Anda di halaman 1dari 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/233677530

Good-bye, Mr. Chips: Male Teacher Shortages and Boys' Reading


Achievement

Article  in  Sex Roles · May 2007


DOI: 10.1007/s11199-007-9206-4

CITATIONS READS

39 3,434

4 authors, including:

Laura Sokal
The University of Winnipeg
44 PUBLICATIONS   320 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Consortium of Inclusive Teacher Education and Development (CITED) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Laura Sokal on 15 February 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Sex Roles (2007) 56:651–659
DOI 10.1007/s11199-007-9206-4

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Good-bye, Mr. Chips: Male Teacher Shortages and Boys’


Reading Achievement
Laura Sokal & Herb Katz & Les Chaszewski &
Cecilia Wojcik

Published online: 22 May 2007


# Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2007

Abstract One hundred seventy-five third- and fourth-grade A drop in the number of male teachers, especially in
boys who were identified as struggling readers participated elementary schools, has been interpreted as a contributor to
in a 10-week intervention to determine the effect of the boys’ under-achievement (BBC News 2005; Carrington
reading teachers’ sex on boys’ reading performance, self- and Skelton 2003; Eng 2004; Hetzner 2003; Mills et al.
perceptions as readers, and view of reading as a masculine, 2004; Ontario Public School Boards’ Association 2000;
feminine, or gender-neutral activity. Findings indicated no Sax 2005; Tinklin et al. 2001). This interpretation is
main effects on reading performance, yet main effects of supported by recent research by Skelton (2003), who
teacher’s sex on two sub-scales of reader self-perception surveyed 118 male and 92 female pre-service teachers
emerged. Evidence of interactions showed that boys across 20 institutions in England and found that 88% of
responded differentially to the intervention—they responded men and 73% of women surveyed believed that more male
better to female teachers. Further analysis revealed that the teachers are needed as role models in primary schools.
boys’ responses varied as much as a response to individual Eighty-eight percent of men and 84% of women believe
male teachers as to the teacher’s sex. that male teachers have a crucial role to play in encouraging
boys’ positive attitudes toward studying. These beliefs in
Keywords Reading . Male teachers . Gender the need for male models have been used to shape
interventions aimed at addressing boys’ reading needs. So
Competent reading is the strongest predictor of school success compelling is the perceived need for more male teachers
(Adams 1990; Hoffert and Sandberg 2001), therefore it is that human rights legislation in Australia was recently
important to ensure that all young children receive effective challenged in order to provide male-only scholarships in
reading instruction to become proficient readers and suc- teacher’s colleges (A good man is hard to find 2004).
cessful students. Educators are compelled to pay attention to That there has been a decrease in the representation of
the convergence of evidence from recent national and men within the teaching force is not in dispute: In Canada,
international test results that show that girls out-perform the percentage of male teachers decreased 6% over the
boys in reading and writing (Council of Ministers of 1990s—from 41% in 1989 to 35% in 1999 (Statistics
Education, Canada 2001; Gambell and Hunter 2000; Mullis Canada 2003). In Australia, the number of male teachers
et al. 2003) and to form appropriate pedagogical responses to dropped 5% from 1987 to 1997 (Lewis et al. 1999). In the
increase the achievement of boys without diminishing girls’ United States, the percentage of men teaching in elementary
achievement. schools dropped 4% between 1981 and 2005—from 18 to
L. Sokal (*) : L. Chaszewski : C. Wojcik
14% (Klecker 1997; Martin 2005). The effect of this
Faculty of Education, University of Winnipeg, 515 Portage demographic change on boys’ reading achievement is not
Avenue, Winnipeg, MB R3B 2E9, Canada clear, however, as the evidence is “limited and contradic-
e-mail: lj.sokal@uwinnipeg.ca tory” (Dee 2006, p. 7). Interventions based on the effects of
male teachers must be grounded in empirical support rather
H. Katz than on intuition (Dee 2006), therefore the purpose of the
University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada current study was to examine the causal effects of male
652 Sex Roles (2007) 56:651–659

