modeling performances on the IAT participant would require more time during im-
in terms of relational responding, and to develop plicit test trials to observe the stimulus, respond
testing methodologies that utilize insights from to it privately, discriminate that response privately
both threads of research. In the following section, along with the controlling source as a specific prior
the authors will outline a behavioral model of the history of trained or derived stimulus associations,
IAT, before moving on to detail two new implicit discriminate that this history is socially undesir-
testing methodologies which are built upon func- able, and then alter the response consciously in
tional foundations. a manner referred to loosely as an example of
social desirability. Rather, under time pressure the
Connecting the Threads: A participant likely simply responds directly to the
Functional Account of the stimulus on screen, without additional mediating
Implicit Association Test sequences of private verbal behavior.
From within a functional Relational Frame
The Watt et al. procedure showed that a func- approach, the IAT is viewed as a measure of an
tional process-based account of attitudes could individual’s verbal history vis-à-vis an assessment
be harnessed to create a subtle test for the verbal of their ability to form stimulus relations under
histories that underpin the attitude construct, at time constraints. IAT effects are conceived in
least from a behavioral perspective. However, the terms of participants’ fluency with the relevant
Watt et al. procedure was developed prior to the verbal categories and their degree of experience at
IAT and the widespread acceptance of the social juxtaposing members of those verbal categories.
cognitive / associationistic approach to attitudes. For instance, an individual who has many dealings
Their definition of attitudes did not make contact with people of a specific race, and has encountered
with the yet-to-emerge phenomenon of implicit both pleasant and unpleasant individuals from this
attitudes. Indeed, from a behavioral perspective, racial group, will likely find it easy to juxtapose
the term “implicit” may not merit a unique ac- racial and evaluative terms in an IAT according
count. It likely refers only to the relative speed of to the test rules across the two test blocks. Such
a response and therefore the relative probability an individual will show no IAT effect (i.e., no
that the response was mediated by further ver- response time or accuracy differential across the
bal behavior, usually private. The processes of test blocks). On the other hand, if they have expe-

172

Function over Form

rienced mostly unpleasant individuals from one types of studies provide useful information that
racial group or other, the juxtaposition of response can inform the ongoing debate as to the form of
rules across the IAT blocks will likely expose a the implicit attitude construct, opening up a wider
fluency differential across those two blocks (i.e., array of explanations that go beyond the underlying
an IAT effect; see the behavioral account of Roche, associative narrative that informs most theorizing
Ruiz, O’Riordan & Hand, 2005). on the subject (Hughes et al., 2011). Further, this
The behavioral model of the IAT was tested research thread has birthed two new functionally
empirically by Gavin et al. (2008) using nonsense oriented implicit tests which are built on the in-
syllables as stimuli and experimentally produced sights gained through functional research, meaning
derived relations between them as laboratory that their core processes are well articulated and
analogs of verbal relations between words in supported by a wealth of basic research.
the vernacular. Two equivalence relations were
established in the usual manner, leading to the Functional Implicit Tests
two classes of nonsense syllables, labeled here as
A1-B1-C1 and A2-B2-C2, where the A-C relations The Implicit Relational
were derived, not reinforced. An IAT-type test Assessment Procedure
was then administered to measure participants’
ability to respond in the same way to common The behavioral methodologies described thus far
class member pairs (e.g., A1 and C1) compared were focused largely on identifying behavioral pro-
to cross-class pairs (e.g., A1 and C2). Not surpris- cesses and were prototypical in nature rather than
ingly, more errors were made in responding under designed for use in real world studies of attitudes.
rule conditions in which a common response was In recent years, two distinct behavioral implicit
required for incompatible, compared to compat- tests have been developed for real world applica-
ible stimuli. In effect, typical IAT outcomes tion. The first of these is the Implicit Relational
were generated using only directly manipulated Assessment Procedure (IRAP; Barnes-Holmes,
variables and laboratory created stimuli, and in Hayden, Barnes-Holmes, & Stewart, 2006). This
purely functional terms. test is in many ways procedurally similar to the
These entirely laboratory produced IAT effects IAT. However, each trial of the IRAP displays
were subsequently shown to be manipulable vis- two stimuli (rather than just one) on screen (e.g.,
à-vis reversals of some of the baseline relations “Child” and “Sexual”) along with a contextual cue
underlying the derived equivalence relations that specifies the relation between the two stimuli
(Ridgeway et al., 2010). Such findings strengthen (e.g., “Same” or “Opposite.”). The participant is
the behavior-analytic position that the IAT test for- required to respond quickly to this resulting rela-
mat is sensitive to a participant’s history of relating tional statement (“Child SAME Sexual” or “Child
the test stimuli, perhaps even including histories OPPOSITE Sexual”) with a key press that cor-
of stimulus relating that a participant would wish responds to one of two response options (e.g., for
to conceal. However, they also shed new light “TRUE” press z, for “FALSE” press m). Feedback
on the fundamental processes that may entirely is only presented if the response in incorrect as
underpin the IAT effect and demonstrate that defined by the block rules (a red X is displayed)
informative research can be done on implicit test or if the response latency is lower than the stated
processes from a functional perspective, without criterion (the words “too slow” are displayed).
necessary recourse to associationistic explanatory Like the IAT, trials in the IRAP are organized into
mechanisms. For the cognitive researcher, these blocks. In one type of block, the responses defined

173

Function over Form

as correct are those that are consistent with social probability of this initial response is a function of
norms (e.g. Child OPPOSITE Sexual – TRUE = participants’ verbal and non-verbal history with
Correct) while the other block requires responses the stimuli and the current contextual cues (i.e.,
that are inconsistent with social norms. the relational stimulus, such as the word opposite
A recent IRAP study by Dawson and Barnes- on screen). The most probable response will likely
Holmes (2009) tested for pre-existing child-sex be emitted first. If this immediate response coheres
stimulus relations in a child sex offender popula- with the response required by the current IRAP
tion. Sixteen participants who had been convicted trial rule, then the response latency will be lower.
of a contact sexual offence against a child (the If the required response is in opposition with the
offender group) and sixteen male non-offenders participants’ immediate relational response, then
recruited from a college population (the control the correct (as defined by the contingencies of
group) completed an IRAP procedure and a Cogni- the specific trial) response will be emitted more
tive Distortion Scale (CDS; Gannon, 2009). The slowly. Across multiple trials, the average latency
IRAP stimuli consisted of two category labels on inconsistent trials will be higher than the aver-
(“Child” and “Adult”) and two sets of target stimuli age latency for consistent trials.
(“sexual” words and “non-sexual” words). During The foregoing provides a basic explanation
the consistent blocks, participants were required for the IRAP effect, but the REC model extends
to respond with “true” to the relations “Adult – the explanation to account for why implicit
Sexual” and “Child – Nonsexual” while in the measures and explicit questionnaire methods so
inconsistent blocks participants were required often diverge in their results. More specifically,
to respond in the opposite way. The IRAP suc- when completing questionnaires or other so called
cessfully detected a difference in response time “explicit” measures of attitudes, the participant is
differentials across the control group and the under little time pressure and can therefore engage
offender group. Furthermore, the IRAP was able in complex and extended relational responding
to identify the specific relation on which the two (i.e., thinking) which allows them to produce a
groups differed. Specifically, on Child-Sexual tri- response which is coherent with other responses
als, the offender group did not show a significant in their behavioral repertoire (see Barnes-Holmes,
IRAP effect, responding equally quickly to Child- Hayes & Dymond, 2001) such as; “it is wrong to
Sexual-False and Child-Sexual-True, whereas categorize children as sexual.” It is also possible
non-offenders tended to show a reaction time under these circumstances to produce a response
differential across these blocks. that coheres with the social expectations of others.
Dermot Barnes-Holmes and colleagues have However, when exposed to the IRAP (or other
devised a behavior-analytic and functional ac- implicit test) procedure, participants are under
count of the IRAP and related implicit test effects significant pressure to respond quickly (com-
based on Relational Frame Theory, which they monly within 2000 ms) and, therefore have little
call the Relational Elaboration Coherence Model time to engage in the elaborate private relational
(REC; Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, Stewart, responding necessary to produce alternative so-
& Boles, 2010). It is a more formally elaborated cially desirable responses. In effect, the most likely
account than the loose working definition of im- responses under time constraint conditions are
plicitness provided above and first proposed by those that are immediate and brief and therefore
Roche et al. (2005). According to the REC model, direct measures of history, unmediated by local
each individual trial on the IRAP produces an im- relational activity.
mediate and brief response to the relation presented The IRAP has a well worked out functional
before the participant presses a response key. The explanatory account accompanying its procedure,

174

Function over Form

and addresses the concerns of the experimental Response speed is constrained not by instruction
analyst of behavior in how it approaches the matter but by a finite response window (usually 3s). There
of implicit testing. Nevertheless, the test procedure are no instructions or response rules, other than to
is sufficiently intensive for the participant that produce as many correct responses as possible. The
rapid administrations of the test are not possible. metric of interest is the differential in the number
This is because the test consists of a practice of trials required to reach response fluency across
block, several re-administrations of test blocks, two critical test blocks.
and a relatively high rate of subject attrition due A FAST contains two “test blocks”, each of
to a failure to reach fluency criteria within the which uses the same four stimuli. Two stimuli are
test itself. However, a further behavior-analytic the stimuli of interest, suspected to be related in
test has been developed to address this and other the participant’s history. The other two are novel,
concerns. Specifically the Function Acquisition unrelated stimuli. In this regard the FAST is not
Speed Test (FAST; O’Reilly, Roche, Ruiz, Tyndall unlike a single target IAT. In the “consistent”
& Gavin, 2012; O’Reilly, Roche, Ryan & Campion, block, the same response is reinforced (e.g., press
2013) was developed within a functional research z) for both of the target stimuli of interest, while
paradigm but with the express purpose of being another response (e.g., press m) is reinforced for
easy and fast to administer. The test also addresses the novel innocuous stimuli. These responses are
the problem of the relative nature of biases and consistent with the participant’s learning history,
preferences as indexed by the IAT and the IRAP. and so should quickly result in stable, high rate
Before this issue is addressed, however, a brief responding by the participant. In the “inconsistent
outline of the FAST methodology is merited. block”, the reinforced responses are inconsistent
with the participant’s learning history insofar as
The Function Acquisition different responses (“press z” and “press m”) are
Speed Test (FAST) required for each of two (thought-to-be-related)
target stimuli. In effect, the juxtaposition of current
The FAST requires participants to complete a and past reinforcement contingencies during the
number of simple discrimination blocks with inconsistent block functions as a type of learning
minimal instructions, and no instructions at all disrupter. The current learning contingencies need
relating to appropriate responses. Each trial to overcome the “behavioral inertia” of the pre-ex-
presents a single stimulus, and participants are perimental contingencies that maintain a specific
required to learn, via trial and error and correc- stimulus relation in order for fluent responding to
tive feedback, whether to respond with a “left” be produced (i.e., learning). In simple terms, the
key press (e.g., press “a” or press “z”) or a “right” “inconsistent” response classes are more difficult
key press (e.g. press “j” or press “m”). For two of for participants to acquire. The differences in rates
the stimuli, a “left” key press is reinforced, while of response class acquisition across the two test
for the other two, a “right” key press is reinforced. blocks is used to index the pre-existing “strength”
Participants are required to continue the block until of the relation under investigation.
their responses are fluent; that is both rapid and Critically, the FAST also contains a “base-
accurate as defined by the production of a specific line” block, in which all four stimuli are novel
succession of correct responses (usually 10). As and unrelated (e.g., nonsense syllables). The
a result there is no predetermined block length. purpose of this block is to measure the rate of
Blocks are completed when response fluency has acquisition of an arbitrary response class under
been achieved. Feedback is presented following all test conditions but without any interfering effect
trials and there is no error correction procedure. from pre-experimental contingencies. The number

175

Function over Form

of trials to criterion on the baseline block places being studied be stated in symmetrical terms.
differences in acquisition speed across the main While this provides a useful index of whether an
FAST blocks in context. For example, for a par- attitude is present for a participant, the specific
ticipant who completes such tasks very quickly, a pattern of relational responding that characterizes
small difference in acquisition rates between the the attitude may be crucial to the development of
inconsistent and consistent test blocks is more interventions to change those attitudes.
meaningful that the same raw fluency difference
is for a participant with large baseline trial re-
quirements. This relationship between differences CONCLUSION
in learning speed across critical FAST blocks,
and baseline acquisition speed is the basis of a While there remains much to be done in the de-
Strength of Relation index used to quantify the velopment of the FAST, its development and that
“strength” of a pre-existing relation between the of the IRAP illustrate that behavior analysis has
stimuli of interest. much to offer the field of implicit testing by way
The FAST has been shown to be capable of of ground-up functional accounts of the behavioral
detecting relations between laboratory created processes relevant to attitude measurement and in
stimuli and relations (O’Reilly et al., 2012) as the development of appropriate methodologies
well as derived relations (O’Reilly et al, 2013). to this end. The functional account of attitudes
It has also been used with real world stimuli to detailed in this chapter led directly to these new
measure categorization of teenaged females as methodologies, and the understanding of the pro-
sexual (see Roche et al., 2012), but is still in early cesses leveraged by these tests means that strong
stages of development. predictions can be made and empirically tested,
One exciting feature of the FAST worth outlin- thus rapidly advancing knowledge with regard to
ing at this stage relates to its circumnavigation of the conditions under which attitudes, implicit and
the problem of relativism in inferences regarding otherwise, are formed and maintained, and how
attitudinal biases. Specifically, the IAT provides they can be changed. The functional approach of-
only a relative measure of association strength fers the hope of providing an account that renders
(De Houwer, 2002, Greenwald, Nosek & Banaji, explicit those very processes social cognitivists
2004). That is, an IAT trial involves both respond- refer to as implicit.
ing towards one category while responding away
from the other, and thus it is impossible to deter-
mine which of these response forms is at work REFERENCES
in controlling the participant’s correct response
rate differentials across test blocks, or the extent Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. Murchison
to which one or the other contributes to the total (Ed.), Handbook of Social Psychology. Worcester,
effect. More specifically, given the result of a MA: Clark University Press.
race IAT, only the question; “Is White preferred Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart,
to Black” is answered. Even though the bias may I., & Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the Implicit
be reliably recorded it remains unclear whether or Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) and
not the bias is characterized by a participant having the Relational Elaboration and Coherence (REC)
a history of pro-white responding or a history of model. The Psychological Record, 60, 527–542.
anti-black responding. In effect, the structure of
the IAT (and the IRAP) requires that the attitude

176

Function over Form

Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayden, E., Barnes-Holmes, Dawson, D. L., Barnes-Holmes, D., Gresswell, D.
Y., & Stewart, I. (2008). The Implicit Relational M., Hart, A. J., & Gore, N. J. (2009). Assessing
Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a response-time the Implicit Beliefs of Sexual Offenders Using
and event-related-potentials methodology for test- the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure
ing natural verbal relations: A preliminary study. A First Study. Sexual Abuse, 21(1), 57–75.
The Psychological Record, 58, 497–516. PMID:19218478
Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayes, S. C., & Dymond, De Houwer, J. (2003). The extrinsic affec-
S. (2001). Self and self-directed rules. In S. C. tive Simon task. Experimental Psychology,
Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), 50(2), 77–85. doi:10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.77
Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian ac- PMID:12693192
count of language and cognition (pp. 119–139).
De Houwer, J. (2006a). Using the Implicit As-
New York: Plenum Press.
sociation Test does not rules out an impact of
Barnes-Holmes, D., Keane, J., Barnes-Holmes, conscious propositional knowledge on evaluative
Y., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). A derived transfer conditioning. Learning and Motivation, 1–12.
of emotive functions as a means of establishing
De Houwer, J. (2006b). What are implicit measures
differential preferences for soft drinks. The Psy-
and why are we using them. In The handbook of
chological Record, 50, 493–511.
implicit cognition and addiction. Academic Press.
Blackledge, J. T. (2003). An introduction to re-
De Houwer, J. (2011). Why the Cognitive Ap-
lational frame theory: Basics and applications.
proach in Psychology Would Profit From a
Behavior Analyst Today, 3(4), 421–433.
Functional Approach and Vice Versa (2011).
Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Gonzales, P. M., & Chris- Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2),
tie, C. (2006). Decoding the implicit association 202–209. doi:10.1177/1745691611400238
test: Implications for criterion prediction. Jour-
De Houwer, J., Beckers, T., & Moors, A. (2007).
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2),
Novel attitudes can be faked on the Implicit As-
192–212. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.07.003
sociation Test. Journal of Experimental Social
Carpenter, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, Psychology, 43(6), 972–978. doi:10.1016/j.
D. (2003). Equivalence-equivalence as a model jesp.2006.10.007
of analogy: Further analyses. The Psychological
De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A.,
Record, 53, 349–371.
& Moors, A. (2009). Theoretical claims necessitate
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). basic research: Reply to Gawronski, Lebel, Peters,
A relational frame training intervention to raise and Banse (2009) and Nosek and Greenwald
Intelligence Quotients: A pilot study. The Psy- (2009). Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 377–379.
chological Record. doi:10.1037/a0015328 PMID:19379021

177

Function over Form

Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). American= Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Attitudes:
white? Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Foundations, functions, and consequences. In M.
chology, 88(3), 447–466. doi:10.1037/0022- A. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), The handbook of
3514.88.3.447 PMID:15740439 social psychology (pp. 139–160). London: Sage.
Dixon, M. R., Dymond, S., Rehfeldt, R. A., Roche, Fiedler, K., Messner, C., & Bluemke, M. (2006).
B., & Zlomke, K. R. (2003). Terrorism and rela- Unresolved problems with the ‘I’, the ‘A’, and
tional frame theory. Behavior and Social Issues, the ‘T’: A logical and psychometric critique of
12(2), 129. doi:10.5210/bsi.v12i2.40 the Implicit Association Test (IAT). European
Review of Social Psychology, 17(1), 74–147.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. E., Kawakami, K., &
doi:10.1080/10463280600681248
Hodson, G. (2002). Why can’t we just get along?
Interpersonal biases and interracial distrust. Cul- Gavin, A., Roche, B., & Ruiz, M. (2008). A be-
tural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology, haviorally modifed and functionally transparent
8(2), 88–102. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.8.2.88 Implicit Association Test to measure levels of
PMID:11987594 culturally appropriate sexual categorization of
children among a sample of normal males and
Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1995). A transforma-
females. Academic Press.
tion of self-discrimination response functions
in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable Gawronski, B., LeBel, E. P., & Peters, K. R. (2007).
relations of sameness, more-than, and less-than. What do implicit measures tell us? Scrutinizing the
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, validity of three common assumptions. Perspec-
64(2), 163–184. doi:10.1901/jeab.1995.64-163 tives on Psychological Science, 2(2), 181–193.
PMID:16812766 doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00036.x
Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2000). Under- Gawronski, B., LeBel, E. P., Peters, K. R., &
standing complex behavior: The transformation of Banse, R. (2009). Methodological issues in the
stimulus functions. The Behavior Analyst, 23(2), validation of implicit measures: Comment on De
239. PMID:22478349 Houwer, Teige-Mocigemba, Spruyt, and Moors
(2009). Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 369–372.
Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (Eds.). (2013). Advances
doi:10.1037/a0014820 PMID:19379019
in relational frame theory: Research and applica-
tion. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. Greenwald, A., Nosek, B., & Banaji, M. (2003).
Understanding and using the Implicit Association
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C.,
Test: I: An improved scoring algorithm. Journal
& Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in auto-
of Personality and Social Psychology.
matic activation as an unobtrusive measure of
racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit
of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), Social Cognition: Attitudes, Self-Esteem, and
1013–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013 Stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27.
PMID:8531054 doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4 PMID:7878162

178

Function over Form

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., Schwartz, Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J.
J. L. K., & Attitudes, M. I. (1998). Measuring (1991). The transfer of contextual control over
individual differences in implicit cognition: The equivalence classes through equivalence classes:
implicit association test. Journal of Personality, A possible model of social stereotyping. Jour-
74(6), 1464–1480. PMID:9654756 nal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
56(3), 505–518. doi:10.1901/jeab.1991.56-505
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R.,
PMID:1774542
& Klauer, K. C. (2005). Validity of the salience
asymmetry interpretation of the implicit associa- Lane, K. A., Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Gre-
tion test: Comment on Rothermund and Wentura enwald, A. G. (2007). Understanding and using
(2004). Academic Press. the Implicit Association Test: IV: Procedures and
validity. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.),
Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B.
Implicit measures of attitudes: Procedures and
(2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinner-
controversies (pp. 59–102). New York: Guilford
ian account of human language and cognition.
Press.
New York: Plenum.
Leslie, J. C., Tierney, K. J., Robinson, C. P., &
Heagle, A. I., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2006). Teaching
Keenan, M. et  al. (1993). Differences between
perspective-taking skills to typically developing
clinically anxious and non-anxious subjects in a
children through derived relational responding.
stimulus equivalence training task involving threat
Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Inter-
words. The Psychological Record, 43, 153–161.
vention, 3, 1–34.
Lipkens, R., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993).
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T.,
Longitudinal study of the development of derived
Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2004). A meta-analysis on
relations in an infant. Journal of Experimental
the correlation between the Implicit Association
Child Psychology, 56(2), 201–239. doi:10.1006/
Test and explicit self-report measures. Unpub-
jecp.1993.1032 PMID:8245768
lished manuscript, University of Trier, Germany.
Lowe, C. F. (1979). Determinants of human oper-
Houwer, J. D. (2002). The Implicit Association
ant behavior. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.),
Test as a tool for studying dysfunctional associa-
Advances in analysis of behavior: Reinforcement
tions in psychopathology: Strengths and limita-
and the Organization of behavior (vol. 1, pp. 159-
tions. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experi-
192). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
mental Psychiatry, 33(2), 115–133. doi:10.1016/
S0005-7916(02)00024-1 PMID:12472175 McConnell, A. R., Rydell, R. J., Strain, L. M.,
& Mackie, D. M. (2008). Forming implicit and
Hughes, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2011). The
explicit attitudes toward individuals: Social group
Dominance of Associative Theorizing in Implicit
association cues. Journal of Personality and Social
Attitude Research: Propositional and Behavioral
Psychology, 94(5), 792–807. doi:10.1037/0022-
Alternatives. The Psychological Record, 61,
3514.94.5.792 PMID:18444739
465–496.

179

Function over Form

McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes- O’Toole, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Three
Holmes, D. (2004). Perspective-taking as rela- chronometric indices of relational responding as
tional responding: A developmental profile. The predictors of performance on a brief intelligence
Psychological Record, 54(1), 115–144. test: The importance of relational flexibility. The
Psychological Record, 59, 119–132.
Merwin, R. M., & Wilson, K. G. (2005). Pre-
liminary findings on the effects of self-referring O’Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smyth, S.
and evaluative stimuli on stimulus equivalence (2007). A derived transfer of functions and the
class formation. The Psychological Record, 55, Implicit Association Test. Journal of the Ex-
561–575. perimental Analysis of Behavior, 88(2), 263–283.
doi:10.1901/jeab.2007.76-06 PMID:17970419
Moran, L., Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Ming,
S. (2010). Brief report: The training and as- Olsen, J. M., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). Attitudes
sessment of relational precursors and abilities and attitude change. Annual Review of Psy-
(TARPA): A preliminary analysis. Journal of chology, 44(1), 117–154. doi:10.1146/annurev.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(9), ps.44.020193.001001
1149–1153. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-0968-0
Olson, M. A., & Kendrick, R. V. (2008). Origins
PMID:20151185
of attitudes. Attitudes and Persuasion, 111-130.
Moxon, P., Keenan, M., & Hine, L. (1993). Gender-
Plaud, J. J. (1995). The generalized expectancy
role stereotyping and stimulus equivalence. The
bias: An explanatory enigma. Behavioral and
Psychological Record, 43(3), 381–393.
Brain Sciences, 18(02), 311–312. doi:10.1017/
Murphy, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Es- S0140525X00038644
tablishing derived manding for specific amounts
Reese, H. W. (1968). The perception of stimulus
with three children: An attempt at synthesizing
relations: Discrimination learning and transposi-
Skinner’s Verbal Behavior with relational frame
tion. New York: Academic Press.
theory. The Psychological Record, 59, 75–91.
Rehfeldt, R. A., Dillen, J. E., Ziomek, M. M., &
Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/
Kowalchuk, R. K. (2007). Assessing Relational
no-go association task. Social cognition, 19(6),
Learning Deficits in Perspective-Taking in Chil-
625–666. Nosek, B. A., & Hansen, J. J. (2008). The
dren with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum
associations in our heads belong to us: Searching
Disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23.
for attitudes and knowledge in implicit evalua-
tion. Cognition and Emotion, 22(4), 553–594. Ridgeway, I., Roche, B., Gavin, A., & Ruiz, M. R.
doi:10.1080/02699930701438186 (2010). Establishing and eliminating IAT effects
in the laboratory: Extending a behavioral model of
O’Reilly, A., Roche, B., Ruiz, M., Ryan, A., &
the Implicit Association Test. European Journal
Campion, G. (in press). A Function Acquisition
of Behavior Analysis, 11, 133–150.
Speed Test for equivalence relations (FASTER).
The Psychological Record. Roche, B., & Barnes, D. (1996). Arbitrarily ap-
plicable relational responding and sexual catego-
O’Reilly, A., Roche, B., Ruiz, M. R., Tyndall, I., &
rization: A critical test of the derived difference
Gavin, A. (2012). The Function Acquisition Speed
relation. The Psychological Record, 46, 451–475.
Test (FAST): A behavior-analytic implicit test for
assessing stimulus relations. The Psychological
Record, 62, 507–528.

180

Function over Form

Roche, B., Barnes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (1997). Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and
Incongruous stimulus pairing and conditional behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors
discrimination training: Effects on relational Cooperative.
responding. Journal of the Experimental Analy-
Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional
sis of Behavior, 68(2), 143–160. doi:10.1901/
discrimination vs. matching to sample: An ex-
jeab.1997.68-143 PMID:16812852
pansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the
Roche, B., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37(1), 5–22.
D., Stewart, I., & O’Hora, D. (2002). Relational doi:10.1901/jeab.1982.37-5 PMID:7057129
Frame Theory: A new paradigm for the analysis
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. Acton, MA:
of social behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 25,
Copley Publishing Group. doi:10.1037/11256-000
75–91. PMID:22478379
Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2003).
Roche, B., O’Reilly, A., Gavin, A., Ruiz, M., &
Children’s emergent preferences for soft drinks:
Arancibia, G. (2012). Using behavior-analytic
Stimulus-equivalence and transfer. Journal of Eco-
implicit tests to assess sexual interests among
nomic Psychology, 24(5), 603–618. doi:10.1016/
normal and sex-offender populations. Socioaffec-
S0167-4870(03)00004-7
tive Neuroscience and Psychology, 2(0), 17335.
doi:10.3402/snp.v2i0.17335 PMID:24693346 Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B.
(2004). A Functional-Analytic Model of Analogy
Roche, B., O’Riordan, M., Ruiz, M., & Hand,
Using the Relational Evaluation Procedure. The
K. (2005). A relational frame approach to the
Psychological Record, 54, 531.
psychological assessment of sex offenders. In M.
Taylor & E. Quayle (Eds.), Viewing Child Pornog- Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Weil, T. (2009).
raphy on the Internet: Understanding the Offence, Training analogical reasoning as relational
managing the Offender, and Helping the Victims responding. Derived relational responding: Ap-
(pp. 109–125). Dorset: Russell House Publishing. plications for learners with autism and other
developmental disabilities. Oakland, CA: New
Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2004). Under-
Harbinger.
lying processes in the Implicit Association Test:
Dissociating salience from associations. Journal Tyndall, I. T., Roche, B., & James, J. E. (2004).
of Experimental Psychology. General, 133(2), The relation between stimulus function and
139–165. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.139 equivalence class formation. Journal of the Ex-
PMID:15149248 perimental Analysis of Behavior, 81(3), 257–266.
doi:10.1901/jeab.2004.81-257 PMID:15357509
Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual
equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Tyndall, I. T., Roche, B., & James, J. E. (2009).
Research, 14(1), 5–13. doi:10.1044/jshr.1401.05 The interfering effect of emotional stimulus func-
PMID:5550631 tions on stimulus equivalence class formation:
Implications for the understanding and treatment
Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emer-
of anxiety. European Journal of Behavior Analysis,
gent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. E. Zeiler
10, 121–140.
(Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units
(pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum
Associates.

181

Function over Form

Villatte, M., Monest ès, J. L., McHugh, L., Frei Conditional Discrimination: A discrimina-
xa i Baqu é, E., & Loas, G. (2010b). Assessing tion between stimuli, in which the reinforcement
perspective taking in schizophrenia using rela- of responding to one stimulus (e.g., a choice from
tional frame theory. The Psychological Record, an array) is conditional upon the presence of a
60, 413–436. further stimulus (i.e., the conditional stimulus).
Construct: A hypothetical construct is an
Villatte, M., Monestès, J.-L., McHugh, L., Freixa
explanatory variable that is not directly observ-
i Baqué, E., & Loas, G. (2010a). Adopting the
able, but which nevertheless has heuristic value.
perspective of another in belief attribution: Contri-
For example, the concepts of intelligence and
bution of relational frame theory to the understand-
motivation are used to explain phenomena in
ing of impairments in schizophrenia. Journal of
psychology, but neither is directly observable or
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
deemed to be extant entities.
41(2), 125–134. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.11.004
Functional Analysis: An analysis of behavior
PMID:20034611
in terms of its antecedents, consequences and
Watt, A., Keenan, M., Barnes, D., & Cairns, E. over-arching context.
(1991). Social categorisation and stimulus equiva- Operant Conditioning: A process by which
lence. The Psychological Record, 41, 33–50. behavior rate is altered as a function of delivered
consequences (appetitive or aversive) that im-
Weil, T. M., Hayes, S. C., & Capurro, P. (2011).
mediately follow responses according to a known
Establishing a deictic relational repertoire in young
schedule.
children. The Psychological Record, 61, 5.
Relational Responding: Responding to rela-
Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). tions between stimuli rather than to the stimuli in
Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level isolation. Example of relations include; similarity,
and its relationship with questionnaire measures. difference, and temporal sequence.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Stimulus Equivalence: A phenomenon
72(2), 262–274. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.262 whereby untrained relations between at least three
PMID:9107001 stimuli emerge spontaneously and bear the defin-
ing characteristics of reflexivity, symmetry, and
transitivity. Reflexivity refers to identity match-
ing (e.g., A is A); symmetry refers to functional
KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS reversibility (e.g., given A is B, B is A is derived
spontaneously); and, transitivity refers to the
Attitude: An attitude is usually defined as
functional equivalence of stimuli related to each
being a combination of cognitive (i.e. proposi-
other only via a third stimulus (e.g., given A is
tional) and affective evaluations of an object with
B and B is C, A is C is derived spontaneously).
a variable strength.

182
183

Chapter 9
The Role of Implicit
Mechanisms in Buffering Self-
Esteem from Social Threats
Jordan B. Leitner
University of Delaware, USA

Chad E. Forbes
University of Delaware, USA

ABSTRACT
Previous research has demonstrated that people have the goal of self-enhancing, or viewing themselves
in an overly positive light. However, only recent research has examined the degree to which the rela-
tionship between self-enhancement goals and outcomes are a result of explicit deliberative mechanisms
or implicit automatic mechanisms. The current chapter reviews evidence on unconscious goal pursuit,
autobiographical memory, social neuroscience, and implicit self-esteem that suggests that implicit
mechanisms play a powerful role in producing self-enhancement outcomes. Furthermore, this chapter
reviews evidence that these implicit mechanisms are activated by social threats and thus contribute to
successful coping. Finally, the authors discuss the implications of implicit self-enhancement mechanisms
for targets of stigma, individuals who frequently encounter threats to well-being.

INTRODUCTION can influence a wide range of social behaviors.


Moreover, most people are motivated to see
Psychology has a rich history of studying self- themselves in a positive light, and indeed report
esteem, or the global evaluations a person holds of moderate to high self-esteem (Greenwald, 1980;
the self. Indeed, at the time of writing this chapter, Taylor & Brown, 1988). Previous theoretical
a PSYCinfo keyword search for “self-esteem” frameworks suggest that this goal of maintaining
returned 25,928 publications. Our fascination high self-esteem stems from positive self-views
with self-esteem is well-deserved, as it colors mitigating the effects of stress and failure (Steele,
every moment of our conscious experience, and 1998), reducing existential stress (Greenberg et al.,

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch009

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

1992), increasing mental health (Taylor & Brown, neuroscience literatures that suggests that self-
1988), and serving as a barometer of social ac- enhancement goals catalyze implicit mechanisms,
ceptance and liking (Leary & Baumeister, 2000). which in turn produce self-enhancement outcomes.
The goal of achieving and maintaining high Finally, we discuss ways in which implicit self-
self-esteem fundamentally alters attention, per- enhancement mechanisms may be activated by
ception, memory, and behavior in ways that cast social stress, and therefore help targets of social
the self in a positive light. Previous research has threat cope with stressful situations.
referred to the influence of such goals on percep-
tion and behavior as self-enhancement. However,
as research has largely focused on explicit self- EXPLICIT SELF-ENHANCEMENT
enhancement outcomes, little is known about the OUTCOMES
mechanisms of self-enhancement or the speeds at
which they occur. In the current chapter, explicit Previous work has demonstrated that explicit self-
self-enhancement outcomes refer to the conscious enhancement outcomes are evident in perceptions
consequences of self-enhancement goals (e.g., of autobiographical events, current abilities, and
consciously evaluating one’s own abilities as predicted life outcomes (Taylor & Brown, 1988).
overly positive), whereas self-enhancement For example, individuals report more internal and
mechanisms refer to the processes through global attributions for positive outcomes than
which self-enhancement goals produce these negative outcomes (Alicke, 1985; Campbell &
consequences (e.g., greater encoding of positive Sedikides, 1999; Mezulis, Abramson, Hyde, &
than negative feedback). We define an explicit Hankin, 2004), consider scientific research less
mechanism as one that is slow, controlled, and credible if it implies they are susceptible to a
linked to conscious self-perception, whereas an disease (Kunda, 1987), and are unrealistically
implicit mechanism is one that is fast, automatic, optimistic about future events (Puri & Robinson,
and outside of conscious awareness. As such, 2007; Weinstein, 1980). Demonstrating that these
an explicit self-enhancement mechanism would outcomes emerge in domains where self-assess-
involve a deliberative and conscious internal dia- ment accuracy is critical, students consistently
logue (e.g., “If I think about my experiences in a underestimate the time necessary to complete
certain way, then I will feel good about myself”), academic tasks (Bueler, Griffin, & Ross, 1994),
whereas an implicit self-enhancement mechanism and surgical residents are overly confident in
would not involve such dialogue. their ability to diagnose medical conditions that
The aim of this chapter is to review evidence they fail to detect in X-rays (Oksam, Kingma, &
suggesting that the link between self-enhancement Klasen, 2000).
goals and explicit self-enhancement outcomes is Although many explicit self-enhancement
driven by mechanisms that are largely implicit. outcomes correspond with inaccurate assess-
That is, people who have the goal of self-enhanc- ments of ability (Dunning, Heath, Suls, 2004),
ing may consciously perceive themselves in an other manifestations of self-enhancement involve
overly positive light (i.e., the explicit outcome focusing attention on more successful domains.
of self-enhancement), but be completely unaware For instance, self-affirmation theory (Steele,
of how and when they modified perceptions of 1988) posits that affirming the self in successful
themselves (i.e., the implicit mechanisms of self- domains attenuates the effect of negative feedback
enhancement). We begin by reviewing explicit on self-esteem. Supportive of this view, individuals
self-enhancement outcomes, after which we de- who define themselves in many unique ways (e.g.,
scribe evidence from behavioral, cognitive, and “mother”, “lawyer”, “chess player”) demonstrate

184

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

more positive mental health (Linville, 1987), as able in self-relevant domains (Tesser, Campbell,
these individuals can self-affirm in any number of & Smith, 1984), and increase the self-relevance of
domains when they encounter negative feedback. a domain after outperforming a close other (Tes-
Related research has demonstrated that when in a ser & Paulhus, 1983). The motivation to perceive
stressful situation, individuals explicitly decrease one’s own abilities as better than others is also
the degree to which they base self-esteem on the evident in the “better than average effect”, wherein
negative feedback (Leitner, Jones & Hehman, most people report that they are above average, a
2013; Major, Spencer, Schmader, Wolfe, & mathematical impossibility. Demonstrating this
Crocker, 1998). effect, 94% of college professors rated their work
Additionally, self-enhancement goals influ- as above average (Cross, 1977), and individuals
ence the type of interpersonal comparisons that a overwhelmingly assert that they are better-than-
person makes. Specifically, people form self-eval- average drivers (Svenson, 1981), and above
uations by comparing the self to others (Festinger, average on socially desirable personality traits
1954), and when self-enhancement motivations (Pedregon, Farley, Davis, Wood, & Clark, 2012).
produce comparisons to less successful others (i.e., In sum, research has demonstrated that self-
downward social comparison), the comparison can enhancement goals influence explicit perceptions
evoke feelings of superiority (Wills, 1981). Self- of ability and interpersonal comparisons. How-
enhancement goals can also evoke comparisons ever, it remains unclear whether the mechanisms
with more successful others (i.e., upward social explaining the link between self-enhancement
comparison), particularly when the comparison goals and these explicit outcomes are deliber-
allows a person to “bask in the reflective glory” ate and explicit, or automatic and implicit. For
of another’s success (Cialdini et al., 1976). example, when individuals estimate their ability
The dynamics of explicit interpersonal com- as above average, do they explicitly calculate
parisons and their relationship to self-esteem their own ability, average ability, and the relative
are outlined in the Self-Evaluation Maintenance difference between their own and average ability,
Model (SEM; Tesser, 1988). According to this or is the process accomplished largely outside
perspective, the impact of another person’s per- of their conscious awareness? Moreover, little is
formance on self-esteem depends on three factors: known about whether self-enhancement goals alter
the other’s performance in relation to one’s own implicit perceptual processes that occur when a
performance, the psychological closeness of the person first encounters evaluative feedback.
other, and the self-relevance of the performance
domain. As the closeness of the other increases, the
impact of an interpersonal comparison on the self SELF-ENHANCEMENT
increases. Thus, whereas being outperformed by MECHANISMS
a close other in a self-relevant domain diminishes
self-esteem, being outperformed by a close other in In order to understand the roots of self-enhance-
a non-self-relevant domain increases self-esteem ment and self-esteem, it is critical to determine
as one’s self can bask in the reflective glory of the degree to which self-enhancement mechanisms
the other person’s success. bias information processing at a fundamental
Importantly, self-enhancement goals can in- level, or after much deliberation and effort. Past
fluence the three factors of the SEM model. For research suggests these mechanisms may manifest
example, individuals hinder the performance of at largely implicit speeds of processing. To gain
close others in self-relevant domains (Pemberton further insight into the degree to which self-
& Sedekides, 2001), choose friends that are less enhancement goals evoke implicit mechanisms,

185

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

we turn to research on unconscious goal pursuit, well-integrated knowledge network that contains
autobiographical memory, social neuroscience, information about the factors that facilitate or
and implicit self-esteem. inhibit goal pursuit (Dijksterhuis &Aarts, 2012).
Accordingly, to the extent that self-enhancement
Insight from Unconscious goals are frequently activated, implicit mechanisms
Goal Pursuit should contribute to self-enhancement outcomes.
However, more explicit self-enhancing mecha-
The notion that self-enhancement goals modulate nisms may be recruited when self-enhancement
implicit mechanisms is supported by a large body goals are not frequently activated. Supportive of
of research showing that goal pursuit operates this possibility, participants who were subliminally
outside of conscious awareness (Aarts & Dijk- primed with the goal to achieve showed greater
sterhuis, 2000; Bargh et al. 2001; Fitzsimons & conscious awareness of the goal when then task
Bargh, 2003; Fitzsimons et al., 2008; Kruglanski was difficult (Bongers, Dijksterhuis, & Spears,
et al. 2002). For example, Fitzsimons and Bargh 2010). Furthermore, recent work demonstrated
(2003) demonstrated that subliminally present- that unconsciously activated goals in person-
ing participants with the names of their parents ally valuable domains (e.g., academics), taxed
increased achievement-related goals, whereas executive control in unrelated domains (Marien,
priming participants with the names of friends Custers, Hassin, & Aarts, 2012), suggesting that
increased helping-related goals. Given that most unconscious goals “hijack” explicit resources
individuals have the goal to maintain and achieve when the goal is particularly valuable. Together,
high self-esteem, this suggests implicit processes these results suggest that implicit mechanisms play
should contribute to self-enhancement in impor- a critical role in pursuing self-enhancement goals,
tant ways. but that explicit mechanisms are recruited when
Indeed, research has found that manipulat- the self-enhancement goal is difficult to attain.
ing implicit processes influences explicit self- In sum, research suggests that self-enhance-
enhancement outcomes. For example, Custer and ment goals influence processes that operate outside
Aarts (2005) paired a neutral task with subliminally conscious awareness, and that implicit mechanisms
presented positive, negative, or neutral words. should be sufficient when self-enhancement goals
Participants reported greater desire for, and worked are easy to attain. One factor that might influence
harder on, tasks that were primed with positive the difficulty of self-enhancement goal attainment
words. Furthermore, once an implicit goal is acti- is the discrepancy between a person’s current
vated, failing to meet that goal influences explicit self-esteem and desired self-esteem. Specifically,
outcomes. For instance, Bongers, Dijksterhuis, and self-enhancement should be supported by implicit
Spears (2009) showed that when participants were mechanisms when there is a small discrepancy
subliminally primed to succeed, poor performance between a person’s current and desired self-esteem,
on a task corresponded with more external per- whereas self-enhancement may need to recruit
formance attributions, and lowering standards of more explicit, deliberate mechanisms when this
what constituted good performance. Thus, these discrepancy is large. Thus, self-enhancement may
findings suggest that implicit mechanisms shape occur more implicitly for chronically high self-
explicit perceptions of own abilities. esteem individuals, who frequently self-enhance
Implicit goal pursuit is thought to arise when and have a small discrepancy between their current
the goal is activated frequently, and is part of a and desired level of self-esteem.

186

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Insight from Memory Research suggest that self-enhancement is supported by


recalling memories related to desirable traits in
One way that self-enhancement goals may influ- more general terms.
ence explicit perceptions of ability is through The link between self-enhancement goals and
implicit autobiographical memory processes. explicit perceptions of ability may additionally be
Indeed, explicit perceptions of ability rely driven by implicit temporal distancing processes
on autobiographical information (Markus & that characterize positive events as occurring
Kunda, 1986), and research suggests that self- more recently than negative events. For instance,
enhancement goals influence the regulation of people maintain the illusion of self-improvement
autobiographical memory. For instance, Sanitioso, by disparaging past selves and complimenting
Kunda, and Fong (1990) led people to believe that more recent selves (Wilson & Ross, 2001), and
either extraversion or introversion led to success, remember negative events as happening more
after which participants recalled autobiographical distant in the past, especially when motivated to
events in which they were extraverted or intro- maintain high self-esteem (Ross & Wilson, 2002).
verted. Participants who believed that extraversion Relatedly, self-enhancement goals influence per-
corresponded with success recalled examples of ceived temporal distance of future autobiographi-
extroverted behavior with greater frequency and cal events, as students who anticipate success on
speed, whereas participants who believed that a midterm construe the exam as temporally closer
introversion corresponded with success recalled than students who anticipate failure (Peetz, Wilson,
examples of introverted behavior with greater &Strahan, 2009).
frequency and speed. Building on these find- In sum, self-enhancement goals influence
ings, Santisoso and Wlodarski (2004) showed the spontaneous recall of autobiographical in-
that memory accuracy is greater when recalling formation, such that people remember favorable
autobiographical information about a trait linked autobiographical information with greater ac-
to success. curacy, speed, and abstraction than unfavorable
Furthermore, research has demonstrated that information. These findings are consistent with the
self-enhancement goals influence the generality constructionist view of autobiographical memory,
or specificity of autobiographical memories. Gen- which suggests that current goals influence how
eral memories are abstract and represent repeated autobiographical memories are constructed (Con-
events over an extended period of time (e.g., “I way & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000), as well as research
went out on many dinner dates last year”), whereas showing that links between regulatory goals and
specific memories represent discrete episodes memory are unconscious (Holland, Tamir, &
(e.g. “I went out to a Thai restaurant with Molly Kensinger, 2010). Thus, to the extent that a person
on Valentine’s Day”; Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, has self-enhancement goals, implicit mechanisms
2000). Participants who were made to believe should shape autobiographical memory in ways
that extraversion predicted success recalled more that render the self in a positive light.
general extraversion-related memories and more
specific introversion-related memories, whereas Insight from Social Neuroscience
participants who were made to believe that intro-
version predicted success showed the opposite Social neuroscience methodologies can provide a
pattern (Brunot & Santitoso, 2004). As more window into how self-enhancement goals influ-
general memories, relative to specific memories, ence early perceptual and encoding processes
are more easily integrated into the self-concept that underlie autobiographical memory. In par-
(Klein, Loftus, & Sherman, 1993), these findings ticular, the timing of brain activity can be used

187

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

as an index for implicit and explicit mechanisms. prefrontal cortex activity (Taylor, Burkland, Eisen-
Implicit mechanisms should be reflected by fast, berger, & Lehman, 2008), a brain region integral
immediate brain activity (e.g., 32ms post-stimulus; for cognitive and affective control (Lieberman, et
Forbes, 2012), whereas explicit mechanisms al., 2007).Consistent with this, the self-enhancing
should be reflected by brain activity that occurs at participants in Taylor et al.’s (2008) threat regula-
least several hundred milliseconds after stimulus tion task also showed decreased amygdala reactiv-
processing begins. Similarly, implicit processes ity, a brain region involved in affective processes
should involve fewer iterations of processing in at multiple levels of awareness (Pasley et al. 2004).
a given neural network (Cunningham & Zelazo, However, it is difficult to determine whether these
2007). Accordingly, this section utilizes social findings implicate implicit or explicit mechanisms,
neuroscience research to inform our understand- as they utilized fMRI, a method with low temporal
ing of how self-enhancement goals modulate resolution that is ill-equipped for examining the
implicit perceptual processes to produce explicit time course of such processes. Thus, methods
self-enhancement outcomes. with higher temporal resolution may be better at
Evidence for self-enhancement processes can dissociating faster implicit processes from slower
be garnered from studies involving the medial explicit processes.
frontal cortex (MFC), as the MFC represents Electroencephalography (EEG) is a method
both goal monitoring and knowledge about the of recording large scale neural interactions at the
self (Amodio and Frith, 2006). Indeed, fMRI scalp with temporal precision that can be used to
research has identified regions within the MFC dissociate neurocognitive processes occurring
associated with monitoring the valence of self- early versus late in the information processing
relevant information (Beer & Hughes, 2010; stream (Luck & Kappenman, 2012). Previous EEG
Hughes & Beer, 2013; Masten et al., 2009; Moran research has demonstrated that goals influence
et al., 2006; Sommerville et al., 2006), as well as the earliest stages of perception, as evidenced by
shaping perceptions of relative social standing changes in electrocortical activity around 100ms
(Sommerville, Kelley, Heatherton, 2010). Addi- following a stimulus (DiRusso et al., 2003; Go-
tionally, using transcranial magnetic stimulation, a mez-Gonzales et al., 1994; Hillyard et al., 1998;
technique that engenders temporary disruption of Rutman et al., 2009). Of relevance to implicit self-
local neural activity, Kwan et al. (2007) found that enhancement mechanisms, Krusemark, Campbell,
disrupting local activity in the prefrontal portion and Clementz (2008) presented participants with
of the MFC reduced the degree to which partici- bogus positive or negative feedback and then
pants rated themselves more positively than their prompted them to make an internal or external
best friend. Finally, fMRI data suggest that when attribution for the feedback (i.e., attribution
presented with negative information, chronically stimulus). Highlighting the interaction between
optimistic individuals exhibited decreased activity self-enhancement goals and fast perceptual pro-
in the inferior prefrontal region of the MFC, which cesses, EEG activity just 320ms following the
in turn predicted more positive perceptions of the attribution stimulus predicted whether participants
event (Sharot, Korn, & Dolan, 2011). ultimately made a self-serving attribution in the
Self-enhancement goals may additionally 2500ms that followed.
modulate a network of brain regions outside the Further examining the relationships between
MFC. For instance, individuals more likely to self-enhancement goals, early electrocortical ac-
demonstrate self-enhancing outcomes (i.e., those tivity, and explicit self-enhancement outcomes,
with high self-esteem and optimism) respond to Leitner, Hehman, Jones, and Forbes (in press)
a threat regulation task with greater ventrolateral recorded EEG while presenting participants with

188

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

equal amounts of positive and negative feedback with positive versus negative attributes. Implicit
(i.e., social acceptance or rejection) during a social self-esteem can be measured with the Implicit
interaction task. Following this task, participants Association Test (IAT), which assesses the as-
estimated the number of times they were accepted sociative strength between self-related concepts
and rejected. Results showed that participants and valenced attributes (Greenwald & Farnham,
who explicitly reported greater self-enhancement 2000). Supportive of the view that self-enhancing
motivations responded to rejection feedback with goals modulate implicit mechanisms, individuals
decreased MFC alpha power (i.e., the degree to are typically faster at responding to self-related
which collections of neurons within MFC fire in words when they are paired with positive, com-
unison at a specific frequency), which is a neural pared to negative, characteristics. Remarkably,
profile that is associated with motivated attention most individuals also show strong associations
and encoding (Klimesch, 2011). Furthermore, for objects related to the self, such as birthday
decreased MFC alpha power to rejection feed- numbers or name initials (i.e., the name letter
back predicted fewer explicit rejection estimates, test; Bosson, Swann, & Pennebaker, 2000; Nuttin,
and mediated the relationship between self- 1985; Pelham, Mirenberg, & Jones, 2002). Thus,
enhancement motivations and explicit estimates these findings suggest that that self-enhancement
of feedback in the task. Importantly, the neural goals help forge stronger associations between
effects in these relationships emerged in the first the self and positive attributes, which in turn may
500ms of processing feedback, suggesting that increase the accessibility of positive attributes.
self-enhancement goals influence relatively fast Interestingly, implicit and explicit self-esteem
perceptual processes, which in turn, influence are influenced by different situational factors
explicit self-enhancement outcomes. (Swann & Bosson, 2010), and they often diverge
In sum, social neuroscience research suggests (Jones et al., 2002; Karpinski & Hilton, 2001).
that self-enhancement goals alter neural function- When this divergence results in high explicit and
ing within the first few hundred milliseconds of low implicit self-esteem, it is known as “fragile”
viewing a stimulus. These early biases in neural self-esteem. Individuals with fragile self-esteem
activity are (a) likely linked to implicit cognitive are thought to compensate for this discrepancy
mechanisms that operate outside of conscious by explicitly exaggerating their strengths (Bau-
awareness, and (b) predict whether a person will meister, Smart, Boden, 1996; Ziegler-Hill, 2007).
demonstrate explicit self-enhancement outcomes. Thus, an interesting possibility is that, for fragile
Together, findings suggest that self-enhancement self-esteem individuals, self-enhancement relies
goals influence implicit processes to produce ex- on more explicit (and less implicit) strategies, as
plicit self-enhancement outcomes. Accordingly, it there may be greater discrepancy between their
is reasonable to expect implicit self-enhancement current self-esteem and desired self-esteem. In
mechanisms to alter the strength with which a contrast, self-enhancement may rely on more
person associates the self with positive and nega- implicit mechanisms for individuals with both
tive attributes. high explicit and implicit self-esteem, as there
may be a smaller discrepancy between current
Insight from Implicit Self-Esteem self-esteem and desired self-esteem.
However, we interpret research on implicit
Though few studies have examined implicit self- self-esteem cautiously. Some research has ques-
enhancement mechanisms directly, past research tioned the construct validity of implicit self-esteem
has provided insight into implicit self-esteem, or measures, as these measures have low temporal
the strength with which a person associates the self stability, poor convergent validity with typical

189

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

covariates of self-esteem, and may be influenced self-enhancement mechanisms that attune basic
by self-presentation strategies (Buhrmeister, perceptual processes to more positive self-relevant
Blanton, & Swann, 2011). Implicit self-esteem information. As such, individuals may cope
might also be a multi-dimensional construct that with social stressors by relying on implicit self-
is not adequately captured by conventional mea- enhancement mechanisms. In this next section, we
sures (Swann & Bosson, 2010), and it is unclear review evidence that implicit self-enhancement
whether implicit and explicit self-esteem measures mechanisms help people cope with social threats.
capture the same underlying construct (Epstein, We then examine how implicit self-enhancement
2003; Fazio & Towles-Schwen, 1999). mechanisms provide a buffer for individuals who
In sum, to the extent that implicit self-esteem are particularly susceptible to social threats: tar-
measures capture the strength of associations gets of stigma.
between the self and valenced attributes, implicit
self-enhancement mechanisms forge strong asso- Implicit Coping
ciations between the self and positive attributes.
Additionally, it is possible that the discrepancy be- One way self-enhancement mechanisms may
tween implicit and explicit self-esteem determines contribute to coping with stress is by uncon-
the degree to which self-enhancement relies on sciously directing attention away from threatening
explicit or implicit mechanisms. However, future stimuli. Indeed, previous research has shown that
research is needed to clarify the nature of implicit activating an unconscious goal directs attention
self-esteem, and its relationship to implicit self- away from goal-inconsistent information (Papies,
enhancing mechanisms. Strobe, & Aarts, 2008). Thus, to the extent that
the goal to self-enhance manifests implicitly,
coping processes should direct attention away
IMPLICIT SELF-ENHANCEMENT from self-effacing information. Supportive of this
AND THREAT view, Dijskterhuis (2004) manipulated implicit
self-esteem via subliminal priming, and found that
Given that self-enhancement outcomes could be participants with augmented implicit self-esteem
produced by either explicit or implicit mechanisms, showed diminished affective reactivity to negative
it is important to understand the contexts that feedback. Notably, increasing implicit self-esteem
may render implicit mechanisms advantageous. in this manner did little to increase participants’
One major function of implicit self-enhancement reactivity to positive feedback, suggesting that
mechanisms may be to provide a buffer from social implicit self-enhancement mechanisms may be
threats. Threatening situations can be cognitively especially attuned to navigating attention away
taxing, and thus limit the degree of executive from negative feedback.
resources that a person can devote to explicit Should implicit mechanisms contribute to
self-enhancement. As implicit processes require coping with threat, it would be important for im-
few executive resources (Muraven & Baumeister, plicit self-enhancement mechanisms to become
2000), implicit self-enhancement mechanisms activated in the face of threatening feedback.
may be particularly adaptive in stressful situa- Indeed, previous research demonstrated that fol-
tions. Moreover, as self-enhancement ceases once lowing threats to self-worth, individuals are more
self-esteem reaches a satisfactory set point (Tesser likely to self-enhance (Brown, 2012; Campbell &
et al., 2001), implicit self-enhancement mecha- Sedikides, 1999). Additionally, individuals high
nisms may be activated by negative social cues. in explicit self-esteem respond to self-concept
Specifically, social stress may trigger implicit threats with increased implicit self-esteem

190

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

(Jones, et al., 2002), suggesting that implicit self- mine well-being and performance, and concludes
enhancement mechanisms respond to threat by with an examination of how self-enhancement
strengthening associations between the self and mechanisms serve to mitigate this treat.
positive attributes. Other research supports this A bevy of research has been devoted to
idea, finding that social exclusion increases the understanding how stigma has deleterious con-
accessibility of positive emotions (Dewall et al., sequences on everything ranging from health
2011) and selective attention to signs of social disparities (Clark, Anderson, Cark, & Williams,
acceptance (Dewall et al., 2009). Thus, these find- 1999) to information processing (Sedlovskaya,
ings converge to suggest that in the face of social Purdie-Vaughns, Eibach, & LaFrance, 2013) to
threats, self-enhancement goals evoke implicit test performance (Steele, 1997). Specific to perfor-
self-enhancement mechanisms, which contribute mance, past research demonstrates that stigmatized
to positive perceptions and well-being. individuals tend to underperform on tasks of merit
Similarly, individuals who explicitly self- in situations where their group membership and
enhance show decreased physiological responses relevant negative group stereotype are primed. This
to stress. For instance, in response to stressful phenomenon, known as stereotype threat (Steele
laboratory tasks, those who explicitly report more & Aronson, 1995; Schmader, Johns, & Forbes,
positive self-perceptions show decreased blood 2008), likely stems from a situational stressor
pressure reactivity (Taylor, Lerner, Sherman, that stigmatized targets experience when they
Sage, & McDowell, 2003) and cortisol reactivity fear their actions may confirm the negative group
(Taylor et al., 2008). High self-enhancing students stereotype. This situational stressor has the ironic
also react to stressful situations with more posi- effect of inadvertently engendering confirmation
tive vagal reactivity (Gramzow, Willard, Mendes, of the negative group stereotype.
2008), an autonomic nervous system response Stereotype threat has been shown to uniquely
that corresponds with positive affective states affect how stigmatized individuals perceive
(Porges, 2007). As these physiological responses themselves. For instance, stereotype threatened
are linked to the autonomic nervous system and individuals tend to experience more negative,
are not dependent on conscious awareness, implicit self-critical cognitions, that in turn, mediate un-
mechanisms are at least partially responsible for derperformance on difficult tasks (Cadinuet al.,
coping with social stress. 2005). Perceptions of underperformance also lead
to more self-doubt, particularly when people reap-
Targets of Stigma praise negative emotions and arousal experienced
during performance (Schmader, Forbes, Zhang, &
Given that implicit self-enhancement mechanisms Mendes, 2009; Study 3). These negative self-per-
protect individuals from social threat, these pro- ceptions may stem from an inherent bias towards
cesses should have important implications for negative cognitions and stereotype-confirming
negatively stereotyped individuals who chronically information in stereotype threatening situations.
encounter socially stressful situations. Specifi- For instance, Forbes and Leitner (2014) found that
cally, implicit self-enhancing mechanisms may stereotype threatened women exhibited a neural at-
help targets of stigma maintain self-worth and tentional bias towards negative feedback as early as
positive performance in the face of situations that 75ms after receiving feedback. More specifically,
threaten to confirm negative stereotypes about negative feedback evoked greater communication
their groups. This next section reviews research between brain regions integral for attention and
demonstrating how the threat of stigma can under- working memory (anterior cingulate cortex and

191

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), and increased guez, & Ruble, 2010), and reappraise negative,
neural activity in the temporal gyrus, which in self-oriented emotions when sympathetic arousal
turn undermined performance. Importantly, this levels are high (Schmader et al., 2009).
processing bias may ultimately facilitate enhanced Of relevance to individual differences in coping
encoding of negative performance feedback that, abilities, insight can also be gained from recent
in turn, undermines performance and perpetuates findings in the social neuroscience literature per-
feelings of anxiety (Forbes, Duran-Jordan, Mager- taining to the default mode network (DMN). The
man and Leitner, in press). DMN represents a network of neural regions that
In order to cope with the threats associated exhibit increases in neural activity when individu-
with stigma, negatively stereotyped individu- als are focusing attention internally or are engaged
als employ self-enhancement mechanisms. For in self-oriented processing (Raichle et al., 2001;
instance, stereotype threatened individuals are Buckner et al., 2008). The extent to which these
more likely to generate external attributions prior regions communicate with one another (i.e., phase
to performance, or self-handicap (Stone, 2002), lock) may provide an index of more efficacious
disengage their self-worth from negative feed- coping with situational stressors (Spreng & Grady,
back on supposed intelligence tests (Leitner, et 2009). Building on this basic framework, Forbes,
al., 2013; Major et al., 1998) and deny evidence Duran-Jordan, et al. (under review) obtained as-
of incompetence in response to stereotype threat sessments of DMN phase locking in minorities and
manipulations (von Hippel et al., 2005). Though it whites at rest and then had them complete a task
is not entirely clear whether these coping mecha- designed to elicit comparable amounts of error and
nisms occur automatically and without conscious correct feedback under stereotype threat. Partici-
awareness, the result of these strategies is the pants then reported how many errors they made
maintenance of a positive self-concept in the face on the supposed intelligence test and their self-
of negative stereotypes. doubt. Results revealed that minorities reported
By extension, individual differences in con- more accurate performance perceptions and less
tending with self-threats has been shown to self-doubt to the extent they exhibited better phase
mitigate stereotype threat effects. For instance, locking between DMN regions at rest. Conversely,
individuals high in self-monitoring (i.e., those whites did not exhibit any of these relationships,
inclined to adjust behaviors in accordance with suggesting that individual differences in DMN
others’ expectations or social norms) are less phase-locking moderated self-perceptions only
susceptible to stereotype threat effects (Inzlicht in stressful, self-threatening contexts.
et al., 2006). One reason for this is that higher Finally, positive implicit associations between
self-monitoring affords individuals greater self- the self and the stigmatized domain can bolster
regulatory resources and coping skills in stressful motivation. Forbes and Schmader (2010) had
situations (Inzlicht et al., 2006; Miller, Omens, women and men complete a task that manipulated
& Delvadia, 1991; Seeley & Gardner, 2003). implicit associations between math and positive
Stigmatized individuals are also less susceptible or negative constructs. Across multiple studies,
to stereotype threat effects to the extent they women who implicitly associated the math domain
take a promotion (i.e., seeking success) oriented with positive constructs chose to spend more time
self-regulatory focus (Seibt & Forster, 2004; working on math related tasks and solving math
Higgins, 1998), appraise performance situations problems. This effect was exacerbated under ste-
as challenging (Alter, Aronson, Darley, Rodri- reotype threat, and under conditions that strength-

192

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

ened the negative women-math stereotype. Thus, enhancement domain. It will be critical to examine
these findings suggest that implicit processes are the factors that influence the frequency and dif-
involved in how a person copes with the threat ficulty of self-enhancement (e.g., depression), and
of stigma. if these factors predict whether self-enhancement
In sum, the threat of negative stereotypes can processes occur implicit or explicitly.
evoke biases in attention and working memory It will also be important to refine measures
that undermine well-being and performance. of implicit self-esteem. We propose that implicit
Importantly, however, intrapersonal regulatory self-esteem might play a role in the degree to
mechanisms can attenuate the deleterious effects of which self-enhancement mechanisms are implicit
stereotype threat. While few studies have directly or explicit. However, various measures of implicit
examined the degree to which these regulatory self-esteem have relatively poor construct validity
mechanisms are implicit or explicit, recent work and inconsistent predictive validity (Buhrmesier,
suggests that implicit mechanisms are particularly et al., 2011). Thus, it will be critical to design
important in buffering individuals from the stress frameworks that conceptualize the nuances of
associated with stigma. implicit self-esteem, and develop measurement
tools to capture these nuances.
Finally, future work should examine how im-
FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS plicit self-enhancement mechanisms contribute to
coping with social threat. Targets of stigma face
While we propose that implicit mechanisms the threat of devaluation, exclusion, and confirma-
play a major role in achieving self-enhancement tion of negative stereotypes. Building on Forbes
outcomes, the relative role of explicit mecha- and Schmader (2010), it would be important to
nisms remains unclear. For example, how are investigate how implicit processes contribute to
implicit mechanisms influenced when someone motivation and performance for stigmatized indi-
consciously and deliberately considers strategies viduals across domains. Additionally, it would be
they might employ to feel better about themselves important to better understand the relationships
(e.g., positive self-talk, mindfulness mediation)? between self-enhancement goals and implicit
Conversely, how are explicit, deliberative pro- coping mechanisms. One intriguing possibility is
cesses influenced when implicit self-enhancement that there exists a feedback loop, wherein self-en-
processes are at work? Future research should in- hancement goals modulate implicit mechanisms,
vestigate these questions by examining interactions which in turn influence self-enhancement goals.
between implicit and explicit self-enhancement Thus, methodologically refined research might
processes. elucidate the extent which self-enhancement goals
Relatedly, little is known about the factors and implicit mechanisms shape one other.
determining whether self-enhancement fluctuates
between an implicit and explicit process. Previous
research suggests that pursuing a goal frequently CONCLUSION
automates goal pursuit into an unconscious process
(Aarts & Dijksterhuis 2000; Sheeran et al., 2005), The current chapter makes a distinction between
whereas difficult goal pursuit recruits working self-enhancement goals, self-enhancement
memory resources and pushes goal pursuit into outcomes (i.e., the explicit consequences of
conscious awareness (Bongers, Dijksterhuis, & self-enhancement goals), and self-enhancement
Spears, 2010; Marien et al., 2012). However, no mechanisms (i.e., the processes through which
research has directly tied these findings to the self- such outcomes emerge). Previous research has

193

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

revealed that self-enhancement goals correspond Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting
with myriad self-enhancement outcomes, in which of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social
people view their past, present, and future, as overly cognition. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 7(4),
positive. However, only recently has research 268–277. doi:10.1038/nrn1884 PMID:16552413
examined the degree to which these outcomes
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A.,
are a result of explicit deliberative mechanisms,
Barndollar, K., & Trötschel, R. (2001). The auto-
or implicit automatic mechanisms. The evidence
mated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of
reviewed in this chapter indicates that implicit
behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social
processes play a powerful role in producing self-
Psychology, 81(6), 1014–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-
enhancement outcomes. Research on unconscious
3514.81.6.1014 PMID:11761304
goal pursuit, social neuroscience, autobiographi-
cal memory, and implicit self-esteem converge to Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J.
suggest that, without conscious awareness, self- I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem
enhancing mechanisms shape the lens through cause better performance, interpersonal success,
which we see ourselves in the world. Given that happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychologi-
these implicit mechanisms help people maintain cal Science in the Public Interest, 4(1), 1–44.
positive self-views, implicit self-enhancement doi:10.1111/1529-1006.01431
appears to be especially important for targets of
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M.
social threats to navigate stressful situations.
(1996). Relation of threatened egotism to vio-
lence and aggression: The dark side of high self-
esteem. Psychological Review, 103(1), 5–33.
REFERENCES
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.5 PMID:8650299
Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as Beer, J. S., & Hughes, B. L. (2010). Neural
knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-di- systems of social comparisons and the “above
rected behavior. Journal of Personality and Social average” effect. NeuroImage, 49(3), 2671–
Psychology, 78(1), 53–63. doi:10.1037/0022- 2679. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.075
3514.78.1.53 PMID:10653505 PMID:19883771
Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2009).
determined by the desirability a controllability of The unconscious eye opener: Pupil dilation re-
trait adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social veals strategic recruitment of mental resources
Psychology, 49(6), 1621–1630. doi:10.1037/0022- upon subliminal reward cues. Psychological
3514.49.6.1621 Science, 20, 1313–1315. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
Alter, A., Aronson, J., Darley, J., Rodriguez, C., & 9280.2009.02443.x PMID:19788532
Ruble, D. N. (2010). Rising to the threat: Reducing Bongers, K. C., Dijksterhuis, A., & Spears, R.
stereotype threat by reframing the threat as a chal- (2010). On the role of consciousness in goal
lenge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, pursuit. Social Cognition, 28(2), 262–272.
46(1), 166–171. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.014 doi:10.1521/soco.2010.28.2.262

194

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Bongers, K. C. A., Dijksterhuis, A., & Spears, R. Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Rosabianca, A., &
(2009). Self-esteem regulation after success and Kiesner, J. (2005). Why do women underperform
failure to attain unconsciously activated goals. under stereotype threat? Evidence for the role of
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), negative thinking. Psychological Science, 16(7),
468–477. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.12.007 572–578. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01577.x
PMID:16008792
Bosson, J. K., Swann Jr, W. B., & Pennebaker,
J. W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-
implicit self- esteem: The blind men and the threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-
elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and analytic integration. Review of General Psychol-
Social Psychology, 79, 631-643. ogy, 3(1), 23–43. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23
Brown, J. D. (2012). Understanding the Better Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thorne, A., Walker,
Than Average Effect Motives (Still) Matter. M. R., Freeman, S., & Sloan, L. R. (1976). Basking
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, in reflected glory: Three (football) field studies.
38(2), 209–219. doi:10.1177/0146167211432763 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
PMID:22205623 34(3), 366–375. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.3.366
Brunot, S., & Sanitioso, R. B. (2004). Motivational Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., &
influence on the quality of memories: Recall of Williams, D. R. (1999). Racism as a stressor
general autobiographical memories related to for African Americans: A biopsychosocial
desired attributes. European Journal of Social model. The American Psychologist, 54(10),
Psychology, 34(5), 627–635. doi:10.1002/ejsp.220 805–816. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.10.805
PMID:10540593
Buckner, R. L., Andrews‐Hanna, J. R., & Schacter,
D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network. Annals of Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000).
the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 1–38. The construction of autobiographical memo-
doi:10.1196/annals.1440.011 PMID:18400922 ries in the self-memory system. Psychological
Review, 107(2), 261–288. doi:10.1037/0033-
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994).
295X.107.2.261 PMID:10789197
Exploring the” planning fallacy”: Why people
underestimate their task completion times. Jour- Cross, K. P. (1977). Not can, but will college
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), teaching be improved? New Directions for
366–381. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.366 Higher Education, 17(17), 1–15. doi:10.1002/
he.36919771703
Buhrmester, M. D., Blanton, H., & Swann, W. B.
Jr. (2011). Implicit self-esteem: Nature, measure- Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). At-
ment, and a new way forward. Journal of Person- titudes and evaluations: A social cognitive neuro-
ality and Social Psychology, 100(2), 365–385. science perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
doi:10.1037/a0021341 PMID:21038971 11(3), 97–104. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.005
PMID:17276131

195

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005). Positive af- Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The
fect as implicit motivator: On the noncon- MODE model of attitude behavior processes.
scious operation of behavioral goals. Journal In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual Process
of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(2), Theories in Social Psychology (pp. 97–116). New
129–142. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.2.129 York, NY: Guilford Press.
PMID:16162049
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social compari-
DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., & Rouby, D. A. son processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117–140.
(2009). Social exclusion and early-stage inter- doi:10.1177/001872675400700202
personal perception: Selective attention to signs
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Think-
of acceptance. Journal of Personality and So-
ing of you: Nonconscious pursuit of interper-
cial Psychology, 96(4), 729–741. doi:10.1037/
sonal goals associated with relationship partners.
a0014634 PMID:19309198
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
DeWall, C. N., Twenge, J. M., Koole, S. L., Bau- 84(1), 148–164. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.148
meister, R. F., Marquez, A., & Reid, M. W. (2011). PMID:12518976
Automatic emotion regulation after social exclu-
Forbes, C. E., Cox, C., Schmader, T., & Ryan,
sion: Tuning to positivity. Emotion (Washington,
L. (2012). Negative stereotype activation alters
D.C.), 11(3), 623–636. doi:10.1037/a0023534
interaction between neural correlates of arousal,
PMID:21668113
inhibition and cognitive control. Social Cogni-
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: The tive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(7), 771–781.
merits of unconscious thought in preference doi:10.1093/scan/nsr052 PMID:21954239
development and decision making. Journal
Forbes, C. E., Duran-Jordan, K., Magerman, A.,
of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5),
& Leitner, J. B. (in press). Unforgettable: Ste-
586–598. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.586
reotype threatening contexts enhance encoding
PMID:15535773
of negative feedback.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, at-
Forbes, C. E., & Leitner, J. B. (2014). Stereotype
tention, and (un) consciousness. Annual Review
threat engenders neural attentional bias towards
of Psychology, 61(1), 467–490. doi:10.1146/
negative feedback to undermine performance.
annurev.psych.093008.100445 PMID:19566422
Biological Psychology.
Dunning, D., Heath, C., & Suls, J. M. (2004).
Forbes, C. E., Leitner, J. B., Duran-Jordan, K.,
Flawed self-assessment implications for health,
Magerman, A., Schmader, R., & Allen, J. B. (in
education, and the workplace. Psychological
press). Spontaneous default mode network phase-
Science in the Public Interest, 5(3), 69–106.
locking modulates performance perceptions under
doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2004.00018.x
stereotype threat. Social Cognitive and Affective
Epstein, S. (2003). Cognitive-experiential self- Neuroscience.
theory of personality. In Millon, T., & Lerner, M.
Forbes, C. E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Retraining
J. (Eds), Comprehensive Handbook of Psychology,
attitudes and stereotypes to affect motivation and
Volume 5: Personality and Social Psychology (pp.
cognitive capacity under stereotype threat. Jour-
159-184). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5),
740–754. doi:10.1037/a0020971 PMID:20822288

196

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Gomez-Gonzalez, C. M. G., Clark, V. P., Fan, S., Holland, A. C., Tamir, M., & Kensinger, E. A.
Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Sources of (2010). The effect of regulation goals on emo-
attention-sensitive visual event-related potentials. tional event- specific knowledge. Memory (Hove,
Brain Topography, 7(1), 41–51. doi:10.1007/ England), 18(5), 504–521. doi:10.1080/0965821
BF01184836 PMID:7803199 1.2010.481628 PMID:20503130
Gramzow, R. H., Willard, G., & Mendes, W. Hughes, B. L., & Beer, J. S. (2012). Orbitofrontal
B. (2008). Big tales and cool heads: Academic cortex and anterior cingulate cortex are modulated
exaggeration is related to cardiac vagal reactiv- by motivated social cognition. Cerebral Cortex,
ity. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 8(1), 138–144. 22(6), 1372–1381. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr213
doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.1.138 PMID:18266525 PMID:21862446
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Inzlicht, M., Aronson, J., Good, C., & McKay,
Rosenblatt, A., Burling, J., & Lyon, D. et al. (1992). L. (2006). A particular resiliency to threatening
Why do people need self - esteem? Converging environments. Journal of Experimental Social
evidence that self-esteem serves an anxiety-buff- Psychology, 42(3), 323–336. doi:10.1016/j.
ering function. Journal of Personality and Social jesp.2005.05.005
Psychology, 63(6), 913–922. doi:10.1037/0022-
Jones, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C.,
3514.63.6.913 PMID:1460559
& Hetts, J. J. (2002). Name letter preferences
Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: are not merely mere exposure: Implicit egotism
Fabrication and revision of personal history. as self-regulation. Journal of Experimental So-
The American Psychologist, 35(7), 603–618. cial Psychology, 38(2), 170–177. doi:10.1006/
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.35.7.603 jesp.2001.1497
Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes
the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and the Implicit Association Test. Journal
and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5),
Psychology, 79(6), 1022–1038. doi:10.1037/0022- 774–778. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.774
3514.79.6.1022 PMID:11138752 PMID:11708556
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Sherman, J. W. (1993).
Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. The role of summary and specific behavioral
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, memories in trait judgments about the self. Per-
30, 1–46. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60381-0 sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19(3),
305–311. doi:10.1177/0146167293193007
Hillyard, S. A., Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. (1998).
Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mecha- Klimesch, W. (2011). Evoked alpha and early
nism of selective attention: Electrophysiological access to the knowledge system: The P1 inhibi-
and neuroimaging evidence. Philosophical Trans- tion timing hypothesis. Brain Research, 1408,
actions of the Royal Society of London. Series 52–71. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.003
B, Biological Sciences, 353(1373), 1257–1270. PMID:21774917
doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0281 PMID:9770220

197

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Krusemark, E. A., Campbell, W. K., & Clem- Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett,
entz, B. A. (2008). Attributions, deception, and M. J., Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M.
event related potentials: An investigation of (2007). Putting feelings into words: Affect labeling
the self‐serving bias. Psychophysiology, 45(4), disrupts amygdala activity to affective stimuli. Psy-
511–515. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00659.x chological Science, 18(5), 421–428. doi:10.1111/
PMID:18282197 j.1467-9280.2007.01916.x PMID:17576282
Kunda, Z. (1987). Motivated inference: Self-serv- Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cog-
ing generation and evaluation of causal theories. nitive buffer against stress-related illness and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, depression. Journal of Personality and Social
53(4), 636–647. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.636 Psychology, 52(4), 663–676. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.52.4.663 PMID:3572732
Kwan, V. S., Barrios, V., Ganis, G., Gorman, J.,
Lange, C., & Kumar, M. et al. (2007). Assessing the Luck, S. J., & Kappenman, E. S. (2012). ERP
neural correlates of self-enhancement bias: A tran- components and selective attention. In S. J. Luck
scranial magnetic stimulation study. Experimental & E. S. Kappenmann (Eds.), The Oxford Hand-
Brain Research, 182(3), 379–385. doi:10.1007/ book of Event-Related Potential Components (pp.
s00221-007-0992-2 PMID:17607566 295–328). New York: Oxford University Press.
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The Major, B., Spencer, S., Schmader, T., Wolfe,
nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer the- C., & Crocker, J. (1998). Coping with negative
ory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, stereotypes about intellectual performance: The
32, 1–62. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80003-9 role of psychological disengagement. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(1), 34–50.
Leitner, J. B., Hehman, E., Jones, J. M., & Forbes,
doi:10.1177/0146167298241003
C. E. (in press). Self-enhancement influences
medial frontal cortex alpha power to social rejec- Marien, H., Custers, R., Hassin, R. R., & Aarts,
tion feedback. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. H. (2012). Unconscious goal activation and the
PMID:24738770 hijacking of the executive function. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 103(3), 399–415.
Leitner, J. B., Jones, J. M., & Hehman, E.
doi:10.1037/a0028955 PMID:22686538
(2013). Succeeding in the face of threat:
The adaptive role of engaegment regulation. Markus, H., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, malleability of the self-concept. Journal of Per-
39(1), 17–27. doi:10.1177/0146167212463083 sonality and Social Psychology, 51(4), 858–866.
PMID:23125031 doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.4.858 PMID:3783430

198

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., Papies, E. K., Stroebe, W., & Aarts, H. (2008). The
Pfeifer, J. H., McNealy, K., Mazziotta, J. C., & allure of forbidden food: On the role of attention
Dapretto, M. (2009). Neural. correlates of social in self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social
exclusion during adolescence: Understanding the Psychology, 44(5), 1283–1292. doi:10.1016/j.
distress of peer rejection Social. Cognitive, and Af- jesp.2008.04.008
fective Neuroscience, 4(2), 143–157. doi:10.1093/
Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C., & Schultz, R. T.
scan/nsp007 PMID:19470528
(2004). Subcortical discrimination of unperceived
Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., & objects during binocular rivalry. Neuron, 42(1),
Hankin, B. L. (2004). Is there a universal positiv- 163–172. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00155-2
ity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of PMID:15066273
individual, developmental, and cultural differences
Pedregon, C. A., Farley, R. L., Davis, A., Wood,
in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychologi-
J. M., & Clark, R. D. (2012). Social desirabil-
cal Bulletin, 130(5), 711–747. doi:10.1037/0033-
ity, personality questionnaires, and the “better
2909.130.5.711 PMID:15367078
than average” effect. Personality and Individual
Miller, M. L., Omens, R. S., & Delvadia, R. (1991). Differences, 52(2), 213–217. doi:10.1016/j.
Dimensions of social competence: Personality and paid.2011.10.022
coping style correlates. Personality and Individual
Peetz, J., Wilson, A. E., & Strahan, E. J. (2009).
Differences, 12(9), 955–964. doi:10.1016/0191-
So far away: The role of subjective temporal dis-
8869(91)90185-E
tance to future goals in motivation and behavior.
Moran, J. M., Macrae, C. N., Heatherton, T. F., Social Cognition, 27(4), 475–495. doi:10.1521/
Wyland, C. L., & Kelley, W. M. (2006). Neuro- soco.2009.27.4.475
anatomical evidence for distinct cognitive and
Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Jones, J.
affective components of self. Journal of Cognitive
T. (2002). Why Susie sells seashells by the sea-
Neuroscience, 18(9), 1586–1594. doi:10.1162/
shore: Implicit egotism and major life decisions.
jocn.2006.18.9.1586 PMID:16989558
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self- 82(4), 469–487. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.469
regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does PMID:11999918
self- control resemble a muscle? Psychological
Pemberton, M., & Sedikides, C. (2001). When
Bulletin, 126(2), 247–259. doi:10.1037/0033-
do individuals help close others improve? The
2909.126.2.247 PMID:10748642
role of information diagnosticity. Journal of
Nuttin, J. M. (1985). Narcissism beyond Gestalt Personality and Social Psychology, 81(2),
and awareness: The name letter effect. European 234–246. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.234
Journal of Social Psychology, 15(3), 353–361. PMID:11519929
doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420150309
Porges, S. W. (2007). The Polyvagal perspec-
Oskam, J., Kingma, J., & Klasen, H. J. (2000). tive. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116–
Clinicians’ recognition of 10 different types of 143. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009
distal radial fractures. Perceptual and Motor Skills, PMID:17049418
91(3), 917–924. doi:10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.917
PMID:11153870

199

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Puri, M., & Robinson, D. T. (2007). Optimism and Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008).
economic choice. Journal of Financial Economics, An integrated process model of stereotype
86(1), 71–99. doi:10.1016/j.jfineco.2006.09.003 threat effects on performance. Psychological
Review, 115(2), 336–356. doi:10.1037/0033-
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A.
295X.115.2.336 PMID:18426293
Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman,
G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Sedlovskaya, A., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Eibach, R.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences P., LaFrance, M., Romero-Canyas, R., & Camp, N.
of the United States of America, 98(2), 676–682. P. (2013). Internalizing the closet: Concealment
doi:10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 PMID:11209064 heightens the cognitive distinction between public
and private selves. Academic Press.
Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). It feels like
yesterday: Self-esteem, valence of personal past Seeley, E. A., & Gardner, W. L. (2003). The
experiences, and judgments of subjective distance. “selfless” and self-regulation: The role of
Personality Processes and Individual Differences, chronic other orientation in averting self-regula-
82, 792–803. PMID:12003478 tory depletion. Self and Identity, 2(2), 103–117.
doi:10.1080/15298860309034
Rutman, A. M., Clapp, W. C., Chadick, J. Z.,
& Gazzaley, A. (2009). Early top-down control Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat
of visual processing predicts working memory and performance: How self-stereotypes influence
performance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, processing by inducing regulatory foci. Journal of
22(6), 1224–1234. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21257 Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1), 38–56.
PMID:19413473 doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.38 PMID:15250791
Sanitioso, R., Kunda, Z., & Fong, G. T. (1990). Sharot, T., Korn, C. W., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). How
Motivated recruitment of autobiographical memo- unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of
ries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, reality. Nature Neuroscience, 14(11), 1475–1479.
59(2), 229–241. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.229 doi:10.1038/nn.2949 PMID:21983684
PMID:2213492
Sheeran, P., Aarts, H., Custers, R., Rivis, A., Webb,
Sanitioso, R. B., & Wlodarski, R. (2004). In T. L., & Cooke, R. (2005). The goal-dependent
search of information that confirms a desired automaticity of drinking habits. The British
self perception: Motivated processing of social Journal of Social Psychology, 44(1), 47–63.
feedback and choice of social interactions. Per- doi:10.1348/014466604X23446 PMID:15901391
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(4),
Somerville, L. H., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley,
412–422. doi:10.1177/0146167203261882
W. M. (2006). Anterior cingulate cortex responds
PMID:15070471
differentially to expectancy violation and social
Schmader, T., Forbes, C. E., Zhang, S., & rejection. Nature Neuroscience, 9(8), 1007–1008.
Mendes, W. B. (2009). A metacognitive per- doi:10.1038/nn1728 PMID:16819523
spective on the cognitive deficits experienced
Somerville, L. H., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T.
in intellectually threatening environments. Per-
F. (2010). Self-esteem modulates medial prefrontal
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(5),
cortical responses to evaluative social feedback.
584–596. doi:10.1177/0146167208330450
Cerebral Cortex, 20(12), 3005–3013. doi:10.1093/
PMID:19228598
cercor/bhq049 PMID:20351022

200

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Spreng, R. N., & Grady, C. L. (2010). Patterns Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and
of brain activity supporting autobiographical well-being: A social psychological perspective
memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2),
relationship to the default mode network. Journal 193–210. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1112–1123. PMID:3283814
doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21282 PMID:19580387
Taylor, S. E., Burklund, L. J., Eisenberger, N. I.,
Steele, C. M. (1988). In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), The Lehman, B. J., Hilmert, C. J., & Lieberman, M.
psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the D. (2008). Neural bases of moderation of corti-
integrity of the self (Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). Ad- sol stress responses by psychosocial resources.
vances in Experimental Social Psychology New Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
York: Academic Press. 95(1), 197–211. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.197
PMID:18605860
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How
stereotypes shape intellectual identity and per- Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K.,
formance. The American Psychologist, 52(6), Sage, R. M., & McDowell, N. K. (2003). Are
613–629. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613 self-enhancing cognitions associated with
PMID:9174398 healthy or unhealthy biological profiles? Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(4),
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype
605–615. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.605
threat and the intellectual test performance of Afri-
PMID:14561115
can Americans. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 69(5), 797–811. doi:10.1037/0022- Tesser, A. (1988). In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Toward
3514.69.5.797 PMID:7473032 a self-evaluation maintenance model of social be-
havior (Vol. 21, pp. 181–227). Advances in Experi-
Stone, J. (2002). Battling doubt by avoiding
mental Social Psychology New York: Academic
practice: The effects of stereotype threat on self-
Press. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60227-0
handicapping in white athletes. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(12), 1667–1678. Tesser, A., Campbell, J., & Smith, M. (1984).
doi:10.1177/014616702237648 Friendship choice and performance: Self- evalu-
ation maintenance in children. Journal of Per-
Svenson, O. (1981). Are we all less risky and
sonality and Social Psychology, 46(3), 561–574.
more skillful than our fellow drivers? Acta Psy-
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.561
chologica, 47, 143-148.
Tesser, A., Crepaz, N., Beach, S. R. H., Cor-
Swann, W. B. Jr, & Bosson, J. (2010). Self and
nell, D., & Collins, J. C. (2000). Confluence
identity. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lin-
of self-esteem regulation mechanisms: On
dzey (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (5th
integrating the self-zoo. Personality and So-
ed., pp. 589–628). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
cial Psychology Bulletin, 26(12), 1476–1489.
doi:10.1177/01461672002612003

201

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Tesser, A., & Paulhus, D. (1983). The definition Baumeister, R. F., Hamilton, J. C., & Tice, D.
of self: Private and public self-evaluation mainte- M. (1985). Public versus private expectancy
nance strategies. Journal of Personality and Social of success: Confidence booster or performance
Psychology, 44(4), 672–682. doi:10.1037/0022- pressure? Journal of Personality and Social
3514.44.4.672 Psychology, 48(6), 1447–1457. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.48.6.1447
von Hippel, W., von Hippel, C., Conway, L.,
Preacher, K. J., Schooler, J. W., & Radvansky, G. Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996).
A. (2005). Coping with stereotype threat: Denial Self-regulation failure: An overview. Psychologi-
as an impression management strategy. Journal of cal Inquiry, 7(1), 1–15. doi:10.1207/s15327965p-
Personality and Social Psychology, 89(1), 22–35. li0701_1
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.22 PMID:16060740
Crocker, J., & Luhtanen, R. K. (2003). Level
Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism of self-esteem and contingencies of self-worth:
about future life events. Journal of Personal- Unique effects on academic, social and finan-
ity and Social Psychology, 39(5), 806–820. cial problems in college students. Personal-
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806 ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(6),
701–712. doi:10.1177/0146167203029006003
Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward social comparison
PMID:15189626
principles in social psychology. Psychological
Bulletin, 90(2), 245–271. doi:10.1037/0033- Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma
2909.90.2.245 and self-esteem: The self-protective properties of
stigma. Psychological Review, 96(4), 608–630.
Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.608
to champ: People’s appraisals of their earlier and
current selves. Journal of Personality and Social Crocker, J., Sommers, S. R., & Luhtanen,
Psychology, 80(4), 572–584. doi:10.1037/0022- R. K. (2002). Hopes dashed and dream ful-
3514.80.4.572 PMID:11316222 filled: Contingencies of self-worth and gradu-
ate school admissions. Personality and So-
cial Psychology Bulletin, 28(9), 1275–1286.
doi:10.1177/01461672022812012
ADDITIONAL READING
Crocker, J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingen-
Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D. A. (1994). The cies of self-worth. Psychological Review, 108(3),
self in social contexts. Annual Review of Psy- 593–623. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.593
chology, 45(1), 297–332. doi:10.1146/annurev. PMID:11488379
ps.45.020194.001501
Fivush, R. (2011). The development of auto-
Baumeister, R. (1997). Esteem threat, self-regula- biographical memory. Annual Review of Psy-
tory breakdown, and emotional distress as factors chology, 62(1), 559–582. doi:10.1146/annurev.
in self- defeating. Review of General Psychology, psych.121208.131702 PMID:20636128
1(2), 145–174. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.1.2.145
Gaertner, and Toguchi (2003). Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 89, 531-538.

202

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual dif- Kwan, V. S. Y., John, O. P., Kenny, D. A., Bond,
ferences in two emotion regulation processes: Im- M. H., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Reconceptualizing
plications for affect, relationships, and well-being. individual differences in self-enhancement bias:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, An interpersonal approach. Psychological Review,
85(2), 348–362. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 111(1), 94–110. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.94
PMID:12916575 PMID:14756588
Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self Leary, M. R., Terry, M. L., Allen, A. B., &
and self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychol- Tate, E. B. (2009). The concept of ego threat
ogy, 62(1), 363–390. doi:10.1146/annurev. in social and personality psychology: Is ego
psych.121208.131616 PMID:21126181 threat a viable scientific construct? Person-
ality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3),
Heine, S. J. (2005). Where is the evidence for pan
151–164. doi:10.1177/1088868309342595
cultural self-enhancement? A reply to Sedikides,
PMID:19648508
Heine, S. J., & Hamamura, T. (2007). In
Marsh, H. W. (1986). Global self-esteem: Its re-
search of East Asian self-enhancement. Per-
lation to specific facets of self-concept and their
sonality and Social Psychology Review,
importance. Journal of Personality and Social
11(1), 1–24. doi:10.1177/1088868306294587
Psychology, 51(6), 1224–1236. doi:10.1037/0022-
PMID:18453453
3514.51.6.1224
Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata,
Murphy, M. C., Steele, C., & Gross, J. (2007). Sig-
T., Ide, E., Leung, C., & Matsumoto, H. (2001).
naling threat: How situational cues affect women
Divergent consequences of success and failure
in math, science and engineering settings. Psycho-
in Japan and North America: An investigation of
logical Science, 18(10), 879–885. doi:10.1111/
self-improving motivations and malleable selves.
j.1467-9280.2007.01995.x PMID:17894605
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
81(4), 599–615. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.599 Ng, F. F., Pomerantz, E. M., & Lamn, S. (2007).
PMID:11642348 European American and Chinese parents’ re-
sponses to children’s success and failure: Impli-
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H., & Kita-
cations for children’s responses. Developmental
yama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for posi-
Psychology, 43(5), 1239–1255. doi:10.1037/0012-
tive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106(4),
1649.43.5.1239 PMID:17723048
766–794. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.766
PMID:10560328 Nussbaum, A. D., & Steele, C. M. (2007). Situ-
ational disengagement and persistence in the face
Higgins, E. T., Shah, J., & Friedman, R. (1997).
of adversity. Journal of Experimental Social
Emotional responses to goal attainment: Strength
Psychology, 43(1), 127–134. doi:10.1016/j.
of regulatory focus as moderator. Journal of Per-
jesp.2005.12.007
sonality and Social Psychology, 72(3), 51–525.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.3.515 PMID:9120782

203

The Role of Implicit Mechanisms in Buffering Self-Esteem

Paulhus, D. L., Graf, P., & Van Selst, M. (1989). Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-
Attentional load increases the positivity of self- regulatory failure: A resource-depletion ap-
presentation. Social Cognition, 7(4), 389–400. proach. Psychological Science, 11(3), 249–254.
doi:10.1521/soco.1989.7.4.389 doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00250 PMID:11273412
Paulhus, D. L., & Levitt, K. (1987). Desirable re- Wood, A. M., Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Baliousis,
sponding triggered by affect: Automatic egotism? M., & Joseph, S. (2008). The authentic personality:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, A theoretical and empirical conceptualization and
52(2), 245–259. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.2.245 the development of the Authenticity Scale. Jour-
nal of Counseling Psychology, 55(3), 385–399.
Pelham, B. W., & Swann, W. B. (1989). From
doi:10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.385
self-conceptions to self-worth: On the sources and
structure of global self-esteem. Journal of Per- Zeigler-Hill, V. (2007). Contingent self-esteem
sonality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 672–680. and race: Implications for the black self-esteem
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.672 PMID:2795437 advantage. The Journal of Black Psychology,
33(1), 51–74. doi:10.1177/0095798406295096
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003).
Pancultural self-enhancement. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 60–79.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.60 PMID:12518971 KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Vevea, J. L. Explicit Self-Enhancement Mechanism: A
(2005). Pancultural self-enhancement reloaded: conscious, goal-directed process that produces
A meta-analytic reply to Heine (2005). Journal self-enhancement outcomes.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(4), Explicit Self-Enhancement Outcome: A
539–551. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.539 conscious perception in which a person views
PMID:16287417 their autobiographical history, present abilities,
Steele, C. M. (2010). Whistling Vivaldi and other or future outcomes as overly positive.
clues to how stereotypes affect us. New York, NY: Explicit Self-Esteem: The global and con-
W.W. Norton & Company. scious attitude a person holds of the self.
Implicit Coping: An unconscious goal-
Taylor, S. E. (2010). Health. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, directed process that mitigates social stress.
& G. Lindzey (Eds.), The Handbook of Social Implicit Self-Enhancement Mechanism: An
Psychology (5th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. unconscious, goal-directed process that produces
Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., self-enhancement outcomes.
Sage, R. M., & McDowell, N. K. (2003). Por- Implicit Self-Esteem: The automatic and
trait of the self-enhancer: Well adjusted and well unconscious associations that a person holds of
liked or maladjusted and friendless? Journal the self.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), Stereotype Threat: The stress response as-
165–176. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.165 sociated with the fear of confirming a negative
PMID:12518977 group stereotype.

204
205

Chapter 10
How Social Factors Influence
Implicit Knowledge
Construction on the Internet
Albena Antonova
Sofia University, Bulgaria

ABSTRACT
The influence of Internet on knowledge acquisition increases as it became the primary source of refer-
ence, especially for young generations. Therefore, it is important to understand how social factors and
Internet influences not only explicit but also implicit knowledge construction. Several theories explain
the impact of the social groups on the knowledge formation as Social Cognitive Theory of Bandura and
Lay Epistemic Theory of Kruglanski. Thus, the authors propose an analysis of the implicit knowledge
construction processes through the theoretical overview of Internet communication. More specifically,
they discuss how the mechanisms of Internet increase the social and group impact on the implicit knowl-
edge processes.

INTRODUCTION access to academic sources and online libraries,


large availability to platforms of researchers and
Information technologies and Internet changed universities, and even specific “knowledge mar-
substantially the attitude toward knowledge and ketplaces” (as for example: Innocentive.com),
knowing as complex psychological and social bring scientific knowledge closer to the wider
processes. During the last decades, Internet and audiences. This creates an attitude of expectation
information technologies profoundly transformed that knowledge is automatically and largely avail-
the way people conceptualize what is knowledge able “on demand”. While these expectations are
and how to perceive its value. A lot of informa- evident for explicit knowledge, we still don’t know
tion and data can be freely found on Internet how Internet influences implicit and unconscious
and the search engines display a large number knowledge construction. In literature, scholars de-
of answers on any specific question. Improved fine explicit knowledge as a fact, rationally proved

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch010

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

and displayed in documents and references. On processes. Referring to the SECI model of Nonaka
the other hand the implicit knowledge is linked et Takeuchi (1994), Chua (2002) emphasizes that
to unconscious thinking, to rumors, impressions, three of the four phases of knowledge construc-
attitudes and emotions. tion consist of social mechanisms: socialization,
As technology barriers have been slowly externalization and combination. This clearly
removed, Internet nowadays provides many new shows that the process of knowledge conversion
forms of synchronous and asynchronous com- involves different social interactions. But how
munication that are publicly available and free. these processes are influenced by Internet?
With exploding social technologies and web 2.0 Our specific interest is focused on Internet as
instruments, everybody can easily express and this is universal platform, facilitating and enabling
create content on Internet, that can reach millions social interactions. Thus Internet in the context
of people for few seconds. In that way, Internet of this research will be more widely understood
differs substantially from other media as television, as a global infrastructure and platform for social
radio and print, allowing two-way communication, networking, including web 2.0 facilities and social
networking and debate. Thus not only everybody technologies such as blogs, wikis, forums, social
can become a content provider and express his network sites, bookmarks, social sharing of mul-
or her views, but in the same time he or she has timedia, social references, tagging and others. We
access to global communities. Therefore, in these will not focus specifically on the multiple func-
new technology realms, it becomes increasingly tionalities of Internet as cloud computing service
easy to influence thinking of larger social groups. provider (as SaaS, PaaS, IaaS), neither its capacity
Even, it is possible “fake” profiles to influence for groupware, voice over IP, internet television
substantial social debates and to have impact and radio as well as many others. The interest of
on considerable social processes of knowledge the topic is motivated as well by the anticipated
construction. new technology wave of “Internet of everything”.
The research problem that we will investigate in In this context, researchers expect fast adoption of
this chapter is to specify the mechanisms through new generations of Internet technologies, consist-
which Internet can influence individual implicit ing of more devices, services and interconnected
knowledge construction. As Internet become systems including people and sophisticated ubiqui-
more popular as even before, it is important to tous technologies as Internet of things, intelligent
identify the mechanisms of these technologies to robots and artificial intelligence systems. Such a
affect implicit knowledge. What is the impact of step will change even further people’s communica-
social groups and how they influence the implicit tion and collaboration habits, largely influencing
knowledge construction? More specifically, we the knowledge construction processes. Thus the
will focus on investigating how social factors present analysis will open new perspectives for
influence the process of knowledge justification discussing how new technologies influence the
that is one of the most substantial factors for implicit knowledge construction.
knowledge creation.
It is admitted that the process of knowledge cre- Theoretical Background
ation is essentially social and symbolic in nature.
The social cognition includes shared language, Knowledge is traditionally defined as “justified
code and narratives, shared representations, inter- true belief”1. Although in traditional epistemology,
pretations and systems of meaning among parties. philosophers identified an exclusion (the Gettier
Moreover, as stated in Chua (2002), the process of case), some recent studies (Starmans& Friedman,
knowledge creation is largely influenced by social 2012) prove that even in Gettier situation, people

206

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

attribute knowledge based on their understanding improve largely the individual capacity for knowl-
of justified true belief. Knowledge as “justified true edge justification, enhancing the personal ability
belief” is composed of three main prepositions, for perception (wider access and precise analysis
including the content -the fact, the evidence, (the of data and information from the external envi-
statement which is true), the agent (the self) who ronment), induction (development of scenarios,
is confident (believe in the statement), and finally better analysis of cause-effect structures), memory
the methods for justification (that the agent can (improved access for information storage and
apply, to assure the relevance of its confidence). retrieval) and reason (application of logical rules
According to this definition, knowledge is a func- on different situation). However, in the same time,
tion of an agent, who believes (it is not necessary Internet increases the impact of social factors on
to prove it, or to explain the reasons for it) that knowledge justification methods, improving the
a fact is true, relying on correct methods for jus- access to social groups, social group knowledge
tification. Dienes & Perner (1999) reformulate and social opinions (testimony).
the proposed definition of knowledge, stating In his knowledge conversion theory, Nonaka
that knowledge is an attitude (belief) towards a (1994) emphasized that knowledge is not static but
proposition (statement) which is true. They em- dynamic, largely influenced by the social process
phasized the importance of the attitude formed of justification of individual beliefs (Nonaka,
by the agent (or the self), and thus investigate 1994). His main interest is how to improve the
knowledge as predominant psychology construct. process of knowledge justification and the attitude
For Kruglanski et al., (2010), the source of all (belief, acceptance and conviction) transforming
knowledge is evidence. Cook and Brown (1999) knowledge from individual to group (and organiza-
distinguish between knowledge as something that tional) knowledge. The focus of our research will
people possess (epistemology of possession), ver- be put it in inverse or to specify how knowledge
sus knowing as action (epistemology of practice). justification methods come from the group to the
They state that knowledge is a tool of knowing. individual knowledge formation processes.
Further Cook and Brown (1999) summarize that
in epistemology literature it prevails the focus of Implicit and Explicit Knowledge
individual versus group knowledge and of explicit
versus implicit knowledge. While discussing the terms explicit and implicit
Considering knowledge as the process of in practice, “explicit” reveals something that is
knowing, we will focus more specifically on the evident, directly addressed, displayed, verbalized;
justification processes, explaining how social while “implicit” is something that is pre-supposed,
factors influence knowledge justification. As not directly mentioned, not verbalized (and
shown in the work of Ichikawa (2013), knowledge un-conscious). Explicit knowledge is acquired
justification methods are based on 6 main reli- through attentive and intentional monitoring of
able sources of reason. In details, the knowledge external and internal events and can be used for
justification methods include: (1) perception deliberate control of behavior. Implicit knowledge
(analysis of external information), (2) introspec- on opposite, is acquired unintentionally, with
tion (analysis of internal factors), (3) induction subjects being not or little aware of its presence.
(cause-effect structures), (4) memory (references In pedagogy research, implicit knowledge is be-
and access to facts about past events), (5) reason coming important factor for reaching excellence,
(logical rules), and (6) testimony (social factors) especially when mastery is involved in practicing,
(Ichikawa et al., 2013). Thus in the knowledge as for example speaking fluently foreign language,
justification context, information technologies playing music and others. Reaching implicit

207

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

knowledge in this context means mastery of un- Social Factors for Knowledge
conscious application of cognitive processes. In the Construction
psychology literature, Schacter (1992) discovers
that the terms “explicit” and “implicit” knowledge Many researchers argue that knowledge is social
are used as synonymous and equivalent in mean- and not individual phenomena. Different theories
ing to “conscious” and “unconscious” knowledge. confirm the important role of the social groups
He finds out that the two sets of terms can be and social factors on the process of knowledge
used interchangeably (Schacter, 1992). Based formation. For example, knowledge, theories,
on his research he concludes that dissociations and understanding embodied in the meaning of
between implicit and explicit knowledge seem to categories, schemata and cognitive structures are
arise as a natural consequence of the functional collectively produced and validated. Categories
architecture of the brain and reflects the activ- are not only cognitive structures but they are also
ity of computation that are routinely performed implicit social norms. The shared social identity
during the course of perceiving, recognizing, and of group members make it possible for them to
remembering (Schacter,1992). The “conscious produce socially validated knowledge, shared be-
awareness” or “conscious” knowledge is, indeed, liefs about way of perceiving thinking and doing
prototypically explicit (Dienes and Perner, 1999). that we assume to be appropriate in terms of the
Analyzing implicit knowledge from psychological demands of objective reality. Reality is always
perspective Dienes and Perner (1999) figure out interpreted from the perspective of a socially
that knowledge can be implicit if the content, the defined perceiver and thus all cognition is social
attitude of knowing and the holder of that attitude cognition (Stets & Burke, 2000). Further, it was
(the self) can be represented implicitly. Further discovered, that physical reality testing (direct
they identify two main sources of implicitness: individual perceptual, cognitive and behavioral
(1) the presupposition (extending implicitly the testing) and social reality testing (consensual vali-
meaning of a fact and adding new information) dation seeking the agreement of in-group others)
and (2) attributing characteristics of the fact to are not alternatives, but interdependent aspects of
the instance or part of a category (implicitly achieving valid social cognition.
transferring the characteristics of a category to A social group is a set of individuals who hold
specific instance). These two main characteristics a common social identification or view them-
of implicit knowledge will be further discussed selves as members of the same social category
in our analysis. (Stets & Burke, 2000). There are imposed two
Moreover, Dienes and Perner (1999) have group-forming mechanisms. Through a social
identified four main components of a known fact comparison process, persons who are similar to the
about which they can ask whether they need to self are categorized with the self and are labeled
be represented explicitly or can be left implicit: the in-group, while persons who differ from the
self are categorized as out-group. More interest-
1. Properties, e.g.: ‘F’, ‘being a cat’. ing is the process of social comparison that is a
2. Individuals, e.g.: ‘b’, ‘particular individual selective application of the accentuation effect.
in front of me’. In order to enhance self-esteem, through social
3. The predication of the property to the indi- comparison, in-group dimensions are evaluated
vidual, e.g.: ‘Fb’, ‘this is a cat’. positively and the out-group dimensions are ranked
4. Temporal context and factuality (vs. fiction), negatively. Thus through the process of social
e.g.: ‘It is a fact of this world that at time t, comparison, social groups influence the attitude
Fb’, ‘It is a fact that this is currently a cat’. of the individual. These findings correspond to

208

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

the statement of Mussweiler (2003), who find out to the social context. The perceiver gains identity
that human judgment is comparative in nature. from being placed in context – social contextual
When people evaluate a given target, they don’t definitions of the perceiver. Stability in self-cate-
do so in a vacuum. Rather, such evaluations are gorization is likely to arise from stability of social
made within and in relation to a specific context. reality that provides contexts for self-definition,
In fact, any evaluation is relative in that it refers high-order knowledge frameworks; social groups,
to a comparison of the evaluated target with a subcultures, social institutions that provide per-
pertinent norm or standard. As each person is a ceivers with stable norms, values and motives,
member of a unique combination of social catego- social influence and communication processes
ries, the set of social identities make the person that translate particular conceptions of self and
self-concept unique. others into social norms and validate the broader
elaborative ideologies used in theory construction.
In-Group and Out-Group Knowledge Further it is interesting to note that the same
Formation Mechanisms information may be accepted or rejected by the
same kind of people in different social contexts
As discovered by Turner et al, (1994), social iden- depending on how they categorize self and others
tity refers to the social categorization of the self at the time. It is demonstrated that the same infor-
and the others, and self-categories that define the mation is accepted when it comes from in-group,
individual in terms of his or her shared similari- and rejected when it comes from an out-group. The
ties with members of certain social categories in group norm, which specifies the correct judgment,
contrast to other social categories. Social identity will vary moving toward either risk or caution. The
refers to social categorical self (us-vs-them); study of influences of the minorities on the change
moreover, the theory reveals that as shared social of personal categories, show interdependence if
identity become salient, individual self-perception this is in-group or out-group. The theories and
tends to become depersonalized. knowledge that one will draw on to make sense of
The standards or the pertinent norms for social a situation vary with self-categorization and are
comparisons largely depend from unique combi- context dependent. It is a well researched fact that
nation of social categories and the in-group and the validity of knowledge is a function of one’s
out-group dimensions. Moreover, depending on subjectively defined own position.
social factors and context it is formed the judg-
ment. As Mussweiler (2003) reveals, most often a Social Interactions and
test for similarities produces assimilation whereas Knowledge Creation
test for dissimilarity produces contrast. Thus the
in-group dimensions and social categories will lead Investigating the model of knowledge creation,
to positive results (similarity, positive judgments), Chua (2002) highlights that knowledge creation
while out-group dimensions will lead to negative is largely influenced by social processes. Even in
results (contrast judgments). Consequently, any the knowledge conversion model of SECI (Nonaka
factor that influences the nature of the initial hy- & Takeuchi, 1995) three of the four phases consist
pothesis is likely to have an effect on comparison of socialization: socialization, externalization
consequences (Mussweiler 2003). and combination. The social interactions among
Moreover, self-categories are social definitions organizational members have three important
of the individual. They represent the perceiver in dimensions – structural, relational and cognitive
social terms, in terms of social relationships of dimensions. Thus structural dimension concerns
similarities and differences to others in relation the properties of the social systems and refer to

209

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

impersonal configuration of linkages between experience), vicarious verification (observing


people or units. Social networks facilitate and others people transactions and effects), social
speed up the knowledge creation, while “who” verification (people evaluating the soundness of
you know affects “what” you know. More im- their views by checking them against what oth-
portantly, the network configuration properties ers believe) and logical verification (deducing
as density and connectivity affect the flexibil- from knowledge that is known) (Bandura, 2001).
ity and ease of knowledge exchange. Relational Observational learning or models of vicarious
dimension describes the personal relationships and social verifications enable people to acquire
between individuals. It depends from the levels implicitly (unintentionally) new knowledge with-
of care, the norms of cooperation and the sense of out having direct experience or applying direct
identification to a group. The “care” summarizes cognitive rules. Bandura claims that observational
trust, active empathy, access to help and leniency learning allows media to transmit new ways of
in judgment. Trust has cognitive and affective thinking and behavior simultaneously to count-
aspects. The norms are degree of consensus in less people in widely dispersed locales. During
the social system and can influence the creation the course of their daily lives, people have direct
of knowledge. It was identified strong correlation contact with only a small sector of the physical
between the level of social interaction and the and social environment. Thus, their conceptions
quality of knowledge created. In particular, the of social reality are greatly influenced by vicari-
relational dimension of the social interaction is ous experiences – what they see, hear and read,
stronger predictor of the quality of the knowledge, without direct experiential correctives. To a large
compared to structural or cognitive dimension extent, people act on their images of reality. The
(Chua 2002). more people’s images of reality depend on the
media’s symbolic environment, the greater is its
Knowledge Justification Methods social impact. Observational learning influences
adoption of rules that can be used to judge the
Knowledge justification is the process of verifica- reality or to generate new instances of behavior.
tion of evidences, coming from different sources. Acquisition of generative rules from modeled
In this section we will explore how social fac- information involves three main processes –
tors influence individual knowledge formation extracting generic features from various social
mechanisms. exemplars, integrate the extracted information
to composite rules, and finally use the rules to
Social Cognitive Theory of Bandura produce new instances of behaviors. Through
abstract modeling, people acquire standards for
According to the Social Cognitive Theory (Ban- categorizing and judging events, linguistic rules
dura, 2001) the social mechanisms of cognition of communication, thinking skills and personal
and the social origins of thought are the mecha- standards for regulating motivation and conduct.
nisms through which social factors exert their Further Bandura (2001) states that much of
influence on cognitive functioning. Effective the social construction of the reality in shaping
cognitive functioning requires reliable ways of dis- of public consciousness occurs through electronic
tinguishing between accurate and faulty thinking. acculturation. More importantly, electronic media
Accurate thinking needs application of validation transform how social systems operate and serve as
mechanisms. There are explored four different a major vehicle for sociopolitical changes. Com-
though verification and validation models: enac- munication systems and media operate through
tive verification (fit between thoughts and direct two pathways – direct pathway and socially medi-

210

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

ated pathway. In direct pathway, communication conscious, and explicit, as it can form judgments
media promote changes by informing, enabling, considered as associative, unconscious, or ‘mecha-
motivating and guiding participants, while in the nistic.’ The unimodel focuses on the evidences as
socially mediated pathway, media influences are the basis of all knowledge. The unimodel ‘zooms
used to link participants to social networks, which in’ on the informational aspect of the epistemic
increase the influence of the lead individuals. This process, investigating conditions under which
way media influences the lead viewers to discuss given information would affect individuals’ judg-
or negotiate matters of importance with others, ments. The unimodel comprises both cognitive
allowing tailored communication, that is more (informational availability and accessibility, the
relevant, credible, better remembered and more presence of cognitive resources) and motivational
effective in influencing behavior than general (e.g. need for closure) factors affecting informa-
messages. Thus social groups can be regarded as tional impact. Based on their research, Kruglanski
reinforcing and personalizing the social influence et al. (2010) affirm that growing evidences sup-
on individuals. Bandura (2001) further states that port that the subjective relevance of information
identifying the verification of thought by com- determines its impact on judgments. The subjective
parison with distorted media versions of social relevance depends on the relation between task
reality can foster shared misconceptions of people, demands of knowing and the necessary cognitive
places and things. Even more, social verification and motivational resources.
can foster bizarre views of reality if the shared
beliefs of the reference group with which one af- The Need for Cognitive Closure
filiate are peculiar and the group is encapsulated
from outside social ties and influences. The need for cognitive closure addresses the mo-
tivation for knowledge construction. People are
The Knowledge Formation and usually capable of forming judgments quickly on
the Lay Epistemic Theory the basis of the available evidence. The magnitude
of an individual’s need for closure is determined
Kruglanski et al. (2010) formulates the Lay epis- by the perceived benefits of closure and the costs
temic theory (LET) in order to explain the general of lacking closure. For instance, different mecha-
knowledge formation processes. According to the nisms influence the need for cognitive closure.
LET process of knowledge formation the cycle The attitude that tend to delay the cognitive clo-
of all knowledge construction (including different sure include: accuracy concerns, accountability
knowledge domains, scientific and general knowl- concerns, need for cognition, while the factors for
edge), comprises three general and interconnected accelerating it can be for example authoritarian-
stages: unimodel of social judgment, cognitive ism or dogmatism, time or resource constraints,
closure and epistemic authority. lack of cognitive capacity and others. Thus social
factors can largely influence the individual need
Unimodel of Social Judgment for cognitive closure, increasing the impact of
external environment to the cognitive process.
The LET theory assumes that all knowledge is As the need for cognitive closure represents
derived from evidence. In other words, to construct a desire for firm knowledge, this requires firm
new knowledge, or to form a new judgment, the evidential basis. Therefore, individuals high on
individual is assumed to use some inference rule the need for closure should be more likely to form
of an “IF-THEN” type. It should be noted that it is rules, including general rules that they can use
not necessary these inferences to be deliberative, across a broad spectrum of situations. For example,

211

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

high (versus low) need for closure individuals re- cesses of LET theory for constructing knowledge
port higher confidence in their judgments, possibly – unimodel judgment of evidences, cognitive
reflecting a high self-ascribed epistemic authority, closure and epistemic authority. In order to justify
or possession of a general ‘I am right’ heuristic. the evidences, individual applies different valida-
Moreover, individuals under high need for closure tion mechanisms. Thus in Internet people usually
exhibit group-centric tendencies (Kruglanski et al., explore “on demand” knowledge constructs, re-
2010), which includes the striving for consensus, sponding on “how to…” and “why…” questions.
or subscription to the ‘group is right’ heuristic. In order to consciously judge the evidences, or
Individuals under high need for closure prefer an apply explicit knowledge processing mechanisms
autocratic, or hierarchical group structure granting as induction or introspection mechanisms, indi-
epistemic authority to few leaders, suggesting a vidual has to activate the “if-then” rules. Thus,
use of the ‘experts are correct’ heuristic. first the rule has to exist and then to be recalled
in memory. Depending on the previous history
Epistemic Authority of the rule activation, the recalling of the rule
can be hard or easy. When the difficulty or the
A special category of evidence concerns other complexity of the evidence increases, it requires
people’s opinions, especially if those people con- more cognitive and motivational resources for
stitute ‘epistemic authorities’ for the individual. processing. Thus, when the evidences are complex,
These people can be domain experts, people with or need additional processing it can be harder for
specific role or just social group which opinion individuals to access resources and mechanisms
can influence the cognitive process and the need for verification and validation. Therefore it can
for cognitive closure. Generally epistemic author- become easier to accept the opinion of epistemic
ity represents the social source of information in authority, or socially available knowledge, ac-
the knowledge formation process. Individuals cepted by wider social groups. According to the
attribute more important information from their theory of cognitive closure, the motivation for
highest domain-specific epistemic authority than knowledge search depends from various factors.
information from the second highest (or any other) Individuals who have a high need for cognitive
epistemic authority. The epistemic authority closure are more likely to form rules, including
reflects the assumption that human knowledge general and common rules, which can use in a
is socially constructed and that it is influenced wide range of situations. People with high need
by others whose judgments one respects. It also for cognitive closure report stronger confidence
touches on the developmental aspect of knowl- in their judgment, probably on the basis of their
edge construction and the diversification of one’s epistemological authority or the general heu-
information sources in accordance with their ristics – “I’m right”. Usually these individuals
perceived expertise. with higher need for cognitive closure are in
the centre of group-forming tendencies, which
Discussion: How Social Factors includes striving for consensus or maintenance
Influence Implicit Knowledge of heuristic - the social group should be rights.
Formation on Internet Thus, if applying the LET theory, the simplified
and clear views (of knowledge) is more likely to
Internet provides a uniform platform for com- be accepted by the social group, as it requires
munication and immediate access to wide range less cognitive-motivational resources. It is more
of information and data. We can assume that in efficient to create a general consensus within
Internet, individual activates the main three pro- the group members with the epistemological au-

212

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

thority and then faster cognitive closure is easily can become automatic and implicit, requiring less
carried out. Conversely, more sophisticated and cognitive resources. The same implies as well for
more complex knowledge can only be accepted collaborative filtering – where users vote or rate
from the out-group or from other epistemological content and the combined ratings serve to get it
authority, since rarely the group members own promoted or made visible in an information system.
resources and cognitive models and the ability This implicit activity will be largely dominated
to demonstrate and test the complex knowledge. and influenced by social groups, reproducing the
The necessity for cognitive closure thus largely content that will be accepted and approved by
depends on the credibility and acceptance of the the others. This can be largely observed in many
external or out-group epistemological authority. on-line e-commerce platforms, where the main
A key challenge in social knowledge exchanges is source of success is due on the social networks.
making a judgment on the authority and credibility The internet and social marketing proves as well
of fellow users. Common examples include reputa- the increased social impact of technologies.
tion scores (awarded to those answering queries Another important factor for promotion of
well, answering a certain number of queries, or the implicit and unconscious knowledge acquisi-
having their answer accepted as the best offered) tion in Internet is the dominance of multimedia
(Matthews & Stephens, 2010). and audio-visual content. Pictures transfer much
It is interesting to note the finding of Thelwall more contextual information, including location,
(2009) that in Internet, the Social Networks Sys- expressions, participants, emotions, attitudes,
tems (SNS) support homophily group formation experiences and others, contributing to bigger
mechanisms. He finds out a clear tendency for extend to the transfer of implicit and unconscious
friendships and any other interpersonal relation- knowledge. Therefore video, visual information
ships to occur between similar people. Based upon and pictures easily transfer and promote implicit
a survey of predominantly US research, it seems “cognition” or understanding.
that gender, sexuality, religion, race and age simi- Some more factors include as well the domi-
larity are all important predictors of friendship. nance of short comments and short narratives,
Therefore even in Internet, the social groups tend emphasizing on emotional, rather than on rational
to be homogenous. analyzes. The preferences for small content are
Another factor for social influence on Internet well displayed in multiple researches, showing that
comes from some of the Internet and social activi- “snack” content is more easily to be read, to be
ties. While routine processes reduces the need for noticed and to be re-sent afterwards (re-tweeted,
conscious control, and thus some estimates that it liked, re-plied etc). With the exploding “smart”
can happen outside of conscious judgment. There- technologies as smart telephones, tablets, smart
fore, the mechanistic thinking process can activate watches and smart glasses, the users will become
implicit knowledge. As the implicit knowledge much more “consumers” of Internet, and not
is largely due on presupposition and attributing content developers, writers, bloggers etc. Thus
characteristics of a category to specific instance we can assume that the future technologies of
(Dienes & Perner, 1999), we can expect that in Internet will largely promote the user behavior of
Internet, social groups can largely influence the consumption of visual information and pictures,
implicit cognitive processes, reflecting the at- reading short-style and easy for understanding
titude and the judgment processes. For example, content, then making detailed analysis and enter-
different functions in Social Networks as “lik- ing into fruitful discussions.
ing”, sharing comments, transferring and sharing That way, Internet can further facilitates the
content to other Internet and social network sites transfer of implicit knowledge between in-group

213

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

members, making it more dominant for individual Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory of
choices, knowledge and understanding. Thus in- mass communications. In J. Bryant & D. Zill-
group implicit knowledge can largely become a man (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory
dominant way of perceiving and understanding and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
the global reality.
Chua, A. (2002). The influence of social
interaction on knowledge creation. Jour-
nal of Intellectual Capital, 3(4), 375–392.
CONCLUSION AND FURTHER
doi:10.1108/14691930210448297
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS
Cook, S. D., & Brown, J. S. (1999). Bridging
The knowledge construction is dynamic, social epistemologies: The generative dance between
process and depends from individual judgment organizational knowledge and organizational
and validation mechanisms. While information knowing. Organization Science, 10(4), 381–400.
technologies improve in general the judgment doi:10.1287/orsc.10.4.381
mechanisms, Internet and web 2.0 technologies
Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (1999). A theory of
mainly promote social and group cognition. The
implicit and explicit knowledge. Behavioral and
social cognition includes shared language, code
Brain Sciences, 22(05), 735–808. doi:10.1017/
and narratives, shared representations, interpre-
S0140525X99002186 PMID:11301570
tations and systems of meaning among parties.
Many of today’s online social exchange platform Ichikawa, J., & Steup, M. (2013). The Analysis
designs have evolved from, or remain very close of Knowledge. The Stanford Encyclopedia of
to the original bulletin board model (Matthews Philosophy (Fall 2013 Edition). Retrieved from
& Stephens, 2010). Thus, in Internet, the process http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2013/en-
of knowledge creation is largely influenced by tries/knowledge-analysis/
social factors and social platforms. The implicit
Kruglanski, A., Orehek, E., Dechenese, M., &
knowledge is constructed and transferred indirectly
Pierro, A. (2010). Lay Epistemic Theory: The
through different social mechanisms. Although the
Motivational, Cognitive and social aspects of
fact that social technology provides many tools
knowledge formation. Social and Personal-
and mechanisms for knowledge construction,
ity Psychology Compass, 10(10), 939–950.
they largely support social and group dominating
doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00308.x
paradigms.
Future work will need to provide more evi- Matthews, P., & Stephens, R. (2010). Sociable
dences for the Internet consumption models and knowledge sharing online: Philosophy, patterns
the way people perceive the influence of social and intervention. Aslib Proceedings, 62(6),
groups on their knowledge creation. With evolving 539–553. doi:10.1108/00012531011089667
smart technologies and Internet of things, we can
Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison Processes
anticipate these patterns to increase in the future.
in Social Judgment: Mechanisms and Con-
sequences. Psychological Review, 110(3),
472–489. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.472
REFERENCES
PMID:12885111
Ayer, A. J. (1956). The Problem of Knowledge.
New York: St Martin’s Press.

214

How Social Factors Influence Implicit Knowledge Construction on the Internet

Nonaka, I. (1994). A dynamic theory of organiza- Explicit Knowledge: A fact, rationally proved
tional knowledge creation. Organization Science, and displayed in documents and references.
5(1), 14–37. doi:10.1287/orsc.5.1.14 Knowledge Justification: According to the
definition of Knowledge as Justified True Belief,
Nonaka, I., & Takeuchi, H. (1995). The Knowl-
knowledge is a function of an agent, who believes
edge-Creating Company: How Japanese Com-
(it is not necessary to prove it, or to explain the
panies Create the Dynamics of Innovation. New
reasons for it) that a fact is true, relying on cor-
York: Oxford University Press.
rect methods for justification. Thus knowledge
Schacter, D. L. (1992). Implicit knowledge: New justification is one of the main components for
perspectives on unconscious processes. Proceed- knowledge acquisition. Knowledge justification
ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the methods are based on 6 main reliable sources of
United States of America, 89(23), 11113–11117. reason and include: (1) perception (analysis of
doi:10.1073/pnas.89.23.11113 PMID:1454787 external information), (2) introspection (analysis
of internal factors), (3) induction (cause-effect
Starmans, C., & Friedman, O. (2012). The folk
structures), (4) memory (references and access to
conception of knowledge. Cognition, 124(3),
facts about past events), (5) reason (logical rules),
272–283. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.017
and (6) testimony (social factors);
PMID:22705197
Social Factors: A social group is a set of indi-
Stets, J. E., & Burke, P. J. (2000). Identity theory viduals who hold a common social identification
and social identity theory. Social Psychology or view themselves as members of the same social
Quarterly, 63(3), 224–237. doi:10.2307/2695870 category (Stets & Burke, 2000). The shared social
identity of group members make it possible for
Thelwall, M. (2009). Homophily in MySpace.
them to produce socially validated knowledge,
Journal of the American Society for Informa-
shared beliefs about way of perceiving thinking
tion Science and Technology, 60(2), 219–231.
and doing that we assume to be appropriate in
doi:10.1002/asi.20978
terms of the demands of objective reality.
Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., Lay Epistemic Theory (LET): According to
& McGarty, C. (1994). Self and collective: the LET theory identified by Kruglanski (2010)
Cognition and social context. Personality and the cycle of all knowledge comprises three general
Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5), 454–454. and interconnected stages: unimodel of social
doi:10.1177/0146167294205002 judgment, cognitive closure and epistemic author-
ity. This theory explain the social mechanisms of
knowledge creation.

KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS


ENDNOTES
Implicit Knowledge: As implicit is something
that is pre-supposed: not directly mentioned, not

1
This statement is attributed to Plato, cited
verbalized (and un-conscious), implicit knowledge
in Ayer (1951) and largely used as starting
can be defined as unconscious understanding,
point in epistemology, philosophy and psy-
rumors, impressions, attitudes and emotions. It is
chology sciences.
sometimes expressed as unconscious knowledge.

215
216

Chapter 11
Hidden Curriculum
Determinants in (Pre)
School Institutions:
Implicit Cognition in Action

Lucija Jančec
Society “Our Children” Opatija, Croatia

Sanja Tatalović Vorkapić


University of Rijeka, Croatia

Jurka Lepičnik Vodopivec


University of Primorska, Slovenia

ABSTRACT
The hidden curriculum is described in terms of values, attitudes, norms, rules, and rituals that are taught
in school, but its main characteristic is its unplanned and officially unorganized form. It appears in every
day kindergarten and school routine where children and pupils learn it as the official curriculum. From
the psychological aspect, it comes close to implicit learning. In this chapter, the authors argue that the
influence of the hidden curriculum and the implicit learning deserve far more attention and scientific
research because of their significant contribution to the learning outcomes and for their constant presence
in the education process: in a learning organization that means all the time and for all the participants.

INTRODUCTION ing to accept it or not. In an educational system,


knowledge production and transfer go through
Pupils in school and children in preschool, apart phases of the official; explicit; written curriculum
from learning about formal school or kindergar- and the process curriculum, unwritten; hidden
ten curriculum content, learn ‘something else’ curriculum. Bašić (2000) and Pastuović (1999)
in their preschool or classroom groups (Oelkers refer to Gordon (1982) when they say that the
& Prior, 1982), whether their members are will- term ‘hidden’ curriculum includes a part of an

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch011

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

educational reality that is not included in the gives more importance to the hidden curriculum
official programme, curriculum or other public when children are younger, for the influence is
documents. The hidden curriculum is important stronger when children age is lower.
for its influence on educational outputs or its set
of affective output (e.g. learned values, norms,
attitudes, social skills), necessary for successful BACKGROUND
social integration.
One cannot just be a teaching expert and ob- The term ‘curriculum’ explains the holistic pro-
jectively educate without simultaneously working cess which contains educational goals, contents,
in the process of upbringing. Bašić (2000, p.171) teaching media, methods, situations, strategies and
analogizes in a hidden curriculum context to evaluation patterns (Bratko, Ljubin & Matijević,
Watzlawick’s, “one cannot not communicate”. 2000). Simply, the term ‘curriculum’ can be seen
Class is a social event in general, “because every as an explanation of what happens in the classroom
behavior – in a teacher and student case – has a or in the laboratory (Eliason & Jenkins, 1986),
communicative and a social effect” (Oelkers & while the curricular programmes should answer
Prior, 1982, p. 64). Tillmann (as cited in Bašić, the following questions: WHY? (refers to goals
2000) sees the hidden curriculum as a regulation of and objectives), WHAT? (refers to contents and
educational interaction in school and appreciable activities) and HOW? (refers to various goals,
instrument of school interaction. In other words, an objectives and planned contents realization).
unmentioned learning plan encompasses aimed but Several groups of people with determinate roles
often unnoticed standardizing and setting norms and assignments are included in the creation
for human relations which sets up a situation of the curriculum. Marsh (2004) describes key
form for acquiring socially eligible interpersonal concepts for understanding curriculum dynamics
interactions and learning how to retrieve quality and gives five basic curriculum categories: pupil
relationships in children age. and teacher’s point of view, planning and devel-
Public, formal and written curriculum is based opment of curriculum, curriculum management
on explicit content, but the grounds of the hidden and curriculum ideology. There are six forms of
curriculum are based on the contents of the of- curriculum in the same book. The term of ‘hidden
ficial curriculum (Cindrić, Miljković & Strugar, curriculum’ is associated with pupils perspective,
2010). School and kindergarten curriculum are and according to Bašić (2000), it presents the op-
commonly accepted as explicit, aware, intentional posite or at least a difference to the public, formal
and formally planned with certain goals. With curriculum. It is assumed that the content of the
this didactic curriculum, which was determined hidden curriculum can be established only after
in advance, children and pupils experience the the content of official curriculum. In a scientific
hidden curriculum described as unofficial and discourse, the term ‘hidden curriculum’ was intro-
unknowingly planned. At this point, we can use duced by Phillip W. Jackson in 1968 and Benson
one working definition and refer to the hidden Snyder in 1971 (Pastuović, 1999, pp.134-136),
curriculum when talking about values and rela- but some authors (Larcher, as cited in Bregar
tions among groups and as a tool of social learning Golobič, 2012) mention Ivan Illich and his book
for children and pupils, but Glatthorn (as cited in ‘Deschooling society’ from 1971 as the founder
Domović domus.srce.hr/...) argues that it refers of the hidden curriculum. A reference from the
to the implicit practice which is specific for a international educational encyclopedia confirms
certain educational environment. Kroflič (2001) Jackson as the original founder of the concept and

217

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

the term of hidden curriculum (Gordon, 1982) and systems throughout the curriculum, the school and
gives him extra legitimacy. The hidden curriculum the classroom life. Pupils are informed about the
can be determined in more ways (Gordon, 1982): hidden curriculum through daily routines, curricu-
lar content and social relationships (Kentli, 2009).
1. As affective educational outputs, such as This approach lays the groundwork for overall
taught values, norms, attitudes necessary for characteristics of the hidden curriculum, and at
the social integration. They are not written the same time, it confirms that school exists for
in the programme or as a part of any insti- the interests of the wider community. In addition,
tutional rule, but it is better for the pupil to Lynch (1989) finds a number of limitations for
accept them; this approach: first, it falls under the consensus
2. As a context in which organized learning in theory which accepts consensus and stability and
a school environment is defined; besides the rejects changes caused by conflict. The other
existing programme, it influences values, thing is that when we look at the norms and belief
attitudes and habits of school participants as system, school is transferring as unproblematic
well as social learning. Commonly, school and indisputable, this approach treats pupils as
relations are a replication of social relations; passive recipients defined in reductionist behav-
3. As a process, within which the difference ioral terms. Pupils are only seen as the products
between the hidden and the official cur- of socialization without the ability to make any
riculum is stressed, and it is related to ways meaning to themselves. In other perspective, Apple
of teaching. While teaching, teacher sends (1982), Anyon (1980) and Giroux (1983) describe
certain non-verbal messages according to how the hidden curriculum practices are realized.
which pupils interpret curriculum and add Their common point is that the social reproduction
it value. emerges in the social organization of the school
and the authority relationships between teachers
These actions can be aware or unaware but and pupils (Kentli, 2009). Jackson in 1968 (as
they certainly affect pupils’ attitudes. That can cited in 1990) in his book ‘Life in Classrooms’
especially be seen in a process of evaluation and defines the hidden curriculum as learning to wait
testing. Pupils will favor exactly the same contents quietly, restraint exercises, completing the work,
as the teacher does. Kentli (2009) describes ten keeping busy, cooperating, showing allegiance to
directions in defining and explaining hidden cur- both teachers and peers, being neat and punctual,
riculum, but she concludes that the hidden cur- and conducting oneself courteously. As mentioned
riculum, as the process of socializing in school, before, he is the hidden curriculum pioneer, he
can be identified through social interactions in approached the phenomenon from the scientific
school environments and serves for the transfer and anthropological perspective and created the
of delicate messages about values, attitudes and meaning of the term ‘hidden curriculum’, which
principles. The hidden curriculum can be detected is used for describing the ‘other’ curriculum, the
by the evaluation of the environment and unex- one existing parallel with the official one (Jack-
pected and unintentional interactions between a son, 1990). Jackson gave the most significant
teacher and a student (Kentli, 2009). contribution to the conception and the term hid-
Durkheim (1961), Drebeen (1967) and Jackson den curriculum, where he identified phenomena
(1968) laid the groundwork for a general defini- in classroom consisted in the social relation of
tion of the hidden curriculum as an element of schooling. According to his analysis, values,
socialization in schools. It is not a formal school dispositions, social and behavioral expectations
curriculum, but it includes norms, values and belief that bring rewards for pupils in school and their

218

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

learning is a feature of the hidden curriculum. Foucault (as cited in Dolar Bahovec & Kodelja,
His version of the hidden curriculum is marked 1996) made a great effort in the conceptual context
by his exceptional and intuitive method that made of the hidden curriculum, and provided a new
him valuable and until then unnoticed understand- aspect of everyday practice in kindergarten. He
ings about holistic and widely defined classroom analyses intern ideology of educational institu-
life that discovered hidden dimension of school tions, its constitutional rules and everyday prac-
events which are consequences of written, formal tices by which inequality and domination are set up
curriculum. and sustained. Intern ideology of kindergarten as
Apple (1982), the most prominent author of an institution of ‘discipline and punishment’ is, in
all mentioned above, in his book “Education and Foucault’s opinion, a real question of microphys-
power” and other books, emphasizes that the ics of authority, which brings new perspective in
hidden curriculum includes various interests, the hidden curriculum. According to Foucault, in
culture forms and patterns, struggles, arrange- addition to the entire assemble of practices asso-
ments and compromises. The main characteristic ciated with relation toward the body, the way of
of his approach is his definition of the hidden sitting at work, holding a pen while drawing and
curriculum through a hegemonistic concept. This writing, waiting for lunch and lying while sleep-
concept staggers and defines school not only as ing, it is a moment of surveillance, ‘normaliza-
a distributor but as a generator of the culture be- tion’ of children, which was founded in technical
ing crucial for the pupils’ socialization. In other and scientific knowledge about children. Dolar
words, according to Apple, pupils are confronted Bahovec & Kodelja (1996) claim that Foucault’s
with different norms and cultures through rules view of everyday practices in kindergartens and
and activities during schooling and classroom life schools, and different perception of childhood in
that shapes the social life in school. In his work different historic periods, is actually the reason
‘Ideology and curriculum’ (1995) he claims that that everyday life should be subjected to genea-
hidden curriculum meets the ideological needs logical research.
of capital. King and Apple (1992) say that the
hidden curriculum is taught in school, but it plays 1. Aspects of Hidden Curriculum
a more significant role in kindergarten, where in Kindergarten and School
children become competent for compliance with
rules, norms, values needed for functioning in a Jackson (1990) does not use terms from the theory
life of an institution: of ideology, but in his research path, he confronts
the school ideology, described by Louis Althusser
The meaning of objects and events becomes (1980): not in programme, lecture or lesson-book
clear to children when they participate in social contents, but in every day school practice, school
environment. The usage of aids (for playing and routines that helps establishing and maintaining
learning), the nature of authority, quality of mu- school (institutional) order. The second name for
tual relations, spontaneous comments and other the hidden curriculum is therefore ‘curriculum
perspectives of everyday life in a kindergarten in use’ (Apple, 1992, p.51). Hidden curriculum
group or classroom, it all contributes to growing is an inner part of the curriculum; it is the cur-
awareness of a child’s role in a group and his riculum in use. Therefore, in a spontaneous, self-
understanding of the social environment. (King understanding approach, that hides the separation
& Apple, 1992, p. 53) of school demands on institutional demands for

219

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

accommodation and enlightenment demands of practices that are specific for a particular educa-
schooling, classification of ‘official curriculum’ tional setting. Bloom (as cited in Dolar Bahovec
and ‘curriculum in use’ is noticed. Bandura (as & Bregar Golobič, 2004) claims that the hidden
cited in Vizek Vidović, Rijavec, Vlahović-Štetić curriculum looks more efficient than the formal,
& Miljković, 2003) proposed an explanation of because we memorize its lessons as it is present
what pupils learn in school in his social learn- through the whole period of schooling. The hidden
ing theory. According to that theory, people are curriculum messages are daily experiences that
in a certain way products of their surroundings are firmly taught. Dealing with different situations
but they can also influence it. One can learn to and social interactions in schools is the experi-
modify its own behavior when influenced by other ence that stays for life. It seems that kindergarten
people during the social interactions. That learn- and school teachers are guided more by their own
ing is called observational learning and comes school and schooling experience than theory and
in two forms: modeling and vicarious learning. professional knowledge. The following citation
Many authors support the concept that teachers, illustrates it more precisely:
by their behavior in school, implicitly transfer
their own values and attitudes to pupils (Gordon, This emphasis on the early years of schooling is
1982; McDiarmid, 1987; Verhoeve, 2008; Hod- quite intentional, for it is during that period that
kinson, 2005; Weldon, 2010; Zembylas, 2011). the young child comes to grips with the fact of
By observing their teachers-models, pupils can institutional life. Also during these formative years
learn new behaviors, determine pre-existing and they develop adaptive strategies that will stay with
(de)motivate themselves with the intention of them throughout their education and beyond. Life
doing socially (un)desirable ones. Observational in high school and college classrooms is surely
learning refers to situations when the pupil sees different from life in the lower grades, but behind
and takes into consideration the consequences the obvious differences there is a basic similar-
of the model’s behavior. It happens in class even ity. In a fundamental sense, school is school, no
when teachers are not aware of it - in case of matter where it happens. (Jackson, 1990, p.xxi)
inconsistency when they talk in one way and act
in another, pupils will imitate their behavior and The hidden curriculum is a common char-
not their words (Vizek Vidović et al., 2003). For acteristic of all school institutions as well as all
example, scared and anxious children imitate a modern time institutions (military, prison, hospi-
model which shows fear more often than self- tals, etc.) and is not limited by boundaries; it is a
confident and secure children. Besides that, the transversal characteristic of institutions (Bregar
importance of personality of an observed model Golobič, 2012). The book ‘Kindergartens for the
is well known since Plato and his The Republic. present time’, which was the consequence of a re-
Contemporary researchers investigate deeper in search project with the same title, Dolar Bahovec
the importance of personality traits in educational & Kodelja (1996) describes the organization of
processes (Tatalović Vorkapić, 2012). Schools everyday life in Slovenian kindergartens in two
support goals and functions of a wider social com- dimensions: in the context of the Foucault’s idea
munity through attitudes, norms, beliefs, values of body discipline in institutions of modern time,
and hypothesis learning that are all expressed as which represents conceptual thinking about the
rules, rituals and regulations (Seddon, 1983), and hidden curriculum, and on the other side, in the
all represented in the hidden curriculum. Glatthorn theory of children rights. This project sought to
(as cited in Domović, domus.srce.hr/...) argues extend the current psychological and pedagogical
that the hidden curriculum refers to the implicit discourses and upbringing of a child and place it in

220

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

the broader field of education theory and cultural dimension they really have in shaping the future
studies. In 66 Slovenian kindergartens observa- generations and the attention they deserve on the
tions were carried out using lists for observing the micro - and macro - level.
environment, feeding, sleeping and other aspects; Many contemporary authors (Kessler, 1992;
the task was completed with the answers of kin- Stoll & Fink, 2000; Bruner, 2000; Carre, 2001;
dergarten teachers acquired through a question- Wilson, 1996; Malaguzzi, 1998) point out that
naire. The results showed that in about one-fifth values, beliefs, attitudes and expectations of kin-
of the observed kindergartens certain unwanted dergarten and school teachers shape their entire
forms of organization and order are present: un- educational approach. Mayall (as cited in Dahl-
necessary regulations that make the daily routine, berg, Moss, & Pence, 1999) claims that children
feeding, toilet habits, sleep, lack of alternatives live in conditions of childhood that were created
regarding to different aspects of the kindergarten, for them by adults, according to their understand-
poor respect for the right to diversity, choices, ing of childhood and children, and their image of
uniqueness. The core of the study conducted in what a child should be. Nimmo (1998) also believes
kindergartens was finding the manner in which that the entire educational teacher work is based
kindergarten teacher organize time and space on his/her image as a child, his/her expectations
of their work, supervise emotional reactions of that reflect his/her cultural perspective, values,
children and their physiological needs, how they aspirations and beliefs about learning and child
use praises and reproach, exercise their authority, upbringing. Bruner (2000) argues that different
respond to spontaneous remarks, allow the children approaches to learning as well as various forms
to participate in deciding and choosing etc., it was of teaching - imitation, lessons, revealing, col-
observed how the kindergarten teacher organizes laboration, are all reflection of different beliefs
and prepares materials and games for children and premises about pupils - as creators, experts,
without the influence of children. Although the direct participants, collaborative thinkers. Beliefs,
toys on the shelves and on the floor were avail- attitudes and expectations of teachers are woven
able to children all the time, time organization into the structural context of the institution and
did not allow using them or playing with them. are realized as a particular theory or orientation
For most of the day children were not allowed to curriculum (Kessler, 1992), because the beliefs,
play with toys that were arranged to invite them preferences and values of people affect their ac-
to play, but they have learned to take them off tions and thinking, says Fullan (as cited in Stoll &
the shelf only when they got the explicit permis- Fink, 2000), directly determines the perspective
sion. When they act according to the rules their of curriculum. The values represent a set of com-
behavior is approved, when not, they are warned. mon beliefs, opinions and attitudes about what
Kindergarten teachers thought that good children is right, good or desirable, and they are formed
were the children who followed their instructions through the process of socialization. Although
and were quiet. In his earlier work, Apple (1979) each individual may have their own value sys-
states that children learn skills in a way that helps tem, there are some general or social values that
them leading a successful life in kindergarten. dominate in each community. In dynamic terms,
For example, they learn to share toys with other values represent a sort of organization of human
children, listen carefully, clean up, and follow the needs, desires and goals that are established among
routine activities of the group. All this supports them, certain relationship priorities and hierarchies
the power of the hidden curriculum and stereo- of importance. This organization facilitates and
types about daily routines in kindergarten, what makes individual’s orientation, decision-making

221

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

and integrated actions easier, what values can decisions and judgments, help in an effort to
be understood as a kind of criteria priorities that rationalize and shape the attitudes and opinions,
guide human behavior (Petz, 1992). Values are the facilitate one’s communication with others and
result of interfering individual (needs) and social represent an important base in the understanding
(social demands) factors that represent cognitive of the social environment. From the perspective
expression of individual needs in socially ac- of educational efforts, the spiritual values have
ceptable terms as a result of the internalization particular importance: ethical, intellectual, aes-
of social demands. Larger number of authors thetic, religious, cultural, rational, personal and
point out the insufficient values research (Sagiv family. All these values have the aim of achieving
& Schwartz, 2000; Rohan, 2000), despite strong educational purposes and tasks of education and
influence of values on attitudes and behavior. The are rightly called educational values (Vukasović,
research results indicate that there is a universal 1999). Educational values are closely linked to
structure of value system and that is possible to educational goals and standards. The crucial fea-
compare individual and group values according ture of a value is the act of evaluation that decides
to values priorities (Schwartz, 1994). Although on the acceptance of objective goal or standard
recently values are also researched according to by individuals. Values are based on standards,
other dispositions, such as personality traits (Ro- but also on the evaluation of something as good/
han, 2000) and mental health (Sagiv & Schwartz, bad, valid, right/wrong (Gudjons, 1994, p. 154).
2000), their predictability for attitudes and be- Ledić (1999) researched the values that are
havior is traditionally expected and researched. promoted in textbooks for the subject Croatian
Very few authors dealt with the classification of language for elementary school that were used in
value functions. For most of them, the definition the school year 1996/97, as well as textbooks for
of value is also a determinant of value function. elementary schools that were in use in the school
Woolfolk (1995) sees values as certain guided year 1986/87. In her study she started from the
behavior functions: premise that school as a social institution of great
importance for development of the society, with
1. Conditional commitment of special position a goal to question the promotion of these values
in relation to social problems, in recent educational practice in our schools, and
2. Directing the choice of a particular political observing them in relation to values that were
or religious ideology, promoted a decade earlier, because the values
3. Contribute to our presentation of our promoted in texts are one in a series of elements
environment, that affect the moral atmosphere in school. For
4. Are crucial in comparative processes, example, justice and freedom as the dominant
5. Encourage individual in the evaluation and values appear only in 15 texts in new generation
judgment, textbooks. The results of this research may lead
6. Allow the influence on other, to the conclusion that the Croatian ideology re-
7. Make it possible to rationalize the opinions places another: the dominant socialist values in the
and attitudes. 1980s were replaced by the national in the 1990s.
The analysis of textbooks raises many questions,
According to Radin (2001), it is indisputable including questions about the impact of ‘hidden
that values have a great guiding function in terms curriculum’ and the real impact of the values
of behavior. Apart from influencing attitudes promoted on the formation of pupils’ values. A
regarding relevant social issues, they affect the special series of questions was related to teachers
individual’s everyday life, encourage one in their themselves: how they see the issues of values,

222

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

whether, how and to what extent they transmit it others. Therefore, based on a research, the need
and how school affects the formation of values for further cross-curricular studies was found,
in children and the youth. This research was also related to the role of texts in pupils’ textbooks in
associated with examining the teachers’ attitudes promoting educational values which would give
through interviews and questionnaires where a significant contribution to the enrichment of the
all the values/messages obtained by textbooks educational function of school.
analysis were lined up. The statements acquired by Yet, in the chapter, we consider some more
interviewing teachers and their attitudes expressed dimensions of ‘hidden curriculum’ and observe
by ranking scale are mutually consistent, but they implicit learning in kindergartens and schools
differ (it is suggested) from the priorities that are so we will discuss its visibility and importance
implicitly or explicitly included in textbooks. A through presented results in researches about
group of national values that was found to be personality traits, but also in researches about
particularly emphasized in new textbooks does empathy and (pre)school teacher’s attitudes about
not stand out in any special way (more on results space characteristics.
in Ledić, 1999).
Nearly a decade after the research on values in 2. Personality Traits of (Pre)School
school, the following study observed myths and Teachers in an Implicit Learning
legends in textbooks for lower elementary grades and Hidden Curriculum Context
(Mlinarević, Buljubašić Kuzmanović & Sablić, of Educational Institution
2007), assuming that school should influence
development of the pupil’s value-system. But if the No matter what definition of the hidden curricu-
problem is observed from the pedagogical context, lum we take and analyze, it is clear that within
then it enters a discussion about education goals its frame, children learn on two levels: conscious
that can be observed as relatively steady quality and unconscious or unintentional, and implicit
that is directed toward specific experiences and learning/memorizing. Understanding the memory
actions among which skills, attitudes, beliefs, as implicit or explicit is well known and it is ap-
values, interests, different knowledge and habits plied on whether we want to remember something
are included (Brezinka, 1994). The analysis shows on purpose – this being the explicit memory or
that proportionately to pupils’ age, with larger unintentionally - the implicit (Zarevski, 2002).
volume and complexity of teaching content, the Hence, when talking about implicit learning, we
prevalence of myths and legends and educational refer to the process where we become sensitive
values grows (Mlinarević, Buljubašić Kuzmanović for certain rules in the environment, but with a
& Sablić, 2007). The content analysis has allo- highlighted intention of absence for learning and
cated 78 texts containing myths and legends and absence of willing to actually learn, that all to-
eight categories of educational values (univer- gether results with kind of knowledge that is hard
sal, democratic, religious, national, personality to express and articulate (Cleeremans, 2002). It
characteristics, family, environmental, aesthetic is interesting that the terms implicit learning and
and cultural) that promote them, and which are hidden curriculum share a common characteristic
components of desirable ethical principles and of having several definitions, where only Frensch
moral education that contribute to the overall (1998) presented eleven definitions of the implicit
development of pupils. These are certainly not just learning. Besides that, the implicit learning is a
values that should be recognized but they are also phenomenon which is present every day (Ellis
life principles that pupils need to use in order to & Hunt, 1993). Therefore, it often seems that
become part of them and their interactions with we know far more than we can explain (Shanks,

223

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

2004). For example activities such as riding a from pupils. Furthermore, all children and pupils
bike, playing tennis or driving, they all include do not react in the same way on the hidden cur-
handling numerous complex motoric skills that riculum and the implicit learning. The research
are difficult to verbalize. Additionally, as said on the implicit learning correlations show that
before, people have abilities to learn new informa- high impulsivity, openness for experiences and
tion without any intention and the power of such intuition are personality traits which are positively
expression of knowledge is very low. The analysis correlated with the implicit learning (Kaufman,
of core characteristics of the implicit learning DeYoung, Gray, Jiménez, Brown & Mackintosh,
and definitions of the hidden curriculum indicate 2010). Additionally, some social factors such as
numerous similarities. One of them is the fact that social class, race, ethnicity, linguistic roots and
in the hidden curriculum one learns parts that are sex are significant determinates of the hidden
not included in the official curriculum - social curriculum and children/pupils’ and kindergarten/
interactions and children are not intentionally school teacher’s behaviors. Some of those elements
directed in the implicit knowledge and contents, have positive and some have negative influence
they are mostly not even aware of it. Questions on children’s behavior which is expected in the
how and why implicit knowledge and contents official curriculum (Massialas, 1996). It is as-
are remembered arise. Assor & Gordon (1987) sumed that the strategies of implementations used
declare basic grounds for memorizing contents of by teachers exist to decrease negative effects of
the hidden curriculum in the learning and teaching the hidden curriculum, referring to:
process; these contents are presented many times
and always in the same ways. In other words, those 1. Elimination of harmful practice “monitor-
contents are strongly redundant, and that is the ing” pupils;
basic principle for learning hidden curriculum 2. Replacement intrinsic for extrinsic rewards,
contents. In the educational system, the efficiency especially avoiding grades as a disciplinary
of learning the official and planned curriculum tool;
contents is confronted with the efficiency of the 3. Initiation of a great number of methods
hidden curriculum contents. Educators presume that include group work representing one
that learning is a natural consequence, especially of efficient ways for demystification the
if quality and efficient methods are used. But traditional, manipulative role of a teacher,
that presumption is not always true. Regarding as ensuring clear social context to highlight
this fact, it is estimated that parts of the official social responsibility and group solidarity;
curriculum contents are significantly lower (it is 4. Change of time spent in school in a way of
10%) than the rest of all the obtained knowledge exchange independent and group work in a
– the rest is ascribed to the hidden curriculum suitable manner, that enables development
(Massialas, 1989). Goodlad (1988) claims pupils of personal communication with the teacher;
are influenced by classroom and school climate 5. Creating “peer leaders”, competent for taking
and experience democratic processes, indepen- care of social relations in a group in a way
dent thinking, creativity, personal autonomy to achieve a group goal with less interven-
and learning for learning itself in large amounts. tions from the teacher as possible (Giroux
Anyon (1980) finds what is dominantly expressed & Penna, 1983).
and expected in some school programmes. For
example, to express your creativity in school In other words, the hidden curriculum is used
with the art programme indicates what kind of in a positive way when life in school imitates real
characteristics will be fostered and expected life in most realistic way. It is then that the children

224

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

get the opportunity to learn skills in construc- other characteristics that come from the personality
tive decision making, what is actually more than of teacher himself (Cartwright, 1999). Researches
necessary in real life. of the kindergarten and school climate show that
Next to the mentioned elements of the hidden the personality of kindergarten/school teachers
curriculum, which are acquired based on the im- is the one thing that determinates it distinctively
plicit learning principle, it is inevitable to avoid (Vasconcelos, 2002; Vizek-Vidović et al., 2003).
the personality of all subjects in learning and the It is clear that the educational process is not just a
teaching process. Onatra & Pena (2004) in their cognitive and technical process, but an extremely
work about the hidden curriculum emphasize complex, personal, social practice that involves
personality traits of pupils and the significant a whole person, whether we speak about a child
influence of teachers who actually shape their per- or a teacher (Hamachek, 1999; Oakes & Lipton,
sonality. These authors underline that the concept 2003; Britzman, 2003; Cochran-Smith, 2005), and
of the hidden curriculum is beyond the discussion that is why it is important to carry out scientific
about conscious and unconscious determinates researches for the personality traits of kindergar-
of formal context of learning and teaching. They ten/school teachers. There are limited numbers of
say that the hidden curriculum shapes personal- researches in this field, especially when thinking
ity of children and ascribes to every one of them of kindergarten teachers (Ayers, 1989; Yonemura,
the role one should have in the society. In that 1986), but a certain research implemented in Croa-
process, the interaction with teacher is extremely tia can be a solid ground for further observations
important, because she/he influences directly about this topic. Slunjski (2003) described in her
on the social relations development in a group. book the enneagram approach of studying nine
Therefore, from the fact that teachers have the different faces of kindergarten teacher, where she
closest interaction with pupils in the process, next also analyzed their advantages and imperfections
to teaching objective knowledge and facts, they in the work with children.
transfer their own attitudes and beliefs and shape The most studies analyzed the personalities
pupils personality. According to the development of kindergarten teacher through the Eysenck’s
of pupils’ personality, three main aspects of the model and the Big Five model of personality. In
hidden curriculum are summed up: a) teachers at- one of these studies (Tatalović Vorkapić, 2012),
titude as the knowledge holder; b) teachers attitude both of these personality models were analyzed
as the authority holder; c) teachers attitude as the by 92 kindergarten teachers, with the mean age
acceptation and rejection holder using their evalu- of 30 years (21 to 49 years), that show consider-
ation tools. These three aspects enable influencing ably higher levels of extraversion, agreeableness,
on a child objectively and subjectively, and are consciousness, openness to experience and social
responsible for the personalization process in the conformity than normative samples. This finding
teaching and learning process. As earlier said, the was expected, regarding assumed personality traits
combination of the teaching style, attitudes and of professionals working with children. An open,
values of a teacher, and the hidden curriculum in communicative, agreeable, positive, conscious,
kindergarten/school which appears (unplanned) flexible and adjustable kindergarten teacher
every day, are significantly determined by the certainly ensures quality work with children and
kindergarten/school teachers’ personality. Kin- their parents. In the same study, significantly
dergarten/school teachers say that the professional lower levels of psychoticism and neuroticism than
education and vocational trainings which result normative samples were determined, as expected.
in essential competences are deficient for quality Similar results were found in further researches.
work with children – that are exactly values and Gović (2012) has conducted a study about flow

225

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

at work and personality traits in a sample of 96 counties and 20 kindergartens in Croatia, with
kindergarten teachers from six kindergartens in the mean age of 43 years, mean of 19 years of
Primorsko-goranska county in Croatia, with the period of employment, high levels of all empathy
mean age of 38 years, ranged from 22 to 64 years, dimensions (perspective taking, fantasy, empathic
mean of 14 years of period of employment. Beside concern and personal distress), and personality
high level of flow at work, high level of extraver- traits of extraversion, agreeableness, conscious-
sion, agreeableness, consciousness, openness to ness, openness to experience are determined. As
experience and low neuroticism were determined expected, a significantly low level of neuroticism
and expected. According to expectations, high is found. An interesting finding is the correlation
level of flow at work is positively correlated with by increasing age and years of period of employ-
extraversion, agreeableness, consciousness, open- ment personal distress gets higher, and level of
ness to experience and has a significantly negative extraversion is reduced. A negative relationship of
relationship with neuroticism. age and extraversion is expected, and this finding
A further research (Jelić, 2013) shows that determined at kindergarten teachers is not signifi-
personality, hope and optimism relation, level of cantly different from findings in other professions.
satisfaction with life, accepting their skills, abili- But, the significant increase of personal distress
ties, as well as general and specific competences with age was not expected and certainly requires
are extremely important for successful kinder- a further empirical investigation and could be
garten teachers. Jelić (2013), in a sample of 339 explained by characteristics in kindergarten teach-
kindergarten teachers from eight counties and 20 ers’ work with preschool children.
kindergartens in Croatia, analyzed the relation of Besides that, it is not significant, but with the
their personality traits and hope, optimism and age comes declining in empathy at kindergarten
satisfaction with life. The results show signifi- teacher in the Martić (2013) research, similar
cantly high values of all personality traits except result as in research Tatalović Vorkapić & Ružić
neuroticism, which is least represented at person- (2013). That research is about level of empathy
ality traits of kindergarten teacher, as expected. at three study groups of future preschool teacher
All personality traits are positively related with (N=100), with the mean age of 20 (18-25), where
hope, optimism and satisfaction with life, except four dimensions of empathy were analyzed: fan-
neuroticism that has a negative relationship to tasy, perspective taking, empathic concern and
these three constructs. personal distress. Results showed significant
According to previous researches (Ivon & higher levels of all dimensions of empathy in a
Sindik, 2008) which show the significance of kin- relationship with other - not kindergarten teacher
dergarten teachers’ empathy in their work, Martić students, as expected. But unexpected result,
(2013) analyzed the relationship of empathy with which could have significant implications for
personality traits of kindergarten teachers. For changing study programme if it will empirically
kindergarten teacher’s work performance it is repeat, showed negative relationship with study
not just about his/her knowledge, skills and com- year and fantasy and empathic concern. In other
petences, but his/her holistic personality, where words, significant decrease of fantasy and em-
highly empathic behavior, high levels of extraver- pathic concern in a relationship with study year
sion, agreeableness, consciousness, openness to is found, and it is absolutely not desirable because
experience is expected. In a sample of 205 kinder- study programme should encourage existing high
garten teachers from Istarska, Brodsko-posavska, level of empathy freshman come to university. In
Bjelovarsko–bilogorska & Primorsko–goranska addition to the previous research, the extension

226

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

of decrease in empathy and in practice itself can 21-49), mean of 6 year work time, show that 25
be seen in a kindergarten teacher practice, what participants have phlegmatic (lower extraversion
has a certain implication for changing vocational and neuroticism), 18 melancholic (lower extraver-
training programmes. sion and higher neuroticism), 23 sanguine (higher
Next research is searching for answers whether extraversion and lower neuroticism) and 19 have
future kindergarten teacher at the moment of entry choleric characteristics (higher extraversion and
an university have higher level of extraversion, neuroticism) according to Eysencks temperament
agreeableness, consciousness, openness to expe- types. As assumed, findings show the greatest
rience and lower level of neuroticism (Tatalović number of lower level with neuroticism (phleg-
Vorkapić & Puljić, 2013). matic and sanguine). In relation to age, phlegmatic
In a sample of 64 students of the Faculty of kindergarten teacher are the oldest, and choleric
Teacher Education in Rijeka, Study of Early and the youngest.
Preschool Education, in age of 19-23, personal- If we compare the results of kindergarten
ity traits was examined based on the Five Factor teachers and findings in personality traits of
model. As expected, findings show higher levels school teachers, the significant difference is the
of extraversion, agreeableness, consciousness, level of neuroticism. Although the expectations
openness to experience and a lower level of of personality traits for kindergarten and school
neuroticism at future kindergarten teachers. No teachers are the same, the findings indicate a
significant relationship of two generations and higher level of neuroticism with school teach-
age of students with personality traits, which were ers (Arif, Rashid, Tahira, & Akhter, 2012) and
expected, were found. The discussion part high- a higher level of vulnerability to stress and burn
lights the importance of a kindergarten teacher in out (Brkić & Rijavec, 2011). To explain this
his/her educational work and general environment phenomenon, a further research should be held,
that he/she creates in his/her work with children, yet some suggestions could already be given. The
as guidance for further researches. first one is about different responsibilities at work
The relationship of personality traits, satis- between preschool and school teacher. The assess-
faction with life and professional burn out of ment and evaluation of children’s work system is
kindergarten teachers was also researched and it present just in school, not in kindergartens, and
has confirmed previous findings about personality it represents a huge additional working burden
traits (Tatalović Vorkapić & Lončarić, 2013). In for school teachers. Secondly, in kindergarten
a sample of 295 kindergarten teachers (4 male) groups the responsibility is divided between two
from all over Croatia, with the mean age of 37, kindergarten teachers, which is not the case in
the results show a significant direct influence of schools where one teacher is responsible for the
extraversion and neuroticism on satisfaction with whole class. Another aspect that should also be
life, what has confirmed former researches. Except taken into consideration, is the fact that kinder-
direct effects, extraversion has an indirect posi- garten teachers are much more involved in physi-
tive effect on satisfaction with life, mediated by cal activities (systematically or not) than school
reduced emotional exhaustion, while neuroticism teachers. It is known that persons who intensively
has an indirect negative effect on satisfaction with do sport activities have significant lower level of
life mediated by increased emotional exhaustion neuroticism, different from those who do not do
and perception of reduced personal achievement. any (Han, Kim, Lee, Bae, Bae, Kim, Sim, Sung
The next research (Tatalović Vorkapić et al., & Lyoo, 2006; Trninić, Barančić & Nazor, 2008).
2014) consists of a sample of 85 kindergarten All these implications of personality traits
teachers with the mean age of 31 (ranged from discussed here can be considered in a light of

227

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

hidden curriculum and its constitutive part, where actions can be understood as their answer to the
the personality of a (pre)school teacher has enor- social environment. They do not take their experi-
mous significance in every day institutional life ence passively, but they interpret and establish it,
of a child. most commonly in a process of mutual interaction
with other children and adults. Learning is also a
3. Interactive and Communicative social process based on a cooperative construc-
Aspect of the Upbringing tion of knowledge, which means that child’s
Process and the Role of knowledge is built in a social interaction (ibid,
Empathy: Multidimensional p.49), and on an unconscious level as implicit
Observing of Empathy learning. Recent publications emphasize interac-
tive and communicative aspect of an educational
Rinaldi (2002) claims that teachers’ values deter- process, and according to that aspect, upbringing
mine the culture of an institution. She describes takes place in mutual relations, and mutual rela-
that values represent a point of reference of our tions are the element on which the efficiency of
judgments and behavior on which we establish upbringing depends. The role of kindergarten and
(or do not establish) our connection within a school teachers is especially highlighted in mutual
social referential group. Valli & Hawley (2002) relations and communication between children,
explain that teachers’ knowledge and opinions pupils, kindergarten and school teachers, as well
“filter” their knowledge, and based on values, as their ability to empathize, understand, and
opinions and attitudes one cannot influence them share with others, children and parents (Lepičnik
in traditional educational ways and trainings, e.g. Vodopivec, 2004).
lectures or workshops where teachers want to be According to Apple (1992), who studied the
taught how “should they do their jobs”. For that hidden curriculum in American kindergartens,
reason, the focus of professional development of hidden curriculum is extremely important as it
kindergarten and school teachers in a learning or- helps the children to learn habits within first few
ganization is creating preconditions where teacher months in kindergarten. Another significant role
can articulate and explore values and opinions is that of kindergarten teachers, represented by
of his own (Stoll & Fink, 2000; Slunjski, 2006). their characteristics and certain skills, yet the
The above mentioned refers to an environment most important is the communication compe-
in which kindergarten and school teachers are tence. Recent publications emphasize interactive
important not just for the children, but for the and communicative aspect of the educational
adults as well because adults and children are a process, and according to that aspect, upbringing
part of a learning organization. Preschool children takes place in mutual relations, it is based on a
teaching is a result of an interaction between creative participation, and depends on interaction
children experiences with materials, ideas and and communication between pupils and teachers
people, so we can say that a child is developing, (Bratanić, 1991), and kindergarten/school teach-
creating, constructing (and not just adopting from ers personality traits (as described in previous
adults) their own knowledge through their activi- subheading). Reardon (1998) emphasizes the
ties and experiences, with the help of parents and ability of empathy as an important part of com-
kindergarten teacher (Slunjski, 2001). There is a munication competence. The results of empathy
strong connection between social and cognitive research in an education process (Bratanić, 2001)
development: for instance, cognitive conflict can show that one can influence abilities for empathy
lead to changes in a cognitive but also in a social development in children and pupils by intentional
aspect of a child’s development. Many children’s activities of kindergarten and school teachers.