teachers on boys’ reading achievement within a controlled gather information about gender roles from their teachers
experimental design with the goal of guiding policy alongside their observations of other sex role models. The
decisions about the strategic hiring of more male teachers. question yet to be theoretically addressed, however,
involves the relative influence of male teachers on boys’
gender development. Although most theories suggest that
Boys’ Experiences of Literacy as Feminine same-sex models are important, few researchers have
explored teacher effects in isolation. Gender schema
Research suggests that one reason for some boys’ lack of theories have not yet addressed the relative influence of
engagement with reading is their perception that it is a gender models within various contexts or developmental
feminine activity (Baron 1996; Brophy 1985; Cummings stages, thus they provide little theoretical contribution to the
1994; Government of the UK 2000; Hermine 1998; male teacher debate.
McKenna 1997; Pottorff et al. 1996). Indeed, Katz and Numerous large-scale and international studies, however,
Sokal (2003) showed that by second grade, 24% of the have demonstrated that male students do not perform
Canadian boys they studied viewed reading as feminine. significantly better for male teachers than they do for
But where does this perception come from, and why is it female teachers (see Allan 1993; Butler and Christianson
held by some boys and not by others? A variety of people and 2003; Carrington and Skelton 2003; Carrington et al. 2005;
factors contribute to the perception that reading is feminine, and Coulter and McNay 1993; Ehrenberg et al. 1995; Froude
children’s differential environmental experiences either support 2002; Martin 2003; Sokal et al. 2005).
or refute this perception. Parental modeling has been broadly In contrast, Dee (2006) recently found that 13-year-old
explored (see Tenenbaum and Leaper 2002) and is related to boys (and girls) performed better for same-sex teachers.
the ideas children develop about gender. In the home, the Based on the results of a large retrospective study of over
average middle-class child is read to for approximately 24,000 eighth-grade students, the author predicted that
1,000 h before beginning school (Adams 1990). In many 1 year with a male teacher of Language Arts would
cases the reader is female (Millard 1997; Pottorff et al. 1996). eliminate one-third of the 1.5 year reading gap between
When children later enter day care and school, their teachers female and male students. It should be noted, however, that
and models of reading are predominantly female (Basow Dee’s findings were generated through post-hoc analysis of
1992; Delamont 1990). The culturally influenced predomi- large databases rather than by experimental design. Fur-
nance of women in the early life of children may create the thermore, his findings were not based on random assign-
conditions for some children to perceive reading as a gender- ment: In fact, his data suggest that students are strategically
marked behaviour (Millard 1997). assigned to specific teachers by sex. For example, male
students with low achievement orientation may have been
The Importance of Male Models strategically assigned to a male teacher as a means of
remediating students’ performance.
Gender schema theories (Martin and Halverson 1981) In addition to research that demonstrates performance
predict that children’s cognitive structures develop within differences, a dated, yet carefully constructed, experimental
a reciprocal relationship with their environmental experi- design to examine effects of teachers’ sex on reading
ences. Children’s observations of same-sex role models are attitudes was conducted by Canadian researcher Dan
important to gender identity development (Golombok and Shapiro. In Shapiro’s (1980) study of second grade boys
Fivush 1994; Martin and Halverson 1981), and, as a result and girls assigned to same-sex and crossed-sex teachers,
of their interactions with same-sex models, by age 6 a 6-month intervention yielded more negative attitudes
children can reliably make predictions about same-sexed toward reading in all groups except for girls taught by
children’s behaviours. Children learn the cultural stereo- male teachers. However, attitudes toward reading in boys
types associated with both sexes, but they learn about their taught by male teachers decreased less than those of boys
own sex more quickly and elaborately. Yet, by age 8 they taught by female teachers, which suggests that boys’ ex-
can also make predictions about the behaviours of children periences with literacy should be examined with consid-
of the other sex (Martin 1993). eration to affective factors in addition to performance
Although some theories of gender development place factors.
special emphasis on the importance of same-sex parents as Furthermore, evidence suggests these affective factors
models, gender schema theory suggests that information are multi-directional: Dee (2006) found that female teachers
about gender roles permeates children’s environments. perceived male students as more disruptive and that male
Television, siblings, peers, parents, music, advertising, students were less enthusiastic about classes taught by
schooling and the like are rich sources of gendered women. This finding suggests that the differential perfor-
information. From this theoretical perspective, children mance that Dee found may be a product of reciprocal
Sex Roles (2007) 56:651–659 653