228

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

Empathic kindergarten and school teachers affect ence in physical and social environment; that all
the empathy development in kindergarten children should be implied in the kindergarten teacher’s
(Lepičnik Vodopivec, 2004) and pupils in schools, role in a child’s learning process.
especially those who can identify themselves with An empathic kindergarten teacher is compe-
children’s position, accept them for who they are, tent to detect and discover children’s feelings,
and create everyday situations where children can emotional-motivational factors of their behavior,
pursue respect toward their environment through choose suitable educational facilities and methods
personal experiences. and adjust communication to every child (Lepičnik
Miljak (1996) claims that in hidden curricu- Vodopivec, 2004), as it is with school teacher.
lum, except for attitudes, opinions and values, Hoffman (1987) claims that empathy has a great
empathy is considered as a personality of every role in a process of establishing a relation with self
individual, and all of that constitutes implicit and others. Lepičnik Vodopivec (2004) implies in
pedagogy. Research that sets off the importance her research work that kindergarten teachers have
of implicit pedagogy, or in other words, individual developed ability to accurately realize children
(private) teacher’s theory (Petrović Sočo, 2009), experience in different environments. Kindergar-
show that with changing implicit pedagogy of a ten teachers with their empathy, their calmness
teacher, the culture of an institution changes too. and social trust influence creating satisfactory
The author emphasizes socio-educational dimen- emotional conditions in an educational process in
sion for achieving quality that will have an impact kindergartens. The results show that kindergarten
on education and development of young children. teachers are distinctively restrained from open-
In a three year research, where ethnographic and ness to new information, which is explained by a
action approach were combined, three kindergar- long-term behavioristic concept in kindergartens.
ten institutions participated. More understanding According to the fact that empathy is an im-
and satisfying of developmental and individual portant component of social cognition, which is
children’s needs were in institutions where the tightly connected to knowledge, it can be assumed
most intensive changes of implicit pedagogues that empathic kindergarten teachers encourage
of kindergarten teacher occurred. In a construc- empathy development and empathic knowledge
tively conceived learning environment, the role (knowledge that is holistic, participating, synergic,
of a kindergarten teacher and his/her activities responsible for the present and future time, knowl-
are based on child’s activities, so the role can be edge that respects life, diversity and nonviolence
defined as a fine adjustment with children’s ac- and stimulates quality of life) in children, and
tivities, based on an attentive observing (Slunjski, Rifkin (1986) claims it has an extremely impor-
2001). Devries & Kohlberg (1987) highlight the tant role in a process of development of critical
importance of good psychological education for thought in children. Empathy consists of cogni-
kindergarten teachers, necessary for understanding tive and emotional component and they make an
and valuation of spontaneous child activities, thus interdependent system where one affects the other
these kind of activities would not be perceived as and vice versa (Deutsh & Madle, 1975; Hoffman
“waste of time”. Furthermore, that knowledge is 1977; Feshbach 1976, all to Davis 1980, 1983a,
necessary not just for the activity organization and 1983b, as cited in Tatalović 1997). Therefore,
child’s thinking stimulation, but also for develop- emotional and cognitive components interact in
ing egalitarian and partner relations with children. a process of empathy, so in this segment we can
Thus, it can be said that a child constructs its own also see the role of implicit learning. With regard
knowledge, gives its own meanings to his experi- to complexity of cognitive processes and emotional

229

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

reactions in an empathy process, empathy is a find school, its environment, furniture, decoration
more multidimensional construct which represents as a “third teacher”, and exactly the same name is
constellation of many aspects. Some of the aspects used in the developed and world famous Reggio
refer to taking over the perspective and some to conception where the space is extremely important
abilities of emotional reactions in emotional com- (Edwards, Gandini, & Forman, 1998). The Reggio
munication (Riggio, Tucker & Coffaro, as cited conception finds a child competent, intelligent,
in Pokrajac-Bulian & Tatalović, 2001). The most curious, creative and moreover, a social being.
famous multidimensional approach of empathy That is the reason for one of the most important
was Mark H. Davis’ approach from 1980. tasks of Reggio teacher, to create rich environ-
ment surroundings which promote autonomy of
4. Hidden Curriculum and Space: child activities and encourage them for different
Importance of ‘Educationality’ research activities. A special attention is given to
of Space and (Pre)School the creation of space conditions for socializing
Teacher’s Attitudes About and communication of children and to continuous
Space Characteristics development of their miscellaneous expressive
modalities (“hundred languages of children”)
Discussion of hidden curriculum revolves on by which they can symbolically represent their
everyday use of our surroundings, in a physical knowledge, ideas and feelings (Forman, 1998;
sense: buildings, kindergartens, schools, architec- Goldhaber, Smith & Sortino, 1997; Gandini,
ture. According to many authors from educational 1998). Taylor (2009) finds school environment to
and architecture field, the space is actually a be “three-dimensional work book” and a part of
characteristic that should be considered the same hidden curriculum that enables different learning
as the essence of kindergartens and schools. Barši experiences, positive or negative. The same is
(1996) uses a Japanese example for illustration with the kindergarten environment. For children,
of importance of space, education method called the kindergarten is a place which they experience
tsutaeai: as a home so it should represent a pleasant not a
stressful environment. It depends on a lot of factors,
… tsutaeai does not just describe relations not just the kindergarten as a building from the
among children and their teachers, but relations inside and outside but kindergarten environment,
of children and things like space, material, de- space organization, inner and outdoor settlement
tails, ornaments. Therefore, relation of life and (Barši, 1996; Miljak & Vujičić 2000, 2002; Miljak,
architecture is an important element of tsutaeai 2009). These authors show how they think about
education, and a building which is rich in its particular kindergarten and school institutions,
message is called tsutaeai . (Maruyama, as cited but that can be applied to every educational in-
in Barši 1996, p. 191) stitution. For early child experiences it is specific
that there is time for building the child’s world.
Recent publications refer that except for In that time, child experiences kindergarten as
what children learn in kindergarten and school, “unique and the only one in the world-like one
important questions are “What is good thinking home, one mother and one father” (Barši, 1996,
space?” (Dolar Bahovec, 2012), “Where should I p. 200). These grounds assure safety that cannot
sit?” (Sudec, 2012), they address the problem of be replaced later (ibid.). Gandini (1998) describes
co-creational relation of learning and school and school classrooms and says that they should look
school’s environment architecture, as for kinder- like kind of studios, different space dimensions
gartens too. Nicholson (2005) claims that we can including central space for gatherings, working

230

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

spaces for cooperative learning groups of differ- Šuklje Erjavec (2012, pp. 163-164) emphasizes
ent sizes and private spaces where children can that poorly arranged and unattended outdoor
think and work independently or individually with spaces send out completely different messages
a teacher or another child. Greenman (Gandini, and co-create completely different relations than
1998) links space and personal experiences: space arranged and equipped spaces. Therefore, it is
that surrounds us affects the way we feel, think important to know what behaviors, customs and
and behave, has an extremely strong influence. expectations children learn in kindergarten and
As all we do, space environment can make it school. Day & Midbjer (2007, as cited in Cencič
easier or harder. 2012) claim that space messages are not just
According to Cencič and Dolar Bahovec (2012) visual but school (as well as the kindergarten)
in an editorial of “Contemporary pedagogy”, sends messages with its smell, school bell..., and
which is thematically dedicated to architecture all of it influences on feelings, values, relations,
and (hidden) messages of the school space, they cognition etc. All buildings and spaces teach us,
say that aspects of teaching and learning highlight but there are just few of them that were planned
that learning is realized in different ways, in dif- with that purpose. The mentioned authors conclude
ferent cities and places and different periods so it that school environment and school space “teach
is necessary that kindergarten and school environ- us in silence and all the time”.
ment stimulate learning, their learning out of their Cencič (2012) emphasizes that in the context of
classroom groups, for instance in hallways and creation more a simulative learning environment,
kindergarten and school environments, adding that active learning is significant, which includes learn-
outdoor space is also a constituent part of learn- ing through work and interaction, and develops
ing. The histories of science, art and philosophy different cognitive and social aspects in children,
have shown the importance of space for opinion their feelings, aesthetics and physical development
itself and that space conditions are actually very (Ciaccio, 2004). In other words, it is a holistic de-
diverse. Authors add that space where the educa- velopment because it includes cognitive, conative
tion process is performed is an additional factor of and psychometric dimension of learning. Thereby,
teaching because young people absorb symbolic school playgrounds are not only purposed for mo-
messages from school building and its surround- tion and physical development but for active learn-
ing. If teaching is a communication process and ing and for research, because they are a component
space an additional factor of teaching, then the of kindergarten and school environments (Moore,
school space is in interaction with all its users. 2006). In another research, findings suggest great
Nicholson (2009, p.45) says that school space is influence of school space on feelings and health
sending numerous messages about what is impor- of pupils and teachers and on learning results. For
tant and what is not. He also claims that school instance, a modest learning environment makes it
space reflects what and how pupils learn and how difficult for teachers to teach and pupils to learn
they are being taught. In philosophical reflections (Barret & Zhang, 2009). These authors represent
about space, thinking and learning in kindergarten results which confirm relationship of pupils’
and school, it turns out that space is an element health and learning. For example, pupils are less
and an inherent part of curriculum itself of every sick and have better learning results in an appro-
educational institution, and not an extern factor priate or eco schools. Stimulative school space
which could be taken into consideration later. The has important and positive role in teaching and
space is also a part of the hidden curriculum; it is learning and affects on better pupils’ results and
right there and for that reason we hardly observe better teachers’ work. Woolner (2010) adds that in
it and change it (Bregar Golobič, 2012). inappropriate school space, employees and pupils

231

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

feel less effective and less motivated, and aim to of learning environment and the depth, quality
be more absent from school, they behave worse of learning. Desforges (2001) says that learning
and have lower learning success. The following is especially sensible for environment, Brown
findings (LPA, as cited in Cencič, 2012) show (2001) says that learning is a complex process and
that learning results improved 10 to 21 percent in strongly influenced by environment and Jelavić
school spaces which had natural light, good natu- (1995) says that learning is an act of creation of
ral ventilation and pleasant view in comparison personal experience most sensible to condition
to results of pupils in classrooms with minimal of place. Organization of space and material
nature light and other less pleasant conditions. equipment of educational institution is also a very
The size of school, classes and special spaces or important condition for quality learning of pupils
corners in classrooms positively affected on pupils’ (Cohen, Manion & Morrison, 1996; Kinstler &
success (Moore & Leckney, as cited in Cencič, Gamble, 2001; Kyriacou, 2001). For Kinstler &
2012). Results also show that teachers as well as Gamble (2001), the quality of physical environ-
pupils become aware of physical environment and ment and material equipment of institution are
realize its weaknesses, that school environment the most important organizational preconditions
affects the moral of pupils and employees, their of pupils’ learning because it strongly determi-
attention and as mentioned before, the learning nates nature of learning. Rosenholtz (as cited in
results (Woolner, 2010). Prosser, 1999) says that the physical environment
In a research of elementary school teacher at- has potential to empower the learning process but
titudes about the school space, there was a sample also to impoverish it. Kerry & Tollitt (as cited in
of 36 elementary schools and 251 educational Cohen, Manion & Morrison, 1996, p. 162) find
workers (Cencič, 2012), the goal was to find out space “essential origin of learning”, especially in
what kind of attitudes elementary school teach- learning of small children where they come close
ers have about relationship of schools space and to Kroflič (2001) and his hidden curriculum pos-
learning, ecology of school space and how do tulate: greater meaning for younger children. For
teachers value connection of school space and that reason, one of the basic parts of kindergarten
environment. The results show that teachers agree and school teachers in a learning organization
with the claim that school should be connected is designing of space-material environment for
with the local community, but do not agree that learning which also refers to the child learning
school playgrounds should be open for other users. support. Space organization essentially deter-
Teachers from the rural area agree with that more minates, among other things, quality of social
than the urban area teachers. Moreover, teachers interactions between pupils, as pupils with teach-
from the rural area were more in favor of having ers. In a learning organization, structure – space
a small school garden, which shows that they organization is focused on stimulating contacts,
are more supportive of the idea of connecting communication and interaction (Slunjski, 2006,
the school with the environment than urban area pp. 76-77). The author adds that the stimulating
teachers. Slunjski (2006) shows how by creating material itself without abilities to communicate
space and material conditions the learning orga- and discussions with other children could lead to
nization produces and co-creates kindergarten education of the so called Piaget child (Dahlberg
curriculum. We will expand this claim and say it et al. 1999), from which it is expected to create
can be applied to other educational institutions, its own isolated knowledge, out of social context.
not just the ones who follow kindergarten cur- Important criteria for space quality of educational
riculum. Many authors (Brown, 2001; Desforges, institution are also communication abilities or
2001; Wilson, 1996) highlight the connection social interactions of children (Bredekamp &

232

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

Rosegrant, 1992; Gandini, 1998; Malaguzzi, CONCLUSION


1998). Gandini (1998) emphasizes that one of
the ground criteria of space environment is the All this constitutes a learning organization, which
strength of space in which it stimulates contacts, is alive, changing and valued and includes a con-
communication and mutual relation among chil- tinuous learning process for children and adults
dren. Space organization highly reflects the image and encourages ‘the feeling of open possibili-
of a certain teacher about a child, its beliefs and ties’ (Bruner, 2000, p. 55). The phenomenon of
attitudes about children and process of their learn- the hidden curriculum described in this paper
ing, as following authors claim: space for learning contributes to emphasize neglecting importance
in classroom usually reflects teacher’s attitudes of interdependence of changing the culture and
about what and how children learn (Tann, 2001). structure of educational organizations. It includes
Hendrick (1986) also connects organization and implicit learning, learning out of structural dimen-
space equipment with educational philosophy of a sions of institutions whereby generally are thought
teacher: in planning a space environment, teachers as its space, material equipment, time structuring
should always reconsider basic educational goals activities for children, educators and teachers, and
which they find important and which they aim, in all other organizational issues. The term ‘culture’
order for their educational philosophy to become usually considers values, attitudes, norms and
alive. Space design and material conditions for expectations of kindergarten and school teachers.
children’s education and learning in educational In this work we can see how its importance is
institutions can represent material objectification hidden and implicit because it is always in front
of certain educational philosophy of the institution, of us; it is hard to see and is equally difficult to
as can be seen in some, so called alternative edu- change, such as describing the appearance of the
cational concepts (Slunjski, 2006). One example hidden curriculum (Bregar Golobič, 2012). Yet
is the Montessori educational concept, built on further research of implicit learning and hidden
Maria Montessori principles (Seitz & Hallwachs, curriculum is necessary in order to observe how
1997; Spodek & Saracho, 1991; Kamenov, 1987) the common vision of children, kindergarten and
that is based on special learning materials she school teachers might change and modernize, so
developed in order to fulfill five basic requests that each participant in the educational process
(more in Kamenov, 1987). Quality and encour- facilitates the opportunities for learning and well-
aging space environment also supports adults being considering their learning needs in each
learning in an educational institution, having in space and each learning community they are in.
mind more chances of observing and investigat-
ing children in more diverse activities and situa-
tions than these activities in poor environments FUTURE TRENDS
(Slunjski, 2006). Senge et al. (2003) illustrates:
until people do not turn their work spaces into The empirical and the theoretical findings show
learning spaces, learning will always remain just that it is evident that we should create a set of
a good idea – brief, but not central. One can say researches in the field of teachers’ personality
that the space-material stimulating environment traits, as it has so far been done systematically
is a base for mutual, continuous learning of adults for teachers, so there would be a real space for
and children at the same time. comparison. Based on the results of these com-

233

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

parisons, a new draft of the research could be Apple, M. W. (1979). Ideology and Cur-
created and be focused on the analysis of the riculum. London: Routledge and Kegan.
differences and the creation of guidelines for doi:10.4324/9780203241219
changes of academic programs, and selection
Apple, M., W. (1992). School, teacher, authority.
ways of candidates’ admission to the Teacher
Ljubljana: Scientific and Publistic Center.
Education Studies. Furthermore, given the current
and anticipated personality of kindergarten and Apple, M. W. (1995). Education and power. New
school teachers, it could be empirically examined York: Routledge.
what exactly occurs through the implicit learning
Arif, M. I., Rashid, A., Tahira, S. S., & Akhter,
within the educational process, and in what ways
M. (2012). Personality and Teaching: An Inves-
to effectively improve the quality of learning
tigation into Prospective Teachers’ Personality.
outcomes that affect the hidden curriculum and
International Journal of Humanities and Social
implicit learning that takes place under hidden
Science, 2(17), 161–171.
curriculum to be positive. A certain number of
studies have examined the hidden curriculum, Assor, A., & Gordon, D. (1987). The implicit
and implicit learning in certain groups of students learning theory of hidden-curriculum research.
(Lempp & Seale, 2004; Ozolins, Hall & Peterson, Journal of Curriculum Studies, 19(4), 329–339.
2008; Barani, Azma, Seyyedrezai, 2011) but on doi:10.1080/0022027870190404
a scientific level none of them directly questions
Ayers, W. (1989). The good preschool teacher:
the relationship between personality traits, em-
Six preschool teachers reflect on their lives. New
pathy, spatial features and attitudes toward space
York: Teachers College Press.
(teachers and/or children) and implicit learning,
which ought to be studied in the future. In addi- Barani, G., Azma, F., & Seyyedrezai, S. H. (2011).
tion, there are research chalanges for empathy of Quality indicators of hidden curriculum in Centers
kindergarten and school teachers which affects of higher education. Procedia: Social and Behav-
the learning environment, as well as a research ioral Sciences, 30, 1657–1661. doi:10.1016/j.
on spatial messages solutions in which learning sbspro.2011.10.321
takes place, including attitudes of kindergarten
Barrett, P., & Zhang, Y. (2009). Optimal Learn-
and school teachers toward the space features.
ing Spaces. Design Implications for Primary
Schools. SCRI Research Report. Retrieved August
7, 2013, from http://www.oecd.org/education/
REFERENCES
innovation-education/centreforeffectivelearnin-
Althusser, L. (1980). Ideology and Ideological genvironmentscele/43834191.pdf
State Apparatuses. In Ideology and aestetc effect Barši, J. (1996). The meanings of kindergarten
(pp. 35-99). Ljubljana: Cankarjeva Založba. spaces. In Kindergartens for present time (pp. 191-
Anyon, J. (1980). Social class and the hidden 200). Ljubljana: Center for Cultural Researches at
curriculum of work. Journal of Education, 162, Educational Institute and Association for Cultural
67–92. Researches.

234

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

Bašić, S. (2000). Concept of hidden curriculum. Cindrić, M., Miljković, D., & Strugar, V. (2010).
Napredak, 141(2), 170–180. Didactic and curriculum. Zagreb: IEP-D2.
Bratanić, M. (1991): Micropedagogy. Zagreb: Cleeremans, A. (2002). Models of Implicit
Školska Knjiga. Learning. In Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science.
Macmillan Publishers.
Bratko, D., Ljubin, T., & Matijević, M. (2000).
Pupils evaluation of curriculum “Active/effective Cochran-Smith, M. (2005). The new teacher edu-
school”. Napredak, 141(2), 156–169. cation: For better or for worse? 2005 presidential
address. Educational Researcher, 34(7), 3–17.
Bredekamp, S., & Rosegrant, T. (1992). Reaching
doi:10.3102/0013189X034007003
Potentials: Transforming Early Childhood Cur-
riculum and Assessment (pp. 1–2). New York: Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (1996). A
National Association for the Education. Guide to Teaching Practice. London: Routledge.
Bregar Golobič, K. (2012). What school do we Dahlberg, G., Moss, P., & Pence, A. (1999).
want? Space as an element (of hidden curriculum). Beyond Quality in Early Childhood Education
Sodobna pedagogika, 1, 52-72. and Care: Postmodern Perspectives. London:
Falmer Press.
Brezinka, W. (1994). Pedagogy and educational
reality. Theleme: Časopis za Istraživanje Edu- Desforges, C. (Ed.). (2001). Successful learn-
kacije i Kulture, 40(1/4), 49-68. ing and teaching – Psychological approaches.
Zagreb: Educa.
Britzman, D. (2003). Practice makes practice: A
critical study of learning to teach. Albany, NY: DeVries, R., & Kohlberg, L. (1987). Programmes
State University of New York Press. of early education: The constructivistic view.
White Plains, NY: Longman.
Brkić, I., & Rijavec, M. (2011). Origin of stress,
coping with the stress and life satisfaction of Dolar Bahovec, E. (2012). What is good think-
teachers. Napredak, 152(2), 211–225. ing space? Philosopy, architecture, schooling.
Sodobna Pedagogika, 1, 18-33.
Brown, G. (2001). What does learning all includes.
In Successful learning and teaching – Psychologi- Dolar Bahovec, E., & Bregar Golobič, K. (2004).
cal approaches (pp. 17-37). Zagreb: Educa. School and kindergarten trough the mirror.
Manual for the kindergartens, schools and parents.
Bruner, J. (2000). The culture of education. Za-
Ljubljana: DZS.
greb: Educa.
Dolar Bahovec, E., & Kodelja, Z. (Eds.). (1996).
Carre, C. (2001). What schould be learned in
Kindergartens for present time. Ljubljana: Center
school? In Successful learning and teaching – Psy-
for cultural researches at Educational institute and
chological approaches (pp. 37-57). Zagreb: Educa.
Association for cultural researches.
Cencič, M. (2012). Dimensions of school space.
Domović, V. (n.d.). Retrieved March 5. 2012, from
Didaktika Slovenica, 27(3-4), 117–150.
domus.srce.hr/.../5_kurikulum%20usmjeren%20
Ciaccio, J. (2004). Totally Positive Teaching: A na%20kompetencije%20-%20osnovni%20poj-
five – Stage Approach to Energizing Students and movi_20-2-09.pdf
Teachers. Alexandria: Association for Supervision
ans Curriculum Development.

235

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

Edwards, C., Gandini, L., & Forman, G. (Eds.). Goodlad, J. (1984). A place called school: Pros-
(1998). The Hundred Languages of Children: pects for the future. New York: McGraw-Hill.
The Reggio Emilia Approach. London: Ablex
Gordon, D. (1982). The concept of the hidden
Publishing Greenwich.
curriculum. Journal of Philosophy of Education,
Eliason, C. F., & Jenkins, L. T. (1986). A Practical 16(2), 187–198. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9752.1982.
Guide to Early Childhood Curriculum. Columbus, tb00611.x
OH: Merrill Publishing Company.
Gović, A. (2012). Experience of flow at work and
Ellis, H. C., & Hunt, R. R. (1993). Fundamen- personality traits of kindergarten teacher. Faculty
tals of cognitive psychology. The McGraw-Hill of Teacher Education, University of Rijeka.
Companies, Inc.
Gudjons, H. (1994). Pedagogy, fundamental
Forman, G., & Fyfe, B. (1998). Negotiated Learn- knowledges. Zagreb: Educa.
ing Through Design, Documentation and Dis-
Hamachek, D. (1999). Effective Teachers: What
course. In C. Edwards, L. Gandini, & G. Forman
they do, how they do it, and the importance of
(Eds.), The Hundred Languages of Children: The
self-knowledge. In R. P. Lipka & T. M. Brinthaupt
Reggio Emilia Approach (pp. 239–260). London:
(Eds.), The role of Self in Teacher Development
Ablex Publishing Greenwich.
(pp. 189–224). Albany, NY: State University of
Frensch, P. A. (1998). One concept, multiple New York Press.
meanings: On how to define the concept of implicit
Han, D. H., Kim, J. H., Lee, Y. S., Bae, S. J.,
learning. In M. A. Stadler & P. A. Frensch (Eds.),
Bae, S. J., & Kim, H. J. et al. (2006). Influence
Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 47–104).
of temperament and anxiety on athletic perfor-
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
mance. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine,
Gandini, L. (1998). Educational and Caring 5, 381–389. PMID:24353455
Spaces. In C. Edwards, L. Gandini, & G. Forman
Hendrick, J. (1986). Total Learning, Curriculum
(Eds.), The Hundred Languages of Children: The
for the Young Child. Columbus, OH: Merrill
Reggio Emilia Approach (pp. 161–178). London:
Publishing Company.
Ablex Publishing Greenwich.
Hendrick, J. (1997). First step Toward Teaching
Giroux, H., & Penna, A. (1983). Social Education
the Reggio Way (pp. 198–209). Columbus, OH:
in the Classroom: The Dynamics of the Hidden
Merrill/Prentice – Hall.
Curriculum. In H. Giroux & P. David (Eds.), The
Hidden Curriculum and Moral Education (pp. Hodkinson, P. (2005). Learning as cultural and
100–121). Berkeley, CA: McCutchan Publishing relational: Moving past some troubling dualisms.
Corporation. Cambridge Journal of Education, 35(1), 107–119.
doi:10.1080/0305764042000332524
Goldhaber, J., Smith, D., & Sortino, S. (1997).
Observing, Recording, Understanding: The Role Hoffman, M. L. (1987). The contribution of
of Documentation in Early Childhood Teacher empathy to justice and moral judgement. In N.
Education. In J. Hendricks (Ed.), First step To- Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its
ward Teaching the Reggio Way (pp. 198–209). development (pp. 47–80). New York: Cambridge
Columbus, OH: Merrill/Prentice – Hall. University Press.

236

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

Ivon, H., & Sindik, J. (2008). Connection between Kroflič, R. (1997). Between obedience and res-
preschool teachers’ empathy and imagination and posability. Ljubljana: Vija.
certain characteristics of preschool child’s behav-
Kyriacou, C. (2001). Basic teaching virtues.
ior and play. Magistra Iadertina, 3(3), 21–38.
Zagreb: Educa.
Jackson, P. W. (1990). Life in clasrooms. New
Ledić, J. (1999). School and values. Rijeka: Faculty
York: Teachers College Press.
of Humanities and Social Sciences.
Jelavić, F. (1995). Didactic basics of teaching.
Lempp, H., & Seale, C. (2004). The hidden
Jastrebasko: Slap.
curriculum in undergraduate medical educa-
Jelić, J. (2013). Personality traits, hope, optimism tion: Qualitative study of medical students’
and life satisfaction od kindergarten teacher. Fac- perceptions of teaching. BMJ (Clinical Re-
ulty of Teacher Education, University of Rijeka. search Ed.), 329(7469), 770–773. doi:10.1136/
bmj.329.7469.770 PMID:15459051
Kamenov, E. (1987). Experimental program-
memes for early education. Beograd: Zavod za Lepičnik Vodopivec, J. (2004): Empathy of kin-
udžbenike i nastavna sredstva. dergarten teachers as an element of hidden cur-
riculum in kindergartens. Sodobna Pedagogika,
Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R.,
2, 140-151.
Jiménez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010).
Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116(3), Malaguzzi, L. (1998). History, ideas and philoso-
321–340. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.011 phy. In C. Edwards, L. Gandini, & G. Forman
PMID:20573341 (Eds.), The Hundred Languages of Children: The
Reggio Emilia Approach (pp. 41–89). London:
Kentli, F. D. (2009). Comparison of hidden cur-
Ablex Publishing Greenwich.
riculum theories. European Journal of Educa-
tional Studies, 1(2), 83–88. Marsh, C. J. (2004). Key concepts for understand-
ing curriculum. London: RoutlegeFalmer, Taylor
Kessler, S. (1992). The Social Context of the
& Francis Group.
Early Childhood Curriculum. In Reconceptual-
izing the early childhood curriculum: Beginning Martić, K. (2013). Is the empathy of kindergarten
the dialogue. (pp. 21-42). New York: Teachers teacher related to their personality? Faculty of
College Press. Teacher Education, University of Rijeka.
Kessler, S., & Swadener, B. B. (Eds.). (1992). Massialas, B. G. (1989). The Inevitability of
Reconceptualizing the early childhood curricu- Issue-Centered Discourse in the Classroom. Social
lum: Beginning the dialogue. New York: Teachers Studies, 80(5), 173–175. doi:10.1080/00377996.
College Press. 1989.9957472
King, N., & Apple, M. W. (1992). What do school Massialas, B. G. (1996). The hidden curriculum
teach. In M. W. Apple (Ed.), School, teacher, and social studies. In Critical Issues in Teaching
authority (pp. 43-64). Ljubljana: Znanstveno in Social Studies (pp. 61-79). Wadsworth.
publicistično središče.
McDiarmid, G. (1987). The Development of
Kinsler, K., & Gamble, A. M. (2001). Reforming a Conceptual-Empirical Theory of Curricu-
schools. London: Continuum. lum. Interchange, 18(3), 38–62. doi:10.1007/
BF01807219

237

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

Miljak, A. (1996). Humanistic approach to theory Oakes, J., & Lipton, M. (2003). Teaching to change
and practice of kindergarten education. Zagreb: the world. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Persona.
Oelkers, J., & Prior, H. (1982). Soziales Lernen
Miljak, A. (2007): Learning by doing and partici- in der Schule. Koeningstein.
pating in a community of professional kindergarten
Onatra, A., & Pena, M. (2004). The hidden cur-
teacher. In Pedagogy: Towards to lifelong learning
riculum. Profile, 158-161.
and knowledge society (pp. 201-214). Zagreb:
Croatian Educational Society. Ozolins, I., Hall, H., & Peterson, R. (2008). The stu-
dent voice: Recognising the hidden and informal
Miljak, A. (2009). Life of children in kindergartens.
curriculum in medicine. Medical Teacher, 30(6),
Zagreb: SM Naklada.
606–611. doi:10.1080/01421590801949933
Miljak, A., & Vujičić, L. (Eds.). (2000). Kin- PMID:18608968
dergarten as child’s house. Poreč: Dječji vrtić
Pastuović, N. (1999). Educology. Zagreb: Znamen.
“Radost”. Poreč.
Petrović Sočo, B. (2009). Changing context and
Miljak, A., & Vujičić, L. (Eds.). (2002). Kin-
educational practice in kindergartens. Zagreb:
dergarten corresponds to child’s nature “child’s
Mali profesor.
house”. Rovinj: Kindergarten “Neven” Rovinj.
Petz, B. (1992). Psyhological dictionary. Zagreb:
Mlinarević, V., Buljubašić, V., & Sablić, M.
Prosvjeta.
(2007): Promoting educational values in myths and
legends in lower grades in elementary scools. In Platon. (2004). The Republic. Zagreb: Naklada
Golden days 8: Myths and legend (pp. 173-187). Jurčić.
University Josip Juraj Strosmayer Osijek, Faculty
Pokrajac-Bulian, A., & Tatalović, S. (2001). A
of Humanities and Social Sciences Osijek, Faculty
multidimensional approach to measuring of sex
of Humanities and Social Sciences Pečuh, Matica
and age differences in empathy. Psihologijske
hrvatska Osijek.
Teme, 10, 77–88.
Moore, R. (2006). Playgounds: A 150 year old
Prosser, J. (Ed.). (1999). School culture. London:
model. In Safe and Healthy School Environments
Paul Chapman Publishing.
(pp. 86-103). Retrieved September 26, 2013, from
http://web.seru.sa.edu.au/pdfs/SERUpdate%20 Radin, F. (2001). Values and unacceptible values.
April%202013%20Play.pdf In V. Ilisin, A. Marinovic-Bobinac, & F. Radin
(Eds.), Children and media (pp. 79–90). Zagreb:
Nicholson, E. (2005). The School Building as
IDIZ.
Third Teacher. In M. Dudek (Ed.), Children Spaces
(pp. 44–65). London: Routledge. Readon, K. K. (1998). Interpersonal communica-
tion. Zagreb: Alinea.
Nimmo, J. (1998). The Child in Community:
Constrains From the Early Childhood Lore. In Rifkin, J. (1986). Entropy - A new world view.
C. Edwards, L. Gandini, & G. Forman (Eds.), Zagreb: Naprijed.
The Hundred Languages of Children: The Reggio
Emilia Approach (pp. 295–312). London: Ablex
Publishing Greenwich.

238

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

Rinaldi, C. (2002). Infant-Todler Centers and Slunjski, E. (2006). Creation of kindergarten cur-
Preschools as Places of Culture. In C. Giudici, riculum in a kindergarten – Learning organization.
C. Rinaldi, & M. Krechevsky (Eds.), Making Mali profesor.
learning visible: Children as individual and group
Spodek, B., & Saracho, O. M. (1991). Issues in
learners (pp. 36–43). Reggio Emilia, Italy: Reg-
Early Childhood Curriculum. New York: Teach-
gio Children. Project Zero & Reggio Children.
ers College Press.
Rohan, M. J. (2000). Rose by any name? The
Stoll, L., & Fink, D. (2000). Changing our schools
values construct. Personality and Social Psy-
– How to enhance efficiency and quality of our
chology Bulletin, 4(3), 255–277. doi:10.1207/
schools. Zagreb: Educa.
S15327957PSPR0403_4
Sudec, K. (2012): Where to sit? Co-creational
Sagiv, L., & Schwartz, S. H. (2000). Value pri-
process of learning with inner architecture of
orities and subjective wellheing: Direct relations
school. Sodobna pedagogika, 1, 140-154.
and congruity effects. European Journal of So-
cial Psychology, 30(2), 177–198. doi:10.1002/ Šuklje Erjavec, I. (2012). Meaning and posabili-
(SICI)1099-0992(200003/04)30:2<177::AID- ties of using outdoor school space in educational
EJSP982>3.0.CO;2-Z process. Sodobna pedagogika, 1, 156-174.
Schwartz, S. H. (1996). Value priorities and be- Tann, S. (2001). Organization of learning. In
haviour: applying a Theory of intergated value Successful learning and teaching – Psychological
sisterns. Seligman, U.C., Olson, J.M., Zanna, approaches (pp. 161-176). Zagreb: Educa.
M.P. (Eds). The Psyhology of values: The Ontario
Tatalović, S. (1997). Adolescents’ empathy in
Symposium (vol. 8, pp. 1-24). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
relation to some aspects of self-esteem (Gradu-
Seddon, T. (1983). The hidden curriculum: An ate thesis). Department of Psychology, Facults
overview. Curriculum Perspectives, 3, 1–6. of Humantities and Social Sciences, University
of Rijeka.
Seitz, M., & Hallwachs, U. (1997). Montessori
or Waldorf. Zagreb: Educa. Tatalović Vorkapić, S. (2012). The significance
of preschool teacher’s personality in early child-
Senge, P., Kleiner, A., Roberts, C., Ross, R., Roth,
hood education: Analysis of Eysenck’s and Big
G., Smith, B. (2003). The dance of change. Zagreb:
Five Dimensions of personality. International
Mozaik knjiga.
Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences,
Shanks, D. R. (2004). Implicit learning. In Hand- 2(2), 28–37. doi:10.5923/j.ijpbs.20120202.05
book of cognition. Academic Press.
Tatalović Vorkapić, S., & Puljić, I. (2013). Im-
Slunjski, E. (2001). Integrated kindergarten portance of students’ personality traits for their
curriculum, child’s project work. Zagreb: Mali future work with preschool children. Metodički
profesor. obzori, 8(2), 34-46.
Slunjski, E. (2003). Nine faces of one kindergarten
teacher/parent. Mali profesor.

239

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

Tatalović Vorkapić, S., & Lončarić, D. (2013). Verhoeve, S. (2008). Peace-Teaching: Evaluating
Does the professional burnout mediate the effects the Role of Education in Post-Conflict Cambodia.
of personality traits on life satisfaction among Undercurrent, 5(3), 45–52.
preschool teachers? Psychological Topics, 22(3),
Vizek Vidović, V., Rijavec, M., & Vlahović-Štetić,
431–455.
V., & Miljković, D. (2003). Psychology of educa-
Tatalović Vorkapić, S. & Ružić, N. (2013). tion. Zagreb: IEP.
Measuring empathy in future preschool teachers:
Vukasović, A. (1999). Purposes and tasks of
Implications for study programme modification.
education. In Basics of contemporary pedagogy
International Journal of Psychology and Behav-
(pp. 131-148). Zagreb: Hrvatski pedagoško-
ioral Sciences, 3(6).
književni zbor.
Tatalović Vorkapić, S., Vujičić, L., & Čepić,
Weldon, G. (2010). Post-conflict teacher develop-
R. (2014). Preschool, Teacher Identity. Book
ment: Facing the past in South Africa. Journal of
Chapter to be Appeared. In P. Breen (Ed.), Cases
Moral Education, 39(3), 353–364. doi:10.1080/0
on Teacher Identity, Diversity, and Cognition in
3057240.2010.497615
Higher Education. IGI-Global. doi:10.4018/978-
1-4666-5990-2.ch002 Wilson, B. (1996). Introduction: What is a con-
structivist learning environment? In B. Wilson
Taylor, A. (2009). Linking Architecture and
(Ed.), Constructivist learning environments (pp.
Education: Sustainable Design for Learning
3–8). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technol-
Environments. Albuquerque, NM: University of
ogy Publications.
New Mexico Press.
Woolfolk, A. E. (1995). Educational psychology.
Trninić, V., Barančić, M., & Nazor, M. (2008).
New York: Allyn&Bacon.
The five-factor model of personality and aggres-
siveness in prisoners and athletes. Kinesiology, Woolner, P. (2010). The Design of Learning
40(2), 170–181. Spaces: Future Schools. London: Continuum
International Publishing Group.
Valli, L., & Hawley, W. D. (2002). Designing
and Implementing School – Based Professional Yonemura, M. (1986). A teacher at work: Pro-
Development. In The Keys to Effective Schools fessional development and the early childhood
–Educational Reform as Continuous Improvement educator. New York: Teachers College Press.
(pp. 86-96). Corwin Press, Inc.
Zarevski, P. (2002). Psychology of memory and
Vasconcelos, T. (2002). “I am like this because I learning. Jastrebarsko: Naklada Slap.
just can’t be different…” Personal and professional
Zembylas, M. (2011). Peace and Human Rights
dimensions of Ana’s teaching: Some implications
Education: Dilemmas of Compatibility and
for teacher education. In D. Rothenberg (Ed.),
Prospects for Moving Forward. Prospects: Quar-
Proceedings of the Lilian Katz Symposium (pp.
terly Review of Comparative Education, 41(4),
191-199). Early Childhood & Parenting (ECAP)
567–579. doi:10.1007/s11125-011-9212-8
colaborative University of Illinois, Urbana-
Champaign.

240

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

ADDITIONAL READING Hoffman, M. L. (1987). The contribution of


empathy to justice and moral judgement. In N.
Apple, M., W. (1992). School, teacher, authority. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its
Ljubljana: Znanstveno in publicistično središče. development (pp. 47–80). New York: Cambridge
Apple, M. W. (1995). Education and power. New University Press.
York, London: Routledge. Jackson, P. W. (1990). Life in clasrooms. New
Barrett, P., & Zhang, Y. (2009). Optimal Learn- York: Teachers College Press.
ing Spaces. Design Implications for Primary Kroflič, R. (1997). Between obedience and res-
Schools. SCRI Research Report. Retrieved August posability. Ljubljana: Vija.
7, 2013, from http://www.oecd.org/education/
innovation-education/centreforeffectivelearnin- Ledić, J. (1999). School and values. Filozofski
genvironmentscele/43834191.pdf fakultet: Rijeka.

Barši, J. (1996). The meanings of kindergarten Lepičnik Vodopivec, J. (2004): Empathy of kin-
spaces. In: Dolar Bahovec, E., Kodelja Z. (Eds.) dergarten teachers as an element of hidden cur-
Kindergartens for present time (pp. 191-200). riculum in kindergartens. Sodobna pedagogika.
Ljubljana: Center za kulturološke raziskave pri 2/2004. 140-151.
Pedagoškem inštitutu in Društvo za kulturološke Malaguzzi, L. (1998). History, ideas and philoso-
raziskave. 191-200. phy. In C. Edwards, L. Gandini, & G. Forman
Bredekamp, S., & Rosegrant, T. (1992). Reaching (Eds.), The Hundred Languages of Children: The
Potentials: Transforming Early Childhood Cur- Reggio Emilia Approach (pp. 41–89). London:
riculum and Assessment (pp. 1–2). New York: Ablex Publishing Greenwich.
National Association for the Education. Marsh, C. J. (2004). Key concepts for understand-
Bruner, J. (2000). The culture of education. Za- ing curriculum. London, New York: Routlege
greb: Educa. Falmer, Taylor & Francis Group.

Desforges, C. (Ed.). (2001). Successful learn- Miljak, A. (2009). Life of children in kindergartens.
ing and teaching – psychological approaches. Zagreb: SM Naklada.
Zagreb: Educa. Petrović Sočo, B. (2009). Changing context and
Dolar Bahovec, E., & Kodelja, Z. (Eds.). (1996). educational practice in kindergartens. Zagreb:
Kindergartens for the present time. Ljubljana: Mali profesor.
Center za kulturološke raziskave pri Pedagoškem Rifkin, J. (1986). Entropy - a new world view.
inštitutu in Društvo za kulturološke raziskave. Zagreb: Naprijed.
Gordon, D. (1982). The concept of the hidden Rinaldi, C. (2002). Infant-Todler Centers and
curriculum. Journal of Philosophy of Education, Preschools as Places of Culture. In C. Giudici,
16(2), 187–198. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9752.1982. C. Rinaldi, & M. Krechevsky (Eds.), Making
tb00611.x learning visible: Children as individual and group
learners (pp. 36–43). Reggio Emilia, Italy: Reg-
gio Children. Project Zero & Reggio Children.

241

Hidden Curriculum Determinants in (Pre)School Institutions

Slunjski, E. (2006). Creation of kindergarten cur- KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS


riculum in a kindergarten – learning organization.
Mali profesor: Zagreb. Big Five Model: Is a model of personality
that describes it by five broad domains or dimen-
Spodek, B., & Saracho, O. M. (1991). Issues in sions: openness, conscientiousness, extraversion,
Early Childhood Curriculum. New York, London: agreeableness, and neuroticism.
Teachers College Press. Davis’ Multidimensional Model of Em-
Stoll, L., & Fink, D. (2000). Changing our schools pathy: Is four-dimensional model for empathy,
– How to enhance efficiency and quality of our consisted of perspective taking, personal distress,
schools. Zagreb: Educa. fantasy and empathic concern.
Empathy: Is ability for identifying and un-
Sudec, K. (2012): Where to sit? Co-creational derstanding other people’s problems, situations
process of learning with inner architecture of and feelings.
school. Sodobna pedagogika. 1/2012. 140-154. Eysenck’s Personality Model: Is a model
Tatalović Vorkapić, S. (2012). The significance of personality types which examines conative
of preschool teacher’s personality in early child- aspects, emotions, cognitive behaviour, and body
hood education: Analysis of Eysenck’s and Big configuration; which in turn shapes personality
Five Dimensions of personality. International traits: extraversion/introversion, emotional stabil-
Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, ity/neuroticism and psychoticism.
2(2), 28–37. doi:10.5923/j.ijpbs.20120202.05 Hidden Curriculum: Is all everyday present
routines and micro phenomenon’s in educational
Tatalović Vorkapić, S. i Puljić, I. (2013). Impor- work with children, from the way the kindergarten/
tance of students’ personality traits for their future school teacher forms environment for learning to
work with preschool children. Metodički obzori, his own values, opinions, attitudes, personality
8(2), 34-46. which he brings in his professional work.
Tatalović Vorkapić, S., & Lončarić, D. (2013). Implicit Learning: Is process where we be-
Does the professional burnout mediate the effects come sensitive for certain rules in environment,
of personality traits on life satisfaction among but with highlighted intention absence for learning
preschool teachers? Psychological Topics, 22(3), and absence of willing to actually learn, that all
431–455. together results with kind of knowledge that is
hard to express and articulate.
Tatalović Vorkapić, S. & Ružić, N. (2013). Personality: Is a specific combination of
Measuring empathy in future preschool teachers: emotional, attitudinal, and behavioral response
Implications for study programme modification. patterns of an individual.
International journal of psychology and behav- Temperament: Is the set of relatively stable
ioral sciences, 3(6), 188-195. characteristics of an individual, rather biologically
Tatalović Vorkapić, S., Vujičić, L., & Čepić, determined which manifest in formal aspects of
R. (2014). Preschool, Teacher Identity. Book reactions resulted (in this case) from the com-
Chapter to be Appeared. In P. Breen (Ed.), Cases bination of Eysenck’s basic personality traits:
on Teacher Identity, Diversity, and Cognition in extraversion and neuroticism.
Higher Education. IGI-Global. doi:10.4018/978-
1-4666-5990-2.ch002

242
243

Chapter 12
Unraveling a Progressive
Inquiry Script in Persistent
Virtual Worlds:
Theoretical Foundations and Decision
Processes for Constructing a Socio-
Cultural Learning Framework

Nikolaos Pellas
University of the Aegean, Greece

ABSTRACT
The radical utilization of collaborative learning processes in Three-Dimensional (3D) multi-user virtual
worlds has been widely investigated. However, a study to delineate a comprehensive pedagogical model
aimed at facilitating the same productive-argumentative knowledge practices is still lacking. This chapter
presents the most reliable principles of contemporary socio-cognitive learning theories and teaching
models. These socio-cognitive constructs can reinforce the majority of collaborative social-cultural
students’ pursuits and capabilities in the learning process in order to better handle their cohesion or
coordination with other peers in 3D multi-user virtual worlds. Consequently, with the adaptation of the
Progressive Inquiry (PI) teaching model, this chapter proposes a novel framework for the implementa-
tion of this model in online collaborative processes that can be held in 3D multi-user virtual worlds.
Educational implications for a reputable instructional format with the conjunction of the PI model and
further suggestions are also discussed.

INTRODUCTION sources arranged from various electronic forms


is currently well-known today. Many facilitating
The fact that e-learning is one nascent and in- ways, such as the abduction of information or
novative course delivery method for students’ production-based literacy in online services have
access to an abundance of Web-based services and resulted as an ever-continuing wave, supporting

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-6599-6.ch012

Copyright © 2015, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

various educational applications. Students can • The assumption that every student as a
gain knowledge from a medium used in order to member of the community that has differ-
be implemented learning process at a distance ent ways of approaching the knowledge
(computer, internet, e-learning platforms) which should also be considered as an important
created in a different theoretical edifice in which reason for the featuring familiarization
students converged should try to co-exist and co- with the environment.
construct through their own concepts an innovative • The balance between different approaches
new “knowledge field”. Thus, this dimension has of knowledge, such as empirical, prescrip-
prompted researchers to determinate collaborative tive, and self-organized learning.
practices in order to facilitate a more sustainable • The principles of “internal” (user-based)
future for e-Education. or “external” (team-based) evaluation
A growing academic literature body (Trush- may import the original specification,
ell, Byrne & Simpson, 2012; Tsai, 2007; Tsai & for rethinking new packages of innova-
Chuang, 2005; Wang & Reeves, 2005; Wang et tive educational programs for all levels of
al., 2006) has already addressed to the emerging education, aimed at satisfying the princi-
issues of the online learning with some interesting ple of all-round development of students’
principles. These are the following: personality.

• The conformation of users to utilize mul- Despite the use of this great innovation from
timedia and online learning environments. the vast majority of other media sources, it is
• The principle of an active learning process also crucial to decode some parameters for their
through an educational context. In this ap- effectiveness that include:
proach, users convoked several teaching
practices based on their active construction • The recognition of a unique self-existence
of knowledge for each group according to that pervaded each student.
their personal needs and demands. • The movement of knowledge from the
• The principle of cooperativeness can fos- “traditional” (instructive) approach to a
ter the interaction between students and new one, where team members acting col-
the instructor’s facilitating role, in order lectively and harmoniously together for a
to provide the necessary feedback for their common purpose.
actions. However, considerable care must • The gradual dissociation (fading scaffold-
be given to the “space” or the “environ- ing) of experts through active processes
ment” which should allow the positive in- and active supply for the construction of a
terdependence, responsibility and use of situated knowledge field.
students’ (e-)skills to be solved problem- • The distribution of projects and construc-
based learning situations. tive applications standards of learning
• The principle of propagation and equal op- through the collaborative exchange of
portunities for all students through a het- knowledge and users’ experiences that is
erogeneous group. necessary for the implementation of a suc-
• The recognition of each student’s person- cessful program.
ality, as a unique entity and personality
for his/her participation in collaborative Nowadays, the integration of ICT (Information
activities. and Communication Technologies) in the field

244

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

of Education can actually contribute to learning, • Presenting a macroscopic or microscopic


but only when it is used in appropriately well- complexity of virtual “spaces,” “places”
designed activities with clearly defined objectives. even from a real or root from a fantastic
Therefore, to be meaningful this approach, several world.
educational computer applications should be based • Providing multiple versions of reality by
explicitly or implicitly on learning theories and avoiding the accessible learning process
psycho-pedagogical theories that reflect the real (instructive approach) that privileges the
needs of the educational community. On the other one-sided dimension of a single reality.
side, the emergence of innovative educational • Accommodating simultaneously experi-
technologies has caused serious changes in the ences, according to the primary description.
present educational system. Computer-supported • Focusing on the active construction of
collaborative learning (CSCL) has proved to be knowledge, since each user can provide
as one of the most eligible and reliable contem- the ability to represent knowledge direct-
porary approaches that convenes basic principles ly from the system and on the other side,
of learning progressions between users (instructor the system should handle errors (feedback
and students), both in electronic (e-)learning en- prompt) or provide the appropriate tools to
vironments and virtual worlds. Especially for the solve the problem.
latter category, the emphasis on co-constructing • Supporting the collaboration and interac-
substantial social characters with cyber entities tion between users (spatially distributed or
(avatars), in conjunction with performances of new not) in a common “place” or “space” of ac-
roles through multiple-communication channels or tion, where the storage of information can
the study or the study of utilizing computational be done automatically in order to be reused
tools, has brought changes in the traditional “status the learning material. However, in this case
quo” of e-Education. it is sufficient that the collective form of
The technological infrastructure that supported work must not overlap the offer from each
at the traditional 2D LMS (Learning Management person in the group.
Systems) and subsequent developments of their • Including a range of authentic or at least
descendants, i.e. 3D systems are beyond [C-] pragmatic activities that resemble those
VEs ([Collaborative-] Virtual Environments) that made in reality.
that also includes virtual worlds; they seemed to
offer several advantages for a learning process Perhaps an important skill acquired by users
in blended/online collaborative settings mainly during their involvement in 3D technologically-
regards. Virtual worlds have “a natural appearance advanced environments is the upgraded percep-
of realness” through to an interactive 3D multi- tions for the accurate decisions, even in a short
user “space” or “place,” in which distributed users period of time. Users in each learning activity
may recreate moments of their private life with that takes place in a virtual environment amidst
their “alter-ego” (avatars). a dipole covered not only for their responses to
Creating learning environments in virtual the environment, but also to their actions with the
worlds can provide a different and often success- proper or quick decision to solve a problem based
ful cognitive achievement of goals or learning on the stimuli during their performance to gain or
outcomes through reliable properties, because it enhance a learning skill (Karim & Sanza, 2013).
can be easily: In an attempt to better understand the implicit

245

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

learning consequences, some researchers (Gabett the definition given that even the recent research
& Masters, 2011; Jackson & Farrow, 2005) have has not been found yet in several outlets to be
tried to discover innovative behavioral models of developed a theoretical learning framework based
visual search which may had a direct correlation to on students’ decisions and foundations and to
the learning material that students need to conquer empower the cohesion of team-based in a teach-
in the environment. ing process held in 3D multi-user virtual worlds.
For Baddeley (2003) implicit learning can be In the present research investigates the decision
achieved by the distraction of a secondary stimulus. making situations in collaborative processes. This
During the latent process, it is not involving the case however, includes two types of learning: (a)
active memory, but allows students to perform the implicit practice is one in which there is no
alongside other functions such as decision mak- declarative knowledge and is not done intention-
ing and c cooperation, if the action created in ally (Raab, 2003) and (b) the explicit or marked
social contexts is considered as very important. In practice generated when the target is defined by
contrast to the manifest process that involves the verbal manner (Perring, 1996) through rules that
active memory, consciously by creating declara- describe how “if you do this - then it will become
tive knowledge. With the execution that requires another” (if - then).
simultaneous processes and multiple information The medley of social, psychological and bio-
sources, the control system is getting overloaded logical factors has led the contemporary psycho-
and can reduce the performance. Other research- pedagogical research to explore a new field of
ers (Jackson & Farrow, 2005; Zhang, Patricia de scientific approaches and teaching practices re-
Pablos & Xu, 2013) have argued that the implicit garding the redefinition of socio-cognitive frame-
practice enables the use of several sources of infor- works for adult learners’ participation through the
mation that help correct and fast decision making, use of new technologies for the recruitment of
particularly in situations of fatigue or anxiety. knowledge. Despite the main founders of learning
Previous research efforts (MacMahon & Masters, theories which have placed the phenomenon of
2002; Jackson & Farrow, 2005) have shown that learning in social experiences, responsibilities and
training with implicit way (subconsciously) has a roles of adults in a socio-cultural environment in
positive effect on the students’ development and relation to current theories that explain learning
improvement of their perception or cognitive skills. with multimedia resources, and probably boundar-
Cleeremans and Jimenez (2002) have defined the ies or learning situations, until now a framework
implicit learning as the process where humans are that endorses inquiry-based activities and decision
becoming sensitive to certain regularities in the processes is still infrequent. A fundamental prac-
environment: (a) in the absence of intention to tically-infused implication is the understanding
learn about those regularities, (b) in the absence of theoretical foundations and decision processes
of awareness that one is learning, and (c) in such according to a progressive inquiry model script in
a way that the resulting knowledge is difficult to motivation affords educators or scholars an insight
be expressed. into students’ needs and demands and is a useful
However the research problem which remains precursor to targeting students and in effectively
unknown is how educators and scholars find new fulfilling these needs.
methods guiding students to acquire the prospect- The contribution of this study represents one
ing information based on collaborative learning of the most expressions initial attempts to better
models in order to reach students foster the per- understand the students’ decisions via a progres-
ceptive understanding of the learning material in sive inquiry (PI) script in virtual worlds, and better
a learning environment. It is very important that inform at least theoretically some understandings

246

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

based on students’ needs or decisions in a tech- users (students and instructors) that must
nologically-advanced multi-user environment. be taken into account from educational
The added value of this novel framework that is developers, instructors and scholars for de-
proposed in this study was focused on its peda- signing, implementing, and enhancing the
gogical use in virtual worlds which in their turn proposed innovative models through vir-
have become even more useful for distance (col- tual learning environments.
laborative) learning courses, where the separated
spatially and temporally trainees: (a) working in The core aim of this study is twofold: (a) to
groups and co-construct knowledge, (b) coexist- review the recent literature of socio-cognitive
ing in a common virtual space and interact via contemporary learning theories and teaching
real time or asynchronous communication form, models, as roots that can be endorsed in a CSCL
which often offered (bulletin board, written text, script and (b) to articulate a novel methodological
voice dialing, etc.). During the teaching process framework according to the Progressive Inquiry
can be achieved more easily the re-organization model for 3D multi-user virtual worlds.
of students’ prior knowledge, the revision of
their beliefs and creating new representations.
Essentially the design of these environments BACKGROUND
for educational purposes should be based on an
analysis of pedagogical requirements which can Contemporary Learning Theories
be adopted and implemented to support and may
further cause the enhancement of social interac- The selection, presentation and exploration of
tions among users, and thus a teaching-didactic contemporary socio-cultural learning theories
framework should be considered at the beginning are not random in this study. Thus, it was pro-
of each learning process. Moreover, the frame- vided their development from those theories and
work that can be proposed should successfully models which considered as more necessary for
connect pedagogical principles and technologies collaborative activities in modern environments
that can offer users supporting functions such as given the fact that:
information display, communication, cooperation
and learning. • A study, which can adequately clarify the
Based on the preceding discussion, the outline common components that should be fo-
objectives of this study are: cused on a contemporary social-cultural
research is still lacking. More specifically,
• To point out affordances from the exist- a socio-cognitive framework considered as
ing virtual worlds as candidate platforms urgent to investigate, formulate and under-
for e-learning processes emphasized to the stand users (instructors and students) clear-
exploration, exploitation and transfer of ly the organizational-educational contexts
knowledge in a 3D common virtual place. which are needed.
• To present the design principles, features, • The theoretical evaluation of digital sourc-
and collaborative sets of a Progressive es can lead to the selection of socio-cultur-
Inquiry (PI) model, relating to construct- al boundaries of learning models that must
ing a collaborative inquiry-based script for be defined or amplified more easily if the
virtual worlds. instructors and scholars have known the
• To identify a set of action strategies, criti- basic axes for their utilization.
cal success, and weighty decisions from

247

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

• From the beginning of this study the path are impacts, conditions and interactions. Devel-
of knowledge that should be followed and opments in the field of cognition have led to the
lead to some new data incorporated in the awareness of the social learning nature and im-
field of learning which is described exten- portance of the framework for mental activities.
sively. It should not be forgotten that at this By this extension, the shifted interest both in the
stage it is even more necessary to clarify organization of cooperative activities around the
a teaching model that may respond to the human is knowledge and computer interaction
learning needs addressing, not only inter- (Littleton & Light, 1999; Thompson & Lynch,
ests but also students’ problems. 2004) in collaborative learning settings through
computers was already recognized by scholars (Li
Socio-Cultural Collaborative et al. 2011; Wang, 2010). The cognitive theory
Principles via Electronic Sources stressed the need to design learning environments
that may support the interaction between students
The valuable proliferation of collaborative learn- (users) and facilitate the collaborative learning
ing (CL) is characterized with some positive ef- process (Järvenoja & Järvelä, 2009).
fects, according to a plethora of researches. This Instead, theories that support CSCL approaches
approach aimed to afford the contributing role of can be used for the implementation of these courses
cooperative learning versus the instructive learn- transferring the knowledge domain. Moreover,
ing approach (Dillenbourg, 1999; Hernández-Leo it’s crucial to observe that the “socio-cognitive
et al., 2004; Lehtinen, 2003; Strijbos, Martens & theories,” revolved around to the pole of the “hu-
Jochems, 2004). There is growing common con- man” and “cultural considerations of society.” In
sensus among the same researchers for the posi- general, these theoretical approaches have argued
tive effect of collaboration in learning; however a that the construction of knowledge can take place
questioning point of view about the mechanisms in collaborative environments through discussions
and reasons of why this occurs is still infrequent. that include the creation and understanding of
According to the above from the review of communication (between users or groups) for the
the literature, it can be distinguished alternative implementation of learning activities. In socio-
approaches for collaborative learning. These are cultural approaches, where it should be given
as follows: more emphasis, the basic assumption is that when
a person participated in a social system, then the
1. Incentive-motivation and social cohesion of “culture” of this system and “cognitive” tools
team-based activities. utilized as for communication forms, operating as
2. Cognitive approaches, i.e. socio-constructiv- a source of learning and development, not only for
ist approach (based on the theory of Piaget an individual, but also for the team (or the com-
and mechanism of cognitive conflict). munity). The characteristics of these approaches
3. Socio-cultural approaches as descendants of summarized as follows:
learning theories (influenced by Vygotsky’s
theory and approach of the Distributed • The active learning construction contrib-
Learning theory). uted to in-depth understanding.
• The learning progression may take place
On the other hand, Dillenbourg (1996) have in a socio-cognitive place in the context
argued three broods of concepts and mechanisms of autonomous activity, mental and social
which CSCL attempts to interpret the cognitive support.
progress about the collaborative learning. These

248

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

• The community, in which learning must the historical and social dimension in which the
help students to disseminate cultures and operation occurred and displayed in two several
practices. ways: (a) at the social, and then (b) at the personal
• The conversation (discourse) enabled to level (Vygotsky, 1978). The core of Vygotsky’s
the participation and negotiation among theoretical construct is that the social interaction
members of the community. determines a fundamental role in the development
of knowledge. The importance of social factors
The combination of social, psychological and and humans’ environments in the development
biological factors has led the modern pedagogi- and direction of learning activities, influenced
cal research to explore a new field of scientific and defined the character and evolution, based
approaches and educational practices related to on (Daniels, 2001; John-Steiner & Mahn, 1996;
the redefinition of learners’ use of contemporary Wertsch, 1985):
technologies for the recruitment of knowledge.
Although, main founders of adult learning theories • The guided learning process is understand-
(Kirschner, Martens, & Strijbos, 2004; Mezirow, able as the construction of knowledge dur-
1978; Onrubia & Engel, 2009) have placed the ing the discussion process, under the in-
phenomenon of learning in social experiences, structor’s guidance.
responsibilities and roles of participants in the • The fading scaffolding is referred as the
social-cultural environment, or on prior experi- main support and assistance of the instruc-
ences in relation to current theories that explain tor to his/her tutors for the construction of
learning with multimedia. Nowadays, the integra- the new knowledge. The instructor in this
tion of ICT in the field of Education has contributed case is not one person that simply gives to
to students’ participation, but only when properly the student a rich learning environment,
used through well-structured activities with clearly helping to be growth their self-develop-
defined objectives. ment, but it refers as an active “facilitator”
The change of the theoretical background (mentor) of social and cultural meanings
that most innovative learning environments that negotiates with the student and helps
can replicate has dictated the movement from a him/her internalize all that support his/her
teacher-centered instruction to an exploratory, socio-cognitive development.
independent based on collaborative interactions
among users. Therefore, effective computer-based The contribution of Vygotsky’s theory in the
educational applications should be based explicitly teaching process, have two notable consequences:
or implicitly on theories of learning and psycho-
pedagogical theories that reflect on the real needs • The establishment of circumstances under
of the educational community (Huang, 2002). the principle of cooperativeness among
groups with different skill levels and socio-
Social Constructivism cognitive background.
• The prominent principles of assistance
With the Social constructivism theory Lev Vy- from the gradual reduction of experienced
gotsky (1896-1934) emphasized to the internal people, which are gradually taking more
principles of humans’ thoughts in collaborative and more accountability from other stu-
activities and external communications for their dents, a well-known approach called “fad-
coordination (Rogoff et al., 1995). A human mental ing scaffolding” learning procedure.
development is an inseparably linked process to

249

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Computer-Supported Collaborative social action. The research for personal mean-


Learning (CSCL) Theory ings in modern societies and reflections on what
knowledge can be considered as valid and how
A more prior version of the social construction different approaches have greater severity in the
and collaboration for the knowledge acquisition context of each situation, without canceling the
was the social theory of Stahl (2003) that intro- other approaches (Van Aalst, 2006; Van de Vold,
duced its own version of social theory issues about 2010) were highlighted with:
computer-supported collaborative learning, as
users selectively worked in an attempt to be clear 1. The importance of the dialogue and negotia-
synthesized insights from their ongoing dialogue. tion between humans.
The social theory based on an extensive analysis 2. The epistemological beliefs that partici-
of high school students in a learning activity with pants in the dialogue must be tolerant to the
simulation software (Stahl, 2012). The proposed multiplicity of interpretations. This change
model provides an examination of how social was not only proclaimed only for skills or
interactions through concepts, such as tool, status, abilities achieved, but also for the identifica-
performance, intuitive knowledge, perspectives tion of personal meaning through a learning
that can be internalized between users. procedure.
This model examines (Stahl, Koschmann, &
Suthers, 2006; Stahl, 2012; Suthers, 2006): The CSCL approach refers to some of the
most novel principles for contemporary learning
• The sequence of collaborative interactions environments (Bransford, Brown & Cocking,
among users. 1999; Wang, 2006) with:
• The reason that knowledge can be shared
and developed in this interactive knowl- • Emphasizing on the social nature of learn-
edge field. ing and knowledge through the co-con-
• The negotiating meaning of tools and arti- struction of knowledge.
facts that students utilized. • Strengthening students’ performance in
• These meanings make up a social world these new roles as team members gained
in which users operated to understand the an innovative knowledge domain together.
new knowledge in collaborative settings. • Supporting communication and computa-
The current model includes issues of epis- tional tools or artifacts that facilitate the
temology, semiotics, hermeneutics and learning process sufficiently.
ontology.
In general, studies that access to the imple-
Computer-Supported Collaborative mentation of this example have demonstrated not
Learning Approaches only opportunities but also limitations (Lehtinen
et al, 1999). Specifically, the same students they
The added value of CSCL may reinforce by social distinguished technological, organizational and
and cultural changes (i.e. the strong interaction pedagogical constraints, for establishing suc-
of people from different cultural traditions for the cessful collaborative environments. A socially
production and usability of knowledge) which are interactive environment in which users can imple-
currently experiencing the views and practices of ment collaborative scenarios, may allow a deeper

250

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

processing of knowledge through the discussion • Designing collaborative learning activities.


with cognitive, meta-cognitive strategies, and with • Modeling practices of knowledge gaits, ac-
the expression of emotions. cording to participant needs.
The conditions for the realization of the above • Receiving information, tools and knowl-
are aware of the educational function that students edge configuration from the natural or arti-
have not only from their prior knowledge, from the ficial environment.
creation of “norms” (i.e. key rules are a manda- • Encouraging the facilitation of knowledge
tory requirement for the holder of a position in the based on groups’ demands.
society which are certain rights and obligations • The assessing of the state order and knowl-
on members’ expectations of a community or a edge base established.
group and procedures of a classroom’s routine). • Identifying a moral leadership from the in-
Otherwise, the understanding of knowledge is structor or experts.
no longer the center of the class activity, and the • Negotiating the new knowledge field by
argument degenerates to quote opinions and digital achieving the triggering event with other
resources that support the CSCL as “a modern peers.
way of communication between students”. This
premise has been frequently recapitulated to the However, with the anticipated growth and
empowerment of new students’ roles (Figure 1). development of modern environments, which are
The operation of an order accompanied by the more responsive to the demands and needs of users
implementation of multiple team responsibilities, for collaborative learning, there is a heightened
such as (Tsai, Shen & Tsai, 2011; Tung & Chang, interest and development of instructional models
2007; Turhan, Yaris & Nural, 2005; Twedden which can in nowadays inspire a meaningful
Levinsen, 2007): learning procedure.