interactions between teachers’ and students’gendered sample was diverse: Seventy-six percent of the schools
behaviours and expectations. This finding is noteworthy were located in the inner city, and 24% were not. The
because, although numerous studies have been done of majority of the children’s parents (55%) self-identified as
effects of teacher sex on student achievement little research belonging to non-Canadian ethnic groups exclusively or in
has examined whether male and female teachers use combination with self-identification as Canadian (e.g.,
different teaching strategies, whether there are differences Asian-Canadians). One-third of the families self-identified
in implementation of the strategies, and/or whether there as having Aboriginal ancestry. One-third of the children’s
are differential effects of strategies on students’ achieve- mothers and one-fifth of the children’s fathers had not
ment (Sakhnandan et al. 2000). completed high school. Approximately one-fifth of the
Even though many studies fail to support the wisdom of mothers and fathers held university degrees. Approximately
hiring male teachers as a pedagogical approach to boys’ two-thirds of the families lived in poverty (incomes less
under-achievement in reading, Dee’s (2006) and Shapiro’s than $40, 000 per year), although 63% were employed
(1980) studies show that the findings are anything but either full-time (42%) or part-time (21%).
conclusive. The lack of conformity in the findings support
Connell’s (1996) claim that more complex systems are at Instruments
work and only an examination of systems of relationships
will illuminate gender development in the classroom Alberta Diagnostic Reading Program (ADRP) (Alberta
setting. The non-conformity in findings also speaks to the Education 1986)
non-conformity in boys. That is, for interventions to be
successful, they must be sensitive to the nuances of specific This informal reading inventory yields data about
school settings and specific types of boys. Interventions students’ reading performance, including instructional
aimed at all boys fail to recognize the individuality of boys’ reading level measured in terms of early-, middle-, and
reading needs and will miss the mark for many boys. late-in-year grade. The Alberta Diagnostic Reading
In the current study, we focused on boys in third or Program was chosen from among a variety of informal
fourth grade who were identified as struggling readers by reading inventories to address possible inequities be-
their teachers. We chose this group for four reasons. First, tween students’ background knowledge. Many of its
although boys and girls enter kindergarten with similar passages are based on western Canadian experiences and
performance in reading, by the spring of third grade boys include culturally relevant events such as ice skating.
have lower reading scores (Freeman 2004), which makes Participants were asked to read graded narrative and
this an opportune time for reading intervention. Second, it expository passages. As grades three and four reading is
is well-established that many children who have not learned widely viewed as a meaning-making activity, and to ensure
to read by fourth grade will experience long-term difficul- reliability in administration of a complex test by research
ties in school. Third, although we were curious about the assistants, instructional reading level was measured only by
effects of male teachers, we were also eager to provide extra responses to comprehension questions. Instructional Read-
reading assistance to 180 mainly inner-city boys who were ing Level was determined by a minimum of seven and a
not perceived as successful readers. A 10-week, one-on-one maximum of eight correct responses to ten comprehension
reading intervention that incorporates best practices was questions, comprised of four categories. Inferring questions
intended to help all of the participants to become better required the student to state information left out by the
readers. Finally, attitudinal and demographic data collected author. Analyzing questions required the student to state
before the intervention allowed us to determine differential important facts or details. Associating questions required
effects on various categories of boys within this diverse the student to provide specific meanings for words.
population. For these four reasons, third- and fourth-grade Synthesizing questions required the student to state main
struggling readers were recruited. ideas and their relationships as sequential, causal, spatial,
comparison, or contrast. This test was administered prior to
and at the end of the 10-week intervention. The final
Method application of this measure began at the last tested
instructional reading level. Pre- to post- comparisons
Participants suggested changes in reading performance.

The participants (N=180) were third-grade (n=87) and Gendered Activities Q-sort (Sokal et al. 2006)
fourth-grade (n=93) boys who attended 12 schools in
Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada (see Table 1). All boys were This measure provides children with pictures that depict
identified by their teachers as struggling readers. Our nine activities, such as playing football, watching televi-
654 Sex Roles (2007) 56:651–659

Table 1 Demographic information. girls?” Responses categories were counter balanced. The
Descriptor Frequency Percentage children’s classification of the picture that depicts reading
was used to infer their views of reading as masculine,
Grade feminine, or gender-neutral.
Grade 3 87 49
Grade 4 93 51
Readers’ Self-Perception Scale (RSPS) (Henck and Melnick
School location
1995)
Inner city 136 76
Not inner city 44 24
Language in the home Coming to view oneself as a reader is critical in a child’s suc-
English 157 92 cessful passage to becoming a proficient reader (Stanovich,
Other 13 8 1986). This scale includes 33 statements on a Likert scale,
Ethnicity which represent five aspects of reader self-efficacy (general
Canadian 77 45 perception, progress, observational comparison, social feed-
Aboriginal 23 13.5
back, and physiological state). Example statements are: “I
Aboriginal and Canadian 35 20.5
Other 14 8
think I am a good reader” (general perception, one question),
Other and Canadian 22 13 “I am getting better at reading” (progress, 9 questions), “I
Mother’s education read better than other kids in my class” (observational
Less than high school 48 29 comparison, 6 questions), “My teacher thinks that I am a
High school 55 33 good reader” (social feedback, 9 questions) and “I feel good
Trade school 26 16 inside when I read” (physiological state, 8 questions).
University 36 22 Scoring is comprised on adding the total of scores out of 5
Father’s education
for each question within each category. Repeated adminis-
Less than high school 49 30
High school 48 30 tration yields evidence of possible changes in a boy’s view of
Trade school 20 12 himself as a reader. This self-perception scale was used prior
University 32 20 to and at the conclusion of the 10-week intervention and it
Not applicable 12 7 took 15 min to administer. Piloting of the instrument
Family income indicated that children became bored with this instrument
Less than $20,000 61 39 when it was administered in its totality. Therefore, the
$20,000–$40,000 45 29
instrument was administered in portions between the admin-
$40,001–$60,000 31 20
istrations of other instruments.
Over $60,000 20 13
Number of siblings
0 29 17 Procedure
1 60 35
2 42 24 Once ethics approval had been granted and consent from
3 21 12 the parents, teachers, and administrators was given, the 180
4 8 5 children were then randomly assigned to work with either a
5 11 6
male or a female research assistant for the duration of the
6 0 0
7 1 1
intervention. Initial visits by the 14 research assistants
Employment status involved developing rapport with the children and admin-
Full time 72 42 istering the RSPS, ADRP, and the Gendered Activities Q-
Part time 36 21 sort. These same instruments were again administered at the
Unemployed 63 37 end of the study.
Number of books in the home The research assistants (RAs) who worked with the
None 3 2 children in our study were all enrolled in their third or fourth
A few 72 42
year of a 5-year Bachelor of Education program. They were
Many 96 56
selected based on references from their placement super-
visors and co-operating teachers over their 3 years of practice
teaching. All RAs also had significant experience working
sion, and reading. Children are asked to classify the pictures with children outside of the classroom settings, such as
into categories of usually done by girls, usually done by tutoring, day care work, camp counseling, and church
boys, or usually done by both boys and girls. For example, groups. The project supervisors visited the study schools
the students were asked: “Who usually plays football? on several occasions to observe the RAs working with the
Usually boys? Usually girls? Or usually both boys and boys in order to ensure the consistency of the treatment.
Sex Roles (2007) 56:651–659 655

Each week for 10 weeks, the RAs visited the Post-treatment


individual children at school to conduct 30 min of
reading. Assent was secured from the boys before each Over the course of the treatment, five boys (< 3%) moved away
session began. In all cases, the texts used for the reading from the participating schools and, therefore, discontinued
were selected based on research about books that hold participation in the study. For the other 175 boys, t-tests
high interest for boys (Worthy et al. 1999). As part of a indicated that there were significant gains in reading perfor-
larger study, some books were read from printed texts and mance over the course of the 10-week intervention, t (174)=
others were read in pdf. format from a computer screen. 5.13, p<.001, M grade equivalent gain =.32, as well as significant
Text formats were evenly distributed between the treatment gains in their general self-perceptions as readers, t (174)=2.65,
groups. As the sessions progressed, boys requested books p=.01, progress , t (174)=2.02, p=.04, and observational
about specific topics (e.g., insects) or books from a specific comparison, t (174)=4.15, p<.001.
series (e.g., Dav Pilky’s Captain Underpants). These An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted with
requests were used as guides in subsequent book purchas- reading teacher’s sex as the independent factor. The
ing. All RAs had access to the same 50 book titles during dependant variables were the changes in boys’ scores over
the study. the 10 weeks in reading performance, gendered view of
Paired Reading, the program used in all the reading reading, and the five sub-scales of reading self-perceptions.
sessions, is a reading practice approach developed by the The ANOVA indicated no significant main effects of RA’s
Northern Alberta Reading Specialists’ Council (1991) sex on reading performance or views of reading as a
based on Topping’s (1987) research. The process includes feminine activity, yet there were significant main effects of
duet reading, during which student and tutor read simulta- RA’s sex on changes in boys’ general perceptions of
neously, and solo reading during which the student reads themselves as readers, F (1, 174)=5.29, p=.02, as well as
independently. Best reading instructional practices are built their social feedback scores (how they thought that other
into the program. Program evaluation (Northern Alberta children and teachers viewed them as readers), F(1,174) =
Reading Specialists’ Council 1991) suggests that strong 18.72, p<.001. Analysis of the means of children’s general
gains in word identification and text comprehension result self-perceptions and social feedback scores showed more
from use of this approach. positive results for those boys who worked with female
At the end of the project, all of the books used in the RAs than with male RAs (see Table 2).
research, approximately $7,000 of high interest books, were In order to control for individual effects of the RAs, two
donated to the participating schools. The children who within-gender MANOVAs were conducted. For students
participated in the project made the presentation to the who worked with female RAs, results indicated no
schools, and the books are now housed in the children’s significant differences in any of the dependent variables
classrooms for use by all the class members. between children who worked with particular female RAs
(F range=.23–2.10, p>.05)
For students who worked with male RAs, results
Results indicated significant differences in changes to feminine
views of reading based on working with specific male RAs,
Pre-treatment F(6, 87)=2.47, p=.03, as well as significant differences
between male RAs on one sub-scale of reader self-
The mean reading level for all boys prior to the intervention perceptions–social feedback, F(6, 87) = 2.42, p = .03.
was grade 1.6 (performance equivalent to average perfor- Finally, children’s reading performance gains were not
mance of students who have completed a little more that half affected differentially in students who worked with
of first grade), which represents a mean grade equivalent specific men.
reading level of 1.3 for third grade boys and 1.9 for fourth
grade boys. Descriptive analyses of frequencies garnered
from the Gendered Activities Q-sort administered prior to the Discussion
intervention indicated that 10 boys (6%) viewed reading as a
masculine activity, 154 boys (86%) viewed reading as a The main questions that guided the current investigation
gender-neutral activity, and 16 boys (9%) viewed reading as were provoked by issues of boys’ under-achievement in
a feminine activity. Self-perceptions as readers indicated the reading and their assumed relationship to the relative lack
following sub-scale means and ranges, respectively: general of male teachers. The findings of the current study do not
perception (4.0, 1–5), progress (38.3, 9–45), observational support any differential effects on achievement when boys
comparison (19.8, 6–32), social feedback (35.8, 12–45), and are taught by male or female teachers, although reader self-
physiological state (32.3, 8–40). perception differences emerged. These findings together
656 Sex Roles (2007) 56:651–659