Figure 1. The circle of an e-collaboration approach

251

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

TEACHING MODELS learning by using computers and exploring the


area in collaborative learning settings, at least in
It is essential as the time passing to understand the last decade characterized by a series of devel-
the difference between theories of learning and opments that have shaped a different landscape
models for the acquisition of knowledge based in the area of intelligence. There have been many
on philosophical theorems, where any depth after others in a theoretical or researching level, such as
years of research leading to some conclusions. (Brown, Collins & Duguid, 1989; Gunawardena,
The models on the other side must: et al. 2010; Lee & Tan, 2010; Stacey, 2002) who
recognized the following:
• Clarify the process of different learning
pathways. • The failure of education systems, which
• Underline the effective use that requires a do not yield the expected results, without
specialized knowledge from the instructor motivating students to participate in team-
in order to implement an organizational- based activities.
teaching plan. • Awareness of learning differences, practic-
• “Prototype” the theory through an ener- es inside and outside educational settings,
getic gait that should be followed by the in- with parallel activities directly mirrored on
structor and students, as it is a continuous students’ needs and interests.
sequence that describes the whole work- • Highlighting limitations of the dominant
flow of activities. paradigm for the cognitive science. This
period tended to eliminate data that are
On the other side, models for the systematic now starting to be associated with socio-
investigation ratified the epistemological cores psychological theories, explaining the en-
of learning perceptions that refer to the imple- ergetic situation of students in cooperative
mentation and teaching level of activities. For the settings.
purposes of this study, it was referred as models, • The variety of empirical findings on how
because these teaching approaches combine learn- the context is interdependent and inextrica-
ing theories and teaching practices (Thomas & bly connected with the intellect, highlight-
Storr, 2005). Meanwhile, Lipponen and Lallimo ing the importance of social interaction on
(2004) had as a starting point that is difficult to cognitive development.
interpret research results in a CSCL script focused • The obvious interest in how people learn
on successful examples of collaborative learning, other cultures beyond their social-lin-
in order to identify common elements between guistic, the research and study models of
them. In particular, they focused on developing teaching and learning cultures outside the
knowledge and authentic learning. This analysis Western world (e.g. communities of prac-
showed the successful use of collaborative tech- tice and studied and translated the work of
nologies has a common denominator that can Vygotsky in English that led to the renewal
be identified through proper design practices, in of relevant interest).
which collaborative technologies used.
Collaborative learning has been a key com- The Progressive Inquiry Model
ponent of contemporaries Information and Com-
munication Technologies (ICT) transactions, but As Van Joolingen, De Jong, and Dimitrakopoulou
these were the organic part, being a purveyor of (2007) have first noticed that IBL (inquiry-based
the service end. Although, the phenomenon of learning), can engage learners’ approaches to build

252

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

their personal approaches offered in an area closer cent findings collaboratively. Another background
to the perception and reality. De Jong (2006) has goal for the evaluated project was to advance
also alleged that the engagement of students comes virtual learning practices on all levels in Finnish
from: orientation, main hypothesis, experimenta- schools (Ministry of Education, 1999), and from
tion, creating models and theories and last but not this was emerged a need to develop models for
least evaluation. Participation in inquiry-based practical applications of technology supported
activities, include: virtual learning in real school contexts.
Meanwhile, there are few studies considered
1. The critical examination of practices. a collaborative knowledge building in distance
2. The development of comprehensive and learning situations. In this study, one aim was
relevant to students’ aspects knowledge. to give students an experience of technology-
3. The production of a “practical” understand- supported virtual working. Therefore, challenges
ing of what they do and what are finally of organizing the learning community in the project
trying to achieve. can be compared to the challenges for building
virtual communities in general. Virtual commu-
This model is an extension based on the model nities characterized as successful when elements
of “knowledge building” that Scardamalia & Be- like sharing goals and resources, students’ active
reiter (1994) have suggested, but also it enriched participation and reciprocal interaction, sense of
with dynamic and pragmatic (or at least authentic) belonging, trust in others shared through social
dimensions of the scientific investigation. The conventions (Preece, 2000). The development of
same authors have also stressed that schools should a networked community requires the management
be restructured towards the “knowledge building” of the mechanisms that “describe the general
in which students and teachers participated in the decision-making, responsibility allocating, and
construction of a collective knowledge in research communication methods that will guide the group”
groups where the object of an activity is a prob- (Schuler, 1996, p. 338).
lem-solving learning tasks. Instructors of Higher Students in CSCL may argue that the participa-
Education, nowadays have an ambitious goal of tion in virtual learning environments, according
introducing students to practice with collaborative to students’ needs, can create a strong sense of
knowledge building in learning communities. As “presence” in the community (Lakkala, IIomaki,
Hakkarainen and Sintonen (2002) have reported, Lallimo, & Hakkarainen, 2003). Progressive
the basic idea behind the research is that learn- inquiry consists of a series of individual steps
ing analogically to the scientific discovery and (Muukkonen, Hakkarainen & Lakkala, 1999;
formulation of theories can be considered as a Lehtinen, 2003): (a) a framework, (b) participation
process of working towards a comprehensive and in research questions, (c) creation of contemporary
thorough understanding of an issue (phenomenon, theories, (d) critical evaluation of the knowledge’s
or problem etc). promotion, (e) search for new scientific infor-
The progressive inquiry model which devel- mation (f) participating in a deeper exploratory
oped by Muukkonen, Hakkarainen, and Lakkala process, and (g) sharing experience and expertise.
(1999), can be a purposeful pedagogical model This model firstly has been used in Computer
for promoting knowledge-building practices in Supported Intentional Learning (CSILE) and
schools with the support of CSCL. In this notion, Knowledge Forum (evolution of CSILE), which
students engaged in a research learning process includes units (modules) that support the step
by defining problems and proposing theoretical progressive investigation that mentioned above
approaches by information sources and explain re- (Rahikainen, Lallimo, & Hakkarainen, 2001).

253

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

This process of progressive inquiry (Michail, 1. The (spatial) immersion in an environment


Teal, & Basta, 2006; Muukkonen Hakkarainen created and endorsed users’ impressions in
& Lakkala, 1999) attempts to engage students in a 3D virtual space.
a self-directed inquiry process to: 2. The interaction suggested that objects in the
virtual world responding absolutely to users’
1. State their initial and often “fuzzy” questions. actions and influence their participation in
2. Produce and work on “theoretical the activity.
foundations”. 3. The sense of imagination to encompass in a
3. Evaluate and redirect their inquiry process 3D virtual place the ability of human’s mind
collaboratively. to compose mental images of situations that
4. Generate subordinate research questions. do not exist in reality, but expressed in desires
5. Produce explanations from the whole learn- or goals of a specific subject.
ing community.
The term “virtual world” should not be con-
The facilitating role of the instructor for col- fused with the term virtual reality which has a more
laborative and inquiry-based learning approaches general meaning. This term often identified with
is to create the appropriate framework, providing the term virtual reality valuable for exploration,
anchors between the theoretical representations describing in this way all data and processes that
and the real-life experiences that students report, make up a virtual reality system, while in other
and keep the process active and in focus during cases used to describe the portrayed elements and
the progression of the course. objects (dimensional model) of an environment
(Bishop, Gary, Henry, & Fuchs, 1992). Virtual
reality employed primarily to the mechanisms
VIRTUAL WORLDS AND through which people can interact with com-
VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS puter simulations, but it does not have time zone
schedules or activities. Virtual worlds often con-
The virtual reality (VR) is an innovative tech- sidered as 3D multi-user computer-generated and
nological breakthrough. It has enabled humans persistent environments that avatars can coexist
to overcome the traditional way of interacting and communicate with others, but in reality can
with their personal computers (PCs) through be increasingly utilized as powerful platforms
a visually-rich media interface, with a mouse of education and business. Based on broadband
and keyboard. Moreover, all users enter (“move technologies and channeling images (streaming)
through the computer screen”) and navigate to a from of the company or the administrator’s servers
three-dimensional (3D), artificially-infused world. to the end-user computer (PC).
Virtual reality reported as a “ticket to enter in an- It is crucial to observe that there is no clear
other world” (Pimentel & Teixeira, 1993, p. 45), and strict definition of the term “virtual world”
or as “an open world where people’s minds are or “virtual environment,” and so we try to give
the only limitation” (Minsky, 1975, p. 211). The below, some of the most prevalent. The term
concept of virtual reality is defined through the itself is controversial while this sometimes leads
user’s experience without limits. Specifically, the to misunderstandings and apparently more hours
main aspects that bring virtual reality to a simula- of philosophical discussions. With the average
tion environment are (Burdea & Coiffet, 2003): “virtual environment”, it is definitely a computer

254

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

system based on applications of 3D VR technol- co-presence.” This concept refers to the collab-
ogy, which simulates in the PC screen a “physical” orative effort, coexistence and common presence
space or place, like this in real life and aims to between distributed users in a virtual community
present a visually interactive and multi-sensory that continues to exist, even after a user exits from
“world,” which is exclusively for users and influ- the environment that grew and user-made changes
ence their actions in a positive way. to its state are-to some extent-permanent. The in-
A virtual world (VW) referred as the result teractive and social forms of modeling allow and
of a virtual environment operational structure, influence the design of learning activities, in ac-
including the representation of a natural environ- cordance with the contemporary social-cognitive
ment and “variants”. Fantastic placements are theories of learning. The rich social features, offer
users’ “virtual rooms” freely operated without to participants the option of digital communica-
restrictions and commitments. This distinction tion (via 3D VoIP) and sharing of experiences,
clearly indicates that virtual world’s rules for within a 3D multi-user “natural space or place,”
building and interpreting data is supporting the in which the visually- rich graphical user interface
operation of the “physical” environment as a causes a sense of co-construction of knowledge
computer system. A 3D environment hosted on in a global digital community.
a server or even in a form of software, which is 2. Open Source Virtual Worlds (OSVWs): The
stored on the personal computer’s (PC’s) hard polymorphic dimensions of an OSVW (Open
disk, giving users the real impression that should Simulator, Realxtend, Reaction Grid etc.) refer
be established on a “solid state,” where every- to the “technocratic nature” depicted through to
thing looks and moves normally. Moreover, the its freeware open source server platform. Many
most distinguished contribution that 3D VR lays of these worlds are working in a tandem compat-
is the ease managing for one or more subjects, ibility with other open source also with the Second
even when it involves hazardous or inadequate Life’s client. An OSVW is written in visual C#
processes, which could not be performed in a real and designed to be easily expanded through the
environment. In this context, students learn from use of plug-in that operated in two modes: (a)
their mistakes, without dangerous effects on their standalone or (b) grid mode. In standalone mode,
actions, and they “produce” their own feedback a single process handles the entire simulation.
and reuse it (debugging). Although, in this “real- In grid mode, various aspects of the simulation
ity” that depicts through virtual worlds, we can separated among multiple processes, which can
distinguish two significant pillars that enroll all exist on different machines. Standalone mode
of these characteristics: is simpler to configure, but limited to a smaller
1. Social Virtual Worlds (SVWs): In the first number of users because only one member is also
category these virtual worlds have been created the administrator of the server-mode. Grid mode
for the communication, both of social media and has the potential to scale as the number of users
social networking friends based on the Web 2.0 grows. Approximately, the user is the administra-
transactions. The social aspect and interaction tor of the server and creates objects or introduces
of a SVW (Second Life, The Sims online, and new things like textures that are free of charge.
WeeWorld) have attracted in the last decade more Each user has access to the virtual world created
cyber entities (avatars), and do not offer to students by the administrator. An OSVW, moreover, uses
a longer isolation in front of a computer screen, an “open-ended” architecture known as “hyper
but it has many characteristics of “persistence grid,” allowing users to teleport between multiple

255

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

“Open Sim-based” virtual worlds by providing initial aim was to entertain the users (Normand et
a hyperlinked map which indexes public grids. al., 1999). MUVEs designed for the educational
This allows multiple links for public grids to community incorporate learning objects or prob-
retain teleportation to each other without being lems in a virtual environment or a 3D context
on the same grid. (Jones & Bronack, 2007). Users can explore the
environment and examine situations with 3D visual
Virtual Worlds in E-Education objects or artifacts. This environment provides
a-synchronous tools of communication between
Virtual reality can be described as an exceptionally users (Schroeder, 2008). Educational MUVEs
powerful tool in education (Hollins & Robbins, designed are to support research for practical
2008). Perhaps the most prestigious educational learning and conceptual understanding (Delwiche,
dimension of VR is the ability to provide “cy- 2006). Usually there is not being one right way
berspace” for exploration and not only to allow of completing this work or solving a problem,
students to study, as with a printed book, or something that makes many teachers reject for
browse as with the supernatant (Coyne, 1994). In their practices. Various solutions are justified, as
this respect, virtual reality actively promotes the in real life, but some solutions may be better than
experiential learning, combined with the experi- others, while great importance of their utilization
ence virtual environments, in which is varying the moderated on users’ cooperation (Peachy, 2007).
position, scale, density of information, interaction, Despite that MUVEs designed solely for
response, time and the degree of user’s engage- entertainment, on the other side, educational
ment (Doyle, 2008). MUVEs are:
At the conceptual approach the importance of
construction and development of a virtual reality • Creating online learning communities and
focused on students’ cognitive, spiritual, social initial can also be used for instructors’
and emotional situations is urgent (Yamashita, professional development (Bull & Kajder,
2006). According to Rovai (2001), instructors 2004).
who use interactive teaching methods can pro- • Engaging users in actions on scientific is-
mote interaction and sense of community among sues by promoting social interaction among
students. According to these dimensions, a virtual users (Kafai, 2006).
learning environment exploits and highlights the • Providing an easy to use virtual environ-
characteristics of teaching principles following ment and collaboration between members
the theory of social constructivism and the way in blended settings (Vadillo & Matute,
for the development of new teaching approaches 2009).
and theories. In an attempt to approach more • Promoting the scientific research (Clarke,
directly and exactly the separation of social and 2006).
open source virtual worlds in e-Education due to
their usage, it is important to keep in mind the 2. Collaborative virtual environments (CVEs):
following meanings: CVEs are 3D learning environments, spatially
1. Multi-user virtual environments (MUVEs): distributed, where students work in teams with
In the educational process introduced to check exceptional communication tools (e.g. VoIP, chat
if firstly can improve activities such as Distance text and gestures etc.). CVEs can transform PCs
Learning (Addison & O ‘Hare, 2008). A MUVE into three-dimensional navigable and populated
is a popular form of multimedia places, and its places, supporting collaborative work and social

256

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

entertainment. These types of worlds have a shared with a goal and strengthen achievements
virtual environment in a server that works as a through social activities that support the
virtual machine, where users can participate re- ongoing collaboration between users, in
gardless of age. Research in the area of Epistemol- order to succeed a goal.
ogy; placed the acquisition of knowledge within a • Providing flexibility and support multi-
system of interactions among individuals, social ple views of learning (formal or informal
and material context (Curtis & Lawson, 2001). The viewpoints) that referred to the provision
acquisition of knowledge does no longer depend of different aspects of the target or opin-
only on stimuli or cognitive processes of users, ions that adapted to serve users’ needs of
by a set of factors is consistent with the interac- users with different roles (Greenhalgh &
tion of users with his environment (Stenkuehler Benford, 1995b).
& Williams, 2006).
CVEs in these circumstances can become an However, a CVE that supports these factors
area of convergence and implementation of socio- is a demanding task. The main factors contribut-
cognitive learning theories, and developments in ing to this problem presented from Gutwin and
educational technology. The valuation of options Greenberg (2001), and these are:
during the learning process revealed through the
social interactions of disparate and anonymous • A CVE can reproduce only a part of the
teammates, both in the world, and beyond its perceptual information available in a “real”
limits. Some studies measured on students’ cog- environment.
nitive performance (Adams, 2010; Chi, Slotta, • The potential interaction in a CVE limited
& de Leeuw, 1994; Stahl, 2001; Vamvakoussi by the physical workplace, and often, of
&Vosniadou, 2004) have suggested that learning the supplied information in a natural envi-
requires the reorganization of prior knowledge, ronment which is may not be available in
the revision of old beliefs and reconstruction of a CVE.
new representations. The additional investigations
lead to the development of computer visualization Collaboration in a CVE is also necessary to be
learning environments, intelligent virtual reality aware of situations in a public workplace, aiming to
micro-worlds. achieve the objective. Students should try to solve
For the situation awareness, it must be con- problems that arose from everyday experience. In
stantly updated on changes in the environment addition, collaborative environments designed to
(Heath & Luff, 1996). The awareness is the situa- provide flexible issues and possibilities of users’
tion’s knowledge which is arising from exploration representations. The strategy focuses on depth
and interaction of students with the environment rather than to cover all the material in depth.
(Dourish & Bellotti, 1992). The awareness of a The CVE facilitates meta-cognitive awareness
situation as a secondary objective is crucial to by allowing students to express their comments
complete the learning goal. Just briefly, users need and representations of knowledge about an object
to know what happens (Dede, 2005) by: and then compare it with others (Dexter & Riedel,
2003). Students made aware of what they know
• Supporting the dialogue and communica- and what to learn. Meanwhile, the variety and
tion (negotiation and co-ordination): the information can offer solutions to synchronous
necessary transfer of information is essen- complex problems from different perspectives
tial to support cooperation and has to do (Wilson & Harris, 2004).

257

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Utilizing Learning Theories crucial where the learning takes place in context
with Virtual Worlds and applied learning should be active through this
experience. For Jarmon, Traphagan, Mayrath, and
Constructivist learning approaches, such as prob- Trivedi (2009), students learned in Second Life,
lem-based or collaborative design-based scripts and as a result it was proclaimed that can be an
or scenarios (Koutsabasis, Vosinakis, Malisova, effective environment for a project-based experi-
& Paparounas, 2012; Pellas & Kazanidis, 2013) ential learning approach, because the connection
can initially be utilized in virtual worlds, allow- between the real and the virtual world offering to
ing the implementation of experiential learning users tangible experiences.
procedures. There are appeared some other stud- As it was noticed above, a growing literature
ies that have used learning models. Vasileiou body has already identified contemporary innova-
and Paraskeva (2010) have used the cognitive tions of various learning theories and practices,
apprenticeship model based on Situated Learn- with emphasis on problem-based learning, col-
ing theory (Collins et al., 1989; Järvelä, 1995) laborative designing, experimental, experiential,
as an instructional model for teaching students and especially constructivist approaches. The
in collaborative practices to deal with realistic, prevalence and convergence of the Contemporary
complex, and daily learning tasks. By developing theories can make the learning procedure valu-
learning scenarios that involve team-based activi- able for the elicitation of new knowledge through
ties, instructors can help learners to benefit from virtual worlds.
the strengths or interests of other team members
and ultimately develop their own skills in order The Justification for Choosing
to feel more confident (Cram, Hedberg, Gosper the Progressive Inquiry Model
& Dick, 2011).
Benvenuti et al. (2008) have argued that ca- The Progressive Inquiry (PI) model can provide
pabilities of 3D virtual worlds beyond the con- to the educational research a prominent “context”
structive approach that can make each of these in which students engaged and discovered the new
environments useful for education and research, knowledge. The components of this “action-based
to persuade some others as well as aspects that framework” includes the potential relationships
do not. The same researchers have also found that between knowledge, values and management prac-
students’ engagement in Professional Education tices that lead to the social, cognitive and emotional
through virtual worlds with constructivist courses development, which has widely emphasized by the
was promising, in terms of their real-life experi- theorists of Cognitive Psychology. The framework
ence. Santos (2011) has framed for a situated that described earlier, requiring the construction
perspective with the utilization of virtual worlds of new analysis tools and modeling interactions
in education, something that Hayes (2006) has and includes four components:
already proposed through Second Life for adult
education. In another significant approach, Fal- 1. The criteria for determining the status of
coner (2012) has measured the ethical and practi- an individual or collaborative activity (in-
cal barriers that inherited in real-world exercises dividual or collective division of labor).
through virtual worlds and situated learning theory 2. The interactions occurred among users
with positive aspects. (negotiation cognitive resources, etc.).
Jestice and Kahai (2010) suggested that 3D 3. The performed procedures, such as the
worlds can offer unique experiences according construction of a conceptual model, the
to learning theories, but pointed out that it is foundation and concepts.

258

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

4. The effect of the activity in the learning A Rationale to Transfer the


process, (i.e. the cognitive outcome). A main Knowledge in Virtual Worlds
thing is to understand that the relationship
between these axes, whereas a state that Various applications of a 3D system designed
determines causes or different patterns of to assist learning and use the classified different
individualism and cooperation can trigger types of interaction for users, which achieved by
students’ cognitive mechanisms and produce immersing the user in the system with keyboard
the anticipated learning outcomes. and mouse. Many of these applications that men-
tioned above drawing ideas from the distributed
Summarizing the above that it is vital to iden- constructionism scattered, while allowing multiple
tify all those additional parameters (at least those users to be in a public place and simultaneously
which can be associated with the past or the current create the same time representation of the object
theories and formalities principles, regarding the investigated (Resnick, 1996b).
cognitive process under certain conditions) that From 1990 onwards, it seems that the baton
have neither been taken into account at all, nor was taken by Koster (2004), where he firstly
been adequately addressed with positive learning introduced the terms of:
outcomes. Indicative examples of this growing
concern, Hakkarainen, Palonen, Paavola, and 1. The “persistence” of an environment (i.e.
Lehtinen (2004) have believed that one of the a virtual world that continues to exist even
main dilemmas for the future is whether it can after a user exits from the world and that
be accepted the theoretical variety, diversity and user-made changes to its state are, to some
the need for integration and synthesis. It should extent, permanent).
be also noted that despite the problems with the 2. The concept of “multi-participation,” where
composition of previous research results (Rahi- multiple users enter into a public place or
kainen, Jarvela, & Salovaara, 2000; Rahikainen, area (3D cyberspace) at the same time with
Lallimo, & Hakkarainen, 2001) and the theoretical other cyber entities (avatars).
diversity, the example of collaborative learning
with computer support is a substantial improve- The element of “persistence” beyond the pres-
ment over the earlier examples. Specifically, some ent reference of the symmetric multi-purpose
elements of this approach, including: situation can be achieved by simultaneous and
independent use of various communication chan-
1. The emphasis on the meaning and develop- nels. The concept of “persistence” involves efforts
ment of a distributed work. of other users to foster self-organization of a virtual
2. The focus on social interaction and com- community, collective action frameworks and co-
munication between users’ via artifacts and responsibility for finding solutions to problems
visual tools. (Gamor, 2012). The plurality of interactive and
3. The analysis tools and mediated nature of multimedia elements make us describe a virtual
cognition. world as an “eco-system”, which based on the
4. The description as a model that shows the dis- environment that gives the user an environment
tributed knowledge-based CSCL approach. action, but also enhances human intuitive and
5. The technocratic and parallel the socio- “kinesthetic” interactive communication and in-
cultural infrastructure that encompasses. teraction of its members. It should not be denied

259

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

that most users enter in such worlds, more daring, quiring a new personality that helps her/
friendships, inclusion in community practice, him to overcome fears and anxieties that
which in real life if they could do. might leave him behind in his studies and
However, Hay et al. (2002) have presented a involvement.
variety of attempts to exploit the VR applications • Quality of services, as in many cases, users
in education with a top priority to link learning are starting to get bored with ordinary 2D
and knowledge through: applications and treat them as electronic
portfolios. Likewise, the corollary of inter-
1. The correlation and exploration concepts or activity and social formalization of mod-
elements necessary through three-dimen- eling, allow users to design learning ac-
sional representations. tivities, in conjunction with contemporary
2. The standardization or integration of mod- pedagogical approaches.
els can be raised to visualize events or • Promotion of the principles governing
phenomena. the implementation framework of mod-
3. The interaction between student-artifacts ern learning theories. As previously men-
to understand the added debt of beneficial tioned, the vast majorities of characteris-
cooperation in these environments. tics that virtual worlds enrolled are quite
encouraging for the active construction of
Many of these worlds identified as descen- knowledge and follow the footsteps of the
dants of 2D [A-] LMS ([Asynchronous] Learning Papert’s constructivist theory in “micro-
Management Systems), providing to users an worlds” (Papert, 1980).
alternative platform with multiple channels of
communication in order to facilitate the acquisition Functional Design Features
of knowledge by prototyping and modeling 3D
visual infrastructures like those of the real world. A plethora of virtual worlds (see Second Life,
At this point, we believe it is now imperative to Open Simulator, Reaction Grid etc.) are down-
analyze decision for the use of 2D LMS over that loadable and can be run in front of the computer
of the 3D virtual world. Thus, the points that seem screen, regarding the tools and services provided
to outweigh the latter category are the following: by each client viewer. Indeed, the tools can be a
formidable ally against the cognitive overload that
• The use of multimedia tools and multi-di- affects students, from their precise first steps into
mensional channels of communication for the virtual world. The deciphering is particularly
users who dispersed either by their physi- noteworthy as a wide range of client viewers, in
cal space or in the virtual environment. which users can download and “play” from one
• The freedom of choice and utilization of world to another, preserving their features, such as:
different tailored virtual spaces, according
to users’ needs and demands. This occurs • Functional design features for the commu-
particularly in the pre-constructed virtual nication among users via with synchronous
worlds, where the user has the authority and asynchronous forms and interactions
to define the framework that she/he really among users (3D VoIP, chat text, gestures
wants. IM).
• Highlighting the dynamic contribution of • Functional design features for the virtu-
each student through an iconic figure, ac- ally persistent workplace that can enhance

260

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

students’ awareness, which is beneficial (2003) has involved the use of feedback in online
because includes multiple forms of aware- learning environments with the overall impact on
ness, and presence in the virtual space. teamwork and a second proposed active construc-
• Functional design features that promote tion of knowledge in online learning environments
educational roles included the facilitating (Tsai, 2008). The lack of theoretical reflection on
role of the instructor and on the other the the part of the students expressed that they used,
instructor’s guidance to achieve student’s it expressed a lack of awareness and concepts
specific goals. that allow:
• Functional design features to facilitate the
managerial responsibilities of students’ • Watching new phenomena of social inter-
groups. This opportunity refers to the action and learning in the classroom.
operation during members’ cooperative • Managing the changes of roles and
tasks, in combination with activities that responsibilities.
take place in specially designated areas, • Monitoring the course by creating new
such as bulletin boards, boards for sharing knowledge courses with different episte-
documents and tools that can either exist mological backgrounds.
from the authority or should be created by
using the supplied programming language As a result, the current approach provides a
(virtual artifacts). pedagogical-theoretical framework for establish-
• Functional design features for custom- ing a collaborative practice, as well as studying
izing the display of multiple or complex and analyzing the cooperative behavior of coop-
representations of real life, and interactive erative groups. The result of this process allows
systems such as observation or phenom- a better understanding of the interactions within
ena that have functions similar to those of the group, and determines the best way to monitor
reality. and evaluate individual and team performances
• Functional design features for microscopic when users support a collaborative activity. Spe-
and macroscopic imaging of realistic or at cifically for this attempt, this study proposes a
least pragmatic situations through the mod- methodological design that guides to the process
eling and prototyping of virtual artifacts. of a social-cognitive learning framework (i.e.
instructors and students) throughout the duration.
Unveiling Several Emerging Issues Providing a well-designed methodology is not
only necessary for individual, but also for team
The successes of collaborative learning scenarios success that can be achieved at different levels:
depend to a large extent on the possibility of data team composition, development of collaborative
processing relating to teamwork, and the achieve- activity, monitoring, evaluation and support. The
ment of an efficient model for the implementation unique design for each of these levels depends on
a collaborative activity. This issue focuses to the the learning scenario that takes place.
attention of many researchers and designers of
collaborative environments (Tynjälä & Häkkinen, Factors and Research Interests
2005). In the literature, however, it was not yet Influencing the Choice to
identified a comprehensive and well-rounded Use Virtual Worlds
approach to provide efficient management and
processing activities that take place in a coopera- Nowadays, it is a common premise that the evo-
tive group activity. A first step from Thurmond lution of the personal computers’ technological

261

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

power and of course Web-based technologies has abundance that delighted is difficult to derive a
given the opportunity to develop users’ innova- common pedagogical model, which implement
tive environments both for entertainment and and incorporate the designing process.
learning. The new technological infrastructure In this view, it is necessary to present innovative
to accompany the rapid spread of broadband methods that it can be established an evaluation
networks and its additional developments, such framework with educational terms for each virtual
as Web 2.0 applications (blogs, wikis, and virtual environment. Frequently this can recapitulate
worlds) and the interactive distributed 3D virtual the expanded use of ICT in Higher Education in
environments (3DVEs) have changed far beyond conjunction with modern pedagogical theories
the traditional methods of e-Education. More- that emphasized to the inquiry-based learning;
over, the rapid development of electronic social problem-solving or decision-making processes
networking has changed the “cyberspace” in a influenced most strongly across the range of
rapidly growing communication system, bringing teaching and learning process and tend to change
various facets of the “networked collectivity.” the traditional way of (e-) educational practices.
The new technological infrastructure that virtual It is necessary to be underlined “the transfer of
environments support, can transform the existing knowledge” through virtual environments, in a
distributed networks, into habitable and navigable contemporary practical-teaching framework for
3D virtual locations. Indeed, these collaborative collaborative e-learning processes, suggesting the
works in simulated sharing experiences can be primarily carried out study between distributed
supported by participants with the average age, users. They usually employed in a learning activ-
showing the emerging relationship between hu- ity or a project, giving a more social interactive
man and computer communications. dimension to their experience.
The pedagogical value of 3D environments in The advantages of cooperation in this effort
recent years became even more useful for distance focused on conducting effective trainings, acquir-
learning courses were segregated spatially and ing new social skills and intercultural relations,
temporally users are being engaged in cooperative and finally increasing their self-esteem. It should
or collaborative tasks. These sessions are coex- be noted that through collaborative interactions
isting in common virtual spaces and interact via and involvement on users’ cognitive mechanisms
synchronous or asynchronous communication during the learning cycle, there is always some
formats, which often offered (bulletin board, “warranty” that produced before the learning
written text, dialing voice, etc.). Previous studies outcome.
(Duncan, Miller, & Jiang, 2012; Dickey, 2005; Within the field of e-Education, this belongs
2011; Pellas, 2012; Pellas & Kazanidis, 2013) on in this work expresses questions to instructors
students’ cognitive performance with the utiliza- and researchers of:
tion of virtual worlds have shown that during the
teaching process learners can achieve more easily • Which learning trends should be followed
the coordination of prior knowledge in order to in a CSCL script based on the PI model
revise antecedent beliefs and new representations held in persistent virtual environments in
of learning in collaborative settings. Even though, order to become more effective for knowl-
all those findings have shown the need of using edge’s acquisition?
new teaching methods that may be applied in any • How can educators and scholars con-
innovative learning environment. However, the duct innovative course delivery methods

262

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

(blended or online) in order to be used tor’s responsibility (as coordinator) which takes
technologically-advanced environments as a team member. The success of the coordinator’s
“educational platforms” in collaborative role depends on the degree of integration of the
learning processes? processes undertaken, including:

• Planning and identifying the cooperative


METHOD goals.
• Setting and monitoring virtual meetings and
The Project is Setting lectures in well-structured environments.
• Organizing and management of joint work-
The present study was implemented in distance ing in the virtual environment.
learning courses for students of the Secondary • Monitoring of process integration, infor-
education in “ICT and Informatics courses”. These mation about team members for any delays
courses took place over a period of 4 weeks, and or possible insolvency of the collaborative
involved at least two (2) instructors and over ninety process.
(90) students, spread across more than 4 online • Mediating and supporting team members
working groups of five (5) to six (6) participants when it’s necessary.
of each team. In the first collaborative experience, • Preparing the final version of the product
students required to work in a scenario that repre- at the end of the phase.
sents a real problem of a business or organization • Ensuring stability and cohesion of the ini-
(collaborative learning through case studies). tial workplace.
Secondly, the proposed method is currently based • Delivering the final assessment and report
on the experience of a collaborative learning on the operation of the collaborative team.
activity. The design methodology of a collabora-
tive practice consists of five well-separated and The above circumstances describe how
structured processes and sub-processes (phases): all members together must study as a team to
inquiry-based definition, design, implementation, implement various learning tasks containing all
testing, and documentation and distribution of significant events that occurred during the coop-
exchangeable virtual objects. erative interaction. This plan typically achieved
This study explains the theoretical-instruc- via asynchronous - synchronous communication
tional design-based collaborative practices that a tools. All interactions among users take place via
progressive inquiry-model can offer. The design the FreeSWITCH module. This Groupware tool
of each objective in this procedure includes sev- supports asynchronous and synchronous commu-
eral interrelated processes appropriate learning, nication of distributed students via the Internet.
which students required to complete individually The platform is a groupware system based on the
(e.g. reading) or collaboratively (e.g. exercises, web where users can share information on common
texturing and scripting), in order to achieve the workplace, as well as concert and coordinate their
teaching goal. Also, some phases discussed at a actions. A FreeSWITCH module server manages
group level necessarily. At the beginning of course a number of shared workspaces (places where
it was shared a document explaining to students they’re shared information), which are accessible
the process of teamwork that must be followed to members of a group through a common browser.
to achieve objectives. An “expert” with a special The FreeSWITCH server module manages many
interest in design and analytical goals is the instruc- common workspaces for different groups, where

263

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

users can belong to different shared workspaces • The exploration of different processes in-
(e.g. there is a workspace for each project which volved the creation of teams cooperatively
includes a user). aimed at the acquisition of learning. More
A workspace in virtual worlds can contain specifically when or how these processes
different types of information, which typically affect the formation of a group, and then
represent data objects organized in a hierarchi- the extent to ensure the sufficient creation
cal structure. These objects can be of different of teams that work effectively at the level
types, such as folders, links to web pages, files, of cooperation and learning must be also
graphics, sex work, discussion areas, or objects configured.
specified by the user (address book, calendars, • The establishment of the educative func-
etc.). A FreeSWITCH module keeps members of tion and structure of learning groups, iden-
a group for the actions of their partners in a shared tifying by the distinction of group learning
workspace and provides detailed information on and social goals, relationships between
their previous cooperation. The main objective members, their interactions and roles that
of this area of cooperation is to enable students define the nature and temperament of the
to interact to form cooperation groups. Indeed, it group.
can be used for discussions, involving all students, • The implementation the composition of
in order to complete the project, and as well as the group suggested via four phases con-
to exchange information between instructors and sisting of well-defined processes (Figure 2)
students. The second workplace is personal space aimed to involve students in activities that
for each cooperative group, which records the data lead to the creation well-organized learn-
and constructs and interactions between members ing groups.
of the team that designed the integration of col-
laborative processes. The instructor in this case plays a pivotal role
and identifies four specific virtual workspaces
The Decision Processes for the representing the four processes. The overall idea
Formation and Consolidation for of the approach explained to students at the begin-
Synthesizing Students’ Groups ning of collaborative practice, as it is described
in Table 1.
In this section discussed the main overarching
question. This can be addressed on: how can be Theoretical Foundations and
better structured and organized a team-based Decision Processes According to
learning activity with a progressive inquiry script the Progressive Inquiry Model
held in virtual worlds?
In particular, it was needed to be investigated The organization of activities in a virtual environ-
the factors that must determine the achievement ment should be done on this trail that users can
of a group, namely to the processes that take deal with it, like an ecosystem in which cyber-
place, governing and influence the formation. entities interact without limits. In this way they
Thusly, it can be observed the establishment and try to lead an economy of time and fatigue other
consolidation of a group as a unique dynamic users to achieve main objectives. The central
process guided by the following two goals and question of a learning activity is the cognitive
inspired through this study: framework that can provide effective ways, means,

264

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Figure 2. Sythenizing students’ groups

Table 1. Organizing a CSCL script

Collaborative Instructor’s Triggers Students’ Triggers Team’s Formation


Organization in a
CSCL Script
1. Team’s Synthesis a) Familiarization and a) Providing information about a) Continuing and fulfill update the
preparation of new teams. the team building to the instructor. learning process.
b) Starting a new phase of b) Referring some evidences b) Initial positive and negative
collaboration. about each member of the team. principles of the process should be
concerned.
2. Introduction to the a) Providing the necessary Constructing a theoretical a) Sense of security for each student
Topic information to students. framework with valuable to develop personal triggers.
b) Analyzing the project. information from their previous b) Distribution of students’
knowledge, collaboration efforts experiences from previous processes
in interactive environments
3. Negotiation with the a) Each team must know In the negotiation phase, students a) Understanding the functional and
Prior Knowledge about the collaborative efforts need to provide alternative multimedia sources or artifacts.
and the scope of the learning options for better solutions about b) Distribution of the knowledge field
process in an environment. the project or even more for the that was already appearing.
effectiveness of their team.
4. Coordination of a) Students’ introduction to a) Collaboration between users to a) Acclimatizing in a virtual space
Teams the virtual world. solve problem-based activities. where they need to collaborate and
b) Operational structure for b) Better cooperation in the learn innovative e-skills.
the collaborative production of production of efficient metaphors. b) Affecting new affordances from the
learning. virtual world.

265

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

experiences and needs of supply, treatment and 1. Hypothetical-deductive strategy for the ini-
the construction of new knowledge. Inasmuch as, tial reception of knowledge (see Phase 1).
Table 2 presents a methodological framework for 2. Modeling for teaching general issues of the
describing the procedure of a learning progression original concerns (see Phase 2).
in a virtual world, with the assistance of the pro- 3. Organization of declarative knowledge and
gressive inquiry model (Muukkonen, Lakkala, & application of a conceptual strategic ap-
Hakkarainen, 2009). Thus, a didactic goal-relevant proach (see Phase 3).
taxonomy should include (see Table 2):

Table 2. A socio-cultural framework

Initiatives for Designing a Phases of the Progressive Inquiry Model Decisions Processes in a CSCL Script
Methodological Framework
Phase 1: Distribution of a New A. Distributed expertise is a central concept Students’ decisions process in a CSCL
Knowledge Field in a Virtual World of this model. The progressive inquiry script: Introduction to the virtual world -
model intends to engage the community Students must understand metaphors and
in a shared process of knowledge affordances from the interactive virtual
advancement, in order to convey, world.
simultaneously, the cognitive goals of Instructor’s decision processes in a
collaboration. CSCL script: Presenting and discussing
B. Diversity of expertise among new concepts, scope of introduction and
participants, and interaction with experts’ describing activities that will follow.
cultures by promoting knowledge
advancement.
C. Acting as a member of the community
that shares cognitive responsibility for the
success of its inquiry. This responsibility
involves not only completing tasks or
delivering productions on time, but
also learners taking responsibility for
discovering what they need to be known,
(e.g. goal setting, planning, and monitoring
the inquiry process).
Phase 2: Creating the Contextual The process begins by creating the main Students’ decisions process in a CSCL
Background context of the inquiry to central conceptual script: Transferring the knowledge field in
principles of the domain or complex real- the VW, and exploiting the differences of a
world problems. The learning community traditional and a 3D classroom.
established by a joint planning and setting Instructor’s decision processes in a CSCL
up common goals. It is indispensable to script: Reinforcement students’ views for
create a social culture that supports the learning with sharing artifacts, tools, and
collaborative sharing of knowledge and facilitating the learning progression.
ideas that are in the process of formulation
and improvement.
Phase 3: Describing the Situation of the An essential issue of the progressive Students’ decisions process in a CSCL
Learning Progression inquiry is setting up research questions script: Using functional artifacts and
generated by the students to the direct tools that a client viewer provide or
problems. Seeking questions (how or even instructor can construct for the
what) is especially valuable in this implementation.
process. Students come to treat studying Instructor’s decision processes in a CSCL
as a problem-solving process addressing script: Presenting different tools, teaching
problems in order to understand the units that are emerging from a traditional
theoretical constructs, methods, and classroom.
practices of scientific culture.

continued on following page

266

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Table 2. Continued

Initiatives for Designing a Phases of the Progressive Inquiry Model Decisions Processes in a CSCL Script
Methodological Framework
Phase 4: Explaining Novel Gaits of this D) It is also notable for students to explain Students’ decision process in a CSCL
Framework and Designing Further phenomena with their own existing script: Participating in collaborative
Actions background knowledge by “constructing activities.
their own theoretical foundations,” before Instructor’s decision processes in a CSCL
using information sources. This serves a script: First evaluation internal (individual
number of goals: or team-based) as students need to find
a) Making visible the prior (intuitive) more information about the project.
conceptions of the issues.
b) Trying to explain to others, students
effectively testing the coherence of their
own understanding, and fill the gaps and
contradictions in their own knowledge
more apparent.
c) Creating a new 3D socially engaged
culture in which knowledge treated
as essentially by evolving objects and
artifacts. Thoughts and ideas that presented
are not final and unchangeable, but rather
utterances in an ongoing discourse.
Phase 5: Initial Evaluation Efforts E) Critical evaluation addresses to the need Students’ decisions process in a CSCL
assessing the strengths, and weaknesses of script: Learning basic e-skills (navigate,
theories and explanations that produced, play with media sources, texturing or
in order directed and regulate the scripting virtual objects) – Organization in
community’s joint cognitive efforts. action-groups.
Critical evaluation can assist the Instructor’s decision processes in a CSCL
community members to rise above its script: Conducting of activities-first
earlier achievements, creating a higher- interaction with objects and learning how
level synthesis of the results of inquiry to use brainstorm-based functions (like IM
processes. or 3D VoIP).
Phase 6: The Students’ Engagement with F) Students also guided to be engaged in Students’ decision process in a CSCL
the Learning Materials “searching deeper knowledge” in order script: Exploiting and exploiting the
to find answers to their personal queries. initial students’ workflow-Organizing
Furthermore, searching for relevant and presenting heterogeneous group for
learning materials, students have an learning competencies.
excellent opportunity for self-directed Instructor’s decision processes in a CSCL
inquiry and hands-on practices in order script: The instructor of a course must
to grasp the differences between various decide how many learning materials should
concepts and theories. be offered to the students and how much
they should search out for themselves.
Questions stemming from true wonderment
on students’ part that can easily extend
the scope of materials beyond what
an instructor can foresee or provide
suggestions.
Phase 7: Implementation of the G) Generating various questions are a Students’ decision process in a CSCL
Scaffolding Process part of the process of advancing inquiry; script: Presenting their projects (assessment
learners may transform initial and activity) -Commemorate the instructor’s
unspecified questions into subordinate and assessment and prompt feedback for further
more questions, based on their evaluation reuse.
of producing new knowledge. This Instructor’s decisions processes in a CSCL
transformation helps to refocus the inquiry. script: Feedback from the instructor-
Directing students to return to previously Sending comments to students via IM or
stated problems, to make more subordinate voice chat-Continuing activities for the
questions and answer them, are ways to exploration and construction of 3D models
scaffold the inquiry. for the lessons.

continued on following page

267

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Table 2. Continued

Initiatives for Designing a Phases of the Progressive Inquiry Model Decisions Processes in a CSCL Script
Methodological Framework
Phase 8: Final Knowledge Fairs and H) Constructing new theoretical Students’ decisions process in a CSCL
Reports assumptions may be raised from the script: Manipulation and configuration
questions and scientific knowledge that collaborative tools of the VW and
participants attained. This process includes construction of appropriate 3D artifacts.
final summaries and conclusions of the There should be development of students’
community’s inquiry. If all productions (and instructor’s) social meta-cognition
shared database in a collaborative (Salomon & Perkins, 1998): students
environment meaningfully organized, learning to understand the cognitive value
participants should have an easy access to of social collaboration and gaining the
prior productions and theories, making the capacity to utilize socially distributed
development of conceptions and artifacts a cognitive resources
visible process.

4. Refinement of procedural knowledge in a a synthesis of old and union attitudes to beliefs


learning substance and meaning (see Phases of new generation demands of society with the
4 and 5). individual’s needs.
5. Development of meta-cognitive strategies Similarly noteworthy, the utilization of pro-
and solutions through guided reflection gressive inquiry model can provide:
strategies and exploratory missions (see
Phases 6, 7 and 8). 1. The knowledge contained within this en-
vironmental framework, the abstract or
general nature. It is necessary to obtain that
DISCUSSION knowledge applied and learned in collabora-
tive environments, authentic contexts, i.e.
New technologies have an appreciable momentum in contexts that include knowledge through
in this effect that might be further exploited in the empirical conditions.
teaching process, depending on the subject area 2. The new knowledge in communities requires
contributing among to the enhanced quality and social interaction and teamwork.
improvement of teaching practice in a direction 3. The learning process is accomplished
where students will investigate, discover, and learn through the interactions among members.
how to set goals for each lesson. The instructor’s 4. The instructor should continually offer new
target for the ultimate achievement of a learning opportunities for students in order to become
goal is not only to observe the learning procedure, shareholders of learning communities within
but also to help students interact with multiple and outside the classroom.
learning materials collaboratively with other peers. 5. Links between knowledge and action should
The aim of the current pedagogical process as be provided. Thereof, learning is an integral
it outlined above is the formation or stabilization, part of everyday life. Obviously, learning in
of students’ behaviors, according to predetermined problem-based situations through students’
goals or applicable norms in a social group. The experiences seemed to be significant for the
educational process always carried out in a specific knowledge’s establishment.
socio-cultural context, where students attempted

268

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

In any occasion it is necessary to find the pillars 1. A significant increase in the participation
that consist the proposed process as an effective of students of Higher education.
learning progression that has two aims, firstly 2. The satisfaction of participants from the
to enhance the teaching skills of the teacher and process and higher motivation.
secondly the design of the teaching acts so that 3. The best combination of features and pre-
knowledge is essential, comprehensive, users’ sentation of personal interaction among
understandable and engrossing. Effective learning learners. It has also been conducted that
(or meaningful learning) can be based on: cooperative distance learning increases
academic performance in several cases.
1. The relevant prior knowledge to correlate This study contributes to the development
the previous with new knowledge, of a sense of community among participants
2. The substantive material the new knowledge in the process, not something that exists
should be associated with older or newer in online personal training methods. This
technology should include assumptions, often happens by transferring situations
concepts, proposals and substantive sense. that occur in natural classrooms, the on-line
environments.

CONCLUSION Another crucial issue of lack from a single


theoretical framework or coherent theory in the
From the aforementioned reasons, it can be fi- field of collaborative learning with computer
nally concluded the necessity of designing and support was already emphasized positively by a
implementing learning procedures in a virtual growing literature body (e.g. Lehtinen, 2003; Lip-
environment because 3D environment can offer ponen & Lallimo, 2004). In conclusion, despite
multi-sensorial communication, thereby it must the problems with the synthesis of research results
meet the needs of contemporary learning theories. and theoretical diversity, the example of CSCL is
It is necessary to have an instructional framework an extraordinarily considerable improvement on
to bring greater knowledge transfer, suggestive the previous examples. In particular, elements of
ways of presenting educational content and better the approach, such as:
learning outcomes. The current model underlined
by giving to students full control and an increased 1. The emphasis on development and meaning.
sense of responsibility for the development of 2. The focus on social interaction and
the learning process by providing an educational communication.
tool for teaching, learning and self-study with the 3. The examination of tools and mediated nature
simultaneous consolidation of the concepts of of cognition.
programming, covering all types of learners learn- 4. The emphasis on learning as opposed to the
ing (visual, auditory, kinesthetic) in the presence first years of technological infrastructure
of sound, integrated programming environment paved the way for a better quality and more
and visualization. complete study of collaborative learning
By evaluating the implementation of collabora- with computer support.
tive learning in a virtual world, it can be stressed
that a collaborative distance learning process has Although, it is also crucial to observe some
significant advantages compared with the indi- educational implications that are depicted through
vidual training version of the web, but also with the utilization of the PI model. These are:
the classical approach to education. These include:

269

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

• The social interaction among participants: on everything around the world, the theoretical
Learning is primarily a social process and, framework that delineated by the socio-cultural
therefore the students’ engagement in col- psychology can contribute as a catalyst effect
laborative learning situations can create a for each study and thereby it can promote and
collaborative and productive atmosphere, further evolution of collaborative learning with
as regards the essential part of learning. computer support.
• The meaningful activities: It is a common
conviction nowadays that students’ en-
gagement in collaborative activities seems FUTURE-DRIVEN RESEARCH
particularly useful both in their personal DIRECTIONS
lives and practices followed in real life.
• The configuration and reconstruction of The students’ engagement for social interaction
new information with the existing knowl- and collaborative learning in virtual worlds is
edge: Usually prior knowledge may be in- an issue that must concern many researchers
complete or incorrect. At this point, it is and instructors. Virtual worlds are being used
necessary to recognize that instructor’s and as educational platforms by many educational
students’ prior knowledge can play a piv- institutions, like colleges, libraries or even urban
otal role to address these difficulties. services. Generally, virtual worlds can be used
• The use different learning strategies: as an educational platform by many educational
Learning strategies are noteworthy because institutions, like colleges, libraries and urban
can help students to understand and solve services.
problems deeply. Each strategy has, how- It should be also noted that the essential e-
ever, some initial difficulty of enforcement, learning process may now enter the e-learning 2.0
but the results can provide is manifold. as a term emerged after the advent of Web 2.0 and
the term is used to describe new ways of thinking
Particularly insightful is the fact that, despite about e-learning. The e-learning 2.0 is based on
the socio-cultural origins of the field of collabora- models of cooperation and the theory of social
tive learning with computer support, dialogue has constructivism, uses virtual classrooms in real
not yet been systematically exploited from a study time (real time virtual classrooms) in combination
in the social nature of cognition and interactive with virtual (learning) environments. This mode
elements. This particular interactive approach of e-leaning can based on the models of coopera-
(discursive approach) of intelligence has a long tion and more generally to socio-cultural theories
history within the socio-cultural psychology. can use also “blended” virtual and face-to-face
The contribution of social-cultural frameworks classrooms in real time. These developments
in virtual worlds enhances the phenomenon of highlight the need for a strong first development
collaborative computer-supported learning, but of a methodology that can utilize both virtual
it is not limited to the above. worlds and other services of Web 2.0, which al-
In all these circumstances a collaborative pro- ready are known and co-ordinate students to use
cess requires an adequate theoretical framework new possibilities that are offered. It is necessary,
for describing the interactions which include both to identify principles, to provide essential fields of
social interaction with others (present or not) and use and reinforcement needs, to identify research
natural, artificial or mental tools (present or not). questions and directions.
Given the fact that social interaction engraved

270

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

REFERENCES Bruckman, A. S. (1997). MOOSE Crossing: Con-


struction, community, and learning in a networked
Adams, A. (2010). Rehearsal or Reorganization: virtual world for kids. (Unpublished Doctoral
Two patterns of literacy strategy use in secondary Dissertation). Massachusetts.
mathematics classes. The Montana Math Enthu-
siast, 7(2&3), 371–390. Bull, G., & Kajder, S. (2004). Tapped In. Learn-
ing and Leading with Technology, 31(5), 34–37.
Addison, A., & O’ Hare, L. (2008). How can
Massive Multi-user Virtual Environments and Burdea, G., & Coiffet, P. (2003). Virtual Reality
Virtual Role-play enhance traditional teaching Technology. Wiley Publishing.
practice? In Proceedings of Researching Learning Chen, W. (2005). Supporting teachers’ inter-
in Virtual Environments International Conference vention in collaborative knowledge building.
(pp. 7-16). The Open University. Retrieved 6 October 2012 from http://halshs.
Barab, S., Thomas, M., Dodge, T., Carteaux, R., archives-ouvertes.fr/docs/00/19/01/63/PDF/
& Tuzun, H. (2005). Making learning fun: Quest Chen_2004.pdf
Atlantis, a game without guns. Educational Tech- Chi, M. T. H., Slotta, J. D., & de Leeuw, N. (1994).
nology Research and Development, 53(1), 86–107. From things to processes: A theory of conceptual
doi:10.1007/BF02504859 change for learning science concepts. Learning
Benvenuti, L., Hennipman, E.-J., Oppelaar, E., and Instruction, 4(1), 27–43. doi:10.1016/0959-
& van der Veer, G. (2008). Experiencing educa- 4752(94)90017-5
tion with 3D Virtual Worlds. In Proceedings of Clarke, J., Dede, C., Ketelhut, D. J., & Nelson,
IADIS International Conference on Cognition and B. (2006). A design-based research strategy to
Exploratory Learning in Digital Age (CELDA promote scalability for educational innovations.
2008) (pp. 285-300). Freiburg, Germany: IADIS. Educational Technology, 46(3), 27–36.
Berns, A., Gonzalez-Pablo, A., & Camacho, D. Cleeremans, A., & Jimenez, L. (2002). Implicit
(2013). Game-like language in 3-D virtual environ- learning and consciousness: A granted dynamic
ments. Computers & Education, 60(1), 210–220. perspective. In Implicit learning and conscious-
doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2012.07.001 ness: An empirical, philosophical and computa-
Bishop, G., & Fuchs, H. (1992). Research directions tional consensus in the making (pp. 1-36). Taylor
in virtual environments: Report of an NSF Invita- and Francis.
tional Workshop. SIGGRAPH Computers Graph- Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Newman, S. E. (1989).
ics, 26(3), 153–177. doi:10.1145/142413.142416 Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the crafts
Bricken, W. (1990, May). Learning in Virtual Re- of reading, writing, and mathematics. In L. B.
ality. Retrieved 3 October 2012 from http://www. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction:
wbricken.com/pdfs/03words/03education/02vr- Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 453–494).
education/01learn-in-VR.pdf Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

271

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Collins, A., Brown, S., & Newman, E. (1987). Dexter, S., & Riedel, E. (2003). Why improv-
Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the craft of ing pre-service teacher educational technology
reading, writing and mathematics. Cambridge, preparation must go beyond the college’s walls.
MA: Centre for the Study of Reading: University Journal of Teacher Education, 54(4), 334–346.
of Illinois. doi:10.1177/0022487103255319
Conole, G., & Fill, K. (2005). A learning design Dickey, E. (2005). Three-dimensional virtual
toolkit to create pedagogically effective learn- worlds and distance learning: Two case studies of
ing activities. Journal of Interactive Media in Active World as a medium of distance education.
Education. Retrieved 3 October 2012 from http:// British Journal of Educational Technology, 36(3),
www-jime.open.ac.uk/2005/08/conole-2005-08- 439–451. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8535.2005.00477.x
paper.html
Dickey, M. (2011). The pragmatics of virtual
Coyne, R. (1994). Heidegger & Virtual reality: worlds for K-12 educators: Investigating the af-
The implication of Heidegger’s thinking for com- fordances and constraints of Active Worlds and
puter representations. Leonardo, 27(1), 65–73. Second Life with K-12 in-service teachers. Edu-
doi:10.2307/1575952 cational Technology Research and Development,
59(1), 1–20. doi:10.1007/s11423-010-9163-4
Cram, A., Hedberg, J., Gosper, M., & Dick, G.
(2011). Situated, embodied and social problem Dillenbourg, P. (1996). Some technical implica-
solving in virtual worlds. Research in Learning tions the distributed cognition approach on the
Technology, 19(3), 259–271. doi:10.1080/21567 design of interactive learning environments.
069.2011.624172 Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education,
7(2), 161–180.
Curtis, D., & Lawson, L. (2001). Exploring collab-
orative online learning. Journal of Asynchronous Dillenbourg, P. (1999). Introduction: What do
Learning Networks, 5(1), 21–34. you mean by “collaborative learning”? In P. Dil-
lenbourg (Ed.), Collaborative Learning: Cogni-
Daniels, H. (2001). Vygotsky and Pedagogy.
tive and computational approaches (pp. 1–19).
Routledge Falmer.
Amsterdam: Pergamon, Elsevier Science.
De Jong, T. (2006). Computer simulations-
Dourish, P., & Bellotti, V. (1992) Awareness and
technological advantages in inquiry learning.
Collaboration in Shared workspaces. In Pro-
Science, 312(5773), 532–533. doi:10.1126/sci-
ceedings of the ACM Conference on Computer-
ence.1127750 PMID:16645080
Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW’92) (pp.
Dede, C. (2005). Planning for neomillenial 107-114). Toronto, Canada: ACM Press.
learning styles: Implecations for investestments
Doyle, D. (2008). Immersed in Learning: Devel-
in technology and faculty. In Educating the Net
oping and Supporting creative practice in virtual
Generation. Educause. Retrieved 3 October
worlds. In Proceedings of Researching Learning
2012 from http://net.educause.edu/ir/library/pdf/
in Virtual Environments International Conference
pub7101o.pdf
(pp. 121-129). The Open University.
Delwiche, A. (2006). Massively Multiplayer On-
Duncan, I., Miller, A., & Jiang, S. (2012). A
line Games (MMO’s) in the new media classroom.
taxonomy of virtual worlds usage in education.
Journal of Educational Technology & Society,
British Journal of Educational Technology, 43(6),
9(3), 160–172.
949–964. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8535.2011.01263.x

272

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Falconer, L. (2012). Situated learning in virtual Hartikainen, A. (2008). Making meanings: Pupil
simulations: Researching the authentic dimension talk in inquiry-oriented instruction. NORDINA,
in virtual worlds. Journal of Interactive Learning 4(1), 64–76.
Research. Retrieved 3 October 2012 from http://
Hay, E., Elliot, D., & Kim, B. (2002). Collabora-
eprints.uwe.ac.uk/16628/
tive network-based virtual reality: The past, the
Gamor, K. I. (2012). Exploiting the Power of present, and the future of the virtual solar system.
Persistence for Learning in Virtual Worlds. In In G. Stahl (Ed.), Proceedings of the Computer
B. Khan (Ed.), User Interface Design for Virtual Support Collaborative Learning: Foundations for
Environments: Challenges and Advances (pp. a CSCL Community (pp. 519-520). Hillsdale, NJ:
142–155). Hershey, PA: IGI Global. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Good, R. (2004). 3D virtual spaces for learning Hayes, E. (2006). Situated learning in virtual
and collaboration. Retrieved 5 August 2012 from worlds: The learning ecology of Second Life. In
http://www.masternewmedia.org/2004/09/27/3d_ Proceedings of Adult Education Research Confer-
virtual_spaces_for_learning.html ence 2006. Retrieved 3 August 2012 from http://
www.adulterc.org/Proceedings/2006/Proceed-
Greenhalgh, C., & Benford, S. (1995b). MAS-
ings/Hayes.pdf
SIVE: A Collaborative Virtual Environment
for Teleconferencing. ACM Transactions on Heikkila, S. (2004). Process inquiry challenges
Computer Human Interfaces, 2(3), 239–261. product challenges. Retrieved from http://www.el-
doi:10.1145/210079.210088 earningeuropa.info/files/media/media13769.pdf
Gunawardena, C. N., Hermans, M. B., Sanchez, Hernández-Leo, D., Jorrín-Abellán, I., Vil-
D., Richmond, C., Bohley, M., & Tuttle, R. (2009). lasclaras-Fernández, E., Asensio-Pérez, J., &
A theoretical framework for building online com- Dimitriadis, Y. (2010). A multicase study for
munities of practice with social networking tools. the evaluation of a pattern-based visual design
Educational Media International, 46(1), 3–16. process for collaborative learning. Journal of Vi-
doi:10.1080/09523980802588626 sual Languages and Computing, 21(3), 313–331.
doi:10.1016/j.jvlc.2010.08.006
Hakkarainen, K. (2003). Progressive inquiry in
a Computer-supported Biology class. Journal of Hollan, J., Hutchins, E., & Kirsh, D. (2000).
Research in Science Teaching, 40(10), 1072–1088. Distributed Cognition: Toward a New Foundation
doi:10.1002/tea.10121 for Human- Computer Interaction Research. ACM
Transactions on Human-Computer Interaction,
Hakkarainen, K., & Paavola, S. (2005). From mo-
7(2), 174–196. doi:10.1145/353485.353487
nological and dialogical to trialogical approaches
to learning. Retrieved 3 October 2012 from http:// Hollins, P., & Robbins, S. (2008). The educational
escalate.org.il/construction_knowledge/papers/ affordances of Multi User Virtual environments
hakkarainen.pdf (MUVE). In Proceedings of Researching Learning
in Virtual Environments International Conference
Hakkarainen, K., Palonen, T., Paavola, S., &
(pp. 172-180). The Open University.
Lehtinen, E. (2004). Communities of networked
expertise: Professional and educational perspec-
tives. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

273

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Jackson, R. C., & Farrow, D. (2005). Implicit Kafai, Y., & Resnick, M. (1996). Constructionism
training: How, when and why? Human Move- in Practice: Designing, Thinking, and Learning in
ment Science, 24(3), 308–325. doi:10.1016/j. a Digital World. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
humov.2005.06.003 PMID:16087263
Kafai, Y. B. (2006). Playing and making games
Jarmon, L., Traphagan, T., Mayrath, M., & Trivedi, for learning: Instructionist and constructionist
A. (2009). Virtual world teaching, experiential perspectives for game studies. Games and Culture,
learning, and assessment: An interdisciplinary 1(1), 36–40. doi:10.1177/1555412005281767
communication course in Second Life. Comput-
Kapnas, G., Ntoa, S., Margetis, G., Antona, M.,
ers & Education, 53(1), 169–182. doi:10.1016/j.
& Stephanidis, C. (2011). Seven Wonders: An
compedu.2009.01.010
Interactive Game for Learning English as a Foreign
Järvelä, S. (1995). The cognitive apprenticeship Language in Junior High-School. In C. Stephanidis
model in a technology rich learning environment: (Ed.), Posters, Part II, HCII 2011 (pp. 499–503).
Interpreting the learning interaction. Learning and doi:10.1007/978-3-642-22095-1_100
Instruction, 5(2), 237–259. doi:10.1016/0959-
Karim, A. & Sanza, C. (2013). Learning by implicit
4752(95)00007-P
imitation in virtual worlds. Path Planning 2013.
Järvenoja, H., & Järvelä, S. (2009). Emotion control
Kirschner, P., Martens, R., & Strijbos, J. (2004).
in collaborative learning situations - Do students
CSCL in Higher Education? A framework for
regulate emotions evoked from social challenges?
designing multiple collaborative environments.
The British Journal of Educational Psychology,
In J. Strijbos, P. Kirschner, & R. Martens (Eds.),
79(3), 463–481. doi:10.1348/000709909X402811
What are we know about CSCL? (pp. 3-30).
PMID:19208290
Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Jestice, R. J., & Kahai, S. (2010). The effective-
Koster, R. (2004). A virtual world by any other
ness of virtual worlds for education: an empirical
name? Retrieved 14 October 2012 from http://
study. Paper presented at the Sixteenth Americas
terranova.blogs.com/terra_nova/2004/06/a_vir-
Conference on Information Systems (AMCIS).
tual_world.html
Lima, Peru.
Koutsabasis, P., & Vosinakis, S. (2012). Rethink-
John-Steiner, V., & Mahn, H. (1996). Sociocul-
ing HCI Education for Design: Problem-Based
tural approaches to learning and development: A
Learning and Virtual Worlds at an HCI Design
Vygotskian framework. Educational Psycholo-
Studio. International Journal of Human-Computer
gist, 31(2), 191–206. doi:10.1080/00461520.19
Interaction, 28(8), 485–499. doi:10.1080/10447
96.9653266
318.2012.687664
Jones, G., & Bronack, C. (2007). Rethinking
Kreijns, K. (2004). Sociable CSCL environments:
cognition, representations and processes in 3D
Social affordances, social ability and social pres-
online social learning environments. In D. Gib-
ence. Retrieved 3 October 2012 from http://wenku.
son, C. Aldridge, & M. Prenky (Eds.), Games
baidu.com/view/3ddc7c848762caaedd33d452.
and simulations in online learning: Research and
html
development Frameworks (pp. 1–20). Hersey, PA:
Information Science Publishing.

274

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Lakkala, M., IIomaki, L. Lallimo, J. & Hakkara- Lipponen, L., & Lallimo, J. (2004). From collab-
inen, K. (2003). Virtual communication in middle orative technology to collaborative use of technol-
school students’ and teachers’ inquiry. In Proceed- ogy: Designing learning oriented infrastructures.
ings of the CSCL (pp. 443-452). Maastricht, the Educational Media International, 41(2), 111–116.
Netherlands: Maastricht McLuhan Institute. doi:10.1080/09523980410001678566
Lakkala, M. (2008). Principles of progressive Ludvidgsen, S., & Morch, A. (2006). Catego-
inquiry. EAAIL. Retrieved 12 October 2012 rization in knowledge building: Task specific
from http://deafed-childabuse-neglect-col.wiki. argumentation in co-located CSCL environment.
educ.msu.edu/file/view/Lakkala+M+-+2008+- Retrieved 23 August 2012 from http://halshs.
+Progressive+inquiry+model_introduction.pdf archives-ouvertes.fr/docs/00/19/05/22/PDF/lud-
vigs_morch.pdfv1.pdf
Land, S., & Hannafin, M. (2000). Student-centered
learning environments. In Theoretical foundations MacMahon, K. M. A., & Masters, R. S. W. (2002).
of Learning Environments (pp. 1–23). Lawrence The effects of secondary tasks on implicit motor
Erlbaum Associates. skill performance. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 33, 307–324.
Lee, C., & Cheen, T. (2010). Scaffolding writing
using feedback in students graphic organize-novice Malamos, A., Mamakis, G., Sympa, P., Kotanitsi,
writers’ relevance of ideas and cognitive tools. E., Gonel Crespo, A., & Zubizarraeta Lopez,
Educational Media International, 47(2), 135–152. A. (2008). Technical aspects in using X3D in
doi:10.1080/09523987.2010.492678 virtual reality mathematics Education (EViE-m
platform). In Proceedings of 5th WSEAS/IASME
Lehtinen, E. (2003). Computer-supported col-
International Conference on engineering educa-
laborative learning: An approach to powerful
tion (EE’08). Herakleion.
learning environments. In Advances in learning
and instruction series: Powerful learning environ- Mezirow, J. (1978). Perspective Transfor-
ments: Unraveling basic components and dimen- mation. Adult Education, 28(2), 100–110.
sions (pp. 35-65). Pergmaon. doi:10.1177/074171367802800202
Lehtinen, E. (2003). Computer-Supported Col- Michail, N., Teal, G., & Basta, J. (2006). Pro-
laborative Learning: an Approach to Powerful gressive learning processes model-interpretive
Learning Environments. In E. De Corte, L. methodological framework for human systems
Verschaffel, N. Entwistle, & J. Van Merriënboer inquiries. In Proceedings of the 50th Annual Meet-
(Eds.), Powerful Learning Environments: Unrav- ing of the International Society for the Systems
elling Basic Components and Dimensions (pp. Sciences (ISSS) (pp. 34-39). Sonoma, CA: ISSS.
35–53). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing
Li, J., Peng, J., Zhang, W., Han, F., & Yuan, W. knowledge. In P. H. Winston (Ed.), The Psychology
(2011). A computer-supported platform based on of Computer Vision (pp. 211–277). McGraw-Hill.
clouds. Journal of Computer Information Systems,
7(11), 3811–3818.

275

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Muukkonen, H., Hakkarainen, K., & Lakkala, Pellas, N., & Kazanidis, I. (2013). The impact of
M. (1999). Collaborative Technology for Facili- computer self-efficacy, situational interest and
tating Progressive Inquiry: the Future Learning academic self-concept in virtual communities of
Environment Tools. In C. Hoadley & J. Roschelle inquiry during the distance learning procedures
(Eds.), Proceedings of the CSCL ’99 Conference through Second Life. World Wide Web Journal.
(pp. 406–415). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. doi:10.1007/s11280-013-0266-9
doi:10.3115/1150240.1150291
Pellas, N., & Kazanidis, I. (2013). On the value of
Normand, V., Babski, C., Benford, S., Bullock, Second Life for students’ engagement in hybrid
A., Carion, S., Farcet, N., et al. (1999). The CO- and online university-level courses: A comparative
VEN project: exploring applicative, technical study from Higher education in Greece. Educa-
and usage dimensions of collaborative virtual tion and Information Technologies. doi:10.1007/
environments. Proceedings of Presence: Teleop- s10639-013-9294-4
erators and Virtual Environments, 8(2), 218–236.
Perring, W. J. (1996). Implicit learning. In J.
doi:10.1162/105474699566189
Hoffmann & W. Kintsch (Eds.), Learning (pp.
Onrubia, J., & Engel, A. (2009). Strategies for 37–56). Gottingen: Hogrefe.
collaborative writing and phases of knowdge
Poldga, H., Valyataga, T., Leinomen, T., Ellonen,
construction in CSCL environments. Computers
A., & Priha, M. (2006). Progress inquiry learning
& Education, 53(4), 1256–1265. doi:10.1016/j.
object templates (PILOT). International Journal
compedu.2009.06.008
of E-Learning, 5(1), 103-111.
Onrubia, J. & Engel, A. (2012). The role of teacher
Preece, J. (2000). Online communities: Designing
assistance on the effects of a macro-script in col-
usability, supporting sociability. New York: John
laborative writing tasks. IJCSCL, 7 (1), 161-186.
Wiley & Sons.
Papert, S. (1995). Mindstorms: Children, Comput-
Puntambekar, S. (2006). Analyzing collaborative
ers, and Powerful Ideas. Basic Books.
interactions: Divergence, shared understand-
Pea, R. (1995). Practices of distributed intelligence ing and construction of knowledge. Computers
and designs for education. In Distributed cogni- & Education, 47(3), 332–351. doi:10.1016/j.
tions: Psychological and educational consider- compedu.2004.10.012
ations (pp. 47–87). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Raab, M., & Johnson, J. (2007). Implicit Learn-
University Press.
ing as a Means to Intuitive Decision Making in
Peachy. (2007). MUVE history. Retrieved from 20 Sports. Retrieved 23 October 2013 from http://
August 2012 from http://www.open.ac.uk/wikis/ www.cs.uu.nl/docs/vakken/mpap/xtra/Sanza.
muve_teaching_and_learning/MUVE_history pdfhttp://www.users.muohio.edu/johnsojg/lab/
papers/RJ_Ch08.pdf
Pellas, N. (2012). A conceptual “cybernetic”
methodology for organizing and managing the e- Rahikainen, M., Jarvela, S., & Salovaara, H.
learning process through [D-] CIVEs: The case of (2000). Motivational Processes in CSILE-Based
“Second Life”. In P. Renna (Ed.), Production and Learning. In FishmanB.O’Connor-DivelbissS.
manufacturing system management: Coordination (Eds.), Fourth International Conference of the
approaches and multi-site Planning. Hershey, PA: Learning Sciences (pp. 50-61). Mahwah, NJ:
IGI Global. Erlbaum.

276

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Rahikainen, M., Lallimo, J., & Hakkarainen, K. Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1999). Schools as
(2001). Progressive inquiry in CSILE environ- knowledge building organizations. In D. Keating &
ment: teacher guidance and students’ engagement. C. Hertzman (Eds.), Today’s children, tomorrow’s
In P. Dillenbourg, A. Eurelings, & K. Hakkarainen society: The developmental health and wealth of
(Eds.), European Perspectives on Computer- nations (pp. 274–289). New York: Guilford.
Supported Collaborative Learning: Proceedings
Schroeder, R. (2008). Defining Virtual Worlds and
of the First European Conference on CSCL (pp.
Virtual Environments. Virtual Worlds Research:
520-528). Maastricht, The Netherlands: Maas-
Past, Present & Future, 1(1). Retrieved 13 Au-
tricht McLuhan Institute.
gust 2012 from http://www.journals.tdl.org/jvwr/
Resnick, M. (1996b). Towards a practice of con- article/download/294/248
structional design. In L. Schauble & R. Glaser
Schuler, D. (1996). New community networks:
(Eds.), Innovations n learning: New environments
Wired For Change. New York: Addison Wesley.
for education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Sherman, W. R., & Craig, A. C. (2003). Under-
Rogers, J. (2000). Communities of Practice: A
standing Virtual Reality—Interface, Application,
framework for fostering coherence in virtual
and Design. Elsevier Science.
learning communities. Journal of Educational
Technology & Society, 3(3), 384–392. Stacey, E. (2002). Learning links online: Estab-
lishing constructivist and collaborative learning
Rogoff, B., Radziszewska, B., & Masiello, T.
environments. In S. McNamara & E. Stacey (Eds.),
(1995). Analysis of developmental processes in
Untangling the web: Establishing learning links:
socio-cultural activity. In L. Martin & K. Nelson
Proceedings ASET 2002. Retrieved 23 August
(Eds.), Sociocultural Psychology: Theory and
2012 from http://www.aset.org.au/confs/2002/
Practice of Doing and Knowing (pp. 125–149).
stacey.html
Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
CBO9780511896828.008 Stahl, G. (2012). Cognizing mediating: Unpacking
the entanglement of artifacts with collective minds.
Rovai, P. (2001). Building Classroom Commu-
IJCSCL, 7 (2). DOI: 10.1007/s11412-012-9148-x
nity at a Distance: A Case Study. Educational
Technology Research and Development Journal, Stahl, G., Koschmann, T., & Suthers, D. (2006).
49(4), 35–50. Computer-supported collaborative learning: An
historical perspective. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.),
Santos, A. (2011). Learning in Virtual Worlds:
Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences
A Situated Perspective. In G. Vincenti & J. Bra-
(pp. 409–426). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
man (Eds.), Teaching through Multi-User Virtual
University Press.
Environments: Applying Dynamic Elements to the
Modern Classroom (pp. 120–138). Hershey, PA: Stahl, S. (1991). Defining the role of prior knowl-
Information Science Reference. edge and vocabulary in reading comprehension:
The retiring of number 41. Journal of Reading
Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1994). Computer
Behavior, 23(4), 487–508.
support for knowledge-building communities.
Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3(3), 265–283.
doi:10.1207/s15327809jls0303_3

277

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Steinkuehler, A., & Williams, D. (2006). Where Thurmond, V. A. (2003). Defining interaction
everybody knows your (screen) name: Online and strategies to enhance interactions in Web-
games as “third places”. Journal of Computer- based courses. Nurse Educator, 28(5), 237–
Mediated Communication, 11(4), 885–909. 241. doi:10.1097/00006223-200309000-00013
doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00300.x PMID:14506357
Strijbos, J., Martens, R., & Jochems, W. (2004). Trushell, J., Byrne, K., & Simpson, R. (2012).
Designing for interaction: Six steps to design- Cheating behaviors, the Internet and education
ing computer-supported group-based learn- undergraduate students. Journal of Computer
ing. Computers & Education, 42(4), 403–424. Assisted Learning, 28(2), 136–145. doi:10.1111/
doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2003.10.004 j.1365-2729.2011.00424.x
Strohecker, C. (2006). Embedded microworlds Tsai, C. (2007). The relationship between Internet
for a multiuser environment. MERL TR95-07. perceptions and preferences towards Internet-
Cambridge, MA: Mitsubishi Electric Research based learning environment. British Journal
Laboratory. of Educational Technology, 38(1), 167–170.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8535.2006.00627.x
Suthers, D. (2006). Technology affordances for
inter-subjective meaning making: A research Tsai, C. (2008). The preferences toward con-
agenda for CSCL. International Journal of structivist Internet-based learning environments
Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, among university students in Taiwan. Computers
1(3), 315–337. doi:10.1007/s11412-006-9660-y in Human Behavior, 24(1), 16–31. doi:10.1016/j.
chb.2006.12.002
Takyana, K. & Wilson, J. (2006). Mapping student
learning through toy the collaborative inquiry Tsai, C., & Chuang, S. (2005). The correlation
process: The progressive e-poster. CAL-Laborate, between epistemological beliefs and preferences
24-32. toward Internet-based learning environments.
British Journal of Educational Technology, 36(1),
Thomas, A., & Storr, C. (2005). WebCT in occu-
97–100. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8535.2004.00442.x
pational therapy clinical education: Implementing
and evaluating a tool for peer learning and interac- Tsai, C.-W. (2011). Achieving effective learn-
tion. Occupational Therapy International, 12(3), ing effects in the blended course: A combined
162–179. doi:10.1002/oti.3 PMID:16398204 approach of online self-regulated learning and
collaborative learning with initiation. Cyber-
Thomas, R., & Macgregor, K. (2005). Online
psychology, Behavior, and Social Networking,
project-based learning: How collaborative strat-
14(9), 505–510. doi:10.1089/cyber.2010.0388
egies and problem solving processes impact
PMID:21288076
performance. Journal of Interactive Learning
Research, 16, 83–170. Tsai, C.-W., Shen, P.-D., & Tsai, M.-C. (2011).
Developing an appropriate design of blended
Thompson, F., & Lynch, J. (2003-2004). Web-
learning with web-enabled self-regulated learning
based instruction: Who is inclined to resist it
to enhance students’ learning and thoughts regard-
and why? Journal of Educational Computing
ing online learning. Behaviour & Information
Research, 29(3), 375–385. doi:10.2190/3VQ2-
Technology, 30(2), 261–271. doi:10.1080/0144
XTRH-08QV-CAEL
929X.2010.514359

278

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Tung, F., & Chang, S. (2007). Exploring adoles- Van Joolingen, W., De Jong, T., & Dimitrako-
cents’ intentions regarding the online learning poulou, A. (2005). Issues in computer supported
courses in Taiwan. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, inquiry learning in science. Journal of Computer
10(5), 729–730. doi:10.1089/cpb.2007.9960 Assisted Learning, 23(2), 111–119. doi:10.1111/
PMID:17927546 j.1365-2729.2006.00216.x
Turhan, K., Yaris, F., & Nural, E. (2005). Does in- Vasileiou, V., & Paraskeva, F. (2010). Teaching
structor evaluation by students using a Web-based Role-Playing Instruction in Second Life: An Ex-
questionnaire impact instructor performance? ploratory Study. Journal of Information, Informa-
Advances in Health Sciences Education: Theory tion Technology, and Organizations, 5, 25–50.
and Practice, 10(1), 5–13. doi:10.1007/s10459-
Vygotsky, L. (1935/1978). Mind in society: The
004-0943-7 PMID:15912280
development of higher psychological processes.
Tweddell Levinsen, K. (2007). Qualifying online Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
teachers – Communicative skills and their impact
Wang, C.-M., & Reeves, T. C. (2007). Cyberspace
on e-learning quality. Education and Information
and online education: The influences of global
Technologies, 12(1), 41–51. doi:10.1007/s10639-
cyber culture on international students. In K.
006-9025-1
Amant (Ed.), Linguistic and cultural online com-
Tynjälä, P., & Häkkinen, P. (2005). E-learning at munication issues in the global age (pp. 239–252).
work: Theoretical underpinnings and pedagogical Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference/IGI
challenges. Journal of Workplace Learning, 17(5/6), Global. doi:10.4018/978-1-59904-213-8.ch015
318–336. doi:10.1108/13665620510606742
Wang, H. (2007). Performing a course material
Vadillo, M. A., & Matute, H. (2009). Learning in enhancement process with asynchronous interac-
virtual environments: Some discrepancies between tive online system. Computers & Education, 48(4),
laboratory- and Internet-based researches on as- 567–581. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2005.03.007
sociative learning. Computers in Human Behavior,
Wang, K. H., Wang, T. H., Wang, W. L., & Huang,
25(2), 402–406. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.08.009
S. C. (2006). Learning styles and formative assess-
Vamvakoussi, X., & Vosniadou, S. (2004). Un- ment strategy: Enhancing student achievement in
derstanding the structure of the set of rational Web-based learning. Journal of Computer Assisted
numbers: A conceptual change approach. Learning Learning, 22(3), 207–217. doi:10.1111/j.1365-
and Instruction, 14(5), 453–467. doi:10.1016/j. 2729.2006.00166.x
learninstruc.2004.06.013
Wang, M.-J. (2010). Online collaboration and
van Aalst, J. (2006). Rethinking the nature of offline interaction between students using asyn-
online work in synchronous learning networks. chronous tools in blended learning. Australasian
British Journal of Educational Technology, 37(2), Journal of Educational Technology, 26, 830–846.
279–288. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8535.2006.00557.x
Wertsch, J. (1985). Vygotsky and the social forma-
van de Vord, R. (2010). Distance students and tion of mind. Harvard University Press.
online research: Promoting information literacy
Wilson, V., & Harris, M. (2004). Creating change?
through media literacy. The Internet and Higher
A review of the impact of Design and Technol-
Education, 13(3), 170–175. doi:10.1016/j.ihe-
ogy in schools in England. Journal of Technology
duc.2010.03.001
Education, 15, 46–65.