provide evidence of the very complex gendered interactions 1983; Stake and Katz 1982) and that sincere praise and
within our schools (Martino and Blye 2006; Mills et al. recognition of achievement increases children’s perceptions
2004; Sokal 2005), and they support claims that the effects of their competence at a task (Guthrie and Wigfield 2000).
of male models must be systematically examined within a Other researchers, however, have found no gender differ-
larger range of social contexts (Martino and Meyenn 2002). ences in elementary and middle school teachers’ use of
Despite the lack of findings regarding achievement, praise and criticism (Merrett and Wheldall 1992), which
other interesting and surprising results emerged. The first leaves us with little explanation for this unexpected finding.
finding occurred even before the intervention began. In our Although boys did respond better to female RAs in
sample of 180 third- and fourth-grade struggling readers, terms of the boys’ self-perceptions as readers, it is
only 9% viewed reading as a feminine activity. This small important to note that there were also significant differences
percentage was unexpected given that in an earlier study in how boys responded to individual male RAs. Boys who
24% of male second grade readers with typical reading worked with some male RAs—but not others—developed a
skills viewed reading as feminine. This finding may be a less feminized view of reading and had better perceptions
developmental artifact in that boys’ views of reading may of their social feedback about their reading skills. This
become less feminine as boys develop. Alternatively, the finding serves as a warning that children’s differential
difference may be an artifact of the characteristics of the responses to teaching interventions can be attributed to
study group: Fewer boys who struggle with reading may many teacher-specific and context-specific factors besides
view reading as feminine than do boys who are typical teacher sex. Gender-related observations must be examined
readers. This latter explanation seems counter-intuitive— within the context of the very complex gendered inter-
boys who struggle may seek explanations that protect their actions within our schools (Martino and Blye 2006; Mills
egos, as categorizing reading as feminine would most et al. 2004; Martino and Meyenn 2002; Sokal 2005).
certainly do. In either case, more research—specifically Given our evolving understanding of masculinities
longitudinal studies– would help clarify whether these (Connell 1996), diversity within the category of “boys/
perceptions change over time and context. men”—including our male research assistants—should not
Our findings regarding between-sex differences suggest be surprising. Although prior research has supported that
that struggling male readers develop more positive self- many men and boys in early years environments perceive
perceptions as readers when they work with female RAs. pressure to conform to the institutionalized hegemonic
Perhaps female RAs were more consistently expressive in masculinity (Eng 2004; Lewis et al. 1999), clear variation is
their praise of the boys’ progress, and this encouragement evident on the effects different male teachers provoked in
in turn promoted better self-perceptions. The lack of our study’s students. The sex of the reading teachers in our
differences in effects between individual female RAs study did not have consistent effects on all boys and even
supports this conclusion. Research suggests that female boys within the same treatment groups responded differen-
teachers provide more verbal and non-verbal praise to tially, which suggests that the placement of more male
students than do male teachers (Simpson and Erickson teachers as a correctional strategy for boys’ under-achieve-