279

Unraveling a Progressive Inquiry Script

Yamashita, S. (2006). What Makes for Effective Open Simulator (Open Sim): Open Sim
Online Community Building? 10 Field-Tested standalone server is characterized as a multi-func-
Strategies You Can Use to Boost Student Success. tional server or multi-user 3D distributed virtual
Paper presented at the World Conference on E- environment platform. It frequently recapitulates
Learning in Corporate, Government, Healthcare, the “equipment” one of the most well-known simu-
and Higher Education. Honolulu, HI. lated virtual environments, like those of Second
Life (SL). The “open-ended” architecture of this
Zhang, X., Ordσρez de Pablos, P., & Xu, Q.
world can be used as a social virtual world or for
(2013). Culture effects on the knowledge sharing
specific applications such as education, training,
in multi-national virtual classes: A mixed method.
and visualization.
Computers in Human Behavior.
Scaffolding: An instructional technique in-
volving the provision of supportive assistance
or learning guidance, particularly when they are
KEY TERMS AND DEFINITIONS first introduced to new concepts and skills, with
the aim of limiting the complexities of the learn-
Avatar: The on-screen persona that represents ing context. It comes from Vygotsky’s “Zone of
user’s alter-ego as he/she interacts in the VW Proximal Development” (ZPD). This concept re-
with others. fers to the gap between learners’ current knowledge
E-Learning 2.0: The term “e-learning 2.0” and also with the ability level (i.e. what they can
is a “neologism” for distributed collaborative perform without assistance) and his/her emerging
learning through PCs & Web-based technologies or potential knowledge and ability level (i.e. what
([D-] CSCL), using the Web 2.0. This defini- he or she can be challenged to accomplish with
tion has begun for the initial transformation of assistance in the form of scaffolding).
conventional distance learning systems, which Virtual World: Three dimensional (3D) ar-
are used widely through the Internet. Unlike that tificial immersive spaces or places that simulate
of the application of “traditional” e-learning, the real-world spatial awareness in a virtually-rich
impulse that gives us this new generation of e- persistent workflow.
learning, focusing on cooperation and the social Web 2.0: The Web 2.0 goes beyond to the
production of knowledge. However, it is useful limited computer-generated platform, where users
to mention that e-learning and e-learning 2.0, is can perform on collaborative Web-based applica-
a single bit of distance learning. tions. Typical applications of Web 2.0 are social
Engagement: The user’s state of action and networking media, such as wikis and weblogs or
contemplation towards a set of stimuli or concep- other applications which are already well-known
tually interrelated activities or events in a virtual like Facebook, Twitter or YouTube.
environment.

280
281

Afterword

The Need for Implicit Cognition Research in the


Developing Field of Social Psychology in China

I have directed my school’s scientific research and foreign affairs for many years, participating in long-
term partnerships with American, Canadian, and French universities. I feel that an important sign that
a country is modernizing is that people become more important than physical objects. In recent years,
the governments of many countries have begun to emphasize “people-oriented” governance principals.
From an academic perspective, “people-oriented” governance demands psychology research. For
example, “a country located in eastern and central Asia and on the western Pacific Rim whose south-
eastern border reaches the ocean and northwestern border extends into the interior” is a statement of
geographical fact. However, if the country is China, this knowledge may fall within the scope of cognitive
psychology. Without human thought and consciousness, the natural world itself is essentially meaning-
less. For thousands of years, Chinese culture has emphasized the relationships between man and others,
environment and society. These relationships are not only the objects of social cognition but also of human
cognition more broadly. However, I believe that social cognition, especially implicit cognition, has not
yet received widespread attention in China. Though implicit cognition research began to be conducted
only recently in China, it is undergoing rapid development.
Dr. Zheng Jin is my colleague at Zhengzhou Normal University. He has been devoted to the study
of social cognition and language cognition for many years, despite his young age. He is the editor of
this book, which selectively discusses issues related to implicit cognition and gathers the latest research
findings in this field from more than ten countries. The contents of this publication cover many topics,
including implicit learning, implicit research methods, implicit attitudes and education information tech-
nology, and reflect the latest developments in the field of implicit cognition. This book is an important
resource for Chinese scholars who want to understand the research findings related to implicit cognition.
It is designed not only to help Chinese people who desire to study and apply implicit cognition theories
understand the cutting-edge research in the field but also provides a critical reference for the develop-
ment of cognitive psychology and social psychology in China. This publication is suitable for students
and researchers in the field of social psychology. In addition, the development of social cognition is a
key measure of the education standards of teaching colleges and plays a decisive role in discipline con-
struction. I sincerely hope that this edited book contributes to the improvement of Zhengzhou Normal
University’s international influence in the field of psychology.

Lizhu Jiang
Zhengzhou Normal University, China

282

Lizhu Jiang, professor at Zhengzhou Normal University, is also the vice-president of this university. She oversees the devel-
opment and administration of Foreign Affairs and Scientific Research schemes. She was also for many years involved in the
management of the Academic Committee. She appears frequently as a senior visiting keynote speaker at universities in America,
Canada, and abroad, including recent addresses in Australia and the United Kingdom.
283

Related References

To continue our tradition of advancing information science and technology research, we have compiled a list of recom-
mended IGI Global readings. These references will provide additional information and guidance to further enrich your
knowledge and assist you with your own research and future publications.

Abdelaziz, H. A. (2013). From content engagement to Albers, M. J. (2012). How people read. In Human-
cognitive engagement: Toward an immersive web-based information interaction and technical communication:
learning model to develop self-questioning and self-study Concepts and frameworks (pp. 367–397). Hershey, PA:
skills. [IJTD]. International Journal of Technology Diffu- Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
sion, 4(1), 16–32. doi:10.4018/jtd.2013010102 0152-9.ch011

Acha, V., Hargiss, K. M., & Howard, C. (2013). The Albers, M. J. (2012). What people bring with them. In
relationship between emotional intelligence of a leader Human-information interaction and technical communica-
and employee motivation to job performance. [IJSITA]. tion: Concepts and frameworks (pp. 61–113). Hershey,
International Journal of Strategic Information Tech- PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
nology and Applications, 4(4), 80–103. doi:10.4018/ 4666-0152-9.ch003
ijsita.2013100105
Ally, M. (2012). Designing mobile learning for the user. In
Agrawal, P. R. (2014). Digital information management: B. Khan (Ed.), User interface design for virtual environ-
preserving tomorrow’s memory. In S. Dhamdhere (Ed.), ments: Challenges and advances (pp. 226–235). Hershey,
Cloud computing and virtualization technologies in PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
libraries (pp. 22–35). Hershey, PA: Information Science 61350-516-8.ch014
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4631-5.ch002
Almeida, L., Menezes, P., & Dias, J. (2013). Augmented
Akram, H. A., & Mahmood, A. (2014). Predicting per- reality framework for the socialization between elderly
sonality traits, gender and psychopath behavior of Twitter people. In M. Cruz-Cunha, I. Miranda, & P. Gonçalves
users. [IJTD]. International Journal of Technology Diffu- (Eds.), Handbook of research on ICTs for human-centered
sion, 5(2), 1–14. doi:10.4018/ijtd.2014040101 healthcare and social care services (pp. 430–448). Hershey,
PA: Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
Akyol, Z. (2013). Metacognitive development within
1-4666-3986-7.ch023
the community of inquiry. In Z. Akyol & D. Garrison
(Eds.), Educational communities of inquiry: Theoretical Alonso, E., & Mondragón, E. (2011). Computational
framework, research and practice (pp. 30–44). Hershey, models of learning and beyond: Symmetries of asso-
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1- ciative learning. In E. Alonso & E. Mondragón (Eds.),
4666-2110-7.ch003 Computational neuroscience for advancing artificial intel-
ligence: Models, methods and applications (pp. 316–332).
Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-60960-021-1.ch013
Related References

Ancarani, A., & Di Mauro, C. (2013). The human side Ávila, I., Menezes, E., & Braga, A. M. (2014). Strategy
of supply chains: A behavioural perspective of supply to support the memorization of iconic passwords. In K.
chain risk management. In Supply chain management: Blashki & P. Isaias (Eds.), Emerging research and trends
Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applications (pp. in interactivity and the human-computer interface (pp.
1453–1476). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference; 239–259). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-2625-6.ch086 10.4018/978-1-4666-4623-0.ch012

Andres, H. P. (2013). Collaborative technology and Bachvarova, Y., & Bocconi, S. (2014). Games and social
dimensions of team cognition: Test of a second-order networks. In T. Connolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter,
factor model. [IJITPM]. International Journal of Infor- & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy, and as-
mation Technology Project Management, 4(3), 22–37. sessment in serious games (pp. 204–219). Hershey, PA:
doi:10.4018/jitpm.2013070102 Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
4773-2.ch010
Arora, A. S., Raisinghani, M. S., Leseane, R., & Thomp-
son, L. (2013). Personality scales and learning styles: Bagley, C. A., & Creswell, W. H. (2013). The role of
Pedagogy for creating an adaptive web-based learning social media as a tool for learning. In E. McKay (Ed.),
system. In M. Raisinghani (Ed.), Curriculum, learning, ePedagogy in online learning: New developments in web
and teaching advancements in online education (pp. mediated human computer interaction (pp. 18–38). Her-
161–182). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; shey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
10.4018/978-1-4666-2949-3.ch012 1-4666-3649-1.ch002

Ashcraft, D., & Treadwell, T. (2010). The social psy- Balogh, Š. (2014). Forensic analysis, cryptosystem imple-
chology of online collaborative learning: The good, mentation, and cryptology: Methods and techniques for
the bad, and the awkward. In web-based education: extracting encryption keys from volatile memory. In S.
concepts, methodologies, tools and applications (pp. Sadkhan Al Maliky & N. Abbas (Eds.), Multidisciplinary
1146-1161). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference. perspectives in cryptology and information security (pp.
doi:10.4018/978-1-61520-963-7.ch078 381–396). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-5808-0.ch016
Aston, J. (2013). Database narrative, spatial montage,
and the cultural transmission of memory: An anthro- Banas, J. R., & Brown, C. A. (2012). Web 2.0 visualiza-
pological perspective. In D. Harrison (Ed.), Digital tion tools to stimulate generative learning. In D. Polly, C.
media and technologies for virtual artistic spaces (pp. Mims, & K. Persichitte (Eds.), Developing technology-rich
150–158). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; teacher education programs: Key issues (pp. 77–90). Her-
10.4018/978-1-4666-2961-5.ch011 shey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
1-4666-0014-0.ch006
Asunka, S. (2013). Collaborative online learning in non-
formal education settings in the developing world: A best Bancroft, J., & Wang, Y. (2013). A computational simu-
practice framework. In V. Wang (Ed.), Technological lation of the cognitive process of children knowledge
applications in adult and vocational education advance- acquisition and memory development. In Y. Wang (Ed.),
ment (pp. 186–201). Hershey, PA: Information Science Cognitive informatics for revealing human cognition:
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2062-9.ch015 Knowledge manipulations in natural intelligence (pp.
111–127). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
Attia, M. (2014). The role of early learning experience in
10.4018/978-1-4666-2476-4.ch008
shaping teacher cognition and technology use. In P. Breen
(Ed.), Cases on teacher identity, diversity, and cognition Bartsch, R. A. (2011). Social psychology and instructional
in higher education (pp. 1–21). Hershey, PA: Information technology. In Instructional design: Concepts, methodolo-
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5990-2.ch001 gies, tools and applications (pp. 1237–1244). Hershey,
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
60960-503-2.ch508

284
Related References

Bertolotti, T. (2013). Facebook has it: The irresistible Blasko, D. G., Lum, H. C., White, M. M., & Drabik, H.
violence of social cognition in the age of social networking. B. (2014). Individual differences in the enjoyment and
In R. Luppicini (Ed.), Moral, ethical, and social dilem- effectiveness of serious games. In T. Connolly, T. Hainey,
mas in the age of technology: Theories and practice (pp. E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.), Psychol-
234–247). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; ogy, pedagogy, and assessment in serious games (pp.
10.4018/978-1-4666-2931-8.ch016 153–174). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch008
Berwick, R. C. (2013). Songs to syntax: Cognition, com-
binatorial computation, and the origin of language. In Y. Borah, P. (2014). Interaction of incivility and news
Wang (Ed.), Cognitive informatics for revealing human frames in the political blogosphere: Consequences and
cognition: Knowledge manipulations in natural intel- psychological mechanisms. In A. Solo (Ed.), Handbook of
ligence (pp. 70–80). Hershey, PA: Information Science research on political activism in the information age (pp.
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2476-4.ch005 407–424). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-6066-3.ch024
Best, C., O’Neill, B., & Gillespie, A. (2014). Assistive
technology for cognition: An updated review. In G. Naik & Borri, D., & Camarda, D. (2011). Spatial ontologies in
Y. Guo (Eds.), Emerging theory and practice in neuropros- multi-agent environmental planning. In J. Yearwood & A.
thetics (pp. 215–236). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Stranieri (Eds.), Technologies for supporting reasoning
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-6094-6.ch011 communities and collaborative decision making: Coopera-
tive approaches (pp. 272–295). Hershey, PA: Information
Bhattacharya, A. (2014). Organisational justice percep-
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-091-4.ch015
tion: A work attitude modifier. In N. Ray & K. Chakraborty
(Eds.), Handbook of research on strategic business Boukhobza, J. (2013). Flashing in the cloud: Shedding
infrastructure development and contemporary issues in some light on NAND flash memory storage systems. In D.
finance (pp. 296–322). Hershey, PA: Business Science Kyriazis, A. Voulodimos, S. Gogouvitis, & T. Varvarigou
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5154-8.ch021 (Eds.), Data intensive storage services for cloud environ-
ments (pp. 241–266). Hershey, PA: Business Science
Biggiero, L. (2012). Practice vs. possession: Epistemologi-
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-3934-8.ch015
cal implications on the nature of organizational knowledge
and cognition. In M. Mora, O. Gelman, A. Steenkamp, & Boyle, E. (2014). Psychological aspects of serious games.
M. Raisinghani (Eds.), Research methodologies, innova- In T. Connolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. More-
tions and philosophies in software systems engineering no-Ger (Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment
and information systems (pp. 82–105). Hershey, PA: in serious games (pp. 1–18). Hershey, PA: Information
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch001
0179-6.ch005
Boyle, E., Terras, M. M., Ramsay, J., & Boyle, J. M.
Bishop, J. (2014). The psychology of trolling and lurking: (2014). Executive functions in digital games. In T. Con-
The role of defriending and gamification for increasing nolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger
participation in online communities using seductive (Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious
narratives. In J. Bishop (Ed.), Gamification for human games (pp. 19–46). Hershey, PA: Information Science
factors integration: Social, education, and psychological Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch002
issues (pp. 162–179). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5071-8.ch010

285
Related References

Breen, P. (2014). Philosophies, traditional pedagogy, and Cawthon, S. W., Harris, A., & Jones, R. (2010). Cogni-
new technologies: A report on a case study of EAP teach- tive apprenticeship in an online research lab for graduate
ers’ integration of technology into traditional practice. In students in psychology. [IJWLTT]. International Journal
P. Breen (Ed.), Cases on teacher identity, diversity, and of Web-Based Learning and Teaching Technologies, 5(1),
cognition in higher education (pp. 317–341). Hershey, 1–15. doi:10.4018/jwltt.2010010101
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
Cederborg, T., & Oudeyer, P. (2014). Learning words by
4666-5990-2.ch013
imitating. In Computational linguistics: Concepts, meth-
Buchanan, A. (2014). Protective factors in family relation- odologies, tools, and applications (pp. 1674–1704). Her-
ships. In M. Merviö (Ed.), Contemporary social issues in shey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
east Asian societies: Examining the spectrum of public 1-4666-6042-7.ch084
and private spheres (pp. 76–85). Hershey, PA: Information
Cervantes, J., Rodríguez, L., López, S., Ramos, F., &
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5031-2.ch004
Robles, F. (2013). Cognitive process of moral decision-
Burke, M. E., & Speed, C. (2014). Knowledge recovery: making for autonomous agents. [IJSSCI]. International
Applications of technology and memory. In M. Michael & Journal of Software Science and Computational Intel-
K. Michael (Eds.), Uberveillance and the social implica- ligence, 5(4), 61–76. doi:10.4018/ijssci.2013100105
tions of microchip implants: Emerging technologies (pp.
Chadwick, D. D., Fullwood, C., & Wesson, C. J. (2014).
133–142). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
Intellectual disability, identity, and the internet. In As-
10.4018/978-1-4666-4582-0.ch005
sistive technologies: Concepts, methodologies, tools, and
Burusic, J., & Karabegovic, M. (2014). The role of applications (pp. 198–223). Hershey, PA: Information
students’ personality traits in the effective use of social Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4422-9.ch011
networking sites in the educational context. In G. Mallia
Chan, E. C., Baciu, G., & Mak, S. C. (2010). Cognitive
(Ed.), The social classroom: Integrating social network
location-aware information retrieval by agent-based
use in education (pp. 224–243). Hershey, PA: Information
semantic matching. [IJSSCI]. International Journal of
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4904-0.ch012
Software Science and Computational Intelligence, 2(3),
Caixinha, A., Magalhães, V., & Alexandre, I. M. (2013). 21–31. doi:10.4018/jssci.2010070102
Do you remember, or have you forgotten? In R. Martinho,
Chen, C. (2014). Differences between visual style and
R. Rijo, M. Cruz-Cunha, & J. Varajão (Eds.), Information
verbal style learners in learning English. [IJDET]. Inter-
systems and technologies for enhancing health and social
national Journal of Distance Education Technologies,
care (pp. 136–146). Hershey, PA: Medical Information
12(1), 91–104. doi:10.4018/ijdet.2014010106
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-3667-5.ch009
Chen, K., & Barthès, J. A. (2012). Giving personal as-
Carbonaro, N., Cipresso, P., Tognetti, A., Anania, G.,
sistant agents a case-based memory. In Y. Wang (Ed.),
De Rossi, D., Pallavicini, F., et al. (2014). Psychometric
Developments in natural intelligence research and
assessment of cardio-respiratory activity using a mobile
knowledge engineering: Advancing applications (pp.
platform. [IJHCR]. International Journal of Hand-
287–304). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
held Computing Research, 5(1), 13–29. doi:10.4018/
10.4018/978-1-4666-1743-8.ch021
ijhcr.2014010102

Castellani, M. (2011). Cognitive tools for group decision


making: The repertory grid approach revisited. In J. Year-
wood & A. Stranieri (Eds.), Technologies for supporting
reasoning communities and collaborative decision making:
Cooperative approaches (pp. 172–192). Hershey, PA:
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-
091-4.ch010

286
Related References

Chen, S., Tai, C., Wang, T., & Wang, S. G. (2011). Social Correa, T., Bachmann, I., Hinsley, A. W., & Gil de Zúñiga,
simulation with both human agents and software agents: H. (2013). Personality and social media use. In E. Li,
An investigation into the impact of cognitive capacity on S. Loh, C. Evans, & F. Lorenzi (Eds.), Organizations
their learning behavior. In S. Chen, Y. Kambayashi, & and social networking: Utilizing social media to engage
H. Sato (Eds.), Multi-agent applications with evolution- consumers (pp. 41–61). Hershey, PA: Business Science
ary computation and biologically inspired technologies: Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4026-9.ch003
Intelligent techniques for ubiquity and optimization (pp.
Cowell, R. A., Bussey, T. J., & Saksida, L. M. (2011).
95–117). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science
Using computational modelling to understand cognition
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60566-898-7.ch006
in the ventral visual-perirhinal pathway. In E. Alonso &
Chiriacescu, V., Soh, L., & Shell, D. F. (2013). Understand- E. Mondragón (Eds.), Computational neuroscience for
ing human learning using a multi-agent simulation of the advancing artificial intelligence: Models, methods and ap-
unified learning model. [IJCINI]. International Journal plications (pp. 15–45). Hershey, PA: Medical Information
of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence, 7(4), Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-021-1.ch002
1–25. doi:10.4018/ijcini.2013100101
Crespo, R. G., Martíne, O. S., Lovelle, J. M., García-
Christen, M., Alfano, M., Bangerter, E., & Lapsley, D. Bustelo, B. C., Díaz, V. G., & Ordoñez de Pablos, P.
(2013). Ethical issues of ‘morality mining’: Moral identity (2014). Improving cognitive load on students with dis-
as a focus of data mining. In H. Rahman & I. Ramos (Eds.), abilities through software aids. In Assistive technologies:
Ethical data mining applications for socio-economic de- Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applications (pp.
velopment (pp. 1–21). Hershey, PA: Information Science 1255–1268). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer-
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4078-8.ch001 ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4422-9.ch066

Cipresso, P., Serino, S., Gaggioli, A., & Riva, G. (2014). Cró, M. D., Andreucci, L., Pinho, A. M., & Pereira, A.
Modeling the diffusion of psychological stress. In J. (2013). Resilience and psychomotricity in preschool edu-
Rodrigues (Ed.), Advancing medical practice through cation: A study with children that are socially, culturally,
technology: Applications for healthcare delivery, manage- and economically disadvantaged. In M. Cruz-Cunha, I.
ment, and quality (pp. 178–204). Hershey, PA: Medical Miranda, & P. Gonçalves (Eds.), Handbook of research
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- on ICTs for human-centered healthcare and social care
4619-3.ch010 services (pp. 366–378). Hershey, PA: Medical Information
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-3986-7.ch019
Code, J. (2013). Agency and identity in social media. In
S. Warburton & S. Hatzipanagos (Eds.), Digital identity Cummings, J. J., & Ross, T. L. (2013). Optimizing the
and social media (pp. 37–57). Hershey, PA: Information psychological benefits of choice: information transpar-
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-1915-9.ch004 ency and heuristic use in game environments. In R. Ferdig
(Ed.), Design, utilization, and analysis of simulations and
Combs, R. M., & Mazur, J. (2014). 3D modeling in a
game-based educational worlds (pp. 142–157). Hershey,
high school computer visualization class: Enacting a
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
productive, distributed social learning environment. In
4018-4.ch009
K-12 education: Concepts, methodologies, tools, and
applications (pp. 1020–1040). Hershey, PA: Information Curumsing, M. K., Pedell, S., & Vasa, R. (2014). Designing
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4502-8.ch061 an evaluation tool to measure emotional goals. [IJPOP].
International Journal of People-Oriented Programming,
Cook, R. G., & Sutton, R. (2014). Administrators’ assess-
3(1), 22–43. doi:10.4018/ijpop.2014010102
ments of online courses and student retention in higher
education: Lessons learned. In S. Mukerji & P. Tripathi
(Eds.), Handbook of research on transnational higher edu-
cation (pp. 138–150). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4458-8.ch008

287
Related References

Cuzzocrea, F., Murdaca, A. M., & Oliva, P. (2013). Using Demirbilek, M. (2010). Cognitive load and disorientation
precision teaching method to improve foreign language issues in hypermedia as assistive technology. In S. Seok,
and cognitive skills in university students. In A. Cartelli E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook of research on
(Ed.), Fostering 21st century digital literacy and technical human cognition and assistive technology: Design, acces-
competency (pp. 201–211). Hershey, PA: Information sibility and transdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 109–120).
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2943-1.ch014 Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch007
DaCosta, B., & Seok, S. (2010). Human cognition in the
design of assistive technology for those with learning Derrick, M. G. (2013). The inventory of learner per-
disabilities. In S. Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), sistence. In M. Bocarnea, R. Reynolds, & J. Baker
Handbook of research on human cognition and assistive (Eds.), Online instruments, data collection, and elec-
technology: Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary tronic measurements: Organizational advancements (pp.
perspectives (pp. 1–20). Hershey, PA: Medical Information 271–290). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch001 10.4018/978-1-4666-2172-5.ch016

DaCosta, B., & Seok, S. (2010). Multimedia design of De Simone, C., Marquis, T., & Groen, J. (2013). Optimiz-
assistive technology for those with learning disabilities. ing conditions for learning and teaching in K-20 education.
In S. Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook In V. Wang (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching and
of research on human cognition and assistive technology: learning in K-20 education (pp. 535–552). Hershey, PA:
Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary perspectives Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
(pp. 43–60). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science 4249-2.ch031
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch003
Doolittle, P. E., McNeill, A. L., Terry, K. P., & Scheer, S.
Danielsson, U., & Öberg, K. D. (2011). Psychosocial B. (2011). Multimedia, cognitive load, and pedagogy. In
life environment and life roles in interaction with daily Instructional design: Concepts, methodologies, tools and
use of information communication technology bound- applications (pp. 1564–1585). Hershey, PA: Information
aries between work and leisure. In D. Haftor & A. Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-503-2.ch706
Mirijamdotter (Eds.), Information and communication
Dourlens, S., & Ramdane-Cherif, A. (2013). Cogni-
technologies, society and human beings: Theory and
tive memory for semantic agents architecture in robotic
framework (Festschrift in honor of Gunilla Bradley) (pp.
interaction. In Y. Wang (Ed.), Cognitive informatics for
266–282). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
revealing human cognition: Knowledge manipulations in
10.4018/978-1-60960-057-0.ch020
natural intelligence (pp. 82–97). Hershey, PA: Information
Daradoumis, T., & Lafuente, M. M. (2014). Studying Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2476-4.ch006
the suitability of discourse analysis methods for emo-
Dubbels, B. (2011). Cognitive ethnography: A meth-
tion detection and interpretation in computer-mediated
odology for measure and analysis of learning for game
educational discourse. In H. Lim & F. Sudweeks (Eds.),
studies. [IJGCMS]. International Journal of Gaming
Innovative methods and technologies for electronic dis-
and Computer-Mediated Simulations, 3(1), 68–78.
course analysis (pp. 119–143). Hershey, PA: Information
doi:10.4018/jgcms.2011010105
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4426-7.ch006
Dunbar, N. E., Wilson, S. N., Adame, B. J., Elizondo, J.,
Deka, G. C. (2014). Significance of in-memory comput-
Jensen, M. L., Miller, C. H., et al. (2013). MACBETH:
ing for real-time big data analytics. In P. Raj & G. Deka
Development of a training game for the mitigation of cogni-
(Eds.), Handbook of research on cloud infrastructures for
tive bias. [IJGBL]. International Journal of Game-Based
big data analytics (pp. 352–369). Hershey, PA: Information
Learning, 3(4), 7–26. doi:10.4018/ijgbl.2013100102
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5864-6.ch014

288
Related References

Dunham, A. H., & Burt, C. D. (2012). Mentoring and the Estrada-Hernández, N., & Stachowiak, J. R. (2010).
transfer of organizational memory within the context of Evaluating systemic assistive technology needs. In S.
an aging workforce: Cultural implications for competitive Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook of
advantage. In Organizational learning and knowledge: research on human cognition and assistive technology:
Concepts, methodologies, tools and applications (pp. Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary perspectives
3076–3099). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference; (pp. 239–250). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science
10.4018/978-1-60960-783-8.ch817 Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch016

Duţu, A. (2014). Understanding consumers’ behaviour Fitzpatrick, M., & Theoharis, R. (2010). Assistive technol-
change in uncertainty conditions: A psychological per- ogy for deaf and hard of hearing students. In S. Seok, E.
spective. In F. Musso & E. Druica (Eds.), Handbook of Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook of research on
research on retailer-consumer relationship development human cognition and assistive technology: Design, acces-
(pp. 45–69). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference; sibility and transdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 179–191).
10.4018/978-1-4666-6074-8.ch004 Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch012
Durrington, V. A., & Du, J. (2013). Learning tasks, peer
interaction, and cognition process an online collaborative Francis, A. G., Jr., Mehta, M., & Ram, A. (2012). Emo-
design model. [IJICTE]. International Journal of Informa- tional memory and adaptive personalities. In Machine
tion and Communication Technology Education, 9(1), learning: Concepts, methodologies, tools and applica-
38–50. doi:10.4018/jicte.2013010104 tions (pp. 1292–1313). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-818-7.ch507
Dwyer, P. (2013). Measuring collective cognition in online
collaboration venues. In N. Kock (Ed.), Interdisciplin- Gagliardi, F. (2014). A cognitive machine-learning
ary applications of electronic collaboration approaches system to discover syndromes in erythemato-squamous
and technologies (pp. 46–61). Hershey, PA: Information diseases. In J. Rodrigues (Ed.), Advancing medical practice
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2020-9.ch004 through technology: Applications for healthcare delivery,
management, and quality (pp. 66–101). Hershey, PA:
Egan, R. G., & Zhou, M. (2011). Re-conceptualizing
Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
calibration using trace methodology. In G. Dettori & D.
1-4666-4619-3.ch005
Persico (Eds.), Fostering self-regulated learning through
ICT (pp. 71–88). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer- Gaines, B. R., & Shaw, M. L. (2013). Sociocognitive in-
ence; 10.4018/978-1-61692-901-5.ch005 quiry. In Data mining: Concepts, methodologies, tools, and
applications (pp. 1688–1708). Hershey, PA: Information
El-Farargy, N. (2013). Refresher training in clinical
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2455-9.ch088
psychology supervision: A blended learning approach.
In A. Benson, J. Moore, & S. Williams van Rooij (Eds.), Gardner, M. K., & Hill, R. D. (2013). Training older
Cases on educational technology planning, design, and adults to improve their episodic memory: Three different
implementation: A project management perspective (pp. approaches to enhancing numeric memory. In R. Zheng,
295–317). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; R. Hill, & M. Gardner (Eds.), Engaging older adults with
10.4018/978-1-4666-4237-9.ch016 modern technology: Internet use and information access
needs (pp. 191–211). Hershey, PA: Information Science
El Louadi, M., & Tounsi, I. (2010). Do organizational
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-1966-1.ch010
memory and information technology interact to affect
organizational information needs and provision? In M. Ghili, S., Nazarian, S., Tavana, M., Keyvanshokouhi,
Jennex (Ed.), Ubiquitous developments in knowledge S., & Isaai, M. T. (2013). A complex systems paradox
management: Integrations and trends (pp. 1–20). Hershey, of organizational learning and knowledge management.
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1- [IJKBO]. International Journal of Knowledge-Based Or-
60566-954-0.ch001 ganizations, 3(3), 53–72. doi:10.4018/ijkbo.2013070104

289
Related References

Gibson, D. (2012). Designing a computational model Guger, C., Sorger, B., Noirhomme, Q., Naci, L., Monti,
of learning. In Machine learning: Concepts, methodolo- M. M., & Real, R. … Cincotti, F. (2014). Brain-computer
gies, tools and applications (pp. 147–174). Hershey, PA: interfaces for assessment and communication in disorders
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960- of consciousness. In G. Naik & Y. Guo (Eds.), Emerging
818-7.ch203 theory and practice in neuroprosthetics (pp. 181-214).
Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science Reference.
Gibson, M., Renaud, K., Conrad, M., & Maple, C.
doi:10.4018/978-1-4666-6094-6.ch010
(2013). Music is the key: Using our enduring memory
for songs to help users log on. In IT policy and ethics: Güngör, H. (2014). Adolescent suicides as a chaotic phe-
Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applications (pp. nomenon. In Ş. Erçetin & S. Banerjee (Eds.), Chaos and
1018–1037). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer- complexity theory in world politics (pp. 306–324). Her-
ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2919-6.ch046 shey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
1-4666-6070-0.ch022
Godine, N., & Barnett, J. E. (2013). The use of telepsy-
chology in clinical practice: Benefits, effectiveness, and Hai-Jew, S. (2013). Interpreting “you” and “me”: Per-
issues to consider. [IJCBPL]. International Journal of sonal voices, PII, biometrics, and imperfect/perfect
Cyber Behavior, Psychology and Learning, 3(4), 70–83. electronic memory in a democracy. In C. Akrivopoulou
doi:10.4018/ijcbpl.2013100105 & N. Garipidis (Eds.), Digital democracy and the impact
of technology on governance and politics: New globalized
Gökçay, D. (2011). Emotional axes: Psychology, psycho-
practices (pp. 20–37). Hershey, PA: Information Science
physiology and neuroanatomical correlates. In D. Gökçay
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-3637-8.ch003
& G. Yildirim (Eds.), Affective computing and interaction:
Psychological, cognitive and neuroscientific perspectives Hainey, T., Connolly, T. M., Chaudy, Y., Boyle, E., Beeby,
(pp. 56–73). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; R., & Soflano, M. (2014). Assessment integration in
10.4018/978-1-61692-892-6.ch003 serious games. In T. Connolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G.
Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy,
Goswami, R., Jena, R. K., & Mahapatro, B. B. (2013).
and assessment in serious games (pp. 317–341). Hershey,
Psycho-social impact of shift work: A study of ferro-
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
alloy industries in Orissa. In P. Ordóñez de Pablos (Ed.),
4773-2.ch015
Business, technology, and knowledge management in
Asia: Trends and innovations (pp. 166–174). Hershey, Hainey, T., Soflano, M., & Connolly, T. M. (2014). A ran-
PA: Business Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- domised controlled trial to evaluate learning effectiveness
2652-2.ch013 using an adaptive serious game to teach SQL at higher
education level. In T. Connolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G.
Graff, M. (2011). Can cognitive style predict how
Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy,
individuals use web-based learning environments? In
and assessment in serious games (pp. 270–291). Hershey,
Instructional design: Concepts, methodologies, tools and
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
applications (pp. 1553–1563). Hershey, PA: Information
4773-2.ch013
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-503-2.ch705
Haque, J., Erturk, M., Arslan, H., & Moreno, W. (2011).
Griffiths, M., Kuss, D. J., & Ortiz de Gortari, A. B.
Cognitive aeronautical communication system. [IJITN].
(2013). Videogames as therapy: A review of the medical
International Journal of Interdisciplinary Telecommu-
and psychological literature. In M. Cruz-Cunha, I. Mi-
nications and Networking, 3(1), 20–35. doi:10.4018/
randa, & P. Gonçalves (Eds.), Handbook of research on
jitn.2011010102
ICTs and management systems for improving efficiency
in healthcare and social care (pp. 43–68). Hershey, PA:
Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
1-4666-3990-4.ch003

290
Related References

Hauge, J. B., Boyle, E., Mayer, I., Nadolski, R., Riedel, J. Honey, R. C., & Grand, C. S. (2011). Application of
C., Moreno-Ger, P., et al. (2014). Study design and data connectionist models to animal learning: Interactions
gathering guide for serious games’ evaluation. In T. Con- between perceptual organization and associative processes.
nolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger In E. Alonso & E. Mondragón (Eds.), Computational
(Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious neuroscience for advancing artificial intelligence: Mod-
games (pp. 394–419). Hershey, PA: Information Science els, methods and applications (pp. 1–14). Hershey, PA:
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch018 Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
1-60960-021-1.ch001
Haykin, S. (2013). Cognitive dynamic systems. In Y. Wang
(Ed.), Cognitive informatics for revealing human cogni- Hoppenbrouwers, S., Schotten, B., & Lucas, P. (2012). To-
tion: Knowledge manipulations in natural intelligence (pp. wards games for knowledge acquisition and modeling. In
250–260). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; R. Ferdig & S. de Freitas (Eds.), Interdisciplinary advance-
10.4018/978-1-4666-2476-4.ch016 ments in gaming, simulations and virtual environments:
Emerging trends (pp. 281–299). Hershey, PA: Information
Henderson, A. M., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2014). Learning
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-0029-4.ch018
words from experience: An integrated framework. In Com-
putational linguistics: Concepts, methodologies, tools, and Huang, C., Liang, C., & Lin, E. (2014). A study on emotion
applications (pp. 1705–1727). Hershey, PA: Information releasing effect with music and color. In F. Cipolla-Ficarra
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-6042-7.ch085 (Ed.), Advanced research and trends in new technologies,
software, human-computer interaction, and communi-
Hendrick, H. W. (2011). Cognitive and organizational
cability (pp. 23–31). Hershey, PA: Information Science
complexity and behavior: Implications for organizational
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4490-8.ch003
design and leadership. In D. Haftor & A. Mirijamdotter
(Eds.), Information and communication technologies, soci- Huang, W. D., & Tettegah, S. Y. (2014). Cognitive load
ety and human beings: Theory and framework (Festschrift and empathy in serious games: A conceptual framework.
in honor of Gunilla Bradley) (pp. 147–159). Hershey, PA: In J. Bishop (Ed.), Gamification for human factors inte-
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960- gration: Social, education, and psychological issues (pp.
057-0.ch013 17–30). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-5071-8.ch002
Henrie, K. M., & Miller, D. W. (2013). An examination
of mediation: Insights into the role of psychological Huijnen, C. (2011). The use of assistive technology to
mediators in the use of persuasion knowledge. In R. Eid support the wellbeing and independence of people with
(Ed.), Managing customer trust, satisfaction, and loyalty memory impairments. In J. Soar, R. Swindell, & P. Tsang
through information communication technologies (pp. (Eds.), Intelligent technologies for bridging the grey digital
106–117). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference; divide (pp. 65–79). Hershey, PA: Information Science
10.4018/978-1-4666-3631-6.ch007 Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-825-8.ch005

Ho, W. C., Dautenhahn, K., Lim, M., Enz, S., Zoll, C., & Huseyinov, I. N. (2014). Fuzzy linguistic modelling in
Watson, S. (2012). Towards learning ‘self’ and emotional multi modal human computer interaction: Adaptation
knowledge in social and cultural human-agent interactions. to cognitive styles using multi level fuzzy granulation
In Virtual learning environments: Concepts, methodolo- method. In Assistive technologies: Concepts, methodolo-
gies, tools and applications (pp. 1426–1445). Hershey, gies, tools, and applications (pp. 1481–1496). Hershey,
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1- PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
4666-0011-9.ch705 4666-4422-9.ch077

Holt, L., & Ziegler, M. F. (2013). Promoting team learning


in the classroom. In V. Wang (Ed.), Technological applica-
tions in adult and vocational education advancement (pp.
94–105). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-2062-9.ch008

291
Related References

Hussey, H. D., Fleck, B. K., & Richmond, A. S. (2014). Johnson, V., & Price, C. (2010). A longitudinal case study
Promoting active learning through a flipped course design. on the use of assistive technology to support cognitive
In J. Keengwe, G. Onchwari, & J. Oigara (Eds.), Promot- processes across formal and informal educational settings.
ing active learning through the flipped classroom model In S. Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook
(pp. 23–46). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; of research on human cognition and assistive technology:
10.4018/978-1-4666-4987-3.ch002 Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary perspectives
(pp. 192–198). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science
Ilin, R., & Perlovsky, L. (2011). Cognitive based dis-
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch013
tributed sensing, processing, and communication. In B.
Igelnik (Ed.), Computational modeling and simulation Kalyuga, S. (2012). Cognitive load aspects of text pro-
of intellect: Current state and future perspectives (pp. cessing. In C. Boonthum-Denecke, P. McCarthy, & T.
131–161). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; Lamkin (Eds.), Cross-disciplinary advances in applied
10.4018/978-1-60960-551-3.ch006 natural language processing: Issues and approaches (pp.
114–132). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
Ilin, R., & Perlovsky, L. (2010). Cognitively inspired
10.4018/978-1-61350-447-5.ch009
neural network for recognition of situations. [IJNCR].
International Journal of Natural Computing Research, Kammler, D., Witte, E. M., Chattopadhyay, A., Bauw-
1(1), 36–55. doi:10.4018/jncr.2010010102 ens, B., Ascheid, G., Leupers, R., & Meyr, H. (2012).
Automatic generation of memory interfaces for ASIPs.
Jenkins, J. L., Durcikova, A., & Burns, M. B. (2013).
In S. Virtanen (Ed.), Innovations in embedded and
Simplicity is bliss: Controlling extraneous cognitive load
real-time systems engineering for communication (pp.
in online security training to promote secure behavior.
79–100). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
[JOEUC]. Journal of Organizational and End User Com-
10.4018/978-1-4666-0912-9.ch005
puting, 25(3), 52–66. doi:10.4018/joeuc.2013070104
Khan, T. M. (2011). Theory of mind in autistic children:
Jennings, D. J., Alonso, E., Mondragón, E., & Bonardi,
multimedia based support. In P. Ordóñez de Pablos,
C. (2011). Temporal uncertainty during overshadowing:
J. Zhao, & R. Tennyson (Eds.), Technology enhanced
A temporal difference account. In E. Alonso & E. Mon-
learning for people with disabilities: Approaches and
dragón (Eds.), Computational neuroscience for advancing
applications (pp. 167–179). Hershey, PA: Information
artificial intelligence: Models, methods and applications
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-923-1.ch012
(pp. 46–55). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-021-1.ch003 Khetrapal, N. (2010). Cognition meets assistive technol-
ogy: Insights from load theory of selective attention. In
Jin, S., DaCosta, B., & Seok, S. (2014). Social skills
S. Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook of
development for children with autism spectrum disorders
research on human cognition and assistive technology:
through the use of interactive storytelling games. In B.
Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary perspectives
DaCosta & S. Seok (Eds.), Assistive technology research,
(pp. 96–108). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science
practice, and theory (pp. 144–159). Hershey, PA: Medical
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch006
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
5015-2.ch010 Khetrapal, N. (2010). Cognitive science helps formulate
games for moral education. In K. Schrier & D. Gibson
Johnson, R. D., De Ridder, D., & Gillett, G. (2014).
(Eds.), Ethics and game design: Teaching values through
Neurosurgery to enhance brain function: Ethical dilem-
play (pp. 181–196). Hershey, PA: Information Science
mas for the neuroscientist and society. In S. Thompson
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-845-6.ch012
(Ed.), Global issues and ethical considerations in human
enhancement technologies (pp. 96–118). Hershey, PA: Kickmeier-Rust, M. D., Mattheiss, E., Steiner, C., &
Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978- Albert, D. (2011). A psycho-pedagogical framework
1-4666-6010-6.ch006 for multi-adaptive educational games. [IJGBL]. Inter-
national Journal of Game-Based Learning, 1(1), 45–58.
doi:10.4018/ijgbl.2011010104

292
Related References

Kiel, L. D., & McCaskill, J. (2013). Cognition and com- Knafla, B., & Champandard, A. J. (2012). Behavior trees:
plexity: An agent-based model of cognitive capital under Introduction and memory-compact implementation. In
stress. In S. Banerjee (Ed.), Chaos and complexity theory A. Kumar, J. Etheredge, & A. Boudreaux (Eds.), Algo-
for management: Nonlinear dynamics (pp. 254–268). rithmic and architectural gaming design: Implementation
Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference; 10.4018/978- and development (pp. 40–66). Hershey, PA: Information
1-4666-2509-9.ch012 Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-1634-9.ch003

Kiili, K., & Perttula, A. (2013). A design framework for Kokkinos, C. M., Antoniadou, N., Dalara, E., Koufogazou,
educational exergames. In S. de Freitas, M. Ott, M. Popes- A., & Papatziki, A. (2013). Cyber-bullying, personality and
cu, & I. Stanescu (Eds.), New pedagogical approaches coping among pre-adolescents. [IJCBPL]. International
in game enhanced learning: Curriculum integration (pp. Journal of Cyber Behavior, Psychology and Learning,
136–158). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; 3(4), 55–69. doi:10.4018/ijcbpl.2013100104
10.4018/978-1-4666-3950-8.ch008
Konrath, S. (2013). The empathy paradox: Increasing
Kiliç, F. (2011). Structuring of knowledge and cognitive disconnection in the age of increasing connection. In R.
load. In G. Kurubacak & T. Yuzer (Eds.), Handbook of Luppicini (Ed.), Handbook of research on technoself:
research on transformative online education and liberation: Identity in a technological society (pp. 204–228). Hershey,
Models for social equality (pp. 370–382). Hershey, PA: PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960- 2211-1.ch012
046-4.ch020
Kopp, B., & Mandl, H. (2012). Supporting virtual
Kim, E. B. (2011). Student personality and learning out- collaborative learning using collaboration scripts and
comes in e-learning: An introduction to empirical research. content schemes. In Virtual learning environments:
In S. Eom & J. Arbaugh (Eds.), Student satisfaction and Concepts, methodologies, tools and applications (pp.
learning outcomes in e-learning: An introduction to em- 470–487). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
pirical research (pp. 294–315). Hershey, PA: Information 10.4018/978-1-4666-0011-9.ch303
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-615-2.ch013
Kumar, S., Singhal, D., & Murthy, G. R. (2013). Doubly
Kinsell, C. (2010). Investigating assistive technologies us- cognitive architecture based cognitive wireless sensor
ing computers to simulate basic curriculum for individuals networks. In N. Chilamkurti (Ed.), Security, design, and
with cognitive impairments. In S. Seok, E. Meyen, & B. architecture for broadband and wireless network technolo-
DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook of research on human cogni- gies (pp. 121–126). Hershey, PA: Information Science
tion and assistive technology: Design, accessibility and Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-3902-7.ch009
transdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 61–75). Hershey, PA:
Kutaula, S., & Talwar, V. (2014). Integrating psychologi-
Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
cal contract and service-related outcomes in emerging
61520-817-3.ch004
economies: A proposed conceptual framework. In A.
Kirwan, G., & Power, A. (2012). Can forensic psychol- Goyal (Ed.), Innovations in services marketing and
ogy contribute to solving the problem of cybercrime? In management: Strategies for emerging economies (pp.
G. Kirwan & A. Power (Eds.), The psychology of cyber 291–306). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference;
crime: Concepts and principles (pp. 18–36). Hershey, 10.4018/978-1-4666-4671-1.ch016
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
Kyriakaki, G., & Matsatsinis, N. (2014). Pedagogical
61350-350-8.ch002
evaluation of e-learning websites with cognitive objec-
Klippel, A., Richter, K., & Hansen, S. (2013). Cognitively tives. In D. Yannacopoulos, P. Manolitzas, N. Matsatsinis,
ergonomic route directions. In Geographic information & E. Grigoroudis (Eds.), Evaluating websites and web
systems: Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applica- services: Interdisciplinary perspectives on user satisfac-
tions (pp. 250–257). Hershey, PA: Information Science tion (pp. 224–240). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2038-4.ch017 Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5129-6.ch013

293
Related References

Kyritsis, M., Gulliver, S. R., & Morar, S. (2014). Cogni- Li, R. (2013). Traditional to hybrid: Social media’s role
tive and environmental factors influencing the process in reshaping instruction in higher education. In A. Sigal
of spatial knowledge acquisition within virtual reality (Ed.), Advancing library education: Technological in-
environments. [IJALR]. International Journal of Artificial novation and instructional design (pp. 65–90). Hershey,
Life Research, 4(1), 43–58. doi:10.4018/ijalr.2014010104 PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
4666-3688-0.ch005
Lai, S., & Han, H. (2014). Behavioral planning theory.
In J. Wang (Ed.), Encyclopedia of business analytics Li, X., Lin, Z., & Wu, J. (2014). Language processing in
and optimization (pp. 265–272). Hershey, PA: Business the human brain of literate and illiterate subjects. In Com-
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5202-6.ch025 putational linguistics: Concepts, methodologies, tools, and
applications (pp. 1391–1400). Hershey, PA: Information
Laffey, J., Stichter, J., & Schmidt, M. (2010). Social orthot-
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-6042-7.ch068
ics for youth with ASD to learn in a collaborative 3D VLE.
In S. Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook Li, X., Ouyang, Z., & Luo, Y. (2013). The cognitive load
of research on human cognition and assistive technology: affects the interaction pattern of emotion and working
Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary perspectives memory. [IJCINI]. International Journal of Cogni-
(pp. 76–95). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science tive Informatics and Natural Intelligence, 6(2), 68–81.
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch005 doi:10.4018/jcini.2012040104

Lawson, D. (2013). Analysis and use of the life styles Lin, T., Li, X., Wu, Z., & Tang, N. (2013). Automatic
inventory 1 and 2 by human synergistics international. cognitive load classification using high-frequency interac-
In M. Bocarnea, R. Reynolds, & J. Baker (Eds.), Online tion events: An exploratory study. [IJTHI]. International
instruments, data collection, and electronic measurements: Journal of Technology and Human Interaction, 9(3),
Organizational advancements (pp. 76–96). Hershey, PA: 73–88. doi:10.4018/jthi.2013070106
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
Linek, S. B. (2011). As you like it: What media psychology
2172-5.ch005
can tell us about educational game design. In P. Felicia
Lee, L., & Hung, J. C. (2011). Effect of teaching using (Ed.), Handbook of research on improving learning and
whole brain instruction on accounting learning. In Q. Jin motivation through educational games: Multidisciplinary
(Ed.), Distance education environments and emerging approaches (pp. 606–632). Hershey, PA: Information
software systems: New technologies (pp. 261–282). Her- Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-495-0.ch029
shey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
Linek, S. B., Marte, B., & Albert, D. (2014). Background
1-60960-539-1.ch016
music in educational games: Motivational appeal and cog-
Levin, I., & Kojukhov, A. (2013). Personalization of learn- nitive impact. In J. Bishop (Ed.), Gamification for human
ing environments in a post-industrial class. In M. Pătruţ & factors integration: Social, education, and psychological
B. Pătruţ (Eds.), Social media in higher education: Teach- issues (pp. 259–271). Hershey, PA: Information Science
ing in web 2.0 (pp. 105–123). Hershey, PA: Information Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5071-8.ch016
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2970-7.ch006
Logeswaran, R. (2011). Neural networks in medicine.
Li, A., Li, H., Guo, R., & Zhu, T. (2013). MobileSens: In Clinical technologies: Concepts, methodologies, tools
A ubiquitous psychological laboratory based on mobile and applications (pp. 744–765). Hershey, PA: Medical
device. [IJCBPL]. International Journal of Cyber Behav- Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-
ior, Psychology and Learning, 3(2), 47–55. doi:10.4018/ 561-2.ch308
ijcbpl.2013040104
Low, R. (2011). Cognitive architecture and instructional
design in a multimedia context. In Instructional design:
Concepts, methodologies, tools and applications (pp.
496–510). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-60960-503-2.ch301

294
Related References

Low, R., Jin, P., & Sweller, J. (2014). Instructional design Manrique-de-Lara, P. Z. (2013). Does discretionary
in digital environments and availability of mental resources internet-based behavior of instructors contribute to stu-
for the aged subpopulation. In Assistive technologies: dent satisfaction? An empirical study on ‘cybercivism’.
Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applications (pp. [IJCBPL]. International Journal of Cyber Behavior,
1131–1154). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer- Psychology and Learning, 3(1), 50–66. doi:10.4018/
ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4422-9.ch059 ijcbpl.2013010105

Lu, J., & Peng, Y. (2014). Brain-computer interface for Markov, K., Vanhoof, K., Mitov, I., Depaire, B., Iva-
cyberpsychology: Components, methods, and applica- nova, K., Velychko, V., & Gladun, V. (2013). Intelligent
tions. [IJCBPL]. International Journal of Cyber Behav- data processing based on multi-dimensional numbered
ior, Psychology and Learning, 4(1), 1–14. doi:10.4018/ memory structures. In X. Naidenova & D. Ignatov (Eds.),
ijcbpl.2014010101 Diagnostic test approaches to machine learning and com-
monsense reasoning systems (pp. 156–184). Hershey, PA:
Ludvig, E. A., Bellemare, M. G., & Pearson, K. G.
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
(2011). A primer on reinforcement learning in the brain:
1900-5.ch007
Psychological, computational, and neural perspectives.
In E. Alonso & E. Mondragón (Eds.), Computational Martin, J. N. (2014). How can we incorporate relevant
neuroscience for advancing artificial intelligence: Models, findings from psychology into systems methods? [IJSS].
methods and applications (pp. 111–144). Hershey, PA: International Journal of Systems and Society, 1(1), 1–11.
Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978- doi:10.4018/ijss.2014010101
1-60960-021-1.ch006
Mayer, I., Bekebrede, G., Warmelink, H., & Zhou, Q.
Lützenberger, M. (2014). A driver’s mind: Psychology (2014). A brief methodology for researching and evaluat-
runs simulation. In D. Janssens, A. Yasar, & L. Knapen ing serious games and game-based learning. In T. Con-
(Eds.), Data science and simulation in transportation re- nolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger
search (pp. 182–205). Hershey, PA: Information Science (Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4920-0.ch010 games (pp. 357–393). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch017
Mancha, R., Yoder, C. Y., & Clark, J. G. (2013). Dynam-
ics of affect and cognition in simulated agents: Bridging Mazumdar, B. D., & Mishra, R. B. (2011). Cognitive
the gap between experimental and simulation research. parameter based agent selection and negotiation process
[IJATS]. International Journal of Agent Technologies for B2C e-commerce. In V. Sugumaran (Ed.), Intelligent,
and Systems, 5(2), 78–96. doi:10.4018/jats.2013040104 adaptive and reasoning technologies: New developments
and applications (pp. 181–203). Hershey, PA: Information
Manchiraju, S. (2014). Predicting behavioral intentions
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-595-7.ch010
toward sustainable fashion consumption: A comparison of
attitude-behavior and value-behavior consistency models. McLaren, I. (2011). APECS: An adaptively parameterised
In H. Kaufmann & M. Panni (Eds.), Handbook of research model of associative learning and memory. In E. Alonso
on consumerism in business and marketing: Concepts and & E. Mondragón (Eds.), Computational neuroscience
practices (pp. 225–243). Hershey, PA: Business Science for advancing artificial intelligence: Models, methods
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5880-6.ch011 and applications (pp. 145–164). Hershey, PA: Medical
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-
Mancilla, R. L. (2013). Getting smart about split atten-
021-1.ch007
tion. In B. Zou, M. Xing, Y. Wang, M. Sun, & C. Xiang
(Eds.), Computer-assisted foreign language teaching and
learning: Technological advances (pp. 210–229). Hershey,
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
2821-2.ch012

295
Related References

McMurray, B., Zhao, L., Kucker, S. C., & Samuelson, L. Mpofu, S. (2014). Memory, national identity, and free-
K. (2013). Pushing the envelope of associative learning: dom of expression in the information age: Discussing
Internal representations and dynamic competition trans- the taboo in the Zimbabwean public sphere. In A. Solo
form association into development. In L. Gogate & G. (Ed.), Handbook of research on political activism in the
Hollich (Eds.), Theoretical and computational models of information age (pp. 114–128). Hershey, PA: Information
word learning: Trends in psychology and artificial intel- Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-6066-3.ch007
ligence (pp. 49–80). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Mulvey, F., & Heubner, M. (2014). Eye movements and
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2973-8.ch003
attention. In Assistive technologies: Concepts, methodolo-
Mena, R. J. (2014). The quest for a massively multiplayer gies, tools, and applications (pp. 1030–1054). Hershey,
online game that teaches physics. In T. Connolly, T. PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.), 4666-4422-9.ch053
Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious games
Munipov, V. (2011). Psychological and social problems
(pp. 292–316). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer-
of automation and computerization. In D. Haftor & A.
ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch014
Mirijamdotter (Eds.), Information and communication
Misra, S. (2011). Cognitive complexity measures: An technologies, society and human beings: Theory and
analysis. In A. Dogru & V. Biçer (Eds.), Modern software framework (Festschrift in honor of Gunilla Bradley) (pp.
engineering concepts and practices: Advanced approaches 136–146). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
(pp. 263–279). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer- 10.4018/978-1-60960-057-0.ch012
ence; 10.4018/978-1-60960-215-4.ch011
Najjar, M., & Mayers, A. (2012). A cognitive compu-
Mok, J. (2010). Social and distributed cognition in col- tational knowledge representation theory. In Machine
laborative learning contexts. In S. Dasgupta (Ed.), Social learning: Concepts, methodologies, tools and applica-
computing: Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applica- tions (pp. 1819–1838). Hershey, PA: Information Science
tions (pp. 1838–1854). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-818-7.ch708
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60566-984-7.ch121
Nakamura, H. (2011). Cognitive decline in patients with
Moore, J. E., & Love, M. S. (2013). An examination of Alzheimer’s disease: A six-year longitudinal study of
prestigious stigma: The case of the technology geek. In B. mini-mental state examination scores. In J. Wu (Ed.), Early
Medlin (Ed.), Integrations of technology utilization and detection and rehabilitation technologies for dementia:
social dynamics in organizations (pp. 48–73). Hershey, Neuroscience and biomedical applications (pp. 107–111).
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1- Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science Reference;
4666-1948-7.ch004 10.4018/978-1-60960-559-9.ch013

Moore, M. J., Nakano, T., Suda, T., & Enomoto, A. Nankee, C. (2010). Switch technologies. In S. Seok, E.
(2013). Social interactions and automated detection tools Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook of research on
in cyberbullying. In L. Caviglione, M. Coccoli, & A. human cognition and assistive technology: Design, acces-
Merlo (Eds.), Social network engineering for secure web sibility and transdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 157–168).
data and services (pp. 67–87). Hershey, PA: Information Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science Reference;
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-3926-3.ch004 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch010

Moseley, A. (2014). A case for integration: Assessment Nap, H. H., & Diaz-Orueta, U. (2014). Rehabilitation
and games. In T. Connolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, gaming. In J. Bishop (Ed.), Gamification for human
& P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy, and as- factors integration: Social, education, and psychological
sessment in serious games (pp. 342–356). Hershey, PA: issues (pp. 122–147). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5071-8.ch008
4773-2.ch016

296
Related References

Naranjo-Saucedo, A. B., Suárez-Mejías, C., Parra- O’Connell, R. M. (2014). Mind mapping for critical
Calderón, C. L., González-Aguado, E., Böckel-Martínez, thinking. In L. Shedletsky & J. Beaudry (Eds.), Cases
F., & Yuste-Marco, A. … Marco, A. (2014). Interactive on teaching critical thinking through visual representa-
games with robotic and augmented reality technology tion strategies (pp. 354–386). Hershey, PA: Information
in cognitive and motor rehabilitation. In Robotics: Con- Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5816-5.ch014
cepts, methodologies, tools, and applications (pp. 1233-
Okrigwe, B. N. (2010). Cognition and learning. In S.
1254). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference.
Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook of
doi:10.4018/978-1-4666-4607-0.ch059
research on human cognition and assistive technology:
Ndinguri, E., Machtmes, K., Hatala, J. P., & Coco, M. Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary perspectives
L. (2014). Learning through immersive virtual environ- (pp. 388–400). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science
ments: An organizational context. In J. Keengwe, G. Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch027
Schnellert, & K. Kungu (Eds.), Cross-cultural online
Ong, E. H., & Khan, J. Y. (2013). Cognitive cooperation
learning in higher education and corporate training (pp.
in wireless networks. In M. Ku & J. Lin (Eds.), Cogni-
185–199). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
tive radio and interference management: Technology and
10.4018/978-1-4666-5023-7.ch010
strategy (pp. 179–204). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Ninaus, M., Witte, M., Kober, S. E., Friedrich, E. V., Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2005-6.ch010
Kurzmann, J., & Hartsuiker, E. … Wood, G. (2014).
Orlova, M. (2014). Social psychology of health as a
Neurofeedback and serious games. In T. Connolly, T.
social-psychological situation. In Ş. Erçetin & S. Banerjee
Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.),
(Eds.), Chaos and complexity theory in world politics (pp.
Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious games
331–335). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
(pp. 82-110). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer-
10.4018/978-1-4666-6070-0.ch024
ence. doi:10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch005
Ortiz Zezzatti, C. A., Martínez, J., Castillo, N., González,
Nobre, F. S. (2012). The roles of cognitive machines in
S., & Hernández, P. (2012). Improve card collection
customer-centric organizations: Towards innovations in
from memory alpha using sociolinguistics and Japanese
computational organizational management networks. In
puzzles. In C. Ortiz Zezzatti, C. Chira, A. Hernandez, &
F. Nobre, D. Walker, & R. Harris (Eds.), Technologi-
M. Basurto (Eds.), Logistics management and optimiza-
cal, managerial and organizational core competencies:
tion through hybrid artificial intelligence systems (pp.
Dynamic innovation and sustainable development (pp.
310–326). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
653–674). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-0297-7.ch012
10.4018/978-1-61350-165-8.ch035
Orr, K., & McGuinness, C. (2014). What is the “learn-
Norris, S. E. (2014). Transformative curriculum design
ing” in games-based learning? In T. Connolly, T. Hainey,
and program development: Creating effective adult
E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.), Psychol-
learning by leveraging psychological capital and self-
ogy, pedagogy, and assessment in serious games (pp.
directedness through the exercise of human agency. In V.
221-242). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference.
Wang & V. Bryan (Eds.), Andragogical and pedagogical
doi:10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch011
methods for curriculum and program development (pp.
118–141). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; Ota, N., Maeshima, S., Osawa, A., Kawarada, M., &
10.4018/978-1-4666-5872-1.ch007 Tanemura, J. (2011). Prospective memory impairment in
remembering to remember in mild cognitive impairment
Norris, S. E., & Porter, T. H. (2013). Self-monitoring scale.
and healthy subjects. In J. Wu (Ed.), Early detection and
In M. Bocarnea, R. Reynolds, & J. Baker (Eds.), Online
rehabilitation technologies for dementia: Neuroscience
instruments, data collection, and electronic measurements:
and biomedical applications (pp. 98–106). Hershey, PA:
Organizational advancements (pp. 118–133). Hershey,
Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
1-60960-559-9.ch012
4666-2172-5.ch007

297
Related References

Ouwehand, K., van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2013). The Pereira, R., Hornung, H., & Baranauskas, M. C. (2014).
use of gesturing to facilitate older adults’ learning from Cognitive authority revisited in web social interaction.
computer-based dynamic visualizations. In R. Zheng, R. In M. Pańkowska (Ed.), Frameworks of IT prosumption
Hill, & M. Gardner (Eds.), Engaging older adults with for business development (pp. 142–157). Hershey, PA:
modern technology: Internet use and information access Business Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4313-
needs (pp. 33–58). Hershey, PA: Information Science 0.ch010
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-1966-1.ch003
Phebus, A. M., Gitlin, B., Shuffler, M. L., & Wildman,
Özel, S. (2012). Utilizing cognitive resources in user J. L. (2014). Leading global virtual teams: The support-
interface designs. In B. Khan (Ed.), User interface design ing role of trust and team cognition. In E. Nikoi & K.
for virtual environments: Challenges and advances (pp. Boateng (Eds.), Collaborative communication processes
115–123). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; and decision making in organizations (pp. 177–200).
10.4018/978-1-61350-516-8.ch007 Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
1-4666-4478-6.ch010
Parsons, T. D., & Courtney, C. G. (2011). Neurocognitive
and psychophysiological interfaces for adaptive virtual Plunkett, D., Banerjee, R., & Horn, E. (2010). Supporting
environments. In M. Ziefle & C. Röcker (Eds.), Human- early childhood outcomes through assistive technology.
centered design of e-health technologies: Concepts, In S. Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook
methods and applications (pp. 208–233). Hershey, PA: of research on human cognition and assistive technology:
Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978- Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary perspectives
1-60960-177-5.ch009 (pp. 339–359). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch024
Peden, B. F., & Tiry, A. M. (2013). Using web surveys
for psychology experiments: A case study in new media Prakash, S., Vaish, A., Coul, N., Saravana, G. K.,
technology for research. In N. Sappleton (Ed.), Ad- Srinidhi, T. N., & Botsa, J. (2014). Child security in
vancing research methods with new technologies (pp. cyberspace through moral cognition. In Cyber behavior:
70–99). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applications (pp.
10.4018/978-1-4666-3918-8.ch005 1946–1958). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer-
ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5942-1.ch102
Perakslis, C. (2014). Willingness to adopt RFID implants:
Do personality factors play a role in the acceptance of Prescott, J., & Bogg, J. (2013). Self, career, and gender
uberveillance? In M. Michael & K. Michael (Eds.), issues: A complex interplay of internal/external factors. In
Uberveillance and the social implications of microchip Gendered occupational differences in science, engineer-
implants: Emerging technologies (pp. 144–168). Hershey, ing, and technology careers (pp. 79–111). Hershey, PA:
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
4582-0.ch006 2107-7.ch004

Pereira, G., Brisson, A., Dias, J., Carvalho, A., Dimas, Prescott, J., & Bogg, J. (2013). Stereotype, attitudes,
J., & Mascarenhas, S. … Paiva, A. (2014). Non-player and identity: Gendered expectations and behaviors. In
characters and artificial intelligence. In T. Connolly, T. Gendered occupational differences in science, engineer-
Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.), ing, and technology careers (pp. 112–135). Hershey, PA:
Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious games Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
(pp. 127-152). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer- 2107-7.ch005
ence. doi:10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch007
Pressey, A., Salciuviene, L., & Barnes, S. (2013). Uncov-
ering relationships between emotional states and higher-
order needs: Enhancing consumer emotional experiences
in computer-mediated environment. [IJOM]. International
Journal of Online Marketing, 3(1), 31–46. doi:10.4018/
ijom.2013010103

298
Related References

Qin, X., Li, C., Chen, H., Qin, B., Du, X., & Wang, S. Revett, K. (2012). Cognitive biometrics: A novel approach
(2014). In memory data processing systems. In J. Wang to continuous person authentication. In I. Traore & A.
(Ed.), Encyclopedia of business analytics and optimiza- Ahmed (Eds.), Continuous authentication using biomet-
tion (pp. 1182–1191). Hershey, PA: Business Science rics: Data, models, and metrics (pp. 105–136). Hershey,
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5202-6.ch109 PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
61350-129-0.ch006
Ramos, I., & Oliveira e Sá, J. (2014). Organizational
memory: The role of business intelligence to leverage Rødseth, I. (2011). A motive analysis as a first step in
the application of collective knowledge. In H. Rahman designing technology for the use of intuition in criminal
& R. de Sousa (Eds.), Information systems and technol- investigation. In A. Mesquita (Ed.), Sociological and
ogy for organizational agility, intelligence, and resilience philosophical aspects of human interaction with technol-
(pp. 206–223). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference; ogy: Advancing concepts (pp. 276–298). Hershey, PA:
10.4018/978-1-4666-5970-4.ch010 Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-
575-9.ch015
Ratten, V. (2013). The development of social e-enterprises,
mobile communication and social networks: A social Rothblatt, M. (2014). Mindclone technoselves: Multi-
cognitive perspective of technological innovation. [JECO]. substrate legal identities, cyber-psychology, and biocy-
Journal of Electronic Commerce in Organizations, 11(3), berethics. In Cyber behavior: Concepts, methodologies,
68–77. doi:10.4018/jeco.2013070104 tools, and applications (pp. 1199–1216). Hershey, PA:
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
Reddy, Y. B. (2013). Nanocomputing in cognitive radio
5942-1.ch062
networks to improve the performance. In N. Meghana-
than & Y. Reddy (Eds.), Cognitive radio technology ap- Rückemann, C. (2013). Integrated information and
plications for wireless and mobile ad hoc networks (pp. computing systems for advanced cognition with natural
173–193). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; sciences. In C. Rückemann (Ed.), Integrated information
10.4018/978-1-4666-4221-8.ch010 and computing systems for natural, spatial, and social
sciences (pp. 1–26). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Redien-Collot, R., & Lefebvre, M. R. (2014). SMEs’
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2190-9.ch001
leaders: Building collective cognition and competences
to trigger positive strategic outcomes. In K. Todorov & Rudnianski, M., & Kravcik, M. (2014). The road to criti-
D. Smallbone (Eds.), Handbook of research on strate- cal thinking and intelligence analysis. In T. Connolly, T.
gic management in small and medium enterprises (pp. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.),
143–158). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference; Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious games
10.4018/978-1-4666-5962-9.ch008 (pp. 47–61). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch003
Remmele, B., & Whitton, N. (2014). Disrupting the magic
circle: The impact of negative social gaming behaviours. In Rufer, R., & Adams, R. H. (2012). Adapting three-
T. Connolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno- dimensional-virtual world to reach diverse learners in an
Ger (Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in MBA program. In H. Yang & S. Yuen (Eds.), Handbook of
serious games (pp. 111–126). Hershey, PA: Information research on practices and outcomes in virtual worlds and
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch006 environments (pp. 606–619). Hershey, PA: Information
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-762-3.ch033
Renaud, P., Chartier, S., Fedoroff, P., Bradford, J., &
Rouleau, J. L. (2011). The use of virtual reality in clinical Saadé, R. G. (2010). Cognitive mapping decision sup-
psychology research. In Clinical technologies: Concepts, port for the design of web-based learning environments.
methodologies, tools and applications (pp. 2073–2093). [IJWLTT]. International Journal of Web-Based Learning
Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science Reference; and Teaching Technologies, 5(3), 36–53. doi:10.4018/
10.4018/978-1-60960-561-2.ch805 jwltt.2010070103

299
Related References

Saeed, N., & Sinnappan, S. (2014). Comparing learning Seo, K. K., Byk, A., & Collins, C. (2011). Cognitive
styles and technology acceptance of two culturally differ- apprenticeship inspired simulations. In Gaming and
ent groups of students. In T. Issa, P. Isaias, & P. Kommers simulations: Concepts, methodologies, tools and applica-
(Eds.), Multicultural awareness and technology in higher tions (pp. 346–358). Hershey, PA: Information Science
education: Global perspectives (pp. 244–264). Hershey, Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-195-9.ch202
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
Serenko, N. (2014). Informational, physical, and psy-
4666-5876-9.ch012
chological privacy as determinants of patient behaviour
Samuelson, L. K., Spencer, J. P., & Jenkins, G. W. (2013). in health care. In V. Michell, D. Rosenorn-Lanng, S.
A dynamic neural field model of word learning. In L. Gulliver, & W. Currie (Eds.), Handbook of research on
Gogate & G. Hollich (Eds.), Theoretical and computa- patient safety and quality care through health informatics
tional models of word learning: Trends in psychology and (pp. 1–20). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science
artificial intelligence (pp. 1–27). Hershey, PA: Information Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4546-2.ch001
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2973-8.ch001
Shibata, T. (2013). A human-like cognitive computer
Sanjram, P. K., & Gupta, M. (2013). Task difficulty and based on a psychologically inspired VLSI brain model. In
time constraint in programmer multitasking: An analysis J. Wu (Ed.), Technological advancements in biomedicine
of prospective memory performance and cognitive work- for healthcare applications (pp. 247–266). Hershey, PA:
load. [IJGC]. International Journal of Green Computing, Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
4(1), 35–57. doi:10.4018/jgc.2013010103 1-4666-2196-1.ch027

Sato, Y., Ji, Z., & van Dijk, S. (2013). I think I have heard Shirkhodaee, M., & Rezaee, S. (2013). Evaluating the
that one before: Recurrence-based word learning with persuasive and memory effects of viral advertising.
a robot. In L. Gogate & G. Hollich (Eds.), Theoretical [IJOM]. International Journal of Online Marketing, 3(3),
and computational models of word learning: Trends in 51–61. doi:10.4018/ijom.2013070104
psychology and artificial intelligence (pp. 327–349). Her-
Simzar, R., & Domina, T. (2014). Attending to student
shey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
motivation through critical practice: A recommendation
1-4666-2973-8.ch014
for improving accelerated mathematical learning. In
Scalzone, F., & Zontini, G. (2013). Thinking animals S. Lawrence (Ed.), Critical practice in P-12 education:
and thinking machines in psychoanalysis and beyond. In Transformative teaching and learning (pp. 66–116). Her-
F. Orsucci & N. Sala (Eds.), Complexity science, living shey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
systems, and reflexing interfaces: New models and per- 1-4666-5059-6.ch004
spectives (pp. 44–68). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Smart, P. R., Engelbrecht, P. C., Braines, D., Strub, M.,
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2077-3.ch003
& Giammanco, C. (2010). The network-extended mind.
Schafer, S. B. (2013). Fostering psychological coherence: In D. Verma (Ed.), Network science for military coalition
With ICTs. In M. Cruz-Cunha, I. Miranda, & P. Gonçalves operations: Information exchange and interaction (pp.
(Eds.), Handbook of research on ICTs for human-centered 191–236). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
healthcare and social care services (pp. 29–47). Hershey, 10.4018/978-1-61520-855-5.ch010
PA: Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
Smith, M. A. (2011). Functions of unconscious and
1-4666-3986-7.ch002
conscious emotion in the regulation of implicit and ex-
Scheiter, K., Wiebe, E., & Holsanova, J. (2011). Theoreti- plicit motivated behavior. In D. Gökçay & G. Yildirim
cal and instructional aspects of learning with visualiza- (Eds.), Affective computing and interaction: Psycho-
tions. In Instructional design: Concepts, methodologies, logical, cognitive and neuroscientific perspectives (pp.
tools and applications (pp. 1667–1688). Hershey, PA: 25–55). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960- 10.4018/978-1-61692-892-6.ch002
503-2.ch710

300
Related References

Somyürek, S. (2012). Interactive learning in workplace Suárez, M. G., & Gumiel, C. G. (2014). The use of senso-
training. In J. Jia (Ed.), Educational stages and interactive rial marketing in stores: Attracting clients through their
learning: From kindergarten to workplace training (pp. senses. In F. Musso & E. Druica (Eds.), Handbook of
498–514). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; research on retailer-consumer relationship development
10.4018/978-1-4666-0137-6.ch027 (pp. 258–274). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-6074-8.ch014
Soliman, F. (2014). Could knowledge, learning, and in-
novation gaps be spiralling? In F. Soliman (Ed.), Learning Sugiura, M. (2013). A cognitive neuroscience approach
models for innovation in organizations: Examining roles to self and mental health. In J. Wu (Ed.), Biomedical
of knowledge transfer and human resources management engineering and cognitive neuroscience for healthcare:
(pp. 1–29). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference; Interdisciplinary applications (pp. 1–10). Hershey, PA:
10.4018/978-1-4666-4884-5.ch001 Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
1-4666-2113-8.ch001
Spadaro, L., Timpano, F., Marino, S., & Bramanti, P.
(2013). Telemedicine and Alzheimer disease: ICT- Sujo-Montes, L. E., Armfield, S. W., Yen, C., & Tu, C.
based services for people with Alzheimer disease and (2014). The use of ubiquitous learning for children with
their caregivers. In V. Gulla, A. Mori, F. Gabbrielli, & Down Syndrome. In F. Neto (Ed.), Technology platform
P. Lanzafame (Eds.), Telehealth networks for hospital innovations and forthcoming trends in ubiquitous learning
services: New methodologies (pp. 191–206). Hershey, (pp. 160–176). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer-
PA: Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978- ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4542-4.ch009
1-4666-2979-0.ch013
Tamba, H. (2013). Workers’ mental health problems and
Spiegel, T. (2014). An overview of cognition roles in future perspectives in Japan: Psychological job stress
decision-making. In J. Wang (Ed.), Encyclopedia of research. In J. Wu (Ed.), Biomedical engineering and
business analytics and optimization (pp. 74–84). Hershey, cognitive neuroscience for healthcare: Interdisciplin-
PA: Business Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- ary applications (pp. 370–379). Hershey, PA: Medical
5202-6.ch008 Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
2113-8.ch038
Stachon, Z., & Šašinka, C. (2012). Human cognition:
People in the world and world in their minds. In T. Tatachari, S., Manikandan, K. S., & Gunta, S. (2014). A
Podobnikar & M. Čeh (Eds.), Universal ontology of geo- synthesis of organizational learning and knowledge man-
graphic space: Semantic enrichment for spatial data (pp. agement literatures. In M. Chilton & J. Bloodgood (Eds.),
97–122). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; Knowledge management and competitive advantage:
10.4018/978-1-4666-0327-1.ch005 Issues and potential solutions (pp. 122–147). Hershey,
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
Stefurak, J. R., Surry, D. W., & Hayes, R. L. (2011). Tech-
4666-4679-7.ch008
nology in the supervision of mental health professionals:
Ethical, interpersonal, and epistemological implications. Taxén, L. (2010). Cognitive grounding. In L. Taxen (Ed.),
In D. Surry, R. Gray, Jr., & J. Stefurak (Eds.), Technology Using activity domain theory for managing complex sys-
integration in higher education: Social and organizational tems (pp. 108–124). Hershey, PA: Information Science
aspects (pp. 114–131). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60566-192-6.ch006
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-147-8.ch009
Te’eni, D. (2012). Knowledge for communicating
Stieglitz, S. (2014). The American memory project. In J. knowledge. In Organizational learning and knowledge:
Krueger (Ed.), Cases on electronic records and resource Concepts, methodologies, tools and applications (pp.
management implementation in diverse environments (pp. 1656–1665). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference;
106–116). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-783-8.ch501
10.4018/978-1-4666-4466-3.ch006

301
Related References

Terras, M. M., & Ramsay, J. (2014). E-learning, mobility, Torres, G., Jaime, K., & Ramos, F. (2013). Brain architec-
and time: A psychological framework. In E. Barbera & P. ture for visual object identification. [IJCINI]. International
Reimann (Eds.), Assessment and evaluation of time factors Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence,
in online teaching and learning (pp. 63–90). Hershey, PA: 7(1), 75–97. doi:10.4018/jcini.2013010104
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
Trajkovski, G., Stojanov, G., Collins, S., Eidelman, V.,
4651-3.ch003
Harman, C., & Vincenti, G. (2011). Cognitive robotics
Thapa, A. (2013). A study on worker’s perceptions of and multiagency in a fuzzy modeling framework. In
psychological capital on their earnings. [IJABE]. Inter- G. Trajkovski (Ed.), Developments in intelligent agent
national Journal of Applied Behavioral Economics, 2(3), technologies and multi-agent systems: Concepts and
27–42. doi:10.4018/ijabe.2013070103 applications (pp. 132–152). Hershey, PA: Information
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-171-3.ch009
Thatcher, A., & Ndabeni, M. (2013). A psychological
model to understand e-adoption in the context of the digi- Tran, B. (2014). Rhetoric of play: Utilizing the gamer
tal divide. In Digital literacy: Concepts, methodologies, factor in selecting and training employees. In T. Con-
tools, and applications (pp. 1402–1424). Hershey, PA: nolly, T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- (Eds.), Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious
1852-7.ch074 games (pp. 175–203). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch009
Thompson, K., & Markauskaite, L. (2014). Identifying
group processes and affect in learners: A holistic approach Tran, B. (2014). The psychology of consumerism in
to assessment in virtual worlds in higher education. In S. business and marketing: The macro and micro behaviors
Kennedy-Clark, K. Everett, & P. Wheeler (Eds.), Cases of Hofstede’s cultural consumers. In H. Kaufmann &
on the assessment of scenario and game-based virtual M. Panni (Eds.), Handbook of research on consumerism
worlds in higher education (pp. 175–210). Hershey, PA: in business and marketing: Concepts and practices (pp.
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- 286–308). Hershey, PA: Business Science Reference;
4470-0.ch006 10.4018/978-1-4666-5880-6.ch014

Tiwary, U. S., & Siddiqui, T. J. (2014). Working to- Travica, B. (2014). Homo informaticus. In Examining the
gether with computers: Towards a general framework informing view of organization: Applying theoretical and
for collaborative human computer interaction. In Assis- managerial approaches (pp. 34–66). Hershey, PA: Business
tive technologies: Concepts, methodologies, tools, and Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5986-5.ch002
applications (pp. 141–162). Hershey, PA: Information
Twomey, K. E., Horst, J. S., & Morse, A. F. (2013).
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4422-9.ch008
An embodied model of young children’s categorization
Titus, C. S. (2013). The use of developmental psychology and word learning. In L. Gogate & G. Hollich (Eds.),
in ethics: Beyond Kohlberg and Seligman? In F. Doridot, Theoretical and computational models of word learn-
P. Duquenoy, P. Goujon, A. Kurt, S. Lavelle, N. Patrignani, ing: Trends in psychology and artificial intelligence (pp.
et al. (Eds.), Ethical governance of emerging technologies 172–196). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
development (pp. 266–286). Hershey, PA: Information 10.4018/978-1-4666-2973-8.ch008
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-3670-5.ch018
Ursyn, A. (2014). Cognitive processes involved in vi-
Tomono, A. (2013). Display technology of images with sual thought. In Perceptions of knowledge visualization:
scents and its psychological evaluation. In T. Nakamoto Explaining concepts through meaningful images (pp.
(Ed.), Human olfactory displays and interfaces: Odor 131–173). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
sensing and presentation (pp. 429–445). Hershey, PA: 10.4018/978-1-4666-4703-9.ch005
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
2521-1.ch022

302
Related References

Ursyn, A. (2014). Communication through many senses. van Rosmalen, P., Wilson, A., & Hummel, H. G. (2014).
In Perceptions of knowledge visualization: Explaining Games for and by teachers and learners. In T. Connolly,
concepts through meaningful images (pp. 25–60). Her- T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.),
shey, PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978- Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious games
1-4666-4703-9.ch002 (pp. 243–269). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer-
ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch012
Ursyn, A. (2014). Four trapped in an elevator. In Compu-
tational solutions for knowledge, art, and entertainment: Vega, J., Perdices, E., & Cañas, J. M. (2014). Atten-
Information exchange beyond text (pp. 322–329). Hershey, tive visual memory for robot localization. In Robotics:
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applications (pp.
4627-8.ch016 785-811). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference.
doi:10.4018/978-1-4666-4607-0.ch038
Usart, M., & Romero, M. (2014). Time factor assessment in
game-based learning: Time perspective and time-on-task Vinther, J. (2012). Cognitive skills through CALL-en-
as individual differences between players. In T. Connolly, hanced teacher training. In F. Zhang (Ed.), Computer-en-
T. Hainey, E. Boyle, G. Baxter, & P. Moreno-Ger (Eds.), hanced and mobile-assisted language learning: Emerging
Psychology, pedagogy, and assessment in serious games issues and trends (pp. 158–187). Hershey, PA: Information
(pp. 62–81). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61350-065-1.ch008
10.4018/978-1-4666-4773-2.ch004
Vogel, E. H., & Ponce, F. P. (2011). Empirical issues
Usoro, A., Majewski, G., & Bloom, L. (2012). Individual and theoretical mechanisms of Pavlovian conditioning.
and collaborative approaches in e-learning design. In In E. Alonso & E. Mondragón (Eds.), Computational
Virtual learning environments: Concepts, methodologies, neuroscience for advancing artificial intelligence: Models,
tools and applications (pp. 1110–1130). Hershey, PA: methods and applications (pp. 81–110). Hershey, PA:
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666- Medical Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-
0011-9.ch514 1-60960-021-1.ch005

van den Brink, J. C. (2014). How positive psychology can Vragov, R. (2013). Detecting behavioral biases in mixed
support sustainable project management. In Sustainable human-proxy online auction markets. [IJSITA]. Interna-
practices: Concepts, methodologies, tools and applica- tional Journal of Strategic Information Technology and
tions (pp. 958–973). Hershey, PA: Information Science Applications, 4(4), 60–79. doi:10.4018/ijsita.2013100104
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4852-4.ch053
Wagner, C. L., & Delisi, J. (2010). Multi-sensory en-
van der Helden, J., & Bekkering, H. (2014). The role of im- vironments and augmentative communication tools. In
plicit and explicit feedback in learning and the implications S. Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook of
for distance education techniques. In T. Yuzer & G. Eby research on human cognition and assistive technology:
(Eds.), Handbook of research on emerging priorities and Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary perspectives
trends in distance education: Communication, pedagogy, (pp. 121–131). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science
and technology (pp. 367–384). Hershey, PA: Information Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch008
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-5162-3.ch025
Walk, A. M., & Conway, C. M. (2014). Two distinct
van Mierlo, C. M., Jarodzka, H., Kirschner, F., & Kirsch- sequence learning mechanisms for syntax acquisi-
ner, P. A. (2012). Cognitive load theory in e-learning. tion and word learning. In Computational linguistics:
In Z. Yan (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cyber behavior (pp. Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applications (pp.
1178–1211). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer- 540–560). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-0315-8.ch097 10.4018/978-1-4666-6042-7.ch025

303
Related References

Wang, H. (2014). A guide to assistive technology for Wang, Y., Patel, S., & Patel, D. (2013). The cogni-
teachers in special education. In Assistive technologies: tive process and formal models of human attentions.
Concepts, methodologies, tools, and applications (pp. [IJSSCI]. International Journal of Software Science and
12–25). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; Computational Intelligence, 5(1), 32–50. doi:10.4018/
10.4018/978-1-4666-4422-9.ch002 ijssci.2013010103

Wang, J. (2012). Organizational learning and technology. Wang, Y., Pedrycz, W., Baciu, G., Chen, P., Wang, G.,
In V. Wang (Ed.), Encyclopedia of e-leadership, counsel- & Yao, Y. (2012). Perspectives on cognitive computing
ing and training (pp. 154–170). Hershey, PA: Information and applications. In Y. Wang (Ed.), Breakthroughs in
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61350-068-2.ch012 software science and computational intelligence (pp.
1–12). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
Wang, K. Y. (2014). Mixing metaphors: Sociological and
10.4018/978-1-4666-0264-9.ch001
psychological perspectives on virtual communities. In
Cross-cultural interaction: Concepts, methodologies, tools Wang, Y., Widrow, B. C., Zhang, B., Kinsner, W., Suga-
and applications (pp. 116–132). Hershey, PA: Information wara, K., Sun, F., et al. (2013). Perspectives on the field
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-4979-8.ch008 of cognitive informatics and its future development. In Y.
Wang (Ed.), Cognitive informatics for revealing human
Wang, Y. (2013). Neuroinformatics models of human
cognition: Knowledge manipulations in natural intel-
memory: Mapping the cognitive functions of memory
ligence (pp. 20–34). Hershey, PA: Information Science
onto neurophysiological structures of the brain. [IJCINI].
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2476-4.ch002
International Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural
Intelligence, 7(1), 98–122. doi:10.4018/jcini.2013010105 Was, C. A., & Woltz, D. J. (2013). Implicit memory and
aging: Adapting technology to utilize preserved memory
Wang, Y. (2013). The cognitive mechanisms and formal
functions. In R. Zheng, R. Hill, & M. Gardner (Eds.),
models of consciousness. [IJCINI]. International Journal
Engaging older adults with modern technology: Internet
of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence, 6(2),
use and information access needs (pp. 1–19). Hershey,
23–40. doi:10.4018/jcini.2012040102
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-
Wang, Y. (2013). Towards the synergy of cognitive infor- 4666-1966-1.ch001
matics, neural informatics, brain informatics, and cognitive
Wei, H. (2013). A neural dynamic model based on ac-
computing. In Y. Wang (Ed.), Cognitive informatics for
tivation diffusion and a micro-explanation for cognitive
revealing human cognition: Knowledge manipulations in
operations. [IJCINI]. International Journal of Cogni-
natural intelligence (pp. 1–19). Hershey, PA: Information
tive Informatics and Natural Intelligence, 6(2), 1–22.
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2476-4.ch001
doi:10.4018/jcini.2012040101
Wang, Y., Berwick, R. C., Haykin, S., Pedrycz, W.,
Wexler, R. H., & Roff-Wexler, S. (2013). The evolution
Kinsner, W., & Baciu, G. … Gavrilova, M. L. (2013).
and development of self in virtual worlds. [IJCBPL].
Cognitive informatics and cognitive computing in year
International Journal of Cyber Behavior, Psychology
10 and beyond. In Y. Wang (Ed.), Cognitive informatics
and Learning, 3(1), 1–6. doi:10.4018/ijcbpl.2013010101
for revealing human cognition: Knowledge manipula-
tions in natural intelligence (pp. 140-157). Hershey, PA: Wickramasinghe, N. (2012). Knowledge economy for
Information Science Reference. doi:10.4018/978-1-4666- innovating organizations. In Organizational learning and
2476-4.ch010 knowledge: Concepts, methodologies, tools and applica-
tions (pp. 2298–2309). Hershey, PA: Business Science
Wang, Y., Fariello, G., Gavrilova, M. L., Kinsner, W.,
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-60960-783-8.ch616
Mizoguchi, F., Patel, S., et al. (2013). Perspectives on
cognitive computers and knowledge processors. [IJCINI].
International Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural
Intelligence, 7(3), 1–24. doi:10.4018/ijcini.2013070101

304
Related References

Widrow, B. C., & Aragon, J. (2012). Cognitive memory: Winsor, D. L., & Blake, S. (2012). Socrates and Descartes
Human like memory. In Y. Wang (Ed.), Breakthroughs meet the E*Trade baby: The impact of early technology
in software science and computational intelligence (pp. on children’s developing beliefs about knowledge and
84–99). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference; knowing. In S. Blake, D. Winsor, & L. Allen (Eds.), Child
10.4018/978-1-4666-0264-9.ch006 development and the use of technology: Perspectives,
applications and experiences (pp. 1–20). Hershey, PA:
Wilson, M. S., & Pascoe, J. (2010). Using software to
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61350-
deliver language intervention in inclusionary settings. In
317-1.ch001
S. Seok, E. Meyen, & B. DaCosta (Eds.), Handbook of
research on human cognition and assistive technology: Yakavenka, H. (2012). Developing professional compe-
Design, accessibility and transdisciplinary perspectives tencies through international peer learning communities.
(pp. 132–156). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science In V. Dennen & J. Myers (Eds.), Virtual professional
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61520-817-3.ch009 development and informal learning via social networks
(pp. 134–154). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer-
Wilson, S., & Haslam, N. (2013). Reasoning about human
ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-1815-2.ch008
enhancement: Towards a folk psychological model of
human nature and human identity. In R. Luppicini (Ed.), Yamaguchi, M., & Shetty, V. (2013). Evaluating the
Handbook of research on technoself: Identity in a techno- psychobiologic effects of fragrances through salivary
logical society (pp. 175–188). Hershey, PA: Information biomarkers. In T. Nakamoto (Ed.), Human olfactory dis-
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2211-1.ch010 plays and interfaces: Odor sensing and presentation (pp.
359–369). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
Wilson, S. G. (2014). Enhancement and identity: A social
10.4018/978-1-4666-2521-1.ch017
psychological perspective. In S. Thompson (Ed.), Global
issues and ethical considerations in human enhancement Yan, Z., & Zheng, R. Z. (2013). Growing from childhood
technologies (pp. 241–256). Hershey, PA: Medical Infor- into adolescence: The science of cyber behavior. In R.
mation Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-6010-6. Zheng (Ed.), Evolving psychological and educational
ch014 perspectives on cyber behavior (pp. 1–14). Hershey, PA:
Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
Widyanto, L., & Griffiths, M. (2013). An empirical
1858-9.ch001
study of problematic internet use and self-esteem. In R.
Zheng (Ed.), Evolving psychological and educational Yildirim, G., & Gökçay, D. (2011). Problems associated
perspectives on cyber behavior (pp. 82–95). Hershey, with computer-mediated communication cognitive psy-
PA: Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1- chology and neuroscience perspectives. In D. Gökçay &
4666-1858-9.ch006 G. Yildirim (Eds.), Affective computing and interaction:
Psychological, cognitive and neuroscientific perspectives
Winsor, D. L. (2012). The epistemology of young children.
(pp. 244–261). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer-
In S. Blake, D. Winsor, & L. Allen (Eds.), Child develop-
ence; 10.4018/978-1-61692-892-6.ch011
ment and the use of technology: Perspectives, applications
and experiences (pp. 21–44). Hershey, PA: Information Younan, Y., Joosen, W., Piessens, F., & Van den Eynden,
Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-61350-317-1.ch002 H. (2012). Improving memory management security
for C and C. In K. Khan (Ed.), Security-aware systems
applications and software development methods (pp.
190–216). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-1580-9.ch011

305
Related References

Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2013). A sensory-motor solution Zhang, J., Luo, X., Lu, L., & Liu, W. (2013). An acqui-
to early word-referent learning. In L. Gogate & G. Hollich sition model of deep textual semantics based on human
(Eds.), Theoretical and computational models of word reading cognitive process. [IJCINI]. International Journal
learning: Trends in psychology and artificial intelligence of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence, 6(2),
(pp. 133–152). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer- 82–103. doi:10.4018/jcini.2012040105
ence; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2973-8.ch006
Zheng, R. Z. (2013). Effective online learning for older
Yu, J., Chen, Z., Lu, J., Liu, T., Zhou, L., Liu, X., et al. people: A heuristic design approach. In R. Zheng, R.
(2013). The important role of lipids in cognitive impair- Hill, & M. Gardner (Eds.), Engaging older adults with
ment. In Bioinformatics: Concepts, methodologies, tools, modern technology: Internet use and information access
and applications (pp. 268-272). Hershey, PA: Medical needs (pp. 142–159). Hershey, PA: Information Science
Information Science Reference. doi:10.4018/978-1-4666- Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-1966-1.ch008
3604-0.ch014
Zhou, M., & Xu, Y. (2013). Challenges to use recom-
Yu, Y., Yang, J., & Wu, J. (2013). Cognitive functions mender systems to enhance meta-cognitive functioning in
and neuronal mechanisms of tactile working memory. online learners. In Data mining: Concepts, methodologies,
In J. Wu (Ed.), Biomedical engineering and cognitive tools, and applications (pp. 1916–1935). Hershey, PA:
neuroscience for healthcare: Interdisciplinary applications Information Science Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-
(pp. 89–98). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science 2455-9.ch099
Reference; 10.4018/978-1-4666-2113-8.ch010
Ziaeehezarjeribi, Y., & Graves, I. (2013). Behind the
Zelinski, E. M. (2013). How interventions might improve MASK: Motivation through avatar skills and knowledge.
cognition in healthy older adults. [IJGCMS]. International In R. Ferdig (Ed.), Design, utilization, and analysis of
Journal of Gaming and Computer-Mediated Simulations, simulations and game-based educational worlds (pp.
5(3), 72–82. doi:10.4018/jgcms.2013070105 225–239). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference;
10.4018/978-1-4666-4018-4.ch014

306
307

Compilation of References

Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowl- Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined
edge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. by the desirability a controllability of trait adjectives.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(6),
53–63. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.53 PMID:10653505 1621–1630. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.6.1621

Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2003). The silence of the Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (1999). Task-switching: Positive
library: Environment, situational norm, and social behav- and negative priming of task-set. In G. W. Humphreys,
ior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), J. Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space, and
18–28. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.18 PMID:12518968 action: Studies in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 273–296).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Abbott, A. (2013). Disputed results a fresh blow for
social psychology: Failure to replicate intelligence- Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. Murchison (Ed.),
priming effects ignites row in research community. Nature, Handbook of Social Psychology. Worcester, MA: Clark
497(7447), 16. doi:10.1038/497016a PMID:23636371 University Press.

Abrams, M., & Reber, A. S. (1988). Implicit learning: Alter, A., Aronson, J., Darley, J., Rodriguez, C., & Ruble,
Robustness in the face of psychiatric disorders. Journal of D. N. (2010). Rising to the threat: Reducing stereotype
Psycholinguistic Research, 17, 425–439. PMID:3193378 threat by reframing the threat as a challenge. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 166–171.
Abrams, R. L., & Greenwald, A. G. (2000). Parts outweigh
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.014
the whole (word) in unconscious analysis of meaning. Psy-
chological Science, 11(2), 118–124. doi:10.1111/1467- Althusser, L. (1980). Ideology and Ideological State
9280.00226 PMID:11273418 Apparatuses. In Ideology and aestetc effect (pp. 35-99).
Ljubljana: Cankarjeva Založba.
Adams, A. (2010). Rehearsal or Reorganization: Two
patterns of literacy strategy use in secondary mathematics Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds:
classes. The Montana Math Enthusiast, 7(2&3), 371–390. The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature
Reviews. Neuroscience, 7(4), 268–277. doi:10.1038/
Addison, A., & O’ Hare, L. (2008). How can Massive
nrn1884 PMID:16552413
Multi-user Virtual Environments and Virtual Role-play
enhance traditional teaching practice? In Proceedings of Amodio, D. M., & Mendoza, S. A. (2010). Implicit
Researching Learning in Virtual EnvironmentsInterna- intergroup bias: Cognitive, affective, and motivational
tional Conference (pp. 7-16). The Open University. underpinnings. In B. K. Payne & B. Gawronski (Eds.),
Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement,
Agosta, S., Ghirardi, V., Zogmaister, C., Castiello, U., &
Theory, and Applications (pp. 353–374). New York:
Sartori, G. (2011). Detecting fakers of the autobiographi-
Guildford Press.
cal IAT. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(2), 299–306.
doi:10.1002/acp.1691
Compilation of References

Anderson, L.W., & Krathwohl. (Eds.). (2001). A Tax- Arif, M. I., Rashid, A., Tahira, S. S., & Akhter, M.
onomy for Learning, Teaching, and Assessing: A Revision (2012). Personality and Teaching: An Investigation into
of Bloom’s Taxonomy of Educational Objectives. New Prospective Teachers’ Personality. International Journal
York: Longman. of Humanities and Social Science, 2(17), 161–171.

Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power, optimism, Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele,
and risk-taking. European Journal of Social Psychology, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When White men can’t do
36(4), 511–536. doi:10.1002/ejsp.324 math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 29–46.
Anderson, C., & Thompson, L. L. (2004). Affect from
doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.1371
the top down: How powerful individuals’ positive affect
shapes negotiations. Organizational Behavior and Hu- Aronson, J., Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (1998).
man Decision Processes, 95(2), 125–139. doi:10.1016/j. Stereotype threat and the academic underperformance
obhdp.2004.05.002 of minorities and women. In J. K. Swim & C. Stangor
(Eds.), Prejudice: The target’s perspective (pp. 83–103).
Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory, and thought.
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
012679130-3/50039-9
Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of
Asgari, S., Dasgupta, N., & Stout, J. G. (2012). When
memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
do counterstereotypic ingroup members inspire versus
22(3), 261–295. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90201-3
deflate? The effect of successful professional women
Anderson, J. R. (2001). Kognitive Psychologie. Heidel- on young women’s leadership self-concept. Personal-
berg, Germany: Spektrum. ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(3), 370–383.
doi:10.1177/0146167211431968 PMID:22205625
Anderson, J. R., & Pirolli, P. L. (1984). Spread of activa-
tion. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Mem- Assor, A., & Gordon, D. (1987). The implicit learning the-
ory, and Cognition, 10(4), 791–798. doi:10.1037/0278- ory of hidden-curriculum research. Journal of Curriculum
7393.10.4.791 Studies, 19(4), 329–339. doi:10.1080/0022027870190404

Anderson, M. (1992). Intelligence and development: A Avons, S. E., Russo, R., Cinel, C., Verolini, V., Glynn,
cognitive theory. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. K., McDonald, R., & Cameron, M. (2009). Associative
and repetition priming with the repeated masked prime
Anyon, J. (1980). Social class and the hidden curriculum technique: No priming found. Memory & Cognition, 37(1),
of work. Journal of Education, 162, 67–92. 100–114. doi:10.3758/MC.37.1.100 PMID:19103980
Apple, M., W. (1992). School, teacher, authority. Lju- Ayer, A. J. (1956). The Problem of Knowledge. New York:
bljana: Scientific and Publistic Center. St Martin’s Press.
Apple, M. W. (1979). Ideology and Curriculum. London: Ayers, W. (1989). The good preschool teacher: Six pre-
Routledge and Kegan. doi:10.4324/9780203241219 school teachers reflect on their lives. New York: Teachers
Apple, M. W. (1995). Education and power. New York: College Press.
Routledge. Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2009).
Arcuri, L., Castelli, L., Galdi, S., Zogmaister, C., & Predicting actual behavior from the explicit and implicit
Amadori, A. (2008). Predicting the vote: Implicit atti- self-concept of personality. Journal of Personality and So-
tudes as predictors of the future behavior of decided and cial Psychology, 97(3), 533–548. doi:10.1037/a0016229
undecided voters. Political Psychology, 29(3), 369–387. PMID:19686006
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00635.x Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.

308
Compilation of References

Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Barani, G., Azma, F., & Seyyedrezai, S. H. (2011). Qual-
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, ity indicators of hidden curriculum in Centers of higher
49A(1), 5–28. doi:10.1080/713755608 education. Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences, 30,
1657–1661. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.10.321
Baddeley, A. D. (2000a). The episodic buffer: A new com-
ponent of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Barbot, A., & Kouider, S. (2012). Longer is not better:
4(11), 417–423. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 Nonconscious overstimulation reverses priming influences
PMID:11058819 under interocular suppression. Attention, Perception &
Psychophysics, 74(1), 174–184. doi:10.3758/s13414-
Baddeley, A. D. (2000b). Short-term and working mem-
011-0226-3 PMID:22033950
ory. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford
handbook of memory (pp. 77–92). Oxford, UK: Oxford Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity:
University Press. Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social
cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memoy.
of social cognition (pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and
motivation (pp. 47–89). Hove, UK: Academic Press. Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life.
In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (pp.
Baddely, A. D., & Logie, R. H. (1999). The multiple-com-
1–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
ponent model. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of
working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these
executive control (pp. 28–61). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology
University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139174909.005 of nonconscious social behavior. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 36(2), 147–168. doi:10.1002/ejsp.336
Baker, T. B., Brandon, T. H., & Chassin, L. (2004). Moti-
PMID:19844598
vational influences on cigarette smoking. Annual Review
of Psychology, 55(1), 463–491. doi:10.1146/annurev. Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C.
psych.55.090902.142054 PMID:14744223 (1996). The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditional
automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task.
Banaji, M. R. (2001). Iimplicit attitudes can be measured.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32(1),
In H. L. Roediger, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. Surprenant
104–128. doi:10.1006/jesp.1996.0005
(Eds.), The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Remember-
ing Robert G. Crowder (pp. 117–150). Washington, DC: Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automatic-
American Psychological Association. ity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and
stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory of mass
Social Psychology, 71(2), 230–244. doi:10.1037/0022-
communications. In J. Bryant & D. Zillman (Eds.), Media
3514.71.2.230 PMID:8765481
effects: Advances in theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum. Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar,
K., & Trötschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Noncon-
Bandura, A. (2004). Health promotion by social cognitive
scious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal
means. Health Education & Behavior, 31(2), 143–164.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 1014–1027.
doi:10.1177/1090198104263660 PMID:15090118
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1014 PMID:11761304
Barab, S., Thomas, M., Dodge, T., Carteaux, R., & Tuzun,
Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., &
H. (2005). Making learning fun: Quest Atlantis, a game
Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the Implicit Relational As-
without guns. Educational Technology Research and
sessment Procedure (IRAP) and the Relational Elaboration
Development, 53(1), 86–107. doi:10.1007/BF02504859
and Coherence (REC) model. The Psychological Record,
60, 527–542.

309
Compilation of References

Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayden, E., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Bassili, J. N., Smith, M. C., & MacLeod, C. M. (1989).
Stewart, I. (2008). The Implicit Relational Assessment Auditory and visual word-stem completion: Separating
Procedure (IRAP) as a response-time and event-related- data-driven and conceptually driven processes. Quarterly
potentials methodology for testing natural verbal rela- Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A(3), 439–453.
tions: A preliminary study. The Psychological Record, doi:10.1080/14640748908402375
58, 497–516.
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs,
Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayes, S. C., & Dymond, S. (2001). K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better per-
Self and self-directed rules. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes- formance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier
Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: A lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest,
post-Skinnerian account of language and cognition (pp. 4(1), 1–44. doi:10.1111/1529-1006.01431
119–139). New York: Plenum Press.
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996).
Barnes-Holmes, D., Keane, J., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggres-
Smeets, P. M. (2000). A derived transfer of emotive func- sion: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological
tions as a means of establishing differential preferences Review, 103(1), 5–33. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.5
for soft drinks. The Psychological Record, 50, 493–511. PMID:8650299

Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development Beer, J. S., & Hughes, B. L. (2010). Neural systems
of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from of social comparisons and the “above average” effect.
ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, NeuroImage, 49(3), 2671–2679. doi:10.1016/j.neuroim-
17(1), 53–58. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x age.2009.10.075 PMID:19883771
PMID:16371144
Bennett, J. (2007). Great actors access vulnerability. The
Barrett, P., & Zhang, Y. (2009). Optimal Learning Spaces. Jason Bennett Actor’s Workshop. Retrieved from http://
Design Implications for Primary Schools. SCRI Research www.jbactors.com/actingreading/actorsaccessvulner-
Report. Retrieved August 7, 2013, from http://www.oecd. ability.html
org/education/innovation-education/centreforeffective-
Benvenuti, L., Hennipman, E.-J., Oppelaar, E., & van der
learningenvironmentscele/43834191.pdf
Veer, G. (2008). Experiencing education with 3D Virtual
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Worlds. In Proceedings ofIADIS International Conference
Personality and performance at the beginning of the on Cognition and Exploratory Learning in Digital Age
new millennium. What do we know and where do we go (CELDA 2008) (pp. 285-300). Freiburg, Germany: IADIS.
next? International Journal of Selection and Assessment,
Bergeron, D. M., Block, C. J., & Echtenkamp, B. A.
9(1&2), 9–30. doi:10.1111/1468-2389.00160
(2006). Disabling the able: Stereotype threat and women’s
Barši, J. (1996). The meanings of kindergarten spaces. In work performance. Human Performance, 19(2), 133–158.
Kindergartens for present time (pp. 191-200). Ljubljana: doi:10.1207/s15327043hup1902_3
Center for Cultural Researches at Educational Institute
Bergson, H. (1911). Matter and Memory. London: Swan
and Association for Cultural Researches.
Sonnenschein. Retrieved from https://archive.org/details/
Bašić, S. (2000). Concept of hidden curriculum. Napredak, matterandmemory00berguoft
141(2), 170–180.
Berlin, I., Singleton, E. G., Pedarriosse, A.-M., Lancre-
Bassett, J. F., & Dabbs, J. M. (2005). A portable version of non, S., Rames, A., Aubin, H.-J., & Niaura, R. (2003).
the go/no-go association task (GNAT). Behavior Research The Modified Reasons for Smoking Scale: Factorial
Methods, 37(3), 506–512. doi:10.3758/BF03192721 structure, gender effects and relationship with nicotine
PMID:16405147 dependence and smoking cessation in French smokers.
Addiction (Abingdon, England), 98(11), 1575–1583.
doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00523.x PMID:14616184

310
Compilation of References

Bermeitinger, C. (submitted). Moving single dots as primes Bermeitinger, C., Kuhlmann, M., & Wentura, D. (2012).
for static arrow targets: Negative effects at very long SOAs. Reading a standing wave: Figure-ground-alternation
masking of primes in evaluative priming. Conscious-
Bermeitinger, C., Frings, C., & Wentura, D. (submitted).
ness and Cognition, 21(3), 1109–1121. doi:10.1016/j.
Which cognitive mechanisms cause evaluative priming?
concog.2012.03.015 PMID:22521264
New insights from event-related potentials.
Bermeitinger, C., Machmer, A. M., Schramm, J., Mertens,
Bermeitinger, C., Koch, F., & Wilborn, D. L. (2011). Em-
D., Wilborn, D. L., & Bonin, L. et  al. (2013). Facial
bodied Emotions – Verkörperte Gefühle: Emotionen als
feedback of surprise in implicit sequence learning.
Sprache des Körpers? L.O.G.O.S. INTERDISZIPLINÄR,
International Journal of Psychology & Psychological
19, 244-258.
Therapy, 13, 145–162.
Bermeitinger, C. (2009). Facts and artifacts about tu-
Bermeitinger, C., & Unger, B. (2013). Subliminal persua-
reens and artichokes: Natural and artifactual categories
sion: Psychological and juristic history and current state
investigated with semantic priming. Göttingen: Cuvillier.
of affairs. In D. Halkias (Ed.), Psychology and the search
Bermeitinger, C. (2013). Response priming with apparent for certainty in everyday life (pp. 149–162). Athens,
motion primes. Psychological Research, 77(4), 371–387. Greece: Atiner.
doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0436-x PMID:22526718
Bermeitinger, C., & Unger, B. (2014). Influences on the
Bermeitinger, C., Feldkötter, A. L., Hildebrand, J., marking of examinations. Psychology (Savannah, Ga.),
Schmieder, L., & Sellner, L. (2012). Strawberry arouses 5, 91–98.
forever? Effects of pre-information and real consump-
Bermeitinger, C., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2008).
tion of a common aphrodisiac on arousal ratings. In M.
Nature and facts about natural and artifactual catego-
Mokryš & A. Lieskovský (Eds.), Proceedings in advanced
ries: Sex differences in the semantic priming paradigm.
research in scientific areas (pp. 751–754). Žilina, Slovak
Brain and Language, 106(2), 153–163. doi:10.1016/j.
Republic: EDIS.
bandl.2008.03.003 PMID:18442848
Bermeitinger, C., Frings, C., & Wentura, D. (2008). Re-
Bermeitinger, C., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2011). How
versing the N400: Event-related potentials of a negative
to switch on and switch off semantic priming effects for
semantic priming effect. Neuroreport, 19(15), 1479–1482.
natural and artifactual categories: Activation processes
doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e32830f4b0b PMID:18797301
in category memory depend on focusing specific feature
Bermeitinger, C., Goelz, R., Johr, N., Neumann, M., Ecker, dimensions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(3), 579–
U. K. H., & Doerr, R. (2009). The hidden persuaders break 585. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0067-z PMID:21380588
into the tired brain. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
Bermeitinger, C., Wentura, D., Koppermann, C., Hauser,
chology, 45(2), 320–326. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.10.001
M., Grass, B., & Frings, C. (2012). The direction of masked
Bermeitinger, C., & Kappes, C. (2013). Threat priming: auditory category priming correlates with participants’
Age, focus, and timing. In K. A. Moore, K. Kaniasty, P. prime discrimination ability. Advances in Cognitive
Buchwald, & A. Sesé (Eds.), Stress and Anxiety: Applica- Psychology, 8(3), 210–217. doi:10.5709/acp-0116-y
tion to health and well-being, work stressors, and anxiety PMID:22956986
assessment (pp. 219–226). Berlin, Germany: Logos.
Berner, M. P., & Maier, M. A. (2004). The direction
Bermeitinger, C., & Kiefer, M. (2012). Embodied con- of affective priming as a function of trait anxiety when
cepts. In S. C. Koch, T. Fuchs, M. Summa, & C. Müller naming target words with regular and irregular pro-
(Eds.), Body memory, metaphor and movement (pp. nunciation. Experimental Psychology, 51(3), 180–190.
121–140). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi:10.1075/ doi:10.1027/1618-3169.51.3.180 PMID:15267126
aicr.84.11ber

311
Compilation of References

Berns, A., Gonzalez-Pablo, A., & Camacho, D. (2013). Bloom, B. S. (Ed.). (1956). Taxonomy of educational
Game-like language in 3-D virtual environments. Com- objectives: The classification of educational goals: Hand-
puters & Education, 60(1), 210–220. doi:10.1016/j. book I, cognitive domain. New York: Longmans, Green.
compedu.2012.07.001
Bloom, B. S., Englhart, M. D., Furst, E. J., Hill, W. H.,
Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1987). Priming of & Krathwohl, D. R. (Eds.). (1956). Taxonomy of educa-
chords: Spreading activation or overlapping frequency tional objectives, the classification of educational goals,
spectra? Perception & Psychophysics, 41(6), 519–524. Handbook I: cognitive domain. New York: Longmans.
doi:10.3758/BF03210486 PMID:3615148
Bloom, B. S., & Krathwohl, D. R. (1956). Taxonomy of
Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2009). The un- Educational Objectives: The Classification of Educa-
conscious eye opener: Pupil dilation reveals strategic tional Goals, by a committee of college and university
recruitment of mental resources upon subliminal reward examiners: Handbook I: Cognitive Domain. New York:
cues. Psychological Science, 20, 1313–1315. doi:10.1111/ Longman, Green.
j.1467-9280.2009.02443.x PMID:19788532
Bodenhausen, G. V., & Todd, A. R. (2010). Automatic
Bishop, G., & Fuchs, H. (1992). Research directions aspects of judgment and decision making. In B. K. Payne
in virtual environments: Report of an NSF Invitational & B. Gawronski (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social
Workshop. SIGGRAPH Computers Graphics, 26(3), Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications (pp.
153–177. doi:10.1145/142413.142416 278–294). New York: Guildford Press.

Blackledge, J. T. (2003). An introduction to relational Bohn, M., & Bermeitinger, C. (2012). Affektives Prim-
frame theory: Basics and applications. Behavior Analyst ing. In C. Bermeitinger (Ed.), Paradigmen der Kogni-
Today, 3(4), 421–433. tiven Psychologie: Affektive Reize I (pp. 13–38). Berlin:
Uni-Edition.
Blair, I. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes
and prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, Bo, J., Borza, V., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Age-related
6(3), 242–261. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0603_8 declines in visuospatial working memory correlate with
deficits in explicit motor sequence learning. Journal
Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2010). The influence of
of Neurophysiology, 102(5), 2744–2754. doi:10.1152/
affect on higher level cognition: A review of research
jn.00393.2009 PMID:19726728
on interpretation, judgment, decision making and
reasoning. Cognition and Emotion, 24(4), 561–595. Bo, J., Jennett, S., & Seidler, R. D. (2011). Working mem-
doi:10.1080/02699930903132496 ory capacity correlates with implicit serial reaction time
task performance. Experimental Brain Research, 214(1),
Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Gonzales, P. M., & Christie, C.
73–81. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2807-8 PMID:21809082
(2006). Decoding the implicit association test: Implica-
tions for criterion prediction. Journal of Experimental Bo, J., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Visuospatial working
Social Psychology, 42(2), 192–212. doi:10.1016/j. memory capacity predicts the organization of acquired
jesp.2005.07.003 explicit motor sequences. Journal of Neurophysiol-
ogy, 101(6), 3116–3125. doi:10.1152/jn.00006.2009
Bley-Vroman, R. (1988). The fundamental character
PMID:19357338
of foreign language learning. In W. Rutherford & M.
Sharwood Smith (Eds.), Grammar and second language Bongers, K. C. A., Dijksterhuis, A., & Spears, R. (2009).
teaching: A book of reading (pp. 19–30). Rowley, MA: Self-esteem regulation after success and failure to attain
Newbury House. unconsciously activated goals. Journal of Experimen-
tal Social Psychology, 45(3), 468–477. doi:10.1016/j.
Bloom, B. S. (1984). Taxomony of educational objectives.
jesp.2008.12.007
Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

312
Compilation of References

Bongers, K. C., Dijksterhuis, A., & Spears, R. (2010). On Bratanić, M. (1991): Micropedagogy. Zagreb: Školska
the role of consciousness in goal pursuit. Social Cogni- Knjiga.
tion, 28(2), 262–272. doi:10.1521/soco.2010.28.2.262
Bratko, D., Ljubin, T., & Matijević, M. (2000). Pupils
Bosak, J., & Sczesny, S. (2008). Am I the right candi- evaluation of curriculum “Active/effective school”. Na-
date? Self-ascribed fit of women and men to a leadership predak, 141(2), 156–169.
position. Sex Roles, 58(9-10), 682–688. doi:10.1007/
Bredekamp, S., & Rosegrant, T. (1992). Reaching Po-
s11199-007-9380-4
tentials: Transforming Early Childhood Curriculum and
Bosson, J. K., Swann Jr, W. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Assessment (pp. 1–2). New York: National Association
Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self- esteem: The for the Education.
blind men and the elephant revisited? Journal of Person-
Bregar Golobič, K. (2012). What school do we want?
ality and Social Psychology, 79, 631-643.
Space as an element (of hidden curriculum). Sodobna
Bosson, J. K., Haymovitz, E. L., & Pinel, E. C. (2004). pedagogika, 1, 52-72.
When saying and doing diverge: The effects of stereotype
Brennan, E. M., & Brennan, J. S. (1981). Accent scaling
threat on self-reported versus non-verbal anxiety. Journal
and language attitudes: Reactions to Mexican American
of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(2), 247–255.
English speech. Language and Speech, 24(3), 207–221.
doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00099-4
Bresnahan, M. I., & Kim, M. S. (1993). The impact of
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter,
positive and negative messages on change in attitude to-
C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and
ward international teaching assistants. Folia Linguistica,
cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.
27(3-4), 347–363. doi:10.1515/flin.1993.27.3-4.347
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624 PMID:11488380
Bresnahan, M. J., Ohashi, R., Nebashi, R., Liu, W. Y., &
Bowers, J. S., & Marsolek, C. J. (Eds.). (2003). Rethinking
Shearman, S. M. (2002). Attitudinal and affective response
implicit memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
toward accented English. Language & Communication,
Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G., & Saklofske, H. (Eds.). 22(2), 171–185. doi:10.1016/S0271-5309(01)00025-8
(2008). Handbook of Personality Theory and Testing.
Brezinka, W. (1994). Pedagogy and educational reality.
London: Sage.
Theleme: Časopis za Istraživanje Edukacije i Kulture,
Bradac, J. J., Cargile, A. C., & Hallett, J. S. (2001). Lan- 40(1/4), 49-68.
guage attitudes: Retrospect, conspect and prospect. In W.
Bricken, W. (1990, May). Learning in Virtual Reality.
P. Robinson & H. Giles (Eds.), The New Handbook of
Retrieved 3 October 2012 from http://www.wbricken.
Language and Social Psychology (pp. 137–155). Chich-
com/pdfs/03words/03education/02vr-education/01learn-
ester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
in-VR.pdf
Brandtstädter, J. (2002). Searching for paths to success-
Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & McCaslin, M. J. (2009). Chang-
ful development and aging: Integrating developmental
ing attitudes on implicit versus explicit measures: What
and action-theoretical perspectives. In L. Pulkkinnen &
is the difference. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol
A. Caspi (Eds.), Paths to successful development (pp.
(Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures
380–408). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
(pp. 285–326). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
doi:doi:10.1017/CBO9780511489761.016 doi:10.1017/
CBO9780511489761.016 Britzman, D. (2003). Practice makes practice: A critical
study of learning to teach. Albany, NY: State University
Brandtstädter, J. (2009). Goal pursuit and goal adjust-
of New York Press.
ment: Self-regulation and intentional self-development in
changing developmental contexts. Advances in Life Course Brkić, I., & Rijavec, M. (2011). Origin of stress, coping
Research, 14(1-2), 52–62. doi:10.1016/j.alcr.2009.03.002 with the stress and life satisfaction of teachers. Napredak,
152(2), 211–225.

313
Compilation of References

Brooks, P. J., & Kempe, V. (2012). Individual differ- Buhrmester, M. D., Blanton, H., & Swann, W. B. Jr.
ences in adult foreign language learning: The mediating (2011). Implicit self-esteem: Nature, measurement, and
effect of metalinguistic awareness. Memory & Cogni- a new way forward. Journal of Personality and Social
tion, 41(2), 281–296. doi:10.3758/s13421-012-0262-9 Psychology, 100(2), 365–385. doi:10.1037/a0021341
PMID:23055121 PMID:21038971

Broverman, I. K., Vogel, S. R., Broverman, D. M., Clark- Bull, G., & Kajder, S. (2004). Tapped In. Learning and
son, F. E., & Rosenkrantz, P. S. (1972). Sex-role stereo- Leading with Technology, 31(5), 34–37.
types: A current reappraisal. The Journal of Social Issues,
Burdea, G., & Coiffet, P. (2003). Virtual Reality Technol-
28(2), 59–78. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1972.tb00018.x
ogy. Wiley Publishing.
Brown, B. (2010, December). The power of vulnerability.
Burke, D. M., & Yee, P. L. (1984). Semantic priming during
TEDxHouston. TED Conferences, LCC. Retrieved from
sentence processing by young and older adults. Develop-
http://www.ted.com/talks/brene_brown_on_vulnerability.
mental Psychology, 20(5), 903–910. doi:10.1037/0012-
html
1649.20.5.903
Brown, B. (2011, January). Brene Brown: The Power of
Burnette, J. L., Pollack, J. M., & Hoyt, C. L. (2010).
Vulnerability. Retrieved from: http://www.youtube.com/
Individual differences in implicit theories of leadership
watch?v=iCvmsMzlF7o
ability and self‐efficacy: Predicting responses to stereotype
Brown, G. (2001). What does learning all includes. In threat. The Journal of Leadership Studies, 3(4), 46–56.
Successful learning and teaching – Psychological ap- doi:10.1002/jls.20138
proaches (pp. 17-37). Zagreb: Educa.
Burt, C. (1948). Factor analysis and canonical correla-
Brown, J. D. (2012). Understanding the Better Than tions. British Journal of Psychology. Statistical Section,
Average Effect Motives (Still) Matter. Personal- 1, 95–106.
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(2), 209–219.
Butler, S. (1872). Erewhon. London: Penguin Books.
doi:10.1177/0146167211432763 PMID:22205623
Retrieved from http://www.joh.cam.ac.uk/erewhon-1872
Bruckman, A. S. (1997). MOOSE Crossing: Construction,
Butler, S. (1880). Unconscious Memory. London: Da-
community, and learning in a networked virtual world for
vid Bogue. Retrieved from http://www.joh.cam.ac.uk/
kids. (Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation). Massachusetts.
unconscious-memory-1880
Bruner, J. (2000). The culture of education. Zagreb: Educa.
Butler, S. (1903). The Ways of all Flesh. London: Grant
Brunot, S., & Sanitioso, R. B. (2004). Motivational Richards. Retrieved from http://www.joh.cam.ac.uk/
influence on the quality of memories: Recall of general way-all-flesh-1903
autobiographical memories related to desired attributes.
Butler, L. T., & Berry, D. C. (2001). Implicit memory:
European Journal of Social Psychology, 34(5), 627–635.
Intention and awareness revisited. Trends in Cogni-
doi:10.1002/ejsp.220
tive Sciences, 5(5), 192–197. doi:10.1016/S1364-
Buckner, R. L., Andrews‐Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. 6613(00)01636-3 PMID:11323263
(2008). The brain’s default network. Annals of the New
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Sheridan, J. F., & Mc-
York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 1–38. doi:10.1196/
Clintock, M. K. (2000). Multilevel integrative analyses
annals.1440.011 PMID:18400922
of human behavior: Social neuroscience and the comple-
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994). Explor- menting nature of social and biological approaches. Psy-
ing the” planning fallacy”: Why people underestimate chological Bulletin, 126(6), 829–843. doi:10.1037/0033-
their task completion times. Journal of Personality and 2909.126.6.829 PMID:11107878
Social Psychology, 67(3), 366–381. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.67.3.366

314
Compilation of References

Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Rosabianca, A., & Kiesner, J. Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. (1959). Modern Language
(2005). Why do women underperform under stereotype Aptitude Test Form A. New York: The Psychological
threat? Evidence for the role of negative thinking. Psy- Cooperation.
chological Science, 16(7), 572–578. doi:10.1111/j.0956-
Carr, T. H., & Dagenbach, D. (1990). Semantic priming
7976.2005.01577.x PMID:16008792
and repetition priming from masked words: Evidence
Campbell, J. I., & Reynvoet, B. (2009). Context-de- for a center-surround attentional mechanism in percep-
pendent semantic priming in number naming. Journal tual recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(2), 341–350.
Cognition, 35(6), 1552–1556. doi:10.1037/a0017012 doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.2.341 PMID:2137871
PMID:19857024
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A relational
Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat frame training intervention to raise Intelligence Quotients:
magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic inte- A pilot study. The Psychological Record.
gration. Review of General Psychology, 3(1), 23–43.
Cencič, M. (2012). Dimensions of school space. Didaktika
doi:10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23
Slovenica, 27(3-4), 117–150.
Cargile, A. C. (1997). Attitudes toward Chinese-accented
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2012). Mor-
speech: An investigation in two contexts. Journal of
bidity and mortality weekly report. Weekly, 61. Retrieved
Language and Social Psychology, 16(4), 434–444.
from http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/pdf/wk/mm6144.pdf
doi:10.1177/0261927X970164004
Chan, E., Ybarra, O., & Schwarz, N. (2006). Revers-
Cargile, A. C., & Giles, H. (1997). Understanding language
ing the affective congruency effect: The role of target
attitudes: Exploring listener affect and identity. Language
word frequency of occurrence. Journal of Experimen-
& Communication, 17(3), 195–217. doi:10.1016/S0271-
tal Social Psychology, 42(3), 365–372. doi:10.1016/j.
5309(97)00016-5
jesp.2005.04.008
Cargile, A. C., & Giles, H. (1998). Language attitudes
Chartrand, T. L., & Jefferis, V. E. (2004). Priming. In M.
toward varieties of English: An American-Japanese con-
Lewis-Beck, A. Bryman, & T. Futing Liao (Eds.), The
text. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 26(3),
Sage encyclopedia of social science research methods
338–356. doi:10.1080/00909889809365511
(pp. 854–855). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Cargile, A. C., Giles, H., Ryan, E. B., & Bradac, J. J. (1994). doi:10.4135/9781412950589.n747
Language attitudes as a social process: A conceptual model
Chassin, L., Presson, C. C., Sherman, S. J., Seo, D.-C., &
and new directions. Language & Communication, 14(3),
Macy, J. T. (2010). Implicit and explicit attitudes predict
211–236. doi:10.1016/0271-5309(94)90001-9
smoking cessation: Moderating effects of experienced
Carpenter, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2003). failure to control smoking and plans to quit. Psychology
Equivalence-equivalence as a model of analogy: Further of Addictive Behaviors, 24(4), 670–679. doi:10.1037/
analyses. The Psychological Record, 53, 349–371. a0021722 PMID:21198227

Carpenter, S. M., Peters, E., Vastfjall, D., & Isen, A. M. Chen, W. (2005). Supporting teachers’ intervention in col-
(2013). Positive feelings facilitate working memory and laborative knowledge building. Retrieved 6 October 2012
complex decision making among older adults. Cognition from http://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/docs/00/19/01/63/
and Emotion, 27(1), 184–192. doi:10.1080/02699931.20 PDF/Chen_2004.pdf
12.698251 PMID:22764739
Chen, S., Lee-Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). Re-
Carre, C. (2001). What schould be learned in school? lationship orientation as a moderator of the effects of
In Successful learning and teaching – Psychological ap- social power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
proaches (pp. 37-57). Zagreb: Educa. ogy, 80(2), 173–187. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.173
PMID:11220439

315
Compilation of References

Chi, M. T. H., Slotta, J. D., & de Leeuw, N. (1994). From Cleeremans, A., & Jimenez, L. (2002). Implicit learn-
things to processes: A theory of conceptual change for ing and consciousness: A granted dynamic perspective.
learning science concepts. Learning and Instruction, 4(1), In Implicit learning and consciousness: An empirical,
27–43. doi:10.1016/0959-4752(94)90017-5 philosophical and computational consensus in the making
(pp. 1-36). Taylor and Francis.
Christie, M. A., & Darlymple-Alford, J. C. (2004). A new
rat model of the human serial reaction time task: Con- Clore, G. L., & Colcombe, S. (2003). The parallel worlds
trasting effects of caudate and hippocampal lesions. The of affective concepts and feelings. In J. Musch & K. C.
Journal of Neuroscience, 24(5), 1034–1039. doi:10.1523/ Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective
JNEUROSCI.3340-03.2004 PMID:14762121 processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 335–369). Mah-
wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Christie, M. A., & Hersch, S. M. (2004). Demonstration
of nondeclarative sequence learning in mice: Develop- Cochran-Smith, M. (2005). The new teacher edu-
ment of an animal analog of the human reaction time cation: For better or for worse? 2005 presidential
task. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), address. Educational Researcher, 34(7), 3–17.
11(6), 720–723. doi:10.1101/lm.85404 PMID:15537741 doi:10.3102/0013189X034007003

Chua, A. (2002). The influence of social interaction on Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (1996). A Guide
knowledge creation. Journal of Intellectual Capital, 3(4), to Teaching Practice. London: Routledge.
375–392. doi:10.1108/14691930210448297
Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning
Ciaccio, J. (2004). Totally Positive Teaching: A five – and retention of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia: Dis-
Stage Approach to Energizing Students and Teachers. sociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science,
Alexandria: Association for Supervision ans Curriculum 210(4466), 207–210. doi:10.1126/science.7414331
Development. PMID:7414331

Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thorne, A., Walker, M. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activa-
R., Freeman, S., & Sloan, L. R. (1976). Basking in tion theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review,
reflected glory: Three (football) field studies. Journal 82(6), 407–428. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407
of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(3), 366–375.
Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Newman, S. E. (1989).
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.3.366
Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the crafts of reading,
Cindrić, M., Miljković, D., & Strugar, V. (2010). Didactic writing, and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Know-
and curriculum. Zagreb: IEP-D2. ing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert
Glaser (pp. 453–494). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Clarke, J., Dede, C., Ketelhut, D. J., & Nelson, B. (2006).
A design-based research strategy to promote scalability Collins, A., Brown, S., & Newman, E. (1987). Cognitive
for educational innovations. Educational Technology, apprenticeship: Teaching the craft of reading, writing and
46(3), 27–36. mathematics. Cambridge, MA: Centre for the Study of
Reading: University of Illinois.
Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Williams, D.
R. (1999). Racism as a stressor for African Americans: Conole, G., & Fill, K. (2005). A learning design toolkit to
A biopsychosocial model. The American Psychologist, create pedagogically effective learning activities. Journal
54(10), 805–816. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.10.805 of Interactive Media in Education. Retrieved 3 October
PMID:10540593 2012 from http://www-jime.open.ac.uk/2005/08/conole-
2005-08-paper.html
Cleeremans, A. (2002). Models of Implicit Learning. In
Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Macmillan Publishers.

316
Compilation of References

Conway, A. R. A., Getz, S. J., Macnamara, B., & Engel Cram, A., Hedberg, J., Gosper, M., & Dick, G. (2011).
de Abreu, P. M. J. (2011). Working memory and intel- Situated, embodied and social problem solving in virtual
ligence. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The worlds. Research in Learning Technology, 19(3), 259–271.
Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 394–418). Cam- doi:10.1080/21567069.2011.624172
bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
Croizet, J., Després, G., Gauzins, M., Huguet, P., Leyens,
CBO9780511977244.021
J., & Méot, A. (2004). Stereotype threat undermines
Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, intellectual performance by triggering a disruptive
D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working mental load. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s letin, 30(6), 721–731. doi:10.1177/0146167204263961
guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 769–786. PMID:15155036
doi:10.3758/BF03196772 PMID:16523997
Cross, K. P. (1977). Not can, but will college teaching be
Conway, A. R. A., Macnamara, B., & Engel de Abreu, improved? New Directions for Higher Education, 17(17),
P. M. J. (2013). Working memory and intelligence. An 1–15. doi:10.1002/he.36919771703
overview. In T. P. Alloway & R. G. Alloway (Eds.), Work-
Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes
ing memory: The connected intelligence (pp. 13–35).
and evaluations: A social cognitive neuroscience per-
London: Psychology Press.
spective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 97–104.
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.005 PMID:17276131
construction of autobiographical memories in the self-
Curtis, D., & Lawson, L. (2001). Exploring collabora-
memory system. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261–288.
tive online learning. Journal of Asynchronous Learning
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.261 PMID:10789197
Networks, 5(1), 21–34.
Cook, S. D., & Brown, J. S. (1999). Bridging episte-
Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005). Positive affect as implicit
mologies: The generative dance between organizational
motivator: On the nonconscious operation of behavioral
knowledge and organizational knowing. Organization
goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Science, 10(4), 381–400. doi:10.1287/orsc.10.4.381
ogy, 89(2), 129–142. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.2.129
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality in adult- PMID:16162049
hood: A six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and
Cvencek, D., Greenwald, A. G., Brown, A. S., Gray, N. S.,
spouse ratings on the NEO Personality Inventory. Journal
& Snowden, R. J. (2010). Faking of the Implicit Associa-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 853–863.
tion Test is statistically detectable and partly correctable.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.853 PMID:3379583
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 32(4), 302–314.
Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-process model of doi:10.1080/01973533.2010.519236
working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.),
Czyzewska, M., & Ginsburg, H. J. (2007). Explicit and
Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active
implicit effects of anti-marijuana and anti-tobacco TV
maintenance and executive control (pp. 62–101). Cam-
advertisements. Addictive Behaviors, 32(1), 114–127.
bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.03.025 PMID:16675148
CBO9781139174909.006
Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. H., & Barnhardt, T. M. (1990).
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term
Inhibitory semantic priming of lexical decisions due to
memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Be-
failure to retrieve weakly activated codes. Journal of
havioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87–185. doi:10.1017/
Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
S0140525X01003922 PMID:11515286
tion, 16(2), 328–340. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.2.328
Coyne, R. (1994). Heidegger & Virtual reality: The
implication of Heidegger’s thinking for computer repre-
sentations. Leonardo, 27(1), 65–73. doi:10.2307/1575952

317
Compilation of References

Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. H., Menzer, D., Duquette, P. J., De Houwer, J. (2003). A structural analysis of indirect
Chalk, H. M., & Rupard, M. et al. (2007). Adventures in measures of attitudes. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.),
inhibition: Plausibly, but not certifiably, inhibitory pro- The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in
cesses. In D. S. Gorfein & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Inhibi- cognition and emotion (pp. 219–244). Mahwah, NJ:
tion in cognition (pp. 45–63). Washington, DC: American Lawrence Erlbaum.
Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/11587-003
De Houwer, J. (2003). The extrinsic affective Si-
Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. H., & Wilhelmsen, A. (1989). mon task. Experimental Psychology, 50(2), 77–85.
Task-induced strategies and near-threshold priming: doi:10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.77 PMID:12693192
Conscious influences on unconscious perception.
De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and
Journal of Memory and Language, 28(4), 412–443.
why are we using them? In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy
doi:10.1016/0749-596X(89)90020-X
(Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition and addic-
Dahlberg, G., Moss, P., & Pence, A. (1999). Beyond Qual- tion (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publisher.
ity in Early Childhood Education and Care: Postmodern doi:10.4135/9781412976237.n2
Perspectives. London: Falmer Press.
De Houwer, J. (2006a). Using the Implicit Association
Daniels, H. (2001). Vygotsky and Pedagogy. Routledge Test does not rules out an impact of conscious proposi-
Falmer. tional knowledge on evaluative conditioning. Learning
and Motivation, 1–12.
Danner, D., Hagemann, D., Schankin, A., Hager, M., &
Funke, J. (2011). Beyond IQ: A latent state-trait analysis De Houwer, J. (2011). Why the Cognitive Approach in
of general intelligence, dynamic decision making, and im- Psychology Would Profit From a Functional Approach
plicit learning. Intelligence, 39(5), 323–334. doi:10.1016/j. and Vice Versa (2011). Perspectives on Psychological
intell.2011.06.004 Science, 6(2), 202–209. doi:10.1177/1745691611400238

Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). De Houwer, J., Beckers, T., & Moors, A. (2007). Novel
Clearing the air: Identity safety moderates the effects of attitudes can be faked on the Implicit Association Test.
stereotype threat on women’s leadership aspirations. Jour- Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(6),
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(2), 276–287. 972–978. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.007
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.276 PMID:15841859
De Houwer, J., Custers, R., & De Clercq, A. (2006).
Dawson, D. L., Barnes-Holmes, D., Gresswell, D. M., Do smokers have a negative implicit attitude toward
Hart, A. J., & Gore, N. J. (2009). Assessing the Implicit smoking? Cognition and Emotion, 20(8), 1274–1284.
Beliefs of Sexual Offenders Using the Implicit Relational doi:10.1080/02699930500484506
Assessment Procedure A First Study. Sexual Abuse, 21(1),
De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1998). An affective variant
57–75. PMID:19218478
of the Simon paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 12(1),
De Houwer, J. (2006b). What are implicit measures and 45–62. doi:10.1080/026999398379772
why are we using them. In The handbook of implicit
De Houwer, J., & Hermans, D. (Eds.). (2010). Cognition
cognition and addiction. Academic Press.
and emotion: Reviews of current research and theories.
De Houwer, J. (2001). A structural and process analysis New York, NY: Psychology Press.
of the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimen-
De Houwer, J., & Moors, A. (2007). How to define and
tal Social Psychology, 37(6), 443–451. doi:10.1006/
examine the implicitness of implicit measures. In B.
jesp.2000.1464
Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of
attitudes: Procedures and controversies (pp. 179–194).
New York: Guilford Press.

318
Compilation of References

De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & Dell’Acqua, R., & Grainger, J. (1999). Unconscious se-
Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analy- mantic priming from pictures. Cognition, 73(1), B1–B15.
sis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 347–368. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00049-9 PMID:10536225
doi:10.1037/a0014211 PMID:19379018
Delwiche, A. (2006). Massively Multiplayer Online
De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & Moors, Games (MMO’s) in the new media classroom. Journal
A. (2009). Theoretical claims necessitate basic research: of Educational Technology & Society, 9(3), 160–172.
Reply to Gawronski, Lebel, Peters, and Banse (2009) and
Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2010). Introduction to
Nosek and Greenwald (2009). Psychological Bulletin,
the special issue: Emotional states, attention, and work-
135(3), 377–379. doi:10.1037/a0015328 PMID:19379021
ing memory. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 189–199.
de Jong, P. J., van den Hout, M. A., Rietbroek, H., & doi:10.1080/02699930903412120
Huijding, J. (2003). Dissociations between implicit and
Desforges, C. (Ed.). (2001). Successful learning and
explicit attitudes toward phobic stimuli. Cognition and
teaching – Psychological approaches. Zagreb: Educa.
Emotion, 17(4), 521–545. doi:10.1080/02699930302305
Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). American= white?
De Jong, T. (2006). Computer simulations-technological
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(3), 447–
advantages in inquiry learning. Science, 312(5773),
466. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.447 PMID:15740439
532–533. doi:10.1126/science.1127750 PMID:16645080
DeVries, R., & Kohlberg, L. (1987). Programmes of
Deary, I. J. (2001a). Human intelligence differences: A
early education: The constructivistic view. White Plains,
recent history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(3), 127–130.
NY: Longman.
doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01621-1 PMID:11239813
DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., & Rouby, D. A. (2009).
Deary, I. J. (2001b). Human intelligence differences:
Social exclusion and early-stage interpersonal percep-
Towards a combined experimental-differential approach.
tion: Selective attention to signs of acceptance. Journal
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(4), 164–170. doi:10.1016/
of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 729–741.
S1364-6613(00)01623-5 PMID:11287270
doi:10.1037/a0014634 PMID:19309198
Dede, C. (2005). Planning for neomillenial learning styles:
DeWall, C. N., Twenge, J. M., Koole, S. L., Baumeister,
Implecations for investestments in technology and faculty.
R. F., Marquez, A., & Reid, M. W. (2011). Automatic
In Educating the Net Generation. Educause. Retrieved
emotion regulation after social exclusion: Tuning to
3 October 2012 from http://net.educause.edu/ir/library/
positivity. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 11(3), 623–636.
pdf/pub7101o.pdf
doi:10.1037/a0023534 PMID:21668113
Degner, J. (2009). On the (un)controllability of affective
Dexter, S., & Riedel, E. (2003). Why improving pre-
priming: Strategic manipulation is feasible but can possibly
service teacher educational technology preparation must
be prevented. Cognition and Emotion, 23(2), 327–354.
go beyond the college’s walls. Journal of Teacher Educa-
doi:10.1080/02699930801993924
tion, 54(4), 334–346. doi:10.1177/0022487103255319
Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic prejudice in
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005).
childhood and early adolescence. Journal of Personality
Sources of openness/intellect: Cognitive and neuropsycho-
and Social Psychology, 98(3), 356–374. doi:10.1037/
logical correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal
a0017993 PMID:20175618
of Personality, 73(4), 825–858. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
DeKeyser, R. M. (2000). The robustness of critical period 6494.2005.00330.x PMID:15958136
effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second
Language Acquisition, 22, 499–533.

319
Compilation of References

Dickey, E. (2005). Three-dimensional virtual worlds and Dillenbourg, P. (1999). Introduction: What do you mean
distance learning: Two case studies of Active World as a by “collaborative learning”? In P. Dillenbourg (Ed.),
medium of distance education. British Journal of Educa- Collaborative Learning: Cognitive and computational
tional Technology, 36(3), 439–451. doi:10.1111/j.1467- approaches (pp. 1–19). Amsterdam: Pergamon, Elsevier
8535.2005.00477.x Science.

Dickey, M. (2011). The pragmatics of virtual worlds Dixon, M. R., Dymond, S., Rehfeldt, R. A., Roche,
for K-12 educators: Investigating the affordances and B., & Zlomke, K. R. (2003). Terrorism and relational
constraints of Active Worlds and Second Life with K-12 frame theory. Behavior and Social Issues, 12(2), 129.
in-service teachers. Educational Technology Research doi:10.5210/bsi.v12i2.40
and Development, 59(1), 1–20. doi:10.1007/s11423-
Do Yeong, K. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the
010-9163-4
Implicit Association Test (IAT). Social Psychology
Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (1999). A theory of implicit Quarterly, 66(1), 83–96. doi:10.2307/3090143
and explicit knowledge. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Dolar Bahovec, E. (2012). What is good thinking space?
22(05), 735–808. doi:10.1017/S0140525X99002186
Philosopy, architecture, schooling. Sodobna Pedagogika,
PMID:11301570
1, 18-33.
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: The merits of
Dolar Bahovec, E., & Kodelja, Z. (Eds.). (1996). Kinder-
unconscious thought in preference development and deci-
gartens for present time. Ljubljana: Center for cultural
sion making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
researches at Educational institute and Association for
ogy, 87(5), 586–598. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.586
cultural researches.
PMID:15535773
Dolar Bahovec, E., & Bregar Golobič, K. (2004). School
Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and
and kindergarten trough the mirror. Manual for the kin-
(un) consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 61(1),
dergartens, schools and parents. Ljubljana: DZS.
467–490. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100445
PMID:19566422 Domović, V. (n.d.). Retrieved March 5. 2012, from
domus.srce.hr/.../5_kurikulum%20usmjeren%20na%20
Dijkstra, A., De Vries, H., & Roijackers, J. (1998).
kompetencije%20-%20osnovni%20pojmovi_20-2-09.pdf
Long-term effectiveness of computer-generated tai-
lored feedback in smoking cessation. Health Education Donovan, J. J., Dwight, S. A., & Hurtz, G. M. (2003). An
Research, 13(2), 207–214. doi:10.1093/her/13.2.207 assessment of the prevalence, severity, and verifiability
PMID:10181019 of entry-level applicant faking using the randomized re-
sponse technique. Human Performance, 16(1), 81–106.
Dijkstra, A., De Vries, H., Roijackers, J., & van Breukelen,
doi:10.1207/S15327043HUP1601_4
G. (1998). Tailored interventions to communicate stage-
matched information to smokers in different motivational Dosher, B. A., & Rosedale, G. (1989). Integrated retrieval
stages. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, cues as a mechanism for priming in retrieval from memory.
66(3), 549–557. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.66.3.549 Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 118(2),
PMID:9642894 191–211. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.118.2.191
Dillenbourg, P. (1996). Some technical implications the Dourish, P., & Bellotti, V. (1992) Awareness and Collabo-
distributed cognition approach on the design of interactive ration in Shared workspaces. In Proceedings of the ACM
learning environments. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work
in Education, 7(2), 161–180. (CSCW’92) (pp. 107-114). Toronto, Canada: ACM Press.

320
Compilation of References

Dovidio, J. F., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). New technolo- Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. H. (2007). Women and the laby-
gies for the direct and indirect assessment of attitudes. rinth of leadership. Harvard Business Review. Cambridge,
In J. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about questions: Meaning, MA: Harvard University Press.
memory, expression and social interaction in surveys (pp.
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory
204–237). New York: Sage.
of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review,
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. E., Kawakami, K., & Hodson, 109(3), 573–598. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573
G. (2002). Why can’t we just get along? Interpersonal PMID:12088246
biases and interracial distrust. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic
Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992).
Minority Psychology, 8(2), 88–102. doi:10.1037/1099-
Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis.
9809.8.2.88 PMID:11987594
Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 3–22. doi:10.1037/0033-
Doyle, D. (2008). Immersed in Learning: Developing 2909.111.1.3
and Supporting creative practice in virtual worlds. In
Eder, A. B., Leuthold, H., Rothermund, K., & Schwein-
Proceedings of Researching Learning in Virtual Envi-
berger, S. R. (2012). Automatic response activation
ronmentsInternational Conference (pp. 121-129). The
in sequential affective priming: An ERP study. Social
Open University.
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(4), 436–445.
Draine, S. C. (1998). Inquisit [Computer Software]. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr033 PMID:21642351
Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software.
Edwards, C., Gandini, L., & Forman, G. (Eds.). (1998).
Duncan, I., Miller, A., & Jiang, S. (2012). A taxonomy The Hundred Languages of Children: The Reggio Emilia
of virtual worlds usage in education. British Journal of Approach. London: Ablex Publishing Greenwich.
Educational Technology, 43(6), 949–964. doi:10.1111/
Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked
j.1467-8535.2011.01263.x
stimuli on motor activation: Behavioral and electrophysi-
Dunning, D., Heath, C., & Suls, J. M. (2004). Flawed ological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
self-assessment implications for health, education, and the Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 1737–1747.
workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.6.1737 PMID:9861720
5(3), 69–106. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2004.00018.x
Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Links between con-
Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1995). A transformation of scious awareness and response inhibition: Evidence from
self-discrimination response functions in accordance with masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3),
the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, more-than, 514–520. doi:10.3758/BF03196307 PMID:12412891
and less-than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Eitam, B., Hassin, R. R., & Schul, Y. (2008). Noncon-
Behavior, 64(2), 163–184. doi:10.1901/jeab.1995.64-163
scious goal pursuit in novel environments: The case of
PMID:16812766
implicit learning. Psychological Science, 19(3), 261–267.
Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2000). Understanding doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02078.x PMID:18315799
complex behavior: The transformation of stimulus func-
Eliason, C. F., & Jenkins, L. T. (1986). A Practical Guide
tions. The Behavior Analyst, 23(2), 239. PMID:22478349
to Early Childhood Curriculum. Columbus, OH: Merrill
Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (Eds.). (2013). Advances Publishing Company.
in relational frame theory: Research and application.
Ellis, H. C., & Hunt, R. R. (1993). Fundamentals of
Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
cognitive psychology. The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.
Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. H. (2004). Women and men as
leaders. In J. Antonakis, A. T. Cianciolo, & R. J. Sternberg
(Eds.), The nature of leadership (pp. 279-301). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

321
Compilation of References

Epstein, S. (2003). Cognitive-experiential self-theory of Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Attitudes: Founda-
personality. In Millon, T., & Lerner, M. J. (Eds), Compre- tions, functions, and consequences. In M. A. Hogg & J.
hensive Handbook of Psychology, Volume 5: Personality Cooper (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp.
and Social Psychology (pp. 159-184). Hoboken, NJ: 139–160). London: Sage.
Wiley & Sons.
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures
Eriksen, C. W. (1995). The flankers task and response in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. An-
competition: A useful tool for investigating a variety of nual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 297–327. doi:10.1146/
cognitive problems. Visual Cognition, 2(2-3), 101–118. annurev.psych.54.101601.145225 PMID:12172003
doi:10.1080/13506289508401726
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (in press). The MODE model:
European Parliament. (2013). Tobacco: Larger warnings, Attitude-behavior processes as a function of motivation
flavours banned, e-cigarettes regulated. Retrieved October and opportunity. In J. W. Sherman, B. Gawronski, & Y.
17, 2013, from http://www.europarl.europa.eu/news/en/ Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories of the social mind.
news-room/content/20131004IPR21539/html/Tobacco- New York: Guilford Press.
larger-warnings-flavours-banned-e-cigarettes-regulated
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., &
Evans, J.B., & Frankish, K. (Eds.). (2009). In two minds: Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of
Dual processes and beyond. Oxford, UK: Oxford Uni- attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
versity Press. ogy, 50(2), 229–238. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229
PMID:3701576
Falconer, L. (2012). Situated learning in virtual simu-
lations: Researching the authentic dimension in vir- Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE
tual worlds. Journal of Interactive Learning Research. model of attitude behavior processes. In S. Chaiken & Y.
Retrieved 3 October 2012 from http://eprints.uwe. Trope (Eds.), Dual Process Theories in Social Psychology
ac.uk/16628/ (pp. 97–116). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Fazio, R. H. (1990). In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Multiple pro- Fehrer, E., & Raab, D. (1962). Reaction time to stim-
cesses by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE uli masked by metacontrast. Journal of Experimental
model as an integrative framework (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109). Psychology, 63(2), 143–147. doi:10.1037/h0040795
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.New York, PMID:13892020
NY: Academic Press.
Feldman, J., Kerr, B., & Streissguth, A. P. (1995).
Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associ- Correlational analyses of procedural and declarative
ated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, learning performance. Intelligence, 20(1), 87–114.
15(2), 115–142. doi:10.1080/02699930125908 doi:10.1016/0160-2896(95)90007-1

Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object-evaluation associa- Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social compari-
tions of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25(5), 664– son processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117–140.
703. doi:10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.603 PMID:19424447 doi:10.1177/001872675400700202

Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, Fiedler, K. (2003). The hidden vicissitudes of the priming
C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an paradigm in evaluative judgment research. In J. Musch &
unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affec-
pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, tive processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 109–137).
69(6), 1013–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013 Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
PMID:8531054
Fiedler, K., & Bluemke, M. (2005). Faking the IAT: Aided
and unaided response control on the implicit associa-
tion tests. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27(4),
307–316. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2704_3

322
Compilation of References

Fiedler, K., Messner, C., & Bluemke, M. (2006). Unre- Forman, G., & Fyfe, B. (1998). Negotiated Learning
solved problems with the ‘I’, the ‘A’, and the ‘T’: A logical Through Design, Documentation and Discourse. In C.
and psychometric critique of the Implicit Association Test Edwards, L. Gandini, & G. Forman (Eds.), The Hundred
(IAT). European Review of Social Psychology, 17(1), Languages of Children: The Reggio Emilia Approach
74–147. doi:10.1080/10463280600681248 (pp. 239–260). London: Ablex Publishing Greenwich.

Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Thinking of you: Fox, E. (1995). Negative priming from ignored distrac-
Nonconscious pursuit of interpersonal goals associated tors in visual selection: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin
with relationship partners. Journal of Personality and & Review, 2(2), 145–173. doi:10.3758/BF03210958
Social Psychology, 84(1), 148–164. doi:10.1037/0022- PMID:24203653
3514.84.1.148 PMID:12518976
Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emo-
Flaganan, D. P., & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.). (2005). Con- tions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action
temporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19(3), 313–332.
issues. New York: Guilford Press. doi:10.1080/02699930441000238 PMID:21852891

Fockenberg, D. A., Koole, S. L., & Semin, G. R. (2008). Frensch, P. A. (1998). One concept, multiple meanings:
Priming in concert: Assimilation and contrast with mul- On how to define the concept of implicit learning. In M.
tiple affective and gender primes. Social Cognition, 26(6), A. Stadler & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of implicit
647–669. doi:10.1521/soco.2008.26.6.647 learning (pp. 47–104). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Pub-
lications.
Fong, C. T., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2002). Dueling expe-
riences and dual ambivalences: The emotional and Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presenta-
motivational ambivalence of women in high status tion rate and individual differences in short-term memory
positions. Motivation and Emotion, 26(1), 105–121. capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory
doi:10.1023/A:1015198209285 & Cognition, 22(1), 95–110. doi:10.3758/BF03202765
PMID:8035689
Forbes, C. E., Duran-Jordan, K., Magerman, A., & Leitner,
J. B. (in press). Unforgettable: Stereotype threatening Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). When and
contexts enhance encoding of negative feedback. why do implicit measures predict behavior? Empirical evi-
dence for the moderating role of opportunity, motivation,
Forbes, C. E., Cox, C., Schmader, T., & Ryan, L. (2012).
and process reliance. European Review of Social Psychol-
Negative stereotype activation alters interaction between
ogy, 19(1), 285–338. doi:10.1080/10463280802556958
neural correlates of arousal, inhibition and cognitive
control. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Frings, C. (2004). Inhibition von Distraktorinformation.
7(7), 771–781. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr052 PMID:21954239 Kassel: University Press.