Table 2 Pre-and Post inter-


vention means and standard Sex of RA Variable Pre-test Post-test
deviations.
M SD M SD

Male General perception 4.22 .98 4.41 .95


Female General perception 3.69 1.28 4.03 1.13
Male Progress 38.81 6.60 39.40 7.14
Female Progress 37.69 6.43 38.90 6.27
Male Observational comparison 20.32 5.99 21.45 5.83
Female Observational comparison 19.24 5.35 20.85 5.15
Male Social feedback 36.86 6.45 36.42 6.84
Female Social feedback 34.76 6.15 35.17 7.00
Male Physiological states 33.47 6.60 33.03 7.14
Female Physiological states 31.16 6.90 32.10 7.20
Male Feminine view of reading 2.04 .40 2.00 .34
Female Feminine view of reading 2.02 .36 2.05 .28
Male Instructional reading level 1.38 1.31 1.62 1.37
Female Instructional reading level 1.82 1.40 2.25 1.48
Sex Roles (2007) 56:651–659 657

ment may be naïve (Skelton 2003). Understanding and we may promote blanket interventions that are ineffective
responding to the needs of different categories of boys, or even harmful while, at the same time, we fail to
however intriguing, requires further research investigation acknowledge the nuances of gender issues in boys’
and validation. schooling (Martino and Blye 2006; Mills et al. 2004; Sokal
2005). Instead, interventions must be mindful of the
Limitations limitations of considering only the embodied man and
instead should be expanded to look at the interactions
Although the design of the current study maximized benefits between boys, girls, male teachers, female teachers, and
to the students and to the research community, it is not home conditions in order to understand the systems in
without limitations. First, it must be remembered that which gender and achievement interact. Attributes and
experimental research with children has implicit restrictions. practices of successful teachers must be explored within the
Although we strove to ensure consistency of treatment dynamics of our gendered classrooms rather than on the
within treatment groups, it must be remembered that the assumption of homogeneity in our male students—and our
children also attended their normal classes with their normal male teachers—that is based on stereotypes rather than on
teachers, each of whom had different levels of training and research. Greater care and critical examination of these
different numbers of years of classroom experience, and the issues is necessary in order to ensure that boys’ needs are
children also interacted with other models within their homes met at school.
and elsewhere. Second, given the transient nature of the
schools within which we worked—some had turnover rates
Acknowledgement We wish to acknowledge those people who
as high a 75% per year—it is likely that the diversity of contributed to this research: the classroom teachers and resource teachers,
children’s prior experiences with male and female teachers the student research assistants at the University of Winnipeg, and the boys
varied greatly. It would have been interesting to collect and their families. We appreciate your dedication to this work.
information about children’s past experiences with both male
and female teachers, and these variables will be considered
in our future research. Although these extraneous and References
possibly confounding variables and interactions may have
had concurrent effects on boys’ gender development and A good man is hard to find (2004, March 12). Fewer than one in three
views of reading, examination of the relative effects of those teachers male. Sydney: Daily Telegraph.
other socializing agents (teachers, parents, friends) was Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
beyond the scope of this project. Third, it must be Alberta Education (1986). Alberta diagnostic reading program.
Edmonton, Alberta: Alberta Education.
remembered that research assistants are not teachers and Allan, J. (1993). Male elementary teachers: Experiences and perspec-
that the treatment does not fully emulate usual classroom tives. In C. Williams (Ed.), Doing “woman’s work”: Men in
conditions. Finally, the term of this investigation was short. It non-traditional occupations (pp. 113–127). CA: Sage, Newbury
is possible that the effects of the variables under examination Park.
Baron, J. (1996). Sexism attitudes toward reading in the adult learner
may change with a longer duration. Despite this limitation, population (Report No. CS 012 410). Union, New Jersey: Kean
the short duration of the study is reflective of the short College of New Jersey. (ERIC Reproduction Document No.
durations some of the participants spent at one school. Thus, ED393092)
these findings are applicable and meaningful to teachers of Basow, S. (1992). Gender stereotypes and roles. Pacific Grove, CA:
Brooks/Cole.
children who move frequently. Furthermore, it is possible BBC News (2005, October 13). Call for male primary teachers.
that, with time, the children’s gains in self-perceptions as Accessed at http://newsvote.bbc.co.uk/mpapps/pagetools/print/
readers promoted by female but not male teachers might news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/education.
produce changes in the children’s reading performance. An Brophy, J. E. (1985). Interactions of male and female students with
male and female teachers. In L. Wilkinson & C. Marrett (Eds.),
investigation of this hypothesis over a longer duration is Gender influences in classroom interactions (pp. 115–142).
currently underway. Cambridge, MA: Academic.
Butler, D., & Christianson, R. (2003). Mixing and matching: The
Final Thoughts effect on student performance of teaching assistants of the same
gender. Political Science and Politics, 36, 781–786.
Carrington, B., & Skelton, C. (2003). Re-thinking role models: Equal
The findings of the current study do not support strategic opportunities in teacher recruitment in England and Wales.
hiring of male teachers as a way to address boys’ under- Journal of Educational Policy, 12, 253–265.
achievement in reading. They do, however, provide Carrington, B., Tymms, P., & Merrell, C. (2005, August). Role
models, school improvement and the ‘gender gap’—Do men
evidence of the heterogeneity of boys and their reading bring out the best in boys and women the best in girls? Paper
needs. By incorrectly attributing causality between the presented at the European Conference for Research on Learning
presence of male teachers and boys’ schooling outcomes, and Instruction conference. Cyprus: University of Nicosia.
658 Sex Roles (2007) 56:651–659