Forbes, C. E., & Leitner, J. B. (2014). Stereotype threat Frings, C., Amendt, A., & Spence, C. (2011). When see-
engenders neural attentional bias towards negative feed- ing doesn’t matter: Assessing the after-effects of tactile
back to undermine performance. Biological Psychology. distractor processing in the blind and the sighted. Journal
of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Forbes, C. E., Leitner, J. B., Duran-Jordan, K., Mager-
Performance, 37(4), 1174–1181. doi:10.1037/a0022336
man, A., Schmader, R., & Allen, J. B. (in press). Spon-
PMID:21553991
taneous default mode network phase-locking modulates
performance perceptions under stereotype threat. Social Frings, C., Bermeitinger, C., & Gibbons, H. (2011).
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. Prime retrieval of motor responses in negative priming:
Evidence from lateralized readiness potentials. Brain Re-
Forbes, C. E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Retraining atti-
search, 1407, 69–78. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.037
tudes and stereotypes to affect motivation and cognitive
PMID:21764039
capacity under stereotype threat. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 99(5), 740–754. doi:10.1037/
a0020971 PMID:20822288

323
Compilation of References

Frings, C., Bermeitinger, C., & Wentura, D. (2008). Center- Gandini, L. (1998). Educational and Caring Spaces. In C.
surround or spreading inhibition: Which mechanism Edwards, L. Gandini, & G. Forman (Eds.), The Hundred
caused the negative effect from repeated masked semantic Languages of Children: The Reggio Emilia Approach
primes? Experimental Psychology, 55(4), 235–243. (pp. 161–178). London: Ablex Publishing Greenwich.
doi:10.1027/1618-3169.55.4.234 PMID:18683620
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working
Frings, C., Bermeitinger, C., & Wentura, D. (2011). memory and language. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Inhibition from blinked category labels: Combining the
Gavin, A., Roche, B., & Ruiz, M. (2008). A behaviorally
attentional blink and the semantic priming paradigm.
modifed and functionally transparent Implicit Association
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(4), 514–521. doi:1
Test to measure levels of culturally appropriate sexual
0.1080/20445911.2011.527328
categorization of children among a sample of normal
Frings, C., Göbel, A., Mast, F., Sutter, J., Bermeitinger, males and females. Academic Press.
C., & Wentura, D. (2011). Category priming with aliens:
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative
Analysing the influence of targets’ prototypicality on the
and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative
centre surround inhibition mechanism. Memory (Hove,
review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psycho-
England), 19(6), 585–596. doi:10.1080/09658211.2011
logical Bulletin, 132(5), 692–731. doi:10.1037/0033-
.592498 PMID:21919586
2909.132.5.692 PMID:16910748
Frings, C., & Neubauer, A. (2005). Are masked-stimuli-
Gawronski, B., LeBel, E. P., & Peters, K. R. (2007). What
discrimination-tests in masked priming studies measures
do implicit measures tell us? Scrutinizing the validity
of intelligence? An alternative task for measuring inspec-
of three common assumptions. Perspectives on Psy-
tion time. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(7),
chological Science, 2(2), 181–193. doi:10.1111/j.1745-
1181–1191. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.024
6916.2007.00036.x
Frumkin, L. (2007). Influences of accent and eth-
Gawronski, B., LeBel, E. P., Peters, K. R., & Banse, R.
nic background on perceptions of eyewitness testi-
(2009). Methodological issues in the validation of implicit
mony. Psychology, Crime & Law, 13(3), 317–331.
measures: Comment on De Houwer, Teige-Mocigemba,
doi:10.1080/10683160600822246
Spruyt, and Moors (2009). Psychological Bulletin, 135(3),
Furnham, A., & Boo, H. C. (2011). A literature review of 369–372. doi:10.1037/a0014820 PMID:19379019
the anchoring effect. Journal of Socio-Economics, 40(1),
Gawronski, B., & Payne, B. K. (2010). Handbook of
35–42. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2010.10.008
implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and ap-
Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The moti- plications. New York: Guilford Press.
vational dimensional model of affect: Implications
Gebauer, G. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric
for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive cat-
intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning.
egorization. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 322–337.
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Mem-
doi:10.1080/02699930903378305
ory, and Cognition, 33(1), 34–54. doi:10.1037/0278-
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, 7393.33.1.34 PMID:17201553
D. H. (2006). Power and perspectives not taken. Psycho-
Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., & Hesse, F. W. (1994).
logical Science, 17(12), 1068–1074. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
Experimental induction of emotional states and their
9280.2006.01824.x PMID:17201789
effectiveness: A review. British Journal of Psychology,
Gamor, K. I. (2012). Exploiting the Power of Persistence 85(1), 55–78. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02508.x
for Learning in Virtual Worlds. In B. Khan (Ed.), User
Giles, H., Hewstone, M., Ryan, E. B., & Johnson, P.
Interface Design for Virtual Environments: Challenges
(1987). Research on language attitudes. In H. U. Ammon,
and Advances (pp. 142–155). Hershey, PA: IGI Global.
N. Dittmar, K. J. Mattheier (Eds.), Sociolinguistics (2nd
ed., pp. 585-597). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

324
Compilation of References

Giles, H., Williams, A., Mackie, D. M., & Rosselli, F. Goldhaber, J., Smith, D., & Sortino, S. (1997). Observing,
(1995). Reactions to Anglo- and Hispanic-American- Recording, Understanding: The Role of Documentation
Accented speakers: Affect, identity, persuasion, and the in Early Childhood Teacher Education. In J. Hendricks
English-only controversy. Language & Communication, (Ed.), First step Toward Teaching the Reggio Way (pp.
15(2), 107–120. doi:10.1016/0271-5309(94)00019-9 198–209). Columbus, OH: Merrill/Prentice – Hall.

Gill, M. (1994). Accent and stereotypes: Their effect Gomez-Gonzalez, C. M. G., Clark, V. P., Fan, S., Luck, S.
on perceptions of teachers and lecture comprehension. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Sources of attention-sensitive
Journal of Applied Communication Research, 22(4), visual event-related potentials. Brain Topography, 7(1),
349–361. doi:10.1080/00909889409365409 41–51. doi:10.1007/BF01184836 PMID:7803199

Giroux, H., & Penna, A. (1983). Social Education in the Gonzales, P. M., Blanton, H., & Williams, K. J. (2002).
Classroom: The Dynamics of the Hidden Curriculum. The Effects of stereotype threat and double-minority
In H. Giroux & P. David (Eds.), The Hidden Curriculum status on the test performance of Latino women. Per-
and Moral Education (pp. 100–121). Berkeley, CA: Mc- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(5), 659–670.
Cutchan Publishing Corporation. doi:10.1177/0146167202288010

Glaser, J. (2003). Reverse priming: Implications for the Good, R. (2004). 3D virtual spaces for learning and col-
(un)conditionality of automatic evaluation. In J. Musch laboration. Retrieved 5 August 2012 from http://www.
& K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Af- masternewmedia.org/2004/09/27/3d_virtual_spaces_
fective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 87–108). for_learning.html
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Good, J. J., Woodzicka, J. A., & Wingfield, L. C.
Glaser, J., & Banaji, M. R. (1999). When fair is foul and foul (2010). The effects of gender stereotypic and counter-
is fair: Reverse priming in automatic evaluation. Journal stereotypic textbook images on science performance.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(4), 669–687. The Journal of Social Psychology, 150(2), 132–147.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.4.669 PMID:10531668 doi:10.1080/00224540903366552 PMID:20397590

Glock, S., & Kneer, J. (2009). Are deterrent pictures effec- Goodlad, J. (1984). A place called school: Prospects for
tive? The impact of warning labels on cognitive dissonance the future. New York: McGraw-Hill.
in smokers. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being,
Gordon, D. (1982). The concept of the hidden curriculum.
1, 356–373. doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01019.x
Journal of Philosophy of Education, 16(2), 187–198.
Glock, S., Kovacs, C., & Unz, D. (2013). Implicit attitudes doi:10.1111/j.1467-9752.1982.tb00611.x
toward smoking: How the smell of cigarettes influences
Goschke, T., & Dreisbach, G. (2008). Conflict-
college-age smokers and non-smokers. Journal of Health
triggered goal shielding: Response conflicts attenuate
Psychology. doi:10.1177/1359105313476974
background monitoring for prospective memory cues.
Glock, S., Unz, D., & Kovacs, C. (2012). Beyond fear Psychological Science, 19(1), 25–32. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
appeals: Contradicting positive smoking outcome expec- 9280.2008.02042.x PMID:18181788
tancies to influence smokers’ implicit attitudes, percep-
Gović, A. (2012). Experience of flow at work and per-
tion, and behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 37(4), 548–551.
sonality traits of kindergarten teacher. Faculty of Teacher
doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.11.032 PMID:22178602
Education, University of Rijeka.
Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-
Gramzow, R. H., Willard, G., & Mendes, W. B. (2008).
dependent memory in two natural environments: On land
Big tales and cool heads: Academic exaggeration is re-
and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66(3),
lated to cardiac vagal reactivity. Emotion (Washington,
325–331. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01468.x
D.C.), 8(1), 138–144. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.1.138
PMID:18266525

325
Compilation of References

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the
Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-
of activation and responses. Journal of Experimental concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychology. General, 4(4), 480–506. doi:10.1037/0096- 79(6), 1022–1038. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022
3445.121.4.480 PMID:1431740 PMID:11138752

Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Rosenb- Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L.
latt, A., Burling, J., & Lyon, D. et al. (1992). Why do K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit
people need self - esteem? Converging evidence that cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Per-
self-esteem serves an anxiety-buffering function. Journal sonality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 913–922. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464 PMID:9654756
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.913 PMID:1460559
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., Schwartz, J. L. K., &
Greenhalgh, C., & Benford, S. (1995b). MASSIVE: A Attitudes, M. I. (1998). Measuring individual differences
Collaborative Virtual Environment for Teleconferencing. in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal
ACM Transactions on Computer Human Interfaces, 2(3), of Personality, 74(6), 1464–1480. PMID:9654756
239–261. doi:10.1145/210079.210088
Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Attitudinal
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Klauer, dissociation: What does it mean? In R. E. Petty, R. H.
K. C. (2005). Validity of the salience asymmetry inter- Fazio, & P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new
pretation of the implicit association test: Comment on implicit measures (pp. 65–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rothermund and Wentura (2004). Academic Press.
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003).
Greenwald, A., Nosek, B., & Banaji, M. (2003). Under- Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I.
standing and using the Implicit Association Test: I: An An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and
improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022-
Social Psychology. 3514.85.2.197 PMID:12916565

Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E., &
and revision of personal history. The American Psycholo- Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Im-
gist, 35(7), 603–618. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.35.7.603 plicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive
validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit so-
97(1), 17–41. doi:10.1037/a0015575 PMID:19586237
cial cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes.
Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27. doi:10.1037/0033- Griffith, R. L., & Peterson, M. H. (2008). The failure of
295X.102.1.4 PMID:7878162 social desirability measures to capture applicant faking
behavior. Industrial and Organizational Psychology:
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham,
Perspectives on Science and Practice, 1(3), 308–311.
S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified
doi:10.1111/j.1754-9434.2008.00053.x
theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem,
and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109(1), 3–25. Grumm, M., & von Collani, G. (2007). Measuring Big-Five
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.3 PMID:11863040 personality dimensions with the implicit association test-
Implicit personality traits or self-esteem? Personality and
Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996).
Individual Differences, 43(8), 2205–2217. doi:10.1016/j.
Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic acti-
paid.2007.06.032
vation. Science, 273(5282), 1699–1702. doi:10.1126/
science.273.5282.1699 PMID:8781230

326
Compilation of References

Gudjons, H. (1994). Pedagogy, fundamental knowledges. Han, D. H., Kim, J. H., Lee, Y. S., Bae, S. J., Bae, S. J.,
Zagreb: Educa. & Kim, H. J. et al. (2006). Influence of temperament and
anxiety on athletic performance. Journal of Sports Science
Gunawardena, C. N., Hermans, M. B., Sanchez, D., Rich-
and Medicine, 5, 381–389. PMID:24353455
mond, C., Bohley, M., & Tuttle, R. (2009). A theoretical
framework for building online communities of practice Hansen, C. H., & Shantz, C. A. (1995). Emotion-
with social networking tools. Educational Media Inter- specific priming: Congruence effects on affect and
national, 46(1), 3–16. doi:10.1080/09523980802588626 recognition across negative emotions. Personal-
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(6), 548–557.
Gupta, V. K., Turban, D. B., & Bhawe, N. M. (2008).
doi:10.1177/0146167295216001
The effect of gender stereotype activation on entrepre-
neurial intentions. The Journal of Applied Psychology, Hartikainen, A. (2008). Making meanings: Pupil talk in
93(5), 1053–1061. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.5.1053 inquiry-oriented instruction. NORDINA, 4(1), 64–76.
PMID:18808225
Hay, E., Elliot, D., & Kim, B. (2002). Collaborative
Gustafsson, U., & Björklund, F. (2008). Women self- network-based virtual reality: The past, the present, and
stereotype with feminine stereotypical traits under ste- the future of the virtual solar system. In G. Stahl (Ed.),
reotype threat. Current Research in Social Psychology, Proceedings of the Computer Support Collaborative
13, 219–231. Learning: Foundations for a CSCL Community (pp. 519-
520). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hahn, A., & Renner, B. (1998). Perception of health
risks: How smoker status affects defensive opti- Hayes, E. (2006). Situated learning in virtual worlds: The
mism. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 11(2), 93–112. learning ecology of Second Life. In Proceedings of Adult
doi:10.1080/10615809808248307 Education Research Conference 2006. Retrieved 3 August
2012 from http://www.adulterc.org/Proceedings/2006/
Haines, E. L., & Kray, L. J. (2005). Self-power associa-
Proceedings/Hayes.pdf
tions: The possession of power impacts women’s self-
concepts. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(5), Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001).
643–662. doi:10.1002/ejsp.252 Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of
human language and cognition. New York: Plenum.
Hakkarainen, K., & Paavola, S. (2005). From monologi-
cal and dialogical to trialogical approaches to learning. Heagle, A. I., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2006). Teaching
Retrieved 3 October 2012 from http://escalate.org.il/ perspective-taking skills to typically developing children
construction_knowledge/papers/hakkarainen.pdf through derived relational responding. Journal of Early
and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 3, 1–34.
Hakkarainen, K. (2003). Progressive inquiry in a Comput-
er-supported Biology class. Journal of Research in Science Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C.,
Teaching, 40(10), 1072–1088. doi:10.1002/tea.10121 & Fagerström, K. O. (1991). The Fagerström Test for
Nicotine Dependence: A revision of the Fagerström Toler-
Hakkarainen, K., Palonen, T., Paavola, S., & Lehtinen,
ance Questionnaire. British Journal of Addiction, 86(9),
E. (2004). Communities of networked expertise: Profes-
1119–1127. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x
sional and educational perspectives. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
PMID:1932883
Hamachek, D. (1999). Effective Teachers: What they do,
Heikkila, S. (2004). Process inquiry challenges product
how they do it, and the importance of self-knowledge. In
challenges. Retrieved from http://www.elearningeuropa.
R. P. Lipka & T. M. Brinthaupt (Eds.), The role of Self in
info/files/media/media13769.pdf
Teacher Development (pp. 189–224). Albany, NY: State
University of New York Press.

327
Compilation of References

Hendrick, J. (1986). Total Learning, Curriculum for Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H.,
the Young Child. Columbus, OH: Merrill Publishing & Schmitt, M. (2004). A meta-analysis on the correlation
Company. between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-
report measures. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Hendrick, J. (1997). First step Toward Teaching the
Trier, Germany.
Reggio Way (pp. 198–209). Columbus, OH: Merrill/
Prentice – Hall. Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Nosek, B. A., & Schmitt,
M. (2005). What moderates implicit—explicit consis-
Hendricks, P. S., & Brandon, T. H. (2005). Smoking expec-
tency? European Review of Social Psychology, 16(1),
tancy associates among college smokers. Addictive Behav-
335–390. doi:10.1080/10463280500443228
iors, 30(2), 235–245. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.05.006
PMID:15621395 Holcomb, P. J., & Anderson, J. E. (1993). Cross-modal
semantic priming: A time-course analysis using event-
Hering, E. (1870/1880). On memory. In S. Butler (Ed.),
related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Pro-
Unconscious memory (New Ed., pp. 63-86). London:
cesses, 8(4), 379–411. doi:10.1080/01690969308407583
Fifield.
Holland, A. C., Tamir, M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2010).
Hermans, D., Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P.
The effect of regulation goals on emotional event- specific
(2003). Affective priming with subliminally presented
knowledge. Memory (Hove, England), 18(5), 504–521.
pictures. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,
doi:10.1080/09658211.2010.481628 PMID:20503130
57(2), 97–114. doi:10.1037/h0087416 PMID:12822839
Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., & Aarts, H. (2005). Smells
Hernández-Leo, D., Jorrín-Abellán, I., Villasclaras-
like clean spirit. Psychological Science, 16(9), 689–693.
Fernández, E., Asensio-Pérez, J., & Dimitriadis, Y. (2010).
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01597.x PMID:16137254
A multicase study for the evaluation of a pattern-based
visual design process for collaborative learning. Journal Hollan, J., Hutchins, E., & Kirsh, D. (2000). Distrib-
of Visual Languages and Computing, 21(3), 313–331. uted Cognition: Toward a New Foundation for Hu-
doi:10.1016/j.jvlc.2010.08.006 man- Computer Interaction Research. ACM Transac-
tions on Human-Computer Interaction, 7(2), 174–196.
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regu-
doi:10.1145/353485.353487
latory focus as a motivational principle. Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 1–46. doi:10.1016/ Hollins, P., & Robbins, S. (2008). The educational af-
S0065-2601(08)60381-0 fordances of Multi User Virtual environments (MUVE).
In Proceedings of Researching Learning in Virtual En-
Hillyard, S. A., Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. (1998). Sensory
vironmentsInternational Conference (pp. 172-180). The
gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective
Open University.
attention: Electrophysiological and neuroimaging evi-
dence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory
of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 353(1373), mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T.
1257–1270. doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0281 PMID:9770220 Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory,
and language (pp. 53–112). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Hodkinson, P. (2005). Learning as cultural and re-
lational: Moving past some troubling dualisms. Houwer, J. D. (2002). The Implicit Association Test as a
Cambridge Journal of Education, 35(1), 107–119. tool for studying dysfunctional associations in psychopa-
doi:10.1080/0305764042000332524 thology: Strengths and limitations. Journal of Behavior
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33(2), 115–133.
Hoffman, M. L. (1987). The contribution of empathy to
doi:10.1016/S0005-7916(02)00024-1 PMID:12472175
justice and moral judgement. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer
(Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 47–80). New
York: Cambridge University Press.

328
Compilation of References

Howe, M. J. A. (1996). Concepts of ability. In I. Dennis Huijding, J., & de Jong, P. (2006). Automatic associations
& P. Tapsfield (Eds.), Human abilities: Their nature with the sensory aspects of smoking: Positive in habitual
and measurement (pp. 39–48). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence smokers but negative in non-smokers. Addictive Behav-
Erlbaum. iors, 31(1), 182–186. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.04.014
PMID:15919160
Hoyt, C. L. (2005). The role of leadership efficacy and
stereotype activation in women’s identification with lead- Huijding, J., de Jong, P. J., Wiers, R. W., & Verkooijen,
ership. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, K. (2005). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward smoking
11(4), 2–14. doi:10.1177/107179190501100401 in a smoking and a nonsmoking setting. Addictive Behav-
iors, 30(5), 949–961. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.09.014
Hoyt, C. L., & Blascovich, J. (2007). Leadership efficacy
PMID:15893091
and women leaders’ responses to stereotype activation.
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10(4), 595–616. Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming due to as-
doi:10.1177/1368430207084718 sociation strength or featural overlap? A micro-analytic
review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 785–813.
Hoyt, C. L., & Blascovich, J. (2010). The role of leadership
doi:10.3758/BF03196544 PMID:15000531
self-efficacy and stereotype activation on cardiovascular,
behavioral and self-report responses in the leadership Ichikawa, J., & Steup, M. (2013). The Analysis of Knowl-
domain. The Leadership Quarterly, 21(1), 89–103. edge. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall
doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.10.007 2013 Edition). Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/
archives/fall2013/entries/knowledge-analysis/
Hoyt, C. L., Johnson, S. K., Murphy, S., & Skinnell,
K. (2010). The impact of blatant stereotype activation Inzlicht, M., Aronson, J., Good, C., & McKay, L. (2006).
and group sex-composition on female leaders. The A particular resiliency to threatening environments. Jour-
Leadership Quarterly, 21(5), 716–732. doi:10.1016/j. nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(3), 323–336.
leaqua.2010.07.003 doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.05.005

Hoyt, C. L., & Simon, S. (2011). Female lead- Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2000). A threatening
ers: Injurious or inspiring role models for women? intellectual environment: Why females are susceptible
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 35(1), 143–157. to experiencing problem-solving deficits in the pres-
doi:10.1177/0361684310385216 ence of males. Psychological Science, 11(5), 365–371.
doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00272 PMID:11228906
Hsu, N., & Schütt, Z. (Eds.). (2012). Psychology of prim-
ing. New York: Nova. Ivon, H., & Sindik, J. (2008). Connection between pre-
school teachers’ empathy and imagination and certain
Huber, D. E. (2008). Immediate priming and cognitive
characteristics of preschool child’s behavior and play.
aftereffects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Gen-
Magistra Iadertina, 3(3), 21–38.
eral, 137(2), 324–347. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.324
PMID:18473662 Jackson, D. O. (2013). Implicit second language learning
and individual differences. In J. M. Bergsleithner, S. N.
Hughes, B. L., & Beer, J. S. (2012). Orbitofrontal cortex
Forota, & J. K. Yoshioka (Eds.), Noticing and second
and anterior cingulate cortex are modulated by motivated
language acquisition: Studies in honor of Richard Schmidt
social cognition. Cerebral Cortex, 22(6), 1372–1381.
(pp. 271–287). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii, Na-
doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr213 PMID:21862446
tional Foreign Language Resource Center.
Hughes, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2011). The Dominance
Jackson, P. W. (1990). Life in clasrooms. New York:
of Associative Theorizing in Implicit Attitude Research:
Teachers College Press.
Propositional and Behavioral Alternatives. The Psycho-
logical Record, 61, 465–496.

329
Compilation of References

Jackson, R. C., & Farrow, D. (2005). Implicit train- Jaśkowski, P., & Verleger, R. (2007). What determines the
ing: How, when and why? Human Movement Science, direction of subliminal priming. Advances in Cognitive
24(3), 308–325. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.003 Psychology, 3(1), 181–192. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-
PMID:16087263 0024-1 PMID:20517508

Jacobs, A. M., & Grainger, J. (1991). Automatic letter Jelavić, F. (1995). Didactic basics of teaching. Jastre-
priming in an alphabetic decision task. Perception & basko: Slap.
Psychophysics, 49(1), 43–52. doi:10.3758/BF03211615
Jelić, J. (2013). Personality traits, hope, optimism and life
PMID:2011452
satisfaction od kindergarten teacher. Faculty of Teacher
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociaton framework: Education, University of Rijeka.
Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory.
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental
Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 513–541.
ability. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.
doi:10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F
Jestice, R. J., & Kahai, S. (2010). The effectiveness of
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New
virtual worlds for education: an empirical study. Paper
York: Holt. doi:10.1037/11059-000
presented at the Sixteenth Americas Conference on In-
Janacsek, K., Fiser, J., & Nemeth, D. (2012). The best time formation Systems (AMCIS). Lima, Peru.
to acquire new skills: Age-related differences in implicit
Jo, E., & Berkowitz, L. (1994). A priming effect analysis of
sequence learning across the human lifespan. Develop-
media influences: An update. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann
mental Science, 15(4), 496–505. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
(Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research.
7687.2012.01150.x PMID:22709399
LEA’s communication series (pp. 43–60). Hillsdale, NJ:
Janacsek, K., & Nemeth, D. (2013). Implicit sequence Lawrence Erlbaum.
learning and working memory: Correlated or compli-
John-Steiner, V., & Mahn, H. (1996). Sociocultural ap-
cated? Cortex, 49(8), 2001–2006. doi:10.1016/j.cor-
proaches to learning and development: A Vygotskian
tex.2013.02.012 PMID:23541152
framework. Educational Psychologist, 31(2), 191–206.
Jarmon, L., Traphagan, T., Mayrath, M., & Trivedi, A. doi:10.1080/00461520.1996.9653266
(2009). Virtual world teaching, experiential learning, and
Jones, G., & Bronack, C. (2007). Rethinking cognition,
assessment: An interdisciplinary communication course
representations and processes in 3D online social learning
in Second Life. Computers & Education, 53(1), 169–182.
environments. In D. Gibson, C. Aldridge, & M. Prenky
doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2009.01.010
(Eds.), Games and simulations in online learning: Re-
Järvelä, S. (1995). The cognitive apprenticeship model search and development Frameworks (pp. 1–20). Hersey,
in a technology rich learning environment: Interpreting PA: Information Science Publishing.
the learning interaction. Learning and Instruction, 5(2),
Jones, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Hetts,
237–259. doi:10.1016/0959-4752(95)00007-P
J. J. (2002). Name letter preferences are not merely mere
Järvenoja, H., & Järvelä, S. (2009). Emotion control in exposure: Implicit egotism as self-regulation. Journal
collaborative learning situations - Do students regulate of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(2), 170–177.
emotions evoked from social challenges? The British doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1497
Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(3), 463–481.
Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A
doi:10.1348/000709909X402811 PMID:19208290
Decade of System Justification Theory: Accumulated
Jaśkowski, P., Białuńska, A., Tomanek, M., & Verleger, Evidence of Conscious and Unconscious Bolstering of
R. (2008). Mask- and distractor-triggered inhibitory pro- the Status Quo. Political Psychology, 25(6), 881–919.
cesses in the priming of motor response: An EEG study. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x
Psychophysiology, 45, 70–85. PMID:17887961

330
Compilation of References

Kafai, Y. B. (2006). Playing and making games for Kappes, C., Bermeitinger, C., & Greve, W. (submitted).
learning: Instructionist and constructionist perspectives Fear-of-crime priming by means of a threat decision task
for game studies. Games and Culture, 1(1), 36–40. in younger and older adults.
doi:10.1177/1555412005281767
Karim, A. & Sanza, C. (2013). Learning by implicit imita-
Kafai, Y., & Resnick, M. (1996). Constructionism in tion in virtual worlds. Path Planning 2013.
Practice: Designing, Thinking, and Learning in a Digital
Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the
World. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and
Kahan, T. A. (2000). Negative priming from masked Social Psychology, 81(5), 774–778. doi:10.1037/0022-
words: Retrospective prime clarification or center-sur- 3514.81.5.774 PMID:11708556
round inhibition? Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Karremans, J. C., Stroebe, W., & Claus, J. (2006). Beyond
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(6), 1392–1410.
Vicary’s fantasies: The impact of subliminal priming and
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.6.1392 PMID:11185772
brand choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Kahler, C. W., Daughters, S. B., Leventhal, A. M., Gwalt- 42(6), 792–798. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.002
ney, C. J., & Palfai, T. P. (2007). Implicit associations
Kaufman, J. C., Kaufman, A. S., Kaufman-Singer, J., &
between smoking and social consequences among smokers
Kaufman, N. L. (2005). The Kaufman Assessment Bat-
in cessation treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
tery for Children – (2nd ed.) and the Kaufman Adolescent
45(9), 2066–2077. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.004
and Adult Intelligence Test. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L.
PMID:17448442
Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment:
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and Theories, tests, and issues (pp. 344-370). London: The
choice: Mapping bounded rationality. The American Guilford Press.
Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720. doi:10.1037/0003-
Kaufman, S. B. (2011). Intelligence and the cogni-
066X.58.9.697 PMID:14584987
tive unconscious. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp.
York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. 442–467). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
doi:10.1017/CBO9780511977244.023
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2007, February). Frames
and brains: Elicitation and control of response tendencies. Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Brown, J.,
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 45–46. doi:10.1016/j. & Mackintosh, N. (2009). Associative learning predicts
tics.2006.11.007 PMID:17188556 intelligence above and beyond working memory and pro-
cessing speed. Intelligence, 37(4), 374–382. doi:10.1016/j.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory:
intell.2009.03.004
An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2),
263–291. doi:10.2307/1914185 Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jimenez,
L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit learning
Kamenov, E. (1987). Experimental programmemes for
as an ability. Cognition, 116(3), 321–340. doi:10.1016/j.
early education. Beograd: Zavod za udžbenike i nastavna
cognition.2010.05.011 PMID:20573341
sredstva.
Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Reis, D. L., & Gray, J.
Kapnas, G., Ntoa, S., Margetis, G., Antona, M., &
R. (2011). General intelligence predicts reasoning abil-
Stephanidis, C. (2011). Seven Wonders: An Interactive
ity even for evolutionarily familiar content. Intelligence,
Game for Learning English as a Foreign Language in
39(5), 311–322. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2011.05.002
Junior High-School. In C. Stephanidis (Ed.), Posters,
Part II, HCII 2011 (pp. 499–503). doi:10.1007/978-3-
642-22095-1_100

331
Compilation of References

Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., & Hoffmann, J. (2007). Mecha-
Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects nisms of subliminal response priming. Advances in
of training in the negation of stereotypic associations Cognitive Psychology, 3(1), 307–315. doi:10.2478/
on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and v10053-008-0032-1 PMID:20517516
Social Psychology, 78(5), 871–888. doi:10.1037/0022-
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1995). Memory and consciousness: An
3514.78.5.871 PMID:10821195
appreciation of Claparede and Recognition et Moiiete.
Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S., Bargh, J. A., & Ross, L. (2004). Consciousness & Cognition. International Journal (To-
Material priming: The influence of mundane physical ronto, Ont.), 4(4), 379–386.
objects on situational construal and competitive be-
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Memory research: The conver-
havioral choice. Organizational Behavior and Human
gence of theory and practice. In D. Hermann, C. McEvoy,
Decision Processes, 95(1), 83–96. doi:10.1016/j.ob-
C. Hertzog, P. Hertel, & M. K. Johnson (Eds.), Basic and
hdp.2004.06.003
applied memory research: Theory in context (Vol. 1, pp.
Keller, J., & Dauenheimer, D. (2003). Stereotype threat 5–25). Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum.
in the classroom: Dejection mediates the disrupting
Kihlstrom, J. F., Dorfman, J., & Park, L. (2007).
threat effect on women’s math performance. Personal-
Implicit and Explicit Memory and Learning. In M.
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(3), 371–381.
Velmans & S. Schneider (Eds.), The Blackwell Com-
doi:10.1177/0146167202250218 PMID:15273014
panion to Consciousness. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). doi:10.1002/9780470751466.ch42
Power, approach and inhibition. Psychological Review,
Kindlon, D. J. (2006). Alpha girls: Understanding the
110(2), 265–28. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.265
new American girl and how she is changing the world.
PMID:12747524
New York, NY: Rodale Press.
Kentli, F. D. (2009). Comparison of hidden curriculum
King, N., & Apple, M. W. (1992). What do school teach.
theories. European Journal of Educational Studies, 1(2),
In M. W. Apple (Ed.), School, teacher, authority (pp.
83–88.
43-64). Ljubljana: Znanstveno in publicistično središče.
Kessler, S. (1992). The Social Context of the Early
Kinsler, K., & Gamble, A. M. (2001). Reforming schools.
Childhood Curriculum. In Reconceptualizing the early
London: Continuum.
childhood curriculum: Beginning the dialogue. (pp. 21-
42). New York: Teachers College Press. Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E.
S. (2009). Accent trumps race in guiding children’s
Kessler, S., & Swadener, B. B. (Eds.). (1992). Reconcep-
social preferences. Social Cognition, 27(4), 623–634.
tualizing the early childhood curriculum: Beginning the
doi:10.1521/soco.2009.27.4.623 PMID:21603154
dialogue. New York: Teachers College Press.
Kirschner, P., Martens, R., & Strijbos, J. (2004). CSCL in
Kiefer, M. (2007). Top-down modulation of unconscious
Higher Education? A framework for designing multiple
‘automatic’ processes: A gating framework. Advances
collaborative environments. In J. Strijbos, P. Kirschner,
in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1), 289–306. doi:10.2478/
& R. Martens (Eds.), What are we know about CSCL?
v10053-008-0031-2 PMID:20517515
(pp. 3-30). Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional sensitization
Klapp, S. T. (2005). Two versions of the negative compat-
of unconscious cognition: Task sets modulate subsequent
ibility effect: Comment on Lleras and Enns (2004). Journal
masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental
of Experimental Psychology. General, 134(3), 431–435.
Psychology. General, 139(3), 464–489. doi:10.1037/
doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.3.431 PMID:16131274
a0019561 PMID:20677895

332
Compilation of References

Klapp, S. T. (2007). Nonconscious control mimics a pur- Klauer, K. C., Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Spruyt, A.
poseful strategy: Strength of Stroop-like interference is au- (2009). Contrast effects in spontaneous evaluations: A
tomatically modulated by proportion of compatible trials. psychophysical account. Journal of Personality and So-
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception cial Psychology, 96(2), 265–287. doi:10.1037/a0013248
and Performance, 33(6), 1366–1376. doi:10.1037/0096- PMID:19159132
1523.33.6.1366 PMID:18085949
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Sherman, J. W. (1993). The role
Klapp, S. T., & Hinkley, L. B. (2002). The negative compat- of summary and specific behavioral memories in trait judg-
ibility effect: Unconscious inhibition influences reaction ments about the self. Personality and Social Psychology
time and response selection. Journal of Experimental Bulletin, 19(3), 305–311. doi:10.1177/0146167293193007
Psychology. General, 131(2), 255–269. doi:10.1037/0096-
Klimesch, W. (2011). Evoked alpha and early access
3445.131.2.255 PMID:12049243
to the knowledge system: The P1 inhibition timing hy-
Klauer, K. C., & Dittrich, K. (2010). From sunshine to pothesis. Brain Research, 1408, 52–71. doi:10.1016/j.
double arrows: An evaluation window account of negative brainres.2011.06.003 PMID:21774917
compatibility effects. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
Klotz, W., & Neumann, O. (1999). Motor activation with-
ogy. General, 139(3), 490–519. doi:10.1037/a0019746
out conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking.
PMID:20677896
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception
Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2002). Goal-dependent and and Performance, 25(4), 976–992. doi:10.1037/0096-
goal-independent effects of irrelevant evaluations. Per- 1523.25.4.976
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(6), 802–814.
Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1994). Information
doi:10.1177/0146167202289009
acquired during artificial grammar learning. Journal
Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: Find- of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and
ings and theories. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The Cognition, 20(1), 79–91. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.1.79
psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition PMID:8138790
and emotion (pp. 7–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1996). Artificial
Klauer, K. C., Musch, J., & Eder, A. B. (2005). Priming grammar learning depends on implicit learning of both
of semantic classifications: Late and response related, abstract and exemplar-specific information. Journal of
or earlier and more central? Psychonomic Bulletin & Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cog-
Review, 12(5), 897–903. doi:10.3758/BF03196783 nition, 22(1), 169–181. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.169
PMID:16524008 PMID:8648284

Klauer, K. C., Roßnagel, C., & Musch, J. (1997). Koch, S. C. (2005). Evaluative affect display toward male
List-context effects in evaluative priming. Journal of and female leaders of task-oriented groups. Small Group Re-
Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cog- search, 36(6), 678–703. doi:10.1177/1046496405281768
nition, 23(1), 246–255. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.1.246
Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., & Ristikari, T.
PMID:9028030
(2011). Are leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis
Klauer, K. C., & Stern, E. (1992). How attitudes guide of three research paradigms. Psychological Bulletin,
memory-based judgments: A two-process model. Jour- 137(4), 616–642. doi:10.1037/a0023557 PMID:21639606
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28(2), 186–206.
Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1991).
doi:10.1016/0022-1031(92)90038-L
The transfer of contextual control over equivalence classes
Klauer, K. C., & Teige-Mocigemba, S. (2007). Control- through equivalence classes: A possible model of social
lability and resource dependence in automatic evalua- stereotyping. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
tion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(4), Behavior, 56(3), 505–518. doi:10.1901/jeab.1991.56-505
648–655. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.06.003 PMID:1774542

333
Compilation of References

Koster, R. (2004). A virtual world by any other name? Krüger, D., Klapötke, S., Bode, S., & Mattler, U. (2013).
Retrieved 14 October 2012 from http://terranova.blogs. Neural correlates of control operations in inverse prim-
com/terra_nova/2004/06/a_virtual_world.html ing with relevant and irrelevant masks. NeuroImage,
64, 197–208. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.018
Koutsabasis, P., & Vosinakis, S. (2012). Rethinking HCI
PMID:22989624
Education for Design: Problem-Based Learning and
Virtual Worlds at an HCI Design Studio. International Krüger, D., Klapötke, S., & Mattler, U. (2011). PRP-
Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 28(8), 485–499. paradigm provides evidence for a perceptual origin of the
doi:10.1080/10447318.2012.687664 negative compatibility effect. Consciousness and Cogni-
tion, 20(3), 866–881. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.014
Krank, M., Wall, A.-M., Stewart, S. H., Wiers, R. W., &
PMID:20947385
Goldman, M. S. (2005). Context effects on alcohol cogni-
tions. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, Kruglanski, A., Orehek, E., Dechenese, M., & Pierro, A.
29(2), 196–206. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000153545.36787. (2010). Lay Epistemic Theory: The Motivational, Cogni-
C8 PMID:15714043 tive and social aspects of knowledge formation. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(10), 939–950.
Krathwohl, D. R. (2002b, Autumn).The Role of Metacog-
doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00308.x
nitive Knowledge in Learning, Teaching, and Assessing.
Theory into Practice, 41 (4), 220-227. Krusemark, E. A., Campbell, W. K., & Clementz, B.
A. (2008). Attributions, deception, and event related
Krathwohl, D. R. (2002a). A revision of Bloom’s Taxon-
potentials: An investigation of the self‐serving bias.
omy: An overview. Theory into Practice, 41(4), 212–218.
Psychophysiology, 45(4), 511–515. doi:10.1111/j.1469-
doi:10.1207/s15430421tip4104_2
8986.2008.00659.x PMID:18282197
Krathwohl, D. R., Bloom, B. S., & Masia, B. B. (1964).
Kunda, Z. (1987). Motivated inference: Self-serving
Taxonomy of educational objectives: The classification of
generation and evaluation of causal theories. Journal
educational goals: Handbook 11: The affective domain.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(4), 636–647.
New York: David McKay.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.636
Kray, L. J., Thompson, L., & Galinsky, A. (2001). Battle
Küper, K., Groh-Bordin, C., Zimmer, H. D., & Ecker, U.
of the sexes: Gender stereotype confirmation and reactance
K. H. (2012). Electrophysiological correlates of exem-
in negotiations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
plar-specific processes in implicit and explicit memory.
ogy, 80(6), 942–958. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.942
Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(1),
PMID:11414376
52–64. doi:10.3758/s13415-011-0065-7 PMID:22038704
Kreijns, K. (2004). Sociable CSCL environments: So-
Kutas, M., Lindamood, T., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984).
cial affordances, social ability and social presence.
Word expectancy and event-related brain potentials dur-
Retrieved 3 October 2012 from http://wenku.baidu.com/
ing sentence processing. In S. Kornblum & J. Roquin
view/3ddc7c848762caaedd33d452.html
(Eds.), Preparatory states and processes (pp. 217–238).
Kristiansen, G. (2001). Social and linguistic stereotyping: Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
A cognitive approach to accents. Estudios Ingleses De La
Kwan, V. S., Barrios, V., Ganis, G., Gorman, J., Lange, C.,
Universidad Complutense, 392(9), 129–146.
& Kumar, M. et al. (2007). Assessing the neural correlates
Kroflič, R. (1997). Between obedience and resposability. of self-enhancement bias: A transcranial magnetic stimula-
Ljubljana: Vija. tion study. Experimental Brain Research, 182(3), 379–385.
doi:10.1007/s00221-007-0992-2 PMID:17607566

334
Compilation of References

Kyriacou, C. (2001). Basic teaching virtues. Zagreb: Latu, I. M., Mast, M., Lammers, J., & Bombari, D. (2013).
Educa. Successful female leaders empower women’s behavior in
leadership tasks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
Lakkala, M. (2008). Principles of progressive inquiry.
ogy, 49(3), 444–448. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.01.003
EAAIL. Retrieved 12 October 2012 from http://deafed-
childabuse-neglect-col.wiki.educ.msu.edu/file/view/ Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and
Lakkala+M+-+2008+-+Progressive+inquiry+model_ function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. Advances in
introduction.pdf Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 1–62. doi:10.1016/
S0065-2601(00)80003-9
Lakkala, M., IIomaki, L. Lallimo, J. & Hakkarainen, K.
(2003). Virtual communication in middle school stu- Ledić, J. (1999). School and values. Rijeka: Faculty of
dents’ and teachers’ inquiry. In Proceedings of the CSCL Humanities and Social Sciences.
(pp. 443-452). Maastricht, the Netherlands: Maastricht
Lee, C., & Cheen, T. (2010). Scaffolding writing using
McLuhan Institute.
feedback in students graphic organize-novice writers’
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. relevance of ideas and cognitive tools. Educational Media
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. International, 47(2), 135–152. doi:10.1080/09523987.2
010.492678
Lambert, W. E. (1967). A social psychology of bilin-
gualism. The Journal of Social Issues, 23(2), 91–109. Lehtinen, E. (2003). Computer-supported collaborative
doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1967.tb00578.x learning: An approach to powerful learning environments.
In Advances in learning and instruction series: Powerful
Land, S., & Hannafin, M. (2000). Student-centered learn-
learning environments: Unraveling basic components and
ing environments. In Theoretical foundations of Learning
dimensions (pp. 35-65). Pergmaon.
Environments (pp. 1–23). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Lehtinen, E. (2003). Computer-Supported Collaborative
Lane, K. A., Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Greenwald, A.
Learning: an Approach to Powerful Learning Environ-
G. (2007). Understanding and using the Implicit Associa-
ments. In E. De Corte, L. Verschaffel, N. Entwistle, &
tion Test: IV: Procedures and validity. In B. Wittenbrink
J. Van Merriënboer (Eds.), Powerful Learning Environ-
& N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes:
ments: Unravelling Basic Components and Dimensions
Procedures and controversies (pp. 59–102). New York:
(pp. 35–53). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Guilford Press.
Leipold, B., Bermeitinger, C., Greve, W., Meyer, B., Ar-
Lane, K. A., Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Greenwald, A.
nold, M., & Pielniok, M. (subm.). Short-term induction
G. (2007). Understanding and using the Implicit Associa-
of assimilation and accommodation.
tion Test: IV: What we know (so far) about the method. In
B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit Measures Leitner, J. B., Hehman, E., Jones, J. M., & Forbes, C. E.
of Attitudes (pp. 59–102). New York: The Guilford Press. (in press). Self-enhancement influences medial frontal
cortex alpha power to social rejection feedback. Journal
Laran, J., Dalton, A. N., & Andrade, E. B. (2011). The
of Cognitive Neuroscience. PMID:24738770
curious case of behavioral backlash: Why brands pro-
duce priming effects and slogans produce reverse prim- Leitner, J. B., Jones, J. M., & Hehman, E. (2013). Suc-
ing effects. The Journal of Consumer Research, 37(6), ceeding in the face of threat: The adaptive role of engae-
999–1014. doi:10.1086/656577 gment regulation. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 39(1), 17–27. doi:10.1177/0146167212463083
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in
PMID:23125031
behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in
behavior (pp. 112–136). New York: John Wiley.

335
Compilation of References

Lempp, H., & Seale, C. (2004). The hidden curriculum Lipkens, R., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993). Longitu-
in undergraduate medical education: Qualitative study of dinal study of the development of derived relations in an
medical students’ perceptions of teaching. BMJ (Clini- infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56(2),
cal Research Ed.), 329(7469), 770–773. doi:10.1136/ 201–239. doi:10.1006/jecp.1993.1032 PMID:8245768
bmj.329.7469.770 PMID:15459051
Lippi-Green, R. (1997). English with An Accent: Lan-
Lepičnik Vodopivec, J. (2004): Empathy of kindergarten guage, Ideology, and Discrimination in the United States.
teachers as an element of hidden curriculum in kinder- London: Routledge.
gartens. Sodobna Pedagogika, 2, 140-151.
Lipponen, L., & Lallimo, J. (2004). From collaborative
Leslie, J. C., Tierney, K. J., Robinson, C. P., & Keenan, M. technology to collaborative use of technology: Design-
et al. (1993). Differences between clinically anxious and ing learning oriented infrastructures. Educational Media
non-anxious subjects in a stimulus equivalence training International, 41(2), 111–116. doi:10.1080/095239804
task involving threat words. The Psychological Record, 10001678566
43, 153–161.
Lips, H. M. (2000). College students’ visions of power and
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). possibility as moderated by gender. Psychology of Women
All frames ar not created equal: A typology and critical Quarterly, 24(1), 39–43. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.
analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and tb01020.x
Human Decision Processes, 76(2), 149–188. doi:10.1006/
Lleras, A., & Enns, J. T. (2004). Negative compatibility
obhd.1998.2804 PMID:9831520
or object updating? A cautionary tale of mask-dependent
Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General,
S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M. (2007). Putting feelings 133(4), 475–493. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.475
into words: Affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity to PMID:15584802
affective stimuli. Psychological Science, 18(5), 421–428.
Lockwood, P. (2006). ‘Someone like me can be suc-
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01916.x PMID:17576282
cessful’: Do college students need same-gender role
Li, J., Peng, J., Zhang, W., Han, F., & Yuan, W. (2011). models? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30(1), 36–46.
A computer-supported platform based on clouds. Journal doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2006.00260.x
of Computer Information Systems, 7(11), 3811–3818.
Loersch, C., Durso, G. R. O., & Petty, R. E. (2013). Vicis-
Lindemann, S. (2003). Koreans, Chinese or Indians? At- situdes of desire: A matching mechanism for subliminal
titudes and ideologies about non-native English speakers persuasion. Social Psychological and Personality, 4(5),
in the United States. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 7(3), 624–631. doi:10.1177/1948550612471975
348–364. doi:10.1111/1467-9481.00228
Loersch, C., & Payne, B. K. (2011). The situated in-
Lingnau, A., & Vorberg, D. (2005). The time course of ference model: An integrative account of the effects
response inhibition in masked priming. Perception & of primes on perception, behavior, and motivation.
Psychophysics, 67(3), 545–557. doi:10.3758/BF03193330 Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 234–252.
PMID:16119400 doi:10.1177/1745691611406921

Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buf- Loftus, E. F. (1975). Spreading activation within semantic
fer against stress-related illness and depression. Journal categories: Comments on Rosch’s ‘cognitive representa-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(4), 663–676. tions of semantic categories’. Journal of Experimental
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.4.663 PMID:3572732 Psychology. General, 104(3), 234–240. doi:10.1037/0096-
3445.104.3.234

336
Compilation of References

Logie, R. H. (2003). Spatial and visual working memory. Major, B., Spencer, S., Schmader, T., Wolfe, C., &
In The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 37–78). Crocker, J. (1998). Coping with negative stereotypes
Hove, UK: Academic Press. about intellectual performance: The role of psychologi-
cal disengagement. Personality and Social Psychology
Lowe, C. F. (1979). Determinants of human operant be-
Bulletin, 24(1), 34–50. doi:10.1177/0146167298241003
havior. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.), Advances in
analysis of behavior: Reinforcement and the Organization Malaguzzi, L. (1998). History, ideas and philosophy. In C.
of behavior (vol. 1, pp. 159-192). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Edwards, L. Gandini, & G. Forman (Eds.), The Hundred
Languages of Children: The Reggio Emilia Approach (pp.
Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without associa-
41–89). London: Ablex Publishing Greenwich.
tion: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 7(4), 618–630. doi:10.3758/BF03212999 Malamos, A., Mamakis, G., Sympa, P., Kotanitsi, E., Gonel
PMID:11206202 Crespo, A., & Zubizarraeta Lopez, A. (2008). Technical
aspects in using X3D in virtual reality mathematics Edu-
Luck, S. J., & Kappenman, E. S. (2012). ERP components
cation (EViE-m platform). In Proceedings of 5th WSEAS/
and selective attention. In S. J. Luck & E. S. Kappen-
IASME International Conference on engineering educa-
mann (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related
tion (EE’08). Herakleion.
Potential Components (pp. 295–328). New York: Oxford
University Press. Mandel, N., & Johnson, E. J. (2002). When web pages
influence choice: Effects of visual primes on experts
Ludvidgsen, S., & Morch, A. (2006). Categorization in
and novices. The Journal of Consumer Research, 29(2),
knowledge building: Task specific argumentation in co-
235–245. doi:10.1086/341573
located CSCL environment. Retrieved 23 August 2012
from http://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/docs/00/19/05/22/ Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previ-
PDF/ludvigs_morch.pdfv1.pdf ous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87(3), 252–271.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.252
Machado, L., Wyatt, N., Devine, A., & Knight, B. (2007).
Action planning in the presence of distracting stimuli: An Marien, H., Custers, R., Hassin, R. R., & Aarts, H. (2012).
investigation into the time course of distractor effects. Unconscious goal activation and the hijacking of the
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception executive function. Journal of Personality and Social
and Performance, 33(5), 1045–1061. doi:10.1037/0096- Psychology, 103(3), 399–415. doi:10.1037/a0028955
1523.33.5.1045 PMID:17924806 PMID:22686538

Mackintosh, N. J. (2011). IQ and human intelligence (2nd Markus, H., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and malle-
ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. ability of the self-concept. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 51(4), 858–866. doi:10.1037/0022-
MacMahon, K. M. A., & Masters, R. S. W. (2002). The
3514.51.4.858 PMID:3783430
effects of secondary tasks on implicit motor skill per-
formance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (Eds.). (1985). Relapse
33, 307–324. prevention. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Major, J. C. (2008). List composition effects for masked Marsh, C. J. (2004). Key concepts for understanding
semantic primes: Evidence inconsistent with activation curriculum. London: RoutlegeFalmer, Taylor & Francis
accounts. Theses and Dissertations (Comprehensive). Pa- Group.
per 1059. Retrieved from http://scholars.wlu.ca/etd/1059

337
Compilation of References

Martić, K. (2013). Is the empathy of kindergarten teacher Mattler, U. (2005). Inhibition and decay of motor and
related to their personality? Faculty of Teacher Educa- nonmotor priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(2),
tion, University of Rijeka. 285–300. doi:10.3758/BF03206492 PMID:15971692

Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role models: Mattler, U. (2006). On the locus of priming and inverse
Protecting women’s math test performance. Personal- priming effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(6),
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(9), 1183–1193. 975–991. doi:10.3758/BF03193359 PMID:17153192
doi:10.1177/01461672022812004
Mattler, U. (2007). Inverse target- and cue-priming effects
Marx, D. M., & Stapel, D. A. (2006). It’s all in the tim- of masked stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
ing: Measuring emotional reactions to stereotype threat Human Perception and Performance, 33(1), 83–102.
before and after taking a test. European Journal of Social doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.83 PMID:17311481
Psychology, 36(5), 687–698. doi:10.1002/ejsp.310
Mattler, U., & Fendrich, R. (2007). Priming by motion
Massialas, B. G. (1996). The hidden curriculum and social too rapid to be consciously seen. Perception & Psycho-
studies. In Critical Issues in Teaching Social Studies (pp. physics, 69(8), 1389–1398. doi:10.3758/BF03192954
61-79). Wadsworth. PMID:18078229

Massialas, B. G. (1989). The Inevitability of Issue- May, C. P., Kane, M. J., & Hasher, L. (1995). Determi-
Centered Discourse in the Classroom. Social Studies, nants of negative priming. Psychological Bulletin, 118(1),
80(5), 173–175. doi:10.1080/00377996.1989.9957472 35–54. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.35 PMID:7644605

Masson, M. E. J. (1999). Semantic priming in a re- Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2006). Evidence for episodic
current network: Comment on Dalrymple-Alford and retrieval of inadequate prime responses in auditory
Marmurek (1999). Journal of Experimental Psychology. negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(3), 776–794. Human Perception and Performance, 32(4), 932–943.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.3.776 doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.932 PMID:16846289

Masson, M. E. J., & Isaak, M. I. (1999). Masked prim- Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2007). Negative priming as a
ing of words and nonwords in a naming task: Further memory phenomenon: A review of 20 years of negative
evidence for a nonlexical basis for priming. Memory & priming research. Zeitschrift für Psychologie. The Journal
Cognition, 27(3), 399–412. doi:10.3758/BF03211536 of Psychology, 215, 35–51.
PMID:10355231
McConnell, A. R., Rydell, R. J., Strain, L. M., & Mackie,
Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., Pfeifer, J. D. M. (2008). Forming implicit and explicit attitudes to-
H., McNealy, K., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2009). ward individuals: Social group association cues. Journal
Neural. correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 792–807.
Understanding the distress of peer rejection Social. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.792 PMID:18444739
Cognitive, and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 143–157.
McCrae, R. R. (1994). Openness to experience: Expanding
doi:10.1093/scan/nsp007 PMID:19470528
the boundaries of factor 5. European Journal of Personal-
Matsuda, M. J. (1991). Voices of America: Accent, an- ity, 8(4), 251–271. doi:10.1002/per.2410080404
tidiscrimination law, and a jurisprudence for the last Re-
McCrae, R. R., & Allik, J. (2002). The five-factor model of
construction. The Yale Law Journal, 100(5), 1329–1407.
personality across cultures. New York: Kluwer Academic/
doi:10.2307/796694
Plenum Publishers. doi:10.1007/978-1-4615-0763-5
Matthews, P., & Stephens, R. (2010). Sociable
McDiarmid, G. (1987). The Development of a Conceptual-
knowledge sharing online: Philosophy, patterns and
Empirical Theory of Curriculum. Interchange, 18(3),
intervention. Aslib Proceedings, 62(6), 539–553.
38–62. doi:10.1007/BF01807219
doi:10.1108/00012531011089667

338
Compilation of References

McGeorge, P., Crawford, J. R., & Kelly, S. W. (1997). Miljak, A. (2007): Learning by doing and participating
The relationships between psychometric intelligence and in a community of professional kindergarten teacher. In
learning in an explicit and an implicit task. Journal of Pedagogy: Towards to lifelong learning and knowledge
Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cog- society (pp. 201-214). Zagreb: Croatian Educational
nition, 23(1), 239–245. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.1.239 Society.
PMID:9028029
Miljak, A., & Vujičić, L. (Eds.). (2002). Kindergarten
McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, corresponds to child’s nature “child’s house”. Rovinj:
D. (2004). Perspective-taking as relational responding: Kindergarten “Neven” Rovinj.
A developmental profile. The Psychological Record,
Miljak, A. (1996). Humanistic approach to theory and
54(1), 115–144.
practice of kindergarten education. Zagreb: Persona.
McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R., & Lord, C. (2003). Alle-
Miljak, A. (2009). Life of children in kindergartens.
viating women’s mathematics stereotype threat through
Zagreb: SM Naklada.
salience of group achievements. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 39(1), 83–90. doi:10.1016/S0022- Miljak, A., & Vujičić, L. (Eds.). (2000). Kindergarten
1031(02)00513-9 as child’s house. Poreč: Dječji vrtić “Radost”. Poreč.
McNamara, T. P. (2005). Semantic priming: Perspectives Miller, M. L., Omens, R. S., & Delvadia, R. (1991). Di-
from memory and word recognition. New York: Psychol- mensions of social competence: Personality and coping
ogy Press. doi:10.4324/9780203338001 style correlates. Personality and Individual Differences,
12(9), 955–964. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90185-E
Merwin, R. M., & Wilson, K. G. (2005). Preliminary
findings on the effects of self-referring and evaluative Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P. M., & Seiffert,
stimuli on stimulus equivalence class formation. The A. E. (1998). Selective attention: A reevaluation of the
Psychological Record, 55, 561–575. implications of negative priming. Psychological Review,
105(2), 203–229. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.203
Meulemans, T., Van der Linden, M., & Perruchet, P.
PMID:9577237
(1998). Implicit sequence learning in children. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 69(3), 199–221. Milyavsky, M., Hassin, R. R., & Schul, Y. (2012). Guess
doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2442 PMID:9654439 what? Implicit motivation boosts the influence of sublimi-
nal information on choice. Consciousness and Cognition,
M e z i row, J . ( 1 9 7 8 ) . Pe r s p e c t i ve Tra n s fo r-
21(3), 1232–1241. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.06.001
m a t i o n . Ad u l t E d u c a t i o n , 2 8( 2 ) , 1 0 0 – 1 1 0 .
PMID:22776628
doi:10.1177/074171367802800202
Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing knowl-
Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., & Hankin,
edge. In P. H. Winston (Ed.), The Psychology of Computer
B. L. (2004). Is there a universal positivity bias in at-
Vision (pp. 211–277). McGraw-Hill.
tributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, devel-
opmental, and cultural differences in the self-serving at- Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H. (2007). Extending
tributional bias. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 711–747. statistical learning farther and further: Long-distance
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.711 PMID:15367078 dependencies, and individual differences in statistical
learning and language. In McNamaraD. S.TraftonJ. G.
Michail, N., Teal, G., & Basta, J. (2006). Progressive
(Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Annual Conference of
learning processes model-interpretive methodological
the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1307-1312). Austin,
framework for human systems inquiries. In Proceedings
TX: Cognitive Science Society.
of the 50th Annual Meeting of the International Society
for the Systems Sciences (ISSS) (pp. 34-39). Sonoma,
CA: ISSS.

339
Compilation of References

Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature Moran, L., Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Ming, S. (2010).
and organization of individual differences in ex- Brief report: The training and assessment of relational
ecutive functions: Four general conclusions. Current precursors and abilities (TARPA): A preliminary analy-
Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. sis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
doi:10.1177/0963721411429458 PMID:22773897 40(9), 1149–1153. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-0968-0
PMID:20151185
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A.
H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and Moxon, P., Keenan, M., & Hine, L. (1993). Gender-role
diversity of executive functions and their contributions stereotyping and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological
to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analy- Record, 43(3), 381–393.
sis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. doi:10.1006/
Mulac, A., Hanley, T. D., & Prigge, D. Y. (1974). Ef-
cogp.1999.0734 PMID:10945922
fects of phonological speech foreignness upon three
Mlinarević, V., Buljubašić, V., & Sablić, M. (2007): dimensions of attitude of selected American listeners.
Promoting educational values in myths and legends in The Quarterly Journal of Speech, 60(4), 411–420.
lower grades in elementary scools. In Golden days 8: doi:10.1080/00335637409383250
Myths and legend (pp. 173-187). University Josip Juraj
Mullenix, J. W., Kilbey, M. M., Fisicaro, S. A.,
Strosmayer Osijek, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sci-
Farnsworth, L. M., & Torrento, R. (2003). The rep-
ences Osijek, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences
resentation of smoking expectancies by single-word
Pečuh, Matica hrvatska Osijek.
adjectives and their relationsship to smoking charac-
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Field, M., & De Houwer, J. teristics. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 5, 681–694.
(2003). Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in doi:10.1080/1462220031000158654 PMID:14577985
smokers: Relationship between attentional biases and
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation
implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence. Addic-
and depletion of limited resources: Does self- control
tion (Abingdon, England), 98(6), 825–836. doi:10.1046/
resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247–
j.1360-0443.2003.00392.x PMID:12780371
259. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247 PMID:10748642
Mogg, K., Field, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional
Murphy, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Establishing
and approach biases for smoking cues in smokers: An
derived manding for specific amounts with three children:
investigation of competing theoretical views of addiction.
An attempt at synthesizing Skinner’s Verbal Behavior
Psychopharmacology, 180(2), 333–341. doi:10.1007/
with relational frame theory. The Psychological Record,
s00213-005-2158-x PMID:15696322
59, 75–91.
Moore, R. (2006). Playgounds: A 150 year old model. In
Musch, J. (2000). Affektives Priming: Kongruenzeffekte
Safe and Healthy School Environments (pp. 86-103). Re-
bei der evaluativen Bewertung. Inauguraldissertation zur
trieved September 26, 2013, from http://web.seru.sa.edu.
Erlangung des Grades eines Doktors der Philosophie an
au/pdfs/SERUpdate%20April%202013%20Play.pdf
der Fakultät für Sozial- und Verhaltenswissenschaften
Moran, J. M., Macrae, C. N., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, der Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg.
C. L., & Kelley, W. M. (2006). Neuroanatomical evidence
Musch, J., Elze, A., & Klauer, K. C. (1998). Gibt es
for distinct cognitive and affective components of self.
Wortlängeneffekte in der evaluativen Entscheidung-
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(9), 1586–1594.
saufgabe? Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie,
doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1586 PMID:16989558
45, 109–119.

340
Compilation of References

Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison Processes in Social Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual
Judgment: Mechanisms and Consequences. Psycho- word recognition: A selective review of current findings
logical Review, 110(3), 472–489. doi:10.1037/0033- and theories. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.),
295X.110.3.472 PMID:12885111 Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp.
264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Muukkonen, H., Hakkarainen, K., & Lakkala, M. (1999).
Collaborative Technology for Facilitating Progressive Neely, J. H., & Keefe, D. E. (1989). Semantic context
Inquiry: the Future Learning Environment Tools. In C. effects on visual word processing: A hybrid prospective-
Hoadley & J. Roschelle (Eds.), Proceedings of the CSCL retrospective processing theory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.),
’99 Conference (pp. 406–415). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in
Erlbaum. doi:10.3115/1150240.1150291 research and theory (pp. 207–248). New York: Academic
Press. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60538-1
Naismith, S. L., Hickie, I. B., Ward, P. B., Scott, E., &
Little, C. (2005). Impaired implicit sequence learning Neely, J. H., Keefe, D. E., & Ross, K. L. (1989). Semantic
in depression: A probe for frontostriatal dysfunction? priming in the lexical decision task: Roles of prospec-
Psychological Medicine, 36(03), 313–323. doi:10.1017/ tive prime-generated expectancies and retrospective
S0033291705006835 PMID:16359605 semantic matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1003–1019.
Naismith, S. L., Lagopoulos, J., Ward, P. B., Davey, C.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.15.6.1003 PMID:2530303
G., Little, C., & Hickie, I. B. (2010). Fronto-striatal cor-
relates of impaired implicit sequence learning in major Neill, W. T. (1997). Episodic retrieval in negative prim-
depression: An fMRI study. Journal of Affective Disor- ing and repetition priming. Journal of Experimental
ders, 125(1-3), 256–261. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2010.02.114 Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(6),
PMID:20219248 1291–1305. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.6.1291

Nakamura, D. (2010). Are intelligence and working Neill, W. T. (2007). Mechanisms of transfer-inappropriate
memory isomorphic? Aoyama Gakuin. English Studies, processing. In D. S. Gorfein & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), In-
34, 25–64. hibition in cognition (pp. 63–78). Washington, DC: Ameri-
can Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/11587-004
Nakamura, D. (2013a). Methodological considerations
in studying awareness during learning. Part 1: Implicit Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J. Jr, Boykin, A.,
learning. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 44, 102–117. Brody, N., & Ceci, S. J. et al. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns
and unknowns. The American Psychologist, X(2), 77–101.
Nakamura, D. (2013b). Methodological considerations
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77
in studying awareness during learning. Part 2: Second
language acquisition. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 44, Neumann, O. (1990). Direct parameter specification and
337–353. the concept of perception. Psychological Research, 52(2-
3), 207–215. doi:10.1007/BF00877529 PMID:2281129
Nakash, O., & Brody, L. (2007). The effects of power im-
balances and gender on autobiographical memory. Self and Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to
Identity, 6(4), 304–321. doi:10.1080/15298860601109495 nonreportable masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct
parameter specification? In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch
Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval
(Eds.), Attention and performance XV: Conscious and
from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading
nonconscious information processing (pp. 124–150).
activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Experimental Psychology. General, 106(3), 226–254.
doi:10.1037/0096-3445.106.3.226

341
Compilation of References

Nguyen, H.-H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2009). Does stereotype Normand, V., Babski, C., Benford, S., Bullock, A.,
threat affect test performance of minorities and women? Carion, S., Farcet, N., et al. (1999). The COVEN project:
A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. The Journal exploring applicative, technical and usage dimensions
of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1314–1334. doi:10.1037/ of collaborative virtual environments. Proceedings of
a0012702 PMID:19025250 Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments,
8(2), 218–236. doi:doi:10.1162/105474699566189
Nicholson, E. (2005). The School Building as Third
doi:10.1162/105474699566189
Teacher. In M. Dudek (Ed.), Children Spaces (pp. 44–65).
London: Routledge. Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go as-
sociation task. Social cognition, 19(6), 625–666. Nosek,
Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science,
B. A., & Hansen, J. J. (2008). The associations in our
216(5827), 1002–1005. doi:10.1126/science.1136930
heads belong to us: Searching for attitudes and knowledge
PMID:17510358
in implicit evaluation. Cognition and Emotion, 22(4),
Nilsson, L. G. (2002). Presentation speech for the 2002 553–594. doi:10.1080/02699930701438186
Bank of Sweden prize in economic sciences in memory
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002).
of Alfred Nobel. Retrieved from http://www.nobelprize.
Math = male, me = female, therefore math ≠ me. Jour-
org/nobel_prizes/economic-sciences/laureates/2002/
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 44–59.
presentation-speech.html
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.44 PMID:12088131
Nimmo, J. (1998). The Child in Community: Constrains
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005).
From the Early Childhood Lore. In C. Edwards, L. Gan-
Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test:
dini, & G. Forman (Eds.), The Hundred Languages of
II: Method variables and construct validity. Personal-
Children: The Reggio Emilia Approach (pp. 295–312).
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(2), 166–180.
London: Ablex Publishing Greenwich.
doi:10.1177/0146167204271418 PMID:15619590
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional require-
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007).
ment of learning: Evidence from performance measure.
The Implicit Association Test at age 7: A methodological
Cognitive Psychology, 19(1), 1–32. doi:10.1016/0010-
and conceptual review. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Automatic
0285(87)90002-8
processes in social thinking and behavior (pp. 265–292).
Nonaka, I. (1994). A dynamic theory of organizational Psychology Press.
knowledge creation. Organization Science, 5(1), 14–37.
Nosek, B. A., & Smyth, F. L. (2007). A multitrait-
doi:10.1287/orsc.5.1.14
multimethod validation of the Implicit Association Test:
Nonaka, I., & Takeuchi, H. (1995). The Knowledge- Implicit and explicit attitudes are related but distinct
Creating Company: How Japanese Companies Create the constructs. Experimental Psychology, 54(1), 14–29.
Dynamics of Innovation. New York: Oxford University doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14 PMID:17341011
Press.
Nuttin, J. M. (1985). Narcissism beyond Gestalt and
Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: awareness: The name letter effect. European Journal
Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. Davidson, of Social Psychology, 15(3), 353–361. doi:10.1002/
G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and ejsp.2420150309
self-regulation: Advances in research and theory. New
York: Plenum.

342
Compilation of References

O’Reilly, A., Roche, B., Ruiz, M., Ryan, A., & Campion, G. Onrubia, J. & Engel, A. (2012). The role of teacher as-
(in press). A Function Acquisition Speed Test for equiva- sistance on the effects of a macro-script in collaborative
lence relations (FASTER). The Psychological Record. writing tasks. IJCSCL, 7 (1), 161-186.

O’Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smyth, S. (2007). Onrubia, J., & Engel, A. (2009). Strategies for collab-
A derived transfer of functions and the Implicit As- orative writing and phases of knowdge construction in
sociation Test. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of CSCL environments. Computers & Education, 53(4),
Behavior, 88(2), 263–283. doi:10.1901/jeab.2007.76-06 1256–1265. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2009.06.008
PMID:17970419
O’Reilly, A., Roche, B., Ruiz, M. R., Tyndall, I., & Gavin,
Oakes, J., & Lipton, M. (2003). Teaching to change the A. (2012). The Function Acquisition Speed Test (FAST):
world. New York: McGraw-Hill. A behavior-analytic implicit test for assessing stimulus
relations. The Psychological Record, 62, 507–528.
OECD. (2012a). Health at a Glance: Europe 2012. OECD
Publishing. doi: Osborne, J. W. (2001). Testing stereotype threat: Does
anxiety explain race and sex differences in achievement?
OECD. (2012b). Health at a Glance: Asia/Pacific 2012.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 262(3), 291–310.
OECD Publishing.
doi:10.1006/ceps.2000.1052 PMID:11414722
Oelkers, J., & Prior, H. (1982). Soziales Lernen in der
Oskam, J., Kingma, J., & Klasen, H. J. (2000). Clinicians’
Schule. Koeningstein.
recognition of 10 different types of distal radial fractures.
Okimoto, T. G., & Brescoll, V. L. (2010). The price Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91(3), 917–924. doi:10.2466/
of power: Power seeking and backlash against female pms.2000.91.3.917 PMID:11153870
politicians. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
Oswald, D. L., & Harvey, R. D. (2001). Hostile environ-
letin, 36(7), 923–936. doi:10.1177/0146167210371949
ments, stereotype threat, and math performance among un-
PMID:20519573
dergraduate women. Current Psychology (New Brunswick,
Olsen, J. M., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). Attitudes and attitude N.J.), 19(4), 338–356. doi:10.1007/s12144-000-1025-5
change. Annual Review of Psychology, 44(1), 117–154.
O’Toole, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Three chro-
doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.001001
nometric indices of relational responding as predictors of
Olson, M. A., & Kendrick, R. V. (2008). Origins of at- performance on a brief intelligence test: The importance
titudes. Attitudes and Persuasion, 111-130. of relational flexibility. The Psychological Record, 59,
119–132.
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the
influence of extra-personal associations on the Im- Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old
plicit Association Test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal infants show increased helping following priming with
of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(5), 653–667. affiliation. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1189–1193.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.653 PMID:15161392 doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02419.x PMID:19674388

Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2009). Implicit and explicit Ozolins, I., Hall, H., & Peterson, R. (2008). The stu-
measures of attitudes: The perspective of the MODE dent voice: Recognising the hidden and informal cur-
model. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), riculum in medicine. Medical Teacher, 30(6), 606–611.
Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. doi:10.1080/01421590801949933 PMID:18608968
19–63). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Onatra, A., & Pena, M. (2004). The hidden curriculum.


Profile, 158-161.

343
Compilation of References

Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational Pea, R. (1995). Practices of distributed intelligence and
and experiential information processing styles to personal- designs for education. In Distributed cognitions: Psy-
ity, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal chological and educational considerations (pp. 47–87).
of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 972–987. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.972 PMID:10402681
Pedregon, C. A., Farley, R. L., Davis, A., Wood, J. M.,
Pantos, A. J., & Perkins, A. (2013). Measuring implicit & Clark, R. D. (2012). Social desirability, personal-
and explicit attitudes toward foreign accented speech. ity questionnaires, and the “better than average” effect.
Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 32(1), 1, Personality and Individual Differences, 52(2), 213–217.
3–20. doi:10.1177/0261927X12463005 doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.022

Papert, S. (1995). Mindstorms: Children, Computers, and Peetz, J., Wilson, A. E., & Strahan, E. J. (2009). So far
Powerful Ideas. Basic Books. away: The role of subjective temporal distance to future
goals in motivation and behavior. Social Cognition, 27(4),
Papies, E. K., Stroebe, W., & Aarts, H. (2008). The
475–495. doi:10.1521/soco.2009.27.4.475
allure of forbidden food: On the role of attention in self-
regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Jones, J. T. (2002).
44(5), 1283–1292. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.008 Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: Implicit ego-
tism and major life decisions. Journal of Personality and
Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C., & Schultz, R. T. (2004).
Social Psychology, 82(4), 469–487. doi:10.1037/0022-
Subcortical discrimination of unperceived objects during
3514.82.4.469 PMID:11999918
binocular rivalry. Neuron, 42(1), 163–172. doi:10.1016/
S0896-6273(04)00155-2 PMID:15066273 Pellas, N. (2012). A conceptual “cybernetic” methodol-
ogy for organizing and managing the e-learning process
Pastuović, N. (1999). Educology. Zagreb: Znamen.
through [D-] CIVEs: The case of “Second Life”. In
Payne, B. K., & Cameron, C. D. (2010). Divided minds, P. Renna (Ed.), Production and manufacturing system
divided morals: How implicit social cognition underpins management: Coordination approaches and multi-site
and undermines our sense of social justice. In B. K. Payne Planning. Hershey, PA: IGI Global.
& B. Gawronski (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social
Pellas, N., & Kazanidis, I. (2013). On the value of Sec-
Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications (pp.
ond Life for students’ engagement in hybrid and online
445–462). New York: Guildford Press.
university-level courses: A comparative study from
Payne, B. K., Cheng, S. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. Higher education in Greece. Education and Information
D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution Technologies. doi:10.1007/s10639-013-9294-4
as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and
Pellas, N., & Kazanidis, I. (2013). The impact of com-
Social Psychology, 89(3), 277–293. doi:10.1037/0022-
puter self-efficacy, situational interest and academic
3514.89.3.277 PMID:16248714
self-concept in virtual communities of inquiry during the
Payne, B. K., McClernon, F. J., & Dobbins, I. G. (2007). distance learning procedures through Second Life. World
Automatic affective responses to smoking cues. Experi- Wide Web Journal.
mental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 15(4), 400–
Pemberton, M., & Sedikides, C. (2001). When do individu-
409. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.15.4.400 PMID:17696687
als help close others improve? The role of information
Peachy. (2007). MUVE history. Retrieved from 20 August diagnosticity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
2012 from http://www.open.ac.uk/wikis/muve_teach- ogy, 81(2), 234–246. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.234
ing_and_learning/MUVE_history PMID:11519929

344
Compilation of References

Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. J. (2001). Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication
Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC): LIWC2001. and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to At-
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. titude Change. New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-
1-4612-4964-1
Perkins, A. W., & Forehand, M. R. (2010a). Implicit social
cognition and indirect measures in consumer behavior. In Petty, R. E., Fazio, R. H., & Briñol, P. (2009). The new
B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit implicit measures: An overview. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio,
Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications & P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit
(pp. 535–547). New York: Guilford. measures (pp. 3–18). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Perring, W. J. (1996). Implicit learning. In J. Hoffmann Petz, B. (1992). Psyhological dictionary. Zagreb: Pros-
& W. Kintsch (Eds.), Learning (pp. 37–56). Gottingen: vjeta.
Hogrefe.
Phelan, J. E., & Rudman, L. A. (2010). Prejudice toward
Perruchet, P. (2008). Implicit learning. In H. L. Roediger female leaders: Backlash effects and women’s impression
(Ed.), Learning and memory: Cognitive psychology of management dilemma. Social and Personality Psychol-
memory (vol. 2, pp. 597-621). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. ogy Compass, 4(10), 807–820. doi:10.1111/j.1751-
9004.2010.00306.x
Perugini, M., Richetin, J., & Zogmaister, C. (2010).
Prediction of behavior. In B. Gawronski & B.K. Payne Pink, D. (2006). A Whole New Mind: Why Right-Brainers
(Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measure- Will Rule the Future. New York, NY: Penguin Group.
ment, Theory, and Applications (pp. 256-278). New York:
Platon. (2004). The Republic. Zagreb: Naklada Jurčić.
Guilford Press.
Plaud, J. J. (1995). The generalized expectancy bias: An
Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of implicit and
explanatory enigma. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
explicit attitudes. The British Journal of Social Psychol-
18(02), 311–312. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00038644
ogy, 44(1), 29–45. doi:10.1348/014466604X23491
PMID:15901390 Pokrajac-Bulian, A., & Tatalović, S. (2001). A multidimen-
sional approach to measuring of sex and age differences
Perugini, M., O’Gorman, R., & Prestwich, A. (2007). An
in empathy. Psihologijske Teme, 10, 77–88.
ontological test of the IAT: Self-activation can increase
predictive validity. Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 134– Poldga, H., Valyataga, T., Leinomen, T., Ellonen, A.,
147. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.2.134 PMID:17472097 & Priha, M. (2006). Progress inquiry learning object
templates (PILOT). International Journal of E-Learning,
Peters, E., Hess, T. M., Vastfjall, D., & Auman, C. (2007).
5(1), 103-111.
Adult age differences in dual information processes:
Implications for the role of affective and deliberative Porges, S. W. (2007). The Polyvagal perspective. Biologi-
processes in older adults’ decision making. Perspectives cal Psychology, 74(2), 116–143. doi:10.1016/j.biopsy-
on Psychological Science, 2(1), 1-23. Retrieved from cho.2006.06.009 PMID:17049418
http://130.154.3.8/content/dam/rand/www/external/
labor/aging/rsi/rsi_papers/2008/peters1.pdf Poropat, A. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor
model of personality and academic performance. Psycho-
Peters, G.-J. Y., Ruiter, R. a. C., & Kok, G. (2013). logical Bulletin, 135(2), 322–338. doi:10.1037/a0014996
Threatening communication: A critical re-analysis and PMID:19254083
a revised meta-analytic test of fear appeal theory. Health
Psychology Review, 7, S8-S31. doi:10.1080/17437199. Pothos, E. M. (2007). Theories of artificial grammar
2012.703527 learning. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 227–244.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.227 PMID:17338598
Petrović Sočo, B. (2009). Changing context and educa-
tional practice in kindergartens. Zagreb: Mali profesor.

345
Compilation of References

Praamstra, P., & Seiss, E. (2005). The neurophysiol- Rahikainen, M., Lallimo, J., & Hakkarainen, K. (2001).
ogy of response competition: Motor cortex activation Progressive inquiry in CSILE environment: teacher
and inhibition following subliminal response priming. guidance and students’ engagement. In P. Dillenbourg,
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 483–493. A. Eurelings, & K. Hakkarainen (Eds.), European
doi:10.1162/0898929053279513 PMID:15814007 Perspectives on Computer-Supported Collaborative
Learning:Proceedings of the First European Conference
Preece, J. (2000). Online communities: Designing usabil-
on CSCL (pp. 520-528). Maastricht, The Netherlands:
ity, supporting sociability. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Maastricht McLuhan Institute.
Pretz, J. E., Totz, K. S., & Kaufman, S. B. (2010). The
Rahikainen, M., Jarvela, S., & Salovaara, H. (2000).
effects of mood, cognitive style, and cognitive ability on
Motivational Processes in CSILE-Based Learning. In
implicit learning. Learning and Individual Differences,
FishmanB.O’Connor-DivelbissS. (Eds.), Fourth Interna-
20(3), 215–219. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.003
tional Conference of the Learning Sciences (pp. 50-61).
Prokhorov, A. V., De Moor, C., Pallonent, U. E., Hud- Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
mon, K. S., Koehly, L., & Hu, S. (2000). Validation of
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers,
the modified Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire with
W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default
salivary cotinine among adolescents. Addictive Behaviors,
mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Acad-
25(3), 429–433. doi:10.1016/S0306-4603(98)00132-4
emy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(2),
PMID:10890296
676–682. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 PMID:11209064
Prosser, J. (Ed.). (1999). School culture. London: Paul
Ramachandran, V. S., & Anstis, S. M. (1983). Extrapola-
Chapman Publishing.
tion of motion path in human visual perception. Vision Re-
Puntambekar, S. (2006). Analyzing collaborative interac- search, 23(1), 83–85. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(83)90044-5
tions: Divergence, shared understanding and construction PMID:6868383
of knowledge. Computers & Education, 47(3), 332–351.
Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1988). A retrieval theory of
doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2004.10.012
priming in memory. Psychological Review, 95(3), 385–
Puri, M., & Robinson, D. T. (2007). Optimism and eco- 408. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.3.385 PMID:3406246
nomic choice. Journal of Financial Economics, 86(1),
Readon, K. K. (1998). Interpersonal communication.
71–99. doi:10.1016/j.jfineco.2006.09.003
Zagreb: Alinea.
Quillian, M. R. (1968). Semantic Memory. In M. Minsky
Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial gram-
(Ed.), Semantic information processing (pp. 216–260).
mar. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
6(6), 855–863. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(67)80149-X
Raab, M., & Johnson, J. (2007). Implicit Learning as a
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge.
Means to Intuitive Decision Making in Sports. Retrieved
Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 118(3),
23 October 2013 from http://www.cs.uu.nl/docs/vakken/
219–235. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.118.3.219
mpap/xtra/Sanza.pdfhttp://www.users.muohio.edu/john-
sojg/lab/papers/RJ_Ch08.pdf Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge:
An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York, NY:
Radin, F. (2001). Values and unacceptible values. In V.
Oxford University Press.
Ilisin, A. Marinovic-Bobinac, & F. Radin (Eds.), Children
and media (pp. 79–90). Zagreb: IDIZ.

346
Compilation of References

Reber, A. S., & Allen, R. (2000). Individual differences Ridgeway, C. L. (2001). Gender, status, and leader-
in implicit learning. In R. G. Kunzendorf & B. Wallace ship. The Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 637–655.
(Eds.), Individual differences in conscious experience (pp. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00233
227–247). Philadelphia: John Benjamins. doi:10.1075/
Ridgeway, I., Roche, B., Gavin, A., & Ruiz, M. R. (2010).
aicr.20.11reb
Establishing and eliminating IAT effects in the laboratory:
Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., & Hernstadt, R. (1991). Extending a behavioral model of the Implicit Association
Implicit learning: Individual differences and IQ. Journal Test. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11, 133–150.
of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cog-
Rifkin, J. (1986). Entropy - A new world view. Zagreb:
nition, 17(5), 888–896. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.17.5.888
Naprijed.
PMID:1834770
Rinaldi, C. (2002). Infant-Todler Centers and Preschools
Reese, H. W. (1968). The perception of stimulus relations:
as Places of Culture. In C. Giudici, C. Rinaldi, & M.
Discrimination learning and transposition. New York:
Krechevsky (Eds.), Making learning visible: Children as
Academic Press.
individual and group learners (pp. 36–43). Reggio Emilia,
Rehfeldt, R. A., Dillen, J. E., Ziomek, M. M., & Kowalchuk, Italy: Reggio Children. Project Zero & Reggio Children.
R. K. (2007). Assessing Relational Learning Deficits in
Riskind, J. H., & Gotay, C. C. (1982). Physical posture:
Perspective-Taking in Children with High-Functioning
Could it have regulatory or feedback effects on motivation
Autism Spectrum Disorder. The Psychological Record,
and emotion? Motivation and Emotion, 6(3), 273–298.
57, 23.
doi:10.1007/BF00992249
Reich, R. R., Goldman, M. S., & Noll, J. (2004). Using
Robinson, M. D., Meier, B. P., Zetocha, K. J., & McCaul,
the false memory paradigm to test two key elements of
K. D. (2005). Smoking and the Implicit Association Test:
alcohol expectancy theory. Experimental and Clinical
When the contrast category determines the theoretical
Psychopharmacology, 12(2), 102–110. doi:10.1037/1064-
conclusions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27(3),
1297.12.2.102 PMID:15122954
201–212. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2703_2
Reid, S. A., & Giles, H. (2005). Intergroup relations:
Robinson, P. (2005a). Cognitive complexity and task
Its linguistic and communicative parameters. Group
sequencing: A review of studies in a Componential
Processes & Intergroup Relations, 8(3), 211–214.
Framework for second language task design. International
doi:10.1177/1368430205053938
Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching,
Resnick, M. (1996b). Towards a practice of constructional 43(1), 1–32. doi:10.1515/iral.2005.43.1.1
design. In L. Schauble & R. Glaser (Eds.), Innovations
Robinson, P. (2005b). Cognitive abilities, chunk-strength
n learning: New environments for education. Mahwah,
and frequency effects in implicit artificial grammar and
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
incidental L2 learning: Replications of Reber, Walkenfeld,
Reuss, H., Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., & Hommel, B. (2011). and Hernstadt (1991) and Knowlton and Squire (1996) and
Unconscious activation of task sets. Consciousness their relevance for SLA. Studies in Second Language Ac-
and Cognition, 20(3), 556–567. doi:10.1016/j.con- quisition, 15, 413–438. doi:10.1017/S0272263100012365
cog.2011.02.014 PMID:21396830
Robinson, P. (2007). Aptitudes, abilities, contexts, and
Richetin, J., Perugini, M., Adjali, I., & Hurling, R. (2007). practice. In R. M. DeKeyser (Ed.), Practice in a second
The moderator role of intuitive versus deliberative deci- language: Perspectives from applied linguistics and cog-
sion making for the predictive validity of implicit and nitive psychology (pp. 256–286). New York: Cambridge
explicit measures. European Journal of Personality, 21(4), University Press.
529–546. doi:10.1002/per.625

347
Compilation of References

Robinson, P. (2010). Implicit artificial grammar learning Rohan, M. J. (2000). Rose by any name? The values
and incidental natural second language acquisition: How construct. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
comparable are they? Language Learning, 60(supplement 4(3), 255–277. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0403_4
2), 245–263. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00608.x
Rohner, J. C., & Björklund, F. (2006). Do self-presentation
Roche, B., & Barnes, D. (1996). Arbitrarily applicable concerns moderate the relationship between implicit and
relational responding and sexual categorization: A critical explicit homonegativity measures? Scandinavian Journal
test of the derived difference relation. The Psychological of Psychology, 47(5), 379–385. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
Record, 46, 451–475. 9450.2006.00522.x PMID:16987207

Roche, B., Barnes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (1997). Incon- Rohr, M., Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2012). Masked
gruous stimulus pairing and conditional discrimination emotional priming beyond global valence activations.
training: Effects on relational responding. Journal of Cognition and Emotion, 26(2), 224–244. doi:10.1080/0
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68(2), 143–160. 2699931.2011.576852 PMID:21970398
doi:10.1901/jeab.1997.68-143 PMID:16812852
Roid, G. L., & Pomplun, M. (2005). Interpreting the
Roche, B., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Stew- Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales. In D. P. Flanagan &
art, I., & O’Hora, D. (2002). Relational Frame Theory: P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assess-
A new paradigm for the analysis of social behavior. The ment: Theories, tests, and issues (5th ed., pp. 325–343).
Behavior Analyst, 25, 75–91. PMID:22478379 New York: The Guilford Press.

Roche, B., O’Reilly, A., Gavin, A., Ruiz, M., & Arancibia, Rooke, S. E., Hine, D. W., & Thorsteinsson, E. B. (2008).
G. (2012). Using behavior-analytic implicit tests to assess Implicit cognition and substance use: A meta-analysis.
sexual interests among normal and sex-offender popula- Addictive Behaviors, 33(10), 1314–1328. doi:10.1016/j.
tions. Socioaffective Neuroscience and Psychology, 2(0), addbeh.2008.06.009 PMID:18640788
17335. doi:10.3402/snp.v2i0.17335 PMID:24693346
Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). It feels like yesterday:
Roche, B., O’Riordan, M., Ruiz, M., & Hand, K. (2005). Self-esteem, valence of personal past experiences, and
A relational frame approach to the psychological assess- judgments of subjective distance. Personality Processes
ment of sex offenders. In M. Taylor & E. Quayle (Eds.), and Individual Differences, 82, 792–803. PMID:12003478
Viewing Child Pornography on the Internet: Understand-
Rothermund, K., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Gast, A., &
ing the Offence, managing the Offender, and Helping the
Wentura, D. (2009). Minimizing the influence of recod-
Victims (pp. 109–125). Dorset: Russell House Publishing.
ing in the IAT: The Recoding-Free Implicit Association
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit Test (IAT-RF). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy-
memory in normal human subjects. In F. Boller & J. Graf- chology, 62, 84–98. doi:10.1080/17470210701822975
man (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (pp. 63–131). PMID:18609400
Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (1998). Ein fairer Test
Rogers, J. (2000). Communities of Practice: A framework für die Aktivationsausbreitungshypothese: Untersuchung
for fostering coherence in virtual learning communities. affektiver Kongruenzeffekte in der Stroop Aufgabe.
Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 3(3), Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 45, 120–135.
384–392. PMID:9677859

Rogoff, B., Radziszewska, B., & Masiello, T. (1995). Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying
Analysis of developmental processes in socio-cultural processes in the Implicit Association Test: Dissociating
activity. In L. Martin & K. Nelson (Eds.), Sociocultural salience from associations. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Theory and Practice of Doing and Knowing Psychology. General, 133(2), 139–165. doi:10.1037/0096-
(pp. 125–149). Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/ 3445.133.2.139 PMID:15149248
CBO9780511896828.008

348
Compilation of References

Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & De Houwer, J. (2005). Rutman, A. M., Clapp, W. C., Chadick, J. Z., & Gazzaley,
Retrieval of incidental stimulus-response associations as a A. (2009). Early top-down control of visual process-
source of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psy- ing predicts working memory performance. Journal of
chology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 482– Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1224–1234. doi:10.1162/
495. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.3.482 PMID:15910132 jocn.2009.21257 PMID:19413473

Rovai, P. (2001). Building Classroom Community at a Ruys, K. I., & Stapel, D. A. (2008). Emotion elicitor
Distance: A Case Study. Educational Technology Research or emotion messenger? Subliminal priming reveals
and Development Journal, 49(4), 35–50. two faces of facial expressions. Psychological Science,
19(6), 593–600. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02128.x
Rubin, D. L., & Smith, K. A. (1990). Effects of accent,
PMID:18578850
ethnicity, and lecture topic on undergraduates’ percep-
tions of nonnative English-speaking teaching assistants. Ryan, E. B. (1982). Prolegomena for developing a so-
International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 14(3), cial psychological theory of language attitudes. In E.
337–353. doi:10.1016/0147-1767(90)90019-S B. Ryan & H. Giles (Eds.), Attitudes Toward Language
Variation: Social and Applied Contexts (pp. 208–223).
Rudman, L. A. (2004). Sources of implicit attitudes. Cur-
London: Arnold.
rent Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 79–82.
doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00279.x Ryan, E. B. (1983). Social Psychological Mechanisms
Underlying Native Speaker Evaluations of Non-Native
Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2008). The social psychol-
Speech. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 5(2),
ogy of gender: How power and intimacy shape gender
148–159. doi:10.1017/S0272263100004824
relations. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Ryan, E. B., Carranza, M. A., & Moffie, R. W. (1977).
Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., & McGhee, D. E.
Reactions toward varying degrees of accentedness in
(2001). Implicit self-concept and evaluative implicit
the speech of Spanish-English bilinguals. Language and
gender stereotypes: Self and ingroup share desirable
Speech, 20, 267–273. PMID:613187
traits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(9),
1164–1178. doi:10.1177/0146167201279009 Rydell, R. J., Sherman, S. J., Boucher, K. L., & Macy, J. T.
(2012). The role of motivational and persuasive message
Rudman, L. A., & Kilianski, S. E. (2000). Implicit and
factors in changing implicit attitudes toward smoking.
explicit attitudes toward female authority. Personality
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 34(1), 1–7. doi:1
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(11), 1315–1328.
0.1080/01973533.2011.637847 PMID:23606782
doi:10.1177/0146167200263001
Ryle, G. (1949). The Concept of Mind. Chicago, IL: Uni-
Rudman, L. A., & Phelan, J. E. (2010). The effect of prim-
versity of Chicago Press. Retrieved from https://archive.
ing gender roles on women’s implicit gender beliefs and
org/details/conceptofmind032022mbp
career aspirations. Social Psychology, 41(3), 192–202.
doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000027 Sagiv, L., & Schwartz, S. H. (2000). Value pri-
orities and subjective wellheing: Direct relations
Rudman, L. A., Phelan, J. E., & Heppen, J. B. (2007).
and congruity effects. European Journal of Social
Developmental sources of implicit attitudes. Personality
Psychology, 30(2), 177–198. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(12), 1700–1713.
0992(200003/04)30:2<177::AID-EJSP982>3.0.CO;2-Z
doi:10.1177/0146167207307487 PMID:18000104
Sanitioso, R. B., & Wlodarski, R. (2004). In search of
Ruiter, R. A. C., & Kok, G. (2005). Saying is not (always)
information that confirms a desired self perception:
doing: Cigarette warning labels are useless. European
Motivated processing of social feedback and choice of
Journal of Public Health, 15, 329–330. doi:10.1093/
social interactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
eurpub/cki095 PMID:15985460
letin, 30(4), 412–422. doi:10.1177/0146167203261882
PMID:15070471

349
Compilation of References

Sanitioso, R., Kunda, Z., & Fong, G. T. (1990). Motivated Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2002). Motor activation
recruitment of autobiographical memories. Journal of with and without inhibition: Evidence for a threshold
Personality and Social Psychology, 59(2), 229–241. mechanism in motor control. Perception & Psycho-
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.229 PMID:2213492 physics, 64(1), 148–162. doi:10.3758/BF03194564
PMID:11916298
Santos, A. (2011). Learning in Virtual Worlds: A Situated
Perspective. In G. Vincenti & J. Braman (Eds.), Teaching Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2006). Active masks and
through Multi-User Virtual Environments: Applying Dy- active inhibition: A comment on Lleras and Enns (2004)
namic Elements to the Modern Classroom (pp. 120–138). and on Verleger, Jaśkowski, Aydemir, van der Lubbe, and
Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference. Groen (2004). Journal of Experimental Psychology. Gen-
eral, 135(3), 484–494. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.484
Sartori, G., Agosta, S., Zogmaister, C., Ferrara, S., &
PMID:16846277
Castiello, U. (2008). How to accurately assess autobio-
graphical events. Psychological Science, 19(8), 772–780. Schlaghecken, F., Klapp, S. T., & Maylor, E. A. (2009). Ei-
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02156.x PMID:18816284 ther or neither, but not both: Locating the effects of masked
primes. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 276(1656),
Savine, A. C., Beck, S. M., Edwards, B. G., Chiew,
515–521. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0933 PMID:18945665
K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Enhancement of cog-
nitive control by approach and avoidance motiva- Schlaghecken, F., Rowley, L., Sembi, S., Simmons, R.,
tional states. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 338–356. & Whitcomb, D. (2007). The negative compatibility
doi:10.1080/02699930903381564 PMID:20390042 effect: A case for self-inhibition. Advances in Cognitive
Psychology, 3(1), 227–240. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-
Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1994). Computer
0027-y PMID:20517511
support for knowledge-building communities. Journal
of the Learning Sciences, 3(3), 265–283. doi:10.1207/ Schmader, T., Forbes, C. E., Zhang, S., & Mendes, W.
s15327809jls0303_3 B. (2009). A metacognitive perspective on the cogni-
tive deficits experienced in intellectually threatening
Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1999). Schools as
environments. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
knowledge building organizations. In D. Keating & C.
letin, 35(5), 584–596. doi:10.1177/0146167208330450
Hertzman (Eds.), Today’s children, tomorrow’s society:
PMID:19228598
The developmental health and wealth of nations (pp.
274–289). New York: Guilford. Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence
that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity.
Schacter, D. L. (1992). Implicit knowledge: New perspec-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 440–
tives on unconscious processes. Proceedings of the Na-
452. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440 PMID:14498781
tional Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
89(23), 11113–11117. doi:10.1073/pnas.89.23.11113 Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An in-
PMID:1454787 tegrated process model of stereotype threat effects on
performance. Psychological Review, 115(2), 336–356.
Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (Eds.). (1994). Memory
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336 PMID:18426293
systems 1994. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Schmidt, F., Haberkamp, A., & Schmidt, T. (2011). Dos
Schlaghecken, F., Bowman, H., & Eimer, M. (2006). Disso-
and don’ts in response priming research. Advances in
ciating local and global levels of perceptuo-motor control
Cognitive Psychology, 7(-1), 120–131. doi:10.2478/
in masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
v10053-008-0092-2 PMID:22253674
Human Perception and Performance, 32(3), 618–494.
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.618 PMID:16822128

350
Compilation of References

Schmidt, T. (2002). The finger in flight: Real-time motor Schröder-Abé, M., Rudolph, A., Wiesner, A., & Schütz, A.
control by visually masked color stimuli. Psychological (2007). Self-esteem discrepancies and defensive reactions
Science, 13(2), 112–118. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00421 to social feedback. International Journal of Psychology,
PMID:11933993 42(3), 174–183. doi:10.1080/00207590601068134

Schmidt, T. (2007). Measuring unconscious cognition: Schroeder, R. (2008). Defining Virtual Worlds and Virtual
Beyond the zero-awareness criterion. Advances in Cog- Environments. Virtual Worlds Research: Past, Present &
nitive Psychology, 3(1), 275–287. doi:10.2478/v10053- Future, 1(1). Retrieved 13 August 2012 from http://www.
008-0030-3 PMID:20517514 journals.tdl.org/jvwr/article/download/294/248

Schmidt, T., Niehaus, S., & Nagel, A. (2006). Primes and Schuette, R. A., & Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitude ac-
targets in rapid chases: Tracing sequential waves of motor cessibility and motivation as determinants of biased
activation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120(5), 1005–1016. processing: A test of the MODE model. Personal-
doi:10.1037/0735-7044.120.5.1005 PMID:17014252 ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(7), 704–710.
doi:10.1177/0146167295217005
Schmidt, T., & Seydell, A. (2008). Visual attention am-
plifies response priming of pointing movements to color Schuler, D. (1996). New community networks: Wired For
targets. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(3), 443–455. Change. New York: Addison Wesley.
doi:10.3758/PP.70.3.443 PMID:18459255
Schultheiss, O. C., Pang, J. S., Torges, C. M., Wirth, M.
Schmidt, T., & Vorberg, D. (2006). Criteria for unconscious M., & Treynor, W. (2005). Perceived facial expressions
cognition: Three types of dissociation. Perception & of emotion as motivational incentives: Evidence from a
Psychophysics, 68(3), 489–504. doi:10.3758/BF03193692 differential implicit learning paradigm. Emotion (Wash-
PMID:16900839 ington, D.C.), 5(1), 41–54. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.41
PMID:15755218
Schmukle, S. C., Back, M. D., & Egloff, B. (2008). Validity
of the five-factor model for the implicit self-concept of Schwartz, S. H. (1996). Value priorities and behaviour:
personality. European Journal of Psychological Assess- applying a Theory of intergated value sisterns. Seligman,
ment, 24(4), 263–272. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.263 U.C., Olson, J.M., Zanna, M.P. (Eds). The Psyhology
of values:The Ontario Symposium (vol. 8, pp. 1-24).
Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2005). A latent state-trait
Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
analysis of implicit and explicit personality measures.
European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21(2), Schwartz, B. L., Howard, D. V., Howard, J. H., Hovagui-
100–107. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.21.2.100 mian, A., & Deutsch, S. I. (2003). Implicit learning of
visuo-spatial sequences in schizophrenia. Neuropsychol-
Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Greenwald, A. G.
ogy, 17(3), 517–533. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.517
(2008). Implicit Association Tests: A landmark for the
PMID:12959517
assessment of implicit personality self-concept. In G. J.
Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), Hand- Schwarzer, R. (2001). Social-cognitive factors in changing
book of Personality Theory and Testing (pp. 508–528). health-related behaviors. Current Directions in Psycholog-
London: Sage. ical Science, 10(2), 47–51. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00112

Schnabel, K., Banse, R., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2006). Assess- Seddon, T. (1983). The hidden curriculum: An overview.
ment of implicit personality self-concept using the Implicit Curriculum Perspectives, 3, 1–6.
Association Test (IAT): Concurrent assessment of anxious-
Sedlovskaya, A., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Eibach, R. P.,
ness and angriness. The British Journal of Social Psychol-
LaFrance, M., Romero-Canyas, R., & Camp, N. P. (2013).
ogy, 45(2), 373–396. doi:10.1348/014466605X49159
Internalizing the closet: Concealment heightens the
PMID:16762106
cognitive distinction between public and private selves.
Academic Press.

351
Compilation of References

Seeley, E. A., & Gardner, W. L. (2003). The “selfless” Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching
and self-regulation: The role of chronic other orientation you: Priming god concepts increases prosocial behavior
in averting self-regulatory depletion. Self and Identity, in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science,
2(2), 103–117. doi:10.1080/15298860309034 18(9), 803–809. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01983.x
PMID:17760777
Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and
performance: How self-stereotypes influence processing Sharkey, A. J. C., & Sharkey, N. E. (1992). Weak contextual
by inducing regulatory foci. Journal of Personality and constraints in text and word priming. Journal of Mem-
Social Psychology, 87(1), 38–56. doi:10.1037/0022- ory and Language, 31(4), 543–572. doi:10.1016/0749-
3514.87.1.38 PMID:15250791 596X(92)90028-V

Seitz, M., & Hallwachs, U. (1997). Montessori or Waldorf. Sharot, T., Korn, C. W., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). How
Zagreb: Educa. unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality.
Nature Neuroscience, 14(11), 1475–1479. doi:10.1038/
Sela, A., & Shiv, B. (2009). Unraveling priming: When
nn.2949 PMID:21983684
does the same prime activate a goal versus a trait?
The Journal of Consumer Research, 36(3), 418–433. Sheeran, P., Aarts, H., Custers, R., Rivis, A., Webb, T.
doi:10.1086/598612 L., & Cooke, R. (2005). The goal-dependent automaticity
of drinking habits. The British Journal of Social Psy-
Senge, P., Kleiner, A., Roberts, C., Ross, R., Roth, G.,
chology, 44(1), 47–63. doi:10.1348/014466604X23446
Smith, B. (2003). The dance of change. Zagreb: Mozaik
PMID:15901391
knjiga.
Sherman, S. J., Rose, J. S., Koch, K., Presson, C. C., &
Shadel, W. G., Shiffman, S., Niaura, R., Nichter, M., &
Chassin, L. (2003). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward
Abrams, D. B. (2000). Current models of nicotine de-
cigarette smoking: The effects of context and motivation.
pendence: What is known and what is needed to advance
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22(1), 13–39.
understanding of tobacco etiology among youth. Drug and
doi:10.1521/jscp.22.1.13.22766
Alcohol Dependence, 59(Suppl 1), S9–S22. doi:10.1016/
S0376-8716(99)00162-3 PMID:10773435 Sherman, W. R., & Craig, A. C. (2003). Understanding
Virtual Reality—Interface, Application, and Design.
Shang, J., Fu, Q., Dienes, Z., Shao, C., & Fu, X. (2013).
Elsevier Science.
Negative affect reduces performance in implicit sequence
learning. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e54693. doi:10.1371/journal. Shiffman, S., & Paty, J. (2006). Smoking patterns and
pone.0054693 PMID:23349953 dependence: Contrasting chippers and heavy smokers.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(3), 509–523.
Shanks, D. R. (2004). Implicit learning. In Handbook of
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.509 PMID:16866591
cognition. Academic Press.
Shiffrin, R., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and
Shanks, D. R. (2005). Implicit learning. In K. Lamberts &
automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual
R. Goldstone (Eds.), Handbook of cognition (pp. 202–220).
learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psy-
London: Sage. doi:10.4135/9781848608177.n8
chological Review, 84(2), 127–190. doi:10.1037/0033-
Shanks, D. R., & St John, M. F. (1994). Characteristics of 295X.84.2.127
dissociable human learning systems. Behavioral and Brain
Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equiva-
Sciences, 17, 367–447. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00035032
lences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14(1),
5–13. doi:10.1044/jshr.1401.05 PMID:5550631

352
Compilation of References

Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal Somerville, L. H., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley, W. M.
classes. In T. Thompson & M. E. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis (2006). Anterior cingulate cortex responds differen-
and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hill- tially to expectancy violation and social rejection. Nature
sdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates. Neuroscience, 9(8), 1007–1008. doi:10.1038/nn1728
PMID:16819523
Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior:
A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative. Somerville, L. H., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F.
(2010). Self-esteem modulates medial prefrontal corti-
Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional dis-
cal responses to evaluative social feedback. Cerebral
crimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the
Cortex, 20(12), 3005–3013. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq049
testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
PMID:20351022
of Behavior, 37(1), 5–22. doi:10.1901/jeab.1982.37-5
PMID:7057129 Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. London:
Macmillan.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New
York: Free Press. Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999).
Stereotype threat and women’s math performance.
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. Acton, MA: Copley
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 4–28.
Publishing Group. doi:10.1037/11256-000
doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.1373
Slunjski, E. (2001). Integrated kindergarten curriculum,
Spodek, B., & Saracho, O. M. (1991). Issues in Early Child-
child’s project work. Zagreb: Mali profesor.
hood Curriculum. New York: Teachers College Press.
Slunjski, E. (2003). Nine faces of one kindergarten teacher/
Spreng, R. N., & Grady, C. L. (2010). Patterns of brain
parent. Mali profesor.
activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospec-
Slunjski, E. (2006). Creation of kindergarten curriculum tion, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the
in a kindergarten – Learning organization. Mali profesor. default mode network. Journal of Cognitive Neurosci-
ence, 22(6), 1112–1123. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21282
Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2003). Children’s PMID:19580387
emergent preferences for soft drinks: Stimulus-equivalence
and transfer. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24(5), Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Hermans, D. (2009). Modu-
603–618. doi:10.1016/S0167-4870(03)00004-7 lation of automatic semantic priming by feature-specific
attention allocation. Journal of Memory and Language,
Smith, D. B., & Robie, C. (2004). The implications 61(1), 37–54. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.03.004
of impression management for personality research in
organizations. In B. Schneider & D. B. Smiths (Eds.), Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., & Eelen, P.
Personality and organizations (pp. 111–138). Hillsdale, (2007). Affective priming of non-affective semantic cat-
NJ: Erlbaum. egorization responses. Experimental Psychology, 54(1),
44–53. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.44 PMID:17341014
Smith, J. L., & White, P. H. (2002). An examination
of implicitly activated, explicitly activated, and nul- Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The medial
lified stereotypes on mathematical performance: It’s temporal lobe memory system. Science, 253(5026), 1380–
not just a woman’s issue. Sex Roles, 47(3/4), 179–191. 1386. doi:10.1126/science.1896849 PMID:1896849
doi:10.1023/A:1021051223441
Stacey, E. (2002). Learning links online: Establishing
Snow, R. E. (1994). Abilities in academic tasks. In R. J. constructivist and collaborative learning environments.
Sternberg & R. W. Wager (Eds.), Mind in context (pp. In S. McNamara & E. Stacey (Eds.), Untangling the web:
3–37). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Establishing learning links: Proceedings ASET 2002.
Retrieved 23 August 2012 from http://www.aset.org.au/
confs/2002/stacey.html

353
Compilation of References

Stahl, G. (2012). Cognizing mediating: Unpacking the Steiger, J. H. (1980b). Testing pattern hypotheses on
entanglement of artifacts with collective minds. IJCSCL, correlation matrices: Alternative statistics and some em-
7 (2). DOI:10.1007/s11412-012-9148-x pirical results. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 15(3),
335–352. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr1503_7
Stahl, G., Koschmann, T., & Suthers, D. (2006). Com-
puter-supported collaborative learning: An historical Steinkuehler, A., & Williams, D. (2006). Where every-
perspective. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook body knows your (screen) name: Online games as “third
of the learning sciences (pp. 409–426). Cambridge, UK: places”. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,
Cambridge University Press. 11(4), 885–909. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00300.x

Stahl, S. (1991). Defining the role of prior knowledge Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. New York: Cam-
and vocabulary in reading comprehension: The retiring of bridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511584152
number 41. Journal of Reading Behavior, 23(4), 487–508.
Sternberg, R. J., & Kaufman, S. B. (Eds.). (2011). The
Stanovich, K. E., & Toplak, M. E. (2012). Defining Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 442–467). Cam-
features versus incidental correlates of Type 1 and Type bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
2 processing. Mind & Society, 11(1), 3–13. doi:10.1007/ CBO9780511977244
s11299-011-0093-6
Stets, J. E., & Burke, P. J. (2000). Identity theory and
Starmans, C., & Friedman, O. (2012). The folk conception social identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63(3),
of knowledge. Cognition, 124(3), 272–283. doi:10.1016/j. 224–237. doi:10.2307/2695870
cognition.2012.05.017 PMID:22705197
Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2004). A
Steele, C. M. (1988). In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), The psychol- Functional-Analytic Model of Analogy Using the Rela-
ogy of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self tional Evaluation Procedure. The Psychological Record,
(Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). Advances in Experimental Social 54, 531.
PsychologyNew York: Academic Press.
Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Weil, T. (2009). Training
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes analogical reasoning as relational responding. Derived
shape intellectual identity and performance. The Ameri- relational responding: Applications for learners with
can Psychologist, 52(6), 613–629. doi:10.1037/0003- autism and other developmental disabilities. Oakland,
066X.52.6.613 PMID:9174398 CA: New Harbinger.

Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and Steyer, R., Schmitt, M., & Eid, M. (1999). Latent
the intellectual test performance of African Americans. state-trait theory and research in personality and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797– individual differences. European Journal of Per-
811. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 PMID:7473032 sonality, 13(5), 389–408. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-
0984(199909/10)13:5<389::AID-PER361>3.0.CO;2-A
Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the Implicit Association Test
immune to faking? Experimental Psychology, 51(3), 165– Stoll, L., & Fink, D. (2000). Changing our schools –
179. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.51.3.165 PMID:15267125 How to enhance efficiency and quality of our schools.
Zagreb: Educa.
Steffens, M. C., & Schulze König, S. (2006). Predicting
spontaneous Big-Five behaviour with Implicit Association Stone, A. (2012). Centre-surround inhibition is a general
Tests. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, aspect of famous person recognition: Evidence from
22(1), 13–20. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.22.1.13 negative semantic priming from clearly visible primes.
Memory & Cognition, 40(4), 652–662. doi:10.3758/
Steiger, J. H. (1980a). Tests for comparing elements
s13421-011-0176-y PMID:22203608
of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87(2),
245–251. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.87.2.245

354
Compilation of References

Stone, J. (2002). Battling doubt by avoiding practice: Svenson, O. (1981). Are we all less risky and more skillful
The effects of stereotype threat on self- handicapping in than our fellow drivers? Acta Psychologica, 47, 143-148.
white athletes. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
Swann, W. B. Jr, & Bosson, J. (2010). Self and identity. In
letin, 28(12), 1667–1678. doi:10.1177/014616702237648
S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook
Stone, J., Lynch, C. I., Sjomeling, M., & Darley, J. M. of Social Psychology (5th ed., pp. 589–628). New York,
(1999). Stereotype threat effects on Black and White NY: McGraw-Hill.
athletic performance. Journal of Personality and Social
Swanson, J. E., Rudman, L. A., & Greenwald, A. G.
Psychology, 77(6), 1213–1227. doi:10.1037/0022-
(2001). Using the Implicit Association Test to inves-
3514.77.6.1213
tigate attitude-behavior consistency for stigmatised
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impul- behavior. Cognition and Emotion, 15(2), 207–230.
sive determinants of social behavior. Personality and doi:10.1080/02699930125706
Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247. doi:10.1207/
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (2004). The social identity
s15327957pspr0803_1 PMID:15454347
theory of intergroup behavior. In J. T. Jost & J. Sidanius
Strijbos, J., Martens, R., & Jochems, W. (2004). Designing (Eds.), Political Psychology: Key Readings (pp. 276–293).
for interaction: Six steps to designing computer-supported New York: Psychology Press.
group-based learning. Computers & Education, 42(4),
Takyana, K. & Wilson, J. (2006). Mapping student learn-
403–424. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2003.10.004
ing through toy the collaborative inquiry process: The
Stroebe, W., Jonas, K., & Hewstone, M. R. C. (2003). progressive e-poster. CAL-Laborate, 24-32.
Sozialpsychologie: Eine Einführung. Berlin: Springer.
Tann, S. (2001). Organization of learning. In Successful
Strohecker, C. (2006). Embedded microworlds for a learning and teaching – Psychological approaches (pp.
multiuser environment. MERL TR95-07. Cambridge, MA: 161-176). Zagreb: Educa.
Mitsubishi Electric Research Laboratory.
Tatalović, S. (1997). Adolescents’ empathy in relation to
Sudec, K. (2012): Where to sit? Co-creational process some aspects of self-esteem (Graduate thesis). Depart-
of learning with inner architecture of school. Sodobna ment of Psychology, Facults of Humantities and Social
pedagogika, 1, 140-154. Sciences, University of Rijeka.

Šuklje Erjavec, I. (2012). Meaning and posabilities of us- Tatalović Vorkapić, S. & Ružić, N. (2013). Measuring
ing outdoor school space in educational process. Sodobna empathy in future preschool teachers: Implications for
pedagogika, 1, 156-174. study programme modification. International Journal of
Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 3(6).
Sumner, P. (2007). Negative and positive masked-priming:
Implications for motor inhibition. Advances in Cognitive Tatalović Vorkapić, S., & Puljić, I. (2013). Importance
Psychology, 3(1), 317–326. doi:10.2478/v10053-008- of students’ personality traits for their future work with
0033-0 PMID:20517517 preschool children. Metodički obzori, 8(2), 34-46.

Süssenbach, P., Niemeier, S., & Glock, S. (2013). Effects Tatalović Vorkapić, S. (2012). The significance of pre-
of and attention to graphic warning labels on cigarette school teacher’s personality in early childhood education:
packages. Psychology & Health, 28(10), 1192–1206. Analysis of Eysenck’s and Big Five Dimensions of person-
doi:10.1080/08870446.2013.799161 PMID:23713958 ality. International Journal of Psychology and Behavioral
Sciences, 2(2), 28–37. doi:10.5923/j.ijpbs.20120202.05
Suthers, D. (2006). Technology affordances for inter-
subjective meaning making: A research agenda for Tatalović Vorkapić, S., & Lončarić, D. (2013). Does the
CSCL. International Journal of Computer-Supported professional burnout mediate the effects of personality
Collaborative Learning, 1(3), 315–337. doi:10.1007/ traits on life satisfaction among preschool teachers? Psy-
s11412-006-9660-y chological Topics, 22(3), 431–455.

355
Compilation of References

Tatalović Vorkapić, S., Vujičić, L., & Čepić, R. (2014). Tesser, A. (1988). In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Toward a self-
Preschool, Teacher Identity. Book Chapter to be Ap- evaluation maintenance model of social behavior (Vol.
peared. In P. Breen (Ed.), Cases on Teacher Identity, 21, pp. 181–227). Advances in Experimental Social
Diversity, and Cognition in Higher Education. IGI-Global. PsychologyNew York: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/
doi:10.4018/978-1-4666-5990-2.ch002 S0065-2601(08)60227-0

Taylor, A. (2009). Linking Architecture and Education: Tesser, A., Campbell, J., & Smith, M. (1984). Friendship
Sustainable Design for Learning Environments. Albu- choice and performance: Self- evaluation maintenance in
querque, NM: University of New Mexico Press. children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
46(3), 561–574. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.561
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-
being: A social psychological perspective on mental Tesser, A., Crepaz, N., Beach, S. R. H., Cornell, D., &
health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193–210. Collins, J. C. (2000). Confluence of self-esteem regula-
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193 PMID:3283814 tion mechanisms: On integrating the self-zoo. Personal-
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(12), 1476–1489.
Taylor, S. E., Burklund, L. J., Eisenberger, N. I., Lehman,
doi:10.1177/01461672002612003
B. J., Hilmert, C. J., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). Neu-
ral bases of moderation of cortisol stress responses by Tesser, A., & Paulhus, D. (1983). The definition of self:
psychosocial resources. Journal of Personality and Private and public self-evaluation maintenance strategies.
Social Psychology, 95(1), 197–211. doi:10.1037/0022- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(4),
3514.95.1.197 PMID:18605860 672–682. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.4.672

Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., Thelwall, M. (2009). Homophily in MySpace. Journal
& McDowell, N. K. (2003). Are self-enhancing cogni- of the American Society for Information Science and
tions associated with healthy or unhealthy biological Technology, 60(2), 219–231. doi:10.1002/asi.20978
profiles? Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Thomas, A., & Storr, C. (2005). WebCT in occupational
ogy, 85(4), 605–615. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.605
therapy clinical education: Implementing and evaluating
PMID:14561115
a tool for peer learning and interaction. Occupational
Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., & Sherman, J. W. Therapy International, 12(3), 162–179. doi:10.1002/
(2010). Practical guide to Implicit Association Test and oti.3 PMID:16398204
related tasks. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.),
Thomas, K. M., Hunt, R. H., Vizueta, N., Sommer,
Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement,
T., Durston, S., Yang, Y., & Worden, M. S. (2004).
Theory, and Applications (pp. 117-139). New York:
Evidence of developmental differences in implicit se-
Guilford Press.
quence learning.An fMRI study of children and adults.
Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Klauer, K. C. (2008). ‚Automatic’ Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1339–1351.
evaluation? Strategic effects on affective priming. Journal doi:10.1162/0898929042304688 PMID:15509382
of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 1414–1417.
Thomas, R., & Macgregor, K. (2005). Online project-based
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.004
learning: How collaborative strategies and problem solv-
Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Klauer, K. C. (2013). On the ing processes impact performance. Journal of Interactive
controllability of evaluative-priming effects: Some limits Learning Research, 16, 83–170.
that are none. Cognition and Emotion, 27(4), 632–657.
Thompson, F., & Lynch, J. (2003-2004). Web-based
doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.732041 PMID:23075050
instruction: Who is inclined to resist it and why? Journal
Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., & Rothermund, of Educational Computing Research, 29(3), 375–385.
K. (2008). Minimizing method-specific variance in the doi:10.2190/3VQ2-XTRH-08QV-CAEL
IAT: The Single Block IAT. European Journal of Psycho-
logical Assessment, 24(4), 237–245. doi:10.1027/1015-
5759.24.4.237

356
Compilation of References

Thurmond, V. A. (2003). Defining interaction and Tsai, C., & Chuang, S. (2005). The correlation between
strategies to enhance interactions in Web-based courses. epistemological beliefs and preferences toward Internet-
Nurse Educator, 28(5), 237–241. doi:10.1097/00006223- based learning environments. British Journal of Educa-
200309000-00013 PMID:14506357 tional Technology, 36(1), 97–100. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
8535.2004.00442.x
Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges
and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and nonauto- Tsai, C.-W. (2011). Achieving effective learning effects
matic processes. Psychological Review, 97(2), 147–168. in the blended course: A combined approach of online
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.97.2.147 PMID:2186423 self-regulated learning and collaborative learning with
initiation. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Net-
Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory
working, 14(9), 505–510. doi:10.1089/cyber.2010.0388
priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Ex-
PMID:21288076
perimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychol-
ogy, 37(4), 571–590. doi:10.1080/14640748508400920 Tsai, C.-W., Shen, P.-D., & Tsai, M.-C. (2011). Devel-
PMID:4081101 oping an appropriate design of blended learning with
web-enabled self-regulated learning to enhance students’
Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect
learning and thoughts regarding online learning. Behav-
inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of
iour & Information Technology, 30(2), 261–271. doi:10
conflicting views. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
.1080/0144929X.2010.514359
Psychology, 54A(2), 321–343. doi:10.1080/713755969
PMID:11394050 Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Toburen, T., & Meier, B. P. (2010). Priming god-related
concepts increases anxiety and task persistence. Journal Tulving, E., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming and hu-
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(2), 127–143. man memory systems. Science, 247(4940), 301–306.
doi:10.1521/jscp.2010.29.2.127 doi:10.1126/science.2296719 PMID:2296719

Topolinski, S., & Sparenberg, P. (2012). Turning the Tung, F., & Chang, S. (2007). Exploring adolescents’
hands of time: Clockwise movements increase preference intentions regarding the online learning courses in
for novelty. Social Psychological & Personality Science, Taiwan. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 10(5), 729–730.
3(3), 308–314. doi:10.1177/1948550611419266 doi:10.1089/cpb.2007.9960 PMID:17927546

Trninić, V., Barančić, M., & Nazor, M. (2008). The five- Turhan, K., Yaris, F., & Nural, E. (2005). Does instructor
factor model of personality and aggressiveness in prisoners evaluation by students using a Web-based questionnaire
and athletes. Kinesiology, 40(2), 170–181. impact instructor performance? Advances in Health
Sciences Education: Theory and Practice, 10(1), 5–13.
Trushell, J., Byrne, K., & Simpson, R. (2012). Cheat-
doi:10.1007/s10459-004-0943-7 PMID:15912280
ing behaviors, the Internet and education undergraduate
students. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 28(2), Turner, J. C., & Giles, H. (1981). Introduction: The so-
136–145. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2011.00424.x cial psychology of intergroup behaviour. In J. C. Turner
& H. Giles (Eds.), Intergroup Behaviour. Oxford, UK:
Tsai, C. (2007). The relationship between Internet per-
Blackwell Publishers, Ltd.
ceptions and preferences towards Internet-based learning
environment. British Journal of Educational Technology, Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & McGarty,
38(1), 167–170. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8535.2006.00627.x C. (1994). Self and collective: Cognition and social con-
text. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5),
Tsai, C. (2008). The preferences toward constructivist
454–454. doi:10.1177/0146167294205002
Internet-based learning environments among university
students in Taiwan. Computers in Human Behavior, 24(1), Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory
16–31. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2006.12.002 capacity task dependent? Memory & Cognition, 29,
484–492.

357
Compilation of References

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect van Aalst, J. (2006). Rethinking the nature of online work
theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal in synchronous learning networks. British Journal of Edu-
of Risk and Uncertainty, 5(4), 297–323. doi:10.1007/ cational Technology, 37(2), 279–288. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
BF00122574 8535.2006.00557.x

Tweddell Levinsen, K. (2007). Qualifying online teachers van de Vord, R. (2010). Distance students and online
– Communicative skills and their impact on e-learning research: Promoting information literacy through media
quality. Education and Information Technologies, 12(1), literacy. The Internet and Higher Education, 13(3),
41–51. doi:10.1007/s10639-006-9025-1 170–175. doi:10.1016/j.iheduc.2010.03.001

Tyndall, I. T., Roche, B., & James, J. E. (2004). The Van Joolingen, W., De Jong, T., & Dimitrakopoulou, A.
relation between stimulus function and equivalence class (2005). Issues in computer supported inquiry learning in
formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be- science. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 23(2),
havior, 81(3), 257–266. doi:10.1901/jeab.2004.81-257 111–119. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2006.00216.x
PMID:15357509
van Vianen, A. M., & Fischer, A. H. (2002). Illuminating the
Tyndall, I. T., Roche, B., & James, J. E. (2009). The glass ceiling: The role of organizational culture preferenc-
interfering effect of emotional stimulus functions on es. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol-
stimulus equivalence class formation: Implications for ogy, 75(3), 315–337. doi:10.1348/096317902320369730
the understanding and treatment of anxiety. European
VanPatten, B. (1994). Evaluating the role of consciousness
Journal of Behavior Analysis, 10, 121–140.
in second language acquisition: Terms, linguistic features
Tynjälä, P., & Häkkinen, P. (2005). E-learning at work: and research methodology. AILA Review, 11, 27–36.
Theoretical underpinnings and pedagogical challenges.
Vasconcelos, T. (2002). “I am like this because I just
Journal of Workplace Learning, 17(5/6), 318–336.
can’t be different…” Personal and professional dimen-
doi:10.1108/13665620510606742
sions of Ana’s teaching: Some implications for teacher
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Individual dif- education. In D. Rothenberg (Ed.), Proceedings of the
ferences in working memory capacity and learning: Lilian Katz Symposium (pp. 191-199). Early Childhood
Evidence from the serial reaction time task. Memory & & Parenting (ECAP) colaborative University of Illinois,
Cognition, 33(2), 213–220. doi:10.3758/BF03195310 Urbana-Champaign.
PMID:16028576
Vasileiou, V., & Paraskeva, F. (2010). Teaching Role-
Vadillo, M. A., & Matute, H. (2009). Learning in virtual Playing Instruction in Second Life: An Exploratory Study.
environments: Some discrepancies between laboratory- Journal of Information, Information Technology, and
and Internet-based researches on associative learn- Organizations, 5, 25–50.
ing. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(2), 402–406.
Vath, N., & Schmidt, T. (2007). Tracing sequential waves
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.08.009
of rapid visuomotor activation in lateralized readiness
Valli, L., & Hawley, W. D. (2002). Designing and Imple- potentials. Neuroscience, 145(1), 197–208. doi:10.1016/j.
menting School – Based Professional Development. In neuroscience.2006.11.044 PMID:17258867
The Keys to Effective Schools –Educational Reform as
Vecchione, M., Dentale, F., Alessandri, G., & Barbaranelli,
Continuous Improvement (pp. 86-96). Corwin Press, Inc.
C. (2014). Fakability of Implicit and Explicit Measures
Vamvakoussi, X., & Vosniadou, S. (2004). Understanding of the Big Five: Research findings from organizational
the structure of the set of rational numbers: A concep- settings. International Journal of Selection and Assess-
tual change approach. Learning and Instruction, 14(5), ment, 22(2), 211–218. doi:10.1111/ijsa.12070
453–467. doi:doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.013
doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.013

358
Compilation of References

Veltkamp, M., Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2009). Un- Villatte, M., Monestès, J.-L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué,
ravelling the motivational yarn: A framework for E., & Loas, G. (2010a). Adopting the perspective of
understanding the instigation of implicitly motivated another in belief attribution: Contribution of relational
behaviour resulting from deprivation and positive affect. frame theory to the understanding of impairments in
European Review of Social Psychology, 20(1), 345–381. schizophrenia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Ex-
doi:10.1080/10463280903388665 perimental Psychiatry, 41(2), 125–134. doi:10.1016/j.
jbtep.2009.11.004 PMID:20034611
Veltkamp, M., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2011). Motivat-
ing consumer behavior by subliminal conditioning in Visveswaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1999). Meta-analysis
the absence of basic needs: Striking even while the iron of fakability estimates: Implications for personality
is cold. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(1), 49–56. measurement. Educational and Psychological Measure-
doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2010.09.011 ment, 59(2), 197–210. doi:10.1177/00131649921969802

Verhoeve, S. (2008). Peace-Teaching: Evaluating the Role Vizek Vidović, V., Rijavec, M., & Vlahović-Štetić, V., &
of Education in Post-Conflict Cambodia. Undercurrent, Miljković, D. (2003). Psychology of education. Zagreb:
5(3), 45–52. IEP.

Verleger, R., Jaśkowski, P., Aydemir, A., van der Lubbe, von Hartmann, E. (1931). Philosophy of the unconscious:
R. H. J., & Groen, M. (2004). Qualitative differences Speculative results according to the inductive method of
between conscious and nonconscious processing? On physical science. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
inverse priming induced by masked arrows. Journal of (Original work published 1868)
Experimental Psychology. General, 133(4), 494–515.
von Hippel, W., von Hippel, C., Conway, L., Preacher, K.
doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.494 PMID:15584803
J., Schooler, J. W., & Radvansky, G. A. (2005). Coping
Versace, R., & Allain, G. (2001). Negative priming in with stereotype threat: Denial as an impression manage-
a gender decision task and in a semantic categorization ment strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
task. Acta Psychologica, 108(1), 73–90. doi:10.1016/ chology, 89(1), 22–35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.22
S0001-6918(01)00041-5 PMID:11485194 PMID:16060740

Verwijmeren, T., Karremans, J. C., Stroebe, W., & Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., &
Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2011). The workings and limits of Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual
subliminal advertising: The role of habits. Journal of perception and action priming. Proceedings of the National
Consumer Psychology, 21(2), 206–213. doi:10.1016/j. Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
jcps.2010.11.004 100(10), 6275–6280. doi:10.1073/pnas.0931489100
PMID:12719543
Vianello, M., Robusto, E., & Anselmi, P. (2010). Im-
plicit conscientiousness predicts academicperformance. Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., &
Personality and Individual Differences, 48(4), 452–457. Schwarzbach, J. (2004). Invariant time course of priming
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.019 with and without awareness. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schröger,
& H. Müller (Eds.), Psychophysics beyond sensation:
Villatte, M., Monest ès, J. L., McHugh, L., Frei xa i Baqu
Laws and invariants of human cognition (pp. 271–288).
é, E., & Loas, G. (2010b). Assessing perspective taking
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
in schizophrenia using relational frame theory. The Psy-
chological Record, 60, 413–436.

359
Compilation of References

Vukasović, A. (1999). Purposes and tasks of education. In Wang, M.-J. (2010). Online collaboration and offline
Basics of contemporary pedagogy (pp. 131-148). Zagreb: interaction between students using asynchronous tools
Hrvatski pedagoško-književni zbor. in blended learning. Australasian Journal of Educational
Technology, 26, 830–846.
Vygotsky, L. (1934). Thinking and speaking. Cambridge,
MA: The MIT Press. Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1970). Amnesic
syndrome: Consolidation or retrieval? Nature, 228(5272),
Vygotsky, L. S. (1933/1966). Play and its role in the mental
628–630. doi:10.1038/228628a0 PMID:4990853
development of the child. Social Psychology, 12(6), 62–76.
Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1974). The effect of
Vygotsky, L. S. (1935). Mental development of children
prior learning on subsequent retention in amnesic patients.
during education. Moscow: Uchpedzig.
Neuropsychologia, 12(4), 419–428. doi:10.1016/0028-
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and Language. Cam- 3932(74)90072-4 PMID:4437740
bridge, MA: MIT Press. doi:10.1037/11193-000
Wasserman, J. D., & Tulsky, D. S. (2005). A history of
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978a). Mind in Society. Cambridge, intelligence assessment. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison
MA: Harvard University Press. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories,
tests, and issues (pp. 3–22). New York: Guilford Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978b). Mind in society: The develop-
ment of higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V. Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-
John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Trans. & switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic
Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. stimulus: Task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive
Psychology, 46(4), 361–413. doi:10.1016/S0010-
Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The genesis of higher mental 0285(02)00520-0 PMID:12809680
functions. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity
in Soviet Psychology. Armonk, NY: Sharpe. Waters, A. J., Carter, B. L., Robinson, J. D., Wetter, D. W.,
Lam, C. Y., & Cinciripini, P. M. (2007). Implicit attitudes
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. to smoking are associated with craving and dependence.
Vygotsky (R. W. Rieber & A. Carton, Eds.). New York: Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 91(2-3), 178–186.
Plenum Press. (Original work published 1934) doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.024 PMID:17658701
Wang, C.-M., & Reeves, T. C. (2007). Cyberspace and Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X:
online education: The influences of global cyber culture Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-
on international students. In K. Amant (Ed.), Linguistic Expanded Form. Unpublished manuscript, University of
and cultural online communication issues in the global age Iowa, Iowa City, IA.
(pp. 239–252). Hershey, PA: Information Science Refer-
ence/IGI Global. doi:10.4018/978-1-59904-213-8.ch015 Watt, A., Keenan, M., Barnes, D., & Cairns, E. (1991).
Social categorisation and stimulus equivalence. The
Wang, H. (2007). Performing a course material enhance- Psychological Record, 41, 33–50.
ment process with asynchronous interactive online system.
Computers & Education, 48(4), 567–581. doi:10.1016/j. Weil, T. M., Hayes, S. C., & Capurro, P. (2011). Estab-
compedu.2005.03.007 lishing a deictic relational repertoire in young children.
The Psychological Record, 61, 5.
Wang, K. H., Wang, T. H., Wang, W. L., & Huang, S. C.
(2006). Learning styles and formative assessment strategy: Weinert, S. (2009). Implicit and explicit models of learn-
Enhancing student achievement in Web-based learning. ing: Similarities and differences from a developmental
Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 22(3), 207–217. perspective. Linguistics, 47(2), 241–271. doi:10.1515/
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2006.00166.x LING.2009.010

360
Compilation of References

Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future White, M. J., & Li, Y. (1991). Second-language fluency
life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and person perception in China and the United States.
39(5), 806–820. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806 Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 10(2),
99–113. doi:10.1177/0261927X91102002
Weisbrod, M., Trage, J., Hill, H., Sattler, H., Maier, S., &
Kiefer, M. et al. (1999). Emotional priming in depressed Whitmarsh, S., Udden, J., Barendregt, H., & Petersson, K.
patients. German Journal of Psychiatry, 2, 19–47. M. (2013). Mindfulness reduces habitual responding based
on implicit knowledge: Evidence from artificial grammar
Weldon, G. (2010). Post-conflict teacher development:
learning. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 833–845.
Facing the past in South Africa. Journal of Moral Educa-
doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.05.007 PMID:23774456
tion, 39(3), 353–364. doi:10.1080/03057240.2010.497615
Widiger, T. A., Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2002). A
Wentura, D. (1999). Activation and inhibition of affec-
proposal for Axis II: Diagnosing personality disorders us-
tive information: Evidence for negative priming in the
ing the five-factor model. In P. T. Costa & T. A. Widiger
evaluation task. Cognition and Emotion, 13(1), 65–91.
(Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model
doi:10.1080/026999399379375
of personality (2nd ed., pp. 431–456). Washington, DC:
Wentura, D. (2000). Dissociative affective and associa- American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10423-
tive priming effects in the lexical decision task: Yes vs. 025
no responses to word targets reveal evaluative judg-
Wiers, R. W., & de Jong, P. J. (2006). Implicit and ex-
ment tendencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
plicit alcohol, smoking and drug-related cognitions and
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(2), 456–469.
emotions. In J. Z. Arlsdale (Ed.), Advances in social
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.2.456 PMID:10764106
psychology research (Vol. 005, pp. 1–35). New York,
Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2005). Repeated masked NY: Nova Science Publishers.
category primes interfere with related exemplars: New
Wiers, R. W., de Jong, P. J., Havermans, R., & Jelicic,
evidence for negative semantic priming. Journal of Ex-
M. (2004). How to change implicit drug use-related
perimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
cognitions in prevention: A transdisciplinary integration
tion, 31(1), 108–120. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.1.108
of findings from experimental psychopathology, social
PMID:15641909
cognition, memory, and experimental learning psychol-
Wentura, D., Kulfanek, M., & Greve, W. (2005). Masked ogy. Substance Use & Misuse, 39(10-12), 1625–1684.
affective priming by name letters: Evidence for a corre- doi:10.1081/JA-200033206 PMID:15587947
spondence of explicit and implicit self-esteem. Journal
Wiers, R. W., Luitgaarden, J., Wildenberg, E., & Smulders,
of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(6), 654–663.
F. T. Y. (2005). Challenging implicit and explicit alcohol-
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.02.005
related cognitions in young heavy drinkers. Addiction
Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (2003). The ‘meddling-in’ (Abingdon, England), 100(6), 806–819. doi:10.1111/
of affective information: A general model of automatic j.1360-0443.2005.01064.x PMID:15918811
evaluation effects. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.),
Wiers, R. W., & Stacy, A. W. (2006). Implicit cognition and
The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in
addiction. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
cognition and emotion (pp. 51–86). Mahwah, New Jersey:
15(6), 292–296. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00455.x
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Wiers, R. W., Wood, M. D., Darkes, J., Corbin, W. R.,
Wertsch, J. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of
Jones, B. T., & Sher, K. J. (2003). Changing expectan-
mind. Harvard University Press.
cies: Cognitive mechanisms and context effects. Al-
Wheeler, S. C., & DeMarree, K. G. (2009). Multiple coholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 27(2),
mechanisms of prime-to-behavior effect. Social and 186–197. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000051023.28893.8A
Personality Psychology Compass, 3/4(4), 566–581. PMID:12605068
doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00187.x

361
Compilation of References

Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward social comparison prin- World Health Organization. (2013). WHO report on the
ciples in social psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90(2), global tobacco epidemic, 2013: Enforcing bans on tobacco
245–271. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.90.2.245 advertising: Executive summary. WHO.

Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: Xie, Q., Gao, X., & King, R. B. (2013). Thinking styles in
People’s appraisals of their earlier and current selves. Jour- implicit and explicit learning. Learning and Individual Dif-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(4), 572–584. ferences, 23, 267–271. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.014
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.572 PMID:11316222
Yamashita, S. (2006). What Makes for Effective Online
Wilson, B. (1996). Introduction: What is a constructivist Community Building?10 Field-Tested Strategies You Can
learning environment? In B. Wilson (Ed.), Constructivist Use to Boost Student Success. Paper presented at the
learning environments (pp. 3–8). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: World Conference on E-Learning in Corporate, Govern-
Educational Technology Publications. ment, Healthcare, and Higher Education. Honolulu, HI.

Wilson, V., & Harris, M. (2004). Creating change? A Yang, H., Yang, S., & Isen, A. M. (2013). Positive af-
review of the impact of Design and Technology in schools fect improves working memory: Implications for con-
in England. Journal of Technology Education, 15, 46–65. trolled cognitive processing. Cognition and Emotion,
27(3), 474–482. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.713325
Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence
PMID:22917664
for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship
with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on atten-
Social Psychology, 72(2), 262–274. doi:10.1037/0022- tion: A review of attentional processing of emotional
3514.72.2.262 PMID:9107001 information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 3–47.
doi:10.1080/02699930903205698
Wittenbrink, B., & Schwarz, N. (2007). In B. Wittenbrink
& N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit Measures of Attitudes (pp. Yi, Y. (1990). Cognitive and affective priming effects of
1–3). New York: Guilford. the context for print advertisements. Journal of Advertis-
ing, 19(2), 40–48. doi:10.1080/00913367.1990.10673186
Wittgenstein, L. (1960). Tractatus logico-philosophicus.
London: Routledge. Yonemura, M. (1986). A teacher at work: Professional
development and the early childhood educator. New York:
Woolfolk, A. E. (1995). Educational psychology. New
Teachers College Press.
York: Allyn&Bacon.
Young, D. M., Rudman, L. A., Buettner, H. M., &
Woolhouse, L. S., & Bayne, R. (2000). Personality and the
McLean, M. C. (2013). The influence of female role
use of intuition: Individual differences in strategy and per-
models on women’s implicit science cognitions.
formance on an implicit learning task. European Journal
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(3), 283–292.
of Personality, 14(2), 157–169. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-
doi:10.1177/0361684313482109
0984(200003/04)14:2<157::AID-PER366>3.0.CO;2-L
Yovel, I., & Friedman, A. (2013). Bridging the gap between
Woolner, P. (2010). The Design of Learning Spaces:
explicit and implicit measurement of personality: The
Future Schools. London: Continuum International Pub-
questionnaire-based implicit association test. Personality
lishing Group.
and Individual Differences, 54(1), 76–80. doi:10.1016/j.
World Health Organization. (2011). WHO report on paid.2012.08.015
the global tobacco epidemic, 2011: Warning about the
dangers of tobacco. WHO.

362
Compilation of References

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Zhang, X., Ordσρez de Pablos, P., & Xu, Q. (2013). Culture
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2, Pt.2), effects on the knowledge sharing in multi-national virtual
1–27. doi:10.1037/h0025848 PMID:5667435 classes: A mixed method. Computers in Human Behavior.

Zarevski, P. (2002). Psychology of memory and learning. Zhu, J., & Weiss, L. (2005). The Wechsler Scales. In
Jastrebarsko: Naklada Slap. D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary
intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp.
Zembylas, M. (2011). Peace and Human Rights Education:
297–324). New York: The Guilford Press.
Dilemmas of Compatibility and Prospects for Moving
Forward. Prospects: Quarterly Review of Comparative Zimmerer, V. C., Cowell, P. E., & Varley, R. A. (2011).
Education, 41(4), 567–579. doi:10.1007/s11125-011- Individual behavior in learning of an artificial grammar.
9212-8 Memory & Cognition, 39(3), 491–501. doi:10.3758/
s13421-010-0039-y PMID:21264600

363
364

About the Contributors

Zheng Jin is an Associate Professor in Institute of Educational Science at Zhengzhou Normal Uni-
versity in China. He is also a collaborative researcher in the University of California at Davis since 2012.
He has been responsible for the coordination of several funded scientific research projects at National
and Ministerial level. In addition to his research into Implicit Cognition, he has published many articles,
book chapters, and conference papers on “psycholinguistics and perception” to understand Ecological
Models that link perception with actions in cognitive processing, perceptual and motor skill, and so on.

***

Albena Antonova is lecturer at Sofia University, Faculty of Economics and Business Administra-
tion in the field of IST Implementation and Application in Business and Economics. She works on
number of research implementation projects, related to knowledge management, e-learning, competence
development, and serious games. Her main research interests include knowledge management, serious
games, business and management education, service science, e-learning and TEL models and methods,
e-Business, technology entrepreneurship, open innovations, and innovations management and living
labs. She has more than 30 publications in the field.

Claudio Barbaranelli is full professor of Psychometry and researcher at the Department of Psy-
chology, Sapienza University of Rome. He is the director of the Psychometry Laboratory (http://dippsi.
psi.uniroma1.it/node/5669), where his research group is doing research on personality assessment and
measurement, implicit measurement, social-cognitive models, occupational health and stress, work safety
and security, and problem gambling. He is author of more than 100 articles in national and international
journals, 5 books, and 10 psychological tests.

Christina Bermeitinger studied Psychology at the University of Göttingen, Germany (2001-2005).


She changed to the Saarland University, Germany, where she received her Doctor degree in 2009. Since
November 2009, Christina Bermeitinger is head of the Allgemeine Psychologie Department at the
University of Hildesheim, Germany. Christina Bermeitinger is interested in cognitive psychology. Her
main research is on attention, memory, consciousness, motor preparation, spatial representations, and
emotional as well as motivational influences. Further, Christina Bermeitinger is interested in develop-
mental questions, movement sciences, and media-related topics. Christina Bermeitinger mainly uses
experimental designs and cognitive paradigms to study these questions.
About the Contributors

Leslie R. Brody is Professor of Psychology at Boston University, past Director of the BU Clini-
cal Psychology PhD program (1991 – 1996) and was the Marion Cabot Putnam Fellow at the Bunting
Institute, Radcliffe College (1994 – 1995). She specializes in how gender and culture impact emotional
functioning and how gender roles relate to mental and physical well-being. Most recently, she is working
on gender role behaviors as predictors of health in women with HIV. Her books include Gender, Emo-
tion, and the Family (Harvard U. Press, 1997) and Daughters of Kings: Growing up as a Jewish Woman
in America (Faber & Faber, 1999).

Aoife Cartwright is a John Hume scholar and doctoral student at the National University of Ireland,
Maynooth. Her research involves the construction and validation of functionally understood implicit test
methodologies based on developments in the behavior-analytic tradition, with a particular emphasis on
the development of tests for the analysis of implicit homonegative prejudice.

Caroline M. Crawford, EdD, is an Associate Professor of Instructional Technology at the University


of Houston-Clear Lake in Houston, Texas, United States of America. Dr. Crawford earned her doctoral
degree from the University of Houston in Houston, Texas, United States of America, in 1998, with
specialization areas in Instructional Technology and Curriculum Theory, and began her tenure at the
University of Houston-Clear Lake (UHCL) the same year. At this point in Dr. Crawford’s professional
career, her main areas of interest focus upon communities of learning, communities of practice, the ap-
propriate and successful integration of technologies into the learning environment, and the instructional
design endeavours that support learner engagement and fulfilling professional understanding and imple-
mentation of the subject matter; the learning environment may be envisioned as face-to-face, hybrid,
and online (virtual or text-driven, open or protected) environments.

Francesco Dentale is Post Doc researcher of Psychometry at the Department of Psychology. He


is member of the Psychometry Laboratory, where the research group is doing research on personality
assessment and measurement, implicit measurement, social-cognitive models, occupational health and
stress, work safety and security, problem gambling. He is author of many international and national
publications focused especially on the implicit measurement of psychological constructs.

Chad E. Forbes is an Assistant Professor of Psychology at the University of Delaware. With a back-
ground spanning from molecular biology to complex social processes, Dr. Forbes utilizes behavioral
methodologies as well as Electroencephalography (EEG), Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
(fMRI), lesion studies, and genetic approaches to investigate social phenomena. Specifically, he exam-
ines how priming negative stereotypes affects our perceptions as well as stigmatized individuals in our
society (e.g. minorities and women) to ironically engender situations that inadvertently reinforce the
stereotype. Dr. Forbes is currently funded by the National Science Foundation to develop a model that
outlines how and why minorities and women are more likely to leave academics and STEM (Science,
Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics) fields, respectively, as well as how these phenomena can
be reversed. He has numerous publications on these topics in a wide array of journals, including Annual
Reviews of Neuroscience, Cerebral Cortex, and the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and
was recently recognized as a “Rising Star” by the American Psychological Association.

365
About the Contributors

Sabine Glock studied Psychology at the University of Saarland. After working as junior researcher at
the Department of Social Psychology at the University of Saarland for two years, she started to work at
the University of Luxembourg. In 2010, she received her PhD from the University of Saarland. Her PhD
thesis focused on smoking and the smoking self. Since 2014, she is a member of the School of Educa-
tion at the Bergische Universität Wuppertal, Germany. She is concerned with educational psychology,
particularly with educational decision making and factors influencing educational decisions. Her main
research interest is implicit cognition, particularly implicit attitudes. Her research focuses on the role of
implicit attitudes for smoking and alcohol consumption. Thereby, she also concentrates on interventions
that can influence implicit cognition in order to decrease smoking or alcohol consumption.

Lucija Jančec, PhD Student, works in a NGO (Non-Govermental Organisation) called “Naša djeca”
(English: Our Children) in Opatija and is external teaching assistant at Faculty of Teacher Education,
University of Rijeka in following subjects: Family Pedagogy, Pedagogy of Early Education and Teach-
ing, Methodology of Early Learning and Teaching, Research and Cognitive Integrated Curriculum I,
General Pedagogy. Her general interests are working with gifted children and children with disabilities
and their parents, but also research of the hidden curriculum in education, its interrelatedness with other
fields of research through interdisciplinary studies. Her study of the hidden curriculum is an important
feature in the raising of awareness of microelements in pedagogical work be it in institutions as well
as out of institutional life. She has been actively involved within various interdisciplinary parents and
experts groups.

Gwendolyn A. Kelso is a Doctoral candidate in Clinical Psychology at Boston University. She is


interested in the influence of social and cultural factors on psychological functioning.

Jordan B. Leitner is a postdoctoral fellow at University of California, Berkeley. Dr. Leitner inves-
tigates how self-regulatory motivations and social threats influence attention, memory, and behavior.
Specifically, Dr. Leitner aims to understand how self-enhancement and self-protective motivations
influence attention and perception of negative stereotypes and social rejection. Dr. Leitner also exam-
ines the intrapersonal and interpersonal factors that influence the self-regulation of behavior. To best
understand the implicit and explicit processes that contribute to these phenomena, Dr. Leitner employs
a multi-method approach, incorporating EEG/ERP methods with cognitive and behavioral techniques.
Dr. Leitner has published his work in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, and Psychological Science.

Daisuke Nakamura is a part-time lecturer at Aoyama Gakuin University, Tokyo, Japan. He has a
PhD in literature from the department of English at Aoyama Gakuin University. His current research
interests are individual differences in implicit learning, methodological considerations of consciousness
in relation to both natural and artificial language learning, and frequency effects on construction learning.
His research has been published in International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching
and Polish Psychological Bulletin.

366
About the Contributors

Pellas Nikolaos is a PhD Candidate at the Department of Product and Systems Design Engineering,
University of the Aegean, in Greece. He holds a Master’s degree in Education. His thesis was about
the utilization of social virtual worlds in e-Education. His research interests include the use of ICT in
e-Εducation with more emphasis on contemporary instructional formats with open-source virtual worlds
as “Metaverse” skins for collaborative learning tasks. He has extensive experience in the utilization of
virtual worlds in Secondary and Higher Education, and more than 17 publications in journals, book
chapters, and international (or national) conferences.

Anthony O’Reilly was awarded his MSc in Forensic Psychology by the University of Kent in 2008
and his PhD in Psychology in 2013 from NUI Maynooth. His research has thus far primarily focused on
the experimental analysis of implicit attitudes and the development of functionally understood methodolo-
gies for detecting the histories of verbal behaviour that underpin attitudes. More generally, his research
seeks to extend the functional understanding of the processes and histories that generate and maintain
attitudes and other cultural/verbal behaviour.

Andrew J. Pantos is an Assistant Professor of Linguistics at Metropolitan State College of Denver. He


earned his PhD from Rice University in 2010 and a JD from the University of Texas in 1984. Dr. Pantos
is a sociolinguist who focuses on language attitudes and their cognitive processing, identity, discourse,
sociophonetics, forensic linguistics, and issues of language and gender and sexuality.

Ineke M. Pit ten-Cate studied experimental Psychology at the University of Utrecht, The Nether-
lands and Pedagogy at the University of Nijmegen, The Netherlands. She has worked both clinically
and academically in different international settings and has been particularly interested in the processes
that underlie psychosocial (dys)function of children with chronic illness and physical disability and
their families. She was awarded her PhD in 2004 by the University of Southampton based on her thesis,
“Family Adjustment to Disability and Chronic Illness in Children.” Her studies have focused on chil-
dren’s developmental outcomes and brain-behavior links as well as the adjustment of (grand)parents and
siblings. Since joining the university in Luxembourg in May 2011, she has been involved in teaching
and research concerning the diagnostic competence of teachers in regards to the judgment of student
achievement and associated decision making.

Bryan Roche was awarded a PhD in Psychology in 1995 from University College Cork. His research
interests are in the experimental analysis of complex human behaviour, and in particular in the analysis
of language and cognitive processes. He employs the concepts of Relational Frame Theory in the de-
velopment of implicit testing methods, the analysis of human avoidance learning and anxiety, human
sexuality, as well as in the development of teaching protocols to enhance general cognitive functioning
(RaiseYourIQ.com). He was co-editor of the first major text on relational frame theory, Relational Frame
Theory, in 2001 as well as the follow-up text, Advances in Relational Frame Theory, in 2013. He is
author of over 70 research papers and book chapters.

367
About the Contributors

Marion S. Smith, PhD, is an Associate Professor in the Department of Management Science at Texas
Southern University’s Jesse H. Jones School of Business in Houston, Texas, United States of America.
Dr. Smith’s areas of expertise include statistical analysis, programming, management and reasoning,
online, blended, and face-to-face understanding of the andragogical learning environment, storytelling
as a cognitive support tool within mathematical and statistics learning environments, and innovative
ideas revolving around the instructional integration of technological supports. Dr. Smith earned her
PhD from the University of Houston, her MBA from Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, and her BS from
Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute.

Sanja Tatalović Vorkapić, Ph.D., Assistant Professor, currently teaches students at Faculty of
Teacher Education, University of Rijeka in: Developmental psychology, Psychology of early learning and
teaching, General psychology, Emotional intelligence, Developmental psychopathology, Methodology
of quantitative research and Positive psychology. Her contemporary research interests include biological
basis of personality, personality of (pre)school teachers and child personality, contemporary issues from
developmental psychopathology and methodology of quantitative research, positive psychology (well-
being, optimism, life satisfaction, virtues) and its relationship with other fields through interdisciplinary
studies, and also study programs for (pre)school teachers. She has published numerous psychology related
articles and has been actively involved within various interdisciplinary research projects.

Michele Vecchione is researcher of “Psychometric Theory” at the department of Psychology (Sa-


pienza University of Rome). He is member of the Psychometric Laboratory, where the research group
is doing research on personality assessment and measurement, implicit measurement, social-cognitive
models, occupational health and stress, work safety and security, and problem gambling. He is the author
of several international and national publications focused on personality assessment.

Jurka Lepičnik Vodopivec is a full professor of Pedagogy at the Faculty of Education at the University
of Primorska. Her professional and scientific work focuses on teaching and researching communication,
education for a sustainable future, and media studies. Recently, she had engaged in the professional
development of preschool teachers, competences and ICT competences of preschool teachers and chil-
dren, and in hidden curriculum. She is the head of the Department for the Educational Sciences and the
Deputy Dean for the International Cooperation, Quality, and Development at the Faculty of Education
at the University of Primorska.

368
369

Index

A E
Affective 5, 16, 18, 24-26, 30, 35-39, 41-42, 59, 72, E-learning 2.0 270, 280
84, 91, 105-108, 111, 117, 136, 139, 145, 148- Emotion 17-18, 61-62, 65, 76, 118, 121, 124-129,
150, 152-154, 161, 163, 182, 188, 190-191, 131, 136-137
210, 217 Empathy 210, 223, 226-230, 234, 242
Affective Priming 24, 35-38, 41, 145, 148-150, 152- Engagement 74, 86, 90-91, 94, 97-98, 122, 253, 256,
154, 161 258, 270, 280
Affective Priming Score 149, 161 Evaluative Processing 117
Affective Reactions 106-107, 117, 139 Explicit Attitudes 104, 107, 109-113, 117, 139
Affective Simon Task 107, 145, 161 Explicit Emotion 137
Affect Misattribution Procedure 145, 161 Explicit Knowledge 73, 205, 207-208, 212, 215
anchoring 20 Explicit Self-Enhancement Mechanism 184, 204
APE (Associative Propositional Evaluation) Model Explicit Self-Enhancement Outcome 204
117 Explicit Self-Esteem 189-190, 204
Associative Processing 107, 117 Explicit Stereotype Threat 118-119, 122, 124-125,
audio IAT 107-108 127, 129, 131, 137
autobiographical memory 183, 186-187, 194 Extrinsic Affective Simon Task 107, 161
autobiographical narratives 124-125
Avatar 280 F
B Female Gender Role 137
Foreign Accent 117
Behavioral Process Model of Personality (BPMP) Functional Analysis 182
2, 15
Big Five Model 225, 242 G
Big Five Personality Factors 15
Bloom’s Taxonomy of the Cognitive Domain 86-87, gender roles 119-120, 131-132
90-92, 95-96, 98, 103 generation problem 41-42
Go 161
C
H
cognitive domain 86-87, 89-98, 103
Conditional Discrimination 165, 169, 182 Hidden Curriculum 216-225, 228-234, 242
Higher Order Thinking Skills 86, 90, 98, 103
D
Dissonance 89, 93, 97-98, 103
Index

I P
IAT (Implicit Attitudes Test) 1-2, 10, 15, 104, 107, Positive Priming Effects 28-29, 32-33, 38, 59
120, 145, 161, 163, 165, 172, 189 pre-activation 16, 18, 20, 25, 27, 30, 35-37
Implicit attitudes measures 154-155, 161 Prime 16-36, 38-43, 59-60
Implicit Coping 190, 193, 204 Priming 16-32, 34-43, 59-60, 69, 74, 113, 145, 148-
Implicit Emotion 129, 137 150, 152-154, 161, 186, 190
implicit knowledge 205-208, 212-215, 224 Psychometric Intelligence 61-62, 64-67, 74, 76-77,
Implicit Learning 61-67, 69-70, 72-74, 76-77, 84, 84
90, 216, 223-225, 228-229, 233-234, 242, 245- Public Service Announcement 151, 161
246
Implicit Measures 1-2, 4-6, 8-10, 27, 131, 139, 148, Q
154, 164, 174
Implicit Memory 18, 86-91, 97-98, 103, 113, 163 Questionnaire Based-IAT (qIAT) 15
implicit processes 73, 118, 123, 132, 186, 188-190,
193-194 R
Implicit Self-Enhancement Mechanism 184, 204 reduction problem 41
Implicit Self-Esteem 183, 186, 189-190, 193-194, Reflective–Impulsive Model (RIM) 15
204 Relational Responding 166-169, 172, 174, 176, 182
Implicit Social Cognition 104, 106, 117, 164-165 Response Priming 16, 18, 24, 26, 28-29, 31-33, 36-
Implicit Stereotype Threat 118-119, 124-125, 127, 38, 43, 59
129-131, 137
Individual Differences 5, 61, 84, 192 S
ISC (Implicit Social Cognition) 117
Scaffolding 280
K Self-Concept of Personality 1-2, 4, 8, 10, 15
Self-enhancement 183-194, 204
knowledge construction 205-206, 208, 211-212, 214 self-esteem 5, 8, 122, 163, 183-190, 193-194, 204,
Knowledge Justification 206-207, 210, 215 208, 262
Semantic Priming 16, 18, 22, 24, 26-27, 29, 31, 34-
L 37, 41, 59, 69
language attitudes 104-107, 111-113 Social Cognitive Theory 205, 210
Lay Epistemic Theory (LET) 205, 211, 215 Social Factors 205-212, 214-215, 224, 249
Lexical Decision Task 23, 26, 59 social neuroscience 183, 186-189, 192, 194
Stereotype 2, 17, 21, 23, 27, 105, 107, 112-113,
M 117-132, 137, 163, 191-193, 204, 221
Stereotype Threat 118-132, 137, 191-193, 204
motor level 39 stigma 183, 190-193
Stimulus Equivalence 164-166, 169-170, 182
N Stimulus Response Compatibility Task 145, 161
Negative Priming 16, 18, 24-25, 27-33, 35, 37-40, T
59
Negative Priming Effects 28-30, 32-33, 35, 37-38, Target 3-4, 18, 20-22, 24-36, 38-40, 59-60, 73, 76,
40, 59 109, 145, 161, 174-175, 209, 246, 268
Nonnative Accent 105, 117 task set 32
taxonomy 86-87, 90-92, 94-96, 98, 103, 266
O Temperament 227, 242
threat 24, 118-132, 137, 184, 188, 190-193, 204
Open Simulator (Open Sim) 280
Operant Conditioning 166-167, 182
Outcome expectancy 161

370
Index

U W
unconscious goal pursuit 183, 186, 194 Web 2.0 206, 214, 255, 262, 270, 280
Working Memory 61, 67, 71, 84, 191, 193
V
Virtual World 254-255, 258-260, 266, 269, 280
Vulnerability 2, 86-87, 89, 91, 93-98, 103, 129, 227

371

Anda mungkin juga menyukai