Connell, R. (1996). Teaching the boys: New research on masculinity, Martin, A. (2003). Primary school boys’ identity formation and the male
and gender strategies for schools. Teachers College Record, 98, role model: An exploration of sexual identity and gender identity in
206–236. the UK through attachment theory. Sex Education, 3, 257–271.
Coulter, R., & McNay, M. (1993). Exploring men’s experiences as Martin, A. (2005, November 6). Wanted: Male teachers for young
elementary school teachers. Canadian Journal of Education, 18, students. Chicago Tribune News, p. 1.
398–413. Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model
Council of Ministers of Education, Canada (2001). Measuring up: the of gender typing in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1132.
performance of Canada’s youth in reading, mathematics and Martino, J., & Blye, F. (2006). The tyranny of surveillance: male
science. The Oganization for Economic Co-operation and teachers and the policing of masculinities in a single sex school.
Development Programme for International Student Assessment Gender and Education, 18(1), 17–33.
study. Toronto: Council of Ministers of Education, Canada. Martino, W., & Meyenn, B. (2002). ‘War, guns, and cool, tough
Cummings, N. (1994). Eleventh graders view gender differences in things’: Interrogating single-sex classes as a strategy for engaging
reading and math. Journal of Reading, 38, 196–199. boys in English. Cambridge Journal of Education, 32, 303–324.
Dee, T. (2006). Teachers and the gender gaps in student achievement. McKenna, E. (1997). Gender differences in reading attitudes.
[National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper # Unpublished master’s thesis, Kean College of New Jersey.
11660]. Available at http://www.swarthmore.edu/SocSci/tdee1/ Merrett, F., & Wheldall, K. (1992). Teachers’ use of praise and
Research/w11660revised.pdf. reprimands to boys and girls. Educational Review, 44(1), 73–79.
Delamont, S. (1990). Sex roles and the school. London: Routledge. Millard, E. (1997). Differently literate. London: Palmer Press.
Ehrenberg, R., Goldhaber, D., & Brewer, D. (1995). Do teachers’ race, Mills, M., Martino, W., & Lingard, B. (2004). Attracting, recruiting,
gender and ethnicity matter? Evidence from the National and retaining male teachers: Policy issues in the male teacher
Education Longitudinal Study of 1988. Industrial and Labor debate. British Journal of Sociology of Education, 25, 355–369.
Relations Review, 48, 547–561. Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzalez, E. J., & Kennedy, A. M.
Eng, J. (2004). Male elementary teachers: Where are they? Educa- (2003). Program in International Reading Literacy Study 2001
tional Insights, 8(3). Available: http://www.ccfi.educ.ubc.ca/ International Report: Institute of Educational Sciences’ study of
publication/insights/v08n03/articles/eng.html. reading literacy achievement in primary schools. Chestnut Hill,
Freeman, C. (2004). Trends in educational equity of girls and women: MA: Boston College.
2004 [NCES 2005-016). Washington, DC: US Government Northern Alberta Reading Specialists’ Council (1991).Paired Read-
Printing Office. ing: A reading practice approach. Kelowna, British Columbia:
Froude, L. (2002, March 8). Study defies the ‘boys need men’ credo. Filmwest.
Times Educational Supplement, 4471, 3–9. Ontario Public School Boards’ Association (2000). Fast reports: Are
Gambell, T., & Hunter, D. (2000). Surveying gender differences in schools failing boys? Ontario Provincial School Boards’ Asso-
Canadian school literacy. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 32, ciation Fast Reports, 12(24). Available: http://www.opsba.org/
689–719. pubs/fast/2000/00-09-15.html.
Golombok, S., & Fivush, R. (1994). Gender development. London: Pottorff, D. D., Phelps-Zientarski, D., & Skovera, M. E. (1996).
Cambridge University Press. Gender perceptions of elementary and middle school students
Government of the UK (2000). The standards site: Gender and about literacy at school and at home. Journal of Research and
achievement [on-line]. Available: http://www.standards.dfee.gov. Development in Education, 29, 203–211.
uk/genderandachievement/data_1.2.9.html. Sakhnandan, L., Lee, B., & Kelleher, S. (2000). An investigation into
Guthrie, J., & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and motivation in gender differences in achievement: phase 2—School and class-
reading. In M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, D. Pearson, & R. Barr room strategies. London: National Foundation for Educational
(Eds.), Handbook of reading research: (pp. 403–422). Maywah, Research.
NJ: Erlbaum. Sax, L. (2005). Why gender matters. New York: Doubleday.
Henck, W., & Melnick, S. (1995). The Reader Self-Perception Scale Shapiro, D. (1980). Primary children’s attitudes toward reading in
(RSPS): A new tool for measuring how children feel about male and female teachers’ classrooms: An exploratory study.
themselves as readers. The Reading Teacher, 48, 470–482. Journal of Reading Behavior, 12(3), 155–157.
Hermine, F. (1998). Gender differences in attitude toward reading in a Simpson, A., & Erickson, M. (1983). Teachers’ verbal and nonverbal
sample of the Jewish community (Report no. CS 013 099). communication patterns as a function of teacher race, student
Union, New Jersey: Kean University. (ERIC Reproduction gender, and student race. American Educational Research
Document No. ED417378). Journal, 20, 183–198.
Hetzner, A. (2003, March 17). As fewer men teach, educators bemoan Skelton, C. (2003). Male primary teachers and perceptions of
loss of make models. Milwaukee Journal Sentinel. Available: masculinity. Educational Review, 55, 195–210.
http://nl.newsbank.com/nl-search/we/Archives?p_action=list& Sokal, L. (2005). Answering the question, “Which boys?” A new lens for
p_topdoc=5. viewing boys’ literacy scores. English Quarterly, 37(2), 36–41.
Hoffert, S., & Sandberg, J. F. (2001). How American children spend Sokal, L., Katz, H., Adkins, M., Grills, T., Stewart, C., Priddle, G., et al.
their time. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 63, 295–308. (2005). Factors affecting inner-city boys’ reading: Are male teachers
Katz, H., & Sokal, L. (2003). Masculine literacy: One size does not fit the answer? Canadian Journal of Urban Research, 14, 107–130.
all. Reading Manitoba, 24(1), 4–8. Sokal, L., Monette, J., McBey, S., & Wojcik, C. (2006, April). Factors
Klecker, B. (1997, November). Male elementary school teachers’ affecting boys’ reading gains. Paper presented at the annual
ratings of job satisfaction by years of teaching experience. Paper meetings of the American Educational Research Association, San
presented at the annual meeting of the Mid-south Educational Francisco, CA.
Research Association, Memphis, Tennessee. Stake, J., & Katz, J. (1982). Teacher–pupil relationships in the elementary
Lewis, E., Butcher, J., & Donnan, P. (1999). Men in primary teaching: school classroom: Teacher-gender and pupil-gender differences.
An endangered species? Australian Catholic University. Avail- American Educational Research Journal, 19, 465–471.
able: http://www.aare.edu.au/99pap/but99238.htm. Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences
Martin, C. L. (1993). New Directions for assessing children’s gender of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading
knowledge. Developmental Review, 13,184–204. Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.
Sex Roles (2007) 56:651–659 659

Statistics Canada (2003). Education indicators in Canada (3rd ed.). and pupil performance in Scotland’s schools. Edinburgh: Scottish
Publication Number 81-852-XPE. Ottawa: Statistics Canada, Executive Education Department.
November 25. Topping, K. (1987). Paired Reading: A powerful technique for parent
Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2002). Are parents’ gender schemas use. The Reading Teacher, 40, 608–614.
related to their children’s gender-related cognitions? Develop- Worthy, J., Moorman, M., & Turner, M. (1999). What Johnny likes to
mental Psychology, 38, 615–630. read is hard to find at school. Reading Research Quarterly, 34,
Tinklin, T., Croxford, L., Duckiln, A., & Frame, B. (2001). Gender 12–27.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

View publication stats

Anda mungkin juga menyukai